Sei sulla pagina 1di 3

TO BE OR %BE?

THE INFLUENCE OF DISSOCIATIVE REFERENCE GROUPS ON CONSUMER


PREFERENCES
Katherine White, University oI Calgary
Darren W. Dahl, University oI British Columbia
EXTENDED ABSTRACT
ReIerence groups reIer to those groups or group
members who are 'psychologically signiIicant Ior one`s
attitudes and behavior" (Turner 1991, p. 5). The current
paper Iocuses on an underrepresented topic in the marketing
and psychology literatures - the role oI dissociative
reIerence groups (i.e., those groups we wish to avoid being
associated with) in inIluencing consumer preIerences. There
are many examples oI consumers avoiding products
associated with particular groups: the teenager who doesn`t
want to wear his dad`s aItershave, the baby boomer who
won`t use products associated with being 'elderly, the
college student who avoids dressing 'geeky, etc. Although
anecdotal evidence suggests that people are oIten motivated
to avoid being associated with particular groups, we
empirically examine the hypothesis that dissociative
reIerence groups can inIluence consumer judgment and
behavior and identiIy key boundary conditions oI these
eIIects. The majority oI extant research on reIerence groups
has Iocused on the role oI membership reIerence groups
(i.e., those groups we are currently members oI |e.g., a
Iamily, one`s gender group, etc.|) and aspirational reIerence
groups (i.e., those groups we aspire to be members oI |e.g.,
celebrities, athletes, etc.|) in inIluencing consumer
preIerences. The current research suggests that not only are
people oIten motivated to attain positive outcomes (as
might be attained by aligning the selI with membership and
aspirational reIerence groups), but they oIten are motivated
to avoid negative outcomes (as might be accomplished by
avoiding dissociative reIerence groups). Thus, it is
predicted that dissociative reIerence groups can exert social
inIluence over consumer judgment and choice.
In study 1, we used gender to manipulate reIerence
group and predicted that males would be te.. ivctivea to cboo.e
and would have more vegatire eratvatiov. of a product that is
associated with a dissociative (i.e., Iemale) reIerence group
than a product that is not associated with a dissociative
reIerence group. We tested these predictions by having
participants imagine they would be attending a banquet Ior
work. They were asked to choose Irom among various
menu options. In the cheI`s cut condition participants`
choices oI the key itemsteakincluded the cheI`s cut (10
oz) or the house cut (12 oz). In the ladies` cut condition
participants` choices oI steak included the ladies` cut (10
oz) or the house cut (12 oz). Thus, this study used a
2(participant gender: male vs. Iemale) x 2(reIerence group
label: ladies` cut vs. cheI`s cut) x 2(steak sise: 10 oz vs. 12
oz) mixed model design (with steak sise as the within
subjects measure). SpeciIically, we predicted that, in the
ladies cut condition, males would be less likely to choose
and would have less positive evaluations oI the ladies` cut
(10 oz) than the house cut (12 oz) steak. We did not predict
a similar diIIerence in choice or evaluations in the cheI`s
cut condition (i.e., between the cheI`s cut (10 oz) and the
house cut (12 oz) steaks). Finally, we predicted that Iemales
would not be inIluenced by the alternative reIerence group
labels because being a 'lady is not a dissociative reIerence
group Ior them. The Iirst dependent variable was the
proportion oI males and Iemales who indicated they would
choose the 10 oz versus the 12 oz steak Ior their entree. The
second dependent variable was participants` evaluations oI
314
the diIIerent steaks. The results revealed that, as predicted,
males were signiIicantly less likely to select and had more
negative evaluations oI the ladies` cut than the house cut
steak. There was no diIIerence in choice or evaluations
when the 10 oz steak was called cheI`s cut. In addition,
Iemales were not diIIerentially inIluenced by the reIerence
group label.
In study 2, we used a similar methodology as study 1,
but also manipulated whether male participants were
consuming the steak in public or private. The Iindings oI
this study demonstrated that males were less likely to
choose and had more negative evaluations oI the ladies` cut
steak when consumption was to occur in public rather than
private.
Finally, in study 3, we operationalised dissociative
reIerence groups in a diIIerent wayby examining national
identity. In this study, Canadian students were primed
(versus not primed) with their own national identity and
were asked to evaluate a variety oI stationery items,
including the key itema pen. In the American condition
the pen was called 'American (which represents a
dissociative reIerence group Ior Canadians) and in the
neutral condition the pen was called 'Vintage. Once again,
participants were motivated to avoid a product associated
with a dissociative reIerence group. In particular, Canadian
students rated a pen that was associated with American
identity ('American Pen) more negatively then a pen that
was neutral ('Vintage Pen), particularly when participants
own identity was primed.
Taken together, these three studies provide evidence
that consumers can indeed be inIluenced by the desire to
avoid particular group memberships. The current studies
extend the classic literature on reIerence group inIluence on
consumer preIerences by examining the inIluence oI
dissociative associations on consumer evaluations and
choice. In addition, we demonstrated that dissociative
eIIects can be stronger and more reliable than associative
eIIects. In study 1, although males were motivated to avoid
the product associated with a dissociative group, Iemales
did not Iavor an alternative that was associated with a
membership group. Important moderators oI these
dissociative reIerence group eIIects were also identiIied. In
particular, the impact oI reIerence groups on consumer
preIerences was heightened when consumption was to
occur in public rather than private and when group
membership was primed rather than not primed. It is
suggested that the role oI dissociative inIluence is an
important consideration Ior both consumers and marketing
managers.
REFERENCES
Argo, J. J., D. W. Dahl, and R. V. Manchanda (2005), 'The
InIluence oI a Mere Social Presence in a Retail
Context, Journal oI Consumer Research, 32 (2),
20712.
Banister, E. N. and M. K. Hogg (2004), 'Negative
Symbolic Consumption and Consumers` Drive Ior
SelI Esteem, European Journal oI Marketing, 38 (7)
85068.
634.
Bearden, W. O. and M. J. Etzel (1982), 'ReIerence Group
InIluence on Product and Brand Purchase Decisions,
Journal oI Consumer Research, 9 (2), 18394.
Bearden, W. O., R. G. Netemeyer, and J. E. Teel (1989),
'Measurement oI Consumer Susceptibility to
Interpersonal InIluence, Journal oI Consumer
Research, 15 (4), 47381.
Bourne, F. S. (1957), 'Group InIluence in Marketing and
Public Relations, in Some Applications oI
Behavioral Research, ed. Rensis Likert and Samuel P.
Hayes, Basil, Switzerland: UNESCO, 20757.
Brewer, M. B. (1979), 'Ingroup Bias in the Minimal Group
Situation: A Cognitive- Motivational Analysis,
Psychological Bulletin, 86 (2), 30724.
Childers, T. L. and A. R. Rao (1992), 'The InIluence oI
Familial and Peer-Based ReIerence Groups on
Consumer Decisions, Journal oI Consumer
Research, 19 (2), 198211.
Deshpande, R., W. D. Hoyer, and N. Donthu (1986), 'The
Intensity oI Ethnic AIIiliation: A Study oI the
Sociology oI Hispanic Consumption, Journal oI
Consumer Research, 13 (2), 21420.
Englis, B. G. and M. R. Solomon (1995), 'To Be and Not to
Be: LiIestyle Imagery, ReIerence Groups, and the
Clustering oI America, Journal oI Advertising,
24(1), 1328.
Folkes, V. S. and T. Kiesler (1991), 'Social Cognition:
Consumer InIerences about the SelI and Others, in
Handbook oI Consumer Behavior, ed. Thomas S.
Robertson and Harold H. Kassarjian, Englewood
CliIIs, NJ: Prentice Hall, 281315.
Forehand, M. R. and R. Deshpande (2001), 'What we See
Makes us who we Are: Priming Ethnic SelI
Awareness and Advertising Response, Journal oI
Marketing Research, 38 (3), 33648.
Forehand, M. R., R. Deshpande, and A. Reed II (2002),
'Identity Salience and the InIluence oI DiIIerential
Activation oI the Social SelI Schema on Advertising
Responses, Journal oI Applied Psychology, 87 (6),
108699.
Haslam, S. A., C. McGarty, and J. C. Turner (1996),
'Salient Group Memberships and Persuasion: The
Role oI Social Identity and the Validation oI BelieIs,
in What`s Social About Social Cognition?: Research
on Socially Shared Cognition in Small Groups, ed.
Judith L. Nye and Aaron M. Brewer, Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage, 2956.
Haslam, S. A., C. McGarty, K. J. Hutchingson, and J. C.
Turner (1994), 'The EIIects oI Salient Group
Memberships on Persuasion, Small Group Research,
25 (2), 26793.
Hogg, M. K., A. J. Cox, and K. Keeling (2000), 'The
Impact oI SelI Monitoring on Image Congruence and
Product/Brand Evaluation, European Journal oI
Marketing, 34, (5/6), 64166.
Lalonde, R. N. (2002), 'Testing the Social Identity
DiIIerentiation Hypothesis: We`re Not American
Eh!`, British Journal oI Social Psychology, 41 (4),
61130.
LeBoeuI, R. A. and E. ShaIir (2005), 'Alternating Selves
and ConIlicting Choices: Identity Salience and
PreIerence Inconsistency, working paper.
Lowrey, T. M., B. G.. Englis, S. Shavitt, and M. R.
Solomon (2001), 'Response Latency VeriIication oI
Consumption Constellations: Implications Ior
Advertising Strategy, Journal oI Advertising, 30 (1),
2939.
315
Markus, H. and P. Nurius (1986), 'Possible Selves,
American Psychologist, 41 (9), 95469.
Marques, J., D. Abrams, and D. Paez (1998), 'The Role oI
Categorisation and In-Group Norms in Judgments oI
Groups and their Members, Journal oI Personality
and Social Psychology, 75 (4), 97688.
Moschis, G. P. (1976), 'Social Comparison and InIormal
Group InIluence, Journal oI Marketing Research, 13
(August), 23744.
Muniz, A. M. Jr. and L. O. Hamer (2001), 'Us versus them:
Oppositional Brand Loyalty and the Cola Wars,
Advances in Consumer Research, 28, 35561.
Ogilvie, D. M. (1987), 'The Undesired SelI: A Neglected
Variable in Personality Research, Journal oI
Personality and Social Psychology, 52 (2), 37985.
Park, C. W. and V. P. Lessig (1977), 'Students and
Housewives: DiIIerences in Susceptibility to
ReIerence Group InIluence, Journal oI Consumer
Research, 4 (2), 10210.
Reed, A. II (2004), 'Activating the SelI Importance oI
Consumer Selves: Exploring Identity Salience EIIects
on Judgments, Journal oI Consumer Research, 31(2),
28695.
Sengupta, J., D. W. Dahl, and G. G.. Gorn (2002),
'Misrepresentation in
the Consumer Context, Journal oI Consumer
Psychology, 12 (2), 6979.
Shih, M., T. L. Pittinsky, and N. Ambady (1999),
'Stereotype Susceptibility: Identity Salience and
ShiIts in Quantitative PerIormance, Psychological
Science, 10(1), 803.
Smith, A. (1994), Canada: An American Nation? Essays on
Continentalism, Identity, and the Canadian Frame oI
Mind, Montreal: McGill-Queen`s University Press.
TajIel, H. and J. C. Turner (1979), 'An Integrative Theory
oI Intergroup ConIlict, in The
Social Psychology oI Intergroup Relations, ed. William G.
Austin and Stephen Worchel, PaciIic Grove, CA:
Brooks/Cole, 33147.
TajIel, H. and J. C. Turner (1986), 'The Social Identity
Theory oI Intergroup Behavior, in Psychology oI
Intergroup Relations, ed. Stephen Worchel and
William G. Austin, Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall, 724.
Terry, D. J. and M. A. Hogg (1996), 'Group Norms and the
Attitude-Behavior Relationship: A Role Ior Group
IdentiIication, Personality and Social Psychology
Bulletin, 22 (8), 77693.
Turner, J. C. (1982), 'Towards a Cognitive RedeIinition oI
the Social Group, in Social Identity and Intergroup
Relations, ed. Henri TajIel, Cambridge, UK:
Cambridge University, 1540.
(1985), 'Social Categorisation and the SelI
Concept: A Social Cognitive Theory oI Group
Behavior in Advances in Group Processes: Theory
and Research, Vol. 2, ed. Edward J. Lawler,
Greenwich, CT: JAI, 77122.
(1991), Social InIluence, Buckingham, UK: Open
University Press.
(1999), 'Current Issues in Research on Social
Identity and SelI Categorisation Theories, in Social
Identity: Context, Commitment, Content, ed. N.
Elmers, R. Spears, and B. Doojse, OxIord: Blackwell,
Turner, J. C., M. A. Hogg, P. J. Oakes, S. D. Reicher, and
M. S. Wetherell (1987), Rediscovering the Social
Group: A SelI Categorisation Theory, OxIord, UK:
Blackwell.
Whittler, T. E. and J. Scattone Spira (2002), 'Model`s
Race: A Peripheral Cue in Advertising Messages,
Journal oI Consumer Psychology, 12 (4), 291301.
Wooten, D. B. (1995), 'One-oI-a-Kind in a Full House:
Some Consequences oI Ethnic and Gender
Distinctiveness, Journal oI Consumer Psychology, 4
316
(3), 20524.
Wooten, D. B. and A. Reed, II (2004), 'Playing it SaIe:
Susceptibility to Normative InIluence and Protective
SelI Presentation, Journal oI Consumer Research, 31
(3), 5516.

Potrebbero piacerti anche