Sei sulla pagina 1di 30

1 Self-Concept in Ethnic Minority Youth: Research and Implications John Kearney Elon University The plight of ethnic minority

children and adolescents has been well documented and reported on for a long time. These youth consistently struggle to find success academically and socially. Many reasons for this have been proposed, ranging from underfunded schools, gang violence, lack of proper role models and parental care, among others. Since the late 1960s activists and advocacy groups have pushed American psychologists to include ethnic minorities as an individual focus of research (Pickren, 2004). The goal of this paper is to examine how the self-concept is involved in the development of minority youth, both positively and negatively. Ultimately, we will discuss and explore how proposed interventions and potential future programs can be utilized to use the self-concept to better the lives of these individuals. A variety of aspects pertaining to the self-concept and how current psychological research has explored these ideas will be examined. In the first section we will discuss basic issues concerning the selfconcept. We will examine why these are of particular importance to ethnic minority youth, and why these individuals deserve our attention. Next, we will look at various types of self-concept and how these have been studied within this group. These will include ethnic and academic facets of self-concept. These differ in how the individual views themselves in the terms of these three focuses. We will then turn our attention to various contexts in which self-concept is of importance to the lives of these individuals. These will include threats of stereotype and prejudice, salience of race differences, and family influences. Last, we will examine various proposed treatment programs, and explore new programs that we will construct based on our previous discussions.

2 First we must define what we mean by self. Psychologists view the self as two constructs, the I and the Me. We will define I as the entity which is constantly perceiving and experiencing the world around us. This construct serves the purpose to differentiate ourselves from other things and people in our world, as well as maintain a sense of continuity in our lives; I saw this person, and then I said this to them. If the I is the objective perception of our world, then the Me is ones subjective psychological experience. This construct is made up of self-referent thoughts, such as considering oneself as intelligent or funny. The Me is generally thought to be composed of the two terms called self-concept and self-esteem. The Me will be the focus of our study, with our focus on self-concept, although we will often encounter self-esteem in our discussion. Particularly we will pay attention to the development of the Me through middlechildhood and adolescence. These ages present the times when self-concept is the most malleable, as we gain cognitive abilities and truly begin to define ourselves apart from others. Why is studying this aspect of the self so important? As we will see, our self-concept plays a key role in how we think and feel about ourselves, as well as what attitudes we create about the world and how we behave in that world. In this way, the self-concept could be argued as one of the most important psychological factors to be studied. The group on which this literature review focuses, ethnic minority youth, was chose deliberately. Why focus on children and adolescents? Many aspects of the self are being directly formed during these years, especially during adolescence. Various aspects of life during these ages are formative and carry into adulthood, so it is important to study what is happening during this time period and how it effects formation of the self-concept. This is especially true when considering the possible negative behaviors that can be occurring during adolescence in particular, and what effect this may have on self-concept formation. It has been found that co-occurrence of harmful behaviors

3 is prominent in teenagers. One study found that on average teenagers participate in at least four different negative behaviors, including smoking, alcohol abuse, minor and major delinquencies, sexual activity, aggression, and gambling (Willoughby, et al., 2004). Ethnic minority youth are presented with various environmental stressors that are unique to their demographic, and so it is arguably even more important to study these individuals. Some have suggested that race is not a relevant variable for personality research (Edwards, 1974), but our review of the literature will show that there are in fact differences between ethnic majority and minority youth in this area of psychology, and that minority youth face a unique set of circumstances in their development that influence how they form ideas about self. For example, it has been suggested that an overwhelming amount of those youth who are repeatedly admitted to psychiatric care are African Americans, confirming reports that African Americans are overrepresented in psychiatric settings (U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, 2001). Additionally, the individual and community poverty often experienced by these youth has been connected to beliefs supporting aggression and aggressive behavior (Guerra, Huesmann, Tolan, Van Acker, & Eron, 1995). It is important to note that while the majority of the research we discuss will draw their samples from largely Black populations, other ethnic minorities will be involved as well, including Hispanic and Asian individuals. Why look at various aspects of the self-concept? By doing so we can look at particular parts of peoples self-concept, in order to create a fuller picture of their subjective psychological experiences and examine the potential consequences. William James (1890) divided the self into three subcategories: material self, social self, and spiritual self. We will divide the self into further, more specific categories. These categories will apply to certain areas of ones life that people are more likely to actually think about themselves in, such as ethnic and academic selves.

4 That is to say, people are more likely to regularly consider who they are ethnically or academically then in the three categories James suggests. We include ethnic identity because we are studying ethnic minorities, with the goal of gaining insight into how the development or lack of development, of this aspect of self-concept can be beneficial or detrimental to psychological well-being. We will then look at how ethnic minority youth think of themselves academically, gaining insight into the persistent academic gap between minority youth and their White counterparts. What factors outside of the individual effect how and why self-concept develops and manifests itself? We will first look at stereotypes and prejudice, and how the perceived presence of these can affect self-concept. Next, we will examine what makes aspects of the self more salient in certain circumstances, and the potential implications of this. Last, we will examine how parental and family influences affect the development and maintenance of certain aspects of the self-concept.

Sub-Sets of Self-Concept
People form a self-concept towards many aspects of their lives. Current psychological research has focused on a few of these aspects in order to gain further understanding into the components that form a persons overall self-concept. Our goal is to look at specific aspects of the lives of ethnic minority youth, and examine how and why these areas self-concept develop and manifest, as well as the positive and negative consequences they may have. Ethnic Self-Concept We begin our examination of the literature by focusing on how racial minority youth identify ethnically. Research has found that having various forms of ethnic identity has a host of consequences. While there are different measures used to operationalize ethnic identity, one of

5 the most common is the Multigroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM), and its various subsets (Phinney, 1992). Another popular measure, the Ethnic Identity scale, has been developed to address some of the limitations of the MEIM (Umaa-Taylor, Yazedjian, Bamaca-Gomez, 2004). Our goal in this section is to try to form an answer to the question: Is it good for racial minority youth to have a strong ethnic identity? Without even looking at the literature, one could logically make an argument for ethnic identity being a good or a bad aspect of self. It makes sense that it would be a good aspect, seeing as many people in the US seem to have pride in their heritage, whether it traces back to somewhere in Europe, Asia, South America, or perhaps is simply pride in being American. One would think that having this pride would increase selfesteem. However, when thinking specifically about African American youth, one might come to other conclusions. Maybe it is better not to associate with a particular group if the dominant stereotypes about that group are negative. If a young person identifies solely with their own ethnic group, does that limit them to their own ethnic group socially? Can you be too ethnically identified? And in any case, arent we all striving for a world that doesnt see color, where everyone is equal and no thought is given to race? Why should we encourage pride in being Black, when we certainly wouldnt encourage people to voice pride in being White? Overall, research has provided some evidence that having a strong ethnic identity can be helpful, but there seems to be a point where ethnic identity can be detrimental. We will now look more specifically at the current literature. What are the potential benefits and consequences of having a high or low ethnic identity? One study found that young Black female adolescents were significantly less likely to have risky sexual attitudes if they reported a high ethnic identity. In this case ethnic identity was measured by self-reported affiliation attitudes and belongingness in the individuals personal ethnic group

6 (Belgrave, Van Oss Marin, Chambers, 2000). Another study of young Black women showed that both high private and public racial identity were negatively correlated to depression. In this case the ethnic component of self is aiding in better psychological well-being. The relation between high private racial regard and lower depression was especially strong when ethnicity was a central part of the womens self-concept. Finally, self-esteem played a mediating role between private/public self-regard and depression. That is to say higher levels of regard were related to higher self-esteem, which in turn related to lower depression. (Settles, Navarette, Pagano, Abdou, Sidanius, 2010). Another study supports the idea that the development of a strong ethnic identity is a positive thing. The results of this longitudinal study showed that increases in ethnic identity from early to late elementary school aged children was related to higher self-esteem, better academic performance, and lower parent-reported aggressive behavior. The relationship was modest, but statistically significant (Smith, Levine, Smith, Dumas, Prinz, 2009). These findings suggest that having a strong ethnic identity is a positive thing for ethnic minority youth. Further longitudinal studies would help us understand how increases or a lack of increases in ethnic identity through childhood and adolescence facilitate positive youth outcomes and personal development for these individuals. With proper scientific support, school psychologists working with these children would be better equipped to help this specific group of children. We will discuss this possibility in more detail in the treatment section of this text. Moving on to a different group of research, it appears that too much or a certain extreme manifestation of ethnic identity can be very harmful. William Cross (1995) presented a model for the shifting ethnic identity. This model is helpful for our discussion in that several studies refer to the principles laid out in Crosss model, which consists of five stages: Preencounter, Encounter, Immersion, Internalization, and Internalization-commitment. The Pre-

7 encounter stage is when a person hasnt begun the identity development process, and is marked by pro-White/anti-Black attitudes. These individuals do not value race as an essential part of their identity, instead concerning themselves with the values of the majority, or what they consider they societal norm. During the Encounter stage involves the beginning of internalizing ones ethnicity as part of their identity. In the Immersion stage one begins to delve into their culture and begin to attempt to show their culture in their attitudes and behaviors. This could include changes in dress, friends, and general interests. The Internalization stage involves a decrease in anti-White sentiment and security in identifying oneself with the Black group. The final stage in Crosss model, Internalization-commitment, involves long-term commitment to both furthering the ideals and needs of the Black group as well as for society as a whole. These individuals are comfortable with both Black and White peers, and Cross argues this stage is only attainable by experiencing all the previous stages, and is likely to occur later in adulthood. Thinking about these stages it seems reasonable that older children and adolescents may be anywhere on the first four of these stages, and research has been done to see how these stages manifest and what consequences they have for the individual. While they do not use the specific terms presented in Crosss model, Thompson, Anderson, and Bakeman (2000) found that the type of racial identity attitudes an individual has relates to the amount of acculturative stress they experience. Acculturative stress is related to pressures of conforming ones own culture with the dominant culture. Those who have rigid attitudes towards race, everything considered White viewed positively, everything considered Black viewed negatively, or vice-versa, are more likely to experience acculturative stress than those who take pride in their culture yet have generally more flexible racial attitudes. In this case these individuals could be placed in the pre-encounter and immersion stages of Crosss model.

8 This study also found a positive correlation between racial socialization and racial identity attitudes, particularly that individuals who received high levels of racial socialization were more likely to have pro-Black and anti-White attitudes. It is important to note that this study used college aged participants, with an average age of 20. It can be argued that Crosss identity development stages are more likely to occur during adolescence, and that individuals who remain at pre-encounter or immersion stages into adulthood are less likely to change their views and progress toward another stage. While this study suggests that racial socialization has a negative effect on ethnic identity development, more research must be done in this area before any broad conclusions can be made about socializations effect on ethnic identity development. These studies show the importance of noting the terminology used when discussing racial identity. Cokley (2005) made the differentiation between racialized and nonracialized ethnic identity. In this study it was found that current measurements of Afrocentric cultural values and positive ethnic identity were negatively related to measurements of racialized ethnic identity. Individuals with these results were said to have nonracialized ethnic identity, which is characterized by validation of Afrocentric values, strong ethnic identity, rejection of internalized racialism, and lack of anti-White attitudes. Racialized ethnic identity is characterized by antiWhite attitudes, beliefs about Afrocentrism, a belief in the natural ability of Blacks, a strong ethnic identity, and negative endorsement of multiculturalist inclusive attitudes. These distinctions are not made in all literature on this topic, and it is important to consider all aspects that may contribute or be a part of ethnic self-concept, and their potential consequences. This study also discussed problems with the term Afrocentric. One set of results showed that endorsing Afrocentric values is negatively related to anti-White attitudes, while another set showed a positive relationship between these two variables. The use of the word Afrocentric is

9 difficult because it is never clearly defined in the literature, and it is fair to assume that participants who rate statements using this word have different definitions for it. This issue is one that is present throughout the literature on this topic, and one that must be resolved before this area of study can begin to make further progress. One study of male African American college students found that those who showed preencounter or immersion racial identity (Cokleys racialized ethnic identity) showed more signs of psychological distress and had lower self-esteem than those individuals who showed positive internalization of racial identity. These latter individuals averaged higher self-esteem and better self-reinforcement against racism. However, this study also found that those who identified more with the overall Black group reported lower self-esteem (Pierre and Mahalik, 2005). This finding suggests two possible interpretations. Either Black men with low self-esteem identify more with Black individuals than White, or Black men feel worse about themselves when identifying themselves more with their Black peers than White peers. Lets take a moment to reflect on these studies and their implications for our discussion. We initially wanted to answer the question of whether having an ethnic identity is a good thing. There is some literature that suggests that it is. However, further study into what constitutes ethnic identity show that the picture is more complicated for ethnic identity than a simple yes its good or no its bad. There seem to be differing levels of ethnic identity development, which may not be fully explained by Crosss model. The last study we discussed shows this to be evident, since it appears that those who would fall into the internalization stage have lower selfesteem. Extensive further research is necessary before psychologists can create a specific accurate model of how ethnic identity develops. Let us remember that our overall goal is to improve the lives of these individuals, and a model such as this would be very helpful in

10 achieving this goal. Ones first thought in investigating the literature was that there would be a point of being ethnically identified that would be too much. Looking at it from Crosss model, as many have, it seems that there is a point where the development of ethnic identity is actually not enough, which is observed in the pre-encounter and immersion stages. How can we encourage individuals to progress to a healthy level of ethnic identification, and what do these individuals looks like? Pierre and Mahaliks study above shows that group association may be an important factor in developing a healthy ethnic identity. This important aspect of ethnic identity is not always explicitly discussed in the literature, yet is seems to have real consequences. RotheramBorus (1990) examined this in a large, ethnically diverse group of high school students, asking them to categorize their ethnic reference-group as mainstream, bicultural, or ethnically identified. Not surprisingly White students were significantly more likely to identify themselves as mainstream, while ethnic minority students, including Hispanics, Blacks, and AsianAmericans mostly identified themselves as bicultural (40-47%) or ethnically identified (26%). The prevalence of the bicultural label suggests that these students identify both with their own ethnic culture and with the White mainstream culture, thus would likely fall into fourth or fifth stages of Crosss model. The most interesting results came from those who rated themselves as ethnically identified. While these individuals did not show any differences in self-esteem, grade point average, or social competence, they did hold more separatist attitudes, engage in less cross-ethnic contact outside of school, and report more cross-ethnic conflict. So where do we draw the line? The individuals in Rotheram-Boruss study who reported as ethnically identified still had positive academic and social outcomes, yet also reported worrying attitudes and behaviors. This article also continues to highlight the need for further

11 work to understand how and why ethnic identity develops. Are these negative findings truly the case of lack of development, as Cross would argue? Or are these individuals taking ethnicity too far, to the point where the negative consequences start to pile up? One might view this problem as simply one of semantics, but this is not the case. Answering these questions has important implications for potential treatment and prevention strategies in addressing ethnicity problems for these youth. Do we need to encourage these individuals into the next stage of development, or do we need to work to tone ethnic identity down to a more adaptive level? As we will see later, oftentimes ethnicity is a far more salient problem for these youth, so it is of the utmost importance to further our knowledge in this area in order to improve the lives of these children and adolescents. Academic Self-Concept Receiving successful or substandard education is one of the biggest obstacles that many ethnic minority children will face in their young lives. These children endure unique problems in their lives that can have detrimental effects on their academics, even if not directly related to school (Schwartz & Gorman, 2003; Fite and Pardini, 2009; McLoyd, 1998). While the trend is narrowing, there has been a consistent academic achievement gap between Black and White children in this country. This gap includes high school graduation rates, scores in reading and math, and amount of students who go to college. So where is the problem? Many point to underfunded public school systems in urban environments, which are often largely constituted by ethnic minorities. This issue certainly plays a large role, as research has shown that schools in affluent areas spend more money per student, while budget cuts often hit schools in low-income areas even harder than those in affluent areas. These low-income area schools often consist mainly of ethnic minority students. So should we simply allocate more money to these schools,

12 and then the problem is solved? As with any important issue, there is no one easy solution. In this section we will look at psychological issues that affect the academic life of ethnic minority students, and how these childrens attitudes towards academics can play an important role in their success. Academic disidentification In attempting to determine where the problem is for these disadvantaged children, we must look at what is actually happening in these schools and in the minds of the students. One possible stereotype that negatively affects Black students academic outcomes is academic disidentification, which is the lack of association between ones identity and academics in general. More generally, this would be the belief that being successful academically is a White thing, and thus Black students do not put as much effort to academics. This belief might act as a self-fulfilling prophecy, as Black students do not believe they can or will do well academically, thus put in less effort in school. There resulting poor performance confirms and continues the stereotype. This effect has also been called academic disidentification (Cokley, 2002). It is fair to assume that children are born with a positive identification to academics; they want to do well. If this is the case then it suggests that something happens during the education process which alters the way they view themselves and their academic possibilities or prowess. Once these stereotypes are learned, one theory argues that students disidentify with academics in order to avoid confirmed the stereotype that Black students do not perform well academically (Steele, 1992). This phenomenon could also serve to protect self-esteem. Research has been done to show this phenomenon at work, and try to understand what its causes are. Research suggests that this stereotype is real and could begin at an early age. A study of ethnically diverse 5th grade students aimed to see if there is a difference in how self-concept

13 could play a role in childrens verbal learning styles. The authors found that Black children with high levels of self-concept were more likely to recall negative adjectives, while White students with high self-concept recalled positive words more frequently (August & Felker, 1977). This finding suggests that not only can peoples ethnicity affect how they feels about themselves, as we have discussed, but this can also affect learning styles. In interpreting these results it is important to mention the phenomenon of the self-referent effect, which states that individuals remember items better if they can refer those items to themselves. Although it is speculation, it is possible that these students view themselves negatively based on their ethnicity and/or their perceived academic possibilities, leading to greater memorization and recall of negative words. Similar research suggests Black girls are less likely to attribute academic success to personal attributes, while being more likely to blame oneself for bad results, while this effect was far less predominant in White children (Brown, Furr, Fulkerson, Ware, Voight, 1984). These sorts of findings are not limited to younger children, and have been linked directly to an individuals academic self-concept, or how they incorporate academics into their identity. It has been found that while there is a positive correlation between academic self-concept and academic performance in Black and White older students, the magnitude and strength of this effect is significantly lower for Black males (Cokley, 2002). It has even been suggested that even bringing up ones ethnicity can have negative academic effects. Probing about an individuals ethnic identification resulted in lower reported school belonging than when only school belonging was reported alone. This occurrence has been found in middle school, high school, and college students who are ethnic minorities (Mallet et al., 2011).

14 Our society should be worried by these findings, for it seems clear that ethnic minority children are being negatively affected by education and/or societal messages, and putting them at a disadvantage academically. Where are these attitudes and beliefs coming from? It is a fair assumption that the majority of these children are not actively being told that they will not and cannot do well in school, so what is happening? Are teachers treating these kids in such a way that portrays they are destined to fail? Is social media, such as music and television, instilling the belief that academic success is impossible? Are parents not properly fostering their children and encouraging them to succeed academically? Or maybe these children simply look at their world and see no reason to believe in themselves. It is a complicated issue, but one of extreme importance. The goal of eliminating both the stereotype of Black children in academics as well as the achievement gap must start with fully understanding not only the surrounding factors, but those who are being affected. It is easy to throw blame at underfunding, non-motivated teachers, or negative environments, but we must remember these are not the most important things. What is most important is thinking about those who suffer because of those outside factors, and to truly gain insight into the problem we must look into the minds and experiences of these children. Seeking further understanding of the origins to these negative self-beliefs should be a priority for future research. Only then can we create plans to fix these problems.

Context
In this section we will examine the various contexts that effect self-concept and youth outcomes. What factors affect how and why self-concept manifests, and what are the consequences? We will begin with two quick looks at aspects pertaining particularly to ethnicity. First we will examine self-concept in the context of racial prejudice and discrimination, followed

15 by salience of race. Last we will take a deeper look into the role of parents and family and how they influence the development of self-concept. Racial Prejudice & Discrimination Prejudice can be defined as a false and generally negative attitude towards members of a particular group. Prejudice often includes negative feelings, stereotype beliefs, and discriminatory behavior (Plous, 2003). Discrimination for our purposes can be defined as negative or abusive behavior towards someone of a different race. From our previous discussion of ethnic identity we concluded that there is evidence that a healthy ethnic identity can be beneficial. However, it has also been found that children with a stronger ethnic identity recognize ethnic bias and prejudice earlier and more frequently than those with weaker ethnic identities. Particularly, Black, Hispanic, and White children in 6th and 8th grade were found to generally have the same amount of awareness of racial bias. However, 4th grade minority students were conscious of racial bias significantly more than White children at this age (Brown, Alabi, Huynh, Masten, 2011). It could be argued that children of this age are too young to truly understand prejudice, but this would mean that they could be making all kinds of false or negative beliefs and attitudes about prejudice, which could be even more harmful. While one might not be particularly worried about racism in elementary school, perhaps it is something that we should be paying more attention to, especially for ethnic minority youth. Just as individuals differ in how they deal with stress, it is fair to assume that people would vary in how they deal with racist discrimination and the accompanying stress. This is an important area of research because if psychologists can distinguish what personal differences cause someone to deal with discrimination stress better than another, it could help in developing treatments and interventions for those who are more susceptible to this stressor. Particularly in the late 1990s there was a push

16 to study what factors might play a mediating role between perceived racism and mental health. This is a difficult thing to study however, as researchers cannot experimentally manipulate racist discrimination; that is, they cannot apply discrimination ethically. The literature on this topic is inconclusive. It would be logical to think that there would be a positive correlation between perceived racism and mental health issues, and some studies have found this result (Sue & Sue, 1990; Landrine & Klonoff, 1996; Szalacha et al., 2003). However, a study of young Black adults had found no relationship between perceived racism and mental health. Additionally individuals with high self-esteem reported poorer mental health when they had high levels of perceived racist discrimination, while those with low self-esteem did not (Fischer & Shaw, 1999). Such odd findings could be due to methodological flaws or flaws in the measures used, but either way the conclusion is that more research needs to be done in this area to gain insight into how prejudice and discrimination may be affecting ethnic minority youth. Salience of Ethnicity The distinctiveness postulate states that what aspects of identity people characterize themselves as depend on how those characteristics differ from an individuals environment. Simply, one is conscious of certain personal characteristics only insofar that they are different. This idea has been applied to the context in which ethnic identity manifests in ethnic minority youth, and it has been suggested that ethnic consciousness is significantly more salient for Black and Hispanic children than White children (McGuire, McGuire, Child, Fugioka, 1978). Interestingly this effect was found across ages, including 1st, 3rd, 7th, and 11th grade students. Again we have evidence that race may be an important factor in the lives of younger children. This has implications for everyone involved in the childs life. Another example of the distinctiveness postulate is one that we are all familiar with: When there is only one person of a

17 certain race in a group that shares ethnicity with each other. Whether its one Black individual in a group of White individuals or vice-versa, this is a scenario that is often portrayed in a comedic sense in movies and on television. The racially unique individual here is in racial solo status, and theory states that this increases racial identity in Black individuals. Sekaquaptewa, Walden, and Thompson (2007) found evidence to support this theory, and stated that this occurs because race is more salient in these situations. Individuals who were put in solo status conditions experienced increased racial identification, race-reflection, and performance apprehension. However, the degree to which individuals experience these was positively correlated to how important ethnicity was to their identity. Reflecting back on our discussion of ethnic identity, we see here how ethnic identity can be a potential detriment at times, here causing performance anxiety. In terms of salience, it is a relevant topic to explore because if we can determine what scenarios and circumstances causes salience, and if the result is positive or negative characteristics, we can work to encourage or avoid these situations and contexts. Parental and Family Influence There exists a large body of literature about the beneficial effects parents and families can have on their children. However, much of this research has focused on White families, with relatively little attention given to racial minorities in terms of the relationship between parenting and various youth outcomes. We will discuss existing research, particularly how parents affect the development of ethnic identity in their children. The development of self involves individuals, their relationships, and relations with family of origin. One measure of overall familial relationships has been positively correlated with child resilience to environmental stressors and academic achievement, while negatively associated with aggressive behaviors (Skowron, 2005). Positive parent-child relations have been related to higher self-esteem, lower

18 rates of anti-social behavior (Arbona & Power, 2003), increased communication between parents and children (Fulkerson et al., 2010), and less negative impact of racism on psychological functioning (Bynum, Burton, Best, 2007). Just like the sitcom, research has shown that Family Matters. We all know that family, parents in particular, are important to a childs development. In this section we want to answer a simple question: Why? What sort of behaviors do parents of ethnic minority youth engage in that are either beneficial or detrimental to their childs development, particularly the development of self-related characteristics? How exactly does the parent-child relationship relate to various aspects of childrens and adolescents self-concept? We have previously concluded that a healthy level of positive ethnic identity can have several beneficial effects. For children and adolescents, parents can play a large role in the development of ethnic identity. How much the parents encourages incorporating ethnicity into a childs sense of self could have obvious implication for the development of ethnic identity. In fact, Ramirez Garcia, Manongdo, and Cruz-Santiago (2010) related this sort of encouragement to lower adolescent substance abuse, while Smalls (2010) found that the fostering of ethnic pride by parents was related to higher levels of academic engagement. Additionally, Rodriguez, UmaaTaylor, Smith, and Johnson (2009) wrote a short review of seven studies which found that ethnic socialization in ethnic minority families contributes to better psychological and academic youth outcomes. As is often the case for studies of youth and adolescents, a good longitudinal study can be incredibly insightful. One such study involved measuring the development of ethnic identity over a 4-year period in a racially diverse sample. While they found that most individuals experienced an overall increase in ethnic identity over time, these effects were significantly stronger when youth also reported more positive family relations and better perceptions of their parents (Huang, 2011). Most would agree that good parent-child relationships have many

19 beneficial effects of children and their development. This study suggests that one of these effects pertains to ethnic identity, which we have concluded can be beneficial to overall well-being. Could the development of ethnic identity play a mediating role in these results? Future research is needed to begin to answer that question. While it is fair to conclude that familial relations play an important role in the development of many positive consequences for ethnic minority youth, it is important to remember the flip-side of this conclusion. That is, negative family relations could have the opposite effects for all of the studies we have discussed. While it is important to take these studies and think about how psychologists can help parents do the right thing with their children, it is equally important to consider how research can aid us in helping parents avoid doing the wrong thing. We will not discuss this area in depth in this review, but research has shown that demanding or stressful family relations can have several negative effects on these youth, including depression, lower self-esteem, and lower optimism (Taylor, Budescu, McGill, 2011). Prevention and Treatment Learning about the workings of the human mind is interesting, but the goal of psychological study must be to take these findings and create ways to apply them in order to better peoples lives. While the findings we have discussed may be interesting, their real value comes from how they can be used to create prevention and treatment plans. This includes preventing and treating the negative symptoms, but also fostering the positive consequences. We will now look at several strategies and intervention plans studied by the psychological community. Our review will focus mainly on programs implemented in schools. Perhaps the most basic strategy is afterschool programs. It is generally believed that afterschool programs can have positive effects on children, whether they are clubs, social groups,

20 or sports. These programs can teach children responsibility, teamwork, increase their social skills, or just remove some time that young students pay otherwise spend getting into trouble (Posner and Vandell, 1999). However, low-funding can severely limit the choices children and parents have for afterschool programs. These programs are especially important to schools in urban, low-income areas, where children have fewer positive outlets and more opportunities for getting into trouble. To think that programs that have the potential to be effective at preventing negative outcomes while fostering positive ones are being made unavailable to those students who need it most is unacceptable. Further research by the psychological community could be a powerful tool in the designing, implementing, and maintaining of afterschool programs. Another program which has found success is Head Start, which is the largest publicly funded early childhood education and care program in the United States. The programs main goal is to ready young children for academic success. Zhai, Brooks-Gunn, and Waldfogel (2011) found that children who participated in this program had higher cognitive ability, increased social competence, and fewer attention problems. The program did not have any effect on behavioral problems. The Head Start program is good, but obviously not perfect. School psychologists should look into this program in order to learn for themselves, but also to contribute into making the program even more effective. The unfortunate truth is that many families will not have access to programs like these, aimed at preventing problems and nurturing positive growth. Luckily, more creative plans have been designed as well. Friedrich-Cofer, Huston-Stein, McBride Kipnis, Susman, and Clewett (1979) studied the effects of pro-social television programs at one Head Start center, and found that these programs increased positive social interactions with peers and adults, as well as imaginative play when supplemented by related materials. With further research and

21 modification, these types of television programs could be made available to parents to use in their homes. In the time since this study childrens television has changed, with some programs now explicitly stating how they are aiding your childs development. Continued psychological research could lead to entire television channels dedicated to these types of programs, which could be immensely helpful for parents. One plan that has found broad success is peer-assisted learning (PAL). PAL is a broad term for interventions that include peer tutoring and cooperative learning activities. A review of several studies by Ginsburg-Block, Rohrbeck, and Fantuzzo (2006) found that this type of intervention was successful in creating positive academic and social outcomes in elementary aged students, and these effects were more pronounced for urban, low-income, ethnic minority students. This could be an effective, cost efficient strategy for urban schools to foster the academic and social success of their young students. Many programs that are designed to treat behavioral or emotional problems involve some sort of interpersonal education or skill development. Some believe that children would have fewer of these problems if schools addressed interpersonal skill development explicitly. School is certainly a place to acquire social skills, but children and adolescents may benefit from classes dedicated solely to interpersonal skill development. Gunlicks-Stoessel, Mufson, Jekal, and Turner (2010) found that interpersonal psychotherapy was useful in treating ethnic minority adolescents suffering from depression, especially when these individuals report high levels of conflict at home and/or social dysfunction with their peers. Instead of waiting for the depression, an interpersonal skills class may have a preventative effect, or at least prevent some of the social dysfunction that contributes to the depression. Another study done by Elias and Haynes (2008) found a relationship between academic outcomes and social-emotional competency. In this case those students with higher

22 initial social-emotional competency and those who showed greater improvements in competency had better academic outcomes throughout the school year. This suggests that school psychologists and even teachers should be aware and pay attention to young students social and emotional experiences in order to foster academic success, perhaps with specially designed classes. These kinds of classes exist in some private schools, and psychological research on the effectiveness of such classes could be very helpful to schools, even if it is not the most cost effective solution.

By reading through the research it becomes clear that one important aspect of interventions is timing. In general, earlier is better. The review done by Ginsburg-Block and her colleagues (2006) found that PAL interventions were significantly more effective when administered at an early age. Bradshaw, Zmuda, Kellam, and Ialongo (2009) illustrated the benefits of early prevention programs in their study, where they found that classroom and family based intervention programs led to higher academic achievement and involvement in elementary school students when administered early in the childrens academic careers. The responsibility for taking advantage of early prevention strategies is completely on the parents. Kilgore, Snyder, and Lentz (2000) found that coercive parent discipline and poor parental monitoring of their children at age 4 was a reliable predictor of behavior problems at age 6. Parents must realize and take responsibility for the role they play in affecting their children and making their lives more or less difficult. Psychological research on how parents could play key roles in prevention is vital, but getting that information to the parents is even more important, if more difficult. Last, the easiest prevention strategy that could be implemented is simply having support available. Diemer, Kauffman, Koenig, Trahan, and Hsieh (2006) found that perceived support

23 from peers, family, and community is directly related to ethnic minority adolescents ability to challenge racism and social injustice. The ability to rise up and succeed in a low-income urban life is difficult enough, and simply having a support system can go great lengths in helping these individuals believe in themselves and their ability to succeed despite the odds. In conclusion, while we have gained insight into the area of self in ethnic minority youth, there is still significant work to be done. As society changes, so does the mindset of the individuals within that society, as well as the world that is shaping our youth. Research must continue, and must not stop until the inequalities and unique struggles that these children face no longer exist. By looking at how these individuals form their concept of self, we gain insight not only into their minds, but into the world around them. Psychologists are trained to know how to reveal these insights, and so it is their duty not only to do so, but then to take that knowledge and apply it in order to change the lives of these youth for the better. The existing literature is a good start.

24 References Arbona, C., & Power, T. G. (2003). Parental attachment, self-esteem, and antisocial behaviors among African American, European American, and Mexican American adolescents. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 50, 40-51. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.50.1.40 August, G. J., & Felker, D. W. (1977). Role of affective meaningfulness and self-concept in the verbal learning styles of White and Black children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 69, 253-260. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.69.3.253 Belgrave, F. Z., Van Oss Marin, B., & Chambers, D. B. (2000). Culture, contextual, and intrapersonal predictors of risky sexual attitudes among urban African American girls in early adolescence. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 6, 309-322. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.6.3.30 Bradshaw, C. P., Zmuda, J. H., Kellam, S. G., & Ialongo, N. S. (2009). Longitudinal impact of two universal preventive interventions in first grade on educational outcomes in high school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 926-937. doi:10.1037/a0016586 Brown, D., Furr, S., Fulkerson, K., Ware, W. B., & Voight, N. L. (1984). Locus of control, sex role orientation, and self-concept in Black and White third- and sixth-grade male and female leaders in a rural community. Developmental Psychology, 20, 717-721. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.20.4.717 Brown, C., Alabi, B. O., Huynh, V. W., & Masten, C. L. (2011). Ethnicity and gender in late childhood and early adolescence: Group identity and awareness of bias. Developmental Psychology, 47, 463-471. doi:10.1037/a0021819

25 Bynum, M., Burton, E., & Best, C. (2007). Racism experiences and psychological functioning in African American college freshmen: Is racial socialization a buffer? Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 64-71. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.13.1.64 Cokley, K. O. (2002). Ethnicity, gender and academic self-concept: A preliminary examination of academic disidentification and implications for psychologists. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 8, 378-388. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.8.4.379 Cokley, K. O. (2005). Racial(ized) identity, ethnic identity, and Afrocentric values: Conceptual and methodological challenges in understanding African American identity. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 517-526. doi:10.1037/0022-0167.52.4.517 Cross, W. E., Jr. (1995). The psychology of nigrescence: Revising the Cross model. In J. G.Ponterotto, J. M.Casas, L. A.Suzuki, & C. M.Alexander (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural counseling (pp. 93122). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Edwards, D. W. (1974). Blacks versus whites: When is race a relevant variable?. Journal of Personality And Social Psychology, 29, 39-49. doi:10.1037/h0035720 Elias, M. J., & Haynes, N. M. (2008). Social competence, social support, and academic achievement in minority, low-income, urban elementary school children. School Psychology Quarterly, 23, 474-495. doi:10.1037/1045-3830.23.4.474 Fischer, A. R., & Shaw, C. M. (1999). African Americans' mental health and perceptions of racist discrimination: The moderating effects of racial socialization experiences and selfesteem. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 46, 395-407. doi:10.1037/00220167.46.3.395

26 Fite, P. J., Wynn, P., & Pardini, D. A. (2009). Explaining discrepancies in arrest rates between Black and White male juveniles. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 77, 916927. doi:10.1037/a0016626 Friedrich-Cofer, L. K., Huston-Stein, A., McBride Kipnis, D., Susman, E. J., & Clewett, A. S. (1979). Environmental enhancement of prosocial television content: Effects on interpersonal behavior, imaginative play, and self-regulation in a natural setting. Developmental Psychology, 15, 637-646. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.15.6.637 Fulkerson, J. A., Pasch, K. E., Stigler, M. H., Farbakhsh, K., Perry, C. L., & Komro, K. A. (2010). Longitudinal associations between family dinner and adolescent perceptions of parentchild communication among racially diverse urban youth. Journal of Family Psychology, 24, 261-270. doi:10.1037/a0019311 Ginsburg-Block, M. D., Rohrbeck, C. A., & Fantuzzo, J. W. (2006). A meta-analytic review of social, self-concept, and behavioral outcomes of peer-assisted learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 732-749. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.4.732 Guerra, N. G., Huesmann, L., Tolan, P. H., Van Acker, R., & Eron, L. D. (1995). Stressful events and individual beliefs as correlates of economic disadvantage and aggression among urban children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 63, 518-528. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.63.4.518 Gunlicks-Stoessel, M., Mufson, L., Jekal, A., & Turner, J. (2010). The impact of perceived interpersonal functioning on treatment for adolescent depression: IPT-A versus treatment as usual in school-based health clinics. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 260-267. doi:10.1037/a0018935

27 Huang, C. Y., & Stormshak, E. A. (2011). A longitudinal examination of early adolescence ethnic identity trajectories. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17, 261270. doi:10.1037/a0023882 James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1) New York: Holt Mallett, R. K., Mello, Z. R., Wagner, D. E., Worrell, F., Burrow, R., & Andretta, J. R. (2011). Do I belong? It depends on when you ask. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17, 432-436. doi:10.1037/a0025455 McGuire, W. J., McGuire, C. V., Child, P., & Fujioka, T. (1978). Salience of ethnicity in the spontaneous self-concept as a function of one's ethnic distinctiveness in the social environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 511-520. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.5.511 McLoyd, V. C. (1998). Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development. American Psychologist, 53, 185-204. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.185 Phinney, J. S. (1992). The multigroup ethnic identity measure: A new scale for use with diverse groups. Journal of Adolescent Research, 7, 156176. Pickren, W. E. (2004). Between the cup of principle and the lip of practice: Ethnic minorities and American psychology, 1966-1980. History Of Psychology, 7, 45-64. doi:10.1037/10934510.7.1.45 Plous, S. (2003). The psychology of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination: An overview. In S. Plous (Ed.), Understanding Prejudice and Discrimination (pp. 3-48). New York: McGraw-Hill.

28 Posner, J. K., & Vandell, D. (1999). After-school activities and the development of low-income urban children: A longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 35, 868-879. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.868 Ramrez Garca, J. I., Manongdo, J. A., & Cruz-Santiago, M. (2010). The family as mediator of the impact of parentyouth acculturation/enculturation and inner-city stressors on Mexican American youth substance use. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 404-412. doi:10.1037/a0019725 Rodriguez, J., Umaa-Taylor, A., Smith, E., & Johnson, D. J. (2009). Cultural processes in parenting and youth outcomes: Examining a model of racial-ethnic socialization and identity in diverse populations. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 15, 106-111. doi:10.1037/a0015510 Rotheram-Borus, M. J. (1990). Adolescents' reference-group choices, self-esteem, and adjustment. Journal of Personality And Social Psychology, 59, 1075-1081. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.5.1075 Ryder, A. G., Alden, L. E., & Paulhus, D. L. (2000). Is acculturation unidimensional or bidimensional? A head-to-head comparison in the prediction of personality, self-identity, and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 49-65. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.1.49 Schwartz, D., & Gorman, A. (2003). Community violence exposure and children's academic functioning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 163-173. doi:10.1037/00220663.95.1.163 Sekaquaptewa, D., Waldman, A., & Thompson, M. (2007). Solo status and self-construal: Being distinctive influences racial self-construal and performance apprehension in African

29 American women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 13, 321-327. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.13.4.321 Settles, I. H., Navarrete, C., Pagano, S. J., Abdou, C. M., & Sidanius, J. (2010). Racial identity and depression among African American women. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 248-255. doi:10.1037/a0016442 Skowron, E. A. (2005). Parent differentiation of self and child competence in low-income urban families. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 337-346. doi:10.1037/00220167.52.3.337 Smalls, C. (2010). Effects of mothers' racial socialization and relationship quality on African American youth's school engagement: A profile approach. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 476-484. doi:10.1037/a0020653 Smith, C., Levine, D. W., Smith, E., Dumas, J., & Prinz, R. J. (2009). A developmental perspective of the relationship of racialethnic identity to self-construct, achievement, and behavior in African American children. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 15, 145-157. doi:10.1037/a0015538 Spaeth, M., Weichold, K., Silbereisen, R. K., & Wiesner, M. (2010). Examining the differential effectiveness of a life skills program (IPSY) on alcohol use trajectories in early adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 78, 334-348. doi:10.1037/a0019550 Steele, C. (1992). Race and the schooling of African-Americans. Atlantic Monthly, 269, 6878. Taylor, R. D., Budescu, M., & McGill, R. (2011). Demanding kin relations and depressive symptoms among low-income African American women: Mediating effects of self-

30 esteem and optimism. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 17, 303-308. doi:10.1037/a0024189 Thompson, C., Anderson, L. P., & Bakeman, R. A. (2000). Effects of racial socialization and racial identity on acculturative stress in African American college students. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 6, 196-210. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.6.2.196 Umaa-Taylor, A. J., Yazedjian, A., & Bamaca-Gomez, M. (2004). Developing the ethnic identity scale using Eriksonian and social identity perspectives. Identity, 4, 938. Willoughby, T., Chalmers, H., & Busseri, M. A. (2004). Where is the syndrome? Examining cooccurrence among multiple problem behaviors in adolescence. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 1022-1037. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.72.6.1022

Potrebbero piacerti anche