Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

NANCY Food Science

LARSON-POWERS & Technology, University

and ROSE MARIE of California,

PANGBORN Davis, CA 95616

PAIRED COMPARISON AND TIME-INTENSITY MEASUREMENTS O F THE SENSORY PROPERTIES O F BEVERAGES AND GELATINS CONTAINING SUCROSE O R SYNTHETIC SWEETENERS

ABSTRACT
paired comparison methods, concentrations of 0.75% and 0.86% calcium cyclamate and of 0.17% and 0.19% aspartame were equivalent in sweetness to 10% sucrose in distilled water at 3 and 22C, respectively. Inherent bitterness of the compounds prevented precise assessment of relative sweetnessfor sodium saccharin in distilled water, and for the saccharin and cyclamate in flavored drinks. By application of linear regression to the paired comparison data, 0.07% aspartame was calculated as equal in sweetness to 10% sucrose in lemon, strawberry and orange drinks. Because the underlying bitterness of saccharin interfered with assessmentof its sweetness,a time-intensity technique was applied. Using a chart recorder to monitor time, time-intensity (T-l) measurements were made of the intensity and the duration of sweetness, bitterness, sourness and flavor in distilled water, and the same characteristics, plus flavor in three flavored drinks, and two flavored gelatins, sweetened with sucrose,cyclamate, or saccharin. T-l curves for the sensory properties of aspartame closely resembled those for sucrose in all media. Cyclamate and saccharin imparted a marked, persistent bitterness to all carriers. In gelatin, samples containing 18% sucrose were firmer initially and took longer to manipulate to a liquid in the mouth than did gelatins containing 0.105% aspartame, 0.55% cyclamate, or 0.05% saccharin.
By

INTRODUCTION
THE DEVELOPMENT and testing of synthetic sweeteners involves a lengthly sequential procedure. First, it must be established that the new ingredient is safe for human consumption, that it is stable under normal conditions of handling and storage, and that it is not detrimental to the physical or functional attributes of the products in which it is to be used. Of prime importance are the sensory properties - appearance, aroma, texture, taste and aftertaste. Traditionally, a single sensory parameter is measured - sweetness equivalence to sucrose, usually by paired comparison methods, In the interest of expediency and efficiency, most investigations are conducted using model systems (compounds dispersed in distilled water), with the hope that similar relationships would hold in more complex food and beverage systems. We felt that measurements of perceived intensity, sensory quality, and stimulus duration in several systems would be useful supplements to the conventional, unidimensional relative sweetness measure ment. The present investigation was undertaken to compare the relative taste properties of aspartame, sodium saccharin and calcium cyclamate with those of sucrose in distilled water, in lemon, orange and strawberry drinks, and in orange- and strawberry-flavored gelatins, using both paired comparison and a new approach to time-intensity (T-I), wherein time is continuously monitored by a strip chart recorder (Larson, 1975).

phenylalanine methyl ester, Searle Biochemics, lot 5270-7). Drinks and gelatins were prepared from powdered basessupplied by General Foods Corp., Tarrytown, NY (Table 1). Sample preparation The water solutions were prepared (w/v) using doubledistilled water and sweetener, covered, and allowed to stand at 22C for 16 hr prior to tests conducted at room temperature, and at 3C for 16 hr prior to tests conducted with solutions at refrigerator temperature. The chilled samples were poured into the test beakers 1 hr before the test session, covered, and replaced in the refrigerator to allow the temperature to equilibrate to 3C. The three flavored drinks were prepared from a stock solution of the powdered base and doubledistilled water. The stock was added to the solid sweeteners, agitated to dissolve them, diluted to volume at 22C, and stored at 3C for approximately 12 hr. Stock solutions of the two gelatins were prepared by mixing the base with 3/4 of the total volume of boiling, distilled water and stirring for 2 min by hand. After mixing for 10 min with a magnetic stirrer, the gelatin was cooled to room temperature and brought to volume. Aliquots of the sweetener, which had been dissolved in distilled water (w/v) to a specific volume, dependent upon the desired concentration, were combined with the gelatin solution, brought to volume, and poured into enamel pans to a height of 2 cm. Any surface foam was removed, and the pans were covered with a plastic wrap and stored at 3C +_1C for 18 hr. All gelatins were tested within 24 hr of preparation, at which time they were served as 2 cm cubes at 7 * 1C. Sensory procedures The paired comparison, constant stimulus, forcedchoice method (Amerine et al., 1965) was used to compare seven concentrations of each of the three synthetic sweetenerswith solutions of 10% sucrose,at 3 and at 22C. In the three flavored drinks, five concentrations of each synthetic sweetener were compared with samples containing 10% sucrose. Judges selected the sweeter sample within each pair, with a total of three replications obtained per judge per paired concentration. The percentage of the responsesselecting the 10% sucrosesample as sweeter

Table

l-Composition

of powdered

bases used for drinks Drinks (w/w%) Orange 48.71 42.35 6.61 0.61 1.55 0.17

andgelatins

Strawberry Citric acid Monocalcium phosphate Flavoring FD&C color Vitamin C Vitamin A Clouding agent 52.36 43.64 0.74 1.49 1.80 0.17

Lemon 69 98 22.07 290 0.01 0.81 0.09 4.23

MATERIALS&METHODS
Ingredients The sweeteners used were: sucrose (99% pure, C & H Sugar Co.), sodium saccharin (Sherwin Williams, lot x5768), calcium cyclohexylsulfamate (Searle Biochemics, lot x5735), and aspartame (L-aspartyl-LStrawberry Gelatin Adipic/fumaric -Sodium citrate Flavoring Color 70.9 20.3 6.7 1.7 0.4

Gelatins

(w/w%) Orange 70.7 20.1 6.6 2.4 0.2

acid

Present address:

1015 Campbell,

Presser,

WA 99350

Volume 43 (1978kJOURNAL

OF FOOD SCIENCE-

41

Fig. l-Time intensity vates strip-chart and until extinction point.

method. Judge places sample in mouth, actirecords perceived intensity on moving chart

ASPARTAME W Fig. 2-Sweetness of aspartame relative to 10% sucrose water at 3 and 22C. showing both linear and parabolic to the data. in distilled lines fitted

was plotted against the concentration of the synthetic sweetener, and a regression line fitted to the data. Equisweetness was defined as that concentration of synthetic sweetener at which 50% of the responses indicated the synthetic was sweeter than sucrose, as read from the linear regression line. For the water solutions, only, parabolic lines were fitted to the data by computer, also. Gelatins were not evaluated by paired comparison due to difficulties encountered with the liquids. Judges consisted of nine females and four males, students and employees, selected on the basis of interest in participation and consistency of response to perceived sweetnessand bitterness in paired presentation of test solutions. Within all test series, samples of approximately 35 ml of solution were served in randomized order in 50-ml beakers coded randomly with numbers from l-999. Distilled water was provided for oral rinsing and judges were instructed not to swallow samples. Test sessions were conducted three to four times per week, between 10:00 am and noon, in individually partitioned booths maintained at 21 + 2C. For the T-I measurements, time was monitored by a strip chart recorder (Heathkit Multi-speed Servo Chart Recorder, Model lR-18M). The paper was set to advance at a rate of 0.05 in/set when liquids were being tested, and at 0.1 in/set for testing gelatin. Perceived intensity was recorded manually with a felt-tip pen on the moving chart, using a lOOdivision, unstructured scale labeled none to extreme on the stationary paper-cutter bar (Fig. 1). The bar served both as a guideline for marking intensity, and to cover the previous response, forcing judges to concentrate on the immediate intensity. Samples were presented in 50-ml beakers identified with small, gummed labels marked with the sequence and with the single sensation being measured, e.g., l-sweet, or 2-bitter. The gummed label was transferred from the beaker onto the chart paper to identify the curve, and to focus the judge attention on the single sensation under consideration. s The judge placed the entire sample into the mouth, retaining the IO ml of liquid for 20 set, or the 2-cm cube of gelatin for 10 set before expectoration, while recording the intensity on the moving chart. For sweetnessmeasurements in distilled water (Fig. 2, 3, and 4), judges first recorded initial intensity on the stationary chart. Then, a second sample of the same stimulus was placed in the mouth while simultaneously initiating the advancement of the chart paper. Beginning from the initial intensity point, the judge marked perceived intensity on the moving chart, expectorating at the designated times, and continuing to mark the moving chart until the sensation was no longer perceived, i.e., to the extinction point (Fig. 1). In addition to recording taste and flavor intensities, hardness and rate of breakdown of the gelatins were assessedby the T-I method. First the judge placed the gelatin cube between the molar teeth, bit down and expectorated, recording the initial hardness or firmness on the stationary chart. Next, the second sample of the same gelatin was placed between the molars, bitten and manipulated between the tongue and palate to a complete liquid, recording the perceived rate of breakdown on the moving chart. For both liquids and gelatin, a total of eight samples were tested per session, with distilled water provided for oral rinsing. For evaluation of the water solutions and the three drinks by the T-l method, nine judges, with experience on the previous paired tests, participated. For evaluation of gelatins, a new group of subjects was screened and trained, with ten being selected on the basis of reproducibility of judgment, two of whom had participated in the previous taste studies. To tabulate the T-I data, points on the graphs were joined to form a smooth curve, with mean intensities calculated for the entire panel at 47%~ intervals for the liquids, and at 2-set intervals for the gelatins. These mean intensities were then replotted against time for each sensation for each sweetener. The total area under the curve was estimated using third-order exponential smoothing, based on an input of points from a digitizing table to a calculator (Hewlett-Packard, 9810). Analysis of variance was applied to the initial intensity values read from the individual charts, and to the values representing areasunder the curves.

RESULTS & DISCUSSION


-0 0.4 0.5 0.8 0.6 0.7 CYCLAMATE ("lo) 0.9 1.0 Paired comparison method Sensory responses to the sweetness of aspartame, cyclamate and saccharin compared to 10% sucrose in distilled water are plotted in Figures 2, 3, and 4, respectively. For aspartame, it was necessary to test a different concentration series at each solution temperature to bracket the sweetness match (Fig. 2).

Fig. 3-Sweetness of calcium cyclamate distilled water at 3 and 22 C, showing fines fitted to the data.

relative to 70% sucrose in both linear and parabolic

42 -JOURNAL

OF FOOD SCIENCE-

Volume 43 (1978)

SENSORY PROPERTIES
Table 2-Concentration range, equisweet sucrose, paired comparison method concentrations and correlation coefficients for solutions

OF BEVERAGESAND
of synthetic sweeteners

GELATINS..
compared to 10%

Aspartame Cone range tested % Equiv. sweetnessa % Cont. range tested %

Cyclamate Equiv. sweetnessa %

Saccharin++ Cone range tested 36

Medium of dispersion Distilled 22C 3C water

0.075-0.290 0.065-0.290 0.05-0.15 0.05-0.270 0.05-0.10

0.190 0.170 0.069 0.068 0.066

0.903X 0.955 0.955 0.986 0.963**

0.15-l 0.04-I

.o .o

0.86 0.75 ++b ++ -I+

0.818 0.9371 -

0.03-0.50 0.05-0.35 0.05-0.25 0.02-0.30 0.05-0.15

Drinks Strawberry Orange Lemon

0.25-0.85 0.20-I .25 0.45-l .15

*Significant at p < 0.05 l * Significant at p < 0.01 * * * Significant at p < 0.001 a Values determined from linear regression line. b++Equivalent sweetness concentration point was

not

determinable.

The aspartame data fit more closely to a parabolic (r = 0.965 at 22C and r = 0.990 at 3 p < 0.01) than a linear regresC, sion (r = 0.903 and 0.955, respectively, p < 0.01 and 0.001). The cyclamate data (Fig. 3) also fit more closely to a parabolic (r = 0.962, p < 0.01) than a linear regression (r = 0.818, p < 0.05). Using the conventional measurement of reading values from a linear regression line, 10% sucrose was determined to be equivalent to 0.190% aspartame and 0.86% cyclamate in solutions tested at 22 C, and to 0.170% aspartame and 0.75% cyclamate at 3C (Table 2). Lower aspartame values have been reported in the literature, e.g., a 10% sucrose match of 0.075% aspartame (Beck, 1974) and 0.050% aspartame (Guadagni et al., 1974). The latter investigators did not specify solution temperatures, but we presume samples were served at room temperature. Although sucrose and cyclamate have been tested by several investigators (Vincent et al., 1955; Schutz and Pilgrim, 1957; Kamen, 1959; Hellaur, 1966; Stone and Oliver, 1969; Yamaguchi et al., 1970a, b none compared cyclamate directly to 10% sucrose. The best estimate of the sweetness of calcium cyclamate from these literature values can be expressed as a relative sweetness of 30-80 compared to sucrose being equal to 1. The linear regression lines in Figure 3 are highly questionable, particularly for solutions tested at 22 C, because of the obvious plateauing of responses at the 50% mark for concentrations ranging from 0.6-1.0% cyclamate. An inspection of the individual data showed a time-order bias for the cyclamatesucrose comparison, only. The second sample within a paired set was significantly selected as sweeter 226 times in 364 trials, or 62% of the time (p < O.OOl), compared to the chance selection of 50%. This overselection of the second sample, despite random presentation, strongly suggests mutual synergism of the two compounds, agreeing with Kamen (1959) and Yamaguchi et al. (1970b) who reported sweetness synergism of intermediate concentrations of sucrose and cyclamate. It should also be pointed out that several investigators have observed a bitter off-taste in cyclamate solutions, starting with concentrations of 1.3% (Helgren et al., 1955). Vincent et al. (1955) found a linear relationship between the percentage of their 76 judges reporting off-taste and the log of the concentration of sodium cyclamate. Twenty percent detected an off-taste at 0.42%, and 50% at 0.66% cyclamate. The off-taste was perceived at higher levels of relative sweetness for the sodium salt than for the calcium salt of cyclamate. Using 150 female testers, Tiwald (197 1) reported significant differences between equisweet solutions of 0.17% sodium cyclamate and

n 13 026 039
l

4
0.05 0.10 0.15 0.20 0.25 0.30 0.35 0.40 0.45 0.50

(T,

SODIUM

SACCHARIN

(%I

Fig. 4-Sweetness of sodium saccharin relative to 10% sucrose in distilled water at 22C, demonstrating the impossibility of fitting a line to the data to determine the relative sweetness.

7.5% sucrose, which he attributed to the bitter aftertaste of the former. Figure 4 dramatically illustrates the impossibility of using the paired comparison technique to establish the concentration of saccharin equivalent to 10% sucrose. A 20increment concentration series, ranging from 0.03-0.05% saccharin resulted in a tracking-type response pattern which never approximated the 50% response criteria. A second concentration series of three levels of saccharin, 0.10, 0.20 and 0.30%, fared no better, despite the increased number of replications. A reappraisal of the recent literature proved futile, as a 10% sucrose standard was not tested by paired comparison. The closest concentration values were 9.12% sucrose Z 0.03% saccharin (Schutz and Pilgrim, 1957) determined by a single stimulus, 9-point intensity scale, and 9.18% sucrose g 0.08% saccharin (Yamaguchi et al., 1970a) established by paired comparison. Neither investigator reported difficulty in testing the relative sweetness of this compound. In his review of a large number of early articles on relative sweetness, Nieman (1958) reported a summary value of 30,000 for saccharin relative to 100 for 10% sucrose. As shown by Amerine et al. (1965), TZufel and Klemm (1925) reported a value of 18,900 for saccharin relative to 100 for 10% sucrose. Vincent et al. (1955) calculated that sodium saccharin was 240-350 times sweeter than sucrose at a level of Volume 43 (1978)-JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE43

Dislllled Water At 22C Distilled Water 22'C 60 t\ \


SWEET

Sucrose 10%

4n2

......Asporlomr 0.075% 2 I -.- Arportomc 0.13% 21 ---- Asporlomc 0 177X

20 .\

BITTER

20
TINE (SEC) TIME (SEC)

TIMEfsecl Fig. &Time-intensity curves for sourness, for 10% sucrose and three concentrations water at 22DC.

40 60 TIME(secl

80

Fig. 5-Time-intensity of four sweeteners

curves in distilled

for sourness, bitterness, water at 22OC.

and sweetness

bitterness, and sweetness of aspartame in distilled

0.007-0.0 125%, but at higher concentrations the sweetness dropped rapidly due to increasing bitterness of the saccharin. This was substantiated by Moskowitz (1970), using magnitude estimation. Although we did not quantitate the bitterness intensity in the paired comparisons, we concur with Moskowitz (1970) that the bitterness of the saccharin concentrations increases at a faster rate than does the sweetness. These changes in sweetness-bitterness functions with increasing concentration -preclude the establishment by paired comparison of a concentration at which 50% of the responses would indicate the saccharin solution was sweeter than 10% sucrose. No difficulty was encountered in calculating sweetness equivalence by paired comparison for aspartame in the three flavored drinks. The aspartame values equivalent to 10% sucrose were so similar for the three drinks, that a single concentration of 0.07% aspartame was used for subsequent testing of all drinks.

Our experience with paired comparison testing of cyclamate, and particularly with saccharin, emphasized the need for a sensitive, indirect estimate of the multiple sensory properties of these compounds, hence the development of the T-I technique. For the T-I method the final concentrations selected for each sweetener are specified on the individual graphs (Fig. 5-9). Time-Intensity method Average T-I curves for the four sweeteners dispersed in distilled water and tested at 22 are shown in Figure 5. SimiC lar curves were obtained with solutions tested at 3 except C, that sourness of the three synthetic sweeteners was significantly lower (p < 0.001) at the lower temperature, as were sweetness and sourness for all compounds. Bitterness intensity was the same, possibly because solutions may have been closer to mouth temperature when they made contact with the bitter

LEMONDRINK 3C AT STRAWBERRY DRINK AT 3C

64

FLAVOR

SOUR
10% -sucrose _--. AlpOrfome 0.07% -- -Cyclomote 0.65% .- saccharin 0.10%

50 -

BITTER ...... .... 1

2 5. t

.-'"'.<,,~~~ BITTER '.

SWEET t

a()-

TIME (set) Fig. 7a-Time-intensity sweetness in strawberry curves for flavor, drink at 3C. bitterness, sourness and Fig. 7b-Time-intensity sweetness in lemon

TIME hxl curves for at 3 C. flavor, bitterness, sourness and

drink

44 -JOURNAL

OF FOOD SCIENCE-Volume

43 (1978)

SENSORY PROPERTIES

OF BEVERAGES

AND GELATINS..

TIME Irecl

TIME (seconds) Fig. 8b-Time-intensity curves for orange gelatin with four sweeteners: (n = 30 for hardness and flavor; n = 24 for bitterness; n = 27 for sweetness). Fig. g--Average T-l curves for strawberry flavor in gelatin for each of ten judges, demonstrating variation in initial intensity, maximum intensity and duration In = 3).

Fig. 8a-Time-intensity curves for strawberry gelatin with four sweeteners: In = 30 for hardness and flavor; n = 24 for bitterness; n = 27 for sweetness).

receptors at the base of the tongue. At both temperatures, the greater sourness and bitterness of saccharin and of cyclamate, compared to sucrose was quite evident. Also, it is very obvious that 0.10% saccharin was considerably less sweet initially, with a shorter sweetness duration, compared to the other sweeteners. The aspartame samples were initially sweeter than the sucrose solutions, but at the lower temperature, the sweetness duration of both solutions was identical. The persistence of all taste sensations in the synthetic sweeteners tested at 22 is C striking, particularly the bitterness and sourness of saccharin. Table 3 presents values in cm* for the total areas under the curves, with the corresponding LSD values. Since the concentration of 0.177% aspartame, calculated from the paired comparison experiment as equivalent to 10% sucrose proved to be sweeter, two lower concentrations were compared to sucrose using the T-I method (Fig. 6). The concentration of 0.075% was taken from a literature value (Beck, 1974), while the level of 0.13% was derived arithmetically from the data in Figure 5, i.e., 0.177% aspartame /64 initial intensity = 0.13% aspartame/49 initial intensity (the starting point for 10% sucrose). The latter aspartame concentration gave sweetness, sourness, and bitterness curves remarkably similar to those for 10% sucrose when tested experimentally (Fig. 6). This demonstrates the use of T-I data to interpolate and match sweetness intensities for compounds with no interfering secondary tastes. T-I curves for the orange and strawberry drinks were almost identical, hence only the latter data are shown (Fig. 7a), contrasted with curves from the lemon drink (Fig. 7b). The main difference in the curves for strawberry (pH 3.0) and lemon (pH 2.75 was the greater perceived sourness of the latter. The data suggest that sourness supressed perceived sweetness intensity, which agrees with previous findings (Pangborn, 1963). Flavor intensity curves were similar in both drinks for sucrose, aspartame, and cyclamate, with significantly less initial flavor for drinks sweetened with saccharin. In both drinks, the saccharin samples were very bitter, cyclamate samples were moderately bitter, and the sucrose and aspartame samples had a slight, transient bitterness. Bitterness curves for samples containing saccharin and for those containing cyclamate suggest a slight increase in intensity up to the time of expectoration (20 set), then a pronounced drop. This slight delay in perception of maximum bitterness, only, might be related to the delay in the stimuli reaching the circumvallate

Table d--Mean areas under the curve (cm ) for solutions, drinks gelatins sweetened with sucrose or three synthetic sweeteners, methoda SUC Distilled Sour Bitter water 3C 22Oc 3C 22 c 3C 22C Strawberry Orange Lemon Strawberry Orange Lemon Strawberry Orange Lemon Strawberry Orange Lemon Strawberry Orange Strawberry Orange Strawberry Orange Strawberry Orange 5.5 6.5 7.3 6.0 37.1 38.1 43.4 36.9 49.6 7.1 13.6 16.9 17.7 20.0 64.9 31.2 30.2 23.4 53.4 56.8 0.8 0.7 48.6 49.3 68.9 57.0 10.7 14.4 15.7 20.3 61.9 76.2 53.5 47.5 62.0 10.4 7.8 20.2 18.3 20.4 59.4 44.6 43.3 39.6 25.5 32.4 6.0 8.8 52.7 56.1 77.9 82.1 14.6 22.7 43.1 41.5 44.0 53.7 61.7 48.8 71.7 40.9 36.4 41.3 17.7 26.1 56.0 56.3 51.3 48.6 26.0 25.9 20.5 la.4 63.2 73.1 82.0 70.0 17.0 28.1 57.3 65.6 22.6 26.7 35.3 37.1 37.5 51.7 53.3 65.3 30.9 33.2 75.1 16.2 20.4 11.0 29.7 33.5 30.3 35.4 47.1 59.9 55.3 48.8 APM WC SAC

end T-l

LSD

Sweet
Drinks Flavor

7.1 7.1 13.3 13.3 13.1 13.1 8.2 9.1 8.3 9.5 10.7 11 .l 8.1 6.7 10.1 10.0 10.9 10.1 6.3 7.6 7.5 11.7 11.5 13.9 17.5 16.4

Bitter

Sour

Sweet

Gelatins Hardness Bitter Flavor Sweet

=SUC, APM, CYC and SAC are, respectively. sucrose, aspartame, cyclamate, and saccharin. For concentration, consult Fig. 6-8. b least significant difference among sweetener means at p < 0.05.

papillae at the base of the tongue, the receptors primarily responsive to bitter compounds. Marked differences can be seen in sweetness intensity and in duration, with aspartame and cyclamate being the greatest Volume 43 /1978)-JOURNAL OF FOOD SCIENCE45

and saccharin being the least. Quantification of areas under the curve in cm* and the corresponding LSD values (Table 3) emphasize the magnitudes of greater bitterness and sourness, and of lesser sweetness and flavor of the saccharin samples. The data indicate that lower concentrations of aspartame and of cyclamate should be used to match the initial sweetness intensity of 10% sucrose. Applying the ratio described previously, one could predict that the initial sweetness intensity of sucrose in these drinks would be equivalent to 0.06% aspartame, and to 0.45-0.47% cyclamate. Due to the unavailability of the same judges, we were unable to verify these predicted concentration matches. Despite the almost identical curves obtained for T-I measurements of orange and of strawberry gelatin;both figures are included to illustrate the reproducibility of the panel using this method (Figs. 8a and 8b). As would be anticipated, gelatins sweetened with sucrose were firmer (p < 0.001) and broke down in the mouth more slowly than did the synthetically-sweetened samples. Maximum firmness was perceived at 0 time, whereas maximum flavor and sweetness were perceived after 10 set of oral manipulation, and maximum bitterness after approximately 15 sec. Again, greater bitterness and less flavor and sweetness were assigned to samples containing saccharin. A good matching of curves was obtained for flavor and for sweetness of samples with 0.55% cyclamate, 0.105% aspartame, and 18% sucrose, also demonstrated in Table 3. An increase in the amount of saccharin would definitely have increased bitterness, but might not have increased sweetness, due to the interference of bitterness. Figure 9 is included to illustrate the typical range of individual judge responses to gelatin samples, using the T-I method. Although all ten judges were requested to begin marking the moving chart at the same time (2 set after placing the gelatin cube in the mouth), they differed markedly in their perception of initial flavor intensity, maximum flavor intensity, and extinction point. Most likely these differences are caused by variations in sensory sensitivity to the stimulus and to variations in the usage of the scalar points. Despite the large between-judge variation, observed in all sensory tests, there was excellent within-judge reproducibility of response. The latter was demonstrated by the general lack of significant F-ratios for the judge X replication interactions in the analysis of variance of these T-I data. We feel that the T-I technique described herein for liquids and gelatins has considerable potential for testing sensory attributes of a variety of foods and beverages, after trained judges master the mechanics of marking intensity on a moving chart. The use of a recorder to monitor time is an improvement over the T-I methods described by Nielson (1957) and by Jellinek (1964) in which judges watch the second hand of a clock, by Todd et al. (197 1) where judges used a stop-watch to time duration of bitterness of beer, and by McNulty and Moskowitz (1974) where 1-set auditory signals indicated the time intervals.

In conclusion, it should be emphasized that paired comparison methods could be inappropriate with stimuli which exhibit masking or synergistic effects, or with stimuli which differ in more than one parameter. REFERENCES
Amerine, M.A.. Pangborn, R.M. and Roessler, E.B. 1965. Principles of Sensory Evaluation of Food. Academic Press, New York. Beck, C.I. 1974. Sweetness, character and applications of aspartic acid based sweeteners. In Symposium: Sweeteners, Ed. Inglett, G.E., Avi Publishing Co., Inc.. Westport, CT. Beck, K.M. 1975. Practical considerations for synthetic sweeteners. Food Product Dev. g(4): 47. Guadagni, D.G., Maier, V.P. and Turnbaugh, J.G. 1974. Effect of subthreshold concentrations of limonin. naringin and sweeteners on bitterness perception. J. Sci. Food & Agric. 25(11): 1349. Helgren, F.J., Lynch, M.J. and Kirchmeyer. F.J. 1955. A taste panel study of the saccharinoff-taste. J. Amer. Pharm. Assoc. 44(6): HeBarn, D. 1966. Sweetness of cyclamate. sodium cyclohexyl sulfamate, Z. Biol. 115: 313. JeIIinek, G. 1964. Introduction to and critical review of modern methods of sensory analysis (odour. taste and flavour evaluation) with special emuhasis on descriutive analvais (Flavour Profile method). J..Nutr. & Diet. l(3): 219. Kanien, J. 1959. Interaction of sucrose and calcium cyclamate on perceived intensity of sweetness. Food Research 24(3): 279. Larson, N.L. 1975. Sensory Properties of Flavored Beverages and Gelatins Containing Sucrose or Synthetic Sweeteners. MS. thesis, University of California. Davis. McNuItv, P.B. and Moskowitz. H.R. 1974. Time-intensity curves for flavored oil-in-water emulsions. J. Food Sci. 39(l): 55. Moskowitz. H.R. 1970. Sweetness and mtensitv of artificial sweeteners Perception &-Psychophysics 8(l): 40. Nielson, A.J. 1958. Time-intensity studies. In Flavor Research and Food Acceptance. Reinhold Publ. Corp., New York. Nieman. C. 1958. Relative Susskraft van Zuckerarten. Zuckeru. Siisswarewirthsch. ll(9): 420. 465. 505, 632. 610, 752, 791, 840,
878,933,974.1051,1088. 353.

Pangborn. R.M. 1963. Relative taste intensities of selected sugars and organic acids J. Food Sci. 28(6): 726. Schulz, H.G. and Pilgrim. F.J. 1957. Sweetness of various compounds. and its measurement. Food Research 22(2): 206. Stone, H. and Oliver, S. 1969. Measurement of the relative sweetness of selected sweeteners and sweetener mixtures. J. Food Sci. 35(2): 215. Taufel, K. and Klemm. B. 1925. Untersuchungen iiber natilrliche und kiinstliche Siistoffe. 1. Studien iiber den Siissungsgrad van Saccharin und DuIcin. Z. Untersuch. Nahr. Genussm. 50: 264. Tiwam. H. 1971. eber den geschmack van natrium&lamai m vergleich zu sucker. Zucker 24(8): 227. Todd, P.H., Bensinger, M.G., Johnson, P.A. and Worden, L.R. 1971. Organoleptic evaluation of iso-alpha-acids and other hop bittering substances. Proc. Amer. Sot. Brewing Chem. 9. Vincent, H.C.. Lynch, M.J., Pohley, F.M., Helgren, F.J. and Kirchmeyer, F.J. .1955. A taste panel study of cyclamate-saccharin mixtu;; and of its components. Amer. Pharm. Assoc. J., Sci. Ed. 44(6): Yama&chi. S.. Yoshikawa. T., Ikeda. S. and Ninomiya. T. 1970a. Studies on the taste of some sweet substances. 1. Measurement of ielative sweetness. Agric. & Bio. Chem. 34(2): 181. Yamaguchi, S., Yoshikawa. T.. Ikeda, S. and Ninomiya, T. 1970b. Studies on the taste of some sweet substances. 2. Interrelationships among them. Agric. & Bio. Chem. 34(2): 187. M S received 512177; revised 814177; accepted 8112177.

Based on a thesis submitted by the senior author to the Univ. of California in partial fulfillment of the MS. degree. The technical assistance of Mrs. Cathy Tassan is gratefully acknowledged. The research was supported, in part, by Searle Biochemics. Arlington Heights, IBinois.

46 -JOURNAL

OF FOOD SCIENCE-Volume

43 (19781

Potrebbero piacerti anche