Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

Psychology of Addictive Behaviors In the public domain

2008, Vol. 22, No. 1, 47–57 DOI: 10.1037/0893-164X.22.1.47

Comorbidity of Substance Dependence and Depression: Role of Life Stress


and Self-Efficacy in Sustaining Abstinence

Susan R. Tate Johnny Wu


Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System and University University of California, San Diego
of California, San Diego

John R. McQuaid Kevin Cummins


Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System and University University of California, San Diego
of California, San Diego

Chris Shriver and Marketa Krenek Sandra A. Brown


Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System and University
of California, San Diego
The authors examined life stress and self-efficacy as predictors of time to relapse for 113 adults with
comorbid major depressive disorder and alcohol and/or substance dependence in a randomized clinical
trial comparing 2 psychotherapy interventions (integrated cognitive– behavioral therapy and 12-step
facilitation therapy). Life stress, self-efficacy, and substance use were assessed at treatment entry, 12
weeks (mid-treatment), and 24 weeks (end of treatment). Time to relapse was defined as the number of
days from treatment initiation until first alcohol and/or drug use. Half of the sample relapsed within the
study period of 24 weeks. There was no significant difference between treatment groups. Individuals
experiencing life stressors were more likely to relapse early than those not experiencing life stressors.
Lower self-efficacy also predicted earlier relapse. Chronic stress levels and self-efficacy were stable
across time for most individuals. In contrast, acute stress events occurred at differing times, and survival
analyses provided evidence of heightened relapse risk in the month following acute stressors. The
interaction of self-efficacy and life stress was not significant. The results highlight the significance of life
stress and self-efficacy as predictors of early relapse.

Keywords: alcohol dependence, substance dependence, life stress, self-efficacy, survival analysis

Given the high prevalence of comorbid substance use and de- stand factors associated with treatment outcomes for individuals
pressive disorders (Regier et al., 1990), it is important to under- with these concomitant disorders. Individuals diagnosed with both
depression and alcohol dependence have exhibited poorer drinking
outcomes than alcohol-dependent individuals without depression
Susan R. Tate and John R. McQuaid, Veterans Affairs San Diego (Greenfield et al., 1998). The presence of comorbid depression has
Healthcare System, and Department of Psychiatry, University of Califor- also been shown to predict earlier relapse to alcohol and drug use
nia, San Diego; Johnny Wu, Department of Psychology, University of among adolescents with alcohol or substance dependence (Corne-
California, San Diego; Kevin Cummins, Department of Psychology and lius et al., 2004). Higher levels of depressive symptoms have also
Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego; Chris predicted earlier treatment attrition, greater urges to use sub-
Shriver and Marketa Krenek, Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare stances, and alcohol relapse (R. A. Brown et al., 1998). These
System; Sandra A. Brown, Veterans Affairs San Diego Healthcare System,
findings highlight challenges associated with addiction treatment
and Department of Psychology and Department of Psychiatry, University
of California, San Diego. in the context of depressive disorders. Findings are mixed, how-
Johnny Wu is now at the Department of Psychology, University of ever, with a number of studies not detecting different addiction
Washington; Marketa Krenek is now at the Department of Psychology outcomes for substance-dependent adults with and without depres-
Syracuse University. sion (Carroll, Nich, & Rounsaville, 1995; Charney, Paraherakis,
This research was supported by a Veterans Affairs Medical Research Negrete, & Gill, 1998; O’Sullivan et al., 1988; Sellman & Joyce,
Merit Review Grant awarded to Sandra A. Brown and a Veterans Affairs 1996; Tate, Brown, Unrod, & Ramo, 2004).
Medical Research Merit Review Entry Program Grant to Susan R. Tate. A significant complication for dual-diagnosis treatment is the
This study was completed as an honors thesis in the psychology honors
potential for alcohol or drug use to undermine psychotherapeutic
program by Johnny Wu at University of California, San Diego, supervised
and pharmacological intervention efforts for both disorders. Alco-
by Susan R. Tate and Sandra A. Brown.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Sandra hol and drug use can cause or exacerbate depression symptoms,
A. Brown, Department of Psychology (0109), University of California, San either through direct effects (e.g., alcohol, sedatives) or during
Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, La Jolla, CA 92093-0109. E-mail: withdrawal states (e.g., cocaine, amphetamines). Substance use
sanbrown@ucsd.edu can also compromise the effectiveness of pharmacotherapy inter-

47
48 TATE ET AL.

ventions by causing serious side effects, potentiating effects of higher levels of depression symptoms to lower self-efficacy
some psychotropic medications (thereby increasing risk of over- (Haukkala, Uutela, Vartiainen, McAlister, & Knekt, 2000; Kanfer
dose), and decreasing adherence to medication regimens (Catz, & Zeiss, 1983). In a dual-diagnosis sample, individuals with
Heckman, Kochman, & DiMarco, 2001; National Institute on greater psychiatric distress were also more tempted to drink
Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2005). Finally, alcohol and sub- (Velasquez, Carbonari, & DiClemente, 1999), which highlights the
stance use are associated with increases in suicidal ideation and importance of self-efficacy for those with comorbidity.
suicide attempts, shifting intervention efforts to crisis management Thus far, we have discussed self-efficacy and life stress sepa-
rather than other therapeutic goals (e.g., Claassen et al., 2007; rately. The strength of an individual’s self-efficacy conviction is
Goldstein & Levitt, 2006; Shen et al., 2006). proposed to influence persistence in the face of obstacles and
In addition to depression symptoms, numerous studies have aversive life experiences (Bandura, 1992). Addiction relapse mod-
demonstrated a relationship between life stress and worse post- els suggest that individuals experiencing life stressors may remain
treatment drinking outcomes (Billings & Moos, 1983; S. A. abstinent in part because of their self-efficacy in being able to
Brown, Vik, Patterson, Grant, & Schuckit, 1995; Canton et al., resist the urge to drink or use (Witkiewitz & Marlatt, 2004). This
1988; Vuchinich & Tucker, 1996). The predictive quality of life suggests the possibility that life stress and self-efficacy may jointly
stressors also extends to posttreatment outcomes for cocaine (Mc- affect the relapse process. However, we found no studies exploring
Mahon, 2001) and opiate users (Grey, Osborn, & Reznikoff, this potential interaction in comorbid samples of substance use–
1986). The negative impact of life stress may be particularly disordered individuals.
relevant for substance-dependent individuals with mood disorders, Survival analysis is a technique that evaluates the time it takes
as life stress has also been associated with recurrence of depressive for an event to happen and has been widely used for examining
episodes, depression treatment outcomes, and attrition (e.g., Mon- time to relapse (e.g., Brecht, von Mayrhauser, & Anglin, 2000;
roe, Kupfer, & Frank, 1992; Monroe, Roberts, Kupfer, & Frank, Cornelius et al., 2004; Greenfield et al., 2000; Jones & McMahon,
1996). Indeed, some research supports the stress-generation hy- 1994; Saunders, Baily, Phillips, & Allsop, 1993). All of these
pothesis, which proposes that individuals with depression generate studies, however, used either unchanging predictors (e.g., gender,
additional stressors as a result of their symptoms, behaviors, and ethnicity) or potentially changing variables measured at a single
social interactions (Hammen, 1991; Monroe, Slavich, Torres, & time point (e.g., intake alcohol expectancies, intake self-efficacy).
Gotlib, 2007), thus heightening risk of substance relapse. Evalu- Many predictors, including life stress and self-efficacy, are dy-
ating the impact of life stress on outcomes is complex, however, namic and likely alter risk of relapse over time. Survival analysis
given fluctuations in stress over time. In addition, the temporal is well suited for analyzing such time-varying predictors (Hosmer
nature of a stressor may affect risk of relapse. Stressors differ in & Lemeshow, 1999; Willett & Singer, 1993; Willett, Singer, &
their temporal characteristics, with some stressors persisting for Martin, 1998). For example, Hillegers et al. (2004) used survival
extended periods, while other stressors occur on a specific date and analyses to examine the relationship between stressful life events
are more short lived (Shiffman, 1989). Although much evidence as a time-varying predictor and the subsequent development of
has demonstrated a relationship between discrete stressful life mood disorders in adolescents. Among other applications, Willett
events and substance use, chronic stressors have received much and Singer (1993) provided an example of survival techniques
less attention. Our research has shown, however, that chronic applied to cocaine relapse (Havassy, Hall, & Wasserman, 1991)
stressors also increase the risk of resuming posttreatment sub- using multiple assessments of mood as a time-varying predictor.
stance use (S. A. Brown et al., 1990; Tate, McQuaid, & Brown, The survivor function depicts the proportions of the sample who
2005). do not experience a given event—in this case, substance use—in
Self-efficacy is another personal characteristic that changes over each time period. We employ a Cox (1972) proportional-hazards
time (Finney, Noyes, Coutts, & Moos, 1998; Rychtarik, Prue, model to determine whether time to relapse is influenced by life
Rapp, & King, 1992). Thus, the relationship between self-efficacy stress, self-efficacy, or their interaction. Our study is unique in that
and addiction relapse may be most appropriately evaluated in we have a comorbid sample with major depression and alcohol or
models that consider such temporal changes. In the context of substance use disorder, and we have modeled the change in pre-
substance abuse, self-efficacy is defined as an individual’s belief in dictors over time. We hypothesize that (a) severe life stress (both
his or her ability to resist the urge to drink or use. Cognitive– chronic stressors and discrete life events) will be associated with
behavioral models of addiction relapse postulate that those with shorter survival times and (b) higher self-efficacy will be associ-
high self-efficacy in their ability to abstain from alcohol and drugs ated with longer survival times. In addition to our primary hypoth-
are more likely to use coping responses and less likely to drink or eses, we also test whether the interaction between self-efficacy and
use than those with low self-efficacy. Past research supports this life stress influences time to relapse beyond the unique contribu-
relationship between self-efficacy and treatment outcomes for al- tion of these factors.
cohol (e.g., Greenfield et al., 2000; Rychtarik et al., 1992;
Sitharthan & Kavanagh, 1990), marijuana (e.g., Stephens, Wertz,
Method
& Roffman, 1993), and cocaine (e.g., Avants, Margolin, & Kosten,
1996). Self-efficacy may be particularly important for substance- Participants
dependent individuals with comorbid depression, as depression
may stem, in part, from conditions that lead individuals to believe Participants were patients at the Veterans Affairs San Diego
that they are not able to successfully execute behaviors required to Healthcare System, drawn from sequential referrals to the Sub-
manage prospective situations (i.e., low self-efficacy; Bandura, stance Abuse Mental Illness Program, an abstinence-based outpa-
1982). Consistent with this conceptualization, research has linked tient dual-diagnosis clinic. Participants were included in the study
SELF-EFFICACY, STRESS, AND TIME TO RELAPSE 49

if they met current criteria for Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of cognitive– behavioral depression treatment (Muñoz, Ying, Perez-
Mental Disorders (4th ed.; DSM–IV; American Psychiatric Asso- Stable, & Miranda, 1993) and the cognitive– behavioral coping
ciation, 1994) major depressive disorder and alcohol, cannabis, or skills training of Project MATCH (Kadden et al., 1994). ICBT
stimulant dependence with recent substance use (within 3 months focused on altering dysfunctional cognitions, practicing positive
prior to intake) and accepted the program goal of abstinence from activities, and developing interpersonal communication skills. TSF
alcohol and drug use. Participants were excluded if they (a) met consisted of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcohol-
criteria for DSM–IV bipolar disorder or any psychotic disorder, (b) ism Project MATCH TSF intervention (Nowinski, Baker, & Car-
met current criteria for DSM–IV opiate dependence through intra- roll, 1994), modified to be delivered in a group format rather than
venous administration, (c) could not accurately recall events be- individual sessions. In addition to the study treatment groups,
cause of memory deficits, or (d) lived too far away to attend participants in both interventions also received monthly medica-
psychotherapy appointments twice a week. Participants agreed to tion management appointments with the dual-diagnosis program
(a) randomization to one of two psychotherapy groups; (b) face- psychiatrist, who used standardized Veterans Affairs protocol for
to-face research assessments at intake, mid-treatment (12 weeks), major depressive disorder (e.g., selective serotonin reuptake inhib-
and end of treatment (24 weeks); and (c) random toxicology itors and atypical antidepressants).
screens. Additionally, all participants received pharmacotherapy Within 1 week of obtaining informed consent for study partic-
via a standardized Veterans Affairs protocol for major depression ipation, a trained research assistant completed a diagnostic assess-
and agreed not to participate in any other formal treatment for ment using the Composite International Diagnostic Interview
substance dependence or depression during the 24 weeks of treat- (Robins et al., 1988) to confirm inclusion criteria. Current analyses
ment, with the exceptions of community 12-step meetings and are focused on assessments of self-efficacy, life stress, and sub-
residential treatment program required meetings. Ninety percent of stance use conducted at three interview time points: (a) intake, (b)
consecutive referrals who met study criteria consented to partici- mid-treatment (Week 12, end of Phase 1), and (c) end of treatment
pate in the study. Of those who refused consent, 1 person felt the (Week 24, end of Phase 2).
assessments were overwhelming, 1 person expressed a preference
to receive treatment focused on his comorbid anxiety disorder, and
the remainder refused randomization to treatment condition. Measures
A total of 168 veterans gave informed consent to participate and
Self-efficacy. The 50-item self-report Drug-Taking Confi-
were randomized to one of the two interventions. Thirty-two
participants (19.0%) who did not complete their intake assessment dence Questionnaire (DTCQ; Annis & Martin, 1985) assesses
were not included in the analyses: Six did not attend any therapy coping self-efficacy for both alcohol and drug use and is a reliable
sessions (1 moved out of the area, and the others were not able to and valid indicator of self-efficacy (alcohol sample, ␣ ⫽ .98;
be contacted), and 26 participants attended at least one session but cocaine sample, ␣ ⫽ .98; Sklar, Annis, & Turner, 1997). Reliabil-
were not responsive to outreach efforts by either therapists or ity coefficients were comparable in our sample (intake, ␣ ⫽ .98;
research staff. The remaining 136 participants were evaluated mid-treatment, ␣ ⫽ .98). Participants rated their confidence in
throughout treatment. One participant gave informed consent but their ability to resist the urge to drink alcohol or use substances on
refused further study participation at the time of the first research a 0 –5 scale (0 ⫽ not at all confident to 5 ⫽ very confident) in 50
assessment, and 1 participant was deceased prior to completing high-risk relapse situations.
treatment. Twenty-one additional participants (15.0%) were ex- Depression symptoms. The Hamilton Depression Rating Scale
cluded from analysis because of missing mid-treatment data. The (HDRS; Hamilton, 1960) is a widely used structured clinical
final sample included 113 individuals, with an average age of 48.9 interview that assesses depression symptoms experienced over the
years (SD ⫽ 7.4). The majority were male (94.7%) and Caucasian prior week. The HDRS consists of 21 items scored on a 0 – 4 scale,
(72.6%) and had completed high school or the equivalent (96.5%). with demonstrated sensitivity and specificity in alcohol-dependent
Most participants met lifetime DSM–IV criteria for alcohol depen- populations (Willenbring, 1986). Standards vary, but scores higher
dence (90.3%), slightly more than half met criteria for stimulant than 20 are considered indicative of clinically depressed patients
dependence (54.8%), and 29.2% met criteria for marijuana depen- (S. A. Brown et al., 1994). We report both mean scores and
dence. Included and excluded cases did not significantly differ on percentages above and below the cutoff at mid-treatment.
demographic or dependence characteristics. Life stress. A trained interviewer administered the Psychiatric
Epidemiology Research Interview—Modified (Hirschfield et al.,
1977) at each assessment. This measure is a 133-item stressor
Design and Procedure
checklist followed by a semistructured interview. For each stressor
Participants in this study were enrolled in a randomized clinical reported, the interviewer probed for a detailed description of the
trial comparing integrated cognitive– behavioral therapy (ICBT) experience, including date of occurrence, context, duration, con-
and 12-step facilitation therapy (TSF) for comorbid substance sequences, and proximity to substance use. The interviewer then
dependence and depression (S. A. Brown et al., 2006). Both presented this information to a panel of at least two raters who had
interventions were delivered in a group format and consisted of achieved adequate reliability. The panel rated stressors according
two consecutive 12-week phases. Phase 1 consisted of 1-hr group to the objective rating criteria of the Bedford College Life Events
sessions that met twice weekly for a total of 24 sessions from and Difficulties Schedule (G. W. Brown, Bifulco, Harris, &
Weeks 1 through 12, and Phase 2 consisted of 1-hr group sessions Bridge, 1986). Two coinvestigators on the study who had exten-
that met once a week for a total of 12 sessions from Weeks 13 sive prior experience with this rating system rated stressors and
through 24. ICBT combined the interventions and structure of trained new raters.
50 TATE ET AL.

Following the training, new raters first observed and later par- culties, and acute events as predictor variables. As previously
ticipated in rating sessions with at least two previously trained noted, survival analysis is widely used to analyze time-to-event
raters, using the actual data from the study, until reliability was data (e.g., Singer & Willett, 1993), and this methodology has been
achieved (r ⬎.90). On the basis of the detailed presentation by the extended to handle time-varying covariates (Hosmer & Leme-
interviewer of each stressor, raters determined whether an occur- show, 1999). All analyses were conducted with STATA statistical
rence qualified as a difficulty (ongoing chronic stressor lasting at software (Version 9.1; StataCorp., College Station, TX).
least 4 weeks) or a stressful event (acute stressor with a discrete The study period was predetermined to last until the end of
onset; G. W. Brown & Harris, 1978). All chronic difficulties and treatment for each participant (range ⫽ 24 –27 weeks). Participants
acute events were rated on the likelihood that the stressor would who did not relapse during the study period were fixed-right
seriously threaten an individual’s personal or social well-being censored (Singer & Willett, 1993). A relapse was defined as the
(Dohrenwend & Dohrenwend, 1974). Raters classified reported day an individual first used any amount of alcohol or drugs (Jones
stressors (both chronic difficulties and acute events) as severe, & McMahon, 1994). Survival time was measured as the number of
nonsevere, or not posing adequate objective threat to warrant days until initial substance use, beginning from each participant’s
rating as a stressor on the basis of the standardized Bedford date of treatment initiation. An individual’s substance use status
College Life Events and Difficulties Schedule manual (covering was coded for each day in our sample (abstinence vs. substance
516 pages of stressor examples and ratings). Because life stress can use). Although we did not anticipate treatment group differences in
be the result of recent substance use (rather than preceding sub- outcomes on the basis of our previous analyses (S. A. Brown et al.,
stance use), raters also coded whether each stressor was a result of 2006), we included treatment groups (ICBT and TSF) in the model
recent substance use. Substance-induced stressors were excluded to control for possible differential treatment effects on time to
from analyses. In addition, we excluded health stressors, because relapse. We examined self-efficacy as a time-variant covariate
prior research has conceptualized health problems as motivating because it was assumed to change over time as a result of treat-
abstinence rather than relapse, as is the case for other stressors ment. We calculated the item mean self-efficacy score (average
(Monti et al., 1999; Smith, Hodgson, Bridgeman, & Shepherd, score across DTCQ items) using the intake assessment to predict
2003), and we have demonstrated that health stressors are associ- survival time in Phase 1 and the mid-treatment scores to predict
ated with reduced likelihood and severity of substance relapse survival time in Phase 2.
(e.g., Tate et al., 2005). Two qualitatively distinct stress measures were included in our
Substance use. The Timeline Follow-Back (TLFB; Sobell & model. Chronic difficulties in each stressor domain (e.g., financial,
Sobell, 1992) was used to measure all substance use during the 3 work, relationship) were scored as none ⫽ 0, nonsevere ⫽ 1, and
months prior to each assessment, including the date of first use severe ⫽ 2. For each phase, difficulty scores in each domain were
following treatment initiation. We selected the date of first use for our summed to provide an index of overall chronic stress. The statis-
outcome because of the importance of abstinence in dually diagnosed tical distribution of the summed indexes was highly skewed, with
samples, noted previously, and because the goals and interventions of the majority of participants scoring 1–2. The limited research
our dual-diagnosis program are abstinence based. The TLFB mea- examining difficulties has commonly dichotomized the presence
sures the type of substance the participant used and the number of or absence of a difficulty in samples with lower levels of chronic
days he or she used drugs as well as the quantity and frequency of difficulties (e.g., Monroe et al., 2007). Given the high levels of
alcohol use (Ehrman & Robbins, 1994; Fals-Stewart, O’Farrell, Frei- chronic difficulties in our sample, we trichotomized each partici-
tas, McFarlin, & Rutigliano, 2000). The TLFB has demonstrated pant’s index such that 0 indicated having no difficulties
reliability and validity in alcohol- and substance-dependent (Fals- (summed index ⫽ 0; 13.3% of the sample at intake), 1 indicated
Stewart et al., 2000; Maisto, Sobell, & Sobell, 1979) and comorbid low chronic difficulty levels (summed indexes ⫽ 1–2; 58.4% at
(Carey, 1997; Carey, Carey, Maisto, & Henson, 2004) treatment intake), and 2 indicated high chronic difficulty levels (summed
samples. Additionally, random toxicology screens were used to en- indexes 3–7; 28.3% at intake). Like self-efficacy, chronic dif-
hance reliability for participants’ self-reports of substance use. Sixty- ficulties varied by phase of treatment (i.e., having a score for
six percent of participants had a urine toxicology screen at some time both Phase 1 and Phase 2).
in Phase 1 or 2, and toxicology results matched TLFB daily self- The second stress measure included in our model represented
reports 82.2% of the time (excluding one toxicology note of an acute events. The acute event measure was dichotomously coded
inadequate sample). Two participants reported alcohol use that was for absence or presence of an acute event. As our prior research has
not detected by the toxicology screen. Five participants denied alcohol documented that substance relapse is related to acute events rated
or drug use at any time on the TLFB, but the toxicology screen as severe (S. A. Brown, Vik, Patterson, Grant, & Schuckit, 1995),
indicated use (2 used marijuana, 3 used stimulants). An additional 12 we excluded nonsevere events from our analysis. We assumed that
participants reported use and had toxicology results indicating use, but if a relapse was related to an acute event, the relapse would likely
the dates of use differed for self-report and toxicology results. If either occur within 30 days following the event (Tate, 2000). Thus, a
source (self-report or toxicology) indicated use, use was coded for participant was coded as having exposure to an acute event for a
analyses. If both sources indicated use, the earlier of the two dates was 30-day window following onset of each acute event. Acute events,
used. like substance use, were not constrained to changing at the mid-
treatment assessment but could change on any day in the time
Statistical Analysis period.
In summary, survival analysis involves computing the pro-
Time to relapse was analyzed with a Cox proportional-hazards portion of a group that experiences the event of interest in a
model performed with treatment type, self-efficacy, chronic diffi- given period—in our study, on a daily basis. The survivor
SELF-EFFICACY, STRESS, AND TIME TO RELAPSE 51

function is a plot of these daily proportions accumulated across (39% in the 1st month, and 30% in the 2nd month). The remaining
time. One can compute daily risks to examine whether risk (or 31% of the relapses were spread across the remainder of the study
hazard) differs systematically for different groups within a period. Of the 56 relapses, 31 individuals (55%) drank alcohol, 8
sample. As described by Willett and Singer (1993), survival (14%) used stimulants (i.e., cocaine or methamphetamine), 8
analyses resemble logistic regression models with daily absti- (14%) used marijuana, and 9 (17%) reported multiple substances
nence or relapse as the outcome variable and model variables as used. All participants who relapsed used a substance for which
predictors accumulated over time. As separate proportions are they had met diagnostic criteria.
calculated for each day, both static predictors (ICBT vs. TSF
treatment group) and time-varying predictors (self-efficacy,
Depression Symptoms and Substance Use
chronic difficulties, acute events) can be included, coded on a
day-to-day basis. Finally, the interaction of life stress and Sample characteristics by relapse status are depicted in Table
self-efficacy was evaluated in an extended model. Two inter- 1. Although it is not a focus of this study and was not included
action terms were added to the initial Cox proportional-hazards in the model, depression was examined as a predictor of relapse
model: Self-Efficacy ⫻ Chronic Difficulties and Self- in each phase via logistic regressions. Depression symptoms
Efficacy ⫻ Acute Events. Thus, we evaluated the ongoing risk were not related to likelihood of relapse in either phase; intake
of relapse given an individual’s treatment (ICBT vs. TSF), HDRS predicting relapse in Phase 1, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 113) ⫽ 0.14,
current self-efficacy, acute events, and chronic difficulties and p ⫽ .71, odds ratio ⫽ 1.01; mid-treatment HDRS predicting
the interaction of self-efficacy and life stress. relapse in Phase 2, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 49) ⫽ 0.21, p ⫽ .66, odds ratio ⫽
0.99. Depression scores for 45% of the participants were below
Results the HDRS clinically depressed cutoff of 20 at the mid-treatment
assessment. Remission of depression symptoms at mid-
Substance Use Outcomes treatment was not related to relapse in Phase 2, ␹2(1, N ⫽ 49) ⫽
Fifty-six (50%) individuals relapsed within the study period, 38 0.002, p ⫽ .97 (22.7% of participants below the cutoff relapsed,
(34%) reported no alcohol or drug use, and 19 (17%) reported no and 22.2% of participants above the cutoff relapsed).
alcohol and drug use in Phase 1 and were censored because of
missing Phase 2 outcome or predictor variables (i.e., 17 were Model Predicting Time to Relapse
missing mid-treatment DTCQs, and 2 were missing Phase 2
TLFBs). There were no significant differences between the 19 The Cox proportional-hazards model was used to predict time to
censored and 94 remaining cases on any demographic or model relapse via treatment type, self-efficacy, chronic difficulties, and
variables. Most individuals who relapsed did so in the first 60 days acute events. Covariates were first examined for both proportion-

Table 1
Sample Characteristics by Relapse Status in Phases 1 and 2

Survivors Phase 1 Phase 2 Censoreda


(abstainers) relapse relapse (missing data)
Characteristic (n ⫽ 38) (n ⫽ 45) (n ⫽ 11) (n ⫽ 19)

Treatment (% TSF / % ICBT) 45/55 44/56 46/55 63/37


Mean (SD) depression
Intake 28.6 (12.0) 29.1 (12.4) 25.3 (10.9) 29.3 (12.4)
Mid-treatment 23.2 (13.5) 27.3 (13.3) 21.2 (11.8) 27.5 (10.6)
Depression (% in remission)b
Mid-treatment 45 26 46 20
Mean (SD) self-efficacy
Intake 3.5 (1.0) 3.0 (1.2) 3.6 (1.1) 3.5 (1.2)
Mid-treatment 4.0 (1.0) 3.2 (1.1) 4.0 (0.8) 3.8 (1.5)
Self efficacy (% low/middle/high)
Intake 0/42/58 13/40/47 9/18/73 5/37/58
Mid-treatment 0/24/76 3/47/50 0/18/82 0/50/50
Chronic difficulties (% none/low/high)
Phase 1 16/60/24 13/47/40 0/73/27 16/74/10
Phase 2 21/55/24 16/51/33 0/80/20 6/76/18
Acute events (%)
Phase 1c 24 21 9 11
Phase 2 5 18 36 26

Note. TSF ⫽ twelve-step facilitation therapy; ICBT ⫽ integrated cognitive-behavioral therapy.


a
The censored group was composed of survivors (abstainers) in Phase 1 who were excluded because of missing data in Phase 2. Mid-treatment self-efficacy
scores were available for only 2 participants, and mid-treatment depression scores were available for 10 participants in this group. bHamilton Depression
Rating Scale scores less than 20 were coded as remitted. cNineteen percent of Phase 1 survivors experienced an acute event in the 12 weeks of Phase
1 (across survivor and censored groups). In contrast, 21% of the Phase 1 relapse group experienced an acute life event prior to relapse (M ⫽ 32 days, SD ⫽
24 days, Mdn ⫽ 30 days).
52 TATE ET AL.

ality of hazards and multicollinearity. None of the covariates Table 1 lists DTCQ scores by relapse status for each phase. In
significantly interacted with time, and there was no evidence of Phase 2, little difference was seen between self-efficacy scores by
multicollinearity. relapse status, as those with low self-efficacy relapsed early (see
Time to relapse was predicted by the model, ␹2(4, N ⫽ 113) ⫽ Figure 1b). As continuous self-efficacy measures (either mean or
20.44, p ⬍ .001. Type of treatment received was not significantly total scores) are most common in the research literature (e.g.,
associated with risk of substance relapse during treatment in this Blume, Schmaling, & Marlatt, 2001; Maisto, Clifford, Long-
comorbid sample (hazard ratio ⫽ 1.13, 95% confidence interval abaugh, & Beattie, 2002), analyses were conducted with mean
[CI] ⫽ 0.67–1.92, p ⫽ .65; see Figure 1a). Median survival times DTCQ scores. For presentation in the figure, self-efficacy was split
were 148 days for TSF (n ⫽ 54; 48% of the sample) and 122 days into three levels to simplify display and interpretation (Singer &
for ICBT (n ⫽ 59; 52% of the sample). Table 1 shows sample Willett, 1993). We produced survival functions by trichotomizing
characteristics by relapse status, and Table 2 provides the median DTCQ scores: (a) low indicates individuals who had scores of
survival days (time to relapse) for participants by treatment, self- 0.00 –1.66, (b) medium indicates individuals who had scores of
efficacy level, and life stressors (chronic difficulties and acute
1.67–3.33, and (c) high indicates individuals who had scores of
events).
3.34 –5.00. As shown in Figure 1b, individuals with higher levels
of self-efficacy had longer survival times, and lower self-efficacy
Self-Efficacy and Time to Relapse was predictive of relapse early in treatment. Table 2 lists the
We assumed self-efficacy would change over time as a result of median survival days by these categories. Using the trichotomized
treatment, and DTCQ scores increased significantly from the in- categories for the 49 participants who survived to the second
take assessment to the Phase 1 assessment at mid-treatment, phase, we found that 33 participants (67.3%) did not change
t(84) ⫽ 2.98, p ⫽ .004. Self-efficacy was a significant predictor in self-efficacy levels (26 started and remained high, 7 started and
the model ( p ⫽ .003). Each unit increase in the DTCQ decreased remained in the middle category, and no one started and remained
the risk of relapse by a hazard ratio of 0.71 (95% CI ⫽ 0.56 – 0.89). in the low category) and 16 individuals (32.7%) did change self-

Figure 1. Estimated survivorship functions following treatment initiation as related to (a) treatment type, (b)
self-efficacy, (c) chronic difficulties, and (d) acute events. TSF ⫽ 12-step facilitation therapy; ICBT ⫽ integrated
cognitive– behavioral therapy.
SELF-EFFICACY, STRESS, AND TIME TO RELAPSE 53

Table 2 Both types of stress were significant predictors in the model. For
Survival Characteristics by Treatment, Self-Efficacy, Acute chronic difficulties ( p ⫽ .02), each unit increase in the trichoto-
Events, and Chronic Difficulties mized stress score increased the risk of relapse by a hazard ratio of
1.66 (95% CI ⫽ 1.08 –2.55). Increasing levels of chronic difficul-
Variable No. participantsa Median survival days ties shortened survival time (see Figure 1c). Among the 49 par-
Treatment ticipants who did not relapse in Phase 1, the majority did not
TSFb 54 148 change chronic difficulty levels in Phase 2 (n ⫽ 43). Six individ-
ICBTc 59 122 uals had trichotomized difficulty scores that changed levels at
Self-efficacyd mid-treatment (1 increased and 4 decreased); none of these 6
Low 8 18
Medium 47 85 participants relapsed in Phase 2. In the group with no chronic
High 74 170 difficulties, fewer than half of the cases relapsed; hence, median
Chronic difficulties survival time was beyond our study period (see Table 2).
None 17 — Acute events were also significant in the model ( p ⫽ .009), such
Moderate 69 170
Severe 33 62
that having an acute event increased the risk of relapse by a hazard
Acute events ratio of 2.91 (95% CI ⫽ 1.30 – 6.51). As depicted in Figure 1d,
None 110 — individuals who were under the influence of an acute event (bot-
Any 20 41 tom function), compared to those who were not (top function), had
shorter survival times. Because events are acute experiences oc-
Note. Dashes indicate that fewer than half of the participants relapsed;
hence, median survival time was beyond the study period. TSF ⫽ twelve- curring on a discrete date and we assumed increased risk could
step facilitation therapy; ICBT ⫽ integrated cognitive-behavioral therapy. persist for 30 days (Tate, 2000), individuals fluctuated between
a
Number of participants who were in each condition at least once during functions depending on whether they had experienced an acute
b c
the study period. Twelve-step facilitation therapy. Integrated event within the prior 30 days. Thus, a participant’s risk of relapse
d
cognitive– behavioral therapy. Self-efficacy scores were categorized
into low (0.00 –1.66), medium (1.67–3.33), and high (3.34 –5.00) groups. was depicted by the “absent” curve until the day of an acute event.
As of that date, risk of relapse was depicted by the “present” curve
for the subsequent 30 days, at which time the risk returned to the
decreased risk levels depicted in the absent curve. Five individuals
efficacy levels (4 decreased from the high to the middle level, 1 reported multiple concurring acute events. The median survival
person increased from low to high, and 11 increased from middle time for individuals who did not experience an acute event was
to high). Twenty-four percent of those with unchanged self- beyond our study period (see Table 2).
efficacy relapsed in Phase 2, compared to 25% of participants with Finally, the interactions of chronic difficulties with self-efficacy
decreased self-efficacy and 17% of participants with increased and acute events with self-efficacy were added to the initial model.
self-efficacy. Of note, only 2 participants with low self-efficacy The inclusion of the interaction terms did not significantly improve
did not relapse in Phase 1: One person relapsed in Phase 2 despite the model, ⌬␹2(2, N ⫽ 113) ⫽ 2.60, p ⫽ .27. Interpretation of this
a self-reported increase from low to high self-efficacy at mid- finding is guarded given the limited range of self-efficacy ob-
treatment, and 1 person was censored because of missing Phase 2 served in our sample over the time frame, as the majority of
data. participants with low self-efficacy relapsed early.

Stressors and Time to Relapse


Discussion
A range of acute events and chronic difficulties was experienced
during the study period. Eighty percent of the total sample had a The present study demonstrates significant relationships be-
chronic financial difficulty during the study, 20% had a chronic tween self-efficacy, life stress, and time to relapse during the initial
legal difficulty, 20% had a chronic relationship difficulty, 19% had 6 months in treatment for adults with comorbid major depressive
a chronic housing difficulty, and a single individual had a chronic disorder and substance dependence. Consistent with prior alcohol
work difficulty. Twelve percent of the sample had no chronic and drug research (Greenfield et al., 2000; Rychtarik et al., 1992;
difficulty during the entire study period, 46% had one chronic Stephens et al., 1993), we found that individuals with higher levels
difficulty, and 42% had multiple chronic difficulties. The domains of self-efficacy maintained abstinence from alcohol and drugs
of acute events experienced were as follows: Six percent of the longer than those with lower levels of self-efficacy. Indeed, a
total sample had a relationship event, 6% had a legal event, 4% had prominent feature of the survival function (Figure 1b) was the
a housing event, 3% had a death event, 3% had a work event, and sharp decline in abstinence seen in those with low self-efficacy in
a single individual had a financial event. Table 1 lists the percent- the early weeks of treatment. The difference in survival between
ages of participants with chronic difficulties and acute events by those with middle and high levels of self-efficacy was much less
relapse status. For the survivor and censored groups, the acute pronounced. Our findings vividly portray the heightened risk of
events percentage reflects participants who experienced an acute relapse for those with low self-efficacy early in treatment. Spec-
event at any time in Phase 1 (initial 12 weeks) or Phase 2 (sub- ulation regarding the impact of changes in self-efficacy during
sequent 12 weeks). For the relapse groups, the percentage reflects treatment is limited by the fact that almost everyone with low
participants who experienced an acute event prior to the day of self-efficacy in our sample relapsed very early in treatment. For-
relapse (Phase 1, M ⫽ 32 days, SD ⫽ 24 days; Phase 2, M ⫽ 122 tunately, a minority of patients fell into the low self-efficacy range.
days, SD ⫽ 31 days). Future development and testing of intensive interventions aimed at
54 TATE ET AL.

this small portion of high-risk individuals early in treatment may in the event curve over time suggests that acute events continued
improve outcomes. to pose risk throughout the treatment phases we examined. Tem-
Both chronic and acute stress elevated relapse risk among the porary increases in the frequency of treatment provider contact
dually diagnosed sample of this study. Those with higher levels of (e.g., booster sessions) or stress-specific adjunct interventions,
chronic difficulties relapsed sooner and were at greater risk of such as grief counseling, may be of benefit.
relapsing. High levels of chronic difficulties more than doubled the Our findings do not support an interaction between self-efficacy
risk of relapse compared to individuals with no chronic difficulty. and life stressors in predicting time to relapse. It is possible that we
As chronic stress became increasingly severe and extended across lacked adequate power to detect an interaction of this magnitude,
multiple domains (e.g., financial, legal, and housing), the risk of or the complexities associated with these variables in our sample
relapse increased. Assessing such protracted stress identifies indi- might have limited our ability to detect such moderating effects.
viduals at high risk for early return to drinking or using. Chronic For example, an individual’s self-efficacy estimates may reflect
life difficulties may contribute to increased likelihood of resuming self-confidence in the context of chronic difficulties, which would
substance use by posing more adaptational demands on the indi- reduce the likelihood of detecting an interaction. Additionally,
vidual, through protracted exposure to negative affective states since individuals with low self-efficacy relapsed early in our study,
(e.g., depression, anxiety, and worry), or through depletion of little variation in self-efficacy remained across later time periods,
coping resources (e.g., social support and financial resources). limiting statistical power for detecting interaction effects over the
Substance abusers coping with chronic life stress may also be study period. Future studies with these variables are needed to
susceptible to developing a sense of learned helplessness when ascertain whether the distributional characteristics of our predictor
managing persistent problems with newly acquired coping skills variables over time are replicated across settings.
that are perceived as more effortful and less immediately effective The likelihood of early relapse was similar across the two types
than substance use. Many addiction treatment programs incorpo- of treatment in our study (ICBT vs. TSF). We had not anticipated
rate stress management interventions focused on enhancing coping differences on the basis of our prior analyses of substance use
skills in high-risk situations. Our findings suggest that the inclu- outcomes during treatment (percentage of days abstinent, average
sion of stress management interventions may be particularly useful drinks per drinking day) in this sample (S. A. Brown et al., 2006).
for comorbid populations, given that the vast majority of our The addiction portions of our interventions were based on manuals
participants reported experiencing these protracted stressors. Ad- developed for Project MATCH, and few differences were detected
ditionally, these chronic stressors were stable over time for most of between the TSF and cognitive– behavioral coping skills condi-
our sample. In contrast to discrete high-risk situations, which are tions in that study (Project MATCH Research Group, 1998).
often the focus of addiction stress management interventions, the Additionally, neither severity of depression symptoms nor depres-
chronicity of these difficulties highlights the need for coping skills sion remission predicted relapse within our sample of depressed
training specific to managing prolonged stress. In addition to stress substance-dependent adults. In some prior studies comparing de-
management skills training, interventions targeting specific types pressed and nondepressed substance-dependent patients, depres-
of stressors may be beneficial. For example, the most common sion predicted worse addiction outcomes (e.g., R. A. Brown et al.,
type of difficulty in our study was in the financial domain, and 1998; Cornelius et al., 2004; Greenfield et al., 1998). However, our
vocational rehabilitation programs may be helpful for substance findings suggest that variance in depression levels above clinical
abusers experiencing problems in this domain. Providing other thresholds is not related to substance relapse. Of note, participants
resources (e.g., legal or relationship counseling) as an adjunct to in our remitted group at mid-treatment were not asymptomatic but
treatment may ameliorate or postpone the likelihood of turning to continued to experience symptoms. Depression research has sug-
substance use for individuals coping with these types of chronic gested that residual depressive symptoms constitute a “subthresh-
difficulties. old continuation of an active major depressive episode” (Judd,
Acute stress events nearly tripled the risk of relapse, signifi- Paulus, Zeller, 1999, p. 764; Judd et al., 2000), with heightened
cantly reducing survival times. This finding supports prior research risk for depression relapse. Thus, remitted participants in our
demonstrating that these types of stressors are related to poorer sample are not comparable to nondepressed participants in prior
substance use outcomes (S. A. Brown et al., 1995; Canton et al., studies.
1988; McMahon, 2001; Vuchinich & Tucker, 1996). Despite re- Our results should be interpreted in the context of potential
cent studies using survival analysis to examine the relationship sample and methodological limitations. These findings were based
between stressful life events and depression (Hillegers et al., 2004; on a sample of predominantly White men and may not generalize
Kendler, Kuhn, & Prescott, 2004), our study is the first to examine to other groups. Our sample included substance-dependent indi-
the time-limited relationship between the occurrence of an acute viduals with comorbid depression, and future research is needed to
stress event and relapse within 30 days following the event. For- determine whether the relationships between hypothesized predic-
tunately, acute events designated as severe on the Bedford College tors and time to relapse extend to other common types of comor-
Life Events and Difficulties Schedule (Bifulco et al. 1989; G. W. bidity (e.g., anxiety disorders, schizophrenia, and bipolar disor-
Brown et al., 1986) life stress rating criteria tend to be infrequent der). Although our trichotomization of chronic difficulties
occurrences (e.g., death of a loved one, divorce, eviction, and appeared to capture qualitative distinctions among stress levels,
incarceration). In contrast to the protracted nature of negative more research is needed to validate this classification. In addition,
affect associated with chronic difficulties, the intensity of emo- larger studies should examine whether various domains of stress
tional response to acute events may overwhelm substance abusers, (e.g., relationship, financial, housing) differentially affect time to
which suggests distinct coping and intervention needs for comor- relapse. Our prior research suggested that relapse was most likely
bid populations during addiction treatment. The continuing decline to occur within 30 days following a acute event, but an individual
SELF-EFFICACY, STRESS, AND TIME TO RELAPSE 55

may not be free of negative impact of the event after this period. Blume, A. W., Schmaling, K. B., & Marlatt, G. A. (2001). Motivating
More research is needed to examine the length of impact for acute drinking behavior change: Depressive symptoms may not be noxious.
stress events on subsequent substance use. As mentioned previ- Addictive Behaviors, 26, 267–272.
ously, we selected initial substance use as our outcome variable on Brecht, M. L., von Mayrhauser, C., & Anglin, M. D. (2000). Predictors of
the basis of the abstinence goals of our dual-diagnosis program and relapse after treatment for methamphetamine use. Journal of Psychoac-
the potential for substance use to negatively impact dual-diagnosis tive Drugs, 32, 211–220.
Brown, G. W., Bifulco, A., Harris, T. O., & Bridge, L. (1986). Life stress,
treatment. However, addiction models posit different predictors for
chronic subclinical symptoms and vulnerability to clinical depression.
initial substance use episodes versus protracted use (e.g., Marlatt &
Journal of Affective Disorders, 11, 1–19.
Gordon, 1980), and our prior research has also detected differences Brown, G. W., & Harris, T. O. (1978). Social origins of depression: A
in predictors for initiation versus continuation of posttreatment study of psychiatric disorder in women. New York: Free Press.
substance use (Tate, 2000; Tate et al., 2005). Thus, more studies Brown, R. A., Monti, P. M., Myers, M. G., Martin, R. A., Rivinus, T.,
are needed to examine the relationship of life stress and self- Dubreuil, M. E., et al. (1998). Depression among cocaine abusers in
efficacy to other important substance outcomes. Finally, it was treatment: Relation to cocaine and alcohol use and treatment outcome.
beyond the scope of this study to incorporate depression symptoms American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 220 –225.
in our survival models. Future models including both depression Brown, S. A., Glasner, S. V., Tate, S. R., McQuaid, J. R., Chalekian, J., &
and addiction outcomes in dual-diagnosis studies are needed. Granholm, E. (2006). Integrated cognitive behavioral therapy versus
In summary, the present study highlights the importance of twelve step facilitation therapy for substance dependent adults with
self-efficacy, chronic difficulties, and acute events on time to depressive disorders. Journal of Psychoactive Drugs, 38, 449 – 460.
relapse for substance-dependent adults with depression. We have Brown, S. A., Inaba, R. K., Gillin, J. C., Schuckit, M. A., Stewart, M. A.,
demonstrated that individuals with comorbid substance depen- & Irwin, M. R. (1994). Alcoholism and affective disorder: Clinical
course of depressive symptoms. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152,
dence and depression are at a heightened risk for relapse and
45–52.
earlier resumption of substance use when experiencing life stres-
Brown, S. A., Vik, P. W., McQuaid, J. R., Patterson, T. L., Irwin, M. R.,
sors, both chronic difficulties and acute events. Consistent with
& Grant, I. (1990). Severity of psychosocial stress and outcome of
prior research and theory, self-efficacy was also related to risk of alcoholism treatment. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 99, 344 –348.
resuming substance for comorbid adults. As previously noted, Brown, S. A., Vik, P. W., Patterson, T. L., Grant, I., & Schuckit, M. A.
extending abstinence may be particularly important in treatment of (1995). Stress, vulnerability and adult alcohol relapse. Journal of Studies
individuals with substance use and other comorbid psychiatric on Alcohol, 56, 538 –545.
disorders. Alcohol and substance use can undermine pharmaco- Canton, G., Giannini, L., Magni, G., Bertinaria, A., Cibin, M., & Gallim-
therapy interventions through decreased medication adherence, berti, L. (1988). Locus of control, life events, and treatment outcome in
missed appointments, and compromised evaluation of response to alcohol dependent patients. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 78, 18 –23.
medication. Further, substance relapse early in the course of treat- Carey, K. (1997). Reliability and validity of the Time-Line Follow-Back
ment will likely impair one’s ability to learn and effectively utilize interview among psychiatric outpatients: A preliminary report. Psychol-
new skills needed to address either disorder. Our findings clarify ogy of Addictive Behaviors, 11, 26 –33.
particular areas of vulnerability, aiding clinicians in identifying Carey, K. B., Carey, M. P., Maisto, S. A., & Henson, J. M. (2004).
individuals at greater risk. Future research is needed to evaluate Temporal stability of the Timeline Followback Interview for alcohol and
drug use with psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 65,
whether additional provider contact or targeted intervention efforts
774 –781.
would prove beneficial in ameliorating the heightened risks posed
Carroll, K. M., Nich, C., & Rounsaville, B. J. (1995). Differential symptom
by life stressors.
reduction in depressed cocaine abusers treated with psychotherapy and
pharmacotherapy. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 251–
References 259.
Catz, S. L., Heckman, T. G., Kochman, A., & DiMarco, M. (2001). Rates
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man-
and correlates of HIV treatment adherence among late middle-aged and
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
older adults living with HIV disease. Psychology, Health & Medicine,
Annis, H. M., & Martin, G. (1985). The Drug-Taking Confidence Ques-
6(1), 47–58.
tionnaire. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Addiction Research Foundation of
Charney, D. A., Paraherakis, A. M., Negrete, J. C., & Gill, K. J. (1998).
Ontario.
The impact of depression on the outcome of addictions treatment.
Avants, S. K., Margolin, A., & Kosten, T. R. (1996). Influence of treatment
readiness on outcomes of two pharmacotherapy trials for cocaine abuse Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 15(2), 123–130.
among methadone-maintained patients. Psychology of Addictive Behav- Claassen, C. A., Trivedi, M. H., Rush, A. J., Husain, M. M., Zisook, S.,
iors, 10, 147–156. Young, E., et al. (2007). Clinical differences among depressed patients
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanisms in human agency. American with and without a history of suicide attempts: Findings from the
Psychologist, 37, 122–147. STAR*D trial. Journal of Affective Disorders, 97, 77– 84.
Bandura, A. (1992). Self-efficacy mechanism in psychobiologic function- Cornelius, J. R., Maisto, S. A., Martin, C. S., Bukstein, O. G., Salloum,
ing. In R. Schwarzer (Ed.), Self-efficacy: Thought control of action (pp. I. M., Daley, D. C., et al. (2004). Major depression associated with
355–394). Washington, DC: Hemisphere. earlier alcohol relapse in treated teens with AUD. Addictive Behaviors,
Bifulco, A., Brown, G. W., Edwards, A., Harris, T., Neilson, E., Richards, 29, 1035–1038.
C. & Robinson, R. (1989). Bedford College Life Events and Difficulties Cox, D. R. (1972). Regression models and life tables. Journal of the Royal
Schedule. London: University of London. Statistical Society, Series B, 34, 187–202.
Billings, A. G., & Moos, R. H. (1983). Psychosocial processes of recovery Dohrenwend, B. S., & Dohrenwend, B. P. (1974). Stress life events: Their
among alcoholics and their families: Implications for clinicians and nature and effects. New York: Wiley.
program evaluators. Addictive Behaviors, 8, 205–218. Ehrman, R. N., & Robbins, S. J. (1994). Reliability and validity of 6-month
56 TATE ET AL.

timeline reports of cocaine and heroin use in a methadone population. Maisto, S. A., Clifford, P. R., Longabaugh, R., & Beattie, M. (2002). The
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 843– 850. relationship between abstinence for one year following pretreatment
Fals-Stewart, W., O’Farrrell, T. J., Freitas, T. T., McFarlin, S. K., & assessment and alcohol use and other functioning at two years in
Rutigliano, P. (2000). The Timeline Follow-Back reports of psychoac- individuals presenting for alcohol treatment. Journal of Studies on
tive substance use by drug-abusing patients: Psychometric properties. Alcohol, 63, 397– 403.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 134 –144. Maisto, S. A., Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1979). Comparison of
Finney, J. W., Noyes, C. A., Coutts, A. I., & Moos, R. H. (1998). alcoholics’ self-reports of drinking behavior with reports of collateral
Evaluating substance abuse treatment process models: I. Changes on informants. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 106 –
proximal outcome variables during 12-step and cognitive-behavioral 122.
treatment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 59, 371–380. Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (1980). Determinants of relapse: Implica-
Goldstein, B. I., & Levitt, A. J. (2006). Is current alcohol consumption tions for the maintenance of behavior change. In O. Park & S. H.
associated with increased lifetime prevalence of major depression and Davidson (Eds.), Behavioral medicine: Changing health lifestyles (pp.
suicidality? Results from a pilot community survey. Comprehensive 410 – 452). New York: Brunner/Mazel.
Psychiatry, 47, 330 –333. McMahon, R. C. (2001). Personality, stress, and social support in cocaine
Greenfield, S., Hufford, M., Vagge, L., Muenz, L., Costello, M., & Weiss, relapse prediction. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment, 21, 77– 87.
R. (2000). The relationship of self-efficacy expectancies to relapse Monroe, S. M., Kupfer, D. J., & Frank, E. (1992). Life stress and treatment
among alcohol dependent men and women: A prospective study. Journal course of recurrent depression: I. Response during index episode. Jour-
of Studies on Alcohol, 61, 345–351. nal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60, 718 –724.
Greenfield, S. F., Weiss, R. D., Muenz, L. R., Vagge, L. M., Kelly, J. F., Monroe, S. M., Roberts, J. E., Kupfer, D. J., & Frank, E. (1996). Life stress
Bello, L. R., et al. (1998). The effect of depression on return to drinking: and treatment course of recurrent depression: II. Postrecovery associa-
A prospective study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 259 –265. tions with attrition, symptom course, and recurrence over 3 years.
Grey, C., Osborn, E., & Reznikoff, M. (1986). Psychosocial factors in Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 313–328.
outcome in two opiate addiction treatments. Journal of Clinical Psy- Monroe, S. M., Slavich, G. M., Torres, L. D., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Major
chology, 42, 185–189. life events and major chronic difficulties are differentially associated
Hamilton, M. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurolog- with history of major depressive episodes. Journal of Abnormal Psy-
ical and Neurosurgical Psychiatry, 23, 56 – 62. chology, 116, 116 –124.
Hammen, C. (1991). Generation of stress in the course of unipolar depres- Monti, P. M., Colby, S. M., Barnett, N. P., Spirito, A., Rohsenow, D. J.,
sion. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 555–561. Myers, M., et al. (1999). Brief intervention for harm reduction with
Haukkala, A., Uutela, A., Vartiainen, E., McAlister, A., & Knekt, P. alcohol-positive older adolescents in a hospital emergency department.
(2000). Depression and smoking cessation: The role of motivation and Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 67, 989 –994.
self-efficacy. Addictive Behaviors, 25, 311–316. Muñoz, R. F., Ying, Y., Perez-Stable, E. J., & Miranda, J. (1993). The
Havassy, B. E., Hall, S. M., & Wasserman, D. A. (1991). Social support prevention of depression: Research and practice. Baltimore: Johns Hop-
and relapse: Commonalities among alcoholics, opiate users, and ciga- kins University Press.
rette smokers. Addictive Behaviors, 16, 235–246. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. (2005). Harmful
Hillegers, M. H. J., Burger, H., Wals, M., Reichart, C. G., Verhulst, F. C., interactions: Mixing alcohol with medicines (NIH Publication No. 03–
Nolen, W. A., et al. (2004). Impact of stressful life events, familial 5329). Rockville, MD: National Institutes of Health.
loading and their interaction on the onset of mood disorders. British Nowinski, J., Baker, S., & Carroll, K. (1994). Twelve step facilitation
Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 97–101. therapy manual. Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse
Hirschfield, R. M. A., Lerman, G. L., Schless, A. P., Endicott, J., Licht- and Alcoholism.
enstaeder, S., & Clayton, P. J. (1977). Modified Life Events sections of O’Sullivan, K., Rynne, C., Miller, J., O’Sullivan, S., Fitzpatrick, V., Hux,
the Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Interview (PERI-M). Bethesda, M., et al. (1988). A follow-up study on alcoholics with and without
MD: National Institute of Mental Health. co-existing affective disorder. British Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 813–
Hosmer, D. W., & Lemeshow, S. (1999). Applied survival analysis: Re- 819.
gression modeling of time to event data. New York: Wiley. Project MATCH Research Group. (1998). Matching alcoholism treatments
Jones, B. T., & McMahon, J. (1994). Negative alcohol expectancy predicts to client heterogeneity: Treatment main effects and matching effects on
post-treatment abstinence survivorship: The whether, when and why of drinking during treatment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 59, 631– 639.
relapse to first drink. Addiction, 89, 1653–1665. Regier, D. A., Farmer, M. E., Rae, D. S., Locke, B. Z., Keith, S. J., Judd,
Judd, L. L., Paulus, M. J., Schettler, P. J., Akiskal, H. S., Endicott, J., Leon, L. L., et al. (1990). Comorbidity of mental disorders with alcohol and
A. C., et al. (2000). Does incomplete recovery from first lifetime major other drug abuse. Journal of the American Medical Association, 264,
depressive episode herald a chronic course of illness? American Journal 2511–2518.
of Psychiatry, 157, 1501–1504. Robins, L. N., Wing, J., Wittchen, H. U., Helzer, J. E., Babor, T. F., Burke,
Judd, L. L., Paulus, M. P., & Zeller, P. (1999). The role of residual J., et al. (1988). The Composite International Diagnostic Interview: An
subthreshold depressive symptoms in early episode relapse in unipolar epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different
major depressive disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56, 764 – diagnostic systems and in different cultures. Archives of General Psy-
765. chiatry, 45, 1069 –1077.
Kadden, K., Carroll, K., Donovan, D., Cooney, N., Monti, P., Abrams, D., Rychtarik, R. G., Prue, D. M., Rapp, S. R., & King, A. C. (1992).
et al. (1994). Cognitive-behavioral coping skills therapy manual. Be- Self-efficacy, aftercare, and relapse in a treatment program for alcohol-
thesda, MD: National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. ics. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 53, 435– 440.
Kanfer, R., & Zeiss, A. M. (1983). Depression, interpersonal setting, and Saunders, B., Baily, S., Phillips, M., & Allsop, S. (1993). Women with
judgments of self-efficacy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 92, 319 – alcohol problems: Do they relapse for reasons different to their male
329. counterparts? Addiction, 88, 1413–1422.
Kendler, K. S., Kuhn, J. W., & Prescott, C. A. (2004). Childhood sexual Sellman, J. D., & Joyce, P. R. (1996). Does depression predict relapse in
abuse, stressful life events and risk for major depression in women. the 6 months following treatment for men with alcohol dependence?
Psychological Medicine, 24, 1475–1482. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 30, 573–578.
SELF-EFFICACY, STRESS, AND TIME TO RELAPSE 57

Shen, X., Hackworth, J., McCabe, H., Lovett, L., Aumage, J., O’Neill, J., Tate, S. R., Brown, S. A., Unrod, M., & Ramo, D. E. (2004). Context of
& Bull, M. (2006). Characteristics of suicide from 1998 –2001 in a relapse for substance-dependent adults with and without comorbid psy-
metropolitan area. Death Studies, 30, 859 – 871. chiatric disorders. Addictive Behaviors, 29, 1707–1724.
Shiffman, S. (1989). Conceptual issues in the study of relapse. In M. Tate, S. R., McQuaid, J. R., & Brown, S. A. (2005). Characteristics of life
Gossop (Ed.), Relapse and addictive behavior (pp. 149 –179). New stressors predictive of substance treatment outcomes. Journal of Sub-
York: Tavistock/Rutledge. stance Abuse Treatment, 29, 107–115.
Singer, J. D., & Willett, J. B. (1993). It’s about time: Using discrete-time Velasquez, M. M., Carbonari, J. P., & DiClemente, C. C. (1999). Psychi-
survival analysis to study duration and the timing of events. Journal of atric severity and behavior change in alcoholism: The relation of the
Educational Statistics, 18, 155–195. transtheoretical model variables to psychiatric distress in dually diag-
Sitharthan, T., & Kavanagh, D. J. (1990). Role of self-efficacy in predict- nosed patients. Addictive Behaviors, 24, 481– 496.
ing outcomes from a programme for controlled drinking. Drug and Vuchinich, R. E., & Tucker, J. A. (1996). Alcoholic relapse, life events,
Alcohol Dependence, 27, 87–94. and behavioral theories of choice: A prospective analysis. Experimental
Sklar, S. M., Annis, H. M., & Turner, N. E. (1997). Development and and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, 19 –28.
validation of the Drug-Taking Confidence Questionnaire: A measure of Willenbring, M. L. (1986). Measurement of depression in alcoholics.
coping self-efficacy. Addictive Behaviors, 22, 655– 670. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 47, 367–372.
Smith, A. J., Hodgson, R. J., Bridgeman, K., & Shepherd, J. P. (2003). A Willett, J. B., & Singer, J. D. (1993). Investigating onset, cessation, relapse,
randomized controlled trial of a brief intervention after alcohol-related and recovery: Why you should, and how you can, use discrete-time
facial injury. Addiction, 98, 43–52. survival analysis to examine event occurrence. Journal of Consulting
Sobell, L. C., & Sobell, M. B. (1992). Timeline Follow-Back: A technique and Clinical Psychology, 61, 952–965.
for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption. In R. Litten & J. Allen Willett, J. B., Singer, J. D., & Martin, N. C. (1998). The design and
(Eds.), Measuring alcohol consumption: Psychosocial and biochemical analysis of longitudinal studies of development and psychopathology in
methods (pp. 41–72). Totowa, NJ: Humana Press. context: Statistical models and methodological recommendations. De-
Stephens, R. S., Wertz, J. S., & Roffman, R. A. (1993). Adult marijuana velopment and Psychopathology, 10, 395– 426.
dependence. In J. S. Baer, G. A. Marlatt, & R. J. McMahon (Eds.), Witkiewitz, K., & Marlatt, G. A. (2004). Relapse prevention for alcohol
Addictive behaviors across the life span: Prevention and policy issues and drug problems. American Psychologist, 59, 224 –235.
(pp. 202–218). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
Tate, S. R. (2000). Development of a substance relapse model integrating
temporally proximal and distal contextual factors. Dissertation Abstracts Received September 26, 2006
International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 61, 2784. (UMI Revision received May 23, 2007
No. 9974113). Accepted May 24, 2007 䡲

E-Mail Notification of Your Latest Issue Online!


Would you like to know when the next issue of your favorite APA journal will be available
online? This service is now available to you. Sign up at http://notify.apa.org/ and you will be
notified by e-mail when issues of interest to you become available!

Potrebbero piacerti anche