Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

LSHSS

A Comparison of Oral and Written


English Styles in African American
Students at Different Stages
of Writing Development
Lennette J. Ivy
University of Mississippi, Oxford
Julie J. Masterson
Missouri State University, Springfield

W riting, a component of the human productive


language system, is essential to literacy devel-
opment (Glassner, 1981; Kroll, 1981; Scott &
Rogers, 1996) and successful academic achievement (Stewart,
1985). In years past, literacy has been equated more with reading, a
2004). Research investigating the writing skills of African American
(AA) school-age students is emerging (Craig et al., 2009; Scott &
Rogers, 1996; Thomas-Tate et al., 2006; Thompson et al., 2004;
Wolfram & Whiteman, 1971). National assessments of writing skills
in students report a divergence in the skills of AA students compared
receptive process, than with writing, an expressive process (Hendrix, to those of Caucasian students (Greenwald et al., 1999; Horkay,
1981). As a result, national concerns regarding literacy problems 2000; Jerry & Ballator, 1999; Persky, Daane, & Jin, 2003).
have focused mostly on reading, excluding writing in many instances Differences in writing levels between AA and Caucasian stu-
(Whiteman & Hall, 1981). In recent years, more efforts have been dents across the United States show trends similar to documented
made to address writing as an important aspect of literacy develop- differences in reading levels between AA and Caucasian students.
ment (Apel & Thomas-Tate, 2009; American Speech-Language- Reports from the 2002 National Assessment of Educational Pro-
Hearing Association [ASHA], 2000; Berninger, Abbott, & Swanson, gress (NAEP) in Writing indicate that approximately 75% of AA
2010; Bloome, Champion, Katz, Morton, & Muldrow, 2001; Craig fourth and eighth graders reached at or above the basic level of
& Washington, 2004b; Craig, Zhang, Hensel, & Quinn, 2009; achievement, whereas 90% of Caucasian (same-grade) students at-
Greenwald, Persky, Campbell, & Mazzeo, 1999; Thomas-Tate, tained the same levels of performance (Persky et al., 2003). The
Apel, & Miskowski, 2006; Thompson, Craig, & Washington, 2007 NAEP writing assessment was administered to eighth graders,

ABSTRACT: Purpose: The purpose of this investigation was to Results: Findings indicated comparable use of dialect in spoken
compare the rates of using African American English (AAE) and written modalities for 3rd graders, but a difference in use
grammatical features in spoken and written language at different between the modalities for 8th graders. The 8th graders used more
points in literacy development. Based on Kroll’s model (1981), dialectal features in speaking than writing.
a high degree of similarity in use between the modalities was Conclusion: These results suggest that there is likely a period
expected at Grade 3, and lower similarity was expected at Grade 8. in writing development when speakers of AAE learn to dialect
Method: Spoken and written language samples were analyzed for switch in their writing.
the occurrence of 6 AAE morphosyntactic features. Fifteen third
graders and 15 eighth graders were asked to respond to interview
questions and to retell stories in both modalities. Percentage use of
the AAE grammatical features and a dialectal density measure KEY WORDS: African American English, spoken language,
were used to measure rates of AAE occurrence. written language, development, dialect density

LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 42 • 31–40 • January 2011 * American Speech-Language-Hearing Association 31
and those results revealed that approximately 81% of AA eighth Craig, 2006; Green, 2002; van Keulen et al., 1998; Wolfram et al.,
graders performed at or above the basic level of achievement com- 1999).
pared to 93% of Caucasian eighth graders (Salahu-Din, Persky, Earlier research has shown that features of AAE do occur in the
& Miller, 2008). Although the 2007 NAEP results indicate a decrease writing of AA children who are speakers of the dialect (Cronnell,
in the gap compared to the 2002 findings, the results continue to 1981; Smitherman, 1993; Terrebonne & Terrebonne, 1976;
show evidence of an achievement gap between majority and non- Whiteman, 1981; Wolfram & Schilling-Estes, 1998; Wolfram &
majority schoolchildren. Whiteman, 1971). The features frequently found in the writing
The results from NAEP suggest that our educational system con- of AAE speakers include (a) verbal –s absence (e.g., She go__),
tinues to lag in its ability to help AA students close this academic (b) plural –s absence (e.g., four mile_), (c) possessive –s absence
gap. One contributing factor is that some teachers and AA students (e.g., John hat), (d) –ed absence resulting from consonant cluster
come from different cultural backgrounds, so they bring to the reduction (e.g., Yesterday they miss), and (e) copula is and are
educational setting different expectations about language use absence (e.g., We going to the game.). AAE features that have
(Wolfram, Adger, & Christian, 1999). The speech of AA children appeared relatively infrequently in writing when compared with
differs from the English used in schools in systematic ways, and their incidence in spoken language include (a) multiple negation,
AA speech patterns should be considered by teachers when develop- (b) the use of ain’t, and (c) the use of habitual be. Social evalua-
ing lessons that are used for literacy instruction. In order to do tion may explain why some AAE features are more susceptible to
this effectively, it is important for educators and policy makers to reflection in writing than others. Items that are highly stigmatized
understand how African American English (AAE) differs linguis- (e.g., ain’t) are probably the first to be changed during the course
tically and systematically from classroom English (Green, 2002). of a child’s education (Whiteman, 1981; Wolfram & Schilling-
AAE is a variety of English that is spoken by some, but not all, Estes, 1998). Although AAE is commonly reflected by the omis-
AAs. The amount of AAE used by AA children who are speakers sion of inflectional suffixes (Terrebonne & Terrebonne, 1976;
of the dialect is dependent on various factors such as age, socio- Whiteman, 1981; Wolfram & Schilling-Estes, 1998; Wolfram &
economic status (SES), situational contexts, and overall linguistic Whiteman, 1971), dialect influence is not solely responsible for
complexity (Battle, 1996; Craig & Washington, 1994, 2000; Isaac, such omission in writing. The developmental process of acquiring
1996; Washington & Craig, 1994, 1998; Washington, Craig, & inflectional suffixes in writing also could be a contributing factor.
Kushmaul, 1998). Researchers agree that AAE is not random, care- Support for this is found in Whiteman’s (1981) study comparing
less, or deviant in nature; rather, it is a structured linguistic system eighth-grade AA and Caucasian students, which revealed some
(Craig et al., 2009; Green, 2002; Rickford, 1999). AAE is char- verbal –s absence and plural –s absence in the writing of both
acterized by unique phonological, morphological, syntactic, and groups of children.
semantic features (Green, 2002; Mufwene, 1998; Rickford, 1999). The ability to code switch or dialect shift from AAE to MAE is a
Although AAE is a variety of English, there are distinct system- skill that is acquired gradually by AA school-aged children. Craig
atic contrasts in the linguistic features between this variety and and Washington (2004a) reported dialect shifting at first grade
Mainstream American English (MAE), particularly in the phono- for spoken language in their investigation of students in preschool
logical and morphosyntactic systems (Apel & Thomas-Tate, 2009; through fifth grade. From data elicited through picture descriptions,
Bailey & Thomas, 1998; Craig et al., 2009; Green, 2002; Rickford, Craig and Washington observed a decrease in the use of zero past
1999; Stockman, 1996). Some common phonological examples tense, zero plural, and zero –ing between kindergarten and first
of AAE use include consonant cluster reduction when the cluster is through fifth grades. Their results suggest that developmental fac-
followed by an obstruent with the same place of articulation (e.g., tors could contribute to the differences in the features used.
firs time for first time), unstressed syllable deletion (e.g., fore for Similarly, Craig, Thompson, Washington, and Potter (2003)
before), and the substitution of t/d and f/v in words in which MAE observed a decrease in the usage rate of phonological features of
requires th. It is important to note that occurrences of these features AAE during oral reading at third grade. Students in the higher grades
are based on positional constraints. Morphosyntactic examples of (i.e., third through fifth grade) read more passages and longer
AAE use include use of the indefinite article a regardless of the passages, yielding more words and more opportunities for AAE
vowel context (e.g., I have a orange); omission of inflectional mark- productions. The dialect density measure (DDM) was used to
ers such as past tense –ed (e.g., the door is clos__), plural –s (e.g., control for differences in the number of words produced by the
I have two shoe_), and possessive –s (e.g., the boy_ grandmotherI); participants. Craig et al. observed grade-level differences in the use
and the third person singular present tense –s marking (e.g., he of AAE phonological and morphophonological features between
walk for he walks) (Apel & Thomas-Tate, 2009; Bailey & Thomas, second-grade DDMs and third-grade DDMs. The DDMs were two
1998; Craig & Washington, 2004a, 2004b; Green, 2002; Rickford, to three times greater for the second grade when compared to the
1999; Stockman, 1996). other grade levels. DDMs based on morphological features were
Communication and culture are inseparable phenomena, and not significantly different among the grade levels.
children express their culture through their communication styles Of the nine phonological features examined in Craig et al.
(van Keulen, Weddington, & DeBose, 1998). Children whose (2003), all were produced at least once during the oral reading,
speech consists of a high frequency of AAE use may find school except for the feature devoicing of final consonants (e.g., /hIs/ for
challenging, particularly if there is a teacher–child mismatch in /hIz/), which was not produced even though there were multiple
cultural backgrounds. Because most schools are aligned with middle- opportunities. Monophthongization of diphthongs (e.g., /Ar/ for
class cultural traditions, teachers may perceive the speech of chil- /AOr/), substitutions for dental fricatives, and consonant cluster
dren who speak AAE as deviant. Such attitudes toward AAE can reduction were used widely across the participants. Of the morpho-
result in negative expectations that might affect the teacher’s behav- syntactic features produced, zero past tense, zero article, and
ior toward children who speak this variety of English (Conner & indefinite article were observed across the participants. These

32 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 42 • 31–40 • January 2011
findings indicate that dialect shifting when speaking appears at two minimal. However, students in middle school who are at Phase 4
periods during early elementary grades—at first grade for spoken and typically use unique styles for writing versus speaking might be
discourse, and at third grade for oral reading. expected to manifest maximum dialect shifting.
A more recent investigation by Craig et al. (2009) demonstrated The purpose of the current study was to investigate the occur-
the ability of AA students to dialect shift in their writing. This study rence of a developmental shift in writing development among
examined the relationship between reading achievement scores AA school-aged children. We addressed the following research
and dialect shifting ability from oral to written language in first questions:
through fifth grade. The investigators used a picture description task & Do oral and written language samples elicited from AA third
to elicit oral samples and an untimed story generation task to elicit graders differ in their rate of use of AAE grammatical features?
written samples. The AAE features coded included 23 morpho-
& Do oral and written language samples elicited from AA eighth
syntactic features, nine phonological features, and nine morpho-
graders differ in their rate of use of AAE grammatical features?
phonological features. DDMs based on use of all AAE features
were calculated for the oral and written narratives. Whereas the & Does grade level affect the rate of use of AAE grammatical
overall mean for DDM in the oral narrative task was 0.103, the features in spoken versus written language?
mean DDM in the writing task was 0.04. The differences between
the two modalities were significant and were associated with a large
effect size (d = 0.85).
In order to investigate the relationship between reading perfor- METHOD
mance and dialect shifting from oral to written language, Craig
et al. (2009) standardized the DDMs for each grade. Consequently, Participants
grade was not used as an independent variable when exploring
Thirty AA students attending elementary and middle schools
the association between dialect shifting and reading skills. Craig
in two school districts in north Mississippi participated in this
et al. found that the rate of AAE used in the writing task predicted
study—15 third graders (9 males, 6 females) ranging in age from
reading achievement; however, use of AAE in speaking did not.
8;0 (years;months) to 9;11 (Mage = 8;4), and 15 eighth graders
These findings indicate that the ability to dialect shift from AAE
(5 males, 10 females) ranging in age from 13;8 to 14;9 (Mage = 13;6).
to MAE in writing has a positive impact on reading outcomes
Approval to conduct research with these participants was obtained
beyond the influences that represent good writing skills. Craig et al.
from the Internal Review Board at The University of Memphis.
cautioned that although their findings documented a relationship
Grades 3 and 8 were selected based on Kroll’s (1981) model of
between dialect shifting and reading achievement, the direction
writing development and implications from previous investigations
of this relationship is not known.
(Carlisle, 1996; Gillam & Johnston, 1992; Kroll, 1981; Kroll &
The studies previously reviewed indicate that dialect shifting
Lemper, 1981; Rubin, Patterson, & Kantor; 1991; Windsor, Scott,
begins in the early grades, manifested first through contextual
& Street, 2000). Third graders were selected for this study because
differences in oral language and later through differences between
they represent a developmental level before the differentiation
oral and written language. However, to date, there has not been
between spoken and written language, and eighth graders were
an investigation regarding developmental influences on dialect
selected because they represent a level well beyond when the
shifting from oral to written language.
differentiation is expected to occur. We expected the similarities be-
Kroll (1981) proposed a model of writing development that high-
tween spoken and written language used by the third graders to
lights the difference between speaking and writing, which varies at
include comparable occurrences of dialect use in both modalities.
different points in development. The first phase, preparation (i.e.,
The shift to modality differentiation by eighth grade should involve
emergent), is the period when children learn the technical skills to
greater formality in writing, and consequently, less dialect use.
represent the written symbols they produce in speech. This phase
Information regarding SES was available for 8 third graders and
characterizes the skills of children who are in their late preschool and
10 eighth graders. All but one student in each grade level qualified
kindergarten years. Phase 2, consolidation of oral and written lan-
for the school’s free lunch program. The school district in which
guage, is the period when children’s writing resembles their speech.
the remaining participants were enrolled would not release informa-
This phase characterizes the skills of children in the primary ele-
tion regarding eligibility or ineligibility to participate in the fed-
mentary grades. Phase 3, differentiation of oral and written language,
erally funded free or reduced-price lunch program.
is the period when children learn the differences in writing and
All potential participants were passing at grade level in reading
speaking with regard to style and structure. This phase characterizes
and language arts in the classroom and on the Mississippi Cur-
the skills that can be found in children in fourth and fifth grades
riculum Test (Mississippi Department of Education, 2007). The
(Kroll, 1981; Singer, 1995). Phase 4, systematic integration of speak-
Mississippi Curriculum Test measures students’ academic achieve-
ing and writing, is the period when children are learning to apply the
ment in reading, language, and mathematics in Grades 2 through 8.
rules and styles from either mode (i.e., speaking or writing). This
None of the participants had ever been enrolled in special education
phase characterizes the skills of mature or conventionally literate
services as reported by their teachers and the school counselors. All
individuals (Kroll, 1981; Singer, 1995; Sulzby, 1996). The model
participants were speakers of AAE.
proposed by Kroll suggests a relationship between speaking and
writing that changes across the course of writing development.
Kroll’s model might be used to make predictions regarding dialectal Procedure
shifts between oral and written language. During the primary ele-
mentary grades, when children are at Phase 2 and their writing Each participant was seen individually for three sessions in the
resembles their speech, expectations for dialect shifting would be school setting. The length of each session was 40–50 min. During

Ivy & Masterson: A Comparison of Oral and Written English 33


the first session, the students were (a) engaged in an informal con- determined for all spoken samples. A graduate student viewed the
versation to determine if they were AAE speakers, (b) given a videotapes and transcribed approximately one third of each spoken
hearing screening, and (c) administered the Peabody Picture Vocab- sample into a word processor. Percentage of agreement was de-
ulary Test—III (PPVT–III; Dunn & Dunn, 1997). During the termined by dividing the total number of agreements by the total
second session, the participants performed two of the four experi- number of agreements plus the number of disagreements between
mental tasks. During the third session, the participants performed the first author and the second viewer and multiplying the total by
the two remaining experimental tasks. 100. The percentage of agreement for the transcriptions was 90%.
Receptive language abilities were documented with the
PPVT–III, which was reported to be a relatively culturally fair Measures of AAE Use
assessment of at-risk AA preschool children (Washington & Craig,
1999). Norm-referenced, expressive spoken and written language Percentage occurrence of AAE features. Six AAE grammatical
measures were not used for inclusion in this investigation. At the features were included in this investigation: (a) absence of verbal –s,
time of data collection, there was no culturally valid spoken lan- (b) absence of plural –s, (c) absence of possessive –s, (d) absence
guage measure available to assess the skills of AA children (Craig of regular past tense –ed, (e) absence of regular be copula, and
& Washington, 2002; Craig, Washington, & Thompson-Porter, 1998; (f ) absence of be auxiliary in spoken and written narrative sam-
Washington, 1996; Washington & Craig, 1994, 1998; Washington ples. These specific AAE features were chosen because previous
et al., 1998). All students had normal hearing sensitivity at the research indicated that they occur frequently within the writing of
frequencies of 1000 Hz, 2000 Hz, and 4000 Hz at 25 db. AA students (Cronnell, 1981, 1984; Smitherman, 1993; Terrebonne
The participants performed four experimental tasks: (a) spoken & Terrebonne, 1976; Whiteman, 1981; Wolfram & Schilling-Estes,
video narration, (b) written video narration, (c) spoken interview, 1998; Wolfram & Whiteman, 1971). Opportunities (i.e., obligatory
and (d) written interview. The task order was counterbalanced context for use of the feature in MAE) to use each feature were
across participants. identified, and actual occurrences of each feature were noted. The
Video narration task. Video narration tasks provide the advan- total number of opportunities for the six features was divided by the
tages of content stability, a high interest level, and high processing number of occurrences of the six features to determine the overall
demands as a language sampling context (Dollaghan, Campbell, percentage occurrence. A sample of the descriptions used to identify
& Tomlin, 1990). The video narration tasks were expected to pro- the opportunities and occurrences of the AAE features are shown in
vide opportunities for use of the past tense, possessive, and plural the Appendix.
morphological structures. Video segments of two Little Rascals Coding reliability for opportunities and occurrences of AAE
episodes (i.e., “The Pooch” [Roach & McGowan, 1932] and “The features was determined by selecting one fourth of each spoken
First Roundup” [Roach & Meins, 1934]) were used. The length of the and written sample for reliability calculations. The samples were
video segments was 10 min, to increase the chances of obtaining recoded for the opportunities and occurrences of AAE features by
samples of adequate length (Scott & Windsor, 2000; Windsor et al., three AA graduate students who were speakers of AAE. Two of the
2000). The informality of the content provided opportunities for par- students were in their third semester of graduate study, and one
ticipants to use more instances of AAE in their spoken and written was in the fourth semester. First, the students were trained on the
language samples (Newkirk, 2001; Washington & Craig, 1994, 1998; coding procedures. Each student was given spoken and written
Washington et al., 1998). The video presentations were counter- samples for 10 participants (i.e., five from both grade levels), and
balanced to optimally minimize an order of video interference effect. they met with the first author for a group training session. Training
After the video was completed, participants were instructed to included (a) viewing the videos to provide a perspective for the
tell or write about the content. They were allotted 3 min to complete related samples, (b) examining the definitions for coding AAE
spoken narratives and 10 min to complete written narratives. Par- features, (c) coding the samples of two participants for a total of
ticipants were instructed not to worry about spelling, but to write eight samples, and (d) discussing agreements and disagreements as
words the way they thought they should be spelled. related to the definitions for coding. Percentage of reliability was
Interview task. The use of an interview context for eliciting a calculated by dividing the total number of agreements by the total
spontaneous language sample has been demonstrated as effective number of agreements plus the number of disagreements between
and reliable (Evans & Craig, 1992; Washington et al., 1998). The the first author and the three students, and multiplying the total by
first author provided the participants with facts from general themes 100. The reliability for opportunities for the use of AAE features
as conversational starters. After each fact was presented, the par- yielded a percentage of agreement of 91%. The reliability for the
ticipants were asked two to four open-ended questions to which actual occurrence of AAE feature use yielded a percentage of agree-
there were no right or wrong answers, and no special knowledge ment of 80%. Despite the training provided to the student coders,
was required. There were 16 facts presented, with a total of 44 the low agreement regarding occurrences was due to errors made
conversational questions. Half of the questions were presented in by the student coders, who worked independently.
each modality. DDM. The amount of AAE that occurs in a spontaneous lan-
Written samples were reviewed with each participant by having guage sample varies among children, and the contexts of some AAE
them read what they had written immediately upon completion features are not easily detectable until the feature has been produced
of the task to resolve instances of illegible handwriting and mis- (i.e., nonspecific linguistic contexts that depend on the speaker’s
spellings. Spoken samples were recorded using a Panasonic Video intent). Consequently, a DDM was calculated for the samples in
Cassette Recorder Model PV-L352. An Emerson color TV-VCR both spoken and written conditions. The DDM is not affected by
Model EWC1301 was used to play back participants’ spoken nar- opportunities for AAE use, but rather reflects the proportion of
ratives, and responses were transcribed verbatim from the video- dialectal use in overall linguistic production (Craig & Washington,
tapes into a word processor. Reliability of the transcriptions was 2000, 2002; Craig et al., 1998). In the current study, the DDM was

34 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 42 • 31–40 • January 2011
calculated by summing the number of times the six AAE gram- p = .542. They used AAE features similarly in both modalities
matical features were used and dividing this number by the number (Mspeaking = 10.1, SD = 7.6; Mwriting = 12.3, SD = 11.2). The effect
of words within the first 50 complete communication units (CCUs) size estimate (Cohen’s d) was .33, indicating a small difference
in each sample. A CCU is a type of C-unit, which is an indepen- between AAE use in each modality by the third graders. However,
dent clause and its modifiers. The C-unit reflects less cultural– there was a statistically significant difference for the eighth graders,
linguistic bias because it is based on words rather than morphemes, t(1, 28) = 7.215, p < .0001. These results indicate that the eighth
which are affected by dialectal variation (Nelson, 2010). Four graders used AAE features significantly more when speaking
spoken samples from third graders contained <50 CCUs (i.e., 15, (M = 12.4, SD = 6.1) than when writing (M = 3.3, SD = 3.3). There
29, 47, 48), and two spoken samples from eighth graders contained was a large effect size (1.08) associated with the difference between
<50 CCUs (i.e., 42, 46). All CCUs contained in the six samples AAE use in each modality by the eighth graders.
were included for analysis. The written samples were expected to
have fewer CCUs because of differences in the method of produc- DDM
tion and the time required to make the product. Consequently, all
CCUs produced in the written samples of the third graders were DDMs were based on the proportion of AAE features used to the
included in the calculation of the DDM. The number of CCUs in number of words in the first 50 CCUs. These results are shown in
the third graders’ written samples ranged from 20 to 42. There were Table 2. With the DDM as the dependent measure, the ANOVA
two eighth-grade written samples that contained 50 CCUs. The re- yielded a statistically significant main effect for modality, F(1, 28) =
maining 13 samples ranged in length from 25 to 49 CCUs. 6.3, p = .018, a significant grade effect, F(1, 28) = 4.89, p = .035,
and a significant interaction between modality and grade, F(1, 28) =
14.198, p = .001. To follow up the significant interaction, grade was
held constant and t tests on modality were performed. There was
RESULTS no difference in DDM between spoken and written language sam-
ples produced by the third graders, t(1, 14) = .734, p = .475.
This study employed a mixed design, with modality (spoken, Similarly, the estimated effect size (d = .10) indicated very small
written) as the within-subjects variable and grade (third, eighth) as modality differences in the DDMs of the third graders. In the eighth
the between-subjects variable. The dependent variables were the graders, the spoken samples were characterized by significantly
overall percentage occurrence of six AAE features and dialectal
higher dialectal density than the written samples, t(1, 14) = .6.09,
density (proportion of AAE features to words). SPSS (1989–2007) p < .0001. The effect size associated with this difference also was
software was used to run two 2 (grade) × 2 (modality) analyses large (d = 1.08).
of variance (ANOVAs) for repeated measures in order to analyze
the data. Standard effect sizes (Cohen’s d) also were calculated on
each comparison to determine the importance, or meaningfulness,
of the modality differences for each grade. According to Cohen DISCUSSION
(1977), a d of .25 is a small effect, .50 is a medium effect, and ≥.80
is a large effect. The purpose of this investigation was to compare the occurrence
of AAE in spoken and written language to determine if the per-
Percentage Occurrence of AAE Features centage occurrence of dialectal use was affected by developmental
influences on modality differences. The specific questions were
The overall percentage occurrence of AAE features was calcu- whether the use of AAE differed in spoken versus written language
lated by dividing the total number of opportunities for all six fea- and if grade level influenced the relative use of dialect in each
tures to occur by the number of times the AAE features actually modality. The percentage occurrence for six AAE features and a
occurred. These results are given in Table 1. On the percentage measure of dialectal density were used to measure dialect use in
occurrence of AAE features, the ANOVA revealed no statistically these modalities at the two grade levels.
significant main effect for modality, F(1, 28) = 3.674, p = .066, The third graders used AAE features with relatively equal fre-
or grade, F(1, 28) = 2.550, p = .122. However, there was a sig- quencies in their speech and writing, whereas the eighth graders
nificant interaction between grade and modality, F(1, 28) = 9.546, decreased their use of AAE during writing. There were differences
p = .004. To follow up the significant interaction, grade was held in the use of AAE features in written language between the two
constant and t tests on modality were performed. For the third graders, grade levels but not in spoken language. The lack of difference in
the difference was not statistically significant, t(1,14) = .625, the spoken modality between the two grades could be attributed to

Table 1. Percentage occurrence of African American English (AAE) in grammatical structures providing
opportunities by grade and modality.

Spoken Written
Grade M SD Range M SD Range t p d

Third 10.1 7.6 0–25.6 12.3 11.2 2.0–38.5 .625 .542 0.33
Eighth 12.4 6.1 3.7–26.5 3.3 3.3 0–12.5 7.215 < .0001 1.08

Ivy & Masterson: A Comparison of Oral and Written English 35


Table 2. Dialectal density measures by grade and modality.

Spoken Written
Grade M SD Range M SD Range t p d

Third 0.011 0.007 .000–.023 0.012 0.006 .003–.030 .734 .475 0.10
Eighth 0.013 0.008 .002–.022 0.004 0.004 .000–.013 6.09 < .0001 1.44

several factors: (a) the tasks the students were asked to complete this ability. Because Craig et al. standardized DDMs for each
were intended to elicit AAE, (b) the interactions between the stu- grade, their findings do not address whether there were differences
dents and the conversational partner were relatively informal, and in dialect shifting across grades. The current investigation suggests
(c) the conversational partner was AA. The use of spoken AAE that dialect shifting in writing is likely to begin occurring devel-
within this context was appropriate for both grade levels. opmentally among AA students after third grade.
It should be noted that the elicitation conditions (narrative, inter- In the current investigation, the DDM for the third graders was
view) were identical for both modalities. Nevertheless, the eighth 0.011 and 0.013 for spoken and written, respectively, and .012
graders used fewer AAE features in the formal register, writing. and .004, respectively, for the eighth graders. These results were
These findings suggest that the older AA children possessed morpho- considerably lower when compared to those reported by Craig et al.
syntactic awareness skills that enabled them to dialect shift between (2009), who found DDMs of 0.103 and 0.040 for speech and writ-
spoken and written modalities. Further, the data support the oc- ing, respectively. The differences in density for both modalities
currence of a developmental shift in the older students’ use of AAE between the two studies are likely due to differences in sampling
features during the acquisition of writing skills. The findings of this conditions. In the current investigation, samples were collected by
investigation are discussed in terms of (a) supporting evidence asking the participants to narrate a video and respond to interview
for there being a sociocultural relationship in the development of questions. Craig et al. used a picture description task to elicit
speaking and writing that changes during the span of development oral language samples and untimed story generations about topics
and (b) the potential implications for pedagogical styles, clinical of choice to elicit written language samples. Perhaps the video
strategies, and future research. and interview tasks were perceived as somewhat academic by our
Kroll’s (1981) model of writing development proposed that the participants, and consequently resulted in less overall use of AAE.
differences and similarities between the oral and written discourse The current study has several potential implications for prac-
of typically developing children may be the result of develop- tice. First, educators should encourage the development of meta-
mental trends in acquiring these language skills. The similar use awareness of dialect shifting by making the need for this skill as
of AAE in oral and written conditions by the third graders in the transparent and explicit as possible to their students. This can be
current study are in line with Kroll’s writing development model, achieved by having students practice dialect shifting between their
indicating that young writers have an immature ability to reflect home language and school language while they are engaged in
on the differences between speech and writing. In other words, classroom writing activities. For example, the AAE feature, plural
the youngest writers in this study had not yet developed a meta- –s absence, could be targeted during a language activity unit on
awareness of dialect shifting (Collins, 1981; Kroll, 1978; Vygotsky, plurals. First, the clinician or teacher could begin the session with an
1934/1962) in their speech or writing. On the other hand, the introduction to the concept of plurals, and by pronouncing all words
eighth graders (13- and 14-year-olds) demonstrated skills of meta- to be used in the unit, drawing attention to the various sounds of
awareness of dialect shifting between speaking and writing. These plural –s. Next, the teacher could read sentences to the students
data corroborate research (Kroll, 1981; Singer, 1995; Sulzby, containing words with the plural –s and point out the salient clues
1996) that proposes a decrease in the interrelatedness of speech (e.g., number words two and above; and demonstrative pronouns
and writing in the course of writing development, with the writing these and those) in the sentences that indicate the need for plural –s.
register becoming relatively more formal. Afterward, the students could engage in activities to identify the
The results also suggest that as typically developing children context clues in sentences. Next, various writing activities could
matriculate through grade school, they should acquire the ability be assigned to the students to complete in class and for homework
to apply the rules of writing that will allow them to differentiate their (Seals, Pollard-Durodola, Foorman, & Bradley, 2007). In-class
writing style from their speaking style, no matter what dialect review of the written activities would strengthen the students’ meta-
they speak. This investigation revealed that a transition does occur awareness of the morphosyntactic feature of plural –s. If clinicians
as children get older, but it also suggests that dialect speakers and teachers began this type of practice (i.e., scaffolding) at early
may require some scaffolding to heighten their awareness of the levels of development, students may be more likely to make the
reality that the rules of academic writing may contrast with the rules developmental transition to dialect shifting in formal writing tasks.
and use of their spoken dialect. The results of the current study In addition, children with weaker language skills will get the scaf-
support the conclusions reported by Craig et al. (2009) and Thompson folding they need to make the transition between their home
et al. (2004). Like Craig et al., this investigation confirms that language and school language.
many AAE-speaking students are able to dialect shift. Whereas 85% The development of meta-awareness skills related to dialect
of the participants in the Craig et al. study demonstrated this ability, shifting can positively impact the academic achievement of AAE
only 67% of the participants in the current study demonstrated dialect speakers. As predicted by the dialect shifting–reading

36 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 42 • 31–40 • January 2011
achievement hypothesis (Craig et al., 2009) and noted by Thompson writing at the college and university level. If university writing
et al. (2004), children with dialect shifting ability have better centers and teachers are going to help AA students with their writing
academic performance than children without this meta-awareness difficulties due to dialect interference, more research is needed to
skill. Therefore, it stands to reason that purposeful guidance by determine if contrastive analysis is beneficial at this point in writing
clinicians or teachers in the skill of shifting from the home language development. Finally, the results of the typically developing AA
to the school language could positively affect the persistent achieve- students in this investigation may have implications for best prac-
ment gap that our educational system continues to experience. tices when providing language intervention to children with lan-
Whether this skill is termed code-switching, dialect shifting, or guage disorders who speak AAE. Dialect shifting in children with
meta-awareness, the end result is children who are bi-dialectal, with language disorder is an area of research to be investigated.
the ability to manage the linguistic demands (i.e., both spoken and
written) of their classes and the sociocultural language demands
of their families, friends, and communities.
This comparison of oral and written English in typically devel- REFERENCES
oping AA third and eighth graders adds to our knowledge base American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (2000). Roles and
and to the developmental profile regarding the acquisition of lan- responsibilities of speech-language pathologists with respect to reading
guage skills in this population. The results indicate that typically and writing in children and adolescents. Rockville, MD: Author.
developing AA children should be expected to acquire language Apel, K., & Thomas-Tate, S. (2009). Morphological awareness skills of
skills in the same developmental time frame as other English- fourth-grade African American students. Language, Speech, and Hearing
speaking children; however, the sociolinguistic differences of these Services in Schools, 40, 312–324.
children must be an aspect of consideration in helping them reach Bailey, G., & Thomas, E. (1998). Some aspects of African-American
their full academic potential. Vernacular English. In S. S. Mufwene, J. R. Rickford, G. Bailey, &
One of the challenges in studies that compare spoken and written J. Baugh (Eds.), African-American English: Structure, history and use
language use is to determine an appropriate system for coding in (pp. 85–109). New York, NY: Routledge.
each. In this investigation, we used a single coding system to iden- Battle, D. E. (1996). Language learning and use by African American
tify the AAE forms in both the oral and written samples. Although children. Topics in Language Disorders, 16(4), 22–37.
language produced in oral and written forms is similar, speech Berninger, V., Abbott, R., & Swanson, H. (2010). Relationship of word-
and writing are not completely parallel to one another. For example, and sentence-level working memory to reading and writing in second,
some irregular forms of past tense words were included in this study fourth, and sixth grade. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in
as opportunities for regular past tense –ed absence (e.g., kept and Schools, 41, 179–193.
left) based on the fact that the irregular past tense is marked after Bloome, D., Champion, T., Katz, L., Morton, M. B., & Muldrow, R.
a consonant without the addition of a syllable. However, considera- (2001). Spoken and written narrative development: African American
tion was not given to the sound changes that occur when the ir- preschoolers as storytellers and storymakers. In J. L. Harris, A. G. Kamhi,
regular past tense forms are spoken. For example, there is vowel & K. E. Pollock (Eds.), Literacy in African American communities
alternation when the past tense of keep (i.e., kept) is spoken (e.g., (pp. 45–76). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
/i/ changes to /e/). Likewise, the production of left as the irregular Carlisle, J. F. (1996). An exploratory study of morphological errors in
past tense of leave results in a consonant change (e.g., /v/ changes children’s written stories. Reading and Writing, 8, 61–72.
to /f/). In their written forms, the past tense of keep and leave are un- Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences.
questionably irregular past tense absence, not regular past tense –ed New York, NY: Academic Press.
absence (e.g., kept not keeped, and left not leaved ). Implications Collins, J. L. (1981). Spoken language and the development of writing
of coding decisions such as these should be considered in future abilities (Report No. CS 206 187). Retrieved from ERIC database.
studies. (ED199187)
It should be noted that we were given information regarding the Conner, C. M., & Craig, H. K. (2006). African American preschoolers’
SES of our participants from only some of the schools. Conse- language, emergent literacy skills, and use of African American English:
quently, we can document that 57% of the participants came from A complex relation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
households with low incomes. Future research is necessary to de- Research, 49, 771–792.
termine whether our findings are specific to AAE speakers from Craig, H. K., Thompson, C. A., Washington, J. A., & Potter, S. L.
low-SES environments or can be expected from most AAE users. (2003). Phonological features of child African American English. Journal
The current findings present several areas for additional of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 623–635.
research. A comparison of the writing of AA children taught to use a Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (1994). The complex syntax skills
contrastive analysis approach between their speech and their writing of poor, urban, African-American preschoolers at school entry. Language,
and a control group of AA students without such training strate- Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25, 181–190.
gies could increase our knowledge of effective and efficient scaf- Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2000). An assessment battery for
folding techniques. Investigations of this type could examine the identifying language impairments in African American children. Journal
effects of various factors on scaffolding techniques such as SES, of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 366–379.
gender differences, the onset of the training, and genre differences. Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2002). Oral language expectations
Such investigations could potentially impact the achievement gap for African-American preschoolers and kindergartners. American Journal
in a positive manner. Educators would have evidence that peda- of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 59–70.
gogical scaffolding provided to AA children could increase aca- Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2004a). Grade-related changes in the
demic achievement and decrease the achievement gap in writing. production of African American English. Journal of Speech, Language,
Also, the findings from this investigation have implications for and Hearing Research, 47, 450–463.

Ivy & Masterson: A Comparison of Oral and Written English 37


Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2004b). Language variation and Mufwene, S. S. (1998). The structure of the noun phrase in African-
literacy learning. In K. Apel, B. Ehren, E. Silliman, & C. A. Stone (Eds.), American vernacular English. In S. S. Mufwene, J. R. Rickford, G. Bailey,
Handbook of language and literacy development and disorders & J. Baugh (Eds.), African-American English: Structure, history and use
(pp. 228–247). New York, NY: Guilford Press. (pp. 69–81). New York, NY: Routledge.
Craig, H. K., Washington, J. A., & Thompson-Porter, C. (1998). Nelson, N. W. (2010). Language and literacy disorders: Infancy through
Average C-unit lengths in the discourse of African American children adolescence. Boston, MA: Pearson Education.
from low-income, urban homes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Newkirk, B. L. (2001, April). Regional differences and selected forms of
Hearing Research, 41, 433–444. African American English in 3 year old African American Head Start
Craig, H. K., Zhang, L., Hensel, S. L., & Quinn, E. J. (2009). African students. Paper presented at the annual conference of the National Black
American English-speaking students: An examination of the relationship Association of Speech, Language and Hearing, Milwaukee, WI.
between dialect shifting and reading outcomes. Journal of Speech, Persky, H. R., Daane, M. C., & Jin, Y. (2003). The Nation’s report card:
Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 839–855. Writing 2002 (NCES 2003-529). Washington, DC: U.S. Department
Cronnell, B. (1981). Dialect and writing: A review. Retrieved from ERIC of Education.
database. (ED 211997) Rickford, J. R. (1999). African American Vernacular English. Malden,
Cronnell, B. (1984). Black-English influences in the writing of third- and MA: Blackwell.
sixth-grade Black students. Journal of Educational Research, 77(4), Roach, H., & McGowan, R. (1932). The pooch. The Little Rascals col-
233–236. lector’s edition, Vol. 2 [VHS]. Greenwich, CT: Cabin Fever Entertainment.
Dollaghan, C. A., Campbell, T. F., & Tomlin, R. (1990). Video narration Roach, H., & Meins, G. (1934). The first roundup. The Little Rascals col-
as a language sampling context. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, lector’s edition, Vol. 2 [VHS]. Greenwich, CT: Cabin Fever Entertainment.
55, 582–590. Rubin, H., Patterson, P. A., & Kantor, M. (1991). Morphological devel-
Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—III. opment and writing ability in children and adults. Language, Speech, and
Circle Pines, MN: AGS. Hearing Services in Schools, 22, 228–235.
Evans, J. L., & Craig, H. K. (1992). Language sample collection and Salahu-Din, D., Persky, H., & Miller, J. (2008). The Nation’s report
analysis: Interview compared to freeplay assessment contexts. Journal card: Writing 2007 (NCES 2008-468). Washington, DC: U.S. Depart-
of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 343–353. ment of Education.
Gillam, R. B., & Johnston, J. R. (1992). Spoken and written language Scott, C. M., & Rogers, L. M. (1996). Written language abilities of African-
relationships in language/learning-impaired and normally achieving American children and youth. In A. G. Kamhi, K. E. Pollock, & J. L.
school-age children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, Harris (Eds.), Communication development & disorders in African
1303–1315. American children: Research, assessment, and intervention (pp. 307–332).
Glassner, B. M. (1981). Writing as an integrator of hemispheric function. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
In B. Kroll & P. Vann (Eds.), Exploring speaking–writing relationships: Scott, C. M., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance mea-
Connections and contrasts (pp. 142–153). Urbana, IL: The National sures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse of school-
Council of Teachers of English. age children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech,
Green, L. J. (2002). African American English. New York, NY: Cambridge Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 324–339.
University Press. Seals, L. M., Pollard-Durodola, S. D., Foorman, B. R., & Bradley, A. M.
Greenwald, E. A., Persky, H. R., Campbell, J. R., & Mazzeo, J. (1999). (2007). Vocabulary power: Lessons for students who use African
The NAEP 1998 writing report card for the nation and the states (NCES American Vernacular English. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
1999-462). Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education, Office of Singer, B. D. (1995). Written language development and disorders: Selected
Educational Research and Improvement. principles, patterns, and intervention possibilities. Topics in Language
Disorders, 16(1), 83–98.
Hendrix, R. (1981). The status and politics of writing instruction. In M. F.
Whiteman (Ed.), Writing: Variation in writing: Functional and linguistic– Smitherman, G. (1993). “The blacker the berry, the sweeter the juice”:
cultural differences (pp. 53–70). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. African American student writers and the national assessment of
educational progress (Report No. CS 214 189). Retrieved from ERIC
Horkay, N. (2000). The NAEP guide (NCES 2000-456). Washington, DC: database. (ED 366944)
U.S. Department of Education, Office of Educational Research.
SPSS. (1989–2007). SPSS for Windows (Version 16.0.1) [Computer
Isaac, G. J. (1996). Persistence of non-standard dialect in school-age software]. Chicago, IL: Author.
children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 434–441.
Stewart, S. R. (1985). Development of written language proficiency:
Jerry, L. J., & Ballator, N. L. (1999). The NAEP 1998 writing state report Methods for teaching text structure. In C. S. Simon (Ed.), Communication
for Mississippi (NCES 1999-463 MS). Washington, DC: U.S. Depart- skills and classroom success (pp. 341–361). San Diego, CA: College
ment of Education, Office of Educational Research and Improvement. Hill Press.
Kroll, B. M. (1978). Cognitive egocentrism and the problem of audience Stockman, I. J. (1996). Phonological development and disorders in African
awareness in written discourse. Research in the Teaching of English, American children. In A. G. Kamhi, K. E. Pollock, & J. L. Harris (Eds.),
12(3), 269–281. Communication development and disorders in African American children:
Kroll, B. M. (1981). Developmental relationships between speaking and Research, assessment, and intervention (pp. 117–153). Baltimore, MD:
writing. In B. M. Kroll & R. J. Vann (Eds.), Exploring speaking–writing Brookes.
relationships: Connections and contrasts (pp. 32–54). Urbana, IL: Sulzby, E. (1996). Roles of oral and written language as children approach
National Council of Teachers of English. conventional literacy. In C. Pontecorvo, M. Orsolini, B. Burge, & L. B.
Kroll, B. M., & Lemper, J. D. (1981). Effect of mode of communication Resnick (Eds.), Children’s early text construction (pp. 25–46). Mahwah,
on the informational adequacy of children’s explanations. The Journal NJ: Erlbaum.
of Genetic Psychology, 138, 27–35. Terrebonne, N. G., & Terrebonne, R. A. (1976). The patterning of
Mississippi Department of Education. (2007). Mississippi Curriculum language variation in writing (Report No. CS 204 457). Retrieved from
Test. Jackson, MS: Author. ERIC database. (ED 162312)

38 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 42 • 31–40 • January 2011
Thomas-Tate, S., Apel, K., & Miskowski, M. (2006, November). Whiteman, M. F. (1981). Dialect influence in writing. In M. F. Whiteman
African-American students’ use of morphology in spoken and written (Ed.), Writing: The nature, development, and teaching of written com-
narratives. Paper presented at the annual convention of the American munication (Vol. 1, pp. 153–166). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Speech-Language-Hearing Association, Miami, FL. Whiteman, M. F., & Hall, W. S. (1981). Introduction. In M. F. Whiteman
Thompson, C. A., Craig, H. K., & Washington, J. A. (2004). Variable (Ed.), Writing: The nature, development, and teaching of written com-
production of African American English across oracy and literacy con- munication (Vol. 1, pp. 1–10). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
texts. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 35, 269–282. Windsor, J., Scott, C. M., & Street, C. K. (2000). Verb and noun mor-
van Keulen, J. E., Weddington, G. T., & DeBose, C. E. (1998). Speech, phology in the spoken and written language of children with language
language, learning and the African American child. Needham Heights, learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing
MA: Allyn & Bacon. Research, 43, 1322–1336.
Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. (E. Hanfmann & G. Vakar, Wolfram, W., Adger, C. T., & Christian, D. (1999). Dialects in schools
Eds. & Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published and communities. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
1934). Wolfram, W., & Schilling-Estes, N. (1998). American English. Malden,
Washington, J. A. (1996). Issues in assessing the language abilities of MA: Blackwell.
African American children. In A. G. Kamhi, K. E. Pollock, & J. L. Harris Wolfram, W., & Whiteman, M. F. (1971). The role of dialect interference
(Eds.), Communication development and disorders in African American in composition (Report No. AL 002-696). Retrieved from ERIC database.
children: Research, assessment, and intervention (pp. 35–54). Baltimore, (ED 045971)
MD: Brookes.
Washington, J. A., & Craig, H. K. (1994). Dialectal forms during dis-
Received September 23, 2009
course of poor, urban, African American preschoolers. Journal of Speech
and Hearing Research, 37, 816–823. Revision received May 13, 2010
Accepted July 10, 2010
Washington, J. A., & Craig, H. K. (1998). Socioeconomic status and
DOI: 10.1044/0161-1461(2010/09-0069)
gender influences on children’s dialectal variations. Journal of Speech,
Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 618–626.
Contact author: Lennette J. Ivy, The University of Mississippi,
Washington, J. A., & Craig, H. K. (1999). Performances of at-risk,
Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, 302 George Hall,
African-American preschoolers on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary
University, MS 38677. E-mail: livy@olemiss.edu.
Test—III. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 30,
75–82.
Washington, J. A., Craig, H. K., & Kushmaul, A. J. (1998). Variable
use of African American English across two language sampling contexts.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1115–1124.

Ivy & Masterson: A Comparison of Oral and Written English 39


APPENDIX. DESCRIPTIONS OF OPPORTUNITIES AND OCCURRENCES OF AFRICAN AMERICAN
ENGLISH FEATURES

Feature Description

Verbal –s absence (VS) Opportunity – Any present tense verb ending in –s, used in 3rd person (e.g., She goes,
he does, she thinks, he works)
Occurrence – Absence of the –s from present tense verbs used in 3rd person (e.g., She go,
he do, she think, he work)
Plural –s absence (PL) Opportunity – Any regular lexical noun with the plural –s (e.g., toys, cars); irregular
plurals were not included (e.g., children, men)
Occurrence – The plural –s omitted from regular lexical nouns (e.g., four mile, two dog)
Possessive –s absence (POSS) Opportunity – Any regular lexical noun with the ’s (e.g., the dog’s collar, Spanky’s dog);
simple possessive pronouns were not included (e.g., my, their, your, hers)
Occurrence – Any regular noun in the possessive case without the ’s (e.g., Pete collar,
Spanky dog)
Regular past tense –ed absence
Regular verb with a pronunciation that Opportunity – Examples: walked, roped, cooked
ends with a consonant (PT-C) Note. The past tense verbs kept and left should be included as opportunities. The past tense
is marked after a consonant without the addition of a syllable.
Occurrence – Examples: walk yesterday, rope cattle this morning, cook dinner yesterday
Regular verb with a pronunciation that Opportunity – Examples: skied, tried
ends with a vowel (PT-V) Note. The past tense verbs said, paid, laid should be included as opportunities. The rationale
is that these are (i.e., say, pay, lay) regular verbs ending with a vowel.
Occurrence – Examples: I ski last winter, I try yesterday, I say go to bed
Regular verb with a pronunciation that Opportunity – Examples: skated, ended
ends with a /t / or /d/ (PT-T/D) Occurrence – Examples: ice skate last winter, it end at midnight (The focus is on the phonemic
makeup of the words – pronunciation.)
Copula absence (COPAb) Copulas are be verbs (i.e., is, am, are, was, were) functioning as the main verb in a sentence.
Opportunity – Examples: He is a doctor, I am happy, we are the winners, she was there
yesterday, Spanky and Stymie were hungry this morning
Occurrence – Examples: He a doctor, I happy, we the winners, she there yesterday, they
hungry this morning
be-Auxiliary absence (beAUXAb) be-Auxiliaries (be-A) are be verbs (i.e., is, am, are, was, were) functioning as a helping verb
when a lexical verb is also present. (This is limited to be verbs and we’re looking for
ABSENCE.)
Opportunity – Examples: She is helping, they are singing, she was jumping this morning
Occurrence – Examples: She helping, they singing, she jumping this morning

40 LANGUAGE, SPEECH, AND HEARING SERVICES IN SCHOOLS • Vol. 42 • 31–40 • January 2011
Copyright of Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools is the property of American Speech-Language-
Hearing Association and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv
without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

Potrebbero piacerti anche