Sei sulla pagina 1di 12

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

The Role of Aesthetic Sensitivity in Body Dysmorphic


Disorder
Christina Lambrou, David Veale, and Glenn Wilson
Online First Publication, January 31, 2011. doi: 10.1037/a0022300

CITATION
Lambrou, C., Veale, D., & Wilson, G. (2011, January 31). The Role of Aesthetic Sensitivity in
Body Dysmorphic Disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. Advance online publication. doi:
10.1037/a0022300
Journal of Abnormal Psychology © 2011 American Psychological Association
2011, Vol. ●●, No. ●, 000 – 000 0021-843X/11/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0022300

The Role of Aesthetic Sensitivity in Body Dysmorphic Disorder

Christina Lambrou, David Veale, and Glenn Wilson


King’s College London

Individuals with a higher aesthetic sensitivity may be more vulnerable to developing body dysmor-
phic disorder (BDD). Aesthetic sensitivity has 3 components: (a) perceptual, (b) emotional, and (c)
evaluative. Individuals with BDD (n ⫽ 50) were compared with a control group of individuals with
an education or employment in art and design related fields (n ⫽ 50) and a control group of
individuals without aesthetic training (n ⫽ 50). A facial photograph of each participant was
manipulated to create a 9-image symmetry continuum. Presented with the continuum on a computer,
participants were required to select and rate the image representing their self-actual, self-ideal, idea
of perfect, most physically attractive, most pleasure, and most disgust. Control symmetry continua
examined the specificity of the disturbance. As predicted, BDD participants displayed no distortion
in their perceptual processing but were disturbed in their negative emotional/evaluative processing
of their self-image. A significant discrepancy between their self-actual and self-ideal, resulting from
an absent self-serving bias in their self-actual (a bias exhibited by controls) appears to be the source
of their disturbance. They also overvalued the importance of appearance and self-objectified. These
aesthetic evaluations may predispose individuals to BDD and/or maintain the disorder.

Keywords: body dysmorphic disorder, aesthetic sensitivity, body image, self-serving bias, depression

The wish to be attractive is a normal desire. Many express evaluating harmony and balance in appearance. This relates to the
dissatisfaction, to some degree, with at least one facet of their concept of aestheticality, a term coined by Harris (1982) to de-
appearance. For those with body dysmorphic disorder (BDD), scribe an innate sensitivity to aesthetic perception, an attribute that
however, the concern with an imagined or slight defect in their varies among individuals; a high aestheticality would augment an
appearance is excessive, causing them significant distress and/or individual’s self-consciousness and distress over any defect in
impairment in their social and/or occupational functioning. Etio- their appearance, such that they seek cosmetic surgery. Veale et al.
logical understanding of BDD is still in its infancy, and it remains (1996) suggested that being more aesthetically sensitive was a
enigmatic. That individuals with BDD appreciate art and beauty to possible risk factor in the development of BDD. To our knowl-
a greater degree than comparative psychiatric groups is suggested edge, this is the first study to investigate whether aesthetic sensi-
by their choice of occupation and/or education (Veale, Ennis, & tivity may play such a role. As well as expanding on the original
Lambrou, 2002), which raises an interesting question about the theory by Harris (1982) and applying the ideas to BDD, we used
definition of BDD as a preoccupation with an imagined defect or novel techniques to test the hypotheses formulated.
a minor physical anomaly. Perhaps individuals with BDD are more Akin to the perceptual and affective/attitudinal components of
aesthetically sensitive than the mental health professionals who body image, this study proposed that aesthetic sensitivity has three
diagnose them and who are therefore unable to appreciate art and components: (a) perceptual (the ability to differentiate variations in
beauty to the same degree (Veale & Lambrou, 2002). aesthetic proportions); (b) emotional (the degree of emotion expe-
rienced when presented with beauty or ugliness); and (c) evalua-
Aesthetic Sensitivity Model tive (aesthetic standards, values, and identity). The question is
whether those with BDD exhibit a perceptual distortion and/or an
Aesthetic sensitivity can be defined as an awareness and appre- emotional/evaluative disturbance.
ciation of beauty and harmony. Individuals with BDD may be
more aware of subtle differences in facial asymmetry or the size of
secondary sexual facial characteristics, or they may be better at Components of Aesthetic Sensitivity

Aesthetic Perceptual Sensitivity


Aesthetic perceptual sensitivity has two components: (a) per-
Christina Lambrou, David Veale, and Glenn Wilson, Department of ceptual understanding/awareness (i.e., idea of perfect) and (b)
Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, London,
perceptual accuracy (i.e., self-actual). Although research on per-
United Kingdom.
This study is part of a doctoral dissertation by Christina Lambrou.
ceptual understanding/awareness was not available prior to this
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Christina study, limited empirical research was available for perceptual
Lambrou, Department of Psychology, Box PO77, Institute of Psychiatry, accuracy. Mainstream opinion is that self-perception in those with
King’s College London, De Crespigny Park, London SE5 8AF, United BDD is distorted. However, consistent with the hypothesis of this
Kingdom. E-mail: christina.lambrou@kcl.ac.uk study, Thomas and Goldberg (1995) found that individuals with

1
2 LAMBROU, VEALE, AND WILSON

BDD were more accurate in assessing their facial proportions than 2008), who have higher beauty standards than the rest of the
controls or individuals seeking cosmetic surgery. Note that they population and demand perfection in themselves as an ideal (Veale
explored accuracy in ability, which is not necessarily the same as & Lambrou, 2002). Relative to controls, those with BDD display
accuracy in perception. a greater discrepancy between their self-actual and their self-ideal
Mirror gazing heightens self-awareness and accuracy (Jerome, (Veale, Kinderman, Riley, & Lambrou, 2003). The question that
1992) and reduces attractiveness ratings of one’s own face (Mulk- needs to be addressed is the source of their self-discrepancy. Is it
ens & Jansen, 2009) in those dissatisfied with their appearance. due to a distorted self-actual, an exaggerated self-ideal, or both?
When viewing their appearance in the mirror (a common compul- Aesthetic standard. The discrepancy between an individual’s
sive behavior in BDD), those with BDD may selectively attend to idea of perfect and their ideal personal standard represents their
specific features. This may explain their accuracy and the main- aesthetic standard. Individuals with BDD may display a minimal
tenance of their symptoms (Veale, 2004). Depression is the most discrepancy between their self-ideal and their idea of perfect
common comorbid disorder in BDD (e.g., Gunstad & Phillips, because they are demanding perfection in their appearance.
2003). The relationship with depression may account for their Attractiveness standard. By the nature of their disorder, indi-
perceptual accuracy. The premise of this study, which is based on viduals with BDD may value attractiveness more than the rest of the
the idea of depressive realism (Alloy & Abramson, 1979), was that population (e.g., Buhlmann, Teachman, Naumann, Fehlinger, & Rief,
mildly to moderately depressed individuals (this would likely 2009). This may partly explain why they are more stringent when
include most individuals with BDD) would be more accurate in rating their own attractiveness (e.g., Buhlmann et al., 2008).
their self-actual perception. In contrast, nondepressed individuals Aesthetic values. Not everyone who believes they are ugly or
would display a self-serving bias. that they have a defect in their appearance develops BDD. Indi-
The accurate self-estimations reported by Thomas and Goldberg viduals with BDD may place a greater value on the importance of
(1995) may be tapping the perceptual component of body image, appearance in their identity, which would predispose them to
which may not be distorted in BDD. In contrast, the perceptual and/or perpetuate the disorder.
distortion described by some clinicians may be an account of their Aesthetic identity. Veale (2004) posited that those with BDD
response to their internal body image (e.g., Osman, Cooper, Hack- are more likely to judge themselves almost exclusively by their
mann, & Veale, 2004), which involves the emotional/evaluative appearance and view the self as an aesthetic object. This process,
component of body image and may be disturbed in BDD. known as self-objectification, leads to an assiduous engagement in
appearance enhancing behaviors, self-consciousness, and body
Aesthetic Emotional Sensitivity shame (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997), which characterize BDD.

Harris (1982) suggested that a consequence of increased aes- Present Study


theticality is that an individual reacts with a greater emotional
response to beauty or ugliness. Individuals with BDD experience The aim of the present study was to investigate three proposed
more aversion to their face than controls (Feusner et al., 2010). components of aesthetic sensitivity (perceptual, emotional, and
When rating their own face or body, the emotional response for evaluative) to determine whether for those with BDD, their views
those with BDD may be a mixture of self-disgust (e.g., when of their appearance are due to a perceptual distortion and/or an
viewing themselves in a mirror), depression at the failure to emotional/evaluative disturbance. The central premise was that
achieve an aesthetic standard, and anxiety about the future conse- rather than having a distortion in their perceptual processing,
quences of being ugly (Veale & Lambrou, 2002). individuals with BDD would have an enhanced understanding of
This study assessed the emotional experience in relation to aesthetic proportions and an increased accuracy in their self-actual
pleasure and disgust to reveal the precise nature of the hypothe- estimation. The source of their disturbance would be in their
sized emotional disturbance in BDD. Because of their heightened emotional/evaluative processing, which is specific to the self.
aestheticality, individuals with BDD may recognize beauty or Individuals with an interest in art and beauty, such as those with an
ugliness on a perceptual level. However, on an emotional level, education or employment in art and design related fields, might have
they may exhibit a negative bias, experiencing less pleasure and developed an enhanced understanding or appreciation of aesthetic
more disgust than those without BDD when viewing attractive and proportions compared with individuals without aesthetic training.
unattractive versions of their own face, respectively. Therefore, art and design controls as well as non-art controls were
used. Conceptualized as the reference group, the selection of art and
Aesthetic Evaluations design controls was based on the expectation that they would resem-
ble individuals with BDD in their perceptual processing, expressed as
Individuals with BDD may hold certain dysfunctional aesthetic an increased understanding of aesthetic proportions, and that they
evaluations, which interact with aesthetic perceptual and emotional would resemble non-art controls in their emotional/evaluative pro-
sensitivity to predispose them to and/or maintain the disorder. cessing. Each of the three proposed components of aesthetic sensitiv-
Aesthetic evaluations divide into three broad categories: (a) aes- ity has a perceptual and emotional/evaluative element to illustrate the
thetic standards, (b) aesthetic values, and (c) aesthetic identity. dichotomy. For instance, in the assessment of aesthetic emotional
Aesthetic standards. There are three types of standards: sensitivity, participants selected the images that gave them the most
personal, aesthetic, and attractiveness. pleasure and the most disgust (perceptual) and reported their pleasure
Personal standard (self-ideal and self-discrepancy). Perhaps and disgust ratings (emotional/evaluative).
individuals with BDD evaluate their appearance negatively be- Two control conditions (other face and building) were included
cause they are perfectionists (Buhlmann, Etcoff, & Wilhelm, to test whether the hypothesized emotional/evaluative disturbance
ROLE OF AESTHETIC SENSITIVITY IN BDD 3

in those with BDD is specific to their own face. The hypothesized (d) snowball sampling. The ethics committees of the Institute of
increased perceptual understanding would generalize to other faces Psychiatry and the Priory Hospital North London approved the
(facial control condition) and inanimate objects in the general study protocol.
surroundings, represented by a building (nonfacial control condi-
tion). In the control conditions, individuals with BDD would be Computer Graphic Techniques
indistinguishable from art and design controls.
We investigated the three proposed components of aesthetic
Method sensitivity by manipulating a digital photograph of each partici-
pant’s face, the control faces, and the building, using Adobe
Photoshop 7.0 and Ulead MorphStudio 1.0 to create nine image
Participants
symmetry continua. Participants viewed the images in SuperLab
The study sample comprised 150 participants in three groups: on a laptop.
(a) 50 individuals with BDD (test group), (b) 50 art and design Generation of individual test photographs. The first author
controls (nonclinical control group), and (c) 50 non-art controls took a digital colored photograph of each participant’s face under
(nonclinical control group). Male and female adults ages 18 standardized conditions. Each participant sat upright in front of a
through 40 were recruited. This age group was selected because it white background, with a light source positioned on either side to
is representative of the time when individuals are more commonly reduce shadowing. A 3.0 mega-pixel digital camera, placed on a
and most affected by BDD. The groups were age and gender tripod and positioned a constant distance of 0.6 m from the
matched. participant, was used to take the frontal view photographs. To
The inclusion criteria for the BDD group were (a) a primary eliminate emotional expression as a possible confounding variable,
diagnosis of BDD based on the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual participants received instructions to look directly at the camera and
of Mental Disorders (4th ed., DSM–IV; American Psychiatric display a neutral facial expression.
Association, 1994); (b) a total score of at least 20 on the Yale– The male and female individuals recruited to represent the two
Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale modified for BDD (BDD- control facial images were selected because they matched the age
YBOCS; Phillips et al., 1997), including a score of at least 2 on criteria of the participants and were judged by 10 volunteers to fall
Item 1 (1–3 hr per day of preoccupation with the perceived defect); within the average range of attractiveness. The first author took
and (c) facial concerns as the main preoccupation, because partic- their original digital colored photograph under the same standard-
ipants were rating their faces and facial concerns are the most ized conditions outlined for participants.
common concerns in BDD. Most participants (88%) had multiple A building represented the nonfacial inanimate control stimulus
concerns, with facial concerns being the main for each BDD to compare with the faces because buildings are comparable with
participant. Concerns with skin (70%), whole face (44%), eyes faces in terms of visual complexity; buildings contain internal
(38%), and nose (38%) were the most common. Aside from their features, have a global structure, and are, to some degree, sym-
primary BDD diagnosis, as confirmed by the Structured Clinical metrical. The first author used a 3.0 mega-pixel digital camera,
Interview for DSM–IV (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1997), placed on a tripod, to generate the original digital colored photo-
their comorbid disorders were major depression (n ⫽ 21), delu- graph of the building. The building was Grovelands House, a
sional disorder (n ⫽ 19), social phobia (n ⫽ 5), obsessive– Grade I listed Regency mansion designed by the celebrated archi-
compulsive disorder (n ⫽ 4), alcohol misuse (n ⫽ 2), adjustment tect John Nash in 1797.
disorder (n ⫽ 1), and bulimia nervosa (n ⫽ 1). Participants with Definition of perfection. Empirical evidence implies that
BDD were either individuals receiving treatment at the Priory attractiveness increases in male and female faces as symmetry is
Hospital North London or individuals who had contacted a BDD enhanced and that attractiveness decreases as symmetry is reduced
support group. (Rhodes, Proffitt, Grady, & Sumich, 1998). For the purposes of
The two nonclinical control groups excluded (a) those who this study, perfection was therefore defined by symmetry. To
responded yes to “Have you ever been diagnosed with a psychi- simplify the design, the manipulations were limited to this one
atric disorder?” and/or “Are you currently suffering with a psy- dimension of beauty.
chiatric disorder?” and (b) those who had excessive appearance Symmetry continuum manipulations. The first author cre-
concerns defined by a total score of 20 or above on the BDD- ated a symmetry continuum of nine images (one real image and
YBOCS, including a score of at least 2 on Item 1. The only eight manipulated images) for each participant, for the male con-
difference between the control groups was the additional inclusion trol facial image, for the female control facial image, and for the
criterion of an education or occupation in art and design, necessary building image. The real image, that is, the original digital photo-
for the art and design group. This included a current or completed graph, represented the midpoint on the symmetry continuum.
education (at least advanced level) or training in art, fine art, art Creating the images representing symmetry and asymmetry.
history, architecture, or design or an occupation as an artist, an art The first author created the two extremes of the continuum, sym-
teacher, an architect, or a graphics, fashion, or textile designer. To metry (most attractive) and asymmetry (least attractive), using
classify a participant, both current and past occupation, training, Adobe Photoshop 7.0. For all the manipulations, the feather was
and education in art and design were used. For instance, a partic- set at 10 pixels to reduce the visibility of harsh lines in the image
ipant with an art degree but working as a waiter qualified for the from using the lasso or marquee tools to select isolated features or
art and design group. The two control groups were recruited by (a) larger sections of the image.
advertisements in a local newspaper, (b) email circulars in 11 Symmetry. To create the facial and building images represent-
universities and colleges, (c) leaflets delivered to 1,000 homes, and ing symmetry, the first step was to duplicate a layer of the real
4 LAMBROU, VEALE, AND WILSON

image to allow for the manipulations. Half of the facial or building related to depression. Total scores range from 0 to 63 (none or
image was then flipped horizontally (with the edit, transform, and minimal: ⬍10; mild to moderate: 10 –18; moderate to severe:
flip horizontal commands). The first author used a randomized 19 –29; severe: 30 – 63). Internal consistency was high in the
process to determine whether to flip the left side or right side. This present sample (Cronbach’s ␣ ⫽ .948).
involved tossing a coin. The randomized order matched across the Values Scale. A simple scale specifically devised for the
three groups. study measured aesthetic values. Given a relative scale, respon-
To circumvent the technical problems reported in previous dents are required to allocate 100 points to a choice of 10 values
research, the first author took care to ensure that, when creating the (physical appearance, family, friends, health, academic/
symmetrical image, the image maintained the original widths of occupational success, religion, art, music, money, scientific truth)
the face and building, as well as the widths and positioning of the and to rate each one on degree of importance. Values that hold no
internal features. By using the scale option in the edit tool, the importance to them receive a zero rating. The final points yield a
transform tool restored any slight deviations from the original. total score of 100 for all respondents.
Skin blemishes transferred to the other side of the face because of Self-Objectification Questionnaire. The Self-Objectification
the flipping, were removed with the cloning stamp and healing Questionnaire (Noll & Fredrickson, 1998), a 10-item ranking scale
brush tools. The final step was to flatten the layers and save the was used to measure aesthetic identity. Total scores range from
image. The first author saved the images in a bitmap format ⫺25 to 25. A higher positive score indicates a greater emphasis on
because it was the only format common to the software used in the appearance and, thus, a higher level of self-objectification.
manipulation and in the presentation of the images.
Asymmetry. To create the facial and building images repre- Procedure
senting asymmetry, the first step was to duplicate a layer of the real
image to allow for the manipulations. For the facial images, one Participants met with the first author on two occasions. In the
side of the jaw line was pinched (with the filter, distort, and pinch first meeting, participants read the information form and had the
tools) by 30% to reduce the symmetrical proportions of the face. opportunity to ask questions. After consenting to the study, par-
To increase the asymmetry of the internal features, the same side ticipants had their photograph taken in preparation for the second
of the mouth was pinched by 30% to reduce the size on one side, meeting, where they viewed the real image and the manipulated
and the same side of the nose was pinched by ⫺30% to increase versions of their image and the control images on a computer.
the size on one side. For the building image, one side of the Participants were then administered the BDD-YBOCS and the
building was pinched by 10% to reduce the symmetrical propor- BDI.
tions of the building. On the same side of the building, pinching In the second meeting, before the computer study commenced,
the outer window on the first floor by 20% and outer window on participants viewed the questions of the study. They received a
the second floor by 20% increased the asymmetry of the internal thorough explanation of the procedure with the opportunity to ask
features. The final step was to flatten the layers and save the questions for clarification.
images in a bitmap format. Participants were then presented with the nine images of their
The asymmetry manipulations occurred on either the left side or face (one real and eight manipulated) simultaneously on the com-
the right side of the face and the building depending on the side puter screen. They were required to select and rate, in the follow-
originally flipped to create the symmetry image. If, to create ing order, their actual self, their ideal self, their perfect face, the
symmetry, this entailed flipping the left side to the right side, then most physically attractive image and give their attractiveness rat-
the asymmetry manipulations were on the right side and vice versa. ing for their selected image (scale: 0 ⫽ extremely unattractive,
Creating the images within the continuum. The first author 10 ⫽ extremely attractive), the image that gave them the most
created the six images within the continuum, using Ulead pleasure and give their pleasure rating for their selected image
MorphStudio 1.0. In this software, an option is available to com- (scale: 0 ⫽ extreme disgust, 10 ⫽ extreme pleasure), and the
pose an image by morphing two images to varying percentages. image that gave them the most disgust and give their disgust rating
The real image and symmetry were morphed to create three images for their selected image (scale: 0 ⫽ extreme pleasure, 10 ⫽
of varying symmetry: 25% symmetry, 50% symmetry, and 75% extreme disgust). Viewing and rating time was 1 min for each
symmetry. The real image and asymmetry were morphed to create selection question. A blank screen preceded each new selection
three images of varying asymmetry: 25% asymmetry, 50% asym- question and series presentation. A neutral instructional format
metry, and 75% asymmetry. was used (i.e., “please select which image represents your actual
self”) to eliminate a possible instructional effect. Participants sub-
sequently completed the Values Scale and the Self-Objectification
Questionnaires
Questionnaire.
BDD-YBOCS. The BDD-YBOCS (Phillips et al., 1997), a Next, participants viewed and rated the images of the facial
12-item semistructured interview was administered to assess the control condition, followed by the nonfacial control condition. The
severity of BDD symptoms during the past week. Total scores same procedure and question order used for the self-image condi-
range from 0 to 48 (mild: 20 –26; moderate: 27–34; severe: 35⫹). tion applied, with two exceptions. First, to make their judgments
Internal consistency was high in the present sample (Cronbach’s for actual other and actual building, participants viewed the real
␣ ⫽ .951). image for 10 s. This image was followed by the nine images of the
Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). The BDI (Beck, Ward, symmetry continuum for 1 min for the image selection. Second, in
Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961), a 21-item self-report inven- contrast to the facial image conditions, participants did not select
tory, was used to assess the presence and severity of symptoms their most attractive building or rate its attractiveness. To reduce
ROLE OF AESTHETIC SENSITIVITY IN BDD 5

the risk of overtesting, male participants viewed only the male BDI ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .85). Control groups did not differ. The
control face, and female participants viewed only the female groups were equivalent in age and gender ratio (see Table 1).
control face. All participants were screened to ensure they were not
familiar with the control face.
There were two presentation orders for the position of the Aesthetic Perceptual Sensitivity
images on the screen, with the allocation of the order randomized
Table 2 presents the group means, standard deviations, test
by tossing a coin. Participants viewed the same presentation, in
statistics, significance values, and effect sizes for the variables of
either the first or the second order, across the three conditions. The
aesthetic perceptual sensitivity. The findings were consistent with
numbers viewing each presentation were equal across the three
expectations.
groups. At the conclusion of the study, each participant was
Perceptual understanding/awareness (idea of perfect).
debriefed.
BDD participants and art and design controls (who did not differ)
selected a significantly more symmetrical self-image for their idea
Statistical Analysis of perfect than did non-art controls ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .62). Their
Multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAs) were conducted perceptual superiority relative to non-art control participants ex-
to confirm the body image components. A series of one-way tended to the control conditions (other face: p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .31;
analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were conducted on each variable. building: p ⬍ .001, r ⫽ .29).
When variances were considered heterogeneous (variance ratio ⬎ Perceptual accuracy. BDD participants were significantly
3:1), a Brown–Forsythe F* test was selected. Significant main more accurate in perceiving their actual self than were control
effects were followed up with planned comparisons designed participants ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .59). The groups did not differ in their
accordingly to test each specific prediction. A post hoc Tukey accuracy for the control conditions.
honestly significant difference test followed up an unexpected Effects of mirror checking. BDD participants checked their
outcome. Cohen’s f (for ANOVAs) or Pearson’s correlation coef- appearance in a mirror significantly more frequently on a weekly
ficient r (for planned comparisons) were calculated to measure basis than did control participants, t(147) ⫽ 5.992, p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽
effect sizes (small: f ⫽ 0.10, r ⫽ .10; medium: f ⫽ 0.25, r ⫽ .30; .44 (BDD: M ⫽ 3.9, SD ⫽ 1.0; art and design: M ⫽ 2.9, SD ⫽ 1.0;
large: f ⫽ 0.40, r ⫽ .50). Correlational analyses were performed to non-art: M ⫽ 2.8, SD ⫽ 1.0). However, mirror checking did not
assess the depressive realism explanation. The following coding influence the self-actual estimation results. When it was included
system was used in the analyses for the images selected from the as a covariate, the pattern of results did not change.
symmetry continuum: symmetry ⫽ 4; 75% symmetry ⫽ 3; 50% Depressive realism. There was a significant negative corre-
symmetry ⫽ 2; 25% symmetry ⫽ 1; real image ⫽ 0; 25% asym- lation between self-actual estimation and depression (BDI total
metry ⫽ ⫺1; 50% asymmetry ⫽ ⫺2; 75% asymmetry ⫽ ⫺3; score; r ⫽ ⫺.52, p ⬍ .0001). Participants with mild-to-moderate
asymmetry ⫽ ⫺4. In light of the multiple comparisons, a Bonfer- depression were the most accurate. Participants with minimal or no
roni corrected alpha level was set at .001 for all analyses. depression showed the greatest positive distortion. Of the BDD
group, 90% were at least mildly depressed. When we controlled
Results for BDD severity (BDD-YBOCS total score), the significant as-
sociation ceased (r ⫽ ⫺.16, p ⫽ .058). There was a significant
negative correlation between self-actual estimation and BDD se-
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics
verity (r ⫽ ⫺.54, p ⬍ .0001). BDD participants with moderate
Relative to control participants, BDD participants scored signif- BDD were the most accurate. BDD participants with severe BDD
icantly higher on the BDD-YBOCS ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .86) and the showed the greatest negative distortion. A significant positive corre-

Table 1
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of BDD and Control Participants

Art and design


BDD participants controls Non-art controls
(n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50)
Effect size
Variable M n SD M n SD M n SD Test statistic p Cohen’s f

Demographic characteristic
Age 27.7 6.9 26.2 6.5 26.3 5.1 F(2, 147) ⫽ 0.988 .375 0.11
Gender
Male 18 16 18
Female 32 34 32 ␹2(2) ⫽ 0.235 .930
Clinical characteristic
BDD-YBOCS 29.6aⴱ 6.3 8.2b 5.7 8.4b 6.1 F(2, 147) ⫽ 209.218 ⬍.0001 1.67
BDI 24.4aⴱ 10.6 4.9b 4.7 4.9b 4.5 Fⴱ(2, 86.546) ⫽ 121.431 ⬍.0001 1.27

Note. Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p ⬍ .001 in planned comparisons. BDD ⫽ body dysmorphic disorder; BDD-YBOCS ⫽
Yale–Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale modified for body dysmorphic disorder; BDI ⫽ Beck Depression Inventory.

p ⬍ .0001.
6 LAMBROU, VEALE, AND WILSON

Table 2
Group Differences in Aesthetic Perceptual Sensitivity

Art and
BDD design Non-art
participants controls controls
(n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50)

Variable and condition M SD M SD M SD Test statistic: F(2, 147) p Effect size Cohen’s f

Perceptual understanding
Self 3.4aⴱ 0.7 3.2aⴱ 0.7 1.9b 1.1 46.452 ⬍.0001 0.79
Other 2.9aⴱ 0.9 2.9aⴱ 1.0 2.1b 1.5 7.983 ⬍.001 0.33
Building 3.3a 0.7 3.2a 0.8 2.7b 1.2 7.351 ⬍.001 0.31
Perceptual accuracy
Self ⫺0.4aⴱ 1.0 0.9b 0.9 1.3b 1.0 41.154 ⬍.0001 0.74
Other 0.3 1.1 0.3 1.5 0.4 1.7 0.119 .888 0.04
Building 0.4 1.1 0.6 1.8 0.6 2.1 0.183a .833 0.05

Note. Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p ⬍ .001 in planned comparisons. BDD ⫽ body dysmorphic disorder; self ⫽ self-image
condition; other ⫽ other face condition; building ⫽ building condition.
a ⴱ
F (2, 121.279).

p ⬍ .0001.

lation was observed between depression and BDD severity (r ⫽ .82, Pleasure. BDD participants and art and design controls (who
p ⬍ .0001). did not differ) selected a significantly more symmetrical self-
image than did non-art controls to represent the image that gave
Aesthetic Emotional Sensitivity them most pleasure ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .48). When rating their
selected self-image, BDD participants experienced significantly
Table 3 presents the group means, standard deviations, test less pleasure than did control participants ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .63).
statistics, significance values, and effect sizes for the variables of Control groups did not differ. The perceptual superiority in BDD
aesthetic emotional sensitivity. The outcomes were as predicted. participants and art and design controls extended to the control

Table 3
Group Differences in Aesthetic Emotional Sensitivity

BDD Art and design Non-art


participants controls controls
(n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50)
Variable and
condition M SD M SD M SD Test statistic: F(2, 147) p Effect size Cohen’s f

Pleasure
Perceptual selection
Self 2.9aⴱ 0.9 2.6aⴱ 1.0 1.5b 1.2 23.146 ⬍.0001 0.55
Other 2.6aⴱ 1.1 2.4aⴱ 1.1 0.9b 1.9 20.330 ⬍.0001 0.52
Building 2.7aⴱ 1.4 2.8aⴱ 1.2 1.9b 1.4 6.951 ⬍.001 0.30
Rating
Self 2.8aⴱ 1.8 5.2b 1.3 5.3b 1.3 47.308 ⬍.0001 0.79
Other 5.6 1.4 5.8 1.2 6.0 1.3 0.942 .392 0.11
Building 6.1 1.8 6.5 1.6 6.1 1.5 1.252 .289 0.13
Disgust
Perceptual selection
Self ⫺2.9aⴱ 1.0 ⫺2.8aⴱ 0.9 ⫺1.9b 1.2 12.562 ⬍.0001 0.40
Other ⫺3.0aⴱ 1.0 ⫺2.9aⴱ 1.1 ⫺1.6b 1.7 16.748 ⬍.0001 0.47
Building ⫺3.1aⴱ 0.9 ⫺2.8aⴱ 1.1 ⫺1.6b 1.6 21.111a ⬍.0001 0.53
Rating
Self 8.0aⴱ 1.7 5.9b 1.5 5.4b 1.5 36.035 ⬍.0001 0.69
Other 5.0 1.7 4.6 1.3 4.4 1.5 2.177 .117 0.17
Building 5.8 1.9 5.3 1.7 4.9 1.7 3.384 .037 0.21

Note. Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p ⬍ .001 in planned comparisons. BDD ⫽ body dysmorphic disorder; self ⫽ self-image
condition; other ⫽ other face condition; building ⫽ building condition.
a ⴱ
F (2, 113.275).

p ⬍ .0001.
ROLE OF AESTHETIC SENSITIVITY IN BDD 7

conditions (other face: p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .46; building: p ⬎ .0001, pectations, BDD participants were not demanding a higher self-
r ⫽ .29). The groups did not differ in their pleasure rating for the ideal standard than were control participants. However, there were
control conditions. significant group differences in the degree of self-discrepancy (see
Disgust. BDD participants and art and design controls (who Figure 1). Consistent with our prediction, BDD participants ex-
did not differ) selected a significantly less symmetrical self-image pressed a greater discrepancy between their self-actual and self-
than did non-art controls to represent the image that gave them ideal than did control participants ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .62). Control
most disgust ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .45). When rating their selected groups did not differ. The groups did not diverge in their ideal
self-image, BDD participants experienced significantly more dis- standard or their actual/ideal discrepancy for the control condi-
gust than did control participants ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .57). Control tions.
groups did not differ. The perceptual superiority in BDD partici- Aesthetic standard. There was a significant group effect in
pants and art and design controls extended to the control condi- the discrepancy between their self-ideal and perfect self. Contrary
tions (other face: p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .43; building: p ⬎ .0001, r ⫽ .55). to expectations, planned comparisons revealed that BDD partici-
The groups did not differ in their disgust rating for the control
pants and control participants did not differ ( p ⫽ .011, r ⫽ .21),
conditions.
and control groups did diverge ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .45). A post hoc
Tukey honestly significant difference test identified the source of
Aesthetic Evaluative Sensitivity the significant group effect. BDD participants and art and design
Table 4 presents the group means, standard deviations, test controls (who did not differ) expressed a greater discrepancy
statistics, significance values, and effect sizes for the variables of between their self-ideal and perfect self than did non-art controls
aesthetic evaluative sensitivity. With the exception of personal and ( ps ⬍ .0001). The groups did not differ in their aesthetic standard
aesthetic standard, the results were as predicted. for the control conditions.
Personal standard. There were no significant differences Attractiveness standard. Relative to non-art controls, BDD
between the groups in their self-ideal selections. Contrary to ex- participants and art and design controls (who did not differ)

Table 4
Group Differences in Aesthetic Evaluative Sensitivity

Art and
BDD design Non-art
participants controls controls
(n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50) (n ⫽ 50)

Variable and condition M SD M SD M SD Test statistic: F(2, 147) p Effect size Cohen’s f

Aesthetic standards
Personal standard
Ideal
Self 2.2 0.9 1.9 1.2 1.9 1.2 1.701 .186 0.15
Other 2.5 1.2 2.4 1.2 2.2 1.4 1.036 .357 0.12
Building 2.7 1.1 2.5 1.5 2.3 1.5 1.076 .343 0.12
Actual/ideal discrepancy
Self 2.6aⴱ 1.3 1.0b 1.0 0.6b 1.0 48.028 ⬍.0001 0.80
Other 2.2 1.3 2.1 1.8 1.8 1.7 1.170 .313 0.12
Building 2.3 1.2 1.9 1.9 1.7 2.2 1.426a .243 0.14
Aesthetic standard (perfect vs. ideal)
Self 1.2aⴱ 1.2 1.3aⴱ 1.2 0.0b 0.9 22.104 ⬍.0001b 0.48
Other 0.4 1.0 0.5 1.1 ⫺0.1 1.4 2.959 .055 0.20
Building 0.6 0.9 0.7 1.1 0.3 1.1 1.978 .142 0.16
Attractiveness Standard
Perceptual selection
Self 2.8aⴱ 0.8 2.5aⴱ 1.3 1.6b 1.2 15.852c ⬍.0001 0.46
Other 2.6aⴱ 1.0 2.4aⴱ 1.2 1.3b 1.9 12.316d ⬍.0001 0.41
Rating
Self 3.3aⴱ 2.1 5.1b 1.2 4.8b 1.5 16.636 ⬍.0001 0.47
Other 5.2 1.6 5.6 1.3 5.8 1.5 1.769 .174 0.15
Aesthetic values
Values Scale–physical appearance 28.8aⴱ 16.7 8.6b 4.6 9.1b 5.6 60.073e ⬍.0001 0.89
Aesthetic identity

Self-Objectification Questionnaire 8.7 a 11.2 ⫺2.5b 9.9 ⫺2.1b 11.3 17.032 ⬍.0001 0.48

Note. Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p ⬍ .001 in planned comparisons. BDD ⫽ body dysmorphic disorder; self ⫽ self-image
condition; other ⫽ other face condition; building ⫽ building condition.
a
Fⴱ(2, 126.940). b A post hoc Tukey honestly significant difference test identified the source of the significant group effect. c Fⴱ(2, 133.473). d Fⴱ(2,
108.377). e Fⴱ(2, 67.935).

p ⬍ .0001.
8 LAMBROU, VEALE, AND WILSON

-4 -3 -2 -1 0 1 2 3 4

|_____|_____|_____|_____|_____|_____|______|_____|

100% 75% 50% 25% RI 25% 50% 75% 100%


AS AS AS AS S S S S

BDD participants

Art and design controls

Non-art controls

Figure 1. Group differences in self-actual versus self-ideal discrepancy. AS ⫽ asymmetry; RI ⫽ real image;
S ⫽ symmetry; BDD ⫽ body dysmorphic disorder.

selected a significantly more symmetrical self-image for their most not influence the results. There were no significant differences
physically attractive image ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .41). BDD participants between the two BDD subgroups for any variable in this study
rated their selected self-image significantly lower in attractiveness ( ps ⬎ .05).
than did control participants ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .53). Control groups
did not differ. The perceptual superiority in BDD participants and
Discussion
art and design controls extended to the other face condition ( p ⬍
.0001, r ⫽ .46). The groups did not differ in their attractiveness It was hypothesized that a higher aesthetic sensitivity contrib-
ratings for the control other face. utes to the development and maintenance of BDD. This study
Aesthetic values. BDD participants were significantly more examined the central premise that rather than being distorted in
likely to value the importance of physical appearance than were their perceptual processing, those with BDD have an enhanced
control participants ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .75). Control groups did not understanding of aesthetic proportions and an increased accuracy
differ. in their self-actual estimation. We expected the source of their
Aesthetic identity. BDD participants reported a significantly disturbance to be in their emotional/evaluative processing, which is
higher positive self-objectification score than did control partici- specific to the self. This would manifest in art and design controls
pants ( p ⬍ .0001, r ⫽ .43). Control groups did not differ. resembling BDD participants in their superior perceptual understand-
ing of aesthetic proportions and non-art controls in their emotional/
Confirmation of Body Image Components: Perceptual evaluative processing. Overall, the findings corroborate these hypoth-
and Emotional/Evaluative eses. Effect sizes for these key findings were medium to large.
Individuals with BDD and art and design controls seemed to
We postulated that the following variables assessed the percep-
have a clearer idea of the criteria of attractiveness levels defined by
tual component of body image: (a) perfect, (b) pleasure perceptual
symmetry. They displayed a greater awareness of their aesthetic
selection, (c) disgust perceptual selection, and (d) physical attrac-
facial proportions relative to non-art controls, and this extended to
tiveness perceptual selection. MANOVAs conducted with these
another person’s face and a building. This reinforces the idea that
variables revealed, as expected, a significant group effect for the
individuals with BDD possess a more critical eye and appreciation
self-image condition, F(8, 290) ⫽ 11.896, p ⬍ .0001, for the other
of aesthetics, which they then apply to their own appearance
face condition, F(8, 290) ⫽ 6.788, p ⬍ .0001, and for the building
(Veale & Lambrou, 2002).
condition, F(6, 292) ⫽ 7.281, p ⬍ .0001.
BDD participants were also more accurate in perceiving their
In addition, we postulated that the following variables would
actual self compared with controls. According to Thomas (1990),
assess the emotional/evaluative component of body image: (a)
the body image of each healthy member of the population tends to
actual, (b) pleasure rating, (c) disgust rating, (d) ideal, (e) actual/
mirror their position within the attractiveness distribution. Dissat-
ideal discrepancy, (f) physical attractiveness rating, (g) aesthetic
isfaction arises when there is a mismatch between perceived ap-
values, and (h) aesthetic identity. MANOVAs conducted with
pearance and actual appearance. However, the results of this study
these variables revealed, as expected, a significant group effect for
suggest that individuals with BDD, who are particularly dissatis-
the self-image condition, F(14, 284) ⫽ 11.650, p ⬍ .0001, but not
fied, present a closer match between perceived appearance and
for the other face condition, F(10, 288) ⫽ 0.922, p ⫽ .513, or for
actual appearance. On average, they displayed only a slight neg-
the building condition, F(8, 290) ⫽ 1.700, p ⫽ .089.
ative bias in their estimation of themselves. Hence, they did not
display a distortion in their perceptual processing. Rather this
Influence of the BDD Concerns
study substantiates preliminary evidence (Thomas & Goldberg,
Of the BDD group, 52% were preoccupied with at least one of 1995) that individuals with BDD are superior in their discrimina-
the facial features altered in the manipulations. However, this did tory abilities. In contrast, control participants expressed a pro-
ROLE OF AESTHETIC SENSITIVITY IN BDD 9

nounced positive bias toward symmetry, overestimating the attrac- tracker could register participants’ eye movements when they are
tiveness of their actual self. In other words, control participants looking at themselves in the mirror to examine how they scan their
were looking at themselves through rose-tinted spectacles. face. Individuals with BDD display visual processing and frontos-
Control participants’ overestimation of attractiveness was spe- triatal abnormalities when viewing their own face, which imply a
cific to their own face and is consistent with previous findings. bias for detail encoding and analysis rather than holistic processing
Jansen, Smeets, Martijn, and Nederkoorn (2006) observed that, (Feusner et al., 2010). It is therefore likely that individuals with BDD
contrary to eating-symptomatic participants who expressed a real- approach the mirror with a more analytical eye, deliberately assessing
istic view of their attractiveness, control participants overestimated features and focusing on their perceived defects. This approach
their own independently rated attractiveness. Similarly, Noles, heightens their self-perception and reinforces their dissatisfaction.
Cash, and Winstead (1985) reported that, although depressed stu- The increased perceptual accuracy in the BDD group was spe-
dents underestimated their attractiveness (akin to the slight nega- cific to their own faces. The groups did not differ in the control
tive distortion in the BDD group), they were nevertheless more conditions. Thus, BDD participants did not display impairments in
accurate in their self-appraisals than nondepressed students, who their face- or object-recognition abilities, further substantiating the
positively exaggerated their level of attractiveness. Consistent with idea that there is no perceptual deficit. Indeed, Stangier, Adam-
the depressive realism explanation, those with mild-to-moderate Schwebe, Müller, and Wolter (2008) found that individuals with
depression in the present study were the most accurate in their BDD were more accurate than control participants in recognizing
self-perception. Participants with minimal or no depression aesthetic alterations to other faces. The accuracy may not extend to
showed the greatest positive distortion. other body image conditions. Individuals with eating disorders
The findings suggest that overestimating personal attractiveness were less accurate than controls at detecting the facial flaws of
may be important for psychological well-being, providing protec- others (Legenbauer, Kleinstäuber, Müller, & Stangier, 2008). The
tion from developing body image conditions and depression. Self- outcome might have been different had these authors assessed the
perceived facial appearance correlates positively with global self- body, because weight and shape are the concerns in eating disor-
worth (Pope & Ward, 1997). The present results imply that ders. The role of facial versus body concerns on aesthetic sensi-
individuals with BDD are not using a self-serving bias when tivity needs to be investigated.
evaluating their physical appearance. Thus, their minor imperfec- The emotional sensitivity results confirm the main hypothesis that
tions disappoint them, and they experience negative emotions, individuals with BDD are superior in their perceptual processing and
such as depression. Future studies could examine whether individ- disturbed in their emotional processing. BDD participants and art and
uals with BDD also fail to use self-serving attributional biases to design controls were perceptually superior to non-art controls. They
assess self-relevant information unrelated to physical appearance. selected a more symmetrical face to represent the self-image provid-
There was no perceptual accuracy in the severely depressed, all ing the most pleasure and a less symmetrical face to represent the
of whom were BDD participants. The situation is probably made self-image providing the most disgust. Crucially, when rating their
more complex by the fact that clinicians have described individ- selected self-image for most pleasure and most disgust, BDD partic-
uals with BDD who clearly display distortion in their self-portraits. ipants experienced less pleasure and more disgust, respectively, than
Severity of the BDD condition may play an important role. Per- either control group. Surprisingly, although the insula, one of the main
haps at some point on the BDD severity scale, individuals with regions implicated in self-recognition, plays a role in the experience of
BDD become exclusively preoccupied with their internal body emotions such as disgust, insula hyperactivity is not evident in those
image. Presumably, at this stage they have a grossly distorted with BDD (Feusner et al., 2010).
internal body image because of the limited validation with objec- The perceptual superiority observed in BDD participants and art
tive reality. The present study provides preliminary support for this and design controls extended to another person’s face and a
idea, because those with severe BDD showed the greatest negative building. The emotional ratings were the pivotal difference be-
distortion in their self-actual estimation. Participants with moderate tween the self-image and the control conditions. In contrast to their
BDD were the most accurate. Although BDD severity ranged from self-image, where they exhibited a marked negative emotional
mild to severe in the present BDD sample, most BDD participants had bias, BDD participants resembled controls in their pleasure and
moderate BDD, which may account for their lack of distortion. disgust ratings for the other face and the building. This corrobo-
It is important to disentangle the effects of the BDD symptom- rates the premise that the emotional disturbance in those with BDD
atology and of depression on participants’ self-actual estimations. is specific to their own face.
Replication with a sample of depressed and nondepressed BDD The aesthetic evaluations component of aesthetic sensitivity is
participants will determine whether depression influences percep- subdivided into standards, values, and identity. Contrary to the
tual accuracy over and above the effects accounted for by BDD. commonly held view and our prediction, individuals with BDD did
Future research could also compare BDD participants with de- not demand a higher standard of beauty for themselves compared
pressed participants. Aesthetic values and identity could be the key with individuals from the healthy population. BDD participants
factors separating them. Only those with BDD will overvalue the and controls did not differ, with each selecting a modest self-ideal.
importance of appearance and self-objectify, which may promote Of interest, Silver and Reavey (2010) reported that for some with
BDD symptomatology. BDD, their ideal self was an idealized version of their perceived
Although BDD participants checked their appearance in the childhood self. The groups were also analogous in their modest
mirror more often than controls, this did not account for their ideal standard for another person’s face and for a building.
perceptual accuracy. The quality of the mirror-checking experi- In line with Veale et al. (2003), the key difference between the
ence may be more important than the quantity of checks (e.g., groups was the self-discrepancy. As a result of overestimating their
Mulkens & Jansen, 2009). To enhance ecological validity, an eye actual attractiveness, controls expressed only a marginal difference
10 LAMBROU, VEALE, AND WILSON

between their actual self and their ideal self, suggesting that they attractiveness. Perhaps for BDD participants in the present study, their
were satisfied with their perceived appearance. Korabik and Pitt appearance dissatisfaction and negative self-appraisals were, to some
(1980) observed that individuals with a high self-concept tended to extent, justified, as a panel in Jansen et al. (2006) found in an
see themselves as closer to the ideal than they actually were. eating-symptomatic group. Alternatively, perhaps BDD participants
Relative to controls, BDD participants expressed a greater discrep- and controls did not differ in their actual attractiveness; the discrep-
ancy between their perceived actual self and their desired ideal ancy in their ratings may have been merely a reflection of control
self, because of an absent self-serving bias in their self-actual participants overestimating their attractiveness to maintain their pos-
estimation. Thus, the crucial determining factor for those with itive self-concept. These issues need to be addressed.
BDD, and probably the source of their disturbance, is that a Physical appearance was the most important value only for BDD
discrepancy exists between their actual and ideal, rather than that participants. They valued its importance three times more than control
they possess an unrealistic ideal or a distorted actual self. participants did, implying that they may be valuing appearance to a
According to Higgins (1987), individuals displaying a discrep- dysfunctional degree. Consistent with the physical attractiveness ste-
ancy between their actual self and their ideal self are vulnerable to reotype, Buhlmann et al. (2009) reported that BDD participants at-
dejection-related emotions, such as depression. This may explain tached more meaning and consequences to thoughts about the impor-
the increased depressive symptomatology in the present BDD tance of appearance (e.g., attractive people are more competent).
sample. As they did not differ from controls in their self-ideal Valuing the importance of appearance is likely to reinforce processing
standards, treatment strategies that aim to develop self-serving of the self as an aesthetic object (Veale, 2004). BDD participants were
biases, such as those evident in controls, may prove effective in more likely to self-objectify than controls. It is reasonable to hypoth-
reducing the self-discrepancy in those with BDD and, conse- esize that these aesthetic evaluations predispose an individual to
quently, may alleviate their symptomatology. BDD, as well as maintaining the condition.
BDD participants resembled control participants in their dis- One must interpret the present outcomes in light of several limita-
crepancy between actual and ideal for the control conditions. Their tions. The main limitation was the unavoidable selection bias in the
significant actual versus ideal discrepancy was therefore unique to BDD group. Only those who were willing to view and rate their image
their self-image. This further attests to the idea that for those with agreed to participate. BDD participants were also preselected for
BDD, the source of their disturbance is the specific discrepancy their predominant facial concerns and for falling within a restricted
between their perceived actual self and their desired ideal self. age range of 18 to 40 years. Further studies would verify whether
Contrary to expectations, BDD participants expressed a discrep- the results generalize to those who refused to participate, those
ancy between their perfect and ideal self. They selected a more primarily concerned with nonfacial features, or those ages above
symmetrical self-image for their perfect self than for their ideal 40 or below 18. Given that BDD usually begins in adolescence,
self, verifying the earlier observation that most do not desire prospective longitudinal studies based on young vulnerable sam-
perfection in their appearance. Indeed, they chose images analo- ples may elucidate whether a high aesthetic sensitivity predisposes
gous to those chosen by art and design controls. an individual to BDD and/or is a consequence of the disorder.
On a perceptual level, BDD participants and art and design Comorbidity was present in the BDD group. Although this is
controls displayed a higher attractiveness standard, probably the typical in BDD, the question does arise as to whether the comorbid
result of their increased understanding of aesthetic proportions. disorders partially influenced the outcomes. Comparing a BDD
They were superior to non-art controls in selecting a more sym- group without comorbidities with related clinical control groups,
metrical self-image to represent their most physically attractive such as groups with eating disorders, depression, obsessive–
image. This perceptual superiority extended to the control other compulsive disorder, and social phobia, would address issues
face. Akin to the trend observed for emotional sensitivity, what regarding a possible degree of overlap in the pathogenesis of BDD
differentiated the BDD group was their self-rating. They rated the and these comorbid disorders. Future investigations could use
attractiveness of only their chosen self-image markedly lower than electroculography to measure how BDD and comparative groups
both control groups. There were no group differences when rating differentiate and/or resemble one another in their strategies when
the attractiveness of the control other face. This did not concur in viewing, selecting, and rating the images. It would have been
Buhlmann et al.’s (2008) study, in which they found that partici- preferable for participants to view both the male and female
pants with BDD rated attractive faces as more attractive compared control images to allow comparisons of cross-gender differences,
with participants with obsessive– compulsive disorder and control but there was a risk of overtesting. In addition, the order in which
participants. However, participants in the present study rated the the three conditions were presented should have been randomized.
image they perceived as the most physically attractive as opposed The present study explored the role of aesthetic sensitivity in BDD.
to rating images classified by others as attractive. Results support the distinction between perceptual and emotional/
This study did not include the objective rating of the partici- evaluative modalities of body image. Results also substantiate the
pants’ actual attractiveness. Should research unequivocally show underlying premise of the study that, rather than suffering from a
individuals with BDD to be less attractive than the general popu- perceptual deficit, individuals with BDD possess an increased under-
lation and than others with a psychiatric disorder, this would raise standing of aesthetic proportions, which extends to other faces and a
some questions regarding the validity of the BDD diagnosis. building, and a superior accuracy in their self-actual estimation. The
Thomas and Goldberg (1995) provided modest support for the source of the disturbance is in their emotional/evaluative processing
possibility of those with BDD being less attractive, but they did not when viewing their self-image. Such findings hold clear implications
assess symmetry or include a control psychiatric group. Buhlmann for treatment. Rather than engaging in a debate with them about the
et al. (2008), however, reported that BDD, obsessive– compulsive existence of their perceived defects, it may be more effective for
disorder, and control groups did not differ in their independently rated clinicians to focus on their emotional/evaluative processing.
ROLE OF AESTHETIC SENSITIVITY IN BDD 11

A higher aesthetic sensitivity may contribute to an explanation profile self perception. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 10, 482–
of why a small defect in their appearance severely disturbs those 489. doi:10.1111/j.1559-1816.1980.tb00727.x
with BDD. As predicted, participants with BDD responded the Legenbauer, T., Kleinstäuber, M., Müller, T., & Stangier, U. (2008). Are
same way as art and design controls in the control conditions. It is individuals with an eating disorder less sensitive to aesthetic flaws than
healthy controls? Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 65, 87–95. doi:
therefore important to understand what differentiates these groups.
10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.02.014
This will not only aid the development of effective treatment Mulkens, S., & Jansen, A. (2009). Mirror gazing increases attractiveness in
strategies but also contribute to the prediction of which individuals satisfied, but not in dissatisfied women: A model for body dysmorphic
are vulnerable to BDD. Differences in personality and psycholog- disorder? Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,
ical factors, such as the experience of shame, may be pivotal to 40, 211–218. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.10.001
appreciating the idiosyncratic pathways of BDD. Noles, S. W., Cash, T. F., & Winstead, B. A. (1985). Body image, physical
The promising outcomes of this study highlight that aesthetic attractiveness, and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psy-
sensitivity plays a role in BDD and indicate potential therapeutic chology, 53, 88 –94. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.53.1.88
strategies. With the development of research methods and the ques- Noll, S. M., & Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). A mediational model linking
tions generated from this study, future research on self-perception in self-objectification, body shame, and disordered eating. Psychology of
Women Quarterly, 22, 623– 636. doi:10.1111/j.1471-6402.1998.tb00181.x
BDD may unlock the mysteries of this enigmatic disorder.
Osman, S., Cooper, M., Hackmann, A., & Veale, D. (2004). Spontaneously
occurring images and early memories in people with body dysmorphic
disorder. Memory, 12, 428 – 436. doi:10.1080/09658210444000043
References
Phillips, K. A., Hollander, E., Rasmussen, S. A., Aronowitz, B. R., De-
Caria, C., & Goodman, W. K. (1997). A severity rating scale for body
Alloy, L. B., & Abramson, L. Y. (1979). Judgment of contingency in
dysmorphic disorder: Development, reliability, and validity of a modi-
depressed and nondepressed students: Sadder but wiser? Journal of
fied version of the Yale–Brown Obsessive–Compulsive Scale. Psycho-
Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 441– 485. doi:10.1037/0096-
pharmacology Bulletin, 33, 17–22.
3445.108.4.441
Pope, A. W., & Ward, J. (1997). Self-perceived facial appearance and
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical man-
psychosocial adjustment in preadolescents with craniofacial anomalies.
ual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.
Cleft Palate Craniofacial Journal, 34, 396 – 401. doi:10.1597/1545-
Beck, A. T., Ward, C. H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961).
1569(1997)0342.3.CO;2
An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry,
Rhodes, G., Proffitt, F., Grady, J. M., & Sumich, A. (1998). Facial
4, 561–571.
symmetry and the perception of beauty. Psychonomic Bulletin & Re-
Buhlmann, U., Etcoff, N. L., & Wilhelm, S. (2008). Facial attractiveness
view, 5, 659 – 669.
ratings and perfectionism in body dysmorphic disorder and obsessive–
Silver, J., & Reavey, P. (2010). “He’s a good-looking chap aint he?”: Narrative
compulsive disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 540 –547. doi:
and visualisations of self in body dysmorphic disorder. Social Science &
10.1016/j.janxdis.2007.05.004
Medicine, 70, 1641–1647. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.11.042
Buhlmann, U., Teachman, B. A., Naumann, E., Fehlinger, T., & Rief, W.
Stangier, U., Adam-Schwebe, S., Müller, T., & Wolter, M. (2008). Dis-
(2009). The meaning of beauty: Implicit and explicit self-esteem and
crimination of facial appearance stimuli in body dysmorphic disorder.
attractiveness beliefs in body dysmorphic disorder. Journal of Anxiety
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 435– 443. doi:10.1037/0021-
Disorders, 23, 694 –702. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2009.02.008
843X.117.2.435
Feusner, J. D., Moody, T., Hembacher, E., Townsend, J., McKinley, M.,
Thomas, C. S. (1990). Stress and facial appearance. Stress Medicine, 6,
Moller, H., & Bookheimer, S. (2010). Abnormalities of visual processing
299 –304. doi:10.1002/smi.2460060408
and frontostriatal systems in body dysmorphic disorder. Archives of General
Thomas, C. S., & Goldberg, D. P. (1995). Appearance, body image and
Psychiatry, 67, 197–205. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.190
distress in facial dysmorphophobia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 92,
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Williams, J. B. W. (1997).
231–236. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1995.tb09574.x
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders: Clinician
Veale, D. (2004). Advances in a cognitive behavioural model of body
Version. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
dysmorphic disorder. Body Image, 1, 113–125. doi:10.1016/S1740-
Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. (1997). Objectification theory: Toward
1445(03)00009-3
understanding women’s lived experiences and mental health risks. Psy-
Veale, D., Ennis, M., & Lambrou, C. (2002). Possible association of body
chology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206. doi:10.1111/j.1471-
dysmorphic disorder with an occupation or education in art and design.
6402.1997.tb00108.x
American Journal of Psychiatry, 159, 1788 –1790. doi:10.1176/
Gunstad, J., & Phillips, K. A. (2003). Axis I comorbidity in body dysmor-
appi.ajp.159.10.1788
phic disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 44, 270 –276. doi:10.1016/
Veale, D., Gournay, K., Dryden, W., Boocock, A., Shah, F., Willson, R.,
S0010-440X(03)00088-9
& Walburn, J. (1996). Body dysmorphic disorder: A cognitive behav-
Harris, D. L. (1982). Cosmetic surgery—where does it begin? British
ioural model and pilot randomised controlled trial. Behaviour Research
Journal of Plastic Surgery, 35, 281–286.
and Therapy, 34, 717–729. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(96)00025-3
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect.
Veale, D., Kinderman, P., Riley, S., & Lambrou, C. (2003). Self-
Psychological Review, 94, 319 –340. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.94.3.319
discrepancy in body dysmorphic disorder. British Journal of Clinical
Jansen, A., Smeets, T., Martijn, C., & Nederkoorn, C. (2006). I see what
Psychology, 42, 157–169. doi:10.1348/014466503321903571
you see: The lack of a self-serving body-image bias in eating disorders.
Veale, D. M., & Lambrou, C. (2002). The importance of aesthetics in body
British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 123–135. doi:10.1348/
dysmorphic disorder. CNS Spectrums, 7, 429 – 431.
014466505X50167
Jerome, L. (1992). Body dysmorphic disorder: A controlled study of
patients requesting cosmetic rhinoplasty. American Journal of Psychia- Received November 8, 2009
try, 149, 577–578. Revision received August 3, 2010
Korabik, K., & Pitt, E. J. (1980). Self concept, objective appearance, and Accepted August 4, 2010 䡲

Potrebbero piacerti anche