Sei sulla pagina 1di 12

Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Cancer Treatment Reviews


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/ctrv

Complications of Treatment

Chemotherapy and cognition: International cognition and cancer task force T


recommendations for harmonising preclinical research

Gordon Winocura,b,c, , Ian Johnstond, Hélène Castele,f,g
a
Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre, Toronto, Canada
b
Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, Canada
c
Departments of Psychology and Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada
d
School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia
e
Normandie Univ, UNIROUEN, INSERM, DC2N, 76000 Rouen, France
f
Institute for Research and Innovation in Biomedicine (IRIB), 76000 Rouen, France
g
Cancer and Cognition Platform, Ligue Nationale contre le Cancer, Caen, France

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Cancer survivors who undergo chemotherapy for non-CNS tumours often report substantial cognitive dis-
Chemotherapy turbances that adversely affect quality of life, during and after treatment. The neurotoxic effects of anti-cancer
Cancer drugs have been confirmed in clinical and pre-clinical research. Work with animals has also identified a range of
Preclinical research factors and underlying mechanisms that contribute to chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment. However,
Cognitive function
there is a continuing need to develop standard cognitive testing procedures for validation and comparison
purposes, broaden the search for biological and neurochemical mechanisms, and develop improved animal
models for investigating the combined effects of treatment, the disease, and other potential factors (e.g., age,
stress). In this paper, a working group, formed under the auspices of the International Cognition and Cancer Task
Force, reviews the state of pre-clinical research, formulates strategic priorities, and provides recommendations to
guide animal research that meaningfully informs clinical investigations.

Introduction investigating the effects of chemotherapy on cognition in animal


models see [6]. Very quickly, studies confirmed that commonly used
Significant advances in cancer treatment in recent years have en- anti-cancer drugs reliably produced cognitive impairment in healthy
abled cancer survivors to live longer and with improved prospects for adult rodents [e.g., 7–11]. Significantly, the most frequently reported
better quality of life. At the same time, increased survival rates have deficits were related to attentional processes, learning, memory, and
focussed attention on side effects of powerful drugs that can undermine executive function – essentially the same pattern as that observed in
day-to-day function. In particular, a substantial number of cancer pa- clinical investigations. These findings undermined the argument that
tients receiving chemotherapy report cognitive disturbances that in- cognitive impairment is secondary to stress-related factors and pointed
clude attentional problems, memory loss, and confused thought pro- to a direct toxic effect on neurocognitive function. As well, through
cesses (‘chemobrain’ or ‘cognitive fog’), that are often accompanied by preclinical research, there have been significant advances in our ap-
mood disorders and fatigue see [1–5]. Although patient complaints of preciation of lost and spared cognitive abilities, recovery over time,
chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment (CICI) have been sup- biological mechanisms, as well as associated non-cognitive abnormal-
ported by neuropsychological investigations, their importance was not ities (e.g., fatigue, emotional reactivity).
fully appreciated when considered in the context of living with a life- The International Cancer and Cognition task Force (ICCTF), re-
threatening disease. Moreover, when the condition was acknowledged, cognizing the importance of preclinical research for advancing the field,
there was uncertainty over the extent to which cognitive deficits should determined that the time is right to examine progress made by animal
be attributed to changes in brain function, or to the psychological researchers, identify priorities for future directions, and develop stan-
challenges of coping with cancer and a difficult treatment. dardized experimental procedures. As a first step, a meeting of clinical
The developing controversy underscored the need for preclinical scientists and basic researchers was convened at its 2016 conference in
research and, by the mid-2000s, labs around the world were Amsterdam, from which emerged a broad consensus that a coordinated


Corresponding author at: Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Health Sciences, 3560 Bathurst St, Toronto, Ontario M6A 2E1, Canada.
E-mail address: gwinocur@research.baycrest.org (G. Winocur).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ctrv.2018.05.017
Received 22 February 2018; Received in revised form 30 May 2018; Accepted 31 May 2018
0305-7372/ © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

Table 1
Common tests used in preclinical research to assess chemotherapy effects on cognitive functions mediated by hippocampus or frontal lobes.
Cognitive function Test References

Hippocampus – sensitive Spatial memory Morris water maze Dubois et al. [22]η,π, [23]η; Fardell et al. [7,24]; Rendeiro et al. [55]; Seigers et al. [20]; Winocur
et al. [11,15,57,58,89]
Novel location recognition Acharya et al. [126]; Christie et al. [127]; Li et al. [79]; Lyons et al. [25,128]; Mustafa et al. [9];
Seigers et al. [117]; Callaghan & O’Mara [40]; Salas-Ramirez et al. [42]; Kitamura et al. [129]
Barnes maze Seigers et al. [117]
Context memory Context fear conditioning Acharya et al. [126]; Christie et al. [127]; Fremouw et al. [130]; Macleod et al. [21]; Seigers et al.
[20,117]
Conditioned emotional ElBeltagy et al. [90]
response
Recognition memory Object recognitionπ Acharya et al. [126]; Dubois et al.[22,23]λ; Fardell et al. [7,24,131]; Fremouw et al. [130]ϕ;
Seigers et al. [117]; Yang et al. [132]; Callaghan & O’Mara [40]ϕ
Internal inhibition Passive avoidance Konat et al. [91]; Yang et al. [132]
Retroactive interference Winocur et al. [56]

Frontal lobe-sensitive Rule learning Operant nose-poke Walker et al. [118]


Non-matching-to-sample Winocur et al. [11,15,57,58,61,89]
Temporal order Temporal familiarity Acharya et al. [126]
Conditional learning Conditional associative Winocur et al. [15]
learning

η
Normal spatial learning and recent memory.
π
Impaired remote spatial memory.
λ
Hyper-reactivity to novelty interpreted as defect in flexibility.
ϕ
Normal recognition memory.

and collaborative research effort was needed. The result, at the in- contextual cues in the test environment [17]. The location recognition
vitation of the ICCTF executive, is the following review which includes memory test [18] compares animals’ responses to identical objects in
a formulation of strategic priorities and specific recommendations to familiar or new locations, while the object recognition memory test
guide animal research as it strives to complement and meaningfully [19] compares responses to familiar and unfamiliar objects. Animals
inform clinical science and practice. See [5] for a similar review of exhibit recognition memory when they spend more time exploring
clinical research]. unfamiliar locations or objects, respectively. Rats and mice with hip-
pocampal dysfunction are reliably impaired on all three tasks and,
Cognitive assessment significantly, similar deficits on the MWM [11,20], CFC [21], and lo-
cation and object recognition tests [22–25] have been reported in ro-
Recommendation: As a validation of new research programs into CICI dents administered anti-cancer drugs1. Standard operating procedures
in animals, initial investigations should include reliable tests of memory for these tasks and others to follow are provided in Appendix A.
and executive function in accordance with standard operating proce- Frontal lobe impairment is associated with disruption of a wide
dures. It is further recommended that subsequent investigations within range of attentional and executive functions. In the animal literature,
the program include at least one such reliable test. such processes are often measured by tests of non-matching-to-sample
Clinical neuropsychological studies and preclinical research into the (NMTS) and conditional associative (CAL) learning. Both tasks in-
behavioural effects of anti-cancer drugs have converged in identifying a corporate working memory and rule-learning components in success-
reliable pattern of core cognitive deficits that reflect dysfunction in fully making goal-oriented decisions. The NMTS test consists of a series
hippocampal and frontal-lobe brain regions [6,12–15]. Table 1 provides of paired trials in which, initially, a distinctive stimulus signals reward,
a summary of tests commonly used in animal research to assess the followed immediately by a test trial in which the familiar stimulus is
effects of chemotherapy on cognitive functions mediated by these re- paired with a novel stimulus. On the test trial, the animal must respond
gions. As a validation of research strategies, particularly as they relate to the new stimulus to obtain reward. In CAL, the animal is trained to
to new programs, and to ensure meaningful comparison of results associate different stimuli with different responses to obtain rewards.
across labs, it is strongly recommended that investigators include, in Animals with frontal lobe damage are impaired on both tasks [26,27].
their test batteries, a hippocampus-sensitive test of memory and a test Developed as land-based tests, they have been adapted for use in a
of executive function that depends on the frontal lobes. It is further water-filled pool in which animals are highly motivated to perform.
recommended that subsequent investigations within the program in- These versions require less training than land-based tests and reliably
clude at least one such validation measure. yield deficits in chemotherapy-treated rodents [11,15]; see Appendix A
The Morris Water Maze (MWM) test of spatial memory, contextual for standard operating procedures].
fear conditioning (CFC), and location and object recognition memory
tests are widely accepted tests of hippocampal memory. These straight-
forward tests are well-characterized, and can be administered re- Assessment of non-cognitive responses
peatedly. The latter is particularly important when longitudinal as-
sessment is desirable, or when potential treatments are being evaluated Recommendation: Preclinical research into CICI should be broadened
and it is necessary to compare performance before treatment, im- to include greater assessment of non-cognitive responses (e.g., stress,
mediately after, and over the long term. emotional reactivity) to drugs.
The MWM [16] is a test of context-dependent spatial memory that is Cancer patients undergoing chemotherapy must cope with many
conducted in a circular pool filled with opaque water. The animal is challenges. Not surprisingly, therefore, they often exhibit signs of
required to use environmental cues to learn and remember the fixed emotional instability that adversely affect their quality of life. Clinical
location of a submerged platform on which it can climb and escape the
water. CFC is a test of non-spatial memory in which an association is 1
As noted in Table 1, the effects of chemotherapy on the object recognition test are
formed between a shock-induced fear response and the configuration of somewhat more variable than the new location test.

73
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

investigators have argued justifiably that such responses contribute to Mechanisms of CICI
impaired cognitive function in this population. Indeed, current thinking
acknowledges that ‘chemobrain’ is multifactorial and impacted by Biological
clusters of symptoms that include heightened stress, fatigue, mood al-
terations, and sleep disturbances, amongst others [28,29]. Che- Recommendation: Animal investigators are urged to broaden the
motherapeutic agents themselves can produce many of these non-cog- search for biological mechanisms (e.g., neuroinflammation, HPA-axis
nitive symptoms, which can interact with the drugs’ effects on cognitive system dysfunction, mitochondrial damage), underlying CICI.
systems, and possibly through common neural pathways [30,31]. The identification of biological mechanisms that underlie CICI is
Preclinical research can play an important role in achieving a better essential to [1] understanding the full effects of the drugs, [2] estab-
understanding of co-occurring symptoms associated with che- lishing causal links between such changes and treatment-related cog-
motherapy, and their relationship to cognitive performance. In rodents, nitive deficits and [3] the development of cognitive treatment strate-
emotionality and stress-related responses have been tested reliably in gies. While the search for causal links is still in early stages,
the elevated plus maze [32,33]. This test evaluates stress avoidance by considerable progress has been made in recent years [28,45,46]. To a
requiring the animal to choose between secure parts of the maze (en- large extent, attention has focussed on neuroinflammation, with nu-
closed arms) and aversive parts (open arms). Anxiety-related behaviour merous clinical studies pointing to an association between the release of
is assessed by the number of entries and time spent in the open arms, pro-inflammatory cytokines (e.g., IL-1, IL-6, TNF-α) and behavioural
relative to the enclosed arms. Other reliable tests of emotional re- symptoms (e.g., mood changes, sleep disturbances, cognitive impair-
activity in rodents include the sucrose preference test [34], a functional ment, fatigue) that have come to be identified with CICI. There is also
measure of reward systems that models anhedonia, and the forced swim evidence that elevated cortisol levels and general malfunction of the
test [35], which models depressive-like behaviour by measuring im- HPA axis system contribute to a similar range of symptoms. Biological
mobility in inescapable stressful situations. The tail suspension [36] is a changes associated with disruption of the two systems interact with
common alternative to the forced swimming test, but it is more suitable each other and there is growing consensus that chronically high stress
for use with mice than rats. These and other tests commonly used to levels associated with cancer and treatment commonly trigger both sets
measure emotional reactivity are cited in Table 2. Animal researchers of responses [47].
have begun to use some of these tests and the general finding has been Other potential biomarkers of CICI include disruption of the
that drug-induced cognitive deficits are often accompanied by increases monoamine neurotransmitter system [48], mitochondrial impairment
in anxiety or depressive-like behaviour e.g., [37–40]; see Appendix A and oxidative stress [49], and changes in brain structure [50] and
for standard operating procedures]. function [51] that may reflect disruption of functional brain networks
Fatigue, possibly related to loss of muscle strength, sleep dis- [52]. Recently, attention has been drawn to chemotherapy-induced
turbances, or over-exertion, is a common complaint of cancer survivors changes in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) compounds (e.g., phospholipids,
following chemotherapy. Fatigue in rodents can be assessed in lysophosphatidyl-acetylcholine) that serve as markers of white matter
straightforward ways by measuring activity in their home cages, open integrity and, potentially, indicators of cognitive impairment. Altera-
field environments, or running wheels. For example, reductions in tions in CSF resulting from chemotherapy have also been associated
spontaneous locomotion and running wheel activity have been reported with significant increases in tau production which are reliably linked to
in mice treated with paclitaxel [41], and in rats receiving a combination neurodegenerative disease [51]. Many of these biological changes (e.g.,
of cyclophosphamide and doxorubicin [42]. neuroinflammation, CSF and mitochondrial disruptions) have been as-
It is worth mentioning an ingenious test of ‘behavioural economics’ sociated with both chemotherapy and tumour growth, reinforcing the
developed by Salamone and colleagues [43] to measure fatigue and notion that the search for mechanisms underlying CICI must be con-
effort-related symptoms. In this task, the rodent is placed in an operant sidered in terms of their relationship to multiple factors.
conditioning chamber where it can obtain a desirable food reward by Relationships between biological changes and cancer-related cog-
pressing a lever a fixed number of times. Alternatively, it can directly nitive impairment are being vigorously pursued in clinical investiga-
access less palatable lab chow with minimal effort. The degree of effort tions. As Seigers and Fardell [6] pointed out, preclinical research is also
the animal is prepared to put forth to obtain reward can be determined essential to this process and, in at least one important area, has led the
by varying the lever-press/reward ratio. When confronted with a way. Animal studies have shown that anti-cancer drugs suppress the
choice, well-trained normal rats will get most of their food by lever- production of new cells and their functional integration into the adult
pressing for the desirable food pellets, and will consume relatively little hippocampus [neurogenesis – 20,23,53–56]. This significant finding
lab chow. Animals that are less motivated to work for food, as a result helps to explain the loss of hippocampus-dependent memory and re-
of stress or emotional unresponsiveness, prefer the freely available, but lated functions that are reliably observed following chemotherapy. This
less palatable, lab chow. This test has the added advantage of tapping relationship was confirmed in complementary animal studies showing
into related affective processes, such as motivation and attaching value that recovery from chemotherapy-induced memory loss following
to rewards, that factor into symptom clusters that contribute to CICI. physical exercise [57] and environmental enrichment [58] is positively
While adapted for use in animal models of anhedonia and related correlated with restoration of new cell production.
conditions [44], it has not been used in a model of chemobrain, al- Preclinical research has linked CICI to increased cell death and al-
though it appears well suited for this purpose (see Appendix A). terations in synaptic plasticity in specific brain regions (e.g.,

Table 2
Common tests used in preclinical research to assess emotional reactivity.
Behaviour Test References

Emotionality, anxiety, conflict resolution Elevated plus maze Lister et al. [32]; Pellow et al. [33]
Light-dark emergence test Smith et al. [133]; Kitamura et al. [129]
Tail suspension test Can et al. [134]; Steru et al. [36]
Stress, depressive behaviour Forced swim test Can et al. [135]; Porsolt et al. [136]; Callaghan & O’Mara [40]
Sucrose preference test Willner et al. [34]; Krishnan et al. [137]; El Yacoubi et al. [138]
Impulsive behaviour Resident intruder aggression test Mineur et al. [139]
Compulsive behaviour Marble burying test Nicolas et al. [140]

74
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

hippocampus, prefrontal cortex), as well as mitochondrial damage, observed deficits (e.g., skill acquisition, problem solving). With respect
neuroinflammation, and HPA-system dysfunction [6,59]. The meth- to memory loss, chemotherapy affects autobiographical episodic
odologies for assessing co-varying changes in cognitive and biological memory, especially when it is necessary to suppress related, but in-
systems are clearly available. For many labs, the challenge will be to appropriate, memories that arise during the retrieval process. However,
formulate multi-disciplinary, comprehensive research programs that memory for semantic or schematic information (general knowledge,
take into account multiple operative components (e.g., psychosocial, well-learned habits and routines) has not been investigated to the same
cognitive, neurobiological) and enable meaningful distinctions between degree. This type of memory entails other forms of processing and re-
correlational and causative relations. presentation, and it is not clear how it is impacted by anti-cancer drugs.
Tasks for addressing such issues are available, or can be developed,
Cognitive thereby enabling preclinical research to play a central role in describing
fundamental cognitive processes that are directly affected by che-
Recommendation: Animal scientists should intensify the investiga- motherapeutic agents.
tion of basic cognitive mechanisms (e.g., attentional processes) asso-
ciated with broad categories of behavioural symptoms resulting from Risk/protective factors and treatment
chemotherapy.
Research into basic cognitive operations that are affected by anti- Recommendation: Preclinical researchers should attach priority to
cancer drugs has lagged behind the search for biological mechanisms. It identifying risk factors for CICI and contributing to the development of
is clear from subjective reports and neuropsychological investigations interventions aimed at minimizing cognitive loss following treatment.
that, following chemotherapy, cancer survivors have difficulty focusing In their review of candidate mechanisms of chemobrain, Ahles and
and sustaining attention. This condition, sometimes termed, ‘atten- Saykin [1] outlined various genetic variabilities that could increase the
tional fatigue’ [60], is manifested in various ways including heightened risk of cognitive impairment following chemotherapy. Particular at-
distractibility, repetitive thought processes, and poor multi-tasking. tention has focussed on Apoliprotein-E (APO-E), a class of apoliproteins
These symptoms suggest a breakdown in an internal regulatory system that is responsible for transporting cholesterol and other fats through
that, in situations involving competing demands, serves to inhibit in- the bloodstream. Individuals carrying an APOE-4 allele are at increased
fluences that interfere with performing the task at hand. Damage to risk for age-related cognitive decline and dementia of the Alzheimer’s
such a system could account for many, if not all, of the cognitive deficits type [64]. Ahles et al. [65] found that cancer survivors with the APOE-4
exhibited by such individuals. allele experienced more cognitive impairment following chemotherapy
Winocur et al. [56] tested the hypothesis that chemotherapy induces than those who did not. Other genetic polymorphisms involving, for
a state of internal disinhibition and exaggerated susceptibility to in- example, the catechol-o-methyltransferase gene have also been linked
terference in rats. A retroactive interference paradigm was used in to cognitive deficits in chemotherapy-treated cancer patients [66].
which animals were initially trained on a discrimination task and, fol- A number of psychosocial and lifestyle variables influence the re-
lowing an interpolated high- or low-interference experience, were re- sponse of cancer survivors to chemotherapy. Cognitive reserve refers to
tested on the same task. Chemotherapy and control groups did not the brain’s capacity to draw on enriching experiences during youth to
differ in original learning, but drug-treated rats in the high-interference protect against later assaults on neurocognitive function [67]. Built up
condition were severely impaired in relearning the discrimination. The through stimulating experiences, such as education and environmental
deficit was correlated with suppression of neurogenesis levels, a finding enrichment, cognitive reserve enables compensatory mechanisms to
in line with reports of hippocampal involvement in this and other high- reduce the damaging effects of neurodegenerative disease, traumatic
interference tasks [61]. Given the pervasiveness of attentional deficits brain injury, and vascular damage. Ahles et al. [2] found that low
in patients receiving chemotherapy, it is likely that other brain regions cognitive reserve, as measured by the Wide Range Achievement Test,
are involved as part of a complex neural circuitry. Indeed, Wimmer was a major factor in the poor performance on standard neuropsycho-
et al. [62], using a cross-modal divided attention task designed for logical tests by breast cancer patients who had received chemotherapy.
mice, identified a network involving the thalamic reticular system, In the same study, Ahles et al. [2] found that the effects of che-
under prefrontal cortical control, that appears to support information motherapy on cognitive performance were exacerbated in older parti-
filtering in high-interference situations. cipants. Age is known to affect recovery from cancer and, in particular,
Inhibitory control is a complex process that involves several fun- acute lymphoblastic leukemia (ALL), which occurs predominantly in
damental components. In addition to sensory gating, as demonstrated children under the age of five and adults over 50. Five-year survival
by Wimmer et al. [62], the ability to evaluate conflicting information rates following intrathecal treatment that prevents spread of disease or
and exercise response control is required, and chemotherapy could neurotoxic effects of other forms of treatment (e.g., cranial radio-
adversely affect any or all of these operations. In an initial attempt to therapy) to the brain, are as high as 80% in children but only between
address these questions, Winocur (in preparation) examined the effects 30% and 40% in adults [68]. These are significant findings in that age is
of chemotherapy in three passive avoidance tasks. Each task had an the single most important risk factor for the development of many
essential inhibitory component but they differed in critical ways. One forms of cancer. The aging brain is susceptible to insult so the combined
was a basic test of inhibitory avoidance in which the animal had to effects of disease, treatment, and age are likely to produce exaggerated
suppress the tendency to step down from a platform onto a grid floor cognitive deficits. It is surprising, therefore, that there has been so little
that delivered a foot shock; a second task, conducted in a runway, re- research into this question. Among the few studies, a large prospective
quired the animal to suppress a learned approach response to avoid trial of older breast cancer patients found that cognitive decline fol-
shock; the third task was similar to the second but, before testing, the lowing chemotherapy was greatest in the oldest individuals, particu-
animal was cued with the distinctive features of the shock-associated larly when the regimen included docetaxel [69]. In another study also
goal box. Chemotherapy-treated rats exhibited normal response in- involving breast cancer patients, cognitive function recovered more
hibition in tasks 1 and 3 but were impaired on task 2, indicating that slowly in older patients [70]. An appreciation of the impact of aging is
the problem was likely related to a deficit in processing competing needed to better understand the dynamics of CICI and is crucial for its
information associated with approach and avoidance responses. prevention and treatment.
These findings provide direction for further investigation of the Physical activity is widely advocated in promoting brain health and
nature of chemotherapy-induced attentional deficits. It has also been in protecting against cognitive losses resulting from various conditions,
suggested that the cognitive aging process offers a model for CICI [2,63] including stroke and normal and pathological aging. Cancer survivors,
and that generalised cognitive slowing can account for some of the who engage in regular exercise, consistently experience better physical

75
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

and mental health, longer survival times, superior cognitive perfor- moderate, suggesting that such individuals may be good candidates for
mance, and overall higher quality of life [71–76]. cognitive rehabilitation programs. Few such programs have been de-
As indicated above, stress associated with a cancer diagnosis is a signed specifically for this population but there have been promising
contributing factor to CICI. Cancer patients are prone to post-traumatic developments. For example, Ferguson et al. [80] have developed a
stress following diagnosis [77] and at least one study has demonstrated behavioural training program (Memory and Attention Adaptation
that this form of stress exacerbates the cognitive impairment subse- Training – MATT) in which patients learn compensatory strategies for
quently experienced by breast cancer patients following chemotherapy improving cognitive function and coping with the challenges of daily
[78]. It follows that interventions aimed at reducing post-traumatic living. In a trial involving breast cancer patients, these investigators
stress could ameliorate behavioural symptoms resulting from treat- reported that training resulted in significant improvement on various
ment. measures of cognitive performance and quality-of-life. Wolf et al. [81]
Most of the risk factors identified in clinical studies are being in- developed a metacognitive strategy training program (MCST) that tar-
vestigated in preclinical research, and with similar results. For example, gets primarily executive function. In a preliminary pre-post, within-
following chemotherapy, rats, like humans, perform better on cognitive subjects study of 14 breast cancer patients, the group receiving training
tests when allowed to engage in physical activity [24,57]. Similarly, improved significantly on several cognitive tests including the Cogni-
chemotherapy-treated rats reared in diverse environments that provide tive Failures Test, the Dysexecutive Questionnaire, and the Delis-Kaplan
opportunities for social stimulation and new learning, exhibit less Executive Function Test, as well as several psychosocial measures. It is
cognitive impairment than similarly treated rats housed in standard lab noteworthy that behavioural improvements were accompanied by en-
cages [58]. hanced functional connectivity in 6/10 patients who underwent func-
In a recently completed study, Winocur et al. (in preparation) ex- tional neuroimaging. Kesler at al. [82] also reported significant cogni-
amined the effects of building up cognitive reserve and chemotherapy tive recovery in breast cancer patients who underwent training that,
in a transgenic model of breast cancer (see next section for a description like the MCST, targeted executive functions.
of the model). At the age of three months, transgenic and control mice These studies represent progress but, in absolute terms, the ob-
were trained on a battery of cognitive tests and then, six months later, served benefits were modest. As well, it is not clear if they were long-
after tumours had developed and the mice received chemotherapy, a lasting or how well they generalised to real-world situations.
new set of tests was administered. Tumour growth and chemotherapy Nevertheless, the results provide direction and a foundation for a
were each associated with cognitive impairment in mice not receiving broader approach in developing interventions for cancer survivors. For
early cognitive training. At test, all the groups in the early training example, the results of clinical and animal studies involving physical
condition (tumourigenic mice receiving saline, chemotherapy-treated activity and social stimulation point to the potential gains of in-
normal mice, and tumourigenic mice that had received chemotherapy) corporating exercise regimens and techniques for enhancing lifestyle
out-performed their counterparts that had not received training. The into rehabilitation programs that emphasize the application of atten-
results confirm the benefits of early enrichment and point to cognitive tional and information-processing strategies.
reserve as a powerful protector of neurocognitive function from the No pharmacotherapies have been approved specifically for CICI
later effects of peripheral cancers and chemotherapy treatment. although several drugs have been tested [83,84], with mixed results.
There has been little preclinical research into age as a factor in CICI. For example, the stimulant, modafinil, was shown to reduce fatigue and
In a notable exception, Dubois et al. [23] compared the impact of improve cognitive function in breast cancer patients treated with che-
chemotherapy on cognitive function in young (8 weeks) and aged motherapy [85], but another stimulant, methylphenidate, was not ef-
(20 months) mice in relation to the proliferation of hippocampal neural fective [86]. As noted above, patients with ALL often receive intrathecal
stem cells (NSC). An interesting result was that age-related emotional treatment prophylactically to protect the brain but cognitive loss is
and cognitive deficits were associated with a significant decrease of known to occur whether or not cranial radiation is also administered.
hippocampal-dividing NSC. Chemotherapy equally affected the pool of There is clinical evidence that the glutamate receptor antagonist, dex-
NSC in both young and aged mice, but there was evidence that di- tromethorphan, can counteract the secondary effects of intrathecal
minished cognition in the aged animals affected their ability to with- MTX treatment [87] and, in an animal model, Vijayanathan et al. [88]
stand the drug-induced effects. Recently, Winocur (in preparation) ex- showed that the reversal extends to cognitive deficits.
amined the effects of chemotherapy and aging in young (6 months) and The anticholinesterase drug, donepezil, extensively used to slow the
older (16 months) rats on cognition. Age and chemotherapy each con- progression of dementia in early Alzheimer’s Disease, has been shown
tributed to cognitive impairment but the combined effects were ex- to reduce cognitive deficits resulting from a combination of MTX + 5-
ponentially greater than the singular effects. The limited evidence from FU in rats [89]. However, there is little clinical evidence of this effect.
animal studies is consistent with the view that normal aging exacer- Other drugs that have been shown to reduce cognitive impairment
bates the neurotoxic effects of chemotherapy, and that the two factors following chemotherapy in animals include the SSRI antidepressant,
interact with each other. fluoxetine [90], the anti-oxidant, N-acetylcysteine [91], and the ribo-
Similarly, there has been little investigation into the effects of nucleotide reductase inhibitor, PAN-811[92] but again, they have not
chemotherapy on developing brains. One study examined the potential been tested adequately in clinical trials
for early life low-dose chemotherapy to induce long-term cognitive Preclinical research has demonstrated that it has a role to play in
impairments. Li et al. [79] administered low-dose methotrexate (MTX; finding ways to prevent and treat cancer-related cognitive impairment.
1 mg/kg) to juvenile rats beginning just after weaning (post-natal day Ultimately, real progress will depend on the coordinated efforts of in-
16) for 8 weeks. When tested as adults, MTX-treated rats demonstrated dividuals with a broad range of expertise in cancer pathology, neu-
impairments in location recognition tests but not in object recognition roscience, neuropsychology, and patient care. These efforts must in-
tests. Moreover, analysis of CSF showed suppressed folate persisting for clude developing strategies for designing clinical studies to validate the
at least 3 weeks after treatment. These results point to the value of findings of animal studies that identify cognitive changes and under-
preclinical models in examining the effects of anti-cancer drugs on the lying mechanisms. The best outcomes are likely to be realized through
cognitive development of children, and the potential effects on life-long close cooperation between clinical and basic researchers, and with
cognitive function treatment programs that combine the strengths of pharmacological and
The discovery of risk/protective factors is a reminder that cancer- behavioural approaches. With that in mind, it may be informative to
related cognitive impairment is not immutable and that, even when examine cognitive recovery programs that have had success with other
spontaneous recovery is limited, interventions are possible. The degree compromised populations – e.g., normal older adults, patients with
of impairment in cancer survivors is usually described as mild to Mild Cognitive Impairment, individuals with traumatic brain injury

76
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

[e.g., 93–95]. An important advantage of tumour transplantation is that it can be


performed with either established human tumour cell lines or human
Creating better animal models tumour cells. Examples include patient-derived xenograft models of
breast cancer in which a patient’s tumour cells are propagated through
Recommendation: In developing animal models of CICI, greater use successive generations of mice and then implanted orthotopically into
should be made of tumour-bearing rodents. the mammary fat pad of murine hosts. This allows the investigation of
In order to determine unconfounded effects of chemotherapy on interactive effects of human cancer and chemotherapy on cognition
neurocognitive function, animal researchers typically experiment with under controlled conditions that are not possible in clinical studies.
healthy, young adult rodents. A major limitation of this approach is that However, severely compromised immunodeficient mouse strains must
anti-cancer drugs are never prescribed for healthy people – only for be used in human xenograft tumour models so that their immune
people who have been diagnosed with cancer. Thus, the most com- system does not reject the tumour cells. Thus, in addition to being ex-
monly used models do not take into account the potential effects of the pensive and time-consuming, human xenograft tumour models have the
disease itself on cognitive function. The importance of considering tu- disadvantage of requiring animals to be continuously maintained in a
mour development as a contributing factor in cognitive impairment is completely sterile environment. The necessity of sterility and the dif-
underscored by mounting clinical evidence that cancerous patients can ficulties with animal handling pose serious challenges for some cogni-
exhibit cognitive deficits at the time of diagnosis, before receiving any tive testing protocols. A major advantage of transgenic or genetically
form of treatment [96–100]. modified models is that the animals are immunocompetent and can be
There has been progress in developing cancer-bearing mouse exposed to the environments which are routine in established cognitive
models, although such models are just beginning to be incorporated testing protocols.
into behavioural research. Transgenic mice that are genetically en-
gineered to develop human forms of cancer are especially promising. Other therapies
One transgenic model that has received considerable attention in the
biological literature is the well-characterized and commercially avail- This review has focused on cognitive changes associated with che-
able FVB/N-Tg (MMTV-neu) 202 Mul/J mouse that expresses similar motherapy but many other cancer treatments are available and, for the
pathology to that seen in breast cancer in women [101]. By most part, their effects on cognitive function have been insufficiently
8–10 months, tumours typically appear in 70–80% of the animals. Life studied. It is well known that cranial radiotherapy seriously impairs
expectancy, when untreated, is an additional 4–6 months, which makes neurocognitive function in children and adults [59], but peripheral
them good candidates for cognitive assessment. Winocur and colleagues radiotherapy can also affect cognition. Shibayam et al. [107] showed
[102] confirmed CICI in this model and, consistent with clinical reports, that breast cancer patients, who received adjuvant regional radio-
also observed cognitive deficits in tumour-bearing mice that had not therapy, exhibited significant memory loss and related cognitive im-
received chemotherapy. An investigation of underlying biological me- pairment, relative to patients not exposed to radiotherapy. Interest-
chanisms showed that impaired cognition in treated and untreated ingly, the authors identified elevated levels of pro-inflammatory
animals was related, in varying degrees, to changes in neurogenesis cytokines, specifically IL-6, as a factor in the deficits. To our knowledge,
levels, brain volume (particularly in regions implicated in cognitive there have been no investigations of cognitive changes following per-
performance), and dysregulation of cytokine activity. These results re- ipheral radiotherapy in animals.
inforce the potential of this transgenic mouse for studying CICI in breast Targeted therapies target specific molecules within cancer cells that
cancer, and underscore the need to develop animal models that in- send signals in support of cell division and tumour growth. For ex-
corporate disease-related factors as well as other potential variables ample, anti-angiogenic therapy (AAT) is designed to suppress the
that influence CICI in humans. growth of new blood vessels that are needed for tumour development.
Another commercially available transgenic breast cancer model is Joly et al. [3] reported that patients with metastatic renal cell carci-
the MMTV-PyMT mouse [103], which develops highly aggressive noma, who received AAT [vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF)
mammary tumours. By 14–16 weeks, females develop tumours in inhibitor] exhibited cognitive deficits, as well as deterioration in quality
mammary glands which rapidly metastasize to lung. This model can be of life. In parallel work, Castel and her colleagues (in preparation)
used for certain types of cognitive assessment, but a limiting factor is observed spatial learning deficits in mice following VEGF inhibitor
that the animals generally exhibit high tumour load and metastatic treatment. Finally, Dubois et al. [23] tested the effects of AAT in a study
disease within 8–12 weeks of initial tumours. of mice receiving the mTOR inhibitor, everolimus. They found no al-
Another way to mimic human cancer in animals is to utilise tumour terations in cognitive performance or emotional reactivity. Nor was
transplantation techniques in which cancer cells are implanted under there an effect on neural cell proliferation in the hippocampus. How-
the skin or into a specific tissue/organ in immunocompromised mice ever, everolimus-treated mice did experience significant fatigue, weight
tumour xenograft model – [104]. Yang et al. [105], using this tech- loss, and altered activity in brain areas involved in the sleep/wake
nique, found that tumour-bearing mice (xenografted with a colon car- cycle. Although the results of the Dubois et al. [22] study were essen-
cinoma cell line CT26) exhibited depressive behaviour and impaired tially negative with respect to changes in learning and memory, on
object recognition memory. These effects were associated with a balance, the evidence does point to the likelihood that cognition is
number of biological changes including increased levels of mRNA en- compromised following AAT.
coding pro-inflammatory cytokines in the hippocampus, plasma glu- New generation hormonal therapies, which are used increasingly in
cocorticoid level, along with reduced levels of hippocampal brain-de- cancer treatment, can also lead to cognitive decline. Recent reviews
rived neurotrophic factor mRNA, and hippocampal neurogenesis. indicate that men receiving androgen deprivation therapy (ADT) for
Seigers et al. [106] compared biological and behavioural changes in prostate cancer perform worse than controls on tests of cognitive
Buffalo rats that were subcutaneously injected with Morris Hepatoma function [108,109]. However, other investigations have failed to yield
7777 cells or a phosphate buffered saline (PBS) control solution. Tu- such effects [110].
mour development was associated with decreased hippocampal cell Anti-estrogen drugs, such as tamoxifen, are commonly administered
proliferation, reduced food intake and body weight. However, there to prevent or treat breast cancer. Reduced estrogen levels have been
were differences between rats receiving PBS and normal or cancerous linked to cognitive losses in post-menopausal women [111] and in early
rats receiving methotrexate, suggesting that, in the heterotopic tumour stage Alzheimer Disease [112]. It follows that tamoxifen could impair
model, inhibition of hippocampal proliferation is mediated by che- cognitive performance in cancer patients but the evidence is mixed
motherapy. [113,114]. Aromatase inhibitors, enzymes that block the production of

77
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

estrogen, are commonly used in conjunction with or as a replacement its own or in combination with other drugs, has been administered to
for tamoxifen. While aromatase inhibitors are potentially effective as a rodents at dosages that vary between 50 mg/kg [15] and 150 mg/kg
cancer treatment, their effects on cognitive function are uncertain [119]. In general, decisions regarding dosages are often made on the
[113,115,116]. basis of dose–response tests that identify a threshold at which sickness
Even less is known conclusively about the impact of other cancer behaviour is observed. Alternatively, estimates of clinically relevant
treatments (e.g., surgery, immunotherapy, neural stem cell transplan- dosages are made but they are not necessarily reliable. The conversion
tation) on cognition and psychosocial activities. As these therapies of drug dosages to animals must take into account several factors, in-
improve and survival rates increase, greater appreciation of secondary cluding differences in pharamacokinetics and metabolism, that could
effects will be needed to ensure quality of life. It is likely that different affect toxicity. Calculating dosage on the basis of the animal’s estimated
treatments affect cognition in different ways and, since patients often surface area can help to account for differences in metabolic activity
receive more than one treatment, an understanding of singular and [120]. As well, Nair & Jacob [121] have devised a promising conver-
combined effects of the various treatments will be required. As with sion algorithm, based on allometric scaling methods, that appears
chemotherapy, strategies that incorporate preclinical and clinical re- promising for extrapolating clinically relevant drug dosages from hu-
search in collaborative ways will be needed. With respect to the con- mans to animals.
tributions of preclinical research, the issues identified in this review are Another important consideration is the scheduling of treatments.
as relevant to the other therapies as they are to chemotherapy, and Many animal studies examine the effect of a single acute dose of che-
should provide useful direction in developing appropriate and com- motherapy drugs on subsequent behaviour and cognitive abilities.
plementary animal models. While these studies provide important information about the potential
for anti-cancer drugs to cause neurocognitive insult, they fail to address
Summary and other considerations the potential of repeated cycles of chemotherapy treatment for causing
progressive and long-term damage to the nervous system. For instance,
Preclinical research has played a significant role in establishing CICI a single acute dose of oxaliplatin produces impairment on the location
as a legitimate clinical problem that should be taken into account in recognition test that typically resolves after a month (Johnston, in
treatment programs. Work with animal models has confirmed the pat- preparation), whereas 3 cycles of the same dose produces impairment
tern of drug-induced cognitive deficits first described by clinical sci- that persists for at least 11 months after treatment [122].
entists, and has led the way in identifying putative biological me- Finally, much of the preclinical research into CICI has been con-
chanisms that result in impaired neurocognitive function. It is ducted on female animals and that is because, for some time, patient
encouraging that many labs are becoming increasingly engaged in this complaints were mainly from female survivors of breast cancer, and
field of research but, at the same time, heterogeneity in methodology most of the clinical research was conducted on this population. It is now
and redundancies in strategy could undermine continued progress. clear that CICI occurs in patients with other types of cancer, including
The present review includes a number of key recommendations, prostate [123] and testicular [124], which of course, affect only men, as
notably the adoption of standard cognitive testing procedures, parti- well as colorectal [100] and lymphoma [125], which affect both men
cularly as new programs are being launched. In light of evidence that a and women in large numbers. Moreover, many drugs are used to treat
range of non-cognitive behavioural changes often accompany cognitive several forms of cancer. Unless there is a specific reason for not doing
deficits, it was recommended that tests of emotional reactivity (e.g., so, it is advisable that male and female animals be used in preclinical
stress, fatigue) also be included in standard protocols. As well, the need research to assess sex differences in response to the drugs.
to accelerate the search for risk factors and underlying mechanisms in The development of improved animal models that accurately reflect
order to better understanding the disorder and develop effective CICI in humans will enhance our appreciation of the disorder and ad-
treatments, was emphasized. vance the field in practical terms. Also needed, particularly in the
A number of issues, considered beyond the scope of this review, current era of limited funding opportunities, is the formation of colla-
deserve mention. For example, whereas some chemotherapeutic agents borative networks of basic and clinical scientists, as well as open
have been extensively examined with respect to their effects on cog- communication and the sharing of resources and expertise between
nition (e.g., MTX, 5-FU, doxorubicin, docetaxel), most have not. Studies research centres. The guidelines outlined here are not intended to be
that investigated multiple drugs typically found that cognitive functions exhaustive – rather, along with the proposed recommendations, they
under hippocampal or frontal-lobe control are most often affected. In are intended to highlight the central role of preclinical research in
what appears to be the most ambitious study of this type, Seigers et al. understanding, preventing, and treating CICI, and to help in the plan-
[117] treated mice with cyclophosphamide, docetaxel, doxorubicin, 5- ning and implementation of sound, translationally relevant, research
FU, MTX, or topotecan, and tested them on several learning and strategies.
memory tasks. All drugs affected hippocampus-sensitive, object and
spatial recognition memory, and three (cyclophosphamide, docetaxel,
doxorubicin) resulted in impairment on the Barnes test of spatial Acknowledgements
memory, another test of hippocampal memory. Performance on a
choice reaction-time test that required directed and sustained attention The authors are grateful to the ICCTF executive, Dr. Tim Ahles, Dr.
as well as inhibitory control, frontal-lobe mediated processes, were Sanne Schagen, Dr. Janette Vardy, and Dr. Jeffrey Wefel for their
disrupted by cyclophosphamide, docetaxel, and topotecan. Walker et al. support and for having commented on earlier versions of this paper.
[118] administered tamoxifen, MTX, and 5-FU, individually or in Thanks also to Dr. Melanie Sekeres, Dr. Peter Cole, and Dr. Jeanne
combination, to mice and tested them on a variable-interval operant Mandelblatt for their helpful contributions to the final draft. The
conditioning task that also depended on frontal-lobe controlled execu- technical assistance of Nick Hoang is gratefully acknowledged.
tive processes. All groups were impaired on this task with the extent of The preparation of the paper was supported by grants to GW from
impairment related to drug and dosage. Clearly, it is necessary to cast a the Canadian Institutes for Health Research and the Canadian Cancer
wider net in studying the impact of commonly-used anti-cancer drugs Society, to HC from the French National Institute for Health, the
on specific cognitive domains. Northwest Cancéropôle, the Ligue Nationale contre le Cancer, and the
The importance of paying close attention to drug dosage to ensure French National Cancer Institute, and to IJ from the New South Wales
valid comparisons between preclinical findings and clinical cases Cancer Council.
cannot be overestimated. Even a cursory review of the literature reveals
that drug dosages vary widely in animal studies. For example, 5-FU, on

78
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

Conflict of interest Morris water maze (MWM) test of spatial memoryApparatus. The MWM
spatial memory test is administered in a circular pool located in the
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. All au- centre of a standard testing room. The pool is filled with opaque water
thors have read and approved the final version of the review. and maintained at room temperature (21 °C). An inverted flower pot,
situated a few cm below the surface, can serve as a platform on which
the animal can climb to escape the water. Throughout testing, the water
Author’s Contributions should be cleaned after each trial and changed every 2–3 days.
The pool can be divided into six zones of approximately equal size.
GW is the lead author of the paper. HC and IJ helped conceptualize Swimming patterns and escape latencies are monitored by a centrally-
the review, made original contributions to various sections, and helped located overhead camera connected to a data processing system. The
with revisions. system enables computation of the time required to mount the platform
(latency) as well as swimming patterns. An error is recorded each time
the animal enters a zone not containing the platform.Procedure. The
Appendix A. Standard operating procedures for behavioural tests task begins with two days of orientation to the maze (5 trials/day) in
which the animal is individually placed in the pool and allowed to swim
Hippocampus-sensitive memory tests to a visible platform. The start point and the location of the platform are
randomized on each trial. By the end of the second day all animals
Contextual fear conditioningApparatus. Contextual Fear Conditioning should be swimming directly to the platform.
(CFC) can be conducted in a chamber (e.g., 50 × 40 × 18-cm) that Training begins the following day. The platform is now positioned a
consists of four clear Plexiglas walls, a hinged clear Plexiglas roof with few cm below the surface and fixed in the centre of one of the zones of
holes to allow ventilation, and a floor made of metal rods, spaced the pool (e.g., north-east). To start a trial, the animal is placed in the
1.3 cm apart. During fear-conditioning, a tone, presented through a water, facing the wall at the edge of one of 5 zones. Animals are never
centrally mounted speaker attached to the roof of the box, is paired placed in the zone containing the platform. Each trial continues until
with a 1 s, 1 mA foot shock that is delivered by a shock generator. A the animal mounts the platform with all four paws, or until 60 s have
video camera and recording equipment mounted above the test elapsed. If the animal fails to find the platform within 60 sec, it is
chamber records freezing behaviour. manually guided to the platform. After 20 s on the platform, the animal
The fear conditioning chamber is positioned on a table, at least 1 m is placed in a holding cage that contains a heat lamp to await the next
above the floor, and situated in the centre of a standard laboratory trial. Five such trials, with an approximate inter-trial interval of
room. Illumination is provided by overhead lighting and all environ- 4–5 min, are administered each day for 5 consecutive days. Escape la-
mental cues (e.g., pictures, bookshelves) must be located in the same tency and errors are recorded for each trial. If the mouse fails to find the
locations throughout the experiment.Fear conditioning training. On platform within 60 s, it can be given an arbitrary error score of 15 and a
Day 1, the animal is transferred to the test room, placed in the con- latency score of 60 s for that trial.
ditioning chamber, and allowed 30 min to explore. This exposure is On the sixth day of training, trials 1 & 2 are administered in the
important as it enables normal animals to become familiar with the usual manner. The third trial serves as a probe trial. For the probe trial,
chamber and form a strong representation of contextual cues within the the platform is removed, and the amount of time the animal spends
environment. The animal is then returned to its home cage. swimming in the platform target zone (north-east zone) is recorded and
The next day, the animal is transferred to the test room, placed in used as an index of the rat’s memory of the platform’s location. After 60
the conditioning chamber, and allowed 3 min to settle down. At the s, the rat is removed and returned to its home cage.
2 min mark, its freezing behaviour is recorded every 5 s for 60 s
Freezing is defined by an immobilized crouching response in which the
only detectable movement is the animal’s breathing. Ten tone (CS) – Novel location recognition (NLR)2Apparatus. The NLR test is typically
shock (US) pairings (tone: 2000 Hz; 80–90 db, 30 s; shock: 1 mA, 1 s) conducted in a plastic box (e.g., 49-cm wide × 66-cm long × 40-cm
are then administered with a variable interval (30–120 s) between high). Stimuli can consist of small ceramic or plastic objects (e.g., for
pairings. After the last shock, freezing behaviour is recorded every 5 s rats – 7–12 cm high × 7–9 cm wide; smaller objects would apply for
for 60 s. The animal is then returned to its home cage.Fear conditioning mice) that vary in colour and size. The objects are secured to the floor of
testing. Testing is conducted 24 h after training. Shock is not ad- the box with Velcro strips. A video camera mounted on the wall directly
ministered during testing. above the box can be used to record the testing session for off-line
Context-only. The animal is placed in the conditioning chamber for analysis.
5 min and its freezing behaviour recorded every 5 s. The animal is then Familiarisation training. Initially, animals receive two sessions in
returned to its home cage. The amount of time spent freezing is the which they spend 45 min in the testing room and 5 min exploring the
measure of fear conditioning to the contextual environment. Animals empty apparatus. Three days of familiarisation training follow. On each
with hippocampal impairment typically freeze less than normal ani- day, the animal is placed in the testing room for 45 min and then in the
mals. empty box for 1 min. The animal is then removed and two identical
Context + CS. One hour later, the animal is returned to the con- objects are placed in different corners of the apparatus. The animal is
ditioning chamber. returned to the box and allowed to explore for 5 min. This procedure is
After 60 s, the tone is sounded for 5 min during which freezing repeated two more times on each day (mean interval between ex-
behaviour is recorded every 5 s. After the tone has ceased, the animal's posures is about 60 min).
freezing behaviour is recorded every 5 s for 120 s. The purpose of this NLR testing. At the beginning of the test session, the animal is
test is to assess fear conditioning to a discrete conditional stimulus – placed in the empty apparatus for 2 min. It is then placed in a holding
hippocampally-impaired and normal animals typically do not differ on cage for 10 min before returned to the apparatus for a 10-minute choice
this test. The animal is then returned to its home cage. trial. In this trial, one of the objects from the familiarisation sessions
remains in the same corner (‘familiar’ location) and the other is moved

2
Dimensions pertaining to apparatus and objects are intended for testing rats. The New
Location and Object Recognition tests can be administered to mice but dimensions must
be scaled down accordingly.

79
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

to a different corner (‘new’ location), and the animal is allowed to ex- Conditional associative learning (CAL)
plore the two objects. Object exploration is scored from video record-
ings. The animal is said to be exploring an object when its nose is within Apparatus
2 cm of the object and the vibrissae were moving. Object exploration is The CAL task is administered in a circular pool (as above), that is
not scored when the animal gnaws the object or uses it to rear upward fitted with a cross-maze.
with its nose facing the ceiling.
A second test can be conducted 24 h later. Following a 2-minute re- Procedure
familiarisation with the empty apparatus and a 10-minute period in a Training begins with three days of orientation to the maze (10
holding cage, the animal is returned to the apparatus. For this test, the trials/day) in which the animal is placed in one of the arms facing the
‘familiar location’ object remains in the same corner but the ‘new lo- edge of the pool. At the centre of the maze, a grey cylinder, suspended
cation’ object is again moved, this time to the remaining open corner. 5 cm above the water and near the entrance of the arm containing a
visible platform, signals the location of the platform at the end of the
arm. The start point and the arm containing the platform are rando-
Object recognition (OR)Apparatus. The type of apparatus and materials
mized on each trial. If the animal fails to find the platform within 60 s,
described for the NLR test can be used for the OR test.
it is manually guided to the platform, where it remains for 20 s. By the
Familiarisation training. Familiarisation training is the same as in
end of the third day all animals should be swimming well and finding
the NLR test.
the platform within 60 s. Animals that take longer than 60 s may be
OR testing. The animal is rehabituated to the empty test apparatus
given an additional day of familiarisation training.
for 2 min. It is then removed and two objects (one novel object and an
For the CAL task, the animal is started in a different arm on each
object from the familiarization phase) are placed in the box. The animal
trial according to a random schedule. On each trial, a black or white
is allowed to explore the objects for 10 min. Object exploration is scored
cylinder is suspended over the centre of the maze, equidistant from the
as in the NLR test from video recordings. Records of object exploration
entrance to each arm. Half of the animals are trained to associate the
should be organized in 1-min blocks, as the most sensitive measures of
black cylinder with turning right, and the white cylinder with turning
object recognition occur early in the test session when the novel object
left, to find the platform at the end of the arm. For the other half, the
is most unfamiliar. The OR test can be repeated 24 h later, using the
opposite is the case. Presentation of the cylinders is organized ac-
same familiar object paired with a new, novel object.
cording to a semi-random schedule that ensures an equal number of
trials with both cylinders over 10 daily trials. For each trial, animals are
Frontal Lobe-sensitive tests of strategic learning (Executive given a maximum of 60 s to find and mount the platform with all four
Function) paws. Animals are allowed 20 s at the end of each trial to rest atop the
platform. Ten such trials are administered each day for 10 days.
Non-Matching-to-Sample (NMTS) Error sequence and latency to find and mount the platform are re-
corded via the data processing system located centrally above the
Apparatus centre of the maze. An error is recorded when the animal enters an
Same as MWM Spatial Memory. incorrect arm with all four paws, or leaves the goal arm without suc-
cessfully mounting the platform. Typically, error scores and latencies
are highly correlated.
Procedure
This test begins with two days of orientation to the maze (5 trials/
Non-cognitive tests
day) in which the animals is placed in the pool and allowed to swim to a
visible platform, which is located beneath a grey cylinder (e.g., 30 cm
Elevated plus maze
long × 3 cm in diameter). The start point and the location of the plat-
form are randomized for each trial. By the end of the second day all rats
Apparatus
should be swimming directly to the platform.
A typical plus-maze for this test consists of two closed arms and two
The NMTS task consists of a series of paired sample and test trials
open arms of equal length (25 cm). The wall surrounding the closed
during which the platform is submerged a few cm below the surface of
arms is 20 cm high. Each open arm is surrounded by a rim about 0.5 cm
the water. In the sample trials, a black or white cylinder (30 cm
high. The maze is located in a quiet room and elevated 50 cm above the
long × 3 cm in diameter), suspended 5 cm above the submerged plat-
floor. The animal’s behaviour can be recorded with a video tracking
form, signals the platform’s location. In the subsequent test trial, both
system.
cylinders are present in different locations. The cylinder that was not
present during the preceding sample trial now signals the platform’s
location. Procedure
For each sample trial of the NMTS test, the animal is placed in the At the beginning of the test, which lasts for 5 min, the animal is
same (e.g., south-east) zone of the pool and allowed to swim to the placed in the centre of the maze with its head in the direction of an
submerged platform under the sample cylinder. The animal remains on open arm. The important measures are the number of entries (defined
the platform for 20 s and is then transferred to a heated holding cage as having all four legs in the arm) into, and time spent in each open and
while the platform is moved and the cylinders repositioned for the test closed arm. Time spent in enclosed arms is considered a sign of high
trial. The animal is then placed in the pool and allowed to swim to the anxiety. It is also possible to measure the number of head-dips, which
submerged platform or until 60 s has elapsed. If the animal fails to find are an indicator of exploration. A large number of head-dips reflect low
the platform within 60 s, it is manually guided to the platform. After 20 anxiety. Stretch-attend-postures can also be measured and interpreted
s on the platform the animal is transferred again to the holding cage, to as anxiety-induced information-taking about the environment.
await the next pair of trials. Ten daily sessions, each consisting of 8
pairs of sample and test trials, are administered. Sucrose preference test
As in spatial memory, the data recording system records the time
required to mount the platform (latency) as well as swimming patterns. Apparatus
An error is recorded each time the animal enters a zone not containing Animals, housed in individual cages, are given access to 2 bottles;
the platform. one contains water and the other contains a 2% sucrose solution.

80
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

Procedure until the number of lever-press responses does not vary by more than
Initially, animals are habituated for 3 days to the presence of the 25% from the mean over a 3-day period. (The operant test can be made
two drinking bottles in their home cage. After this period, they have the more demanding by increasing the response/reinforcement ratio –
choice of drinking the 2% sucrose solution or water for a period of Salamone et al. [44] reported that normal rats will continue to prefer
3 days. During this time, the volume of the solution consumed in each the pellets and press the lever at high rates at FR schedules that exceed
bottle is measured and the sucrose preference is calculated as the per- 60.) At the end of each food-choice session, the number of lever-press
cent of the volume of the sucrose solution drunk relative to the total responses and the amount of lab chow consumed (taking into account
volume of liquid drunk. Normal animals prefer the sweetened drink; spillage) are recorded. Animals that are less motivated to work for food,
failure to do so is a measure of anhedonia/depression. as a result of fatigue, stress, or emotional unresponsiveness, prefer the
freely available, but less palatable, lab chow.
Forced swim test
References
Apparatus
A 2 Liter beaker, filled with 1.500 L of water (25 °C), is located in a [1] Ahles TA, Saykin AJ. Candidate mechanisms for chemotherapy-induced cognitive
quiet testing area that is equipped with a video tracking system. Water changes. Nat Rev Cancer 2007;7:192–201.
[2] Ahles TA, Root JC, Ryan EL. Cancer- and cancer treatment-associated cognitive
is changed between each animal. change: an update on the state of the science. J Clin Oncol 2012;30:3675–86.
[3] Joly F, Giffard B, Rigal O, De Ruiter MB, Small BJ, Dubois M, et al. Impact of
Procedure cancer and its treatments on cognitive function: advances in research from the
paris international cognition and cancer task force symposium and update since
The animal is placed in the water for 5 min and allowed to swim 2012. J Pain Symptom Manage 2015;50:830–41.
freely. The animal is considered to be immobile when there is no [4] Vardy J, Tannock I. Cognitive function after chemotherapy in adults with solid
movement, except for those necessary to keep its head above the water tumours. Crit Rev Oncol Hematol 2007;63:183–202.
[5] Wefel JS, Vardy J, Ahles T, Schagen SB. International Cognition and Cancer Task
surface. The animal’s behaviour is analyzed through video records.
Force recommendations to harmonise studies of cognitive function in patients with
Animals that make little effort to escape and quickly become immobile cancer. Lancet Oncol 2011;12:703–8.
are thought to exhibit signs of depression. [6] Seigers R, Fardell JE. Neurobiological basis of chemotherapy-induced cognitive
impairment: a review of rodent research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2011;35:729–41.
[7] Fardell JE, Vardy J, Logge W, Johnston I. Single high dose treatment with meth-
Tail suspension test (for mice only) otrexate causes long-lasting cognitive dysfunction in laboratory rodents.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2010;97:333–9.
Apparatus [8] Foley JJ, Raffa RB, Walker EA. Effects of chemotherapeutic agents 5-fluorouracil
and methotrexate alone and combined in a mouse model of learning and memory.
The mouse is suspended by the tail from an elevated bar. Its activity, Psychopharmacology 2008;199:527–38.
while dangling, can be recorded with a video tracking system. [9] Mustafa S, Walker A, Bennett G, Wigmore PM. 5-Fluorouracil chemotherapy affects
spatial working memory and newborn neurons in the adult rat hippocampus. Eur J
Neurosci 2008;28:323–30.
Procedure [10] Seigers R, Schagen SB, Beerling W, Boogerd W, van Tellingen O, van Dam FS, et al.
The test consists of a single 6 min session. Records are kept of la- Long-lasting suppression of hippocampal cell proliferation and impaired cognitive
tency to first bout of immobility (as defined by passive hanging with no performance by methotrexate in the rat. Behav Brain Res 2008;186:168–75.
[11] Winocur G, Vardy J, Binns MA, Kerr L, Tannock I. The effects of the anti-cancer
attempt to right itself), and number and duration of immobility bouts. drugs, methotrexate and 5-fluorouracil, on cognitive function in mice. Pharmacol
Animals that exhibit few escape oriented behaviours and become im- Biochem Behav 2006;85:66–75.
mobile are thought to exhibit signs of depression. [12] Hodgson KD, Hutchinson AD, Wilson CJ, Nettelbeck T. A meta-analysis of the
effects of chemotherapy on cognition in patients with cancer. Cancer Treat Rev
2013;39:297–304.
Food choice test [13] Ferguson RJ, McDonald BC, Saykin AJ, Ahles TA. Brain structure and function
differences in monozygotic twins: possible effects of breast cancer chemotherapy. J
Clin Oncol 2007;25:3866–70.
Apparatus
[14] McDonald BC, Saykin AJ. Alterations in brain structure related to breast cancer
A standard operant chamber (30 × 25 × 30 cm) located in a quiet and its treatment: chemotherapy and other considerations. Brain Imaging Behav
room or preferably in a sound-proof chamber. The chamber contains a 2013;7:374–87.
lever that is centrally located in one wall and an adjacent food dis- [15] Winocur G, Henkelman M, Wojtowicz JM, Zhang H, Binns MA, Tannock IF. The
effects of chemotherapy on cognitive function in a mouse model: a prospective
penser that delivers commercially available 45 mg food pellets. study. Clin Cancer Res 2012;18:3112–21.
[16] Morris RG, Garrud P, Rawlins JN, O'Keefe J. Place navigation impaired in rats with
Procedure hippocampal lesions. Nature 1982;297:681–3.
[17] Anagnostaras SG, Maren S, Fanselow MS. Temporally graded retrograde amnesia
Before beginning behavioural training, rats are placed on a 23.5 of contextual fear after hippocampal damage in rats: within-subjects examination.
food-deprivation schedule during which they receive 30 min daily J Neurosci 1999;19:1106–14.
feeding sessions with both standard lab chow and food pellets available. [18] Dix SL, Aggleton JP. Extending the spontaneous preference test of recognition:
evidence of object-location and object-context recognition. Behav Brain Res
After several days, rats receive 30 min daily familiarization training 1999;99:191–200.
sessions in the operant chamber in which they learn to associate the [19] Broadbent NJ, Gaskin S, Squire LR, Clark RE. Object recognition memory and the
lever and dispenser with food pellets. Rats are then trained in 30 min rodent hippocampus. Learn Mem 2010;17:5–11.
[20] Seigers R, Schagen SB, Coppens CM, van der Most PJ, van Dam FS, Koolhaas JM,
sessions to press the lever to obtain food pellets on a continuous re- et al. Methotrexate decreases hippocampal cell proliferation and induces memory
inforcement (CRF schedule in which each lever press produces a single deficits in rats. Behav Brain Res 2009;201:279–84.
pellet. Following familiarization and CRF training, rats are allowed [21] Macleod JE, DeLeo JA, Hickey WF, Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, Bucci DJ. Cancer che-
motherapy impairs contextual but not cue-specific fear memory. Behav Brain Res
20 min access to lab chow, to ensure that they will have consumed
2007;181:168–72.
adequate amounts of food each day. [22] Dubois M, Lapinte N, Villier V, Lecointre C, Roy V, Tonon MC, et al.
When CRF lever pressing has stabilized, the food-choice condition is Chemotherapy-induced long-term alteration of executive functions and hippo-
introduced. For this condition the reinforcement schedule is changed to campal cell proliferation: role of glucose as adjuvant. Neuropharmacology
2014;79:234–48.
fixed ratio (FR) 5 in which the rat must press the lever 5 times to receive [23] Dubois M, Le Joncour V, Tonon MC, Anouar Y, Proust F, Morin F, et al. Evaluation
a single pellet. After several days, when the rat is pressing consistently of the impact of the cancer therapy everolimus on the central nervous system in
on this schedule, 15 g of lab chow is introduced into the chamber and mice. PLoS One 2014;9:1–27.
[24] Fardell JE, Vardy J, Shah JD, Johnston IN. Cognitive impairments caused by ox-
made freely available. Thus, the rat now has a choice of pressing the aliplatin and 5-fluorouracil chemotherapy are ameliorated by physical activity.
lever on FR5 reinforcement schedule to receive desirable food pellets or Psychopharmacology 2012;220:183–93.
simply eating the less preferred lab chow. Testing should be continued [25] Lyons L, Elbeltagy M, Bennett G, Wigmore P. The effects of cyclophosphamide on

81
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

hippocampal cell proliferation and spatial working memory in rat. PLoS One [56] Winocur G, Wojtowicz JM, Tannock IF. Memory loss in chemotherapy-treated rats
2011;6:1–5. is exacerbated in high-interference conditions and related to suppression of hip-
[26] Petrides M. Frontal lobes and working memory: evidence from investigations of pocampal neurogenesis. Behav Brain Res 2015;281:239–44.
the effects of cortical excisions in non-human primates. Handbook of [57] Winocur G, Wojtowicz JM, Huang J, Tannock IF. Physical exercise prevents sup-
Neuropsychology. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1994. p. 59–82. pression of hippocampal neurogenesis and reduces cognitive impairment in che-
[27] Winocur G. A comparison of normal old rats and young adult rats with lesions to motherapy-treated rats. Psychopharmacology 2014;231:2311–20.
the hippocampus or prefrontal cortex on a test of matching-to-sample. [58] Winocur G, Wojtowicz JM, Merkley CM, Tannock IF. Environmental enrichment
Neuropsychologia 1992;30:769–81. protects against cognitive impairment following chemotherapy in an animal
[28] Dantzer R, Meagher MW, Cleeland CS. Translational approaches to treatment-in- model. Behav Neurosci 2016;130:428–36.
duced symptoms in cancer patients. Nat Rev Clin Oncol 2012;9:414–26. [59] Dietrich J, Prust M, Kaiser J. Chemotherapy, cognitive impairment and hippo-
[29] Kim HJ, Barsevick AM, Fang CY, Miaskowski C. Common biological pathways campal toxicity. Neuroscience 2015;309:224–32.
underlying the psychoneurological symptom cluster in cancer patients. Cancer [60] Merriman JD, Von Ah D, Miaskowski C, Aouizerat BE. Proposed mechanisms for
Nurs 2012;35:E1–20. cancer- and treatment-related cognitive changes. Semin Oncol Nurs
[30] Paul ES, Harding EJ, Mendl M. Measuring emotional processes in animals: the 2013;29:260–9.
utility of a cognitive approach. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29:469–91. [61] Winocur G, Becker S, Luu P, Rosenzweig S, Wojtowicz JM. Adult hippocampal
[31] Roelofs S, Boleij H, Nordquist RE, van der Staay FJ. Making decisions under am- neurogenesis and memory interference. Behav Brain Res 2012;227:464–9.
biguity: judgment bias tasks for assessing emotional state in animals. Front Behav [62] Wimmer RD, Schmitt LI, Davidson TJ, Nakajima M, Deisseroth K, Halassa MM.
Neurosci 2016;10:1–16. Thalamic control of sensory selection in divided attention. Nature
[32] Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. 2015;526:705–9.
Psychopharmacology 1987;92:180–5. [63] Mandelblatt JS, Stern RA, Luta G, McGuckin M, Clapp JD, Hurria A, et al.
[33] Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open:closed arm entries in an Cognitive impairment in older patients with breast cancer before systemic therapy:
elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods is there an interaction between cancer and comorbidity? J Clin Oncol
1985;14:149–67. 2014;32:1909–18.
[34] Willner P, Muscat R, Papp M. An animal model of anhedonia. Clin [64] Liu CC, Kanekiyo T, Xu H, Bu G. Apolipoprotein E and Alzheimer disease: risk,
Neuropharmacol 1992;Suppl 1 Pt 15A:550A–1A. mechanisms and therapy. Nat Rev Neurol 2013;9:106–18.
[35] Castagne V, Porsolt RD, Moser P. Use of latency to immobility improves detection [65] Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, Noll WW, Furstenberg CT, Guerin S, Cole B, et al. The re-
of antidepressant-like activity in the behavioral despair test in the mouse. Eur J lationship of APOE genotype to neuropsychological performance in long-term
Pharmacol 2009;616:128–33. cancer survivors treated with standard dose chemotherapy. Psychooncology
[36] Steru L, Chermat R, Thierry B, Simon P. The tail suspension test: a new method for 2003;12:612–9.
screening antidepressants in mice. Psychopharmacology 1985;85:367–70. [66] Cheng H, Li W, Gan C, Zhang B, Jia Q, Wang K. The COMT (rs165599) gene
[37] Merzoug S, Toumi ML, Boukhris N, Baudin B, Tahraoui A. Adriamycin-related polymorphism contributes to chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment in
anxiety-like behavior, brain oxidative stress and myelotoxicity in male Wistar rats. breast cancer patients. Am J Transl Res 2016;8:5087–97.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2011;99:639–47. [67] Stern Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve
[38] Wilson CL, Weber ET. Chemotherapy drug thioTEPA exacerbates stress-induced concept. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2002;8:448–60.
anhedonia and corticosteroid responses but not impairment of hippocampal cell [68] Jabbour E, Thomas D, Cortes J, Kantarjian HM, O'Brien S. Central nervous system
proliferation in adult mice. Behav Brain Res 2013;236:180–5. prophylaxis in adults with acute lymphoblastic leukemia: current and emerging
[39] Yang M, Kim JS, Kim J, Kim SH, Kim JC, Wang H, et al. Neurotoxicity of meth- therapies. Cancer 2010;116:2290–300.
otrexate to hippocampal cells in vivo and in vitro. Biochem Pharmacol [69] Lange M, Laviec H, Castel H, Heutte N, Leconte A, Leger I, et al. Impact of new
2011;82:72–80. generation hormone-therapy on cognitive function in elderly patients treated for a
[40] Callaghan CK, O'Mara SM. Long-term cognitive dysfunction in the rat following metastatic prostate cancer: Cog-Pro trial protocol. BMC Cancer 2017;17:1–8.
docetaxel treatment is ameliorated by the phosphodiesterase-4 inhibitor, rolipram. [70] Huang Z, Zheng Y, Bao P, Cai H, Hong Z, Ding D, et al. Aging, obesity, and post-
Behav Brain Res 2015;290:84–9. therapy cognitive recovery in breast cancer survivors. Oncotarget
[41] Ray MA, Trammell RA, Verhulst S, Ran S, Toth LA. Development of a mouse model 2017;8:12364–73.
for assessing fatigue during chemotherapy. Comp Med 2011;61:119–30. [71] Branstrom R, Petersson LM, Saboonchi F, Wennman-Larsen A, Alexanderson K.
[42] Salas-Ramirez KY, Bagnall C, Frias L, Abdali SA, Ahles TA, Hubbard K. Physical activity following a breast cancer diagnosis: implications for self-rated
Doxorubicin and cyclophosphamide induce cognitive dysfunction and activate the health and cancer-related symptoms. Eur J Oncol Nurs 2015;19:680–5.
ERK and AKT signaling pathways. Behav Brain Res 2015;292:133–41. [72] Ferrer RA, Huedo-Medina TB, Johnson BT, Ryan S, Pescatello LS. Exercise inter-
[43] Salamone JD, Steinpreis RE, McCullough LD, Smith P, Grebel D, Mahan K. ventions for cancer survivors: a meta-analysis of quality of life outcomes. Ann
Haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion suppress lever pressing Behav Med 2011;41:32–47.
for food but increase free food consumption in a novel food choice procedure. [73] Pinto BM, Dunsiger S, Waldemore M. Physical activity and psychosocial benefits
Psychopharmacology 1991;104:515–21. among breast cancer patients. Psychooncology 2013;22:2193–9.
[44] Salamone JD, Correa M, Nunes EJ, Randall PA, Pardo M. The behavioral phar- [74] Ehlers DK, Aguinaga S, Cosman J, Severson J, Kramer AF, McAuley E. The effects
macology of effort-related choice behavior: dopamine, adenosine and beyond. J of physical activity and fatigue on cognitive performance in breast cancer survi-
Exp Anal Behav 2012;97:125–46. vors. Breast Cancer Res Treat 2017;165:699–707.
[45] Castel H, Denouel A, Lange M, Tonon MC, Dubois M, Joly F. Biomarkers associated [75] Fitzpatrick TR, Edgar L, Holcroft C. Assessing the relationship between physical
with cognitive impairment in treated cancer patients: potential predisposition and fitness activities, cognitive health, and quality of life among older cancer survivors.
risk factors. Front Pharmacol 2017;8:1–19. J Psychosoc Oncol 2012;30:556–72.
[46] Lacourt TE, Heijnen CJ. Mechanisms of neurotoxic symptoms as a result of breast [76] Myers JS, Wick JA, Klemp J. Potential factors associated with perceived cognitive
cancer and its treatment: considerations on the contribution of stress, inflamma- impairment in breast cancer survivors. Support Care Cancer 2015;23:3219–28.
tion, and cellular bioenergetics. Curr Breast Cancer Rep 2017;9:70–81. [77] Voigt V, Neufeld F, Kaste J, Buhner M, Sckopke P, Wuerstlein R, et al. Clinically
[47] Andreotti C, Root JC, Ahles TA, McEwen BS, Compas BE. Cancer, coping, and assessed posttraumatic stress in patients with breast cancer during the first year
cognition: a model for the role of stress reactivity in cancer-related cognitive de- after diagnosis in the prospective, longitudinal, controlled COGNICARES study.
cline. Psychooncology 2015;24:617–23. Psychooncology 2017;26:74–80.
[48] Seigers R, Schagen SB, Van Tellingen O, Dietrich J. Chemotherapy-related cogni- [78] Hermelink K, Voigt V, Kaste J, Neufeld F, Wuerstlein R, Buhner M, et al.
tive dysfunction: current animal studies and future directions. Brain Imaging Elucidating pretreatment cognitive impairment in breast cancer patients: the im-
Behav 2013;7:453–9. pact of cancer-related post-traumatic stress. J Natl Cancer Inst 2015;107:1–13.
[49] Vichaya EG, Chiu GS, Krukowski K, Lacourt TE, Kavelaars A, Dantzer R, et al. [79] Li Y, Vijayanathan V, Gulinello ME, Cole PD. Systemic methotrexate induces
Mechanisms of chemotherapy-induced behavioral toxicities. Front Neurosci spatial memory deficits and depletes cerebrospinal fluid folate in rats. Pharmacol
2015;9:1–17. Biochem Behav 2010;94:454–63.
[50] McDonald BC, Conroy SK, Ahles TA, West JD, Saykin AJ. Gray matter reduction [80] Ferguson RJ, McDonald BC, Rocque MA, Furstenberg CT, Horrigan S, Ahles TA,
associated with systemic chemotherapy for breast cancer: a prospective MRI study. et al. Development of CBT for chemotherapy-related cognitive change: results of a
Breast Cancer Res Treat 2010;123:819–28. waitlist control trial. Psychooncology 2012;21:176–86.
[51] de Ruiter MB, Reneman L, Boogerd W, Veltman DJ, van Dam FS, Nederveen AJ, [81] Wolf TJ, Doherty M, Kallogjeri D, Coalson RS, Nicklaus J, Ma CX, et al. The fea-
et al. Cerebral hyporesponsiveness and cognitive impairment 10 years after che- sibility of using metacognitive strategy training to improve cognitive performance
motherapy for breast cancer. Hum Brain Mapp 2011;32:1206–19. and neural connectivity in women with chemotherapy-induced cognitive impair-
[52] Xuan H, Gan C, Li W, Huang Z, Wang L, Jia Q, et al. Altered network efficiency of ment. Oncology 2016;91:143–52.
functional brain networks in patients with breast cancer after chemotherapy. [82] Kesler S, Hadi Hosseini SM, Heckler C, Janelsins M, Palesh O, Mustian K, et al.
Oncotarget 2017;8:105648–61. Cognitive training for improving executive function in chemotherapy-treated
[53] Monje M, Dietrich J. Cognitive side effects of cancer therapy demonstrate a breast cancer survivors. Clin Breast Cancer 2013;13:299–306.
functional role for adult neurogenesis. Behav Brain Res 2012;227:376–9. [83] Fardell JE, Vardy J, Johnston IN, Winocur G. Chemotherapy and cognitive im-
[54] Nokia MS, Anderson ML, Shors TJ. Chemotherapy disrupts learning, neurogenesis pairment: treatment options. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2011;90:366–76.
and theta activity in the adult brain. Eur J Neurosci 2012;36:3521–30. [84] Davis J, Ahlberg FM, Berk M, Ashley DM, Khasraw M. Emerging pharmacotherapy
[55] Rendeiro C, Sheriff A, Bhattacharya TK, Gogola JV, Baxter JH, Chen H, et al. Long- for cancer patients with cognitive dysfunction. BMC Neurol 2013;13:2–9.
lasting impairments in adult neurogenesis, spatial learning and memory from a [85] Kohli S, Fisher SG, Tra Y, Adams MJ, Mapstone ME, Wesnes KA, et al. The effect of
standard chemotherapy regimen used to treat breast cancer. Behav Brain Res modafinil on cognitive function in breast cancer survivors. Cancer
2016;315:10–22. 2009;115:2605–16.

82
G. Winocur et al. Cancer Treatment Reviews 69 (2018) 72–83

[86] Mar Fan HG, Clemons M, Xu W, Chemerynsky I, Breunis H, Braganza S, et al. A tamoxifen and exemestane on cognitive functioning of postmenopausal patients
randomised, placebo-controlled, double-blind trial of the effects of d-methylphe- with breast cancer: results from the neuropsychological side study of the tamox-
nidate on fatigue and cognitive dysfunction in women undergoing adjuvant che- ifen and exemestane adjuvant multinational trial. J Clin Oncol 2010;28:1294–300.
motherapy for breast cancer. Support Care Cancer 2008;16:577–83. [114] Ahles TA, Saykin AJ, McDonald BC, Li Y, Furstenberg CT, Hanscom BS, et al.
[87] Drachtman RA, Cole PD, Golden CB, James SJ, Melnyk S, Aisner J, et al. Longitudinal assessment of cognitive changes associated with adjuvant treatment
Dextromethorphan is effective in the treatment of subacute methotrexate neuro- for breast cancer: impact of age and cognitive reserve. J Clin Oncol
toxicity. Pediatr Hematol Oncol 2002;19:319–27. 2010;28:4434–40.
[88] Vijayanathan V, Gulinello M, Ali N, Cole PD. Persistent cognitive deficits, induced [115] Bender CM, Sereika SM, Brufsky AM, Ryan CM, Vogel VG, Rastogi P, et al. Memory
by intrathecal methotrexate, are associated with elevated CSF concentrations of impairments with adjuvant anastrozole versus tamoxifen in women with early-
excitotoxic glutamate analogs and can be reversed by an NMDA antagonist. Behav stage breast cancer. Menopause 2007;14:995–8.
Brain Res 2011;225:491–7. [116] Phillips KA, Ribi K, Fisher R. Do aromatase inhibitors have adverse effects on
[89] Winocur G, Binns MA, Tannock I. Donepezil reduces cognitive impairment asso- cognitive function? Breast Cancer Res 2011;13:1–7.
ciated with anti-cancer drugs in a mouse model. Neuropharmacology [117] Seigers R, Loos M, Van Tellingen O, Boogerd W, Smit AB, Schagen SB. Cognitive
2011;61:1222–8. impact of cytotoxic agents in mice. Psychopharmacology 2015;232:17–37.
[90] ElBeltagy M, Mustafa S, Umka J, Lyons L, Salman A, Chur-yoe GT, et al. Fluoxetine [118] Walker EA, Foley JJ, Clark-Vetri R, Raffa RB. Effects of repeated administration of
improves the memory deficits caused by the chemotherapy agent 5-fluorouracil. chemotherapeutic agents tamoxifen, methotrexate, and 5-fluorouracil on the ac-
Behav Brain Res 2010;208:112–7. quisition and retention of a learned response in mice. Psychopharmacology
[91] Konat GW, Kraszpulski M, James I, Zhang HT, Abraham J. Cognitive dysfunction 2011;217:539–48.
induced by chronic administration of common cancer chemotherapeutics in rats. [119] Lee GD, Longo DL, Wang Y, Rifkind JM, Abdul-Raman L, Mamczarz JA, et al.
Metab Brain Dis 2008;23:325–33. Transient improvement in cognitive function and synaptic plasticity in rats fol-
[92] Jiang ZG, Winocur G, Wojtowicz JM, Shevtsova O, Fuller S, Ghanbari HA. PAN- lowing cancer chemotherapy. Clin Cancer Res 2006;12:198–205.
811 prevents chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment and preserves neuro- [120] Gouma E, Simos Y, Verginadis I, Lykoudis E, Evangelou A, Karkabounas S. A
genesis in the hippocampus of adult rats. PLoS One 2018;13:1–13. simple procedure for estimation of total body surface area and determination of a
[93] Ball K, Berch DB, Helmers KF, Jobe JB, Leveck MD, Marsiske M, et al. Effects of new value of Meeh's constant in rats. Lab Anim 2012;46:40–5.
cognitive training interventions with older adults: a randomized controlled trial. [121] Nair AB, Jacob S. A simple practice guide for dose conversion between animals
JAMA 2002;288:2271–81. and human. J Basic Clin Pharm 2016;7:27–31.
[94] Levine B, Robertson IH, Clare L, Carter G, Hong J, Wilson BA, et al. Rehabilitation [122] Fardell JE, Vardy J, Monds LA, Johnston IN. The long-term impact of oxaliplatin
of executive functioning: an experimental-clinical validation of goal management chemotherapy on rodent cognition and peripheral neuropathy. Behav Brain Res
training. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2000;6:299–312. 2015;291:80–8.
[95] Petersen RC, Thomas RG, Grundman M, Bennett D, Doody R, Ferris S, et al. [123] Janelsins MC, Kohli S, Mohile SG, Usuki K, Ahles TA, Morrow GR. An update on
Vitamin E and donepezil for the treatment of mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J cancer- and chemotherapy-related cognitive dysfunction: current status. Semin
Med 2005;352:2379–88. Oncol 2011;38:431–8.
[96] Cimprich B, Reuter-Lorenz P, Nelson J, Clark PM, Therrien B, Normolle D, et al. [124] Schagen SB, Boogerd W, Muller MJ, Huinink WT, Moonen L, Meinhardt W, et al.
Prechemotherapy alterations in brain function in women with breast cancer. J Clin Cognitive complaints and cognitive impairment following BEP chemotherapy in
Exp Neuropsychol 2010;32:324–31. patients with testicular cancer. Acta Oncol 2008;47:63–70.
[97] Cruzado JA, Lopez-Santiago S, Martinez-Marin V, Jose-Moreno G, Custodio AB, [125] Kadan-Lottick NS, Zeltzer LK, Liu Q, Yasui Y, Ellenberg L, Gioia G, et al.
Feliu J. Longitudinal study of cognitive dysfunctions induced by adjuvant che- Neurocognitive functioning in adult survivors of childhood non-central nervous
motherapy in colon cancer patients. Support Care Cancer 2014;22:1815–23. system cancers. J Natl Cancer Inst 2010;102:881–93.
[98] Lange M, Heutte N, Rigal O, Noal S, Kurtz JE, Levy C, et al. Decline in cognitive [126] Acharya MM, Martirosian V, Chmielewski NN, Hanna N, Tran KK, Liao AC, et al.
function in older adults with early-stage breast cancer after adjuvant treatment. Stem cell transplantation reverses chemotherapy-induced cognitive dysfunction.
Oncologist 2016;21:1337–48. Cancer Res 2015;75:676–86.
[99] Scherling C, Collins B, Mackenzie J, Bielajew C, Smith A. Pre-chemotherapy dif- [127] Christie LA, Acharya MM, Parihar VK, Nguyen A, Martirosian V, Limoli CL.
ferences in visuospatial working memory in breast cancer patients compared to Impaired cognitive function and hippocampal neurogenesis following cancer
controls: an FMRI study. Front Hum Neurosci 2011;5:1–21. chemotherapy. Clin Cancer Res 2012;18:1954–65.
[100] Vardy JL, Dhillon HM, Pond GR, Rourke SB, Bekele T, Renton C, et al. Cognitive [128] Lyons L, ElBeltagy M, Bennett G, Wigmore P. Fluoxetine counteracts the cognitive
function in patients with colorectal cancer who do and do not receive che- and cellular effects of 5-fluorouracil in the rat hippocampus by a mechanism of
motherapy: a prospective, longitudinal, controlled study. J Clin Oncol prevention rather than recovery. PLoS One 2012;7:1–8.
2015;33:4085–92. [129] Kitamura Y, Hattori S, Yoneda S, Watanabe S, Kanemoto E, Sugimoto M, et al.
[101] Guy CT, Webster MA, Schaller M, Parsons TJ, Cardiff RD, Muller WJ. Expression of Doxorubicin and cyclophosphamide treatment produces anxiety-like behavior and
the neu protooncogene in the mammary epithelium of transgenic mice induces spatial cognition impairment in rats: possible involvement of hippocampal neu-
metastatic disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1992;89:10578–82. rogenesis via brain-derived neurotrophic factor and cyclin D1 regulation. Behav
[102] Winocur G, Berman H, Nguyen M, Binns MA, Henkelman M, van Eede M, et al. Brain Res 2015;292:184–93.
Neurobiological mechanisms of chemotherapy-induced cognitive impairment in a [130] Fremouw T, Fessler CL, Ferguson RJ, Burguete Y. Preserved learning and memory
transgenic model of breast cancer. Neuroscience 2018;369:51–65. in mice following chemotherapy: 5-Fluorouracil and doxorubicin single agent
[103] Guy CT, Cardiff RD, Muller WJ. Induction of mammary tumors by expression of treatment, doxorubicin-cyclophosphamide combination treatment. Behav Brain
polyomavirus middle T oncogene: a transgenic mouse model for metastatic dis- Res 2012;226:154–62.
ease. Mol Cell Biol 1992;12:954–61. [131] Fardell JE, Vardy J, Johnston IN. The short and long term effects of docetaxel
[104] Whittle JR, Lewis MT, Lindeman GJ, Visvader JE. Patient-derived xenograft chemotherapy on rodent object recognition and spatial reference memory. Life Sci
models of breast cancer and their predictive power. Breast Cancer Res 2015;17:17. 2013;93:596–604.
[105] Yang M, Kim J, Kim JS, Kim SH, Kim JC, Kang MJ, et al. Hippocampal dysfunc- [132] Yang M, Kim JS, Song MS, Kim SH, Kang SS, Bae CS, et al. Cyclophosphamide
tions in tumor-bearing mice. Brain Behav Immun 2014;36:147–55. impairs hippocampus-dependent learning and memory in adult mice: possible
[106] Seigers R, Pourtau L, Schagen SB, van Dam FS, Koolhaas JM, Konsman JP, et al. involvement of hippocampal neurogenesis in chemotherapy-induced memory
Inhibition of hippocampal cell proliferation by methotrexate in rats is not po- deficits. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2010;93:487–94.
tentiated by the presence of a tumor. Brain Res Bull 2010;81:472–6. [133] Smith GW, Aubry JM, Dellu F, Contarino A, Bilezikjian LM, Gold LH, et al.
[107] Shibayama O, Yoshiuchi K, Inagaki M, Matsuoka Y, Yoshikawa E, Sugawara Y, Corticotropin releasing factor receptor 1-deficient mice display decreased anxiety,
et al. Association between adjuvant regional radiotherapy and cognitive function impaired stress response, and aberrant neuroendocrine development. Neuron
in breast cancer patients treated with conservation therapy. Cancer Med 1998;20:1093–102.
2014;3:702–9. [134] Can A, Dao DT, Terrillion CE, Piantadosi SC, Bhat S, Gould TD. The tail suspension
[108] McGinty HL, Phillips KM, Jim HS, Cessna JM, Asvat Y, Cases MG, et al. Cognitive test. J Vis Exp 2012;59:1–5.
functioning in men receiving androgen deprivation therapy for prostate cancer: a [135] Can A, Dao DT, Arad M, Terrillion CE, Piantadosi SC, Gould TD. The mouse forced
systematic review and meta-analysis. Support Care Cancer 2014;22:2271–80. swim test. J Vis Exp 2012;59:1–5.
[109] Nelson CJ, Lee JS, Gamboa MC, Roth AJ. Cognitive effects of hormone therapy in [136] Porsolt RD, Le Pichon M, Jalfre M. Depression: a new animal model sensitive to
men with prostate cancer: a review. Cancer 2008;113:1097–106. antidepressant treatments. Nature 1977;266:730–2.
[110] Alibhai SM, Breunis H, Timilshina N, Johnston C, Tomlinson G, Tannock I, et al. [137] Krishnan V, Han MH, Graham DL, Berton O, Renthal W, Russo SJ, et al. Molecular
Impact of androgen-deprivation therapy on physical function and quality of life in adaptations underlying susceptibility and resistance to social defeat in brain re-
men with nonmetastatic prostate cancer. J Clin Oncol 2010;28:5038–45. ward regions. Cell 2007;131:391–404.
[111] Haskell SG, Richardson ED, Horwitz RI. The effect of estrogen replacement [138] El Yacoubi M, Rappeneau V, Champion E, Malleret G, Vaugeois JM. The H/Rouen
therapy on cognitive function in women: a critical review of the literature. J Clin mouse model displays depression-like and anxiety-like behaviors. Behav Brain Res
Epidemiol 1997;50:1249–64. 2013;256:43–50.
[112] Kawas C, Resnick S, Morrison A, Brookmeyer R, Corrada M, Zonderman A, et al. A [139] Mineur YS, Prasol DJ, Belzung C, Crusio WE. Agonistic behavior and unpredictable
prospective study of estrogen replacement therapy and the risk of developing chronic mild stress in mice. Behav Genet 2003;33:513–9.
Alzheimer's disease: the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neurology [140] Nicolas LB, Kolb Y, Prinssen EP. A combined marble burying-locomotor activity
1997;48:1517–21. test in mice: a practical screening test with sensitivity to different classes of an-
[113] Schilder CM, Seynaeve C, Beex LV, Boogerd W, Linn SC, Gundy CM, et al. Effects of xiolytics and antidepressants. Eur J Pharmacol 2006;547:106–15.

83

Potrebbero piacerti anche