Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

Adverbial Extraction: A defense of tracelessness

Ivan A. Sag
Department of Linguistics
Stanford University
Stanford, 94305
sag@stanford.edu

1 Introduction to the coordinate structure, and hence outscopes the


conjunction.
Ever since the ‘adverbs-as-complements’ [A-as-C]
analysis was first proposed by Bouma and van No- In this paper, I explore a small modification of
ord, it has been controversial. Even though a num- the BMS analysis that resolves this problem with-
ber of people have offered extensive motivation for out introducing traces of the sort that Levine argues
this view in various languages (e.g. Przepiórkowski would provide an alternative account of data like (1).
1999, Manning et al. 1999), there are various issues The goal of saving the BMS analysis is worthwhile,
of adequacy that have been raised by researchers it should be noted, as it is the only extant HPSG
in the HPSG research community. Specifically, in extraction analysis which is immediately consistent
a penetrating study, Levine (2002) raises important with the extensive evidence cited for A-as-C and
questions about how the A-as-C analysis developed which also provides a straightforward account of the
by Bouma et al. (2001) [BMS] can be reconciled fact, documented extensively by Hukari and Levine
with examples like (1): (1995), that adverb extraction triggers the same mor-
phosyntactic repercussions as complement extrac-
(1) In how many seconds flat did Robin find a tion in languages (e.g. Chamorro, Palauan, Thomp-
chair, sit down, and whip off her logging boots? son River Salish, Irish,...) that register extraction de-
pendencies locally. Under BMS’s proposal, all verbs
Because in BMS’s analysis, an adverb selected by and complementizers within an extraction domain
a verb identifies its MOD value’s KEY value with are distinguished by having a nonempty SLASH
the verb’s KEY value, (1) poses a dilemma: if the value. Under an approach where adverbial traces ter-
extracted adverb is associated with a dependent of minate filler-gap dependencies, there is no motiva-
each verb (find, sit, and whip), then three contradic- tion for extraction information (a nonempty SLASH
tory KEY values must be equated. Intuitively, (1) value) to be registered on the verb modified by an
requires that the adverb modify the coordinate struc- adverbial trace.
ture (since this sentence has a cumulative reading
and its meaning is a question about the duration of a
tripartite event), yet BMS’s analysis assumes that all 2 Analysis
postverbal adverbials are complements, and hence
lacks any way to associate the adverb with the ap- In unpublished work, Bouma et al. (1998) ob-
propriate modifier position and no way to assign it serve that the BMS analysis requires a stip-
the correct scope. On the other hand, Levine argues, ulation based on a binary relation they call
if there are adverbial traces that can appear wherever successively-out-modify in order to en-
adverbs can appear, then these examples are unprob- sure that the linear order of postverbal modifiers de-
lematic – the adverbial trace is in a position adjoined termines their relative scope:

111
(2) a. Robin reboots the Mac [frequently] [inten- value2 ):
 
 
tionally]. intnl(freq(reboot..))

(4) PHON found  
  LTOP    
b. Robin reboots the Mac [intentionally] [fre-
   find-rel     
quently]. freq(intnl(reboot..))
 SS  LOC  CONT  RELS  LBL ARG1 !

 
  ARG2 "

This stipulation can be eliminated by returning to  #
  $ ,    '

a lexical-rule (LR)-based analysis like that origi-  
HCONS

nally proposed by van Noord and Bouma(see also LTOP 
Przepiórkowski 1999). For convenience, I will for-  ARG-ST NP% , NP& ,  MOD  ...HEAD verb
mulate this as lexical rule as a unary schema that ...LTOP 
simply extends a verb’s ARG-ST list, i.e. as in (3),
where the daughter is the ‘LR input’ and the mother Here the selected adverb, if scopal, will have to in-
clude the verb’s local top, and hence the verb’s pred-
is the ‘LR output’:1
ication, within its scope. The use of , rather than
)+* (
(3) Adverb Addition Schema (AAS): , is crucial to my analysis.
 PHON 
Mother:
 Notice that the mother in (3) (the ‘LR output’)
     says nothing about the KEY value of the verb or that
 SS  LOC  CONT  LTOP HCONS   

 of the MOD value. In addition, when a verb selects
 
 ARG-ST   RELS 
two adverbials, the first adverbial’s local top enters
 (
into an relation with the local top of the second ad-
  LTOP     verbial’s MOD value. This ensures that subsequent
  MOD  LOC  CAT  HEAD verb scopal adverbials will always outscope prior adver-
CONT  LTOP 
bials (and that all such adverbials will include the
verb’s predication in their scope).
Dtr:
 PHON  
 CAT  HEAD verb   
The only two resolved mrs-s that satisfy the con-
 straints imposed by (4) for an example like (5a) are

   shown in (5b,c):
SS  LOC 
CONT  HCONS 
LTOP 
 RELS  (5) a. Kim found a chair in 30 seconds.
ARG-ST  ,
b. LTOP .-
RELS #
:found(k,y), :a(y,./ ,
),
The AAS requires that the local top (  ) of the se-
/ :chair(y), .- :in-30-secs( ) 10
in-30-secs(a (y, chair(y), found(k,y)))
lected adverb is also the verb’s local top. It also en-
sures that the local top (  ) of the daughter verb is ,
c. LTOP -
RELS #  :found(k,y) , - :a(y,./ ,
),
less than or equal to the adverb’s MOD value’s local
top (  ). This means that when a verb combines with
/ :chair(y) ,
:in-30-secs(  ) 10
a (y, chair(y), in-30-secs( found(k,y)))
a scopal adverbial complement, the verb’s predica-
tion will always be within the scope of that adver- 243
The handle ( ) of the quantifer a is within the
bial, as shown in (4). In addition, selected adverbials preposition’s scope in (5b), but outside it in (5c).
must be able to modify verbal expressions (hence the It is important to understand that the adverbial
[HEAD verb] specification in the adverbial’s MOD complement’s scope remains ‘clause-bounded’ un-
der this proposal. A verb like believe or try selects a
1
I am aware that by eliminating DEPS, I raise controversial verbal phrase as complement and lexically identifies
issues about the role of binding theory in the treatment of Prin-
ciple C effects, but these are orthogonal to the matters at hand. the local top of the relevant complement with the ap-
I follow Copestake et al.’s presentation of MRS throughout. In propriate semantic argument (the second argument
particular, lexical constraints are assumed to ensure that the lo-
2


cal top (a handle) of a verb or a scopal adverb is equal to that of Note that no further LOC, CAT, SUBCAT or HEAD iden-
its predication, modulo quantifiers ( ). tity is enforced.

112
of believe-rel or try-rel). Since a VP’s local top will  

PH in,how,many,secs,flat,did,Robin,
be identified with that of the rightmost adverbial in find,a,chair,sit,down,and,whip,off,hr,lgng,boots
an example like (6), all of the adverbs must be within ..SLASH :<;
the scope of the embedding handle-embedding rela-
tion:
 PH  in..  PH  did,R.,find..,sit..,and,whip..
..CAT
PP ..SL : - [CAT
] ; 
5  PH  did,R.  PH  find..,sit..,and,whip..
 ..SL : - ;
VP
(6) ...RELS # -
:try(x, .6 ) 17 .. 
5 5
5 6  PH  find..  PH  sit..  PH  and,whip..
..SL : - ;  ..SL : - ;  ..SL : - ; 
V ADV ADV
...LTOP 687 ...LTOP
7 ... ...LTOP 697
The SLASH values also make their way down to the
verbs find, sit, and whip, where they are ‘amalga-
In short, this proposal entails that the scope inter- mated’ from the selected adverbial, as in the BMS
actions of selected scopal adverbials parallel that analysis. Making normal assumptions about gaps,
of true modifiers, but in the opposite order. (see the CAT value of each selected adverbial is identi-
Copestake et al. (to appear) discussion of Kim ap- fied with the CAT value of its SLASH value. Since
parently almost succeeded. (has only an appar- MOD is within CAT, it follows that the filler’s MOD
ently(almost(succeeded(k))) reading). value must outscope each verbal predication.
Following Copestake et al. (to appear), I assume
that conjunctions embed the local tops of the con-
3 Extracted Adverbials Scope over juncts as their arguments, roughly as in (8):
Conjunctions
 PH  find,a,chair,sit,down,and,whip,off,hr,lgng,bts 
   and-rel    
In head-filler constructions of all sorts, it is rea- 
sonable to assume that the filler daughter’s CAT (8)
 ..RELS  LBL - ,..
and INDEX values are identified with those of the ARGS 
, , /
head daughter’s SLASH member.3 Now reconsider
Levine’s example in (1) above. In this case, the
 PH  find..  PH  sit..  PH  and,whip..
..LTOP
 ..LTOP  ..LTOP / 
CAT and INDEX values of the adverbial filler (the
PP in how many minutes flat) will be identified with
those of the SLASH member, which will in turn
(via standard HPSG principles governing the inher-
itance of SLASH specifications) be identified with Since each conjunct’s local top is embedded as an
the SLASH members of the selected adverbials, as argument of the conjunction, the only way the filler
sketched in (7): adverbial can simultaneously outscope find-rel, sit-
rel, and whip-rel) is for that adverbial to outscope
the and-rel (since, given the nature of MRS, the ad-
(7) verbial’s relation can only appear once in a resolved
mrs structure). The correct result thus results from
the resource-sensitive nature of MRS. Assuming a
3
Given MRS, it would be an unwanted complication to iden- variant of and-rel that provides the appropriate cu-
tify the entire CONT value of filler and the gap in a UDC. Iden- mulative event interpretation discussed by Levine,
tifying the LTOP of the filler daughter with that of the SLASH
value would also impose unwanted scope restrictions when the his example (1) is properly analyzed, as sketched in
filler is scopal. (9):

113
 
 RELS .- .- :how-many(x,
, ),
:second(x), 
(9) LTOP Manning et al., and others. Although I have mod-

:in( ./ ,x), h/ :and(h ,h= ,h> ),  ified the BMS analysis in three ways, by eliminat-

 @ :a(y, #? ,.@ ), #? :chair(y), ing DEPS, returning to van Noord and Bouma’s lex-
:found(k,y) , = :sit-down(k), ical rule analysis of adverb addition, and introduc-
A> :whip-off-h-l-boots(k) (
ing constraints, I preserve the elegant account that

Note that the use of ( , rather than )B* (as in Copes-


BMS provide of the Hukari/Levine (1995) observa-
tion that adverb and complement extraction are both
take et al. to appear), is crucial, as this is what allows morphosyntactically registered in all languages that
the and-rel to ‘slip in’ to the resolved mrs structure. locally register extraction dependencies. No analy-
sis with ‘wh-traces’ has achieved a comparable re-
4 Further Issues sult.
The question remains of how to deal with other ex- References
amples involving adverbs that follow a coordinate-
structure, e.g. (10)[from Levine 2002]. Exactly the Bouma, Gosse, Robert Malouf, and Ivan A. Sag.
same analysis developed above extends to these ex- 1998. Adjunct Scope and Complex Predicates.
amples if they are analyzed in terms of a rightward Paper presented at the 20th Annual Meeting
extraction scheme of the sort that would also treat of the DGfS. Section 8: The Syntax of Ad-
examples like (11a), where a left-adjoined (true) verbials – Theoretical and Cross-linguistic As-
modifier is within the scope of the right adjoined PP pects. Halle, Germany.
modifier:
Bouma, Gosse, Robert Malouf, and Ivan A. Sag.
(10) Robin [found a chair, sat down, and whipped 2001. Satisfying Constraints on Extraction and
off her logging boots] [in twenty seconds flat]. Adjunction. Natural Language and Linguistic
Theory. 19.1: 1–65.
(11) a. Sandy [[rarely visited a friend] because of
illness]. van Noord, Gertjan, and Gosse Bouma. 1994.
The Scope of Adjuncts and the Processing of
b. Sandy [rarely [visited a friend because of Lexical Rules. Proceedings of Coling: Kyoto,
illness]]. Japan.

The because(rarely...) reading associated with Copestake, Ann, Dan Flickinger, Carl Pollard, and
(11a) involves rightward extraction of the because- Ivan A. Sag. To appear. Minimal Recursion
phrase. This should be contrasted with the Semantics: an Introduction. To appear in Re-
rarely(because...) reading associated with (11b), search on Language and Computation.
where the because-phrase is directly realized as a
complement of visited, with rarely modifying the re- Hukari, Thomas E., and Robert D. Levine. 1995.
sultant VP. Adjunct Extraction. Journal of Linguistics 31:
195–226.
5 Conclusion
Levine, Robert D. 2002. Adjunct valents: Cu-
The traceless adverb-as-complement analysis is mulative scoping adverbial constructions
alive and well. It gives a principled answer to the and impossible descriptions. In J. Kim and
important questions raised by Levine about the in- S. Wechsler, eds., Proceedings of the 9th
teraction of adverbial extraction and cumulative con- International Conference on Head-Driven
junction, while at the same time providing a coher- Phrase Structure Grammar, Kyung-Hee Uni-
ent, unified approach for systematizing the massive versity, Seoul. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
evidence for the A-as-C approach provided by van http://cslipublications.stanford.edu/HPSG/3/.
Noord and Bouma (1994), Przepiórkowski 1999, Pp. 209–232.

114
Manning, Christopher, Ivan A. Sag, and Masayo
Iida. 1999. The Lexical Integrity of Japanese
Causatives. In R. D. Levine and G. Green, eds.,
Readings in Modern Phrase Structure Gram-
mar. Cambridge University Press. Pp. 39–79.

Przepiórkowski, Adam. 1999. Case Assign-


ment and the Complement-Adjunct Dichotomy:
A Non-Configurational Constraint-Based Ap-
proach. Doctoral dissertation, University of
Tübingen.

115

Potrebbero piacerti anche