Sei sulla pagina 1di 12

Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314

https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01113-0

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Under Pressure: Differentiating Adolescents’ Expectations


Regarding Stereotypic Masculine and Feminine Behavior
Emma F. Jackson 1 & Kay Bussey 1

Published online: 6 January 2020


# Springer Science+Business Media, LLC, part of Springer Nature 2020

Abstract
The present study provided a novel way to compare the pressure felt by adolescents to engage in same gender behavior and other
gender behavior. A new scale of felt pressure was developed which measured the reactions participants anticipated from others if
they were to engage in masculine or feminine stereotyped behaviors. The scale was tested on a sample of 297 Australian
adolescents between the ages of 12 and 17. Factor analysis indicated two factors which corresponded to Masculine-Typed and
Feminine-Typed behaviors. Items related to feminine-typed behaviors were designated as same gender for young women and
other gender for young men, and vice versa for masculine-typed items. Analyses indicated that young men reported higher felt
pressure to conform to same gender behavior than did young women, and young women reported felt pressure to conform to
other gender behavior whereas young men reported pressure to avoid other gender behavior. In addition, high same gender felt
pressure was associated with higher self-perceived same gender typicality and lower other gender typicality. Conversely, high
other gender felt pressure was associated with high levels of other gender typicality and lower levels of same gender typicality.
The presence of same and other gender felt pressure encourages theorists and practitioners to be mindful of the impact of both
these influences on adolescents’ gender identity development and psychosocial adjustment.

Keywords Adolescence . Gender identity . Masculinity . Femininity . Gender typicality . Felt pressure . Sex differences

Gender is a central component of personal identity, influenc- 2018), including the search for a comprehensive model of
ing appearance, professional and personal activities, academic gender identity (Egan and Perry 2001; Perry et al. 2019;
choices, and relationships (Egan and Perry 2001; Ruble et al. Schmader and Block 2015; Tobin et al. 2010).
2006). The pressure felt by individuals to engage in specific Gender identity is a widely researched and conceptualized
behaviors associated with their gender has a wide impact on construct (Bussey 2011). Traditional approaches to measuring
adolescents’ behavior and mental health. In adolescents, pres- gender identity assessed individual’s self-perceived identifica-
sure to avoid or conform to specific gendered activities is tion with gender stereotypic behaviors and traits (Liben et al.
linked to peer acceptance, school activities, academic choices, 2002). However, Spence (1993) noted that this method of
and negative mental health outcomes (Braun and Davidson assessment did not generalize across other domains of
2017; Bussey and Bandura 1999; Carver et al. 2003; gender-related cognitions. Further, Egan and Perry (2001) ob-
Kornienko et al. 2016). Interest in gender has been growing served that researcher-defined stereotypes did not necessarily
in recent psychological research (Morgenroth and Ryan reflect individual participant’s stereotype knowledge. Egan
and Perry (2001) therefore proposed the multidimensional
model of gender identity to address these concerns. The mul-
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article
(https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01113-0) contains supplementary
tidimensional model theorized that gender identity was multi-
material, which is available to authorized users. faceted with distinct cognitive components, in contrast to tra-
ditional approaches which measured only identification with
* Emma F. Jackson gender stereotypes.
emma.jackson3@hdr.mq.edu.au The components of the multidimensional model include
gender typicality, felt pressure to conform to gendered behav-
1
Centre for Emotional Health, Department of Psychology, Macquarie ior and intergroup bias. According to Egan and Perry, gender
University, 4 First Walk, Sydney, NSW 2109, Australia typicality refers to an individual’s self-perceived similarity to
304 Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314

their gender group, without reference to specific gendered the gender categories of male and female, creating a personal-
behaviors or traits. Intergroup bias is the attitudes and beliefs ized measure of gender identification (Egan and Perry 2001).
regarding gender groups, particularly with respect to positive This concept was further developed by Martin et al. (2017),
regard for one’s own gender group. Felt pressure is the pres- who examined the differences between individual’s perceived
sure felt by individuals to engage in gendered behaviors, mea- similarity to their own gender (same gender typicality) and
sured as an individual’s anticipated consequences for enacting their perceived similarity to the other gender (other gender
different gendered behaviors, where gendered behaviors are typicality). In their study, measures of same gender typicality
the masculine or feminine behaviors stereotypically assigned did not significantly correlate with other gender typicality,
to genders. Masculinity is associated with agentic traits such indicating that gender typicality is better conceptualized by
as being independent, assertive, and aggressive (Dean and considering both same and other genders. This reconceptual-
Tate 2016; Spence 1993) and is reflected in masculine-typed ization is in line with research outside Egan and Perry’s (2001)
behaviors such as sport. Femininity is associated with expres- multidimensional approach, which has suggested that mascu-
sive traits such as being nurturing and supportive (Dean and linity and femininity are associated with unique cognitive fac-
Tate 2016; Spence 1993) and is reflected in feminine-typed tors (Liben et al. 2002; Spence 1993). However, the other
behaviors such as ballet dancing. The components of gender components of gender identity identified by Egan and Perry,
identity manifest differently in each person and interact to such as felt pressure, have yet to be considered with respect to
influence mental health, processing of new experiences, peer the separation of same and other genders.
choice, behavior, and other gender-related domains (Carver Examining the component of felt pressure in Egan and Perry’s
et al. 2003; Corby et al. 2007; DeLay et al. 2018; Egan and (2001) model, there are inconsistencies in defining felt pressure,
Perry 2001; Jewell and Brown 2014; Patterson 2012; Yunger resulting in discrepancies in interpreting previous research. In
et al. 2004). Egan and Perry’s initial conceptualization of felt pressure, it
Recently, Martin et al. (2017) extended the multidimen- was defined as the felt pressure for gender conformity. It was
sional model to measure individuals’ self-perceived typicality proposed this pressure emanated from parents, peers and the self
to the other gender alongside typicality to their own gender. to conform to gender stereotypes (Carver et al. 2003).
This extension aligns with the view that normative popula- Subsequent research by Pauletti et al. (2014) critiqued how felt
tions possess gender cognitions beyond identification with pressure was measured in the original model and observed that in
their own gender (Joel et al. 2014). The present study aims Egan and Perry, felt pressure was operationalized as expected
to extend consideration of other gender identification to an- negative reactions to gender non-conforming behavior.
other of the components of gender identity: felt pressure for Therefore, when Pauletti et al. used Egan and Perry’s measure
gendered behavior. The aim of the present study was therefore of felt pressure, their results were interpreted as felt pressure to
first to develop a new measure for felt pressure and secondly avoid gendered behavior, contrasting previous interpretations as
to examine felt pressure for same and other gender behavior felt pressure to conform to gendered behaviors. An interpretation
and its relationship with gender typicality. that defines felt pressure as conformity to gendered behaviors
infers an individual’s cognitive relationship with same gender
behaviors. Whereas an interpretation that defines felt pressure
as avoidance of gendered behaviors infers individuals’ cognitive
Dual Approach to Gender Typicality and Felt relationship with other gender behaviors. This is an important
Pressure distinction as demonstrated by Martin et al.’s (2017) dual ap-
proach, showing that there are separate cognitions and expecta-
An important contribution of Egan and Perry’s (2001) multidi- tions for same gender knowledge and other gender knowledge.
mensional approach was the separation of self-perceived gen- Pauletti et al. (2014) reinterpretation points to a problem in
der identity from specific gender traits and behaviors. Previous how felt pressure has been understood and measured because
approaches to gender often assessed gender identity by measur- existing research has interpreted felt pressure both as
ing individual engagement in gendered behaviors specified by conformity to and avoidance of gendered behaviors. Yunger
established scales (Liben et al. 2002; Spence 1993), thus mea- et al. (2004) and Corby et al. (2007) interpreted felt pressure as
suring gendered behavior rather than self-perceived gender. In conformity, and Perry et al. (2019) interpreted felt pressure as
contrast, gender typicality is assessed as an individual’s self- avoidance. Thus, existing research is currently confounded by
perceived similarity to a gender category (Egan and Perry contrasting interpretations of the same scale of felt pressure.
2001). Self-perceived similarity is measured by the endorse- Additionally, based on research findings regarding gender
ment of how true statements such as “My personality is similar differences in gender typicality (Carver et al. 2003; Corby et al.
to boys/girls’ personality” are for the individual. By measuring 2007; Jewell and Brown 2014), similar gender differences in felt
gender typicality as self-perceived similarity, individuals pro- pressure may be present. Existing studies report that children
ject their own knowledge of gender traits and behaviors onto differ in same and other gender typicality because boys reported
Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314 305

higher same gender typicality than girls, whereas girls reported femininity, thereby encouraging them to engage in socially
higher other gender typicality than boys (Martin et al. 2017; advantageous masculine-typed behaviors.
Pauletti et al. 2017). Previous research has also found that boys Changes in young girl’s behavior described by Halim et al.
reported higher felt pressure for gendered behavior than did girls (2011) could be interpreted through the lens of the relationship
(Carver et al. 2003; Egan and Perry 2001; Kornienko et al. 2016; between felt pressure and gender typicality. Felt pressure is
Pauletti et al. 2014). Thus, reconceptualizing felt pressure regard- operationalized as individuals’ anticipated reactions from
ing both same and other gender behavior may clarify gender others regarding gendered behavior. In the case of the “Pink
differences in felt pressure. Frilly Dress” phase, young girls may anticipate positive reac-
A reanalysis of felt pressure is clearly required to separate tions from others and themselves regarding feminine-typed
cognitions regarding same and other gender behaviors. behavior, which then corresponds to greater same gender typ-
Therefore, the first aim of the present study is to develop a icality. Subsequently as they age, girls may begin to expect
new measure of felt pressure that distinguishes same and other less pressure to avoid other gender behavior due to social
gender behavior and to examine gender differences in felt valuation of masculinity and thus become more other gender
pressure. The separation of same and other gender felt pres- typical. It is therefore possible that felt pressure may influence
sure is of further value to research because masculinity and the decrease in same gender typicality across girls’ childhood.
femininity can be analyzed separately. Recent research on the multidimensional model provides
cross-sectional evidence in line with this possibility because
same gender felt pressure was found to be positively associ-
Conformity or Avoidance of Femininity ated with same gender typicality and negatively associated
and Masculinity with other gender typicality (Pauletti et al. 2017).
Participants’ gender may further moderate the relationship
The desirability of in-group traits leads to increased conformi- of felt pressure and gender typicality. As noted by Halim et al.
ty to in-group traits and avoidance of out-group traits (2011), girls’ awareness of social devaluation of femininity
(Schmader and Block 2015); however, this effect is different impacts their other gender behavior; however, such a change
for males and females across ages due to perceived devalua- is not evident in boys. Therefore, the association of felt pres-
tion of femininity. Indeed, Messerschmidt (2012), in a com- sure and gender typicality may vary across participants’ gen-
prehensive review of research on masculinity, reports that der because for women, other gender characteristics are so-
avoidance of femininity is often core to men’s gender identity. cially valued, whereas for men, other gender characteristics
However, conformity and avoidance of traits may be con- are devalued. Thus, the second aim of the present study was
founded for women because out-group masculine traits are to examine the relationship between dual felt pressure and
socially desirable (Messerschmidt 2012), and thus women gender typicality, as well as how gender might moderate this
may feel less pressure to avoid other gender behaviors. The relationship.
gender differences in felt pressure noted previously are con-
sistent with these social theories because during childhood
boys reported higher felt pressure to avoid feminine-typed The Present Study
conduct compared to girl’s felt pressure to avoid masculine
conduct (Carver et al. 2003; Cooper 2014; Egan and Perry Building on the dual approach proposed by Martin et al.
2001; Pauletti et al. 2017). It would therefore be expected that (2017), the first aim of our study was to examine the reliability
the inverse would be evident regarding same gender felt pres- and factor structure of a new measure of felt pressure for
sure: Female adolescents would report less felt pressure to gendered behavior which measured same and other gendered
conform to same gender behavior than male. behavior. The development of the new scale followed existing
Differences in felt pressure between males and females of definitions and operationalization of felt pressure as pressure
different ages may also impact gender typicality. Halim et al. from parents, peers, and the self. Same and other gender felt
(2011) suggest that throughout middle childhood, the social pressure were distinguished by asking all participants to re-
valuation of masculinity and femininity differentially impacts spond to items regarding both masculine and feminine stereo-
girls’ and boys’ gender identity development. Pre-school and typed behaviors, which could then be coded as same or other
kindergarten girls go through a phase of strong adherence to dependent on participants’ gender.
same gender behavior, often termed the “Pink Frilly Dress” Alongside measuring felt pressure regarding same and oth-
phase after their choice of clothing (Halim et al. 2011). Then, er gender behavior, our new scale also sought to distinguish
in primary school, adherence to femininity transforms into a felt pressure to avoid gendered behavior from pressure to con-
strong preference for masculine-typed activities. Such a form to gendered behavior. Felt pressure has previously been
change does not occur for boys, and Halim et al. posited this operationalized by measuring the negative responses partici-
to be due to girls’ increasing awareness of the devaluation of pants anticipate from others if they engaged in specified
306 Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314

behaviors (Jewell 2015). In these existing measures of felt Method


pressure, participants indicated whether they themselves or
others would be upset if they engaged in gendered behaviors, Participants
thus measuring felt pressure to avoid the behavior (Pauletti
et al. 2014). In order to measure pressure to avoid or conform Fully 297 Australian high school students participated in the
to behaviors, the new scale gave participants the option to present study and were recruited from nine independent, co-
indicate anticipated pleased reactions or upset reactions. educational schools in New South Wales as part of a larger
Thus, felt pressure to avoid gendered behavior would be pres- longitudinal study. The sample comprised 197 Grade 8 stu-
ent when participants indicated they expected upset reactions dents (66.3%; 115 [58.38%] female, Mage = 13.8, SD = .43),
to gendered behavior. Conversely, felt pressure to conform to and 100 Grade 10 students (33.7%; 50 [50%] female, Mage =
gendered behavior would be present when participants indi- 15.8, SD = .35). The sample was largely of upper middle-class
cated they expected pleased reactions to gendered behavior. socioeconomic status (Australian Curriculum Assessment and
In line with the dual approach by Martin et al. (2017), we Reporting Authority 2016). Students selected all ethnicities
expected that the new measure of felt pressure would reveal which applied to them, and identified as Anglo-Celtic (212,
two factors that would correspond to masculine-typed and 73.5%), European (53, 20.5%), East/Southeast Asian (20,
feminine-typed behaviors (Hypothesis 1). Additionally, due 7.4%), Aboriginal/Torres Strait Islander (14, 5.4%), Middle
to gender differences in felt pressure reported across Eastern (12, 4.0%), South Asian (11, 3.7%), and Pacific
existing literature (Carver et al. 2003; Cooper 2014; Islander (4, 1.3%) with five selecting “other”.
Egan and Perry 2001; Pauletti et al. 2017), we expected
that female and male adolescents would differ in scores
on the new measure of felt pressure (Hypothesis 2). Measures
Specifically, we expected that due to devaluation of fem-
ininity, female adolescents would report less felt pressure Gender Typicality
to conform to same gender behavior than male adoles-
cents (Hypothesis 2a), and male adolescents would report Gender typicality was measured with the scale adapted from
more felt pressure to avoid other gender behavior com- Jewell (2015) which was based on Egan and Perry (2001). In
pared to female adolescents (Hypothesis 2b). Jewell’s study, participants rated only their self-perceived typ-
Our second aim was to examine associations between gen- icality to their own gender, such that male adolescents were
der typicality and felt pressure. Building on previous research asked to rate similarity to other male peers, and female ado-
suggesting that the pressure felt by individuals impacts their lescents to other female peers. To assess other gender typical-
gender typicality (Halim et al. 2011), we hypothesized that ity alongside same gender typicality in the present study, each
there would be a systematic relationship between felt pressure participant rated their similarity to both male and female peers
and gender typicality (Hypothesis 3). Previous research also (e.g., “I fit in with boys” and “I fit in with girls”). The final
indicated a positive association between same gender typical- measure included 10 items, five relating to males and five to
ity and same gender felt pressure, as well as a negative asso- females, and students rated on a 4-point scale the extent to
ciation of other gender typicality with same gender felt pres- which each item was true for them: 0 (Definitely not true for
sure (Pauletti et al. 2017). Drawing on this research to consid- me) to 3 (Very true for me).
er how gender typicality may then relate to other gender felt Following the procedure detailed in Martin et al. (2017),
pressure, the inverse relationship was expected. We predicted items were recoded to create the subscales of same and other
that at higher levels of other gender felt pressure, same gender gender typicality. For participants identifying as female, as
typicality would be low (Hypothesis 3a) and that at higher determined by self-reported demographic information, items
levels of same gender felt pressure, same gender typicality regarding “girls” were coded as “same gender,” and items
would be high (Hypothesis 3b). Regarding other gender typ- regarding “boys” were coded as “other gender” with five
icality, higher levels of other gender typicality were expected items corresponding to same gender and five for other gender.
when participants reported higher levels of other gender felt The recoding process was then repeated in reverse for partic-
pressure (Hypothesis 3c), and lower levels of other gender ipants identifying as male. Final scores were calculated by
typicality were expected when participants reported high summing ratings for items within subscales of same and other
levels of same gender pressure (Hypothesis 3d). gender typicality (with a possible range of 5–42). Higher
Additionally, we hypothesized that gender would moderate scores indicated stronger same or other gender typicality.
the relationship between felt pressure and gender typicality Internal consistency was good in the present study, with al-
(Hypothesis 3e). Specifically, the association between these phas of .87 and .89 for same gender typicality and with alphas
variables was expected to be stronger for young men than of .83 and .82 for other gender typicality (for female and male
for young women. adolescents, respectively).
Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314 307

Felt Pressure for Gendered Behavior online or with a paper copy of the survey under the supervi-
sion of a researcher or teacher, and they provided consent prior
A new measure of felt pressure for gendered behavior was to commencing the survey. Sixteen students completed a pa-
developed for the present study: Felt Pressure for Gendered per version of the survey due to insufficient access to com-
Behavior Scale (FP-GBS). The scale was developed from def- puters. Demographic information was collected including
initions and operationalization of felt pressure described in the grade, age, gender, date of birth, and ethnicity. All scales were
introduction. Items required participants to rate how they an- ordered into two fixed survey forms to counterbalance presen-
ticipated their parents, peers, and themselves would feel if tation of measures. At completion of the survey, students were
they engaged in specified gendered behaviors. Each rating provided with a debriefing statement including advice on how
was done on a 7-point scale from 1 (Very pleased) through 4 to make an appointment with school counsellors if they had
(Wouldn’t care) to 7 (Very upset). Six behaviors, three of experienced distress during the survey.
which were feminine-stereotyped and three of which were
masculine-stereotyped, were included in the scale which were
Missing Data
based on highly gender stereotypic behaviors identified by
Liben et al. (2002) for children and adults. The six items
The proportion of missing data was small and ranged from
related to feminine-typed appearance, feminine-typed activi-
0%–3.3%. The expectation maximization (EM) method in
ties, selection of female friends, masculine-typed appearance,
SPSS was used to manage missing data because EM has the
masculine-typed activities, and selection of male friends. For
advantage of non-random imputation compared to other
example, the item assessing pressure from parents to engage
methods (Allison 2001). Of the 317 students who participated
in a feminine-typed activity was: “If I started a girly activity
in the study, 20 were removed due to <80% completion of the
like ballet my parent would be ….” The corresponding item
relevant measures, and missing data estimation was performed
assessing pressure regarding masculine-typed activities was:
on the remaining 297 participants.
“If I joined a boys’ sports club, my parent would be ….” Each
of the six items was repeated, substituting “my friends would
be …” and “I would be…” in place of “my parent would
be…” to assess pressure from peers and the participant them- Results
selves. (The measure is reported in the Appendix, and a ready-
to-use version is available to download in the online Prior to analyses, the potential clustering of school and
supplement.) age were assessed using a linear mixed model analysis.
To facilitate interpretability, items were rescored such that School and grade were included as random factors and
−3 corresponded to a response of “Very upset,” zero found to have no significant impact on felt pressure so
corresponded to a response of “Wouldn’t care,” and + 3 that they were not controlled in subsequent analyses.
corresponded to a response of “Very pleased.” To calculate Descriptive statistics and correlations for felt pressure
subscales, items were coded to distinguish same and other and gender typicality for male and female adolescents,
gender behavior, wherein masculine-typed items were desig- separately, are reported in Table 1. Correlations showed
nated same gender for male adolescents and other gender for that for both young men and women, same gender typi-
female adolescents, and vice-versa for feminine-typed items. cality and same gender felt pressure were significantly
Lastly, items were summed within subscales for a total same positively correlated, as was other gender typicality and
gender felt pressure score and other gender felt pressure score, other gender felt pressure. Same gender felt pressure and
with each possibly ranging from −27 to +27. Scores below other gender felt pressure were significantly positively
zero indicate expected negative responses or felt pressure to correlated for only young women, whereas same gender
avoid the specified behavior. Scores above zero indicated ex- typicality and other gender felt pressure were significantly
pected positive responses or felt pressure to conform to the negatively correlation for only young men. Correlations
specified behavior. Scores close to zero indicate less felt pres- indicate that there are systematic relationships between
sure because zero corresponds to expected neutral reactions dual felt pressure and gender typicality and that these
from parents, peers, and themselves. differ between genders.
Results are presented in three sections. In the first
Procedure section, the results of an exploratory factor analysis
which tested Hypothesis 1 are presented. The second
Ethics approval was granted by the authors’ institutional re- section presents the results from a mixed design
view board prior to commencement of data collection. ANOVA which tested Hypothesis 2. Finally, the results
Consent was obtained from school principals and parents prior from two linear regression models that tested Hypothesis
to survey administration. Students completed the survey either 3 are presented.
308 Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314

Table 1 Descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables

ns (Male/Female) Male Adolescents Female Adolescents Actual Correlations

Variables M (SD) M (SD) Range 1. 2. 3. 4.

1. Same gender pressure 132/165 9.76 (8.83) 5.90 (8.46) −14 – +27 – .54** .37** −.15
2. Other gender pressure 132/165 −7.79 (10.23) 3.77 (7.42) −27 – +27 −.11 – .10 .25**
3. Same gender typicality 132/165 10.87 (3.75) 10.59 (3.21) 0–15 .31** −.22* – .04
4. Other gender typicality 132/165 4.33 (3.42) 6.87 (3.47) 0–15 −.12 .23** .06 –

Note: Correlations for female adolescents are above the diagonal of the correlation matrix; for male adolescents, below the diagonal
*p < .05. ***p < .001

Hypothesis 1 subjects variable with the levels of male and female and felt
pressure was included as a within-subjects variable of same
An Exploratory Factor Analysis was conducted to test and other gender felt pressure.
Hypothesis 1, which predicted that the FP-GBS would be con- Results revealed a main effect for felt pressure, with higher
sistent with a two-factor structure that corresponded to masculine same gender felt pressure (M = 7.62, SD = 8.83) than other
and feminine stereotyping. Principal axis factoring (PAF) with no gender felt pressure (M = −1.37, SD = 10.51), F(1, 295) =
rotation was conducted on the 18 items developed. Factors with 230.32, p < .001, ηp2 = .44. There was also a main effect for
no rotation best approximated simple structure, and therefore gender because there was a significant difference in felt pres-
unrotated factor loadings were interpreted. The Kaiser–Meyer– sure reported by young men (M = .99, SD = 6.40) and young
Olkin measure verified sampling adequacy (KMO = .81), and
individual items had KMO values above .74, which is greater
Table 2 Factor loadings for the two-factor structure of the felt pressure
than the acceptable limit of .50 (Kaiser 1974). scale
The PAF revealed four factors with eigenvalues above 1.
Examination of the scree plot in accordance with the process Factor
outlined by Costello and Osborne (2005) indicated an elbow Item 1 2
after Factors 1 and 2. Thus, the two-factor solution was
interpreted, which accounted for 54.76% of the variance. Parental reaction
Factor loadings for the two-factor structure are displayed in Feminine-typed appearance .72 .14
Table 2, and the full four-factor loadings and eigenvalues are Feminine-typed activity .80 .12
presented in Table 1s of the online supplement. Female friend selection .52 .27
Consistent with Hypothesis 1, examination of the two-factor Masculine-typed appearance .08 .60
solution showed items regarding feminine-typed behaviors load- Masculine-typed activity −.33 .57
ed onto Factor 1 and masculine-typed items loaded onto Factor 2. Male friend selection −.30 .60
The two-factor structure also generalized across source of pres- Peer reaction
sure, including parents, peers, and self, indicating these combined Feminine-typed appearance .80 .13
to contribute to the overarching factors of Masculine-Typed and Feminine-typed activity .80 .07
Feminine-Typed behaviors. The internal consistency of the scales Female friend selection .64 .24
was good as indicated by Cronbach’s alphas for same gender Masculine-typed appearance −.02 .72
behavior (αmales = .89; αfemales .85) and for other gender behavior Masculine-typed activity −.29 .72
(αmales = .88; αfemales .81). Male friend selection −.29 .59
Self reaction
Hypothesis 2 Feminine-typed appearance .81 .12
Feminine-typed activity .74 .12
We conducted a two-factor mixed-design ANOVA to test Female friend selection .55 .27
Hypothesis 2 which predicted that female and male adoles- Masculine-typed appearance −.03 .56
cents would differ in their levels of same and other gender felt Masculine-typed activity −.26 .60
pressure. Assumptions were tested, and although tests of nor- Male friend selection −.16 .63
mality were violated, skew and kurtosis statistics indicated Eigenvalues 5.54 4.32
normal distribution, thus analyses continued without transfor-
mations. Participants’ gender was included as a between- Note. Factor loadings above .40 are bolded
Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314 309

women (M = 4.84, SD = 6.97), F(1, 295) = 24.03, p < .001, (Additionally, Table 2s in the online supplement presents the
ηp2 = .08. Consistent with Hypotheses 2, these main effects alternative analysis regressing gender typicality onto felt
were qualified by a significant interaction involving felt pres- pressure.) Alternative analysis found a similar pattern of asso-
sure and participants’ gender, F(1, 296) = 141.32, p < .001, ciation as is described in the following. Of note, both same and
ηp2 = .33. To fully explore the interaction and Hypotheses 2a other gender typicality were significant predictors of same
and 2b, t-values were examined against p < .025 (.05/2) as the gender felt pressure, but no significant interactions with par-
Bonferroni adjustment. ticipant gender were present. Whereas same gender typicality
Hypothesis 2a predicted that female adolescents would re- was not a significant predictor of other gender felt pressure,
port less pressure to conform to same gender behavior than the interaction of other gender typicality and participant gen-
male adolescents. Consistent with this hypothesis, results in- der did significantly predict other gender typicality.
dicated that young men (M = 9.76, SD = 8.83) reported higher
same gender felt pressure than did young women (M = 5.90, Hypotheses 3a And 3b
SD = 8.46), t(296) = 3.83, p < .001, ηp2 = .05. One sample t-
tests against zero were conducted to determine whether scores Hypothesis 3a predicted that higher levels of other gender felt
were significantly different from a score of zero which indi- pressure would be associated with lower same gender typical-
cated neutral felt pressure. Results revealed that both young ity, and conversely Hypothesis 3b predicted that higher levels
men’s, t(296) = 12.7, p < .001, and young women’s, t(296) = of same gender felt pressure would be associated with higher
8.96, p = <.001, scores significantly differed from zero, sug- same gender typicality. To test these hypotheses, same and
gesting that both reported felt pressure to conform to same other gender felt pressure were regressed onto same gender
gender behavior, but this pressure, on average, was signifi- typicality. Participants’ gender was also included as a moder-
cantly lower for young women than for young men. ating variable to test Hypothesis 3e.
Hypothesis 2b predicted that male adolescents would re- Consistent with Hypothesis 3a, examination of coefficients
port more felt pressure to avoid other gender behavior than showed that high levels of same gender felt pressure were
female adolescents. Results indicated that young men (M = associated with high levels of reported same gender typicality
−7.79, SD = 10.29) reported lower other gender felt pressure (see Table 3a). In line with Hypothesis 3b, high levels of other
than did young women (M = 3.77, SD = 7.42), t(296) = gender felt pressure were associated with low levels of report-
−11.23, p < .001, ηp2 = .30. Again, one sample t-tests were ed same gender typicality. The main effect of gender was also
conducted to determine whether these scores differed signifi- significant. Female adolescents reported higher same gender
cantly from a neutral score of zero. Results showed that that typicality than male adolescents at average levels of felt pres-
both young men’s, t(296) = −8.70, p < .001, and young sure. However, results from the second step of the regression
women’s, t(296) = 6.53, p = <.001, scores significantly dif- were inconsistent with Hypothesis 3e because the interaction
fered from zero. These findings give support to Hypothesis of felt pressure and participants’ gender did not significantly
2b because the mean score for young men was significantly increase R2.
below zero, indicating felt pressure to avoid feminine-typed
behavior, whereas female adolescents reported felt pressure to Hypotheses 3c and 3d
conform to other gender behavior because their mean score
was significantly above zero. Hypothesis 3c predicted that higher levels of other gender felt
pressure would be associated with higher other gender typi-
Hypothesis 3 cality, and conversely Hypothesis 3d predicted that higher
levels of same gender felt pressure would be associated with
Two regression models tested Hypothesis 3 which predicted a lower other gender typicality. To test these hypotheses, same
systematic relationship between felt pressure and same and and other gender felt pressure were regressed onto other gen-
other gender typicality. The two step-wise regression models der typicality. Again, participants’ gender was included as a
were two-step models. Participants’ gender, same gender felt moderating variable to test Hypothesis 3e.
pressure, and other gender felt pressure were entered in Step 1 Consistent with Hypothesis 3c, examination of coefficients
to assess main effects. Hypothesis 3e predicted that partici- showed that high levels same gender felt pressure were asso-
pants’ gender would moderate the relationship between felt ciated with lower levels of reported other gender typicality
pressure and gender typicality. Thus, the interactions of par- (see Table 3b). In addition, in line with Hypothesis 3d, high
ticipants’ gender with felt pressure were entered in Step 2 to levels of other gender felt pressure were associated with
assess the moderating effect of participants’ gender. higher levels of reported other gender typicality. The main
Assumptions were tested, with no violations, and effect of gender was significant with female adolescents
multicollinearity was not of concern. Table 3 presents the reporting more other gender typicality than male adolescents
standardized regression parameters and R 2 values. at average levels of felt pressure. Further, consistent with
310 Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314

Table 3 Regression of gender and felt pressure predicting gender typicality

Step 1 Step 2

Predictors β b t β b t

(a) Same gender typicality


Same gender felt pressure .38 .15 6.65*** .44 .17 4.83***
Other gender felt pressure −.19 −.06 −2.84* −.20 −.07 −1.64
Participant gender −.14 −1.00 −2.14* −.10 −.71 −1.28
Same gender felt pressure x gender −.11 −.05 −1.01
Other gender felt pressure x gender −.01 −.00 −.08
F 15.80*** 9.69***
df 3 5
dferror 293 291
R2 .14 .14
ΔR2 .00
(b) Other gender typicality
Same gender felt pressure −.18 −.08 −3.37* −.40 −.17 −4.65***
Other gender felt pressure .31 .11 4.80*** .62 .22 5.38***
Participant gender −.14 −1.01 −2.10* −.24 −1.74 −3.14*
Same gender felt pressure x gender .26 .13 2.63*
Other gender felt pressure x gender −.31 −.15 −2.95*
F 24.22*** 17.47***
df 3 5
dferror 293 291
R2 .20 .48
ΔR2 .03*

*p < .05. ***p < .001

Hypothesis 3e, two significant two-way interactions involving gender typicality was significant for female adolescents (β =
participants’ gender and felt pressure were obtained. −.07, p < .046), but not for male adolescents (β = −.04,
p = .251) (see Fig. 1a). Additionally, the positive association
Hypothesis 3e between other gender felt pressure and other gender typicality,
although significant for both young women and men, was
Results provided partial support for Hypothesis 3e which pre- stronger for female adolescents (β = .11, p < .001) than for
dicted that participants’ gender would moderate the relation- male adolescents (β = .07, p = .011) (see Fig. 1b).
ship of felt pressure and gender typicality. Inconsistent with
Hypothesis 3e, the regression model testing Hypotheses 3a
and 3b indicated that participants’ gender did not moderate Discussion
the relationship of felt pressure and same gender typicality
(see Table 3a). The present study extended the current understanding of gen-
However, the model testing Hypotheses 3c and 3d provid- der identity under Egan and Perry’s (2001) multidimensional
ed support for Hypothesis 3e (see Table 3b). Here, partici- model by reconceptualizing felt pressure to include other gen-
pants’ gender was a significant moderator of the relationship der behavior and same gender behavior. Factor analysis of our
between same and other gender felt pressure and other gender new measure of felt pressure revealed that felt pressure
typicality. Two significant interactions were present: (a) par- encompassed two distinct factors that corresponded to stereo-
ticipants’ gender with same gender felt pressure and (b) par- typic masculine- and feminine-related items. Examination of
ticipants’ gender with other gender felt pressure. These inter- differences in reported felt pressure between young men and
actions were further investigated by examining simple slopes young women gave further support for the reconceptualization
computed using PROCESS. because greater insight was gained by distinguishing felt pres-
Analysis of the simple slopes revealed that the negative sure for same and other gender behavior. Young men reported
association between same gender felt pressure and other significantly more overall felt pressure and felt pressure to
Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314 311

7.5
Female adolescents identification with only one gender (Joel et al. 2014). Further
7
consistent with existing literature, results of the present study
Other gender typicality

Male adolescents
6.5 revealed gender differences in felt pressure for same gender
6 behavior (Carver et al. 2003; Egan and Perry 2001; Kornienko
et al. 2016). The present study extended prior research by
5.5
examining gender differences in a dual approach to felt pres-
5 sure and found that male adolescents reported more felt pres-
4.5 sure to conform to same gender behavior than did female
4
adolescents and that female adolescents reported more felt
-1.206 7.617 16.441 pressure to conform to other gender behavior than did male.
Same gender felt pressure The gender differences in felt pressure for other gender
behavior reported in the present study gives new insight into
(a) Same Gender Felt Pressure the kinds of pressure that young men and young women ex-
perience. In line with predictions, young men reported pres-
sure to avoid feminine-typed behavior, consistent with social
7.5 Female adolescents theories which suggest that males perceive a core part of gen-
7 Male adolescents der identity to be avoidance of feminine-typed behaviors
Other gender typicality

6.5 (Messerschmidt 2012). In contrast, young women reported


felt pressure to conform to masculine-typed behavior. These
6
results shift the focus toward the value placed on masculine-
5.5
typed attributes for women and girls, in line with previous
5 research indicating that young children cite masculine-typed
4.5
characteristics as the reason they like other classmates (Braun
and Davidson 2017).
4
-11.8743 -1.3661 9.1421 The present study’s demonstration of gender differences in
Other gender felt pressure other gender felt pressure is of particular interest when viewed
together with previous research regarding felt pressure and
(b) Other Gender Felt Pressure psychological outcomes. Using a scale of felt pressure that
Fig. 1 Moderation effect of participants’ gender on the relationships only measured avoidance of other gender behavior, Carver
between other gender typicality and (a) same gender felt pressure and et al. (2003) reported that, in childhood, felt pressure to avoid
(b) other gender felt pressure. In both panels, pressure is graphed at −1 other gender behavior had a significant negative relationship
SD below the mean, the mean, and + 1 SD above the mean. Black lines, with global self-worth, social competence, and achievement-
whether dashed or solid, indicate significant relationships between pres-
sure and typicality whereas the grey, dashed line for male adolescents in oriented characteristics for girls. Conversely, for boys, higher
Fig. 1a is not significant felt pressure was only associated with lower levels of
relationship-oriented characteristics. Examining previous re-
search in parallel with the present study, the relationship of felt
conform to same gender behavior than young women did. pressure and psychological outcomes becomes clearer. If
Comparatively, young women reported felt pressure to women are pressured to avoid masculine-typed behavior, neg-
conform to other gender behavior whereas young men report- ative self-worth may be linked to an inability to engage in
ed felt pressure to avoid other gender behaviors. Results fur- socially valued masculine-typed behaviors that might improve
ther indicated that felt pressure was systematically associated their social competence and self-worth. However, the present
with gender typicality. Specifically, same and other gender felt study suggests that many young women report felt pressure to
pressure were independently associated with same gender typ- conform to other gender masculine-typed behaviors that pro-
icality and other gender typicality. Gender then moderated the mote increased self-worth. The FB-GBS opens opportunities
associations of same and other gender felt pressure with other to further examine how the different types of felt pressure may
gender typicality such that the associations were significant aid in unravelling the impact of the different values of femi-
only for young men. The following discusses thee results in nine and masculine stereotyping on psychological adjustment.
greater detail. Of key interest in our results regarding the association of
The exploratory factor analysis of the FP-GBS supported felt pressure and gender typicality is the moderating effect of
the two proposed subscales of same gender behavior and other gender on the relationship between felt pressure and other
gender behavior. This separation of same and other genders gender typicality. Our results suggested that female adoles-
supports research which shows that within gender-conforming cents are more susceptible to felt pressure than male adoles-
populations, an individual’s experience of gender goes beyond cents are. One explanation is that the mean level of other
312 Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314

gender typicality is lower in males than in females (Egan and study design prevented clear causal statements to be made
Perry 2001). Perhaps by adolescence low other gender typi- about the relationships among felt pressure, gender, and gen-
cality in young men is consolidated from high same gender der typicality. The sample was also restricted to independent
felt pressure throughout childhood. Intensification of gender schools that have an over-representation of higher socioeco-
identity has been observed in adolescence as male and female nomic families, which may directly impact the negative ex-
adolescents’ gender identities become increasingly differenti- pectations around masculine-typed behaviors. According to
ated (Hill and Lynch 1983). Marasco (2018) the perception of what is a desirable
Alternatively, the effect may lie with young women. masculine-typed behavior changes across contexts, influenced
Masculine-typed behavior is socially acceptable for women by race/ethnicity, social class, and age. For adolescents, school
(Braun and Davidson 2017), and there is strong potential for becomes a self-contained social environment in which the
them to be other gender typical because they may want to definition of masculinity responds to the social norms within
engage in socially advantageous masculine-typed behaviors. each school (Marasco 2018; Messerschmidt 2012). Although
Indeed, the present study indicated that young women’s other there was not a significant clustering effect of school in the
gender typicality was more strongly impacted by felt pressure. current sample, future research should remain aware of the
Thus, when young women expect negative reactions to other possible effect of contextual masculinity.
gender behavior, there may be a larger impact on their other
gender typicality than is observed in men, although the con- Future Directions
verse effect could also be true. Examination of the relationship
of felt pressure and gender typicality longitudinally would The present study provides a basis for future research by giv-
allow for the identification of developmental stages and for ing a description and analysis of how felt pressure for gen-
causal patterns to be detected. Further, felt pressure has the dered behavior presented within a sample of predominantly
potential to be experimentally tested because participant’s ex- gender-conforming Australian adolescents. A longitudinal
pectations of other’s reactions to gendered behavior could be study design would aid in unravelling the impact of felt pres-
manipulated. Such a manipulation would allow the impact of sure on gender typicality over time, along with the impact of
higher or lower felt pressure on gender typicality to be tracked. felt pressure on other psychological processes and outcomes.
The current study had many strengths, the largest being the Extending the sample to include gender-diverse participants,
development of a new scale to measure felt pressure and the potentially as a majority, would reveal how felt pressure may
demonstration by the FP-GBS that felt pressure comprises differentially impact gender-diverse individuals compared to
separate knowledge regarding same and other gender behav- gender-conforming individuals. The construction of the FP-
iors. The sample used in the present study was also of value GBS as measuring felt pressure from parents, peers, and the
due to its large size and also in drawing from a novel popula- self also provides the opportunity to consider each of these
tion outside the United States. However, some limitations to sources of pressure individually and in comparison to one
the present study warrant comment. another.
Furthermore, although the current study developed a new
Limitations measure of felt pressure for gendered behavior, the underlying
factor structure is yet to be confirmed in an independent sam-
First, restrictions were placed on the present study due to the ple. Although the current study was able to show that the
lack of opportunity for gender-diverse participants to identify measure showed gender differences, as expected based on
themselves. Inclusion of a gender diverse option alongside the theory and previous research, further validation studies of
male and female options in demographic information was not the measure should be conducted to confirm the validity (both
included due to wishes of some school administrations. It convergent and discriminant) and reliability of the measure.
could be argued that demographic information assesses a gen-
der binary, failing to provide an opportunity for participants to Practice Implications
volunteer gender diverse identities. Although this restriction
may have not directly impacted the results of our study, it Movement toward a dual approach broadens the scope of the
promoted ideas of a gender binary and possibly that genders multidimensional model of gender identity. Understanding the
outside this binary are deviant. The implication that transgen- interplay of same and other gender-related cognitions opens the
der and gender-diverse children are deviant is not the aim of conversation for mental health professionals to better untangle
the present study, and therefore it is important to note the the influences of gender identity on adolescents’ adjustment,
reason behind the lack of recognition of these individuals. along with how to increase their resilience. Further, the finding
Second, study sampling and design were limitations. The that adolescents experience both same and other gender felt
study was cross-sectional and quasi-experimental because pressure encourages parents and teachers to be cognizant that
participants’ gender could not be randomly allocated. Thus, pressure to avoid or conform to masculine and feminine
Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314 313

stereotypic behavior applies to both males and females. For 4. If I wore the jersey of the sports team I support, my parent
example, in considering the underrepresentation of women in [my friends; I] would be …
STEM, Liben (2016) notes how field interventions have primar- 5. If I joined a boys’ sports club, my parent [my friends; I]
ily focused on increasing the feminine-typing of STEM subjects would be …
and careers, but that these interventions do not achieve their 6. If I was spending a lot of time with boys, my parent [my
goals. The dual approach instead suggests that equal attention friends; I] would be…
should be paid to the impact of felt pressure for masculine-
typed behavior because the relationship of other gender felt pres- Note. Each item is rated on a 7-point scale where 1 = Very
sure was significantly associated with other gender typicality for pleased, 2 = Quite pleased, 3 = A little pleased, 4 = Wouldn’t
female adolescents in our study. care, 5 = A little upset, 6 = Quite upset, 7 = Very upset. For
analysis, items are recoded from −3 (Very pleased) to +3
(Very upset). The first three items measure feminine stereo-
Conclusion typed behavior; the last three, masculine stereotyped.

The present study provides compelling evidence that the mul-


tidimensional model of gender identity’s component of felt
pressure needs to include both same and other gender cogni-
tions. This reconceptualization of felt pressure provides the References
opportunity to more closely examine the relationship of felt
pressure with adjustment. Further, in moving towards a more Allison, P. D. (2001). Quantitative applications in the social sciences:
Missing data. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. https://doi.
comprehensive model of gender identity, the present study org/10.4135/9781412985079.
makes an important contribution. Alongside conceptualizing Australian Curriculum Assessment and Reporting Authority. (2016). My
same and other gender typicality (Martin et al. 2017; Pauletti school website. Retrieved from http://www.myschool.edu.au.
et al. 2017), the present study suggests that multiple compo- Braun, S. S., & Davidson, A. J. (2017). Gender (non)conformity in mid-
dle childhood: A mixed methods approach to understanding gender-
nents of gender identity extend beyond cognitions regarding typed behavior, friendship, and peer preference. Sex Roles, 77(1–2),
only our own gender to the other gender, thereby allowing 16–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0693-z.
gender identity to be understood by jointly considering both Bussey, K. (2011). Gender identity development. In S. J. Schwartz, K.
same and other gender cognitions and behaviors. Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Handbook of identity theory and
research (pp. 603–628). New York: Springer.
Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of gender
Compliance with Ethical Standards Authors declare that development and differentiation. Psychological Review, 106(4),
there is no conflict of interests or external funding for this research. 676–173. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.106.4.676.
Authors declare that the study complied with the ethical standards of Carver, P., Yunger, J., & Perry, D. (2003). Gender identity and adjustment
the Authors’ institution, including participant consent and adherence to in middle childhood. A Journal of Research, 49(3), 95–109. https://
the APA ethical guidelines. doi.org/10.1023/A:1024423012063.
Cooper, P. (2014). Gender self-discrepancies in middle childhood: influ-
Appendix ences on children’s personal and social adjustment (doctoral disser-
tation, Florida Atlantic University). Available from ProQuest disser-
Felt Pressure for Gendered Behavior Scale tations and theses database. (UMI no. 3647552)
Corby, B. C., Hodges, E. V., & Perry, D. G. (2007). Gender identity and
adjustment in black, Hispanic, and white preadolescents.
There are three portions to this 18-item measure, varying the Developmental Psychology, 43(1), 261–266. https://doi.org/10.
six items in each to target a parent, friends, and one’s self. The 1037/0012-1649.43.1.261.
instructions for each section then are: (a) “Think about your Costello, B. A., & Osborne, J. (2005). Best practices in exploratory factor
analysis: Four recommendations for getting the most from your
mother, father or someone else who looks after you a lot. For
analysis. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation, 10(7), 1–9.
the following questions, think about that person as your par- Dean, M. L., & Tate, C. C. (2016). Extending the legacy of sandra bem:
ent.”; (b) “Now think about how your friends would be if you Psychological androgyny as a touchstone conceptual advance for
did these activities.”; and (c) “Now think about how you the study of gender in psychological science. Sex Roles, 76(11–
would be if you did these activities.” 12), 643–654. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0713-z.
DeLay, D., Lynn Martin, C., Cook, R. E., & Hanish, L. D. (2018). The
influence of peers during adolescence: Does homophobic name call-
1. If I wore a skirt and makeup my parent [my friends; I] ing by peers change gender identity? Journal of Youth and
would be … Adolescence, 47(3), 636–649. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-
2. If I started a girly activity like ballet my parent [my 0749-6.
Egan, S. K., & Perry, D. G. (2001). Gender identity: A multidimensional
friends; I] would be … analysis with implications for psychosocial adjustment.
3. If I was spending a lot of time with girls, my parent [my Developmental Psychology, 37(4), 451–463. https://doi.org/10.
friends; I] would be … 1037/0012-1649.37.4.451.
314 Sex Roles (2020) 83:303–314

Halim, M. L., Ruble, D. N., & Amodio, D. M. (2011). From pink frilly psychologists' conceptualization of gender? Fronteirs in
dresses to “one of the boys”: A social-cognitive analysis of gender Psychology, 9, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01320.
identity development and gender bias. Social and Personality Patterson, M. (2012). Self-perceived gender typicality, gender-typed at-
Psychology Compass, 5(11), 933–949. https://doi.org/10.1111/j. tributes, and gender stereotype endorsement in elementary- school-
1751-9004.2011.00399.x. aged children. A Journal of Research, 67(7), 422–434. https://doi.
Hill, J. P., & Lynch, M. E. (1983). The intensification of gender-related org/10.1007/s11199-012-0184-9.
role expectations during early adolescence. In J. Brooks-Gunn & A. Pauletti, R. E., Cooper, P. J., & Perry, D. G. (2014). Influences of gender
Petersen (Eds.), Girls at puberty: Biological and psychosocial identity on children's maltreatment of gender-nonconforming peers:
perspectives (pp. 201–228). New York: Plenum. A person x target analysis of aggression. Journal of Personality and
Jewell, J. A. (2015). Adolescents' gender typicality, psychological well- Social Psychology, 106(5), 843–866. https://doi.org/10.1037/
being, and experiences with teasing, bullying, and rejection (doctor- a0036037.
al dissertation, University of Kentucky). Available from ProQuest Pauletti, R. E., Menon, M., Cooper, P. J., Aults, C. D., & Perry, D. G.
dissertations and theses database. (UMI no. 3731455) (2017). Psychological androgyny and children’s mental health: A
Jewell, J. A., & Brown, C. S. (2014). Relations among gender typicality, new look with new measures. Sex Roles, 76(11–12), 705–718.
peer relations, and mental health during early adolescence. Social https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0627-9.
Development, 23(1), 137–156. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12042. Perry, D. G., Pauletti, R. E., & Cooper, P. J. (2019). Gender identity in
Joel, D., Tarrasch, R., Berman, Z., Mukamel, M., & Ziv, E. (2014). childhood: A review of the literature. International Journal of
Queering gender: Studying gender identity in ‘normative’ individ- Behavioral Development. Advance online publication. doi: https://
uals. Psychology & Sexuality, 5(4), 291–321. https://doi.org/10. doi.org/10.1177/0165025418811129
1080/19419899.2013.830640.
Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Berenbaum, S. A. (2006). Gender devel-
Kaiser, H. F. (1974). An index of factorial simplicity. Psychometrika,
opment. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.),
39(1), 31–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02291575 .
Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality
Kornienko, O., Santos, C. E., Martin, C. L., & Granger, K. L. (2016). Peer
development (Vol. 3, pp. 858–932). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
influence on gender identity development in adolescence.
Sons Inc..
Developmental Psychology, 52(10), 1578–1592. https://doi.org/10.
1037/dev0000200. Schmader, T., & Block, K. (2015). Engendering identity: Toward a clear-
Liben, L. (2016). We've come a long way, baby (but we're not there yet): er conceptualization of gender as a social identity. Sex Roles, 73(11–
Gender past, present, and future. Child Development, 87(1), 5–28. 12), 474–480. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-015-0536-3.
Liben, L. S., Bigler, R. S., Ruble, D. N., Martin, C. L., & Powlishta, K. K. Spence, J. T. (1993). Gender-related traits and gender ideology: Evidence
(2002). The developmental course of gender differentiation: for a multifactorial theory. Journal of Personality and Social
Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluating constructs and path- Psychology, 64(4), 624–635. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.
ways [monograph]. Monographs of the Society for Research in 64.4.624.
Child Development, 67(2, serial no. 231), 1-183. Tobin, D. D., Menon, M., Menon, M., Spatta, B. C., Hodges, E. V., &
Marasco, V. M. (2018). Addressing hegemonic masculinity with adoles- Perry, D. G. (2010). The intrapsychics of gender: A model of self-
cent boys within the counseling relationship. Journal of Child and socialization. Psychological Review, 117(2), 601–622. https://doi.
Adolescent Counseling, 4(3), 226–238. https://doi.org/10.1080/ org/10.1037/a0018936.
23727810.2017.1422647. Yunger, J. L., Carver, P. R., & Perry, D. G. (2004). Does gender identity
Martin, C. L., Andrews, N. C., England, D. E., Zosuls, K., & Ruble, D. N. influence children's psychological well-being? Developmental
(2017). A dual identity approach for conceptualizing and measuring Psychology, 40(4), 572–582. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.
children's gender identity. Child Development, 88(1), 167–182. 40.4.572.
https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12568.
Messerschmidt, J. W. (2012). Engendering gendered knowledge. Men Publisher’s Note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdic-
and Masculinities, 15(1), 56–76. tional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Morgenroth, T., & Ryan, M. K. (2018). Gender trouble in social psychol-
ogy: How can butler's work inform experimental social

Potrebbero piacerti anche