Sei sulla pagina 1di 17

Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

www.elsevier.com/locate/neunet

2007 Special Issue

The apical dendrite theory of consciousness


David LaBerge ∗ , Ray Kasevich
Bard College at Simon’s Rock and Stanley Laboratory of Electrical Physics, Great Barrington, MA, 01230, United States

Abstract

The neural basis of consciousness is theorized here to be the elevated activity of the apical dendrite within a thalamocortical circuit. Both
the anatomical and functional properties of these two brain structures are examined within the general context of the cortical minicolumn, which
is regarded as the functional unit of the cerebral cortex. Two main circuits of the minicolumn are described: the axis circuit, which sustains
activity for extended durations and produces our sensory impressions, and the shell circuit, which performs input–output processing and produces
identifications, categorizations, and ideas. The apical dendrite operates within the axis circuit to stabilize neural activity, which enables conscious
impressions to be steady and to be sustained over long periods of time. In an attempt to understand how the conscious aspect of subjective
impressions may be related to apical dendrite activity, we examine the characteristics of the electric and magnetic fields during the movement of
charges along the apical dendrite. The physical correlate of consciousness is regarded here as the relatively intense electromagnetic field that is
located along the inside and the outside close to the surface of the active apical dendrite.
c 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Consciousness; Apical dendrite; Minicolumn; Electric and magnetic fields

1. Introduction which is based on the corollary discharge of attention


movement (the CODAM model). According to the Theory
The special kind of activity in the brain that is presumed of Neuronal Group Selection (TNGS) of Edelman (2003),
to underlie consciousness continues to elude the grasp of consciousness is produced by reentrant interactions involving
our scientific concepts. However, current theories of the the thalamocortical system, whose circuitry is dominated by
neural correlates of consciousness seem to agree on one recurrent or looping circuits. Newman, Baars, and Cho (1997)
particular aspect of consciousness: its extendable duration. regard the activation loops of the thalamocortical circuit within
Consciousness is an activity that is extended in time and a global workspace model as crucial for generating conscious
typically its duration continues from the time we awake to states.
the time we fall asleep. Grossberg (1999, 2005) describes Unfortunately, mental activities that have extendable
consciousness in terms of “resonance states” that occur in short- durations, even of a few seconds, are not the kinds of
term memory. When top-down expectations select consistent events that the traditional scientific framework of input–output
bottom-up signals and their reciprocal feedback activity processing is structured to investigate and explain. Processing
settles into a steady state, their circuits resonate together; of information is generally regarded as directed toward an
and while this resonance continues, consciousness exists, output, and not toward sustaining itself over prolonged periods
according to his Adaptive Resonance Theory (ART). Taylor of time without apparent outputs. Therefore, if the input-
(1999, 2005, 2007) describes consciousness as “temporally processing–output framework is the current reigning paradigm,
extended neural activity” in a buffer store of working memory, might the restrictive use of it hinder the quest for the neural
correlates of consciousness? In the present paper this paradigm
∗ Corresponding address: P.O. Box 301, So. Egremont, MA, 01258, United is modified to emphasize the input-processing components as
States. Tel.: +1 413 528 1303; fax: +1 413 644 9836. we seek to understand how consciousness may be generated at
E-mail address: dlaberge@earthlink.net (D. LaBerge). the level of the neuron within neural circuits.

c 2007 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.


0893-6080/$ - see front matter
doi:10.1016/j.neunet.2007.09.006
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1005

2. The circuitry in minicolumns of the cortex

2.1. The pyramidal neuron in the minicolumn circuit structure

The theory of consciousness described in the present paper


centers on the activity of a particular kind of cortical neuron: the
pyramidal neuron. The defining feature of the pyramidal neuron
(the pyramid) that separates it from other neurons of the brain
is its very long apical dendrite, which generates the special
kind of electrical activity that is presumed here to underlie the
subjective impressions we call consciousness.
To set the stage for the description of the pyramidal neuron
and its apical dendrite in the circuit activity of the minicolumn,
we show how the pyramidal neuron is distributed within the
cerebral cortex. In Fig. 1, the top two diagrams show how
the cortical fabric, which is extensively convoluted to fit into
the sphere-like shape of the skull, can be transformed into a
flat surface (Van Essen, Drury, Joshi, & Miller, 1998), whose
thickness is defined by the length of columns of neurons.
Fig. 1 indicates that each column of neurons consists of
a cluster of many minicolumns, and the structure of the
minicolumn appears to be organized around the central core
of layer 5 pyramids, which are the pyramids that contain the
longest apical dendrites (Peters & Sethares, 1991). Layer 6
pyramids are not organized within the minicolumn but instead Fig. 1. Locations of the pyramidal neurons in the cerebral cortex. The
are organized within the column, which is a cluster of lateral diagram of the cortex within the skull (upper left) shows a highly
approximately 100 minicolumns (Mountcastle, 1998). The convoluted sheet, which is converted into a flat surface (upper right), whose
area is approximately three times that of the lateral view. This cortical fabric
minicolumn is regarded as the functional unit of the cortex has an average thickness of approximately 3 mm and is constructed of
(Mountcastle, 1957), which is based on the observation that neurons organized in columns, which are clusters of minicolumns. Within
neurons in a given minicolumn share many receptive field each minicolumn are shown the major pyramidal neurons with their vertically
properties. The centrality of the long apical dendrites of layer aligned apical dendrites. The star-shaped cells in the middle of the minicolumns
5 pyramidal neurons in the structure of the minicolumn is are stellate neurons. For clarity, inhibitory neurons, which make up 15%–20%
of the total number of minicolumn neurons, are omitted. Axons, which exit at
apparent in the two diagrams in the lower part of Fig. 1. the bottom of the somas, are also omitted.
Later sections of the present paper will address the possible
centrality of the layer 5 pyramidal neurons in the function of The average length of the layer 5 apical dendrite varies
the minicolumn. with cortical area, with parietal and motor areas showing
The pyramidal neuron is shown diagrammatically in 20%–50% longer lengths than occipital V1 area, based on
Fig. 1 as a triangular shape, representing the soma, with cortical thickness data of Rockel, Hiorns, and Powell (1980).
a relatively long line attached at the top, representing the Also, the average length of the layer 5 apical dendrite varies
apical dendrite. Thus, the axon and the many basal dendrites across the mammalian species of mouse, rat, cat, monkey
of the pyramids are omitted in these diagrams. The other and humans, with the mouse apical dendrite of area V1
neurons shown in the figure are stellate neurons, whose measuring approximately 1/3 the length of the human apical
many radiating lines represent the relatively short dendrites dendrite (LaBerge, 2005, Fig. 1). It seems difficult to avoid
attached to the soma. Visual inspection of Fig. 1 gives the the tantalizing question of why the apical dendrite varies
impression that the considerable majority of neurons in the considerably in length across mammalian species and across
cerebral cortex are pyramidal neurons, which is confirmed cortical areas within a species. Discovering the function that
by Feldman’s (1984) measurements, which estimate their apical dendrite activity serves in cortical processing could help
percentage in the cortex as approximately 70%–80%. The in answering that question.
remaining percentages of stellate and inhibitory neurons
(in area V1) are approximately 5%–8% and 16%–20%, 2.2. The axis and shell circuits within the minicolumn
respectively (Mountcastle, 1998). Under the area of a dime
placed on the human skull over the parietal cortex there are To explore apical dendrite activity in cortical circuitry, we
approximately 18,000,000–20,000,000 pyramidal neurons, of simplify the complex arrangements and interconnections of
which approximately 22% or 4,000,000–4,400,000 are layer neurons within a minicolumn by dividing the minicolumn
5 pyramidal neurons (based a cell count of approximately 70 into two compartments, the axis and the shell. Fig. 2 shows
neurons beneath surface patches of 25 × 30 microns for the diagrammatically how the layer 5 pyramids, with their very
monkey). long apical dendrites, form the axis part of the minicolumn
1006 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

Fig. 3. The two main kinds of minicolumn circuits defining the cortical
minicolumn. The recurrent circuit (left) contains the layer 5 pyramidal neuron
Fig. 2. Most of the neurons of a minicolumn are organized around the long and connects with thalamic neurons; and the input–output circuit that contains
apical dendrites of the layer 5 pyramidal neurons (the axis), with apical layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons (right) restricts its connections to the cortex. The
dendrites of layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons (the shell) arranged in a circular inhibitory neurons of the cortex and of the thalamic reticular nucleus (and dorsal
pattern around the top sector of the apical dendrites of the layer 5 pyramidal thalamus) are omitted for clarity.
neurons.
consciousness. It is assumed in this paper that sustained
while the layer 2 and layer 3 pyramids form the shell part activity in a primary sensory area provides the ongoing
of the minicolumn. Fig. 3 shows the general structure of the activity of background consciousness for that particular sense.
circuit within each of these two parts of the minicolumn. Elevated consciousness for selected aspects of background
The axis circuit of the cortical minicolumn extends to the consciousness is assumed to arise when sustained activity
subcortical thalamus in a reciprocally connected manner, such of a primary sensory area is sent to higher sensory areas,
that a recurrent or looping circuit exists between the layer where a selected part of the sensory scene is amplified
5 pyramid and a thalamic neuron. The axis circuit can be by attentional activity controlled from the frontal lobes.
characterized as an “input-stayput” circuit, because its apparent The elevated attentional activity of a part of the sensory
function is not the processing of inputs into outputs, but instead scene in higher sensory areas is regarded here as foreground
the holding of neural activity over time. The shell circuit is
consciousness.
shown as a one-directional input–output circuit that connects
Fig. 4 shows a diagram of the major connections between
one minicolumn with another, and the processing component
excitatory neurons in the primary visual area (V1), which
consists of connections between layer 2/3 pyramids within the
applies generally to primary auditory (A1) and primary
minicolumn. One could also characterize the axis circuit as
somatosensory (S1) areas as well. The circuit diagram is
a “vertical” circuit, because it extends from the cortex to the
adapted in part from diagrams in two publications by Jones
subcortical thalamus, and the shell circuit as a “horizontal”
(2002, 2007), which summarize what is known to date about the
circuit, because it extends within the cortex, linking one cortical
reciprocal connections between a sensory area and the thalamic
minicolumn to another.
neurons that serve that area.
2.3. The minicolumn circuitry of primary sensory cortical Layer 5 and 6 pyramids participate in the two kinds of
areas corticothalamic loops. Apparently layer 2/3 pyramids do not
participate in major circuit loops; nevertheless they do have
Traditionally, the study of connections of neurons in the six apical dendrites of appreciable length. It has been suggested
layers of the neocortex has been aimed at the discovery of their (LaBerge, 2001, 2005) that these apical dendrites sustain
receptive field properties (e.g., Bolz, Gilbert, & Wiesel, 1989). activity supplied by tonic input from the layer 5 circuit loop,
Here we study the interlaminar connections within the cortical and that this sustained activity, operating at subthreshold firing
minicolumn to discover how they could support sustained levels, modulates the input–output processing of basal dendrites
activity in recurrent circuits that are believed to underlie inputs within the soma of layer 2/3 neurons.
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1007

Fig. 4. Diagram of the major circuits in a minicolumn of a primary sensory area Fig. 5. Diagram of the major circuits in a minicolumn of a higher sensory
(e.g., area V1), involving two kinds of thalamic relay neurons, Tmatrix and area (e.g., area V4). Some of the inputs to the thalamic matrix neuron from
Tcore. Inhibitory neurons are omitted for clarity. Adapted in part from Jones the frontal area connect with thalamocortical circuits in the parietal area before
(2002, 2007). terminating on the thalamic neuron serving this higher sensory area. Adapted
in part from Jones (2002, 2007).
When the inputs to the layer 5 and layer 6 loops of Fig. 4
are compared, it is clear that the layer 6 loop is capable of
supplying large amounts of activity to the apical dendrite of in the higher sensory minicolumn, along with the pattern of
the layer 5 loop (via thalamic Tcore axons terminating on connecting circuitry of neurons.
the many layer 4 stellate neurons), while the layer 5 loop Perhaps the most salient difference between the circuitry of
delivers no activity to the apical dendrite of the layer 6 loop the higher sensory minicolumn and the circuitry of the primary
(and apparently only minor activity to the basal dendrites sensory minicolumn is the change in number of stellate neurons
of the layer 6 pyramid). Thus, while the axons from the in layer 4. The reduction of stellate neurons in the higher
two kinds of thalamic relay neurons drive activity in their sensory minicolumn is reflected by the shrinkage in thickness
respective thalamocortical circuits, the total activity in the layer of layer 4, which is produced also by a corresponding increase
5 circuit includes additional, and relatively strong, inputs from in the thicknesses of layers 3, 5, and 6, which contain pyramidal
the activity of the layer 6 pyramidal circuit. This preferential neurons that make up the vast majority of neurons of the cortex.
convergence of activity on the apical dendrite of the layer 5 Another noticeable difference between the circuitry of primary
pyramid is consistent with the anatomical centrality of the layer and higher sensory minicolumns is the increase in synaptic
5 apical dendrite in the structure of the minicolumn. connections of the thalamocortical axon (from the matrix relay
neuron) onto the distal region of the layer 5 and layer 2/3 apical
2.4. The minicolumn circuitry of higher sensory cortical areas dendrites. Taken together, going from the primary to the higher
sensory minicolumn, these two changes in circuitry suggest that
The neurons in the primary sensory minicolumn send the influence of the thalamic core (T core) neuron on activity of
activity to minicolumns of higher sensory areas along two the layer 5 apical dendrite is reduced, while the influence of the
pathways: a direct axon pathway from the layer 2/3 pyramid thalamic matrix (T matrix) neuron is increased.
of the shell circuit, and an indirect axon pathway from the This shift in the influence of the two kinds of thalamic
layer 5 pyramid of the axis circuit that synapses with a relay neurons on activity of the layer 5 apical dendrite is parallel
thalamic neuron before entering the higher sensory cortical to the shift in influence of bottom-up and top-down sources
minicolumn. Fig. 5 shows the terminations of these pathways of activity, globally considered. The many stellate neurons
1008 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

in layer 4 of the primary sensory minicolumn appear to auditory, or somatosensory), attentional enhancement of the
serve the strong registration in cortex of activity arising from shell circuit activity may be required to produce accurate
sensory receptors (retinal, auditory, somatosensory), but once identifications. It has been suggested (LaBerge, 2001, 2005)
this registration takes place in the primary sensory area of the that tonic enhancement of soma activity in layer 2/3 pyramids
cortex, the need for the special function of the stellate neuron can be produced by inputs from the layer 5 axis circuit, as
is apparently reduced. To summarize this section, there are two shown in Fig. 5. With activity already at an elevated level in
major kinds of inputs to the higher sensory minicolumn: one the soma of the layer 2/3 pyramid, the input to a basal dendrite
from the stimulus registration activity of the primary sensory from a layer 2/3 pyramid in the primary sensory minicolumn
minicolumn, and the other from frontal areas of the cortex. has a higher likelihood of being successfully processed into
an output. This issue will be addressed in more detail in
2.5. Attentional activity in higher sensory minicolumns Section 4.4 of this paper.

How does the circuitry of the higher sensory minicolumn 3. Special functions of apical dendrite activity
operate on the information arising from the primary sensory
area and from the frontal areas? One answer is that the higher 3.1. The issue of stability in corticothalamic circuit activity
sensory minicolumn serves as a site for attention, defined as
the elevation of circuit activity corresponding to a selected Under the assumption that activity in corticothalamic
part (typically a small part) of the total registration of the circuits underlies the extended durations of consciousness,
sensory scene in V1, A1, or S1. The operations of attention it follows that the circuit activity must maintain stability
are presumed to involve both an enhancement of activity in over these durations of time. The cortical environment, as
those minicolumns that correspond to, or code, the selected well as the circuit itself, contains sources of excitatory and
locations and/or appearances of the scene, and a concurrent inhibitory noise. Wide fluctuations in voltage levels at the
suppression of minicolumns that code neighboring parts of the soma of the pyramidal neuron can interrupt ongoing circuit
scene (locations and appearances of distracters). For example, activity when intense voltage peaks distort circuit operations,
under attentional conditions, an orientation-sensitive V4 neuron and when momentary drops in voltage shut down the circuit
shows amplification in responding, while the width and mean operations. To maintain steady levels of soma activity some
of its tuning curve remains unchanged (McAdams & Maunsell, means must be found to oppose the naturally noisy nature of
1999). The control of selective amplification/suppression both the cortical background activity and the circuit operations
during attentional activity is presumed to be produced by axons themselves (Tegner, Compte, & Wang, 2002).
of the frontal cortex that make contact with the thalamic relay Evidently, the brain has solved the stability problem
neurons that serve those minicolumns (see Fig. 5). Hence, for apical dendrite activity, because synchronous oscillations
the thalamic neurons receive activation from two sources: over extended durations have been recorded from implanted
frontal areas of attentional control and the primary sensory electrodes and electrodes on the scalp while animals sustain
minicolumns. The intensity of activity from the frontal area can attention (e.g., Bouyer, Montaron, & Rougeul, 1981). Here we
be varied, but the intensity of activity from the primary sensory describe one way that oscillatory activity in the apical dendrite
minicolumn would seem to remain relatively constant, under can become uniform so that it can promote stability in the
typical conditions. When attentional manipulations produce corticothalamic circuit of which it is a part.
changes in V1 (McAdams & Reid, 2005; Silver, Ress, & Because stability in a recurrent circuit is related to the extent
Heeger, 2007) and/or lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) activity or width of momentary fluctuation of its activity level, one
(O’Connor, Fukui, Pinsk, & Kastner, 2002), the change would indicator of stability is the variability of successive electrical
seem to be minor relative to the change in activity that events at a given location within the circuit. Therefore, to
attentional manipulations can produce in higher sensory areas, increase the stability of circuit activity the variability of activity
given the circuit properties of these areas shown in Figs. 4 level must decrease. The part of the corticothalamic circuit that
and 5. Thus, attention is regarded as the intensification of appears to be most variable is the pyramidal neuron, particularly
a particular part of the sensory scene registered in primary at synaptic sites along the apical dendrite where the almost
sensory areas by means of the amplification of axis circuit simultaneous discharge of many incoming axons induces
activities in minicolumns of higher sensory areas that code that strong electric currents that result in large EPSPs (excitatory
particular part of the sensory scene. postsynaptic potentials). By the time a EPSP propagates to the
The higher sensory minicolumn is presumed to perform a soma, the variability of the EPSP must be substantially reduced,
function in addition to the expression of attentional activity so that a sequence of EPSPs produce a steady level of soma
in an axis circuit. The shell circuit of these minicolumns activity that can generate a synchronous series of axon pulses.
receives inputs from shell circuits of the primary sensory In Fig. 6 are shown four curves that describe the decay of
minicolumn, which are processed into an output that we four levels of initial EPSP voltages as a function of distance
assume constitutes the identification of stimuli coded by that traveled along the apical dendrite from the region of its
minicolumn. Identification of input information is presumed to initiation to the soma. At any given location on the apical
be a brief event, relative to the typically prolonged sustaining dendrite the variability of voltage is indicated by the spread of
of attention to that event. But in a cluttered scene (visual, a Gaussian-shaped distribution. Almost all of the area under
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1009

& Tank, 1996), and the activity at the thousands of spine


synapses on the dendrite maintains membrane depolarization
at subthreshold firing levels. It is estimated that the thousands
of synapses on dendrites are responsible for approximately 80%
of the mainly inhibitory conductance of the dendritic membrane
(Azouz & Gray, 1999; Contreras, Timofeev & Steriade, 1996;
Nowak, Sanchez-Vives, & McCormick, 1997). The chaotic
nature of this background input to the apical dendrite produces
fluctuations that sometimes generate action potentials that may
arise from transient calcium currents (Helmchen, Svoboda,
Denk, & Tank, 1999). Action potentials that originate in
dendrites do not always propagate to the soma (Jarsky, Roxin,
Kath, & Spruston, 2005; Larkum & Zhu, 2002), and may
participate in local changes of synaptic long-term potentiation
(Colbert, 2001; Goldberg, Holthoff, & Yuste, 2002). Recent
Fig. 6. A family of theoretical exponential functions that describe the decay of findings from intracellular recordings of action potentials in
voltage with distance along the apical dendrite from the initiation of the EPSP. vivo during Up states and during visual stimulation indicate that
Four levels of voltage, V , are shown at the initiating synapse, and the distance action potentials are typically initiated in the axon (Shu, Duque,
to the soma is represented by d. The decay constant for this example is k, which Yu, Haider, & McCormick, 2007). The part of the axon that
represents the average of the many active and passive membrane conductances.
appears to be the preferred site of action potential initiation is
All voltage values are increments in voltage from baseline level. The left-to-
right sequence of Gaussian-like distributions describes the reduction of voltage the initial segment of the axon that typically lies 40–55 microns
variability as the EPSP propagates toward the soma. from the soma.
Of particular relevance to the stabilizing of thalamocortical
a distribution of voltages is assumed to lie below the firing circuits is the effect of the active membrane conductances
threshold of an action potential, for reasons indicated in the maintained by tonic activity in the thousands of synaptic spines
next section of this paper. The voltage level at the soma and along the apical dendrite. This spine activity produces the
its variability are shown to decrease with the distance the attenuation of the voltage of the EPSP as it propagates along
EPSP travels to reach the soma. The exponential equation that the apical dendrite shafts illustrated diagrammatically in Figs. 4
describes the rate of EPSP decay with distance was developed and 5. The decaying curves shown in Fig. 6 indicate the
by Rall (1989), and represents the passive decay of voltage attenuating effect of spinal activity between the site of EPSP
in a cable model of the dendrite. The average membrane initiation and the soma.
(outward) conductance is represented by the constant, k, and Therefore, it could be said that the propagating EPSP of
more recent versions of the cable model assume that the the apical dendrite is treated in a “give-and-take” manner.
membrane conductances of dendrites in the awake animal are The EPSP is “given” to the apical dendrite by axon activity
dominated by active inhibitory (outward) currents, with an of groups of layer 4 stellate neurons and by activity of
inhibitory chem/excitatory ratio that may be as high as 5-to-1 thalamic axons. Immediately there ensues a “taking” of the
(Destexhe, Rudolph, & Pare, 2003). EPSP, which is the attenuation of the EPSP produced by the
outward conductances of the membrane produced by low-
3.2. The give-and-take operations of apical dendrite activity level synaptic activity in the thousands of spines that dot the
dendritic shaft. As the EPSP approaches the soma, it decreases
The dendrites of a typical pyramidal neuron contain as many in strength, but, importantly, the variability of the strength
as 10,000 synapses (DeFelipe & Farina, 1992), and a typical also decreases. The average rate of decrease is described by
apical dendrite (of the rat) contains at least 3000 (excitatory) the exponential decay equation based on average membrane
synaptic spines on its major vertical shaft, with a similar conductance. The theoretical convergence of decreasing mean
quantity of spines on oblique dendrites that are branches of voltage with distance can also be described by equations other
the dendrite shaft (Larkman, 1991). Since the length of the than the exponential equation (see, e.g., Koch (1998)).
human apical dendrite is approximately 2 times that of the rat, The variance of the propagating EPSP at the soma is
the estimated quantity of synaptic spines on the major shaft assumed to be relatively high for the first cycle of electrical
of the human apical dendrite could be as high as 6000. The activity through the recurrent thalamocortical circuit. But in
locations of these ubiquitous dendritic spines are indicated in subsequent cycles, the EPSP at the soma moves more closely
an abbreviated manner along the apical dendrites shown in to the mean voltage it will ultimately achieve, so that the
Figs. 4 and 5. Aside from the synapses made by thalamic axons, variability of the EPSP at the asymptote can be regarded as the
the vast majority of synapses are activated from other cortical characteristic level of stability for this recurrent circuit, given
neurons (Braitenberg & Schuz, 1998), which provide most of the particular conditions that are present at the time.
the background noise for the apical dendrite. Synaptic activity Conditions under which the thalamocortical circuit operates
along the apical dendrite produces a rich variety of intrinsic can vary considerably. One important condition is the intensity
electric and chemical effects (Llinas, 1988; Reyes, 2001; Yuste level of the afferent input to the thalamic relay neuron, which
1010 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

can be varied by the brightness contrast of an oriented line in may not change quickly. Hence, adjustments to the “take”
vision. Variability of input to the thalamic relay neuron can also operations on the propagating EPSP in the dendritic spines may
be produced by the focusing of attention to the oriented line in lag well behind changes in the “give” operation of the initiating
the axons arising from frontal areas of attentional control. In EPSP. Therefore, it would seem that this diffuse circuit model
both of these cases the number of active thalamic relay neurons exhibits much less direct coupling between the “give” and the
can vary the level of EPSPs in a layer 5 apical dendrite (directly, “take” operations than would be shown by a specific circuit
or via stellate neurons) by varying the number of active axons model.
that contact that apical dendrite, while maintaining a constant The adjustment of the “give-and-take” treatment of variable
frequency of pulses in the input axons. This inference is based EPSPs of the apical dendrite may take place by means
on the finding that, in the cortex, a branching axon seldom other than those described in the foregoing paragraphs. When
makes more than 10 contacts with any single neuron, and attention to an object is intensified over longer time periods,
frequently with only one or two neurons (Mountcastle, 1998). activity in a thalamocortical circuit is repeated with elevated
When the magnitude of the EPSP that is “given” to the apical intensities of EPSPs occurring in the apical dendrite. As a
dendrite varies, its variability will also change, and this change result, new spines may appear, which induce the apical dendrite
will be maintained as it propagates to the soma (see Fig. 6). shaft to elongate to accommodate the additional spines. A
When the variability of the EPSP at the soma changes, so will longer length of the spine-covered apical dendrite enables the
the stability of the entire thalamocortical circuit. Therefore, to EPSP to undergo additional reduction in variability, which
maintain a high level of stability in the thalamocortical circuit, compensates for the additional variability in the initial EPSP
some means must be found to make appropriate adjustments in that accompanies higher levels of intensity.
the trimming operation of the variable EPSP before it reaches In view of these considerations, it might seem puzzling that
the soma. in area V1 the major input synapses of the stellate neurons
on the apical dendrite of layer 5 pyramids are located at the
3.3. Balancing the “give-and-take” operations in the apical middle part of the layer 5 pyramidal apical dendrite, and not at
dendrite the distal part, whence an initial EPSP could undergo a larger
amount of trimming before it reached the soma. One hypothesis
One possible way to compensate for a large initial EPSP suggests that the activity in the lateral geniculate neurons
“given” to the apical dendrite is to increase the synaptic already possesses a moderate level of stability, because the
strengths of the thousands of spine synapses that “trim” the visual scene that stimulates the retina is typically quite stable.
EPSP as it propagates to the soma. Adjustments to the axon In comparison, major synapses on apical dendrites of layer 5
inputs to these spines could be produced in two general ways. pyramids in higher sensory minicolumns appear to be located
The first way involves specific circuits that respond directly at more distal parts, while midsection stellate neurons supply
to the intensity of the initial EPSP. For example, the layer 6 a minor portion of the synaptic input. The distal synapses
pyramid, whose momentary activity presumably keeps pace on apical dendrites in the higher sensory areas are driven by
with the activity level of the layer 5 pyramid, sends collaterals thalamic pulvinar neurons, which do not receive the major
of its axons into layer 4, where they branch profusely. If enough part of their inputs from the retina. Therefore these EPSPs at
of these axon arborizations contact the spines of the layer 5 the distal apical dendrites exhibit a higher level of variability
apical dendrite, a close link would be formed between the and require more trimming before they reach the soma. Fig. 5
momentary amount of the “give” operation to the consequent shows that the thalamic pulvinar neurons are activated from
amount of the “take” operation. As it turns out, however, the two major sources: the frontal cortical areas, and area V1.
supporting evidence for a high number of layer 6 axon synapses The variability from area V1 is presumed to be low, having
on layer 5 apical dendrites (McGuire, Hornung, Gilbert, & started at a moderate level (from the relatively stable retinal
Wiesel, 1984) is not strong. source) and having received further refinement by the trimming
A second way that the “take” operation may be controlled operation of the V1 layer 5 apical dendrite. But the source of
by the “give” operation is by means of a large and diffuse set variability from frontal cortical areas is presumed to be quite
of circuits that operate in a more indirect way than the kind of large, owing to presumed high fluctuations in frontal cortical
specific circuit just described. When a large EPSP is delivered circuits. Also, the weight of the frontal input component to the
to the apical dendrite by the thalamic relay neurons contacting pulvinar neuron is presumed to be influenced by the intensity
pyramidal and stellate neurons of the cortex, it is likely that level of attentional control. Therefore, the higher the intensity
many local circuits of the local minicolumn increase the level of attentional control, the higher the variability of the activity
of their activity. If these local circuits supply most of the axon delivered to the pulvinar neuron. Hence the EPSP induced at
input to the thousands of spines on an apical dendrite, then an the distal part of the apical dendrite in the higher sensory area
increase in the activity level in these many circuits will result is increased, and a commensurate increase in length of apical
in an upward adjustment in the spine activity, which would dendrite is needed to reduce this increased variability before
increase the local outward membrane conductance, thereby the EPSPs reach the soma.
increasing the trimming of the “take” operation. However, Therefore, these theoretical considerations predict that high
because of the diffuse nature of the connectivity of the many levels of attention directed to a particular object should, over
local minicolumn circuits, it is likely that their activity levels weeks and years, elongate the apical dendrites of layer 5
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1011

pyramidal neurons in the minicolumns (of higher sensory areas) 3.5. Lengths of apical dendrites and the ERP
that code that object. One potential indicator of apical dendrite
length is cortical thickness, which has been measured with The ERP (event-related potential) measured at the scalp is
variations of the fMRI technique. We turn now to an analysis of a transient EEG that is produced by a relatively abrupt onset
the effectiveness of this measure for estimating apical dendrite of stimulus whose amplitude is increased when its location
length. and attribute have been cued in advance of its appearance.
The cue is presumed to induce preparatory attention to the
3.4. Lengths of apical dendrites and cortical thickness location and attributes of the target stimulus, which presumably
produces elevated activity in the columns that code the location
Many apical dendrites of layer 5 and layers 2/3 extend and attributes of the stimulus, relative to the activity levels
to the top of layer 1 and spread laterally in tufts for a short in neighboring columns that code for similar locations and
distance, apparently leaving little space in which to expand attributes. In the typical ERP task, both target and distracter
upward. Therefore, an elongation of the shaft of the apical stimulus appear in a random sequence, with the target appearing
dendrite should shift the soma in the other direction toward the at a low frequency. The task for the observer is to count the
bottom of layer 6. However, in the case of stretching apical number of targets over a marked time period. The finding
dendrites of layer 5 pyramids, it would seem that there is of interest is that the ERP amplitude is higher at the cued
little space within layer 6 to accommodate the relative massive location of the stimulus attributes than at an uncued location
somas of layer 5 pyramids, owing to the already high packing (e.g., DiRusso, Martinez, and Hillyard (2003)).
density of neurons in a minicolumn (Hendry, Schwark, Jones, According to the present theory, when the cue induces
& Yan, 1987; Rockel, Hiorns, & Powell, 1974). In view of these preparatory attention to a particular stimulus content, the
limiting factors, it would seem that an elongation of the apical corresponding minicolumn axis circuits elevate their activity
dendrite shaft would result in an expansion of the individual levels, and they maintain or increase these levels throughout the
cortical layers through which the shaft passes. As a result the time period in which the target stimulus is expected to occur.
total thickness of the cortex would increase in the local cortical When a stimulus of the cued attribute and location appears,
areas containing these elongated apical dendrites. the apical dendrites in the minicolumn axis circuits show a
A fMRI study of meditation practice by Lazar et al. (2005) transient increase in amplitude of electric fields. For vision, the
found selected brain regions in which cortical thickness was pathways that are likely to deliver this increase are the relatively
larger in participants who had extensive meditation experience fast conducting Y -cell driven pathways in the cortex and in
than in controls, and Makris et al. (2006) found smaller the superior colliculus. Both pathways synapse with pulvinar
cortical thickness in cortical areas underlying attention in thalamic neurons before terminating within a minicolumn of
participants diagnosed with ADHD compared with controls. It a higher sensory area (see Fig. 5). The Y -cells in the retina
is tempting to infer that the observed change in thickness was respond only transiently to the onset of a visual stimulus, so
strongly influenced by the changes in apical dendrite lengths that the activity in their pathways presumably has a transient
owing to meditation practice or attentional pathology. However, effect on the apical dendrites where their pathways eventually
even though increases in apical dendrite length could produce terminate.
increases in cortical thickness, would these measured increases In a study by Pantev et al. (1991) the repeated displays of the
in cortical thickness necessarily imply an increase in apical target stimulus produced a transient increase in amplitude of the
dendrite lengths? Elevated activity in minicolumns over time is scalp-recorded EEG 20–130 ms after stimulus onset (the MEG
presumed to generate new synapses, and an increase in number recording was virtually identical in form to the EEG recording).
of synapses (and spines) along with their increased lengths The short interval of amplitude increase was shown to consist
of local axon branches could also increase cortical thickness of 4 or more cycles of 40 Hz oscillations, which suggests that
owing simply to the overall increase in volume of neuronal the sustained preparatory attention was being produced by axis
tissue. However, if an appreciable proportion of new synapses oscillations at 40 Hz, and that when a stimulus appeared, the
appear on an apical dendrite, then it would seem that the oscillations were amplified for a fraction of a second.
apical dendrite would have to elongate to accommodate these Amplifications of the ERP are produced not only by
additional synapses. Also, data from the frontal cortex of rats momentary attention but apparently also by long-term effects
that undergo confinement each day for 21 days show that the of attention during training. Simple and complex tones produce
loss of spines on apical dendrites is accompanied by a 20% higher amplitudes of early ERPs in musicians who have had
reduction in the length of the apical dendrite (Radley et al., many years of musical training compared with non-musicians,
2006). Nevertheless, the more cautious interpretation is that even under passive attention conditions (Kuriki, Kanda, &
observed changes in cortical thickness could conceivably be Hirata, 2006; Shahin, Bosnyak, Trainor, & Roberts, 2003;
produced by an increase in synapses without an increase in Shahin, Roberts, Pantev, Aziz, & Picton, 2007). Also, trained
apical dendrite length. On this view, cortical thickness measures meditators show higher amplitude ERPs (and frequencies)
by themselves are not an unambiguous indicator of underlying compared to controls (Lutz, Greichar, Rawlings, Ricard, &
apical dendrite lengths. What is clearly needed is a technique Davidson, 2004).
that enables apical dendrite lengths to be measured directly in The source of the electric fields that produce an ERP is
live animals and humans. assumed to be the electric dipole, which is defined by the
1012 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

location of the two opposite electric charges along the active


portion of the apical dendrite. The intensity of the dipole,
termed the dipole moment, is influenced by two variables:
the charge at the two poles and the distance between them.
Therefore, an increase in ERP amplitude can be produced
by stronger charges of EPSPs or by a greater length of the
apical dendrite shaft traveled by the EPSP, or by both of
these variables. The present theory suggests that the larger
EPSPs produced by strong sustained attention over years of
training induces an elongation of apical dendrite shafts, so that
distance as well as charge of the dipole would be affected
in expert musicians. However, in the short duration of a task
condition that instructs or otherwise induces the observer to
increase attention there is not enough time to elongate the apical
dendrites, so that the increase in dipole strength is apparently
solely due to increase in the charge produced by the larger
EPSPs of heightened attention.
The foregoing analysis suggests a way that training may
increase ERP amplitude by changes in apical dendrite length Fig. 7. Global ascending pathways that connect posterior cortical columns
over time. Does it follow that an increase in ERP amplitude over with anterior cortical columns in two ways. One way (the lower-tier pathway)
time indicates that the apical dendrite length has increased? connects columns by their axis circuits, and the other way (the upper-tier
Apparently not, because an observed increase in ERP amplitude pathway) connects columns by their shell circuits.
at the scalp could be produced simply by more minicolumns
being recruited, without any change in apical dendrite length the cognitive event that may be called “having an impression”
and without any change in EPSP amplitude induced on an of something. The earliest of these subjective impressions takes
individual apical dendrite. place in the primary sensory areas of the cortex where the
entire array of sensory input is first registered in the cortex
4. Implications of minicolumn circuitry for cognition (see Fig. 4). We regard a scene that contains many impressions
as background consciousness, and the array of impressions is
The two different kinds of minicolumn circuits make typically sustained for very long periods of time while attention
possible two different kinds of cognitive activity, according to selects impressions of specific objects in the scene. Examples
the theory of cortical function described in the present paper. are visual scenes seen through a window, the ambient sounds of
The shell circuits, which connect layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons traffic heard outside a city room, and the continuous sensations
across minicolumns, operate as input–output mechanisms that of the somatosensory landscape of the body.
typically complete their processing in a short fraction of a Foreground consciousness is produced by axis circuits that
second. Shell circuits in early sensory areas that receive V1 produce impressions in higher sensory areas (see Fig. 5);
inputs identify object appearances and locations, and later these axis circuits are presumed to be strongly influenced
sensory areas process these identifications into categories. In by top-down frontal activations which elevate activity in
the frontal areas categories are organized into propositions that minicolumns that code a selected part of the total registered
may be based mainly on activity in the shell circuitry spread input. This attentional activity can take on variable degrees
across many minicolumns. Fig. 7 shows a diagram of the global of spatial focus and intensity, depending on the manner
shell pathway, along which early identifications of stimulus of control activity received from the frontal areas. Thus
objects and locations are processed into the categories and the simultaneous impressions from both foreground and
propositions upon which the operations of intellectual thinking background consciousness together constitute the momentary
operate. content of consciousness.
The second general kind of minicolumn circuit is the axis Fig. 8 shows diagrams of top-down global pathways for
circuit, which connects layer 5 pyramidal neurons between both the shell and axis circuits. Two of the important cognitive
minicolumns through intervening synapses with thalamic functions served by these pathways are attention and imaging.
neurons. Fig. 7 shows a diagram of the global axis pathway, In the case of attention, the top-down axis pathway provides one
which begins with the initial cortical registration of sensory of the two input sources that activate thalamic neurons serving
inputs in primary sensory minicolumns, and extends into the the minicolumns of higher sensory areas in the posterior cortex
frontal cortex where, among other activities, axis circuits are (see Fig. 5). Thus, the sustaining of attention over extended
presumed to perform special supporting functions for the shell- time periods is controlled by sustained activity in axis circuits
based processes of thinking. The axis circuits act as holding of frontal areas.
circuits that sustain activity for extended periods of time. The top-down shell pathway may function to shift attention
When the intensity of the apical dendrite component of the rapidly among an array of stimulus items during search.
thalamocortical holding circuit is sufficiently high, it produces Axons from frontal attentional control areas are presumed
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1013

to the variable lengths of axons that connect axis circuits of


participating minicolumns. When visual objects are displayed
very briefly, the axis circuits may have sufficient time to
produce accurate impressions of attributes but not sufficient
time to produce impressions of precise locations. As a result,
some attributes may be bound to incorrect locations and lose
their correct conjunctive relationships, so that the observer
reports seeing objects whose conjunction of attributes is not
correct but “illusory”.

4.2. Holding circuits in frontal areas

Many of the potential cognitive aspects of axis activity in


frontal minicolumns have not yet been explored. Some of the
possible functions of the thalamocortical holding circuit are
suggested by the way the basal ganglia influences activity in
the frontal cortex. Axons from neurons of the basal ganglia do
not directly synapse on neurons of frontal cortex but instead
Fig. 8. Global descending pathways that connect anterior cortical columns with on thalamic neurons that are connected to frontal cortical
posterior cortical columns in two ways. One way (the lower-tier pathway) neurons (Alexander & Crutcher, 1990). One implication of
connects columns by their axis circuits, and the other way (the upper-tier this neuroanatomical arrangement is that the thalamocortical
pathway) connects columns by their shell circuits. holding circuit is involved when motivational activities in the
to contact layer 2/3 pyramids (through stellate neurons), basal ganglia act upon the cognitive and motor actions that
whose axon collaterals produce strong lateral inhibition in are processed in frontal circuits. Activity held in these circuits
neighboring minicolumns that code for distracter locations and can then operate on shell circuits in a modulatory manner,
appearances. Since the operations of inhibitory neurons are thereby influencing the way that input–output processing takes
typically very rapid compared to the operations of excitatory place in these circuits. For example, holding circuit activity
neurons, the top-down shell pathway can support a series of may stabilize the input–output processes involved in choosing
very rapid attentional selection of minicolumns that code for among several tasks to perform in the Wisconsin Card Sorting
locations. Identifications of each scanned item can be quickly Task, for example, sorting by color, or shape, or number.
accomplished in the shell circuitry of attribute minicolumns A thalamocortical holding circuit, controlled from the basal
(selected by axons from location minicolumns), and because ganglia, may also stabilize the processes involved in the early
the “dwell time” of attention on any particular item is very brief, trials of performing a specific task, such as sorting by color.
the contribution of axis circuitry has a minimal contribution An unstable holding circuit of a newly cued task may induce
to the operation of selecting minicolumns during the scanning the participant to revert to the previous task, which is a finding
operations. Therefore the major circuit operations supporting frequently observed in schizophrenic participants.
rapid search appear to be the lateral inhibitory connections Another subcortical circuit that may influence cortical
within shell circuits. circuits through the thalamic holding circuit is the limbic
circuit. One cortical target of the limbic circuit is the anterior
4.1. Attention and the binding of object attributes cingulate area, which has been shown to be particularly active
during cognitive events that involve conflict. When conflict is
It is generally agreed that the binding of component evoked in a task, it typically persists over a period of time; and
attributes produces the unitary impression of an object. The while the conflict activity is sustained, it is often accompanied
theory of minicolumn circuitries, described in the present paper, by “feelings” or impressions of the conflict. These observations
suggests that the binding of attributes of an object occurs suggest that the holding circuit participates in maintaining
by the set of impressions produced by the simultaneously states of conflict. Evidence supporting this conjecture is
elevated apical dendrite activity in minicolumns that code given by an fMRI study of attention (Buchsbaum et al.,
those attributes. When this set of minicolumns is linked 2006) in which elevation in activity of the anterior cingulate
to minicolumns coding a common location, attention to and its connected anterior thalamic neurons implicates the
that location raises the activation of the connected attribute thalamocortical recurrent circuit that connects these two brain
minicolumns in a correlated manner. Therefore, the binding structures.
could take place along the axis pathways shown in Fig. 8 Frontal holding circuits also would seem to be implicated
that intersect a common location. The amplified activity in the during the effort to recall an item from long-term memory (e.g.,
axis circuits containing the apical dendrites is based in part recalling an image of the Eiffel Tower). Although the typical
on a common frequency of electric oscillations, and but also impression of the effort to recall is not as vivid as the sensory
on the amplitude of the EPSPs in the apical dendrites. Phase impression of the item itself, or an image of the item, there
locking does not seem crucial to binding, on this view, owing may be occasions in which the effort to recall an item believed
1014 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

to be “on the tip of the tongue” is indirectly experienced activity within that axis circuit would seem to be of low
as an impression of tension somewhere in the body. The intensity so that it would be easily obscured by the typically
sustained activity of a bodily somatosensory indicator implies high intensities in axis circuits of sensory areas in the posterior
that a holding circuit is maintaining this activity. Because the cortex.
occasion that gives rise to the effort is the attempt to recall Therefore, the appreciable duration of the “passing moment”
an item, it would seem that the holding circuit is located in implies that axis activity is providing the principal neural
or close to the recall operation. However, one ordinarily is substrate, while the vanishingly small duration of the “present
not conscious of an impression of effort activity in these axis moment” implies that shell activity is providing the principal
circuits because the intensity of activity in these apical dendrites neural substrate. Stated in another way suggested by the present
is overshadowed by the characteristically strong intensities in theory, the passing moment is an impression of which we are
axis circuits of body feelings coded in SS2 minicolumns and conscious, and the present moment is a concept which we
in axis circuits of minicolumns that code ongoing visual and process as an idea.
auditory impressions.
4.4. Interactions between axis and shell circuit activities
4.3. The continuity of successive conscious impressions
A cognitively important relationship between having an
Considered as a unit of the impressions that constitute impression of an object and having an idea about an object is
consciousness, axis circuit activity can occur in many embodied in the connective links between the shell circuit and
minicolumns simultaneously, as is the case in primary sensory the axis circuit within a minicolumn. The main links between
areas where inputs from sensory receptors register many the axis circuit and the shell circuits are shown in Figs. 4 and 5.
details of a scene in parallel. However, in higher sensory Thalamocortical axons from the thalamic matrix neurons send
areas, attentional processes typically operate to select parts collateral axons to the apical dendrites of layer 2/3 pyramidal
of the registered scene, by elevating the axis circuit activity neurons, so that activity in the axis circuit produces and
in minicolumns, which will continue at this higher level maintains activity in the layer 2/3 apical dendrites. Therefore,
of intensity as long as frontal attentional control sites send when minicolumns of higher sensory areas exhibit attentional
activation to the thalamic neurons of these axis circuits. The activity, the elevated activity in the axis circuits of layer 5
elevated activity in these axis circuits does not abruptly return to pyramids induces EPSPs in the layer 2/3 apical dendrites.
baseline when attentional control is shifted to another part of the However, the amplitudes at the soma of layer 2/3 pyramids
sensory scene, but instead the activity decays gradually toward of these EPSPs are assumed to remain below threshold firing
baseline. Attentional effects observed in the primary visual area levels, so that the axons do not exhibit significant rates of
apparently exhibit this overlap of activity at the destination output pulses. For example, while a driver waits for a traffic
and origin sites of attentional shifts (Khayat, Spekreijse, & light to change from red to green and wishes to move quickly
Roelfsema, 2006). This overlap of minicolumn axis activity ahead at the onset of the green light, the driver may direct
during shifts of attention produces continuity in the succession preparatory attention to the location and color of the green
of impressions that would not exist if each axis circuit shuts light during the time interval leading up to the appearance of
down immediately when attentional activation is shifted away the green light. During this preparatory interval, the level of
from it. If attentional control induces relatively high levels attention corresponding to the amplitude of layer 2/3 apical
of intensity in a particular minicolumn (or a column cluster dendrite activity is elevated at the soma, but not to the level
of minicolumns), then the course of the decay process may that will produce axon output. When the green light appears,
be sufficiently long that some elevated activity remains after axonal pulses from V1 minicolumns coding the location and
attention has shifted to another minicolumn and then returned color of the green light produce EPSPs in the basal dendrites of
to the original minicolumn. This is the prediction derived and these layer 2/3 pyramids, which sum with the existing level of
tested for the case of shifting visual attention between spatial activity in the soma and produce axon outputs. Fig. 9 shows two
locations (LaBerge, Carlson, Williams, & Bunney, 1997). diagrams of a layer 2/3 pyramidal neuron, one showing activity
The continuity of successive impressions commonly in the apical dendrite (received from the layer 5 axis circuit),
observed during shifts of attention across visual, auditory and and the other showing no activity in the apical dendrite. When
tactual scenes contrasts with the discontinuities of successive axons from area V1 produce EPSPs in the basal dendrites of
identifications of objects (attended or unattended) during these layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons, the pyramidal neuron with
these shifts of attention. Units of processing are distinct and the attentionally induced higher tonic activity at the soma will
separated, owing largely to their typically short durations emit a train of output pulses in the axon sooner than the other
and their abrupt terminations when an output occurs. When pyramid, which represents a state of no preparatory attention.
identifications evoke categories and ideas further along in the In addition to a shortening of the output latency, the tonic soma
shell global pathway, there may be overlap between ideas when activation by the apical dendrite will increase the rate of pulses
they are stored in working memory circuits. But, if working in the output, so that, in effect, the signal-to-noise level of the
memory circuits are exclusively shell-based, there will be no input pulses received at the basal dendrite is increased.
impressions generated. But even if the working memory circuits But if the level of apical dendrite activity delivered to
contained an axis component, the intensity of apical dendrite the soma of the layer 2/3 pyramid momentarily drifts above
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1015

As a general cognitive principle, shell circuit activity enables


the identification of the axis activity that is taking place in
the same minicolumn (or column cluster of minicolumns). The
attachment of a symbol or label to a stimulus input is based on
the process of identifying a stimulus input from the primary
sensory area and then categorizing it in successively higher
sensory areas. Thus, it could be said that the symbolizing
process produces the basic materials for the construction
of ideas. But the symbolizing process does not capture the
subjective impression itself, because sustained axis activity
Fig. 9. Apical dendrite activity modulates input–output processing in layer 2/3
does not translate or transform into the shell activity by
pyramidal neurons. Here, the same train of input pulses contacts the basal input–output processing circuitry. However, the shell circuits
dendrites of both neurons, but the sustained elevated apical dendrite activity can respond to certain aspects of the axis activity in the
in the neuron on the left increases excitability of the soma, so that fewer input minicolumn they share. In many cases, shell processing will
pulses are needed to produce an output pulse. occur or not occur according to the presence of axis activity,
so that an input to the shell circuit can act as a query about the
firing threshold, then axon output will occur, which typically existence or non-existence of axis activity in that minicolumn
produces false positive responses, called “jumping the gun”. (see Fig. 9). A succession of these queries can therefore
Therefore, to produce axon outputs to the onset of the green provide a means of estimating the duration of axis activity
light with short latency and without momentary fluctuations in a minicolumn or cluster of minicolumns. Also, since the
of apical dendrite activity at the soma that produce false intensity of axis activity affects the latency and strength of the
positive outputs, the mean level of apical dendrite activity output signals in shell processing (described in the foregoing
delivered to the soma must be elevated as close to the firing example of anticipating a green traffic light), an input query to
threshold as the variability of the EPSP will allow. Thus, an the shell circuit provides a means of detecting the intensity of
observer cannot effectively reduce the time to process the green axis activity in a minicolumn or cluster of minicolumns. Thus,
light (in the attention-based higher sensory area) simply by it could be said that the shell circuitry can have knowledge
increasing the amplitude of the EPSP delivered to the layer 2/3 about the duration and intensity of axis activity, but shell
apical dendrites unless the lengths of the apical dendrites are circuitry does not have direct knowledge (e.g., knowledge
sufficiently long to reduce the variability of the larger EPSP by by acquaintance) of “what it is like” to have the impression
the time it reaches the soma. With training, one might expect the produced by an axis circuit. The sharp limitation of knowledge
lengths of the apical dendrites to lengthen, so the modulatory about axis activity in a particular minicolumn appears to be
activity delivered to the soma could be raised to progressively reflected in the manner in which the layer 2/3 pyramidal
higher levels while maintaining the steady stability of low EPSP neurons are arranged around the outside of the apical dendrites
variability. of the layer 5 pyramidal neurons. The etymological form of
There are additional benefits for tasks of sustained attention the word “about”, prior to 900 A.D. was the word abutan,
that are produced by high but steady activity in the apical which meant “on the outside of”. Thus, the shell circuits
dendrites of layer 5 and layer 2/3 pyramids. These pyramids may be viewed as exterior to the interior events of the axis
send collaterals of their axons to neighboring columns, where circuits and specialize in “aboutness”. The symbolic nature of
they synapse on inhibitory neurons that contact pyramids shell activity enables its outputs concerning this “aboutness”
and lower their responsiveness. Also, axis circuits that serve to be communicated to others through language and gestures.
neighboring columns inhibit each other in the thalamus by In contrast, the impressions of axis activity would seem to
their synapses on inhibitory neurons of the thalamic reticular constitute a subjective “inwardness” of the individual’s mental
nucleus (Jones, 2007). When the activity levels of axis circuits life, and as such are not capable of being communicated to other
of a column cluster of minicolumns are elevated, the activity individuals. Furthermore, it could be said that the extended
in neighboring columns that code for potential distracting duration of axis activity confers a concrete “existence” to
signals is concurrently suppressed. When this state of affairs impressions compared to the specious existence of the fleeting
is extended over durations of time, as in anticipation of moments in which ideas are processed. In summary, the
an upcoming green light, or in imaging the arrangement of distinctive structures of the axis and shell circuits within the
furniture in a familiar room, or activating a plan of action, minicolumn appear to define two very different ways in which
there is less likelihood that the onsets of distracting stimuli the cerebral cortex responds to and constructs its knowledge of
will perturb the ongoing axis activity that supports preparatory the sensory inputs from objects of the external world.
attention, imaging, and planning. Thus, high amplitudes of axis
activity can protect axis circuit activity from interference by 4.5. The role of the self in consciousness
distracting events, much as increased rates of axon outputs can
increase the signal-to-noise ratio in input–output processing The global circuits connecting axis and shell circuits also
and protect the output information from interference by ambient serve as a basis for distinguishing two selves, which, for
noise. convenience, we label as the “thinking” self and the “feeling”
1016 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

self. The thinking self is a product of ideas produced by shell consciousness produced by attention can occlude the ongoing
circuits. It includes items of personal history and verbalized background feelings of the body, so that we can apparently
personal characteristics that arise from the way we interpret “forget ourselves” when we are beholding a sunset, or when
our behaviors in social situations. Gazzanga’s theory of the we are listening to a favorite piece of music, or when we are
“Interpreter” (Turk, Heatherton, Macrae, Kelley, & Gazzaniga, reading a novel. Thus, the body-based or body feeling-based
2003) provides an example of the self as “thought about”. In impression of the self is based on background consciousness of
contrast, the feeling self is based on the sustained impressions the current state of the body. As such, this consciousness may
of the body’s landscape, whose intensities can be modulated be regarded as another kind of sensory-based consciousness,
by the emotional activities of the body. The present notions with registration of ongoing bodily feelings taking place in the
concerning the relationship of the feeling self to ongoing primary somatosensory area, S1. Foreground consciousness of
emotional events in the internal organs are influenced by the a bodily feeling, then, is the elevated intensity of an axis circuit
work of Damasio (1994). By analogy with descriptions of coding an attentionally selected part of this registered array of
activities in early cortical areas of vision, audition, and touch, feelings in minicolumns of the higher sensory area S2.
we conjecture that the ongoing inputs from the internal organs
and external bodily surfaces are registered as impressions in 5. Consciousness and the electromagnetic fields of apical
axis circuits of somatosensory area S1 and selectively attended dendrites
in axis circuits of area S2.
The continuing inputs from our body provide impressions The present paper proposes that sustained activity in the axis
that constitute background consciousness of our bodily feeling. circuit of minicolumns is a necessary condition for producing
This ongoing feeling of our body provides the primitive sense and maintaining consciousness in the brain. The specific part
of presence, which then can be attributed to objects of the world of the axis circuit that underlies the subjective aspect of
that we imagine our body can touch. Thus, when the sustained conscious impressions is the electromagnetic activity of the
bodily impressions are paired in a coordinated manner with apical dendrite, according the present theory. In this final
the sustained impressions of objects seen, heard, and especially section of the paper we examine in some detail the properties of
touched, these objects also become present to us. Together with the electromagnetic activity of the apical dendrite to determine
the impression of the presence of the body, the impressions what properties of conscious impressions they may underlie.
of the presence of these objects provide our ongoing sense of In particular, do the properties of electromagnetic activity of
presence of ourselves in the world. For example, as we walk the apical dendrite correspond to the properties of extended
along a path and approach a large tree, we seem to attend more duration and variations in intensity that we have attributed to
and more strongly to our body as well as to the tree. As we close conscious impressions?
the distance between our body and the tree the impressions of The electrical nature of the propagating EPSP in the
both body and tree seem to intensify, and this intensity peaks apical dendrite contrasts sharply with the electrical nature of
if a part of our body actually touches the tree. These sustained the propagating action potential in the myelinated and un-
impressions of the tree and our bodily self are likely to become myelinated axon in several important and related respects.
very intense if the tree we approach is a giant redwood. Firstly, the initiating current and voltage of the apical dendrite
A conductor of an orchestra can experience a similar EPSP can vary considerably in amplitude while the current
impression of the presence of both the self and an external and voltage characteristics of the action potential are virtually
object, but in this case the looming object is the overall constant (and relatively low) in a given axon. Secondly,
orchestral sound as the conductor gradually directs a crescendo the EPSP of the apical dendrite decays substantially as it
from the level of a pianissimo to the level of fortissimo. In a propagates toward the soma, while the action potential decays
similar way, the sounds of music or the sounds of voices at a only very slightly within the inter-nodal segment of the
cocktail party in the hotel room down the hall seem to require myelinated axon, except for the highest frequency components
a minimal level of intensity to give the impression of their of the Fourier spectrum of the action potential, which affect the
presence, but the additional requirement is the relationship to pulse rise-time only. Thirdly, owing to the clustering of several
our current impression of our body. It would seem, therefore, apical dendrites aligned in parallel there is a strengthening
that it is the attention directed to our body while we attend of the overall electric dipole effect, whereas sites of action
to a suitably intense impression of an external object that adds potentials are not consistently aligned within nerve bundles
the impression of presence to the consciousness of that object. of axons. We note that the superposition of electrical fields
It has been suggested that the term “awareness” should be from many parallel and closely spaced electric dipoles with
reserved for such cases of elevated consciousness that arise codirectional currents produces a local enhancement of the
when attention is directed to the body at the same time as electric fields in an axial direction along the inner and outer
attention is directed to an object (LaBerge, 1997). surface of the apical dendrite membrane. This superposition
In view of these considerations, the present theory of fields concentrates the electromagnetic energy within the
of consciousness does not require the self, considered cluster volume, so that the total field energy (inside and
either as the felt-body based on axis activity or as an outside the membrane) undergoes a gain that would not occur
intellectual construction based on shell activity, as a necessary in unclustered, well-separated apical dendrites. This gain or
component of consciousness. High levels of visual or auditory enhancement in localized energy (or power) increases as the
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1017

the analysis presented by Burrell and Peters (1979). When


propagation is in the LFW, as is the case for the EPSP or
action potentials, the extracellular pulse will propagate with
little distortion. The theoretical behavior of the transient pulse
or electromagnetic field propagation from an electrically small
electric dipole source is well-known for such lossy material as
extracellular fluids, and has been rigorously developed by Song
and Chen (1993). For the known frequency content of EPSPs
and action potentials, the electric and magnetic field time
dependence at any localized point in the extracellular region
is identical to the spatially localized current time waveform
of the internal EPSP. Thus, for EPSP propagation along the
apical dendrite we can make the simplifying assumption that
the electric and magnetic fields in the extracellular medium
preserve the current waveform exhibited by the EPSP inside
the dendrite with little distortion.
Therefore, the attenuation of the extracellular electromag-
netic pulse depends only on the cube of the radial distance
from the apical dendrite and simple angular variables within
Fig. 10. (Above) The decay in voltage, V , of an EPSP mass of charge shown at the LFW. We employ here a spherical coordinate system to de-
three locations along the apical dendrite shaft as the EPSP mass propagates scribe the field structure of the EPSP pulse, shown in Fig. 11.
toward the soma. (Below) At each of the three EPSP locations there is an Two electric field components (radial and tangential) and one
(appropriately small) electric dipole accompanied by its electromagnetic field. magnetic field component (circumferential) describe the total
instantaneous field at any point outside the apical dendrite. The
square of the sum of the field amplitudes of individual apical diagram shown in Fig. 11 describes the spatial distribution of
dendrites. the electric field at a point in time for one current dipole, while
To promote an understanding of how these fields could the magnetic field component is always circumferential about
relate closely to consciousness we describe the electromagnetic the axis of the current dipole. The angular dependence of each
characteristics of the transient fields in the extracellular region component specifies that at right angles to the axis of the api-
produced by the propagating EPSP in the apical dendrite. cal dendrite the tangential component of the electric field that is
Because the present theory assumes that the time-dependent parallel (or approximately parallel) to the dendrite is the dom-
charges decay as they propagate along the apical dendrite (see inant electric field component. This parallel component will
Fig. 6) we partition the apical dendrite into a collinear spatial exhibit strong electrical coupling to a neighboring dendrite lo-
array of electric dipoles at any given instant of time. Each dipole cated within several diameters of the dendrite (a typical apical
is a source of electromagnetic wave energy in the form of a dendrite diameter is approximately 2 microns, and 6–8 apical
transient pulse of both localized electric field and magnetic dendrite shafts lie within an area of approximately 12 × 12 mi-
field energy (density) that radiates into the extracellular space crons (Peters & Sethares, 1991, Fig. 5)).
(see Fig. 10). Each electric dipole along the apical dendrite The equations that describe the near electric and magnetic
shaft is derived from the time- and space-dependent current fields produced by the velocity-dependent dipole of the apical
of the moving EPSP waveform multiplied by an appropriately dendrite are as follows.
chosen short length, dz, of the apical dendrite. These dynamic E θ = (I dz/4π σ r 3 ) sin θ, (1)
dipoles are characterized by both magnetic and electric fields,
and their dipole moments depend on three variables: the charge Er = (I dz/2π σ r ) cos θ,
3
(2)
at the two poles, the velocity of the charges, and the distance Hφ = (I dz/4πr 2 ) sin θ, (3)
between them. When these component dipoles are superposed
ν ∼ 1/σ µ0r, (4)
they produce the overall dipole effect of the apical dendrite
activity. where E θ and Er are components of the electric field, Hφ is
The time-dependent EPSP current at any given segment, dz, the magnetic field, ν is the field propagation velocity, I is the
along the dendrite is the excitation source of the dipole. The current, dz is an appropriately small dipole length, r is the radial
pulse shape of the radiation field from this dipole is preserved distance from the dipole, θ is the angle defined by the radial
as it propagates away from the dendrite at a decreasing velocity. direction and the dipole axis, σ is the electrical conductivity
This preservation of pulse shape results from the dominance of the medium, and µ0 is the magnetic permeability of the
of conduction currents over Maxwellian displacement currents medium.
in the extracellular medium (approximating that of seawater), Near the axis of the apical dendrite, the radial component
and from the low frequency characteristic of EPSPs. As that points in the axial direction of EPSP propagation is the
a consequence, the propagation of a radiating pulse may dominant electric field component. Inside the apical dendrite
be described by a low-frequency window (LFW), following this axially directed component is at its strongest level, where
1018 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

6. Other ways of producing consciousness

This paper makes the strong claim that the physical correlate
of consciousness is the near electromagnetic field (see Eqs.
(1)–(3)) located along the surface of the neural membrane.
This field contains the “energy of consciousness” that has
the ability to do work on other electric charges and thereby
influence other neural events (e.g., by modulation effects at the
soma); or it can simply be the state of “being conscious” of
the particular content coded by the apical dendrite (e.g., the
color red). Thus, a major physical property of the field
activity is the intensity of energy, which corresponds to
the cognitive property of intensifying a sensory impression
(e.g., concentrating attention more strongly to the redness of an
apple). Level of intensity determines what content dominates
consciousness, so that low intensity levels of active apical
dendrites in many areas of the brain (e.g., apical dendrites
that code for the ongoing impression of pressure of the feet
on the floor) will normally not be experienced as part of the
momentary content of consciousness. While the long apical
Fig. 11. Detailed diagram (in a spherical coordinate system) of the radiating dendrite seems well-suited to provide variations in intensity and
electromagnetic field from an EPSP mass of charges at a particular location as other properties of consciousness (e.g., extendable duration),
it moves along the apical dendrite. At a given point in space the electric field we cannot rule out the possibility that other structures, organic
intensity is E 0 and the magnetic field intensity is Hφ0 . The radial electric field or inorganic, could also produce electric fields with these kinds
component, Er is highest in the interior of the dendritic shaft, and it decays very
of properties. For example, action potentials in stable circuit
rapidly in the radial direction as 1/r 3 . For clarity, the circumferential magnetic
field distribution has been omitted from the diagram. loops produce extended durations of electric field oscillations;
an example is the high spectral fields produced by spikes at the
axon initial segment of layer 5 pyramidal neurons within the
it is closest to the EPSP current flow mechanism. Eq. (4) corticothalamic circuit loop. Since action potentials fire with
indicates that the electromagnetic pulse emitted by the dipole a constant voltage, variations in intensity would be based on
propagates away from the apical dendrite at a velocity, ν, which the number of participating axons in the near neighborhood;
is proportional to the inverse product of radial distance and however, action potentials at nodes of myelinated axons are not
electrical conductivity. In effect, the velocity of propagation is likely to produce large summation of fields because the nodes
close to the speed of light at the outer surface of the dendrite and do not line up in close neighborhoods across axon clusters.
quickly decays with radial distance r to values several orders of Another, more global, view combines the fields of all axons,
magnitude lower than the speed of light at distances beyond dendrites, and somas in subcortical and cortical regions of the
about a millimeter, according to theoretical predictions. The brain into one complex electromagnetic field, and regards this
velocity decays as 1/r for a constant conductivity and magnetic total brain field as the basis of consciousness (McFadden, 2002;
permeability. Pockett, 2000). Finally, the electromagnetic fields produced by
The relationship between the EPSP voltage waveform and inorganic structures such as wire antennas could, in principle,
current waveform can be derived from classical cable theory serve as the basis of artificial consciousness, but it may
using the well-known concept of wave impedance or cable be somewhat difficult to infer the properties of conscious
characteristic impedance. Figs. 10 and 11 contain examples activity in these structures without additional circuitry (e.g., for
of EPSP voltage waveforms described as Gaussian pulse accessing and identification) which the normal human brain
shapes. The exact time dependence of the electromagnetic field provides.
pulse at a point in the extracellular fluid will depend on the
conductivity, dielectric properties, and thickness of the surface 7. Tentative conclusions
layer of the apical dendrite. Clusters of apical dendrites in close
proximity therefore will develop electric field energy in their In cortical circuits that operate by input–output processing,
core structures from the combined electric field components the strengths of electrical signals typically are at relatively low
parallel to the dendritic axes. The development of magnetic levels. In comparison, the electrical activity in apical dendrites
field energy between parallel electric field dipoles is much less normally operates at relatively high levels, as suggested by
significant compared to electric field energy coupling between the stronger demands upon blood flow of local field potentials
pairs of parallel apical dendrites with internal collinear dipoles compared to that of the multi-unit activity of action potentials
moving at the conduction velocity. According to the present (Logothetis, Pauls, Augath, Trinath, & Oeltermann, 2001).
theory, it is this electromagnetic field intensity that underlies Also, EEG recordings, which are driven by electrical activity
our immediate conscious impressions. of clusters of apical dendrites, can show exceptionally high
D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020 1019

amplitudes during meditation (e.g., Lutz et al. (2004)). Given Burrell, G. A., & Peters, L., Jr. (1979). Pulse propagation in lossy media using
the level of electrical currents/voltage in an individual apical the low-frequency window for video pulse radar application. Proceedings
of IEEE, 67, 981–990.
dendrite, close clustering of apical dendrites increases the
Colbert, C. M. (2001). Back propagating action potentials in pyramidal
concentration of field energies as the square of the number neurons: A putative signaling mechanism for the induction of Hebbian
of active apical dendrites within the cluster. Owing to the synaptic activity. Restoration of Neuroscience, 19, 100–211.
length of the apical dendrites, these high levels of electrical Contreras, D., Timofeev, I., & Steriade, M. (1996). Mechanisms of long lasting
activity will be attenuated before they reach their somas, hyperpolarizations underlying slow sleep oscillations in cat corticothalamic
networks. Journal of Physiology, 494, 251–264.
where they can modulate the input–output activity in circuits
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, reason and the human
that process signals. Thus, the extended length of the apical Brain. New York: Avon Books.
dendrite provides one way to separate the pocket of intense DeFelipe, J., & Farina, I. (1992). The pyramidal neurons of the cerebral cortex:
electrical activity in moderately distal sectors of the apical Morphological and chemical characteristics of the synaptic inputs. Progress
dendrite from the input–output processing at the soma. These in Neurobiology, 3, 563–607.
Destexhe, A., Rudolph, M., & Pare, D. (2003). The high-conductance state of
pockets of intense electrical activity in the apical dendrite are
neocortical neurons in vivo. Nature Reviews of Neuroscience, 4, 739–751.
also separated from neighboring circuits in the intercellular DiRusso, F., Martinez, A., & Hillyard, S. A. (2003). Source analysis of event-
medium by the restriction of their high field strengths to small related cortical activity during visuo-spatial attention. Cerebral Cortex, 13,
regions very near the surface of the apical dendrite, because 486–499.
the near electric field intensity falls off as 1/r 3 , where r Edelman, G. M. (2003). Naturalizing consciousness: A theoretical framework.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of
is the radial distance from a point in the apical dendrite. It America, 100, 5521–5524.
would seem, therefore, that these small, relatively isolated Feldman, M. L. (1984). Morphology of the neocortical pyramidal neuron. In A.
regions in the apical dendrites are able to increase their electric Peters, & E. G. Jones (Eds.), Cerebral cortex. Cellular components of the
field intensity to a relatively high level, and sustain it there cerebral cortex: Vol. 1 (pp. 123–200). New York: Plenum Press.
for extended durations without perturbing the input–output Goldberg, J., Holthoff, K., & Yuste, R. (2002). A problem with Hebb and local
spikes. Trends in Neuroscience, 25, 433–435.
electrical activities of neurons in neighboring circuits. Grossberg, S. (1999). The link between brain learning, attention, and
Therefore the following question arises: What function is consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 8, 1–44.
served by high electric field intensities and their extended Grossberg, S. (2005). Linking attention to learning, expectation, competition,
durations in apical dendrites? We have suggested that high and consciousness. In L. Itti, G. Rees, & J. K. Tsotsos (Eds.), Neurobiology
of attention (pp. 652–662) (Chapter 107).
field intensities benefit input–output processing by their
Helmchen, F., Svoboda, K., Denk, W., & Tank, D. W. (1999). In vivo
stronger modulatory effects at the somas of pyramidal dendritic calcium dynamics in deep-layer cortical pyramidal neurons.
neurons, with consequent increases in the strength of lateral Nature Neuroscience, 2, 989–996.
inhibitory connections that protect the axis circuit activity Hendry, S. H. C., Schwark, H. D., Jones, E. G., & Yan, J. (1987). Numbers
from interference by neighboring minicolumn activity. But and proportions of GABA-immunoreactive neurons in different areas of the
monkey cerebral cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 7, 1503–1519.
these pockets of high electric field intensity are activated and
Jarsky, T., Roxin, A., Kath, W. L., & Spruston, N. (2005). Conditional dendritic
sustained even when they are not modulating input–output spike propagation following distal synaptic activation of hippocampal CA1
processing, for example, when we are attending to sunsets, pyramidal neurons. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1667–1676.
music, and other pleasurable activities. These localized high Jones, E. G. (2002). Thalamic circuitry and thalamocortical synchrony.
electric field intensities may serve to raise our raw impressions Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London, B, 357,
1659–1673.
of the world to the level of consciousness, and thereby provide
Jones, E. G. (2007). The Thalamus (2nd ed.). Cambridge (UK): Cambridge
our mental life with its special kind of existence. University Press.
Khayat, P. S., Spekreijse, H., & Roelfsema, P. R. (2006). Attention lights
References up new object representations before the old ones fade away. Journal of
Neuroscience, 26, 138–142.
Koch, C. (1998). Biophysics of computation: Information processing in the
Alexander, G. E., & Crutcher, M. D. (1990). Functional architecture of single neuron. New York: Oxford University Press.
basal ganglia circuits: Neural substrate of parallel processing. Trends in Kuriki, S., Kanda, S., & Hirata, Y. (2006). Effects of musical experience on
Neuroscience, 13, 266–271. different components of MEG responses elicited by sequential piano-tones
Azouz, R., & Gray, C. M. (1999). Cellular mechanisms contributing to and chords. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 4046–4053.
response variability of cortical neurons in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience, LaBerge, D. (1997). Attention, awareness, and the triangular circuit.
19, 2209–2223. Consciousness and Cognition, 6, 149–181.
Bolz, J., Gilbert, C. D., & Wiesel, T. N. (1989). Pharmacological analysis of LaBerge, D. (2001). Attention, consciousness, and electrical wave activity
cortical circuitry. Trends in Neuroscience, 12, 292–296. within the cortical column. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 43,
Bouyer, J. J., Montaron, M. F., & Rougeul, A. (1981). Fast fronto-parietal 5–24.
rhythms during combined focused attentive behavior and immobility in cat: LaBerge, D. (2005). Sustained attention and apical dendrite activity in recurrent
Cortical and thalamic localizations. Electroencephalography and Clinical circuits. Brain Research Reviews, 50, 86–99.
Neurophysiology, 51, 244–252. LaBerge, D., Carlson, R. L., Williams, J. K., & Bunney, B. G. (1997). Shifting
Braitenberg, V., & Schuz, (1998). Cortex: Statistics and geometry of neuronal attention in visual space: Tests of moving-spotlight models versus and
connectivity (2nd ed.). Berlin: Springer. activity-distribution model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Buchsbaum, M. S., Buchsbaum, B. R., Chokron, S., Tang, C, Wei, T. C., Perception and Performance, 23, 13801392.
& Byne, W. (2006). Thalamocortical circuits: FMRI assessment of the Larkman, A. U. (1991). Dendritic morphology of pyramidal neurons of the
pulvinar and medial dorsal nucleus in normal volunteers. Neuroscience visual cortex of the rat: III. Spine distributions. Journal of Comparative
Letters, 404, 282–287. Neurology, 306, 332–343.
1020 D. LaBerge, R. Kasevich / Neural Networks 20 (2007) 1004–1020

Larkum, M. E., & Zhu, J. J. (2002). Signaling of layer 1 and whisker-evoked Peters, A, & Sethares, C. (1991). Organization of pyramidal neurons in area
Ca2+ and Na+ action potentials in distal and terminal dendrites of rat 17 of monkey visual cortex. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 306,
neocortical pyramidal neurons in vitro and in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience, 1–23.
22, 6991–7005. Pockett, S. (2000). The nature of consciousness: A hypothesis. Lincoln (NE):
Lazar, S. W., Kerr, C. E., Wasserman, R. H., Gray, J. R., Greve, D. N., Writers Club Press.
Treadway, M. T., et al. (2005). Meditation experience is associated with Radley, J. J., Rocher, A. B, Miller, M., Janssen, W. G., Liston, C., Hof, P. R.,
increased cortical thickness. Neuroreport, 16, 1893–1897. et al. (2006). Repeated stress induces dendritic spine loss in the rat medial
Llinas, R. R. (1988). The intrinsic electrophysiological properties of prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 313–320.
mammalian neurons: Insights into central nervous system function. Science, Rall, W. (1989). Cable theory for dendritic neurons. In C. Koch, & I. Segev
242, 1654–1664. (Eds.), Methods in Neuronal Modeling (pp. 9–62). Cambridge (MA): MIT
Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., Augath, M., Trinath, T., & Oeltermann, A. (2001). Press.
Neurophysiological investigation of the basis of the fMRI signal. Nature, Reyes, A. (2001). Influence of dendritic conductances on the input–output
412, 150–157. properties of neurons. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 653–675.
Lutz, A., Greichar, L. L., Rawlings, N. B, & Ricard, M. (2004). Long-term Rockel, A. J., Hiorns, R. W., & Powell, T. P. S. (1974). Numbers of neurons
meditators self-induce high-amplitude gamma synchrony during mental through the full depth of the neocortex. Proceedings of the Anatomy Society
practice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 101, of Great Britain and Ireland, 118, 371.
16369–16373. Rockel, A. J., Hiorns, R. W., & Powell, T. P. S. (1980). The basic uniformity
Makris, N., Biederman, J., Valera, E. M., Bush, G., Kaiser, J., Kennedy, D. in structure of the neocortex. Brain, 103, 221–244.
N., et al. (2006). Cortical thinning of the attention and executive function Silver, H. A., Ress, D, & Heeger, D. J. (2007). Neural correlates of sustained
networks in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cerebral spatial attention in human early visual cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology,
Cortex, 17, 1364–1375. 97, 229–237.
McAdams, C. J., & Maunsell, J. H. (1999). Effects of attention on the reliability Shahin, A., Bosnyak, D. J., Trainor, L. J., & Roberts, L. E. (2003).
of individual neuron in monkey visual cortex. Neuron, 23, 765–773. Enhancement of neuroplastic P2 and N1c auditory evoked potential in
McAdams, C. J., & Reid, R. C. (2005). Attention modulates the responses of musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 5545–5552.
simple cells in monkey primary visual cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, Shahin, A. J., Roberts, L. E., Pantev, C., Aziz, M., & Picton, T. W. (2007).
11023–11033. Enhanced anterior–temporal processing for complex tones in musicians.
McFadden, J. (2002). The conscious electromagnetic information (cemi) field Clinical Neurolphysiology, 118, 209–220.
theory: The hard problem made easy. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 9, Shu, Y., Duque, A., Yu, Y., Haider, B., & McCormick, D. A. (2007). Properties
45–60. of action potential initiation in neocortical pyramidal cells: evidence from
McGuire, B. A., Hornung, J. P., Gilbert, C. D., & Wiesel, T. N. (1984). Patterns whole cell axon recordings. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97, 746–760.
of synaptic input to layer 4 of cat striate cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, Song, J., & Chen, K. (1993). Propagation of EM pulses excited by an electric
3021–3033. dipole in a conducting medium. IEEE Transactions on Antennas and
Mountcastle, V. B. (1957). Modality and topographic properties of single Propagation, 41, 1414–1421.
neurons in cat’s somatic sensory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 20, Taylor, J. T. (1999). The race for consciousness. Cambridge (MA): MIT Press.
408–434. Taylor, J. G. (2005). Mind and consciousness: Towards a final answer? Physics
Mountcastle, V. B. (1998). The cerebral cortex. Cambridge (MA): Harvard of Life Reviews, 2, 1–45.
University Press. Taylor, J. G. (2007). On the neurodynamics of the creation of consciousness.
Newman, J., Baars, B. J., & Cho, S-B. (1997). A neural global workshop model Cognitive Neurodynamics, 1, 97–118.
of conscious attention. Neural Networks, 10, 1195–1206. Tegner, J., Compte, A., & Wang, (2002). The dynamical stability of
Nowak, L. G., Sanchez-Vives, M. V., & McCormick, D. A. (1997). Influence reverberatory neural circuits. Biological Cybernetics, 87, 471–481.
of low and high frequency inputs on spike timing in visual cortical neurons. Turk, D. J., Heatherton, T. F., Macrae, C. N., Kelley, W. M., & Gazzaniga, M. S.
Cerebral Cortex, 7, 487–501. (2003). Out of contact, out of mind: The distributed nature of the self.
O’Connor, D. H., Fukui, M. M., Pinsk, M. A., & Kastner, S. (2002). Attention Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1001, 65–78.
modulates responses in the human lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature Van Essen, D. C., Drury, H. A., Joshi, S., & Miller, M. I. (1998). Functional and
Neuroscience, 5, 1203–1209. structural mapping of human cerebral cortex: Solutions are in the surfaces.
Pantev, C., Makieg, S., Hoke, M., Galambos, R., Hampson, S., & Gallen, C. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 788–795.
(1991). Human auditory evoked gamma-band magnetic fields. Proceedings Yuste, R., & Tank, D. W. (1996). Dendritic integration in mammalian neurons,
of the National Academy of Sciences. USA, 88, 8996–9000. a century after Cajal. Neuron, 16, 701–71.

Potrebbero piacerti anche