Sei sulla pagina 1di 3

Current Biology

Dispatches

Bio-Linguistics: Monkeys Break Through the Syntax


Barrier
W. Tecumseh Fitch
Department of Cognitive Biology, Faculty of Life Sciences, University of Vienna, Vienna, A-1090 Austria
Correspondence: tecumseh.fitch@univie.ac.at
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.04.087

Macaque monkeys can be trained to produce complex spatial sequences beyond the simplest levels of
grammar previously known from animal studies. This indicates cognitive capabilities in the spatial-motor
domain that approach the computational complexity level of human syntax.

The human capacity for language, After intensive training, the monkeys language theory, which tells us what type
allowing us to express any thought we can could learn rules more complex than any of computational system is needed to
think, appears to be unique on the planet: previously demonstrated in nonhuman identify or generate that particular set of
although most animals communicate, species. Most tellingly, they learned to well-formed strings.
none but humans show this unbounded produce mirror sequences following the The most limited class consists of the
expressive power. But our capacity to use pattern ABCjCBA, and generalized this regular languages, which require
and acquire language consists of multiple ability to new sequence lengths. This is computational machinery termed ‘finite
interlocking subcomponents, many of important because such ‘mirror state automata’. Such systems have a
which are shared with other species [1]. grammars’ require computational limited ‘rote’ memory, but limited ability to
For example, humans are the only living capabilities beyond the simplest type, the keep track of past occurrences or
primate species known to be capable of so-called ‘regular’ or ‘finite state’ context. Abundant previous work shows
learning and reproducing novel grammars. Like the grammars of all that many nonhuman species possess, at
vocalizations, including words or human languages, mirror grammars least, this level of computational power.
melodies: but we share this capability with require a learner to possess ‘supra- But human language has many examples
other, more distantly related species regular’ computational abilities, which where such limited systems are
including birds, bats, seals, or elephants requires specific computational inadequate: for example a sentence with
[2–4]. Shared traits are a boon to machinery not needed at the lower sub- an ‘if’ will typically have, some arbitrary
biologists interested in language, as regular level (Figure 1). The new results number of words later, the word ‘then’,
animal models allow us to deploy a thus suggest that the monkey’s brain and such if/then pairs can be nested
panoply of neuroscientific tools to possesses the kind of cognitive within one another. This is one of many
understand their inner workings, and to mechanisms required for human linguistic examples in linguistic syntax where
test evolutionary hypotheses about syntax, at least in this specific cognitive complex tree structures are required to
adaptive function. domain, and after intensive training. capture the empirical facts about human
One component of language has until To fully unpack the significance of this language, and crucially, supra-regular
now resisted the search for parallels in our result requires a bit of computational systems are needed to capture such
animal brethren: syntax. All human theory. Formal language theory is a patterns [9].
languages have at their core complex sets branch of mathematics, originating with Previous animal work on supra-
of rules which enable us to combine the work of Alan Turing [8], that plays a regularity started with the finding of [11]
phonemes into syllables into words into central role in theoretical computer that cotton-top tamarins, while able to
sentences with precise, specific science [9]. It specifies the types of learn a simple regular rule (‘repeat AB
meanings. The last, most complex stage computational mechanisms required to indefinitely’) were unable to learn a closely
(words into sentences) requires rule cope with potentially infinite sets of matched supra-regular grammar termed
systems — ‘grammars’ — previously strings — termed ‘languages’ — that obey AnBn (meaning ‘some number of As
thought to be beyond the capabilities of certain constraints or follow certain followed by the same number of Bs’). This
nonhuman animals [5,6]. In this issue of patterns [10]. Note that, despite using suggested that supra-regularity may
Current Biology, Jiang et al. [7] show that, such words as ‘grammar’ and ‘language’, represent a threshold between humans
with adequate training, monkeys can this body of theory is not limited to human and other species. However, it used a
break beyond this barrier. languages: it applies across diverse habituation paradigm involving very little
Working in Liping Wang’s laboratory, domains including mathematical training. Later work using intensive
together with Stanislas Dehaene, Jiang expressions, music, visual patterns, or training in songbirds appeared to show
and colleagues [7] trained two rhesus even well-formed telephone numbers. success on this same grammar [12] but,
macaques to produce structured Any system where some strings are valid along with several subsequent studies [7],
sequences by pressing a touchscreen at (‘grammatical’ or well-formed) and others has been faulted on methodological
specific locations arrayed around a circle. invalid can be analysed using formal grounds. The central empirical challenge

Current Biology 28, R695–R717, June 18, 2018 ª 2018 Elsevier Ltd. R695
Current Biology

Dispatches

A B should greatly illuminate the brain


Sub-Regular Systems Supra-Regular Systems
Syntax mechanisms underlying human language.
English If-then Swiss German
Phonological Sentences Crossing My money personally is on involvement of
Phenomena in Dependencies
Language the region of the prefrontal cortex known
Finite Regular
Context-
Mildly
Context-
Context- as Broca’s area, which exists in monkeys
free sensitive
sensitive
but is greatly expanded in humans
Well-formed
Telephone Numbers
[17,18]. It is possible that the massive
Computably
(AB)n
Grammar
Mirror
Grammar Copy
Enumerable
Strings
expansion of this region in humans,
Grammar combined with greatly increased
Current Biology
connectivity, underlies the domain-
general dendrophilia typifying our
Figure 1. Macaques can master supra-regular grammars. species [19].
(A) The formal language hierarchy categorizes computational systems at different levels of complexity. In conclusion, after more than a decade
Each small circle is a special case of the outer, enclosing circles. The outermost circle of all represents of searching, a paradigm allowing a
anything that is Turing-machine computable. Until now, high performance on supra-regular systems
was known only for humans. (B) A rhesus macaque working on a touchscreen illustrates the basic nonhuman species to break through the
paradigm developed by Jiang et al. [7], where a circular array of screen locations is used to encode ‘syntax barrier’ has finally been found.
grammars at varying levels of complexity. The monkeys in this study succeeded in mastering two While many other mechanisms are
complex grammars, at the supra-regular level, a first for nonhuman animals.
needed for full human language (including
both complex semantics and the neural
is to exclude the possibility that animals the one hand, the new results are bad control required for speech), one central
‘succeed’ on a supra-regular task by news for those who want to draw a strict component of syntax can now be
inferring ‘shortcuts’ or heuristics at the line separating humans from other explored at the neural level. Be sure to
finite-state level; animals have indeed animals. On the other hand, the fact that watch this space!
been demonstrated in several studies to monkeys require intensive training (tens of
adopt such alternative strategies [13,14]. thousands of trials) to successfully learn
REFERENCES
Although supra-regular, the AnBn the task, while children master it nearly
grammar underlying most of this previous perfectly with almost no training,
1. Fitch, W.T. (2010). The Evolution of Language
work has also been faulted for allowing suggests a major quantitative distinction (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).
strategies such as counting which have between humans and macaques in this
2. Stoeger, A.S., Mietchen, D., Oh, S., de Silva,
little relevance for the structural analyses cognitive domain. While humans may not S., Herbst, C.T., Kwon, S., and Fitch, W.T.
at the heart of human languages. be the only species capable of mastering (2012). An Asian elephant imitates human
The mirror grammar used by Jiang et al. supra-regular systems, we may have an speech. Curr. Biol. 22, 2144–2148.
[7] neatly avoids this problem, as simple unusually strong propensity to do so [15]. I 3. Knörnschild, M. (2014). Vocal production
counting or various finite-state shortcuts have previously dubbed this human learning in bats. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 28,
will not yield success. This grammar, propensity to infer tree structures 80–85.
combined with the novel experimental ‘dendrophilia’ [16]; the new work 4. Nowicki, S., and Searcy, W.A. (2014). The
paradigm, makes the new work a paragon suggests that macaques may be dendro- evolution of vocal learning. Curr. Opin.
of how to perform this type of animal competent with training, but not Neurobiol. 28, 48–53.

research. In addition to the mirror dendrophilic by nature. 5. Fitch, W.T., and Friederici, A.D. (2012).
grammar, the monkeys in this study Future research can proceed on two Artificial grammar learning meets formal
language theory: An overview. Philos. Trans.
learned two additional grammars, major pathways. The first concerns the
R. Soc. Lond. B 367, 1933–1955.
including a ‘repeat’ or ‘copy’ grammar generality of macaque’s capabilities: are
(requiring similar supra-regular resources they limited to this particular visuospatial 6. ten Cate, C. (2016). Assessing the uniqueness
of language: Animal grammatical abilities take
to the mirror grammar). One monkey was domain and manual output modality, or center stage. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 91–96.
able, impressively, to combine a spatial can macaques generalize to other
rule (‘progress around the circle’) with the systems (such as auditory sequences)? If 7. Jiang, X., Long, T., Cao, W., Li, J., Dehaene, S.,
and Wang, L. (2018). Production of supra-
mirror repetition, so that given ‘A’ it could supra-regularity turns out to be domain regular spatial sequences by macaque
produce the subsequent ‘BC’, followed specific, it may be that spatial cognition monkeys. Curr. Biol. 28, 1851–1859.
by the entire ‘CBA’ mirrored sequence. and/or motor control provided the original
8. Turing, A.M. (1937). On computable numbers,
Finally, to put these achievements into evolutionary function of supra-regularity, with an application to the
perspective, the researchers tested pre- later exploited and strengthened during Entscheidungsproblem. Proc. Lond. Math.
Soc. 42, 230–265.
schoolers aged 5–6 years. Human human evolution. Second, now that a task
children learned the tasks easily and exists where monkeys successfully 9. Ja€ger, G., and Rogers, J. (2012). Formal
almost instantly (requiring about five exhibit supra-regular abilities, language theory: refining the Chomsky
hierarchy. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 267,
demonstrations), and vastly out- neuroscientists can begin a full 1956–1970.
performed the monkeys. exploration of the neural mechanisms
This monkey/pre-schooler comparison involved (for example, with intracranial 10. Hopcroft, J.E., Motwani, R., and Ullman, J.D.
(2000). Introduction to Automata Theory,
suggests two rather different recording, functional magnetic resonance Languages and Computation, 2nd Edition
interpretations of the overall results. On imaging, and so on). Such research (Reading, Massachusetts: Addison-Wesley).

R696 Current Biology 28, R695–R717, June 18, 2018


Current Biology

Dispatches
11. Fitch, W.T., and Hauser, M.D. (2004). rules can explain discrimination of putative The evolution of the arcuate fasciculus
Computational constraints on syntactic recursive syntactic structures by a songbird revealed with comparative DTI. Nat. Neurosci.
processing in a nonhuman primate. Science species. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci USA 106, 11, 426–428.
303, 377–380. 20538–20543.
18. Schenker, N.M., Hopkins, W.D., Spocter,
12. Gentner, T.Q., Fenn, K.M., Margoliash, D., and 15. Miller, G.A. (1967). Project Grammarama. In
M.A., Garrison, A.R., Stimpson, C.D., Erwin,
Nusbaum, H.C. (2006). Recursive syntactic Psychology of Communication, G.A. Miller, ed. J.M., Hof, P.R., and Sherwood, C.C. (2010).
pattern learning by songbirds. Nature 440, (New York: Basic Books). Broca’s area homologue in chimpanzees
1204–1207.
16. Fitch, W.T. (2014). Toward a computational (Pan troglodytes): probabilistic mapping,
asymmetry and comparison to humans.
13. Ravignani, A., Westphal-Fitch, G., Aust, U., framework for cognitive biology: unifying
Schlumpp, M., and Fitch, W.T. (2015). More approaches from cognitive neuroscience and Cerebr. Cort. 20, 730–742.
than one way to see it: Individual heuristics in comparative cognition. Physics Life Rev. 11,
avian visual cognition. Cognition 143, 13–24. 329–364. 19. Fitch, W.T. (2018). What animals can teach us
about human language: The phonological
14. van Heijningen, C.A.A., de Vissera, J., 17. Rilling, J.K., Glasser, M.F., Preuss, T.M., Ma, continuity hypothesis. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci.
Zuidema, W., and ten Cate, C. (2009). Simple X., Zhao, T., Hu, X., and Behrens, T.E.J. (2008). 21, 68–75.

Division of Labor: How Microbes Split Their


Responsibility
Babak Momeni
Department of Biology, Boston College, Chestnut Hill, MA 02467, USA
Correspondence: momeni@bc.edu
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.024

Within a biofilm, individual cells might perform only a subset of activities required for overall success of the
biofilm. A new study examining matrix production, a task necessary for biofilm formation, shows possible
mechanisms of genetic or phenotypic division of labor.

Division of labor — subpopulations genetically clonal population gets divided It is challenging to tease apart how
performing different tasks simultaneously into subpopulations with drastically division of labor takes place in natural
within an assembly — is pervasive in different activities [12]. Genetically similar settings in which underlying mechanisms
biology. In multicellular organisms, cells in these biofilms differentiate into are often muddied by the multitude of
differentiation of cells into different tissues cells specialized for motility, matrix unknowns and uncertainties about
and organs is a clear example. Division of production, and sporulation, all of which individuals, interactions, and the
labor can happen among a group of are important for overall success of the environment. Therefore, in the new study,
individuals as well. In termite colonies, for biofilm. But how do the costs and benefits Drago s and colleagues [2] focus on
instance, queens and kings are of division of labor for each cell favor the B. subtilis as a tractable system to explain
specialized for reproduction, workers maintenance of such a scheme amongst the origination and maintenance of
forage and collect food, and soldiers individuals? division of labor. The capability to
primarily defend workers [1]. Division of Revealing the mechanisms of how monitor, control, and manipulate
labor is an important concept, allowing a division of labor is implemented in a subpopulations in this system offers a
better understanding of how complexity biological system is an important direct way to mechanistically explore the
arises and is maintained in biological question. Take persistent microbial maintenance of division of labor. Two
systems. A new study by Drago s et al. [2], infections as an example: synergy among techniques are primarily used: fluorescent
published in this issue of Current Biology, microbes through division of labor can markers are used to monitor the
aims to reveal the molecular mechanism make them more harmful to us. In chronic expression of relevant genes, and specific
used by a common soil bacterium and wounds, Pseudomonas aeruginosa and mutants are constructed to assess the
model system, Bacillus subtilis, to explain Staphylococcus aureus divide virulence impact of corresponding genes. This
the division of labor amongst cells tasks, making it harder for the immune combination allows them to examine both
engaged in biofilm matrix production. system or antibiotics to suppress them genetic and phenotypic aspects of
Microbial assemblies, especially within [13]. Similarly, in the lungs of humans with division of labor.
a biofilm, are already known to cystic fibrosis, the differentiation of In particular, Drago s et al. [2] focus on
demonstrate division of labor [3–11]. In P. aeruginosa is thought to make it the production of an extracellular matrix,
B. subtilis biofilms, for example, even a harder to treat the infection [14,15]. which is comprised of two major

Current Biology 28, R695–R717, June 18, 2018 ª 2018 Published by Elsevier Ltd. R697

Potrebbero piacerti anche