Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Journal of Traumatic Stress

April 2016, 29, 111–119

Numbing of Positive, Negative, and General Emotions: Associations


With Trauma Exposure, Posttraumatic Stress, and Depressive
Symptoms Among Justice-Involved Youth
Patricia K. Kerig, Diana C. Bennett, Shannon D. Chaplo, Crosby A. Modrowski, and Andrew B. McGee
Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, Utah, USA

Increasing attention has been drawn to the symptom of emotional numbing in the phenomenology of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD),
particularly regarding its implications for maladaptive outcomes in adolescence such as delinquent behavior. One change in the definition
of emotional numbing according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; DSM-5; American Psychiatric
Association, 2013) was the limitation to the numbing of positive emotions. Previous research with youth, however, has implicated
general numbing or numbing of negative emotions in PTSD, whereas numbing of positive emotions may overlap with other disorders,
particularly depression. Consequently, the goal of this study was to investigate whether numbing of positive emotions was associated with
PTSD symptoms above and beyond numbing of negative emotions, general emotional numbing, or depressive symptoms among at-risk
adolescents. In a sample of 221 detained youth (mean age = 15.98 years, SD = 1.25; 50.7% ethnic minority), results of hierarchical
multiple regressions indicated that only general emotional numbing and numbing of anger accounted for significant variance in PTSD
symptoms (total R2 = .37). In contrast, numbing of sadness and positive emotions were statistical correlates of depressive symptoms
(total R2 = .24). Further tests using Hayes’ Process macro showed that general numbing, 95% CI [.02, .45], and numbing of anger,
95% CI [.01, .42], demonstrated indirect effects on the association between trauma exposure and PTSD symptoms.

Emotional numbing is a specific posttraumatic symptom involvement (Allwood, Bell, & Horan, 2011; Becker & Kerig,
that has garnered increasing empirical attention. Research on 2011, 2014; Bennett, Kerig, Chaplo, McGee, & Baucom, 2014;
emotional numbing helped to inform the revised criteria for Gaylord-Harden, Dickson, & Pierre, 2015; Kerig & Becker,
the diagnosis of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in the 2010; Kerig & Bennett, 2013; Kerig, Bennett, Thompson, &
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th Becker, 2012). Numbing of emotions in the aftermath of trauma
ed.; DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013) may serve to dampen youths’ awareness of distressing emotions
by establishing that numbing should be considered separately (Kalisch et al., 2005; Orsillo, Theodore-Oklota, Luterek, &
from the symptoms of avoidance with which it was combined Plumb, 2007), but also might increase the likelihood that youth
in the DSM-IV-TR (APA, 2000). This distinction has been con- will externalize that distress and act out against others (Ford,
firmed in samples of adults (e.g., Ayer et al., 2011; King, Leskin, Chapman, Connor, & Cruise, 2012; Lansford et al., 2006).
King, & Weathers, 1998) as well as children and adolescents There are two mismatches, however, between the new
(e.g., Kassam-Adams, Marsac, & Cirilli, 2010; Saul, Grant, & DSM-5 criteria regarding numbing and the extant research base.
Carter, 2008; Wang, Long, Li, & Armour, 2011). In addition, The first mismatch is that the DSM-5 criteria limit the construct
emotional numbing has come to play a key role in theoretical of emotional numbing to the “persistent inability to experience
models attempting to explain the association between expo- positive emotions” (p. 272; italics added) such as happiness
sure to childhood trauma and maladaptive outcomes in ado- or love. In contrast, in keeping with the DSM-IV-TR’s more
lescence, particularly delinquent behavior and justice system encompassing definition of emotional numbing as involving a
“restricted range of affect” (p. 468), much of the empirical lit-
erature has investigated posttraumatic outcomes for both adults
This work was funded in part by a grant from the National Institute of Justice and adolescents that are associated with the general numbing of
(2014-90914-UT-IJ) to the first author.
emotions (Bennett & Kerig, 2014; Bennett et al., 2014; Flack
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patricia K. Jr, Litz, Hsieh, Kaloupek, & Keane, 2000; Orsillo et al., 2007;
Kerig, Department of Psychology, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT
84112. E-mail: p.kerig@utah.edu
Roemer, Orsillo, Borkovec, & Litz, 1998). This general emotio-
nal numbing has also been termed desensitization (Gaylord-
Copyright  C 2016 International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies. View

this article online at wileyonlinelibrary.com Harden, Cunningham, & Zelencik, 2011), detachment (Kalisch
DOI: 10.1002/jts.22087 et al., 2005), or emotion suppression (Kaplow, Gipson, Horwitz,

111
112 Kerig et al.

Burch, & King, 2013). Moreover, other research has estab- All study procedures were approved by the institutional re-
lished that there are significant associations between trauma view boards of the University of Utah and the Utah Department
exposure and the numbing of negative emotions amongst ado- of Human Services. At visitations to the detention center, all le-
lescents, such as sadness or fear (Allwood et al., 2011; Kerig gal guardians were invited to provide signed informed consent
et al., 2012). Therefore, clarification is needed as to whether encompassing all study procedures, after which their children
emotional numbing among at-risk youth in the aftermath of were invited to provide signed assent. To eliminate any percep-
trauma is specific to, and should be limited to, that of positive tions of coercion, no incentives were offered for participation.
emotions. Data were collected during individual interviews conducted by
A second mismatch between the revised diagnosis and a research assistant in a private room within the detention cen-
the empirical base is that, despite well-replicated research ter. Among legal guardians, 57% percent who were approached
demonstrating the superior fit of models that placed numbing ultimately consented to participate in the study, and 69% of
on its own separate factor in DSM-IV-TR (Bennett et al., 2014; youth assented to participate.
Elhai et al., 2011), in DSM-5 numbing was placed in a cluster
involving a large array of symptoms associated with changes in
Measures
cognition and mood in the aftermath of trauma. Recent studies
have called this symptom structure into question, instead The UCLA Posttraumatic Stress Disorder Reaction Index for
linking the numbing of positive emotions as defined by DSM-5 Children/Adolescents–DSM-5 (PTSD-RI; Pynoos & Steinberg,
to other symptoms indicative of anhedonia, such as detachment 2014) is a widely used screening tool to assess trauma exposure
and lack of interest, a finding that has been confirmed in and PTSS that has demonstrated good convergent validity with
samples of adults (Armour et al., 2015; Pietrzak et al., 2015) as other diagnostic measures, high internal consistency, and a con-
well as adolescents (Wang et al., 2015). Symptoms associated firmed factor structure that maps onto the DSM-5 criteria for
with anhedonia have considerable overlap with depression PTSD (Elhai et al., 2013; Steinberg et al., 2013). The first set
(Armour et al., 2012); moreover, it is notable that depression of questions establishes whether youth meet Criterion A for the
is highly comorbid with PTSD and differential diagnosis of PTSD diagnosis by asking whether they have been exposed in
the two disorders is challenging (Elhai et al., 2015; Friedman, their lifetime to any of a series of traumatic events. The number
2013). Thus, investigations are needed that help to clarify of events endorsed also is summed to create a Total Trauma
whether emotional numbing in the aftermath of trauma Exposure score.
exposure is related to symptoms of posttraumatic stress in Additional questions on the PTSD-RI ask youth to iden-
ways that are distinguishable from symptoms of depression. tify the “worst” stressor they have ever experienced and rate
To address these questions, the present study addressed three in relation to that event the extent to which they have expe-
specific aims: (a) to investigate whether in a sample of trauma- rienced in the past month symptoms associated with the four
exposed justice-involved youth the numbing of positive emo- symptoms clusters in the DSM-5 diagnosis of PTSD: intrusion
tions is associated with posttraumatic stress symptoms (PTSS) (Criterion B), avoidance (Criterion C), changes in cognition
above and beyond the numbing of general emotional numb- and mood (Criterion D), and arousal (Criterion E). In addition,
ing, numbing of negative emotions, or depressive symptoms; four questions on the PTSD-RI probe for the presence of dis-
(b) to examine whether, in contrast, the numbing of positive sociative symptoms involving experiences of derealization or
emotions is associated with youth depressive symptoms above depersonalization. Responses to the questions are presented in
and beyond other forms of emotional numbing; and (c) to test a Likert scale format ranging from 0 = none of the time to
whether different forms of emotional numbing have indirect 4 = most of the time and a rating of 3 or higher is considered to
effects on the association between trauma exposure and PTSS indicate symptom presence. A Total PTSS score is calculated
or depressive symptoms. as a sum of all symptom ratings across the clusters. In the cur-
rent sample, Cronbach’s α was .93. In addition, the PTSD-RI
allows for an assessment of the likelihood that youth meet full
Method or partial criteria for a current PTSD diagnosis according to the
DSM-5. Full PTSD is defined as an endorsement of Criterion A
Participants and Procedure
as well as the required number of symptoms for Criteria B, C,
Participants included 221 youth (63 girls, 158 boys) recruited D, and E, whereas partial PTSD is defined as an endorsement
from a short-term juvenile detention center (average length of Criterion A as well the required number of symptoms for
of stay = 4 days) located in the western United States. Con- any three of the other four symptom clusters.
sistent with the demographics of the larger justice-involved The Emotional Numbing and Reactivity Scale (ENRS; Or-
population in this geographical region, youth ranged in age sillo et al., 2007) is a self-report measure designed to assess
from 13 to 19 years (M = 15.98, SD = 1.25) and 49.3% were current experiences of numbness versus reactivity of emotional
White/Caucasian, 31.2% Hispanic/Latino, 4.1% Black/African responses to events or experiences. Subscales assess general
American, 2.7% Pacific Islander/ Native Hawaiian, 4.5% Native numbing (8 items, e.g., “I feel cut off from my emotions”),
American, and 8.1% multiracial/other. numbing of positive emotions (26 items, e.g., “I am unable to

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Numbing of Positive, Negative, or General Emotions 113

feel joy”) as well as numbing of the specific negative emotions In addition, to test other plausible hypotheses about the in-
of fear (6 items), sadness (12 items), and anger (10 items, e.g., terrelationships of the variables measured, a number of alterna-
“I have a hard time feeling ____ even when there are reasons for tive models were examined. First, to ensure that results found
me to feel that way”). Each item is rated on a 5-point Likert-type were not an artifact of the order of entry of the variables into
scale ranging from 1 = not at all typical of me to 5 = entirely the equation, alternative models were run for Aim 1 entering
typical of me. The developers report good internal consisten- the numbing variables and depression simultaneously against
cies, test-retest reliability, and construct validity. Evidence of PTSD symptoms, and for Aim 2, the numbing variables were
the reliability and validity of the self-report of emotional numb- entered simultaneously against depression; models were then
ing using this measure is indicated by its strong psychometrics rerun allowing positive numbing to be entered first in each
and demonstrated utility in a large body of research on adult equation. Second, models were tested with and without the de-
samples as well as its reliability and validity in previous re- mographic variables of ethnicity, age, and gender entered on
search involving adolescents (Allwood et al., 2011; Bennett & the first step of the model; any differences in the findings are
Kerig, 2014; Kerig et al., 2012). In the present sample, internal reported.
consistencies of the scales were as follows: General Numbing Finally, to examine indirect effects of numbing on the asso-
= .71; Positive = .86; Sadness = .83; Anger = .81; and Fear ciations between trauma exposure and the PTSS versus depres-
= .80. sion, we utilized Hayes’ (2013) PROCESS macro, which allows
The short form of the Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI; for the testing of direct and indirect effects while also providing
Kovacs, 1992) is an 11-item self-report, symptom-oriented bootstrapped estimates of the confidence interval (CI) around
scale that was designed for school-aged children and adoles- the indirect effect.
cents. The CDI assesses emotional problems and functional
severity of symptoms including negative mood/somatic symp-
toms, low self-esteem, interpersonal problems, and perceived Results
ineffectiveness. For each item, there are three choices describ-
Descriptive statistics indicated that 92.3% of the sample
ing the frequency with which the symptom is currently experi-
endorsed at least one lifetime traumatic event, thus meeting
enced (e.g., 0 = I am sad once in a while, 1 = I am sad many
Criterion A for the diagnosis of PTSD. On average, youth
times, 2 = I am sad all of the time). The author reports high
endorsed experiencing 6.47 different types of traumatic events
internal consistence, test-retest reliability, and construct valid-
(SD = 4.05) and the average length of time elapsed since
ity in both normative and clinical samples. Cronbach’s α in the
these events was 36.07 months (SD = 38.10). Furthermore,
current sample was .81.
113 youth (51.1%) likely met full criteria for current PTSD
based on their reports on the PTSD-RI. A significant gender
Data Analysis
difference emerged, with girls more likely than boys to
Data were analyzed using SPSS version 23.0. Missing data were likely meet diagnostic criteria (65.1% of girls vs. 45.6% of
addressed using listwise deletion for the subset of 53 youth boys), χ2 (1, N = 221) = 6.86, p = .015. An additional 43
who were not administered the CDI. For Aim 1, investigating youth (20.2%) met partial criteria for PTSD, with no gender
whether numbing of positive emotions explained unique vari- difference in the proportion of boys (5.3%) and girls (13.1%),
ance in Total PTSS after accounting for depressive symptoms χ2 (1, N = 221) = 2.65, p = .10.
and the numbing of general or negative emotions, we conducted Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations for the full
a 4-step hierarchical regression. To account for any variance re- sample are provided in Table 1. Tests for mean differences,
lated to the wide ranges of ethnicities and ages in this dataset, with equal variances not assumed, showed that in comparison
and because previous research has found gender differences in to boys, girls reported higher levels of depressive symptoms,
posttraumatic stress reactions in this population, ethnicity and t(76.77) = 3.45, p = .001, d = 0.62 and PTSS, t(94.62) = 2.71,
age were entered on the first step of the regression equation. De- p = .008, d = 0.43, including scores for Criterion C, t(211) =
pressive symptoms was entered on the second step, followed 2.58, p = .008, d = 0.40, and Criterion D, t(99.92) = 2.76,
on the third step by scores on general numbing, numbing of p = .007, d = 0.42. Compared to girls, boys reported higher
sadness, numbing of anger, and numbing of fear. In the final levels of numbing of sadness, t(218) = −4.56, p < .001,
step, numbing of positive emotions was added to the model. All d = 0.68 and numbing of fear, t(218) = −4.41, p < .001,
variables except ethnicity and gender were centered. For Aim d = 0.65. There were no significant differences between
2, investigating whether numbing of positive emotions was a boys and girls for Criterion B, Criterion E, trauma exposure,
unique correlate of depressive symptoms, we conducted a par- general numbing of emotions, numbing of anger, or numbing
allel 3-step hierarchical regression with depressive symptoms of positive emotions.
as the dependent variable. Again, all variables were centered, Among the youth in this sample, 9.95% endorsed the item
except gender and ethnicity. General numbing was entered next involving positive numbing on the PTSD-RI, and of these youth,
in the regression, followed by numbing of sadness, numbing of 40.9% likely met criteria for the diagnosis of PTSD on the
anger, and numbing of fear in the second step. In the third step, measure. Examination of the concordance between this item
numbing of positive emotions was added to the equation. and youths’ endorsement of positive numbing on the ENRS
Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
114

Table 1
Descriptive Statistics and Intercorrelations Among Study Variables
Variable M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1. Age (years) 15.98 1.25 –
2. Ethnicity – – −.03 –
3. Gender – – −.10 −.04 –
4. Numb–Pos 52.51 12.71 −.05 −.09 .01 –
5. Numb–Gen 17.44 5.20 −.07 .02 −.05 .56 –
6. Numb–Sad 26.63 8.01 −.05 .01 .30 .61 .29 –
7. Numb–Anger 22.93 7.34 .00 −.02 .08 .27 −.06 .35 –
8. Numb–Fear 17.83 8.80 .03 .02 .29 .44 .23 .74 .27 –
Kerig et al.

9. Depressive 8.80 3.40 −.07 .07 −.30 .09 .19 −.26 −.18 −.14 –
10. Trauma Exp 6.47 4.05 .21 .13 −.13 −.04 .15 −.04 −.16 .05 .07 –
11. Criterion B 7.94 5.74 .07 .10 −.13 −.01 .15 −.21 −.21 −.14 .28 .43 –
12. Criterion C 5.68 3.79 .12 .13 −.18 −.01 .21 −.20 −.27 −.17 .30 .31 .76 –
13. Criterion D 10.31 6.15 .14 .02 −.20 .21 .41 −.13 −.27 −.03 .50 .40 .63 .60 –
14. Criterion E 10.41 5.37 .14 .07 −.12 .05 .26 −.06 −.35 .01 .27 .43 .58 .51 .65 –
15. Total PTSD 37.39 20.43 .11 .10 −.20 .10 .34 −.17 −.31 −.08 .43 .46 .87 .80 .86 .81
Note. N = 221. Coefficients ࣙ .14 are significant at p < .05, > .20 are significant at p < .01, and above .27 are significant at p < .001. Numb–Pos = numbing of positive emotions; Numb–Gen =
general numbing; Numb–Sad = numbing of sadness; Numb–Fear = numbing of fear; Numb–Anger = numbing of anger; Depressive = depressive symptoms; Trauma Exp = total trauma exposure;
PTSD = posttraumatic stress disorder.

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Numbing of Positive, Negative, or General Emotions 115

Table 2
Summary of Multiple Regression Analyses of Symptoms of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder and Depression, Displaying Results at Final
Step
Variable B SE R2
PTSD symptoms
Step 1 .10**
Age 2.53 1.30
Ethnicity 1.00 1.23
Gender −10.25** 3.40
Step 2 .14***
***
Depressive symptoms 2.36 0.45
Step 3 .14***
***
General numbing 1.33 0.30
Numbing of sadness −0.46 0.27
Numbing of anger −0.40 0.20
Numbing of fear 0.40 0.35
Step 4 .00
Numbing of positive emotions 0.06 0.16
Depressive symptoms
Step 1 .10**
Age −0.32 0.22
Ethnicity 0.13 0.21
Gender −2.22*** 0.57
Step 2 .09**
*
General numbing 0.13 0.06
Numbing of sadness −0.14** 0.05
Numbing of anger −0.03 0.04
Numbing of fear 0.08 0.07
Step 3 .06**
**
Numbing of positive emotions 0.10 0.03
Note. For posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), N = 221. For depression, n = 152.
* p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001.

showed a correlation between the variables of r = .29, p < .01, than 1% of additional variance in Total PTSS after accounting
and that youth who endorsed the positive numbing item on the for the other variables, and only age, depression, general numb-
PTSD-RI reported significantly higher scores than their peers ing of emotions, and numbing of anger remained significant.
on the ENRS positive numbing scale, t(209) = −2.39, p = .02, The final model accounted for 37.2% of the variance in To-
d = .52. tal PTSS. All variables had acceptable variance inflation factor
As the results of the hierarchical regression reported in and tolerance values, indicating the absence of multicollinear-
Table 2 indicate, age and depressive symptoms were signifi- ity among the study variables (Cohen, Cohen, West, & Aiken,
cant correlates of Total PTSS in the 4-step regression. Specifi- 2003).
cally, the combination of age, gender, and ethnicity accounted When our model was challenged by comparison to an all-
for 9.6% of the variance in PTSS and depressive symptoms possible-subsets regression in which the numbing variables and
explained an additional 14% of the variance. The direction of depression were entered simultaneously after the inclusion of
effects showed that older age and higher depressive symptoms the demographic variables, results indicated no change in the
were associated with higher levels of PTSS. In the third step, results, with age, depression, general numbing, and numbing
general numbing and numbing of anger acted as additional of anger still emerging as the only significant correlates of
correlates of Total PTSS, altogether accounting for an addi- PTSD scores. Similarly, when the order of entry allowed posi-
tional 13.5% of the variance in Total PTSS scores. Notably, tive numbing to be entered first in the equation, followed by the
general numbing of emotions was positively associated with other numbing scores, and then depression, the same pattern of
increased PTSS scores, whereas numbing of anger was associ- results were obtained for the final model. When the regression
ated with decreased PTSS. In the fourth step, the addition of equations were rerun excluding the demographic variables from
numbing of positive emotions to the equation explained less the first step, results indicated only one change in the results, in

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
116 Kerig et al.

Positive
numbing

-0.79 (0.24)**
General
numbing
0.20 (0.09)* 0.92 (0.27)**

Trauma Numbing
-0.28 (0.13)* 0.92 (0.17)** PTSS
exposure of anger

Numbing
of fear
0.37 (0.14)**

Numbing
of sadness

1.81 (0.30)***

Figure 1. N = 221. Paths in the mediation model. Nonsignificant paths are displayed as dashed lines; significant paths are displayed as solid lines. Unstandardized
B coefficients (with standard errors in parentheses) are displayed for all significant paths. Indirect effects, significant for general numbing and numbing of anger,
are omitted from the figure for clarity, but are described in the text. PTSS = posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001.

that numbing of sadness also emerged as a significant negative trauma exposure and PTSS by general numbing, B = 0.19, 95%
correlate of PTSS, B = −0.64, SE = 0.31, p = .04, along with CI [0.02, 0.45], and numbing of anger, B = 0.15, 95% CI [0.01,
depression, general numbing, and numbing of anger. 0.42]. The combination of age, ethnicity, gender, and trauma
As reported in Table 2, in the 3-step hierarchical regression exposure accounted for 18.3% of the variance in general numb-
with depression as the dependent variable, results indicated that ing and 17.6% in numbing of anger. The total model explained
female gender was a significant correlate of depressive symp- 62.2% of the variance in PTSS.
toms in Step 1, with gender, age, and ethnicity accounting for A parallel model for the prediction of depression also was
10.2% of the variance in depression scores. In the second step, conducted; results indicated that none of emotional numbing
female gender, higher general numbing, and lower numbing scales emerged as having significant indirect effects on the
of sadness were additional significant correlates of depressive association between trauma exposure and depression.
symptoms, accounting for an additional 8.6% of the variance in
depression. In the third step, numbing of positive emotions
explained an additional 5.7% of the variance in depressive
Discussion
symptoms over and above the demographic and other emo-
tional numbing variables, and in this step, only lower numbing Consistent with a large body of accumulated research, the
of sadness and higher numbing of positive emotions remained justice-involved youth in this sample exhibited significant
significant predictors of depression. Altogether, the final model levels of trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress symp-
accounted for 24.4% of the variance in depressive symptoms. toms, providing further confirmation that this is a cohort of
Again, we examined competing models by comparing re- youth deserving of attention in the study of traumatic stress
sults achieved by different orders of entry of the variables into (Dierkhising et al., 2013; Ford, Chapman, Mack, & Pearson,
the equation. If the demographic variables were excluded, all 2006; Ford, Grasso, Hawke, & Chapman, 2013; Kerig, 2012).
numbing variables were entered simultaneously. If numbing Further, consistent with a large body of emerging research
of positive emotions was entered in a step prior to the other on youth in the justice system, gender differences emerged
numbing variables, the same pattern emerged, with only numb- with girls reporting higher levels of depression and PTSD and
ing of sadness and numbing of positive emotions significantly being more likely than boys to likely meet criteria for a PTSD
associated with depressive symptoms. diagnosis. Moreover, the prevalence of trauma exposure and
Our third aim was to investigate whether there were indi- PTSS in this sample provided us the opportunity to investigate
rect effects of emotional numbing on the association between an important question regarding the revised DSM-5 diagnostic
trauma exposure and the dependent variables of PTSS versus criteria for PTSD: whether the numbing of positive emotions
depression. In the model with PTSS as the dependent variable is associated with posttraumatic stress over and above the
(see Figure 1), bootstrapped indirect effects results were con- numbing of other emotional states. In addition, this highly
sistent with a partial indirect effect of the association between trauma-exposed sample allowed us to examine the relative

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Numbing of Positive, Negative, or General Emotions 117

contributions of numbing of positive, negative, and general other psychological problems commonly seen among youth
emotions to PTSS versus depression. in the juvenile justice system, such as callous-unemotional
Consistent with the lack of bivariate associations found be- traits and limited prosocial emotions, which have been newly
tween positive numbing and PTSS, the results of the regres- added as a specifier in the DSM-5 diagnosis of conduct disor-
sion analyses indicated that in this sample, numbing of positive der. Research has shown that emotional numbing in the after-
emotions did not explain variance in PTSS above and beyond math of trauma exposure is associated with delinquent behavior
the presence of depressive symptoms, the numbing of nega- among adolescents (Allwood et al., 2011), as well as elevated
tive emotions, or general emotional numbing. Instead, general scores on measures of callous-unemotional traits (Bennett &
emotional numbing and the numbing of the negative emotion Kerig, 2014; Kerig et al., 2012). Consequently, further re-
of anger were the facets of numbing that were most strongly search exploring the boundary between posttraumatic numbing
associated with PTSS. These results suggest the possibility that and youth callousness is warranted, as is care to differenti-
the narrowing of the definition of emotional numbing to include ate the two symptoms in screenings and assessments designed
only positive emotions in the DSM-5 criteria might not accu- to connect justice-involved youth to appropriate mental health
rately characterize the aftermath of trauma exposure in samples services.
such as the justice-involved youth studied here. The results of this study also should be considered in light
In contrast, in this sample, the numbing of positive emotions of its limitations. The present sample was drawn from a ge-
did explain unique variance in depressive symptoms, above ographic region with a proportional representation of ethnic
and beyond other forms of emotional numbing. These findings groups that may not generalize to other areas; in particular,
suggest the possibility that numbing of positive emotions may there were fewer African American youth in the present sample
be more indicative of co-occurring depressive symptoms than than are represented in the juvenile justice system in other lo-
PTSS specifically, in concert with other investigations that have calities in the United States. Only a subset of eligible guardians
pointed to the overlap of dysphoric symptoms in the new DSM- and youth elected to participate in the study, and they may dif-
5 criteria for PTSD and depression (e.g., Armour et al., 2012; fer from other members of this population in ways we could
Elhai et al., 2015; Friedman, 2013). The present study might not assess. Another major limitation is that these data were
help to suggest a future step for research devoted to disentan- gathered cross-sectionally. Although the measures adminis-
gling depression and PTSD by pointing toward the restricted tered suggest a sequential ordering in that all youth reported
definition of emotional numbing in DSM-5 as a potential cul- on past traumatic events and current posttraumatic symptoms,
prit in the overlap between the disorders. Although difficulty the fact that measures were taken at the same time point prevents
experiencing positive emotions is a symptom that co-occurs our drawing causal conclusions. In addition, PTSD symptoms
with depressive disorders, what may be unique to PTSD are were assessed using the youth-report version of the PTSD-RI,
difficulties regarding the ability to experience a range of emo- which does not include an assessment of functional impair-
tions. Interestingly, among the youth in the present sample, ment. Although the high rates of psychiatric diagnoses among
it was reduced numbing of anger that had indirect effects on detained youth generally (i.e., 45% of boys and 30% of girls;
the association between trauma exposure and PTSS. Theoret- Teplin et al., 2015) are suggestive of the presence of func-
ical models of the underlying mechanisms linking trauma to tional impairment in this population, future research should
delinquency have posited that the experience of victimization include measures that allow for the assessment of all criteria
in the absence of validation, support, or rescue may leave youth for the diagnosis of PTSD. A further limitation is that all of
with a powerful but impotent sense of anger and injustice (Ford the data were gathered via youth self-report and via a single
et al., 2006). Attempts to emotionally numb this unarticulated measure for each construct. Future research would benefit from
rage might serve adolescents as an adaptation strategy that, al- the inclusion of reports from knowledgeable observers, such
though perhaps associated with problem behaviors that bring as family members or friends, and the inclusion of multiple
these youth to the attention of the juvenile justice system, also measures obtained over time. Another limitation of this study
serves a protective function against posttraumatic stress reac- is its use of a PTSD symptom to estimate the likelihood of a
tions (Lansford et al., 2006). PTSD diagnosis. Finally, the sample was limited to youth in-
The results of this study also suggested that, despite its an- volved in the juvenile justice system and the results may not
nexation to a DSM-5 cluster involving a variety of other PTSD generalize to other adolescent or adult populations. Although
symptoms, emotional numbing continues to warrant attention justice-involved youth comprise a specific cohort, it is not an
as a specific posttraumatic reaction. For example, research has insignificant one. The number of adolescents involved in the
indicated that adolescents and adults high in emotional numb- justice system in the United States is substantial, with over
ing are among those whose functioning is most impaired by 1.7 million youth processed by the courts each year (Office of
posttraumatic stress (Asmundson, Stapleton, & Taylor, 2004; Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, 2011) and over
Saul et al., 2008) and that emotional numbing can act as a 85,000 youth held in detention settings on any given day
significant impediment to the effectiveness of evidence-based (Mendel, 2011). Furthermore, the vast majority of these youth
interventions for PTSD (Taylor et al., 2001). In addition, behav- are trauma-exposed and exhibit significant symptoms of PTSD
ioral expressions of emotional numbing may also overlap with (see Kerig & Becker, 2012). Nonetheless, this is a group with

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
118 Kerig et al.

specific characteristics and risk factors that may have affected Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple
the present findings. regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum.
In conclusion, the results of this study suggest that further
consideration of the role of emotional numbing in posttraumatic Dierkhising, C. B., Ko, S. J., Woods-Jaeger, B., Briggs, E. C., Lee, R., &
stress reactions is warranted. In particular, at least for some pop- Pynoos, R. S. (2013). Trauma histories among justice-involved youth: Find-
ings from the National Child Traumatic Stress Network. European Journal
ulations, such as justice-involved youth, the numbing of general of Psychotraumatology, 4. doi:10.3402/ejpt.v4i0.20274
rather than positive emotions may play the more salient role in
posttraumatic stress. Given that the DSM-5 criteria limit emo- Elhai, J. D., Biehn, T. L., Armour, C., Klopper, J. J., Frueh, B. C., &
Palmieri, P. A. (2011). Evidence for a unique PTSD construct represented
tional numbing to experiences of positive affect, youth who en- by PTSD’s D1–D3 symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 340–345.
gage in other forms of emotional numbing might be overlooked doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.10.007
for services. Effective evidence-based interventions have been
Elhai, J. D., Contractor, A. A., Tamburrino, M., Fine, T. H., Cohen, G., Shirley,
developed for youth with co-occurring posttraumatic stress and E., & Galea, S. (2015). Structural relations between DSM-5 PTSD and major
justice involvement (see Ford, Kerig, Desai, & Feierman, in depression symptoms in military soldiers. Journal of Affective Disorders,
press); therefore, our accurately identifying these youth and re- 175, 373–378. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.034
ferring them for treatment promises to have a beneficial impact Elhai, J. D., Layne, C. M., Steinberg, A. M., Brymer, M. J., Briggs, E. C.,
on the adolescents, their families, and their communities. Ostrowski, S. A., & Pynoos, R. S. (2013). Psychometric properties of
the UCLA PTSD Reaction Index. Part II: Investigating factor structure in
a national clinic-referred youth sample. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26,
10–18. doi:10.1002/jts.21755
References Flack Jr, W. F., Litz, B. T., Hsieh, F. Y., Kaloupek, D. G., & Keane, T. M.
Allwood, M. A., Bell, D. J., & Horan, J. (2011). Posttrauma numbing of (2000). Predictors of emotional numbing, revisited: A replication and exten-
fear, detachment, and arousal predict delinquent behaviors in early adoles- sion. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 13, 611–618. doi:10.1023/A:100780613
cence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 40, 659–667. 2319
doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.597081
Ford, J. D., Chapman, J., Connor, D. F., & Cruise, K. R. (2012). Complex
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of trauma and aggression in secure juvenile justice settings. Criminal Justice
mental disorders (4th ed., text rev.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric and Behavior, 39, 694–724. doi:10.1177/0093854812436957
Association.
Ford, J. D., Chapman, J., Mack, J. M., & Pearson, G. (2006). Pathways from
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual traumatic child victimization to delinquency: Implications for juvenile and
of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: Author. permanency court proceedings and decisions. Juvenile and Family Court
Journal, 57, 13–26. doi:10.1111/j.1755-6988.2006.tb00111.x
Armour, C., Elhai, J. D., Richardson, D., Ractliffe, K., Wang, L., &
Elklit, A. (2012). Assessing a five factor model of PTSD: Is dyspho- Ford, J. D., Grasso, D. J., Hawke, J., & Chapman, J. F. (2013). Poly-
ric arousal a unique PTSD construct showing differential relationships victimization among juvenile justice-involved youths. Child Abuse & Ne-
with anxiety and depression? Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 368–376. glect, 37, 788–800. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2013.01.005
doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2011.12.002
Ford, J. D., Kerig, P. K., Desai, N., & Feierman, J. (in press). Psychosocial
Armour, C., Tsai, J., Durham, T. A., Charak, R., Biehn, T. L., Elhai, J. interventions for traumatized youth in the juvenile justice system: Research
D., & Pietrzak, R. H. (2015). Dimensional structure of DSM-5 post- evidence base and clinical/legal challenges. Journal of Juvenile Justice.
traumatic stress symptoms: Support for a hybrid anhedonia and exter-
nalizing behaviors model. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 61, 106–113. Friedman, M. J. (2013). Finalizing PTSD in DSM-5: Getting here from
doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.012 there and where to go next. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26, 548–556.
doi:10.1002/jts.21840
Asmundson, G. J. G., Stapleton, J. A., & Taylor, S. (2004). Are avoidance and
numbing distinct PTSD symptom clusters?. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17, Gaylord-Harden, N., Cunningham, J., & Zelencik, B. (2011). Effects of expo-
467–475. doi:10.1007/s10960-004-5795-7 sure to community violence on internalizing symptoms: Does desensitization
to violence occur in African American youth? Journal of Abnormal Child
Ayer, L. A., Cisler, J. M., Danielson, C. K., Amstadter, A. B., Saunders, B. Psychology, 39, 711–719. doi:10.1007/s10802-011-9510-x
E., & Kilpatrick, D. G. (2011). Adolescent posttraumatic stress disorder: An
examination of factor structure reliability in two national samples. Journal Gaylord-Harden, N. K., Dickson, D., & Pierre, C. (in press). Profiles of com-
of Anxiety Disorders, 25, 411–421. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2010.11.004 munity violence exposure among African American youth: An examination
of desensitization to violence using latent class analysis. Journal of Inter-
Becker, S. P., & Kerig, P. K. (2011). Posttraumatic stress symptoms are personal Violence. doi:10.1177/0886260515572474
associated with the frequency and severity of delinquency among de-
tained boys. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 40, 1–7. Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional
doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.597091 process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press.

Bennett, D. C., & Kerig, P. K. (2014). Investigating the construct of Kalisch, R., Wiech, K., Critchley, H. D., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. E., Oakley,
trauma-related acquired callousness among delinquent youth: Differ- D., & Dolan, R. J. (2005). Anxiety reduction through detachment: Subjec-
ences in emotion processing. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 27, 415–422. tive, physiological, and neural effects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience,
doi:10.1002/jts.21931 17, 874–883. doi:10.1162/0898929054021184

Bennett, D. C., Kerig, P. K., Chaplo, S. D., McGee, A. B., & Baucom, Kaplow, J. B., Gipson, P. Y., Horwitz, A. G., Burch, B. N., & King, C. A. (2013).
B. R. (2014). Validation of the five-factor model of PTSD symptom Emotional suppression mediates the relation between adverse life events
structure among delinquent youth. Psychological Trauma, 6, 438–447. and adolescent suicide: Implications for prevention. Prevention Science, 15,
doi:10.1037/a0035303 177–185. doi:10.1007/s11121-013-0367-9

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Numbing of Positive, Negative, or General Emotions 119

Kassam-Adams, N., Marsac, M. L., & Cirilli, C. (2010). Posttraumatic stress Orsillo, S. M., Theodore-Oklota, C., Luterek, J. A., & Plumb, J. (2007). The
disorder symptom structure in injured children: Functional impairment development and psychometric evaluation of the Emotional Reactivity and
and depression symptoms in a confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Numbing Scale. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 830–836.
the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 616–625. doi:10.1097/NMD.0b013e318156816f
doi:10.1097/00004583-201006000-00010
Pietrzak, R. H., Tsai, J., Armour, C., Mota, N., Harpaz-Rotem, I., & South-
Kerig, P. K. (2012). Trauma and juvenile delinquency: Dynamics and devel- wick, S. M. (2015). Functional significance of a novel 7-factor model
opmental mechanisms. Journal of Child and Adolescent Trauma, 5, 83–87. of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms: Results from the National Health and Re-
doi:10.1080/19361521.2012.671743 silience in Veterans Study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 174, 522–526.
doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.007
Kerig, P. K., & Becker, S. P. (2010). From internalizing to externalizing:
Theoretical models of the processes linking PTSD to juvenile delinquency. Pynoos, R. S., & Steinberg, A. M. (2014). UCLA PTSD Reaction Index for
In S. J. Egan (Ed.), Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Causes, symptoms Children/Adolescents–DSM-5. University of California at Los Angeles. Los
and treatment (pp. 1–46). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science. Angeles.

Kerig, P. K., & Becker, S. P. (2012). Trauma and girls’ delinquency. In S. Miller, Roemer, L., Orsillo, S. M., Borkovec, T. D., & Litz, B. T. (1998). Emotional
L. D. Leve, & P. K. Kerig (Eds.), Delinquent girls: Contexts, relationships, response at the time of a potentially traumatizing event and PTSD symp-
and adaptation (pp. 119–143). New York, NY: Springer. tomatology: A preliminary retrospective analysis of the DSM-IV Criterion
A-2. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 29, 123–
Kerig, P. K., & Bennett, D. C. (2013). Beyond fear, helplessness, 130. doi:10.1016/s0005-7916(98)00007-x
and horror: Peritraumatic reactions associated with posttraumatic stress
symptoms among traumatized delinquent youth. Psychological Trauma: Saul, A. L., Grant, K. E., & Carter, J. S. (2008). Post-trumatic reactions in
Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 5, 431–438. doi:10.1037/a002 adolescents: How well do the DSM-IV PTSD criteria fit the real life experi-
9609 ence of trauma exposed youth? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36,
915–925. doi:10.1007/s10802-008-9222-z
Kerig, P. K., Bennett, D. C., Thompson, M., & Becker, S. P. (2012). “Nothing
really matters”: Emotional numbing as a link between trauma exposure and Steinberg, A. M., Brymer, M. J., Kim, S., Briggs, E. C., Ippen, C. G, Os-
callousness in delinquent youth. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 25, 272–279. trowski, S. A., . . . Pynoos, R. S. (2013). Psychometric Properties of the
doi:10.1002/jts.21700 UCLA PTSD Reaction Index: Part I. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 26(1), 1–9.
doi:10.1002/jts.21780
King, D. W., Leskin, G. A., King, L. A., & Weathers, F. W. (1998). Confirmatory
factor analysis of the clinician-administered PTSD Scale: Evidence for the Taylor, S., Fedoroff, I. C., Koch, W. J., Thordarson, D. S., Fecteau, G., &
dimensionality of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Assessment, Nicki, R. M. (2001). Posttraumatic stress disorder arising after road traf-
10, 90–96. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.90 fic collisions: Patterns of response to cognitive–behavior therapy. Journal
of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 541–551. doi:10.1037/0022-
Kovacs, M. (1992). Children’s Depression Inventory manual. Toronto, Canada: 006X.69.3.541
Multi-Health Systems.
Teplin, L., Welty, L. J., Abram, K. M., Dulcan, M. K., Washburn, J. J., McCoy,
Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Stevens, K. I., Dodge, K. A., Bates, J. E., & Pettit, K., & Stokes, M. L. (2015). Psychiatric disorders in youth after detention.
G. S. (2006). Developmental trajectories of externalizing and internalizing Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs,
behaviors: Factors underlying resilience in physically abused children. De- Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention.
velopment and Psychopathology, 18, 35–56. doi:10.1017/S095457940606
0032 Wang, L., Long, D., Li, Z., & Armour, C. (2011). Posttraumatic stress disorder
symptom structure in Chinese adolescents exposed to a deadly earthquake.
Mendel, R. A. (2011). No place for kids: The case for reducing juvenile Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39, 749–758. doi:10.1007/s10802-
incarceration. Baltimore, MD: Annie E. Casey Foundation. doi:http:// 011-9508-4
hdl.handle.net/10244/1035
Wang, L., Zhang, L., Armour, C., Cao, C., Qing, Y., Zhang, J., & Fan, G. (2015).
Office of Juvenile Justic and Delinquency Prevention. (2011). Statistical Assessing the underlying dimensionality of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms in
briefing book. Retrieved from http://www.ojjdp.gov/ojstatbb/court/qa06201. Chinese adolescents surviving the 2008 Wenchuan earthquake. Journal of
asp?qaDate=2008. Anxiety Disorders, 31, 90–97. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.02.006

Journal of Traumatic Stress DOI 10.1002/jts. Published on behalf of the International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.
Copyright of Journal of Traumatic Stress is the property of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. and its
content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the
copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email
articles for individual use.

Potrebbero piacerti anche