Sei sulla pagina 1di 13

Japanese Psychological Research doi: 10.1111/jpr.

12129
2016, Volume 58, No. 4, 342–354

Consumers’ Response to an Advertisement


Using Brand Popularity in a Foreign Market

JI-HERN KIM Sejong University

DONGWON MIN* Dankook University

Abstract: This research examined how domestic consumers respond to an adver-


tisement using brand popularity in a foreign market. By using structural equation
modeling, it shows that the consumers’ response can change as a function of three
characteristics of brand popularity in a foreign market: (a) expertise of foreign consu-
mers, (b) similarity between domestic consumers and foreign consumers, and
(c) animosity to a foreign country. Meanwhile, the similarity effect is found to be
stronger for high-preference heterogeneity than low-preference heterogeneity. This
research documents the importance of understanding the underlying mechanism to
determine the effects of brand popularity in a foreign market on brand attitude and
purchase intention.
Key words: brand popularity, expertise, similarity, animosity, preference
heterogeneity.

Increasing numbers of global companies have (Gmarket). This might be in part due to the
recently used brand popularity in a foreign lower price of MILFORD than Nestlé. How-
market (BPFM) as an advertising cue when ever, because stilltee would not be a price-
they enter a new market. For example, to sensitive item for nursing mothers, MILFORD
overcome the second-mover disadvantages might not overtake Nestlé without popularity
over Nestlé in the Korean stilltee market, claims, such as “No. 1 sales in German
MILFORD used their brand popularity in the market.”
German market as an advertising cue. Stilltee Although BPFM has received more atten-
is a popular herbal tea among European nurs- tion by practitioners, little is known about its
ing mothers, because it is supposedly helpful effect on domestic consumer behavior. Previ-
for breastfeeding by increasing the amount of ous studies have shown that brand popularity
breast milk. Given the fact that nursing is an important factor positively affecting
mothers tend to be very risk-averse consu- brand evaluation (Kim & Min, 2014). Most of
mers, they are more likely to prefer a product these studies, however, have been conducted
made by a well-known company (Nestlé). The in a domestic context. Specifically, they have
sales of MILFORD stilltee, however, were investigated how brand popularity in a domes-
found to be higher than those of Nestlé stilltee tic market affects a domestic consumer’s pur-
in the biggest Korean Internet mall chase decision-making process.

*Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to: Dongwon Min, Department of Business Adminis-
tration, College of Business, Dankook University, 126, Jukjeon-dong, Su-ji, Yong-in, Gyeonggi-do, Seoul 448-
701, Korea. (E-mail: dwmin@dankook.ac.kr)

© 2016 Japanese Psychological Association. Published by John Wiley & Sons Australia, Ltd.
Effect of brand popularity in a foreign market 343

BPFM has a distinctive aspect. The effect of factor influencing consumer attitudes towards
brand popularity on consumer behavior can an advertisement and a brand, and purchase
change according to the characteristics of the intention (Feick & Higie, 1992). The similarity
country referenced in a popularity claim. For effect would be higher for high-preference het-
example, what if MILFORD had used Mexico erogeneity than for low-preference
(e.g., “No. 1 sales in the Mexican market”) heterogeneity.
instead of Germany in a popularity claim? Given the discussion above, this research
The domestic consumers’ response to the was conducted with two main objectives for a
advertisement might have been quite different. better understanding of the underlying mech-
This indicates that the underlying mechanism anism determining the BPFM effects. First, it
to determine the effect of BPFM on consumer aims to examine how three characteristics of
behavior needs to be examined. BPFM (expertise, similarity, animosity) affect
Based on the studies about source effects in consumer responses to an advertisement
persuasion, BPFM effects should depend on (brand attitude and purchase intention). The
the expertise of the foreign consumers refer- second objective is to investigate how the simi-
enced in a popularity claim and similarity larity effect can be moderated by preference
between domestic consumers and the foreign heterogeneity.
consumers (Biswas, Biswas, & Das, 2006).
According to source credibility theory, when
foreign consumers referenced by BPFM are Theoretical Background
perceived to be knowledgeable about a partic-
and Research Model
ular product, domestic consumers are likely to
respond to BPFM positively. Also, according Brand Popularity in a Foreign Market
to source attraction theory, when an unfamil- Brand popularity is defined as the extent to
iar product is endorsed by similar foreign con- which a brand is chosen and purchased by the
sumers rather than dissimilar ones, domestic general population (Kim & Chung, 1997).
consumers are more likely to positively evalu- Brand popularity has a positive impact on con-
ate a product advertised with BPFM sumer behaviors, which can be explained with
(Ibok, 2013). a norm theory (Cialdini, 2003). Social norms
Meanwhile, animosity towards a foreign consist of injunctive norms and descriptive
country in a popularity claim can be another norms. While past research has focused prima-
important factor influencing domestic consu- rily on examining the effect of injunctive
mers’ responses to BPFM. Klein, Ettenson, norms on individual behavior, descriptive
and Morris (1998) first introduced the concept norms have received more attention recently.
of animosity in consumer research, presenting Unlike injunctive norms, which concern what
the finding that it has a negative impact on most people approve of, descriptive norms
purchase intention in the global market place. involve what most people do. Individuals tend
Given that consumers tend not to buy a prod- to take advantage of descriptive norms as a
uct related to a country towards which they shortcut in a decision-making process because
have animosity (Nes, Yelkur, & Silkoset, it is perceived to be less risky and more sensi-
2012), domestic consumers would negatively ble (Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990). Brand
respond to BPFM if they had negative images popularity is related to a descriptive norm,
associated with the foreign country. and thus it can positively affect brand attitude
Besides the source characteristics, character- and purchase intention (Kim & Min, 2014).
istics of the advertised products may affect Meanwhile, most previous studies used mar-
domestic consumers’ responses to an adver- ket share as a proxy for brand popularity and
tisement using BPFM. Especially, preference examined its effect on brand evaluation. It was
heterogeneity (the extent of variation in con- shown that a brand with high market share is
sumer preferences) is known to be a critical perceived to have good quality because a

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


344 J.-H. Kim and D. Min

manufacturer would be forced to reflect the Regardless of the level of expertise actually
improvement requirements from a great num- possessed by a source, the message receiver’s
ber of customers (Liebowitz & Margolis, perception of expertise can change the
1994). However, brand popularity could nega- response to a persuasive message. In sum-
tively affect brand evaluation because of the mary, the perceived level of topic-specific
congestion problem and the loss of exclusivity expertise of the source determines the expert
(Hellofs & Jacobson, 1999). The congestion endorsement effect (Biswas et al., 2006).
problem involves the decrease in utility from According to source credibility theory, con-
overuse of a product. For example, the speed sumers perceive a persuasive message to be
of a computer network decreases as the num- credible when a source has expertise as well as
ber of users grows (Liebowitz & Margolis, trustworthiness (Erdogan, 1999). Thus, they
1994). In the case of a symbolic product, a tend to accept and agree with an opinion from
prestigious image might be lost due to wide- an expert and trustworthy source (Silvera &
spread popularity. Thus, brand popularity pos- Austad, 2004). Previous studies on endorser
itively affects brand evaluation only when effects in an advertisement have mainly
congestion problems and loss of exclusivity focused on expertise rather than trustworthi-
are not critical. ness (Biswas et al., 2006). Expert endorse-
Given this discussion, BPFM is expected to ments enhance the credibility of an
have a positive effect on brand attitude and advertisement, and thus accelerate attitude
purchase intention at the early stages of mar- change (Berry & Shield, 2014).
ket entry of a foreign brand. First, BPFM Consumers referenced by BPFM may play a
hardly results in a congestion problem. role as endorsers of a product in a communi-
Domestic consumers would not worry about a cation. When domestic consumers perceive
decrease in their utility from overuse of a that foreign consumers referenced by BPFM
brand by foreign consumers. Additionally, are knowledgeable about an advertised prod-
BPFM is free from loss of exclusivity due to uct, they are likely to believe that foreign con-
the geographic distance between the domestic sumers are credible and thus evaluate a
and foreign consumers. Moreover, consumers popularity cue more positively. For example,
may infer that a brand can be improved by consumers may believe that French consumers
reflecting feedback from a great number of have expertise in perfumes. Accordingly, if
foreign consumers. In summary, BPFM as an domestic consumers see an advertisement that
advertising cue would be expected to posi- touts “the best-selling perfume in France,”
tively affect brand attitude and purchase they would evaluate the product endorsed by
intention. expert groups positively and have higher
intention to buy it. This leads to the following
Effects of Three Characteristics of BPFM Hypothesis 1.
on Consumer Behavior Hypothesis 1: The expertise of consumers
referenced by BPFM would positively affect
Expertise. Expertise is defined as the brand attitude (Hypothesis 1a: H1a) and pur-
extent to which a communicator is perceived chase intention (Hypothesis 1b: H1b).
to have an ability to make a valid assertion
about a particular topic (Biswas et al., 2006). Similarity. Similarity is defined as the
It accrues from a source’s experience, knowl- extent of resemblance between the communi-
edge, or skill (Silvera & Austad, 2004). The cator and the recipient of a message
level of expertise to influence source effect in (Erdogan, 1999). The level of similarity can be
persuasion is assessed topic-specifically rather assessed, based on various dimensions, such as
than at a general level (Birnbaum & Stegner, age, gender, ethnicity, lifestyle, physical
1979). Moreover, it does not matter whether a appearance, ideology (or values), and product
source is a true expert or not (Erdogan, 1999). usage (Amos, Holmes, & Strutton, 2008). The

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Effect of brand popularity in a foreign market 345

importance of similarity in endorser effects in it (Klein et al., 1998). Recently, some


has been emphasized in previous studies on research contended that animosity negatively
psychology, marketing, communication, and affects product evaluation (Nes et al., 2012).
international business studies (Ibok, 2013). Leong et al. (2008) mentioned that cognitive
Kelman (1961) argued that consumers tend and affective evaluations should be distin-
to change their attitude through a psychologi- guished when analyzing the effects of animos-
cal process of identification when they find ity on product judgment. Then, they revealed
similarity between a source and themselves. that animosity had a greater impact on affec-
According to source attraction theory, consu- tive evaluation than on cognitive evaluation
mers automatically assess the level of similar- across five countries.
ity with a source during communication, which Animosity is not a unidimensional construct;
determines the persuasive effectiveness of the it consists of multi-dimensions. Leong
message (Silvera & Austad, 2004). Consumers et al. (2008) argued that animosity can be
tend to perceive that similar sources are divided into two dimensions: situational and
attractive and credible, and thus adopt their stable animosity. The former involves hostility
argument. Additionally, Aaker, Brumbaugh, induced by a specific circumstance and/or a
and Grier (2000) mentioned that a similar particular event, and thus is a temporary emo-
source in an advertisement is likely to make tion compared to the latter. The latter refers to
consumers perceive that they are the targeted long-lasting hostility driven by an accumulation
audience for the advertisement, which elicits a of situational animosity (Jung et al., 2002). Nes
positive attitude towards an advertised brand. et al. (2012) conducted a critical review of
Given this discussion, when consumers per- research on animosity and classified it as four
ceive that foreign consumers referenced by a dimensions: economic animosity, people ani-
popularity claim have characteristics similar to mosity, military/war animosity, and politics/
their own, they would be expected to positively government animosity. Then, they examined
evaluate an advertisement and the advertised the effects of the four dimensions on purchase
brand. For example, Korean consumers may intention through psychosocial affect. Eco-
perceive that Japanese consumers have more nomic and people animosity were found to
similarity in coffee consumption than Chinese have the greatest effects on purchase intention.
consumers do. Accordingly, if Korean consu- Taken altogether, this research includes ani-
mers see the advertisement that touts “best- mosity to a country referenced by a popularity
selling coffee in Japan” versus in China, brand claim as an important factor affecting con-
attitude and purchase intention would be sumer responses to an advertisement. Animos-
higher. This leads to the following Hypothesis 2. ity is measured with items related to economic
Hypothesis 2: Similarity between domestic con- and people animosity because they have
sumers and foreign consumers referenced by strong effects on consumer behavior. When
BPFM would positively affect brand attitude consumers are exposed to an advertisement
(Hypothesis 2a: H2a) and purchase intention using BPFM, animosity would negatively
(Hypothesis 2b: H2b). affect brand attitude and purchase intention.
This leads to the following Hypothesis 3.
Animosity. Klein et al. (1998) defined ani- Hypothesis 3: Animosity to a foreign country
mosity toward a foreign country as “the rem- referenced by a popularity claim would nega-
nants of antipathy related to previous or tively affect brand attitude (Hypothesis 3a: H3a)
ongoing military, political, or economic and purchase intention (Hypothesis 3b: H3b).
events” (p. 90). It has a negative impact on
purchase intention regardless of product judg- The Moderating Effect of Preference
ment. Although consumers perceive that a Heterogeneity
brand has a high quality, they are not likely to Preference heterogeneity is defined as variation
buy it because hostile countries are involved in consumer tastes and preferences for a

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


346 J.-H. Kim and D. Min

product (or service) (Feick & Higie, 1992). It is The findings of these two studies led to an
said to be high when consumers have wide vari- inference on the interaction effect between
ation and no consensus in product evaluation. similarity and preference heterogeneity on
Preference heterogeneity arises from the fact brand attitude and purchase intention. How-
that each consumer has idiosyncratic criteria ever, it is noteworthy that the studies did not
and standards about the mix of attributes or measure participants’ perception of prefer-
amount of each attribute (Price, Feick, & Higie, ence heterogeneity. More importantly, a
1989). For example, when three consumers buy comparison between the high-heterogeneity
and drink the same beer, they may report their group and the low-heterogeneity group was
experience differently, such as great, average, not conducted. Because each consumer is
and terrible. It comes from the underlying dif- likely to perceive the level of preference het-
ference in evaluating the taste of the beer. erogeneity differently, the group split and
The effect of similarity on brand attitude comparison according to individual percep-
and purchase intention may be stronger for tions of heterogeneity need to be conducted
high-preference heterogeneity than for low- for correctly analyzing preference heteroge-
preference heterogeneity. That is, for high- neity effects.
preference heterogeneity, consumers are Given the above discussion, this research
more likely to positively evaluate and pur- postulates that similarity effects would be
chase a brand that is endorsed by a similar stronger for high-preference heterogeneity
source than by a dissimilar one. Suppose that than low-preference heterogeneity.1 This leads
you are planning to have a family trip and to the following Hypothesis 4.
make a reservation with a hotel. You may Hypothesis 4: The effect of similarity on brand
try to get useful information for choosing a attitude (Hypothesis 4a: H4a) and purchase
hotel. When it comes to choosing a hotel, intention (Hypothesis 4b: H4b) would be
people may have idiosyncratic criteria stronger for high-preference heterogeneity
(e.g., price, location, cleanness) and thus than for low-preference heterogeneity.
preference heterogeneity is high. If one of The four hypotheses mentioned so far are
your friends considers that cleanness is the summarized in Figure 1.
most important factor like you, you may
have a big chance to accept his/her recom-
mendation. This similarity effect is not
Method
important for choosing toilet paper, which
has relatively low preference heterogeneity. Development of Advertisements
Wang (2005) examined the effects of expert Touting Brand Popularity in a Foreign
and regular consumer endorsements on the Market
evaluation of a movie, and revealed that regu- To develop advertisements using BPFM, eight
lar consumer endorsements are more influen- Korean undergraduate students (female: 4)
tial on consumers’ intention to watch a movie. had interviews with a researcher. Eight candi-
A movie belongs to a product category with date stimuli were evaluated and then “Hima-
high-preference heterogeneity, and thus the laya Hand Cream” was selected as a stimulus
similar source effect may be magnified. The
importance of a similar source effect was also
1
found in Silvera and Austad’s (2004) study When preference heterogeneity is extremely high,
consumers would hardly depend on popularity cues
where the stimulus product was a fragrance. and make a decision to buy a product, based on their
This result might also be due to a high- own preference, intuition, and sense of value. Since
preference heterogeneity. Specifically, consu- this research did not consider the extreme case,
mers are likely to have idiosyncratic tastes in hand cream was used as a stimulus product. The
extreme case needs to be considered in future
perfume, and thus they tend to depend on a research. We appreciate an anonymous reviewer for
similar source in brand evaluation. this insightful comment.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Effect of brand popularity in a foreign market 347

product in the advertisements because partici- By revising an original advertisement of


pants (irrelevant of gender) were familiar with Himalaya hand cream used in a real Internet
a product category but were not aware of the mall, four different versions of advertisements
brand name. Moreover, “Himalaya” is known were developed with variation in foreign coun-
as the most famous local cosmetics brand with tries referenced by a popularity claim (see the
high quality in India. However, participants Appendix).
were found to have negative images (e.g., low
quality) associated with cosmetics made in Sampling and Procedure
India. This indicates that Himalaya must be an A total of 225 Korean undergraduate students
appropriate stimulus brand because marketers participated in the survey, and elimination of
may need to use BPFM in launching it in coun- incomplete responses left 216 eligible for anal-
tries where a negative country-of-origin ysis. Participants were randomly exposed to
(COO) effect may occur. one of four advertisements (Japan = 54,
Four countries were selected as foreign mar- China = 53, France = 55, and India = 54).
kets used in popularity claims because consu- Then, they were instructed to imagine that
mers are expected to perceive three they decided to buy a hand cream and saw the
characteristics of BPFM (i.e., expertise, simi- advertisement during Internet shopping. Sub-
larity, and animosity) about them differently. sequently, participants were instructed to
The nations were Japan, China, France, and respond to the items for measuring six con-
India. Specifically, participants perceived that structs included in the research model (see
the French and Japanese have stronger exper- Figure 1). Demographics were measured last,
tise in hand cream than the Chinese and and participants were debriefed.
Indians. In addition, the Japanese and Chinese
were perceived to have more similarity in Measures
hand cream usage than the French and Six constructs in a measurement model were
Indians. Finally, participants were found to scored on a 7-point scale and are shown in
have stronger animosity to Japan and China Table 1. Cronbach’s alpha for all of the items
than to the other two countries. These varia- to measure each of six constructs is higher
tions in three characteristics across countries than .7, and thus scale reliability is secured.
led to the easiness of analyzing their effects on Except for purchase intention, all the con-
brand attitude and purchase intention. structs were measured on a 7-point Likert
scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly
agree).
Expertise and similarity were measured with
four and three items, respectively. The seven
items were adapted from a study by Feick and
Preference
Heterogeneity Higie (1992). As mentioned in a literature
Expertise review, animosity consists of many dimensions
in BPFM H1a H4b H4a
but people and economic animosity have the
Brand
H1b
Attitude strongest impact on consumer behavior (Nes
H2a et al., 2012). Accordingly, animosity was meas-
Similarity
in BPFM ured with five items adapted from a study by
H2b
Klein et al. (1998), one item for people ani-
Purchase
H3a Intention mosity and four items for economic animosity.
Animosity Brand attitude was measured with four items
in BPFM H3b
about quality, reliability, perfection, and safety
adapted from a study by Häubl (1996). Pur-
Figure 1. The conceptual model and hypotheses. Note. chase intention was measured with four items
BPFM = brand popularity in a foreign market. adapted from a study by Gill, Gorssbart, and

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


348 J.-H. Kim and D. Min

Table 1 Operational measures of constructs in a measurement model


Standardized
Operational measures M SD loading t-value

Expertise (α = .917)
E1: (The Japanese) know a variety of hand cream brands 3.48 1.44 .82 -
E2: (The Japanese) seem to have a good sense about a hand 3.43 1.38 .82 14.38
cream
E3: (The Japanese) are knowledgeable about a hand cream 3.21 1.36 .94 17.57
E4: (The Japanese) have a lot of experience using hand cream 3.49 1.56 .86 15.35
Similarity (α = .923)
S1: (The Japanese) and I probably have similar values and beliefs 3.21 1.45 .83 -
about hand cream
S2: (The Japanese) are quite a bit like me in hand cream usage 3.23 1.46 .94 21.74
S3: It’s likely that (the Japanese) and I have similar tastes and 3.12 1.44 .91 17.29
preferences in hand cream
Animosity (α = .881)
A1: I dislike (the Japanese) 2.69 1.56 .79 8.57
A2: (Japan) is not a reliable trading partner 2.92 1.56 .91 9.53
A3: (Japan) wants to gain economic power over Korea 3.65 1.68 .90 14.98
A4: (Japan) has too much economic influence in Korea 3.45 1.72 .63 15.15
A5: (Japan) is doing business unfairly with Korea 2.97 1.38 .57 -
Brand attitude (α = .867)
BA1: Himalaya is a reliable product 4.31 1.26 .88 -
BA2: Himalaya is a good-quality product 4.16 1.26 .92 17.55
BA3: Himalaya is a flawless product 3.34 1.18 .64 10.48
BA4: Himalaya is a safe product 3.94 1.39 .72 12.41
Purchase intention (α = .928)
PI1: Very unlikely 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very likely 3.53 1.38 .86 -
PI2: Very improbable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very probable 3.43 1.40 .92 18.55
PI3: Very impossible 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very possible 3.84 1.40 .89 17.70
PI4: Very non-existent 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Very existent 3.95 1.37 .83 15.50
Preference heterogeneity (α = .788)
PH1: Most people want the same thing from a hand cream 5.72 1.22 .86 -
PH2: Tastes and preferences are not important in how people 5.89 1.19 .88 10.19
choose a hand cream
PH3: Each consumer has idiosyncratic criteria and standards in 5.38 1.37 .55 7.88
evaluation of a hand cream

Note: Four foreign consumers or countries are in brackets.

Laczniak (1988). Preference heterogeneity was endogenous constructs (brand attitude and
measured with three items adapted from a purchase intention), and a moderating con-
study by Price et al. (1989). struct (preference heterogeneity). The results
of CFA showed an acceptable goodness-of-fit
of the measurement model (χ2(215) = 403.05,
GFI = .86, AGFI = .82, RMSEA = .06, CFI =
Results
.95, IFI = .95). As presented in Table 1, the
Measurement Model standardized factor loadings of all items were
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was con- higher than .5 and their coefficients were sta-
ducted to examine discriminant and conver- tistically significant, indicating convergent
gent validity of three exogenous constructs validity (Bagozzi, Yi, & Phillips, 1991). Fur-
(expertise, similarity, and animosity), two thermore, as presented in Table 2, the

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Effect of brand popularity in a foreign market 349

Table 2 Correlation matrix


1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Expertise –
2. Similarity .48 (.13)** –
3. Animosity .13 (.10) .09 (.12) –
4. Brand attitude .30 (.10)** .34 (.11)** .06 (.09) –
5. Purchase intention .33 (.11)** .31 (.12)** −.06 (.10) .63 (.12)** –
6. Preference heterogeneity .07 (.09) −.06 (.10) −.03 (.09) .16 (.09)* .11 (.09) –

*p < .05. **p < .01.

confidence interval (2 SE) around the corre- BPFM had statistically significant impacts on
lation coefficients between two constructs does brand attitude and purchase intention, and the
not include 1.0, and thus discriminant validity directions were as hypothesized. Second,
is also secured (Anderson & Gerbing, 1988). Hypothesis 2 was partially supported. Similar-
ity between domestic consumers and foreign
Structural Model consumers positively affected brand attitude
The three hypotheses (Hypothesis 1–Hypothe- whereas its impact on purchase intention was
sis 3) in the research model were tested by not statistically significant. This indicates that
structural equation modeling. The goodness-of- similarity has an indirect impact on purchase
fit statistics were found to be acceptable intention through brand attitude. Finally,
(χ2(160) = 322.767, GFI = .872, AGFI = .832, Hypothesis 3 was also partially supported.
RMSEA = .069, CFI = .949, IFI = .950), and Animosity negatively affected purchase inten-
all the hypotheses were fully or partially tion while it did not have an impact on brand
supported. attitude. That is, animosity has a direct impact
First, Hypothesis 1 was fully supported. The on purchase intention regardless of brand
expertise of foreign consumers referenced by

E1 Preference
BA1 BA2 BA3 BA4
Heterogeneity
E2
Expertise
E3 in BPFM 0.18*

E4
0.15* Brand
Attitude
S1
0.25**
S2 Similarity
0.58***
in BPFM
S3 0.06

A1
Purchase
0.01
A2 Intention
A3 Animosity
-0.12*
in BPFM
A4

A5 PI1 PI2 PI3 PI4

Figure 2. The results of structural equation modeling. *p < .05 **p < .01. ***p < .001.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


350 J.-H. Kim and D. Min

Table 3 The differences between countries in consumers’ response to an advertisement using


brand popularity in a foreign market
Expertise Similarity Animosity Purchase intention

M SE M SE M SE M SE

France 3.69 .16 3.55 .18 2.07 .13 4.04 .16


Japan 4.28 .15 3.73 .18 3.59 .16 3.90 .16
China 3.08 .15 3.13 .17 4.40 .11 3.26 .18
India 2.55 .14 2.32 .14 2.53 .11 3.54 .17

attitude. The results of the hypotheses testing an advertisement using BPFM were analyzed
are presented in Figure 2. (see the Table 3). The results of analysis of var-
iance and post-hoc tests by the LSD method
Multi-group Comparison: The Moderating showed that all of the differences of expertise
Effect of Preference Heterogeneity between countries were significant, F
To test the moderating effect of preference (3, 212) = 24.12, p < .001, India had a signifi-
heterogeneity on the relationship between cantly lower score on similarity than other
similarity and consumer responses to an countries, F(3, 212) = 13.81, p < .001, all of the
advertisement using BPFM (Hypothesis 4), differences of animosity between countries
sub-group analysis was conducted. Participants were significant, F(3, 212) = 67.12, p < .001,
were divided into two subgroups, a high- and France had a significantly higher score on
preference heterogeneity group (n = 112) and purchase intention than China and India while
a low-preference heterogeneity group Japan had a significantly higher score than only
(n = 104), according to the result of a mean- China, F(3, 212) = 4.42, p < .01.
split (M = 5.67) on preference heterogeneity.
The objective of the subgroup analysis was to
examine whether the effects of similarity on
brand attitude (Hypothesis 4a) and purchase Discussion
intention (Hypothesis 4b) were equal across both Hypothesis 1 was fully supported. However,
groups, respectively. First, the result of χ2 differ- the other hypotheses were partially supported,
ence comparison between the constraint model which was unexpected. The partial support of
(χ2(321) = 574.506) and the free model Hypothesis 2 shows that similarity affects pur-
(χ2(320) = 571.334) revealed that the difference chase intention through brand attitude. This
between the coefficients of two paths (i.e., from indicates that when the advertisement uses a
similarity to brand attitude) was marginally signif- similar source in a popularity claim, it needs to
icant (Δχ2(1) = 3.172, p < .1). Thus, Hypothesis 4a provide additional information for consumers
was supported. However, Hypothesis 4b was not to positively affect brand attitude (e.g., high
supported because the coefficients of two paths quality). Given that Hypothesis 4b was not
(i.e., from similarity to purchase intention) was supported, the enhancement of quality percep-
not significantly different (χ2(321) = 573.320, tion is critical, especially for a product cate-
χ2(320) = 571.334, Δχ2 (1) = 1.986, p > .1). In sum- gory with high-preference heterogeneity
mary, Hypothesis 4 was partially supported. (vs. low-preference heterogeneity).
The partial support of Hypothesis 3 reveals
The Differences between Countries in that animosity has a direct impact on purchase
Consumers’ Response to an Advertisement intention, but not through brand attitude. This
Using BPFM result is consistent with the findings of Klein
Although it is not hypothesized, the differences et al. (1998) that animosity does not affect
between countries in consumers’ response to product judgment but that it negatively affects

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Effect of brand popularity in a foreign market 351

willingness to buy directly. Given Leong required conditions because it is proclaimed


et al.’s (2008) arguments that animosity affects based on sales in foreign countries and thus it
cognitive evaluation more than affective evalu- is not heavily related to the issues on exclusiv-
ation, this research measured brand attitude ity and the congestion problem. Therefore, the
with cognitive items and thus Hypothesis 3a finding of this research that BPFM has a posi-
may not be supported. tive impact on brand attitude supports the
Total effect (TE) analysis was conducted to argument by Hellofs and Jacobson (1999).
compare the three characteristics of BPFM This research extends the scope of extant
(expertise, similarity, and animosity) in the research on effect of a persuasive message
magnitude of the effect on purchase intention. conveying descriptive norm on consumer
The TE was calculated by aggregating the behaviors. Recently, some psychologists
direct effect between the characteristics of emphasized the role of a descriptive norm in
BPFM and purchase intention and the indirect persuasion (Goldstein, Cialdini, & Griskevi-
effect that the characteristics of BPFM have cius, 2008). Kim and Min (2014) mentioned
on purchase intention through brand attitude. that a popularity claim can be a type of
Purchase intention is most influenced by descriptive norm to affect brand evaluation.
expertise (TE = .252), followed by similarity However, they did not examine the effect of
(TE = .144), and animosity (TE = −.118). brand popularity on purchase intention. It was
These findings imply that expertise among the assumed based on the result that brand popu-
three characteristics of BPFM is most impor- larity affects perceived quality and risk percep-
tant in enhancing purchase intention. tion. This research overcomes the limitation
by investigating the effect of brand popularity
on purchase intention.
Meanwhile, Biswas et al. (2006) revealed
Conclusion
that for high-technology oriented products
Theoretical and Managerial Implications (e.g., computers), expert endorsers rather than
By testing four hypotheses regarding brand similar endorsers are more influential in redu-
popularity in a foreign country, this research cing consumers’ risk perception, while for low-
provides researchers with a number of theo- technology oriented products (e.g., treadmills)
retical implications. First, to our knowledge, there is no difference between the two endor-
this is the first reported study to investigate sers. This research used a hand cream as a
the characteristics of BPFM to determine con- stimulus product, which may belong to the
sumer responses to an advertisement. low-technology oriented product category. It
Although retailers often use brand popularity was found that there was no difference
as an advertising cue, little research has been between similar and expert endorsers in
conducted to examine its effects on consumer effects on brand attitude (Similar = .25,
behavior (Kim & Min, 2014). Furthermore, Expert = .18; t = −.117), which is consistent
the brand popularity effect was mostly exam- with the findings of Biswas et al. (2006).
ined in a domestic context (Dean, 1999). Interestingly, however, an expert endorser
Accordingly, little is known about the BPFM was found to have a stronger effect on pur-
effect on consumer behavior. This research chase intention than a similar endorser
addresses that gap by examining the effects of (Expert = .25 vs. Similar = .14), although the
three characteristics of BPFM on brand atti- difference cannot be statistically verified
tude and purchase intention. because structural equation modeling does not
Meanwhile, Hellofs and Jacobson (1999) offer a way of comparing the total effect. This
mentioned that brand popularity positively result is not consistent with the findings of Bis-
affects brand evaluation only when exclusivity was et al. (2006), indicating that their argu-
of a product is not critical and the congestion ments need to be approached with more
problem does not occur. BPFM satisfies the caution.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


352 J.-H. Kim and D. Min

This research also provides practitioners consumers’ preference heterogeneity. Specifi-


with important practical implications. It is cally, marketers have to emphasize how simi-
noteworthy that domestic consumers respond lar consumers referenced by BPFM are with
to an advertisement differently according to domestic consumers in using a product with
foreign countries referenced by brand popu- high-preference heterogeneity.
larity and thus marketers need to be cautious
in selecting a referent country for the develop- Limitations and Future Research
ment of an advertisement using BPFM. As This research has three major limitations that
presented in Table 3, when France is used as a should be rectified in future research. First, this
referent country in an advertisement, Korean research does not consider the COO effect in a
consumers’ intention to purchase the Hima- research model. Overall country image can be a
laya hand cream is the highest (M = 4.04), fol- counteracting force against the effects of exper-
lowed by Japan (M = 3.90), India (M = 3.54), tise, similarity, and animosity on a consumer’s
and China (M = 3.26). This indicates that response to an advertisement with a popularity
Himalaya should use an advertisement con- claim (Paswan & Sharma, 2004). For example,
veying brand popularity in France (No. 1 sales Korean consumers may perceive that Indian
in the French market) or Japan (No.1 sales in consumers have a low level of expertise with a
the Japanese market) rather than in India as a car product. Moreover, the perceived similarity
country of origin. in car consumption may be low. This indicates
More importantly, Himalaya should not that “No.1 sales in India” should not be an
use China as an endorser in an advertise- effective advertising cue for a foreign car brand.
ment because it did not obtain a higher However, if the Korean consumers notice that
score on purchase intention than India. the car is made in Japan, they may have high
China had a higher score for both expertise brand attitude and purchase intention because
and similarity than India, but it also had the of the technology advancement level of COO.
highest score for animosity, which may That is to say, COO image may have an influ-
induce the lowest purchase intention. ence on the effect of BPFM on consumer beha-
Accordingly, marketers must pay special viors. Therefore, COO image should be
attention to animosity (especially, people and controlled in a future research model.
economic animosity) as well as expertise and Second, “No. 1 sales in a foreign country” was
similarity when devising a communication used as an advertising cue to examine domestic
strategy using BPFM. consumers’ response to BPFM. However, mar-
The findings of this research suggest that keters may have difficulty in using this cue
three characteristics of BPFM need to be con- because of the prerequisite condition that sales
sidered in determining the order of entering a of a brand must be the highest in the market.
new market. Specifically, before launching a Accordingly, other popularity cues may be used
brand in other markets, marketers may launch as endorsements. For example, “Hit product”
it in countries that could be good endorsers can be a good alternative in that a brand does
(high expertise/similarity and low animosity) not have to be best-selling in the market.
and try to get good feedback from consumers According to Kim and Min (2014), both the
in these countries. Then, they could develop “No. 1 in sales” and “Hit product” cues have an
an advertisement using good brand perfor- impact on the reduction of risk perception.
mance in these countries as endorsements However, the former is more influential than the
when launching a brand in other markets. latter because it is a stronger argument about
Meanwhile, given that similarity has higher brand popularity. Future research needs to com-
total effects on purchase intention for high- pare the relative magnitude of the effects of two
preference heterogeneity than for low- cues regarding BPFM on purchase intention.
preference heterogeneity, marketers need to Finally, the results of this research should
develop an advertisement to reflect target not be generalized without caution, owing to

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


Effect of brand popularity in a foreign market 353

using limited samples (students), stimulus Feick, L., & Higie, R. A. (1992). The effects of pref-
product (hand cream), and foreign countries erence heterogeneity and source characteristics
referenced by brand popularity. Especially, on ad processing and judgments about endor-
sers. Journal of Advertising, 21(2), 9–24.
future research needs to use different stimulus
Gill, J. D., Gorssbart, S., & Laczniak, R. N. (1988).
products that differ in technology orientation Influence of involvement, commitment and
and preference heterogeneity. familiarity on brand beliefs and attitudes of
viewers exposed to alternative ad claim strate-
gies. Journal of Advertising, 17(2), 33–43.
Goldstein, N. J., Cialdini, R. B., & Griskevicius, V.
References (2008). A room with a viewpoint: Using social
Aaker, J. L., Brumbaugh, A. M., & Grier, S. A. norms to motivate environmental conservation in
(2000). Nontarget markets and viewer distinc- hotels. Journal of Consumer Research, 35,
tiveness: The impact of target marketing on 472–482.
advertising attitudes. Journal of Consumer Psy- Häubl, G. (1996). A cross-national investigation of
chology, 9, 127–140. the effects of country of origin and brand name
Amos, C., Holmes, G., & Strutton, D. (2008). on the evaluation of a new car. International
Exploring the relationship between celebrity Marketing Review, 13(5), 76–97.
endorser effects and advertising effectiveness: Hellofs, L. L., & Jacobson, R. (1999). Market share
A quantitative synthesis of effect size. Interna- and customers’ perception of quality: When can
tional Journal of Advertising, 27, 209–234. firms grow their way to higher versus lower
Anderson, J. C., & Gerbing, D. W. (1988). Struc- quality? Journal of Marketing, 63(1), 16–25.
tural equation modeling in practice: A review Ibok, N. I. (2013). Factors determining the effective-
and recommended two-step approach. Psycho- ness of celebrity endorsed advertisements: The
logical Bulletin, 103, 411–423. case of Nigerian telecommunication industry.
Bagozzi, R. P., Yi, Y., & Phillips, L. W. (1991). Asses- American Journal of Business and Management,
sing construct validity in organizational research. 2, 233–238.
Administrative Science Quarterly, 36, 421–458. Jung, K., Ang, S. H., Leong, S. M., Tan, S. J.,
Berry, T. R., & Shield, C. (2014). Source attribution Pornpitakpan, C., & Kau, A. K. (2002). A typology
and credibility of health and appearance exer- of animosity and its cross-national validation. Jour-
cise and advertisements: Relationship with nal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 525–539.
implicit and explicit attitudes and intentions. Kelman, H. C. (1961). Processes of opinion change.
Journal of Health Psychology, 19, 242–252. Public Opinion Quarterly, 25, 57–78.
Biswas, D., Biswas, A., & Das, N. (2006). The differ- Kim, C. K., & Chung, J. Y. (1997). Brand popular-
ential effects of celebrity and expert endorse- ity, country image and market share: An empir-
ments on consumer risk perceptions. Journal of ical study. Journal of International Business
Advertising, 35(2), 17–31. Studies, 28, 361–386.
Birnbaum, M. H., & Stegner, S. E. (1979). Source Kim, J.-H., & Min, D. (2014). The effects of
credibility in social judgments: Bias, expertise, brand popularity as an advertising cue on per-
and the judge’s point of view. Journal of Per- ceived quality in the context of internet shopping.
sonality and Social Psychology, 37, 48–74. Japanese Psychological Research, 56, 309–319.
Cialdini, R. B. (2003). Crafting normative messages Klein, J. G., Ettenson, R., & Morris, M. D. (1998).
to protect the environment. Current Directions The animosity model of foreign product pur-
in Psychological Science, 12, 105–109. chase: An empirical test in the people’s Republic
Cialdini, R. B., Reno, R. R., & Kallgren, C. A. of China. Journal of Marketing, 62(1), 89–100.
(1990). A focus theory of normative conduct: Leong, S. M., Cote, J. A., Ang, S. H., Tan, S. J.,
Recycling the concept of norms to reduce litter- Jung, K., Kau, A. K., & Pornpitakpan, C.
ing in public places. Journal of Personality and (2008). Understanding consumer animosity in
Social Psychology, 58, 1015–1026. an international crisis: Nature, antecedents, and
Dean, D. H. (1999). Brand endorsement, popularity, consequences. Journal of International Business
and event sponsorship as advertising cues Studies, 39, 996–1009.
affecting consumer pre-purchase attitudes. Jour- Liebowitz, S. J., & Margolis, S. E. (1994). Network
nal of Advertising, 28(3), 1–12. externality: An uncommon tragedy. Journal of
Erdogan, B. Z. (1999). Celebrity endorsement: A lit- Economic Perspectives, 8, 133–150.
erature review. Journal of Marketing Manage- Nes, E. B., Yelkur, R., & Silkoset, R. (2012).
ment, 15, 291–314. Exploring the animosity domain and the role of

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.


354 J.-H. Kim and D. Min

affect in a cross-national context. International Silvera, D. H., & Austad, B. (2004). Factors predict-
Business Review, 21, 751–765. ing the effectiveness of celebrity endorsement
Paswan, A. K., & Sharma, D. (2004). Brand-country advertisements. European Journal of Marketing,
of origin (COO) knowledge and COO image: 38, 1509–1526.
Investigation in an emerging franchise market. Wang, A. (2005). The effects of expert and consumer
Journal of Product & Brand Management, 13, endorsements on audience response. Journal
145–155. of Advertising Research, 45, 402–412.
Price, L. L., Feick, L. F., & Higie, R. A. (1989).
Preference heterogeneity and coorientation as (Received December 10, 2015; accepted June 22, 2016)
determinants of perceived information influ-
ence. Journal of Business Research, 19, 227–242.

Appendix

Sales No.1 in Japanese Hand Cream Market

01 Moisturizer for rough and dry hands

A super creamy balm with oils of Almond, Olive and


Sunflower protect your skin. This intensive cream
nourishes skin so it’s noticeably smoother after each
luxurious application.

02 Refreshingly light texture

The non-greasy and quick absorbing leaves hands soft and


smooth and can be applied as often as needed, providing
long lasting hydration.

03 80 years of history on natural herbs

Himalaya Herbals is a range of 100% natural and safe


products with rare herbs collected from the foothills of the
Himalayas.

© Japanese Psychological Association 2016.

Potrebbero piacerti anche