Sei sulla pagina 1di 18

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/321341590

Understanding the Profoundly Gifted

Presentation · November 2017


DOI: 10.13140/RG.2.2.15385.93282

CITATIONS READS

0 1,535

1 author:

Colleen Farrelly
Jenzabar
41 PUBLICATIONS   63 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

Various network metric and topological data analysis extensions (mostly industry, probably another paper in early 2018) View project

Promoting Diversity in Tech/Data Science View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Colleen Farrelly on 28 November 2017.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Colleen M. Farrelly
Gifted individuals
are typically defined  IQ is theorized to be
as the top few normally-
percent on IQ distributed.
measurements.
 Tests typically set
the test average as
100, with standard
deviations of 15
(most tests).
 This means that
~68% of people fall
within 1 standard
deviation of the
mean.
 Very few people are
thought to exist in
~95% of population the tails.
are within 30 points of
average.
 Several definitions of gifted exist, with
definitions forming a hierarchy. IQ Level of Theoretical
 Higher scores become rarer and rarer under Giftedness Prevalence
the normal distribution. 120 Most gifted ~1 in 11
 Many students within a school system are programs’
pretty bright and qualify as gifted under
school definition (IQ>120). definition
 Mensa-level intelligence occurs at a much 132 Mensa ~ 1 in 50
lower rate (1 in 50).
135 Top 1% ~1 in 100
 Much variation occurs in the top 1% of the IQ
distribution. 145 Highly gifted ~1 in 750
 A teacher who teaches 25 students per year for 30
years likely has taught ~1 high gifted student.
160 Exceptionally/ ~ 1 in 30,000
 That same teacher has 1:40 odds of teaching a
profoundly
student with IQ>160 during that same tenure. gifted
 A population the size of the US would be expected 180 Profoundly ~1 in 20,000,000
to have ~17 individuals at IQ>180.
gifted
 But, deviations from the normal distribution
exist in the right tail…
 Recent studies have shown that profoundly gifted individuals exist at much
higher rates than expected under the normal distribution.
 This is particularly true for those with uneven talents, where only one ability is at
the profoundly gifted level (IQ>160 in that ability).
 About 1 in 10,000 people are profoundly gifted on nonverbal measurements (proxy for
mathematical talent); ~35,000 are thought to exist in the US (likely higher given skilled
immigrants).
 About 1 in 30,000 people are verbally gifted at this level, or ~12,000 individuals in the
US.
 Profound giftedness in multiple areas is rarer but occurs at a much higher level
than expected under the normal distribution.
 Estimates of profound verbal and nonverbal giftedness put this rate at ~1 in 100,000; the
average IQ in previous studies is >190 for most individuals at this level.
 Studies considering other talents, such as spatial or musical ability, put estimates of
profound giftedness across abilities at ~1 in 250,000+.
 ~1,500-4,000 are expected in the US (likely closer to 4,000 given skilled immigrants).
 Very few modern tests can
differentiate ability at this level,
with most tests having a ceiling of
150-160.
 The old Stanford-Binet LM ratio-
based IQ test has a higher ceiling.
 Some extended measures exist for
modern IQ tests (SB-V, WISC-
IV…).
 Talent searches are a common
way to measure deviance IQ at
the profoundly gifted levels in
children and adolescents:
 Scores of >700 on SAT math or
verbal prior to age 13
 ACT reading >34, writing >32,
math >24, or science >30 at ages
12-14
 At lower levels of intelligence,
people generally solve problems
and learn material similarly.
 Those at the lower gifted levels:
 Require fewer repetitions to learn
 Solve problems faster
 Those at the profoundly gifted
level (IQ>160-170):
 Solve problems very differently
 Store knowledge very differently

 Recent studies suggest this


difference in kind is related to a
difference in brain connectivity
patterns (white matter tracts).
 Extreme need for mental stimulation  Preference/ability to think in analogies
 Example: voracious reading in subject of  Translates into the ability to process and
interest or across many subjects explain complex material intuitively
 Combining steps in problem-solving  Interconnecting knowledge
into one large step  Knowledge webs vs. knowledge filing
 Can cause issues in mathematics, cabinets when assimilating new
particularly as children (long division, knowledge
multiplication…)
 Projection of self into a problem
 Intensity of existing personality traits  Example: visualizing a mathematics
 Dabrowski’s overexcitabilities, problem and literally “walking through
amplification of traits like extraversion it” to a solution
or openness to experience  Example: imagining oneself as a novel
character in a particular situation to
 Divergent thinking write a scene of fiction
 Examples: filling in the blank in h_r
with “helicobactor” or “honor” rather  The simple is complex; the complex is
than “her” simple.
 Asynchronous development:
 A child may be mentally 14,
physically 7, and socially 10.
 Learning rates may vary within
and across subjects.
 A child may be at a 4th grade level in
math/science and a 2nd grade level in
the humanities.
 A child may go through a year’s
worth of math in 1 month but a
year’s worth of literature in 2
months (and a year’s worth of social
studies in a weekend).
 This makes meeting the needs of
profoundly gifted learners
challenging, particularly in an age-
based classroom.
 Acceleration
 This can involve moving up in a single subject or
across subjects (whole grade acceleration).
 Radical acceleration involves moving up multiple
grades, either within a subject or across subjects.
 Longitudinal studies suggest most students benefit
academically, socially, and motivationally from
acceleration, including radical acceleration.
 Accelerated students also tend to have higher adult-
level achievement in their fields.
 Many accelerated students are satisfied with their
acceleration, with most dissatisfied students
preferring more acceleration.

 However, many teachers and administrations


don’t welcome acceleration.
 Some believe it is harmful, despite the studies.
 Some are outright hostile to profoundly gifted
students, who often don’t fit educational
assumptions or “molds.”
 Some dismiss these students’ extremely high
scores as incorrect or the result of cheating.
 A follow-up study of profoundly gifted
students at ~ age 23 showed:
 56% planned to pursue doctoral degrees
(or were already) compared to 1% of the
general population.
 Degrees pursued tended to match ability
pattern, with mathematically-gifted
students pursuing STEM degrees.
 Many were already accomplished in
their fields (patents, academic/creative
writing publications, national awards,
Phi Beta Kappa membership).
 Accomplishment rates were higher for
verbally-gifted and evenly-gifted
individuals.
 This same group of 320 individuals achieved much in their fields by age 38:
 133 STEM patents
 392 STEM publications
 687 software contributions
 922 dance and music productions
 191 creative writing publications
 79 works of art
 46 social science/law/business publications
 16 companies founded
 $26 million in grant funding

 These findings have been replicated in other samples of profoundly gifted


individuals.
 Achievements tended to separate into STEM and humanities
accomplishments according to intellectual profile (ability tilt).
 Of these impressive individuals, some
outshone others in their field:
 57% of fine arts accomplishments were
attributed to one person.
 34 of 39 poems were created by one person.
 Three individuals produced 100 software
contributions (44% of total).
 This held in the replication sample:
 One person produced 60 of 68 publications in
chemistry.
 40% of NSF grants went to one researcher.
 43 of 86 Fortune 500 patents were filed by one
person.
 These individuals are rare even within the
rare profoundly gifted population.
 Another rare subpopulation consists
of those who are profoundly gifted
across multiple fields.
 They are about 10-25 times rarer than
individuals who are profoundly gifted
in only one area.
 Their average IQ is higher than
Evenly-gifted unevenly gifted individuals (IQ>200
estimated in one study, vs.
 Their early accomplishments span
STEM and humanities, and little has
been published about their adult-level
accomplishments within and across
fields.
 Recent studies suggest they are a
unique subpopulation relative to the
unevenly gifted at this level of ability.
 A few interesting research directions exist, including:
1. A longitudinal study of wranglers to identify how their academic and achievement
trajectories develop over time, as well as early signs of wrangler potential.
2. Studies examining achievement trajectories over time by type of academic intervention
types in this population, by demographic factors (women, minorities…), or by ability
profile.
3. A more in-depth follow-up of achievement profile among different ability profiles to
understand how unevenly-gifted populations might differ from evenly-gifted
populations in adulthood.
4. A neuroimaging study of different populations (unevenly profoundly gifted, evenly
profoundly gifted, more moderately gifted, and average populations, for instance) to
understand how brain activity patterns and connectivity relates to field-specific and
general talent on verbal and mathematical problems.
 Profoundly gifted individuals are, by definition, rare.
 Small sample sizes present statistical challenges, and few methods exist that can
compare samples of <30 individuals.
 Persistent homology and its simplified cousin, single-linkage hierarchical clustering,
provide statistically robust methods for sample comparison at small sample sizes.
 These methods also provide good visualization methods (example shown below).
 Coyle, T. R., Purcell, J. M., Snyder, A. C., & Richmond, M. C. (2014). Ability tilt on the SAT and ACT predicts specific abilities and
college majors. Intelligence, 46, 18-24.
 Farrelly, C. M. (2017). Topological Data Analysis for Data Mining Small Educational Samples with Application to Studies of the Gifted.

 Gross, M. U. (1992). The use of radical acceleration in cases of extreme intellectual precocity. Gifted child quarterly, 36(2), 91-99.
 Gross, M. U. (2000). Exceptionally and profoundly gifted students: An underserved population. Understanding Our Gifted, 12(2), 3-9.
 Gross, M. U. (2003). Exceptionally gifted children. Routledge.
 Gross, M. U. (2015). Characteristics of Able Gifted Highly Gifted Exceptionally Gifted and Profoundly Gifted Learners. In Applied
Practice for Educators of Gifted and Able Learners (pp. 3-23). SensePublishers.
 Janos, P. M. (1987). A fifty-year follow-up of Terman's youngest college students and IQ-matched agemates. Gifted Child Quarterly,
31(2), 55-58.
 Kell, H. J., Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2013). Who rises to the top? Early indicators. Psychological Science, 24(5), 648-659.
 Lubinski, D. (2009). Exceptional cognitive ability: The phenotype. Behavior genetics, 39(4), 350-358.
 Lubinski, D., Webb, R. M., Morelock, M. J., & Benbow, C. P. (2001). Top 1 in 10,000: a 10-year follow-up of the profoundly gifted.
Journal of applied Psychology, 86(4), 718.
 Makel, M. C., Kell, H. J., Lubinski, D., Putallaz, M., & Benbow, C. P. (2016). When lightning strikes twice: Profoundly gifted,
profoundly accomplished. Psychological Science, 27(7), 1004-1018.
 Prescott, J., Gavrilescu, M., Cunnington, R., O'Boyle, M. W., & Egan, G. F. (2010). Enhanced brain connectivity in math-gifted
adolescents: An fMRI study using mental rotation. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(4), 277-288.
 Ruf, D. L. (2005). Losing our minds: Gifted children left behind. Great Potential Press, Inc..
 Singh, H., & O'boyle, M. W. (2004). Interhemispheric interaction during global-local processing in mathematically gifted adolescents,
average-ability youth, and college students. Neuropsychology, 18(2), 371.
 Wai, J., Cacchio, M., Putallaz, M., & Makel, M. C. (2010). Sex differences in the right tail of cognitive abilities: A 30year examination.
Intelligence, 38(4), 412-423.
 http://www.davidsongifted.org/Young-Scholars
 http://www.hoagiesgifted.org/
 https://cty.jhu.edu/set/index.html
 https://tip.duke.edu/
 http://www.davidsongifted.org/Search-Database/entry/A10108
 https://robinsoncenter.uw.edu/programs/eep/
 http://www.calstatela.edu/academic/eep
 https://www.talentigniter.com/

View publication stats

Potrebbero piacerti anche