Sei sulla pagina 1di 276

1

Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 1: Overview
By Terezinha Nunes, Peter Bryant and Anne Watson, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Summary of findings 3
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: Overview 10

Paper 1: Overview References 36


Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities About the authors
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical Terezinha Nunes is Professor of Educational
representation Studies at the University of Oxford.
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation Peter Bryant is Senior Research Fellow in the
in mathematics Department of Education, University of Oxford.
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning Anne Watson is Professor of Mathematics
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating Education at the University of Oxford.
concepts
Paper 8: Methodological appendix
About the Nuffield Foundation
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while charitable trust established in 1943 by William
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
in secondary schools. Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
being. We fund research and pr actical
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which experiment and the development of capacity
has been published separately as Introduction and to under take them; working across education,
summary of findings. science, social science and social policy. While
most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published responsive grant programmes we also
together as Summary papers. undertake our own initiatives.

All publications are available to download from


our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary of findings

Aims
Our aim in the review is to present a synthesis Pragmatic theories are usually not tested for their
of research on mathematics lear ning by children consistency with empirical evidence, nor examined
from the age of five to the age of sixteen y ears for the parsimony of their explanations vis-à-vis other
and to identify the issues that are fundamental to existing theories; instead they are assessed in m ultiple
understanding children’s mathematics learning. In contexts for their descriptive power, their credibility
doing so, we concentrated on three main questions and their effectiveness in practice.
regarding key understandings in mathematics.
• What insights must students have in order to Our star ting point in the review is that children need
understand basic mathematical concepts? to learn about quantities and the relations between
• What are the sources of these insights and how them and about mathematical symbols and their
does informal mathematics knowledge meanings. These meanings are based on sets of
relate to school lear ning of mathematics? relations. Mathematics teaching should aim to ensure
• What understandings must students have in that students’ understanding of quantities, relations
order to build new mathematical ideas using and symbols go together.
basic concepts?

Conclusions
Theoretical framework
This theoretical approach underlies the six main
While writing the review, we concluded that there sections of the review. We now summarise the main
are two distinct types of theor y about how children conclusions of each of these sections.
learn mathematics.

Explanatory theories set out to explain how Whole numbers


children’s mathematical thinking and knowledge • Whole numbers represent both quantities and
change. These theories are based on empir ical relations between quantities, such as differences
research on children’s solutions to mathematical and ratio. Primary school children must establish
problems as well as on exper imental and longitudinal clear connections between numbers, quantities
studies. Successful theories of this sor t should and relations.
provide insight into the causes of children’s
mathematical development and worthwhile • Children’s initial understanding of quantitative
suggestions about teaching and lear ning mathematics. relations is largely based on correspondence.
One-to-one correspondence underlies their
Pragmatic theories set out to investigate what children understanding of cardinality, and one-to-many
ought to learn and understand and also identify correspondence gives them their first insights
obstacles to learning in formal educational settings. into multiplicative relations. Children should be
4 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

encouraged to think of n umber in terms of these • Two types of quantities that are taught in
relations. primary school must be represented by fractions.
The first involves measurement: if you want to
• Children star t school with varying levels of represent a quantity by means of a n umber
ability in using different action schemes to solve and the quantity is smaller than the unit of
arithmetic problems in the context of stor ies. measurement, you need a fr action; for example,
They do not need to kno w arithmetic facts to a half cup or a quar ter inch. The second involves
solve these problems: they count in different division: if the dividend is smaller than the divisor,
ways depending on whether the prob lems they the result of the division is represented b y a
are solving involve the ideas of addition, fraction; for example, three chocolates shared
subtraction, multiplication or division. among four children.

• Individual differences in the use of action • Children use different schemes of action in these
schemes to solve problems predict children’s two different situations. In division situations, they
progress in learning mathematics in school. use correspondences between the units in the
numerator and the units in the denominator. In
• Interventions that help children lear n to use their measurement situations, they use par titioning.
action schemes to solve problems lead to better
learning of mathematics in school. • Children are more successful in under standing
equivalence of fractions and in order ing fractions
• It is more difficult for children to use numbers to by magnitude in situations that in volve division
represent relations than to represent quantities. than in measurement situations.

• It is crucial for children’s understanding of fractions


Implications for the classroom that they learn about fractions in both types of
Teaching should make it possible for children to: situation: most do not spontaneously transfer what
• connect their knowledge of counting with their they learned in one situation to the other.
knowledge of quantities
• understand additive composition and one-to- • When a fraction is used to represent a quantity,
many correspondence children need to lear n to think about how the
• understand the inverse relation between addition numerator and the denominator relate to the
and subtraction value represented by the fraction. They must think
• solve problems that involve these key about direct and inverse relations: the larger the
understandings numerator, the larger the quantity, but the larger
• develop their multiplicative understanding the denominator, the smaller the quantity.
alongside additive reasoning.
• Like whole numbers, fractions can be used to
represent quantities and relations between
Implications for further research quantities, but they are rarely used to represent
Long-term longitudinal and intervention studies relations in primary school. Older students often
with large samples are needed to suppor t curriculum find it difficult to use fractions to represent relations.
development and policy changes aimed at
implementing these objectives. There is also a
need for studies designed to promote children’s Implications for the classroom
competence in solving problems about relations. Teaching should make it possible for children to:
• use their understanding of quantities in division
situations to understand equivalence and order
Fractions of fractions
• Fractions are used in pr imary school to represent • make links between different types of reasoning
quantities that cannot be represented b y a single in division and measurement situations
whole number. As with whole numbers, children • make links between understanding fractional
need to make connections between quantities quantities and procedures
and their representations in fr actions in order to • learn to use fr actions to represent relations
be able to use fr actions meaningfully. between quantities, as well as quantities.
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Implications for further research situations.This alternative approach has also not
Evidence from experimental studies with larger been systematically assessed yet.
samples and long-term interventions in the classroom
are needed to establish how division situations relate • There is no research that compares the results
to learning fractions. Investigations on how links of these diametrically opposed ideas.
between situations can be built are needed to suppor t
curriculum development and classroom teaching.
Implications for the classroom
There is also a need f or longitudinal studies designed Teaching should make it possible for children to:
to clarify whether separation between procedures • distinguish between quantities and relations
and meaning in fr actions has consequences for • become explicitly aware of the different types
further mathematics learning. of relations in different situations
• use different mathematical representations to
Given the impor tance of understanding and focus on the relevant relations in specif ic problems
representing relations numerically, studies that • relate informal knowledge and formal learning.
investigate under what circumstances pr imary school
students can use fractions to represent relations
between quantities, such as in propor tional Implications for further research
reasoning, are urgently needed. Evidence from experimental and long-term
longitudinal studies is needed on which approaches
to making students aware of relations in problem
Relations and their mathematical situations improve problem solving. A study comparing
representation the alternative approaches – star ting from informal
• Children have greater difficulty in understanding knowledge versus starting from formalisations –
relations than in understanding quantities. This would make a significant contribution to the literature.
is true in the context of both additiv e and
multiplicative reasoning problems.
Space and its mathematical
• Primary and secondar y school students often representation
apply additive procedures to solve multiplicative • Children come to school with a great deal of
problems and multiplicative procedures to solve informal and often implicit knowledge about
additive problems. spatial relations. One challenge in mathematical
education is how best to harness this knowledge
• Teaching designed to help students become aware in lessons about space .
of relations in the context of additive reasoning
problems can lead to significant improvement. • This pre-school knowledge of space is mainl y
relational. For example, children use a stable
• The use of diagr ams, tables and graphs to background to remember the position and
represent relations in multiplicative reasoning orientation of objects and lines.
problems facilitates children’s thinking about the
nature of the relations between quantities. • Measuring length and area poses par ticular
problems for children, even though they are
• Excellent curriculum development work has able to understand the underlying logic of
been carried out to design progr ammes that measurement. Their difficulties concern iteration of
help students develop awareness of their implicit standard units and the need to appl y multiplicative
knowledge of multiplicative relations. This work reasoning to the measurement of area.
has not been systematically assessed so far.
• From an early age children are able to
• An alternative view is that students’ implicit extrapolate imaginary straight lines, which allows
knowledge should not be the star ting point for them to learn how to use Car tesian co-ordinates
students to learn about propor tional relations; to plot specific positions in space with little
teaching should focus on formalisations rather difficulty. However, they need help from teacher s
than informal knowledge and only later seek to on how to use co-ordinates to w ork out the
connect mathematical formalisations with applied relation between different positions.
6 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

• Learning how to represent angle mathematicall y transitive inference, inversion and one-to-one
is a hard task f or young children, even though correspondence relate to problems with geometr y,
angles are an impor tant par t of their everyday such as measurement of length and area.
life. Initially children are more aware of angle in
the context of movement (turns) than in other There is a need f or intervention studies on methods
contexts. They need help from to teacher s to be of teaching children to work out the relation
able to relate angles across diff erent contexts. between different positions, using co-ordinates.

• An important aspect of learning about geometr y


is to recognise the relation betw een transformed Algebra
shapes (rotation, reflection, enlargement). This can • Algebra is the way we express generalisations
be difficult, since children’s preschool experiences about numbers, quantities, relations and functions.
lead them to recognise the same shapes as For this reason, good understanding of connections
equivalent across such transformations, rather than between numbers, quantities and relations is
to be aware of the nature of the tr ansformation. related to success in using algebr a. In par ticular,
understanding that addition and subtr action are
• Another aspect of the under standing of shape inverses, and so are multiplication and division,
is the fact that one shape can be tr ansformed helps students understand expressions and solve
into another by addition and subtraction of its equations.
subcomponents. For example, a parallelogram
can be transformed into a rectangle of the same • To understand algebraic symbolisation, students
base and height by the addition and subtraction have to (a) under stand the underlying operations
of equivalent triangles. Research demonstrates a and (b) become fluent with the notational r ules.
danger that children lear n these transformations These two kinds of lear ning, the meaning and the
as procedures without under standing their symbol, seem to be most successful when
conceptual basis. students know what is being expressed and have
time to become fluent at using the notation.

Implications for the classroom • Students have to learn to recognise the different
Teaching should make it possible for children to: nature and roles of letter s as: unknowns,
• build on spatial relational knowledge from variables, constants and parameters, and also the
outside school meanings of equality and equivalence . These
• relate their knowledge of relations and meanings are not always distinct in algebra and
correspondence to the conceptual basis of do not relate unambiguously to arithmetical
measurement understandings.
• iterate with standard and non-standard units
• understand the difference between • Students often get confused, misapply, or
measurements which are/are not multiplicative misremember rules for transforming expressions
• relate co-ordinates to extrapolating imaginary and solving equations. They often tr y to apply
straight lines arithmetical meanings inappropriately to
• distinguish between scale enlargements and area algebraic expressions. This is associated with
enlargements. over-emphasis on notational manipulation, or
on ‘generalised arithmetic’, in which they ma y
try to get concise answ ers.
Implications for further research
There is a ser ious need for longitudinal research
on the possible connections between children’s Implications for the classroom
pre-school spatial abilities and how well they learn Teaching should make it possible for children to:
about geometry at school. • read numerical and algebraic expressions
relationally, rather than as instr uctions to calculate
Psychological research is needed on: children’s ability (as in substitution)
to make and understand transformations and the • describe generalisations based on proper ties
additive relations in compound shapes; the exact (arithmetical rules, logical relations, structures)
cause of children’s difficulties with iteration; how as well as inductive reasoning from sequences
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• use symbolism to represent relations Understanding relations is necessar y to solve


• understand that letters and ‘=’ have a range of equations meaningfully.
meanings
• use hands-on ICT to relate representations Students have to learn when and how to use
• use algebra purposefully in multiple experiences informal, experiential reasoning and when to
over time use formal, conventional, mathematical reasoning.
• explore and use algebr aic manipulation software. Without special attention to meanings, many
students tend to apply visual reasoning, or be
triggered by verbal cues, rather than analyse
Implications for further research situations to identify var iables and relations.
We need to know how explicit work on
understanding relations between quantities enables In many mathematical situations in secondar y
students to move successfully between arithmetical mathematics, students have to look for relations
to algebraic thinking. between numbers, and variables, and relations
between relations, and proper ties of objects, and
Research on how expressing generality enables know how to represent them.
students to use algebr a is mainly in small-scale
teaching interventions, and the problems of large-
scale implementation are not so well repor ted. We Implications for the classroom
do not know the longer-term comparative effects of Teaching should make it possible for children to:
different teaching approaches to ear ly algebra on • learn new abstract understandings, which is
students’ later use of algebr aic notation and thinking. neither achieved through learning procedures,
nor through problem-solving activities, without
There is little research on higher algebr a, except further intervention
for teaching experiments involving functions. How • use their obvious reactions to perceptions and
learners synthesise their knowledge of elementar y build on them, or understand conflicts with them
algebra to understand polynomial functions, their • adapt to new meanings and dev elop from earlier
factorisation and roots, simultaneous equations, methods and conceptualizations over time
inequalities and other algebr aic objects beyond • understand the meaning of new concepts ‘know
elementary expressions and equations is not kno wn. about’, ‘know how to’, and ‘know how to use’
• control switching between, and comparing,
There is some research about the use of symbolic representations of functions in order to
manipulators but more needs to be lear ned about understand them
the kinds of algebr aic exper tise that develops • use spreadsheets, graphing tools, and other
through their use. software to suppor t application and authentic
use of mathematics.

Modelling, solving problems and


learning new concepts in secondary Implications for further research
mathematics Existing research suggests that where contextual and
Students have to be fluent in under standing methods exploratory mathematics, integrated through the
and confident about using them to kno w why and curriculum, do lead to fur ther conceptual learning it is
when to apply them, but such application does not related to conceptual learning being a rigorous focus
automatically follow the learning of procedures. Students for curriculum and textbook design, and in teacher
have to understand the situation as well as to be able to preparation, or in specifically designed projects based
call on a familiar reper toire of facts, ideas and methods. around such aims. There is therefore an urgent need for
research to identify the key conceptual understandings
Students have to know some elementar y for success in secondar y mathematics. There is no
concepts well enough to apply them and combine evidence to convince us that the new U.K. curricula will
them to form new concepts in secondar y necessarily lead to better conceptual understanding of
mathematics. For example, knowing a range of mathematics, either at the elementar y level which is
functions and/or their representations seems to be necessary to learn higher mathematics, or at higher
necessary to under stand the modelling process, and levels which provide the confidence and foundation
is cer tainly necessar y to engage in modelling. for further mathematical study.
8 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

We need to under stand the ways in which students that many of their difficulties are due to failures to
learn new ideas in mathematics that depend on make the correct logical move that would have led
combinations of earlier concepts, in secondar y them to the cor rect solution. Four different aspects
school contexts, and the char acteristics of of logic have a crucial role in lear ning about
mathematics teaching at higher secondar y level mathematics.
which contribute both to successful conceptual
learning and application of mathematics. The logic of cor respondence (one-to-one and one-to-
many correspondence) The extension of the use of
one-to-one correspondence from sharing to working
out the numerical equivalence or non-equivalence of
Common themes two or more spatial ar rays is a vastly impor tant step
in early mathematical learning. Teaching multiplication
We reviewed different areas of mathematical activity, in terms of one-to-many correspondence is more
and noted that many of them involve common effective than teaching children about m ultiplication
themes, which are fundamental to lear ning as repeated addition.
mathematics: number, logical reasoning, reflection on
knowledge and tools, understanding symbol systems The logic of inversion Longitudinal evidence shows that
and mathematical modes of enquir y. understanding the inverse relation between addition
and subtraction is a strong predictor of children’s
mathematical progress. A flexible understanding of
Number inversion is an essential element in children’s
Number is not a unitar y idea, which children geometrical reasoning as well. The concept of
learn in a linear fashion. Number develops in inversion needs a great deal more prominence
complementary strands, sometimes with than it has now in the school curriculum.
discontinuities and changes of meaning. Emphasis on
procedures and manipulation with n umbers, rather The logic of class inclusion and additive composition
than on under standing the under lying relations and Class inclusion is the basis of the under standing of
mathematical meanings, can lead to over-reliance ordinal number and the number system. Children’s
and misapplication of methods in ar ithmetic, algebra, ability to use this form of inclusion in lear ning about
and problem-solving. For example, if children form number and in solving mathematical prob lems is at
the idea that quantities are onl y equal if they are first rather weak, and needs some support.
represented by the same number, a principle that
they could deduce from lear ning to count, they will The logic of transitivity All ordered series, including
have difficulty understanding the equivalence of number, and also forms of measurement involve
fractions. Learning to count and to under stand transitivity (a > c if a > b and b > c: a = c if a = b
quantities are separate strands of development. and b = c). Learning how to use tr ansitive relations
Teaching can play a major role in helping children in numerical measurements (for example, of area) is
co-ordinate these two forms of knowledge without difficult. One reason is
making counting the only procedure that can be that children often do not grasp the impor tance of
used to think about quantities. iteration (repeated units of measurement).

Successful learning of mathematics includes The results of longitudinal research (although there is
understanding that number describes quantity; being not an exhaustive body of such work) suppor t the
able to make and use distinctions betw een different, idea that children’s logic plays a critical par t in their
but related, meanings of number; being able to use mathematical learning.
relations and meanings to inf orm application and
calculation; being able to use number relations to
move away from images of quantity and use Reflection on knowledge and tools
number as a structured, abstract, concept. Children need to re-conceptualise their intuitiv e
models about the world in order to access the
mathematical models that have been developed in
Logical reasoning the discipline. Some of the intuitive models used by
The evidence demonstrates beyond doubt that children lead them to appropr iate mathematical
children must rely on logic to lear n mathematics and problem solving, and yet they may not know why
9 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

they succeeded. Implicit models can interfere with about the relations between different objects in the
problem solving when students rel y on assumptions systems and schemes that are being represented.
that lead them astray.
Making and using representations Conventional
The fact that students use intuitiv e models number symbols, algebraic syntax, coordinate
when learning mathematics, whether the teacher geometry, and graphing methods, all afford
recognises the models or not, is a reason f or manipulations which might otherwise be impossib le.
helping them to develop an awareness of their Coordinating different representations to explore and
models. Students can explore their intuitive models extend meaning is a fundamental mathematical skill.
and extend them to concepts that are less intuitiv e,
more abstract. This pragmatic theor y has been Action and reflection-on-action In mathematics,
shown to have an impact in pr actice. actions may be physical manipulation, or symbolic
rearrangement, or our obser vations of a dynamic
image, or use of a tool. In all these contexts, we
Understanding symbol systems observe what changes and what stays the same as
Systems of symbols are human in ventions and a result of actions, and make inferences about the
thus are cultural tools that have to be taught. connections between action and effect.
Mathematical symbols are human-made tools that
improve our ability to control and adapt to the Direct and inverse relations It is impor tant in all
environment. Each system makes specific cognitive aspects of mathematics to be ab le to construct and
demands on the lear ner, who has to understand the use inverse reasoning. As well as enabling more
systems of representation and relations that are understanding of relations between quantities, this
being represented; for example place-value notation also establishes the impor tance of reverse chains of
is based on additive composition, functions depict reasoning throughout mathematical problem-solving,
covariance. Students can behave as if they algebraic and geometrical reasoning.
understand how the symbols work while they do
not understand them completely: they can lear n Informal and formal reasoning At first young children
routines for symbol manipulation that remain bring everyday understandings into school and
disconnected from meaning. This is true of rational mathematics can allow them to formalise these and
numbers, for example. make them more precise . Mathematics also provides
formal tools, which do not descr ibe everyday
Students acquire informal knowledge in their experience, but enable students to solve problems
everyday lives, which can be used to giv e meaning in mathematics and in the world which would be
to mathematical symbols lear ned in the classroom. unnoticed without a mathematical per spective.
Curriculum development work that takes this
knowledge into account is not as widespread as one
would expect given discoveries from past research.
Epilogue
Mathematical modes of enquiry We have made recommendations about teaching and
Some impor tant mathematical modes of enquiry learning, and hope to have made the reasoning behind
arise in the topics covered in this synthesis. these recommendations clear to educationalists (in
the extended review). We have also recognised that
Comparison helps us make new distinctions and create there are weaknesses in research and gaps in current
new objects and relations Comparisons are related to knowledge, some of which can be easil y solved by
making distinctions, sor ting and classifying; students research enabled by significant contributions of past
need to learn to make these distinctions based on research. Other gaps may not be so easily solved, and
mathematical relations and properties, rather than we have described some pragmatic theories that are
perceptual similarities. or can be used by teachers when they plan their
teaching. Classroom research stemming from the
Reasoning about properties and relations rather than exploration of these theories can provide new
perceptions Throughout mathematics, students have insights for further research in the future , alongside
to learn to interpret representations before they longitudinal studies which focus on learning
think about how to respond. They need to think mathematics from a psychological per spective.
10 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

Overview

Aims
Our aim in this review is to present a synthesis of true of some of our theoretical anal ysis of the
research on key aspects of mathematics learning by evidence that we read for this review.
children from the age of 5 to the age of 16 y ears:
these are the ages that compr ise compulsory The answers to our questions should allo w us to
education in the United Kingdom In prepar ing the trace students’ learning trajectories. Confrey (2008)
review, we have considered the results of a lar ge defined a learning trajectory as ‘a researcher-
body of research car ried out by psychologists and by conjectured, empirically-supported description of
mathematics educators over approximately the last the ordered network of experiences a student
six decades. Our aim has been to dev elop a encounters through instruction (i.e. activities, tasks,
theoretical analysis of these results in order to attain tools, forms of interaction and methods of
a big picture of how children learn, and sometimes evaluation), in order to move from informal ideas,
fail to learn, mathematics and how they could lear n it through successive refinements of representation,
better. Our main tar get is not to provide an answer articulation, and reflection, towards increasingly
to any specific question, but to identify issues that complex concepts over time.’ If students’ learning
are fundamental to under standing children’s trajectories towards understanding specific concepts
mathematics learning. In our view theories of are generally understood, teachers will be much
mathematics learning should deal with three main better placed to promote their advancement.
questions regarding key understandings in
mathematics: Finally, one of our aims has been to identify a set
• What insights must students have in order to of research questions that stem from our cur rent
understand basic mathematical concepts? knowledge about children’s mathematics learning and
• What are the sources of these insights and how methods that can provide relevant evidence about
does informal mathematics knowledge relate to important, outstanding issues.
school learning of mathematics?
• What understandings must students have in order
to build new mathematical ideas using basic Scope of the review
concepts?
As we reviewed existing research and existing
Theoretical analysis played a major role in this theories about mathematics lear ning, it soon became
synthesis. Many theoretical ideas were already clear to us that there are tw o types of theories
available in the liter ature and we sought to examine about how children learn mathematics. The first are
them critically for coherence and for consistency explanatory theories. These theories seek to explain
with the empirical evidence. Cooper (1998) suggests how children’s thinking and knowledge change.
that there may be occasions when new theoretical Explanatory theories are based on empirical
schemes must be developed to provide an research on the str ategies that children adopt in
overarching understanding of the higher-order solving mathematical problems, on the difficulties
relations in the research domain; this was cer tainly and misconceptions that affect their solutions to
11 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

such problems, and on their successes and their didactic transposition, the theory of situations, social
explanations of their own solutions. They also draw theories regarding the impor tance of conflict and
on quantitative methods to describe age or school cooperation, the role of the teacher, the role and use
grade levels when cer tain forms of knowledge are of language, peer collaboration and argumentation in
attained and to make inferences about the nature of the classroom. These are important ideas but they
relationships observed during learning (for example, apply to other domains of lear ning as well, and we
to help understand the relation between informal decided not to provide an analysis of such theor ies
knowledge and school lear ning of mathematics). but to mention them onl y in the context of specif ic
issues about mathematical lear ning.
We have called the second type of theor y pragmatic.
A pragmatic theory is rather like a road map for Another decision that we made about the scope of
teachers: its aims are to set out what children m ust the synthesis was about how to deal with cultur al
learn and understand, usually in a clear sequence , differences in teaching and lear ning mathematics. The
about par ticular topics and to identify obstacles to focus of the review is on mathematics learning by U.K.
learning in formal educational settings and other students during compulsory education. We recognise
issues which teachers should keep in mind when that there are many differences between learners in
designing teaching. Pragmatic theories are usually different parts of the world; so, we decided to include
not tested for their consistency with empir ical mostly research about learners who can be
evidence, nor examined for the parsimony of their considered as reasonably similar to U.K. students, i.e.
explanations vis-à-vis other existing theor ies; instead those living in Western cultures with a relatively high
they are assessed in multiple contexts for their standard of living and plenty of oppor tunities to
descriptive power, their credibility and their attend school. Thus the description of students who
effectiveness in practice. participated in the studies is not presented in detail
and will often be indicated only in terms of the
Explanatory theories are of great impor tance in country where the research was car ried out. In order
moving forward our understanding of phenomena to offer readers a notion of the time in students’ lives
and have proven helpful, for example, in the domain when they might succeed or sho w difficulties with
of literacy teaching and lear ning. However, with some specific problems, we used age levels or school grade
aspects of mathematics, which tend to be those that levels as references. These ages and years of schooling
older children have to learn about, there simply is are not to be gener alised to very different
not enough explanator y knowledge yet to guide circumstances where, for example, children might be
teachers in many aspects of their mathematics growing up in cultures with different number systems
teaching, but students must still be taught even when or largely without school par ticipation. Occasional
we do not know much about how they think or reference to research with other groups is used b ut
how their knowledge changes over time through this was purposefully limited, and it was included onl y
learning. Mathematics educators have developed when it was felt that the studies could shed light on a
pragmatic theories to fill this gap and to tak e specific issue.
account of the inter play of learning theory with
social and cultural aspects of educational contexts. We also decided to concentr ate on key
Pragmatic theories are designed to guide teachers in understandings that offer the foundation for
domains where there are no satisfactor y explanatory mathematics learning rather than on the different
theories, and where explanator y theory does not technologies used in mathematics. Wartofsky (1979)
provide enough information to design complex conceives technology as any human made tool that
classroom teaching. We have included both types of improves our ability to control and adapt to the
theory in our review. We believe that both types are environment. Mathematics uses many such tools.
necessary in mathematics education b ut that they Some representational tools, such as counting and
should not be confused with each other. written numbers, are par t of traditional mathematics
learning in primary school. They improve our abilities
We decided to concentr ate in our review on issues in amazing ways: for example, counting allows us to
that are specific to mathematics lear ning. We represent precisely quantities which we could not
recognise the significance of general pedagogical discriminate perceptually and written numbers in the
theories that stress, for example, the impor tance Hindu-Arabic system create the possibility of column
of giving learners an active role in developing their arithmetic, which is not easil y implemented with or al
thinking and conceptual under standing, the notion of number when quantities are lar ge or even with
12 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

written Roman numerals. We have argued elsewhere and that they should repor t either the results of
(Nunes, 2002) that systems of signs enhance, empirical research or theoretical schemes f or
structure and empower their users but learners understanding mathematics learning or both. We
must still construct meanings that allow them to also consulted several books in order to read
use these systems. Our choice in this review was researchers’ syntheses of their own empirical work
to consider how learners construct meanings and to access ear lier well-established reviews of
rather than explore in depth the enab ling role of relevant research; we chose books that provide
mathematical representations. We discuss in much useful frameworks for research and theor ies in
greater detail how they learn to use whole and mathematics learning.
rational numbers meaningfully than how they
calculate with these numbers. Similarly, we discuss We hope that this review will become the object
how they might lear n the meaning and power of of discussion within the community of researchers,
algebraic representation rather than how they teachers and policy makers. We recognise that it
might become fluent with algebraic manipulation. is only one step towards making sense of the vast
Psychological theories (Luria, 1973; Vygotsky, 1981) research on how students’ thinking and knowledge
emphasise the empowering role of cultur ally of mathematics develops, and that other steps
developed systems of signs in human reasoning b ut must follow, including a thorough evaluation of
stress that learners’ construction of meanings for this contribution.
these signs undergoes a long development process
in order for the signs to be tr uly empowering.
Similarly, mathematics educators stress that
technology is aimed not to replace , but to enhance
Teaching and learning
mathematical reasoning (Noss and Hoyles, 1992). mathematics: What is the
nature of this task?
Our reason for not focusing on technologies in this
synthesis is that there are so man y technological Learning mathematics is in some ways similar
resources used today for doing mathematics that (but of course not identical) to language lear ning: in
it is not possible to consider even those used or mathematics as well as in language it is necessar y to
potentially useful in pr imary school in the required learn symbols and their meaning, and to know how
detail in this synthesis. We recognise this gap and to combine them meaningfully.
strongly suggest that at least some of these issues
be taken up for a synthesis at a later point, as some Learning meanings for symbols is often more diff icult
important comparative work already exists in the than one might think. Think of learning the meaning
domain of column ar ithmetic (e.g. Anghileri, of the word ‘brother’. If Megan said to her f our-year-
Beishuizen and Putten, 2002; Treffers, 1987) and old friend Sally ‘That’s my brother’ and pointed to
the use of calculator s (e.g. Ruthven, 2008). her brother, Sally might learn to say correctly and
appropriately ‘That’s Megan’s brother’ but she would
We wish to emphasise , therefore, that this review is not necessarily know the meaning of ‘brother’.
not an exhaustive one. It considers a par t of today’s ‘Brother of ’ is a phr ase that is based on a set of
knowledge in mathematics education. There are relationships, and in order to under stand its meaning
other, more specific aspects of the subject which, we need to under stand this set of relationships,
usually for reasons of space , we decided to by-pass. which includes ‘mother of ’ and ‘father of ’. It is in this
We shall explain the reasons for these choices as way that learning mathematics is very like learning a
we go along. language: we need to lear n mathematical symbols
and their meanings, and the meaning of these
symbols is based on sets of relations.
Methods of the review
In the same way that Megan might point to her
We obtained the mater ial for the synthesis through brother, Megan could count a set of pens and sa y:
a systematic search of peer review ed journals, edited ‘There are 15 pens here’. Sally could learn to count
volumes and refereed conference proceedings.1 We and say ‘15 pens’ (or dogs, or stars). But ‘15’ in
selected the papers that we read by first screening mathematics does not just refer to the result of
the abstracts: our main cr iteria for selecting ar ticles counting a set: it also means that this set is
to read were that they should be on a relevant topic equivalent to all other sets with 15 objects, has
13 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

fewer objects than any set with 16 or more , and is that we can use language to represent a large
has more objects than any set with 14 or f ewer. variety of meanings, and mathematics has a similar
Learning about numbers involves more than power. But, of course, mathematics learning differs
understanding the operations that are car ried out to from language learning: mathematics contains its own
determine the word that represents the quantity. In distinct concepts and modes of enquir y which
the context of lear ning mathematics, we would like determine the way that mathematics is used. This
students to know, without having to count, that some specificity of mathematical concepts is reflected in the
operations do and other s do not change a quantity. themes that we chose to analyse in our synthesis.
For example, we would like them to under stand that
there would only be more pens if we added some
to the set, and fewer if we subtracted some from The framework for this review
the set, and that there would still be 15 pens if w e
added and then subtracted As we star t our review, there is a gener al point to
(or vice versa) the same number of pens to and be made about the theoretical position that w e have
from the original set. reached from our review of research on children’s
mathematics. On the whole , the teaching of the
The basic numerical concepts that we want students various aspects of mathematics proceeds in a clear
to learn in primary school have these two sides sequence, and with a cer tain amount of separ ation
to them: on the one hand, there are quantities, in the teaching of different aspects. Children are
operations on quantities and relations betw een taught first about the number sequence and then
quantities, and on the other hand there are symbols, about written numbers and arithmetical operations
operations on symbols and relations between using written numbers. The teaching of the f our
symbols. Mathematics teaching should aim to ensure arithmetical operations is done separately. At school
that students’ understanding of quantities, relations children learn about addition and subtraction
and symbols go together. Anything we do with the separately and before they learn about multiplication
symbols has to be consistent with their under lying and division, which also tend to be taught quite
mathematical meaning as well as logically consistent separately from each other. Lessons about arithmetic
and we are not free to pla y with meaning in start years before lessons about propor tions and the
mathematics in quite the same ways we might play use of mathematical models.
with words.
This order of events in teaching has had a clear
This necessary connection is often neglected in effect on research and theor ies about mathematical
theories about mathematics lear ning and in teaching learning. For example, it is a commonplace that
practices. Theories that appear to be contradictory research on multiplication and division is most often
have often focused either on students’ understanding (though there are exceptions; see Paper 4) carried
of quantities or on their under standing of symbols out with children who are older than those who
and their manipulations. Similarly, teaching is often participate in research on addition and subtraction.
designed with one or the other of these two kinds Consequently, in most theor ies additive reasoning is
of understanding in sight, and the result is that there hypothesised (or assumed) to precede m ultiplicative
are different ways of teaching that have different reasoning. Until recently there have been very few
strengths and weaknesses. studies of children’s understanding of the connection
between the different arithmetical operations
Language learners eventually reach a time when they because they are assumed to be learned relatively
can learn the meaning of new w ords simply by independently of each other.
definitions and connections with other words. Think of
words like ‘gene’ and ‘theory’: we learn their meanings Our review of the relevant research has led us to
from descriptions provided by means of other words us to a different position. The evidence quite clear ly
and from the way they are used in the language. suggests that there is no such sequence , at any rate
Mathematics beyond primary school often works in the onset of children’s understanding of some of
similarly: new mathematical meanings are lear ned by these different aspects of mathematics. Much of this
using previously learned mathematical meanings and learning begins, as our review will sho w, in informal
ways of combining these. There are also other ways in circumstances and before children go to school. Even
which mathematics and language lear ning are similar ; after they begin to lear n about mathematics formally,
perhaps the most impor tant of these other similarities there are clear signs that they can embar k on
14 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

genuinely multiplicative reasoning, for example, at a Thompson (1993) suggested that ‘a person
time when the instr uction they receive is all about constitutes a quantity by conceiving of a quality
addition and subtraction. Similar obser vations can be of an object in such a wa y that he or she
made about learning algebra; there are studies that understands the possibility of measur ing it.
show that quite young children are capable of Quantities, when measured, have numerical value,
expressing mathematical generalities in algebraic but we need not measure them or kno w their
terms, but these are r are: the majority of studies measures to reason about them. You can think
focus on the ways in which lear ners fail to do so of your height, another per son’s height, and the
at the usual age at which this is taught. amount by which one of y ou is taller than the
other without having to know the actual values’
Sequences do exist in children’s learning, but these (pp. 165–166). Children exper ience and lear n
tend not to be about diff erent arithmetic operations about quantities and the relations betw een them
(e.g. not about addition before multiplication). Instead, quite independently of lear ning to count. Similarly,
they take the form of children’s understanding of new they can lear n to count quite independentl y from
quantitative relations as a result of w orking with and understanding quantities and relations betw een
manipulating relations that have been familiar for them. It is cr ucial for children to lear n to make
some time. An example, which we describe in detail both connections and distinctions betw een
in Paper 2, is about the inverse relation between number and quantity. There are different theories
addition and subtraction. Young children easily in psychology regarding how children connect
understand that if you add some new items to a set quantity and number ; these are discussed in
of items and then subtr act exactly the same items, Paper 2.
the number of items in the set is the same as it was
initially (inversion of identity), but it takes some time The review also showed that there are two
for them to extend their knowledge of this relation different types of quantities that primary school
enough to understand that the number of items in children have to understand and that these are
the set will also remain the same if you add some connected to different types of numbers. In
new items and then subtr act an equal number items everyday life, as well as in primary school, children
from the set, which are not the same ones you had learn about quantities that can be counted. Some
added (inversion of quantity: a + b - b = a). Causal are discrete and each item can be counted as a
sequences of this kind play an impor tant par t in the natural unit; other quantities are continuous and we
conclusions that we reach in this review. use measurement systems, count the conventional
units that are part of the system, and attribute
Through our review, we identified some key numbers to these quantities. These quantities
understandings which we think children must achieve which are measured by the successive addition of
to be successful lear ners of mathematics and which items are termed extensive quantities. They are
became the main topics f or the review. In the represented by whole numbers and give children
paragraphs that follow, we present the ar guments their first insights into number.
that led us to choose the six main topics.
Subsequently, each topic is summar ised under a In everyday life children also learn about quantities
separate heading. The research on which these that cannot be counted like this. One reason why
summaries are based is anal ysed in Papers 2 to 7. the quantity might not be countable in this way is
that it may be smaller than the unit; for example, if
The main points that are discussed here , before we you share three chocolate bars among four people,
turn to the summar ies, guided the choice of paper s you cannot count how many chocolate bars each
in the review. one receives. Before being taught about fractions,
some primary school students are aware that you
cannot say that each person would be given one
Quantity and number chocolate bar, because they realise that each
person’s portion would be smaller than one: these
The first point is that there is a distinction to be children conclude that they do not know a number
made between quantity and number and that to say how much chocolate each person will
children must make connections as well as receive (Nunes and Bryant, 2008). Quantities
distinctions between quantity and number in order that are smaller than the unit are represented by
to succeed in learning mathematics. fractions, or more generally by rational numbers.
15 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Rational numbers are also used to represent What is impor tant is relationships among quantities’
quantities which we do not measure directly (p. 165). Elsewhere, Thompson (1994) emphasised
but only through a relation between two other that ‘a quantitative operation is non-numerical; it has
measures. For example, if we want to say something to do with the comprehension (italics in the or iginal)
about the concentration of orange squash in a glass, of a situation.’ (p. 187). So relations, like quantities,
we have to say something about the r atio of are different from numbers but we use numbers to
concentrate used to water. This type of quantity, quantify them.
measurable by ratio, is termed intensive quantity
and is often represented by rational numbers. Paper 4 of this synthesis discusses the quantification
of relations in mathematics, with a focus on the sor ts
In Papers 2 and 3 we discuss how children make of relations that are par t of learning mathematics in
connections between whole and r ational numbers primary school.
and the different types of quantities that they
represent.
The coordination of basic concepts
and the development of higher order
Relations concepts
Our second general point is about relations. Students in secondar y school have the dual task of
Numbers are used to represent quantities as w ell refining what they have learned in primary school
as relations; this is why children must establish a and understanding new concepts, which are based
connection between quantity and number but also on reflections about and combinations of previous
distinguish between them. Measures are numbers concepts. The challenge for students in secondar y
that are connected to a quantity. Expressions such school is to lear n to take a different perspective with
as 20 books, 3 centimetres, 4 kilos, and ½ a respect to their mathematics knowledge and, at the
chocolate are measures. Relations, like quantities, same time, to learn about the power of this new
do not have to be quantified. For example, we can perspective. Students can under stand much about
simply say that two quantities are equivalent or using mathematical representations (numbers,
different. This is a qualitative statement about the diagrams, graphs) for quantities and relations and
relation between two quantities. But we can quantify how this helps them solve problems. Students
relations and we use numbers to do so: for example, who have gone this far under stand the role of
when we compare two measures, we are quantifying mathematics in representing and helping us
a relation. If there are 20 children in a class and 17 understand phenomena, and even generalising
books, we can say that there are 3 more children beyond what we know. But they may not have
than books. The number 3 quantifies the relation. We understood a distinct and cr ucial aspect of the
can say 3 more children than books or 3 books importance of mathematics: that, above and beyond
fewer than children; the meaning does not change helping represent and explore what y ou know, it can
when the wording changes because the number 3 be used to discover what you do not know. In this
does not refer to children or to books, but to the review, we consider two related themes of this
relation between the two measures. second side of mathematics: algebraic reasoning and
modelling. Papers 6 and 7 summar ise the research
A major use of mathematics is to quantify relations on these topics.
and manipulate these representations to expand
our understanding of a situation. We came to the In the rest of this opening paper w e shall summarise
conclusion from our review that under standing our main conclusions from our review. In other
relations between quantities is at the root of words, Papers 2 to 7 contain our detailed reviews
understanding mathematical models. Thompson of research on mathematics lear ning; each of the
(1993) suggested that ‘Quantitative reasoning is the six subsequent sections about a centr al topic in
analysis of a situation into a quantitativ e structure – mathematics learning is a summary of Papers 2 to 7.
a network of quantities and quantitative
relationships… A prominent characteristic of
reasoning quantitatively is that numbers and numeric
relationships are of secondar y impor tance, and do
not enter into the pr imary analysis of a situation.
16 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

Key understandings in number and ordinal number. By cardinal number, we


mean that two sets with the same n umber of items
mathematics: A summary in them are equal in amount. The term ordinal
of the topics reviewed number refers to the fact that n umbers are arranged
in an ordered ser ies of increasing magnitude:
Understanding extensive quantities successive numbers in the counting sequence are
and whole numbers greater than the preceding n umber by 1. Thus, 2 is
a greater quantity than 1 and 3 than 2 and it f ollows
Natural numbers are a way of representing that 3 must also greater than 1.
quantities that can be counted. When children learn
numbers, they must find out not just about the There are three different theories about how
counting sequence and how to count, but also about children come to co-ordinate their kno wledge
how the numbers in the counting system represent of quantities with their knowledge of counting.
quantities and relations between them. We found a
great deal of evidence that children are aware of The first is Piaget’s theory, which maintains that this
quantities such as the size of objects or the amount development is based on children’s schemas of
of items in groups of objects long before they learn action and the coordination of the schemas with
to count or under stand anything about the number each other. Three schemas of action are relevant
system. This is quite clear in their ability to to natural number: adding, taking away, and setting
discriminate objects by size and sets by objects in correspondence. Children must also
number when these discr iminations can be understand how these schemas relate to each other.
made perceptually. They must, for example, understand that a quantity
increases by addition, decreases by subtraction, and
Our review also showed that children learn to count that if you add and take away the same amount to
with surprisingly little difficulty. Counting is an activity an original quantity, that quantity stays the same. They
organised by principles such as the order in variance must also understand the additive composition of
of number labels, one-to-one correspondence number, which involves the coordination of one-to-
between items and counting labels, and the use of one correspondence with addition and subtr action:
the last label to say how many items are in the set. if the elements of two sets are placed in
There is no evidence of children being taught these correspondence but one has more elements than
principles systematically before they go to school the other, the larger set is the sum of the smaller set
and yet most children star ting school at the age of plus the number of elements for which there is no
five years are already able to respect these principles corresponding item in the smaller set. Research has
when counting and identify other people’s errors shown that this insight is not attained b y young
when they violate counting principles. children, who think that adding elements to the
smaller set will make it larger than the lar ger set
However, research on children’s numerical without considering the number added.
understanding has consistently shown that at first
they make very little connection between the A second view, in the form of a nativist theor y,
number words that they lear n and their existing has been suggested by Gelman and Butterworth
knowledge about quantities such as siz e and (Gelman & Butterworth, 2005). They propose that
the amounts of items. Our review showed that from bir th children have access to an innate , inexact
Thompson’s (1993) theoretical distinction between but powerful ‘analog’ system, whose magnitude
quantities and number is hugely relevant to increases directly with the number of objects in an
understanding children’s mathematics. For example, array, and they attach the n umber words to the
many four-year-old children understand how to share properties occasioning these magnitudes. According
objects equally between two or more people , on a to this view both the system f or knowing about
one-for-A, one-for-B basis, but have some difficulty quantities and the pr inciples of counting are innate
in understanding that the number of items in two and are naturally coordinated.
equally shared sets must be the same, i.e. that if
there are six sweets in one set, there must be six A third theoretical alternative, proposed by Carey
in the other set as well. To make the connection (2004), star ts from a standpoint in agreement with
between number words and quantities, children have Gelman’s theory with respect to the innate analog
to grasp two aspects of number, which are cardinal system and counting pr inciples. However, Carey does
17 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

not think that these systems are coordinated establish connections between counting and their
naturally: they become so through a ‘parallel understanding of quantities.
individuation’ system, which allows very young
children to make precise discriminations between Piaget’s studies concentrated on children’s ability
sets of one and two objects, and a little later, to reason logically about quantitative relations. He
between two and three objects. During the same argued that children must understand the inverse
period, these children also learn number words and, relation between addition and subtr action and also
through their recognition of 1, 2 and 3 as distinct additive composition (which he ter med class-
quantities, they manage to associate the r ight count inclusion and was later investigated under the label
words (‘one’, ‘two’ and ‘three’) with the r ight of par t-whole relations) in order to tr uly understand
quantities. This association between parallel number. The best way to test this sor t of causal
individuation and the count list ev entually leads to hypothesis is through a combination of longitudinal
what Carey (2004) calls ‘bootstrapping’: the children and intervention studies. Longitudinal studies with
lift themselves up by their own intellectual the appropriate controls can suggest that A is
bootstraps by inducing a r ule that the next count causally related to B if it is a specific predictor of B
word in the counting system is exactly one more at a later time . Intervention studies can test these
than the previous one . They do so, some time causal ideas: if children are successfull y taught A and,
between the age of three – and f ive years and, as a consequence, their learning of B improves, it
therefore, before they go to school. is safe to conclude that the natur al, longitudinal
connection between A and B is also a causal one .
One impor tant point to note about these three
theories is that they use diff erent definitions of It had been difficult in the past to use this
cardinal number, and therefore different criteria for combination of methods in the analysis of children’s
assessing whether children under stand cardinality mathematics learning for a variety of reasons. First,
or not. Piaget’s criterion is the one that we have researchers were not clear on what sor ts of logical
mentioned already and which we ourselves think to reasoning were vital to lear ning mathematics. There
be right: it is the under standing that two or more are now clearer hypothesis about this: the inverse
sets are equal in quantity when the number of items relation between addition and subtr action and
in them is the same (and vice-v ersa). Gelman’s and additive composition of number appear as key
Carey’s less demanding cr iterion for understanding concepts in the work of different researchers.
cardinality is the knowledge that the last count w ord Second, outcome measures of mathematics lear ning
for the set represents the set’ s quantity: if I count were difficult to find. The current availability of
‘one, two, three’ items and realise that means that standardised assessments, either developed for
that there are three in the set, I understand cardinal research or by policy makers for monitoring the
number. In our view, this second view of cardinality is performance of educational systems, makes both
inadequate for two reasons: first, it is actually based longitudinal and inter vention studies possible, as
on the position of the count w ord and is thus more these can be seen as valid outcome measures. Our
related to ordinal than cardinal n umber; second, it own research has shown that researcher designed
does not include any consideration of the fact that and government designed standardised assessments
cardinal number involves inferences regarding the are highly correlated and, when used as outcome
equivalence of sets. Piaget’s definition of cardinal and measures in longitudinal and inter vention studies,
ordinal number is much more stringent and it has lead to convergent conclusions. Finally, in order to
not been disputed by mathematics educators. He carry out inter vention studies, it is necessar y to
was sceptical of the idea that children w ould develop ways of teaching children the k ey concepts
understand cardinal and ordinal n umber concepts on which mathematics learning is grounded.
simply from learning how to count and the evidence Fortunately, there are cur rently successful
we reviewed definitely shows that learning about interventions that can be used f or fur ther research
quantities and numbers develop independently of to test the effect that learning about these key
each other in young children. concepts has on children’s mathematics learning.

This conclusion has impor tant educational Our review identified two longitudinal studies that
implications. Schools must not be satisfied with show that children’s understanding of logical aspects
teaching children how to count: they must ensure of number is vital for their mathematics lear ning.
that children learn not only to count but also to One was carried out in the United Kingdom and
18 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

showed that children’s understanding of the inverse C Tom has seven books: Jane has five: how many
relation between addition and subtr action and of more books does Tom have than Jane? Compare
additive composition at the beginning of school problem.
are specific predictors of their results in National
Curriculum maths tests (a government designed The main interest of these prob lems is that, although
and administered measure of children’s mathematics they all involve very simple and similar additions and
learning) about 14 months later, after controlling for subtractions, there are vast differences in the level of
their general cognitive ability, their knowledge of their difficulty. When the three kinds of prob lem are
number at school entr y, and their working memory. given in the form that we have just illustrated, the
Compare problems are very much harder than the
The second study, carried out in Ger many, showed Combine and Change problems. This is not because
that a measure of children’s understanding of the it is too difficult for the children to subtr act 5 from 7,
inverse relation between addition and subtr action which is how to solve this par ticular Compare
when they were eight years old was a predictor problem, but because they find it hard to work out
of their performance in an algebra test when they what to do so solve the problem. Compare
were in university; controlling for the children’s problems require reasoning about relations betw een
performance in an intelligence test giv en at age eight quantities, which children find a lot more difficult
had no effect on the strength of the connection than reasoning about quantities.
between their understanding of the inverse relation
and their performance in the algebr a measure. Thus the difficulty of these problems rests on how
well children manage to work out the ar ithmetical
Our review also showed that it is possible to relations that they involve. This conclusion is
improve children’s understanding of these logical supported by the fact that the relatively easy
aspects of number knowledge. Children who were problems become a great deal more diff icult if the
weak in this under standing at the beginning of school mathematical relations are less tr ansparent. For
and improved this understanding through a short example, the usually easy Change problem is a lot
intervention performed significantly better than a harder if the result is giv en and the children have to
control group that did not receiv e this teaching. work out the star ting point. For example, Wendy
Together, these studies allow us to conclude that it is had some pictures on her wall b ut then took 3 of
crucial for children to coordinate their understanding them down: now she has 4 pictures left on the wall:
of these logical aspects of quantities with their how many were there in the first place? The reason
learning of numbers in order to mak e good progress that children find this problem a relatively hard one
in mathematics learning. is that the stor y is about subtr action, but the solution
is an addition. Pupils therefore have to call on their
Our final step in this summar y of research on whole understanding of the inverse relation between
numbers considered how children use additive adding and subtracting to solve this problem.
reasoning to solve word problems. Additive
reasoning is the logical anal ysis of problems that One way of analysing children’s reactions to word
involve addition and subtr action, and of cour se the problems is with the fr amework devised by
key concepts of additive composition and the Vergnaud, who argued that these problems involve
inverse relation between addition and subtr action quantities, transformations and relations. A Change
play an essential role in this reasoning. The chief tool problem, for example, involves the initial quantity and
used to investigate additive reasoning is the word a transformation (the addition or subtr action) which
problem. In word problems a scene is set, usually in leads to a new quantity, while Compare problems
one or two sentences, and then a question is posed. involve two quantities and the relation between
We will give three examples. them. On the whole , problems that involve relations
are harder than those involving transformations, but
A Bob has three marbles and Bill has f our: how other factors, such as the stor y being about addition
many marbles do they have altogether? Combine and the solution being a subtr action or vice versa
problem. also have an effect.

B Wendy had four pictures on her wall and her The main impact of research on word problems has
parents gave her three more: how many does she been to reinforce the idea with which we began this
have now? Change problem. section. This idea is that in teaching children
19 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

arithmetic we must make a clear distinction two quantities are involved, a quantity to be shared
between numerical analysis and the children’s and a number of recipients of the shares.
understanding of quantitative relations. We must
remember that there is a great deal more to Partitioning is the scheme of action most often used
arithmetical learning than knowing how to carry in primary schools in the United Kingdom to
out numerical procedures. The children have to introduce the concept of fr actions. Research shows
understand the quantitative relations in the that children have quite a few problems to solve
problems that they are ask ed to solve and how when they par tition continuous quantities: for
to analyse these relations with numbers. example, they need to anticipate the connection
between number of cuts and n umber of parts, and
some children find themselves with an even number
Understanding rational numbers and of par ts (e.g. 6) when they wanted to ha ve an odd
intensive quantities number (e.g. 5) because they star t out by
partitioning the whole in half. Children also find it
Rational numbers, like whole numbers, can be used very difficult to understand the equivalence between
to represent quantities. There are some quantities fractions when the par ts they are asked to compare
that cannot be represented by a whole number, and do not look the same . For example, if they are
to represent these quantities, we must use rational shown two identical rectangles, each cut in half b ut in
numbers. Quantities that are represented b y whole different ways (e.g. horizontally and diagonally), many
numbers are formed by addition and subtr action: as 9- and 10-year-olds might say that the fr actions are
argued in the previous section, as we add elements not equivalent; in some studies, almost half of the
to a set and count them (or conventional units, in children in these age levels did not recognize the
the case of continuous quantities), we find out what equivalence of two halves that looked rather
number will be used to represent these quantities. different due to being the result of diff erent cuts.
Quantities that cannot be represented b y whole Also, if students are ask ed to paint 2/3 of a f igure
numbers are measured not by addition but by divided into 9 par ts, many 11- to 12-year-olds may
division: if we cut one chocolate, for example, in be unable to do so, even though they can paint 2/3
equal par ts, and want to have a number to represent of a figure divided into 3 par ts; in a study in the
the par ts, we cannot use a whole number. United Kingdom, about 40% of the students did not
successfully paint 2/3 of figures that had been divided
We cannot use whole n umbers when the quantity into 6 or 9 sections.
that we want to represent n umerically:
• is smaller than the unit used for counting, Different studies that we reviewed showed that
irrespective of whether this is a natural unit students who learn about fractions through the
(e.g. we have less than one banana) or a engagement of the par titioning schema in division
conventional unit (e.g. a fish weighs less than a kilo) tend to reply on perception r ather than on the
• involves a ratio between two other quantities (e.g. logic of division when solving prob lems: they are
the concentration of orange juice in a jar can be much more successful with items that can be
described by the ratio of orange concentrate to solved perceptually than with those that cannot.
water; the probability of an event can be described There is a clear lesson here f or education: number
by the ratio between the number of favourable understanding should be based on logic , not on
cases to the total number of cases). These perception alone, and teaching should be designed
quantities are called intensive quantities. to guide children to think about the logic of
rational numbers.
We have concluded from our review that there are
serious problems in teaching children about fr actions The research that we reviewed shows that the
and that intensive quantities are not explicitl y partitioning scheme develops over a long per iod of
considered in the cur riculum. time. This has led some researcher s to develop ways
to avoid asking the children to par tition quantities by
Children learn about quantities that are smaller providing them with pre-divided shapes or with
than the unit through division. Two types of action computer tools that do the partitioning for the
schemes are used by children in division situations: children. The use of these resources has positiv e
partitioning, which involves dividing a whole into effects, but these positive effects seem to be
equal par ts, and correspondence situations, where obtained only after large amounts of instr uction.
20 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

In some studies, the students had difficulties with the they can achieve some insight into the equivalence
idea of improper fr actions even after prolonged of fractions by thinking that, if there are twice as
instruction. For example, one student ar gued with many things to be shared and twice as man y
the researcher during instruction that you cannot recipients, then each one’s share is the same . This
have eight sevenths if you divided a whole into involves thinking about a direct relation betw een
seven parts. the quantities. The partitioning scheme leads to
understanding equivalence in a different way: if a
In contrast to the difficulties that children have with whole is cut into twice as man y par ts, the size of
partitioning, children as young as five or six years in each par t will be halved. This involves thinking about
age are quite good at using cor respondences in an inverse relation between the quantities in the
division, and do so without ha ving to carry out the problem. Research consistently shows that children
actual par titioning. Some children seem to understand direct relations better than inverse
understand even before receiving any instruction relations and this may also be tr ue of rational
on fractions that, for example, two chocolates shared number knowledge.
among four children and four chocolates shared
among eight children will give the children in the The arguments children use when stating that
two groups equivalent shares of chocolate; they fractional quantities resulting from shar ing are or are
demonstrate this equivalence in action b y showing not equivalent have been described in one study in
that in both cases there is one chocolate to be the United Kingdom. These arguments include the
shared by two children. use of correspondences (e.g. sharing four chocolates
among eight children can be sho wn by a diagram to
Children’s understanding of quantities smaller than be equivalent to shar ing two chocolates among four
one is often ahead of their kno wledge of fractional children because each chocolate is shared among
representations when they solve problems using two children), scalar arguments (twice the number of
the correspondence scheme. This is true of children and twice the n umber of chocolates means
understanding equivalence and even more so of that they all get the same), and an under standing of
understanding order. Most children at the age of the inverse relation between the number of par ts
eight or so realise that dividing 1 chocolate among and the size of the par ts (i.e. twice the number of
three children will give bigger pieces than dividing pieces means that each piece is halv ed in size). It
one chocolate among four children. This insight would be impor tant to investigate whether
that they have about quantities is not necessar ily increasing teachers’ awareness of children’s own
connected with their under standing of ordering arguments would help teachers guide children’s
fractions by magnitude: the same children might say learning in this domain of numbers more effectively.
that 1/3 is less than 1/4 because three is less than
four. So we find in the domain of rational numbers Some researchers have argued that a better star ting
the same distinction found in the domain of whole point for teaching children about fractions is the use
numbers between what children know about of situations where children can use cor respondence
quantities and what they know about the numbers reasoning than the use of situations where the
used to represent quantities. scheme of par titioning is the relevant one . Our
review of children’s understanding of the equivalence
Research shows that it is possible to help children and order of fr actions suppor ts this claim. However,
connect their understanding of quantities with their there are no inter vention studies comparing the
understanding of fractions and thus make progress outcomes of these two ways of introducing children
in rational number knowledge. Schools could make to the use of fr actions, and inter vention studies
use of children’s informal knowledge of fractional would be crucial to solve this issue: one thing is
quantities and work with problems about situations, children’s informal knowledge but the outcomes of
without requiring them to use f ormal its formalization through instruction might be quite
representations, to help them consolidate this another. There is now considerably more information
reasoning and prepare them f or formalization. regarding children’s informal strategies to allow for
new teaching programmes to be designed and
Reflecting about these two schemes of action assessed. There is also considerable work on
and drawing insights from them places children in curriculum development in the domain of teaching
different paths for understanding rational number. fractions in primary school. Research that compares
When children use the correspondence scheme, the different forms of teaching (based on par titioning
21 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

or based on cor respondences) and the introduction controls and so it does not allo w for establishing
of different representations (decimal or ordinar y) firmer conclusions.
is now much more feasible than in the past.
Intervention research, which could be carried out Students can learn procedures for comparing, adding
in the classroom, is urgently needed. The available and subtracting fractions without connecting these
evidence suggests that testing this h ypothesis procedures with their under standing of equivalence
appropriately could result in more successful and order of fr actional quantities, independently of
teaching and learning of rational numbers. whether they are taught with ordinar y or decimal
fractions representation. This is not a desired
In the United Kingdom ordinar y fractions continue outcome of instruction, but seems to be a quite
to play an impor tant role in pr imary school common one. Research that focuses on the use of
instruction whereas in some countries greater children’s informal knowledge suggests that it is
attention is given to decimal representation than to possible to help students mak e connections between
ordinary fractions in primary school. Two reasons are their informal knowledge and their lear ning of
proposed to justify the teaching of decimals bef ore procedures but the evidence is limited and the
ordinary fractions . First, decimals are common in consequences of this teaching have not been
metric measurement systems and thus their investigated systematically.
understanding is critical for learning other topics,
such as measurement, in mathematics and science. Research has also shown that students do not
Second, decimals should be easier than ordinar y spontaneously connect their knowledge of fractions
fractions to understand because decimals can be developed with extensive quantities smaller than the
taught as an extension of place value representation; unit with their under standing of intensive quantities.
operations with decimals should also be easier and Students who succeed in under standing that two
taught as extensions of place value representation. chocolates divided among four children and four
chocolates divided among 8 children yield the same
It is cer tainly true that decimals are used in size share do not necessar ily understand that a paint
measurement and thus lear ning decimals is necessar y mixture made with two litres of white and tw o of
but ordinary fractions often appear in algebraic blue paint will be the same shade as one made with
expressions; so it is not clear a priori whether one four litres of white and f our of blue paint.
form of representation is more useful than the other
for learning other aspects of mathematics. However, Researchers have for some time distinguished
the second argument, that decimals are easier than between different situations where fr actions are
ordinary fractions, is not suppor ted in sur veys of used and argued that connections that seem
students’ performance: students find it difficult to obvious to an adult are not necessar ily obvious
make judgements of equivalence and order as m uch to children. There is now evidence that this is so .
with decimals as with ordinar y fractions. Students There is a clear educational implication of this
aged 9 to 11 years have limited success when result: if teaching children about fr actions in the
comparing decimals written with different numbers domain of extensive quantities smaller than the
of digits after the decimal point (e.g. 0.5 and 0.36): unit does not spontaneously transfer to their
the rate of correct responses varied between 36% understanding of intensive quantities, a complete
and 52% in the three diff erent countries that fractions curriculum should include intensive
participated in the study, even though all the children quantities in the progr amme.
have been taught about decimals.
Finally, this review opens the wa y for a fresh research
Some researchers (e.g. Nunes, 1997; Tall, 1992; agenda in the teaching and lear ning of fractions. The
Vergnaud, 1997) argue that different representations source for the new research questions is the finding
shed light on the same concepts from different that children achieve insights into relations between
perspectives. This would suggest that a way to fractional quantities before knowing how to
strengthen students’ learning of rational numbers is represent them. It is possible to envisage a research
to help them connect both representations. Case agenda that would not focus on children’s
studies of students who received instruction that misconceptions about fractions, but on children’s
aimed at helping students connect the tw o forms of possibilities of success when teaching star ts from
representation show encouraging results. However, thinking about quantities r ather than from lear ning
the investigation did not include the appropr iate fractional representations.
22 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

Understanding relations and their proportions previously led to the conclusion that
graphical representation students’ problems with propor tional reasoning
Children form concepts about quantities from their stemmed from their difficulties with multiplicative
everyday experiences and can use their schemas of reasoning. However, there is presently much
action with diverse representations of the quantities evidence to show that, from a relatively early age
(iconic, numerical) to solve problems. They often (about five to six years in the United Kingdom),
develop sufficient awareness of quantities to discuss many children (our estimate is about tw o-thirds)
their equivalence and order as w ell as how quantities already have informal knowledge that allows them
are changed by operations. It is significantly more to solve multiplicative reasoning problems.
difficult for them to become aware of the relations
between quantities and oper ate on relations. Multiplicative reasoning problems are defined by the
fact that they involve two (or more) measures
The difficulty of understanding relations is clear both linked by a fixed ratio. Students’ informal knowledge
with additive and multiplicative relations between of multiplicative reasoning stems from the schema
quantities. Children aged about eight to ten y ears of one-to-many correspondence, which they use
can easily say, for example, how many marbles a boy both in multiplication and division problems. When
will have in the end if he star ted a game with six the product is unknown, children set the elements
marbles, won five in the first game, lost three in the in the two measures in cor respondence (e.g. one
second game, and won two in the third game. sweet costs 4p) and figure out the product (ho w
However, if they are not told ho w many marbles the much five sweets will cost) by counting or adding.
boy had at the start and are asked how many more When the correspondence is unknown (e.g. if you
or fewer marbles this boy will have after playing the pay 20p for five sweets, how much does each
three games, they find this second problem sweet cost), the children share out the elements
considerable harder, par ticularly if the first game (20p shared in five groups) to find what the
involves a loss. correspondence is.

Even if the children are taught how to represent This informal knowledge is currently ignored in
relations and recognise that winning five in the first U.K. schools, probably due to the theor y that
game does not mean having five marbles, they often multiplication is essentially repeated addition and
interpret the results of oper ations on relations as if division is repeated subtr action. However, the
they were quantities. Children find both additive and connections between addition and multiplication on
multiplicative relations significantly more difficult than the one hand, and subtraction and division on the
understanding quantities. other hand, are procedural and not conceptual.
So students’ informal knowledge of multiplicative
There is little evidence that the design of reasoning could be developed in school from an
mathematics curricula has so far tak en into account earlier age.
the impor tance of helping students become a ware
of the difference between quantities and relations. Even after being taught other methods to solv e
Some researchers have carried out experimental proportions problems in school, students continue
teaching studies which suggest that it is possib le to to use one-to-many correspondences reasoning to
promote students’ awareness of additive relations solve propor tions problems; these solutions have
as different from quantities; this was not an easy been called building up methods. For example, if a
task but the instr uction seemed to have positive recipe for four people is to be adapted to serve six
results (but note that there were no control people, students figure out that six people is the
groups). Fur ther research must be carried out to same as four people plus two people; so they figure
analyse how this knowledge affects mathematics out what half the ingredients will be and add this to
learning: longitudinal and inter vention studies the quantity required for four people. Building up
would be cr ucial to clarify this. If positive results methods have been documented in many different
are found, there will be imperative policy countries and also among people with lo w levels of
implications. schooling. A careful analysis of the reasoning in
building-up methods suggests that the students focus
The first teaching that children receive in school on the quantities as they solve these problems, and
about multiplicative relations is about propor tions. find it difficult to focus on the relations between
Initial studies on students’ understanding of the quantities.
23 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Research carried out independently in different mathematical structures and physical knowledge
countries has shown that students sometimes use at a later point. Representations using ordinar y and
additive reasoning about relations when the decimal fractions and the number line are seen as
appropriate model is a multiplicative one. Some the tools that can allow students to abstr act early
recent research has shown that students also use on from the physical situations. Students should lear n
multiplicative reasoning in situations where the early on to represent equivalences betw een ordinary
appropriate model is additive. These results suggest fractions (e.g. 2/4 = 4/8), a representation that would
that children use additive and multiplicative models provide insight into propor tions, and also
implicitly and do not mak e conscious decisions equivalences between ordinary and decimal fractions
regarding which model is appropr iate in a specific (2/4 = 0.5), which would provide insight into the
situation. We concluded from our review that ordering and equivalence of fr actions marked on
students’ problems with propor tional reasoning the number line.
stems from their difficulties in becoming explicitly
aware of relations between quantities. Greater Each of these approaches mak es assumptions about
awareness of the models implicit in their solutions the significance of students’ informal knowledge at
would help them distinguish between situations that the star t of the teaching progr amme. The functional
involve different types of relations: additive, approach assumes that students’ informal knowledge
proportional or quadratic, for example. can be formalised through instruction and that this
will be beneficial to learning. The algebraic approach
The educational implication from these f indings assumes that students’ informal knowledge is an
is that schools should tak e up the task of helping obstacle to students’ mathematics learning. There is
students become more aware of the models that evidence from a combination of longitudinal and
they use implicitly and of ways of testing their intervention methods, albeit with younger children,
appropriateness to par ticular situations. The that shows that students’ knowledge of informal
differences between additive and multiplicative multiplicative reasoning is a causal and positiv e factor
situations rests on the relations between quantities; in mathematics learning. Children who scored higher
so it is likely that the cr itical move here is to help in multiplicative reasoning problems at the star t of
students become aware of the relations between their first year in school performed significantly
quantities implicit in the procedure they use to better in the government designed and school
solve problems. administered mathematics achievement test than
those whose scores were lower. This longitudinal
Two radically different approaches to teaching relationship remained significant after the appropr iate
proportions and linear functions in schools can be controls were taken into account. The intervention
identified in the liter ature. These constitute pragmatic study provides results that are less clear because the
theories, which can guide teacher s, but have as yet children were taught not only about multiplicative
not been tested systematically. The first, described as reasoning but also about other concepts considered
functional and human in focus, is based on the key to mathematics learning. Nevertheless, children
notion that students’ schemas of action should be who were at risk for mathematics learning and
the star ting point for this teaching. Through received teaching that included multiplicative
instruction, they should become progressively more reasoning, along with two other concepts, showed
aware of the relations between quantities that can average achievement in the standardised
be identified in such problems. Diagrams, tables and mathematics achievement tests whereas the
graphs are seen as tools that could help students control group remained in the bottom 20% of the
understand the models of situations that they are distribution, as predicted by their assessment at the
using and make them into models f or other start of school. So, in terms of the assumptions
situations later. regarding the role of inf ormal knowledge, the
functional approach seems to have the edge over
The second, described in the literature as algebraic, the algebraic approach.
proposes that there should be a sharp separation
between students’ intuitive knowledge, in which These two approaches to instr uction also differ in
physical and mathematical knowledge are respect to what students need to know to benefit
intertwined, and mathematical knowledge. Students from teaching and what they lear n during the course
should be led to f ormalisations early on in of instruction. Within the functional approach, the
instruction and re-establish the connections between tools used in teaching are diagr ams, tables, and
24 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

graphs so it is clear that students need to lear n to reason is that many of the spatial relations that
read graphs in order to be ab le to use them as tools children must think about and lear n to analyse
for thinking about relations between quantities and mathematically in geometr y classes are different from
functions. Research has shown that students have the spatial relations that they lear n about in their
ideas about how to read gr aphs before instruction pre-school years. The second is that geometr y makes
and these ideas should be taken into account when great demands on children’s spatial imagination. In
graphs are used in the classroom. It is possible to order to measure length or area or angle , for
teach students to read gr aphs and to use them in example, we have to imagine spaces divided into
order to think about relations in the course of equal units and this tur ns out to be quite hard f or
instruction about propor tions, but much more children to learn to do systematically.
research is needed to show how students’ thinking
changes if they do lear n to use gr aphs to analyse the Nevertheless, pre-school children’s spatial knowledge
type of relation relevant in specific situations. Within and spatial experiences are undoubtedly relevant to
the algebraic approach, it is assumed that students the geometry that they must learn about later, and it
understand the equivalence of fr actions without is impor tant for teachers and researchers alike to
reference to situations. Our review of students’ recognise this. From a very early age children are
understanding of fractions, summarised in the able to distinguish and remember diff erent shapes,
previous section, shows that this is not tr ivial so including basic geometrical shapes. Children are able
it is necessar y to show that students can, in the to co-ordinate visual information about size and
course of this teaching, learn both about fr action distance to recognise objects by their actual size,
equivalence and propor tional relations. and also to co-ordinate visual shape and or ientation
information to recognise objects by their actual
There is no evidence to show how either of these shapes. In social situations, children quite easily work
approaches to teaching works in promoting out what someone else is looking at b y extrapolating
students’ progress nor that one of them is more that person’s line of sight often across quite lar ge
successful than the other. Research that can clar ify distances, which is an impressive feat of spatial
this issue is ur gently needed and could have a major imagination. Finally, they are highly sensitive not just
impact in promoting better lear ning by U.K. to the orientation of lines and of objects in their
students. This is par ticularly impor tant in view of environments, but also to the relation betw een
findings from the international comparisons that orientations: for example, young children can,
show that U.K. students do relatively well in additive sometimes at least, recognise when a line in the
reasoning items but comparatively poorly in foreground is parallel to a stable background feature.
multiplicative reasoning items.
These impressive spatial achievements must help
children in their efforts to understand the geometr y
Understanding space and its that they are taught about at school, but there is little
representation in mathematics direct research on the links betw een children’s
existing informal knowledge about space and the
When children begin to be taught about geometr y, progress that they make when they are eventually
they already know a great deal about space , shape, taught about geometry. This is a worrying gap,
size, distance and or ientation, which are the basic because research of this sor t would help teachers
subject matter of geometr y. They are also quite to make an effective connection between what their
capable of drawing logical inferences about spatial pupils know already and what they ha ve to learn in
matters. In fact, their spatial knowledge is so their initial geometr y classes. It would also give us a
impressive and so sophisticated that one might better understanding of the obstacles that children
expect geometry to be an easy subject f or them. encounter when they are first taught about geometr y.
Why should they have any difficulty at all with
geometry if the subject just in volves learning how Some of these obstacles are immediatel y apparent
to express this spatial knowledge mathematically? when children learn about measurement, first of
length and then of area. In order to lear n how to
However, many children do find geometry hard and measure length, children must grasp the underlying
some children continue to make basic mistakes right logic of measurement and also the role of iter ated
through their time at school. There are two main (i.e. repeated) measurement units, e.g. the unit of 1
reasons for these well-documented difficulties. One cm repeated on a r uler. Using a r uler also involves an
25 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

active form of one-to-one cor respondence, since the rule for finding the area of tr iangles, A = ½
the child must imagine and impose on the line being (base x height), is justified by the fact every triangle
measured the same units that are explicit and can be transformed into a par allelogram with the
obvious on the ruler. Research suggests that children same base and height b y doubling that triangle. Thus,
do have a reasonable understanding of the rules for measuring area rest heavily on the relations
underlying logic of measurement by the time that between geometric shapes. Although Wertheimer
they begin to lear n about geometr y, but that many did some ingenious studies on ho w children were
have a great deal of diff iculty in grasping how to able to use of the relations betw een shapes to help
imagine one-to-one correspondence between the them measure the area of some of these shapes,
iterated units on the r uler and imagined equivalent very little research has been done since then on
units on the line that they are measur ing. One their understanding of this centr ally impor tant
common mistake is to set the 1 cm r ather than aspect of geometr y.
the 0 cm point at one end of the line . The evidence
suggests that many children apply a poorly In contrast, there is a great deal of research on
understood procedure when they measure length children’s understanding of angles. This research
and are not thinking, as they should, of one-to-one shows that children have very little under standing
correspondence between the units on the r uler and of angles before they are taught about geometr y.
the length being measured. There is no doubt that The knowledge that they do ha ve tends to be
teachers should think about how to promote quite disconnected because children often fail to
children’s reflection on measurement procedures. see the connection between angles in dissimilar
Nunes, Light and Mason (1993), for example, contexts, like the steepness of a slope and ho w
showed that using a brok en ruler was one way much a per son has to tur n at a cor ner. There is
to promote this. evidence that children begin to connect what they
know about angles as they gro w older : they
Measurement of area presents additional prob lems. acquire, in the end, a fairly abstract understanding
One is that area is often calculated from lengths, of angle. There is also evidence , mostly from
rather than measured. So, although the measurement studies with the progr amming language Logo, that
is in one kind of unit, e.g. centimetres, the final children learn about angle relatively well in the
calculation is in another, e.g. square centimetres. This context of movement.
is what Vergnaud calls a ‘product of measures’
calculation. Another potential problem is that most Children’s initial uncer tainties with angles contrast
calculations of area are multiplicative: with rectangles sharply to the relative ease with which they adopt
and parallelograms, one has to multiply the figure’s the Cartesian framework for plotting positions in
base by height, and with tr iangles one must calculate any two-dimensional space. This framework requires
base by height and then halve it. There is evidence them to be able to extrapolate imaginary pairs of
that many children attempt to calculate area b y straight lines, one of which is perpendicular to the
adding par ts of the per imeter, rather than by vertical axis and the other to the hor izontal axis, and
multiplying. One consequence of the m ultiplicative then to work out where these imaginar y lines will
nature of area calculations is that doub ling a figure’s meet, in order to plot specif ic positions in space.
dimensions more than doubles its area. Think of a At first sight this might seem an extraordinarily
rectangle with a base of 10 cm and a height of 4 cm, sophisticated achievement, but research suggests
its area is 40 cm 2: if you enlarge the figure by that it presents no intellectual obstacle at all to most
doubling its base and height (20 cm x 8 cm), you children. Their success in extrapolating imaginary
quadruple its area (160 cm 2). This set of relations straight lines and working out their meeting point ma y
is hard for pupils, and for many adults too, to stem for their early experiences in social interactions
understand. of extrapolating such lines when working out what
other people are looking at, but we need longitudinal
The measurement of area also raises the question research to establish whether this is so. Some fur ther
of relations between shapes. For example the proof research suggests that, although children can usually
that the same base b y height r ule for measuring work out specific spatial positions on the basis of
rectangles applies to par allelograms as well rests Cartesian co-ordinates, they often find it hard to use
on the demonstration that a rectangle can be these co-ordinates to work out the relation between
transformed into a par allelogram with the same two or more different positions in space .
height and base without changing its area. In turn
26 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

We also need research on another possib le represent unknowns and variables. Distinguishing
connection between children’s early informal spatial between these meanings is usuall y not taught
knowledge and how well they learn about geometry explicitly, and this lack of instr uction might cause
later on. We know that very young children tell children some difficulty: g, for example, can indicate
shapes apart, even abstract geometric shapes, grams, acceleration due to gr avity, an unknown in an
extremely well, but it is also clear that when children equation, or a var iable in an expression.
begin to learn about geometr y they often find it
hard to decompose complicated shapes into sev eral Within common algebraic usage, Küchemann (1981)
simpler component shapes. This is a worrying identified six different ways adolescents used letter s
difficulty because the decomposition of shapes pla ys in the Chelsea diagnostic test instr ument (Har t et al.,
an impor tant par t in learning about measurement of 1984). Letters could be evaluated in some wa y,
area and also of angles. More research is needed on ignored, used as shor thand for objects or treated as
how children learn that par ticular shapes can be objects used as a specific unknown, as a gener alised
broken down into other component shapes. number, or as a var iable. These interpretations
appear to be task-dependent, so learners had
Overall, research suggests that the relation between developed a sense of what sor ts of question were
the informal knowledge that children build up before treated in what kinds of wa ys, i.e. generalising
they go to school and the progress that they mak e (sometimes idiosyncratically) about question-types
at school in geometr y is a cr ucial one. Yet, it is a through familiarity and prior experience.
relation on which there is v ery little research indeed
and there are few theories about this possible link as The early experiences students have in algebra are
well. The theoretical frameworks that do exist tend therefore very impor tant, and if algebr a is presented
to be pragmatic ones. For example, the Institute as ‘arithmetic with letter s’ there are many possible
Freudenthal group assume a strong link betw een confusions. Algebraic statements are about
children’s preschool spatial knowledge and the relationships between variables, constructed using
progress that they make in learning about geometr y operations; they cannot be calculated to find an
later on, and argue that improving children’s early answer until numbers are substituted, and the same
understanding of space will have a beneficial effect relationship can often be represented in many
on their learning about geometr y. Yet, there is no different ways. The concept of equivalent
good empirical evidence for either of these two expressions is at the heart of algebraic manipulation,
important claims. simplification, and expansion, but this is not al ways
apparent to students. Students who do not
understand this try to act on algebr aic expressions
Algebraic reasoning and equations in ways which have worked in
arithmetical contexts, such as trial-and-error, or
Research on learning algebra has considered a r ange trying to calculate when they see the equals sign,
of new ideas that have to be under stood in school or rely on learnt rules such as ‘BODMAS’ which
mathematics: the use and meaning of letters and can be misapplied.
expressions to represent numbers and variables;
operations and their proper ties; relations, functions, Students’ prior experience of equations is often
equations and inequalities; manipulation and associated with finding hidden numbers using
transformation of symbolic statements. Young arithmetical facts, such as ‘what number, times by 4,
children are capable of understanding the use of a gives 24?’ being expressed as 4p = 24. An algebraic
letter to take the place of an unkno wn number, and approach depends on under standing operations or
are also able to construct statements about functions and their inverses, so that addition and
comparisons between unknown quantities, but subtraction are understood as a pair, and
algebra is much more than the substitution of letter s multiplication and division are under stood as a pair.
for numbers and numbers for letters. Letters are This was discussed in an ear lier section. Later on,
used in mathematics in var ying ways. They are used roots, exponents and logar ithms also need to be
as labels for objects that have no numerical value, seen as related along with other functions and their
such as vertices of shapes or f or objects that do inverses. Algebraic understanding also depends on
have numerical value, such as lengths of sides of understanding an equation as equating two
shapes. They denote fixed constants such as g, e or π expressions, and solving them as f inding out for
and also non-numerical constants such as I and they what values of the var iable they are equal. New
27 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

technologies such as gr aph-plotters and spreadsheets structured in ways that relate to under lying
have made multiple representations available and mathematical structure. Algebraic relationships
there is substantial evidence that students who ha ve represented by graphs, spreadsheets and
these tools available over time develop a stronger diagrammatic forms are often easier to understand
understanding of the meaning of expressions, and than when they are expressed in symbols. For
equations, and their solutions, than equivalent example, students who use function machines are
students who have used only formal pencil-and- more likely to understand the order of oper ations
paper techniques. in inverse functions.

Students have to learn that whereas the The difficulties learners have with algebra in
mathematical objects they have understood in secondary school are near ly all due to their inability
primary school can often be modelled with to shift from ear lier understandings of arithmetic to
material objects they now have to deal with objects the new possibilities afforded by algebraic notation.
that cannot always be easily related to their
understanding of the mater ial world, or to their out- • They make intuitive assumptions and apply
of-school language use. The use of concrete models pragmatic reasoning to a symbol system they do
such as rods of ‘unknown’ related lengths, tiles of not yet understand.
‘unknown’ related areas, equations seen as balances,
and other diagrammatical methods can provide • They need to grasp the idea that an algebraic
bridges between students’ past experience and expression is a statement about relationships
abstract relationships and can enable them to make between numbers and operations.
the shift to seeing relations r ather than number as
the main focus of mathematics. All these metaphors • They may confuse equality with equivalence and try
have limitations and eventually, par ticularly with the to get answers rather than transform expressions.
introduction of negative numbers, the metaphors
they provide break down. Indeed it was this • They get confused between using a letter to stand
realisation that led to the in vention of for something they know, and using it to stand for
algebraic notation. something they do not know, and using it to stand
for a variable.
Students have many perceptions and cognitive
tendencies that can be har nessed to help them lear n • They may not have a purpose for using algebra,
algebra. They naturally try to relate what they are such as expressing a generality or relationship, so
offered to what they already kno w. While this can cannot see the meaning of what they are doing.
be a problem if students refer to computational
arithmetic, or alphabetic meaning of letters New technologies offer immense possibilities for
(e.g. a = apples), it can also be useful if they ref er to imbuing algebraic tasks with meaning, and for
their understanding of relations between quantities generating a need for algebraic expression.
and operations and inverses. For example, when
students devise their own methods for mental The research synthesis sets these obser vations out
calculation they often use relations betw een numbers in detail and focuses on detailed aspects of algebr aic
and the concepts of distr ibutivity and associativity. activity that manifest themselves in school
mathematics. It also formulates recommendations
Students naturally try to generalise when they see for practice and research.
repeated behaviour, and this ability has been used
successfully in approaches to algebr a that focus on
expressing generalities which emerge in Modelling, problem-solving and
mathematical exploration. When learners need to integrating concepts
express generality, the use of letter s to do so mak es
sense to them, although they still have to learn the Older students’ mathematical learning involves
precise syntax of their use in order to comm unicate situations in which it is not immediately apparent
unambiguously. Students also respond to the visual what mathematics needs to be done or applied,
impact of mathematics, and make inferences based nor how this new situation relates to previous
on layout, graphical interpretation and patterns in knowledge. Learning mathematics includes lear ning
text; their own mathematical jottings can be when and how to adapt symbols and meanings to
28 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

apply them in unfamiliar situations and also relationships (beyond linear relationships with which
knowing when and how to adapt situations and they are already familiar), but they also need to think
representations so that familiar tools can be about relations between relations. Our analysis
brought to bear on them. Students need to lear n (see Paper 4) suggested that cur ricula presently do
how to analyse complex situations in a var iety of not consider the impor tant task of helping students
representations, identify variables and relationships, become aware of the distinctions between quantities
represent these and develop predictions or and relations; this task is left to the students
conclusions from working with representations themselves. It is possible that helping students mak e
of variables and relationships. These might be this distinction at an ear lier age could have a positive
presented graphically, symbolically, diagrammatically impact on their later lear ning of algebra.
or numerically.
In the absence of specific instructions, students tend
In secondary mathematics, students possess not only to repeat patterns of learning that have enabled
intuitive knowledge from outside mathematics and them to succeed in other situations o ver time.
outside school, but also a r ange of quasi-intuitive Students tend to star t on new problems with
understandings within mathematics, derived from qualitative judgements based on a particular context,
earlier teaching and gener alisations, metaphors, or the visual appear ance of symbolic representations,
images and strategies that have served them well in then tend to use additive reasoning, then form
the past. In Tall and Vinner’s pragmatic theory, (1981) relationships by pattern recognition or repeated
these are called ‘concept images’, which are a r agbag addition, and then shift to propor tional and relational
of personal conceptual, quasi-conceptual, perceptual thinking if necessar y. The tendency to use addition as
and other associations that relate to the language of a first resor t persists as an obstacle into secondar y
the concept and are loosel y connected by the mathematics. Students also tend to check their
language and observable artefacts associated with arithmetic if answers conflict rather than adapting
the concept. The difference between students’ their reasoning by seeing if answers make sense or
concept images and conventional definitions causes not, or by analysing what sorts of relations are
problems when they come to lear n new concepts important in the problem. Pedagogic intervention
that combine different earlier concepts. They have over time is needed to enab le learners to look for
to expand elementar y meanings to under stand new underlying structure and, where multiple
abstract concepts, and sometimes these concepts representations are available (graphs, data, formulae,
do not fit with the images and models that students spreadsheets), students can, over time, develop new
know. For example, rules for combining quantities do habits that focus on covariation of variables.
not easily extend to negative numbers; multiplication However, they need knowledge and experience of a
as repeated addition does not easily extend to range of functions to dr aw on. Students are unlikely
multiplying decimals. to detect an exponential relationship unless they
have seen one before, but they can descr ibe changes
There is little research and theoretical explor ation between nearby values in additive terms. A shift to
regarding how combinations of concepts are describing changes in multiplicative terms does not
understood by students in general. For example, happen naturally.
it would be helpful to know if students who
understand the use of letters, ratio, angle, functions, We hoped to find evidence about how students
and geometrical facts well have the same difficulties learn to use mathematics to solv e problems when it
in learning early trigonometry as those whose is not immediately clear what mathematics they
understanding is more tenuous. Similarly, it would should be using. Some evidence in elementar y
be helpful to know if students whose algebraic situations has been descr ibed in an ear lier section,
manipulation skills are fluent understand quadratic but at secondar y level there is only evidence of
functions more easily, or differently, from students successful strategies, and not about how students
who do not have this, but do understand come to have these strategies. In modelling and
transformation of graphs. some other problem-solving situations successful
students know how to identify var iables and how to
There is research about how students learn to use form an image of simultaneous variation. Successful
and apply their knowledge of functions, par ticularly students know how to hold one var iable still while
in the context of modelling and prob lem-solving. the change in another is obser ved. They are also able
Students not only have to learn to think about to draw on a reper toire of known function-types to
29 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

say more about how the changes in var iables Five common themes across
are related. It is more common to f ind secondary
students treating each situation as ad hoc and
the topics reviewed
using trial-and-adjustment methods which are In our view, a set of coherent themes cuts across the
arithmetically-based. Pedagogic intervention over rich, and at first sight heterogeneous, topics around
time is needed to enable them to shift towards which we have organised our outline. These themes
seeing relationships, and relations between relations, rise naturally from the material that we have
algebraically and using a range of representational mentioned, and they do not include recent attempts
tools to help them do so . to link brain studies with mathematical education. In
our view, knowledge of brain functions is not yet
The tendencies described above are specific sophisticated enough to account for assigning meaning,
instances of a more general issue. ‘Outside’ forming mathematical relationships or manipulating
experiential knowledge is seldom appropr iate as symbols, which we have concluded are the significant
a source for meaning in higher mathematics, and topics in studies of mathematical lear ning.
students need to lear n how to distinguish between
situations where earlier and ‘outside’ understandings In this section, we summarise five themes that
are, and are not, going to be helpful. For example, is emerged as significant across the research on the
it helpful to use your ‘outside’ knowledge about different topics, summarised in the previous sections.
cooking when solving a r atio problem about the size
of cakes? In abstract mathematics the same is tr ue:
the word ‘similar’ means something rather vague Number
in everyday speech, but has specific meaning in
mathematics. Even within mathematics there are Number is not a unitar y idea that develops
ambiguities. We have to understand, for example, conceptually in a linear fashion. In learning, and in
that -40 is greater in magnitude than -4, but a mathematical meaning, understanding of number
smaller number. develops in complementar y strands, sometimes with
discontinuities and changes of meaning. Emphasis on
All students generalise inductively from the examples calculation and manipulation with n umbers rather
they are given. Research evidence of secondar y than on understanding the underlying relations and
mathematics reveals many typical problems that mathematical meanings can lead to o ver-reliance and
arise because of gener alising irrelevant features misapplication of methods.
of examples, or over-generalising the domain of
applicability of a method, but we found little Most children star t school with everyday
systematic research to show instances where the understandings that can contr ibute to their ear ly
ability to generalise contributes positively to learning learning of number. They understand ‘more’ and ‘less’
difficult concepts, except to gener ate a need to lear n without knowing actual quantities, and can compare
the syntax of algebr a. discrete and continuous quantities of familiar objects.
Whole number is the tool which enab les them to be
Finally, we found considerable evidence that precise about comparisons and relations between
students do, given appropriate experiences over quantities, once they under stand cardinality.
time, change the ways in which they approach
unfamiliar mathematical situations and new Learning to count and under standing quantities are
concepts. We only found anecdotal evidence that separate strands of development which have to be
these new ways to view situations are extended experienced alongside each other. This allows
outside the mathematics classroom. There is comparisons and combinations to be made that are
considerable evidence from long-ter m curriculum expressed as relations. Counting on its own does not
studies that the procedures students ha ve to learn provide for these. Counting on its own also means
in secondar y mathematics are lear nt more easily if that the shift from discrete to contin uous number is
they relate to less f ormal explorations they have a conceptual discontinuity rather than an extension
already undertaken. There is evidence that of meaning.
discussion, verbalisation, and explicitness about
learning can help students mak e these changes. Rational numbers (we have used ‘fraction’ and
‘rational number’ interchangeably in order to f ocus
on their meaning for learners, rather than on their
30 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

mathematical definitions) arise naturally for children understanding of mathematics. However, Piaget’s
from understanding division in sharing situations, theory has fallen out of fa vour in recent years, and
rather than from par titioning wholes. Understanding many leading researchers on mathematics learning
rational numbers as a way of comparing quantities is either ignore or actively dismiss his and his colleagues’
fundamental to the development of multiplicative contribution to the subject. So, our conclusion about
and propor tional reasoning, and to applications in the impor tance of logic may seem a sur prising one
geometry, science, and everyday life. This is not the but, in our view, it is absolutely inescapable. We
same as saying that children should do ar ithmetic conclude that the evidence demonstr ates beyond
with rational numbers. The decimal representation doubt that children rely on logic in lear ning
does not afford this connection (although it is mathematics and that many of their difficulties in
relatively easy to do additive arithmetic with decimal solving mathematical problems are due to failures on
fractions, as long as the same n umber of digits their par t to make the correct logical move which
appears after the decimal point). would have led them to the cor rect solution.

The connection between number and quantity We have reviewed evidence that four different
becomes less obvious in higher mathematics, e.g. on aspects of logic have a crucial role in lear ning about
the co-ordinate plane the n umbers indicate scaled mathematics. Within each of these aspects we have
lengths from the axes, but are more usefully been able to identify definite changes over time in
understood as values of the var iables in a function. children’s understanding and use of the logic in
Students also have to extend the meaning of question. The four aspects follow.
number to include negative numbers, infinitesimals,
irrationals, and possibly complex numbers. Number The logic of correspondence (one-to-one
has to be abstr acted from images of quantity and and one-to-many correspondence)
used as a set of related, continuous, values which Children must understand one-to-one
cannot all be expressed or depicted precisel y. correspondence in order to lear n about cardinal
Students also have to be able to handle number-like number. Initially they are much more adept at
entities in the form of algebraic terms, expressions applying this kind of cor respondence when they
and functions. In these contexts, the idea of n umber share than when they compare spatial arrays of
as a systematically related set (and subsets) is centr al items. The extension of the use of one-to-one
to manipulation and tr ansformation; they behave like correspondence from sharing to working out the
numbers in relations, but are not defined quantities numerical equivalence or non-equivalence of two or
that can be enumerated. Ordinality of number also more spatial arrays is a vastly impor tant step in early
has a place in mathematics, in the domain of mathematical learning.
functions that generate sequences, and also in
several statistical techniques. One–to-many correspondence, which itself is an
extension of children’s existing knowledge of one-to-
Successful learning of mathematics includes one correspondences, plays an essential, but until
understanding that number describes quantity; being recently largely ignored, par t in children’s learning
able to make and use distinctions between different, about multiplication. Researchers and teachers have
but related, meanings of number; being able to use failed to consider that one-to-many correspondence
relations and meanings to inform application and is a possible basis for children’s initial multiplicative
calculation; being able to use number relations to reasoning because of a wide-spread assumption that
move away from images of quantity and use n umber this reasoning is based on children’s additive
as a structured, abstract, concept. knowledge. However, recent evidence on how to
introduce children to multiplication shows that
teaching them multiplication in terms of one-to-many
Logical reasoning plays a crucial correspondence is more effective than teaching them
part in every branch of mathematical about multiplication as repeated addition.
learning
The logic of inversion
The impor tance of logic in children’s understanding The subject of inversion was also neglected until
and learning of mathematics is a centr al theme in our fairly recently, but it is now clear that under standing
review. This idea is not a new one , since it was also that the addition and subtr action of the same
the main claim that Piaget made about children’ s quantity leaves the quantity of a set unchanged is of
31 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

great impor tance in children’s additive reasoning. for children finding it difficult to apply transitive
Longitudinal evidence also shows that this reasoning to measurement successfully is that they
understanding is a strong predictor of children’s often do not gr asp the impor tance of iteration
mathematical progress. Experimental research (repeated units of measurement). These difficulties
demonstrates that a flexible understanding of persist through primary school.
inversion is an essential element in children’ s
geometrical reasoning as well. It is highly likely that One of the reasons wh y Piaget’s ideas about the
children’s learning about the inverse relation importance of logic in children’s mathematical
between multiplication and division is an equally understanding have been ignored recently is
important a par t of mathematical lear ning, but the probably the nature of evidence that he off ered for
right research still has to be done on this question. them. Although Piaget’s main idea was a positiv e
Despite this gap, there is a clear case for giving the one (children’s logical abilities deter mine their
concept of inversion a great deal more prominence learning about mathematics), his empirical evidence
than it has now in the school cur riculum. for this idea was mainly negative: it was about
children’s difficulties with the four aspects of logic
The logic of class inclusion and additive that we have just discussed. A constant theme in
composition our review is that this is not the best wa y to test
Numbers consist of other numbers. One cannot a causal theor y about mathematical lear ning. We
understand what 6 means unless one also kno ws advocate instead a combination of longitudinal
that sets of 6 are composed of 5 + 1 items, or 4 + research with inter vention studies. The results of this
2 items etc. The logic that allows children to work kind of research do strongly suppor t the idea that
out that every number is a set of combination of children’s logic plays a critical par t in their
other numbers is known as class inclusion. This form mathematical learning.
of inclusion, which is also ref erred to as additive
composition of number, is the basis of the
understanding of ordinal number: every number Children should be encouraged to
in the number series is the same as the one that reflect on their implicit models and
precedes it plus one . It is also the basis f or learning
the nature of the mathematical tools
about the decade str ucture: the number 4321
consists of four thousands, three hundreds two tens Children need to re-conceptualise their intuitiv e
and one unit, and this can only be proper ly models about the world in order to access the
understood by a child who has thoroughl y grasped mathematical models that have been developed in
the additive composition of number. This form of the discipline. Some of the intuitive models used by
understanding also allows children to compare children lead them to appropr iate mathematical
numbers (7 is 4 more than 3) and thus to problem solving, and yet they may not know why
understand numbers as a way of expressing they succeeded. This was exemplified by students’
relations as well as quantities. The evidence clear ly use of one-to-many correspondence in the solution
shows that children’s ability to use this f orm of of propor tions problems: this schema of action leads
inclusion in learning about number and in solving to success but students may not be aware of the
mathematical problems is at first rather weak, and invariance of the r atio between the variables when
needs some suppor t. the scheme is used to solve problems. Increasing
students’ awareness of this invariant should improve
The logic of transitivity their mathematical understanding of propor tions.
All ordered series, including number, and also forms
of measurement involve transitivity (a > c if a > b Another example of implicit models that lead to
and b > c: a = c if a = b and b = c). Empirical success is the use of distr ibutivity in oral calculation
evidence shows that children as young as 5-years of multiplication and division. Students who know
of age do to some extent gr asp this set of relations, that they can, instead of multiplying a number by 15,
at any rate with continuous quantities like length. multiply it by 10 and then add half of this to the
However, learning how to use transitive relations product, can be credited with implicit kno wledge of
in numerical measurements (for example, of area) is distributivity. It is possible that they would benefit
an intricate and to some extent a diff icult business. later on, when learning algebra, from the awareness
Research, including Piaget’s initial research on of their use of distr ibitivity in this context. This
measurement, shows that one powerful reason understanding of distributivity developed in a
32 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

context where they could justify it could be used intuitively can become a model f or other situations,
for later learning. which might not be so accessib le to intuition.
Students’ reflection about the mathematics
Other implicit models may lead students astr ay. encapsulated in one concept is termed by Treffers
Fischbein, Deri, Nello and Mar ino (1985) and horizontal mathematising; looking across concepts
Greer (1988) have shown that some implicit models and thinking about the mathematics tools themselv es
interfere with students’ problem solving. If, for leads to vertical mathematising, i.e. a re-construction
example, they make the implicit assumption that in of the mathematical ideas at a higher lev el of
a division the dividend must always be larger than abstraction. This pragmatic theory about how
the divisor, they might shift the n umbers around in students’ implicit models develop can be easily put
implementing the division operation when the to test and could have an impact on mathematics as
dividend is actually smaller than the divisor. So, when well as science education.
students have developed implicit models that lead
them astray, they would also benefit from greater
awareness of these implicit models. Mathematical learning depends on
children understanding systems of
The simple fact that students do use intuitiv e models
symbols
when they are lear ning mathematics, whether the
teacher recognises the models or not, is a reason f or One of the most powerful contributions of recent
wanting to help students develop an awareness of research on mathematical lear ning has come from
the models they use . Instruction could and should work on the relation of logic , which is universal, to
play a crucial role in this process. mathematical symbols and systems of symbols, which
are human inventions, and thus are cultur al tools that
Finally, reflecting on implicit models can help students have to be taught. This distinction plays a role in all
understand mathematics better and also link branches of mathematical lear ning and has ser ious
mathematics with reality and with other disciplines implications for teaching mathematics.
that they learn in school. Freudenthal (1971) ar gued
that it would be difficult for teachers of other Children encounter mathematical symbols
disciplines to tie the bonds of mathematics to reality throughout their lives, outside school as well as in
if these have been cut by the mathematics teacher. the classroom. They first encounter them in lear ning
In order to tie these bonds, mathematics lessons can to count. Counting systems with a base pro vide
explore models that students use intuitively and children with a powerful way of representing
extend these models to scientific concepts that have numbers. These systems require the cognitive skills
been shown to be challenging f or students. One of involved in generative learning. As it is impossible to
the examples explored in a mathematics lesson memorise a very long sequence of words in a fixed
designed by Treffers (1991) focuses on the order, counting systems with a base solv e this
mathematics behind the concept of density. He tells problem: we learn only a few symbols (the labels f or
students the number of bicycles owned by people units, decades, hundred, thousand, million etc.) by
in United Kingdom and in the Nether lands. He also memory and generate the other ones in a r ule-
tells them the population of these tw o countries. based manner. The same is tr ue for the Hindu-Arabic
He then asks them in which countr y there are more place value system for writing numbers: when we
bicycles. On the basis of their intuitiv e knowledge, understand how it works, we do not need to
students can easily engage in a discussion that leads memorise how each number is written.
to the concept of density: the number of bicycles
should be considered in relation to the n umber Mathematical symbols are technologies in the sense
of people. A similar discussion might help students that they are human-made tools that impro ve our
understand the idea of population density and of ability to control and adapt to the environment. Each
density in physics, a concept that has been sho wn to of these systems makes specific cognitive demands
be very difficult for students. The discussion of how from the learner. In order to under stand place-value
one should decide which countr y has more bicycles representation, for example, students’ must
draws on students’ intuitive models; the concept of understand additive composition. If students have
density in physics extends this model. Streefland and explicit knowledge of additive composition and how
Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen (see Paper 4) suggested it works in place-value representation, they are
that a model of a situation that is under stood better placed to lear n column arithmetic, which
33 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

should then enable students to calculate with v ery informal knowledge in their everyday lives, which
large numbers; this task is very taxing without can be used to giv e meaning to mathematical
written numbers. So the costs of lear ning to use symbols learned in the classroom. Research in
these tools are worth paying: the tools enable mathematics education over the last five decades
students to do more than they can do without the or so has helped descr ibe the situations in which
tools. However, research shows that students should these meanings are lear ned and the way in which
be helped to make connections between symbols they are str uctured. Curriculum development work
and meanings: they can behave as if they under stand that takes this knowledge into account has already
how the symbols work while they do not started (a major example is the research b y
understand them completely: they can lear n routines members of the Freudenthal Institute) b ut it is
for symbol manipulation that remain disconnected not as widespread as one w ould expect given the
from meaning. discoveries from past research.

This is also tr ue of rational numbers. Children Children need to learn modes of enquiry
can learn to use wr itten fractions by counting the associated with mathematics
number of par ts into which a whole was cut and We identify some impor tant mathematical modes
writing this below a dash, and counting the n umber of enquiry that arise in the topics covered in this
of par ts painted and wr iting this above the dash. synthesis.
However, these symbols can remain disconnected
from their logical thinking about division. These Comparison helps us make new
disconnections between symbols and meaning are distinctions and create new objects
not restricted to writing fractions: they are also and relations
observed when students learn to add and subtract A cycle of creating and naming new objects through
fractions and also later when students lear n acting on simple objects per vades mathematics, and
algebraic symbols. the new objects can then be related and compared
to create higher-level objects. Making additive and
Plotting variables in the Cartesian plane is another multiplicative comparisons is an aspect of
use of symbol systems that can empo wer students: understanding relations between quantities and
they can, for example, more easily analyse change arithmetic. These comparisons are manifested precisely
by looking at gr aphs than they can by intuitive as difference and ratio. Thus difference and ratio arise
comparisons. Here, again, research has shown how as two new mathematical ideas, which become new
reading graphs also depends on the inter pretations mathematical objects of study and can be represented
that students assign to this system of symbols. and manipulated. Comparisons are related to making
distinctions, sorting and classifying based on
A recurrent theme in the review of research across perceptions, and students need to lear n to make
the different topics was that the disconnection these distinctions based on mathematical relations
between symbols and meanings seems to explain and properties, rather than perceptual similarities.
many of the difficulties faced by primary school
students in learning mathematics. The inevitable Reasoning about properties and relations
educational implication is that teaching aims should rather than perceptions
include promoting connections between symbols Many of the problems in mathematics that students
and meaning when symbols are introduced and used find hard occur when immediate perceptions lead to
in the classroom. misapplication of learnt methods or informal
reasoning. Throughout mathematics, students have to
This point is, of cour se, not new, but it is well learn to interpret representations before they think
worth reinforcing and, in par ticular, it is well worth about how to respond. They need to think about the
remembering in the light of cur rent findings. The relations between different objects in the systems
history of mathematics education includes the and schemes that are being represented.
development of pedagogical resources that w ere
developed to help students attr ibute meaning to Making and using representations
mathematical symbols. But some of these resources, Conventional number symbols, algebraic syntax,
like Dienes’ blocks and Cuisinaire’s rods, are only coordinate geometry, and graphing methods, all afford
encountered by students in the classroom; the manipulations that might otherwise be impossib le.
point we are making here is that students acquire Coordinating different representations to explore and
34 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

extend meaning is a fundamental mathematical skill unnoticed without a mathematical per spective. In
that is implicit in the use of the n umber line to the area of word problems and realistic problems
represent quantities, for example, the use of gr aphs to learning when and how to apply informal and formal
express functions. Equivalent representations, such as reasoning is impor tant. Later on, counter-intuitive
for number, algebraic relationships and functions, can ideas have to take the place of ear ly beliefs, such as
provide new insights through compar ison and ‘multiplication makes things bigger’ and students have
isomorphic analogical reasoning. to be war y of informal, visual and immediate
responses to mathematical stimuli.
Action and reflection-on-action
Learning takes place when we reflect on the effects A recurring issue in the papers is that students find
of actions. In mathematics, actions may be physical it hard to coordinate attention on local and global
manipulation, or symbolic rearrangement, or our changes. For example, young children confuse
observations of a dynamic image, or use of a tool. quantifying ‘relations between relations’ with the
In all these contexts, we observe what changes and original quantities; older children who cannot identify
what stays the same as a result of actions, and make covariation of functions might be able to talk about
inferences about the connections between action and separate variation of variables; students readily see
effect. In early mathematics such reflection is usuall y term-to-term patterns in sequences r ather than the
embedded in children’s classroom activity, such as generating function; changes in areas are confused
when using manipulatives to model changes in with changes in length.
quantity. In later mathematics changes and invariance
may be less obvious, particularly when change is
implicit (as in a situation to be modelled) or useful Epilogue
variation is hard to identify (as in a quadr atic function).
Our aim has been to wr ite a review that summar ises
Direct and inverse relations our findings from the detailed analysis of a lar ge
Direct and inverse relations are discussed in sev eral amount of research. We sought to make it possible
of our papers. While it may sometimes be easier to for educators and policy makers to take a fresh look
reason in a direct manner that accords with action, it at mathematics teaching and lear ning, star ting from
is impor tant in all aspects of mathematics to be ab le the results of research on key understandings, rather
to construct and use inverse reasoning. Addition than from previous tr aditions in the or ganisation of
and subtraction must be understood as a pair, and the curriculum. We found it necessar y to organise
multiplication and division as a pair, rather than as a our review around ideas that are already core ideas
set of four binary operations. As well as enabling in the curriculum, such as whole and r ational
more understanding of relations between quantities, number, algebra and problem solving, but also to
this also establishes the impor tance of reverse chains focus on ideas that might not be identif ied so easily
of reasoning throughout mathematical problem- in the current curriculum organisation, such as
solving, algebraic and geometrical reasoning. For students’ understanding of relations between
example, using reverse reasoning makes it more quantities and their under standing of space.
likely that students will lear n the dualism embedded
in Car tesian representations; that all points on the We have tried to make cogent and convincing
graph fulfil the function, and the function gener ates recommendations about teaching and lear ning,
all points on the gr aph. and to make the reasoning behind these
recommendations clear to educationalists. We
Informal and formal reasoning have also recognised that there are w eaknesses in
At first young children bring everyday understandings research and gaps in cur rent knowledge, some of
into school, and mathematics can allow them to which can be easily solved by research enabled by
formalise these and make them more precise . On the significant contributions of past research. Other gaps
other hand, intuitions about continuity, approximation, may not be so easil y solved, and we have described
dynamic actions and three-dimensional space might some pragmatic theories that are, or can be , used by
be over-ridden by early school mathematics – yet are teachers when they design instr uction. Classroom
needed later on. Mathematics also provides formal research, stemming from the explor ation of these
tools which do not descr ibe everyday outside pragmatic theories, can provide new insights for
experience, but enable students to solve problems in further research in the future .
mathematics and in the world which would be
35 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Endnotes
1 Details of the search process is pro vided in Appendix 1. This
contains the list of data bases and jour nals consulted and the
total number of papers read although not all of these can be
cited in the six paper s that comprise this review.
36 PaperSUMMARY
1: Overview
– PAPER 2: Understanding whole numbers

References

Anghileri, J., Beishuizen, M., and Putten, K. V. (2002). Nunes, T., and Br yant, P. (2008). Rational Numbers
From informal strategies to structured procedures: and Intensive Quantities: Challenges and Insights
mind the gap! Educational Studies in Mathematics, to Pupils’ Implicit Knowledge. Anales de Psicologia,
49, 149–170. in press.
Carey, S. (2004). Bootstrapping and the origins of Ruthven, K. (2008). Creating a calculator-aware
concepts. Daedalus, 133, 59–68. number curriculum. Canadian Journal of Science,
Cooper, H. M. (1998). Synthesizing research: a guide Mathematics and Technology Education, 3, 437–450.
for literature reviews. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Tall, D. (1992). The transition to advanced
Publications. mathematical thinking: Functions, limits, infinity, and
Confrey, J. (2008). A synthesis of research on r ational proof. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of
number reasoning. Paper presented at the Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning
International Conference on Mathematics (pp. 495–511). New York, N.Y.: Macmillan.
Education – ICME 11, Monterrey, Mexico. Thompson, P. W. (1993). Quantitative Reasoning,
Fischbein, E., Deri, M., Nello, M., & Marino, M. (1985). Complexity, and Additive Structures. Educational
The role of implicit models in solving verbal Studies in Mathematics, 25, 165–208.
problems in multiplication and division. Journal for Thompson, P. (1994). The Development of the
Research in Mathematics Education, 16, 3–17. Concept of Speed and Its Relationship to
Freudenthal, H. (1971). Geometry between the Devil Concepts of Rate. In G. Harel and J. Confrey
and the Deep Sea. Educational Studies in (Eds.), The Development of Multiplicative Reasoning
Mathematics, 3, 413–435. in the Learning of Mathematics (pp. 181–236).
Greer, B. (1988). Nonconservation of multiplication Albany, N.Y.: State University of New York Press.
and division: Analysis of a symptom. Journal of Treffers, A. (1987). Integrated column arithmetic
Mathematical Behavior, 7, 281–298. according to progressive schematisation.
Hart. K. (1981) Children’s Understanding of Educational Studies in Mathematics , 18 125–145.
Mathematics 11–16. London (U.K.): Murray. Treffers, A. (1991). Didactical background of a
Küchemann, D. (1981) Algebra, in K. Har t (Ed.) mathematics programme for primary education.
Children’s Understanding of Mathematics 11–16, In L. Streefland (Ed.), Realistic Mathematics
102–119 London (U.K.); Murray. Education in Primary School (pp. 21–56). Utrecht:
Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain. An introduction to Freudenthal Institute, Utrecht University.
neuropsychology. Harmondsworth (U.K.): Penguin. Vergnaud, G. (1997). The nature of mathematical
Noss, R., and Hoyles, C. (1992). Looking back and concepts. In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning
looking forward. In C . Hoyles and R. Noss (Eds.), and Teaching Mathematics. An International
Learning mathematics and LOGO (pp. 431–470). Perspective (pp. 1–28). Hove (UK): Psychology Press.
Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The genesis of higher mental
Nunes, T. (1997). Systems of signs and mathematical functions. In J. V. Wertsch (Ed.), The concept of
reasoning. In T. Nunes amd P. Bryant (Eds.), activity in Soviet Psychology (pp. 57–93). Amonk,
Learning and Teaching Mathematics. An N.Y.: Sharpe.
International Perspective (pp. 29–44). Hove (UK): Wartofsky, M. W. (1979). Models: representation and
Psychology Press. the scientific understanding. Dordrecht: Reidel.
Nunes, T. (2002). The role of systems of signs in
reasoning. In T. Brown and L. Smith (Eds.),
Reductionism and the Development of Knowledge
(pp. 133–158). Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-68-9

www.nuffieldfoundation.org
2
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 2: Understanding whole numbers
By Terezinha Nunes and Peter Bryant, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Summary of paper 2 3
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: Understanding extensive quantities
and whole numbers 7
Paper 1: Overview
Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and References 34
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities About the authors
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical Terezinha Nunes is Professor of Educational
representation Studies at the University of Oxford.
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation Peter Bryant is Senior Research Fellow in the
in mathematics Department of Education, University of Oxford.
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating
concepts About the Nuffield Foundation
Paper 8: Methodological appendix The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
charitable trust established in 1943 by William
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics being. We fund research and pr actical
in secondary schools. experiment and the development of capacity
to under take them; working across education,
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which science, social science and social policy. While
has been published separately as Introduction and most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
summary of findings. responsive grant programmes we also
undertake our own initiatives.
Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published
together as Summary papers.

All publications are available to download from


our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary of paper 2:
Understanding
whole numbers

Headlines
• Whole numbers are used in primary school to In children’s everyday lives and before they start
represent quantities and relations. It is crucial for school, they have experiences of manipulating and
children’s success in learning mathematics in comparing quantities. For example, even at age four,
primary school to establish clear connections many children can share sweets fairly between two
between numbers, quantities and relations. recipients by using correspondences: they share giving
one-for-you, one-for-me, until there are no sweets left.
• Using different schemes of action, such as setting They do sometimes make mistakes but they know
objects in correspondence, children can judge that, when the sharing is done fairly, the two people
whether two quantities are equivalent, and if they are will have the same amount of sweets at the end. Even
not, make judgements about their order of magnitude. younger children know some things about quantities:
These insights are used in understanding the number they know that if you add sweets to a group of
system beyond simply producing a string of number sweets, there will be more sweets there, and if you
words in a fixed order. It takes children some time to take some away, there will be fewer. However, they
make links between their understanding of quantities might not know that if you add a certain number and
and their knowledge of number. take away the same number, there will be just as many
sweets as there were before.
• Children start school with varying levels of
ability in using different action schemes to solve At the same time that young children are developing
arithmetic problems in the context of stories. They these ideas about quantities, they are often learning
do not need to know arithmetic facts to solve to count. They learn to say the sequence of number
these problems: they count in different ways words in the right order, they know that each object
depending on whether the problems they are that they are counting must be counted once and
solving involve the ideas of addition, subtraction, only once, and that it does not matter if you count a
multiplication or division. row of sweets from left to right or from right to left,
you should get to the same number.
• Individual differences in the use of action schemes
to solve problems predict children’s progress in Four-year-olds are thus amazing learners of
learning mathematics in school. mathematics. But they lack one thing which is crucially
important: they do not at first make connections
• Interventions that help children learn to use their between their understanding of quantities and their
action schemes to solve problems lead to better knowledge of numbers. So if
learning of mathematics in school. you ask a four-year-old, who just shared some sweets
fairly between two dolls, to count the sweets that
• It is considerably more difficult for children to use one doll has and then tell you, without counting, how
numbers to represent relations than to represent many sweets the other doll has, the majority (about
quantities. Understanding relations is crucial for their 60%) will tell you that they do not know. Knowing
further development in mathematics in school. that the dolls have the same quantity is not sufficient
4 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

to know that if one has 8 sweets, the other one has development. The actions of adding and taking away
8 sweets also, i.e. has the same number. help them understand part–whole relations. When
they can link their understanding of part–whole
Quantities and numbers are not the same thing. We relations with counting, they will understand many
can use numbers as measures of quantities, but we things about relations between numbers. A critical
can think about quantities without actually having a change in young children’s behaviour when they add
measure for them. Until children can understand the two sets is from ‘count all’ to ‘count on’. If they know
connections between numbers and quantities, they that they have 5 sweets, and you add 4 to the 5, they
cannot use their knowledge of quantities to support could either start from 1 and count all the sweets
their understanding of numbers and vice versa. (count all) or they could point to the 5, and count on
Because the connections between quantities and from there. ‘Count on’ is a sign that the children have
numbers are many and varied, learning about these linked their knowledge of part–whole relations with
connections could take three to four years in the counting sequence: they have understood the
primary school. additive composition number. This explanation works
for the relation between a number and its immediate
An important link that children must make between predecessor and any of its predecessors. It is
number and quantity is the link between the order supported by much research that shows that counting
of number words in the counting sequence and the on is a sign of abstraction in part–whole relations,
magnitude of the quantity represented. How do which opens the way for children to solve many other
children come to understand that the any number problems: they can add a quantity to an invisible set,
in the counting sequence is equal to the preceding count coins of different denominations to form a single
number plus 1? total, and are ready to learn to use place value to
represent numbers in writing.
Different explanations have been proposed in the
literature. One is that they simply see that magnitude Adding and subtracting elements to sets also give
increases as they count. But this explanation does children the opportunity to understand the inverse
not work well: our perception of magnitude is relation between addition and subtraction. This insight
approximate and knowing that any number is equal is not gained in an all-or-nothing fashion: children first
to its predecessor plus 1 is a very precise piece of apply it only to quantities and later on to number also.
knowledge. A second explanation is that children The majority of five-year-olds realises that if you add 3
use perception, language and inferences together to sweets to a set of sweets and then take the same
reach this understanding. Young children discriminate sweets away, the number of sweets in the set remains
well, for example, one puppet from two puppets and the same. However, many of these children will not
two puppets from three puppets. Because they realise that if you add 3 sweets to the set and then
know these differences precisely, they put these take 3 other sweets away, the number of sweets is still
two pieces of information together, and learn that the same. They see that adding and taking away the
two is one more than one, and three is one more same quantity leaves the original quantity the same but
than two. They then make the inference that all this does not immediately mean to them that adding
numbers in the counting sequence are equal to the and taking away the same number also leaves the
predecessor plus one. But this sort of generalisation original number the same. Research shows that the
could not be stretched into helping children step from understanding the inverse relation between
understand that any number is also equal to the addition and subtraction of quantities is a useful start if
last-but-one in the sequence plus 2. This process of one wants to teach children about the inverse relation
putting together perception with language and then between addition and subtraction of number.
generalising is an explanation for only the n + 1 idea;
it would be much better if we could have a more Adding, taking away and understanding part–whole
general explanation of how children understand relations form one part of the story of what
the connection between quantities and the children know about quantities and numbers in the
number sequence. early years of primary school. They relate to how
additive reasoning develops. The other part of the
The third explanation for how children connect story is surprising to many people: children also
heir knowledge of quantities with the magnitude of know quite a lot about multiplicative reasoning
numbers in the counting sequence is that children’s when they start school.
schemes of action play the most important part in this
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Children use two different schemes of action to solve problem if Ali had some Chinese stamps in his
multiplication and division problems before they are collection and gave 2 to his grandfather, leaving his
taught about these operations in school: they use collection with 6. In this second problem, there is a
one-to-many correspondence and sharing. If five- decrease in the quantity but the problem has to be
and six-year-olds are shown, for example, four little solved by an increase in the number, in order to get
houses in a row, told that they should imagine that in us back to Ali’s collection before he gave 2 stamps
each live three dogs, and asked how many dogs live away. There is no controversy in the literature: inverse
in the street, the majority can say the correct number. problems are more difficult than direct problems,
Many children will point three times to each house irrespective of whether the arithmetic operation that
and count in this way until they complete the is used to solve it is addition or subtraction.
counting at the fourth house. They are not
multiplying: they are solving the problem using one- The second difficulty depends on whether the
to-many correspondence. Children can also share numbers in the problem are all about quantities or
objects to recipients and answer problems about whether there is a need to consider a relation
division. They do not know the arithmetic operations, between quantities. In the two problems about Ali’s
but they can use their reasoning to count in different stamps, all the numbers refer to quantities. An example
ways and solve the problem. So children manipulate of a problem involving relations would be: In Ali’s class
quantities using multiplicative reasoning and solve there are 8 boys and 6 girls; how many more boys
problems before they learn about multiplication and than girls in Ali’s class? (Or how many fewer girls than
division in school. boys in Ali’s class?). The number 2 here refers neither
to the number of boys nor to the number of girls: it
If children are assessed in their understanding of the refers to the relation (the difference) between number
inverse relation between addition and subtraction, of boys and girls. A difference is not a quantity: it is a
of additive composition, and of one-to-many relation. Problems that involve relations are more
correspondence in their first year of school, this difficult than those that involve quantities. It should not
provides us with a good way of anticipating whether be surprising that relations are more difficult to deal
they will have difficulties in learning mathematics in with in numerical contexts than quantities: the majority,
school. Children who do well in these assessments if not all, the experiences that children have with
go on to attain better results in mathematics counting have to do with finding a number to
assessments in school. Those who do not do well can represent a quantity, because we count things and not
improve their prospects through early intervention. relations between things. We can re-phrase problems
Children who received specific instruction on these that involve relations so that all the numbers refer to
relations between quantities and how to use them to quantities. For example, we could say that the boys
solve problems did significantly better than a similar and girls need to find a partner for a dance; how many
group who did not receive such instruction. boys won’t be able to find a girl to dance with? There
are no relations in this latter problem, all the numbers
Finally, many studies have used story problems to refer to quantities. This type of problem is significantly
investigate which uses of additive reasoning are easier. So it is difficult for children to use numbers to
easier and which are more difficult for children of represent relations. This could be one step that
primary school age. Two sorts of difficulties have teachers in primary school want to help their children
been identified. take, because it is a difficult move for every child.

The first relates to the need to understand that


addition and subtraction are the inverse of each
other. One story that requires this understanding is:
Ali had some Chinese stamps in his collection and his
grandfather gave him 2; now he has 8; how many
stamps did he have before his grandfather gave him
the 2 stamps? This problem exemplifies a situation in
which a quantity increases (the grandfather gave him
2 stamps) but, because the information about the
original number in his collection is missing, the
problem is not solved by an addition but rather by
a subtraction. The problem would also be an inverse
6 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

Recommendations

Research about mathematical Recommendations for teaching


learning and research

Children’s pre-school knowledge of Teaching Teachers should be aware of the impor tance
quantities and counting develops of helping children make connections between their
separately. understanding of quantities and their kno wledge of counting.

When children star t school, they can solve Teaching The linear view of development, according to
many different problems using schemes of which understanding addition precedes multiplication, is not
action in coordination with counting, supported by research. Teachers should be aware of children’s
including multiplication and division mathematical reasoning, including their ability to solv e
problems. multiplication and division problems, and use their abilities
for fur ther learning.

Three logical-mathematical reasoning Teaching A greater emphasis should be giv en


principles have been identified in research, in the curriculum to promoting children’s understanding of the
which seem to be causally related to inverse relation between addition and subtraction, additive
children’s later attainment in mathematics composition, and one-to-many correspondence. This would help
in primary school. Individual differences in children who star t school at risk for difficulties in learning
knowledge of these principles predict later mathematics to make good progress in the f irst years.
achievement and inter ventions reduce Research Long-term longitudinal and inter vention studies
learning difficulties. with large samples are needed before curriculum and policy
changes can be proposed. The move from the laboratory to
the classroom must be based on research that identif ies
potential difficulties in scaling up successful inter ventions.

Children’s ability to solve word problems Teaching Systematic use of problems involving these
shows that two types of problem cause difficulties followed by discussions in the classroom would give
difficulties for children: those that involve children more oppor tunities for making progress in using
the inverse relation between addition and mathematics in contexts with which they ha ve difficulty.
subtraction and those that involve thinking Research There is a need f or intervention studies designed
about relations. to promote children’s competence in solving problems about
relations. Brief experimental interventions have paved the way
for classroom-based research but large-scale studies are
needed.
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Understanding extensive
quantities and whole
numbers

Counting and reasoning


At school, children’s formal learning about another. If Sean has 15 books and Patrick 17,
mathematics begins with natural numbers (1, 2, Patrick has more books than Sean. Unless children
…17…103 …525…). Numbers are symbols for understand that numbers are a precise way of
quantities: they make it possible for the child to expressing quantities, the number system will have
specify single values precisely and also to work no meaning for them.
out the relations between different quantities. By
counting, the child can tell you that there are 20 Second, they must realise eventually that the number
books in the pile on the teacher’s desk (a single system enhances their knowledge of quantities in an
quantity), and eventually should be able to work increasingly powerful way. They may not be able to
out that there is 1 book for every child in the class look at a pile of books and tell without counting that
if there are 20 children there, or that there are 5 the one with 17 has more books than the one with
more books than children (a relation between two 15; indeed, the thickness of books varies and the pile
quantities) if there are 15 children in the class. of 15 books could well be taller than the pile with 17.
By counting they can know which pile has more
Quantities and numbers are not the same. Thompson books. When they know how to count, we can also
(1993) suggested that ‘a person constitutes a quantity add and subtract numbers, and work out the exact
by conceiving of a quality of an object in such a way relations between them. If we understand lots of
that he or she understands the possibility of measuring things about quantities, e.g. how to create equivalent
it. Quantities, when measured, have numerical value, quantities and how their equivalence is changed, but
but we need not measure them or know their we don’t have numbers to represent them, we
measures to reason about them.You can think of your cannot add and subtract.
height, another person's height, and the amount by
which one of you is taller than the other without In this section, therefore, we will focus on the
having to know the actual values’ (pp. 165–166).1 connections that children make, and sometimes fail
to make, between their growing knowledge about
Children experience and learn about quantities and quantities and the number system. In many ways this
the relations between them quite independently of is an unusual thing to do. Most existing accounts of
learning to count. Similarly, they can learn to count how children learn about number are more
quite independently from understanding quantities restricted. Either they leave out the number system
and relations between them. We shall argue in this altogether and concentrate instead on children’s
section that the most important task for a child who ability to reason about quantities, or they are strictly
is learning about natural numbers is to connect these confined to how well children count sets of objects.
numbers to a good understanding of quantities and
relations. The connection should work at two levels. Piaget’s theory (Piaget, 1952) is an example of the
first kind of theory. His view that children have to be
First, children must realise that their knowledge of able to reason logically about quantity in order to
quantities and numbers should agree with one understand number and the number system is
8 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

almost certainly right, but it left out the possibility Equivalence of sets in one-to-one
that learning to count eventually transforms this correspondence and its connection to
reasoning in children by making it more powerful number words
and more precise. Numbers have both cardinal and ordinal properties.
Two sets have the same cardinal value when the
In the opposite corner, Gelman’s influential theory items in one set are in one-to-one correspondence
(Gelman and Gallistel, 1978), which focuses on how with those in the other. There are as many eggcups in
children count single sets of objects and has little to a box of six egg-cups as there are eggs in a carton of
say about children’s quantitative reasoning, has the six eggs, and if there are six people at the breakfast
serious disadvantage that it by-passes children’s table each will have one of those eggs on its own
reasoning about relations between quantities. In the eggcup to eat. Thus, the eggcups, eggs and people are
end, numbers are only important because they allow all in one-to-one correspondence since there is one
us to represent quantities and make sense of egg and eggcup for each one person, which means
quantitative relations. that each of these three sets has the same number.

The first part of this section is an account of We shall deal with the ordinal properties of number
how children connect numbers with quantity. We in a later section. At the moment, all that we need to
will start this account with a detailed list of the say is that numbers are arranged in an ordered series.
connections that they need to make. We argue that
children need to make three types of connections To return to cardinality, Piaget argued quite
between number words and quantities in order to reasonably that no one can understand the meaning
make the most of what they learn when they begin of ‘six’ unless he or she also understands the
to count: they need to understand cardinality; they number’s cardinal properties, and by this he meant
need to understand ordinal numbers, and they need understanding not only that any set of six contains
to understand the relation between cardinality and the same number of items as any other set of six but
addition and subtraction. The second part of this also that that the items in a set of six are in one-to-
section is an account of how children learn to use one correspondence with any other set of six items.
numbers to solve problems. We argue that numbers So, if we are to pursue the approach of studying the
are used to represent quantities but that children links between children’s quantitative reasoning and
must also learn to use them to represent how they learn about natural numbers, we need to
transformations and relations, and that the different find out how well children understand the principle
meanings that numbers can have affect how easily that sets which are in one-to-one correspondence
children solve problems. with each other are equal in quantity, and also how
clearly they apply what they understand about one-
to-one correspondence to actual numbers like ‘six’.
Giving meaning to numbers Piaget based his claim that young children have a very
Young children’s dissociation of poor understanding of one-to-one correspondence
quantities and numbers on the mistakes that they make when they are shown
one set of items (e.g. a row of eggs) and are asked to
Children may know that two quantities are the same form another set (e.g. of eggcups) of the same
and still not make the inference that the number of number. Four- and five-year-olds often match the new
objects in one is the same as the number of objects set with the old one on irrelevant criteria such as
in the other. Conversely, they may know how to two rows’ lengths and make no effort to put the
count and yet not make use of counting when asked rows into one-to-one correspondence. Their ability
to create two equal sets. We review here briefly to establish one-to-one correspondence between
research within two different traditions, inspired by sets grows over time: it cannot be taken
Piaget’s and Gelman’s theories, that shows that young for granted.
children do not necessarily make a connection
between what they know about quantities and However, even when children do establish a one-to-
what they know about counting. one correspondence between two sets, they do not
necessarily infer that counting the elements in one set
tells them how many elements there are in the other
set. Piaget (1952) established this in an experiment in
9 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

which he proposed to buy sweets from the children, they knew about the relative quantities to the
using a one-to-one exchange between pence and number symbols. Other children, however, did make
sweets. For each sweet that the child gave to Piaget, this connection, which we think is the first significant
he gave the child a penny. As they exchanged pence step in understanding cardinality. Whether all children
and sweets, the child was asked to count how many will have made this connection by the time that they
pence he/she had. Piaget ensured that he stopped this start learning about numbers and arithmetic at
exchange procedure without going over the child’s school depends on many factors: for example, the
counting range. When he stopped the exchange, he age they start school and their previous experiences
asked the child how many pence the child had. The with number are related to whether they have taken
children were able to answer this without difficulty as this important step by then (e.g. socio-economic
they had been counting their coins. He then asked the status related to maths ability at school entry: see
child how many sweets he had. Piaget reports that Ginsburg, Klein, and Starkey, 1998; Jordan,
some children were unable to make the inference Huttenlocher, and Levine, 1992; Secada, 1992).
that the number of sweets Piaget had was the same
as the number of pence that the child himself/herself Counting and understanding relations
had. Unfortunately, Piaget gave no detailed description between quantities
of how the ability to make this inference related to Piaget’s theory of how children develop an
the children’s age. understanding of cardinality was confronted by an
alternative theory, by Gelman’s nativist view of
More recent research, which offers quantitative children’s counting and its connection to cardinal
information, shows that many four-year-olds who do number knowledge (Gelman and Gallistel, 1978).
understand one-to-one correspondence well enough Gelman claimed that children are born with a
to share fairly do not make the inference that genuine understanding of natural number, and that
equivalent sets have the same number of elements. this makes it possible for them to learn and use the
Frydman and Bryant (1988) asked four-year-old basic principles of counting as soon as they begin to
children to share a set of ‘chocolates’ to two learn the names for numbers. She outlined five basic
recipients. At this age, children often share things counting principles. Anyone counting a set of objects
between themselves, and they typically do so on a should understand that:
one-for-A, one-for-B, one-for-A, one-for-B basis. In this • you should count every object once and only once
study, the children established the correspondence (one-to-one correspondence principle)
themselves; this contrasts with Piaget’s study, where • the order in which you count the actual objects
Piaget controlled the exchange of sweets and pence. (from left-to right, from right to left or from the
When the child had done the sharing, the middle outwards) makes no difference (order
experimenters counted out the number of items that irrelevance principle)
had been given to one recipient, which was six. • you should produce the number words in a
Having done this, they asked the child how many constant order when counting: you cannot count
chocolates had been given to the other recipient. 1-2-3 at one time and 1-3-2 at another (fixed
order principle)
None of the children immediately made the • whether the objects in a set are all identical to each
inference that there were the same number of other or all quite different has no effect on their
chocolates in one set as in the other, and therefore number (the abstraction principle)
that there were also six items in the second set. • the last number that you count is the number of
Instead, every single child began to count the second items in the set (cardinal principle).
set. In each case, the experimenter then interrupted
the child’s counting, and asked him or her if there was Each of these principles is justified in the sense that
any other way of working out the number of items in anyone who does not respect them will end up
the second recipient’s share. Only 40% of the group counting incorrectly. A child who produces count
of four-year-olds made the correct inference that the words in different orders at different times is bound
second recipient had also been given six chocolates. to make incorrect judgements about the number of
The failure of more than half of the children is an items in a set. So will anyone who does not obey the
interesting one. The particular pre-school children one-to-one principle.
who made it knew that the two recipients’ shares
were equal, and they also knew the number of items Gelman’s original observations of children counting
in one of the shares. Yet, they did not connect what sets of objects, and the results of some subsequent
10 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

experiments in which children had to spot errors in principles, only apply to what someone must know
other protagonists’ counting (e.g. Gelman and Meck, and do in order to count a single set of objects.
1983), all supported her idea that children obey and They tell us nothing about children’s understanding
apparently understand all five of these principles of numerical relations between sets. Piaget’s
with small sets of items long before they go to research on number, on the other hand, was almost
school. The young children’s success in counting entirely concerned with comparisons between
smaller sets allowed her to dismiss their more different quantities, and this has the confusing
frequent mistakes with large sets of items as consequence that when Gelman and Piaget used
executive errors rather than failures in the same terms, they gave them quite different
understanding. She agued that the children knew meanings. For Piaget, understanding cardinality
the principles of counting and therefore of number, was about grasping that all and only equivalent
but lacked some of the skills needed to carry them sets are equal in number: for Gelman it meant
out. This view became known as the ‘principles- understanding that the last number counted
before-skills hypothesis’. represents the number of items in a single set.
When Piaget studied one-to-one correspondence,
These observations of Gelman’s provoked a great he looked at children’s comparisons between two
deal of useful further research on children’s counting, quantities (eggs and egg cups, for example):
most of which has confirmed her original results, Gelman’s concern with one-to-one
though with some modifications. For example, five- correspondence was about children assigning
year-old children do generally count objects in a one- one count word to each item in a set.
to-one fashion (one number word for each object)
but not all of the time (Fuson, 1988). They tend either Since two sets are equal in quantity if they contain
to miss objects or count some more than once in the same number of items and unequal if they do
disorganised arrays. It is now clear that gestures play not, one way to compare two sets quantitatively is
an important part in helping children keep track to count each of them and to compare the two
during counting (Albilali and DiRusso, 1999) but numbers. Another, for much the same reason, is to
sometimes they point at some of the objects in a use one-to-one correspondence: if the sets are in
target set without counting them. correspondence they are equal; if not, they are
unequal. This prompts a question: how soon and how
Many of the criticisms of Gelman’s hypothesis well do children realise that counting sets is a valid
were against her claims that children understand way, and sometimes the only feasible valid way, of
cardinality. Ironically, even critics of Gelman (e.g. comparing them quantitatively? Another way of
Carey, 2004; LeCorre and Carey, 2007)) have in putting the same question is to ask: how soon and
their own research accepted her all too limited how well do children realise that numbers are a
definition of understanding cardinality (that it is the measure by which they can compare the quantities
realisation that the last number counted represents of two or more different sets.
the number of objects). However, several
researchers have criticised her empirical test of Most of the research on this topic suggests that it
cardinality. Gelman had argued that children, who takes children some time to realise that they can, and
count a set of objects and emphasise the last often should, count to compare. Certainly many pre-
number (‘one-two-three-FOUR’) or repeat it school children seem not to have grasped the
(‘one-two-three-four- there are four’), understand connection between counting and comparing even if
that this last number represents the quantity of the they have been able to count for more than one year.
counted set. However, Fuson (Fuson, and Hall,
1983; Fuson, Richards and Briars, 1982) and One source of evidence comes from the work by
Sophian (Sophian, Wood, and Vong, 1995) both Sophian (1988), who asked children to judge whether
made the reasonable argument that emphasising or someone else (a puppet) was counting the right way
repeating the last number could just be part of an when asked to do two things. The puppet was faced
ill-understood procedure. with two sets of objects, and was asked in some trials
to say whether the two sets were equal or not and in
Although Gelman’s five principles cover some others to work out how many items there were on
essential aspects of counting, they leave others the table altogether. Sometimes the puppet did the
out. The five principles, and the tools that Gelman right thing, which was to count the two sets
devised to study children’s understanding of these separately when comparing them and to count all the
11 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

items together when working out the grand total. a theoretical standpoint, Vergnaud (2008) pointed
At other times he got it wrong, e.g. counted all the out that Gelman’s cardinality criterion should actually
objects as one set when asked to compare the two be viewed as showing that children have some
sets. The main result of Sophian’s study was that the understanding of ordinal, not of cardinal, number:
pre-school children found it very hard to make this Gelman’s criterion is indeed based on the position of
judgement. Most 3-year-olds judged counting each set the number word in the counting sequence, because
was the right way to count in both tasks while 4-year- the children use the last number word to represent
olds judged counting both sets together was the right the set. Vergnaud argues that ordinal numbers cannot
way to count in both tasks. Neither age group could be added whereas cardinal numbers can. He predicts
identify the right way to count reliably. that children whose knowledge of cardinal number is
restricted to Gelman’s cardinality principle will not be
A second type of study shows that even at school able to continue counting to answer how many
age many children seem not to understand fully objects are in a set if you add some objects to the
the significance of numbers when they make set that they have just counted: they will need to
quantitative comparisons. There is, for example, the count again from one. Research by Siegler and
striking demonstration by Pierre Gréco (1962), a Robinson (1982) and Starkey and Gelman (1982)
colleague of Piaget’s, that children will count two produced results in line with this prediction: 3-year-
rows of counters, one of which is more spread out olds do not spontaneously count to solve addition
and longer than the other, and correctly say that problems after counting the first set. Ginsburg, Klein
they both have the same number (this one has six, and Starkey (1998) also interpreted such results as
and so does the other) but then will go on to say indicative of an insufficient development of the
that there are more counters in the longer row concept of cardinality in young children. We return
than in the other. A child who makes this mistake to the definition of cardinality later on, after we have
understands cardinality in Gelman’s sense (i.e. is discussed alternative explanations to Gelman’s theory
able to say how many items in the set) but does of an innate counting principle as the basis for
not know what the word ‘six’ means in Piaget’s learning about cardinality.
sense. Barbara Sarnecka and Susan Gelman (2004)
recently replicated this observation. They report Three further studies will be used here to illustrate
that children three- and four-year-olds know that if that some children who are able to use Gelman’s
a set had five objects and you add some to it, it no cardinality principle do not seem to have a full grasp
longer has five objects; however they did not know of when this principle should be applied; so meeting
that equal sets must have the same number word. the criterion for the cardinality principle does not
mean understanding cardinality.
Another source of evidence is the observation,
repeated in many studies, that children, who can Fuson (1988) showed that three-year-old children who
count quite well, nevertheless fail to count the seem to understand the cardinality principle continue
items in two sets that they have been asked to to use the last number word in the counting sequence
compare numerically (Cowan, 1987; Cowan and to say how many items are in a set even if the counting
Daniels, 1989; Michie, 1984; Saxe, Guberman and started from two, rather than from one. Counting in
Gearhart, 1987); instead they rely on perceptual this unusual way should at least lead the children to
cues, like length, which of course are unreliable. reject the last word as the cardinal for the set.
Children who understand the cardinality of
number should understand that they can make the Using a similar experimental manoeuvre, Freeman,
comparison only by counting or using one-to-one Antonuccia and Lewis (2000) assessed three- and
correspondence, and yet at the age of five and six five-year-olds’ rejection of the last word after counting
years most of them do neither, even when, as in the if there had been a mistake in counting. The children
Cowan and Daniels study, the one-to-one cues are participated in a few different tasks, one of which was
emphasised by lines drawn between items in the a task where a puppet counted an array with either
two sets that the children were asked to compare. 3 or 5 items, but the puppet miscounted, either by
counting an item twice or by skipping an item. The
Finally, the criterion for the cardinality principle has children were asked whether the puppet had counted
itself been criticised as insufficient to show that right, and if they said that the puppet had not, they
children understand cardinality. The criticism is both were asked: How many does the puppet think there
theoretical and also based on empirical results. From are? How many are there really? All children had
12 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

shown that they could count 5 items accurately (2 principle from understanding cardinality: when you
of 22 could count accurately to 6, another 4 could count backwards, the first number label is the cardinal
do so to 7, and the remaining 18 could count items for the set if there is a one-to-one correspondence
accurately to 10). However, their competence in between number labels and objects. Bermejo and
counting was no assurance that they realised that the colleagues showed that four- and six-year-old children
puppet’s answer was wrong after miscounting: only who can say that there are three objects in a set when
about one third of the children were able to say that you count forward cannot necessarily say that if you
the answer was not right after they had detected the count backwards from four and the last number label is
error. The children’s rejection of the puppet’s wrong ‘two’, this does not mean that there are two objects in
answer increased with age: 82% of the five-year-olds the set. In fact, many children did not realize that there
correctly rejected the puppet’s answer in all three was a contradiction between the two answers: for
trials when a mistake had been made. However, the them, the set could have three objects if you count one
majority of the children could not say what the way and two if you count in another way.They also
cardinal for the set was without recounting: the showed that children who were given the opportunity
majority counted the set again in order to answer the to discuss what the cardinal for the set was when the
question ‘how many are there really?’ They neither said counting was done backwards showed marked
immediately the next number when the puppet had progress in other tasks of understanding cardinality,
skipped one nor used the previous number when the which included starting to count from other numbers
puppet double-counted an item. So, quite a few of the in the counting sequence than the number one, as in
younger children passed Gelman’s cardinality principle Fuson’s task.They concluded that reflecting about the
but did not necessarily see that the cardinality use of counting and the different actions involved in
principle should not be applied when the counting achieving a correct counting created opportunities for
principles are violated. Most of the older children, who children to understanding cardinality better.
rejected the use of the cardinality principle, did not
use it to deduce what the correct cardinal should be; The evidence that we have presented so far
instead, all they demonstrated was that they could suggests very strongly and remarkably consistently
replace the wrong routine with the correct one, and that learning to count and understanding relations
then they could say what the number really was. between quantities are two different achievements.
Understanding that the next number is the cardinal On the whole, children can use the procedures for
for the set if the puppet skipped one item, without counting long before they realise how counting
having to count again, would have demonstrated that allows them to measure and compare different
the children have a relatively good grasp of cardinality. quantities, and thus to work out the relations
Freeman and his colleagues reported that only about between them. We think that it is only when
one third of the children who detected the puppet’s children establish a connection between what
error were able to say what the correct number of they know about relations between quantities
items was without recounting. In the subsequent and counting that they can be said to know the
section we return to the importance of knowing what meaning of natural numbers.2
the next number is for the concept of cardinality.

The third study we consider here was by Bermejo,


Morales and deOsuna (2004), who argued that if Summary
children really understand cardinality, and not just the 1 Natural numbers are a way of representing
Gelman’s cardinality principle, they should do better particular quantities and relations between
than just re-implement the counting in a correct way. quantities.
For example, they should be able to know how many
objects are in a set even if the counting sequence is 2 When children learn numbers, they must find out
implemented backwards. If you count a set by saying not just about the counting sequence and ho w to
‘three, two, one’, and you reach the last item when you count, but also about how the numbers in the
say ‘one’, you know that there are three objects in the counting system represent quantities and relations
set. If you count backwards from three and the label between them.
‘one’ does not coincide with the last object, you know
that the set does not have three objects. Just like 3 One basic aspect of this representation is the
starting to count from two, counting backwards is cardinality of number: all sets with the same
another way of separating out Gelman’s cardinality number have the same quantity of items in them.
13 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

4 Another way of expressing cardinality is to say can accelerate this process of reflection on actions,
that all sets with the number are in one-to-one and so can other forms of social interaction, because
correspondence with each other. they may help the children realise the contradictions
that they fall into when they say, for example, that
5 There is evidence that young children’s first two quantities are different and yet they are labelled
successful experiences with one-to-one by the same number. However, the process that
correspondence come through shar ing; however, eventually leads to their understanding of the
even if they succeed in shar ing fairly and know the meanings of natural numbers and the implications
number of items in one set, many do not make the of these representations is the child’s growing
inference that the number of items in the other understanding of relations between quantities.
set is the same.
Piaget’s studies of children’s understanding of the
6 Because of its cardinal proper ties, number is a relations between quantities involved three different
measure: one can compare the quantity of items ideas that he considered central to understanding
in two different sets by counting each set. number: understanding equivalence, order, and class-
inclusion (which refers to the idea that the whole is
7 Several studies have shown that many children as the sum of the parts, or that a set with 6 items
old as six years are reluctant to count, although comprises a set with 5 items plus 1). The methods
they know how to count, when asked to compare used in these studies have been extensively
sets. They resor t to perceptual compar isons criticised, as has the idea that children develop
instead. through a sequence of stages that can be easily
traced and are closely associated with age. However,
8 This evidence suggests that lear ning about to our knowledge the core idea that children come
quantities and learning about numbers develop to understand relations between quantities by
independently of each other in y oung children. But reflecting upon the results of their actions is still a
in order to under stand natural numbers, children very important hypothesis in the study of how
must establish connections between quantities and children learn about numbers. We do not review this
numbers. Thus schools must ensure that children vast literature here as there are several collections of
learn not only to count but also learn to establish papers that do so (see, for example, Steffe, Cobb
connections between counting and their and Glaserfeld, 1988; Steffe and Thompson, 2000).
understanding of quantities. Later sections of this paper will revisit Piaget’s theory
and discuss related research.

Current theories about the origin of This is not the only theory about how children come
to understand the meaning of cardinal numbers. There
children’s understanding of the
are at least two alternative theories which are widely
meaning of cardinal number discussed in the literature. One is a nativist theory,
We have seen that Piaget’s theory defines children’s which proposes that children have from birth access
understanding of number on the basis of their to an innate, inexact but powerful ‘analog’ system,
understanding of relations between quantities; for whose magnitude increases directly with the number
him, cardinality is not just saying how many items of objects in an array, and they attach the number
are in sets but grasping that sets in one-to-one words to the properties occasioning these
correspondence are equivalent in number and vice- magnitudes (Dehaene 1992; 1997; Gallistel and
versa. He argued that children could only be said to Gelman, 1992; Gelman and Butterworth, 2005; Xu
understand numbers if they made a connection and Spelke, 2000; Wynn, 1992; 1998). This gives all
between numbers and the relations between of us from birth the ability to make approximate
quantities that are implied by numbers. He also judgements about numerical quantities and we
argued that this connection was established by continue through life to use this capacity. The
children as they reflected about the effect of discriminations that this system allows us to make are
their actions on quantities: setting items in much like our discriminations along other continua,
correspondence, adding and taking items away are such as loudness, brightness and length. One feature
schemes of action which form the basis for children’s of all these discriminations is that the greater the
understanding of how to compare and to change quantities (the louder, the brighter or the longer they
quantities. Piaget acknowledges that learning to count are) the harder they are to discriminate (known, after
14 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

the great 19th century psycho-physicist who the crucial part in making it possible for children to
meticulously studied perceptual sensitivity, as the learn how to connect number with the counting
‘Weber function’). To quote Carey (2004): ‘Tap out as system. This system makes it possible for infants to
fast as you can without counting (you can prevent recognise and represent very small numbers exactly
yourself from counting by thinking 'the' with each tap) (not approximately like the analog system). The
the following numbers of taps: 4, 15, 7, and 28. If you system only operates for sets of 1, 2 and 3 objects
carried this out several times, you'd find the mean and even within this restricted scope there is marked
number of taps to be 4, 15, 7, and 28, with the range development over children’s first three years.
of variation very tight around 4 (usually 4, occasionally
3 or 5) and very great around 28 (from 14 to 40 taps, Initially the system allows very young children to
for example). Discriminability is a function of the recognise sets of 1 as having a distinct quantity. The
absolute numerical value, as dictated by Weber's law’ child understands 1 as a quantity, though he or she
(p. 63). The evidence for this analog system being an does not at first know that the word ‘one’ applies to
innate one comes largely from studies of infants (Xu this quantity. Later on the child is able to discriminate
and Spelke, 2000; McCrink and Wynn, 2004) and to and recognise – in Carey’s words ‘to individuate’ –
a certain extent studies of animals as well, and is sets of 1 and 2 objects, and still later, around the age
beyond the scope of this review. The evidence for its of three- to four-years, sets of 1, 2 and 3 objects as
importance for learning about number and arithmetic distinct quantities. In Carey’s terms young children
comes from studies of developmental or acquired progress from being ‘one-knowers’ to becoming ‘two-
dyscalculia (e.g. Butterworth, Cipolotti and knowers’ and then ‘three-knowers’.
Warrington, 1996; Landerl, Bevana and Butterworth,
2004). However important this basic system may be During the same period, these children also learn
as a neurological basis for number processing, it is not number words and, though their recognition of 1, 2
clear how the link between an analog and imprecise and 3 as distinct quantities does not in any way
system and a precise system based on counting can depend on this verbal learning, they do manage to
be forged: ‘ninety’ does not mean ‘approximately associate the right count words (‘one’, ‘two’ and ‘three’)
ninety’ any more than ‘eight’ could mean with the right quantities. This association between
‘approximately eight’. In fact, as reported in the parallel individuation and the count list eventually leads
previous section, three- and four-year-olds know that to what Carey (2004) calls ‘bootstrapping’: the children
if a set has 6 items and you add one item to it, it no lift themselves up by their own intellectual bootstraps.
longer has 5 objects: they know that ‘six’ is not the They do so, some time in their fourth or fifth year, and
same as ‘approximately six’. therefore well before they go to school.

A third well-known theoretical alternative, which This bootstrapping takes two forms. First, with the help
starts from a standpoint in agreement with Gelman’s of the constant order of number words in the count
theory, is Susan Carey’s (2004) hypothesis about list, the children begin to learn about the ordinal
three ways of learning about number. Carey accepts properties of numbers: 2 always comes after 1 in the
Gelman and Gallistel’s (1978) limited definition of the count list and is always more numerous than 1, and 3 is
cardinality principle but rejects their conclusions more numerous than 2 and always follows 2. Second,
about how children first come to understand this since the fact that the count list that the children learn
principle. Carey argues that initially (by which she goes well beyond 3, they eventually infer that the
means in the first three years of life), very young number words represent a continuum of distinct
children can represent number in three different quantities which also stretches beyond ‘three’.They also
ways (Le Corre and Carey, 2007). The first is the begin to understand that the numbers above three are
analog system, described in the previous paragraphs. harder to discriminate from each other at a glance than
However, although Carey thinks that this system plays sets of 1, 2 and 3 are, but that they can identify by
a part in people’s informal experiences of quantity counting. In Carey’s words ‘The child ascertains the
throughout their lives, she does not seem to assign meaning of 'two' from the resources that underlie
it a role in children’s learning about the counting natural language quantifiers, and from the system of
system, or in any other part of the mathematics that parallel individuation, whereas she comes to know the
they learn about at school. meaning of 'five' through the bootstrapping process –
i.e., that 'five' means one more than four, which is one
In her theory, the second of Carey’s three systems, more than three – by integrating representations of
which she calls the ‘parallel individuation’ system, plays natural language quantifiers with the external serial
15 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

ordered count list’. Carey called this new understanding Carey’s theory has been subjected to much criticism
‘enriched parallel individuation’ (Carey, 2004; p. 65). for the role that it attributes to induction or analogy
in the use of the ‘next’ principle and to language.
Carey’s main evidence for parallel individuation and Gelman and Butterworth (2005), for example, argue
enriched parallel individuation came from studies in that groups that have very restricted number
which she used a task, originally developed by Wynn, language still show understanding of larger quantities;
called ‘Give – a number’. In this, an experimenter asks their number knowledge is not restricted to small
the child to give her a certain number of objects numerosities as suggested in Carey’s theory. Rips,
from a set of objects in front of them: ‘Could you Asmuth and Bloomfield (2006; 2008) address it
take two elephants out of the bowl and place them more from a theoretical standpoint and argue that
on the table?’ Children sometimes put out the the bootstrapping hypothesis presupposes the very
number asked for and sometimes just grab objects knowledge of number that it attempts to explain.
apparently randomly. Using this task Carey showed They suggest that, in order to apply the ‘next
that different three-, four- and five-year-old children number’ principle, children would have to know
can be classified quite convincingly as ‘one-’ ‘two-’ or already that 1 is a set included in 2, 2 in 3, and 3 in 4.
‘three-knowers’ or as ‘counting-principle-knowers’. If they already know this, then they do not need to
The one-knowers do well when asked to provide use the ‘next number’ principle to learn about what
one object but not when asked the other numbers number words mean.
while the two- and three-knowers can respectively
provide up to two and three objects successfully. Which of these approaches is right? We do not think
The ‘counting-principle-knowers’ in contrast count that there is a simple answer. If you hold, as we do,
quantities above three or four. that understanding number is about understanding
an ordered set of symbols that represent quantitative
The evidence for the existence of these three relations, Piaget’s approach definitely has the edge.
groups certainly supports Carey’s interesting idea Both Gelman’s and Carey’s theory only address the
of a radical developmental change from ‘knowing’ question of how children give meaning to number
some small quantities to understanding that the words: neither entertains the idea that numbers
number system can be extended to other numbers represent quantities and relations between quantities,
in the count list. The value of her work is that it and that it is necessary for children to understand
shows developmental changes in children’s learning this system of relations as well as the fact that the
about the counting system. These had been by- word ‘five’ represents a set with 5 items in order to
passed both by Piaget and his colleagues because learn mathematics. Their research did nothing to dent
their theory was about the underlying logic needed Piaget’s view that children of five years and six years
for this learning and not about counting itself, and are still learning about very basic relations between
also by Gelman, because her theory about counting quantities, sometimes quite slowly.
principles was about innate or rapidly acquired
structures and not about development. However,
Carey’s explanation of children’s counting in terms Summary
of enriched parallel individuation suffers the
limitation that we have mentioned already: it has 1 Piaget’s studies of lear ning about number
no proper measure of children’s understanding of concentrated on children’s ability to reason
cardinality in its full sense. Just knowing that the last logically about quantitative relations, and bypassed
number that you counted is the number of the set their acquisition of the counting system.
is not enough.
2 In contrast many current theories concentrate
The third way in which children learn number, on children learning to count, and omit children’s
according to Carey’s theory, is through a system reasoning about quantitative relations. The most
which she called ‘set-based quantification’: this is notable omission in these theor ies is the question
heavily dependent on language and particularly on of children’s understanding of cardinality.
words like ‘a’ and ‘some’ that are called ‘quantifiers’.
Thus far the implications of this third hypothesised 3 Gelman’s studies of children’s counting,
system for education are not fully worked out, and nevertheless, did establish that even very young
we shall not discuss it further. children systematically obey some basic counting
principles when they do count.
16 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

Ordinal number cards, each of which depicted a different number of


squares. Each three-card sequence constituted either
Numbers, as we have noted, come in a fixed order, an increasing or a decreasing series. In some
and this order represents a quantitative series. sequences the number of squares increased from
Numbers are arranged in an ascending scale: 2 is card to card e.g. 2, 4, 8 and 3, 6, 12: in others the
more than 1 and 3 more than 2 and so on. Also the numbers decreased e.g. 16, 8, 4.
next number in the scale is always 1 more than the
number that precedes it. Ordinal numbers indicate Brannon’s results suggested that 11-month-old infants
the position of a quantity in a series. could discriminate the two kinds of sequence (after
seeing several increasing sequences they were more
Piaget developed much the same argument about interested in looking at a decreasing than at yet
ordinal number as about cardinal number. He claimed another increasing sequence, and vice versa), and
that children learn to count, and therefore to she concluded that even at this young age children
produce numbers in the right fixed order, long before have some understanding of seriation.
they understand that this order represents an ordinal
series. This claim about children’s difficulties with However, her task was a very weak test of the
ordinality was based on his experiments on ‘seriation’ understanding of ordinality. It probably shows that
and also on ‘transitivity’. children of this age are to some extent aware of the
relations ‘more’ and ‘less’, but it does not establish that
In his ‘seriation’ experiments, Piaget and his colleagues the children were acting on the relation between all
(Piaget, 1952) showed children a set of sticks all three numbers in each sequence.
different in length and arranged in order from
smallest to largest, and then jumbled them up and The point here is that in order to understand
asked the child to re-order them in the same way. ordinality the child must be able to co-ordinate
However, the children were asked to do so not by a set of ‘more’ and ‘less’ relations. This means
constructing the visual display all at once, which they understanding that b is smaller than a and at the
would be able to do perceptually and by trial-and- same time larger than c in an a >b > c series. Piaget
error, but by giving the sticks to the experimenter was happy to accept that even very young children
one by one, in the order that they think they should can see quite clearly that b is smaller than a and at
be placed. another time that it is larger than c, but he claimed
that in order to form a series children have to
This is a surprisingly difficult task for young children understand that intermediary quantities like b are
and, at the age range that we are considering here simultaneously larger than some values and smaller
(five- to six-years), children tend to form groups of than others. Of course, Brannon did not show
ordered sticks instead of creating a single ordered whether the young children in her study could or
series. Even when they do manage to put the sticks could not grasp these two-way relations.
into a proper series, they tend then to fail an
additional test, which Piaget considered to be the Piaget’s (1921) most direct evidence for children’s
acid test of ordinal understanding: this was to insert difficulties with two-way relations came from another
another stick which he then gave them into its kind of task – the transitivity task. The relations
correct place in the already created series, which was between quantities in any ordinal series are transitive.
now visible. These difficulties, which are highly reliable If A > B and B > C, then it follows that A > C, and
and have never been refuted or explained away, are one can draw this logical conclusion without ever
certainly important, but they may not be true of directly comparing A with C. This applies to number
number. Children’s reactions to number series may as well: since 8 is more than 4 and 4 more than 2, 8 is
well be different precisely because of the extensive more than 2.
practice that they have with producing numbers in
a fixed order. Piaget claimed that children below the age of roughly
eight years are unable to make these inferences
Recently, however, Brannon (2002) made the striking because they find it difficult to understand that B
claim that even one-year-old-children understand can be simultaneously smaller than one quantity (A)
ordinal number relations. The most direct evidence and larger than another (C). In his experiments on
that Brannon offered for this claim was a study in transitivity Piaget did find that children very rarely
which she showed the infant sequences of three made the indirect inference between A and C on the
17 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

basis of being shown that A > C and B > C, but this Therefore, seriation studies do not deal directl y
was not very strong evidence for his hypothesis with children’s understanding of natural number.
because he failed to check the possibility that the The question of the ser iation of number is still
children failed to make the inference because they an open one.
had forgotten the premises – a reason which has
nothing directly to do with logic or with reasoning.
Cardinality, additive reasoning and
Subsequent studies, in which care was taken to extensive quantities
check how well the children remembered the
premises at the time that they were required to So far we have discussed how children give meaning
make the A > C inference (Bryant and Trabasso, to number and how easy or difficult it is for them to
1971; Bryant and Kopytynska, 1976 ; Trabasso, 1977) make connections between what they understand
consistently showed that children of five years or about quantity and the numbers that they learn
older do make the inference successfully, provided when they begin to count. Now we turn to another
that they remember the relevant premises correctly. aspect of cardinal number, its connection with
Young children’s success in these tasks throws some addition and subtraction – or, more generally, with
doubt on Piaget’s claim that they do not understand additive reasoning. There are undeniable connections
ordinal quantitative relations, but by and large there between the concept of cardinality and additive
is still a host of unanswered questions about reasoning and we shall explore them in this section,
children’s grasp of ordinality. We shall return to which is about the additive composition of number,
the issue of transitivity in the section on Space and in the subsequent section, which is about the
and Geometry. inverse relations between addition and subtraction.

Above all we need a comprehensive set of seriation Piaget (1952) included in his definition of children’s
and transitivity experiments in which the quantities understanding of number their realisation that a
are numbers (discontinuous quantities), and not quantity (and its numerical representation) is only
continuous quantities like the rods of different lengths changed by addition or subtraction, not by other
that have been the staple diet of previous work on operations such as spreading the elements or
these subjects. bunching them together. This definition, he indicated,
is valid for the domain of extensive quantities, which
are measured by the addition of units because the
Summary whole is the sum of the parts. If the quantity is made
of discrete elements (e.g. a set of coins), the task of
1 The count list is ar ranged in order of the magnitude measuring it and assigning a number to it is easy: all
of the quantities represented by the numbers. The the children have to do is to count. If the quantity is
relations between numbers in this series are continuous (e.g. a ribbon), the task of measuring it is
transitive: if A > B and B > C , then A > C . more difficult: normally a conventional unit would be
applied to the quantity and the number that
2 Piaget argued that young children find ordinal represents the quantity is the number of iterations of
relations, as well as cardinal relations, difficult to these units. Extensive quantities differ from intensive
understand. He attributed these difficulties to quantities, which are measured by the ratio between
an inability, on the par t of young children, to two other quantities. For example, the concentration
understand that, in an A > B > C ser ies, B is of a juice is measured by the ratio between amount
simultaneously smaller than A and larger than B. of concentrate and amount of water used to make
the juice. These quantities are considered in Paper 4.
3 Piaget’s evidence for this claim came from studies
of seriation and transitivity. The difficulties that His studies of children’s understanding of the
children have in the ser iation experiment, in which conservation of quantities have been criticised on
they have to construct an ordered ser ies of sticks, methodological grounds (e.g. Donaldson, 1978;
are surprising and very striking. Light, Buckingham and Robbins, 1979; Samuel and
Bryant, 1984) but, so far as we know, his idea that
4 However, the criterion for constructing the series children must realise that extensive quantities
in the seriation experiment (different lengths of change either by addition or by subtraction has
some sticks) cannot be applied by counting. not been challenged.
18 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

Piaget (1952) also made the reasonable suggestion the animals: this had no effect on the children’s
that you cannot understand what ‘five’ is unless you performance, and they continued to exclude the
also know that it is composed of numbers smaller dogs mentally from the class of animals. The only
than it. Any set of five items contains a sub-set of 4 manipulation that helped the children was to ask the
items and another sub-set of 1, or one sub-set of 3 children to first think of the set of animals without
and another of 2. A combination of or, in other separating out the dogs, then replace the dogs with
words, an addition of each of these pairs of sets visual representations that marked their inclusion in
produces a set of five. This is called the additive the class of animals, while the dogs were set in a
composition of number, which is an important aspect separate class: the children were then able to create a
of the understanding of relations between numbers. simultaneous representation of the dogs included in
the whole and as a separate part and answer the
Piaget used the idea of class-inclusion to describe this question correctly. After having answered the question
aspect of number; others (e.g. Resnick and Ford, in this situation, some children went on to answer it
1981) have called it part-whole relations. Piaget’s correctly when other class-inclusion problems were
studies consisted in asking children about the presented (for example, about flowers and roses)
quantitative relations between classes, one of which without the support of the extra visual signs.
was included in the other. For example, in some tasks
children were asked to compare the number of dogs The Piagetian experiments on class-inclusion have
with the number of animals in sets which included been criticised on many grounds: for example, it has
other animals, such as cats. For an adult, there is no been argued that the question the children are asked
need to know the actual number of dogs, cats, and is an anomalous question because it uses disjunction
animals in such a task: there will be always more (dogs or animals) when something can be
animals than dogs because the class of dogs is simultaneously a dog and an animal (Donaldson,
included in the class of animals. However, some 1978; Markman, 1979). However, Piaget’s hypothesis
children aged four- to six-years do not necessarily that part-whole relations are an important aspect of
think like adults: if the number of dogs is quite a bit number understanding has not been challenged. As
larger than the number of cats, the children might discussed in the previous sections, it has been argued
answer that there are more dogs than animals. (e.g. by Rips, Asmuth and Bloomfield, 2008) that it is
According to Piaget, this answer which to an adult most unlikely that a child will understand the
seems entirely illogical, was the result of the children’s ordinality of number until she has grasped the
difficulties with thinking of the class of dogs as connection between the next number and the plus-
simultaneously included in the class of animals and one compositions: i.e. that a set of 5 items contains a
excluded from it for comparison purposes. Once they set of 4 items plus a set of 1, and a set of 4 items is
mentally excluded the dogs from the set of animals, composed of a set of 3 plus 1, and so on.
they could no longer think of the dogs as part of the
set of animals: they would then be unable to focus on For exactly the same reasons, the understanding of
the fact that the whole (the overall class, animals) is additive composition of number is essential in any
always larger than one part (the included class, dogs). comparison between two numbers. To judge the
difference, for example, between 7 and 4, something
Piaget and his colleagues (Piaget, 1952; Inhelder, which as we shall see is not always easy for young
Sinclair and Bovet, 1974) did use a number of children, you need to know that 7 is composed of 4
conditions to try to eliminate alternative hypotheses and 3, which means that 7 is 3 greater than 4.
for children’s difficulties. For example, they asked the
children whether in the whole world there would be Of course, even very young children have a great deal
more dogs or more animals. This question used the of informal experience of quantities increasing or
same linguistic format but could be answered without decreasing as a result of additions and subtractions.
an understanding of the necessary relation between a There is good evidence that pre-schoolers do
part and a whole: the children could think that there understand that additions increase and subtractions
are many types of animals in the world and therefore decrease quantities (Brush, 1978; Cooper, 1984; Klein,
there say that there are more animals than dogs. 1984) but this does not mean that they realise that
Children are indeed more successful in answering this the only changes that affect quantity are addition and
question than the class-inclusion one. Another subtraction. It is possible that their understanding of
manipulation Piaget and his colleagues used was to these changes is qualitative in the sense that it lacks
ask the children to circle with a string the dogs and precision. We can take as an example what happens
19 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

when young children are shown two sets that are However, their counting is not always economic. If, for
unequal and are arranged in one-to-one example, they are given a set of 7 items which they
correspondence, as in Figure 2.1, so that it is possible duly count and then 6 further items are added to this
for the children to see the size of the difference (say set and the children are asked about the total number
one set has 10 objects and the other 7). The in the newly increased set, they tend to count all the
experimenter proposes to add to the smaller set 13 items in front of them including the subset that
fewer items than the difference (i.e. she proposes to they counted before. Such observations have been
add 2 to the set with 7). Some preschoolers judge replicated many times (e.g. Fuson, 1983; Nunes and
that the set to which elements are added will become Bryant, 1996; Wright, 1994) and have given origin to
larger than the other. Others think that it is now the a widely used analysis of children’s progress in
same as the other set. It is only at about six- or seven- understanding cardinality (e.g. Steffe, Cobb and von
years that children actually take into account the Glaserfeld, 1988; Steffe, Thompson and Richards, 1982;
precise difference between the sets in order to know Steffe, von Glasersfeld, Richards and Cobb, 1983). This
whether they will be the same or not after the counting of all the items is not wrong, of course, but
addition of items to the smaller set (Klein, 1984; the repeated counting of the initial items is
Blevins-Knabe, Cooper, Mace and Starkey, 1987). unnecessary. The children could just as well and much
more efficiently have counted on from the initial set
The basic importance of the additive composition of (not ‘1, 2, 3,…..13’ but ‘8, 9, 10…..13’).
number means that learning to count and learning to
add and subtract are not necessarily two successive According to Vergnaud (2008), the explanation for
and separate intellectual steps, as common sense children’s uneconomical behaviour is conceptual: as
might suggest. At first glance, it seems quite a referred in the previous section, he argues that their
plausible suggestion that children must understand understanding of number may be simply ordinal
number and know about the counting system in (i.e. what they know is that the last number word
order to do any arithmetic, like adding and represents the set) and so they cannot add because
subtracting. It seems simply impossible that they ordinal numbers cannot be added. They can, however,
could add 6 and 4 or subtract 4 from 6 without count a new, larger set, and give to it the label of the
knowing what 6 and 4 mean. However, we have now last number word used in counting.
seen that this link between counting and arithmetic
must work in the opposite direction as well, because Studies of young children’s reactions to the kind
it is also impossible that children could know what 6 of situation that we have just described have
or 4 or any other number mean, or anything about consistently produced two clear results. The first is
the relations between these numbers, without also that young children of around the age of five years
understanding something about the additive consistently count all the items. The second is that
composition of number. between the ages of five and seven years, there is a
definite developmental shift from counting-all to
Given its obvious importance, there is remarkably little counting-on: as children grow older they begin to
research on children’s grasp of additive composition of adopt the more economic strategy of counting-on
number. The most relevant information, though it is from the previously counted subset. This new strategy
somewhat indirect, comes from the well-known is a definite sign of children’s eventual recognition of
developmental change from ‘counting-all’ to ‘counting- the additive composition of the new set: they appear
on’. As we have seen, five-year-old children generally to understand that the total number of the new set
know how to count the number of items in a set will contain the original 7 items plus the newly added
within the constraints of one-to-one counting. 6 items. The fact that younger children stick to

Figure 2.1: Two sets in correspondence; the difference between sets is easily seen.
20 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

counting-all does not establish that they cannot The decade structure and additive
understand the additive composition of the new composition
set (as is often the case, it is a great deal easier to
establish that children do understand some principle Additive composition and the understanding of
than that they do not). However, the developmental number and counting are linked for another
change that we have just described does suggest an important reason. The power and the effectiveness of
improvement in children’s understanding of additive counting rest largely on the invention of base systems,
relations between numbers during their first two and these systems depend on additive composition.
years at school. The base-10 system, which is now widespread, frees
us from having to remember long strings of numbers,
The study of the connections that children make, or as indeed any base system does. In English, once we
fail to make, between understanding number, additive know the simple rules for the decimal system and
composition and additive reasoning plainly supports remember the number words for 1 to 20, for the
the Piagetian thesis that children give meaning to decades, and then for a hundred, a thousand and a
numbers by establishing relations between quantities million, we can generate most of the natural numbers
though their schemes of action. They do need to that we will ever need to produce with very little
understand that addition increases and subtraction effort or difficulty. The link between understanding
decreases the number of items in a set. This forms a additive composition and adopting a base system is
foundation for their understanding of the precise way quite obvious. Base systems rest on the additive
in which the number changes: adding 1 to set a composition of number and the decade structure is in
creates a number that is equal to a + 1. This number effect a clear reminder that ‘fourteen’ is a combination
can be seen as a whole that includes the parts a and of 10 and 4, and ‘thirty-five’ of three 10s and 5.
1. Instead of relying on the ‘next number’ induction or
analogy, children use addition and the logic of part- Additive composition is the basic concept that
whole to understand numbers. underlies any counting system with a base, oral or
written. This includes of course the Hindu-Arabic
place-value system that we use to write numbers. For
Summary example, the decimal system explicitly represents the
fact that all the numbers between 10 and a 100 must
1 In order to under stand number as an ordinal be a combination of one or more decades and a
series, children have to realise that numbers are number less than 10: 17 is a combination of 10 and 7
composed of combinations of smaller n umbers. and 23 a combination of two 10s and 3. The digits
express the additive composition of any number
2 This realisation stems from their progressiv e from 10 on: e.g. in 23, 2 represents two 10s which
understanding of how addition affects number: at are added to 3, which represents three units.
first they understand that addition increases the
number of items in a set without being precise Additive composition is also at the root of our ability
about the extent of this increase b ut, as they co- to count money using coins and notes of different
ordinate their knowledge of addition with their denominations. When we have, for example, one 10p
understanding of par t-whole relations, they can and five 1p coins, we can only count the 10p and the
also become more precise about additiv e 1p coins together if we understand about additive
composition. composition.

3 Young children’s tendency to count-all r ather than The data from the ‘shop task’, a test devised by Nunes
to count-on suggests either that they do not and Schliemann (1990), suggest that initially children
understand the additive composition of number find it hard to combine denominations in this way. In
or that their gr asp of additive composition is too the shop task children are shown a set of toys in a
weak for them to take advantage of it. ‘shop’, are given some (real or artificial) money and are
asked to choose a toy that they would like to buy.
4 Their difficulties suggest that children should Then the experimenter asks them to pay a certain
be taught about additive composition, and sum for their choice. Sometimes the child can pay for
therefore about addition, as they lear n about this with coins of one denomination only: for example,
the counting system. the experimenter charges a child 15p for a toy car and
the child has that number of pence to make the
21 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

purchase or the charge is 30p and the child can pay Since children appear to be finding out about the
with three 10p coins. In other trials, the child can pay additive nature of the base-10 system and at the same
only by combining denominations: the car costs 15p time (their first two years at school) about the additive
and the child, having fewer 1p coins than that, must composition of number in general, one can reasonably
pay with the combination of a 10p and five 1p coins. ask what the connection between these two is. One
Although the values that the children are asked to pay possibility is that children must gain a full understanding
when they use only 10p coins are larger than those of the additive composition of number before they can
they pay when using combinations of different values, understand the decade structure. Another is that
children can count in tens (ten, twenty, thirty etc.) instruction about the decade structure is children’s first
using simple correspondences between the counting entrée to additive composition. First they learn that 12
labels and the coins. This task does not require is a combination of 10 and 2 and then they extend this
understanding additive composition. So Nunes and knowledge to other combinations (e.g. 12 is also a
Schliemann predicted that the mixed denomination combination of 8 and 4). The results of a recent study
trials would be significantly more difficult than the by Krebs, Squire and Bryant (2003), in which the same
other trials in the task. They found that the mixed children were given the shop task and counting
denominations trials were indeed much harder for the all/counting on tasks, favour the second hypothesis. All
children than the single denomination trials and that the children who consistently counted on (the more
there was a marked improvement between the ages economic strategy) also did well on the shop task, but
of five and seven years in children’s performance in the there were some children who scored well in the
combined denomination trials. This work was originally shop task but nevertheless tended not to count on.
carried out in Brazil and the results have been However, no child scored well in the counting all/on
confirmed in other research in the United Kingdom task but poorly in the shop task. This pattern suggests
(Krebs, Squire and Bryant, 2003; Nunes et al., 2007). that the cues present in the language help children
learn about the decimal system first and then extend
A fascinating observation in this task is that children their new understanding of additive composition to
don’t change from being unable to carry out the combinations that do not involve decades.
additive composition to counting on from ten as they
add 1p coins to the money they are counting. The
show the same count-all behaviour that they show Summary
when they have a set of objects and more object are
added to the set. However, as there are no visible 1p 1 The decimal system is a good example of an
coins within the 10p, they point to the 10p ten times invented and culturally transmitted mathematical
as they count, or they lift up 10 fingers and say ‘ten’, tool. It enhances our power to calculate and frees
and only then go on to count ‘eleven, twelve, thirteen us from having to remember extended sequences
etc.’ This repeated pointing to count invisible objects of number.
has been documented also by Steffe and his colleagues
(e.g. Steffe, von Glasersfeld, Richards and Cobb, 1983), 2 Once we know the rules for the decade system
who interpreted it, as we do, as a significant step in and the names of the diff erent classes and order s
coordinating counting with a more mature (tens, hundreds, thousands etc.), we can use the
understanding of cardinality. system to count by generating numbers ourselves.

In a recent training study (Nunes et al., 2007), we 3 However, the system also mak es some quite
encouraged children who did not succeed in the difficult intellectual demands. Children find it hard
shop-task to use the transition behaviour we had at first to combine different denominations, such as
observed, and asked them to show us ten with their tens and ones.
fingers; we then pointed to their fingers and the 10p
coin and asked the children to say how much there 4 Teachers should be aware that the ability to
was in each display; finally, we encouraged them to go combine denominations rests on a thorough gr asp
on and count the money. Our study showed that of additive composition.
some children seemed to be able to grasp the idea
of additive composition quite quickly after this 5 There is some evidence that exper ience with the
demonstration and others took some time to do structure of the decimal system may enhances
so, but all children benefited significantly from brief children’s understanding of additive composition.
training sessions using this procedure. There is also evidence that it is possib le to use
22 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

money to provoke children’s progress in multiplication and division. They asked children,
understanding additive composition. aged from six- to ten-years, to choose a number
but not to tell them what this was. Then they asked
the child first to add 3 to this number, next to
The inverse relation between double the sum and then to add 5 to the result of
the multiplication. Next, they asked the child what
addition and subtraction
the result was, and went on to tell him or her what
The research we have considered so far suggests was the number that s/he chose to start with.
that by the age of six or seven children understand Finally the experimenters asked each child to
quite a lot about number: they understand explain how they had managed to work out what
equivalence well enough to know that if two sets this initial number was.
are equivalent they can infer the number that
describes one by counting the other; they Piaget and Moreau reported that this was a difficult
understand that addition and subtraction are the task. The youngest children in the sample did not
operations that change the number in a set; they understand that the experimenters had performed
understand additive composition and what must the inverse operations, subtracting where the child
be added to one set to make it equivalent to the had added and dividing where s/he had multiplied.
other; they understand that they can count on if The older children did show some understanding
you add more elements to a set; and they that this was how the experimenters reached the
understand about ordinal number and can make right number, but did not understand that the order
transitive inferences. However, there is an insight of the inverse operations was important. The
about how addition and subtraction affect the experimenters accounted for the younger children’s
number of elements in a set that we still need to difficulties by arguing that these children had failed to
consider. This is the insight that addition is the understand the adult’s use of inversion (equal
inverse of subtraction and vice versa, and thus that additions and subtractions and equal multiplications
equal additions and subtractions cancel each other and divisions) because they did not understand the
out: 27 + 19 – 19 = 27 and 27 – 19 + 19 = 27. principle of inversion.

It is easy to see that one cannot understand either This was a highly original study but Piaget and
addition or subtraction or even number fully without Moreau’s conclusions from it can be questioned. One
also knowing about the inverse relation of each of alternative explanation for the children’s difficulties is
these operations to the other. It is absolutely essential that they may perfectly have understood the inverse
when adding and subtracting to understand that relations between the different operations, but they
these are reversible actions. Otherwise one will not may still not have been able to work out how the
understand that one can move along the number adult used them to solve the problem. The children,
scale in two opposite directions – up and down. also, had to deal with two kinds of inversion
(addition-subtraction and multiplication-division) in
The understanding of any inverse relation should, order to explain the adult’s correct solution, and
according to Piaget (1952), be particularly hard for so their frequent failures to produce a coherent
young children, since in his theory young children are explanation may have been due to their not knowing
not able to carry out ‘reversible’ thought processes. about one of the inverse relations, e.g. between
Children in the five- to eight-year range do not see multiplication and division, even though they were
that if 4 + 8 = 12, therefore 12 – 8 = 4 because completely at home with the other, e.g. between
they do not realise that the original addition (+8) is addition and subtraction.
cancelled out by the inverse subtraction (–8). This
claim is a central part of Piaget’s theory about Nevertheless, some following studies seemed to
children’s arithmetical learning, but he never tested it confirm that young school-children are often
directly, even though it would have been quite easy unaware aware that inverse transformations cancel
to do so. each other out in a + b – b sums. In two studies
(Bisanz and Lefevre, 1990; Stern, 1992), the vast
In one of his last publications, Piaget and Moreau majority of the younger children did no better with
(2001) did report an ingenious, but rather too inverse a + b – b sums in which they could take
complicated, study of the inverse relations between advantage of the inversion principle than with control
addition and subtraction and also between a + b – c sums where this was not possible. For
23 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

example, Stern reported that only 13% of the seven- particular task young children did a great deal better
year-old children and 48% of the nine-year-olds in her with the inversion problems than with the control
study used the inversion principle consistently, when problems, which is good evidence that they were
some of the problems that they were given were using the inversion principle when they could. In the
a + b – b sums and others a + b – c sums. same study the children were also given equivalent
inversion and control problems as verbal sums (27 +
This overall difficulty was confirmed in a further study 14 – 14): they used the inversion principle much less
by Siegler and Stern (1998), who gave eight-year-old often in this task than in the task with bricks, a result
German children inversion problems in eight which resonates well with Hughes’ (1981) discovery
successive sessions. Their aim was to see whether that pre-school children are much more successful at
the children improved in their use of the inversion working out the results of additions and subtractions
principle to solve problems. The children were also in problems that involve concrete objects than in
exposed to other traditional scholastic problems abstract, verbal sums.
(e.g. a + b – c), which could not be solved by using
the inverse principle. In the last of the eight sessions, The fact that young children are readier to use the
Siegler and Stern also gave the children control inversion principle in concrete than in abstract
problems, which involved sequences such as a + b + problems suggests that they may learn about
b, so that the inversion principle was not appropriate inversion initially through their actions with concrete
for solution. The experimenters recorded how well material. Bryant et al. raised this possibility, and they
children distinguished the problems that could be also made a distinction between two levels in the
solved through the inversion principle from those understanding of the inverse relation between
that had to be solved in some other way. addition and subtraction. One is the level of identity:
when identical stuff is added to and then subtracted
The study showed that the children who were given from an object, the final state of this object is the
lots of inversion problems in the first seven sessions same as the initial state. Young children have many
tended to get better at solving these problems over informal experiences of inverse transformations at
these sessions, but in the final session in which the this level. A child gets his shirt dirty (mud is added to
children were given control as well as inversion it) and then it is cleaned (mud is subtracted) and the
problems they often, quite inappropriately, shirt is as it was before. At meal-times various objects
overgeneralised the inversion strategy to the control (knives, forks etc.) are put on the dining room table
sums: they would give a as the answer in a + b + b and then subtracted when the meal is over; the table
control problems as well as in inverse a + b – b top is as empty after the meal, as it was before.
problems. Their relatively good performance with
the inversion problems in the previous sessions, Note that understanding the inversion of identity
therefore, was probably not the result of an may not involve quantity. The child can understand
increasing understanding of inversion. They seem to that, if the same (or identical) stuff is added and then
have learned some lower-level and totally inadequate removed, the status quo is restored without having to
strategy, such as ‘if the first number (a) is followed by know anything about the quantity of the stuff.
another number (b) which is then repeated, the
answer must be a’. The other possible level is the understanding of the
inversion of quantity. If I have 10 sweets and someone
The pervasive failures of the younger school children gives me 3 more and then I eat 3, I have the same
in these studies to take advantage of the inversion number left as at the start, and it doesn’t matter
principle certainly suggest that it is extremely difficult whether the 3 sweets that I ate are the same 3
for them to understand and to learn how to use this sweets as were given to me or different ones.
principle, as Piaget first suggested. However, in all Provided that I eat the same number as I was given,
these tasks the problems were presented either the quantitative status quo is now restored.
verbally or in written form. Other studies, which
employed sets of physical objects, paint a different In a second study, again using toy bricks, Bryant et al.
picture. (Bryant, Christie and Rendu, 1999; established that five- and six-year-old children found
Rasmussen, Ho and Bisanz, 2003). For example, problems, called identity problems, in which exactly
Bryant et al. used sets of bricks to present five- the same bricks were added to and then subtracted
and six-year-old children with a + b – b inversion from the initial set (or vice versa), easier than other
problems and a + a – b control problems. In this problems, called quantity problems, in which the
24 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

same number of bricks was added and then Thus, the discrepancy between knowing about
subtracted (or vice versa), but the actual bricks inversion and knowing how to calculate went one
subtracted were quite different from the bricks that way but not the other. Gilmore identified a group of
had been added before. Bryant et al. also found a children who could use the inversion principle and
greater improvement with age in children’s yet did not calculate well, but she found no evidence
performance in quantity inversion problems than in at all for the existence a group of children who
identity inversion problems. These results point to a could calculate well but were unable to use the
developmental hypothesis: children’s understanding inversion principle. Children, therefore, do not have
of the inversion of identity precedes, and may to be good at adding and subtracting in order to
provide the basis for, their understanding of the understand the relation between these two
inversion of quantity. First they understand that operations. On the contrary, they may need to
adding and subtracting the same stuff restores the understand the inverse relation before they can
physical status quo. Then they extend this knowledge learn to add and subtract efficiently.
to quantity, realising now that adding and subtracting
the same quantity restores the quantitative status How can knowledge of inversion facilitate children’s
quo, whether the addend and subtrahend are the ability to calculate? Our answer to this is only
same stuff or not. hypothetical at this stage, but it is worth examining
here. If children understand well the principle of
However, the causal determinants of learning about inversion, they may use their knowledge of number
inversion might vary between children. Certainly facts more flexibly, and thus succeed in more
there are many reports of substantial individual problems where calculation is required than children
differences within the same age groups in the who cannot use their knowledge flexibly. For
understanding of the inversion principle. Many of the example, if they know that 9 + 7 = 16 and
seven- and nine-year-olds in Bisanz and LeFevre's understand inversion, they can use this knowledge
study (1990) used the inversion principle to solve to answer two more questions: 16 – 9 = ? and
appropriate problems but over half of them did not. 16 – 7 = ? Similarly, the use of ‘indirect addition’ to
Over half of the ten-year-olds tested in Stern’s solve difficult subtraction problems depends on
(1992) original study did take advantage of the knowing and using the inverse relation between
principle, but around 40% seemed unable to do so. addition and subtraction. One must understand
inversion to be able to see, for example, that an easy
Recent work by Gilmore (Gilmore and Bryant, 2006; way to solve the subtraction 42 – 39 is to convert it
Gilmore and Papadatou-Pastou, 2008) suggests that into an addition: the child can count up from 39 to
the underlying pattern of these individual differences 42, find that this is 3, and will then know that 42 – 39
might take a more complex and also a more must equal 3. In our view, no one could reason this
interesting form than just a dichotomy between way without also understanding the inverse relation
those who do and those who do not understand between addition and subtraction.
the inversion principle. She used cluster analysis
with samples of six- to eight-year-olds who had If this hypothesis is correct, it has fascinating
been given inversion and control problems (again educational implications. Children spend much time
the control problems had to be solved through at home and in school practising number facts,
calculation), and consistently found three groups perhaps trying to memorise them as if they were
of children. One group appeared to have a clear independent of each other. However, a mixture of
understanding of inversion and good calculation skills learning about number facts and about mathematical
as well; these children did better in the inversion principles that help them relate one number fact to
than in the control problems, but their scores in the others, such as inversion, could provide them with
control problems were also relatively high. Another more flexible knowledge as well as more interesting
group consisted of children who seemed to have learning experiences. So far as we know, there is no
little understanding of inversion and whose direct evidence of how instruction that focuses both
calculation skills were weak as well. The remaining on number facts and principles works in comparison
group of children had a good understanding of with instruction that focuses only on number facts.
inversion, but their calculation skills were weak: in However, there is some preliminary evidence on
other words, these children did better in the the role of inversion in facilitating children’s
inversion than in the control problems, but their understanding of the relation between the
scores in the control problems were particularly low. sum a + b = c and c – b (or – a) = ?
25 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Some researchers have called this ‘the complement’ children answered inverse problems (a + b – b),
question and analysed its difficulty in a quite direct control problems (a + b – c) and complement
way by telling children first that a + b = c and then problems (a + b = c; c – a/b = ?). During the training,
immediately asking them the c – a = ? question they only worked on inversion problems. So if the
(Baroody, Ginsburg and Waxman, 1983; Baroody, taught groups improved significantly on the
1999; Baroody and Tiilikainen, 2003; Resnick, 1983; complement problems, this would have to be a
Putnam, de Bettencourt and Leinhardt, 1990). These consequence of realising the relevance of the
studies established that the step from the first to the inverse principle to this type of problem.
second sum is extremely difficult for children in their
first two or three years at school, and most of them For the teaching phase, the children were randomly
fail to take it. Only by the age of about eight years do assigned to one of three groups: a Control group,
a majority of children use the information from the who only received practice in calculation; a Visual
addition to solve the subtraction, and even at this age Demonstration group and a Verbal Calculator group,
many children continue to make mistakes. Would both receiving instruction on the inverse relation.The
they be able to do better if their understanding of form of the instruction varied between the two groups.
the inverse relation improved?
The Visual Demonstration group was taught with the
The study by Siegler and Stern (1998) described support of concrete materials, and started with a
earlier on, with eight-year-olds, seems to suggest that series of trials that took advantage of the identity
it is not that easy to improve children’s understanding inversion. First the children counted the number
of the inverse relation between addition and of bricks in a row of Unifix bricks, which was
subtraction: after solving over 100 inversion subsequently hidden under a cloth so that no
problems, distributed over 7 days, the children did counting was possible after that. Next, the
very poorly in using it selectively; i.e. using it when it experimenter added some bricks to the row and
was appropriate, and not using it when it was not subtracted others. The child was then asked how
appropriate. However, the method that they used many bricks were left under the cloth. The number of
had several characteristics, which may not have bricks added and subtracted was either the same or
facilitated learning. First, the problems were all differed by one; this required the children to attend
presented simply as numbers written on cards, with during all trials, as the answer was not in all examples
no support of concrete materials or stories. Second, the same number as before the additions and
the children were encouraged to answer correctly subtractions, but they could still use the inverse
and also quickly, if possible, but they did not receive principle easily because the difference of one did not
any feedback on whether they were correct. Finally, make the task too different from an exact inversion
they were asked to explain how they had solved the trial. When they had given their answer, they received
problem, but if they indicated that they had used the feedback and explained how they had found the
inverse relation to solve it, they were neither told answer. If they had not used the inversion principle,
that this was a good idea nor asked to think more they were encouraged to think about it (e.g. How
about it if they had used it inappropriately. In brief, it many were added? How many were taken away?
was not a teaching study. Would the number be the same or different?).

Recently we completed two studies on teaching The Verbal Calculator group received the same
children about the inverse relation between addition number of trials but no visual demonstration. After
and subtraction (Nunes, Bryant, Hallett, Bell and they had provided their answer, they were encouraged
Evans, 2008). Our aims were to test whether it is to repeat the trial verbally as they entered the
possible to improve children’s understanding of the operations into a calculator and checked the answer.
inverse relation and to see whether they would Thus they would be saying, for example, ‘fourteen plus
improve in solving the complement problem after eight minus eight is’ and looking at the answer.
receiving instruction on inversion.
As explained, we had three types of problems in
One of the studies was with eight-year-olds, i.e. the pre- and post-test: inversion, control and
children of the same age as those who participated complement problems, which were transfer problems
in the Siegler and Stern study. Our study was for our intervention group, as they had not learned
considerably briefer, as it involved a pre-test, two about these directly during the training. We did not
teaching sessions, and a post-test. In the pre-test the expect the groups to differ in the control trials, as the
26 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

amount of experience they had between pre- unable to find a quite room to work with the
and post-test was limited, but we expected the children without interruptions and the children had
experimental teaching groups to perform significantly difficulty in concentrating.
better than the Control group in the inversion and
transfer problems. The main lesson from this second study was that it is
possible for this intervention to work with such young
The results were clear: children and for the effects to last without further
instruction, but it is not certain that it will do so.
• Both taught groups made more progress than the
Control group from the pre-test to the post-test in Finally, we need to consider whether knowing about
the inversion problems. inversion is really as important as we have claimed
here. Two studies support this claim. The first was by
• The Visual Demonstration group made more Stern (2005). She established in a longitudinal study
progress than the Control group in the transfer that German children’s performance in inversion
problems; the Verbal Calculator group’s tasks, which they solved in their second year at
improvement did not differ from the improvement school, significantly predicted their performance in an
in the Control group in the transfer problems. algebra assessment given about 15 years later, when
they were 23 years old and studying in university. In
• The children’s performance did not improve fact, the brief inversion task that she gave to the
significantly in the control problems in any of children had a higher correlation with their
the groups. performance in the algebra test than the IQ test
given at about the same time as the inversion task.
Thus with eight-year-olds both Visual and Verbal Partialling out the effect of IQ from the correlation
methods can be used to promote children’s reflection between the inversion and the algebra tests did not
about the inverse relation between addition and affect this predictive relation between the inversion
subtraction. Although the two methods did not differ task and the algebra test.
when directly compared to each other, they differed
when compared with the fixed-standard provided by The second study was by our own team (Nunes et
the control group: the Visual Demonstration method al., 2007). It combined longitudinal and intervention
was effective in promoting transfer from the types of methods to test whether the relation between
items used in the training to new types of items, of a reasoning principles and mathematics learning is a
format not presented during the training, and the causal one. The participants in the longitudinal study
Verbal method did not. were tested in their first year in school. In the second
year, they completed the mathematics achievement
In our second teaching study, we worked with much tests administered by the teachers and designed
younger children, whose mean age was just five years. centrally in the United Kingdom. The gap between
We carried out the study using the same methods, our assessment and the mathematics achievement
with a pre-test, two teaching sessions, and a post-test, test was about 14 months. One of the components
but this time all the children were taught using the of our reasoning test was an assessment of children’s
Visual Demonstration method. Because the children understanding of the inverse relation between
were so young, we did not use complement problems addition and subtraction; the others were additive
to assess transfer, but we included a delayed post-test, composition (assessed by the shop task) and
given to the children about three weeks after they correspondence (in particular, one-to-many
had completed the training in order to see whether correspondence). We found that children’s
the effects of the intervention, if any, would remain performance in the reasoning test significantly
significant at a later date without further instruction. predicted their mathematics achievement even after
controlling for age, working memory, knowledge of
The intervention showed significant effects for the mathematics at school entry, and general cognitive
children in one school but not for the children in the ability. We did not report the specific connection
other school; the effects persisted until the delayed between the inversion problems and the children’s
post-test was given. Although we cannot be certain, mathematics achievement in the original paper, so we
we think that the difference between the schools report it here. We used a fixed-order regression
was due to the fact that in the school where the analysis so that the connection between the inversion
intervention did not have a significant effect we were task and mathematics achievement could be
27 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

considered after controlling for the children’s age, intervention in studies carried out in the classroom.
general cognitive ability and working memory. The Experimental studies, such as ours, provide a proof of
inversion task remained a significant predictor of the existence: they show that it is possible to accomplish
children’s mathematics achievement, and explained something under controlled conditions. But they do
12% extra variance. This is a really remarkable result: not show that it is possible to accomplish the same
6 inversion problems given about 14 months before results in the classroom. The step from the laboratory
the mathematics achievement test made a significant to the classroom must be carefully considered (see
contribution to predicting children’s achievement Nunes and Bryant, 2006, for a discussion of this
after such stringent controls. issue). Finally, it is clear to us that developmental
processes that describe children’s development when
Our study also included an intervention component. they do not have any special educational needs (they
We identified children who were underperforming in do not have brain deficiencies, for example, and have
the logical assessment for their age at the beginning hearing and sight within levels that grant them access
of their first year in school and created a control and to information normally accessed by children) may
an intervention group. The control children received need further analysis when we want to understand
no intervention and the intervention group received the development of children who do have special
instruction on the reasoning principles for one hour a educational needs. We exemplify here briefly the
week for 12 weeks during the time their peers were situation of children with severe or profound hearing
participating in mathematics lessons. So they had no loss. The vast majority of deaf children are born to
extra time on maths but specialised instruction on hearing parents (about 90%), who may not know
reasoning principles. We then compared their how to communicate with their children without
performance in the state-designed mathematics much additional learning. Mathematics learning
achievement tests with that of the control group. The involves logical reasoning, as we have argued, and also
intervention group significantly outperformed the involves learning conventional representations for
control group. The mean for the control group in the numbers. Knowledge of numbers can be used to
mathematics assessment was at the 28th percentile accelerate and promote children’s reflections about
using English norms; the intervention group’s mean their schemes of action, and this takes place through
was just above the 50th percentile, i.e. above the social interaction. Parents teach children a lot about
mean. So a group of children who seemed at risk for counting before they go to school (Schaeffer,
difficulties with mathematics caught up through this Eggleston and Scott, 1974; Young-Loveridge, 1989)
intervention. In the intervention study it is not but the opportunities for these informal learning
possible to separate out the effect of inversion; the experiences may be restricted for deaf children. They
children received instruction on three reasoning would enter school with less knowledge of counting
principles that we considered of great importance as and less understanding of the relations between
a basis for their learning. It would be possible to carry addition, subtraction, and number. This does not mean
out separate studies of how each of the three that they have to develop their understanding of
reasoning principles that we taught the children numbers in a different way from hearing children, but
affects their mathematics performance but we did it does mean that they may need to learn in a much
not consider this a desirable approach, as our view is more carefully planned environment so that their
that each one of them is central to children’s learning opportunities are increased and appropriate
mathematics learning. for their visual and language skills. In brief, there may
be special children whose mathematical development
The combination of longitudinal and intervention requires special attention. Understanding their
methods in the analysis of the causes of success and development may or may not shed light on a more
difficulties in learning to read is an approach that was general theory of mathematics learning.
extremely successful (Bradley and Bryant, 1983).
The study by Nunes et al., (2007) shows that this
combination of methods can also be used
successfully in the analysis of how children learn
mathematics. However, three caveats are called for
here. First, the study involved relatively small samples:
a replication with a larger sample is highly
recommended. Second, it is our view that it is also
necessary to attempt to replicate the results of the
28 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

Summary For example, if you buy something that costs £35,


you may pay with two £20-notes. You can calculate
1 The inversion principle is an essential par t of your change by subtraction (40 – 35) or by addition
additive reasoning: one cannot understand either (35 + 5). So, problems are not addition or
addition or subtraction unless one also subtraction problems in themselves, but they can be
understands their relation to each other. defined by the type of reasoning that they require,
additive reasoning.
2 Children probably first recognise the inverse
relation between adding and subtr acting the Although preschoolers’ knowledge of addition and
identical stuff. We call this the inversion of identity. subtraction is limited, as we argued in the previous
section, it is clear that their initial thinking about these
3 The understanding of the inversion of quantity is two arithmetical operations is rooted in their
a step-up. It means under standing that a quantity everyday experiences of seeing quantities being
stays the same if the same n umber of items is combined with, or taken away from, other quantities.
added to it and subtr acted from it, even though They find purely numerical problems like ‘what is 2
the added and subtr acted items are different from and 1 more?’ a great deal more difficult than
each other. problems that involve concrete situations, even when
these situations are described in words and left
4 The inversion of quantity is more diff icult for young entirely to the imagination (Ginsburg, 1977; Hughes,
children to understand, but in tasks that involve 1981; 1986; Levine, Jordan and Huttenlocher, 1992).
concrete objects, many children in the five- to
seven-year age range do grasp this form of The type of knowledge that children develop initially
inversion to some extent. seems to be related to two types of action: putting
more elements in a set (or joining two sets) and
5 There are however strong individual differences taking out elements from one set (or separating two
among children in this f orm of understanding. sets). These schemes of action are used by children
Children in the five- to eight-year range fall into to solve arithmetic problems when they are
three main groups. Those who are good a presented in the context of stories.
calculating and also good at using the in version
principle, those who are weak in both things, By and large, three main kinds of story problem have
and those who are good at using the in version been used to investigate children’s additive reasoning:
principle, but weak in calculating.
• the Change problem (‘Bill had eight apples and then
6 The evidence suggests that children’s he gave three of them away. How many did he
understanding of the inversion principle plays an have left?’).
important causal role in their progress in learning
about mathematics. Children’s understanding of • the Combine problem (‘Jane has three dolls and Mary
inversion is a good predictor of their mathematical has four. How many do they have altogether?’).
success, and improving this understanding has the
result of improving children’s mathematical • the Compare problem (‘Sam has five books and
knowledge in general. Sarah has eight. How many more books does Sarah
have than Sam?’).

Additive reasoning and A great deal of research (e.g. Brown, 1981; Carpenter,
Hiebert and Moser, 1981; Carpenter and Moser, 1982;
problem solving De Corte and Verschaffel, 1987; Kintsch and Greeno,
In this section we continue to analyse children’s 1985; Fayol, 1992; Ginsburg, 1977; Riley, Greeno and
ability to solve additive reasoning problems. Additive Heller, 1983; Vergnaud, 1982) has shown that in
reasoning refers to reasoning used to solve problems general, the Change and Combine problems are
where addition or subtraction are the operations much easier than the Compare problems. The most
used to find a solutions. We prefer to use this interesting aspect of this consistent pattern of results
expression, rather than addition and subtraction is that problems that are solved by the same
problems, because it is often possible to solve the arithmetic operation – or in other words, by the same
same problem either by addition or by subtraction. sum – can differ radically in how difficult they are.
29 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Usually pre-school children do make the appropriate between the decrease in quantity and the operation
moves in the easiest Change and Combine problems: of addition can be solved if the children understand
they put together and count up (counting on or the inverse relation between subtraction and
counting all) and separate and count the relevant set addition: by adding the number that Tim still has
to find the answer.Very few pre-school children seem and the number he gave away, one can find out
to know addition and subtraction facts, and so they how many candies he had before.
succeed considerably more if they have physical objects
(or use their fingers) in order to count. Research by Different analyses of word problems have been
Carpenter and Moser (1982) gives an indication of proposed (e.g. Briars and Larkin, 1984; Carpenter and
how pre-school children perform in the simpler Moser, 1982; Fuson, 1992; Nesher, 1982; Riley, Greeno
problems.These researchers interviewed children (aged and Heller, 1983; Vergnaud, 1982). We focus here on
about four to five years) twice before they had had some aspects of the analysis provided by Gérard
been given any instruction about arithmetic in school; Vergnaud, which allows for the comparison of many
we give here the results of each of these interviews, as different types of problems and can also be used to
there is always some improvement worth noting help understand the level of difficulty of further types
between the testing occasions. of additive reasoning problems, involving directed
numbers (i.e. positive and negative numbers).
For Combine problems (given two parts, find the
whole), 75% and 82% of the answers were correct First, Vergnaud distinguishes between numerical and
when the numbers were small and 50% and 71% relational calculation. Numerical calculation refers to the
when the numbers were larger; only 13% of the arithmetic operations that the children carry out to
responses with small numbers were obtained through find the answer to a problem: in the case of additive
the recall of number facts and this was the largest reasoning, addition and subtraction are the relevant
percentage of recall of number facts observed in their operations. Relational calculation refers to the
study. For Change problems (Tim had 11 candies; he operations of thought that the child must carry out in
gave 7 to Martha; how many did he have left?), the order to handle the relations involved in the problem.
pre-schoolers were correct 42% and 61% with larger For example, in the problem ‘Bertrand played a game
numbers (Carpenter and Moser do not report the of marbles and lost 7 marbles. After the game he had
figures for smaller numbers) at each of the two 3 marbles left. How many marbles did he have before
interviews; only 1% of recall of number facts is the game?’, the relational calculation is the realisation
reported. So, pre-school children can do relatively well that the solution requires using the inverse of
on simple Change and Combine problems before subtraction to go from the final state to the initial
they know arithmetic facts; they do so by putting sets state and the numerical calculation would be 7 + 3.
together or by separating them and counting.
Vergnaud proposes that children perform these
This classification of problems into three types – relational calculations in an implicit manner: to use his
Combine, Change and Compare – is not sufficient expression, they rely on ‘theorems in action’. The
to describe story problem-solving. In a Change children may say that they ‘just know’ that they have
problem, for example, the story might provide the to add when they solve the problem, and may be
information about the initial state and the change unable to say that the reason for this is that addition
(Tim had 11 candies; he gave 7 to Martha); the child is the inverse of subtraction. Vergnaud reports
is asked to say what the final state is. But it is also approximately twice as many correct responses by
possible to provide information, for example, about French pre-school children (aged about five years) to
the transformation and the end state (Tim had a problem that involves no relational calculation
some candies; he gave Martha 7 and he has 4 left) (about 50% correct in the problem: Pierre had 6
and ask the child to say what the initial state was marbles. He played a game and lost 4; how many did
(how many did he have before he gave candies to he have after the game?) than to the problem above
Martha?). This sort of analysis has resulted in more (about 26% correct responses), where we are told
complex classifications, which consider which how many marbles Bertrand lost and asked how
information is given and which information must be many he had before the game.
supplied by the children in the answer. Stories that
describe a situation where the quantity decreases, as Vergnaud also distinguished three types of
in the example above, but have a missing initial state meanings that can be represented by natural
can most easily be solved by an addition. The conflict numbers: quantities (which he calls measures),
30 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

transformations and relations. This distinction has lost 7 marbles. His final result, with the two games
an effect on the types of problems that can be together, was that he had won 3 marbles. What
created starting from the simple classification in happened in the first game?
three types (change, combine and compare) and
their level of difficulty. This de-combination of transformations was still very
difficult for French children in the fourth year in
First, consider the two problems below, the first school (age about nine years): they attained less than
about combining a quantity and a transformation and 50% correct responses.
the second about combining two transformations.
Vergnaud’s hypothesis is that when children combine
• Pierre had 6 marbles. He played one game and transformations, rather than quantities, they have to
lost 4 marbles. How many marbles did he have go beyond natural numbers: they are now operating
after the game? in the domain of whole numbers. Natural numbers
are counting numbers. You can certainly count the
• Paul played two games of marbles. He won 6 in number of marbles that Pierre had before he started
the first game and lost 4 in the second game. the game, count and take away the marbles that he
What happened, counting the two games together? lost in the second game, and say how many he had
left at the end. In the case of Paul’s problem, if you
French children, who were between pre-school and count the marbles that he won in the first game, you
their fourth year in school, consistently performed need to count them as ‘one more, two more, three
better on the first than on the second type of more etc.’: you are actually not counting marbles but
problem, even though the same arithmetic calculation the relation between the number that he now has to
(6-4) is required in both problems. By the second year the number he had to begin with. So if the starting
in school, when the children are about seven years old, point in a problem that involves transformations is
they achieve about 80% correct responses in the first not known, the transformations are now relations. Of
problem, and they only achieve a comparable level of course, children who do solve the problem about
success two years later in the second problem. So, Paul’s marbles may not be fully aware of the
combining transformations is more difficult than difference between a transformation and a relation,
combining a quantity and a transformation. and may succeed exactly because they overlook this
difference. This point is discussed in Paper 4, when we
Brown (1981) confirmed these results with English consider in detail how children think about relations.
students in the age range 11 to 16. In her task,
students are shown a sign-post that indicates that Finally, problems where children are asked to quantify
Grange is 29 miles to the west and Barton is 58 relations are usually difficult as well:
miles to the east; they are asked how do they work
out how far they need to drive to go from Grange • Peter has 8 marbles. John has 3 marbles. How many
to Barton. There were eight choices of operations more marbles does Peter have than John?
connecting these two numbers for the students to
indicate the correct one. The rate of correct The question in this problem is neither about a
responses to this problem was 73%, which contrasts quantity (i.e. John’s or Peter’s marbles) nor about a
with 95% correct responses when the problem transformation (no-one lost or got more marbles): it
referred to a union of sets (a combine problem). is about the relation between the two quantities.
Although most pre-school children can say that Peter
The children found problems even more difficult has more marbles, the majority cannot quantify the
when they needed to de-combine transformations relation (or the difference) between the two. The
than when they had to combine them. Here is an best known experiments that demonstrate this
example of a problem with which they needed to difficulty were carried out by Hudson (1983) in the
de-combine two transformations, because the story United States. In a series of three experiments, he
provides the result of combining operations and the showed the children some pictures and asked them
question that must be answered is about the state two types of question:
of affairs before the combination took place.
• Here are some birds and some worms. How many
• Bruno played two games of marbles. He played the more birds than worms?
first and the second game. In the second game he
31 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• Here are some birds and some worms. The birds Vergnaud (1982) pointed out that relationships
are racing to get a worm. How many birds won’t between people could be used to create problems
get worms? that do not contain the question ‘how many more’.
Among others, he suggested the following example.
The first question asks the children to quantify the
relation between the two sets, of worms and birds; • Peter owes 8 marbles to Henry but Henry owes
the second question asks the children to imagine that 6 marbles to Peter. What do they have to do to
the sets were matched and quantify the set that has get even?
no matching elements. The children in the first year of
school (mean age seven years) attained 64% correct According to his analysis, this problem involves a
responses to the first question and 100% to the composition of relations.
second question; in nursery school (mean age four
years nine months) and kindergarten (mean age 6 Marthe (1979) compared the performance of French
years 3 months), the rate of correct responses was, students in the age range 11 to 15 years in two
respectively, 17% and 25% to the first question and problems involving such composition of relations
83% and 96% to the second question. with their performance in two problems involving
a change situation (i.e. quantity, transformation,
It is, of course, difficult to be completely certain that quantity). In order to control for problem format, all
the second question is easier because the children four problems had the structure a + x = b, in which x
are asked a question about quantity whereas the first shows the place of the unknown. The problems used
question is about a relation. The reason for this large numbers so that students had to go through
ambiguity is that two things have to change at the the relational calculation in order to determine the
same time for the story to be different: in order to numerical calculation (with small numbers, it is
change the target of the question, so that it is either possible to work in an intuitive manner, sometimes
a quantity or a relation, the language used in the starting from a hypothetical amount and adjusting the
problem also varies: in the first problem, the word starting point later to make it fit). An example of a
‘more’ is used, and in the second it is not. problem type using a composition of relations is
shown below.
Hudson included in one of his experiments a pre-
test of children’s understanding of the word ‘more’ • Mr Dupont owes 684 francs to Mr. Henry. But Mr
(e.g. Are there more red chips or more white Henry also owes money to Mr Dupont. Taking
chips?’) and found that they could answer this everything into account, Mr Dupont must give back
question appropriately. He concluded that it was the 327 francs to Mr Henry. What amount did Mr
linguistic difficulty of the ‘How many more…?’ Henry owe to Mr Dupont?
question that made the problem difficult, not simply
the difficulty of the word ‘more’. We are not Marthe did find that problems about relations were
convinced by his conclusion and think that more quite a bit more difficult than those about quantities
research about children’s understanding of how to and transformations; there was a difference of 20%
quantify relations is required. Stern (2005), on the between the rates of correct responses for the
other hand, suggests that both explanations are younger children and 10% for the older children.
relevant: the linguistic form is more difficult and However, the most important effect in these
quantifying relations is also more difficult than using problems seemed to be whether the students had
numbers to describe quantities. to deal with numbers that had the same or different
signs: problems with same signs were consistently
In the domain of directed numbers (i.e. positive easier than those with different signs.
and negative numbers), it is relatively easier to study
the difference between attributing numbers to In summary, different researchers have argued that it
quantities and to relations without asking the ‘how is one thing to learn to use numbers to represent
many more’ question. Unfortunately, studies with quantities and a quite different one to use numbers
larger sample, which would allow for a quantitative to quantify relations. Relations are more abstract and
comparison in the level of difficulty of these more challenging for students. Thompson (1993)
problems, are scarce. However, some indication hypothesises that learning to quantify and think
that quantifying relations is more difficult for about numbers as measures of relations is a crucial
students is available in the literature. step that students must take in order to understand
32 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

algebra. We are completely sympathetic to this depends on children making a coherent


hypothesis, but we think that the available evidence connection between quantitative relations and
is a bit thin. the appropriate numerical analysis.

More than two decades ago, Dickson, Brown and


Gibson (1984) reviewed research on additive reasoning
and problem solving, and pointed out how difficult it is
Overall conclusions and
to come to firm conclusions when no single study has
educational implications
covered the variety of problems that any theoretical • Learning about quantities and numbers are two
model would aim to compare. We have to piece the different matters: children can understand relations
evidence together from diverse studies, and of course between quantities and not know how to make
samples vary across different locations and cohorts. In inferences about the numbers that are used to
the last decade research on additive reasoning has represent the quantities; they might also learn to
received less attention in research on mathematics count without making a connection between
education than before. Unfortunately, this has left some counting and what it implies for the relations
questions with answers that are, at best, based on single between quantities.
studies with limited numbers of students. It is time to
use a new synthesis to re-visit these questions and • Some ideas about quantities are essential for
seek for unambiguous answers within a single research understanding number: equivalence between
programme. quantities, their order of magnitude, and the part-
whole relations implicit in determining the number
Summary of elements in a set.

1 In word problems children are told a br ief story • These core ideas, in turn, require that children
which ends in an ar ithmetical question. These come to understand yet other logical principles:
problems are widely used in school textbooks transitive relations in equivalence and order,
and also as a research tool. which operations change quantities and which do
not, and the inverse relation between addition
2 There are three main kinds of w ord problem: and subtraction. These notions are central to
Combine, Change and Compare . understanding numbers and how they represent
quantities; children who have a good grasp of
3 Vergnaud argued that the cr ucial elements in them learn mathematics better in school.
these problems were Quantities (measures), Children who have difficulties with these ideas
Transformations and Relations. On the whole , and do not receive support to come to grips
problems that involve Relations are harder than with them are at risk for difficulties in learning
those involving Transformations. mathematics, but these difficulties can be
prevented to a large extent if they receive
4 However, other factors also affect the level of appropriate instruction.
difficulty in word problems. Any change in sign is
often hard for children to handle: when the stor y • There is no question that word problems give us a
is about an addition b ut the solution is to subtr act, valuable insight into children’s reasoning about
as in missing addend problems, children often fail addition and subtraction. They demonstrate that
to use the inverse operation. there is a great deal more to understanding these
operations than just learning how to add and
5 Overall the extreme var iability in the level of subtract. Children’s solutions do depend on their
difficulty of different problems, even when these ability to reason about the relations between
demand exactly the same mathematical solution quantities in a logical manner. There is no doubt
(the same simple additions or subtr actions) about these conclusions, even if there is need for
confirms the view that there is a great deal more further research to pin down some of the details.
to arithmetical learning than knowing how to carry
out par ticular operations. • Learning to count and to use numbers to
represent quantities is an important element in
6 Research on word problems suppor ts a different this developmental process. Children can more
approach, which is that ar ithmetical learning easily reason about the relation between
33 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

addition, subtraction, and number when they Endnotes


know how to represent quantities by counting.
But this is not a one-way relation: it is by adding, 1 Gelman and Butterworth (2005) make a similar distinction
subtracting, and understanding the inverse between numerosity and the representation of n umber: ‘we
relation between these operations that children need to distinguish possession of the concept of n umerosity
itself (knowing that any set has a n umerosity that can be
understand additive composition and learn to determined by enumeration) from the possession of
solve additive reasoning problems. rerepresentations (in language) of par ticular numerosities’
(pp. 6). However, we adopt here the ter m ‘quantities’ because
• The major implication from this review is that it has an established definition and use in the context of
schools should take very seriously the need to children’s learning of mathematics.
include in the curriculum instruction that promotes
2 It is noted here that evidence from cases studies of acquired
reflection about relations between quantities, dyscalculia (a cognitive disorder affecting the ability to solve
operations, and the quantification of relations. mathematics problems observed in patients after neurological
damage) is consistent with the idea that under standing
• These reflections should not be seen as quantities and number knowledge can be dissociated:
appropriate only for very young children: when calculation may be impaired and conser vation of quantities
may be intact in some patients whereas in other s calculation
natural numbers start to be used to represent is intact and conceptual knowledge impaired (Mittmair-Delazer,
relations, directed numbers become a new Sailer and Benke, 1995). Dissociations between arithmetic skills
domain of activity for children to re-construct and the meaning of n umbers were extensively described by
their understanding of additive relations. The McCloskey (1992) in a detailed review of cases of acquired
construction of a solid understanding of additive dyscalculia.
relations is not completed in the first years of
primary school: some problems are still difficult for
students at the age of 15.
34 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

References

Alibali, M. W., & DiRusso, A. A. (1999). The function of Bryant, P. E., & Kopytynska, H. (1976). Spontaneous
gesture in learning to count: More than keeping measurement by young children. Nature, 260, 773.
track. Cognitive Development, 14, 37-56. Bryant, P. E., & Trabasso,T. (1971). Transitive inferences and
Baroody, A. J., Ginsburg, H.P., & Waxman, B. (1983). memory in young children. Nature, 232, 456-458.
Children’s use of mathematical structure. Journal for Bryant, P., Christie, C., & Rendu, A. (1999). Children’s
Research in Mathematics Education, 14, 156-168. understanding of the relation between addition
Baroody, A. J., & Tiilikainen, S. H. (2003). Two and subtraction: Inversion, identity and
perspectives on addition development. In A. J. decomposition. Journal of Experimental Child
Baroody & A. Dowker (Eds.), The development Psychology, 74, 194-212.
of arithmetic concepts and skills (pp. 75-126). Butterworth, B., Cipolotti, L., & Warrington, E. K.
Erlbaum: Mahwah, New Jersey. (1996). Shor t-term memory impairments and
Bermejo, V., Morales, S., & deOsuna, J. G. (2004). arithmetical ability. Quarterly Journal of
Supporting children’s development of cardinality Experimental Psychology, 49A, 251-262.
understanding. Learning and Instruction, 14 381-398. Carey, S. (2004). Bootstrapping and the or igin
Bisanz, J., & Lefevre, J.-A. (1990). Strategic and non- of concepts. Daedalus, 133(1), 59-69.
strategic processing in the development of Carpenter, T. P., & Moser, J. M. (1982). The
mathematical cognition. In D. J. Bjorklund (Ed.), development of addition and subtr action
Children’s strategies: Contemporary views of problem solving. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser &
cognitive development (pp. 213-244). T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A
Blevins-Knabe, B., Cooper, R. G., Mace, P. G., & cognitive perspective (pp. 10-24). Hillsdale (NJ):
Starkey, P. (1987). Preschoolers sometimes know Lawrence Erlbaum.
less than we think: The use of quantifiers to solve Carpenter, T. P., Hieber t, J., & Moser, J. M. (1981).
addition and subtraction tasks. Bulletin of the Problem structure and first grade children’s initial
Psychometric Society, 25, 31-34. solution processes for simple addition and
Bradley, L., & Br yant, P. E. (1983). Categorising sounds subtraction problems. Journal for Research in
and learning to read -a causal connection. Nature, Mathematics Education, 12, 27-39.
301, 419-521. Cooper, R. G. (1984). Early number development:
Brannon, E. M. (2002).The development of ordinal discovering number space with addition and
numerical knowledge in infancy. Cognition, 83, 223-240. subtraction. In C . Sophian (Ed.), The origins of
Briars, D. J., & Larkin, J. H. (1984). An integrated model cognitive skill (pp. 157-192). Hillsdale, NJ:
of skills in solving elementary arithmetic word Lawrence Erlbaum Ass.
problems. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 245-296. Cowan, R. (1987). When do children tr ust counting
Brown, M. (1981). Number operations. In K. Hart as a basis for relative number judgements? Journal
(Ed.), Children’s Understanding of Mathematics: of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 328-345.
11-16 (pp. 23-47). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson. Cowan, R., & Daniels, H. (1989). Children’s use of
Brush, L. R. (1978). Preschool children’s knowledge of counting and guidelines in judging relativ e
addition and subtraction. Journal for Research in number. British Journal of Educational Psychology,
Mathematics Education, 9, 44-54. 59, 200-210.
35 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

De Cor te, E., & Verschaffel, L. (1987). The effect of Gilmore, C., & Br yant, P. (2006). Individual differences
semantic structure on first graders’ solution in children’s understanding of inversion and
strategies of elementar y addition and subtraction arithmetical skill. British Journal of Educational
word problems. Journal for Research in Psychology, 76, 309-331.
Mathematics Education, 18, 363-381. Gilmore, C., & Papadatou-Pastou, M. (2009).
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Patterns of Individual Differences in Conceptual
Cognition, 44 1-42. Understanding and Arithmetical Skill: A Meta-
Dehaene, S. (1997). The Number Sense. London: Analysis. In special issue on “Young Children’s
Penguin. Understanding and Application of the Addition-
Dickson, L., Brown, M., & Gibson, O. (1984). Children Subtraction inverse Principle”. Mathematical
learning mathematics: A teacher’s guide to recent Thinking and Learning, in press.
research. Oxford: The Alden Press. Ginsburg, H. (1977). Children’s Arithmetic: The Learning
Donaldson, M. (1978). Children’s minds. London: Process. New York: Van Nostrand.
Fontana. Ginsburg, H. P., Klein, A., & Starkey, P. (1998). The
Fayol, M. (1992). From number to numbers in use: Development of Children’s Mathematical
solving arithmetic problems. In J. Bideaud, C. Thinking: Connecting Research with Pr actice. In
Meljac & J.-P. Fischer (Eds.), Pathways to Number W. Damon, I. E. Siegel & A. A. Renninger (Eds.),
(pp. 209-218). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. Handbook of Child Psychology. Child Psychology in
Freeman, N. H., Antonuccia, C., & Lewis, C. (2000). Practice (Vol. 4, pp. 401-476). New York: John
Representation of the cardinality pr inciple: early Wiley & Sons.
conception of error in a counterfactual test. Gréco, P. (1962). Quantité et quotité: nouvelles
Cognition, 74, 71-89. recherches sur la correspondance terme-a-terme
Frydman, O., & Br yant, P. E. (1988). Sharing and the et la conser vation des ensembles. In P. Gréco & A.
understanding of number equivalence by young Morf (Eds.), Structures numeriques elementaires:
children. Cognitive Development, 3, 323-339. Etudes d’Epistemologie Genetique Vol 13
Fuson, K. (1992). Research on whole number (pp. 35-52). Paris.: Presses Universitaires de France.
addition and subtraction. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Hudson, T. (1983). Correspondences and numerical
Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching differences between sets. Child Development, 54,
and learning (pp. 243-275). New York: Macmilla 84-90.
Publishing Company. Hughes, M. (1981). Can preschool children add and
Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children’s Counting and Concepts subtract? Educational Psychology, 3, 207-219.
of Number. New York: Springer Verlag. Hughes, M. (1986). Children and number. Oxford:
Fuson, K. C., Richards, J., & Briars, D. J. (1982). The Blackwell.
acquisition and elaboration of the number word Inhelder, B., Sinclair, H., & Bovet, M. (1974). Learning
sequence. In C . J. Brainerd (Ed.), Children’s logical and the development of cognition. London:
and mathematical cognition (pp. 33-92). New York: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Springer Verlag. Jordan, N., Huttenlocher, J., & Levine, S. (1992).
Fuson, K., & Hall, J. W. (1983). The Acquisition of Early Differential calculation abilities in young children
Number Word Meanings: A Conceptual Analysis from middle- and low-income families.
and Review. In H. P. Ginsburg (Ed.), The Developmental Psychology, 28, 644-653.
Development of Mathematical Thinking (pp. 50- Kintsch, W., & Greeno, J. G. (1985). Understanding
109). New York: Academic Press. and solving word arithmetic problems.
Gallistel, C. R., & Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal Psychological Review, 92, 109-129.
counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43-74. Klein, A. (1984). The early development of arithmetic
Gelman, R., & Butterworth, B. (2005). Number and reasoning: Numerative activities and logical
language: how are they related? Trends in Cognitive operations. Dissertation Abstracts International,
Sciences, 9, 6-10. 45, 131-153.
Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child’s Krebs, G., Squire, S., & Bryant, P. (2003). Children’s
understanding of number. Cambridge, Mass: understanding of the additive composition of
Harvard University Press. number and of the decimal structure: what is the
Gelman, R., & Meck, E. (1983). Preschoolers’ counting: relationship? International Journal of Educational
Principles before skill. Cognition, 13, 343-359. Research, 39, 677-694.
36 PaperSUMMARY
2: Understanding
– PAPERwhole
2: Understanding
numbers whole numbers

Landerl, K., Bevana, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Piaget, J. (1952). The Child’s Conception of Number.
Developmental dyscalculia and basic n umerical London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
capacities: A study of 8-9 year old students. Piaget, J., & Moreau, A. (2001). The inversion of
Cognition 93, 99-125. arithmetic operations (R. L. Campbell, Trans.).
Le Corre, M., & Carey, S. (2007). One, two, three, In J. Piaget (Ed.), Studies in Reflecting Abstraction
nothing more: an investigation of the conceptual (pp. 69-86). Hove: Psychology Press.
sources of verbal number principles. Cognition, Putnam, R., deBettencour t, L. U., & Leinhardt, G.
105, 395-438. (1990). Understanding of derived fact strategies
Light, P. H., Buckingham, N., & Robbins, A. H. (1979). in addition and subtr action. Cognition and
The conservation task as an interactional setting. Instruction, 7, 245-285.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 49, 304-310. Rasmussen, C., Ho, E., & Bisanz, J. (2003). Use of the
Markman, E. M. (1979). Classes and collections: mathematical principle of inversion in young
conceptual organisation and numerical abilities. children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
Cognitive Psychology, 11, 395-411. 85, 89-102.
Marthe, P. (1979). Additive problems and directed Resnick, L., & Ford, W. W. (1981). The Psychology of
numbers. Paper presented at the Proceedings of Mathematics for Instruction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
the Annual Meeting of the Inter national Group Erlbaum Ass.
for the Psychology of Mathematics Education, Resnick, L. B. (1983). A Developmental theory of
Warwick, UK. Number Understanding. In H. P. Ginsburg (Ed.),
McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2004). Large number The Development of Mathematical Thinking (pp.
addition and subtraction by 9-month-old infants. 110-152). New York: Academic Press.
Psychological Science, 15, 776-781. Riley, M., Greeno, J. G., & Heller, J. I. (1983).
Michie, S. (1984). Why preschoolers are reluctant to Development of children’s problem solving ability
count spontaneously. British Journal of in arithmetic. In H. Ginsburg (Ed.), The
Developmental Psychology, 2, 347-358. development of mathematical thinking
Mittmair-Delazer, M., Sailer, U., & Benke, T. (1995). (pp. 153-196). New York: Academic Press.
Impaired arithmetic facts but intact conceptual Rips, L. J., Asmuth, J., & Bloomfield, A. (2006). Giving
knowledge - A single-case study of dyscalculia. the boot to the bootstr ap: How not to lear n the
Cortex, 31, 139-147. natural numbers. Cognition, 101, B51-B60.
Nesher, P. (1982). Levels of description in the analysis Rips, L. J., Asmuth, J., & Bloomfield, A. (2008).
of addition and subtraction word problems. In T. P. Discussion. Do children lear n the integers by
Carpenter, J. M. Moser & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), induction? Cognition, 106 940-951.
Addition and subtraction. Hillsdale,NJ: Lawrence Samuel, J., & Br yant, P. E. (1984). Asking only one
Erlbaum Ass. question in the conser vation experiment. Journal
Nunes, T., & Br yant, P. (2006). Improving Literacy of Child Psychology and Psychiatr y, 25, 315-318.
through Teaching Morphemes: Routledge. Sarnecka, B. W., & Gelman, S. A. (2004). Six does not
Nunes, T., & Schliemann, A. D. (1990). Knowledge of just mean a lot: preschoolers see number words
the numeration system among pre-schooler s. In as specific. Cognition, 92 329-352.
L. S. T. Wood (Ed.), Transforming early childhood Saxe, G., Guberman, S. R., & Gearhar t, M. (1987).
education: International perspectives (pp. pp.135- Social and developmental processes in children’s
141). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. understanding of number. Monographs of the Society
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Evans, D., Bell, D., Gardner, S., for Research in Child Development, 52, 100-200.
Gardner, A., & Carraher, J. N. (2007). The Schaeffer, B., Eggleston, V. H., & Scott, J. L. (1974).
Contribution of Logical Reasoning to the Number development in young children. Cognitive
Learning of Mathematics in Pr imary School. British Psychology, 6, 357-379.
Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 147-166. Secada, W. G. (1992). Race, ethnicity, social class,
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Hallett, D., Bell, D., & Evans, D. language, and achievement in mathematics. In D.
(2008). Teaching Children about the Inverse A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on
Relation between Addition and Subtraction. mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 623-660).
Mathematical Thinking and Learning, in press. New York: Macmillan.
Piaget, J. (1921). Une forme verbale de la Siegler, R. S., & Robinson, M. (1982). The development
comparaison chez l’enfant. Archives de Psychologie, of numerical understanding. In H. W. Reese & L. P.
18, 141-172. Lipsitt (Eds.), Advances in child development and
behavior. New York: Academic Press.
37 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Siegler, R. S., & Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and Vergnaud, G. (1982). A classification of cognitive tasks
unconscious strategy discoveries: a microgenetic and operations of thought involved in addition
analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology- and subtraction problems. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M.
General, 127, 377-397. Moser & R. T. A (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A
Sophian, C. (1988). Limitations on preschool cognitive perspective (pp. 60-67). Hillsdale (NJ):
children’s knowledge about counting: using Lawrence Erlbaum.
counting to compare two sets. Developmental Vergnaud, G. (2008). The theory of conceptual fields.
Psychology, 24, 634-640. Human Development, in press.
Sophian, C., Wood, A. M., & Vong, C. I. (1995). Making Wynn, K. (1992). Evidence against empir icist accounts
numbers count: the early development of of the origins of numerical knowledge. Mind and
numerical inferences. Developmental Psychology, Language, 7, 315-332.
31, 263-273. Wynn, K. (1998). Psychological foundations of
Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American number: numerical competence in human infants.
Psychologist, 55, 1233-1243. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 296-303.
Starkey, P., & Gelman, R. (1982). The development of Xu, F., & Spelke, E. (2000). Large number discrimination
addition and subtraction abilities prior to formal in 6-month-old infants. Cognition, 74, B1-B11.
schooling in arithmetic. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Young-Loveridge, J. M. (1989). The relationship
Moser & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and between children’s home experiences and their
subtraction: a cognitive per spective (pp. 99-115). mathematical skills on entr y to school. Early Child
Hillsdale,NJ: Elrbaum. Development and Care, 1989 (43), 43 - 59.
Steffe, L. P., & Thompson, P. W. (2000). Radical
constructivism in action : building on the pioneer ing
work of Ernst von Glasersfeld. New York Falmer.
Steffe, L. P., Cobb, P., & Glaserfeld, E. v. (1988).
Construction of arithmetical meanings and
strategies. London Springer-Verlag.
Steffe, L. P., Thompson, P. W., & Richards, J. (1982).
Children’s Counting in Arithmetical Problem
Solving. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser & T. A.
Romberg (Eds.), Addition and Subtraction: A
Cognitive Perspective (pp. 83-96). Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Steffe, L. P., von Glasersfeld, E., Richards, J., & Cobb, P.
(1983). Children’s Counting Types: Philosophy, Theory
and Application. New York: Praeger.
Stern, E. (1992). Spontaneous used of conceptual
mathematical knowledge in elementar y school
children. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 17,
266-277.
Stern, E. (2005). Pedagogy - Learning for Teaching.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, Monograph
Series, II, 3, 155-170.
Thompson, P. W. (1993). Quantitative Reasoning,
Complexity, and Additive Structures. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 3, 165-208.
Trabasso, T. R. (1977). The role of memory as a
system in making transitive inferences. In R. V. Kail
& J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the
development of memory and cognition. Hillsdale, NJ:
Lawrence Erlbaum Ass.
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-69-6

www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers
and intensive quantities
By Terezinha Nunes and Peter Bryant, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Summary of Paper 3 3
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: Understanding rational numbers
and intensive quantities 7
Paper 1: Overview
Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and References 28
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities About the authors
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical Terezinha Nunes is Professor of Educational
representation Studies at the University of Oxford.
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation Peter Bryant is Senior Research Fellow in the
in mathematics Department of Education, University of Oxford.
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating
concepts About the Nuffield Foundation
Paper 8: Methodological appendix The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
charitable trust established in 1943 by William
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics being. We fund research and pr actical
in secondary schools. experiment and the development of capacity
to under take them; working across education,
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which science, social science and social policy. While
has been published separately as Introduction and most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
summary of findings. responsive grant programmes we also
undertake our own initiatives.
Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published
together as Summary papers.

All publications are available to download from


our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary of paper 3:
Understanding
rational numbers and
intensive quantities

Headlines
• Fractions are used in primary school to represent numerator and the denominator relate to the
quantities that cannot be represented by a single value represented by the fraction. They must think
whole number. As with whole numbers, children about direct and inverse relations: the larger the
need to make connections between quantities and numerator, the larger the quantity but the larger
their representations in fractions in order to be the denominator, the smaller the quantity.
able to use fractions meaningfully.
• Like whole numbers, fractions can be used to
• There are two types of situation in which fractions represent quantities and relations between
are used in primary school. The first involves quantities, but in primary school they are rarely
measurement: if you want to represent a quantity used to represent relations. Older students
by means of a number and the quantity is smaller often find it difficult to use fractions to
than the unit of measurement, you need a fraction represent relations.
– for example, a half cup or a quarter inch. The
second involves division: if the dividend is smaller There is little doubt that students f ind fractions a
than the divisor, the result of the division is challenge in mathematics. Teachers often say that it
represented by a fraction. For example, when you is difficult to teach fr actions and some think that it
share 3 cakes among 4 children, each child receives would be better for everyone if children were not
¾ of a cake. taught about fractions in primary school. In order
to understand fractions as numbers, students must
• Children use different schemes of action in these be able to know whether two fractions are
two different situations. In division situations, they equivalent or not, and if they are not, which one is
use correspondences between the units in the the bigger number. This is similar to under standing
numerator and the units in the denominator. In that 8 sweets is the same n umber as 8 marbles and
measurement situations, they use partitioning. that 8 is more than 7 and less than 9, for example.
These are undoubtedly key understandings about
• Children are more successful in understanding whole numbers and fractions. But even after the
equivalence of fractions and in ordering fractions age of 11 many students have difficulty in knowing
by magnitude in situations that involve division than whether two fractions are equivalent and do not
in measurement situations. know how to order some fractions. For example, in
a study carried out in London, students were asked
• It is crucial for children’s understanding of fractions to paint 2/3 of f igures divided in 3, 6 and 9 equal
that they learn about fractions in both types of parts. The majority solved the task cor rectly when
situation: most do not spontaneously transfer what the figure was divided into 3 par ts but 40% of the
they learned in one situation to the other. 11- to 12-year-old students could not solv e it
when the figure was divided into 6 or 9 par ts,
• When a fraction is used to represent a quantity, which meant painting an equivalent fraction
children need to learn to think about how the (4/6 and 6/9, respectively).
4 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

Fractions are used in pr imary school to represent between them, however, research shows that
quantities that cannot be represented by a single children think about the situations diff erently.
whole number. If the teaching of fr actions were to be Children use different schemes of action in each
omitted from the primary school curriculum, children of these situations.
would not have the suppor t of school learning to
represent these quantities. We do not believe that it In measurement situations, they use par titioning. If
would be best to just f orget about teaching fractions a child is asked to show ¾ of a chocolate, the child
in primary school because research shows that will try to cut the chocolate in 4 equal par ts and
children have some informal knowledge that could be mark 3 parts. If a child is asked to compare ¾ and
used as a basis for learning fractions. Thus the question 6/8, for example, the child will par tition one unit in
is not whether to teach fractions in primary school 4 par ts, the other in 8 par ts, and tr y to compare the
but what do we know about their informal knowledge two. This is a difficult task because the par titioning
and how can teachers draw on this knowledge. scheme develops over a long per iod of time and
children have to solve many problems to succeed
There are two types of situation in which fr actions in obtaining equal par ts when par titioning. Although
are used in pr imary school: measurement and partitioning and comparing the par ts is not the only
division situations. way to solve this problem, this is the most lik ely
solution path tried out by children, because they
When we measure anything, we use a unit of draw on their relevant scheme of action.
measurement. Often the object we are measuring
cannot be described only with whole units, and we In division situations, children use a different scheme
need fractions to represent a par t of the unit. In the of action, correspondences. A problem analogous to
kitchen we might need to use a ½ cup of milk and the one above in a division situation is: there are 4
when setting the mar gins for a page in a document children sharing 3 cakes and 8 children shar ing 6
we often need to be precise and def ine the margin identical cakes; if the two groups share the cak es
as, for example, as 3.17 cm. These two examples fairly, will the children in one group get the same
show that, when it comes to measurement, we amount to eat as the children in the other group?
use two types of notation, ordinary and decimal Primary school pupils often approach this problem
notation. But regardless of the notation used, we by establishing correspondences between cakes and
could not accurately describe the quantities in these children. In this way they soon realise that in both
situations without using fr actions. When we speak groups 3 cakes will be shared by 4 children; the
of ¾ of a chocolate bar, we are using fractions in a difference is that in the second group there are tw o
measurement situation: we have less than one unit, lots of 3 cakes and two lots of 4 children, but this
so we need to descr ibe the quantity using a fr action. difference does not affect how much each child gets.

In division situations, we need a fraction to represent a From the beginning of primary school, many children
quantity when the dividend is smaller than the divisor . have some informal knowledge about division that
For example, if 3 cakes are shared among 4 children, it could be used to under stand fractional quantities.
is not possible for each one to have a whole cake, but Between the ages of five and seven years, they are
it is still possible to carry out the division and to very bad at par titioning wholes into equal par ts
represent the amount that each child receiv es using but can be relatively good at thinking about the
a number, ¾. It would be possible to use decimal consequences of sharing. For example, in one study
notation in division situations too, but this is rarely the in London 31% of the f ive-year-olds, 50% of the six-
case. The reason for preferring ordinary fractions in year-olds and 81% of the sev en-year-olds understood
these situations is that there are tw o quantities in the inverse relation between the divisor and the
division situations: in the example, the number of shares resulting from the division: they knew that the
cakes and the number of children. An ordinary more recipients are shar ing a cake, the less each one
fraction represents each of these quantities b y a whole will receive. They were even able to ar ticulate this
number: the dividend is represented b y the numerator, inverse relation when asked to justify their answers.
the divisor by the denominator, and the operation of It is unlikely that they had at this time made a
division by the dash between the two numbers. connection between their understanding of
quantities and fractional representation; actually, it is
Although these situations are so similar f or adults, we unlikely that they would know how to represent the
could conclude that it is not necessar y to distinguish quantities using fractions.
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

The lack of connection between students’ who succeed in compar ing fractional quantities and
understanding of quantities in division situations and fractions after instruction in division situations do
their knowledge about the magnitude of fr actions is no better in a post-test when the questions are
very clearly documented in research. Students who about measurement situations than other children
have no doubt that recipients of a cak e shared in a control group who receiv ed no teaching. The
between 3 people will fare better than those of a converse is also tr ue: children taught in measurement
cake shared between 5 people may, nevertheless, say situations do no better than a control group in
that 1/5 is a bigger fr action than 1/3 because 5 is a division situations.
bigger number than 3. Although they understand the
inverse relation in the magnitude of quantities in a A major debate in mathematics teaching is
division situation, they do not seem to connect this the relative weight to be given to conceptual
with the magnitude of fr actions. The link between understanding and procedural knowledge in teaching.
their understanding of fractional quantities and The difference between conceptual understanding
fractions as numbers has to be developed through and procedural knowledge in the teaching of
teaching in school. fractions has been explored in many studies. These
studies show that students can lear n procedures
There is only one well-controlled experiment which without understanding their conceptual significance.
compared directly young children’s understanding of Studies with adults show that knowledge of
quantities in measurement and division situations. In procedures can remain isolated from under standing
this study, carried out in Portugal, the children were for a long time: some adults who are ab le to
six- to seven-years-old. The context of the problems implement the procedure they lear ned for dividing
in both situations was very similar : it was about one fraction by another admit that they ha ve no idea
children eating cakes, chocolates or pizzas. In the why the numerator and the denominator exchange
measurement problems, there was no shar ing, places in this procedure . Learners who are able to
only par titioning. For example, in one of the co-ordinate their knowledge of procedures with
measurement problems, one girl had a chocolate bar their conceptual understanding are better at solving
which was too lar ge to eat in one go . So she cut her problems that involve fractions than other lear ners
chocolate in 3 equal pieces and ate 1. A boy had an who seem to be good at procedures b ut show less
identical bar of chocolate and decided to cut his into understanding than expected from their knowledge
6 equal par ts, and eat 2. The children were asked of procedures. These results reinforce the idea that it
whether the boy and the gir l ate the same amount is very impor tant to tr y to make links between
of chocolate. The analogous division problem was children’s knowledge of fractions and their
about 3 girls sharing one chocolate bar and 6 bo ys understanding of fractional quantities.
sharing 2 identical chocolate bar s. The rate of correct
responses in the par titioning situation was 10% f or Finally, there is little , if any, use of fr actions to
both six- and seven-year-olds and 35% and 49%, represent relations between quantities in pr imary
respectively, for six- and seven-year-olds in the school. Secondar y school students do not easil y
division situation. quantify relations that involve fractions. Perhaps
this difficulty could be attenuated if some teaching
These results are relevant to the assessment of about fractions in primary school involved
variations in mathematics cur ricula. Different quantifying relations that cannot be descr ibed
countries use different approaches in the initial by a single whole n umber.
teaching of fraction, some star ting from division
and others from measurement situations. There is
no direct evidence from classroom studies to
show whether one star ting point results in higher
achievement in fractions than the other. The scarce
evidence from controlled studies suppor ts the idea
that division situations provide children with more
insight into the equivalence and order of quantities
represented by fractions and that they can lear n how
to connect these insights about quantities with
fractional representation. The studies also indicate
that there is little tr ansfer across situations: children
6 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

Recommendations

Research about mathematical Recommendations for teaching


learning and research

Children’s knowledge of fractional Teaching Teachers should be aware of children’s insights


quantities starts to develop before they regarding quantities that are represented b y fractions and
are taught about fr actions in school. make connections between their understanding of these
quantities and fractions.

There are two types of situation relevant Teaching The primary school curriculum should include the
to primary school teaching in which study of both types of situation in the teaching of fr actions.
quantities cannot be represented by a Teachers should be aware of the different types of reasoning
single whole number: measurement and used by children in each of these situations.
division. Research Evidence from experimental studies with lar ger
samples and long-term interventions in the classroom are
needed to establish whether division situations are indeed
a better star ting point for teaching fractions.

Children do not easily transfer their Teaching Teachers should consider how to establish links
understanding of fractions from division to between children’s understanding of fractions in division and
measurement situations and vice-versa. measurement situations.
Research Investigations on how links between situations can
be built are needed to suppor t curriculum development and
classroom teaching.

Many students do no make links between Teaching Greater attention may be required in the teaching
their conceptual understanding of fractions of fractions to creating links between procedures and
and the procedures that they are taught conceptual understanding.
to compare and oper ate on fractions in Research There is a need f or longitudinal studies designed
school. to clarify whether this separation between procedural and
conceptual knowledge does have impor tant consequences for
further mathematics learning.

Fractions are taught in pr imary school only Teaching Consideration should be given to the inclusion of
as representations of quantities. situations in which fr actions are used to represent relations.
Research Given the impor tance of understanding and
representing relations numerically, studies that investigate
under what circumstances pr imary school students can use
fractions to represent relations between quantities are
urgently needed.
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Understanding rational
numbers and intensive
quantities

Introduction
Rational numbers, like natural numbers, can be used (but see Mack, 1990), but it is reasonable to expect
to represent quantities. There are some quantities that the same hypothesis holds: children should lear n
that cannot be represented by a natural number, and to connect quantities that must be represented by
to represent these quantities, we must use rational rational numbers with their mathematical notation.
numbers. We cannot use natur al numbers when the However, the difficulty of learning to use r ational
quantity that we want to represent n umerically: numbers is much greater than the difficulty of
• is smaller than the unit used for counting, learning to use natur al numbers. This paper discusses
irrespective of whether this is a natural unit why this is so and presents research that shows
(e.g. we have less than one banana) or a when and how children have significant insights into
conventional unit (e.g. a fish weighs less than a kilo) the complexities of r ational numbers.
• involves a ratio between two other quantities (e.g.
the concentration of orange juice in a jar can be In the first section of this paper, we discuss what
described by the ratio of orange concentrate to children must learn about rational numbers and why
water; the probability of an event can be described these might be difficult for children once they have
by the ratio between the number of favourable learned about natural numbers. In the second section
cases to the total number of cases).1 we describe research which shows that these are
indeed difficult ideas for students even at the end of
The term ‘fraction’ is often identified with situations primary school. The third section compares children’s
where we want to represent a quantity smaller than reasoning across two types of situations that ha ve
the unit. The expression ‘rational number’ usually been used in different countries to teach children
covers both sor ts of examples. In this paper, we will about fractions. The fourth section presents a br ief
use the expressions ‘fraction’ and ‘rational number’ overview of research about children’s understanding
interchangeably. Fractions are considered a basic of intensive quantities. The fifth section considers
concept in mathematics lear ning and one of the whether children develop sound understanding of
foundations required for learning algebra (Fennell, equivalence and order of magnitude of fr actions
Faulkner, Ma, Schmid, Stotsky, Wu et al. (2008); so when they learn procedures to compare fr actions.
they are impor tant for representing quantities and The final section summarises our conclusions and
also for later success in mathematics in school. discusses their educational implications.

In the domain of whole n umbers, it has been known


for some time (e .g. Carpenter and Moser, 1982;
Ginsburg, 1977; Riley, Greeno and Heller, 1983) that What children must know in
it is impor tant for the development of children’s order to understand rational
mathematics knowledge that they establish numbers
connections between the numbers and the
quantities that they represent. There is little Piaget’s (1952) studies of children’s understanding
comparable research about r ational numbers of number analysed the crucial question of whether
8 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

young children can under stand the ideas of interpretation for fractions. Many younger students
equivalence (cardinal number) and order (ordinal (about 38% of the 10-year-olds in grade 5) seemed
number) in the domain of natur al numbers. He also to treat the numerator and denominator as
pointed out that lear ning to count may help the independent numbers whereas others (about 20%)
children to understand both equivalence and order. were able to conceive fractions as indicating a par t-
All sets that are represented by the same number whole relation but many (22%) are unable to offer
are equivalent; those that are represented by a a clear explanation for how to interpret the
different number are not equivalent. Their order of numerator and the denominator.
magnitude is the same as the order of the number
labels we use in counting, because each number label Rational numbers are also different from natural
represents one more than the previous one in the numbers in their density (see, for example,
counting string. Brousseau, Brousseau and Warfield, 2007;
Vamvakoussi and Vosniadou, 2004): there are no
The understanding of equivalence in the domain natural numbers between 1 and 2, for example,
of fractions is also crucial, but it is not as simple but there is an infinite number of fractions between
because language does not help the children in 1 and 2. This may seem unimpor tant but it is this
the same way. Two fractional quantities that have difference that allows us to use r ational numbers to
different labels can be equivalent, and in fact there represent quantities that are smaller than the units.
is an infinite number of equivalent fr actions: 1/3, 2/6, This may be another source of diff iculty for students.
6/9, 8/12 etc. are different number labels but they
represent equivalent quantities. Because rational Rational numbers have another proper ty which is
numbers refer, although often implicitly, to a whole , it not shared by natural numbers: every non-zero
is also possible for two fractions that have the same rational number has a multiplicative inverse (e.g. the
number label to represent different quantities: 1/3 of inverse of 2/3 is 3/2). This proper ty may seem
12 and 1/3 of 18 are not representations of unimportant when children are taught about
equivalent quantities. fractions in primary school, but it is impor tant for
the understanding of the division algor ithm (i.e. we
In an analogous way, it is not possible simply to multiply the fraction which is the dividend b y the
transfer knowledge of order from natural to rational inverse of the fr action that is the divisor) and will be
numbers. If the common fr action notation is used, required later in school, when students lear n about
there are two numbers, the numerator and the algebra. Booth (1981) suggested that students often
denominator, and both affect the order of magnitude have a limited under standing of inverse relations,
of fractions, but they do so in diff erent ways. If the particularly in the domain of fr actions, and this
denominator is constant, the larger the numerator, becomes an obstacle to their under standing of
the larger is the magnitude of the fr action; if the algebra. For example, when students think of
numerator is constant, the larger the denominator, fractions as representing the n umber of par ts into
the smaller is the fr action. If both var y, then more which a whole was cut (denominator) and the
knowledge is required to order the fr actions, and it number of par ts taken (numerator), they find it very
is not possible to tell which quantity is more b y difficult to think that fr actions indicate a division and
simply looking at the fr action labels. that it has, therefore, an inverse.

Rational numbers differ from whole numbers also in Finally, rational numbers have two common written
the use of two numerical signs to represent a single notations, which students should lear n to connect: 1/2
quantity: it is the relation betw een the numbers, not and 0.5 are conceptually the same number with two
their independent values, that represents the different notations. There isn’t a similar variation in
quantity. Stafylidou and Vosniadou (2004) analysed natural number notation (Roman numerals are
Greek students’ understanding of this form of sometimes used in specific contexts, such as clocks
numerical representation and obser ved that most and indices, but they probably play little role in the
students in the age r ange 11 to 13 years did not development of children’s mathematical knowledge).
seem to interpret the written representation of Vergnaud (1997) hypothesized that different
fractions as involving a multiplicative relation notations afford the understanding of different
between the numerator and the denominator : aspects of the same concept; this would imply that
20% of the 11-year-olds, 37% of the 12-year-olds students should learn to use both notations f or
and 48% of the 13-year-olds provided this type of rational numbers. On the one hand, the common
9 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

fractional notation 1/2 can be used to help students olds were able to solve this task. We (Nunes, Bryant,
understand that fractions are related to the oper ation Pretzlik and Hurry, 2006) gave the same item more
of division, because this notation can be inter preted recently to a sample of 130 pr imary school students
as ‘1 divided by 2’. The connection between fractions in Years 4 and 5 (mean ages, respectively, 8.6 and 9.6
and division is cer tainly less explicit when the decimal years). The rate of correct responses across these
notation 0.5 is used. It is reasonable to expect that items was 28% for the children in Year 4 and 49% for
students will find it more difficult to understand what the children in Year 5. This low percentage of cor rect
the multiplicative inverse of 0.5 is than the in verse of answers could not be explained b y a lack of
1/2, but unfortunately there seems to be no evidence knowledge of the fr action 2/3: when the diagr am was
yet to clarify this. divided into three sections, 93% of the students in
the study by Har t el al. (1985) gave a correct answer;
On the other hand, adding 1/2 and 3/10 is a in our study, 78% of the Year 4 and 91% of the Year 5
cumbersome process, whereas adding the same students’ correctly shaded 2/3 of the f igure.
numbers in their decimal representation, 0.5 and 0.3,
is a simpler matter. There are disagreements regarding This quantitative information is presented here to
the order in which these notations should be taught illustrate the level of difficulty of these questions.
and the need for students to lear n both notations in A different approach to the analysis of how the level
primary school (see, for example, Brousseau, of difficulty can var y is presented later, in the third
Brousseau and Warfield, 2004; 2007), but, to our section of this paper.
knowledge, no one has proposed that one notation
should be the only one used and that the other one Students often have difficulty in ordering fractions
should be banned from mathematics classes. There is according to their magnitude. Hart et al. (1985) asked
no evidence on whether children f ind it easier to students to compare two fractions with the same
understand the concepts related to r ational numbers denominator (3/7 and 5/7) and tw o with the same
when one notation is used r ather than the other. numerator (3/5 and 3/4). When the fractions have the
same denominator, students can respond cor rectly by
considering the numerators only and ordering them as
Students’ difficulties if they were natural numbers. The rate of correct
responses in this case is relatively high but it does not
with rational numbers effectively test students’ understanding of rational
Many studies have documented students’ difficulties numbers. Hart et al. (1985) observed approximately
both with under standing equivalence and order of 90% correct responses among their students in the
magnitude in the domain of r ational numbers (e.g. age range 11 to 13 years and we (Nunes et al., 2006)
Behr, Harel, Post and Lesh, 1992; Behr, Wachsmuth, found that 94% of the students in Year 4 and 87% of
Post and Lesh, 1984; Har t, 1986; Har t, Brown, the students in Year 5 gave correct responses. In
Kerslake, Küchermann and Ruddock, 1985; Kamii contrast, when the numerator was the same and the
and Clark, 1995; Kerslake, 1986). We illustrate here denominator varied (comparing 3/5 and 3/4), and the
these difficulties with research car ried out in the students had to consider the value of the fractions in a
United Kingdom. way that is not in agreement with the order of natur al
numbers, the rate of correct responses was
The difficulty of equivalence questions varies across considerably lower: in the study by Hart et al.,
types of tasks. Kerslake (1986) noted that when approximately 70% of the answers were correct,
students are given diagrams in which the same shapes whereas in our study the percent of cor rect responses
are divided into different numbers of sections and were 25% in Year 4 and 70% among in Year 5.
asked to compare two fractions, this task is relatively
simple because it is possible to use a perceptual These difficulties are not par ticular to U.K. students:
comparison. However, if students are given a diagram they have been widely reported in the literature on
with six or nine divisions and asked to mark 2/3 of equivalence and order of fr actions (for examples in
the shape, a large propor tion of them fail to mar k the United States see Behr, Lesh, Post and Silver, 1983;
the equivalent fractions, 4/6 and 6/9. Har t, Brown, Behr, Wachsmuth, Post and Lesh 1984; Kouba, Brown,
Kerslake, Küchermann and Ruddock (1985), working Carpenter, Lindquist, Silver and Swafford, 1988).
with a sample of students ( N =55) in the age r ange
11 to 13 years, found that about 60% of the 11- to Difficulties in comparing rational numbers are not
12-year-olds and about 65% of the 12- to 13-y ear- confined to fractions. Resnick, Nesher, Leonard,
10 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

Magone, Omanson and Peled (1989) have shown either in written or in or al form. Two schemes
that students have difficulties in comparing decimal of action that children use in division ha ve been
fractions when the number of places after the decimal analysed in the liter ature: par titioning and
point differs. The samples in their study w ere relatively correspondences (or dealing).
small (varying from 17 to 38) b ut included students
from three different countries, the United States, Israel Behr, Harel, Post and Lesh (1992; 1993) pointed out
and France, and in three gr ade levels (4th to 6th). The that fractions represent quantitites in a diff erent way
children were asked to compare pair s of decimals across two types of situation. The first type is the part-
such as 0.5 and 0.36, 2.35 and 2.350, and 4.8 and 4.63. whole situation. Here one star ts with a single quantity,
The rate of correct responses varied between 36% the whole, which is divided into a cer tain number of
and 52% correct, even though all students had parts (y), out of which a specified number is taken (x);
received instruction on decimals. A more recent study the symbol x/y represents this quantity in ter ms of
(Lachance and Confrey, 2002) of 5 th grade students part-whole relations. Partitioning is the scheme of
(estimated age approximately 10 years) who had action that children use in par t-whole tasks. The most
received an introduction to decimal fr actions in the common type of fraction problem that teachers give
previous year showed that only about 43% were able to children is to ask them to par tition a whole into a
to compare decimal fractions correctly.2 Rittle-Johnson, fixed number of parts (the denominator) and show a
Siegler and Alibali (2001) confirmed students’ certain fraction with this denominator. For example,
difficulties when comparing the magnitude of the children have to show what 3/5 of a pizza is. 4
decimals: the rate of correct responses by the
students (N = 73; 5th grade; mean age 11 years 8 The second way in which fr actions represent
months) in their study was 19%. quantities is in quotient situations. Here one starts
with two quantities, x and y, and treats x as the
In conclusion, the very basic ideas about equivalence dividend and y as the divisor, and by the operation of
and order of fractions by magnitude, without which we division obtains a single quantity x/y. For example, the
could hardly say that the students have a good sense quantities could be 3 chocolates ( x) to be shared
for what fractions represent, seems to elude many among 5 children (y).The fractional symbol x/y
students for considerable periods of time. In the represents both the division (3 divided by 5) and the
section that follows, we will contrast two situations quantity that each one will receiv e (3/5). A quotient
that have been used to introduce the concept of situation calls for the use of correspondences as the
fractions in primary school in order to examine the scheme of action: the children establish
question of whether children’s learning may differ as correspondences between por tions and recipients.
a function of these differences between situations. The por tions may be imagined by the children, not
actually drawn, as they must be when the children
are asked to par tition a whole and show 3/5.5
Children’s schemas
of action in division situations When children use the scheme of par titioning in
part-whole situations, they can gain insights about
Mathematics educators and researchers may not quantities that could help them under stand some
agree on many things, but there is a clear consensus principles relevant to the domain of rational numbers.
among them on the idea that r ational numbers are They can, for example, reason that, the more par ts
numbers in the domain of quotients (Brousseau, they cut the whole into, the smaller the par ts will be.
Brousseau and Warfield, 2007; Kieren, 1988; 1993; This could help them under stand how fractions are
1994; Ohlsson, 1988): that is, numbers defined by the ordered. If they can achieve a higher level of precision
operation of division. So, it seems reasonable to seek in reasoning about par titioning,
the origin of children’s understanding of rational they could develop some understanding of the
numbers in their under standing of division.3 equivalence of fractions: they could come to
understand that, if they have twice as many par ts,
Our hypothesis is that in division situations children each par t would be halved in size. For example, you
can develop some insight into the equivalence and would eat the same amount of chocolate after
order of quantities in fr actions; we will use the ter m cutting one chocolate bar into tw o par ts and eating
fractional quantities to refer to these quantities. one par t as after cutting it into f our par ts and eating
These insights can be dev eloped even in the two, because the number of par ts and the size of the
absence of knowledge of representations for fractions, parts compensate for each other precisely. It is an
11 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

empirical question whether children attain these • The first is the one just pointed out: that, when
understandings in the domain of whole n umbers and children set two measures in correspondence, there
extend them to r ational numbers. is no necessary relation between the size of the
dividend and of the divisor. In contrast, in partitioning
Partitioning is the scheme that is most often used children form the implicit model that the sum of the
to introduce children to fractions in the United parts must not be larger than the whole. Therefore,
Kingdom, but it is not the onl y scheme of action it may be easier for children to develop an
relevant to division. Children use correspondences understanding of improper fractions when they form
in quotient situations when the dividend is one correspondences between two fields of measures
quantity (or measure) and the divisor is another than when they partition a single whole. They might
quantity. For example, when children share out have no difficulty in understanding that 3 chocolates
chocolate bars to a number of recipients, the shared between 2 children means that each child
dividend is in one domain of measures – the could get one chocolate plus a half. In contrast, in
number of chocolate bar s – and the divisor is in partitioning situations children might be puzzled if
another domain – the n umber of children. The they are told that someone ate 3 parts of a
difference between par titioning and correspondence chocolate divided in 2 parts.
division is that in par titioning there is a single whole
(i.e. quantity or measure) and in cor respondence • A second possible difference between the two
there are two quantities (or measures). schemes of action may be that, when using
correspondences, children can reach the conclusion
Fischbein, Deri, Nello and Marino (1985) hypothesised that the way in which partitioning is carried out does
that children develop implicit models of division not matter, as long as the correspondences between
situations that are related to their experiences. We the two measures are ‘fair’. They can reason, for
use their hypothesis here to explore what sorts of example, that if 3 chocolates are to be shared by 2
implicit models of fractions children may develop from children, it is not necessary to divide all 3 chocolates
using the par titioning or the correspondence scheme in half, and then distribute the halves; giving a whole
in fractions situations. Fischbein and colleagues chocolate plus a half to each child would accomplish
suggested, for example, that children form an implicit the same fairness in sharing. It was argued in the first
model of division that has a specific constraint: the section of this paper that this an important insight in
dividend must be larger than the divisor. We ourselves the domain of rational numbers: different fractions
hypothesise that this implicit model is developed only can represent the same quantity.
in the context of par titioning. When children use the
correspondence scheme, precisely because there are • A third possible insight about quantities that can
two domains of measures, young children readily be obtained from correspondences more easily
accept that the dividend can be smaller than the than from partitioning is related to ordering of
divisor: they are ready to agree that it is perfectly quantities. When forming correspondences,
possible to share one chocolate bar among children may realize that there is an inverse
three children. relation between the divisor and the quotient: the
more people there are to share a cake, the less
At first glance, the difference between these two each person will get: Children might achieve the
schemes of action, par titioning and correspondence, corresponding insight about this inverse relation
may seem too subtle to be of interest when we are using the scheme of partitioning: the more parts
thinking of children’s understanding of fractions. you cut the whole into, the smaller the parts.
Certainly, research on children’s understanding of However, there is a difference between the
fractions has not focused on this distinction so far. principles that children would need to abstract
However, our review shows that it is a cr ucial from each of the schemes. In partitioning, they
distinction for children’s learning, both in ter ms need to establish a within-quantity relation (the
of what insights each scheme of action aff ords and more parts, the smaller the parts) whereas in
in terms of the empir ical research results. correspondence they need to establish a
between-quantity relation (the more children,
There are at least f our differences between what the less cake). It is an empirical matter to find out
children might learn from using the par titioning whether or not it is easier to achieve one of
scheme or the scheme of correspondences. these insights than the other.
12 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

• Finally, both partitioning and correspondences • next, he removed the pink flowers and asked the
could help children to understand something children to place a blue flower into each one of the
about the equivalence between quantities, but the same vases;
reasoning required to achieve this understanding
differs across the two schemes of action. When • then, he set all the flowers aside, leaving on the
setting chocolate bars in correspondence with table only the vases, and asked the children to take
recipients, the children might be able to reason from a box the exact number of straws required if
that, if there were twice as many chocolates and they wanted to put one flower into each straw.
twice as many children, the shares would be
equivalent, even though the dividend and the Without counting, and only using correspondences,
divisor are different. This may be easier than the five- and six-year old children were able to make
comparable reasoning in partitioning. In inferences about the equivalence between straws and
partitioning, understanding equivalence is based flowers: by setting two straws in correspondence with
on inverse proportional reasoning (twice as many each vase, they constructed a set of str aws equivalent
pieces means that each piece is half the size) to the set of vases. Piaget concluded that the children’s
whereas in contexts where children use the judgements were based on ‘multiplicative equivalences’
correspondence scheme, the reasoning is based on (p. 219) established by the use of the cor respondence
a direct proportion (twice as many chocolates and scheme: the children reasoned that, if there is a 2-to-1
twice as many children means that everyone still correspondence between flowers and vases and a 2-
gets the same). to-1 correspondence between straws and vases, the
number of flowers and straws must be the same.
This exploratory and hypothetical analysis of how
children can reach an understanding of equivalence In Piaget’s study, the scheme of cor respondence
and order of fr actions when using par titioning or was used in a situation that in volved ratio but not
correspondences in division situations suggests that division. Frydman and Br yant (1988) carried out
the distinction between the two schemas is worth a series of studies where children established
investigating empirically. It is possible that the scheme correspondences between sets in a division situation
of correspondences affords a smoother tr ansition which we have described in more detail in P aper 2,
from natural to rational numbers, at least as far as Understanding whole numbers. The studies showed
understanding equivalence and order of fr actional that children aged four often shared pretend sweets
quantities is concerned. fairly, using a one-for-you one-for-me type of
procedure. After the children had distr ibuted the
We turn now to an empir ical analysis of this question. sweets, Frydman and Br yant asked them to count
The literature about these schemes of action is vast the number of sweets that one doll had and then
but this paper focuses on research deduce the number of sweets that the other doll
that sheds light on whether it is possib le to find had. About 40% of the f our-year-olds were able to
continuities between children’s understanding of make the necessar y inference and say the exact
quantities that are represented by natural numbers number of sweets that the second doll had; this
and fractional quantities. We review research on proportion increased with age . This result extends
correspondences first and then research on Piaget’s observations that children can mak e
partitioning. equivalence judgements not only in multiplication but
also in division problems by using correspondence.

Children’s use of the Frydman and Br yant’s results were replicated in a


number of studies by Davis and his colleagues (Davis
correspondence scheme in and Hunting, 1990; Davis and Pepper, 1992; Pitkethly
judgements about quantities and Hunting, 1996), who refer to this scheme of
Piaget (1952) pioneered the study of ho w and when action as ‘dealing’. They used a var iety of situations,
children use the cor respondence scheme to dr aw including redistribution when a new recipient comes,
conclusions about quantities. In one of his studies, to study children’s ability to use cor respondences in
there were three steps in the method. division situations and to mak e inferences about
equality and order of magnitude of quantities. They
• First, Piaget asked the children to place one pink also argue that this scheme is basic to children’ s
flower into each one of a set of vases; understanding of fractions (Davis and Pepper, 1992).
13 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Correa, Nunes and Br yant (1998) extended these division situations in which the quantities w ere
studies by showing that children can make inferences discrete and the dividends were larger than the
about quantities resulting from a division not onl y divisors to their inferences in situations in which the
when the divisors are the same b ut also when they quantities were continuous and dividends smaller
are different. In order to circumvent the possibility than the divisors. In the discrete quantities tasks, the
that children feel the need to count the sets after children were shown one set of small to y fishes to
division because they think that they could ha ve be distributed fairly among a group of white cats and
made a mistake in sharing, Bryant and his colleagues another set of fishes to be distr ibuted to a group of
did not ask the children to do the shar ing: the brown cats; the number of fish was always greater
sweets were shared by the experimenter, outside the than the number of cats. In the continuous quantities
children’s view, after the children had seen that the tasks, the dividend was made up of f ish-cakes, to be
number of sweets to be shared was the same. distributed fairly among the cats: the number of
cakes was always smaller than the n umber of cats,
There were two conditions in this study: same and varied between 1 and 3 cak es, whereas the
dividend and same divisor versus same dividend number of cats to receive a por tion in each group
and different divisors. In the same dividend and same varied between 2 and 9. Following the paradigm
divisor condition, the children should be ab le to devised by Correa, Nunes and Br yant (1998), the
conclude for the equivalence between the sets that children were neither asked to distribute the fish nor
result from the division; in the same dividend and to par tition the fish cakes. They were asked whether,
different divisor condition, the children should after a fair distr ibution in each group, each cat in one
conclude that the more recipients there are , the group would receive the same amount to eat as
fewer sweets they receive; i.e. in order to answer each cat in the other group .6
correctly, they would need to use the inverse
relation between the divisor and the result as a In some trials, the number of fish (dividend) and cats
principle, even if implicitly. (divisor) was the same; in other tr ials, the dividend
was the same but the divisor was different. So in the
About two-thirds of the five-year-olds, the vast first type of tr ials the children were asked about
majority of the six-year-olds, and all the seven-year- equivalence after sharing and in the second type the
olds concluded that the recipients had equivalent children were asked to order the quantities obtained
shares when the dividend and the divisor were the after sharing.
same. Equivalence was easier than the in verse
relation between divisor and quotient: 34%, 53% The majority of the children succeeded in all the
and 81% of the children in these three age lev els, items where the dividend and the divisor w ere the
respectively, were able to conclude that the more same: 62% of the five-year-olds, 84% of the six-year-
recipients there are, the smaller each one’s share will olds and all the seven-year-olds answered all the
be. Correa (1994) also found that children’s success questions correctly. When the dividend was the same
in making these inferences improved if they solved and the divisors differed, the rate of success was
these problems after practising sharing sweets 31%, 50% and 81%, respectively, for the three age
between dolls; this indicates that thinking about ho w levels. There was no difference in the level of success
to establish correspondences improves their ability attained by the children with discrete versus
to make inferences about the relations between the continuous quantities.
quantities resulting from shar ing.
In almost all the items, the children explained their
In all the previous studies, the dividend was answers by referring to the type of relation betw een
composed of discrete quantities and was lar ger the dividends and the divisor s: same divisor, same
than the divisor. The next question to consider is share or, with different divisors, the more cats
whether children can make similar judgements about receiving a share, the smaller their share . The use
equivalence when the situations involve continuous of numbers as an explanation f or the relative size of
quantities and the dividend is smaller than the the recipients’ shares was obser ved in 6% of answers
divisor: that is, when children have to think about by the seven-year-olds when the quantities were
fractional quantities. discrete and less often than this b y the younger
children. Attempts to use numbers to speak about
Kornilaki and Nunes (2005) investigated this the shares in the contin uous quantities trials were
possibility by comparing children’s inferences in practically non-existent (3% of the sev en-year-olds’
14 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

explanations). Thus, the analysis of justifications in the transition between natural and rational
supports the idea that the children w ere reasoning numbers. In the domain of r ational numbers there is
about relations between quantities rather than using an infinite set of equivalences (e .g. 1/2 = 2/4 = 3/6
counting when they made their judgments of etc) and in the studies that we have described so far
equivalence or ordered the quantities that w ould be the children were only asked to make equivalence
obtained after division. judgements when the dividend and the divisor in the
equivalent fractions were the same. Can they still
This study replicated the previous f indings, which make the inference of equivalence in shar ing
we have mentioned already, that young children can situations when the dividend and the divisor are
use correspondences to make inferences about different across situations, but the dividend-divisor
equivalences and also added new evidence relevant ratio is the same?
to children’s understanding of fractional quantities:
many young children who have never been taught Nunes, Bryant, Pretzlik, Bell, Evans and Wade (2007)
about fractions used correspondences to order asked British children aged between 7.5 and 10 years,
fractional quantities. They did so successfully when who were in Years 4 and 5 in school, to make
the division would have resulted in unitar y fractions comparisons between the shares that would be
and also when the dividend was greater than 1 and received by children in sharing situations where the
the result would not be a unitar y fraction (e.g. 2 fish dividend and divisor were different but their ratio was
cakes to be shared by 3, 4 or 5 cats). the same. Previous research (see, for example, Behr,
Harel, Post and Lesh, 1992; Kerslake, 1986) shows that
A study by knowledge Mamede, Nunes and Br yant children in these age levels have difficulty with the
(2005) confirmed that children can mak e inferences equivalence of fractions. The children in this study had
about the order of magnitude of fractions in sharing received some instruction on fractions: they had been
situations where the dividend is smaller than the taught about halves and quar ters in problems about
divisor (e.g. 1 cake shared by 3 children compared partitioning. They had only been taught about one pair
to 1 cake shared by 5 children). She worked with of equivalent fractions: they were taught that one half
Portuguese children in their first year in school, who is the same as two quarters. In the correspondence
had received no school instr uction about fractions. item in this study, the children were presented with
Their performance was only slightly weaker than that two pictures: in the first, a group of 4 gir ls was going to
of British children: 55% of the six-year-olds and 71% share fairly 1 pie; in the second, a group of 8 boys was
of the seven-year-olds were able to make the going to share fair ly 2 pies that were exactly the same
inference that the lar ger the divisor, the smaller the as the pie that the gir ls had. The question was whether
share that each recipient would receive. each girl would receive the same share as each bo y.
The overall rate of correct responses was 73% (78%
These studies strongly suggest that children can in Year 4 and 70% in Year 5; this difference was not
learn principles about the relationship betw een significant). This is an encouraging result: the children
dividend and divisor from exper iences with sharing had only been taught about halves and quar ters;
when they establish correspondences between the nevertheless, they were able to attain a high r ate of
two domains of measures, the shared quantities correct responses for fractional quantities that could
and the recipients. They also suggest that children be represented as 1/4 and 2/8.
can make a relatively smooth transition from
natural numbers to rational numbers when they In the studies reviewed so far the children w ere
use correspondences to under stand the relations asked about quantities that resulted from division
between quantities. This argument is central to and always included two domains of measures; thus
Streefland’s (1987; 1993; 1997) hypothesis about the children’s correspondence reasoning was
what is the best star ting point for teaching engaged in these studies. However, they did not
fractions to children and has been advanced b y involve asking the children to represent these
others also (Davis and Pepper, 1992; Kieren, 1993; quantities through fractions. The final study reviewed
Vergnaud, 1983). here is a br ief teaching study (Nunes, Bryant, Pretzlik,
Evans, Wade and Bell, 2008), where the children
This research tell an encour aging story about were taught to represent fr actions in the context of
children’s understanding of the logic of division ev en two domains of measures, shared quantities and
when the dividend is smaller than the divisor, but recipients, and were asked about the equivalence
there is one fur ther point that should be considered between fractions. The types of arguments that the
15 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

children produced to justify the equivalence of the • Four children will be sharing three chocolates. (a)
fractions were then analyzed and compared to the Will each child be able to get one bar of chocolate?
insights that we hypothesized would emerge in the (b) Will each child be able to get at least a half bar
context of sharing from the use of the of chocolate? (c) How would you share the
correspondence scheme. chocolate? (The booklets contained a picture with
three chocolate bars and four children and the
Brief teaching studies are of great value in research children were asked to show how they would share
because they allow the researchers to know what the chocolates) Write what fraction each one gets.
understandings children can constr uct if they are
given a specific type of guidance in the inter action After the children had completed these tasks,
with an adult (Cooney, Grouws and Jones, 1988; the researcher told them that they were going to
Steffe and Tzur, 1994; Tzur, 1999; Yackel, Cobb, practice writing fractions which they had not y et
Wood, Wheatley and Merkel, 1990). They also have learned in school. The children were asked to write
compelling ecological validity: children spend much ‘half ’ with numerical symbols, which they knew
of their time in school tr ying to use what they ha ve already. The researcher taught the children to wr ite
been taught to solve new mathematics problems. fractions that they had not y et learned in school in
Because this study has onl y been published in a order to help the children re-inter pret the meaning
summary form (Nunes, Bryant, Pretzlik and Hur ry, of fractions. The numerator was to be used to
2006), some detail is presented here . represent the number of items to be divided, the
denominator should represent the number of
The children (N = 62) were in the age r ange from recipients, and the dash between them two numbers
7.5 to 10 years, in Years 4 and 5 in school. Children in should represent the sign for division (for a discussion
Year 4 had only been taught about half and quar ters of children’s interpretation of fraction symbols in this
and the equivalence between half and two quar ters; situation, see Charles and Nason, 2000, and Empson,
children in Year 5 had been taught also about thirds. Junk, Dominguez and Turner, 2005).
They worked with a researcher outside the
classroom in small groups (12 groups of betw een 4 The equivalence task, presented on the second
and 6 children, depending on the class siz e) and were day, was:
asked to solve each problem first individually, and
then to discuss their answers in the group. The • Six children went to a pizzeria and ordered two
sessions were audio- and video-recorded. The pizzas to share between them. The waiter brought
children’s arguments were transcribed verbatim; the one first and said they could start on it because it
information from the video-tapes was later would take time for the next one to come. (a)
coordinated with the tr anscripts in order to help the How much will each child get from the first pizza
researchers understand the children’s arguments. that the waiter brought? Write the fraction that
shows this. (b) How much will each child get from
In this study the researcher s used problems the second pizza? Write your answer. (c) If you add
developed by Streefland (1990). The children solved the two pieces together, what fraction of a pizza
two of his shar ing tasks on the first day and an will each child get? You can write a plus sign
equivalence task on the second day of the teaching between the first fraction and the second fraction,
study. The tasks were presented in booklets with and write the answer for the share each child gets
pictures, where the children also wrote their in the end. (d) If the two pizzas came at the same
answers. The tasks used in the first day were: time, how could they share it differently? (e) Are
these fractions (the ones that the children wrote
• Six girls are going to share a packet of biscuits. The for answers c and d equivalent?
packet is closed; we don’t know how many biscuits
are in the packet. (a) If each girl received one According to the hypotheses presented in the
biscuit and there were no biscuits left, how many previous section, we would expect children to
biscuits were in the packet? (b) If each girl received develop some insights into r ational numbers by
a half biscuit and there were no biscuits left, how thinking about different ways of sharing the same
many biscuits were in the packet? (c) If some more amount. It was expected that they w ould realize that:
girls join the group, what will happen when the
biscuits are shared? Do the girls now receive
more or less each than the six girls did?
16 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

1 it is possible to divide a smaller n umber by a larger When the dividend is twice as large
number and the divisor is also twice as large,
the result is an equivalent amount
2 different fractions might represent the same
amount The principle that when the dividend is twice as
large and the divisor is also twice as large, the result
3 twice as many things to be divided and twice as is an equivalent amount was expressed in 11 of the
many recipients would result in equivalent amounts 12 groups. For example, one child said, ‘It’s half the
girls and half the pizzas; three is a half of six and one
4 the larger the divisor, the smaller the quotient. is a half of two.’ Another child said, ‘If they have two
pizzas, then they could give the first pizza to three
The children’s explanations for why they thought girls and then the next one to another three gir ls.
that the fractions were equivalent provided evidence (…) If they all get one piece of that each, and they
for all these insights, and more, as described below. get the same amount, they all get the same amount’.

So all three ideas we thought that could appear in


this context were expressed by the children. But
It is possible to divide a smaller
two other principles, which we did not expect to
number by a larger number observe in this cor respondence problem, were also
There was no difficulty among the students in made explicit by the children.
attempting to divide 1 pizza among 6 children. In
response to par t a of the equivalence prob lem, all
children wrote at least one fr action correctly (some
The number of parts and size of
children wrote more than one fr action for the same
answer, always correctly).
parts are inversely proportional
The principle that the number of par ts and size of
In response to par t c, when the children were asked parts are inversely proportional was enunciated in 8 of
how they could share the 2 pizzas if both pizzas the 12 groups. For example, one child who cut the
came at the same time and what fr action would pizzas the second time around in 12 par ts each said,
each one receive, some children answered 1/3 and ‘Because it’s double the one of that [total number of
others answered 2/12 from each pizza, giving a total pieces] and it’s double the one of that [number of
share of 4/12. The latter children, instead of shar ing pieces for each], they cut it twice and each is half the
1 pizza among 3 gir ls, decided to cut each pizza in size; they will be the same’. Another child said, ‘Because
12 par ts: i.e. they cut the sixths in half. one sixth and one sixth is actuall y a different way in
fractions [from 1 third] and it doub led [the number of
pieces] to make it [the size of the piece] littler, and
halving [the number of pieces] makes it [the size of the
Different fractions can represent the
piece] bigger, so I halved it and it became one third’.
same amount
The insight that different fractions can represent
the same amount was expressed in all groups. For
The fractions show the same
example, one child said that, ‘They’re the same
amount of people, the same amount of pizzas, and
part-whole relation
that means the same amount of fr actions. It doesn’t The reasoning that the fr actions show the same
matter how you cut it.’ Another child said, ‘Because it part-whole relation, which we had not expected to
wouldn’t really matter when they shared it, they’d get emerge from the use of the cor respondence
that [3 girls would get 1 pizza], and then they’d get scheme, was enunciated in only one group (out of
that [3 girls would get the other pizza], and then it 12), initially by one child, and was then reiter ated by
would be the same .’ Another child said, ‘It’s the same a second child in her own terms. The first child said,
amount of pizza. They might be different fractions ‘You need three two sixths to make six [6/6 – he
but the same amount [this child had offered 4/12 as shows the 6 pieces mar ked on one pizza], and you
an alternative to 2/6].’ Another child said: ‘Erm, well need three one thirds to mak e three (3/3 – shows
basically just the time doesn’t mak e much difference, the 3 pieces mar ked on one pizza). [He wrote the
the main thing is the n umber of things.’ computation and continued] There’s two sixths, add
17 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

two sixths three times to make six sixths. With one Kieren (1993) also documented children’s use of
third, you need to add one third three times to correspondences to compare fractions. In his
make three thirds.’ problem, the fractions were not equivalent: there
were 7 recipients and 4 items in Group A and 4
To summarize: this brief teaching experiment was recipients and 2 items in Group B . The children
carried out to elicit discussions betw een the were asked how much each recipient would get
children in situations where they could use the in each group and whether the recipients in both
correspondence scheme in division. The first set of groups would get the same amount. Kieren
problems, in which they are ask ed about sharing presents a drawing by an eight-year-old, where
discrete quantities, created a background f or the the items are par titioned in half and the
children to use this scheme of action. The correspondences between the halves and the
researchers then helped them to constr uct an recipients are shown; in Group A, a line without a
interpretation for written fractions where the recipient shows that there is an extr a half in that
numerator is the dividend, the denominator is the group and the child ar gues that there should be
divisor, and the line indicates the operation of one more per son in Group A for the amounts to
division. This interpretation did not replace their be the same . Kieren termed this solution
original interpretation of number of par ts taken ‘corresponding or ‘ratiolike’ thinking’ (p. 54).
from the whole; the two meanings co-existed and
appeared in the children’s arguments as they
explained their answers. In the subsequent
Conclusion
problems, where the quantity to be shared was
continuous and the dividend was smaller than the The scheme of cor respondences develops relatively
divisor, the children had the oppor tunity to early: about one-third of the five-year-olds, half of six-
explore the different ways in which continuous year-olds and most seven-year-olds can use
quantities can be shared. They were not asked to correspondences to make inferences about
actually par tition the pizzas, and some made mar ks equivalence and order in tasks that in volve fractional
on the pizzas whereas other s did not. The most quantities. Children can use the scheme of
salient feature of the children’s drawings was that correspondences to:
they were not concerned with par titioning per se , • establish equivalences between sets that have the
even when the par ts were marked, but with the same ratio to a reference set (Piaget, 1952)
correspondences between pizzas and recipients. • re-distribute things after having carried out one
Sometimes the correspondences were carried out distribution (Davis and Hunting, 1990; Davis and
mentally and expressed verbally and sometimes Pepper, 1992; Davis and Pitkethly, 1990; Pitkethly
the children used dr awings and gestures which and Hunting, 1996);
indicated the correspondences. • reason about equivalences resulting from division
both when the dividend is larger or smaller than
Other researchers have identified children’s use of the divisor (Bryant and colleagues: Correa, Nunes
correspondences to solve problems that involve and Bryant, 1994; Frydman and Bryant, 1988; 1994;
fractions, although they did not necessar ily use this Empson, 1999; Nunes, Bryant, Pretzlik and Hurry,
label in describing the children’s answers. Empson 2006; Nunes, Bryant, Pretzlik, Bell, Evans and Wade,
(1999), for example, presented the following 2007; Mamede, Nunes and Bryant, 2005);
problem to children aged about six to sev en years • order fractional quantities (Kieren, 1993; Kornilaki
(first graders in the USA): 4 children got 3 pancak es and Nunes, 2005; Mamede, 2007).
to share; how many pancakes are needed for 12
children in order for the children to have the same These studies were carried out with children up to
amount of pancake as the first group? She repor ted the age of ten years and all of them produced
that 3 children solved this problem by par titioning positive results. This stands in clear contr ast with the
and 3 solved it by placing 3 pancakes in literature on children’s difficulties with fractions and
correspondence to each group of 4 children. Similar prompts the question whether the difficulties might
strategies were repor ted when children solved stem from the use of par titioning as the star ting
another problem that involved 2 candy bar s shared point for the teaching of fr actions (see also Lamon,
among 3 children. 1996; Streefland, 1987). The next section examines
the development of children’s par titioning action and
its connection with children’s concepts of fr actions.
18 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

Children’s use of the scheme level of success depending on the shape of the whole
of partitioning in making (circular areas are more difficult to par tition than
judgements about quantities rectangles) and on the number of par ts. A major
strategy in carrying out successful par titioning was the
The scheme of par titioning has been also named use of successive divisions in two: so children are able
subdivision and dissection (Pothier and Sawada, to succeed in dividing a whole into f ourths before
1983), and is consistently defined as the process they can succeed with thirds. Successive halving
of dividing a whole into par ts. This process is helped the children with some fr actions: dividing
understood not as the activity of cutting something something into eighths is easier this way. However, it
into par ts in any way, but as a process that m ust be interfered with success with other fr actions: some
guided from the outset by the aim of obtaining a children, attempting to divide a whole into f ifths,
pre-determined number of equal par ts. ended up with sixths by dividing the whole first in
halves and then subdividing each half in three parts.
Piaget, Inhelder and Szeminska (1960) pioneered the
study of the connection between par titioning and Piaget and colleagues also investigated children’s
fractions. They spelled out a number of ideas that understanding of their seventh criterion for a true
they thought were necessary for children to develop concept of fraction, i.e. the conser vation of the
an understanding of fractions, and analysed them in whole. This conservation, they argued, would require
partitioning tasks. The motivation for par titioning was the children to under stand that each piece could
sharing a cake between a number of recipients, but not be counted simply as one piece , but had to be
the task was one of par titioning. They suggested that understood in its relation to the whole. Some
‘the notion of fr action depends on two fundamental children aged six and even seven years failed to
relations: the relation of par t to whole (…) and the understand this, and argued that if someone ate a
relation of par t to par t’ (p. 309). Piaget and colleagues cake cut into 1/2 + 2/4 and a second per son ate a
identified a number of insights that children need to cake cut into 4/4, the second one would eat more
achieve in order to under stand fractions: because he had four par ts and the first one only had
three. Although these children would recognise that
1 the whole must be conceived as divisible, an idea if the pieces were put together in each case they
that children under the age of about tw o do not would form one whole cake, they still maintained
seem to attain that 4/4 was more than 1/2 + 2/4. Finally, Piaget and
colleagues also obser ved that children did not ha ve
2 the number of par ts to be achieved is determined to achieve the highest level of development in the
from the outset scheme of par titioning in order to under stand the
conservation of the whole.
3 the par ts must exhaust the whole (i.e . there
should be no second round of par titioning and no Children’s difficulties with par titioning continuous
remainders) wholes into equal par ts have been confirmed many
times in studies with pre-schooler s and children in
4 the number of cuts and the number of par ts are their first years in school (e .g. Hieber t and
related (e.g. if you want to divide something in 2 Tonnessen, 1978; Hunting and Shar pley, 1988 b,)
parts, you should use only 1 cut) observed that children often do not anticipate the
number of cuts and fail to cut the whole extensiv ely,
5 all the par ts should be equal leaving a par t of the whole un-cut. These studies also
extended our knowledge of how children’s exper tise
6 each par t can be seen as a whole in itself, nested in par titioning develops. For example, Pothier and
into the whole but also susceptible of fur ther Sawada (1983) and Lamon (1996) proposed more
division detailed schemes for the analysis of the development
of par titioning schemes and other researcher s
7 the whole remains invariant and is equal to the (Hiebert and Tonnessen, 1978; Hunting and Shar pley,
sum of the par ts. 1988 a and b; Miller, 1984; Novillis, 1976) found that
the difficulty of par titioning discrete and continuous
Piaget and colleagues observed that children rarely quantities is not the same , as hypothesized by Piaget.
achieved correct partitioning (sharing a cake) before Children can use a procedure f or par titioning
the age of about six y ears. There is variation in the discrete quantities that is not applicable to
19 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

continuous quantities: they can ‘deal out’ the discrete The children in Kamii’s study were considerably older
quantities but not the continuous ones. Thus they than those in the cor respondence studies: they were
perform significantly better with the f ormer than the in the fifth or sixth year in school (approximately 11
latter. This means that the tr ansition from discrete to and 12 years). Both groups of children had been
continuous quantities in the use of par titioning is not taught about equivalent fractions. In spite of having
difficult, in contrast to the smooth tr ansition noted in received instruction, the children’s rate of success
the case of the cor respondence scheme. was rather low: only 44% of the fifth graders and
51% of the sixth gr aders reasoned that they would
These studies showed that the scheme of eat the same amount of chocolate cak e because
partitioning continuous quantities develops slowly, these were halves of identical wholes.
over a longer per iod of time. The next question to
consider is whether par titioning can promote the Kamii and Clark then showed the children two
understanding of equivalence and order ing of identical wholes, cut one in f ourths using a hor izontal
fractions once the scheme has developed. and a vertical cut, and the other in eighths, using only
horizontal cuts. They discarded one fourth from the
Many studies investigated children’s understanding of first ‘chocolate cake’, leaving three fourths be eaten,
equivalence of fractions in par titioning contexts (e.g. and asked the children to tak e the same amount
Behr, Lesh, Post and Silver, 1983; Behr, Wachsmuth, from the other cake, which had been cut into
Post and Lesh, 1984; Larson, 1980; Kerslake, 1986), eighths, for themselves. The percentage of cor rect
but differences in the methods used in these studies answers was this time even lower: 13% of the fifth
render the comparisons between par titioning and graders and 32% of the sixth gr aders correctly
correspondence studies ambiguous. For example, if identified the number of eighths required to tak e the
the studies star t with a representation of the same amount as three f ourths.
fractions, rather than with a problem about
quantities, they cannot be compared to the studies Recently, we (Nunes and Br yant, 2004) included a
reviewed in the previous section, in which children similar question about halves in a sur vey of English
were asked to think about quantities without children’s knowledge of fractions. The children in our
necessarily using fractional representation. We shall study were in their fourth and fifth year (mean ages
not review all studies b ut only those that use eight and a half and nine and a half, respectively) in
comparable methods. school. The children were shown pictures of a boy
and a girl and two identical rectangular areas, the
Kamii and Clark (1995) presented children with ‘chocolate cakes’. The boy cut his cake along the
identical rectangles and cut them into fr actions using diagonal and the gir l cut hers horizontally. The
different cuts. For example, one rectangle was cut children were asked to indicate whether they ate the
horizontally in half and the second was cut across a same amount of cake and, if not, to mark the child
diagonal. The children had the oppor tunity to verify who ate more. Our results were more positive than
that the rectangles were the same size and that the Kamii and Clark’s: 55% of the children in y ear four
two parts from each rectangle were the same in size. (eight and a half year olds) in our study answ ered
They asked the children: if these were chocolate correctly. However, these results are weak by
cakes, and the researcher ate a par t cut from the comparison to children’s rate of correct responses
first rectangle and the child ate a par t cut from the when the problem draws on their under standing of
second, would they eat the same amount? This correspondences. In the Kor nilaki and Nunes study,
method is highly comparable to the studies by 100% of the seven-year-olds (third graders) realized
Kornilaki and Nunes (2005) and b y Mamede (2007), that two divisions that have the same dividend and
where the children do not ha ve to carry out the the same divisor result in equivalent shares. Our
actions, so their difficulty with par titioning does not results with fourth graders, when both the dividend
influence their judgements. They also use similar ly and the divisor were different, still shows a higher
motivated contexts, ending in the question of whether rate of correct responses when cor respondences are
recipients would eat the same amount. However, the used: 78% of the fourth graders gave correct answers
question posed by Kamii and Clar k draws on the when comparing one fourth and two eighths.
child’s understanding of par titioning and the relations
between the par ts of the two wholes because each In the preceding studies, the students had to think
whole corresponds to a single recipient. about the quantities ignoring their perceptual
appearance. Har t et al. (1985) and Nunes et al.
20 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

(2004) presented students with verbal questions, Finally, it is impor tant to compare students’ arguments
which did not contain drawings that could lead to for the equivalence and order of quantities
incorrect conclusions based on perception. In both represented by fractions in teaching studies where
studies, the children were told that two boys had partitioning is used as the basis for teaching. Many
identical chocolate bars; one cut his into 8 par ts and teaching studies that aim at promoting students’
ate 4 and the other cut his into 4 par ts and ate 2. understanding of fractions through par titioning have
Combining the results of these two studies, it is been reported in the literature (e.g., Behr, Wachsmuth,
possible to see how the rate of correct responses Post and Lesh, 1984; Brousseau, Brousseau and
changed across age: 40% at ages 8 to 9 y ears, 74% at Warfield, 2004; 2007; Empson, 1999; Kerslake, 1986;
10 to11 years, 60% at 11 to 12 y ears, and 64% at 12 Olive and Steffe, 2002; Olive and Vomvoridi, 2006;
to 13 years. The students aged 8 to 10 were Saenz-Ludlow, 1994; Steffe, 2002). In most of these
assessed by Nunes et al. and the older ones by Har t studies, students’ difficulties with par titioning are
et al. These results show modest progress on the circumvented either by using pre-divided materials
understanding of equivalence questions presented in (e.g. Behr, Wachsmuth, Post and Lesh, 1984) or by
the context of par titioning even though the using computer tools where the computer carries
quantities eaten were all equivalent to half. out the division as instructed by the student
(e.g. Olive and Steffe, 2002; Olive and Vomvoridi, 2006).
Mamede (2007) carried out a direct compar ison
between children’s use of the cor respondence and Many studies combine par titioning with
the par titioning scheme in solving equivalence and correspondence during instruction, either because
order problems with fractional quantities. In this well- the researchers do not use this distinction (e .g.
controlled study, she used stor y problems involving Saenz-Ludlow, 1994) or because they wish to
chocolates and children, similar pictures and construct instruction that combines both schemes
mathematically identical questions; the division in order to achieve a better instr uctional program
scheme relevant to the situation was the onl y (e.g. Brousseau, Brousseau and Warfield, 2004; 2007).
variable distinguishing the problems. In These studies will not be discussed here. Two studies
correspondence problems, for example, she asked that analysed student’s arguments focus the
the children: in one par ty, three girls are going to instruction on par titioning and are presented here .
share fairly one chocolate cake; in another par ty, six
boys are going to share fairly two chocolate cakes. The first study was car ried out by Berhr, Wachsmuth,
The children were asked to decide whether each Post and Lesh (1984). The researchers used objects
boy would eat more than each girl, each girl would of different types that could be manipulated dur ing
eat more than each boy, or whether they would instruction (e.g. counters, rectangles of the same siz e
have the same amount to eat. In the par titioning and in different colours, pre-divided into fractions
problems, she set the f ollowing scenario: This girl and such as halves, quar ters, thirds, eighths) but also
this boy have identical chocolate cakes; the cakes are taught the students how to use algor ithms (division
too big to eat at once so the gir l cuts her cake in of the denominator by the numerator to find a
3 identical par ts and eats one and the boy cuts his ratio) to check on the equivalence of fr actions. The
cake in 6 identical par ts and eats 2. The children students were in fourth grade (age about 9) and
were asked whether the girl and the boy ate the received instruction over 18 weeks. Behr et al.
same amount or whether one ate more than the provided a detailed analysis of children’s arguments
other. The children (age r ange six to seven) were regarding the ordering of fractions. In summary, they
Portuguese and in their first year in school; they had report the following insights after instr uction.
received no instruction about fractions.
• When ordering fractions with the same numerator
In the correspondence questions, the responses of and different denominators, students seem to be able
35% of the six-year-olds and 49% of the sev en-year- to argue that there is an inverse relation between the
olds were correct; in the par titioning questions, 10% number of parts into which the whole was cut and
of the answers of children in both age lev els were the size of the parts.This argument appears either
correct. These highly significant differences suggest with explicit reference to the numerator (‘there are
that the use of cor respondence reasoning suppor ts two pieces in each, but the pieces in two fifths are
children’s understanding of equivalence between smaller.’ p. 328) or without it (‘the bigger the number
fractions whereas par titioning did not seem to afford is, the smaller the pieces get.’ p. 328).
the same insights.
21 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• A third fraction can be used as a reference point However, one of the protocols (Oliv e and Steffe,
when two fractions are compared: three ninths is 2002) provides evidence for the student’s difficulty
less than three sixths because ‘three ninths is … with improper fractions, which, we hypothesise, could
less than half and three sixths is one half ’ (p. 328). be a consequence of using par titioning as the basis
It is not clear how the students had learned that for the concept of fr actions. The researcher asked Joe
3/6 and 1/2 are equivalent but they can use this to make a stick 6/5 long. Joe said that he could not
knowledge to solve another comparison. because ‘there are only five of them’. After prompting,
Joe physically adds one more fifth to the five already
• Students used the ratio algorithm to verify whether used, but it is not clear whether this ph ysical action
the fractions were equivalent: 3/5 is not equivalent convinces him that 6/5 is mathematicall y appropriate.
to 6/8 because ‘if they were equal, three goes into In a subsequent example , Joe labels a stick made with
six, but five doesn’t go into eight.’ (p. 331). 9 sticks, which had been defined as ‘one seventh’ of
an original stick, 9/7, but according to the researcher s
• Students learned to use the manipulative materials ‘an impor tant per turbation’ remains. Joe later counts
in order to carry out perceptual comparisons: 6/8 8 of a stick that had been designated as ‘one seventh’
equals 3/4 because ‘I started with four parts. Then I but doesn’t use the label ‘eight sevenths’. When the
didn’t have to change the size of the paper at all. I researcher proposes this label, he questions it: ‘How
just folded it, and then I got eight.’ (p. 331). can it be EIGHT sevenths?’ (Olive and Steffe, 2002,
p. 426). Joe later refused to mak e a stick that is 10/7,
Behr et al. report that, after 18 weeks of instruction, even though the procedure is ph ysically possible.
a large propor tion of the students (27%) contin ued Subsequently, on another day, Joe’s reaction to
to use the manipulatives in order to car ry our another improper fraction is: ‘I still don’t under stand
perceptual comparisons; the same propor tion (27%) how you could do it. How can a fraction be bigger
used a third fr action as a reference point and a than itself? (Olive and Steffe, 2002, p. 428; emphasis
similar propor tion (23%) used the r atio algorithm in the original).
that they had been taught to compare fr actions.
According to the researcher s, Joe only sees that
Finally, there is no evidence that the students w ere improper fractions are acceptable when they
able to understand that the number of par ts and size presented a problem where pizzas were to be shared
of par ts could compensate for each other precisely by people. When 12 friends ordered 2 slices each of
in a propor tional manner. For example, in the pizzas cut into 8 slices, Joe realized immediately that
comparison between 6/8 and 3/4 the students could more than one pizza would be required; the traditional
have argued that there were twice as many par ts partitioning situation, where one whole is divided into
when the whole was cut into 8 par ts in comparison equal parts, was transformed into a less usual one ,
with cutting in 4 par ts, so you need to take twice as where two wholes are required but the size of the
many (6) in order to ha ve the same amount. part remains fixed.

In conclusion, students seemed to develop some This example illustrates the difficulty that students
insight into the inverse relation between the divisor have with improper fr actions in the context of
and the quantity but this only helped them when the partitioning but which they can overcome by
dividend was kept constant: they could not extend thinking of more than one whole .
this understanding to other situations where the
numerator and the denominator differed.
Conclusion
The second set of studies that f ocused on
partitioning was carried out by Steffe and his Partitioning, defined as the action of cutting a
colleagues (Olive and Steffe, 2002; Olive and whole into a pre-deter mined number of equal par ts,
Vomvoridi, 2006; Steffe, 2002). Because the aim of shows a slower developmental process than
much of the instr uction was to help the children correspondence. In order for children to succeed,
learn to label fr actions or compose fr actions that they need to anticipate the solution so that the r ight
would be appropriate for the label, it is not possible number of cuts produces the right number of equal
to extract from their repor ts the children’s parts and exhausts the whole . Its accomplishment,
arguments for equivalence of fr actions. however, does not seem to produce immediate
insights into equivalence and order of fr actional
22 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

quantities. Apparently, many children do not see it as hallmarks of adolescent thinking and f ormal
necessary that halves from two identical wholes are operations. They devoted a book to the anal ysis of
equivalent, even if they have been taught about the children’s understanding of probabilities (Piaget and
equivalence of fractions in school. Inhelder, 1975) and descr ibed in great detail the
steps that children take in order to understand the
In order to use this scheme of action as the basis quantification of probabilities. In the most
for learning about fractions, teaching schemes and comprehensive of their studies, the children were
researchers rely on pre-cut wholes or computer shown pairs of decks of cards with diff erent numbers
tools to avoid the difficulties of accurate par titioning. of cards, some marked with a cross and other s
Students can develop insight into the inverse relation unmarked. The children were asked to judge which
between the number of par ts and the size of the deck they would choose to dr aw from if they
parts through the par titioning scheme but there is wanted to have a better chance of drawing a
no evidence that they realize that if you cut a whole card marked with a cross.
in twice as many par ts each one will be half in siz e.
Finally, improper fractions seem to cause uneasiness Piaget and Inhelder obser ved that many of the
to students who have developed their conception of young children treated the n umber of marked and
fractions in the context of par titioning; it is impor tant unmarked cards as if they w ere independent:
to be aware of this uneasiness if this is the scheme sometimes they chose one deck because it had
chosen in order to teach fr actions. more marked cards than the other and sometimes
they chose a deck because it had f ewer blank cards
than the other. This approach can lead to cor rect
responses when either the n umber of marked cards
Rational numbers and children’s or the number of unmarked cards is the same in
understanding of intensive both decks, and children aged about seven years
quantities were able to make correct choices in such problems.
In the introduction, we suggested that r ational This is rather similar to the obser vations of children’s
numbers are necessar y to represent quantities that successes and difficulties in comparing fractions
are measured by a relation between two other reported earlier on: they can reach the cor rect
quantities. These are called intensive quantities and answer when the denominator is constant or when
there are many examples of such quantities both the numerator is constant, as this allow them to
in everyday life and in science . In everyday life, we focus on the other value . When they must think of
often mix liquids to obtain a cer tain taste. If you different denominators and numerators, the
mix fruit concentrate with water to mak e juice, the questions become more difficult.
concentration of this mixture is descr ibed by a
rational number: for example, 1/3 concentrate and Around the age of nine , children star ted making
2/3 water. Probability is an intensive quantity that is correspondences between marked and unmarked
important both in mathematics and science and is cards within each deck and w ere able to identify
measured as the number of favourable cases divided equivalences using this type of procedure . For
by the number of total cases. 7 example, if asked to compare a deck with one
marked and two unmarked cards (1/3 probability)
The conceptual difficulties involved in understanding with another deck with two marked and four
intensive quantities are lar gely similar to those unmarked cards (2/6 probability), the children would
involved in understanding the representation of re-organise the second deck in tw o lots, setting one
quantities that are smaller than the whole. In marked card in cor respondence with two unmarked,
order to understand intensive quantities, students and conclude that it did not mak e any difference
must form a concept that takes two variables which deck they picked a card from. Piaget and
simultaneously into account and realise that there is Inhelder saw these as empir ical propor tional
an inverse relation between the denominator and solutions, which were a step towards the abstraction
the quantity represented. that characterises propor tional reasoning.

Piaget and Inhelder descr ibed children’s thinking Noelting (1980 a and b) replicated these results with
about intensive quantities as one of the man y another intensive quantity, the taste of or ange juice
examples of the development of the scheme of made from a mixture of concentr ate and water. In
proportionality, which they saw as one of the broad terms, he described children’s thinking and its
23 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

development in the same way as Piaget and Inhelder and Carmi, 1982; Stavy and Tirosh, 2000), but there is
had done. This is an impor tant replication of Piaget’s little doubt that students’ difficulties in understanding
results results considering that the content of the intensive quantities are very similar to those that
problems differed marked across the studies, they have when thinking about fr actions which
probability and concentration of juice. represent quantities smaller than the unit. They treat
the values independently, they find it difficult to think
Nunes, Desli and Bell (2003) compared students’ about inverse relations, and they might think of the
ability to solve problems about extensive and relations between the numbers as additive instead of
intensive quantities that involved the same type of multiplicative.
reasoning. Extensive quantities can be represented
by a single whole n umber (e.g. 5 kilos, 7 cows, 4 There is presently little information to indicate
days) whereas intensive quantities are represented whether students can tr ansfer what they have
by a ratio between two numbers. In spite of these learned about fractions in the context of
differences, it is possible to create problems which representing quantities smaller than the unit to the
are comparable in other aspects b ut differ with representation and understanding of intensive
respect to whether the quantities are extensiv e or quantities. Brousseau, Brousseau, and Warfield (2004)
intensive. Intensive quantities problems always suggest both that teacher s believe that students will
involve three variables. For example, three variables easily go from one use of fr actions to another, and
might be amount of or ange concentrate, amount of that nonetheless the differences between these two
water, and the taste of the or ange juice, which is types of situation could actuall y result in interference
the intensive quantity. The amount of or ange rather than in easy tr ansfer of insights across
concentrate is directly related to how orangey the situations. In contrast, Lachance and Confrey (2002)
juice tastes whereas the amount of water is developed a curriculum for teaching third gr ade
inversely related to how orangey the juice tastes. students (estimated age about 8 y ears) about ratios
A comparable extensive quantities problem would in a variety of problems, including intensive quantities
involve three extensive quantities, with the one problems, and then taught the same students in
under scrutiny being inversely propor tional to one fourth grade (estimated age about 9 y ears) about
of the variables and directly propor tional to the decimals. Their hypothesis is that students would
other. For example, the number of days that the show positive transfer from learning about ratios to
food bought by a farmer lasts is directly learning about decimals. They claimed that their
proportional to the amount of f ood purchased and students learned significantly more about decimals
inversely propor tional to the number of animals than students who had not par ticipated in a similar
she has to feed. In our study, we analysed students’ curriculum and whose performance in the same
performance in comparison problems where they questions had been descr ibed in other studies.
had to consider either intensiv e quantities (e.g. how
orangey a juice would taste) or extensive quantities We believe that it is not possible at the moment to
(e.g. the number of days the farmer’s food supply form clear conclusions on whether kno wledge of
would last). Students performed significantly better fractions developed in one type of situation tr ansfers
in the extensive quantities problems even though easily to the other, shows no transfer, or actually
both types of problem involved propor tional interferes with learning about the other type of
reasoning and the same n umber of variables. So, situation. In order to settle this issue, we must
although the difficulties shown by children across carry out the appropr iate teaching studies and
the two types of problem are similar, their level of comparisons.
success was higher with extensiv e than intensive
quantities. This indicates that students f ind it difficult However, there is good reason to conclude that the
to form a concept where tw o variables must be use of rational numbers to represent intensive
coordinated into a single constr uct,8 and therefore quantities should be explicitly included in the
it may be impor tant for schools and teacher s to curriculum. This is an impor tant concept in everyday
consider how they might promote this life and science, and causes difficulties for students.
development in the classroom.

We shall not review the lar ge literature on intensive


quantities here (see, for example, Erickson, 1979;
Kaput, 1985; Schwar tz, 1988; Stavy, Strauss, Orpaz
24 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

Learning to use mathematical performance. Har t et al. (1985) presented students


procedures to determine the with the task of identifying the missing values in
equivalence and order equivalent fractions. The children were presented
with the item below and asked which numbers
of rational numbers should replace the square and the tr iangle:
Piaget’s (1952) research on children’s understanding
of natural numbers shows that young children, aged 2/7 = /14 = 10/
about four, might be able to count two sets of
objects, establish that they have the same number, The rate of correct responses by 11 to 12 and 12- to
and still not conclude that they are equivalent if the 13-year-olds for the second question was about half
sets are displayed in very different perceptual that for the first one: about 56% for the first question
arrangements. Conversely, they might establish the and 24% for the second. The within-fraction method
equivalence between two sets by placing their cannot be easily applied in these cases b ut the factors
elements in correspondence and, after counting the are 2 and 5, and these multiplication tables should be
elements in one set, be unable to infer what the quite easy for students at this age lev el.
number in the other set is (Piaget, 1952; Frydman
and Bryant, 1988). As we noted in Paper 2, We recently replicated these different levels of
Understanding whole numbers, counting is a difficulty in a study with 8- to 10-y ear-olds. The
procedure for creating equivalent sets and placing easiest questions were those where the common
sets in order but many young children who know factor was 2; the rates of correct responses for
how to count do not use counting when ask ed to 1/3 = 2/  and 6/8 = 3/  were 52% and 45%,
compare or create equivalent sets (see, for example, respectively. The most difficult question was 4/12
Michie, 1984, Cowan and Daniels, 1989; Cowan, = 1/ this was only answered correctly by 16%
1987; Cowan, Foster and Al-Zubaidi, 1993; Saxe, of the students. It is unlikely that the difficulty of
Guberman and Gearhar t, 1987). computation could explain the differences in
performance: even weak students in this age r ange
Procedures to establish the equivalence and order should be able to identify 3 as the factor connecting
of fractional quantities are much more complex than 4 and 12, if they had been taught the within-fr action
counting, par ticularly when both the denominator method, or 4 as the factor connecting 1 and 4, if
and the numerator differ. Students are taught they were taught the between-fractions method.
different procedures in different countries. The
procedure that seems most commonl y taught in A noteworthy aspect of our results was the lo w
England is to check the equivalence b y analysing correlations between the different items: although
the multiplicative relation between or within the most were significant (due to the lar ge sample size;
fractions. For example, when comparing 1/3 with N = 188), only two of the nine cor relations were
4/12, students are taught to f ind the factor that above .4. This suggests that the students w ere not
connects the numerators (1 and 4) and then apply able to use the procedure that they learned
the same factor to the denominators. If the consistently to solve five items that had the same
numerator and the denominator of the second format and could be solved by the same procedure .
fraction are the product of the n umerator and the
denominator of the first fraction by the same Our assessment, like the one by Har t et al. (1985),
number, 4 in this case , they are equivalent. An also included an equivalence question set in the
alternative approach is to find whether the context of a story: two boys have identical chocolate
multiplicative relation between the numerator bars, one cuts his into 8 equal par ts and eats 4 and
and the denominator of each fraction is the same the other cuts his into 4 equal par ts and eats 2; the
(3 in this case): if it is, the fractions are equivalent. children are asked to indicate whether the bo ys eat
the same amount of chocolate and, if not, who eats
If students learned this procedure and applied it more. This item is usually seen as assessing children’s
consistently, it should not matter whether the factor understanding of quantities as it is not expressed in
is, for example, 2, 3 or 5, because these are well- fraction terms. In our sample , no student wrote the
known multiplication associations. It should also not fractions 4/8 and 2/4 and compared them by means
matter whether the fr action with larger numerator of a procedure. We analysed the correlations
and denominator is the first or the second. However, between this item and the f ive items described in
research shows that these var iations affect students’ the previous paragraph. If the students used the
25 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

same reasoning or the same procedure to solv e the discrepancy between the two. Thus, some students
items, there should be a high cor relation between seem to learn procedures for finding equivalent
them. This was not so: the highest of the cor relations fractions without an understanding of why the
between this item and each of the f ive previous ones procedures work, others base their approach to
was 0.32, which is low. This result exemplifies the fractions on their under standing of quantities without
separation between understanding fractional mastering the relevant procedures, and yet others
quantities and knowledge of procedures in the seem able to co-ordinate the two forms of
domain of rational numbers. This is much the same knowledge. Our results show that the third group is
as observed in the domain of natur al numbers. more successful not only in a test about fr actions but
also in a test about intensiv e quantities, which did
It is possible that understanding the relations between not require the use of fractions in the representation
quantities gives students an advantage in lear ning the of the quantities.
procedures to establish the equivalence of fractions,
but it may not guarantee that they will actually learn it Finally, we ask whether students are better at using
if teachers do connect their under standing with the procedures to compare decimals than to compare
procedure. When we separated the students into two ordinary fractions. The students in some of the gr ade
groups, one that answered the question about the levels studied by Resnick and colleagues (1989)
boys and the chocolates correctly and the other that would have been taught how to add and subtract
did not, there was a highly significant difference decimals: they were in grades 5 and 6 (the estimated
between the two groups in the r ate of correct age for U.S. students is about 10 and 11 years) and
responses in the procedural items: the group who one of the ear ly uses of decimals in the cur riculum
succeeded in the chocolate question showed 38% in the three par ticipating countries is addition and
correct responses to the procedur al items whereas subtraction of decimals. When students are taught to
the group who failed only answered 18% of the align the decimal numbers by placing the decimal
procedural questions correctly. points one under the other before adding – for
example, when adding 0.8, 0.26 and 0.361 you need
A combination of longitudinal and inter vention to align the decimal points bef ore carrying out the
studies is required to clar ify whether students who addition – they may not realise that they are using a
understand fractional quantities benefit more when procedure that automatically converts the values to
taught how to represent and compare fr actions. the same denominator : in this case, x/1000. It is
There are presently no studies to clarify this matter. possible that students may use this procedure of
aligning the decimal point without full y understanding
Research that analyses students’ knowledge of that this is a conversion to the same denominator
procedures used to find equivalent fractions and its and thus that it should help them to compare the
connection with conceptual knowledge of fractions value of the fr actions: after learning how to add and
has shown that there can be discrepancies betw een subtract with decimals, they may still think that 0.8 is
these two forms of knowledge. Rittle-Johnson, Siegler less than 0.36 but probably would not have said that
and Alibali (2001) argued that procedural and 0.80 is less than 0.36.
conceptual knowledge develop in tandem but
Kerslake (1986) and Byr nes and colleagues (Byr nes, To conclude, we find in the domain of r ational
1992; Byrnes and Wasik, 1991), among others, numbers a similar separ ation between
identified clear discrepancies between students’ understanding quantities and lear ning to operate
conceptual and procedural knowledge of fractions. with representations when judging the equivalence
Recently we (Hallett, Nunes and Bryant, 2007) and order of magnitude of quantities. Students are
analysed a large data set (N = 318 children in Years taught procedures to test whether fr actions are
4 and 5) and obser ved different profiles of relative equivalent but their knowledge of these procedures
performance in items that assess kno wledge of is limited, and they do not appl y it across items
procedures to compare fr actions and understanding consistently. Similarly, students who solve
of fractional quantities. Some children show greater equivalence problems in context are not necessar ily
success in procedural questions than would be experts in solving problems when the fr actions are
expected from their performance in conceptual presented without context.
items, others show better performance in conceptual
items than expected from their perf ormance in the The significance of children’s difficulties in
procedural items, and still other s do not show any understanding equivalence of fr actions cannot be
26 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

overstressed: in the domain of r ational numbers, different way: if a whole is cut into twice as many
students cannot learn to add and subtr act with parts, the size of each part will be halved. This
understanding if they do not realise that fr actions involves thinking about an inverse relation between
must be equivalent in order to be added. Adding 1/3 the quantities in the problem. Research consistently
and 2/5 without tr ansforming one of these into an shows that children understand direct relations
equivalent fraction with the same denominator as better than inverse relations.
the other is like adding bananas and tins of soup: it
makes no sense. Above and beyond the fact that • There are no systematic and controlled
one cannot be said to understand numbers without comparisons to allow for unambiguous conclusions
understanding their equivalence and order, in the about the outcomes of instruction based on
domain of rational numbers equivalence is a core correspondences or partitioning. The available
concept for computing addition and subtr action. evidence suggests that testing this hypothesis
Kerslake (1986) has shown that students lear n to appropriately could result in more successful
implement the procedures for adding and teaching and learning of rational numbers.
subtracting fractions without having a glimpse at
why they convert the fractions into common • Children’s understanding of quantities is often
denominators first. This separation between the ahead of their knowledge of fractional
meaning of fractions and the procedures cannot representations when they solve problems using
bode well for the future of these lear ners. the correspondence scheme. Schools could make
use of children’s informal knowledge of fractional
quantities and work with problems about
situations, without requiring them to use formal
Conclusions and educational representations, to help them consolidate this
implications reasoning and prepare them for formalization.
• Rational numbers are essential for the
representation of quantities that cannot be • Research has identified the arguments that children
represented by a single natural number. For this use when comparing fractions and trying to see
reason, they are needed in everyday life as well as whether they are equivalent or to order them by
science, and should be part of the curriculum in the magnitude. It would be important to investigate
age range 5 to 16. next whether increasing teachers’ awareness of
children’s own arguments would help teachers
• Children learn mathematical concepts by applying guide children’s learning more effectively.
schemes of action to problem solving and reflecting
about them. Two types of action schemes are • In some countries, greater attention is given to
available in division situations: partitioning, which decimal representation than to ordinary fractions
involves dividing a whole into equal parts, and in primary school whereas in others ordinary
correspondence situations, where two quantities fractions continue to play an important role. The
(or measures) are involved, a quantity to be shared argument that decimals are easier to understand
and a number of recipients of the shares. than ordinary fractions does not find support in
surveys of students’ performance: students find it
• Children as young as five or six years in age are difficult to make judgements of equivalence and
quite good at establishing correspondences to order both with decimals and with ordinary
produce equal shares, whereas they experience fractions.
much difficulty in partitioning continuous quantities.
Reflecting about these schemes and drawing insights • Some researchers (e.g. Nunes, 1997; Tall, 1992;
from them places children in different paths for Vergnaud, 1997) argue that different
understanding rational number. When they use the representations shed light onto the same concepts
correspondence scheme, they can achieve some from different perspectives. This would suggest
insight into the equivalence of fractions by thinking that a way to strengthen students’ learning of
that, if there are twice as many things to be shared rational numbers is to help them connect both
and twice as many recipients, then each one’s share representations. Moss and Case (1999) analysed
is the same. This involves thinking about a direct this possibility in the context of a curriculum based
relation between the quantities. The partitioning on measurements, where ordinary fractions and
scheme leads to understanding equivalence in a percentages were used to represent the same
27 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

information. Their results are encouraging, but the Endnotes


study does not include the appropriate controls
that would allow for establishing firmer conclusions. 1 Rational numbers can also be used to represent relations that
cannot be described by a single whole n umber but the
• Students can learn procedures for comparing, representation of relations will not be discussed here .
adding and subtracting fractions without connecting 2 The authors repor t a successful progr amme of instruction
these procedures with their understanding of where they taught the students to estab lish connections
between their understanding of ratios and decimals. The
equivalence and order of fractional quantities, students had received two years of instruction on ratios. A full
independently of whether they are taught with discussion of this very interesting work is not possible here as
ordinary or decimal representation. This is not a the information provided in the paper is insufficient.
desired outcome of instruction, but seems to be a 3 There are different hypotheses regarding what types of
quite common one. Research that focuses on the subconstructs or meanings for rational numbers should be
use of children’s informal knowledge suggests that it distinguished (see, for example, Behr, Harel, Post and Lesh,
1992; Kieren, 1988) and how many distinctions are justifiable.
is possible to help students make connections (e.g.
Mathematicians and psychologists may well use different
Mack, 1990), but the evidence is limited. There is criteria and consequently reach different conclusions.
now considerably more information regarding Mathematicians might be looking for conceptual issues in
children’s informal strategies to allow for new mathematics and psychologists for distinctions that have an
teaching programmes to be designed and assessed. impact on children’s learning (i.e. show different levels of
difficulty or no tr ansfer of learning across situations). We have
decided not to pursue this in detail b ut will consider this
• Finally, this review opens the way for a fresh
question in the final section of the paper.
research agenda in the teaching and learning of
4 Steffe and his colleagues have used a different type of problem,
fractions. The source for the new research where the size of the par t is fixed and the children have to
questions is the finding that children achieve insights identify how many times it fits into the whole .
into relations between fractional quantities before 5 This classification should not be confused with the classif ication
knowing how to represent them. It is possible to of division problems in the mathematics education liter ature.
envisage a research agenda that would not focus Fischbein, Deri, Nello and Mar ino (1985) define par titive
on children’s misconceptions about fractions, but division (which they also term sharing division) as a model f or
situations in which ‘an object or collection of objects is divided
on children’s possibilities of success when teaching
into a number of equal fr agments or sub-collections. The
starts from thinking about quantities rather than dividend must be larger than the divisor ; the divisor (operator)
from learning fractional representations. must be a whole n umber; the quotient must be smaller than
the dividend (operand)… In quotative division or measurement
division, one seeks to deter mine how many times a given
quantity is contained in a lar ger quantity. In this case , the only
constraint is that the dividend m ust be larger than the divisor. If
the quotient is a whole n umber, the model can be seen as
repeated subtraction.’ (Fischbein, Deri, Nello and Mar ino, 1985,
p.7). In both types of prob lems discussed by Fishbein et al., the
scheme used in division is the same , par titioning, and the
situations are of the same type, par t-whole.
6 Empson, Junk, Dominguez and Turner (2005) have stressed that
‘the depiction of equal shares of, for example, sevenths in a
part–whole representation is not a necessar y step to
understanding the fraction 1/7 (for contrasting views, see
Charles and Nason, 2000; Lamon, 1996; Pothier and Sawada,
1983). What is necessar y, however, is understanding that 1/7 is
the amount one gets when 1 is divided into 7 same-siz ed par ts.’
7 Not all intensive quantities are represented b y fractions;
speed, for example, is represented by a ratio, such as in 70
miles per hour.
8 Vergnaud (1983) proposed this hypothesis in his compar ison
between isomorphism of measures and product of measures
problems. This issue is discussed in greater detail in another
paper 4 of this review.
28 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

References

Behr, M., Harel, G., Post, T., and Lesh, R. (1992). Carpenter, T. P., and Moser, J. M. (1982). The
Rational number, ratio, propor tion. In D. A. development of addition and subtraction
Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of Research on problem solving. In T. P. Carpenter, J. M. Moser and
Mathematics Teaching and Learning (pp. 296-333). T. A. Romberg (Eds.), Addition and subtraction: A
New York: Macmillan. cognitive perspective (pp. 10-24). Hillsdale (NJ):
Behr, M. J., Harrel, G., Post, R., & Lesh, R. (1993). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Toward a unified discipline of scientific enquiry. In Charles, K. and Nason, R. (2000). Young children’s
T. Carpenter, P. E. Fennema & T. A. Romberg (Eds.), partitioning strategies. Educational Studies in
Rational Numbers: an integration of research (pp. Mathematics, 43, 191–221.
13-48). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Ass. Cooney, T. J., Grouws, D. A., and Jones, D. (1988). An
Behr, M., Lesh, R., Post, T., and Silver, E. A. (1983). agenda for research on teaching mathematics. In
Rational number concepts. In R. Lesh and M. D. A. Grouws and T. J. Cooney (Eds.), Effective
Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of Mathematical mathematics teaching (pp. 253-261). Reston, VA:
Concepts and Processes (pp. 91-126). New York: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics and
Academic Press. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Behr, M. J., Wachsmuth, I., Post, T. R., and Lesh, R. (1984). Correa, J. (1994). Young children’s understanding of the
Order and equivalence of r ational numbers: A division concept. University of Oxford, Oxford.
clinical teaching experiment. Journal for Research in Correa, J., Nunes, T., and Br yant, P. (1998). Young
Mathematics Education, 15(5), 323-341. children’s understanding of division: The
Brousseau, G., Brousseau, N., and Warfield, V. (2004). relationship between division terms in a
Rationals and decimals as required in the school noncomputational task. Journal of Educational
curriculum Part 1: Rationals as measurement. Psychology, 90, 321-329.
Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 23, 1–20. Cowan, R., and Daniels, H. (1989). Children’s use of
Brousseau, G., Brousseau, N., and Warfield, V. counting and guidelines in judging relative number.
(2007). Rationals and decimals as required in British Journal of Educational Psychology, 59, 200-210.
the school curriculum Part 2: From rationals Cowan, R. (1987). When do children trust counting
to decimals. Journal of Mathematical Behavior as a basis for relative number judgements? Journal
26 281–300. of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 328-345.
Booth, L. R. (1981). Child-methods in secondary Cowan, R., Foster, C. M., and Al-Zubaidi, A. S. (1993).
mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics , Encouraging children to count. British Journal of
12 29-41. Developmental Psychology, 11, 411-420.
Byrnes, J. P. (1992). The conceptual basis of Davis, G. E., and Hunting, R. P. (1990). Spontaneous
procedural learning. Cognitive Development, 7, partitioning: Preschoolers and discrete items.
235-237. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 21, 367-374.
Byrnes, J. P. and Wasik, B. A. (1991). Role of Davis, G. E., and Pepper, K. L. (1992). Mathematical
conceptual knowledge in mathematical problem solving by pre-school children.
procedural learning. Developmental Psychology, Educational Studies in Mathematics, 23, 397-415.
27(5), 777-786. Davis, G. E., and Pitkethly, A. (1990). Cognitive aspects
of sharing. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 21, 145-153.
29 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Empson, S. B. (1999). Equal sharing and shared Hunting, R. P. and Sharpley, C. F. (1988 b). Fractional
meaning: The development of fraction concepts in knowledge in preschool children. Journal for
a First-Grade classroom. Cognition and Instruction, Research in Mathematics Education, 19(2), 175-180.
17(3), 283-342. Kamii, C., and Clark, F. B. (1995). Equivalent Fractions:
Empson, S. B., Junk, D., Dominguez, H., and Turner, E. Their Difficulty and Educational Implications.
(2005). Fractions as the coordination of Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 14, 365-378.
multiplicatively related quantities: a cross-sectional Kaput, J. (1985). Multiplicative word problems and
study of children’s thinking. Educational Studies in intensive quantities: An integrated software response
Mathematics, 63, 1-28. (Technical Report 85-19). Cambridge (MA):
Erickson, G. (1979). Children’s conceptions of heat Harvard University, Educational Technology Center.
and temperature. Science Education, 63, 221-230. Kerslake, D. (1986). Fractions: Children’s Strategies and
Fennell, F. S., Faulkner, L. R., Ma, L., Schmid, W., Stotsky, Errors: A Report of the Strategies and Errors in
S., Wu, H.-H., et al. (2008). Report of the Task Secondary Mathematics Project. Windsor: NFER-
Group on Conceptual Knowledge and Skills Nelson.
Washington DC: U.S. Depar tment of Education, Kieren, T. (1988). Personal knowledge of rational
The Mathematics Advisory Panel. numbers: Its intuitive and formal development. In
Fischbein, E., Deri, M., Nello, M., and Marino, M. (1985). J. Hiebert and M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts
The role of implicit models in solving v erbal and operations in the middle-grades (pp. 53-92).
problems in multiplication and division. Journal for Reston (VA): National Council of Teachers of
Research in Mathematics Education, 16, 3-17. Mathematics.
Frydman, O., and Br yant, P. E. (1988). Sharing and the Kieren, T. E. (1993). Rational and Fr actional Numbers:
understanding of number equivalence by young From Quotient Fields to Recursive
children. Cognitive Development, 3, 323-339. Understanding. In T. Carpenter, E. Fennema and T.
Frydman, O., and Br yant, P. (1994). Children’s A. Romberg (Eds.), Rational Numbers: An
understanding of multiplicative relationships in the Integration of Research (pp. 49-84). Hillsdale, N.J.:
construction of quantitative equivalence. Journal of Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 489-509. Kieren, T. (1994). Multiple Views of Multiplicative
Ginsburg, H. (1977). Children’s Arithmetic: The Learning Structures. In G. Harel and J. Confrey (Eds.), The
Process. New York: Van Nostrand. Development of Multiplicative Reasoning in the
Gréco, P. (1962). Quantité et quotité: nouvelles Learning of Mathematics (pp. 389-400). Albany,
recherches sur la cor respondance terme-a-terme New York: State University of New York Press.
et la conser vation des ensembles. In P. Gréco and Kornilaki, K., and Nunes, T. (2005). Generalising
A. Morf (Eds.), Structures numeriques elementaires: Principles in spite of Procedur al Differences:
Etudes d’Epistemologie Genetique Vol 13 (pp. 35- Children’s Understanding of Division. Cognitive
52). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. Development, 20, 388-406.
Hart, K. (1986). The step to formalisation. Insititute of Kouba, V. L., Brown, C. A., Carpenter, T. P., Lindquist, M.
Education, University of London: Proceedings of M., Silver, E. A., and Swafford, J. (1988). Results of
the 10th International Conference of the the Four th NAEP Assessment of Mathematics:
Psychology of Mathematics Education, pp Number, operations, and word problems.
159-164. Arithmetic Teacher, 35 (4), 14-19.
Hart, K., Brown, M., Kerslake, D., Kuchermann, D., & Lachance, A., and Confrey, J. (2002). Helping students
Ruddock, G. (1985). Chelsea Diagnostic build a path of under standing from ratio and
Mathematics Tests. Fractions 1. Windsor (UK): proportion to decimal notation. Journal of
NFER-Nelson. Mathematical Behavior, 20 503–526.
Hallett, D., Nunes, T., and Br yant, P. (2007). Individual Lamon, S. J. (1996). The Development of Unitizing: Its
differences in conceptual and procedur al Role in Children’s Partitioning Strategies. Journal for
knowledge when learning fractions. under review. Research in Mathematics Education, 27, 170-193.
Hiebert, J., and Tonnessen, L. H. (1978). Development Larson, C. N. (1980). Locating proper fr actions on
of the fraction concept in two physical contexts: number lines: Effect of length and equivalence .
An exploratory investigation. Journal for Research School Science and Mathematics , 53, 423-428.
in Mathematics Education, 9(5), 374-378. Mack, N. K. (1990). Learning fractions with
Hunting, R. P. and Sharpley, C. F. (1988 a). understanding: Building on informal knowledge.
Preschoolers’ cognition of fr actional units. British Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 21,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 58, 172-183. 16-32.
30 PaperSUMMARY
3: Understanding
– PAPERrational
2: Understanding
numbers and
whole
intensive
numbers
quantities

Mamede, E., Nunes, T., & Br yant, P. (2005). The Nunes, T., Br yant, P., Pretzlik, U., Bell, D., Evans, D., &
equivalence and ordering of fractions in part-whole Wade, J. (2007). La compréhension des fr actions
and quotient situations. Paper presented at the chez les enfants. In M. Merri (Ed.), Activité
29th PME Conference, Melbourne; Volume 3, humaine et conceptualisation. Questions à Gérard
281-288. Vergnaud (pp. 255-262). Toulouse, France:
Mamede, E. P. B. d. C. (2007). The effects of Presses Universitaires du Mirail.
situations on children’s understanding of fractions. Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Pretzlik, U., Evans, D., Wade, J., and
Unpublished PhD Thesis, Oxford Brookes Bell, D. (2008). Children’s understanding of the
University, Oxford. equivalence of fractions. An analysis of arguments
Michie, S. (1984). Why preschoolers are reluctant in quotient situations. submitted.
to count spontaneously. British Journal of Nunes, T., Desli, D., and Bell, D. (2003). The
Developmental Psychology, 2, 347-358. development of children’s understanding of
Miller, K. (1984). Measurement procedures and the intensive quantities. International Journal of
development of quantitative concepts. In C . Educational Research, 39, 652-675.
Sophian (Ed.), Origins of cognitive skills. The Piaget, J. (1952). The Child’s Conception of Number.
Eighteen Annual Carnegie Symposium on London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Cognition (pp. 193-228). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Piaget, J., and Inhelder, B. (1975). The Origin of the Idea
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. of Chance in Children. London: Routledge and
Moss, J., and Case, R. (1999). Developing children’s Kegan Paul.
understanding of rational numbers: A new model Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., and Szeminska, A. (1960). The
and an experimental curriculum. Journal for Child’s Conception of Geometr y. New York: Harper
Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 122-147. and Row.
Noelting, G. (1980 a). The development of Ohlsson, S. (1988). Mathematical meaning and
proportional reasoning and the r atio concept applicational meaning in the semantics of
Part I - Differentiation of stages. Educational fractions and related concepts. In J. Hieber t and
Studies in Mathematics, 11, 217-253. M. Behr (Eds.), Number Concepts and Operations
Noelting, G. (1980 b). The development of in the Middle Grades (pp. 53-92). Reston (VA):
proportional reasoning and the r atio concept National Council of Mathematics Teachers.
Part II - Problem-structure at sucessive stages: Olive, J., and Steffe, L. P. (2002). The construction of
Problem-solving strategies and the mechanism of an iterative fractional scheme: the case of Joe .
adaptative restructuring. Educational Studies in Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20 413-437.
Mathematics, 11, 331-363. Olive, J., and Vomvoridi, E. (2006). Making sense of
Novillis, C. F. (1976). An Analysis of the Fr action instruction on fractions when a student lacks
Concept into a Hier archy of Selected necessary fractional schemes: The case of Tim.
Subconcepts and the Testing of the Hier archical Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 25 18-45.
Dependencies. Journal for Research in Mathematics Piaget, J. (1952). The Child’s Conception of Number.
Education, 7, 131-144. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.
Nunes, T. (1997). Systems of signs and mathematical Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., and Szeminska, A. (1960). The
reasoning. In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.), Child’s Conception of Geometr y. New York: Harper
Learning and Teaching Mathematics. An and Row.
International Perspective (pp. 29-44). Hove (UK): Pitkethly, A., and Hunting, R. (1996). A Review of
Psychology Press. Recent Research in the Area of Initial Fraction
Nunes, T., and Br yant, P. (2004). Children’s insights and Concepts. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 30,
strategies in solving fractions problems. Paper 5-38.
presented at the Fir st International Conference of Pothier, Y., and Sawada, D. (1983). Partitioning: The
the Greek Association of Cognitive Psychology, Emergence of Rational Number Ideas in Young
Alexandropolis, Greece. Children. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Pretzlik, U., and Hurry, J. (2006). Education, 14, 307-317.
Fractions: difficult but crucial in mathematics Resnick, L. B., Nesher, P., Leonard, F., Magone, M.,
learning. London Institute of Education, London: Omanson, S., and Peled, I. (1989). Conceptual
ESRC-Teaching and Learning Research bases of arithmetic errors: The case of decimal
Programme. fractions. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education, 20, 8-27.
31 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Riley, M., Greeno, J. G., and Heller, J. I. (1983). Streefland, L. (1993). Fractions: A Realistic Approach.
Development of children’s problem solving ability In T. P. Carpenter, E. Fennema and T. A. Romberg
in arithmetic. In H. Ginsburg (Ed.), The (Eds.), Rational Numbers: An Integration of Research
development of mathematical thinking (pp. 153- (pp. 289-326). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
196). New York: Academic Press. Associates.
Rittle-Johnson, B., Siegler, R. S., and Alibali, M. W. Streefland, L. (1997). Charming fractions or fractions
(2001). Developing Conceptual Understanding being charmed? In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.),
and Procedural Skill in Mathematics: An Iterative Learning and Teaching Mathematics. An
Process. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93(2), International Perspective (pp. 347-372). Hove (UK):
46-36. Psychology Press.
Saenz-Ludlow, A. (1994). Michael’s Fraction Tall, D. (1992). The transition to advanced
Schemes. Journal for Research in Mathematics mathematical thinking: Functions, limits, infinity, and
Education, 25, 50-85. proof. In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of
Saxe, G., Guberman, S. R., and Gearhar t, M. (1987). Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning
Social and developmental processes in children’s (pp. 495-511). New York: Macmillan.
understanding of number. Monographs of the Society Tzur, R. (1999). An Integrated Study of Children’s
for Research in Child Development, 52, 100-200. Construction of Improper Fractions and the
Schwartz, J. (1988). Intensive quantity and referent Teacher’s Role in Promoting That Learning. Journal
transforming arithmetic operations. In J. Hiebert for Research in Mathematics Education, 30, 390-416.
and M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and Vamvakoussi, X., and Vosniadou, S. (2004).
operations in the middle grades (pp. 41-52). Understanding the structure of the set of rational
Hillsdale,NJ: Erlbaum. numbers: a conceptual change approach. Learning
Stafylidou, S., and Vosniadou, S. (2004). The and Instruction, 14 453-467.
development of students’ understanding of the Vergnaud, G. (1997). The nature of mathematical
numerical value of fractions. Learning and concepts. In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning
Instruction 14, 503-518. and Teaching Mathematics. An International
Stavy, R., Strauss, S., Orpaz, N., and Carmi, G. (1982). Perspective (pp. 1-28). Hove (UK): Psychology
U-shaped behavioral growth in ration Press.
comparisons, or that’s funny I would not have Vergnaud, G. (1983). Multiplicative structures.
thought you were U-ish. In S. Strauss and R. Stavy In R. Lesh & M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of
(Eds.), U-shaped Behavioral Growth (pp. 11-36). mathematics concepts and processes
New York: Academic Press. (pp. 128-175). London: Academic Press.
Stavy, R., and Tirosh, D. (2000). How Students (Mis- Yackel, E., Cobb, P., Wood, T., Wheatley, G., and
)Understand Science and Mathematics. Intuitive Merkel, G. (1990). The impor tance of social
Rules. New York: Teachers College Press. interaction in children’s construction of
Steffe, L. P. (2002). A new hypothesis concerning mathematical knowledge. In T. J. Cooney and C . R.
children’s fractional knowledge. Journal of Hirsch (Eds.), Teaching and learning mathematics in
Mathematical Behavior, 102, 1-41. the 1990s. 1990 Yearbook of the National Council
Steffe, L. P., and Tzur, R. (1994). Interaction and of Teachers of Mathematics (pp. 12-21). Reston,
children’s mathematics. Journal of Research in VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics.
Childhood Education, 8, 99- 91 16.
Streefland, L. (1987). How lo Teach Fractions So As to
Be Useful. Utrecht, The Netherlands: The State
University of Utrecht.
Streefland, L. (1990). Free productions in Teaching
and Learning Mathematics. In K. Gravemeijer, M.
van den Heuvel & L. Streefland (Eds.), Contexts
Free Production Tests and Geometr y in Realistic
Mathematics Education (pp. 33-52). Utrecht
Netherlands: Research group for Mathematical
Education and Educational Computer Centre -
State University of Utrecht.
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-70-2

www.nuffieldfoundation.org
4
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 4: Understanding relations
and their graphical representation
By Terezinha Nunes and Peter Bryant, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Summary of paper 4 3
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: Understanding relations and
their graphical representation 7
Paper 1: Overview
Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and References 34
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities About the authors
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical Terezinha Nunes is Professor of Educational Studies
representation at the University of Oxford.
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation Peter Bryant is Senior Research Fellow in the
in mathematics Department of Education, University of Oxford.
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating
concepts About the Nuffield Foundation
Paper 8: Methodological appendix The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
charitable trust established in 1943 by William
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics being. We fund research and pr actical
in secondary schools. experiment and the development of capacity
to under take them; working across education,
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which science, social science and social policy. While
has been published separately as Introduction and most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
summary of findings. responsive grant programmes we also
undertake our own initiatives.
Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published
together as Summary papers.

All publications are available to download from


our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary of paper 4:
Understanding relations
and their graphical
representation

Headlines
• Children have greater difficulty in understanding Children need to lear n to co-ordinate their
relations than in understanding quantities. This is knowledge of numbers with their under standing
true in the context of both additive and of quantities. This is critical for mathematics
multiplicative reasoning problems. learning in primary school so that they can use
their understanding of quantities to suppor t their
• Primary and secondary school students often knowledge of numbers and vice versa. But this is
apply additive procedures to solve multiplicative not all that students need to lear n to be able to
reasoning problems and also apply multiplicative use mathematics sensibly. Using mathematics also
procedures to solve additive reasoning problems. involves thinking about relations betw een
quantities. Research shows quite unambiguously
• Explicit instruction to help students become aware that it is more diff icult for children to solve
of relations in the context of additive reasoning problems that involve relations than to solve
problems can lead to significant improvement in problems that involve only quantities.
children’s performance.
A simple problem about quantities is: Paul had 5
• The use of diagrams, tables and graphs to marbles. He played two games with his fr iend. In the
represent relations facilitates children’s thinking first game, he won 6 marbles. In the second game he
about and discussing the nature of the relations lost 4 marbles. How many marbles does he have
between quantities in problems. now? The same numerical information can be used
differently, making the problem into one which is all
• Excellent curriculum development work has been about relations: Paul played three games of marbles.
carried out to design instruction to help students In the first game, he won 5 marbles. In the second
develop awareness of their implicit knowledge of game, he won 6. In the third game , he lost 4. Did he
multiplicative relations. This programme has not end up winning or losing marb les? How many?
been systematically assessed so far.
The arithmetic that children need to use to solve is
• An alternative view is that students’ implicit the same in both problems: add 5 and 6 and subtr act
knowledge should not be the starting point for 4. But the second problem is significantly more difficult
students to learn about proportional relations; for children because it is all about relations. They don’t
teaching should focus on formalisations rather know how many marbles Paul actually had at any time,
than informal knowledge and seek to connect they only know that he had 5 more after the f irst
mathematical formalisations with applied situations game than before, and 6 more after the second game ,
only later. and 4 fewer after the third game . Some children say
that this problem cannot be solved because we don’t
• There is no research comparing the results of know how many marbles Paul had to begin with: they
these diametrically opposed ideas. recognise that it is possible to operate on quantities,
but do not recognise that it is possib le to operate on
relations. Why should this be so?
4 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

One possible explanation is the way in which we Models of situations are ways of thinking about them,
express relations. When we speak about quantities, we and more than one way may be appropriate. It all
say that Paul won marbles or lost marbles; these are depends on the question that w e want to answer.
two opposite statements. When we speak about Suppose there are 12 gir ls and 18 boys in a class and
relations, statements that use opposite words may they are assigned to single-sex groups dur ing French
mean the same thing: after winning 5 marbles, we can lessons. If there were not enough books for all of
say that Paul now has 5 more marbles or that before them and the Head Teacher decided to give 4 books
he had 5 fewer. In order to gr asp the concept of to the girls and 6 books to the bo ys, would this be
relations fully, students must be able to view these fair? If you give one book to each gir l, there are 8
two different statements as meaning the same thing. girls left without books; if you give one book to each
Research shows that some students are able to treat boy, there are 12 boys left without books. This seems
these different statements as having the same meaning unfair. If you ask all the children to share , 3 girls will
but others find this difficult. Students who realise that share one book and 3 bo ys will share one book. This
the two statements mean the same thing are more seems fair. The first model is additive: the questions it
successful in solving problems about relations. answers are ‘How many more girls than books?’ and
‘How many more boys than books?’ The second
A second plausible explanation is that many children model is multiplicative: it examines the r atio between
do not distinguish clear ly between quantities and girls and books and the r atio between boys and
relations when they use numbers. When they are books. If the Head Teacher is planning to buy more
given a problem about relations, they interpret the books, she needs an additive model. If the Head
relations as quantities. If they are given a problem like Teacher is not planning to b uy more books, the
‘Tom, Fred, and Rhoda put their apples into a bag. ratio is more informative. A model of a situation is
Tom and Fred together had 17 more apples than constructed by the problem solver for a purpose;
Rhoda. Tom had 7 apples. Rhoda had 5 apples. How additive and multiplicative relations answer different
many apples did Fred have?’, they write down that questions about the same situation.
Tom and Fred had 17 apples together (instead of
17 more than Rhoda). When they make this Children, but also adults, often make mistakes in the
interpretation error, the problem seems very easy: choice of operation when solving problems: they
if Tom had 7, Fred had 10. The information about sometimes use additive reasoning when they should
Rhoda seems irrelevant. But of cour se this is not the have used multiplicative reasoning but they can also
solution. It is possible to teach children to represent make the converse mistake, and use multiplicative
quantities and relations differently, and thus to reasoning when additive reasoning would be
distinguish the two: for example, they can be taught appropriate. So, we need to examine research that
to write ‘plus 17’ to show that this is not a quantity explains how children can become more successful in
but a relation. Children aged seven to nine years can choosing the appropriate model to answer a question.
adopt this notation and at the same time improve
their ability to solve relational problems. However, Experts often use diagrams, tables and graphs to help
even after this teaching, they still seem to be tempted them analyse situations. These resources could suppor t
to interpret relations as quantities. So, learning to children’s thinking about situations. But children seem
represent relations helps children take a step towards to have difficulty in using these resources and ha ve to
distinguishing relations and quantities but they need learn how to use them. They have to become literate
plenty of oppor tunity to think about this distinction. in the use of these mathematical tools in order to
interpret them correctly. A question that has not been
A third difficulty is that relational thinking involves addressed in the literature is whether children can
building a model of a prob lem situation in order to learn about using these tools and about anal ysing
treat the relations in the problem mathematically. In situations mathematically at the same time . Research
primary school, children have little oppor tunity to about interpreting tables and graphs has been carried
explore situations in their mathematics lessons out either to assess students’ previous knowledge (or
before solving a problem. If they make a mistake misconceptions) before they are taught or to test
in solving a problem when their computation was ways of making them liter ate in the use of these tools.
correct, the error is explained as ‘choice of the
wrong operation’, but the wrong choice of A remarkable exception is found in the work of
operation is a symptom, not an explanation f or researchers in the Freudenthal Institute . One of their
what went wrong during problem solving. explicit aims for instruction in mathematics is to help
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

students mathematise situations: i.e. to help them of persons in half and also the amount of water
build a model of a situation and later tr ansform it into in half. Additive reasoning does not work: the
a model for other situations through their awareness transformation from 8 to 4 people would mean
of the relations in the model. They argue that we subtracting 4 whereas the par allel transformation
need to use diagrams, tables and graphs during the in the amount of water would be to subtr act 1. So
process of mathematising situations. These are built by the relation is not the same . If they can discover
students (with teacher guidance) as they explore the that multiplicative reasoning preserves the relation,
situations rather than presented to the students ready whereas additive reasoning does not, they could be
made for interpretation. Students are encouraged to encouraged to test whether there is a multiplicative
use their implicit knowledge of relations; by building relation that they can use to f ind the recipe for 6;
these representations, they can become aware of they could come up with x3, trebling the recipe for
which models they are using. The process of solution 2. Streefland’s ratio table can be used as a model for
is thus not to choose an operation and calculate but testing if other situations fit this sor t of multiplicative
to analyse the relations in the prob lem and work reasoning. The table can be expanded to calculate
towards solution. This process allows the students to the amounts of the other ingredients.
become aware of the relations that are conserved
throughout the different steps. An alternative approach in curriculum development is
to start from formalisations and not to base teaching
Streefland worked out in detail how this process on students’ informal knowledge. The aim of this
would work if students were asked to solve Hart’s approach is to establish links between different formal
famous onion soup recipe problem. In this problem, representations of the same relations. A programme
students are presented with a recipe of onion soup proposed by Adjiage and Pluvinage starts with lines
for 8 people and asked how much of each ingredient divided into segments: students learn how to
they would need if they were preparing the soup for represent segments with the same fr action even
6 people. Many students use their everyday though the lengths of the lines diff er (e.g. 3/5 of lines
knowledge of relations in searching for a solution: they of different lengths). Next they move to using these
think that you need half of the or iginal recipe (which formal representations in other types of prob lems: for
would serve 4) plus half of this (which w ould serve 2 example, mixtures of chocolate syr up and milk where
people) in order to have a recipe for 6 people. This the number of cups of each ingredient differs but the
perfectly sound reasoning is actually a mixture of ratio of chocolate to total n umber of cups is the
additive and multiplicative thinking: half of a recipe for same. Finally, students are asked to write abstractions
8 serves 4 people (multiplicative reasoning) and half of that they learned in these situations and formulate
the latter ser ves 2 (multiplicative reasoning); 6 people rules for solving the problems that they solved during
is 2 more than 4 (additiv e); a recipe for 6 is the same the lessons. An example of generalisation expected is
as the recipe for 4 plus the recipe f or 2 (additive). ‘seven divided by four is equal to seven fourths’ or ‘7
÷ 4 = 7/4’. An example of a r ule used in problem
Streefland and his colleagues suggested that solving would be ‘Given an enlargement in which a 4
diagrams and tables provide the sort of cm length becomes a 7 cm length, then any length to
representation that helps students think about the be enlarged has to be multiplied by 7/4.’
relations in the problem. It is illustr ated here by the
ratio table showing how much water should be used There is no systematic research that compares these
in the soup. The table can be used to help students two very different approaches. Such research would
become aware that the first two steps in their provide valuable insight into how children come to
reasoning are multiplicative: they divide the n umber understand relations.
6 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

Recommendations

Research about mathematical Recommendations for teaching


learning and research

Numbers are used to represent quantities Teaching Teachers should be aware of children’s difficulties
and relations. Primary school children in distinguishing between quantities and relations dur ing
often interpret statements about relations problem solving.
as if they were about quantities and thus
make mistakes in solving problems.

Many problem situations involve both Teaching The primary school curriculum should include the
additive and multiplicative relations; which study of relations in situations in a more explicit wa y.
one is used to solve a problem depends Research Evidence from experimental studies is needed on
on the question being ask ed. Both children which approaches to making students a ware of relations in
and adults can make mistakes in selecting problem situations improve problem solving.
additive or multiplicative reasoning to
answer a question.

Experts use diagrams, tables and graphs to Teaching The use of tables and graphs in the classroom may
explore the relations in a prob lem have been hampered by the assumption that students m ust first
situation before solving a problem. be literate in interpreting these representations before they can
be used as tools. Teachers should consider using these tools as
part of the learning process during problem solving.
Research Systematic research on how students use
diagrams, tables and graphs to represent relations dur ing
problem solving and how this impacts their later lear ning is
urgently needed. Experimental and longitudinal methods
should be combined.

Some researchers propose that informal Teaching Teachers who star t from formalisations should tr y
knowledge interferes with students’ to promote links across different types of mathematical
learning. They propose that teaching representations through teaching.
should star t from formalisations which are Research There is a need f or experimental and longitudinal
only later applied to problem situations. studies designed to investigate the progress that students
make when teaching star ts from formalisations rather than
from students’ informal knowledge and the long-ter m
consequences of this approach to teaching students
about relations.
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Understanding relations
and their graphical
representation

Relations and their importance


in mathematics
In our analysis of how children come to under stand because the number refers to the relation between
natural and rational numbers, we examined the the two quantities, how many more or fewer.
connections that children need to mak e between
quantities and numbers in order to under stand When we use qualitative statements about the
what numbers mean. Numbers are cer tainly used relations between two quantities, the quantities may
to represent quantities, but they are also used to or may not have been expressed numerically. For
represent relations. The focus of this section is on example, we can look at the children and the books
the use of numbers to represent relations. Relations in the class and know that there are more children
do not have to be quantified: we can simply say, for without counting them, especially if the difference is
example, that two quantities are equivalent or quite large. So we can say that there are more
different. This is a qualitative statement about the children than books without knowing how many
relation between two quantities. But relations can children or how many books. But in order to quantify
be quantified also: if there are 20 children in the a relation between two quantities, the quantities
class and 17 books, we can say that there are 3 need to be measur able, even if, in the case of
more children than books. The number 3 quantifies differences, we can evaluate the relationship without
the additive relation between 17 and 20 and so we actually measuring them. The ability to express the
can say that 3 quantifies a relation. relationship quantitatively, without knowing the actual
measures, is one of the roots of algebra (see Paper
When we use numbers to represent quantities, the 5). For this reason, we will often use the ter m
numbers are the result of a measurement oper ation. ‘measures’ in this section, instead of ‘quantities’, to
Measures usually rely on culturally developed refer to quantities that are represented n umerically.
systems of representation. In order to measure
discrete quantities, we count their units, and in order It is perfectly possible that when children first appear
to measure continuous quantities, we use systems to succeed in quantifying relations, they are actually
that have been set up to allow us to represent them still thinking about quantities: when they say ‘3
by a number of conventional units. Measures are children more than books’, they might be thinking of
usually described by a number followed by a noun, the poor little things who w on’t have a book when
which indicates the unit of quantity the n umber the teacher shares the books out, not of the relation
refers to: 5 children, 3 centimetres, 200 grams. And between the number of books and the number of
we can’t replace the noun with another noun children. This hypothesis is consistent with results of
without changing what we are talking about. When studies by Hudson (1983), described in Paper 2:
we quantify a relation, the number does not refer to young children are quite able to answer the
a quantity. We can say ‘3 more children than books’ question ‘how many birds won’t get worms’ but they
or ‘3 books fewer than children’: it makes no can’t tell ‘how many more birds than worms’. We, as
difference which noun comes after the n umber adults, may think that they understand something
8 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

about relations when they answer the first question, rate question, such as pr ice per chocolate, you use
but they may be talking about quantities, i.e. the division. So quantifying relations can be done b y
number of birds that won’t get worms. additive or multiplicative reasoning. Additive
reasoning tells us about the diff erence between
There is no doubt to us that children must grasp quantities; multiplicative reasoning tells us about the
how numbers and quantities are connected in ratio between quantities. The focus of this section is
order to understand what numbers mean. But on multiplicative reasoning but a brief discussion of
mathematics is not only about representing additive relations will be included at the outset to
quantities with numbers. A major use of illustrate the difficulties that children face when they
mathematics is to manipulate n umbers that need to quantify and operate on relations. However,
represent relations and ar rive at conclusions before we turn to the issue of quantif ication of
without having to operate directly on the quantities. relations, we want to say why we use the ter ms
Attributing a number to a quantity is measur ing; additive and multiplicative reasoning, instead of
quantifying relations and manipulating them is speaking about the four arithmetic operations.
quantitative reasoning. To quote Thompson (1993):
‘Quantitative reasoning is the anal ysis of a situation Mathematics educators (e.g. Behr, Harel, Post and
into a quantitative structure – a network of Lesh, 1994; Steffe, 1994; Vergnaud, 1983) include
quantities and quantitative relationships… A under the term ‘additive reasoning’ those problems
prominent characteristic of reasoning quantitatively that are solved by addition and subtr action and
is that numbers and numeric relationships are of under the term ‘multiplicative reasoning’ those that
secondary impor tance, and do not enter into the are solved by multiplication and division. This way of
primary analysis of a situation. What is impor tant is thinking, focusing on the problem structure rather
relationships among quantities’ (p. 165). Elsewhere, than on the ar ithmetic operations used to solve
Thompson (1994) emphasised that ‘a quantitative problems, has become dominant in mathematics
operation is nonnumerical; it has to do with the education research in the last three decades or so .
comprehension [italics in the original] of a situation. It is based on some assumptions about ho w children
Numerical operations (which we have termed learn mathematics, three of which are made explicit
measurement operations) are used to evaluate a here. First, it is assumed that in order to understand
quantity’ (p. 187–188). addition and subtraction properly, children must also
understand the inverse relation between them;
In order to reach the r ight conclusions in similarly, in order to under stand multiplication and
quantitative reasoning, one must use an appropr iate division, children must understand that they also are
representation of the relations between the the inverse of each other. Thus a focus on specific
quantities, and the representation depends on what and separate operations, which was more typical of
we want to know about the relation between the mathematics education thinking in the past, is
quantities. Suppose you want to know whether you justified only when the focus of teaching is on
are paying more for your favourite chocolates at computation skills. Second, it is assumed that the
one shop than another, but the boxes of chocolates links between addition and subtr action, on one
in the two shops are of different sizes. Of course the hand, and multiplication and division, on the other,
bigger box costs more money, but are you paying are conceptual: they relate to the connections
more for each chocolate? You don’t know unless you between quantities within each of these domains of
quantify the relation between price and chocolates. reasoning. The connections between addition and
This relation, price per chocolate, is not quantified in multiplication and those between subtraction and
the same way as the relation ‘more children than division are procedural: you can multiply by carrying
books’. When you want to know how many children out repeated additions and divide b y using repeated
won’t have books, you subtract the number of subtractions. Finally, it is assumed that, in spite of the
books from the number of children (or vice v ersa). procedural links between addition and multiplication,
When you want to know the price per chocolate, these two forms of reasoning are distinct enough to
you shouldn’t subtract the number of chocolates be considered as separ ate conceptual domains. So
from the price (or vice versa); you should divide the we will use the ter ms additive and multiplicative
price by the number of chocolates. Quantifying reasoning and relations r ather than refer to the
relations depends on the nature of the question y ou arithmetic operations.
are asking about the quantities. If you are asking how
many more, you use subtraction; if you are asking a
9 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Quantifying additive relations many of the children did not seem to know what to
do when asked to solve a compare problem.
The literature about additive reasoning consistently
shows that compare problems, which involve Transforming relational statements into
relations between quantities, are more difficult equivalent relational statements
than those that involve combining sets or Compare problems can also state how many items
transformations. This literature was reviewed in A has, then the value of the relation betw een A’s
Paper 1. Our aim in taking up this theme again here and B’s quantities, and then ask how much B has.
is to show that there are three sources of diff iculties Two problems used by Vershaffel (1994) will be
for students in quantifying additive relations: used to illustrate this problem type. In the problem
• to interpret relational statements as such, rather ‘Chris has 32 books. Ralph has 13 more books than
than to interpret them as statements about Chris. How many books does Ralph have?’, the
quantities relation is stated as ‘13 more books’ and the answer
• to transform relational statements into equivalent is obtained by addition; this problem type is referred
statements which help them think about the to by Lewis and Mayer (1987) as involving
problem in a different way consistent language. In the problem ‘Pete has 29
• to combine two relational statements into a third nuts. Pete has 14 more n uts than Rita. How many
relational statement without falling prey to the nuts does Rita have?’, the relation is stated as ‘14
temptation of treating the result as a statement more nuts’ but the answer is obtained by
about a quantity. subtraction; this problem types is referred to as
involving inconsistent language. Verschaffel found that
This discussion in the context of additiv e reasoning Belgian students in sixth grade (aged about 12) gave
illustrates the role of relations in quantitativ e 82% correct responses to problems with consistent
reasoning. The review is br ief and selective, because language and 71% cor rect responses to problems
the main focus of this section is on m ultiplicative with inconsistent language. The operation itself,
reasoning. whether it was addition or subtr action, did not
affect the rate of correct responses.
Interpreting relational statements as
quantitative statements Lewis and Mayer (1987) have argued that the r ate
Compare problems involve two quantities and a of correct responses to relational statements with
relation between them. Their general format is: consistent or inconsistent language var ies because
A had x; B has y; the relation between A and B is z. there is a higher cognitive load in processing
This allows for creating a number of different inconsistent sentences. This higher cognitive load is
compare problems. For example, the simplest due to the fact that the subject of the sentence in
compare problems are of the f orm: Paul has 8 the question ‘how many nuts does Rita have?’ is the
marbles; Alex had 5 marbles; how many more does object of the relational sentence ‘Pete has 14 more
Paul have than Alex? or How many fewer does Alex nuts than Rita’. It takes more effort to process these
have than Paul? In these problems, the quantities are two sentences than other two, in which the subject
known and the relation is the unknown. of the question is also the subject of the relational
statement. They provided some evidence for this
Carpenter, Hiebert and Moser (1981) observed that hypothesis, later confirmed by Verschaffel (1994),
53% of the first grade (estimated age about 6 y ears) who also asked the students in his study to retell
children that they assessed in compare prob lems the problem after the students had already
answered the question ‘how many more does A answered the question.
have than B’ by saying the number that A has. This
is the most common mistak e repor ted in the In the problems where the language was consistent,
literature: the relational question is answ ered as a almost all the students who gave the right answer
quantity mentioned in the problem. The explanation simply repeated what the researcher had said: there
for this error cannot be children’s lack of knowledge was no need to rephr ase the problem. In the
of addition and subtr action, because about 85% of problems where the language was inconsistent,
the same children used cor rect addition and about half of the students (54%) who gave correct
subtraction strategies when solving problems that answers retold the problem by rephrasing it
involved joining quantities or a tr ansformation of an appropriately. Instead of saying that ‘Pete has 14 nuts
initial quantity. Carpenter and Moser repor t that more than Rita’, they said that ‘Rita has 14 nuts less
10 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

than Pete’, and thus made Rita into the subject of had 25 apples. How many apples did Fred have?’
both sentences. Verschaffel interviewed some of the (p. 167). This problem includes three quantities
students who had used this cor rect rephrasing by (Tom’s, Fred’s and Rhoda’s apples) and three
showing them the wr itten problem that he had read relations (how many more Fred and Rhoda ha ve
and asking them whether they had said the same than Tom; how many fewer Rhoda has than Tom; a
thing. Some said that they changed the phr ase combination of these two relations). He asked six
intentionally because it was easier to think about the children who had achieved different scores in a pre-
question in this way; they stressed that the meaning test (three with higher and three with middle lev el
of the two sentences was the same . Other students scores) sampled from two grade levels, second
became confused, as if they had said something (aged about seven) and fifth (aged about nine) to
wrong, and were no longer cer tain of their answers. discuss six problems presented over four different
In conclusion, there is evidence that at least some days. The children were asked to think about the
students do reinterpret the sentences as problems, represent them and discuss them.
hypothesised by Lewis and Mayer; some do this
explicitly and others implicitly. However, almost as On the first day the children went directly to tr ying
many students reached cor rect answers without out calculations and represented the relations as
seeming to rephrase the problem, and may not quantities: the statement ‘97 more apples than Tom’
experience the extra cognitive load predicted. was interpreted as ‘97 apples’. They did not know
how to represent ‘97 more’. This leads to the
It is likely that, under many conditions, we rephrase conclusion that Fred has 80 apples because Rhoda
relational statements when solving problems. So two has 17. On the second day, working with problems
significant findings arise from these studies: about marbles won or lost dur ing the games, the
researcher taught the children to use
• rephrasing relational statements seems to be a representations by writing, for example, ‘plus 12’
strategy used by some people, which may place to indicate that someone had won 12 marbles and
extra cognitive demands on the problem solver ‘minus 1’ to indicate that someone had lost 1
but nevertheless helps in the search for a solution marble. The children were able to work with these
representations with the researcher’s suppor t, but
• rephrasing may be done intentionally and explicitly, when they combined two statements, for example
as a strategy, but may also be carried out implicitly minus 8 and plus 14, they thought that the answer
and apparently unintentionally, producing was 6 marbles (a quantity), instead of plus six
uncertainty in the problem solvers’ minds if they (a relation). So at first they represented relational
are asked about the rephrasing. statements as statements about quantities,
apparently because they did not kno w how to
Combining relational statements into a represent relations. However, after having learned
third relational statement how to represent relational statements, they
Compare problems typically involve two quantities continued to have difficulties in thinking only
and a relation between them but it is possible to relationally, and unwittingly converted the result of
have problems that require children to w ork with operations on relations into statements about
more quantities and relations than these simpler quantities. Yet, when asked whether it would always
problems. In these more complex prob lems, it may be true that someone who had w on 2 marbles in a
be necessary to combine two relational statements game would have 2 marbles, the children recognised
to identify a third one . that this would not necessarily be true. They did
understand that relations and quantities are different
Thompson (1993) analysed students’ reasoning in but they interpreted the result of combining tw o
complex comparison problems which involved at relations as a quantity.
least three quantities and three relations. His aim
was to see how children interpreted complex Thompson describes this tension between
relational problems and how their reasoning interpreting numbers as quantities or relations as the
changed as they tackled more prob lems of the same major difficulty that the students faced throughout
type. To exemplify his problems, we quote the first his study. When they seemed to under stand
one: ‘Tom, Fred, and Rhoda combined their apples ‘difference’ as a relation between two quantities
for a fruit stand. Fred and Rhoda together had 97 arrived at by subtraction, they found it difficult to
more apples than Tom. Rhoda had 17 apples. Tom interpret the idea of ‘difference’ as a relation
11 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

between two relations. The children could cor rectly error is not confined to young students: pre-service
answer, when asked, that if someone has 2 marb les elementary school teachers in the United States
more than another per son, this does not mean that (Cramer, Post and Currier, 1993) made the same
he has two marbles; however, after combining two sort of mistake when asked to solve the problem:
relations (minus 8 and plus 14), instead of saying Sue and Julie were r unning equally fast around a track.
that this person ended up with plus 6 marb les, they Sue started first. When she had run 9 laps, Julie had
said that he now had 6 marbles. run 3 laps. When Julie completed 15 laps , how many
laps had Sue r un? The relation between Sue’s and
Julie’s numbers of laps should be quantified
additively: because they were running at the same
Summary
speed, this difference would (in principle) be
1 At first, children have difficulties in using additive constant. However, 32 of 33 pre-ser vice teachers
reasoning to quantify relations; when asked about answered 45 (15 x 3), apparently using the r atio
a relation, they answer about a quantity. between the first two measures (9 and 3 laps) to
calculate Sue’s laps. This latter type of mistak e would
2 Once they seem to conquer this, they continue to not be predicted by Piaget’s theory.
find it difficult to combine relations and stay within
relational reasoning: the combination of two The hypothesis that we will pursue in this chapter,
relations is often converted into a statement following authors such as Thompson 1994) and
about a quantity. Vergnaud (1983), is that additive and multiplicative
reasoning have different origins. Additive reasoning
3 So children’s difficulties with relations are not stems from the actions of joining, separating, and
confined to multiplicative reasoning: they are also placing sets in one-to-one cor respondence.
observed in the domain of additiv e reasoning. Multiplicative reasoning stems from the action of
putting two variables in one-to-many
correspondence (one-to-one is just a par ticular
case), an action that k eeps the ratio between the
Quantifying multiplicative relations
variables constant. Thompson (1994) made this
Research on how children quantify multiplicative point forcefully in his discussion of quantitativ e
relations has a long tr adition. Piaget and his operations: ‘Quantitative operations originate in
colleagues (Inhelder and Piaget, 1958; Piaget and actions: The quantitative operation of combining
Inhelder, 1975) originally assumed that children first two quantities additively originates in the actions of
think of quantifying relations additively and can only putting together to make a whole and separating a
think of relations multiplicatively at a later age . This whole to make par ts; the quantitative operation of
hypothesis led to the prediction of an ‘additive comparing two quantities additively originates in the
phase’ in children’s solution to multiplicative action of matching two quantities with the goal of
reasoning problems, before they would be able to determining excess or deficits; the quantitative
conceive of two variables as linked by a operation of comparing two quantities
multiplicative relation. This hypothesis led to much multiplicatively originates in matching and subdividing
research on the development of propor tional with the goal of shar ing. As one interiorizes actions,
reasoning, which largely suppor ted the claim that making mental operations, these operations in the
many younger students offer additive solutions to making imbue one with the ability to comprehend
proportions problems (e.g. Har t, 1981 b; 1984; situations representationally and enable one to dr aw
Karplus and Peterson, 1970; Karplus, Pulos and Stage, inferences about numerical relationships that are not
1983; Noelting, 1980 a and b). These results are not present in the situation itself ’ (pp. 185–186).
disputed but their interpretation will be examined in
the next sections of this paper because cur rent We suggest that, if students solve additive and
studies suggest an alter native interpretation. multiplicative reasoning problems successfully but
they are guided by implicit models, they will find it
Work carried out mostly by Lieven Verschaffel and difficult to distinguish between the two models.
his colleagues (e.g. De Bock, Verschaffel et al., 2002; According to Fischbein (1987), implicit models
2003) shows that students also make the converse and informal reasoning provide a star ting point
mistake, and multiply when they should be adding in for learning, but one of the aims of mathematics
order to solve some relational problems. This type of teaching in primary school is to help students
12 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

formalize their informal knowledge (Treffers, 1987). Hannah. They use the schema of one-to-man y
In this process, the models will change and become correspondence.
more explicitly connected to the systems of
representations used in mathematics. Different researchers have investigated the use of
one-to-many correspondences by children to solve
In this section, we analyse how students establish multiplication and division problems before they are
and quantify relations between quantities in taught about these oper ations in school. Piaget’s
multiplicative reasoning problems. We first discuss work (1952), described in Paper 3, showed that
the nature of multiplicative reasoning and present children can understand multiplicative equivalences:
research results that descr ibe how children’s they can construct a set A equivalent to a set B b y
informal knowledge of multiplicative relations putting the elements in A in the same r atio that B
develops. In the subsequent section, we discuss the has to a compar ison set.
representation of multiplicative relations in tables
and graphs. Next we analyse how children establish Frydman and Br yant (1988; 1994) also showed
other relations between measures, besides linear that young children can use one-to-many
relations. The final section sets out some h ypotheses correspondences to create equivalent sets.
about the nature of the diff iculty in dealing with They used sharing in their study because y oung
relations in mathematics and a research agenda f or children seem to have much experience with
testing current hypotheses systematically. correspondence when sharing. In a shar ing situation,
children typically use a one-for-you one-for-me
The development of multiplicative reasoning procedure, setting the shared elements (sweets) into
Multiplicative reasoning is impor tant in many ways one-to-one correspondence with the recipients
in mathematics learning. Its role in under standing (dolls). Frydman and Br yant observed that children
numeration systems with a base and place value in the age r ange five to seven years became
was already discussed in P aper 2. In this section, progressively more competent in dealing with one-
we focus on a different role of multiplicative to-many correspondences and equivalences in this
reasoning in mathematics lear ning, its role in situation. In their task, the children were asked to
understanding relations between measures or construct equivalent sets but the units in the sets
quantities, which has already been recognised were of a different value. For example, one doll only
by different researchers (e.g. Confrey, 1994; liked her sweets in double units and the second doll
Thompson, 1994; Vergnaud, 1983; 1994). liked his sweets in single units. The children were
able to use one-to-many correspondence to share
Additive and multiplicative reasoning problems fairly in this situation: when they gave a double to
are essentially different: additive reasoning is used the first doll, they gave two singles to the second.
in one-variable problems, when quantities of the This flexible use of correspondence to construct
same kind are put together, separated or compared, equivalent sets was inter preted by Frydman and
whereas multiplicative reasoning involves two Bryant as an indication that the children’ s use of
variables in a fixed-ratio to each other. Even the the procedure was not merel y a copy of previously
simplest multiplicative reasoning problems involve observed and rehearsed actions: it reflected an
two variables in a fixed ratio. For example, in the understanding of how one-to-many
problem ‘Hannah bought 6 sweets; each sweet costs correspondences can result in equivalent sets. They
5 pence; how much did she spend?’ there are two also replicated one of Piaget’s previous findings:
variables, number of sweets and price per sweet. The some children who succeed with the 2:1 r atio found
problem would be solved by a multiplication the 3:1 ratio difficult. So the development of the
if, as in this example , the total cost is unkno wn. The one-to-many correspondence schema does not
same problem situation could be presented with happen in an all-or-nothing fashion.
a different unknown quantity, and would then be
solved by means of a division: ‘Hannah bought some Kouba (1989) presented young children in the
sweets; each sweet costs 5p; she spent 30p; how United States, in first, second and third gr ade
many sweets did she buy?’ (aged about six to eight y ears), with multiplicative
reasoning problems that are more typical of those
Even before being taught about multiplication and used in school; for example: in a par ty, there were 6
division in school, children can solve multiplication cups and 5 mar shmallows in each cup; how many
and division problems such as the one about marshmallows were there?
13 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Kouba analysed the children’s strategies in great and counting or adding f or the other); few used
detail, and classified them in ter ms of the types of recall of multiplication facts. The recall of number
actions used and the level of abstraction. The level facts was significantly higher after the children had
of abstraction varied from direct representation received instruction, when they were in third gr ade.
(i.e. all the information was represented by the
children with concrete mater ials), through par tial The level of success obser ved by Kouba among
representation (i.e. numbers replaced concrete children who had not yet received instruction is
representations for the elements in a group and the modest, compared to that obser ved in two
child counted in groups) up to the most abstract subsequent studies, where the ratios were easier.
form of representation available to these children, Becker (1993) asked kindergarten children in the
i.e. multiplication facts. United States, aged four to five years, to solve
problems in which the cor respondences were 2:1
For the children in first and second gr ade, who or 3:1. As repor ted by Piaget and by Frydman and
had not received instruction on multiplication and Bryant, the children were more successful with 2:1
division, the most important factor in predicting than 3:1 correspondences, and the level of success
the children’s solutions was which quantity was improved with age. The overall level of correct
unknown. For example, in the problem above, about responses by the five-year-olds was 81%.
the 6 cups with 5 mar shmallows in each cup, when
the size of the groups was known (i.e. the number Carpenter, Ansell, Franke, Fennema and Weisbeck
of marshmallows in each cup), the children used (1993) also gave multiplicative reasoning problems
correspondence strategies: they paired objects (or to U.S. kindergarten children involving
tallies to represent the objects) and counted or correspondences of 2:1, 3:1 and 4:1. They observed
added, creating one-to-many correspondences 71% correct responses to these problems.
between the cups and the mar shmallows. For
example, if they needed to find the total number of The success rates leave no doubt that many young
marshmallows, they pointed 5 times to a cup (or its children star t school with some under standing of
representation) and counted to 5, paused, and then one-to-many correspondence, which they can use
counted from 6 to 10 as they pointed to the to learn to solve multiplicative reasoning problems
second ‘cup’, until they reached the solution. in school. These results do not impl y that children
Alternatively, the may have added as they pointed who use one-to-many correspondence to solve
to the ‘cup’. multiplicative reasoning problems consciously
recognise that in a multiplicative situation there is
In contrast, when the number of elements in each a fixed ratio linking the two variables. Their actions
group was not known, the children used dealing maintain the ratio fixed but it is most likely that this
strategies: they shared out one mar shmallow (or its invariance remains, in Vergnaud’s (1997) terminology,
representation) to each cup, and then another, until as a ‘theorem in action’.
they reached the end, and then counted the n umber
in each cup. Here they sometimes used trial-and- The importance of informal knowledge
error: they shared more than one at a time and Both Fischbein (1987) and Treffers (1987) assumed
then might have needed to adjust the number per that children’s informal knowledge is a star ting
cup to get to the cor rect distribution. point for learning mathematics in school b ut it is
important to consider this assumption fur ther. If
Although the actions look quite diff erent, their children star t school with some inf ormal knowledge
aims are the same: to establish one-to-many that can be used f or learning mathematics in school,
correspondences between the marshmallows it is necessar y to consider whether this kno wledge
and the cups. facilitates their learning or, quite the opposite , is an
obstacle to learning. The action of establishing one-
Kouba observed that 43% of the str ategies used to-many correspondences is not the same as the
by the children, including first, second, and third concept of ratio or as multiplicative reasoning: ratio
graders, were appropriate. Among the first and may be implicit in their actions b ut it is possible that
second grade children, the overwhelming majority the children are more aware of the methods that
of the appropriate strategies was based on they used to figure out the numerical values of the
correspondences, either using direct representation quantities, i.e. they are aware of counting or adding.
or par tial representation (i.e. tallies for one variable
14 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

Children’s methods for solving multiplication cognitive ability and a Working Memory Test,
problems can be seen as a star ting point, if they Counting Recall (Pickering and Gathercole, 2001),
form a basis for fur ther learning, but also an obstacle at school entr y. At the beginning of the study, the
to learning, if children stick to their counting and children’s age ranged from five years and one month
addition procedures instead of lear ning about ratio to six years and six months. About 14 months later,
and multiplicative reasoning in school. Resnick the children were given a state-designed and
(1983) and Kaput and West (1994) argue that an teacher-administered mathematics achievement test,
important lesson from psychological and which is entirely independent of the researcher s and
mathematics education research is that, even after an ecologically valid measure of how much they
people have been taught new concepts and ideas, have learned in school. The children’s performance
they still resor t to their prior methods to solve in the five items on cor respondence at school entr y
problems that differ from the textbook examples on was a significant predictor of their mathematics
which they have applied their new knowledge. The achievement, after controlling for: (1) age at the time
implementation of the one-to-many correspondence of the achievement test; (2) performance on the
schema to solve problems requires adding and BAS-II excluding the subtest of their kno wledge of
counting, and students have been repor ted to resor t numbers at school star t; (3) knowledge of number
to counting and adding even in secondar y school, at school entr y (a subtest of the B AS-II); (4)
when they should be multiplying (Booth, 1981). So performance on the working memory measure;
is this informal knowledge an obstacle to better and (5) performance on the multiplicative reasoning,
understanding or does it provide a basis for learning? one-to-many correspondence items. Nunes et al.
(2007) did not repor t the analysis of longitudinal
It is possible that a precise answer to this question prediction based separately on the items that assess
cannot be found: whether informal knowledge helps multiplicative reasoning; so these results are
or hinders children’s learning might depend on the reported here. The results are presented visuall y in
pedagogy used in their classroom. However, it is Figure 4.1 and descr ibed in words subsequently.
possible to consider this question in pr inciple by
examining the results of longitudinal and The total variance explained in the mathematics
intervention studies. If it is f ound in a longitudinal achievement by these predictors was 66%; age
study that children who star t school with more explained 2% (non significant), the BAS general
informal mathematical knowledge achieve better score (excluding the Number Skills subtest)
mathematics learning in school, then it can be explained a fur ther 49% (p < 0.001); the sub-test on
concluded that, at least in a gener al manner, informal number skills explained a fur ther 6% (p < 0.05);
knowledge does provide a basis for learning. working memory explained a fur ther 4% (p < 0.05),
Similarly, if inter vention studies show that increasing and the children’s understanding of multiplicative
children’s informal knowledge when they are in their reasoning at school entr y explained a fur ther 6%
first year school has a positive impact on their (p = 0.005). This result shows that children’s
school learning of mathematics, there is fur ther understanding of multiplicative reasoning at school
support for the idea that inf ormal knowledge can entry is a specific predictor of mathematics
offer a foundation for learning. In the case of the achievement in the first two years of school. It
correspondence schema, there is clear evidence supports the hypothesis that, in a gener al way, this
from a longitudinal study b ut intervention studies informal knowledge forms a basis for their school
with the appropriate controls are still needed. learning of mathematics: after 14 months and after
controlling for general cognitive factors at school
Nunes, Bryant, Evans, Bell, Gardner, Gardner and entry, performance on an assessment of
Carraher (2007) carried out the longitudinal study. multiplicative reasoning still explained a signif icant
In this study, British children were tested on their amount of variance in the children’s mathematics
understanding of four aspects of logical- achievement in school.
mathematical reasoning at the star t of school; one
of these was multiplicative reasoning. There were It is therefore quite likely that instruction will be an
five items which were multiplicative reasoning important factor in influencing whether students
problems that could be solved by one-to-many continue to use the one-to-many schema of action
correspondence. The children were also given the to solve such problems, even if replacing objects
British Abilities Scale (BAS-II; Elliott, Smith and with numbers but still counting or adding instead of
McCulloch, 1997) as an assessment of their gener al multiplying, or whether they go on to adopt the use
15 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

of the operations of multiplication and division. focus the students’ attention on the invariance of
Treffers (1987) and Gr avemeijer (1997) argue that ratio in the cor respondence situations. They used
students do and should use their inf ormal different sor ts of diagrams which treated the
knowledge in the classroom when lear ning about quantities in correspondence as composite units: for
multiplication and division, but that it should be one example, a plate and six pieces of tableware formed
of the aims of teaching to help them f ormalise this a single unit, a set-place for one person. The ideas
knowledge, and in the process develop a better proposed in these approaches to instruction are
understanding of the ar ithmetic operations very ingenious and merit further research with the
themselves. We do not review this work here but appropriate controls and measures. The lack of
recognise the impor tance of their ar gument, control groups and appropr iate pre- and post-test
particularly in view of the strength of this inf ormal assessments in these inter vention studies makes it
knowledge and students’ likelihood of using it even difficult to reach conclusions regarding the impact of
after having been taught other f orms of knowledge the programmes.
in school. However, it must be pointed out that
there is no evidence that teaching students about Park and Nunes (2001) car ried out a br ief
arithmetic operations makes them more aware of intervention study where they compared children’s
the invariance of the r atio when they use one-to- success in multiplicative reasoning problems after the
many correspondences to solve problems. Kaput children had par ticipated in one of two types of
and West (1994) also designed a teaching intervention. In the first, they were taught about
programme which aimed at using students’ informal multiplication as repeated addition, which is the
knowledge of correspondences to promote their traditional approach used in Br itish schools and is
understanding of multiplicative reasoning. In contrast based on the procedur al connection between
to the programme designed by Treffers for the multiplication and addition. In the second
operation of multiplication and by Gravemeijer for intervention group, the children were taught
the operation of division, Kaput and West’s about multiplication by considering one-to-many
programme used simple calculations and tried to correspondence situations, where these

Figure 4.1: A schematic representation of the degree to which individual differences in mathematics achievement are explained
by the first four factors and the additional amount of variance explained by children’s informal knowledge at school entry.
16 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

correspondences were represented explicitly. A third multiplication and division, making a tr ansition
group of children, the control group, solved addition from small to lar ge numbers easier for the
and subtraction problems, working with the same students. The second type (Kaput and West, 1994;
experimenter for a similar per iod of time. The Park and Nunes, 2001) focused on making the
children in the one-to-many correspondence group students more aware of the schema of one-to-
made significantly more progress in solving many correspondences and the theorems in
multiplicative reasoning problems than those in the action that it represents implicitl y. There is
repeated addition and in the control group . This evidence that, with younger children solving small
study does include the appropriate controls and number problems, an inter vention that focuses
provides clear evidence for more successful lear ning on the schema of cor respondences facilitates the
of multiplicative reasoning when instruction draws development of multiplicative reasoning.
on the children’s appropriate schema of action.
However, this was a very brief intervention with a Finally, it is pointed out that all the examples
small sample and in one-to-one teaching sessions. presented so far dealt with prob lems in which the
It would be necessar y to replicate it with larger children were asked questions about quantities.
numbers of children and to compare its level of None of the problems focused on the relation
success with other inter ventions, such as those used between quantities. In the subsequent section, we
by Treffers and Gravemeijer, where the children’s present a classification of multiplicative reasoning
understanding of the ar ithmetic operations of problems in order to aid the discussion of ho w
multiplication and division was strengthened b y quantities and relations are handled in the context
working with larger numbers. of multiplicative reasoning problems.

Different types of multiplicative


Summary
reasoning problems
1 Additive and multiplicative reasoning have their We argued previously that many children solve
origins in different schemas of action. There does problems that involve additive relations, such as
not seem to be an order of acquisition, with compare problems, by thinking only about quantities.
young children understanding at first only additive In this section, we examine different types of
reasoning and only later multiplicative reasoning. multiplicative reasoning problems and analyse
Children can use schemas of action appropr iately students’ problem solving methods with a view to
both in additive and multiplicative reasoning understanding whether they are consider ing only
situations from an ear ly age. quantities or relations in their reasoning. In order
to achieve this, it is necessar y to think about the
2 The schemas of one-to-many correspondence different types of multiplicative reasoning problems.
and sharing (or dealing) allow young children
to succeed in solving multiplicative reasoning Classifications of multiplicative reasoning situations
problems before they are taught about vary across authors (Brown, 1981; Schwar tz, 1988;
multiplication and division in school. Tourniaire and Pulos, 1985; Vergnaud, 1983), but
there is undoubtedly agreement on what
3 There is evidence that children’s knowledge of characterises multiplicative situations: in these
correspondences is a specific predictor of their situations there are always two (or more) var iables
mathematics achievement and, therefore, that their with a fixed ratio between them. Thus, it is argued
informal knowledge can provide a basis for further that multiplicative reasoning forms the foundation
learning. However, this does not mean that they for children’s understanding of propor tional relations
understand the concept of r atio: the invariance of and linear functions (Kaput and West, 1994;
ratio in these situations is lik ely to be known only Vergnaud, 1983).
as a theorem in action.
The first classifications of problem situations
4 Two types of progr ammes have been proposed considered distinct possibilities: for example, rate and
with the aim of br idging students’ informal and ratio problem situations were distinguished initially.
formal knowledge. One type (Treffers, 1987; However, there seemed to be little agreement
Gravemeijer, 1997) focuses on teaching the amongst researchers regarding which situations
children more about the oper ations of should be classified as rate and which as r atio. Lesh,
17 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Post and Behr (1988) wrote some time ago: ‘there of a quality of an object in such a wa y that we
is disagreement about the essential char acteristics understand the possibility of measur ing it.
that distinguish, for example rates from ratios… In ‘Quantities, when measured, have numerical value,
fact, it is common to f ind a given author changing but we need not measure them or kno w their
terminology from one publication to another’ (p. measures to reason about them’ (p. 166). Two
108). Thompson (1994) and Kaput and West (1994) quantities, area and volume, can be used here to
consider this distinction to appl y not to situations, illustrate the difference between quantities and
but to the mental oper ations that the problem measures.
solver uses. These different mental operations could
be used when thinking about the same situation: Hart (1981 a) pointed out that the square unit can
ratio refers to understanding a situation in ter ms of be used to measure area b y different measurement
the par ticular values presented in the problem (e.g. operations. We can attribute a number to the area
travelling 150 miles over 3 hours) and rate refers to of a rectangle, for example, by covering it with
understanding the constant relation that applies to square units and counting them: this is a simple
any of the pair s of values (in theor y, in any of the 3 measurement operation, based on iteration of the
hours one would have travelled 50 miles). ‘Rate is a units. If we don’t have enough bricks to do this (see
reflectively abstracted constant ratio’ (Thompson, Nunes, Light and Mason, 1993), we can count the
1994, p. 192). number of square units that mak e a row along the
base, and establish a one-to-many correspondence
In this research synthesis, we will work with the between the number of rows that fit along the
classification offered by Vergnaud (1983), who height and the number of square units in each row.
distinguished three types of problems. We can calculate the area of the rectangle b y
conceiving of it as an isomor phism of measures
• In isomorphism of measures problems, there is problem: 1 row corresponds to x units. If we
a simple proportional relation between two attribute a number to the area of the rectangle b y
measures (i.e. quantities represented by numbers): multiplying its base by its height, both measured with
for example, number of cakes and price paid for units of length, we are conceiving this situation as a
the cakes, or amount of corn and amount of corn product of measures: two measures, the length of
flour produced. the base and that of its height, multiplied produce a
third measure, the area in square units. Thus a
• In product of measures problems, there is a quantity in itself is not the same as its measure , and
Cartesian composition between two measures to the way it is measured can change the complexity
form a third measure: for example, the number (i.e. the number of relations to be considered) of
of T-shirts and number of shorts a girl has can be the situation.
composed in a Cartesian product to give the
number of different outfits that she can wear; the Nunes, Light and Mason (1993) showed that
number of different coloured cloths and the children aged 9 to 10 y ears were much more
number of emblems determines the number successful when they compared the area of tw o
of different flags that you can produce. figures if they chose to use br icks to measure the
areas than if they chose to use a r uler. Because the
• In multiple proportions problems, a measure children did not have sufficient bricks to cover the
is in simple proportion to (at least) two other areas, most used calculations. They had three
measures: for example, the consumption of cereal quantities to consider – the number of rows that
in a Scout camp is proportional to number of covered the height, the number of bricks in each
persons and the number of days. row along the base , and the area, and the relation
between number of rows and number of bricks in
Because this classification is based on measures, it the row. These children worked within an
offers the oppor tunity to explore the difference isomorphism of measures situation.
between a quantity and its measure . Although this
may seem like a digression, exploring the difference Children who used a r uler worked within a product
between quantities and measures is helpful in this of measures situation and had to consider three
chapter, which focuses on the quantification of quantities – the value of the base, the value of the
relations between measures. A quantity, as defined height, and the area; and three relations to consider
by Thompson (1993) is constituted when we think – the relation between the base and the height, the
18 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

relation between base and area, and the relation this chapter. We will focus primarily on isomorphism
between the height and the area (the area is of measures situations, because the analysis of how
proportional to the base if the height is constant this type of problem is solved by students of
and propor tional to the height if the base is different ages and by schooled and unschooled
constant). groups will help us under stand the operations of
thought used in solving them.
The students who developed an isomorphism
of measures conception of area w ere able to use A diagram of isomorphism of measures situations,
their conception to compare a rectangular with presented in Figure 4.2 and adapted from Vergnaud
a triangular area, and thus expanded their (1983), will be used to facilitate the discussion.
understanding of how area is measured. The
students who worked within a product of measures This simple schema shows that there are two sets
situation did not succeed in expanding their of relations that can be quantif ied in this situation:
knowledge to think about the area of tr iangles. • the relation between a and c is the same as that
Nunes, Light and Mason speculated that, after between b and d; this is the scalar relation, which
this initial move, students who worked with an links two values in the same measure space
isomorphism of measures model might subsequentl y • the relation between a and b is the same as that
be able to re-conceptualise area once again and between c and d; this is the functional relation, or
move on to a product of measures approach, but the ratio, which links the two measure spaces.
they did not test this h ypothesis.
The psychological difference (i.e. the difference
Hart (1981 a) and Vergnaud (1983) make a similar that it makes for the students) between scalar and
point with respect to the measurement of v olume: it functional relations is very impor tant, and it has
can be measured by iteration of a unit (how many been discussed in the literature by many authors
litres can fit into a container) or can be conceived as (e.g. Kaput and West, 1994; Nunes, Schliemann and
a problem situation involving the relations between Carraher, 1993; Vergnaud, 1983). It had also been
base, height and width, and described as product of discussed previously by Noelting (1980 a and b) and
measures. Volume as a quantity is itself neither a uni- Tourniaire and Pulos (1985), who used the ter ms
dimensional nor a three-dimensional measure and within and between quantities relations. This paper
one measure might be useful f or some purposes will use only the terms scalar and functional relations
(add 2 cups of milk to mak e the pancake batter) or reasoning.
whereas a different one might be useful f or other
purposes (the volume of a tr ailer in a lorry can be ‘For a mathematician, a propor tion is a statement
easily calculated by multiplying the base, the height of equality of two ratios, i.e., a/b = c/d’ (Tourniaire
and the width). Different systems of representation and Pulos, 1985, p. 181). Given this definition, there
and different measurement operations allow us to is no reason to distinguish betw een what has been
attribute different numbers to the same quantity, and traditionally termed multiplication and division
to do so consistently within each system. problems and propor tions problems. We think that
the distinction has been based, perhaps only
Vergnaud’s classification of multiplicative reasoning implicitly, on the use of a r atio with reference to the
situations is used here to simplify the discussion in unit in multiplication and division problems. So one

Measure 1 Measure 2

Figure 4.2: Schema of an isomorphism of measures situation. Measures 1 and 2 are connected by a proportional relation.
19 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

should not be sur prised to see that one-to-many many correspondence schema, which maintains the
correspondences reasoning is used in the beginning ratio invariant without necessarily bringing with it an
of primary school by children to solve simple awareness of the fixed relation between the
multiplicative reasoning problems and continues to variables. Unit ratio is a mathematical ter m but it is
be used by older students to solve propor tions not clear whether the children who w ere explicitly
problems in which the unit r atio is not given in the searching for the price of one notebook in Ricco’s
problem description. Many researchers (e.g. Har t, study were thinking of r atio as the quantification of
1981 b; Kaput and West, 1994; Lamon, 1994; Nunes the relation between notebooks and money. When
and Bryant, 1996; Nunes, Schliemann and Car raher, the child says ‘1 notebook costs 4 cents’, the child is
1993; Piaget, Grize, Szeminska and Bangh, 1977; speaking about two quantities, not necessarily about
Inhelder and Piaget, 1958; 1975; Ricco, 1982; Steffe, the relation between them. A statement about the
1994) have described students’ solutions based on relation between the quantities would be ‘the
correspondence procedures and many different number of notebooks times 4 tells me the total cost’.
terms have been used to refer to these, such as
building up strategies, empirical strategies, halving The use of these informal strategies by students in the
or doubling, and replications of a composite unit. In solution of propor tions problems is consistent with
essence, these strategies consist of using the initial the hypothesis that multiplicative reasoning develops
values provided in the problem and changing them from the schema of one-to-many correspondences:
in one or more steps to ar rive at the desired value. students may be simply using numbers instead of
Hart’s (1981 b) well known example of the onion objects when reasoning about the quantities in these
soup recipe for 4 people, which has to be problems. In the same way that they build up the
converted into a recipe f or 6 people, illustrates this quantities with objects, they can build up the quantities
strategy well. Four people plus half of 4 mak es 6 with numbers. It is unlikely that students are thinking of
people, so the children take each of the ingredients the scalar relation and quantifying it when they solv e
in the recipe in tur n, half the amount, and add this problems by means of building up strategies. We think
to the amount required f or 4 people. that it can be concluded with some certainty that
students realise that whatever transformation they
Students used yet another method in solving make, for example, to the number of people in Har t’s
proportions problems, still related to the idea of onion soup problem, they must also make to the
correspondences: they first find the unit r atio and quantities of ingredients. It is even less likely that they
then use it to calculate the desired value . Although have an awareness of the ratio between the two
this method is taught in some countr ies (see Lave, domains of measures and have reached an
1988; Nunes, Schliemann and Car raher, 1993; Ricco, understanding of a reflectively abstracted constant
1982), it is not necessar ily used by all students after ratio, in Thompson’s terms.
they have been taught; many students rely on
building up strategies which change across different These results provoke the question of the role of
problems in terms of the calculations that are used, teaching in developing students’ understanding of
instead of using a single algor ithm that aims at functional relations. Studies of high-school students
finding the unit ratio. Hart (1981 b) presented the and adults with limited schooling in Br azil throw some
following problem to a lar ge sample of students light on this issue . They show that instruction about
(2257) aged 11 to 16 y ears in 1976: 14 metres of multiplication and division or about propor tions per
calico cost 63p; find the price of 24 metres. She se is neither necessar y for people to be able to solve
reported that no child actuall y quoted the unitar y proportions problems nor sufficient to promote
method in their explanation, even though some students’ thinking about functional relations. Nunes,
children did essentially seek a unitar y ratio. Ricco Schliemann and Carraher (1993) have shown that
(1982), in contrast, found that some students fishermen and foremen in the constr uction industry,
explicitly searched for the unit ratio (e.g. ‘First I need who have little formal school instruction, can solve
to know how much one notebook will cost and proportions problems that are novel to them in three
then we will see’, p. 299, our translation) but others ways: (a) the problems use values that depart from
seem to search for the unit r atio without making the values they normally work with; (b) they are
explicit the necessity of this step in their procedure . asked to calculate in a direction which they normally
do not have to think about; or (c) the content of the
Building up methods and finding the unit r atio may problem is different from the problems with which
be essentially an extension of the use of the one-to- they work in their everyday lives.
20 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

Foremen in the constr uction industry have to work schooling were more successful when they could
with blue-prints as representations of distances in use building up strategies easily, as in problems of
the buildings under construction. They have the type A in Figure 4.3. Problem B uses the same
experience with a cer tain number of conventionally numbers but arranged in a way that building up
used scales (e.g. 1:50, 1:100 and 1:1000). When they strategies are not so easily implemented; the relation
were provided with a scale dr awing that did not fit that is easy to quantify in prob lem B is the functional
these specifications (e.g. 1:40) and did not indicate relation.
the ratio used (e.g. they were shown a distance on
the blue-print and its value in the b uilding), most The difference in students’ rate of success across the
foremen were able to use cor respondences to two types of problem was significant: they solved
figure out what the scale w ould be and then about 80% of type A problems correctly and only
calculate the measure of a wall from its measure on 35% of type B problems. For the adults (fishermen),
the blueprint. They were able to do so even when there was a difference between the rate of correct
fractions were involved in the calculations and the responses (80% correct in type A and 75% correct
scale had an unexpected f ormat (e.g. 3 cm:1 m uses in type B) but this was not statisticall y significant.
different units whereas scales typicall y use the same Their success, however, was typically a result of
unit) because they have extensive experience in prowess with calculations when b uilding up a
moving across units (metres, centimetres and quantity, and very few answers might have resulted
millimetres). Completely illiterate foremen (N = 4), from a quantification of the functional relation.
who had never set foot in a school due to their lif e
circumstances, showed 75% correct responses to These results suggest three conclusions.
these problems. In contrast, students who had been
taught the formal method known as the r ule of • Reasoning about quantities when solving
three, which involves writing an equation of the proportional problems seems to be an extension
form a/b = c/d and solving for the unknown value, of correspondence reasoning; schooling is not
performed significantly worse (60% correct). Thus necessary for this development.
schooling is not necessar y for multiplicative
reasoning to develop and propor tions problems to • Most secondary school students seem to use the
be solved correctly, and teaching students a gener al same schema of reasoning as younger students;
formula to solve the problem is not a guarantee that there is little evidence of an impact of instruction
they will use it when the oppor tunity arises. on their approach to proportional problems.

These studies also showed that both secondar y • Functional reasoning is more challenging and is not
school students and adults with relativ ely little guaranteed by schooling; teaching students a

Measure 1 Measure 2 Measure 1 Measure 2

3 5 3 12

x4
x4
12 ? 5 ?

A B

Figure 4.3: For someone who can easily think about scalar or functional relations, there should be no difference in the level of difficulty of
the two problems. For those who use building up strategies and can only work with quantities, problem A is significantly easier.
21 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

formal method, which can be used as easily for There has been some discussion of the diff erence
both problem types, does not make functional between reasoning about quantities and relations in
reasoning easier (see Paper 6 for further the literature. However, we have not been able to
discussion). find studies that establish whether the difficulty of
thinking about relations might be at the root of
The results obser ved with Br azilian students do students’ difficulties in transforming their informal into
not differ from those obser ved by Vergnaud formal mathematics knowledge. The educational
(1983) in Fr ance, and Har t (1981 b; 1984) in the implications of these hypotheses are considerable but
United Kingdom. The novelty of these studies is there is, to our knowledge, no research that
the demonstration that the informal knowledge examines the issue systematically enough to provide
of multiplicative reasoning and the ability to solv e a firm ground for pedagogical developments. The
multiplicative reasoning problems through importance of the issue must not be underestimated,
correspondences develop into more abstr act particularly in the United Kingdom, where students
schemas that allow for calculating in the absence seem do to well enough in the inter national
of concrete forms of representation, such as comparisons in additive reasoning but not in
maniputatives and tallies. Both the students and multiplicative reasoning problems (Beaton, Mullis,
the adults with low levels of schooling were able Martin, Gonzalez, Kelly AND Smith, 1996, p. 94–95).
to calculate, for example, what should the actual
distances in a building be from their siz e in
blueprint drawings. Relatively unschooled adults
Summary
who have to think about propor tions in the
course of their occupations and secondar y We draw some educational implications from these
school students seem to rel y on these more studies, which must be seen as hypotheses about
abstract schemas to solve propor tions problems. what is impor tant for successful teaching of
The similarity between these two groups, rather multiplicative reasoning about relations.
than the differences, in the forms of reasoning
and rates of success is str iking. These results 1 Before children are taught about m ultiplication
suggest that informal knowledge of and division in school, they already have schemas
correspondences is a powerful thinking schema of action that they use to solv e multiplicative
and that schooling does not easily transform it reasoning problems. These schemas of action
into a more powerful one by incorporating involve setting up cor respondences between
functional understanding into the schema. two variables and do not appear to dev elop
from the idea of repeated addition. This informal
Different hypotheses have been considered in the knowledge is a predictor of their success in
explanation of why this informal knowledge seems learning mathematics and should be dr awn upon
so resistant to change . Har t (1981 b) considered the explicitly in school.
possibility that this may rest on the difficulty of the
calculations but the comparisons made by Nunes, 2 Students’ schemas of multiplicative reasoning
Schliemann and Carraher (1993) rule out this develop sufficiently for them to apply these
hypothesis: the difficulty of the calculations was held schemas to numbers, without the need to use
constant across problems of type A and type B , and objects or tallies to represent quantities. But they
quantitative reasoning on the basis of the functional seem to be connected to quantities, and it
relation remained elusive. appears that students do not f ocus on the
relations between quantities in multiplicative
An alternative explanation, explored by Vergnaud reasoning problems. This informal knowledge
(1983) and Har t (personal communication), is that seems to be resistant to change under current
informal strategies are resistant to change because conditions of instruction.
they are connected to reasoning about quantities,
and not about relations. It makes sense to say that if 3 In many previous studies, researchers drew the
I buy half as much fish, I pay half as much money: conclusion that students’ problems in understanding
these are manipulations of quantities and their proportional relations were explained by their
representations. But what sense does it make to difficulties in thinking multiplicatively. Today, it seems
divide kilos of fish by money? more likely that students’ problems are based on
their difficulty in thinking about relations, and not
22 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

about quantities, since even young children succeed (Gombert, 1992; Karmiloff-Smith, 1992), memory
in multiplicative reasoning problems. (Flavell, 1971) and the under standing of others
(Flavell, Green and Flavell, 1990). It is beyond the
4 Teaching approaches might be more successful in scope of this work to review the liter ature on
promoting the formalisation of students’ informal whether representing something does help us
knowledge if: (a) they dr aw on the students’ become more aware of the represented meaning;
informal knowledge rather than ignore it; (b) they we will treat this as an assumption.
offer the students a way of representing the
relations between quantities and promote an The hypothesis concerning the impor tance of
awareness of these relations; and (c) they use a representations will be used in a diff erent form here.
variety of situational contexts to help students Duval (2006) pointed out that ‘the part played by
extend their knowledge to new domains of signs in mathematics, or more exactly by semiotic
multiplicative reasoning. systems of representation, is not only to designate
mathematical objects or to communicate but also to
We examine now the conceptual under pinnings of work on mathematical objects and with them.’ (p.
two rather different teaching approaches to the 107). We have so far discussed the quantif ication of
development of multiplicative reasoning in search relations, and in par ticular of functional relations, as if
for more specific hypotheses regarding how greater the representation of functional relations could onl y be
levels of success can be achiev ed by U.K. students. attained through the use of n umbers. Now we wish to
make explicit that this is not so . Relations, including
The challenge in attempting a synthesis of results is functional relations, can be represented by numbers
that there are many ways of classifying teaching but there are many other ways in which relations can
approaches and there is little systematic research that be represented before a number is attributed to them;
can provide unambiguous evidence. The difficulty is to put it more forcefully, one could say that relations
increased by the fact that by the time students are can be represented in different ways in order to
taught about propor tions, some time between their facilitate the attribution of a number to them.
third and their sixth year in school, they have
participated in a diversity of pedagogical approaches When students are taught to wr ite an equation
to mathematics and might already ha ve distinct of the form a/b = c/x, for example, to represent a
attitudes to mathematics learning. However, we proportions problem in order to solve for x, this
consider it plausible that systematic investigation of formula can be used to help them quantify the
different teaching approaches would prove invaluable relations in the problem. Har t (1981 b) repor ts that
in the analysis of pathways to help children this formula was taught to 100 students in one
understand functional relations. In the subsequent school where she car ried out her investigations of
section, we explore two different pathways by proportions problems but that it was onl y used by
considering the types of representations that are 20 students, 15 of whom were amongst the high
offered to students in order to help them become achievers in the school. This formula can be used
aware of functional relations. to explore both scalar and functional relations in a
proportions problem but it can also be taught as a
rule to solve the problem without any exploration
of the scalar or functional relations that it
Representing symbolises. In some sense , students can lear n to use
functional relations the formula without developing an awareness of the
The working hypothesis we will use in this section nature of the relations between quantities that are
is that in order to become explicitl y aware of assumed when the formula is applied.
something, we need to represent it. This hypothesis
is commonplace in psychological theor ies: it is par t Researchers in mathematics education have been
of general developmental theories, such as Piaget’s aware for at least two decades that one needs to
theory on reflective abstraction (Piaget, 1978; 2001; explore different forms of representation in order to
2008) and Karmiloff-Smith’s theory of seek the best ways to promote students’ awareness
representational re-descriptions in development of reasoning about the relations in a propor tions
(Karmiloff-Smith, 1992; Karmiloff-Smith and Inhelder, problem. It is likely that the lar ge amount of research
1977). It is also used to descr ibe development in on propor tional reasoning, which exposed students’
specific domains such as language and liter acy difficulties as well as their reliance on their o wn
23 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

methods even after teaching, played a crucial role in highlighted the role that dr awing and visualisation
this process. It did undoubtedly raise teachers’ and can play in making children aware of relations. In an
researchers’ awareness that the representation initial paper, van den Br ink and Streefland (1979)
through formulae (a/b = c/x) or algorithms did not analysed a boy’s reactions to propor tions in
work all that well. In this section, we will seek to drawings and also pr imary school children’s
examine the underlying assumptions of two very reactions in the classroom when visual propor tions
different approaches to teaching students about were playfully manipulated by their teacher.
proportions.
The boy’s reactions were taken from a discussion
between the boy and his father. They saw a poster
for a film, where a man is br avely standing on a
Two approaches to the
whale and tr ying to harpoon it. The whale’s size is
representation of functional relations exaggerated for the sake of sensation. The father
Kieren (1994) suggested that there are tw o asked what was wrong with the picture and the boy
approaches to research about, and to the teaching eventually said: ‘I know what you mean. That whale
of, multiplicative reasoning in school. The first is should be smaller. When we were in England we
analytic-functional: it is human in f ocus, and saw an orca and it was onl y as tall as three men’
investigates actions, action schemes and oper ations (van den Brink and Streefland, 1979, p. 405). In line
used in giving meaning to m ultiplicative situations. with Bryant (1974), van den Br ink and Streefland
The second is algebraic: this focuses on mathematical argued that visual propor tions are par t of the basic
structures, and investigates structures used in this mechanisms of perception, which can be used in
domain of mathematics. Although the investigation learning in a var iety of situations, and suggested that
of mathematics structures is not incompatible with this might be an excellent star t for making children
the analytic-functional approach, these are aware of relations between quantities.
alternatives in the choice of star ting point for
instruction. They delineate radically distinct pathways Van den Brink and Streefland then developed
for guiding students’ learning trajectories. classroom activities where six- to eight-y ear-old
children explored propor tional relations in drawings.
Most of the research car ried out in the past about Finally, the teacher showed the children a picture of
students’ difficulties did not describe what sort of a house and asked them to mark their own height
teaching students had par ticipated in; one of the on the door of the house . The children engaged in
exceptions is Har t’s (1981 b) description of the measurements of themselves and the door of the
teaching in one school, where students were taught classroom in order to tr anspose this size relation to
the a/b = c/d, algebraic approach: the vast majority of the drawing and mark their heights on the door. This
the students did not use this f ormula when they were activity generated discussions relevant to the
interviewed about propor tions in her study, and its question of propor tions but it is not possible to
use was confined to the higher achievers in their tests. assess the effect of this activity on their
It is most urgent that a research progr amme that understanding of propor tions, as no assessments
systematically compares these two approaches should were used. The lesson ended with the teacher
be carried out, so that U.K. students can benefit from showing another par t of the same picture: a girl
better understanding of the consequences of ho w standing next to the house . The girl was much taller
these different pathways contribute to learning of than the house and the children concluded that this
multiplicative reasoning. In the two subsequent was actually a doll house. Surprise and playfulness
sections, we present one well developed programme were considered by Streefland an impor tant factor
of teaching within each approach. in children’s engagement in mathematics lessons.

Van den Brink and Streefland suggested that children


can use perceptual mechanisms to reflect about
The analytic-functional approach:
proportions when they judge something to be out of
from schematic representations of proportion in a picture. They argued that it is not only
quantities in correspondence to of psychological interest but also of mathematical-
quantifying relations didactical interest to discover why children can reason
Streefland and his colleagues (Streefland, 1984; 1985 in ratio and proportion terms in such situations,
a and b; van den Br ink and Streefland, 1979) abstracting from perceptual mechanisms.
24 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

Streefland (1984) later developed fur ther activities ingredients for the taste to be preserved when the
in a lesson ser ies with the theme ‘with a giant’s amounts are adjusted. Streefland argued that ‘the
regard’, which star ted with activities that explored ratio table is a per manent record of propor tion as
the children’s informal sense of propor tions and an equivalence relation, and in this way contributes
progressively included mathematical representations to acquiring the correct concept. Applying the ratio
in the lesson. The children were asked, for example, table contributes to the detachment from the
to imagine how many steps would a normal man context… In this quality the r atio table is, as it were,
take to catch up with one of the giant’ s steps; later, a unifying model for a variety of ratio contexts, as
they were asked to represent the man’s and the well as for the various manifestations of ratio…
giant’s steps on a n umber line and subsequently by The ratio table can contribute to discovering,
means of a table. Figure 4.4 presents one example making conscious and applying all proper ties that
of the type of diagr am used for a visual comparison. characterise ratio-preserving mappings and to their
use in numerical problems’ (Streefland, 1985, a, p.
In a later paper, Streefland (1985 a and b) pur sued 91). Ratio tables are then related to gr aphs, where
this theme fur ther and illustrated how the diagrams the relation between two variables can be discussed
used to represent visual meanings could be used in in a new way.
a progressively more abstract way, to represent
correspondences between values in other problems Streefland emphasises that ‘mathematizing reality
that did not have a visual basis. This was illustrated involves model building’ (Streefland, 1985a, p. 86); so
using, among others, Hart’s (1981 b) onion soup students must use their intuitions to dev elop a
problem, where a recipe f or onion soup for 8 model and then lear n how to represent it in order
people is to be adapted f or 4 or 6 people . The to assess its appropriateness. He (Streefland, 1985 b;
diagram proposed by Streefland, which the teacher in van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2003) argued that
should encourage the pupils to constr uct, shows children’s use of such schematic models of situations
both (a) the cor respondences between the values, that they understand well can become a model for
which the children can find using their own, informal new situations that they would encounter in the
building up strategies, and (b) the value of the scalar future. The representation of their knowledge in
transformation. See Figure 4.5 f or an example. such schematic form helps them under stand what is
implied in the model, and make explicit a relation
Streefland suggested that these schematic that they had used onl y implicitly before.
representations could be used later in Har t’s onion
soup problem in a vertical orientation, more This hypothesis is in agreement with psychological
common for tables than the above diagram, and theories that propose that reflection and
with all ingredients listed on the same table in representation help make implicit knowledge explicit
different rows. The top row would list the number (e.g. Karmiloff-Smith, 1992; Piaget, 2001). However,
of people, and the subsequent rows would list each the concept proposes a pedagogical str ategy in
ingredient. This would help students realise that the Streefland’s work: the model is chosen b y the
same scalar transformation is applied to all the teacher, who guides the student to use it and adds

Figure 4.4:The giant’s steps and the man’s steps on a line; this drawing can be converted into a number line and a table which displays the
numerical correspondences between the giant’s and the man’s steps.
25 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

elements, such as the explicit representation of the percentages (Streefland and van den Heuv el-
scalar factor. The model is chosen because it can be Panhuizen, 1992; van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2003)
easily stripped of the specifics in the situation and and fractions (Streefland, 1993; 1997). Marja van den
because it can help the students mo ve from thinking Heuvel-Panhuizen and her colleagues (Middleton
about the context to discussing the mathematical and van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 1995; Middleton, van
structures (van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2003). So den Heuvel-Panhuizen and Shew, 1998) detailed the
children’s informal knowledge is to be tr ansformed use of the r atio table in teaching students in their
into formal knowledge through changes in 3rd year in school about percentages and
representation that highlight the mathematical connecting percentages, fractions and propor tions.
relations that remain implicit when students f ocus
on quantities. In all these studies, the use of the r atio table is seen
as a tool for computation and also f or discussion of
Finally, Streefland also suggested that teaching the different relations that can be quantified in the
children about ratio and propor tions could star t problem situations. Their advice is that teacher s
much earlier in primary school and should be seen should allow students to use the table at their
as a longer project than prescribed by current own level of understanding but always encourage
practice. Star ting from children’s informal knowledge students to make their reasoning explicit. In this way,
is a crucial aspect of his proposal, which is based students can compare their own reasoning with
on Freudenthal’s (1983) and Vergnaud’s (1979) their peers’ approach, and seek to improve their
argument that we need to know about children’s understanding through such compar isons.
implicit mathematical models for problem situations,
not just their ar ithmetic skills, when we want to Streefland’s proposal is consistent with man y of the
develop their problem solving ability. Streefland educational implications that we drew from previous
suggests that, besides the visual and spatial relations research. It star ts from the representation of the
that he worked with, there are other concepts correspondences between quantities and moves to
which children aged eight to ten years can grasp in the representation of relations. It uses schematic
primary school, such as comparisons between the drawings and tables that bring to the fore of each
density (or crowdedness) of objects in space and student’s activity the explicit representation of the
probabilities. Other concepts, such as percentages two (or more) measures that are in volved in the
and fractions, were seen by him as related to problem. It is grounded on students’ informal
proportions, and he ar gued that connections should knowledge because students use their building up
be made across these concepts. However, Streefland solutions in order to constr uct tables and schematic
considered that they mer ited their own analyses in drawings. It systematizes the students’ solutions in
the mathematics classroom. He argued, citing tables and re-represents them by means of gr aphs.
Vergnaud (1979) that ‘different proper ties, almost After exploring students’ work on quantities,
equivalent to the mathematician, are not all students’ attention is focused on scalar relations,
equivalent for the child (Vergnaud, 1979, p. 264). So which they are asked to represent explicitly using the
he also developed programmes for the teaching of same visual records. It draws on a var iety of contexts

Figure 4.5: A table showing the answers that the children can build up and the representation of the scalar transformations.
26 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

that have been previously investigated and which and animals became a higher-level unit. The cells in
students have been able to handle successfully. Finally, the computer screen were linked up with tables,
it uses graphs to explore the linear relations that are which showed the values cor responding to the cells
implied in propor tional reasoning. that had been filled with these composite units: for
example, if 9 cells had been filled in with the iconic
To our knowledge, there is no systematic representations, the table displayed the values for 1
investigation of how this proposal actually works through 9 of the composite units in columns headed
when implemented either exper imentally or in by the icons for umbrellas and animals. Subsequently,
the classroom. The work by Treffers (1987) and students worked with non-integer values f or the
Gravemeijer (1997) on the f ormalisation of students’ ratios between the quantities: for example, they
understanding of multiplication and division focused could be asked to enlarge a shape and the
on the transition from computation with small to corresponding sides of the two figures had a non-
large numbers. The work by van den Heuvel- integer ratio between them (e.g. one figure had a
Panhuizen and colleagues focused on the use of side 21 cm long and the other had the
ratio tables in the teaching of percentages and corresponding side 35 cm long).
equivalence of fractions. In these paper s, the authors
offer a clear descr iption of how teachers can guide Kaput and West’s programme was delivered over 11
students’ transition from their own intuitions to a lessons in two experimental classes, which included
more formal mathematical representation of the 31 students. Two comparison classes, with a total of
situations. However, there is no assessment of how 29 students, followed the instruction previously used
the programmes work and limited systematic by their teachers and adopted from textbooks. One
description of how students’ reasoning changes comparison class had 13 lessons: the first five lessons
as the programmes develop. were based on a textbook and covered exercises
involving ratio and propor tion; the last eight
The approach by researchers at the Freudenthal consisted of computer-based activities using function
Institute is described as developmental research and machines with problems about rate and profit. The
aims at constructing a curriculum that is designed second comparison class had only three lessons; the
and improved on the basis of students’ responses content of these is not descr ibed by the authors.
(Gravemeijer, 1994). This work is crucial to the The classes were not assigned randomly to these
development of mathematics education. However, it treatments and it is not clear ho w the teachers
does not allow for the assessment of the eff ects of were recruited to par ticipate in the study.
specific teaching approaches, as more experimental
intervention research does. It leaves us with the sense At pre-test, the students in the exper imental and
that the key to formalising students’ multiplicative comparison classes did not differ in the percentage
reasoning may be already to hand but we do not of correct solutions in a multiplicative reasoning test.
know this yet. Systematic research at this stage would At post-test, the students in the exper imental group
offer an invaluable contribution to the under standing significantly out-performed those in both compar ison
of how students learn and to education. classes. They also showed a larger increase in the
use of multiplicative strategies than students in the
Streefland was not the onl y researcher to propose comparison classes. It is not possible from Kaput and
that teaching students about multiplicative relations West’s repor t to know whether these were building
should star t from their informal understanding of up, scalar or functional solutions, as they are
the relations between quantities and measures. considered together as multiplicative solutions.
Kaput and West (1994) developed an experimental
programme that took into account students’ building In spite of the limitations pointed out, the study
up methods and sought to formalise them through does provide evidence that students benefit from
connecting them with tables. Their aim was to help teaching that develops their building up strategies
students create composite units of quantity, where into more formalised approaches to solution, by
the correspondences between the measures were linking the quantities represented by icons of objects
represented iconically on a computer screen. For to tables that represent the same quantities. This
example, if in a problem the quantities are 3 result goes against the view that inf ormal methods
umbrellas for 2 animals, the computer screen would are an obstacle to students’ learning in and of
display cells with images f or 3 umbrellas and 2 themselves; it is more likely that they are an obstacle
animals in each cell, so that the group of umbrellas if the teaching they are exposed to does not b uild
27 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

on the students’ informal strategies and does not Adjiage and Pluvinage (2007) ar gue that it is
help students connect what they kno w with the important to separate the physical from the
new forms of mathematical representation that mathematical initially in order to ar ticulate them
the teacher wants them to learn. later, and propose that three r ational registers
should be used to facilitate students’ attainment of
level 3: linear scale (a number line with resources
such as subdividing, sliding along the line , zooming),
The algebraic approach:
fractional writing, and decimal wr iting should be
representing ratios and equivalences used in the teaching of r atio and propor tions.
In contrast to the functional approach to the
teaching of propor tions that was descr ibed in the It seems quite clear to us that this proposal does
previous section, some researchers have proposed not star t from students’ intuitions or str ategies for
that teaching should not star t from students’ solving multiplicative reasoning problems, but rather
understanding of multiplicative reasoning, but from a aims to formalise the representation of physical
formal mathematical definition of propor tions as the situations from the star t and to teach students ho w
equality of two ratios. We found the most explicit to work with these formalisations. The authors
justification for this approach in a recent paper b y indicate that their progr amme is inspired by Duval’s
Adjiage and Pluvinage (2007). Adjiage and Pluvinage, (1995) theory of the role of representations in
citing several authors (Har t, 1981 b); Karplus, Pulos mathematical thinking but we believe that there is
and Stage, 1983; Lesh, Post, and Behr, 1988), argue no necessary link between the theor y and this
that building up strategies are a weak indicator of particular approach to teaching students about
proportionality reasoning and that the link betw een ratio and propor tions.
‘interwoven physical and mathematical
considerations, present in the build-up strategy’ In order to convey a sense for the programme,
(2007, p. 151) should be the representation of Adjiage and Pluvinage (2007) descr ibe five moments
problems through rational numbers. For example, a experienced by students. The researchers worked
mixture that contains 3 par ts concentrate and 2 with two conditions of implementation, which
parts water should be represented as 3/5, using they termed the full exper iment and the par tial
numbers or marks on a number line. The level 1, experiment. Students in the par tial experiment
which corresponds to an iconic representation of the did not par ticipate in the first moment using a
parts used in the mixture , should be tr ansformed computer; they worked with pencil and paper tasks
into a level 2, numerical representation, and students in moments 1 and 2.
should spend time working on such tr ansformations.
Similarly, a scale dr awing of a figure where one side is • Moment 1 The students are presented with three
reduced from a length of 5 cm to 3 cm should be lines, divided into equal spaces. They are told that
represented as 3/5, also allowing for the move from the lines are drawn in different scales. The lines
an iconic to a numerical representation. Finally, the have different numbers of subdivisions – 5, 3 and 4,
representation by means of an equivalence of r atios, respectively. Points equivalent to 3/5, 2/3 and 1/4
as in 3/5 = 6/10, should be introduced, to transform are marked on the line. The students are asked to
the level 2 into a level 3 representation. The same compare the segments from the origin to the point
results could be obtained by using decimals r ather on the line. This is seen as a purely mathematical
than ordinary fractions representations. question, executed in the computer by students in
the full experiment condition. The computer has
In brief, level 1 allows for an ar ticulation between resources such as dividing the lines into equal
physical quantities: the students may realise that a segments, which the students can use to execute
mixture with 3 par ts concentrate and 2 par ts water the task.
tastes the same as another with 6 par ts concentrate
and 4 par ts water. Level 2 allows for articulations • Moment 2 A similar task is presented with paper
between the physical quantities and a mathematical and pencil.
representation: students may realise that two different
situations are represented by the same number. Level • Moment 3 The students are shown two pictures
3 allows for articulations within the mathematical that represent two mixtures: one is made with
domain as well as conversions from one system of 3 cups of chocolate and 2 of milk (the cups are
representation to another : 3/5 = 0.6 or 6/10. shown in the pictures in different shades) and the
28 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

other with 2 cups of chocolate and 1 of milk. The low rate of success in r atio and propor tions
students are asked which mixture tastes more problems: about 13%. At the end of gr ade 7, they
chocolaty. This problem aims to link the physical attained 39% correct answers whereas the students
and the mathematical elements. in the sample from the same region (baseline
group) attained 15% cor rect responses in the
• Moment 4 The students are asked in what way are national assessment. The students in the full
the problems in moments 2 and 3 similar. Students experimental classes obtained significantly better
are expected to show on the segmented line results than those in the par tial experimental
which portion corresponds to the cups of classes but the researcher s did not provide separate
chocolate and which to the cups of milk. percentages for the two groups. Prospective
teachers attained 83% on similar prob lems. The
• Moment 5 This is described as institutionalization in researchers were not satisfied with these results
Douady’s (1984) sense: the students are asked to because, as they point out, the students performed
make abstractions and express rules. For instance, significantly worse than the prospective teachers,
expressions such as these are expected: ‘7 divided who were taken to represent educated adults.
by 4 is equal to seven fourths (7 ÷ 4 = 7/4)’;
‘Given an enlargement in which a 4 cm length Although there are limitations to this study, it
becomes a 7 cm length, then any length to be documents some progress among the students
enlarged has to be multiplied by 7/4.’ (Adjiage and in the experimental classes. However, it is difficult
Pluvinage, 2007, pp. 160–161). to know from their repor t how much time was
devoted to the teaching progr amme over the two
The teaching programme was implemented over years and how this compares to the instr uction
two school years, star ting when the students were received by the baseline group.
in their 6 th year (estimated age about 11 y ears) in
school. A pre-test was given to them before they In brief, this approach assumes that students’ main
started the programme; the post-test was carried difficulties in solving propor tions problems result from
out at the end of the students’ 7th year (estimated their inability to co-ordinate different forms of
age about 12 years) in school. mathematical representations and to manipulate them.
There is no discussion of the question of quantities
Adjiage and Pluvinage (2007) worked with an and relations and there is no attempt to mak e
experienced French mathematics teacher, who students aware of the relations between quantities
taught two classes using their exper imental in the problems. The aims of teaching are to:
programme. In both classes, the students solved • develop students’ understanding of how to use
the same problems but in one class, referred to as number line and numerical representations
a par tial experimental, the students did not use the together in order to compare rational numbers
computer-based set of activities whereas in the • promote students’ reflection on how the numerical
other one, referred to as full exper imental, they and linear representations relate to problem
had access to the computer activities. The teacher situations that involve physical elements
modified only his approach to teaching r atio and (3 cups of chocolate and 2 of milk)
proportions; other topics in the y ear were taught as • promote students’ understanding of the relations
previously, before his engagement in the exper iment. between the different mathematical
representations and their use in solving problems.
The performance of students in these tw o
experimental classes was compared to results A comparison between this example of the
obtained by French students in the same region (the algebraic approach and the functional approach as
baseline group) in a national assessment and also to exemplified by Streefland’s work suggests that this
the performance of non-specialist, prospective algebraic approach does not offer students the
school-teachers on a r atio and propor tions task. The opportunity to distinguish between quantities and
tasks given to the three groups w ere not the same relations. The three forms of representation offered
but the researchers considered them compar able. in the Adjiage and Pluvinage progr amme focus on
quantities; the relations between quantities are left
Adjiage and Pluvinage repor ted positive results implicit. Students are expected to recognise that
from their teaching progr amme. When the pupils in mixtures of concentrate that are numerically
the experimental classes were in grade 6 they had a represented as 3/5 and 6/10 are equivalent. In the
29 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

number line, they are expected to manipulate the be of interest in this context but this research is
representations of quantities in order to compare not reviewed here, as it does not contr ibute to the
them. We found no evidence in the descr iption of discussion of how graphs can be used to help
their teaching programme that students were asked students understand functional relations (for
to think about their implicit models of the situations complementary reviews, see Friel, Curcio and Br ight,
and explicitly discuss the tr ansformations that would 2001; Mevarech and Kr amarsky, 1997). We focus
maintain the equivalences. here on the possibilities of using gr aphs to help
students understand functional relations.

As repor ted earlier in this chapter, Lieven Verschaffel


Summary
and his colleagues have shown that students make
1 It is possible to identify in the liter ature two rather multiplicative reasoning errors in additive situations
different views of how students can best be taught as well as additive errors in multiplicative situations,
about multiplicative reasoning. Kieren identified and so there is a need for students to be offered
these as the functional and the algebr aic approach. opportunities to reflect on the nature of the relation
between quantities in problems. Van Dooren, Bock,
2 The functional approach proposes that teaching Hessels, Janssens and Verschaffel (2004) go as far as
should star t from students’ understanding of suggesting that students fall prey to what they call an
quantities and seek to make their implicit models illusion of linearity, but we think that they have
of relations between quantities explicit. overstated their case in this respect. In fact, some of
the examples that they use to illustrate the so-called
3 The algebraic approach seeks to represent illusion of linearity are indeed examples of linear
quantities with mathematical symbols and lead functions, but perhaps not as simple as the typical
students to work with symbols as soon as linear functions used in school. In two examples of
possible, disentangling physical and mathematical their ‘illusion of linearity’ discussed here, there is a
knowledge. linear function connecting the two variables but the
problem situation is more complex than many of
4 There is no systematic compar ison between these the problems used in schools when students are
two approaches. Because their explicit description taught about ratio and propor tions. In our view,
is relatively recent, this paper is the f irst detailed these problems demonstrate the impor tance of
comparison of their char acteristics and provides working with students to help them reflect about
a basis for future research. the relations between the quantities in the
situations.

Graphs and In one example, taken from Cramer, Post and


functional relations Currier (1993) and discussed ear lier on in this
paper, two girls, Sue and Julie , are supposed to be
The previous sections focused on the visual running on a tr ack at the same speed. Sue star ted
and numerical representations of relations. This first. When she had run 9 laps, Julie had r un 3 laps.
section will briefly consider the question of the When Julie completed 15 laps, how many laps had
representation of relations in the Car tesian plane. Sue run? Although prospective teachers wrongly
We believe that this is a f orm of representation that quantified the relation between the number of laps
merits fur ther discussion because of the additional in a multiplicative way, we do not think that they f ell
power that it can add to students’ reflections, if for the ‘illusion of linearity’, as argued by De Bock,
properly explored. Verschaffel et al., (2002; 2003). The function actually
is linear, as illustrated in Figure 4.6. However, the
Much research on how students interpret graphs intercept between Sue’s and Julie’s numbers of laps
has shown that graph reading has to be learned, just is not at zero, because Sue must have run 6 laps
as one must learn how to read words or numbers. before Julie starts. So the prospective teachers’ error
Similarly to other aspects of mathematics lear ning, is not an illusion of linearity but an inability to deal
students have some ideas about reading gr aphs with intercepts different from zero.
before they are taught, and researchers agree that
these ideas should be considered when one designs Figure 4.6 shows that three different curves would
instruction about graph reading. Several papers can be obtained: (1) if the girls were running at the
30 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

same speed but one star ted before the other, as student overcome the ‘illusion of linearity’ in a
in the Cramer, Post and Currier problem; (2) if second problem, which we argue also involve
one were running faster than the other and this mislabelling of the phenomenon under study. In
difference in speed were constant; and (3) if they several studies, De Bock, Van Dooren and their
started out running at the same speed b ut one girl colleagues (De Bock, Verschaffel and Janssens, 1998;
became progressively more tired whereas the other De Bock, Van Dooren, Janssens and Verschaffel, 2002;
was able to speed up as she war med up. Students De Bock, Verschaffel and Janssens, 2002; De Bock,
might hypothesise that this latter example is better Verschaffel, Janssens, Van Dooren and Claes, 2003)
described by a quadratic than a linear function, if the claim to have identified this illusion in questions
girl who was getting tired w ent from jogging to exemplified in this problem: ‘Farmer Carl needs
walking, but they could find that the quadr atic approximately 8 hours to manure a square piece of
function would exaggerate the difference between land with a side of 200 m. How many hours would
the girls: how could the strong gir l run 25 laps while he need to manure a square piece of land with a
the weak one ran 5? side of 600 m?’ De Bock, Van Dooren and colleagues
worked with relatively large numbers of Belgian
The aim of this illustr ation is to show that relations students across their many studies, in the age r ange
between quantities in the same context can var y and 12 to 16 years. They summarise their findings by
that students can best investigate the nature of the indicating that ‘the vast majority of students (even
relation between quantities is if they ha ve a tool to 16-year-olds) failed on this type of prob lem because
do so. Streefland suggested that tables and graphs of their alarmingly strong tendency to appl y linear
can be seen as tools that allo w students to explore methods’ (Van Dooren, Bock, Hessels, Janssens and
relations between quantities; even though they could Verschaffel, 2004, p. 487) and that even with
be used to help students’ reasoning in this problem, considerable suppor t many students were not
we do not know of research where it has been used. able to overcome this difficulty. Some students
who did become more cautious about o ver-using a
Van Dooren, et al. (2004) used graphs and tables in linear model, subsequently failed to use it when it
an intervention programme designed to help was appropriate.

Figure 4.6:Three graphs showing different relations between the number of laps run by two people over time
31 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

We emphasise here that in this problem, as in the increase by 1 cm in one side corresponds a 4 cm
previous one, students were not falling prey to an increase in the per imeter.
illusion of linearity. The area of a rectangular f igure is
indeed propor tional to its side when the other side is We think that it would be surprising if the students
held constant; this is a case of m ultiple propor tions had made significant progress in under standing the
and thus the linear relation betw een the side and relations between the quantities through the
the area can only be appreciated if the other side instruction that they received in these problems:
does not change. Because the rectangle in their they were not guided to an appropr iate model of
problem is the par ticular case of a square , if one the situation, and worked with one measure , side,
side changes, so does the other ; with both measures instead of two measures, base and height. One of
changing at the same time, the area is not a simple the students remarked at the end of the
linear function of one of the measures. intervention programme, after ten exper imental
lessons over a two week period: ‘I really do
Van Dooren et al. (2004) describe an inter vention understand now why the area of a square increases
programme, in which students used gr aphs and 9 times if the sides are tr ipled in length, since the
tables to explore the relation betw een the measure enlargement of the area goes in two dimensions. But
of the side of a square , its area and its per imeter. The suddenly I star t to wonder why this does not hold
intervention contains interesting examples in which for the perimeter. The perimeter also increases in
students have the oppor tunity to examine diagr ams two directions, doesn’t it?’ (Van Dooren et al., 2004,
that display squares progressively larger by 1 cm, in p. 496). This student seems to have understood that
which the square units (1 cm2) are clearly marked. the increase in one dimension of the square implies
Students thus can see that when the side of a square a similar increase along the other dimension and
increases, for example, from 1 cm to 2 cm, its area that these are multiplicatively related to the area b ut
increases from 1 cm 2 to 4 cm 2, and when the apparently missed the oppor tunity to understand
increase is from 2 cm to 3 cm, the area increases to that sides are additively related to the perimeter.
9 cm2. The graph associated with this table displays a
quadratic function whereas the gr aph associate with In spite of the shor tcomings of this study, the
the perimeter displays a linear function. intervention illustrates that it is possible to relate
problem situations to tables and graphs
Their programme was not successful in promoting systematically to stimulate students’ reflection about
students’ progress: the experimental group significantly the implicit models. It is a cur rent hypothesis by
decreased the rate of responses using simple many researchers (e.g. Carlson, Jacobs, Coe, Larsen
proportional reasoning to the area prob lems but also and Hsu, 2002; Hamilton, Lesh, Lester and Yoon,
decreased the rate of correct responses to perimeter 2007; Lesh, Middleton, Caylor and Gupta, 2008)
problems, although the perimeter of a square is that modelling data, testing the adequacy of models
connected to its sides by a simple propor tion. through graphs, and comparing different model fits
can make an impor tant contribution to students’
We believe that the lack of success of their understanding of the relations between quantities. It
programme may be due not to a lack of is consistently acknowledged that this process must
effectiveness of the use of gr aphs and tables in be carefully designed: powerful situations must be
promoting students’ reasoning but from their use chosen, clear means of hypothesis testing must be
of an inadequate mathematical anal ysis of the available, and appropriate teacher guidance should
problems. Because the gr aphs and tables used only be provided. Shor tcomings in any of these aspects
two variables, measure of the side and measure of of teaching experiments could easily result in
the area, the students had no oppor tunity to negative results.
appreciate that in the area prob lem there is a
proportional relation between area and each the The hypothesis that modelling data, testing the
two sides. The two sides var y at the same time in adequacy of models through graphs, and comparing
the par ticular case of the square b ut in other different model fits can promote student’s
rectangular figures there isn’t a quadr atic relation understanding of different types of relations
between side and area. The relation between sides between quantities seems entirely plausible but, to
and perimeter is additive, not multiplicative: it our knowledge, there is no research to pro vide clear
happens to be multiplicative in the case of the support to it. We think that there are no w many
square because all sides are equal; so to each ideas in the liter ature that can be implemented to
32 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

assess systematically how effective the use of allows them to solve multiplicative reasoning
graphs and tables is as tools to suppor t students’ problems. We suggest that students’ problems with
understanding of the different types of relation that proportional reasoning stems from their difficulties
can exist between measures. This research has the in becoming explicitly aware of relations between
potential to make a huge contribution to the quantities. This awareness would help them
improvement of mathematics education in the distinguish between situations that involve different
United Kingdom. types of relations: additive, propor tional or
quadratic, for example.

5 Multiplicative reasoning problems are defined


Conclusions by the fact that they in volve two (or more)
and implications measures linked by a fixed ratio. Students’ informal
This review has identified results in the domain of knowledge of multiplicative reasoning stems from
how children learn mathematics that have significant the schema of one-to-many correspondence,
implications for education. The main points are which they use both in multiplication and division
highlighted here. problems. When the product is unknown, children
set the elements in the tw o measures in
1 Children form concepts about quantities from correspondence (e.g. 1 sweet costs 4p) and figure
their everyday experiences and can use their out the product (how much 5 sweets will cost).
schemas of action with diverse representations When the correspondence is unknown (e.g. if you
of the quantities (iconic, numerical) to solve pay 20p for 5 sweets, how much does each sweet
problems. They often develop sufficient awareness cost), the children share out the elements (20p
of quantities to discuss their equivalence and shared in 5 groups) to f ind what the
order as well as how they can be combined. correspondence is.

2 It is significantly more difficult for them to become 6 This informal knowledge is currently ignored in
aware of the relations between quantities and U.K. schools, probably due to the theor y that
operate on relations. Even after being taught how multiplication is essentially repeated addition and
to represent relations, they often inter pret the division is repeated subtr action. However, the
results of operations on relations as if they connections between addition and multiplication,
were quantities. Children find both additive and on one hand, and subtraction and division, on the
multiplicative relations significantly more difficult other hand, are procedural and not conceptual.
than understanding quantities. So students’ informal knowledge of multiplicative
reasoning could be developed in school from an
3 There is little evidence that the design of earlier age.
instruction has so far tak en into account the
importance of helping students become a ware 7 A considerable amount of research car ried out
of the difference between quantities and relations. independently in different countries has shown
Some researchers have carried out experimental that students sometimes use additive reasoning
teaching studies that suggest that it is possib le to about relations when the appropr iate model is
promote students’ awareness of relations. Fur ther a multiplicative one. Some recent research has
research must be carried out to analyse how this shown that students also use m ultiplicative
knowledge affects mathematics learning. If positive reasoning in situations where the appropr iate
results are found, there will be strong policy model is additive. These results suggest that
implications. children use additive and multiplicative models
implicitly and do not mak e conscious decisions
4 Previous research had led to the conclusion that regarding which model is appropr iate in a specific
students’ problems with propor tional reasoning situation. The educational implication from these
stemmed from their difficulties with multiplicative findings is that schools should tak e up the task of
reasoning. However, there is presently much helping students become more aware of the
evidence to show that, from a relatively early age models that they use implicitly and of ways of
(about five to six years in the United Kingdom), testing their appropriateness to par ticular
children already have informal knowledge that situations.
33 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

8 Proportional reasoning stems from children’s use relations but much more research is needed to
of the schema of one-to-many correspondences, show how students’ thinking changes if they do
which is expressed in calculations as b uilding-up learn to use gr aphs in order to anal yse the type
strategies. Evidence suggests that many students of relation that is most relevant in specif ic
who use these str ategies are not aware of situations.
functional relations that characterises a linear
function. This result reinforces the impor tance of
the role that schools could pla y in helping students
become aware of functional relations in
proportions problems.

9 Two radically different approaches to teaching


proportions and linear functions in schools can be
identified in the liter ature. One, identified as
functional and human in focus, is based on the
notion that students’ schemas of action should be
the star ting point for this teaching. Through
instruction, they should become progressively
more aware of the scalar and functional relations
that can be identified in such problems. Diagrams,
tables and graphs are seen as tools that could help
students understand the models that they are
using of situations and mak e them into models f or
other situations later. The second approach,
identified as algebraic, proposes that there should
be a sharp separation between students’ intuitive
knowledge, in which physical and mathematical
knowledge are inter twined, and mathematical
knowledge. Students should be led to
formalisations early on in instr uction and re-
establish the connections between mathematical
structures and physical knowledge at a later point.
Representations using fractions, ordinary and
decimal, and the number line are seen as the tools
that can allow students to abstr act early on from
the physical situations. There is no unambiguous
evidence to show how either of these approaches
to teaching succeeds in promoting students’
progress, nor that either of them is more
successful than the less clear ly ar ticulated ideas
that are implicit in cur rent teaching in the
classroom. Research that can clar ify this issue is
urgently needed and could have a major impact by
promoting better learning in U.K. students.

10 Students need to lear n to read gr aphs in order


to be able to use them as tools f or thinking
about functions. Research has shown that
students have ideas about how to read gr aphs
before instruction and that these ideas should be
taken into account when gr aphs are used in the
classroom. It is possible to teach students to read
graphs and to use them in order to think about
34 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

References

Adjiage, R., and Pluvinage, F. (2007). An experiment Confrey, J. (1994). Splitting, Similarity and Rate of
in teaching ratio and propor tion. Educational Change: A New Approach to Multiplication
Studies in Mathematics, 65, 149-175. and Exponential Functions. In G. Harel & J.
Beaton, A. E., Mullis, I. V. S., Mar tin, M. O., Gonzalez, E. Confrey (Eds.), The Development of
J., Kelly, D. L., & Smith, T. A. (1996). Mathematics Multiplicative Reasoning in the Lear ning of
achievement in the middle school y ears: IEA’s Third Mathematics (pp. 293-332). Albany, New York:
International Mathematics and Science Study State University of New York Press.
(TIMSS). Chestnut Hill, MA, USA: TIMSS Cramer, K., Post, T., and Currier, S. (1993). Learning
International Study Center, Boston College. and teaching ratio and propor tion: research
Becker, J. (1993). Young children’s numerical use of implications. In D. T. Owens (Ed.), Research ideas
number words: counting in many-to-one for the classroom: Middle grades mathematics (pp.
situations. Developmental Psychology, 19, 458-465. 159-178). New York: Macmillan.
Behr, M. J., Harel, G., Post, T., and Lesh, R. (1994). Units De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., and Janssens, D. (1998).
of quantity: A conceptual basis common to additive The predominance of the linear model in
and multiplicative structures. In G. Harel and J. secondary school students’ solutions of word
Confrey (Eds.), The Development of Multiplicative problems involving length and area of similar
Reasoning in the Learning of Mathematics (pp. 121- plane figures. Educational Studies in Mathematics,
176). Albany (NY): SUNY Press. 35, 65-83.
Booth, L. R. (1981). Child-Methods in Secondary De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., and Janssens, D. (2002).
Mathematics. Educational Studies in Mathematics , The effects of different problem presentations
12, 29-41. and formulations on the illusion of linearity in
Brown, M. (1981). Number operations. In K. Hart secondary school students. Mathematical Thinking
(Ed.), Children’s Understanding of Mathematics: and Learning, 4, 65-89.
11-16 (pp. 23-47). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson. De Bock, D., Van Dooren, W., Janssens, D., and
Bryant, P. (1974). Perception and Understanding in Verschaffel, L. (2002). Improper use of linear
Young Children. London and Co. Ltd.: Methuen. reasoning: an in-depth study of the nature and
Carlson, M., Jacobs, S., Coe, E., Larsen, S., and Hsu, E. the irresistibility of secondar y school students’
(2002). Applying covariational reasoning while errors. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 50,
modeling dynamic events. Journal for Research in 311-334.
Mathematics Education, 33, 352–378. De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., Janssens, D., Dooren, W.,
Carpenter, T. P., Ansell, E., Franke, M. L., Fennema, E., and Claes, K. (2003). Do realistic contexts and
and Weisbeck, L. (1993). Models of Problem graphical representations always have a beneficial
Solving: A Study of Kinder garten Children’s impact on students’ performance? Negative
Problem-Solving Processes. Journal for Research in evidence from a study on modelling non-linear
Mathematics Education, 24, 428-441. geometry problems. Learning and Instruction, 13,
Carpenter, T. P., Hiebert, J., and Moser, J. M. (1981). 441-463.
Problem structure and first grade children’s initial Douady, R. (1984). Jeux de cadres et dialectique outil-
solution processes for simple addition and objet dans l’enseignement des mathématiques , une
subtraction problems. Journal for Research in réalisation de tout le cur sus primaire. Unpublished,
Mathematics Education, 12, 27-39. Doctorat d’état, Paris.
35 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Duval, R. (1995). Sémiosis et pensée humaine. Bern: Inhelder, B. and. Piaget, J. (1958). The Growth of Logical
Peter Lang. Thinking for Childhood to Adolescence. London:
Duval, R. (2006). A cognitive analysis of problems of Routledge. and Kegan Paul.
comprehension in a lear ning of mathematics. Kaput, J., and West, M. M. (1994). Missing-Value
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 61, 103-131. proportional reasoning problems: Factors
Elliott, C. D., Smith, P., and McCulloch, K. (1997). affecting informal reasoning patterns. In G. Harel
British Ability Scale-II Technical Manual. London: and J. Confrey (Eds.), The Development of
nferNelson Publishing Company. Multiplicative Reasoning in the Lear ning of
Flavell, J. H. (1971). What is memor y development Mathematics (pp. 237-292). Albany, New York:
the development of? Human Development, 14, State University of New York Press.
272-278. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: a
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1990). developmental perspective on cognitive science.
Developmental Changes in young children’s Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
knowledge about the mind. Cognitive Karmiloff-Smith, A., and Inhelder, B. (1977). ‘If you
Development, 5, 1-27. want to get ahead, get a theor y’. In P. N. Johnson-
Freudenthal, H. (1983). Didactical Phenomenology of Laird and P. C. Wason (Eds.), Thinking. Readings in
Mathematical Structures. Dordrecht: D. Reidel. Cognitive Science (pp. 293-306). Cambridge:
Friel, S. N., Curcio, F. R., and Bright, G. W. (2001). Cambridge University Press.
Making Sense of Graphs: Critical Factors Karplus, R., and Peterson, R. W. (1970). Intellectual
Influencing Comprehension and Instr uctional development beyond elementary school II: ratio,
Implications. Journal for Research in Mathematics a survey. School-Science and Mathematics, 70,
Education, 32, 124-158. 813-820.
Frydman, O., & Br yant, P. E. (1988). Sharing and the Karplus, R., Pulos, S., and Stage, E. K. (1983).
understanding of number equivalence by young Proportional reasoning of early adolescents. In R.
children. Cognitive Development, 3, 323-339. Lesh and M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of
Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic Development. mathematics concepts and processes (pp. 45-90).
Hemel Hempstead: Harverster Wheatsheaf. London: Academic Press.
Gravemeijer, K. (1994). Educational Development Kieren, T. (1994). Multiple Views of Multiplicative
and Developmental Research in Mathematics Structures. In G. Harel and J. Confrey (Eds.), The
Education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Development of Multiplicative Reasoning in the
Education, 25, 443-471. Learning of Mathematics (pp. 389-400). Albany,
Gravemeijer, K. (1997). Mediating between concrete New York: State University of New York press.
and abstract. In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.), Kouba, V. (1989). Children’s solution strategies for
Learning and Teaching Mathematics. An equivalent set multiplication and division word
International Perspective (pp. 315-346). Hove problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics
(UK): Psychology Press. Education, 20, 147-158.
Hamilton, E., Lesh, R., Lester, F., and Yoon, C. (2007). Lamon, S. (1994). Ratio and Proportion: Cognitive
The use of reflection tools in b uilding personal Foundations in Unitizing and Nor ming. In G. Harel
models of problem-solving. In R. Lesh, E. and J. Confrey (Eds.), The Development of
Hamilton and J. Kaput (Eds.), Foundations for the Multiplicative Reasoning in the Lear ning of
future in mathematics education (pp. 349–366). Mathematics (pp. 89-122). Albany, New York:
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. State University of New York Press.
Hart, K. (1981 a). Measurement. In K. Har t (Ed.), Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice. Cambridge:
Children’s Understanding of Mathematics: 11-16 Cambridge University Press.
(pp. 9-22). London: John Murray. Lesh, R., Middleton, J. A., Caylor, E., and Gupta, S.
Hart, K. (1981 b). Ratio and propor tion. In K. Har t (2008). A science need: Designing tasks to
(Ed.), Children’s Understanding of Mathematics: 11- engage students in modeling complex data.
16 (pp. 88-101). London: John Murray. Educational Studies in Mathematics, in press.
Hart, K. (1984). Ratio: children’s strategies and errors. A Lesh, R., Post, T., and Behr, M. (1988). Propor tional
report of the strategies and errors in secondar y reasoning. In J. Hieber t and M. Behr (Eds.),
mathematics project. Windsor: NFER-Nelson. Number Concepts and Operations in the Middle
Hudson, T. (1983). Correspondences and numerical Grades (pp. 93-118). Reston, VA: National Council
differences between sets. Child Development, 54, of Teachers of Mathematics.
84-90.
36 PaperSUMMARY
4: Understanding
– PAPERrelations
2: Understanding
and their whole
graphical
numbers
representation

Lewis, A., and Mayer, R. (1987). Students’ Piaget, J., Grize, J., Szeminska, A., and Bangh, V. (1977).
miscomprehension of relational statements in Epistemology and the Psychology of Functions .
arithmetic word problems. Journal of Educational Dordrecth, Netherlands: D. Reidel.
Psychology, 79, 363-371. Piaget, J., and Inhelder, B. (1975). The Origin of the
Mevarech, Z. R., and Kramarsky, B. (1997). From Idea of Chance in Children . London: Routledge and
Verbal Descriptions to Graphic Representations: Kegan Paul.
Stability and Change in Students’ Alternative Pickering, S., and Gathercole, S. (2001). Working
Conceptions. Educational Studies in Mathematics , memory test batter y for children (WMTB-C)
32, 229-263. Manual. London: The Psychological Corporation.
Middleton, J. A., and Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. Resnick, L. B. (1983). Mathematics and Science
(1995). The ratio table. Mathematics Teaching in Learning: A New Conception. Science, 220, 477-
the Middle School, 1, 282-288. 478.
Middleton, J. A., Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M., and Ricco, G. (1982). Les première acquisitions de la
Shew, J. A. (1998). Using bar representations as a notion de fonction linèaire chez l’enfant de 7 à
model for connecting concepts of r ational 11 ans. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 13,
number. Mathematics Teaching in the Middle 289-327.
School, 3, 302-311. Schwartz, J. (1988). Intensive quantity and referent
Noelting, G. (1980 a). The development of transforming arithmetic operations. In J. Hieber t
proportional reasoning and the ratio concept and M. Behr (Eds.), Number concepts and
Part I - Differentiation of stages. Educational operations in the middle grades (pp. 41-52).
Studies in Mathematics, 11, 217-253. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Noelting, G. (1980 b). The development of Steffe, L. (1994). Children’s multiplying schemes. In G.
proportional reasoning and the ratio concept Harel and J. Confrey (Eds.), The Development of
Part II - Problem-structure at successive stages: Multiplicative Reasoning in the Lear ning of
Problem-solving strategies and the mechanism of Mathematics (pp. 3-40). Albany, New York: State
adaptative restructuring. Educational Studies in University of New York Press.
Mathematics, 11, 331-363. Streefland, L. (1984). Search for the Roots of Ratio:
Nunes, T., & Br yant, P. (1996). Children Doing Some Thoughts on the Long Term Learning
Mathematics. Oxford: Blackwell. Process (Towards...A Theory): Part I: Reflections
Nunes, T., Light, P., and Mason, J. (1993). Tools for on a Teaching Experiment. Educational Studies in
thought: the measurement of lenght and area. Mathematics, 15, 327-348.
Learning and Instruction, 3, 39-54. Streefland, L. (1985 a). Search for the Roots of Ratio:
Nunes, T., Schliemann, A. D., and Carraher, D. W. Some Thoughts on the Long Term Learning
(1993). Street Mathematics and School Process (Towards...A Theory): Part II: The Outline
Mathematics. New York: Cambridge University of the Long Term Learning Process. Educational
Press. Studies in Mathematics, 16, 75-94.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P., Evans, D., Bell, D., Gardner, S., Streefland, L. (1985 b). Wiskunde als activiteit en de
Gardner, A., & Carraher, J. N. (2007). The realiteit als bron. Nieuwe Wiskrant, 5, 60-67.
Contribution of Logical Reasoning to the Lear ning Streefland, L. (1993). Fractions: A Realistic Approach.
of Mathematics in Pr imary School. British Journal In T. P. Carpenter, E. Fennema and T. A. Romberg
of Developmental Psychology, 25, 147-166. (Eds.), Rational Numbers: An Integration of
Park, J., & Nunes, T. (2001). The development of the Research (pp. 289-326). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence
concept of multiplication. Cognitive Development, Erlbaum Associates.
16, 1-11. Streefland, L. (1997). Charming fractions or fractions
Piaget, J. (1952). The Child’s Conception of Number. being charmed? In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (Eds.),
London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Learning and Teaching Mathematics. An
Piaget, J. (1978). The Grasp of Consciousness. London: International Perspective (pp. 347-372). Hove
Routledge Kegan Paul. (UK): Psychology Press.
Piaget, J. (2001). Studies in reflecting abstraction: Streefland, L., and Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M.
translated by R. Campbell. Hove: Psychology Press. (1992). Evoking pupils’ informal knowledge on
Piaget, J. (2008). Intellectual Evolution from percents. Paper presented at the Proceedings of
Adolescence to Adulthood. Human Development, the Sixteenth PME Conference, University of
51(Reprint of Human Development 1972; New Hampshire, Durham, NH.
15:1–12), 40-47.
37 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Thompson, P. W. (1993). Quantitative Reasoning,


Complexity, and Additive Structures. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 25, 165-208.
Thompson, P. (1994). The Development of the
Concept of Speed and Its Relationship to
Concepts of Rate. In G. Harel and J. Confrey (Eds.),
The Development of Multiplicative Reasoning in the
Learning of Mathematics (pp. 181-236). Albany,
New York: State University of New York Press.
Tourniaire, F., and Pulos, S. (1985). Propor tional
reasoning: A review of the liter ature. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 16 181-204.
Treffers, A. (1987). Integrated column arithmetic
according to progressive schematisation.
Educational Studies in Mathematics , 18 125-145.
Van den Brink, J. and Streefland, L. (1979). Young
Children (6-8): Ratio and Propor tion. Educational
Studies in Mathematics, 10, 403-420.
Van de Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (2003). The Didactical
Use of Models in Realistic Mathematics Education:
An Example from a Longitudinal Trajectory on
Percentage. Realistic Mathematics Education
Research: Leen Streefland’s Work Continues.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 54, 9-35.
Van Dooren, W., Bock, D. D., Hessels, A., Janssens, D.,
and Verschaffel, L. (2004). Remedying secondar y
school students’ illusion of linear ity: a teaching
experiment aiming at conceptual change .
Learning and Instruction, 14, 485–501.
Vergnaud, G. (1979). The Acquisition of Arithmetical
Concepts. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 10,
263-274.
Vergnaud, G. (1983). Multiplicative structures. In R.
Lesh and M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of
Mathematics Concepts and Processes (pp. 128-
175). London: Academic Press.
Vergnaud, G. (1994). Multiplicative Conceptual Field:
What and Why? In G. Harel & J. Confrey (Eds.), The
Development of Multiplicative Reasoning in the
Learning of Mathematics (pp. 41-60). Albany, New
York: State University of New York press.
Vergnaud, G. (1997). The nature of mathematical
concepts. In T. Nunes & P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning
and Teaching Mathematics. An International
Perspective (pp. 1-28). Hove (UK): Psychology Press.
Verschaffel, L. (1994). Using Retelling Data to Study
Elementary School Children’s Representations
and Solutions of Compare Problems. Journal for
Research in Mathematics Education, 25, 141-165.
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-71-9

www.nuffieldfoundation.org
5
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 5: Understanding space and
its representation in mathematics
By Peter Bryant, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Summary of Paper 5 3
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: Understanding space and its
representation in mathematics 7
Paper 1: Overview
Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and References 37
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities About the author
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical Peter Bryant is Senior Research Fellow in the
representation Department of Education, University of Oxford.
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation
in mathematics
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning About the Nuffield Foundation
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
concepts charitable trust established in 1943 by William
Paper 8: Methodological appendix Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant being. We fund research and pr actical
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while experiment and the development of capacity
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics to under take them; working across education,
in secondary schools. science, social science and social policy. While
most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which responsive grant programmes we also
has been published separately as Introduction and undertake our own initiatives.
summary of findings.

Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published


together as Summary papers.

All publications are available to download from


our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary of paper 5:
Understanding space
and its representation
in mathematics

Headlines
• Children come to school with a great deal of • An important aspect of learning about geometry
knowledge about spatial relations. One of the most is to recognise the relation between transformed
important challenges in mathematical education is shapes (rotation, reflection, enlargement). This
how best to harness this implicit knowledge in also can be difficult, since children’s pre-school
lessons about space. experiences lead them to recognise the same
shapes as equivalent across such transformations,
• Children’s pre-school implicit knowledge of space is rather than to be aware of the nature of the
mainly relational. Teachers should be aware of kinds transformation. However, there is very little
of relations that young children recognise and are research on this important question.
familiar with, such as their use of stable background
to remember the position and orientation of • Another aspect of the understanding of shape is the
objects and lines. fact that one shape can be transformed into another,
by addition and subtraction of its subcomponents.
• Measuring of length and area poses particular For example, a parallelogram can be transformed
problems for children, even though they are into a rectangle of the same base and height by the
able to understand the underlying logic of addition and subtraction of equivalent triangles and
measurement. Their difficulties concern iteration adding two equivalent triangles to a rectangle creates
of standard units, which is a new idea for them, a parallelogram. Research demonstrates that there is
and also the need to apply multiplicative reasoning a danger that children might learn about these
to the measurement of area. transformations only as procedures without
understanding their conceptual basis.
• From an early age children are able to extrapolate
imaginary straight lines, which allows them to learn • There is a severe dearth of psychological research
how to use Cartesian co-ordinates to plot specific on children’s geometrical learning. In particular we
positions in space with no difficulty. However, they need long-term studies of the effects of intervention
need instruction about how to use co-ordinates to and a great deal more research on children’s
work out the relation between different positions. understanding of transformations of shape.

• Learning how to represent angle mathematically is a At school, children often lear n formally about matters
hard task for young children, even though angles are that they already know a great deal about in an
an important part of their everyday life.There is informal and often quite implicit wa y. Sometimes their
evidence that children are more aware of angle in the existing, informal understanding, which for the most
context of movement (turns) than in other contexts part is based on exper iences that they start to have
and learn about the mathematics of angle relatively long before going to school, fits well with what they
easily in this context. However, children need a great are expected to lear n in the classroom. At other
deal of help from to teachers to understand how to times, what they know already, or what they think
relate angles across different contexts. they know, clashes with the formal systems that they
4 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

are taught at school and can ev en prevent them from knowledge into formal geometrical understanding.
grasping the significance of these formal systems. Yet, it is not al ways that easy. It is an unf ortunate and
well-documented fact that many children have
Geometry is a good and an obvious example. persistent difficulties with many aspects of geometr y.
Geometry lessons at school deal with the use of
mathematics and logic to analyse spatial relations and One evidently successful link between young
the properties of shapes. The spatial relations and the children’s early spatial knowledge and their more
shapes in question are cer tainly a common par t of formal experiences in the classroom is their lear ning
any child’s environment, and psychological research how to use Car tesian co-ordinates to plot positions
has established that from a very early age children are in two-dimensional space. This causes schoolchildren
aware of them and quite familiar with them. It has little difficulty, although it takes some time for them
been shown that even very young babies not only to understand how to work out the relation
discriminate regular geometric shapes but can recognise between two positions plotted in this wa y.
them when they see them at a tilt, thus co-ordinating
information about the orientation of an object with Other links between informal and formal knowledge
information about the pattern of its contour s. are harder for young children. The apparently simple act
of measuring a straight line, for example, causes them
Babies are also able to extrapolate imaginary straight problems even though they are usually perfectly able to
lines (a key geometric skill) at any rate in social make the appropriate logical moves and understand
situations because they can work out what someone the importance of one-to-one correspondence, which
else is looking at and can thus constr uct that person’s is an essential par t of relating the units on a r uler to
line of sight. Another major early achievement by the line being measured. One problem here is that
young children is to master the logic that under lies they find it hard to under stand the idea of iter ation:
much of the formal analysis of spatial relations that iteration is about repeated measurements, so that a
goes on in geometr y. By the time they f irst go to ruler consists of a set of iter ated (repeated) units like
school young children can make logical transitive centimetres. Iteration is necessar y when a par ticular
inferences (A > B , B > C , therefore A > C; A = B, B length being measured is longer than the measur ing
= C, therefore A = C), which are the logical basis of instrument. Another problem is that the one-to-one
all measurement. In their first few years at school correspondence involved in measuring a line with a
they also become adept at the logic of inversion (A ruler is asymmetrical. The units (centimetres, inches) are
+ B – B), which is a logical mo ve that is an essential visible and clear in the r uler but have to be imagined
part of studying the relation betw een shapes. on the line itself. It is less of a sur prise that it also takes
children a great deal of time to come to ter ms with
Finally, there is strong evidence that most of the the fact that measurement of area usuall y needs some
information about space that children use and form of multiplication, e.g. height x width with
remember in their everyday lives is relational in rectangles, rather than addition.
nature. One good index of this is that children’ s
memory of the or ientation of lines is lar gely based The formal concept of angle is another serious
on the relation between these lines and the stumbling block for children even though they are
orientation of stable features in the background. For familiar enough with angles in their everyday spatial
this reason children find it much easier to remember environments. The main problem is that they find it
the orientation of horizontal and vertical lines than hard to grasp that two angles in very different contexts
of diagonal lines, because horizontal and vertical are the same, e.g. themselves turning 90o and the
features are quite common in the child’s stable corner of a page in a book. Abstraction is an essential
spatial environment. For the same reason, young part of geometr y but it has very little to do with
children remember and reproduce r ight angles children’s ordinary spatial perception and knowledge.
(perpendicular lines) better than acute or obtuse
angles. The relational nature of children’s spatial For much the same reason, decomposing a relatively
perception and memor y is potentially a powerful complex shape into several simpler component
resource for learning about geometry, since spatial shapes – again an essential activity in geometr y – is
relations are the basic subject matter of geometr y. something that many children find hard to do. In their
ordinary lives it is usually more impor tant for them to
With so much relevant informal knowledge about see shapes as unities, rather than to be able to break
space and shape to dr aw on, one might think that them up into other shapes. This difficulty makes it hard
children would have little difficulty in translating this for them to work out relationships between shapes.
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

For example, children who easily grasp that a + b – b research, however, concentrated on shor t-term gains
= a, nevertheless often fail to under stand completely in children’s geometric understanding and did not
the demonstration that a rectangle and a answer the question whether these ear ly teaching
parallelogram with the same base and height are programmes would actually help children when they
equal in area because you can transform the begin to learn about geometr y in the classroom.
parallelogram into the rectangle by subtracting a
triangle from one end of the parallelogram and adding There has also been research on teaching children
an exactly equivalent triangle to the other end. about angle, mostly in the context of computer-
based teaching programmes. One of the most
We know little about children’s understanding of interesting points to come out of this research is that
transformations of shape or of an y difficulties that teaching children about angle in ter ms of movements
they might have when they are taught about these (turning) is successful, and there is some evidence
transformations. This is a ser ious gap in research on that children taught this way are quite likely to
children’s mathematical learning. It is well recognised, transfer their new knowledge about angle to other
however, that children and some adults confuse scale contexts that do not involve movement.
enlargements with enlargements of area. They
think that doubling the length of the contour of a However, there has been no concer ted research
geometric shape such as a square or a rectangle also on how teachers could take advantage of children’s
doubles its area, which is a serious misconception. considerable spatial knowledge when teaching them
Teachers should be aware of this potential difficulty geometry. We badly need long-term studies of
when they teach children about scale enlar gements. interventions that take account of children’s
relational approach to the spatial en vironment and
Researchers have been more successful in identifying encourage them to gr asp other relations, such as the
these obstacles than in showing us how to help relation between shapes and the relation between
children to surmount them. There are some studies shapes and their subcomponent par ts, which go
of ways of preparing children for geometry in the beyond their informal spatial knowledge.
pre-school period or in the ear ly years at school. This

Recommendations

Research about mathematical Recommendations for teaching


learning and research

Children’s pre-school knowledge of space Teaching Teachers should be aware of the research on
is relational. They are skilled at using stab le children’s considerable spatial knowledge and skills and should
features of the spatial fr amework to relate their teaching of geometr ical concepts to this
perceive and remember the relative knowledge.
orientation and position of objects in the Research There is a ser ious need for longitudinal research
environment. There is, however, no on the possible connections between children’s pre-school
research on the relation between this spatial abilities and how well they learn about geometr y
informal knowledge and how well children at school.
learn about geometr y.

Children already understand the logic of Teaching The conceptual basis of measurement and not just
measurement in their ear ly school years. the procedures should be an impor tant par t of the teaching.
They can make and understand transitive Teachers should emphasise tr ansitive inferences, inversion of
inferences, they understand the inverse addition and subtraction and also one-to-one cor respondence
relation between addition and subtr action, and should show children their impor tance.
and they can recognise and use one-to- Research Psychologists should extend their research
one correspondence. These are three on transitive inference, inversion and one-to-one
essential aspects of measurement. correspondence to geometrical problems, such as
measurement of length and area.
6 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

Recommendations (continued)

Research about mathematical Recommendations for teaching


learning and research

Many children have difficulties with the Teaching Teachers should recognise this difficulty and
idea of iteration of standard units in construct exercises which involve iteration, not just with
measurement. standard units but with familiar objects lik e cups and hands.
Research Psychologists should study the exact cause of
children’s difficulties with iteration.

Many children wrongly apply additive Teaching In lessons on area measurement, teachers can
reasoning, instead of multiplicative promote children’s use of the reasoning ‘number in a row
reasoning, to the task of measur ing area. times number of rows’ by giving children a number of tiles that
Children understand this multiplicative is insufficient to cover the area. They should also contrast
reasoning better when they first think of it measurements which do, and measurements which do not,
as the number of tiles in a ro w times the rest on multiplication.
number of rows than when they tr y to
use a base times height formula.

Even very young children can easily Teaching Teachers, using concrete mater ial, should relate
extrapolate straight lines and teaching about spatial co-ordinates to children’s everyday
schoolchildren have no difficulty in learning experiences of extrapolating imaginary straight lines.
how to plot positions using Car tesian Research There is a need f or intervention studies on
co-ordinates, but it is difficult for them to methods of teaching children to w ork out the relation
work out the relation between different between different positions, using co-ordinates.
positions plotted in this way.

Research on pre-school inter vention Research There will have to be long-ter m predictive and
suggests that it is possible to prepare long-term intervention studies on this cr ucial, but neglected,
children for learning about geometr y by question
enhancing their understanding of space
and shapes. However, this research has not
included long-term testing and therefore
the suggestion is still tentative.

Children often learn about the relation Teaching Children should be taught the conceptual reasons
between shapes (e.g. between a for adding and subtr acting shape components when studying
parallelogram and a rectangle) as a the relation between shapes.
procedure without understanding the Research Existing research on this topic was done a v ery
conceptual basis for these transformations. long time ago and was not v ery systematic. We need well-
designed longitudinal and inter vention studies on children’s
ability to make and understand such transformations.

There is hardly any research on children’s Teaching Teachers should be aware of the r isk that children
understanding of the tr ansformation of might confuse scale enlar gements with area enlar gements.
shapes, but there is evidence of confusion Research Psychologists could easily study how children
in many children about the effects of understand transformations like reflection and rotation b ut
enlargement: they consider that doubling they have not done so. We need this kind of research.
the length of the per imeter of a square ,
for example, doubles its area.
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Understanding space
and its representation
in mathematics

From informal understanding to


formal misunderstanding of space
This paper is about children’s informal knowledge of Piaget, Inhelder and Szeminska’s (1960) impressive
space and spatial relations and about their f ormal pioneering work on children’s understanding of
learning of geometr y. It also deals with the geometry, which we shall describe later, psychologists
connection between these two kinds of knowledge. have vir tually ignored this aspect of children’ s
This connection is much the same as the one education since then. In contrast, mathematics
between knowledge about quantitative relations on educators have made steady progress in studying
the one hand and about n umber on the other hand, children’s geometry with measures of what children
which we described in Papers 1 and 2. We shall show find difficult and studies of the eff ects of different
how young children build up a lar ge and impressive, kinds of teaching and classroom exper ience.
but often implicit, understanding of spatial relations
before they go to school and ho w this knowledge One effect of this imbalance in the contr ibution of
sometimes matches the relations that they lear n in the two disciplines to research on lear ning about
geometry very well and sometimes does not. geometry is that the existing research tells us
more about educational methods than about the
There is a r ich vein of research on children’s underlying difficulties that children have in learning
spatial knowledge – knowledge which they acquire about geometry. Another result is that some
informally and, for the most par t, long before they go excellent ideas about enhancing children’s
to school – and this research is ob viously relevant to geometrical understanding have been proposed by
the successes and the difficulties that they have when educationalists but are still waiting f or the kind of
they are taught about geometry at school. Yet, with a empirical test that psychologists are good at
few honourable exceptions, the most remar kable of designing and carrying out.
which is a recent thorough review b y Clements and
Sarama (2007 b), there have been very few attempts The central problem for anyone trying to make the
indeed to link research on children’ s informal, and link between children’s informal spatial knowledge
often implicit, knowledge about spatial relations to and their understanding of geometr y is easy to state .
their ability to car ry out the explicit anal yses of It is the star k contrast between children’s impressive
space that are required in geometr y classes. everyday understanding of their spatial environment
and the difficulties that they have in learning how
The reason for this gap is probably the striking to analyse space mathematically. We shall star t
imbalance in the contr ibution made by psychologists our review with an account of the basic spatial
and by maths educators to research on geometr ical knowledge that children acquire inf ormally long
learning. Although psychologists have studied before they go to school.
children’s informal understanding of space in detail
and with great success, they have vir tually ignored
children’s learning about geometr y, at any rate in
recent years. Despite Wertheimer’s (1945) and
8 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

Early spatial knowledge: they do far better when it is possib le to solve the
perception problem on the basis of siz e relations (e.g. it is
always the smaller one) than when they ha ve to
remember its absolute size (e.g. it is always exactly
Shape, size, position and extrapolation so large) (Lawrenson and Br yant, 1972).
of imagined straight lines
Another remarkable early spatial achievement by
Spatial achievements begin early. Over the last 30 infants, which is also relational and is highl y relevant
years, experimental work with young babies has to much of what they later ha ve to learn in
clearly shown that they are born with, or acquire geometry lessons, is their ability to extr apolate
very early on in their life, many robust and effective imaginary straight lines in three dimensional space
perceptual abilities. They can discriminate objects by (Butterworth, 2002). Extrapolation of imagined
their shape, by their size and by their orientation and straight lines is, of course, essential for the use of
they can perceive depth and distinguish differences in Cartesian co-ordinates to plot positions in gr aphs
and in maps, but it also is a basic ingredient of very
distance (Slater, 1999; Slater and Lewis 2002; Slater,
young children’s social communication (Butterworth
Field and Hernadez-Reif, 2002; Bremner, Bryant and
and Cochrane, 1980; Butterworth and Grover, 1988).
Mareschal, 2006).
Butterworth and Jarrett (1991) showed this in a
They can even co-ordinate information about size and study in which they ask ed a mother to sit opposite
distance (Slater, Mattock and Brown, 1990), and they her baby and then to stare at some predeter mined
can also co-ordinate information about an object’s object which was either in front and in full view of
shape and its orientation (Slater and Morrison, 1985). the child or was behind the child, so that he had to
The first co-ordination makes it possible for them to turn his head in order to see it. The question was
recognise a par ticular object, which they first see close whether the baby would then look at the same
up, as the same object when they see it again in the object, and to do this he would have to extrapolate
distance, even though the size of the visual impression a straight line that represented his mother’ s line of
that it now makes is much smaller than it was before. sight. Butterworth and Jarrett found that babies
With the help of the second kind of co-ordination, younger than 12 months manage to do this most of
babies can recognise par ticular shapes even when the time when the object in question was in front of
they see them from completely different angles: the them. They usually did not also tur n their heads to
shape of the impression that these objects mak e on look at objects behind them when these apparently
the visual receptors varies, but babies can still caught their mothers’ attention. But 15-month-old
recognise them as the same by taking the change in children did even that: they followed their mother’s
orientation into account. line of sight whether it led them to objects already
in full view or to ones behind them. A slightly later
We do not yet know how children so young are development that also involves extrapolating
capable of these impressive feats, but it is quite lik ely imaginary straight lines is the ability to point and to
that the answer lies in the relational nature of the look in the direction of an object that someone else
way that they deal with siz e (and, as we shall see is pointing at, which infants manage do with great
later, with orientation), as Rock (1970) suggested proficiency (Butterworth and Morisette, 1996;
many years ago. A person nearby makes a larger Butterworth and Itakura, 2000).
visual impression on your visual system than a
person in the distance b ut, if these two people are
roughly the same size as each other, the relation
Orientation and position
between their size and that of familiar objects near
each of them, such as car s and bus-stops and The orientation of objects and surfaces are a
wheelie-bins, will be much the same. significant and highly regular and predictable par t of
our everyday spatial environments. Walls usually are,
The idea that children judge an object’ s size in terms and usually have to be, vertical: objects stay on
of its relation to the siz e of other objects at the horizontal surfaces but tend to slide off sloping
same distance receives some suppor t from work surfaces. The surface of still liquid is hor izontal: the
on children’s learning about relations. When four- opposite edges of many familiar manufactured
year-old children are asked to discriminate and objects (doors, windows, television sets, pictures,
remember a par ticular object on the basis of its siz e, book pages) are par allel: we ourselves are vertical
9 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

when we walk, horizontal when we swim. Yet, relations. It is that that there are usually ample stable
children seem to have more difficulty distinguishing horizontal and vertical features in the background to
and remembering information about orientation than relate these lines to. Stable, background features that
information about other familiar spatial variables. parallel par ticular lines which are not either v ertical
or horizontal are much less common. If this idea is
Horizontals and verticals are not the problem. right, young children are already relying on spatial
Five-year-old children take in and remember the relations that are at the hear t of Euclidean geometr y
orientation of horizontal and vertical lines extremely to store information about the spatial environment
well (Bryant 1969, 1974; Bryant and Squire, 2001). In by the time that they begin to be taught f ormally
contrast, they have a lot of difficulty in remembering about geometry.
either the direction or slope of ob liquely oriented
lines. There is, however, an effective way of helping However, children do not al ways adopt this excellent
them over this difficulty with oblique lines. If there are strategy of relating the orientation of lines to
other obliquely oriented lines in the background that permanent features of the spatial environment.
are parallel to an oblique line that they are ask ed to Piaget and Inhelder’s (1963) deser vedly famous and
remember, their memor y of the slope and direction often-repeated experiment about children dr awing
for this oblique line improves dramatically (see Figure the level of water in a tilted container is the best
5.1). The children use the par allel relation between example. They showed the children tilted glass
the line that they have to remember and stable containers (glasses, bottles) with liquid in them
features in the background fr amework to store and (though the containers were tilted, the laws of
recognise information about the oblique line. nature dictated that the level of the liquid in them
was horizontal). They also gave the children a picture
This result suggests a reason for the initial r adical of an empty, tilted container depicted as just abo ve
difference in how good their memor y is for vertical a table top which was an ob vious horizontal
and horizontal features and how poor it is for background feature. The children’s task was to dr aw
obliquely oriented ones. The reason, again, is about in the level of the liquid in the dr awing so that it was

Child sees a line Short delay Child shown two


lines and judges

Easy

Difficult

Easy

Figure 5.1: Children easily remember horizontal or vertical lines but not oblique lines unless they can relate oblique lies to a
stable background feature.
10 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

exactly like the liquid in the exper imenter’s hand. The objects that are usually vertical, such as trees and
question that Piaget and Inhelder ask ed was whether chimneys. In the pictures that the children had to
they would draw the liquid as par allel to the table copy, these objects were positioned on obliquely
top or, in other words, as horizontal. oriented surfaces: the trees stood vertically on the
side of a steeply sloping hill and vertical chimneys
Children below the age of roughl y eight years did were placed on sloping roofs. In their copies of
not manage to do this. Many of them drew the liquid these pictures, children younger than about eight
as perpendicular to the sides (when the sides were years usually drew the trees and chimneys as
straight) and parallel to the bottom of the container. perpendicular to their baselines (the side of the hill
It seems that the children could not tak e advantage or the sloping roof) and theref ore with an oblique
of the parallel relation between the liquid and the orientation. Piaget and Inhelder concluded that
table top, probably because they were preoccupied children of this age have not yet realised that the
with the glass itself and did not manage to shift their space around them is full of stab le vertical and
attention to an exter nal feature. horizontal features.

Piaget and Inhelder treated the y oung child’s There is something of a conflict betw een the two sets
difficulties in this dr awing task as a failure on the of results that we have just presented. One (Br yant
child’s par t to notice and tak e advantage of a basic 1969, 1974; Bryant and Squire, 2001) suggests that
Euclidean relation, the parallel relation between young children detect, and indeed rely on, parallel
two horizontal lines. They argued that a child who relations between objects in their immediate
makes this mistake does not have any idea about perception and stable background features. The other
horizontality: he or she is unaware that horizontal (Piaget and Inhelder, 1963) leads to the conclusion
lines and surfaces are an impor tant par t of the that children completely disregard these relations.
environment and that some surfaces, such as still However, this is not a ser ious problem. In the first set
liquid, are constantly horizontal. of experiments the use that children made of parallel
relations was probably implicit. The second set of
Piaget and Inhelder then extended their ar gument to experiments involved drawing tasks, in which the
verticality. They asked children to copy pictures of children had to make explicit judgements about such

When children see 2-line figure A and are asked to copy in the missing line on B either by placing
or drawing a straight wire, they represent the line as nearer to the perpendicular than it is

Figure 5.2: The perpendicular bias


11 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

relations. Children probably perceive and make use of stretch between posts by measuring the line that we
parallel relations without being aware of doing so. The have and the distance between the posts. We compare
implication for teaching children is an interesting one . the two lengths, the length of the line and the distance
It is that one impor tant task for the teacher of from one post to the other indirectl y, by comparing
geometry is to transform their implicit knowledge both directly to the same measur ing instrument – a
into explicit knowledge. tape measure or r uler. We combine two direct
comparisons to make an indirect comparison.
There is another point to be made about the
children’s mistakes in Piaget and Inhelder’s studies. When we put two pieces of information together in
One possible reason, or par tial reason, for these this way in order to produce a new conclusion, we
mistakes might have been that in every case (the are making a logical inference. Inferences about
liquid in a tilted container, trees on the hillside , continua, like length, are called transitive inferences.
chimneys on the sloping roofs) the task was to dr aw We, adults, know that if A = B in length and B = C ,
the crucial feature as non-perpendicular in relation then A is necessarily the same length as C , even
to its baseline. There is plenty of evidence (Ib botson though we have never seen A and C together and
and Bryant, 1976) that, in copying, children find it therefore have not been able to compare them
quite difficult to draw one straight line that meets directly. We also know, of course, that if A > B and B
another straight line, the baseline, when the line that > C (in length), then A > C, again without making a
they have to draw is obliquely oriented to that direct comparison between A and C . In these
baseline (see Figure 5.2). inferences B is the independent measure through
which A and C can be compared.
They tend to misrepresent the line that they are
drawing either as per pendicular to the baseline or Piaget, Inhelder and Szeminska (1960) were the first
as closer to the per pendicular than it should be . to discuss this link between understanding logic and
There are various possible reasons for this being able to measure in their w ell-known book on
‘perpendicular error’, but at the very least it shows geometry. They argued that the main cause of the
that children have some difficulty in representing difficulties that children have in learning about
non-perpendicular lines. The work by Piaget et al. measurement is that they do not understand
establishes that the presence of stab le, background transitive inferences. These authors’ claim about the
features of the spatial environment, like the table importance of transitive inferences in learning about
top, does not help children surmount this bias. measurement is indisputable and an extremely
important one. However, their idea that young
children cannot make or understand transitive
Early spatial knowledge: inferences has always been a controversial one, and
logic and measurement it is now clear that we must make a fundamental
distinction between being able to make the inference
and knowing when this inference is needed and how
Inferences about space and to put it into effect.
measurement
There are usually two consecutive par ts to a
The early spatial achievements that we have transitive inference task. In the first, the child is given
described so far are, broadly speaking, perceptual two premises (A = B , B = C) and in the second he
ones. Our next task is to consider ho w young or she has to tr y to draw an inference from these
children reason about space . We must consider premises. For example, in Piaget’s first study of
whether young children are able to make logical transitive inferences, which was not about length b ut
inferences about space and can under stand other about the behaviour of some fictional people, he first
people’s inferential reasoning about space by the told the children that ‘Mary is naughtier than Sar ah,
age when they first go to school. and Sarah is naughtier than Jane’ and then asked then
‘Who was the naughtier, Mary or Jane?’ Most children
We can star t with spatial measurements. These below the age of roughly nine years found, and still
depend on logical inferences about space. do find, this an extremely difficult question and often
Measurement allows us to make comparisons between say that they cannot tell. The failure is a dr amatic one,
quantities that we cannot compare directly. We can but there are at least tw o possible reasons for it.
work out whether a washing line is long enough to
12 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

One, favoured by Piaget himself, that the failure is a various possible measurement instruments, such as
logical one – that children of this age simpl y cannot strips of paper and a str aight stick, to help her with
put two premises about quantity together logicall y. the task, and the main question that they ask ed was
It is worth noting that Piaget thought that the reason whether the child would use any of these as
that young children did not make this logical move measures to compare the two towers.
was that they could not conceive that the middle
term (B when the premises are A > B and B > C) Children under the age of (roughl y) eight years did
could simultaneously have one relation to A and not take advantage of the measur ing instruments.
another, different, relation to C . Either they tried to do the task by remembering the
original while creating the replica, which did not
The second possible reason for children not making work at all well, or they used their hands or their
the transitive inference is about memor y. The body as a measur ing instrument. For example, some
children may be unable to make the inference simply children put one hand at the bottom and the other
because they have forgotten, or because they did at the top of the or iginal tower and then walked to
not bother to commit to memory in the first place, the other tower trying at the same time to k eep
one or both of the premises. The implication here is their hands at a constant distance from each other .
that they would be able to make the inference if This strategy, which Piaget et al. called ‘manual
they could remember both premises at the time that transfer’, tended not to be successful either, for the
they were given the inferential question. practical reason that the children also had to use and
move their hands to add and subtr act bricks to their
One way to test the second h ypothesis is to make own tower. Older children, in contrast, were happy
sure that the children in the study do remember to use the str ips of paper or the do wel rod as a
the premises, and also to tak e the precaution of makeshift ruler to compare the two towers. Piaget
measuring how well they remember these premises et al. claimed that the children who did not use the
at the same time as testing their ability to dr aw a measuring instruments failed the task because they
transitive inference. Bryant and Trabasso (1971) did were unable to reason about it logicall y. They also
this by repeating the information about the premises argued that children’s initial use of their own body
in the first par t of the task until the children had was a transitional step on the way to true
learned it thoroughly, and then in the second measurement using an ‘independent middle term’.
part checking how well they remembered this
information and testing how well they could answer This might be too pessimistic a conclusion. There is
the inferential questions at the same time . In this an alternative explanation for the reactions of the
study even the four-year-olds were able remember children who did not attempt to use a measure at
the premises and they managed to put them all. It is that children not only have to be able to
together successfully to make the correct transitive make an inference to do well in any measuring task:
inference on 80% of the tr ials. The equivalent figure they also have to realise that a direct compar ison
for the five-year-olds was 89%. will not do, and thus that instead they should make
an indirect, inferential, comparison with the help of a
Young children’s success in this inferential task reliable intervening measure.
suggests that they have the ability to mak e the
inference that underlies measurement, but we still There is some evidence to suppor t this idea. If it is
have to find out how well they apply this ability to right, children should be ready to measure in a task
measurement itself. Here, the research of Piaget et in which it is made completel y obvious that direct
al. (1960) on measurement provides some comparisons would not work. Bryant and Kopytynska
interesting suggestions. These researchers showed (1976) devised a task of this sor t. First, they gave a
children a tower made of br icks of different sizes. group of five- and six-year-old children a version of
The tower was placed on a small tab le and each Piaget et al.’s two towers task, and all of them failed.
child was Then, in a new task, they gave the children two
asked to build another tower of the same height blocks of wood, each with a hole sunk in the middle
on another lower table that was usually, though in such a way that it was impossible to see how
not always, on the other side of the other side of a deep either hole was. They asked the children to find
partition, so that the child had to create the replica out whether the two holes were as deep as each
without being able compare it directly to the original other or not. The children were also given a rod with
tower. Piaget et al. also provided the child with coloured markings. The question was whether the
13 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

children, who did not measure in Piaget et al.’s task, 1 metre is made up of 100 iter ations of 1
would star t to use a measure in this new task in centimetre, and one kilometre consists of 1000
which it was clear that a direct compar ison would be iterations of 1 metre . Children’s first insight into this
useless. iterative system, according to Piaget et al., comes
from their initial exper iences with R < T problems.
Nearly all the children used the rod to measure This is an interesting causal h ypothesis that has some
both holes in the blocks of wood at least once (they serious educational implications. It should be tested.
were each given four problems) and over half the
children measured and produced the r ight answer in
all four problems. It seems that children of f ive years
Conclusions about children’s early
or older are ready to use an inter vening measure to
make an indirect compar ison of two quantities. Their
spatial knowledge
difficulty is in knowing when to distr ust direct • Children have a well-developed and effective
comparisons enough to resor t to measurement. relational knowledge of shape, position, distance,
spatial orientation and direction long before they
go to school. This knowledge may be implicit and
non-numerical for the most part, but it is certainly
Iteration and measurement
knowledge that is related to geometry.
One interesting variation in the study of measurement
by Piaget et al. (1960) was in the length of the str aight • The mistakes that children make in drawing
dowel rod, which was the main measur ing tool in this horizontal and vertical lines are probably due to
task. The rod’s length equalled the height of the preferring to concentrate on relations between
original tower (R = T) in some problems but in others lines close to each other (liquid in a glass is
the rod was longer (R > T) and in others still it was perpendicular to the sides of the glass) rather than
shorter (R < T) than the tower. to separated lines (liquid in a glass is parallel to
horizontal surfaces like table tops). This is a mistake
The older children who used the rod as a measure not in relational perception, but in picking the right
were most successful when R = T. They were slightly relation.
less successful when R > T and they had to mar k a
point on the rod which coincided with the summit • Children are also able to understand and to make
of the tower. In contrast, the R < T problems were transitive inferences, which are the basic logical
particularly difficult, even for the children who tr ied move that underlies measurement, several years
to use the rod as a measure . The solution to such before being taught about geometry.
problems is iteration which, in this case, is to apply
the rod more than once to the to wer: the child has • We do not yet know how well they can cope with
to mark a point to represent the length of the r uler the notion of iteration in the school years.
and then to star t measuring again from this point.
• There is no research on the possible causal links
It is worth noting that iter ation also involves a great between these impressive early perceptual and
deal of care in its ex ecution. You must cover all the logical abilities and the successes and difficulties that
surface that you are measuring, all its length in these children have when they first learn about geometry.
examples, but you must never overlap – never This is a serious gap in our knowledge about
measure any par t of the surface twice . geometrical learning.

Iteration in measurement is interesting because


the people who do it successfull y are actually
constructing their own measure and therefore
certainly have a strong and effective understanding
of measurement. Piaget et al. (1960) also argued that
children’s eventual realisation that iteration is the
solution to some measur ing problems is the basis
for their eventual understanding of the role of
standardised units such as centimetres and metres.
We use these units, they argued, in an iter ative way:
14 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

The connections between on these early skills and enhance them in var ious
children’s knowledge of space ways that will help them lear n about geometr y when
before being taught geometry the time comes.
and how well they learn when
Here the situation is r ather different. Educators have
they are taught about geometry produced systematic programmes to prepare
children for formal instruction in geometr y. Some of
these are ingenious and convincing, and they deser ve
To what extent does children’s early spatial attention. The problem in some cases is a lack of
development predict their success in geometr y later on? empirical evaluation.
The question is simple , clear and overwhelmingly
important. If we were dealing with some other One notable programme comes from the highl y
school subject – say learning to read – we would respected Freudenthal Institute in the Nether lands.
have no difficulty in finding an answer, perhaps more A team of educational researcher s there (van den
than one answer, about the impor tance of early, Heuven-Panhuizen and Buys, 2008) have produced
informal learning and experience, because of the an ingenious and or iginal plan for enhancing
very large amount of work done on the subject. children’s geometric skills before the age when they
With geometry, however, it is different. Having would normally be taught in a f ormal way about the
established that young children do have a rich and subject. We shall concentrate here on the
in many ways sophisticated understanding of their recommendations that van den Heuven-Panhuizen
spatial environment, psychologists seem to have and Buys make for introducing kindergarten children
made their excuses and left the room. Literally to some basic geometr ical concepts. However, we
hundreds of longitudinal and inter vention studies shall begin with the remar k that, though their
exist on what children already know about language recommendations deserve our serious attention, the
and how they learn to read and spell. Yet, as far as Freudenthal team offer us no empir ical evidence at
we know, no one has made a systematic attempt, in all that they really do work. Neither inter vention
longitudinal or inter vention research, to link what studies with pre-tests and post-tests and r andomly
children know about space to how they learn the selected treatment groups, nor longitudinal predictive
mathematics of spatial relations, even though there projects, seem to have played any par t in this
are some extremely interesting and highly specific particular initiative.
questions to research.
The basic theoretical idea behind the Freudenthal
To take one example, what connections are there team’s programme for preparing children for
between children’s knowledge of measurement geometry is that children’s everyday life includes
before they learn about it and how well they learn experiences and activities which are relevant to
to use and under stand the use of r ulers? To take geometry but that the geometr ic knowledge that
another, we know that children have a bias towards kindergarten children glean from these exper iences
representing angles as more per pendicular than they is implicit and unsystematic . The solution that the
are: what connection is there betw een the extent of team offers is to give these young children a
this bias and the success that children ha ve in systematic set of enjoyable game-like activities with
learning about angles, and is the relation a positive or familiar material and after each activity is f inished to
a negative one? These are practicable and immensely discuss and to encour age the children to reflect on
interesting questions that could easil y be answered in what they have just done.
longitudinal studies. It is no longer a matter of what
is to be done . The question that baffles us is: why are Some of these activities are about measurement
the right longitudinal and inter vention studies not (Buys and Veltman, 2008). In one interesting example ,
being done? a teacher encourages the children to find out how
many cups of liquid would fill a par ticular bottle and,
How can we inter vene to prepare young children in when they have done that, to work out how many
the pre-school period for geometr y? If there is a cups of liquid the bottle would provide when the
connection between the remarkable spatial bottle is not completely full. This leads to the idea of
knowledge that we find in quite young children putting marks on the bottle to indicate when it
and their successes and failures in lear ning about contains one or more cups’ worth of liquid. Thus, the
geometry later on, it should be possible to work children experience measurement units and also
15 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

iteration. In another measurement activity children someone has to establish, through empirical
use conventional measures. They are given three research, how right they are.
rods each a metre long and are ask ed to measure
the width of the room. Typically the children star t There are a few empirical studies of ways of
well by forming the rods into a straight 3-metre line, improving spatial skills in pre-school children. In these
but then hit the problem of measuring the remaining the children are given pre-tests which assess how
space: their first reaction is to ask f or more rods, but well they do in spatial tasks which are suitable for
the teacher then provides the suggestion that instead children of that age , then go through inter vention
they try moving the first rod ahead of the third and sessions which are designed to increase some of
then to move the second rod: Buys and Veltman these skills and finally, soon after the end of this
report that the children readil y follow this suggestion teaching, they are given post-tests to measure
and apparently understand the iteration involved improvement in the same skills.
perfectly well.
Two well designed studies car ried out by Casey and
Other exercises, equally ingenious, are about her colleagues take this form (Casey, Erkut, Ceder
constructing and operating on shapes (van and Young, 2008; Casey, Andrews, Schindler, Kersh and
den Heuvel-Panhuizen, Veltman, Janssen and Young, 2008). In both studies the researcher s were
Hochstenbach, 2008). The Freudenthal team use interested in how well five- and six-year-old children
the device of folding paper and then cutting out can learn to compose geometric shapes by
shapes to encourage children to think about the combining other geometric shapes and how well they
relationship between shapes: cutting an isosceles decompose shapes into component shapes, and also
triangle across the fold, for example, creates a whether it is easier to impro ve this par ticular skill
regular parallelogram when the paper is unf olded. when it is couched in the context of a stor y than
The children also play games that take the form of when the context is a more f ormal and abstract one.
four children creating a f our-part figure between
them with many symmetries: each child produces The results of these two studies showed that the
the mirror-image of the figure that the previous special instruction did, on the whole , help children to
child had made (see Figure 5.3). The aim of such compose and decompose shapes and did have an
games is to give children systematic exper ience of effect on related spatial skills in the children who
the transformations, rotation and reflection, and to were taught in this way. They also showed that the
encourage them to reflect on these transformations. narrative context added to the eff ect of teaching
children at this age . Recently, Clements and Sar ama
If this group of researcher s is right, children’s early (2007 a) repor ted a very different study of slightly
knowledge of geometric relationships and younger, nursery children. These researchers were
comparisons, though implicit and unsystematic, plays interested in the effects of a pre-school progr amme,
an impor tant par t in their eventual learning about called Building Blocks, the aim of which is to prepare
geometry. It is a resource that can be enhanced b y children for mathematics in general including
sensitive teaching of the kind that the Freudenthal geometry. This programme is based on a theor y
group has pioneered. They may be right, but about children’s mathematical development: as far as

Figure 5.3: An activity devised by van den Heuvel-Panhuizen,Veltman, Janssen and Hochstenbach: four children devise a four-part
shape by forming mirror-images.
16 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

geometry is concerned Clement and Sar ama’s 5 There is little research on the possib le causal links
strongest interest is in children’s awareness of the between these impressive early perceptual and
composition of shapes and the relationship betw een logical abilities and the successes and diff iculties
different shapes. They also believe that the actual that children have when they first learn about
teaching given to individual children should be geometry. This is a ser ious gap in our knowledge
determined by their developmental levels. Thus the about geometrical learning.
day-to-day instruction in their progr amme depends
on the children’s developmental trajectories.
Clements and Sarama repor t that the young children
taught in the Building Blocks progr ammes improved
Learning about geometry
from pre-test to post-test more r apidly than children The aim of teaching children geometry is to show
taught in other ways in tasks that involved them how to reason logically and mathematically
constructing or relating shapes. about space, shapes and the relation between
shapes, using as tools conventional mathematical
These are interesting conclusions and a good star t. measures for size, angle, direction, orientation and
However, research on the question of the eff ects position. In geometr y classes children lear n to
of intervention programmes designed to prepare analyse familiar spatial exper iences in entirely new
children for geometry need to go fur ther than this. ways, and the exper ience of this novel and explicit
We need studies of the eff ects of pre-school kind of analysis should allow them to perceive and
interventions on the progress that children mak e understand spatial relationships that they knew
when they are eventually taught geometr y at school nothing about before.
a few years later on. We cannot be sure that the
changes in the children’s skills that were detected in In our view the aspects of anal ysing space
these studies would have anything to do with their geometrically that are new to children coming to
successes and failures later on in geometr y. the subject for the first time are:
• representing spatial relations which are already
familiar to them, like length, area and position, in
Summary numbers
• learning about relations that are new to them, at
1 Children have a well-developed and effective any rate in terms of explicit knowledge, such as
relational knowledge of shape, position, distance, angle
spatial orientation and direction long bef ore they • forming new categories for shapes, such as
go to school. This knowledge may be implicit and triangles, and understanding that the properties of
non-numerical for the most par t, but it is cer tainly a figure depends on its geometric shape
knowledge that is related to geometry. • understanding that there are systematic relations
between shapes, for instance between rectangles
2 The mistakes that children make in drawing and parallelograms
horizontal and vertical lines are probably due to • understanding the relation between shapes across
them preferring to concentrate on relations transformations, such as rotation, enlargement and
between lines close to each other (liquid in a glass changes in position.
is perpendicular to the sides of the glass) r ather
than to separated lines (liquid in a glass is par allel
to horizontal surfaces like table tops). This is a Applying numbers to
mistake not in relational perception, but in picking familiar spatial relations
the right relation.
and forming relations
3 Children are also able to understand and to make
between different shapes
transitive inferences, which are the basic logical
move that underlies measurement, several years Length measurement
before being taught about geometr y.
Young children are clear ly aware of length. They
4 We do not yet know how well they can cope with know that they grow taller as they grow older, and
the notion of iter ation in the school years. that some people live closer to the school than
others. However, putting numbers on these changes
17 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

and differences, which is one of their first geometric which was 6 cm. Thus even at the comparatively late
feats, is something new to them. age of 11 years, after several years experience of
using rulers, many children seemed not to
Standard units of measurement are equal subdivisions understand, or at any rate not to under stand
of the measuring instrument, and this means that perfectly, that it is the n umber of units on the r uler
children have to understand that this instr ument, a that the line cor responded to that decided its length.
ruler or tape measure or protr actor, is not just a
continuous quantity but is also subdivided into units A study by Nunes, Light and Mason (1993) gives us
that are exactly the same as each other. The child has some insight into this apparentl y persistent difficulty.
to understand, for example, that by using a r uler, she These experimenters asked pairs of six- to eight-
can represent an object’s length through an iter ation year-old children to work together in a measuring
of measurement units, like the centimetre. task. They gave both children in each pair a piece of
paper with a str aight line on it, and the pair’s task
When children measure, for example, the length of a was to find out whether their lines w ere the same
straight line, they must relate the units on the r uler to length or, if not whose was longer and whose
the length that they are measur ing, which is a one-to- shorter. Neither child could see the other’ s line
one correspondence, but of a relatively demanding because the two children did the task in separate
form. In order to see that the measured length is, for rooms and could only talk to each other o ver a
example, 10 cm long, they have to understand that telephone. Both children in each pair w ere given a
the length that they are measur ing can also be measure to help them compare these lines and the
divided into the same unit and that ten of the units only difference between the pairs was in the
on the ruler are in one-to-one cor respondence with measures that the experimenters gave them.
ten imaginary but exactly similar units on what is
being measured. This is an active form of one-to-one The pairs of children were assigned to three groups.
correspondence, since it depends on the children In one group, both children in each pair w ere given a
understanding that they are converting a continuous string with no markings: this therefore was a measure
into a discontinuous quantity by dividing it into without units. In a second group, each child in every
imaginary units. Here, is a good example of ho w even pair was given a standard r uler, marked in centimetres.
the simplest of mathematical analysis of space makes In the third group the children w ere also given rulers
demands on children’s imagination: they must imagine marked in centimetres, but, while one of the children
and impose divisions on undivided quantities in order had a standard r uler, the other child in the pair was
to create the one-to-one cor respondence which is given a ‘broken’ ruler: it star ted at four centimetres.
basic to all measurement of length. The child with the broken ruler could not produce
the right answer just by reading out the number from
Measuring a straight line with a r uler is probably the the ruler which coincided with the end of the line. If
simplest form of measurement of all, but children the line was, for example, 7 cm long, that number
even make mistakes with this task and their mistak es would be 11 cm. The children in these pairs had to
suggest that they do not at f irst grasp that measuring pay particular attention to the units in the ruler.
the line takes the form of imposing one-to-one
correspondence of the units on the measure with The pairs in the second group did w ell. They came
imagined units on the line . This was cer tainly up with the cor rect solution 84% of the time . The
suggested by the answers that a large number of few mistakes that they made were mostly about
children who were in their first three years of placing the ruler or counting the units. Some children
secondary school (11-, 12-, 13- and 14-year-olds) aligned one endpoint with the 1 cm point on the
gave to a question about the length of a str aight line, ruler rather than the 0 cm point and thus
which was par t of a test devised b y Har t, Brown, overestimated the length by 1 cm. This suggests that
Kerslake, Küchemann and Ruddock (1985). The they were wrongly concentrating on the boundar ies
children were shown a picture of straight line beside between units rather than the units themselves.
a ruler that was mar ked in centimetres. One end of Teachers should be aware that some children have
the line was aligned with the 1 cm mar k on the r uler this misconception. Nevertheless the ruler did, on
and the other end with the 7 cm mar k. The children the whole, help the children in this study since those
were asked how long the line was and, in the who worked with complete r ulers did much better
youngest group, almost as many of them (46%) gave than the children who were just given a string to
the answer 7 cm as gave the right answer (49%) measure with.
18 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

However, the broken ruler task was more difficult. the number of units in the par t of the ruler that is
The children in the standard r uler group were right in correspondence to the line. Thus measurement
84%, and those in the brok en ruler group 63%, of of length is a one-to-one correspondence
the time. Those who got it r ight despite having a problem, and the cor respondence is between units
broken ruler either counted the units or read off the that are displayed on the r uler but have to be
last number (e.g. 11 cm for a 7 cm line) and then imagined on the line itself. This act of imagination
subtracted 4, and since they managed to do this seem obvious and easy to adults b ut may not be
more often than not, their performance established so for young children.
that these young children have a considerable
amount of understanding of how to use the units in 4 Tests and experiments in which the line being
a measuring instrument and of what the units mean. measured is not aligned with z ero show that
However, on 30% of the tr ials the children in this initially children do not completel y understand
group seemed not to under stand the significance of how measurement is based on imagining one-to-
the missing first four centimetres in the r uler. Either one correspondence of iterated units.
they simply read off the n umber that matched the
line’s endpoint (11 cm f or 7 cm) or they did not
subtract the right amount from it.
Measurement of area: learning about
A large-scale American study (Kloosterman, Warfield,
the relationship between the areas of
Wearne, Koc, Martin and Strutchens, 2004; Sowder, different shapes
Wearne, Martin and Strutchens, 2004) later confirmed There is a str iking contrast between young children’s
this striking difficulty. In this study, the children had to apparently effortless informal discriminations of size
judge the length of an object which was pictured just and area and the difficulties that they have in learning
above a ruler, though neither of its endpoints was how to analyse and measure area geometr ically.
aligned with the zero endpoint on that r uler. Less than Earlier in this chapter we repor ted that babies are
25% of the 4 th graders (nine-year-olds) solved the able to recognise objects by their size and can do so
problem correctly and only about 60% of the 8 th even when they see these objects at diff erent
graders managed to find the correct answer. This distance on different occasions. Yet, many children find
strong result, combined with those repor ted by Nunes it difficult at first to measure or to under stand the
et al., suggests that many children may know how to area of even the simplest and most regular of shapes.
use a standard r uler, but do not fully understand the
nature or structure of the measurement units that they All the intellectual requirements f or understanding
are dealing with when they do measure . Their mistake, how to measure length, such as knowing about
we suggest, is not a misunder standing of the function transitivity, iteration, and standardised units, apply as
of a ruler: it is a failure in an activ e form of one-to-one well to measuring area. The differences are that:
correspondence – in imagining the same units on the
line as on the r uler and then counting these units. • area is necessarily a more complex quantity to
measure than length because now children have to
learn to consider and measure two dimensions and
to co-ordinate these different measurements. The
Summary
co-ordination is always a multiplicative one (e.g.
1 Measuring a straight line with a r uler is a procedure base x height for rectangles; πr2 for circles etc.).
and it is also a consider able intellectual feat.
• the standardised units of area – square centimetres
2 The procedure is to place the zero point of the and square metres or square inches etc. – are new
ruler at one end of the str aight line and to read off to the children and need a great deal of
the number of standardised units on the r uler that explanation. This additional step is usually quite a
corresponds to the other end of this line . There is hard one for children to take.
no evidence that this procedure causes y oung
children any consistent difficulty.
Rectangles
3 The intellectual feat is to under stand that the r uler
iterates a standardised unit (e.g. the centimetre) Youngsters are usually introduced to the
and that the length of the line being measured is measurement of area by being told about the
19 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

base x height r ule for rectangles. Thus, rectangles week?’) and those in which the question is couched
provide them with their first experience of square in one unit and the answer in another (‘The page on
centimetres. The large-scale study of 11- to 14-y ear- your book is 15 cm high and 5 cm wide. What is
old children by Har t et al. (1985), which we have the area of this page?’). The answer to the second
mentioned already, demonstrates the difficulties that question must be in square centimetres ev en though
many children have even with this simplest of area the question itself is couched onl y in terms of
measurements. In one question the children w ere centimetres. Vergnaud categorised the first kind of
shown a rectangle, drawn on squared paper, which problem as ‘isomorphism of measures’ and the
measured 4 squares (base) by 2½ squares (height) second as ‘product of measures’. His point was that
and then were asked to draw another rectangle of product of measures problems are intrinsically the
the same area with a base of 5 squares. Only 44% of more difficult of the two because, in order to solve
the 11-year-old children got this r ight: many judged such problems, the child has to under stand how one
that it was impossible to solve this problem. kind of unit can be used to create another .

We have to consider the reason for this difficulty. At first, even covering tasks are difficult for many
One reason might be that children find it hard to young children. Outhred and Mitchelmore (2000)
come to terms with a new kind of measuring unit, gave young children a rectangle to measure and just
the square. In order to explain these new units one 1 cm 2 square tile to help them to do this. The
teachers often give children ‘covering’ exercises. The children also had pencils and w ere encouraged to
children cover a rectangle with squares, usually 1 cm draw on the rectangle itself. Since the children had
squares, arranged in columns and rows and the one tile only to work with, they could only ‘cover’ the
teacher explains that the total number of squares is area by moving that tile about. Many of the younger
a measure of the rectangle’s area. The arrangement children adopted this str ategy but carried it out
of columns and rows also provides a way of rather unsuccessfully. They left gaps between their
introducing children to the idea of m ultiplying height different placements of the tile and there were also
by width to calculate a rectangle’ s area. If the gaps between the squares in the dr awings that they
rectangle has five rows and four columns of squares, made to represent the different positions of the tiles.
which means that its height is 5 cm and its width 4
cm, it is covered by 20 squares. These mistakes deserve attention, but they are hard
to interpret because there are two quite different
This might seem like an easy tr ansition, but it has its ways of accounting for them. One is that these
pitfalls. These two kinds of computation are based on particular children made a genuinely conceptual
completely different reasoning: counting is about mistake about the iter ation of the measuring unit.
finding out the number that represents a quantity They may not have realised that gaps are not allo wed
and involves additive reasoning whereas multiplying – that the whole area m ust be covered by these
the base by the height involves understanding that standardised units. The alternative account is that
there is a multiplicative relation between each of this was an executive, not a conceptual, failure. The
these measures and the area. Therefore, practice on children may have known about the need for
one (counting) will not necessar ily encourage the complete covering, and yet may have been unable
child to adopt the other f ormula (multiplying). to carry it out. Moving a tile around the rectangle ,
Another radical difference is that the covering so that the tile covers every par t of it without any
exercise provides the unit, the square centimetre, overlap, is a complicated task, and children need a
from the star t but when the child uses a ruler to great deal of dexter ity and a highly organised
measure the sides and then to multiply height by memory to carry it out, even if they know exactly
width, she is measur ing with one unit, the what they have to do. These ‘executive’ demands may
centimetre, but creating a new unit, the square have been the source of the children’s problems.
centimetre (for fur ther discussion, see Paper 3). Thus, we cannot say for sure what bear ing this study
has on Vergnaud’s distinction between isomorphism
This could be an obstacle . The French psychologist, and product of measures until w e know whether the
Gerard Vergnaud (1983), rightly distinguishes mistakes that children made in applying the measure
problems in which the question and the answ er are were conceptual or executive ones.
about the same units (‘A plant is 5 cm high at the
beginning of the week and by the end of the w eek Vergnaud’s analysis, however, fits other data that we
it is 2 cm higher. How high is it at the end of the have on children’s measurement of area quite w ell.
20 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

Nunes et al. (1993) asked pairs of eight- and 1 If you ask people to judge the area of different
nine-year-old children to work out whether two rectangles that var y both in height and in width,
rectangles had the same area or not. The dimensions will their judgement be affected by both these
of the two rectangles were always different, even dimensions? In other words, if you hold the width
when their areas were the same (e .g. 5 x 8 and of two rectangles constant will they judge the
10 x 4 cm). The experimenters gave all the children higher of the two as larger, and if you hold their
standard rulers, and also 1 cm 3 bricks to help them height constant will they judge the wider one as
solve the problem. the larger? It is quite possible that young children
might attend to one dimension onl y, and indeed
The experimenters allowed the pairs of children to Piaget’s theory about spatial reasoning implies that
make several attempts to solve each problem until this could happen.
they agreed with each other about the solution.
Most pairs star ted by using their r ulers, as they had 2 If people take both dimensions into account, do
been taught to at school, but many of them then they do so in an additive or a multiplicative way?
decided to use the br icks instead. Overall the The correct approach is the multiplicative one,
children who measured with br icks were much more because the area of a rectangle is its height
successful than those who relied entirel y on their multiplied by its width. This means that the
rulers. This clear difference is a demonstr ation of difference that an increase in the rectangle’ s height
how difficult it is, at first, for children to use one makes to the area of the rectangle depends on its
measurement unit (centimetres) to create another width, and vice versa. An increase of 3 cm in the
(square centimetres). At this age they are happier height of a 6 cm wide rectangle adds another 18
and more successful when working just with direct cm2 to its area, but the same increase in height to
representation of the measurement units that they an 8 cm wide rectangle adds another 24 cm 2. The
have to calculate than when they ha ve to use a r uler additive approach, which is wrong, would be to
to create these units. judge that an equal change in height to tw o
rectangles has exactly the same effect on their
The success of the children who used the br icks was areas, even if their widths differ. This is not tr ue of
not due to them just counting these bricks. They area, but it is tr ue of perimeter. To increase the
hardly ever covered the area and then labor iously height of a rectangle by 3 cm has exactl y the same
counted all the br icks. Much more often, they effect on the per imeter of a 6 cm and an 8 cm
counted the rows and the columns of br icks and wide rectangle (and increase of 6 cm) and the
then either multiplied the two figures or used same goes for increases to the width of rectangles
repeated addition or a mixture of the tw o to come with different heights. Also, the same increase in
up with the cor rect solution (A: ‘Eight bricks in a row. width has exactly the same effect on the two
And 5 rows. What’s five eights?’ B: ‘Two eights is 16 and rectangles’ perimeters, but very different effects on
16 is 32. Four eights is 32. 32. 40’). In fact, the children their area. It follows that anyone who persistently
who used bricks multiplied in order to calculate the makes additive judgements about area is probably
area more than three times as often as the children confusing area with per imeter.
who used the r uler. Those who used r ulers often
concentrated on the per imeter: they measured the The tasks that these two teams of exper imenters
length of the sides and added lengths instead. gave to children and adults in their studies were
remarkably similar, and so we will describe only
This confusion of area and per imeter is a ser ious Wilkening’s (1979) experiment. He showed 5-, 8-
obstacle. It can be tr aced back in time to a and 11-year-old children and a group of adults a
systematic bias in judgements that young children series of rectangles that var ied both in height (6, 12
make about area long bef ore they are taught the and 18 cm) and in width (again 6, 12 and 18 cm). He
principles of area measurement. This bias is towards told the par ticipants that these could be brok en into
judging the area of a f igure by its perimeter. pieces of a par ticular size, which he illustr ated by
showing them also the size of one of these pieces.
The bias was discovered independently in studies by The children’s and adults’ task was to imagine what
Wilkening (1979) in Ger many and Anderson and would happen if each rectangle was brok en up and
Cuneo (1978) and Cuneo (1983) in America. Both the pieces were arranged in a row. How long would
groups of researchers asked the same two questions this row be?
(Wilkening and Anderson, 1982).
21 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

The most striking contrast in the patter n of these not as pronounced as in the adult group . These
judgments was between the five-year-old children changes do not prove that being taught how
and the adults. To put it br iefly, five-year-old children to measure and then becoming increasingl y
made additive judgements and adults made experienced with measuring led to this difference
multiplicative judgements. between the age groups, but they are cer tainly
consistent with that idea. There is an alter native
The five-year-olds plainly did take both height and explanation, which is that adults and older children
width into account, since they routinely judged have more informal experience than 8-year-olds do
rectangles of the same height b ut different widths as of judging and compar ing areas, as for example when
having different areas and they did the same with they have to judge how much paint they need to
rectangles of the same widths b ut different heights. cover different walls. Here is a significant and
This is an impor tant result, and it must be reassuring interesting question for research: do teachers alone
to anyone who has to teach young schoolchildren change our spatial under standing of area or does
about how to measure area. They are apparently informal experience play a par t as well?
ready to take both dimensions into account.

However, the results suggest that y oung children


Summary
often co-ordinate information about height and
width in the wrong way. The typical five-year-old 1 Measuring area is a multiplicative process: we
judged, for example, that a 6 cm diff erence in height usually multiply two simple measurements (e.g.
would have the same effect on 12 cm and 18 cm base by height for rectangles) to produce a total
wide rectangles. In contrast, the adults’ judgements measure of an area. The process also produces a
showed that they recognised that the eff ect would different unit (i.e. product of measures): measuring
be far greater on the 18 cm than on the 12 cm wide the base and height in centimetres and then
figures. This is evidence that young children rely on multiplying them produces a measure in ter ms of
the figures’ perimeters, presumably implicitly, in order square centimetres.
to judge their area. As have already seen, when
children begin to use r ulers many of them fall into 2 Producing a new measure is a diff icult step for
the trap of measuring a figure’s perimeter in order to children to make. They find it easier to measure a
work out its area, (Nunes, Light and Mason, 1993). rectangle when they measure with units which
Their habit of concentr ating on the per imeter when directly instantiate square centimetres than when
making informal judgements about area may well be they use a r uler to measure its base and height in
the basis for this later mistake. The existence among centimetres.
schoolchildren of serious confusion between area
and perimeter was confirmed in later research by 3 The multiplicative aspect of area measurement is
Dembo, Levin and Siegler (1997). also a problem for young children who show a
definite bias to judge the area of a rectangle by
We can end this section with an interesting question. adding its base and height r ather than by
One obvious possible cause of the r adical difference multiplying them. They confuse, therefore,
in the patterns of 5-year-olds’ and adults’ judgements perimeter and area.
might be that the 5-year-olds had not learned how
to measure area while the adults had. In other
words, mathematical learning could alter this aspect
Parallelograms: forming relations
of people’s spatial cognition. The suggestion does not
seem far-fetched, especially when one also consider s
between rectangles and
the performance of the older children in Wilkening’s parallelograms
interesting study. The 5-year-olds had not been The measurement of parallelograms takes us into
taught about measurement at all: the 8-year-olds had one of the most exciting aspects of lear ning about
had some instruction, but not a great deal: the 12- geometry. The base-by-height rule applies to these
year-olds were well-versed in measurement, but figures as well as to rectangles. One way of justifying
probably still made mistakes. Wilkening found some the base by height rule for parallelograms is that any
signs of a multiplicative pattern in the responses of parallelogram can be transformed into a rectangle with
the 8-year-olds, but this was slight: he found stronger the same base and height measurements b y adding
signs of this patter n among the 12-year-olds, though and subtracting congruent areas to the parallelogram.
22 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

Figure 5.4 presents this justification which is a Some classic research by the well-known Austrian
commonplace in geometr y classes. It is based on the psychologist Max Wertheimer (1945) suggests that
inversion principle (see Paper 2). Typically the teacher many children learn the procedure for transforming
shows children a par allelogram and then creates two parallelograms into rectangles quite easily, but apply it
congruent triangles (A and S) by dropping vertical inflexibly. Wertheimer witnessed a group of 11-y ear-
lines from the top two corners of the par allelogram old children learning from their teacher why the
and then extending the baseline to reach the new same base-by-height rule applied to parallelograms
vertical that is exter nal to the par allelogram. Triangle as well. The teacher used the justification that we
A falls outside the original parallelogram, and have already described, which the pupils appeared to
therefore is an addition to the f igure. Triangle S falls understand. However, Wertheimer was not cer tain
inside the original parallelogram, and the nub of the whether these children really had understood the
teacher’s demonstration is to point out that the underlying idea. So, he gave them another
effect of adding Triangle A and subtracting Triangle S parallelogram whose height was longer than its base
would be to tr ansform the figure into a rectangle (diagram 3 in Figure 5.4). When a parallelogram is
with the same base and height as the or iginal oriented in this way, dropping two vertical lines from
parallelogram. Triangles A and S are congruent and its top two corners does not create two congruent
so their areas are equal. Therefore, adding one and triangles. Wertheimer found that most of the
subtracting the other tr iangle must produce a new children tried putting in the two vertical lines, but
figure (the rectangle) of exactl y the same size as the were at a loss when they sa w the results of doing so .
original one (the par allelogram). A few, however, did manage to solve the problem by
rotating the new figure so that the base was longer
This is a neat demonstr ation, and it is an impor tant than its height, which made it possible for them to
one from our point of view, because it is our f irst repeat the teacher’s demonstration.
example of the impor tance in geometr y of
understanding that there are systematic relations The fact that most of the pupils did not cope with
between shapes. Rectangles and par allelograms are Wertheimer’s new figure was a clear demonstr ation
different shapes but they are measured by the same that they had lear ned more about the teacher’s
base-by-height rule because one can tr ansform any procedure than about the under lying idea about
rectangle into any parallelogram, or vice versa, with transformation that he had hoped to convey.
the same base and height without changing the Wertheimer argued that this was probably because
figure’s area. the teaching itself concentr ated too much on the
procedural sequence and too little on the idea of

Figure 5.4: Demonstrating by transforming a parallelogram into a rectangle that the base-by-height rule applies to
parallelograms as well as to rectangles.
23 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

transformation. In later work, that is only repor ted the area of an ir regular polygon (Figure 5.6).
rather informally (Luchins and Luchins, 1970), One good way to solve this difficult problem is
Wertheimer showed children two figures at a time, to par tition the figure by imaging the divisions
one of which could be easil y transformed into a represented by the dotted lines in the r ight hand
measurable rectangle while the other could not figure. This creates the Triangle x and also a rectangle
(for example, the A and B figures in Figure 5.5). which includes another Triangle y. Since the two
Wertheimer repor ted that this is an eff ective way triangles are congruent and Triangle x is par t of the
of preparing children for understanding the relation original polygon while Triangle y is not, the area of
between the area of par allelograms and rectangles. the polygon must add up to the area of the
rectangle (plus Triangle x minus Triangle y).
It is a regrettable irony that this extraordinarily
interesting and ingenious research by a leading Piaget et al. report that the problem flummoxed
psychologist was done so long ago and is so widel y most of the children in their study, but repor t that
known, and yet few researchers since then have some 10-year-olds did come up with the solution
studied children’s knowledge of how to transform that we have just described. They also tell us that
one geometric shape into another to f ind its area. many children made no attempt to break up the
figure but that others, more advanced, were ready to
In fact, Piaget et al. (1960) did do a relevant study, decompose the figure into smaller shapes, but did
also a long time ago. They asked children to measure not have the idea of in eff ect adding to the figure by

Figure 5.5:An example of Wertheimer’s A and B figures.A figures could be easily transformed into a simple rectangle. B figures could not.

Figure 5.6: Piaget et al.’s irregular polygon whose area could be measured by decomposition.
24 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

imagining the BD line which was exter nal to the Children are usually taught about the relationship
figure. Thus, the stumbling block for these relatively between triangles, rectangles and parallelograms quite
advanced children was in adding to the f igure. This early on in their geometr y lessons. However, we know
valuable line of research, long abandoned, needs to of no direct research on ho w well children understand
be restarted. the relationships between different shapes or on the
best way to teach them about these relations.

Triangles: forming relations between


triangles and parallelograms Summary
Other transformations from one shape to another 1 Learning about the measurement of the area of
are equally impor tant. Triangles can be transformed different shapes is a cumulative affair which is
into parallelograms, or into rectangles if they are based not just on f ormulas for measuring par ticular
right-angle triangles, simply by being doubled (see shapes but on grasping the relationships, through
Figure 5.7). Thus the area of a tr iangle is half that of transformations, of different shapes to each other.
a parallelogram with the same base and height. Parallelograms can be tr ansformed into rectangles
by adding and subtr acting congruent triangles:
Hart et al.’s (1985) study shows us that 11- to triangles can be tr ansformed into parallelograms
14-year-old children’s knowledge of the (base x by being doubled.
height)/2 rule for measuring triangles is distinctly
sketchy. Asked to calculate the area of a r ight-angle 2 There is little direct research on children’ s
triangle with a base of 3 cm and a height of 4 cm, understanding of the impor tance of the
only 48% of the children in their third y ear in relationships between shapes in their
secondary school (13- and 14-year-olds) gave the measurement. Wertheimer’s observations suggest
right answer. Only 31% of the first year secondary very plausibly that their under standing depends
school children (11-year-olds) succeeded, while greatly on the quality of the teaching.
almost an equal number of them – 29% – ga ve the
answer 12, which means that they cor rectly 3 We need more research on what are the most
multiplied base by height but forgot to halve the effective ways to teach children about these
product of that multiplication. relationships.

Figure 5.7: A demonstration that any two congruent triangles add up to a measurable
parallelogram and any two congruent right angle triangles add up to a measurable rectangle.
25 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Understanding new In our everyday lives we experience angles in many


relationships: the case different contexts, and it may not at first be easy for
of angle children to connect information about angles
encountered in different ways. The obvious distinction
Angle is an abstr act relation. Sometimes it is the here is between perceiving angles as configurations,
difference between the orientations of two lines or such as the difference between perpendicular and
rays, sometimes the change in y our orientation from non-perpendicular lines in pictures and diagr ams, and
the beginning of a tur n that you are making to its as changes in movement, such as changing direction
end, and sometimes the relation betw een a figure or by making a tur n. These forms of experiencing angles
a movement to permanent aspects of the can themselves be subdivided: it may not be obvious
environment: the angle at which an aeroplane r ises to school children that we make the same angular
after take-off is the relation between the slope of its change in our movements when we walk along a
path and the spatial hor izontal. Understanding that path with a r ight-angle bend as when we turn a
angles are a way of describing such a var iety of door-knob by 90o (Mitchelmore, 1998).
contexts is a basic par t of learning plane geometr y.
Yet, research by psychologists on this impor tant and Another point that children might at first find hard to
fascinating topic is remar kably thin on the ground. grasp is that angles are relational measures. When we
say that the angles in some of the f igures in Figure
Most of the relatively recent studies of children’s and 5.8a are 90 o ones and in other s 45o, we are making a
adults’ learning about angles are about the statement about the relation between the
effectiveness of computer-based methods of orientations of the two lines in each figure and not
teaching. This is estimable and valuable work, but we about the absolute or ientation of any of the
also need a great deal more inf ormation about individual lines, which var y from each other. Also,
children’s basic knowledge about angles and about angles affect the distance between lines, but only in
the obstacles, which undoubtedly do exist, to relation to the distance along the lines: in Figure 5.8b
forming an abstract idea of what angles are . the distance between the lines in the f igure with the
larger angle is greater than in the other f igure when

Figure 5.8: Angles as relations


26 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

the distance is measured at equivalent points along 3 Abstract angle concepts Mitchelmore and
lines in the two figures, but not necessarily otherwise. White’s third step is itself a ser ies of steps. They
claim that children begin to compare angles across
A third possible obstacle is that children might f ind contexts, for example, between slopes and tur ns,
some representations of angle more understandable but that initially these connections across contexts
than others. Angles are sometimes formed by the are limited in scope; for example, these
meeting of two clear lines, like the peak of a roof or researchers repor t that even at the age of 11
the corner of a table. With other angles, such as the years many children cannot connect angles in
inclination of a hill, the angle is clear ly represented by bends with angles in tur ns. So, children form one
one line – the hill itself, but the other, a notional or more restricted abstract angle ‘domains’ (e.g. a
horizontal, is not so clear. In still other cases, such as domain that links inter sections, bends, slopes and
the context of tur ning, there are no clear ly defined turns) before they finally develop this into a
lines at all: the angle is the amount of tur ning. completely abstract concept of standard angles.
Children may not see the connection between these
very different perceptual situations, and they may To test this theoretical fr amework, Mitchelmore and
find some much easier than other s. White gave children of 7, 9 and 11 years pictures of
a wide range of situations (doors, scissors, bends in
There are few theories of how children learn roads etc.) and asked them to represent the angles
about angles, despite the impor tance of the topic . in these, using a bent pipe cleaner to do so , and also
The most comprehensive and in many ways the to compare angles in pair s of different situations. The
most convincing of the theor ies that do exist was study cer tainly showed different degrees of
produced by Mitchelmore and White (2000). The abstraction among these children and provided some
problem that these researcher s tried to solve is how evidence that abstraction about angles increases with
children learn to abstract and classify angles despite age. This is a valuable contribution,
the large variety of situations in which they but we cer tainly need more evidence about this
experience them. They suggest that children’s developmental change for at least two reasons.
knowledge of angles develops in three steps:
One is methodological. The research that we have just
1 Situated angle concepts Children first register described was cross-sectional: the children in the
angles in completely specific ways, according to different age groups were different children. A much
Mitchelmore and White. They may realise that a better way of testing any hypothesis about a series of
pair of scissors, for example, can be more open or developmental cognitive changes is to do a
less open, and that some playground slides have longitudinal study of the ideas that the same children
steeper slopes than other s but they make no link hold and then change over time as they get older. If
between the angles of scissor s and of slides, and the hypothesis is also about what makes the changes
would not even recognise that a slide and a roof happen, one should combine this longitudinal research
could have the same slope. with an inter vention study to see what pro vokes the
development in question. We commented on the
2 Contextual angle concepts The next step that need for combining longitudinal and inter vention
children take is to realise that there are similar ities studies in Paper 2. Once again, we commend this all-
in angles across different situations, but the too-rarely adopted design to anyone planning to do
connections that they do mak e are always research on children’s mathematics.
restricted to par ticular, fairly broad, contexts. Slope,
which we have mentioned already, is one of these The second gap in this theory is its concentr ation on
contexts; children begin to be ab le to compare the children learning what is ir relevant rather than what
slopes of hills, roofs and slides, but they do not is relevant to angle . The main claim is that children
manage to make any connection between these eventually learn, for example, that the same angles
and the angles of, for instance, turns in a road. They are defined by two clear lines in some cases b ut not
begin to see the connection betw een angles in in others, that some angular information is about
very different kinds of tur ns – in roads and in a static relations and some about mo vement, but that
bent nail, for example – but they do not link these it is still exactly the same kind of inf ormation. This
to objects turning round a fixed point, like a door claim is almost cer tainly right, but it does not tell us
or a door-knob. what children learn instead.
27 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

One possibility, suggested by Piaget, Inhelder and called their situated angle concepts. Magina and
Szeminska (1960), is that children need to be ab le to Hoyles (1997) attempted to do this b y investigating
relate angles to the sur rounding Euclidean framework children’s understanding of angle in the context of
in order to reproduce them and compare them to clocks and watches. They asked Brazilian children,
other angles. In one study they ask ed children to copy whose ages ranged from 6 to 14 y ears, to show
a triangle like the one in Figure 5.9. them where the hands on a clock w ould be in half
an hour’s time and also half an hour bef ore the time
They gave the children some rulers, strips of paper it registered at that moment. Their aim was to find
and sticks (but apparently no protractor) to help out if the children could judge the cor rect degree
them do this. Piaget et al. wanted to find out how of turn. Magina and Hoyles repor t that the younger
the children set about reproducing the angles C AB, children’s responses tended to be either quite
ABC and ACB. In the experimenters’ view the best unsystematic or to depend on the initial position
solution was to measure all three lines, and also of the minute hand: these latter children, mostly 8-
introduce an additional vertical line (KB in Figure 5.9) to 11-year-olds, could move the minute hand to
or to extend the hor izontal line and then introduce represent half an hour’s difference well enough when
a new vertical line (CK ’ in Figure 5.9). It is not at all the hand’s initial position was at 6 (half past) on the
surprising that children below the age of roughly ten clock face, but not when it was at 3 (quar ter past).
years did not think of this Euclidean solution. Some Thus, even in this highly familiar situation, many
tried to copy the triangle perceptually. Others used children seem to have an incomplete under standing
the rulers to measure the length of the lines b ut did of the angle as the degree of a tur n. This rather
not take any other measures. Both strategies tended disappointing result suggests that the or igins of
to lead to inaccur ate copies. children’s understanding may not lie in their inf ormal
spatial experiences.
The study is interesting, but it does not establish that
children have to think of angles in ter ms of their One way in which children may learn about angles is
relationship to horizontal and vertical lines in order through movement. The idea of children lear ning
to be able to compare and reproduce par ticular about spatial relations by monitoring their own actions
angles. The fact that the children w ere not given the in space fits well with Piaget’s framework, and it is the
chance to use the usual conventional measure for basis for Logo, the name that Papert (1980) gave to
angles – the protr actor – either in this study or in his well-known computer-system that has often been
Mitchelmore and White’s study needs to be noted. used for teaching aspects of geometr y. In Logo,
This measure, despite being quite hard to use , may children learn to write programmes to move a ‘turtle’
play a significant and possibly even an essential par t around a spatial environment. These programmes
in children’s understanding of angle. consist of a series of instructions that determine a
succession of movements by the tur tle. The
Another way of approaching children’s understanding instructions are about the length and direction of
of angles is through what Mitchelmore and White each movement, and the instr uctions about direction

Figure 5.9: The triangle (represented by the continuous lines) that Piaget, Inhelder and Szeminska asked children to copy,
with the vertical and horizontal (dotted) lines which some of the children created to help them to solve this problem.
28 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

take the form of angular changes e .g. L90 is an which involve no movement at all. We need more
instruction for the tur tle to make a 90 o turn to the research to be sure of this conclusion and, as far as
left. Since the tur tle’s movements leave a trace, studies of the effects of Logo and other computer-
children effectively draw shapes by writing these based programmes are concerned, we need studies
programmes. in which pre-tests are given before the children go
through these programmes as well as post-tests that
There is evidence that exper ience with Logo does follow these classes.
have an effect on children’s learning about angles,
and this in tur n suppor ts the idea that Children do not just lear n about single angles in
representations of movement might be one effective isolation from each other. In fact, to us, the most
way of teaching this aspect of geometr y. Noss interesting question in this area is about their
(1988) gave a group of 8- to 11-y ear-old children, learning of the relations between different angles.
some of whom had attended Logo classes o ver a These relations are a basic par t of geometr y lessons:
whole school year, a series of problems involving pupils learn quite early on in these lessons that, for
angles. On the whole the children who had been to example, when two straight lines intersect opposite
the Logo classes solved these angular problems angles are equal and that alter nate angles in a Z-
more successfully than those who had not. The shape figure are equal also, but how easily this
relative success of the Logo group was par ticularly knowledge comes to them and ho w effectively they
marked in a task in which the children had to use it to solve geometric problems are interesting
compare the size of the tur n that people would have but unanswered questions (at any rate, unanswered
to make at different points along a path, and this is by psychologists). Some interesting educational
not surprising since this specific task resonated research by Gal and Vinner (1997) on 14-year-old
with the instructions that children make when students’ reaction to perpendicular lines suggests that
determining the direction of the tur tle’s movements they have some difficulties in understanding the
(see Figure 5.10). relation between the angles made by intersecting
lines when the lines are perpendicular to each other.
However, the Logo group also did better than the Many of the students did not realise at f irst that if
comparison in more static angular problems. This is one of the four angles made by two intersecting
an interesting result because it suggests that the lines is a r ight angle the other three m ust be so as
children may have generalised what they learned well. The underlying reason for this difficulty needs
about angles and movement to other angular tasks investigation.

You are walking along this path.You start at point A and you finish at point G.

• At which point would you have to turn most?


• At which point would you have to turn least?

finish

Figure 5.10: The judgement about relative amount of turns in Noss’s study.
29 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary to do so. Thus, many young children and some adults


too (Howard, 1978) do not recognise that the level
1 Although young children are aware of orientation, of liquid is parallel to horizontal features of the
they seem to know little about angle (the relation environment like a table top.
between orientations) when they star t on
geometry. 2 Part of the difficulty that they have in Piaget’s
horizontality and verticality tasks is that these
2 The concept of angle is an abstr act one that cuts depend on children being able to represent acute
across very different contexts. This is difficult for and obtuse angles (i.e . non-perpendicular lines). They
children to understand at first. tend to do this inaccurately, representing the line
that they draw as closer to per pendicular than it
3 There is some evidence , mainly from work on actually is. This bias towards the perpendicular may
Logo, that children can lear n about angle through get in the way of children’s representation of angle .
movement.

4 Children’s understanding of the relation between


The role of horizontal and vertical
angles within figures (e.g. when straight lines intersect,
opposite angles are equivalent) is a basic par t of
axes in the Cartesian system
geometry lessons, but there seems to be no research The Euclidean framework makes it possible to pinpoint
on their understanding of this kind of relation. any position in a two-dimensional plane. We owe this
insight to René Descar tes, the 17 th century French
mathematician and philosopher, who was interested in
linking Euclid’s notions with algebra. Descartes devised
Spatial frameworks an elegant way of plotting positions by representing
them in terms of their position along tw o axes in a
Horizontal and vertical lines two dimensional plane. In his system one axis was
vertical and the other hor izontal, and so the two axes
Most children’s formal introduction to geometr y is were perpendicular to each other. Descartes pointed
a Euclidean one. Children are taught about str aight out that all that you need to know in order to find a
lines, perpendicular lines, and parallel lines, and they particular point in two-dimensional space is its position
learn how a quite complex system of geometr y can along each of these two axes. With this information
be derived from a set of simple , comprehensible, you can plot the point b y extrapolating an imaginar y
axioms. It is an exercise in logic, and it must, for straight perpendicular line from each axis. The point at
most children, be their first experience of a formal which these two lines intersect is the position in
and explicit account of two-dimensional space. question. Figure 5.11 shows two axes, x and y, and
The principal feature of this account is the relation points which are expressed as positions on these axes.
between lines such as par allel and perpendicular and
intersecting lines. This simple idea has had a huge impact on science
and technology and on all our dail y lives: for
These fundamental spatial relations are probab ly example, we rely on Car tesian co-ordinates to
quite familiar, but in an implicit way, to the seven- and interpret maps, graphs and block diagrams. The
eight-year-old children when they star t classes in Cartesian co-ordinate system is a good example of
geometry. In spatial environments, and par ticularly in a cultural tool (Vygotsky, 1978) that has tr ansformed
‘carpentered’ environments, there are obvious all our intellectual lives.
horizontal and vertical lines and surfaces, and these
are at right angles (perpendicular) to each other. We To understand and to use the Car tesian system to
reviewed the evidence on children’s awareness of plot positions in two-dimensional space, one has to
these spatial relations in an ear lier par t of this paper, be able to extrapolate two imaginary perpendicular
when we reached the following two conclusions. straight lines, and to co-ordinate the tw o in order to
work out where they inter sect. Is this a difficult or
1 Although quite young children can relate the even an impossible barrier for young children?
orientation of lines to stable background features and Teachers cer tainly need the answer to this question
often rely on this relation to remember or ientations, because children are introduced to graphs and block
they do not always do this when it w ould help them diagrams in primary school, and as we have noted
30 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

these mathematical representations depend on the the square waited two characters, each just about to
use of Car tesian co-ordinates. set off across the square . One of these char acters
was standing on a vertical edge (the right or left side
Earlier in this section we mentioned that, in social of the square) and the other on a horizontal edge
contexts, very young children do extr apolate of the square. The children were told that both
imaginary straight lines. They follow their mother’s characters could only walk in the direction they
line of sight in order, apparently, to look at whatever were facing (each char acter had a rather prominent
it is that is attr acting her attention at the time nose to mark this direction, which was per pendicular
(Butterworth, 1990). If children can extr apolate to the depar ture line, clear), and that the two would
straight lines in three-dimensional space , we can eventually meet at one par ticular position in the
quite reasonably expect them to be able to do so square. It was the child’s task to say which position
in two-dimensional space as well. The Car tesian that would be.
requirement that these extrapolated lines are
perpendicular to their baselines should not be a The task was slightly easier when the choices w ere
problem either, since, as we have seen already, arranged in a gr id than when the ar rangement was
children usually find it easier to create per pendicular irregular, but in both tasks all the children chose the
than non-perpendicular lines. The only requirement right position most of the time . The individual
that this leaves is the ability to w ork out where the children’s choices were compared to chance (if a
paths of the two imaginary straight lines intersect. child followed just one extr apolated line instead of
co-ordinating both, he or she would be right by
A study by Somerville and Br yant (1985) established chance 25% of the time) and it turned out that
that children as young as six years usually have this the number of correct decisions made by every
ability. In the most complex task in this study, young individual child was significantly above chance. Thus,
children were shown a fairly large square space on a all of these 6-year-old children were able to plot the
screen and 16 positions were clearly marked within intersection of two extrapolated, imaginary straight
this space, sometimes arranged in a regular grid and lines, which means that they were well equipped to
sometimes less regularly than that. On the edge of understand Car tesian co-ordinates.

Figure 5.11: Descartes’ co-ordinates: three points (8, 8), (-3, 5) and (-9, -4) are plotted by their positions on the x- and y-axes
31 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Piaget et al. (1960) were less optimistic about intensive instruction, which the researcher s
children’s grasp of co-ordinates, which they tested themselves designed. In the tasks that they gave to
with a copying task. They gave each child two the children Sarama et al. represented the x and y
rectangular sheets of paper, one with a small circle co-ordinates as numbers, which was a good thing
on it and the other a complete b lank. They also to include because it is a fundamental par t of the
provided the children with a pencil and a r uler and Cartesian system, and they also imposed a
strips of paper, and then they ask ed them to put a rectangular grid on many of the spaces that they
circle on the blank sheet in exactly the same position gave the children to work with. Thus, one set of
as the circle on the other sheet. Piaget et al.’s materials was a rectangular grid, presented as a map
question was whether any of the children would use of a grid-plan city with ‘streets’ as the vertical and
a co-ordinate system to plot the position of the ‘avenues’ as the hor izontal lines.
existing circle and would then use the co-ordinates
to position the circle that they had to dr aw on the The children were given various tasks before, during
other sheet. This was a difficult task, which children and after the instr uction. Some of these involved
of six and seven years tended to fail. Most of them relating locations to x and y co-ordinates: the
tried to put the new circle in the right place simply children had to locate positions giv en the co-
by looking from one piece of paper to the other, and ordinates and also to work out the co-ordinates of
this was a most unsuccessful str ategy. particular positions. Sarama et al. reported that most
of the children lear ned about this relation quickl y
There is no conflict between the success of all the and well, as one might expect giv en their evident
children in the Somer ville and Br yant study, and the ability to co-ordinate extr apolated lines in a
grave difficulties of children of the same age in the rectangular context (Somer ville and Bryant, 1985).
Piaget et al. task. In the Piaget et al. task the children However, when they had to co-ordinate inf ormation
had to decide that co-ordinates w ere needed and about two or more locations, they were in greater
then had to measure in order to estab lish the difficulty. For example, some children found it hard to
appropriate position on each axis. In the Somer ville work out the distance between two locations in the
and Bryant study, the co-ordinates were given and all grid-like city, because they thought that the n umber
that the child had to do was to use them in order to of turns in a path affected its distance, and some did
find the point where the tw o extrapolated lines met. not realise that the numerical differences in the x
So, six- and seven-year-old children can establish a and y co-ordinate addresses between the two
position given the co-ordinates but often cannot set locations represents the distance between them.
up these co-ordinates in the f irst place.
Thus, the children under stood how to find two
Some older children in Piaget et al.’s study (all the locations, given their coordinates, but struggled with
successful children given as examples in the book the idea that a compar ison between the two pairs
were eight- or-nine-years-old) did apparently of co-ordinates told them about the spatial relation
spontaneously use co-ordinates. It seems unlikely between these locations. Fur ther observations
to us that these children managed to in vent the showed that the problem that some of the children
Cartesian system for themselves. How could eight- had in working out the relations between two co-
and nine-year-old children come up, in one ordinate pairs was created by a cer tain tension
experimental session, with an idea f or which mankind between absolute and relative information. The
had had to wait till Descar tes had his br illiant insight two co-ordinate pairs 10,30 and 5,0 represent tw o
in the middle of the 17th centur y? absolute positions: however, some children, who
A more plausible reason for these children’s success were given first 10,30 and then 5,0 and ask ed to
is that, being among the older children in Piaget et work out a path between the two, decided that 5,0
al.’s sample, they had been taught about the use of represented the difference between the first and the
Cartesian co-ordinates in maps or gr aphs already. second location and plotted a location f ive blocks to
the right of 10,30. They treated absolute information
It appears that this success is not univ ersal. Many about the second position as relativ e information
children who have been taught about Car tesian co- about the difference between the two positions.
ordinates fail to take advantage of them or to use However, most of the children in this ingenious and
them properly. Sarama, Clements, Swaminathan, important study seem to have overcome this
McMillen and Gomez (2003) studied a group of difficulty during the period of instruction, and to
nine-year-old children while they were being given have learned reasonably well that co-ordinate pair s
32 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

represent the relation between positions as well as 4 Thus, although children have the basic abilities to
the absolute positions themselves. understand and use co-ordinates well, there seem
to be obstacles that prevent them using these
Finally, we should consider children’s understanding of abilities in tasks which involve two or more plotted
the use of Car tesian co-ordinates in graphs. Here, positions. We need research on how to teach
research seems to lead to m uch the same conclusion children to surmount these obstacles.
as we reached in our discussion of the use of co-
ordinates to plot spatial positions. With graphs, too,
children find it easy to locate single positions, but
often fail to take advantage of the information that
Categorising, composing and
graphs provide that is based on the relation between
decomposing shapes
different positions. Bryant and Somer ville (1986) We have chosen to end this chapter on lear ning
gave six- and nine-year-old children a graph that about space and geometr y with the question of
represented a simple linear function. Using much children’s ability to analyse and categorise shapes, but
the same technique as in their previous research on we could just as easil y have star ted the section with
spatial co-ordinates, these researchers measured the this topic, because children are in man y ways exper ts
children’s ability to plot a position on x-axis given the on shape from a very early age. They are born,
position on the y-axis and vice versa. This was quite an apparently, with the ability to distinguish and
easy task for the children in both age groups, and so remember abstract, geometric shapes, like squares,
the main contribution of the study was to estab lish triangles, and circles, and with the capacity to
that children can co-ordinate extr apolated straight recognise such shapes as constant ev en when they
lines in a graph-like task fairly well even before they see them from different angles on different occasions
have had any systematic instruction about graphs. so that the shape of the retinal image that they mak e
varies quite radically over time. We left shape to the
The function line in a gr aph is formed from a ser ies end because much of the lear ning that we have
of positions, each of which is deter mined by discussed already, about measurement and angle and
Cartesian co-ordinates. As in the Sar ama et al. study, spatial co-ordinates, undoubtedly affects and changes
it seems to be hard f or children to grasp what the schoolchildren’s understanding of shape.
relation between these different positions means. An
interesting study by Knuth (2000) showed that There is nearly complete agreement among those
American students ‘enrolled in 1st-year algebra’ who study mathematics education that children’ s
(Knuth does not say how old these students were) knowledge about shapes under goes a series of
are much more likely to express linear functions as radical changes during their time at school. Different
equations than graphically. We do not yet know the theories propose different changes but many of
reasons for this preference these apparent disagreements are really only
semantic ones. Most claim, though in different terms,
that school-children star t by being able to distinguish
Summary and classify shapes in a perceptual and implicit wa y
and eventually acquire the ability to analyse the
1 Cartesian co-ordinates seem to pose no basic properties of shapes conceptually and explicitly.
intellectual difficulty for young children. They are
able to extrapolate imaginary straight lines that are The model developed by the Dutch educationalist
perpendicular to horizontal and vertical axes and van Hiele (1986) is cur rently the best known
to work out where these imaginar y lines would theoretical account of this kind of lear ning. This is a
meet in maps and in gr aphs. good example of what we called a ‘pragmatic theory’
in our opening paper. Van Hiele claimed that children
2 However, it is harder f or children to work out the have to take a sequence of steps in a f ixed order in
relation between two or more positions that are their geometric learning about shape. There are five
plotted in this way, either in an map-lik e or in a such steps in van Hiele’s scheme, but he agreed that
graph-like task. not all children get to the end of this sequence:

3 Students prefer expressing functions as equations


to representing them gr aphically.
33 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Level 1: Visualization/recognition Students can decompose any parallelogram into two identical
recognise and learn to name cer tain geometric triangles) in order to under stand how to measure
shapes but are usually only aware of shapes as a the area of tr iangles. As far as we know, there has
wholes, and not of their proper ties or of their been no direct research on the relationship betw een
components. children’s ability to compose and decompose shapes
and their understanding of the rules for measuring
Level 2: Descriptive/analytic Students begin to simple geometric figures, though such research
recognise shapes by their proper ties. would be easy to do.

Level 3: Abstract/relational Students begin to form The Har t et al. (1985) study included two items
definitions of shapes based on their common which dealt with shapes that w ere decomposed into
properties, and to under stand some proofs. two par ts and these par ts were then re-arranged. In
one item the re-ar ranged par ts were a rectangle and
Level 4: Formal deduction Students understand the two triangles, which are simple and familiar geometr ic
significance of deduction as a wa y of establishing shapes, and in the other they w ere unfamiliar and
geometric theory within an axiomatic system, and more complex shapes. In both cases the children
comprehend the interrelationships and roles of were asked about the effect of the re-ar rangement
axioms, definitions, theorems, and formal proof. on the figure’s total area: was the new figure’s area
bigger or smaller or the same as the area of the
Level 5: Rigour Students can themselves reason original one? A large propor tion of the 11- to 14-
formally about different geometric systems. year-old students in the study (o ver 80% in each
group) gave the correct answer to the first of these
There have been several attempts to elaborate and two questions but the second was far harder : only
refine this system. For example, Clements and Sarama 60% of the 11-year-old group understood that the
(2007 b) argue that it would better to rename the area was the same after the rear rangement of par ts
Visual/recognition stage as Syncretic, given its as before it. There are two reasons for being
limitations. Another development was Guttierez’s surprised at this last result. The first is that it is hard
(1992) sustained attempt to extend the system to 3- to see why there was such a lar ge difference in the
D figures as well as 2-D ones. However, although van difficulty of the two problems when the logic for
Hiele’s steps provide us with a useful and interesting solving both was exactly the same. The second is that
way of assessing improvements in children’s the mistakes which the children made in such
understanding of geometry, they are descriptive. The abundance with the harder problem are in effect
theory tells us about changes in what children do and conservation failures, and yet these children are well
do not understand, but not about the under lying beyond the age when conser vation of area should
cognitive basis for this understanding, nor about the pose any difficulty for them. This needs fur ther study.
reasons that cause children to mo ve from one level
to the next. We shall turn now to what is known and The insight that under standing composition and
what needs to be known about these cognitive bases. decomposition may be a basic par t of children’s
learning about shapes has been investigated in
another way. Clements, Wilson and Sarama (2004)
looked at a group of three- to seven-year-old
Composing and decomposing
children’s ability to assemble target patterns, like the
If van Hiele is r ight, one of the most basic changes in figure of a man, by assembling the right component
children’s analysis of shape is the realisation that wooden shapes. This interesting study produced
shapes, and par ticularly complex shapes, can be evidence of some shar p developmental changes: the
decomposed into smaller shapes. We have already younger children tended to create the f igures bit by
discussed one of the reasons wh y children need to bit whereas the older children tended to create units
be able to compose and decompose shapes, which is made out of several bits (an ar m unit for example)
that it is an essential par t of understanding the and the oldest dealt in units made out of other units.
measurement of the area of different shapes. The next step in this research should be to f ind out
Children, as we have seen, must learn, for example, whether there is a link betw een this development
that you can compose a par allelogram by putting and the eventual progress that children mak e in
together two identical triangles (and thus that you learning about geometr y. Once again we have to
34 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

make a plea for longitudinal studies (which are far transformations, even though some research on
too rare in research on children’s geometry) and perceptual development has come close to doing
intervention studies as well. so. They have shown that children remember
symmetrical figures better than asymmetr ical figures
(Bornstein, Ferdinandsen and Gross, 1981), and
there are obser vations of pre-school children
Summary
spontaneously constructing symmetrical figures in
1 Although young school children are already v ery informal play (Seo and Ginsburg, 2004). However,
familiar with shapes, they have some difficulty with the bulk of the psychological work on rotation and
the idea of decomposing these into par ts, e.g. a reflection has treated these tr ansformations in a
parallelogram decomposed into two congruent negative sense. The researchers (Bomba, 1984;
triangles or an isosceles tr iangle decomposed into Quinn, Siqueland and Bomba, 1985; Bryant,
two right-angle triangles, and also with the inverse 1969,1974) were concerned with children’s
process of composing new shapes b y combining confusions between symmetrical, mirror-image
two or more shapes to mak e a different shape. figures (usually reflections around a vertical axis):
they studied the development of children’s ability to
2 The barrier here may be that these are un usual tell symmetrical figures apar t, not to under stand the
tasks for children who might lear n how to carry relation between them. Here is another br idge still
them out easily given the right experience. This is to be crossed between psychology and education.
a subject for future research.
Enlargement raises some interesting issues about
children’s geometric understanding. We know of no
direct research on teaching children or on children
Transforming shapes: enlargement,
learning about this tr ansformation, at any rate in the
rotation and reflection geometrical sense of shapes being enlar ged by a
We have already stressed the demands that designated scale factor. These scale factors of course
measurement of length and angle mak e on children’s directly affect the perimeter of the shapes: the
imagination, and the same holds f or their learning lengths of the sides of, for example, a right-angle
about the basic tr ansformations of shapes – triangle enlarged by a factor of 2 are twice those of
translation, enlargement, rotation and reflection. the original triangle. But, of course, the relation
Children have to learn to imagine how shapes would between the areas of the tw o triangles is different:
change as a result of each of these tr ansformations the area of the lar ger triangle is 4 times that of the
and we know that this is not al ways easy. The work smaller one.
by Har t (1981) and her colleagues on children’ s
solutions to reflection and rotation prob lems There is a danger that some children, and even some
suggests that these tr ansformations are not always adults, might confuse these different kinds of relation
easy for children to work out. They repor t that there between two shapes, one of which is an enlar gement
is a great deal of change in betw een the ages of 11 of the other. Piaget et al. (1960) showed children a 3
and 16 years in students’ understanding of what cm x 3 cm square which they said represented a
changes and what stays constant as a result of these field with just enough gr ass for one cow, and then
two kinds of tr ansformation. they asked each child to dr aw a larger field of the
same shape which would produce enough for two
One striking pattern repor ted by this research group cows. Since this area measured 9 cm 2 the new
was that the younger children in the group being square would have to have an area of 18 cm. 2 and
studied were much more successful with rotation therefore sides of roughly 4.24 cm. since 4.24 is very
and reflection problems that involved horizontal and nearly the square root of 18. In this study most of
vertical figures than with sloping figures: this result the children under the age of 10 y ears either acted
may be related to the evidence , mentioned earlier, quite unsystematically or made the mistak e of
that much younger children discriminate and doubling the sides of the original square in the new
remember horizontal and vertical lines much better figure that they drew, which meant that their new
than sloping ones. square (an enlargement of the original square by a
scale factor of two) had 6 cm sides and an area of
Psychologists, in contrast to educationalists, have not around 36 cm 2 which is actually 4 times the area of
thrown a great deal light on children’ s learning about the first square. Older children, however, recognised
35 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

the problem as a multiplicative one and calculated General conclusions on


each of the two squares’ areas my multiplying its learning geometry
height by its width. The younger children’s difficulties
echoed those of the slave who, in Plato’s Meno, was 1 Geometry is about spatial relations.
lucky enough to be instr ucted about measuring area
by Socrates himself. 2 Children have become highly familiar with some of
these relations, long before they learn about them
The widespread existence of this apparentl y formally in geometr y classes: others are new to
prevailing belief that doubling the length of the sides them.
of a shape will double its area as well was recently
confirmed by a team of psychologists in Belgium 3 In the case of the spatial relations that they kno w
((De Bock, Verschaffel and Janssens, 1998, 2002; De about already, like length, orientation and position
Bock, Van Dooren, Janssens and Verschaffel, 2002; De relations, the new thing that children ha ve to learn
Bock, Verschaffel, Janssens, Van Dooren and Claes, is to represent them n umerically. The process of
2003). There could be an educational conflict here making these numerical representations is not
between teaching children about scale factors on the always straightforward.
one hand and about propor tional changes in area on
the other. It is possible that the misconceptions 4 Representing length in standard units depends on
expressed by students in the studies by Piaget et al. children using one-to-one cor respondence
(1960) and also by the Belgian team may actually between the units on the ruler and imagined units
have been the result of confusion betw een the use on the line being measured. This may seem to be
of scale factors in drawing and effect of doubling the easy to do to adults, but some children find it
sides of figures. In Paper 4 we discussed in detail the difficult to understand.
difficulties of the studies car ried out by the Belgian
team but we still need to find out, by research, 5 Representing the area of rectangles in standard
whether scale drawing does provoke this confusion. units depends on children understanding two
It would be easy to do such research. things: (a) why they have to multiply the base with
the height in centimetres (b) wh y this
multiplication produces a measure in different
units, square centimetres. Both ideas are difficult
Summary
ones for young children.
1 Understanding, and being able to work out, the
familiar transformations of reflection, rotation and 6 Understanding how to measure par allelograms and
enlargement are a basic par t of the geometry that triangles depends on children lear ning about the
children learn at school. They are another instance relation between these shapes and rectangles.
of the impor tance of grasping the relations Learning about the relations between shapes is a
between shapes in lear ning geometry. significant par t of learning about geometr y and
deserves attention in research done by
2 Psychology tells us little about children’ s psychologists.
understanding of these relations, though it would
be easy enough for psychologists to do empir ical 7 The idea of angle seems to be new to most
research on this basic topic. The reason for children at the time that they begin to lear n about
psychologists’ neglect of tr ansformations is that geometry. Research suggests that it tak es children
they have concentrated on children distinguishing some time to a f orm an abstract concept of angle
between shapes rather than on their ability to that cuts across different contexts. More research
work out the relations between them. is needed on children’s understanding of the
relations between angles in par ticular figures.
3 There is the possibility of a clash betw een learning
about scale factors in enlargement and about the 8 Children seem well-placed to learn about the
measurement of area. This should be investigated. system of Car tesian co-ordinates since they are , on
the whole, able to extrapolate imaginary
perpendicular lines from hor izontal and vertical co-
ordinates and to work out where they intersect.
They do, however, often find co-ordinate tasks
36 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

which involve plotting and working out the


relationship between two or more positions quite
difficult. We need research on the reasons f or this
particular difficulty.

9 There is a ser ious problem about the quality of the


research that psychologists have done on children
learning geometry. Although psychologists have
carried out good work on children’s spatial
understanding, they have done very little to extend
this work to deal with f ormal learning about the
mathematics of space. There is a special need f or
longitudinal studies, combined with inter vention
studies, of the link between informal spatial
knowledge and success in learning geometry.
37 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

References

Anderson, N. H., and Cuneo, D. (1978). The height + Butterworth, G., and Itakura, S. (2000). How the eyes,
width rule in children’s judgments of quantity. head and hand ser ve definite reference. British
Journal of Experimental Psychology- General, 107, Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18, 25-50.
335-378. Butterworth, G., and Jarrett, N. (1991). What minds
Bomba, P. C. (1984). The Development of have in common is space: spatial mechanisms
Orientation Categories between 2 and 4 Months serving joint visual attention in infancy. British
of Age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 55-72.
37, 609-636. Butterworth, G., and Morisette, P. (1996). Onset
Bornstein, M.H., Ferdinandsen, K., and Gross, C. of pointing and the acquisition of language in
(1981) Perception of symmetry in infancy. infancy. British Journal of Develomental Psychology,
Developmental Psychology, 17, 82-86. 14, 219-231.
Bremner, A., Bryant, P., and Mareschal, D. (2006). Buys, K., and Veltman, A. (2008). Measurement in
Object-centred spatial reference in 4-month-old Kindergarten 1 and 2. In M. van den Heuvel and
infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(1), 1-10. K. Buys (Eds.), Young children learn measurement
Bryant, P. E. (1969). Perception and memor y of the and geometr y: a learning-teaching trajectory with
orientation of visually presented lines by children. intermediate attainment targets for the lower grades
Nature, 224, 1331-1332. in Primary school (pp. 15-36). Rotterdam: Sense
Bryant, P. E. (1974). Perception and understanding in Publishers.
young children. London: Methuen. Casey, B., Andrews, N., Schindler, H., Kersh, J. E.,
Bryant, P. E., and Kopytynska, H. (1976). Spontaneous Samper, A., and Copley, J. V. (2008). The
measurement by young children. Nature, 260, 773. development of spatial skills through
Bryant, P. E., and Somerville, S. C. (1986). The spatial interventions involving block building activities.
demands of graphs. British Journal of Psychology, Cognition and Instruction, 26, 1-41.
77, 187-197. Casey, B., Erkut, S., Ceder, I., and Young, J. M. (2008).
Bryant, P. E., and Trabasso, T. (1971). Transitive Use of a stor ytelling context to improve girls’ and
inferences and memory in young children. Nature, boys’ geometry skills in kindergarten. Journal of
232, 456-458. Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 29-48.
Butterworth, G. (2002). Joint visual attention in Clements, D. H., and Sarama, J. (2007 a.). Effects of a
infancy. In G. Bremner and A. Fogel (Eds.), preschool mathematics curriculum: Summative
Blackwell Handbook of Infant Development: research on the Building Blocks project. .Journal
Handbooks of Developmental Psychology (pp. 213- for Research in Mathematics Education, 38,
240). Oxford: Blackwell. 136-163
Butterworth, G., and Cochran, C. (1980). Towards a Clements, D. H., and Sarama, J. (2007 b.). Early
mechanism of joint attention in human infancy. childhood mathematics learning. In F. K. Lester
International Journal of Behavioural Development, (Ed.), Second handbook of research on mathematics
3, 253-272. teaching and learning . (Vol. 1, pp. 461-555).
Butterworth, G., and Grover, L. (1988). The origins of Charlotte, NC:: Information Age Publishing.
referential communication in human infancy. In L. Clements, D., Wilson, D., and Sarama, J. (2004). Young
Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought wihtout language (pp. 5- children’s composition of geometr ic figures: a
24). Oxford: Oxford University Press. learning trajectory. Mathematical Thinking and
Learning, 6(2), 163-184.
38 PaperSUMMARY
5: Understanding
– PAPERspace
2: Understanding
and its representation
whole numbers
in mathematics

De Bock, D., Van Dooren, W., Janssens, D., and Lawrenson, W., and Bryant, P. E. (1972). Absolute and
Verschaffel, L. (2002). Improper use of linear relative codes in young children. Journal of Child
reasoning: An in-depth study of the nature and Psychology and Psychiatry, 12, 25-35.
the irresistibility of secondar y school students’ Luchins, A. S., and Luchins, E. H. (1970). Wertheimer’s
errors. Educational Studies in Mathematics , 50, Seminars Re-visited: Problem solving and thinking
311–334. Vols I to III. Albany, N.Y.: Faculty Student
De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., and Janssens, D. (1998). Association, State University of New York.
The predominance of the linear model in Magina, S., and Hoyles, C. (1997). Children’s
secondary school students’ solutions of word understanding of turn and angle. In T. Nunes and
problems involving length and area of similar P. Bryant (Eds.), Learning and Teaching
plane figures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, Mathematics. An International Perspective (pp. 88-
35, 65–83. 114). Hove (UK): Psychology Press.
De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., and Janssens, D. (2002). Mitchelmore, M. (1998). Young students’ concepts of
The effects of different problem presentations turning. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 265-284.
and formulations on the illusion of linearity in Mitchelmore, M., and White, P. (2000). Development
secondary school students. Mathematical Thinking of angle concepts by progressive abstraction and
and Learning, 4, 65–89. generalisation. Educational Studies in Mathematics ,
De Bock, D., Verschaffel, L., Janssens, D., Van Dooren, 41, 209-238.
W., and Claes, K. (2003). Do realistic contexts and Noss, R. (1988). Children’s learning of geometrical
graphical representations always have a beneficial concepts through Logo. Journal for Research in
impact on students’ performance? Negative Mathematics Education, 18(5), 343-362.
evidence from a study on modeling non-linear Nunes, T., Light, P., and Mason, J. (1993). Tools for
geometry problems. Learning and Instruction, 13, thought: the measurement of lenght and area.
441-463. Learning and Instruction, 3, 39-54.
Dembo, Y., Levin, I., and Siegler, R.S. (1997). A Outhred, L., and Mitchelmore, M. (2000). Young
comparison of the geometric reasoning of students children’s intuitive understanding of rectangular
attending Israeli ultra-orthodox and mainstream area measurement. Journal for Research in
schools. Developmental Psychology, 33, 92-103. Mathematics Education, 31(2), 144-167.
Guttierez, A. (1992). Exploring the link between van Papert, S. (1980). Mindstorms: children, computers and
Hiele’s levels and 3-dimensional geometry. powerful ideas. Brighton: Harvester Press.
Structural Topology, 18, 31-48. Piaget, J., and Inhelder, B. (1963). The Child’s
Hart, K. (1981). Children’s understanding of Conception of Space. London: Routledge. and
mathematics: 11-16. London: John Murray. Kegan Paul.
Hart, K., Brown, M., Kerslake, D., Küchemann, D. and Piaget, J., Inhelder, B., and Szeminska, A. (1960). The
Ruddock, G. (1985) Chelsea Diagnostic child’s conception of geometr y. London: Routledge
Mathematics’ Test: Teacher’s Guide. Windsor: NFER- and Kegan Paul.
Nelson. Quinn, P. C., Siqueland, E. R., and Bomba, P. C. (1985).
Howard, I. P. (1978). Recognition and knowledge of Delayed recognition memor y for orientation by
the water level principle. Perception, 7, 151-160. human infants. Journal of Experimental Child
Ibbotson, A., and Br yant, P. E. (1976). The Psychology, 40, 293-303.
perpendicular error and the vertical effect in Rock, I. (1970). Towards a cognitive theory of
children’s drawing. Perception, 5, 319-326. perceptual constancy. In A. Gilgen (Ed.),
Kloosterman, P., Warfield, J., Wearne, D., Koc, Y., Contemporary Sceintific psychology (pp. 342-378).
Martin, G., and Strutchens, M. (2004). Knowledge New York: Academic Press.
of learning mathematics and perceptions of Sarama, J., Clements, D.H., Swaminathan, S., McMillen,
learning mathematics in 4th-gr ade students. In P. S., and Gomez, G. (2003) Development of
Kloosterman and F. Lester (Eds.), Resutlts and Mathematics Concepts of Two-Dimensional
interpetations of the 2003 mathematics assessment Space in Grid Environments: an exploratory
of the NAEP. Reston, VA: national Council Of study. Cognition and Instruction, 21, 285-324.
Teachers of Mathematics.
Knuth, E.J. (2000) Student under standing of the
Cartesian connection: an exploratory study.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education ,
31, 500-508.
39 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Seo, K-H., and Ginsburg, H. (2004) What is Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in Society. Cambridge,
developmentally appropriate in early childhood Mass: Harvard University Press.
mathematics education? In D.H. Clements, J. Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive Thinking. New York:
Sarama, and A-M. Dibias (Eds.) Engaging young Harper and Row.
children in mathematics: Standards for early Wilkening, F. (1979). Combining of stimulus
childhood mathematics education. (pp.91-104). dimensions in children’s and adult’s judgements
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. of area: an information integration analysis.
Slater, A. (1999). Visual organisation and perceptual Developmental Psychology, 15, 25-53.
constancies in early infancy. In V. Walsh and J. Wilkening, F., and Anderson, N. H. (1982).
Kulikowski (Eds.), Perceptual Constancy: why things Comparison of two-rule assessment
look as they do (pp. 6-30). Cambridge: C.U.P. methodologies for studying cognitive
Slater, A., Field, T., and Hernadez-Reif, M. (2002). The development and knowledge structure.
development of the senses. In Slater and M. Lewis Psychological Bulletin, 92, 215-237.
(Eds.), Introduction to Infant Development (pp. 83-
98). Oxford: O.U.P.
Slater, A., and Lewis, M. (2002). Introduction to infant
development. Oxford: O.U.P.
Slater, A., Mattock, A., and Brown, E. (1990). Size
constancy at bir th: newborn infants’ responses to
retinal and real size. Journal of Experimental Child
Psychology, 49, 314-322.
Slater, A., and Morison, V. (1985). Shape constancy
and slant perception at birth. Perception, 14, 337-
344.
Somerville, S., and Br yant, P. E. (1985). Young
children’s use of spatial coordinates. Child
Development, 56, 604-613.
Sowder, J., Wearne, D., Mar tin, G., and Strutchens, M.
(2004). What grade 8 students know about
mathematics: changes over a decade. In P.
Kloosterman and F. Lester (Eds.), The 1990
through 2000 mathematics assessment of the
NAEP: Results and interpretations (pp. 105-143).
Reston, VA: National Councilof Teachers of
Mathematics.
van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. and Buys, K. (2008)
Young children learn measurement and Geometr y: a
learning-teaching trajectory with intermediate
attainment targets for the lower grades in Primary
school. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.
van den Heuvel, M., Veltman, A., Janssen, C., and
Hochstenbach, J. (2008). Geometry in
Kindergarten 1 and 2. In M. van den Heuvel and
K. Buys (Eds.), Young children learn measurement
and geometr y: a learning-teaching trajectory with
intermediate attainment targets for the lower grades
in Primary school. Rotterdam: Sense Publishers.
van Hiele, P. M. (1986). Structure and insight. Orlando,
Fl: Academic Press.
Vergnaud, G. (1983). Multiplicative structures.
In R. Lesh and M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition
of mathematics concepts and processes
(pp. 128-175). London: Academic Press.
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-72-6

www.nuffieldfoundation.org
6
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning
By Anne Watson, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Summary of Paper 6 3
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: Algebraic reasoning 8

Paper 1: Overview References 37


Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities About the author
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical Anne Watson is Professor of Mathematics
representation Education at the University of Oxford.
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation
in mathematics
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning About the Nuffield Foundation
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
concepts charitable trust established in 1943 by William
Paper 8: Methodological appendix Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant being. We fund research and pr actical
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while experiment and the development of capacity
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics to under take them; working across education,
in secondary schools. science, social science and social policy. While
most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which responsive grant programmes we also
has been published separately as Introduction and undertake our own initiatives.
summary of findings.

Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published


together as Summary papers.

All publications are available to download from


our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary of paper 6:
Algebraic reasoning

Headlines Understanding symbolisation


• Algebra is the way we express generalisations The conventional symbol system is not merel y an
about numbers, quantities, relations and functions. expression of generalised arithmetic; to understand
For this reason, good understanding of connections it students have to understand the meanings of
between numbers, quantities and relations is arithmetical operations, rather than just be able to
related to success in using algebra. In particular, carry them out. Students have to understand ‘inverse’
students need to understand that addition and and know that addition and subtraction are inverses,
subtraction are inverses, and so are multiplication and that division is the inverse of multiplication.
and division. Algebraic representations of relations between
quantities, such as difference and ratio, encapsulate
• To understand algebraic symbolisation, students this idea of inverse. Using familiarity with symbolic
have to (a) understand the underlying operations expressions of these connections, rather than thinking
and (b) become fluent with the notational rules. in terms of generalising four arithmetical operations,
These two kinds of learning, the meaning and the gives students tools with which to under stand
symbol, seem to be most successful when students commutativity and distributivity, methods of solving
know what is being expressed and have time to equations, and manipulations such as simplifying and
become fluent at using the notation. expanding expressions.

• Students have to learn to recognise the different The precise use of notation has to be lear nt as well,
nature and roles of letters as: unknowns, variables, of course, and many aspects of algebr aic notation
constants and parameters, and also the meanings are inherently confusing (e.g. 2r and r 2). Over-
of equality and equivalence. These meanings are reliance on substitution as a method of doing this
not always distinct in algebra and do not relate can lead students to get stuck with ar ithmetical
unambiguously to arithmetical understandings, meanings and rules, rather than being able to
Mapping symbols to meanings is not learnt in recognise algebraic structures. For example, students
one-off experiences. who have been taught to see expressions such as:
97 – 49 + 49
• Students often get confused, misapply, or
misremember rules for transforming expressions as structures based on relationships between
and solving equations. They often try to apply numbers, avoiding calculation, identifying variation, and
arithmetical meanings to algebraic expressions having a sense of limits of var iability, are able to reason
inappropriately. This is associated with over- with relationships more securely and at a younger age
emphasis on notational manipulation, or on than those who have focused only on calculation. An
‘generalised arithmetic’, in which they may try expression such as 3x + 4 is both the answ er to a
to get concise answers. question, an object in itself, and also an algor ithm or
process for calculating a par ticular value. This has
parallels in arithmetic: the answer to 3 ÷ 5 is 3/5.
4 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Time spent relating algebr aic expressions to Expressions linked by the ‘equals’ sign might be
arithmetical structures, as opposed to calculations, not just numerically equal, but also equivalent, yet
can make a difference to students’ understanding. students need to retain the ‘unknown’ concept when
This is especially impor tant when understanding that setting up and solving equations which ha ve finite
apparently different expressions can be equivalent, solutions. For example, 10x – 5 = 5(2x – 1) is a
and that the processes of manipulation (often the statement about equivalence, and x is a variable, but
main focus of algebra lessons) are actually 10x – 5 = 2x + 1 defines a value of the variable for
transformations between equivalent forms. which this equality is tr ue. Thus x in the second case
can be seen as an unkno wn to be found, but in the
first case is a var iable. Use of gr aphical software can
Meanings of letters show the difference visually and powerfully because
and signs the first situation is represented by one line, and the
second by two intersecting lines, i.e. one point.
Large studies of students’ interpretation and use of
letters have shown a well-defined set of possible
actions. Learners may, according to the task and Misuse of rules
context:
• try to evaluate them using irrelevant information Students who rely only on remembered r ules often
• ignore them misapply them, or misremember them, or do not
• used as shorthand for objects, e.g. a = apple think about the meaning of the situations in which
• treat them as objects they might be successfully applied. Many students
• use a letter as a specific unknown will use guess-and-check as a first resor t when
• use a letter as a generalised number solving equations, par ticularly when numbers are
• use a letter as a variable. small enough to reason about ‘hidden numbers’
instead of ‘undoing’ within the algebr aic structure.
Teachers have to understand that students may use Although this is sometimes a successful str ategy,
any one of these approaches and students need to particularly when used in conjunction with gr aphs,
learn when these are appropr iate or inappropriate. or reasoning about spatial str uctures, or practical
There are conventions and uses of letter s situations, over-reliance can obstruct the
throughout mathematics that have to be under stood development of algebraic understanding and
in context, and the statement ‘letters stand for more universally applicable techniques.
numbers’ is too simplistic and can lead to confusion.
For example: Large-scale studies of U.K. school children show
• it is not always true that different letters have that, despite being taught the BIDMAS r ule and its
different values equivalents, most do not kno w how to decide on
• a letter can have different values in the same the order of oper ations represented in an algebraic
problem if it stands for a variable expression. Some researchers believe this to be due
• the same letter does not have to have the same to not fully understanding the under lying operations,
value in different problems. others that it may be due to misinter pretation of
expressions. There is evidence from Australia and
A critical shift is from seeing a letter as representing the United Kingdom that students who are taught
an unknown, or ‘hidden’, number defined within a to use flow diagrams, and inverse flow diagrams, to
number sentence such as: construct and reorganise expressions are better
able to decide on the order implied b y expressions
3+x=8
involving combinations of oper ations. However, it is
to seeing it as a var iable, as in y = 3 + x, or 3 = y – x. not known whether students taught this way can
Understanding x as some kind of gener alized successfully apply their knowledge of order in
number which can take a range of values is seen b y situations in which flow diagrams are inappropriate,
some researchers to provide a bridge from the idea such as with polynomial equations, those involving
of unknown to that of var iables. The use of boxes the unknown on ‘both sides’, and those with more
to indicate unknown numbers in simple ‘missing than one variable. To use algebra effectively,
number’ statements is sometimes helpful, but can decisions about order have to be fluent
also lead to confusion when used f or variables, or and accurate.
for more than one hidden n umber in a statement.
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Misapplying arithmetical to express known general relationships, have been


meanings to algebraic successful both for aiding their under standing of
expressions symbol use, and understanding the underlying
quantitative relations being expressed. For example,
Analysis of children’s algebra in clinical studies with some year 1 children first compare and discuss
12- to 13-year-olds found that the main problems in quantities of liquid in different vessels, and soon
moving from arithmetic to algebra arose because: become able to use letter s to stand for unknown
• the focus of algebra is on relations rather than amounts in relationships, such as a > b; d = e; and so
calculations; the relation a + b = c represents three on. In another example , older primary children could
unknown quantities in an additive relationship generalise the well-known questions of how many
• students have to understand inverses as well as people can sit round a line of tab les, given that there
operations, so that a hidden value can be found can be two on each side of each tab le and one at
even if the answer is not obvious from knowing each of the extreme ends. The ways in which
number bonds or multiplication facts; 7 + b = 4 can students count differ, so the forms of the gener al
be solved using knowledge of addition, but c + 63 statement also differ and can be compared, such as:
= 197 is more easily solved if subtraction is used as ‘multiply the number of tables by 4 and add 2 or ‘it
the inverse of addition is two times one more than the n umber of tables’.
• some situations have to be expressed algebraically
first in order to solve them. ‘My brother is two The use of algebr a to express known arithmetical
years older than me, my sister is five years younger generalities is successful with students who ha ve
than me; she is 12, how old will my brother be in developed advanced mental str ategies for dealing
three years’ time?’ requires an analysis and with additive, multiplicative and proportional
representation of the relationships before solution. operations (e.g. compensation as in 82 – 17 = 87 –
‘Algebra’ in this situation means constructing a 17 – 5). When students are allowed to use their
method for keeping track of the unknown as own methods of calculation they often f ind algebraic
various operations act upon it. structures for themselves. For example, expressing
• letters and numbers are used together, so that 13 x 7 as 10 x 7 + 3 x 7, or as 2 x 7 2 –7, are
numbers may have to be treated as symbols in a enactments of distributivity and learners can
structure, and not evaluated. For example, the represent these symbolically once they know that
structure 2(3+b) is different from the structure of 6 letters can stand for numbers, though this is not
+ 2b although they are equivalent in computational trivial and needs several experiences. Explaining a
terms. Learners have to understand that sometimes general result, or structure, in words is often a
it is best to leave number as an element in an helpful precursor to algebraic representation.
algebraic structure rather than ‘work it out’.
• the equals sign has an expanded meaning; in Fortunately, generalising from experience is a natur al
arithmetic it is often taken to mean ‘calculate’ but in human propensity, but the everyday inductive
algebra it usually means ‘is equal to’ or ‘is equivalent reasoning we do in other contexts is not al ways
to’. It takes many experiences to recognise that an appropriate for mathematics. Deconstruction of
algebraic equation or equivalence is a statement diagrams and physical situations, and identification of
about relations between quantities, or between relationships between variables, have been found to
combinations of operations on quantities. Students be more successful methods of dev eloping a formula
tend to want ‘closure’ by compressing algebraic than pattern-generalisation from number sequences
expressions into one term instead of understanding alone. The use of verbal descriptions has been shown
what is being expressed. to enable students to br idge between observing
relations and writing them algebraically.

Expressing generalisations Further aspects of algebr a arise in the companion


summaries, and also in the main body of Paper 6:
In several studies it has been found that students Algebraic reasoning.
understand how to use algebr a if they have
focused on generalizing with numerical and spatial
representations in which counting is not an option.
Attempts to introduce symbols to v ery young
students as tools to be used when they ha ve a need
6 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Recommendations
Research about mathematical Recommendations for teaching
learning

The bases for using algebraic symbolisation Emphasis should be given to reading numerical and algebraic
successfully are (a) under standing the expressions relationally, rather than computationally. For
underlying operations and relations and algebraic thinking, it is more impor tant to understand how
(b) being able to use symbolism cor rectly. operations combine and relate to each other than how they
are performed. Teachers should avoid emphasising symbolism
without understanding the relations it represents.

Children interpret ‘letter stands for Developers of the cur riculum, advisory schemes of work and
number’ in a var iety of ways, according to teaching methods need to be a ware of children’s possible
the task. Mathematically, letters have interpretations of letters, and also that when cor rectly used,
several meanings according to context: letters can have a range of meanings. Teachers should avoid
unknown, variable, parameter, constant. using materials that oversimplify this variety. Hands-on ICT can
provide powerful new ways to understand these differences in
several representations.

Children interpret ‘=’ to mean ‘calculate’; Developers of the cur riculum, advisory schemes of work and
but mathematically ‘=’ means either ‘equal teaching methods need to be a ware of the difficulties about
to’ or ‘equivalent to’. the ‘=’ sign and use multiple contexts and explicit language .
Hands-on ICT can provide powerful new ways to understand
these differences in several representations.

Students often forget, misremember, Developers of the cur riculum, advisory schemes of work and
misinterpret situations and misapply rules. teaching methods need to tak e into account that algebr aic
understanding takes time, multiple experiences, and clarity of
purpose. Teachers should emphasise situations in which
generalisations can be identified and described to provide
meaningful contexts for the use of algebr aic expressions. Use
of software which car ries out algebraic manipulations should
be explored.

Everyone uses ‘guess-&-check’ if answers Algebra is meaningful in situations f or which specific arithmetic
are immediately obvious, once algebraic cannot be easily used, as an expression of relationships. Focusing
notation is understood. on algebra as ‘generalised arithmetic’, e.g. with substitution
exercises, does not give students reasons for using it.

Even very young students can use letter s Algebraic expressions of relations should be a commonplace
to represent unknowns and variables in in mathematics lessons, par ticularly to express relations and
situations where they have reasoned a equivalences. Students need to have multiple experiences of
general relationship by relating proper ties. algebraic expressions of gener al relations based in proper ties,
Research on inductive generalisation from such as arithmetical rules, logical relations, and so on as w ell as
pattern sequences to develop algebra the well-known inductive reasoning from sequences.
shows that moving from expressing simple
additive patterns to relating proper ties has
to be explicitly suppor ted.
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Recommendations for research

• The main body of Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning


includes a number of areas for which further
research would be valuable, including the following.

• How does explicit work on understanding relations


between quantities enable students to move
successfully from arithmetical to algebraic thinking?

• What kinds of explicit work on expressing


generality enable students to use algebra?

• What are the longer-term comparative effects of


different teaching approaches to early algebra on
students’ later use of algebraic notation and
thinking?

• How do learners’ synthesise their knowledge of


elementary algebra to understand polynomial
functions, their factorisation and roots, simultaneous
equations, inequalities and other algebraic objects
beyond elementary expressions and equations?

• What useful kinds of algebraic expertise could be


developed through the use of computer algebra
systems in school?
8 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Algebraic reasoning

In this review of how students learn algebra we try students’ natural ability to discer n patterns and
to balance an approach which f ocuses on what generalise them, and their growing competence in
learners can do and how their generalising and use understanding and using symbols; however this
of symbols develop (a ‘bottom up’ developmental would not take us very far in consider ing all the
approach), and a view which states what is required aspects of school algebr a. The content of school
in order to do higher mathematics (a ‘top down’ algebra as the development of algebraic reasoning is
hierarchical approach). The ‘top down’ view often expressed by Thomas and Tall (2001) as the shifts
frames school algebra as a list of techniques which between procedure, process/concept, generalised
need to be fluent. This is manifested in research arithmetic, expressions as evaluation processes,
which focuses on errors made by learners in the manipulation, towards axiomatic algebra. In this
curriculum and small-scale studies designed to perspective it helps to see manipulation as the
ameliorate these. This research tells us about generation and transformation of equivalent
development of understanding by identifying the expressions, and the identification of specific
obstacles which have to be overcome, and also values for variables within them.
reveals how learners think. It therefore makes sense
to star t by outlining the different aspects of algebr a. At school level, algebra can be descr ibed as:
However, this is not suggesting that all mathematics • manipulation and transformation of symbolic
teaching and learning should be directed towards statements
preparation for higher mathematics. • generalisations of laws about numbers and patterns
• the study of structures and systems abstracted
By contrast a ‘bottom up’ view usually focuses on from computations and relations
algebraic thinking, taken to mean the expression • rules for transforming and solving equations
and use of gener al statements about relationships • learning about variables, functions and expressing
between variables. Lins (1990) sought a def inition of change and relationships
algebraic thinking which encompassed the different • modelling the mathematical structures of situations
kinds of engagement with algebr a that run through within and outside mathematics.
mathematics. He concluded that algebr aic thinking
was an intentional shift from context (which could Bell (1996) and Kaput (1998; 1999) emphasise the
be ‘real’, or a par ticular mathematical case) to process of symbolisation, and the need to oper ate
structure. Thus ‘algebraic thinking arises when people with symbolic statements and the use them within
are detecting and expressing str ucture, whether in and outside algebra, but algebra is much more than
the context of problem solving concerning numbers the acquisition of a sign system with which to
or some modelled situation, whether in the context express known concepts. Vergnaud (1998) identifies
of resolving a class of prob lems, or whether in the new concepts that students will meet in algebr a as:
context of studying str ucture more generally’ (Lins, equations, formulae, functions, variables and
1990). Thus a complementary ‘bottom up’ view parameters. What makes them new is that symbols
includes consideration of the development of are higher order objects than n umbers and become
9 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

mathematical objects in their own right; arithmetic Part 1: arithmetic, algebra,


has to work in algebraic systems, but symbol systems letters, operations, expressions
are not merely expressions of general arithmetic.
Furthermore, ‘the words and symbols we use to
communicate do not refer directly to reality but to Relationships between arithmetic
represented entities: objects, properties, relationships, and algebra
processes, actions, and constructs, about which there In the United States, there is a strong commitment
is no automatic agreement’ (p.167). to arithmetic, par ticularly fluency with fr actions, to be
seen as an essential precur sor for algebra: ‘Proficiency
In this paper I dr aw on the research evidence about with whole numbers, fractions, and par ticular aspects
the first five of the aspects above. In the next paper I of geometry and measurement are the Cr itical
shall tackle modelling and associated issues, and their Foundation of Algebra. …The teaching of fr actions
relation to mathematical reasoning and application must be acknowledged as critically impor tant and
more generally at school level. improved before an increase in student achiev ement
in Algebra can be expected.’ (NMAP, 2008). While
It would be naïve to write about algebraic reasoning number sense precedes formal algebra in age-related
without repor ting the considerable difficulties that developmental terms, this one-way relationship is far
students have with adopting the conventions of from obvious in mathematical ter ms. In the United
algebra, so the first part of this review addresses the Kingdom where secondar y algebra is not taught
relationship between arithmetic and algebra, and the separately from other mathematics, integration
obstacles that have to be overcome to understand across mathematics makes a two-way relationship
the meaning of letter s and expressions and to use possible, seeing arithmetic as par ticular instances of
them. The second par t looks at difficulties which are algebraic structures which have the added feature
evident in three approaches used to develop that they can be calculated. For example, rather than
algebraic reasoning: expressing generalities; solving knowing the procedures of fractions so that they can
equations; and working with functions. The third be generalised with letters and hence make algebraic
part summarises the findings and makes fractions, it is possible for fraction calculations to be
recommendations for practice and research. seen as enactments of relationships betw een rational
structures, those generalised enactments being
expressed as algorithms.

For this review we see number sense as preceding


formal algebra in students’ learning, but to imagine
that algebraic understanding is merely a
generalisation of arithmetic, or grows directly from it,
is a misleading over-simplification.

Kieran’s extensive work (e.g. 1981, 1989, 1992)


involving clinical studies with ten 12- to 13-y ear-olds
leads her to identify five inherent difficulties in making
a direct shifts between arithmetic and algebra.

• The focus of algebra is on relations rather than


calculations; the relation a + b = c represents two
unknown numbers in an additive relation, and while
3 + 5 = 8 is such a relation it is more usually seen
as a representation of 8, so that 3 + 5 can be
calculated whereas a + b cannot.

• Students have to understand inverses as well as


operations, so that finding a hidden number can
be done even if the answer is not obvious from
knowing number bonds or multiplication facts; 7 +
b = 4 can be done using knowledge of addition,
10 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

but c + 63 = 197 is more easil y done if gap exists, we need to know if it is developmental or
subtraction is used as the in verse of addition 1. epistemological, i.e. do we have to wait till lear ners
Some writers claim that under standing this are ready, or could teaching make a difference? A
structure is algebraic, while others say that doing bottom-up view would be that algebr aic thinking is
arithmetic to find an unknown is arithmetical often counter-intuitive, requires good under standing
reasoning, not algebraic reasoning. of the symbol system, and abstract meanings which
do not arise through normal engagement with
• Some situations have to be expressed algebraically phenomena. Nevertheless the shifts required to
in order to solve them, rather than starting a understand it are shifts the mind is ab le to make
solution straight away. ‘I am 14 and my brother is 4 given sufficient experiences with new kinds of object
years older than me’ can be solved by addition, but and their representations. A top-down view would be
‘My brother is two years older than me, my sister is that students’ prior knowledge, conceptualisations and
five years younger than me; she is 12, how old will tendencies create errors in algebra. Carraher and
my brother be in three years’ time?’ requires an colleagues (Carraher, Brizuela & Earnest, 2001;
analysis and representation of the relationships Carraher, Schliemann & Br izuela, 2001) show that the
before solution. This could be with letters, so that processes involved in shifting from an ar ithmetical
the answer is obtained by finding k where k – 5 = view to an algebr aic view, that is from quantifying
12 and substituting this value into (k + 2) + 3. expressions to expressing relations between variables,
Alternatively it could be done by mapping systems are repeated for new mathematical str uctures at
of points onto a numberline, or using other higher levels of mathematics, and hence are
symbols for the unknowns. ‘Algebra’ in this situation characteristics of what it means to lear n mathematics
means constructing a method for keeping track of at every level rather than developmental stages of
the unknown as various operations act upon it. learners. This same point is made again and again b y
mathematics educators and philosophers who point
• Letters and numbers are used together, so that out that such shifts are fundamental in mathematics,
numbers may have to be treated as symbols in and that reification of new ideas, so that they can be
a structure, and not evaluated. For example, the treated as the elements for new levels of thought,
structure 2(a + b) is different from the str ucture is how mathematics develops both historically and
of 2a + 2b although they are equivalent in cognitively. There is considerable agreement that
computational terms. these shifts require the action of teacher s and
teaching, since they all involve new ways of thinking
• The equals sign has an expanded meaning; in that are unlikely to arise naturally in situations (Filloy
arithmetic it often means ‘calculate’ but in algebra and Sutherland, 1996).
it more often means ‘is equal to’ or even ‘is
equivalent to’. Some of the differences reported in research rest on
what is, and what is not, described as algebraic. For
If algebra is seen solely as generalised arithmetic example, the equivalence class of fr actions that
(we take this to mean the expression of gener al represent the rational number 3/5 is all fractions of
arithmetical rules using letters), many problems arise the form 3k/5k (k Є N)2. It is a cur riculum decision,
for learning and teaching. Some writers describe rather than a mathematical one, whether equivalent
these difficulties as manifestations of a ‘cognitive gap’ fractions are called ‘arithmetic’ or ‘algebra’ but
between arithmetic and algebra (Filloy and Rojano, whatever is decided, learners have to shift from seeing
1989; Herscovics and Linchevski, 1994). For example, 3/5 as ‘three cakes shared between five people’ to a
Filloy and Rojano saw students dealing ar ithmetically quantitative label for a general class of objects
with equations of the form ax + b = c , where a, b structured in a particular quantitative relationship. This
and c are numbers, using inverse operations on the is an example of the kind of shift lear ners have to
numbers to complete the ar ithmetical statement. make from calculating number expressions to seeing
They saw this as ‘arithmetical’ because it depended such expressions as meaningful str uctures.
only on using oper ations to find a ‘hidden’ number.
The same students acted algebr aically with equations Attempts to introduce symbols to v ery young
such as ax + b = cx + d , treating each side as an students as tools to be used when they ha ve a need
expression of relationships and using direct to express general relationships, can be successful
operations not to ‘undo’ but to maintain the equation both for them understanding symbol use, and
by manipulating the expressions and equality. If such a understanding the underlying quantitative relations
11 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

being expressed (Dougher ty, 1996; 2001). In Students who had developed advanced mental
Dougherty’s work, students star ting school strategies, (e.g. compensation as in 82 – 17 = 87 –
mathematics first compare and discuss quantities of 17 – 5) for dealing with additive, multiplicative and
liquid in different vessels, and soon become able to proportional operations, could use letter s in
use letters to stand for unknown amounts. Arcavi conventional algebra once they knew that they
(1994) found that, with a r ange of students from ‘stood for’ numbers. Those who did best at algebra
middle school upwards over several years, symbols were those in schools where teacher s had focused
could be used as tools ear ly on to express on generalizing with numerical and spatial
relationships in a situation. The example he uses is the representations in situations where counting was
well-known one of expressing how many people can not a sensible option.
sit round a line of tab les, given that there can be tw o
on each side and one at each of the extreme ends. There are differences in the meaning of notation as
The ways in which students count diff er, so the forms one shifts between arithmetic and algebra. Wong
of the general statement also differ, such as: ‘multiply (1997) tested and inter viewed four classes of
the number of tables by 4 and add two’ or ‘it is two secondary students to see whether they could
times one more than the n umber of tables’. In Brown distinguish between similar notations used for
and Coles’ work (e.g. 1999, 2001), several years of arithmetic and algebra. For example, in arithmetic the
analysis of Coles’ whole-class teaching showed that expression 3(4 + 5) is both a str ucture of operations
generalising by expressing structures was a powerful and an invitation to calculate, but in algebra a(b + c)
basis for students to need symbolic notation, which is only a structure of operations. Thus students get
they could then use with meaning. For example, to confused when given mixtures such as 3(b + 5)
express a number such that ‘twice the number plus because they can assume this is an in vitation to
three’ is ‘three less’ than ‘add three and double the calculate. This tendency to confuse what is possib le
number’ a student who has been in a class of 12- with numbers and letters is subtle and depends on
year-olds where expression of gener al relationships is the expression. For example, Wong found that
a normal and frequent activity introduced N for expressions such as (2am)n are harder to simplify and
himself without prompting when it is appropr iate. substitute than (hk)n, possibly because the second
expression seems very clearly in the realm of algebr a
When students are allowed to use their own and rules about letters. Where Booth and Kier an
methods of calculation they often f ind algebraic claim that it is not the symbolic conventions alone
structures for themselves. For example, expressing that create difficulties but more often a lack of
13 x 7 as 10 x 7 + 3 x 7, or as 2 x 7 2 - 7, are understanding of the underlying operations, Wong’s
enactments of distributivity (and, implicitly, work helpfully foregrounds some of the inevitable
commutativity and associativity) and can be confusions possible in symbolic conventions. The
represented symbolically, though this shift is not student has to under stand when to calculate , when
trivial (Anghileri, Beishuizen and van Putten,2002; to leave an expression as a statement about
Lampert, 1986). On the other hand, allowing operations, what par ticular kind of number
students to develop a mindset in which an y method (unknown, general or variable) is being denoted, and
that gives a right answer is as good as any other can what the structure looks like with numbers and
lock learners into additive procedures where letters in combination. As an example of the last
multiplicative ones would be more generalisable, difficulty, 2X is found to be harder to deal with than x2
multiplicative methods where exponential methods although they are visually similar in form.
would be more powerful, and so on. But some
number-specific arithmetical methods do exemplify The question for this review is theref ore not whether
algebraic structures, such as the transformation of learners can make such shifts, or when they make
13 x 7 descr ibed above. This can be seen either as them, but what are the shifts they have to make, and
‘deriving new number facts from known number in what circumstances do they make them.
facts’ or as an instance of algebr aic reasoning.
Summary
The impor tance of a link between the kinds of • Algebra is not just generalised arithmetic; there are
transformations necessary for mental arithmetic and significant differences between arithmetical and
algebraic thinking is demonstr ated in a three-year algebraic approaches.
longitudinal teaching and testing progr amme of 116
students aged 12 to 14 (Br itt and Irwin, 2007).
12 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

• The shifts from arithmetic to algebra are the Booth (1984) inter viewed 50 students aged 13 to
kinds of shifts of perception made throughout 15 years, following up with 17 fur ther case study
mathematics, e.g. from quantifying to relationships students. She took a subset of Kuchemann’ s
between quantities; from operations to structures meanings, ‘letters stand for numbers’, and fur ther
of operations. unpicked it to reveal problems based on students’
test answers and follow-up interviews. She identified
• Mental strategies can provide a basis for the following issues which, for us, identify more
understanding algebraic structures. about what students have to learn.

• Students will accept letters and symbols standing • It is not always true that different letters have
for numbers when they have quantitative different values; for example one solution to 3x +
relationships to express; they seem to be able to 5y = 8 is that x = y = 1.
use letters to stand for ‘hidden’ numbers and also
for ‘any’ number. • A letter can have different values in the same
problem, but not at the same time, if it stands for a
• Students are confused by expressions that combine variable (such as an equation having multiple roots,
numbers and letters, and by expressions in which or questions such as ‘find the value of y = x2 + x +
their previous experience of combinations are 2 when x = 1, 2, 3…’)
reversed. They have to learn to ‘read’ expressions
structurally even when numbers are involved. • The same letter does not have to have the same
value in different problems.

Meaning of letters • Values are not related to the alphabet (a = 1, b = 2


…; or y > p because of relative alphabetic position).
Students’ understanding of the meaning of letter s
in algebra, and how they use letters to express • Letters do not stand for objects (a for apples)
mathematical relationships, are at the root of except where the objects are units (such as m for
algebraic development. Kuchemann (1981) identified metres).
several different ways adolescent students used
letters in the Chelsea diagnostic test instr ument • Letters do not have to be presented in alphabetical
(Hart, 1981). His research is based on test paper s of order in algebraic expressions, although there are
2900 students between 12 and 16 (see Appendix 1). times when this is useful3.

Letters were: • Different symbolic rules apply in algebra and


• evaluated in some way, e.g. a = 1 arithmetic, e.g.: ‘2 lots of x’ is written ‘2x’ but two
• ignored, e.g. 3a taken to be 3 lots of 7 are not written ‘27’.
• used as shorthand for objects, e.g. a = apple
• treated as objects As well as in Booth’s study, paper and pencil tests
• used as a specific unknown that were administered to 2000 students in aged 11
• used as a generalised number to 15 in 24 Australian secondary schools in 1992
• used as a variable. demonstrated all the above confusions (MacGregor
and Stacey, 1997).
Within his categorisation there were correct and
incorrect uses, such as students who ascr ibed a value These problems are not resolved easily, because letters
to a letter based on idiosyncr atic decisions or past are used in mathematics in var ying ways. There is no
experience, e.g. x = 4 because it was 4 in the single correct way to use them. They are used
previous question. These interpretations appear to as labels for objects that have no numerical value, such
be task dependent, so learners had developed a as vertices of shapes, or for objects that do have
sense of what sor ts of question were treated in numerical value but are treated as gener al, such as
what kinds of ways, i.e. generalising (sometimes lengths of sides of shapes. They denote fixed constants
idiosyncratically) about question-types through such as g, e or π, also non-numerical constants such as i,
familiarity and prior experience. and also they represent unknowns which have to be
found, and variables. Distinguishing between these
meanings is usually not taught explicitly, and this lack
13 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

of instruction might cause students some difficulty. On – 5 = 5(2x – 1) is a statement about equivalence , and
the other hand it is v ery hard to explain how to know x is a variable, but 10x – 5 = 2x + 1 defines a value of
the difference between a parameter, a constant and a the variable for which this equality is true. Thus x in the
variable (e.g. when asked to ‘vary the constant’ to second case can be seen as an unknown to be found.
explore a structure), and successful students may
learn this only when it is necessar y to make such It is possible to address some of the prob lems by giving
distinctions in par ticular usage. It is particularly hard to particular tasks which force students to sor t out the
explain that the O and E in O + E = O (to indicate difference between parameters and variables (Drijvers,
odd and even numbers) are not algebraic, even though 2001). A parameter is a value that def ines the structure
they do refer to numbers. Interpretation is therefore of a relation. For example, in y = mx + c the variables
related to whether students under stand the algebraic are x and y, while m and c define the relationship and
context, expression, equation, equivalence, function or have to be fixed before we can consider the
other relation. It is not sur prising that Furinghetti and covariation of x and y. In the United Kingdom this is
Paola (1994) found that only 20 out of 199 students dealt with implicitly, and finding the gradient and
aged 12 to 17 could explain the diff erence between intercept in the case just descr ibed is seen as a special
parameters and variables and unknowns (see also kind of task. At A-level, however, students have to find
Bloedy-Vinner, 1994). Bills’ (2007) longitudinal study of coefficients for partial fractions, or the coefficients of
algebra learning in upper secondar y students noticed polynomials which have given roots, and after many
that the letters x and y have a special status, so that years of ‘finding x’ they can find it hard to use par ticular
these letters trigger certain kinds of behaviour (e.g. values for x to identify parameters instead. By that time
these are the variables; or (x.y) denotes the general only those who have chosen to do mathematics need
point). Although any letter can stand for any kind of to deal with it, and those who ear lier could only find
number, in practice there are conventions, such as x the m and c in y=mx + c by using formulae without
being an unknown; x, y, z being variables; a, b, c being comprehension may have given up maths. Fortunately,
parameters/coefficient or generalised lengths, and so on. the dynamic possibilities of ICT off er tools to fully
explore the variability of x and y within the constant
A critical shift is from seeing a letter as representing behaviour of m and c and it is possible that more
an unknown, or ‘hidden’, number defined within a extensive use of ICT and modelling approaches might
number sentence such as: develop the notion of var iable further.

3+x=8
Summary
to seeing it as a var iable, as in y = 3 + x, or 3 = y – x. • Letters standing for numbers can have many
While there is research to sho w how quasi-variables meanings.
such as boxes can help students understand the use
of letters in relational statements (see Car penter and • The ways in which operations and relationships are
Levi, 2000) the shift from unkno wn to variable when written in arithmetic and algebra differ.
similar letters are used to have different functions is
not well-researched. Understanding x as some kind of • Learners tend to fall into well-known habits and
generalised number which can take a range of values assumptions about the use of letters.
is seen by some researchers to provide a bridge from
the idea of unknown to that of variables (Bednarz, • A particular difficulty is the difference between
Kieran and Lee, 1996). unknowns, variables, parameters and constants,
unless these have meaning.
The algebra of unknowns is about using solution
methods to find mystery numbers; the algebra of • Difficulties in algebra are not merely about using
variables is about expressing and tr ansforming relations letters, but about understanding the underlying
between numbers. These different lines of thought operations and structures.
develop throughout school algebra. The ‘variable’ view
depends on the idea that the expressions linked by the • Students need to learn that there are different uses
‘equals’ sign might be not just n umerically equal, but for different letters in mathematical conventions; for
also equivalent, yet students need to retain the example, a, b and c are often used as parameters,
‘unknown’ concept when setting up and solving or generalised lengths in geometry, and x, y and z
equations which have finite solutions. For example, 10x are often used as variables.
14 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Recognising operations methods they used to solve word problems were


bound by context, and depended on counting, adding,
In several intervention studies and textbooks and reasoning with whole and half n umbers. They
students are expected to use algebr aic methods for were unable to express how to solve problems in
problems for which an answer is required, and for terms of arithmetical operations, so that algebraic
which ad hoc methods work perfectly well. This expressions of such operations were of little use ,
arises when solving equations with one unkno wn on being unrelated to their own methods. Similarly, their
one side where the answer is a positive integer (such methods of recording were not conducive to
as 3x + 2 = 14); in word problems which can be algebraic expression, because the roles of different
enacted or represented diagr ammatically (such as ‘I numbers and signs were not clear in the layout. For
have 15 fence posts and 42 metres of wire; how far example, if students calculate as they go along, rather
apart must the fence posts be to use all the wire than maintaining the arithmetical structure of a
and all the posts to mak e a straight fence?’); and in question, much information is lost. For example, 42 –22
these and other situations in which tr ial-and- becomes 16 – 4 and the ‘difference between two
adjustment work easily. Students’ choice to use squares’ is lost; similarly, turning rational or irrational
non-algebraic methods in these contexts cannot numbers into decimal fractions can lose both accuracy
be taken as evidence of problems with algebra. and structure.

In a teaching experiment with 135 students age 12 In Booth’s work it was not the use of letter s
to 13, Bednarz and Janvier (1996) found that a that is difficult, but the underlying arithmetical
mathematical analysis of the operations required for understanding. This again suppor ts the view that it
solution accurately predicted what students would find is not until ad hoc, number fact and guess-and-test
difficult, and they concluded that prob lems where one methods fail that students are lik ely to see a need
could start from what is known and work towards for algebraic methods, and in a cur riculum based on
what is not known, as one does in arithmetical expressions and equations this is lik ely occur when
calculations, were significantly easier than problems in solving equations with non-integer answers, where a
which there was no obvious bridge between knowns full understanding of division expressed as fr actions
and the unknown, and the relationship had to be would be needed, and when working with the
worked out and expressed before any calculations unknown on both sides of an equation. Alternatively,
could be made. Many students tried to work if students are tr ying to express gener al relationships,
arithmetically with these latter kinds of problem, use of letters is essential once they realise that
starting with a fictional number and working forwards, particular examples, while illustrating relationships, do
generating a structure by trial and error rather than not fully represent them. Nevertheless students’
identifying what would be appropriate. This study is invented methods give insight into what they might
one of many which indicate that under standing the know already that is f ormalisable, as in the 13 x 7
meaning of arithmetical operations, rather than merely example given above.
being able to carry them out, is an essential precursor
not only to deciding what operation is the right one to Others have also obser ved the persistence of
do, but also to expressing and under standing structures arithmetic (Kieran, 1992; Vergnaud, 1998). Vergnaud
of relations among operations (e.g. Booth, 1984). The compares two student protocols in solving a
impact of weak arithmetical understanding is also distance/time problem and comments that the
observed at a higher level, when students can confuse additive approach chosen by one is not conceptuall y
the kinds of propor tionality expressed in y = k/x and y similar to the multiplicative chosen by the other, even
= kx, thinking the former must be linear because it though the answers are the same , and that this linear
involves a ratio (Baker, Hemenway and Trigueros,2001). approach is more natur al for students than the
The ratio of k to x in the first case is specific for each multiplicative. Kieran (1983) conducted clinical
value of x, but the ratio of y to x in the second case is interviews with six 13-year-old students to find out
invariant and this indicates a propor tional relationship. why they had difficulty with equations. The students
tended to see tasks as about ‘getting answers’ and
Booth (1984) selected 50 students from f our schools could not accept an expression as meaningful in
to identify their most common er rors and to itself. This was also obser ved by Collis (1971) and
interview those who made cer tain kinds of error. This more recently by Ryan and Williams in their lar ge
led her to identify more closel y how their weakness scale study of students’ mathematical understanding,
with arithmetic limited their progress with algebr a. The drawing on a sample of about 15 000 U .K. students
15 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

(2007). Stacey and Macgregor (2000, p. 159) talk of Summary


the ‘compulsion to calculate’ and comment that at • Learners use number facts and guess-and-check
every stage students’ thinking in algebr aic problems rather than algebraic methods if possible.
was dominated by arithmetical methods, which
deflected them from using algebra. Fur thermore, • Doing calculations, such as in substitution and
Bednarz and Janvier (1996) showed that even those guess-and-check methods, distracts from the
who identified structure during interviews were likely development of algebraic understanding.
to revert to arithmetical methods minutes later. It
seemed as if testing par ticular numbers was an • Substitution can be useful in exploring equivalence
approach that not only overwhelmed any attempts of expressions.
to be more analytical, but also prevented
development of a str uctural method. • Word problems do not, on their own, scaffold a
shift to algebraic reasoning.
This suggests that too much focus on substitution in
early algebra, rather than developing understanding • Learners have to understand operations and their
of how structure is expressed, might allow a inverses.
‘calculation’ approach to per sist when working with
algebraic expressions. If calculation does per sist, then • Methods of recording arithmetic can scaffold a shift
it is only where calculation breaks down that to understanding operations.
algebraic understanding becomes crucial, or, as in
Bednarz and Janvier (1996), where word problems
do not yield to str aightforward application of What shifts have to be made
operations. For a long time in Soviet education word
between arithmetic and algebra?
problems formed the core of algebr a instruction.
Davydov (1990) was concer ned that arithmetic does Changing focus slightly, we now turn to what the
not necessarily lead to awareness of generality, learner has to see differently in order to overcome
because the approach degener ates into ‘letter the inherent problems discussed above. A key shift
arithmetic’ rather than the expression of gener ality. which has to be made is from f ocusing on answers
He developed the approach used by Dougher ty obtained in any possible way, to focusing on
(2001) in which young students have to express structure. Kieran (1989, 1992), reflecting on her long-
relationships before using algebra to generalize term work with middle school students, classifies
arithmetic. For example, students in the first year ‘structure’ in algebra as (1) surface str ucture of
of school compare quantities of liquid (‘do y ou have expression: arrangement of symbols and signs; (2)
more milk than me?’) and express the relationship systemic: operations within an expression and their
as, say, G < R. They understand that adding the same actions, order, use of br ackets etc.; (3) structure of an
amount to each does not mak e them equal, but that equation: equality of expressions and equivalence .
they have to add some to G to mak e them equal.
They do not use n umbers until relationships Boero (2001) identifies transformation and
between quantities are established. anticipation as key processes in algebr aic problem
solving, drawing on long-term research in authentic
Substituting values can, however, help students to classrooms, reconstructing learners’ meanings from
understand and verify relationships: it matters if this what they do and say. He obser ved two kinds of
is for an unknown: 5 = 2x – 7 where only one value transformation, firstly the contextual ar ithmetical,
will do; or for an equation where var iables will be physical and geometric transformations students do
related: y = 2x – 7; or to demonstr ate equivalence: to make the problem meaningful within their current
e.g. does 5(x + y) – 3 = 5x + 5y – 3 or 5x + y – 3? knowledge (see also Filloy, Rojano and Robio,2001);
But using substitution to under stand what secondly, the new kinds of tr ansformation made
expressions mean is not helpful. Fur thermore the available by the use of algebr a. If students’
choice of values offered in many textbooks can anticipation is locked into arithmetical activity: finding
exacerbate misunderstandings about the values answers, calculating, proceeding step-by-step from
letters can have. They can reinforce the view that a known to unknown (see also Dettor i, Garutti, and
letter can only take one value in one situation, and Lemut, 2001), and if their main exper ience of algebra
that different letters have to have different values, is to simplify expressions, then the shift to using the
and even that a = 1, b = 2 etc. new kinds of tr ansformation afforded by algebra is
16 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

hindered. Thus typical secondar y school algebraic indicating missing values in number sentences) as a
behaviour includes reaching for a formula and precursor to understanding generalization. Brown and
substituting numbers into it (Arzarello, Bazzini Coles (1999, 2001) develop a classroom environment
and Chiappini,1994), as is often demonstrated in in a U.K. secondary school in which relationships are
students’ meaningless approaches to finding areas developed which need to be expressed str ucturally,
and perimeters (Dickson, 1989 a). Typically students and algebraic reasoning becomes a tool to make new
will multiply every available edge length to get questions and transformations possible. These studies
area, and add everything to get perimeter. These span ages 6 to lower secondary and provide school-
approaches might also be manifestations of learners’ based evidence that the development of algebraic
difficulties in understanding area (see Paper 5, reasoning can happen in deliber ately-designed
Understanding space and its representation in educational contexts. In all these contexts, calculation
mathematics) which cause them to rely on methods is deliberately avoided by focusing on, quantifiable but
rather than meaning. not quantified, relationships, and using Kieran’s first
level of structure, surface structure, to express
The above evidence confirms that the relationship phenomena at her third level, equality of expressions.
between arithmetic and algebra is not a direct A study with 105 11- and 12-y ear-olds suggests that
conceptual hierarchy or necessarily helpful. Claims explaining verbally what to do in gener al terms is a
that arithmetical understanding has to precede the precursor to understanding algebraic structure
teaching of algebra only make sense if the focus is (Kieran’s third level) (Reggiani 1994). In this section I
on the meaning of oper ations and on arithmetical have shown that it is possible for students to make
structures, such as inverses and fractional equivalence, the necessary shifts given certain circumstances, and
rather than in correct calculation. A focus on answers can identify necessar y experiences which can suppor t
and ad hoc methods can be a distr action unless the the move.
underlying structures of the ad hoc methods are
generalisable and expressed str ucturally. Booth (1984)
found that inappropriate methods were sometimes Summary of what has to be learnt to
transferred from arithmetic; students often did not shift from arithmetic and algebra
understanding the purpose of conventions and • Students need to focus on relations and
notations, for example not seeing a need f or brackets expressions, not calculations.
when there are multiple operations. The possibilities of
new forms of expression and tr ansformation have to • Students need to understand the meaning of
be appreciated, and the visual format of algebraic operations and inverses.
symbolism is not always obviously connected to its
meanings (Wertheimer, 1960; Kirschner, 1989). For • Students need to represent general relations which
example, the meaning of index notation has to be are manifested in situations
learnt, and while y3 can be related to its meaning in
some way, y1/2 is rather harder to interpret without • In algebra letters and numbers are used together;
understanding abstract structure. algebra is not just letters.

In the U.K. context of an integrated curriculum, a non- • The equals sign means ‘has same value as’ and ‘is
linear view of the shift between arithmetic and algebra equivalent to’ – not ‘calculate’.
can be considered. Many researchers have shown
that middle-school students can develop algebraic • Arithmetic can be seen as instances of general
reasoning through a focus on relationships, rather relationships between quantities.
than calculations4. Coles, Dougherty and Arcavi have
already been mentioned in this respect, and Blanton • Division is a tool for constructing a rational
and Kaput (2005) showed in an inter vention-and- expression.
observation study of cohort of 20 primary teachers,
in particular one self-defined as ‘not a maths per son’ in • The value of a number is less important than its
her second year of teaching, could integrate algebraic relation to other numbers in an expression.
reasoning into their teaching successfully, particularly
using ICT as a medium f or providing bridges between • Guessing and checking, or using known number
numbers and structures. Fujii and Stephens (2001, facts, has to be put aside for more general
2008) examined the role of quasi-var iables (signs methods.
17 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• A letter does not always stand for a particular horizontally; a line of repeated quantities can
unknown. only be split up into commensurate lengths. The
language of division in schools is usually ‘sharing’
Without explicit attention to these issues, learners or ‘shared by’ rather than divide, thus triggering an
will use their natur al and quasi-intuitive reasoning to: assignment metaphor. This is a long way from the
• try to match their use of letters to the way they notion of number required in order to, for
use numbers example, find y when 6y = 7. There is evidence
• try to calculate expressions that students under stand some proper ties of
• try to use ‘=’ to mean ‘calculate’ operations better in some contexts than in other s
• focus on value rather than relationship (e.g. Nunes and Br yant, 1995).
• try to give letters values, often based on
alphabetical assumptions. As well as knowing about operations and their
inverses, students need to know that only addition
and multiplication are commutative in arithmetic, so
that with subtraction and division it matter s which
Understanding expressions way round the numbers go. Also in subtraction and
An expression such as 3 x + 4 is both the answ er to multiplication it makes a difference if an unknown
a question, an object in itself, and also an algor ithm number or variable is not the n umber being acted
or process for calculating a par ticular number. This is on in the oper ation. For example, if 7 – p = 4, then
not a new way of thinking in mathematics that onl y to find p the appropriate inverse operation is 7 – 4.
appears with algebra: it is also tr ue that the answer to In other words ‘subtract from n’ is self-inverse. A
3 ÷ 5 is 3/5, something that students are expected to similar issue arises with ‘divide into n’.
understand when they lear n about intensive
quantities and fractions. Awareness of this kind of We are unconvinced by the U.S. National
dual meaning has been called proceptual thinking Mathematics Advisory Panel’s suggestion that fractions
(Gray and Tall, 1994), combining the process with its must be understood before algebra is taught (NMAP,
outcome in the same way as a multiple is a number 2008). Their argument is based on a ‘top-down’
in itself and also the outcome of m ultiplication. The curriculum view and not on research about ho w such
notions of ‘procept’ and ‘proceptual understanding’ ideas are learnt. The problems just described are
signify that there is a need f or flexibility in how we algebraic, yet contribute to a full under standing of
act towards mathematical expressions. fractions as rational structures. There is a strong
argument for seeing the mathematical str ucture of
fractions as the unifying concept which draws together
Operational understanding parts, wholes, divisions, ratio, scalings and multiplicative
Many young students under stand, at least under relationships, but it may only be in such situations as
some circumstances, the inverse relation between solving equations, algebraic fractions, and so on that
addition and subtraction but it takes students students need to extend their view of division and
longer to under stand the inverse relation fractions, and see these as related.
between multiplication and division 5. This may
be par ticularly difficult when the division is not To understand algebraic notation requires an
symbolized by the division sign ÷ b ut by means of understanding that terms made up of additive,
a fraction, as in 1/3. Understanding division when it multiplicative and exponential oper ations, e.g.
is symbolically indicated as a fr action would require (4a3b – 8a), are variables rather than instr uction
students to realise that a symbol such as 1/3 to calculate, and have a str ucture and equivalent
represents not only a quantity (e .g. the amount of forms. It has been suggested that spending time
pizza someone ate when the pizza was cut into relating algebraic terms to arithmetical structures
three parts) but also as an operation. Kerslake can provide a bridge between arithmetic and
(1986) has shown that older primary and younger algebra (Banerjee and Subr amaniam, 2004). More
secondary students in the United Kingdom rarely research is needed, but working this way round,
understand fractions as indicating a division. A rather than introducing ter ms by reverting to
further difficulty is that multiplication, seen as substitution and calculation, seems to have
repeated addition, does not provide a ready image potential.
on which to build an understanding of the inverse
operation. An array can be split up vertically or
18 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Summary (Wertheimer, 1960) and also to kno w when and


• Learners tend to persist in additive methods rather how to handle specifics and when to stay with
than using multiplicative and exponential where structure. The power of such approaches is
appropriate. illustrated in the well-known story of the young
Gauss’ seeing a str uctural way to sum an ar ithmetic
• It is hard for students to learn the nature of progression. In Fujii and Stephens’ work, seeing
multiplication and division – both as inverse of patterns based on relationships between numbers,
multiplication and as the structure of fractions and avoiding calculation, identifying variation, having a
rational numbers. sense of limits of var iability, were all found to be
predictors of an ability to reason with relationships
• Students have to learn that subtraction and division rather than numbers.
are non-commutative, and that their inverses are
not necessarily addition and multiplication. These are fundamental algebr aic shifts. Seeing algebra
as ‘generalised arithmetic’ is not achieved by inductive
• Students have to learn that algebraic terms can reasoning from special cases, but by developing a
have equivalent forms, and are not instructions to structural perspective on number sentences.
calculate. Matching terms to structures, rather than
using them to practice substitution, might be useful.
Summary
• Learners naturally generalise, they look for patterns
Relational reasoning and habits, and familiar objects.

Students may make shifts between arithmetic and • Inductive reasoning from several cases is a natural
algebra, and between operations and relations, way to generalise, but it is often more important to
naturally with enough exper ience, but research look at expressions as a whole.
suggests that teaching can mak e a difference to the
timing and robustness of the shift. Carpenter and • Learners can shift from ‘seeing’ number expressions
Levi (2000) have worked substantially over decades as instructions to calculate to seeing them as
to develop an approach to ear ly algebra based on relationships.
understanding equality, making generalisations
explicit, representing generalisations in various ways • This shift can be scaffolded by teaching which
including symbolically, and talking about justification encourages students not to calculate but to identify
and proof to validate gener alities. Following this and use relations between numbers.
work, Stephens and other s have demonstrated that
students can be taught to see expressions such as: • Learners who are fluent in both ways of seeing
expressions, as structures or as instructions to
97 – 49 + 49 calculate, can choose which to use.

as structures, in Kieran’s second sense of


relationships among operations (see also the Combining operations
paper on natural numbers). In international studies,
students in upper pr imary in Japan gener ally tackled Problems arise when an expression contains more
these relationally, that is they did not calculate all the than one operation, as can be seen in our paper on
operations but instead combined operations and functional relations where young children cannot
inverses, at a younger age than Australian students understand the notion of relations between
made this shift. Chinese students gener ally appeared relations, such as differences of differences. In
to be able to choose between rapid computation arithmetical and algebraic expressions, some relations
and relational thinking as appropr iate, while 14-year- between relations appear as combinations of
old English students var ied between teachers in their operations, and learners have to decide what has
treatment of these tasks (Fujii and Stephens, 2001, to be ‘done’ first and how this is indicated in the
2008; Jacobs, Franke, Carpenter, Levi and Battey, notation. Carpenter and Levi (2000), Fujii and
2007). This ‘seeing’ relationally seems to depend on Stephens (2001, 2008), Jacobs et al. (2007), draw
the ability to discer n details (Piaget, 1969 p. xxv) attention to this in their work on how students read
and application of an intelligent sense of str ucture number sentences. Linchevski and Her scovics (1996)
19 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

studied how 12- and13-year-olds decided on the them, but it is clear from Ry an and Williams’ study
order of operations. They found that students tended that interpretation is also significantly problematic.
to overgeneralise the order, usually giving addition The prevalence of similar er rors in studies 20 years
priority over subtraction; or using oper ations in left apart is evidence that these are due to students’
to right order ; they can show lack of awareness of normal sense-making of algebra, given their previous
possible internal cancellations; they can see br ackets experiences with arithmetic and the inherent non-
as merely another way to write expressions rather obviousness of algebraic notation.
than an instruction to act first, for example: 926 –
167 – 167 and 926 – (167 + 167) yielded diff erent
answers (Nickson, 2000 p. 120); they also did not Summary
understand that signs were somehow attached to • Understanding operations and their inverses is a
the following number. greater problem than understanding the use of
symbols.
Apart from flow diagrams, a common way to teach
about order in the United Kingdom is to off er • Learners tend to use their rules for reading and
‘BODMAS6’ and its var iants as a r ule. However, it is other false priorities when combining operations,
unclear whether such an approach adequatel y i.e. interpreting left to right, doing addition first,
addresses typical errors made by students in their using language to construct expressions, etc. They
use of expressions. need to develop new priorities.

The following expression errors were manifested • New rules, such as BODMAS (which can be
in the APU tests (Foxman et al., 1985). These tests misused), do not effectively and quickly replace old
involved a cohor t of 12 500 students age 11 to 15 rules which are based on familiarity, habit, and
years. There is also evidence in more recent studies arithmetic.
(see Ryan and Williams, 2007) that these are
persistent, especially the first.
Equals sign
• Conjoining: e.g. a + b = ab
A significant body of research repor ts on difficulties
• Powers are interpreted as multiplication, an error about the meaning of the equals sign Sfard and
made by 20% of 15-year-olds Linchevski (1994) find that students who can do 7 x
+ 157 = 248 cannot do 112 = 12 x + 247, but
• Not understanding that having no coefficient means these questions include two issues: the position and
the coefficient is 1 meaning of the equals sign and that algor ithmic
approaches lead to the temptation to subtr act
• Adding all three values when substituting in, say, u + gt smaller from larger, erroneously, in the second
example. They argue that the root prob lem is the
• Expressing the cost of a packet of sweets where x failure to understand the inverse relation between
packets cost 90p as x/90 addition and subtraction, but this research shows
how conceptual difficulties, incomplete
The most obvious explanation of the conjoining error understandings and notations can combine to mak e
is that conjoining is an attempt to express and ‘answer’ multiple difficulties. If students are taught to mak e
by constructing closure, or students may just not know changes to both sides of an equation in order to
that letters together in this notation mean ‘multiply’. solve it (i.e. transform the equation y – 5 = 8 into y
– 5 + 5 = 8 + 5) and they do not see the need to
Ryan and Williams (2007) found a significant number maintain equivalence between the values in the two
of 14-year-olds did not know what to do with an sides of the equation, then the method that they
expression; they tried to ‘solve’ it as if it is an are being taught is m ysterious to them, par ticularly
equation, again possibly a desire for an ‘answer’. They as many of the cases they are off ered at first can be
also treated subtraction as if it is comm utative, and easily solved by arithmetical methods. Booth (1984)
ignored signs associated with n umbers and letters. shows that these er rors combine problems with
Both APU (Foxman et al., 1985) and Har t, (1981) understanding operations and inverses and
concluded that understanding operations was a problems understanding equivalence.
greater problem than the use of symbols to indicate
20 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

There are two possible ways to tackle these problems: possible by algebra, combined with ‘new anticipations’
to identify all the separ ate problems, treat them also made possible by algebra.
separately, and expect learners to apply the relevant
new understandings when combinations occur ; or to Alibali and colleagues (2007) studied 81 middle school
treat algebraic statements holistically and semantically, students over three years to map their under standing
so that the key feature of the above examples is of equations. They found that those who had, or
equality. There is no research which sho ws conclusively developed, a sophisticated under standing of the equals
that one approach is better than the other (a sign were able to deal with equivalent equations, using
statement endorsed in NMAP’s review (2008)). equivalence to transform equations and solve for
unknowns. Kieran and Saldanha (2005) used a
There is semantic and syntactic confusion about the Computer Algebra System to enable five classes of
meaning of ‘=’ that goes beyond learning a notation upper secondary students to explore different
(Kieran, 1981; 1992). Sometimes, in algebra, it is used meanings of ‘=’ and found that given suitable tasks they
to mean that the two expressions are equal in a were able to understand equivalence, generating for
particular instance where their values are equal; other themselves two different understandings: equivalence
times it is used to mean that tw o expressions are as meaning that expressions would give them equal
equivalent and one can be substituted for another in values for a range of input values of the var iables, and
every occurrence. Strictly speaking, the latter is equivalence as meaning that the expressions were
equivalence and might be wr itten as ‘ ’ but we are not basically transformations of the same form. Both of
arguing for this to become a new ‘must do’ for these understandings contribute to meaningful
the curriculum as this would cut across so much manipulation from one form to another. Also focusing
contextual and historical practice. Rather, the on equivalence, Kieran and Sfard (1999) used a
understanding of algebraic statements must be graphical function approach and thus enabled students
situational, and this includes lear ning when to use ‘==’ to recognise that equivalent algebr aic representations of
to mean ‘calculate’; when to use it to mean ‘equal in functions would generate the same graphs, and hence
special cases’ and when to mean ‘equivalent’; and when represent the same relationships between variables.
to indicate that ‘these two functions are related in this
way’ (Saenz-Ludlow and Walgamuth, 1998). These The potential for confusion between equality and
different meanings have implications for how the letter equivalence relates to confusion between finding
is seen: a quantitative placeholder in a structure; a unknowns (such as values of variables when two
mystery number to be found to make the equality non-equivalent expressions are tempor arily made
work; or a variable which co-varies with others within equal) and expressing relationships between
relationships. Saenz-Ludlow and Walgamuth showed, variables. Equivalence is seen when gr aphs coincide;
over a year-long study with children, that the shift equality is seen when graphs intercept.
towards seeing ‘=’ to mean ‘is the same as’ rather than
‘find the answer’ could be made within ar ithmetic with
consistent, intentional, teaching. This was a teaching Summary
experiment with eight-year-olds in which children were • Learners persist in using ‘=’ to mean ‘calculate’
asked to find missing sums and addends in addition because this is familiar and meaningful for them.
grids. The verb ‘to be’ was used instead of the equals
sign in this and several other tasks. Another task • The equals sign has different uses within
involved finding several binary calculations whose mathematics; sometimes it indicates equivalence
answer was 12, this time using ‘=’. Word problems, and sometimes equality; learners have to learn
including some set by the children, were also used. these differences.
Children also devised their own ways to represent and
symbolise equality. We do not have space here to • Different uses of the equals sign carry different
describe more of the exper iment, but at the end the implications for the meaning of letters: they can
children had altered their initial view that ‘=’ was an stand for hidden numbers, or variables, or
instruction to calculate. They understood ‘=’ as giving parameters.
structural information. Fujii and Stephens’ (2001)
research can be interpreted to show that students do • Equivalence is seen when graphs coincide, and can
get better at using ‘new’ meanings of the equals sign be understood either structurally or as generation
and this may be a product of repeated exper ience of of equal outputs for every input; equality is seen
what Boero called the ‘new transformations’ made when graphs intercept.
21 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Equations and inequality them as a method. Most 12-year-olds could reverse


operations but not their order when ‘undoing’ to find
In the CMF study (Johnson, 1989), 25 classes in 21 unknowns in this approach. Booth (1984) and Piaget
schools in United Kingdom were tested to find out and Moreau (2001) show that students who
why and how students between 8 and 13 cling to understand inversion might not under stand that,
guess-and-check and number-fact methods rather when inverting a sequence of oper ations, the inverse
than new formal methods offered by teachers. The operations cannot just be car ried out in any order :
study focused on several topics, including linear the order in which they are car ried out influences the
equations. The findings, dependent on lar ge scale result. Robinson, Ninowski and Gray (2006) also
tests and additional inter views in four schools, are showed that coordinating inversion with associativity
summarised here and can be seen to include several is a greater challenge than using either inversion or
tendencies already described in other, related, associativity by themselves in problem solving.
algebraic contexts. That the same tendencies emer ge Associativity is the proper ty that x + (y + z) is equal
in several algebraic contexts suggest that these are to (x + y) + z, so that we can add either the f irst
natural responses to symbolic stimuli, and hence take two terms, and then the last, or the last two and then
time to overcome. the first. This proper ty applies to multiplication also.
(Incidentally, note that the automatic application of
Students tended to: BODMAS here would be unnecessar y.) Students
• calculate each side rather than operate on them get confused about how to ‘undo’ such related
• not use inverse operations with understanding operations, and how to undo other paired operations
• use ad hoc number-specific methods which are not associative. As in all such matter s,
• interpret a box or triangle to mean ‘missing teaching which is based on meaning has different
number’ but could not interpret a letter for this outcomes (see Brown and Coles, 1999, 2001).
purpose
• not relate a method to the symbolic form of a Once learners understand the meaning of ‘=’ there is
method a range of ‘intuitive’ methods they use to f ind
• be unable to explain steps of their procedures unknown numbers: using known facts, counting,
• confuse a ‘changing sides’ method with a ‘balance’ inverse operations, and trial substitution (Kieran,
metaphor, particularly not connecting what is said 1992). These do not generalize for situations in which
to what is done, or to what is written the unknown appears on both sides, so formal
• test actual numbers rather than use an algebraic methods are taught. Formal methods each carry
method potential difficulties: function machines do not extend
• assume different letters had different values beyond ‘one-sided’ equations; balance methods do not
• think that a letter could not have the value zero. work for negative signs or for non-linear equations;
change-side/change-sign tends to be misapplied rather
They also found that those who used the language than seen as a special kind of tr ansformation.
‘getting rid of ’ were more likely to engage in
superficial manipulation of symbols. They singled out Many errors when solving equations appear to
‘get rid of a minus’ for par ticular comment as it has come from misapplication of r ules and processes
no mathematical meaning. These findings have been rather than a flawed understanding of the equals
replicated in United Kingdom and elsewhere , and sign. Filloy describes several ‘cognitive tendencies’
have not been refuted as evidence of common observed over several studies of students
difficulties with equations. progressing from concrete to abstr act
understandings (e.g. Filloy and Suther land, 1996).
In the same study, students were then taught using a These tendencies are: to cling to concrete models;
‘function machine’ approach and this led to better to use sign systems inappropr iately; to make
understanding of what an equation is and the variable inappropriate generalizations; to get stuck when
nature of x. However, this approach only makes sense negatives appear ; to misinterpret concrete actions.
when an input-output model is appropr iate, i.e. not Problems with the balance metaphor could be a
for equating two functions or for higher order manifestation of the gener al tendency to cling to
functions (Vergnaud 1997). Ryan and Williams (2007) concrete models (Filloy and Rojano, 1989), and the
found that function machines can be used b y most negative sign cannot be related to concrete
students age 12 to 14 to solv e linear equations, but understandings or even to some syntactic r ules
only when provided. Few students chose to introduce which may have been learnt (Vlassis, 2002). Another
22 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

problem is that when the ‘unknown’ is on both sides algebraic expressions without maintaining an
it can no longer be treated with simple in version understanding of the inequality (Linchevski and Sfard,
techniques as finding ‘the hidden number’; 3x = 12 1991; Tsamir and Bazzini, 2001; Tsamir and Almog,
entails answering the question ‘what number must I 2001). One of these studies compares the
multiply 3 by to get 12?’. But when balancing ‘4m + performance of 170 Italian students to that of 148
3’ with ‘3m + 8’ the balance metaphor can suggest Israeli students in higher secondar y school (Tsamir
testing and calculating each side until they match, and Bazzini, 2001). In both countr ies students had
rather than solving by filling-in arithmetical facts. been formally taught about a r ange of inequalities.
Vlassis devised a teaching experiment with 40 lower They were asked whether statement about the set
secondary students in two classes. The first task was S ={ x Є R: x = 3} could be tr ue or not: ‘S can be
a word problem which would have generated two the solution set of an equality and an inequality’.
equal expressions in one variable, and students only Only half the students under stood that it could be
applied trial-and-error to this. The second task was the solution set of an inequality, and those few
a sequence of balance prob lems with diagrams Italians who gave examples chose a quadr atic
provided, and all students could solv e these. The inequality that they already knew about. Some
final task was a sequence of similar prob lems students offered a linear inequality that could be
expressed algebraically, two of which used negativ e solved to include 3 in the answ er. The researchers
signs. These generated a range of erroneous concluded that unless an inequality question was
methods, including failure to identify when to use an answerable using procedural algebra it was too hard
inverse operation, misapplication of r ules, syntactical for them. Another task asked if par ticular solution
mistakes and manipulations whose meaning was sets satisfied 5x4 < 0. Only half were able to say that
hard to identify. In subsequent exercises errors of x = 0, the next most popular answ er being x < 0.
syntax and meaning diminished, but errors with The researchers compared students’ responses to
negative integers persisted. Eight months later, in a both tasks. It seems that the image of ‘imbalance’
delayed inter view, Vlassis’ students were still using often used with algebr aic inequalities is abandoned
correctly the principles represented in the balance when manipulation is done . The ‘imbalance’ image
model, though not using it explicitly, but still had does not extend to quadratic inequalities, for which
problems when negatives were included. In Filloy a graphical image works better, but again a
and Rojano (1989) a related tendency is descr ibed, procedural approach is preferred by many students
that of students creating a per sonal sense of who then misapply it.
concrete action (e.g. ‘I shall move this from here to
here’) and using them as if they are algebr aic rules
(also obser ved by Lima and Tall, 2008). More insight Summary
into how learners understand equations is given by • Once students understand the equals sign, they are
English and Sharry (1996) who asked students to likely to use intuitive number-rules as a first resort.
classify equations into similar types. Some classified
them according to superficial syntactic aspects, and • The appearance of the negative sign creates need
others to under lying algebraic structure. English and for a major shift to abstract meanings of operations
Sharry draw attention to the need f or students to and relations, as concrete models no longer
have experience of suitable structures in order to operate.
reason analogically and identify deeper similarities.
• The appearance of the unknown on both sides of
There is little research in students’ understanding of an equation creates the need for a major shift
inequality in algebra. In number, children may know towards understanding equality and variables.
about ranges of smaller, or larger, or ‘between’
numbers from their position on a n umberline, and • Students appear to use procedural manipulations
children often know that adding the same quantity when solving equations and inequalities without a
to two unequal quantities maintains the inequality. mental image or understanding strong enough to
There are well-known confusions about relative size prevent errors.
of decimal numbers due to misunder standings about
the notation, but beyond the scope of this review • Students appear to develop action-based rules
(Hart, 1981). Research by Tsamir and others when faced with situations which do not have
describe common problems which appear to relate obvious concrete manifestations.
to a tendency to act procedurally with unequal
23 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• Students find it very hard to detach themselves unknown pieces. Equations with the var iable on both
from concrete models, images and instructions and sides are taught to 11 and 12 y ear-olds in Singapore
focus on structure in equations. using such an approach. The introduction of such
methods into classrooms where teacher s are not
experienced in its use has not been researched. It
has some similarities to the approach based on
Manipulatives
Cuisenaire rods championed by Gattegno in Europe .
It is not only arithmetical habits that can cause Whereas use for numbers was widespread in U.K.
obstacles to algebra. There are other algebr aic primary schools, use for algebra was not, possibly
activities in which too strong a memor y for process because the curriculum focus on substitution and
might create obstacles for future learning. For simplification, rather than meaning and equivalence ,
example, a popular approach to teaching algebr a provided an obstacle to sustained use .
is the provision of materials and diagrams which
ascribe unknown numerical (dimensional) meaning
to letters while facilitating their manipulation to Summary
model relationships such as comm utativity and • Manipulatives can be useful for modelling algebraic
distributivity. These appear to have some success in relationships and structures.
the shor t term, but shifts from physical appearance
to mental abstraction, and then to symbolism, are • Learners might see manipulatives as ‘just something
not made automatically by learners (Boulton-Lewis, else to learn’.
Cooper, Atweh, Pillay, Wilss and Mutch,1997). These
manipulatives provide persistent images and • Teachers can help learners connect the use of
metaphors that may be obstr uctions in future objects, the development of imagery and the use of
work. On the other hand, the original approach to symbols through language.
dealing with variables was to represent them as
spatial dimensions, so there are strong histor ical • Students have to appreciate the limitations of
precedents for such methods. There are repor ted concrete materials and shift to mental imagery
instances of success in teaching this, relating to and abstract understandings.
Bruner’s three per spectives, enactive-iconic-
symbolic (1966), where detachment from the
model has been under stood and scaffolded by
Application of formulae within
teaching (Filloy and Suther land, 1996; Simmt and
Kieren, 1999). Detachment from the model has to
mathematics
be made when values are negative and can no Dickson’s study with three classes of ten-y ear-olds
longer be represented concretely, and also with (1989 a) into students’ use of formulae and formal
fractional values and division oper ations. Spatial methods is based on using the f ormula for area of
representations have been used with success rectangle in various contexts. In order to be
where the image is used per sistently in a r ange successful in such tasks, students have to understand
of algebraic contexts, such as expressions and what multiplication is and how it relates to area, e.g.
equations and equivalence, and where teacher s through an array model, how to use the f ormula by
use language to scaffold shifts between concrete, substitution and how the measuring units for area
numerical and relational per spectives. are applied. Some students can then w ork out a
formula for themselves without formal teaching.
Use of rod or bar diagr ams as in Singapore (NMAP, From this study, Dickson (1989a, 1989b) and her
2008; Greenes and Rubenstein, 2007) to represent colleagues found several problems in how students
part/whole comparisons, reasoning, and equations, approach formal methods in ear ly secondary school.
appears to scaffold thinking from actual n umbers to A third of her subjects did not use a f ormal method
structural relationships, so long as they onl y involve at all; a third used it in a test b ut could not explain it
addition and/or repeated addition. Statements in the in interviews; a third used it and explained it. She
problem are translated into equalities between found that they:
lengths. These equal lengths are constructed from
rods which represent both the actual and the • may not have underlying knowledge on which to
unknown numbers. The rod arrangements or values base formalisation (note that formalisation can
can then be manipulated to f ind the value of the happen spontaneously when they do have such
24 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

knowledge) Part 2: problems arising in


• base their reasoning on incorrect method different approaches to
• have a sound strategy that may not match formal developing algebraic reasoning
method
• may be taught methods leading up to formal, but Since the CSMS study (Har t, 1981) there has been
not matching the formal method an expansion of teaching approaches to dev elop
• may retain other methods, which may have limited meaningful algebra as:
application • expressing generalities which the child already
• may retain formalisation but lose meaning, then knows, therefore is expressing something that has
misapply a formal method in future meaning, and comparing equivalent expressions
• pre-formal enactive or iconic experiences may have • describing relationships between expressions as
been forgotten equations, which can then be solved to find
• might be able to use materials to explain formal unknown values (as in word problems)
method • a collection of techniques for transforming
• may interpret formal notation inadequately. equations to either find unknown values or
represent relationships between variables in
The research described above, taken as a whole , different ways
suggests that the problems students have with using • expressing functions and their inverses, in which
formulae in subjects other than mathematics are inputs become outputs according to a sequence
due to: not being fluent with the notation; not of operations; using multiple representations
understanding the underlying operations; experience • modelling situations by identifying variables and
of using such formulae in mathematics lessons being how they co-vary.
limited to abstract or confusing situations, or even to
situations in which an algebr aic formula is not Each of these offers more success in some aspects
necessary. In addition, of course, they may not than an approach based on r ules for manipulating
understand the intended context. expressions, but also highlights fur ther obstacles to
reasoning. Research is patchy, and does not examine
how students learn across contexts and materials
Summary (Rothwell-Hughes, 1979). Indeed, much of the
• Learners are able to construct formulae for research is specifically about learning in par ticular
themselves, at least in words if not symbols, if they contexts and materials.
have sufficient understanding of the relationships
and operations.
Expressing generalisations
• Learners’ problems using formulae have several
possible root causes.
from patterns
1 Underlying knowledge of the situation or One approach to address inherent diff iculties in
associated concepts may be weak. algebra is to dr aw on our natur al propensity to
2 Existing working strategies may not match the observe patterns, and to impose patter ns on
formal method. disparate experiences (Reed 1972). In this
3 Notational problems with understanding how approach, sequences of patterns are presented
to interpret and use the formula. and students asked to deduce formulae to
describe quantitative aspects of a gener al term
in the sequence . The expectation is that this
generates a need for algebraic symbolisation, which
is then used to state what the student can already
express in other ways, numerical, recursive,
diagrammatically or enactively.

This approach is prevalent in the United Kingdom,


Australia and par ts of Nor th America. The NMAP
(2008) review finds no evidence that expressing
generality contributes to algebraic understanding, yet
others would say that this depends on the definition
25 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

of algebraic understanding. Those we offered at the pattern-based approach to algebr a did no better and
start of this chapter include expression of gener ality as no worse on traditional algebra items than students
an indication of under standing. In Australia, there are taught with a more tr aditional approach (MacGregor
contradictory findings about the value of such tasks. and Stacey, 1993, 1995).

The following is an example of one of the items Redden (1994) studied the work of 1400 10- to
which was used in the lar ge scale test administered 13-year-olds to identify the stages through which
to students by MacGregor and Stacey (repor ted in students must pass in such tasks. First they must
Mason and Sutherland, 2002). recognise the number pattern (which might be
multiplicative), then there must be a stimulus to
Look at the numbers in this table and answer the expression, such as being ask ed for the next ter m
questions: and then the value of uncountab le term; they must
then express the gener al rule and use symbols to
x y express it. Some students could onl y process one
piece of data, some could process more pieces of
1 5 data, some gave only a specific example, some gave
the term-to-term formula and a few gave a full
2 6 functional formula. A major shift of perception has to
take place to express a functional f ormula and this is
3 7 more to do with ‘seeing’ the functional relationship, a
shift of perception, than symbolising it. Rowland and
4 8 Bills (1996) describe two kinds of gener alisation:
empirical and structural, the first being more
5 9 prevalent than the second. Amit and Neria (2007)
use a similar distinction and f ound that students who
6 .. had followed a pattern-generalisation curriculum
were able to switch representations meaningfull y,
7 11 distinguish between variables, constants and their
relationships, and shift voluntarily from additive to
8 ... multiplicative reasoning when appropr iate.

.... ... Moss, Beatty and Macnab (2006) w orked with nine-
year-old students in a longitudinal study and found
... that developing expressions for pattern sequences
was an effective introduction to understanding the
nature of rules in ‘guess the rule’ problems. Nearly
(i) When x is 2, what is y? all of the 34 students w ere then able to ar ticulate
(ii) When x is 8, what is y? general descriptions of functions in the classic
(iii) When x is 800, what is y? handshake problem7 which is known to be hard for
(iv) Describe in words how you would find y if you students in early secondary years. By contrast, Ryan
were told that x is ……… and Williams (2007) found in large-scale testing that
(v) Use algebra to write a rule connecting x and y the most prevalent er ror in such tasks for 12- and
……….. 14-year-olds was giving the ter m-to-term formula
rather than the functional formula, and giving an
MacGregor and Stacey found performance on these actual value for the nth term. Cooper and Warren
items varied from school to school. The success of (2007, and Warren and Cooper, 2008), worked for
14-year-old students in wr iting an algebraic rule three years in five elementary classrooms, using
ranged from 18% in one school to 73% in another. In patterning and expressing patter ns, to teach students
general students searched for a term-to-term rule to express generalisations to use var ious
(e.g. Stacey, 1989). They also tested the same students representations, and to compare expressions and
with more traditional items involving substitution to structures. Their students learnt to use algebr aic
show the meaning of notation and tr ansformation, to conventions and notations, and also under stood that
show equivalence and finding unknowns. From this expressions had underlying operational meanings.
study they concluded that students taught with a Clearly, students are capable of learning these aspects
26 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

of algebra in cer tain pedagogic conditions. Among Reed (1972) hypothesised that classifying is a natural
other aspects common to most such studies, Cooper act that enables us to make distinctions, clump ideas,
and Warren’s showed the value of compar ing and hence deal with lar ge amounts of new
different but equivalent expressions that ar ise from information. It is therefore useful to think of what sor t
different ways to generalise the patterns, and also of information learners are tr ying to classify in these
introduced inverse operations in the context of kinds of task. Reed found that people extract
function machines, and a r ange of mental ar ithmetic prototypes from the available data and then see how
methods. If other research about gener alising patterns far other cases are from this prototype . Applying this
applies in this study, then it must be the combination to pattern-growth and sequence tasks makes it
of pattern-growth with these other aspects of algebr a obvious that term-to-term descriptions are far easier
that made the difference in the lear ning of their and likely to be dominant when the data is expressed
students.They point to ‘the impor tance of sequentially, such as in a table. We could legitimately
understanding and communicating aspects of ask the question: is it worth doing these kinds of
representational forms which allowed commonalities activity if the shifts to seeing and then expressing
to be seen across or betw een representations’. functional relationships are so hard to make? Does
this just add more difficulties to an already difficult
As Carraher, Martinez & Schliemann (2007) show, it subject? To answer this, we looked at some studies in
is impor tant to nurture the transition from empirical which claims are made of impro vements in seeing and
(term-to-term) generalizations (called naïve expressing algebraic relationships, and identifying
induction by Radford, 2007), to generalisations that features of pedagogy or innovation which may have
follow from explicit statements about mathematical influenced these improvements. Yeap and Kaur (2007)
relations between independent and dependent in Singapore found a wider range of factors influencing
variables, and which might not be ‘seen’ in the data. success in unfamiliar generalisation tasks than has been
Steele (2007) indicates some of the wa ys in which a reported in studies which focus on rehearsed
few successful 12 to 13 y ear old students go about procedures. In a class of 38 ten-y ear-old students they
this transition when using var ious forms of data, set tasks, then observed and interviewed students
pictorial, diagrammatic and numerical, but bigger about the way they had worked on them. Their aim
studies show that this shift is not automatic and was to learn more about the str ategies students had
benefits from deliberate tuition. Radford further used and how these contributed towards success. The
points out that once a functional relation is task was to find the sums of consecutive odd
observed, expressing it is a fur ther process involving numbers: 1 + 3 + 5 + …+ (2 n – 1). Students were
integration of signs and meaning. Stephens’ work familiar with adding integers from 1 to 100, and also
(see Mason, Stephens and Watson, in press) shows with summing multiples. They were given a sequence
that the oppor tunity and ability to exemplify of subtasks: a table of values to complete , to find the
relationships between variables as number pairs, sum or 1 + 3 + … + 99 and to f ind the sum of 51 +
and to express the relationship within the pair s, are 53 + … + 99. The researchers helped students by
necessary predictors of the ability to f ocus on and offering simpler versions of the same kinds of
express a functional relationship. This research also summation if necessar y. Nearly all students were able
illustrates that such abilities are dev elopmental, and to recognize and continue the pattern of sums (they
hence indicates the kind of lear ning experiences turn out to be the square n umbers); two-thirds were
required to make this difficult shift. able to transfer their sense of str ucture to the ‘sum to
99’ task, but only one-third completed the ‘sum from
Rivera and Becker (2007), looking longitudinally at 51’ task – the one most dissimilar to the table-filling
middle school students’ understanding of sequences tasks, requiring adaptation of methods and use of
of growing diagrammatic patterns in a teaching previous knowledge to make an argument. The
experiment, specify three forms of generalization researcher had a series of designed prompts to help
that students engage with: constructive standard, them, such as to find the sum from 1 to 49, and then
constructive nonstandard, and deconstructive. It is see what else they needed to get the sum to 99.
the deconstruction of diagrams and situations that Having found an answer, students then had to f ind it
leads most easily to the functional formula, they again using a different method. They found that
found, rather than reasoning inductively from success depended on:
numbers. However, their students gener ally reverted • the ability to see structures and relationships
to arithmetical strategies, as repor ted in many other • prior knowledge
studies of this and other shifts to wards algebra. • metacognitive strategies
27 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• critical-thinking strategies pedagogic skill and technical know-how, why should


• the use of organizing heuristics such as a table it be pursued? The reason is that skill in the meaning
• the use of simplifying heuristics such as trying out and use of algebr a enables fur ther generalizations to
simpler cases be made, and transformations of mathematical
• task familiarity relationships to be used and studied. The work
• use of technology to do the arithmetic so that large required to understand the functional relationship is
numbers can be handled efficiently. necessary to operate at a higher level than merely
using algebra to symbolize what you do, as with
As with all mathematics teaching, limited experience term-to-term formulae. It is algebr a that provides the
is unhelpful. Some students only know one way to means to building concepts upon concepts, a key
construct cases, one way to accomplish gener alisation aspect of secondar y mathematics, by providing
(table of values and patter n spotting), and have only expression of abstract relationships in ways that can
ever seen simple cases used to star t sequence be manipulated. In algebra, the products are not
generation, rather than deliberate choices to aid answers, but structures, relationships, and information
observations. Students in this situation may be about relationships and special instances of them.
unaware of the necessity for critical, reflective thinking These tasks provide contexts for that kind of shift,
and the value of simplifying and or ganising data. but do not guar antee that it will take place.
Furthermore, this collection of studies on expressing
generality shows that construction, design, choice and Assumptions, such as that which appear s to be made
comparison of various representational means does in Redden’s study, that understanding term-to-term
not happen spontaneously for students who are relationships is a route to under standing functional
capable of using them. Choosing when and why to relations contradict the experience of
switch representations has long been known to be a mathematicians that algebra expresses the str ucture
mark of successful mathematics students (Krutetskii, of relations, and this can be adduced from single
1976) and therefore this is a str ategy which needs to cases which are gener ic enough to illustrate the
be deliberately taught. Evidence from Blanton and relationship through diagrams or other spatial
Kaput’s intervention study with 20 teacher s (2005) is representations. Numerical data has to be backed up
that many primary children were able to invent and with fur ther information about relationships. For
solve ‘missing number’ sentences using letter s as example, consider this data set:
placeholders, symbolize quantities in patter ns, devise
and use graphical representations for single variables, x y
and some could wr ite simple relations using letter s,
codes, ‘secret messages’ or symbols. The intervention 1 1
was suppor tive professional development which
helped teachers understand what algebraic reasoning 2 4
entails, and gave them resources, feedback, and other
support over five years. Ainley (1996) showed that 3 9
supportive technology can display the purpose of
formal representations, and also remove the technical ... ...
difficulties of producing new representations. Ten-year-
old students in her study had w orked for a few years
in a computer-rich environment and used While it is possible for these values to be examples
spreadsheets to collect data from pur poseful of the function y = x 2 it is also possible that they
experiments. They then generated graphs from the exemplify y = x 2 + (x – 1)(x – 2)(x – 3). Without
data and studied these , in relation to the data, to further information, such as x being the side of a
make conjectures and test them. One task was square and y being its area, we cannot deduce a
designed to lead to a prob lematic situation so that functional formula, and inductive reasoning is
students would have to look for a shor tcut, and she misapplied. There is much that is mathematically
observed that the need to ‘teach the computer’ how interesting in the connection between term-to-term
to perform a calculation led to spontaneous f ormal and functional formulae, such as application of the
representation of a var iable. method of differences, and students have to learn
how to conjecture about algebr aic relationships, but
So, if it is possible for students to learn to make to only approach generalisation from a sequence
these generalizations only with a great deal of perspective is misleading and, as we have seen from
28 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

these studies, very hard without the suppor t of Students in one class of Booth’ s (1984) inter vention
specially-designed tasks comparing and transforming study (which took place with f our classes in lower
equivalent structural generalisations. secondary school) had a teacher who emphasised
throughout that letters had numerical value. These
students were less likely than others to treat a letter
Summary as merely an object. In her study, discussion about
• Learners naturally make generalisations based on the meaning of statements bef ore formal activity
what is most obviously related; this depends on the seemed to be beneficial, and those students who
visual impact of symbols and diagrams. were taught a formal method seemed to under stand
it better some time after the lesson, maybe after
• Seeing functional, abstract, relationships is hard and repeated experiences. However, some students did
has to be supported by teaching. not understand it at all. As with all inter vention
studies, the teaching makes a difference. Linchevski
• Deconstruction of diagrams, relationships, situations and Herscovics (1996) taught six students to collect
is more helpful in identifying functional relationships like terms and then decompose additive terms in
than pattern-generation. order to focus on ‘sides’ or equations as expressions
which needed to be equated. While this led to them
• Development of heuristics to support seeing being better able to deal with equations, there were
structural relationships is helpful. lingering problems with retaining the sign preceding
the letter rather than attaching the succeeding sign.
• There is a further shift from seeing to expressing
functional relationships. Several other inter vention studies (e.g. van Ameron,
2003; Falle, 2005) confirm that the type of equation
• Learners who can express relationships correctly and the nature of its coeff icients often make non-
and algebraically can also exemplify relationships formal methods available to learners, even if they
with number pairs, and express the relationships have had significant recent teaching in f ormal
within the pairs; but not all those who can express methods. These studies fur ther demonstrate that
relationships within number pairs can express the students will use ad hoc methods if they seem more
relationship algebraically. appropriate, given that they under stand the meaning
of an equation; where they did not understand they
• Learners who have combined pattern- often misapplied formal methods. Falle’s study
generalisation with function machines and other included more evidence that the structure a/x = b
ways to see relationships can become more fluent caused par ticular problems as learners interpreted
in expressing generalities in unfamiliar situations. ‘division’ as if it were commutative. As with other
approaches to teaching algebr a, using equations as
• Conflicting research results suggest that the nature the central focus is not trouble-free.
of tasks and pedagogy make a difference to success.

• Functional relationships cannot be deduced from Summary


sequences without further information about • As with all algebraic expressions, learners may react
structure. to the visual appearance without thinking about the
meaning.

Using an equation-centred approach • Learners need to know what the equation is telling
to teaching algebra them.

There are new kinds of prob lem that arise in an • Learners need to know why an algebraic method is
equation-centred approach to teaching algebra in necessary; this is usually demonstrated when the
addition to those described earlier: the solution of unknown is negative, or fractional, and/or when the
equations to find unknown values, and the constr uction unknown is on both sides. They are likely to choose
of equations from situations. The second of these new ad hoc arithmetic methods such as guess- and-
problems is considered in Paper 7. Here we look at check, use of known number facts, compensation or
difficulties that students had in teaching studies designed trial-and-adjustment if these are more convenient.
to focus on typical problems in finding unknowns.
29 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• Learners’ informal methods of making the sides spreadsheet, e.g. sequences can be laid side by side,
equal in value may not match formal methods. input and output values for different functions can
be compared, and graphs can be related directly to
• ‘Undoing’ methods depend on using inverse numerical data.
operations with understanding.
• Large data sets can be used so that questions
• Fluent technique may be unconnected to explaining about patterns and generalities become more
the steps of their procedures. meaningful.

• Learners can confuse the metaphors offered to In Sutherland and Rojano’s work, two small groups
‘model’ solving equations, e.g. ‘changing sides’ with of students 10- to 11-years-old with no formal
‘balance’. algebraic background were given some algebraic
spreadsheet tasks based on area. It was found that
• Metaphors in common use do not extend to they were less likely to use ar ithmetical approaches
negative coefficients or ‘unknowns’ or non-linear when stuck than students repor ted in non-
equations. spreadsheet research, possibly because these
arithmetical approaches are not easil y available in a
• Non-commutative and associative structures are spreadsheet environment. Sutherland and Rojano
not easily used with inverse reasoning. used three foci known to be difficult for students: the
relation between functions and inverse functions, the
• As in many other contexts, division and rational development of equivalent expressions and word
structures are problematic. problems. The arithmetic methods used included
whole/part approaches and tr ying to work from
known to unknown. Most of the problems, however,
Spreadsheets required working from the unknown to the known
to build up relationships. In a similar follow-up study
Learners have to know how to recognise str uctures 15-year-old students progressively modified the
(based on under standing arithmetical operations values of the unknowns until the given totals were
and what they do), express str uctures in symbols, reached (Sutherland and Rojano, 1993). There was
and calculate par ticular cases (to stimulate inductive some improvement in post-tests over pre-tests for
understanding of concepts) in order to use algebr a the younger students, but most still found the tasks
effectively in other subjects and in higher difficult. One of the f our intervention sessions
mathematics. Several researchers have used involved students constructing equivalent
spreadsheets as a medium in which to explore spreadsheet expressions. Some students started by
what students might be ab le to learn constructing expressions that gener ated equality in
(e.g. Schwar tz and Yerushalmy, 1992; Sutherland and specific cases, rather than overall equivalence.
Rojano, 1993; Friedlander and Tabach, 2001). The Students who had star ted out by using par ticular
advantages of using spreadsheets are as f ollows. arithmetical approaches spontaneously derived
algebraic expressions in the pencil-and-paper tasks
• In order to use spreadsheets you have to know the of the post-test. This appears to confound evidence
difference between parameters (letters and numbers from other studies that an ar ithmetical approach
that structure the relationship) and variables, and the leads to obstacles to algebr aic generalization. The
spreadsheet environment is low-risk since mistakes generation of numbers, which can be compared to
are private and can easily be corrected. the desired outputs, and adjusted through adapting
the spreadsheet formula, may have made the need
• The physical act of pointing the cursor provides an for a formula more obvious. The researchers
enactive aspect to building abstract structures. concluded that comparing expressions which
referred only to numbers, to those which referred
• Graphical, tabular and symbolic representations are to variables, appeared to have enabled students to
just a click away from each other and are updated make this critical shift.
together.
A recent area of research is in the use of computer
• Correspondences that are not easy to see in other algebra systems (C AS) to develop algebraic
media can be aligned and compared on a reasoning. Kieran and Saldanha (2005) have had
30 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

some success with getting students to deal with We have discussed the use of function machines to
equations as whole meaningful objects within C AS. solve equations above.

Summary Multiple representations


Use of spreadsheets to b uild formulae: A widespread attempt to overcome the obstacles of
• allows large data sets to be used learning algebra has been to offer learners multiple
• provides physical enactment of formula representations of functions because:
construction • different representations express different aspects
• allows learners to distinguish between variables and more clearly
parameters • different representations constrain interpretations –
• gives instant feedback these have to be checked out against each other
• does not always lock learners into arithmetical and • relating representations involves identifying and
empirical viewpoints. understanding isomorphic structures (Goldin 2002).

By and large these methods offer graphs, equations,


and tabular data and maybe a physical situation or
Functional approach
diagram from which the data has been generated.
Authors var y in their use of the w ord ‘function’. The fundamental idea is that when the main f ocus is
Technically, a function is a relationship of on meaningful functions, rather than mechanical
dependency between variables, the independent manipulations, learners make sensible use of
variables (input) which var y by some external representations (Booth, 1984; Yerushalmy, 1997; Ainley,
means, and the dependent var iables (output) which Nardi and Pratt, 1999; Hollar and Norwood, 1999).
vary in accordance with the relationship . It is the
relationship that is the function, not a par ticular A central issue is that in most contexts f or a letter
representation of it, however in practice authors to represent anything, the student must understand
and teachers refer to ‘the function’ when indicating what is being represented, yet it is often only by the
a graph or equation. An equivalence such as use of a letter that what is being represented can be
temperature conversion is not a function, because understood. This is an essential shift of abstr action. It
these are just different ways to express the same may be that seeing the use of letter s alongside other
thing, e.g. t = 9/5 C + 32 where t is temperature in representations can help develop meaning, especially
degrees Fahrenheit and C temperature in degrees through isomorphisms.
Celsius (Janvier 1996). Thus a teaching approach
which focuses on comparing different expressions of This line of thought leads to a substantial body of
the same generality is concerned with str ucture and work using multiple representations to develop
would afford manipulation, while an approach which understandings of functions, equations, graphs and
focuses on functions, such as using function tabular data. All these studies are teaching
machines or multiple representations, is concerned experiments with a r ange of students from upper
with relationships and change and w ould afford primary to first year undergraduates. What we learn
thinking about pair s of values, critical inputs and from them is a r ange of possibilities for learning and
outputs, and rates of change. new problems to be overcome. Powell and Maher
(2003) have suggested that students can themselv es
discover isomorphisms. Others have found that
Function machines learners can recognise similar structures (English and
Some researchers repor t that students find it hard Sharry, 1996) but need experience or prompts in
to use inverses in the r ight order when solving order to go beyond surface features. This is because
equations. However, in Booth’s work (1984) with surface features contribute to the first impact of any
function machines she found that lower secondary situation, whether they are visual, aural, the way the
students were capable of instructing the ‘machine’ by situation is first ‘read’, or the first recognition of
writing operations in order, using proper algebr aic similarity.
syntax where necessar y, and could make the shift to
understanding the whole expression. They could Hitt (1998) claims that ‘A central goal of
then reverse the flow diagram, maintaining order, mathematics teaching is taken to be that the
to ‘undo’ the function. students be able to pass from one representation
31 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

type to another without falling into contradictions.’ problems and applications of functions, but students
(p. 134). In experiments with teachers on a cour se needed additional teaching to become as fluent in
he asked them to match pictures of v essels with algebra as ‘conventional’ students. But teaching to
graphs to represent the relationship betw een the fluency took only six weeks compared to one year
volume and height of liquid being poured into them. for others. This result seems to confirm that if
The most common er rors in the choice of functions algebra is seen to have purpose and meaning then
were due to misinter pretation of the graphical the technical aspects are easier to lear n, either
representation, and misidentification of the because there is motivation, or because the lear ner
independent variable in the situation. Understanding has already developed meanings for algebraic
the representation, in addition to under standing the expressions, or because they have begun to develop
situation, was essential. The choice of representation, appropriate schema for symbol use. When students
in addition to under standing, is also influential in first had to express functions, and only then had to
success. Arzarello, Bazzini and Chiappini, (1994) gave answer questions about par ticular values, they had
137 advanced mathematics students this prob lem: fewer problems using symbols.
‘Show that if you add a 4-digit n umber to the 4-digit
number you get if you reverse the digits, the answer There were fur ther benefits in the C ARAPACE
is a multiple of eleven’. There were three strategies study: they found that their students could switch
used by successful students, and the most-used was from variable to unknown correctly more easily than
to devise a way to express a 4-digit n umber as the has been found in other studies; the students saw a
sum of multiples of powers of ten. This strategy leads single-value as special case of a function, but their
immediately to seeing that the ter ms in the sum justifications tended to relate to tab ular data and
combine to show multiples of eleven. The were often numerical, not relating to the overall
relationship between the representation and its function or the context. The students had to
meaning in terms of ‘eleven’ was very close. ‘Talk’ consciously reach for algebraic methods, even to use
can structure a choice of representations that most their own algorithms, when the situations became
closely resemble the mathematical meaning harder. Even in a multi-representational environment,
(see also Siegler and Ster n, 1998). using functions algebraically has to be taught; this is
not spontaneous as long as n umerical or graphical
Even (1998) points to the ability to select, use, move data is available. Students preferred to move
between and compare representations as a cr ucial between numerical and graphical data, not symbolic
mathematical skill. She studied 162 early students representations (Brenner, Mayer, Moseley, Brar, Duran,
in 8 universities (the findings are informative for Reed and Webb,1997). This finding must depend on
secondary teaching) and found a difference between task and pedagogy, because by contrast Lehrer,
those who could only use individual data points and Strom and Confrey,(2002) give examples where
those who could adopt a global, functional approach. coordinating quantitative and spatial representations
Nemirovsky (1996) demonstrates that the Car tesian appears to develop algebraic reasoning through
relationship between graphs and values is much representational competence. Even (1998) argues
easier to understand pointwise, from points to line that the flexibility and ease
perhaps via a table of values, than holistically, every with which we hope students will move from
point on a line representing a par ticular relationship. representation to representation depends on what
general strategy students bring to mathematical
Some studies such as Computer-Intensive Algebra situations, contextual factors and previous
(e.g. Heid, 1996) and C ARAPACE (Kieran, Boileau experience and knowledge. We will look fur ther
and Garancon,1996) go some way towards at this in the next paper.
understanding how learners might see the duality of
graphs and values. In a study of 14 students aged Further doubts about a multiple representation
about 13, the C ARAPACE environment (of graphs, approach are raised by Amit and Fried (2005) in
data, situations and functions) seemed to suppor t lessons on linear equations with 13 – 14 y ear-olds:
the understanding of equality and equivalence of ‘students in this class did not seem to get the idea
two functions. This led to findings of a significant that representations are to be selected, applied, and
improvement in dealing with ‘unknown on both translated’. The detail of this is elabor ated through
sides’ equations over groups taught more the failed attempts of one student, who did make
conventionally. The multiple-representation ICT this link, to persuade her peers about it. Hirschhorn
environment led to better perf ormance in word (1993) repor ts on a longitudinal compar ative study
32 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

at three sites in which those taught using m ultiple particular age are, in these circumstances, able to
representations and meaningful contexts did display algebraic behaviour of par ticular kinds. Usually
significantly better in tests than other s taught more these experiments contradict curriculum
conventionally, but that there was no diff erence in expectations of age, or order, or nature, of learning.
attitude to mathematics. All we really learn from this For example, in a teaching experiment over several
is that the confluence of oppor tunity, task and weeks with 8-year-old students, Carraher, Brizuela
explanation are not sufficient for learning. Overall and Schliemann (2000) repor t that young learners
the research suggests that there are some gains in are able to engage with problems of an algebraic
understanding functions as meaningful expressions of nature, such as expressing and f inding the unknown
variation, but that symbolic representation is still hard heights in problems such as: Tom is 4 inches taller
and the least preferred choice. than Maria, Maria is 6 inches shor ter than Leslie;
draw their heights. They found that young learners
The effects of multiple representational could learn to express unknown heights with letter s
environments on students’ problem-solving and in expressions, but were sometimes puzzled by the
modeling capabilities are descr ibed in the next paper. need to use a letter f or ‘any number’ when they had
been given a par ticular instance. This is a real source
for confusion, since Maria can only have one height.
Summary Students can naturally generalise about operations
• Learners can compare representations of a and methods using words, diagrams and actions
relationship in graphical, numerical, symbolic and when given suitable suppor t (Bastable and Schifter,
data form. 2008). They can also see operators as objects
(Resnick, Lesgold and Bill, 1990). These and other
• Conflicting research results suggest that the nature studies appear to indicate that algebr aic thinking can
of tasks and pedagogy make a difference to success. develop in primary school.

• The hardest of these representations for learners is In secondary school, students can work with a wider
the symbolic form. range of examples and a greater degree of complexity
using ICT and graphical approaches than when
• Previous experience of comparing multiple confined to paper and pencil. For example, Kieran and
representations, and the situation being modelled, Sfard (1999) used graphs successfully to help 12- and
helps students understand symbolic forms. 13-yearold students to appreciate the equivalence of
expressions. In another example, Noss, Healy and
• Learners who see ‘unknowns’ as special cases of Hoyles (1997) constructed a matchsticks microworld
equality of two expressions are able to distinguish which requires students to build up LOGO
between unknowns and variables. procedures for drawing matchstick sequences. They
report on how the software suppor ted some 12- to
• Teachers can scaffold the shifts between 13-year-old students in finding alternative ways to
representations, and perceptions beyond surface express patterns and structures of Kieran’s second
features, through language. and third kinds. Microworlds provide support for
students’ shifts from par ticular cases to what has to be
• Some researchers claim that learners have to true, and hence suppor t moves towards using algebra
understand the nature of the representations in as a reasoning tool.
order to use them to understand functions, while
others claim that if learners understand the In a teaching study with 11 y ear-old students, Noble,
situations, then they will understand the Nemirovsky, Wright and Tierney, (2001) suggest that
representations and how to use them. students can recognise core mathematical str uctures
by connecting all representations to per sonally-
constructed environments of their own, relevant for
What students could do if taught, but the task at hand. They asked pairs of students to
are not usually taught proceed along a linear measure , using steps of
different sizes, but the same number of steps each,
Most research on algebr a in secondar y school is and record where they got to after each step . They
of an innovative kind, in which par ticular tasks or used this data to predict where one w ould be after
teaching approaches reveal that learners of a the other had taken so many steps. The aim was to
33 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

compare rates of change. Two further tasks, one a • Students can shift towards looking at relationships if
number table and the other a software-suppor ted encouraged and scaffolded to make the shift,
race, were given and it was noticed from the ways in through language or microworlds, for example.
which the students talked that they were bringing to
each new task the language, metaphors and • Students can shift from seeing letters as unknowns
competitive sense which had been gener ated in the to using them as variables.
previous tasks. This enabled them to progress from
the measuring task to comparing rates in multiple • Students will develop similarities and prototypes to
contexts and representations. This still suppor ts the make sense of their experience and support future
fact that students recognize similarities and look for action.
analogical prototypes within a task, but questions
whether this is related to what the teacher expects in • Students can shift from seeing cases as particular to
any obvious way. In a three-year study with 16 lower seeing algebraic representations as statement about
secondary students, Lamon (1998) found that a year’s what has to be true.
teaching which focused on modelling sequential
situations was so effective in helping students • Comparison of cases and representations can
understand how to express relationships that they support learning about functions and learning how
could distinguish between unknowns, variables and to use algebra to support reasoning.
parameters and could also choose to use algebr a
when appropriate – normally these aspects were not
expected at this stage , but two years further on.

Lee (1996) describes a long ser ies of teaching


experiments: 50 out of 200 first year university
students committed themselves to an extr a study
group to develop their algebraic awareness. This
study has implications for secondar y students, as
their algebraic knowledge was until then r ule-based
and procedural. She forced them, from the star t, to
treat letters as variables, rather than as hidden
numbers. By many measures this group succeeded
in comparative tests, and there was also evidence of
success beyond testing, improvements in attitude
and enjoyment. However, the impact of
commitment to extra study and ‘belonging’ to
a special group might also ha ve played a par t.
Whatever the causal factors, this study shows that
the notion of var iable can be taught to those who
have previously failed to under stand, and can form
a basis for meaningful algebra.

Summary
With teaching:

• Young children can engage with missing number


problems, use of letters to represent unknown
numbers, and use of letters to represent
generalities that they have already understood.

• Young children can appreciate operations as


objects, and their inverses.
34 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Part 3: Conclusions Recommendations


and recommendations For teaching
These recommendations require a change from a
Conclusions fragmented, test-driven, system that encour ages an
emphasis on fluent procedure f ollowed by
Error research about elementar y algebra and application.
pre-algebra is uncontentious and the f indings are
summarised above. However, it is possible for young • Algebra is the mathematical tool for working
learners to do more than is nor mally expected in with generalities, and hence should permeate
the curriculum, e.g. they will accept the use of letter s lessons so that it is used wherever mathematical
to express generalities and relationships which they meaning is expressed. Its use should be
already understand. Research about secondar y commonplace in lessons.
algebra is less coherent and more patch y, but broadly
can be summarised as follows. • Teachers and writers must know about the
research about learning algebra and take it into
Teaching algebra by offering situations in which account, particularly research about common
symbolic expressions make mathematical sense, errors in understanding algebraic symbolisation and
and what learners have to find is mathematically how they arise.
meaningful (e.g. through multiple representations,
expressing generality, and equating functions) is more • Teachers should avoid using published and web-
effective in leading to algebr aic thinking and skill use based materials which exacerbate the difficulties by
than the teaching of technical manipulation and over-simplifying the transition from arithmetic to
solution methods as isolated skills. However, these algebraic expression, mechanising algebraic
methods need to be combined through complex transformation, and focusing on algebra as
pedagogy and do not in themselv es bring about all ‘arithmetic with letters’.
the necessary learning. Technology can play a big par t
in this. There is a difference between using ICT in • The curriculum, advisory schemes of work, and
the learner’s control and using ICT in the teacher’s teaching need to take into account how shifts from
control. In the lear ner’s control the physical actions arithmetical to algebraic understanding take time,
of moving around the screen and choosing between multiple experiences, and clarity of purpose.
representations can be easily connected to the
effects of such moves, and feedback is personalised • Students at key stage 3 need support in shifting
and instant. to representations of generality, understanding
relationships, and expressing these in
There is a tenuous relationship between what it conventional forms.
means to understand and use the affordances of
algebra, as described in the previous paragraph, and • Students have to change focus from calculation,
understanding and using the symbolic f orms of quantities, and answers to structures of operations
algebra. Fluency in under standing symbolic and relations between quantities as variables. This
expressions seems to develop through use, and shift takes time and multiple experiences.
also contributes to effective use – this is a tw o-way
process. However, this statement ignores the • Students should have multiple experiences of
messages from research which is purel y about constructing algebraic expressions for structural
procedural fluency, and which suppor ts repetitive relations, so that algebra has the purpose of
practice of procedures in carefull y constructed expressing generality.
varying forms. Procedural research focuses on
obstacles such as dealing with negativ e signs and • The role of ‘guess-the-sequence-rule’ tasks in the
fractions, multiple operations, task complexity and algebra curriculum should be reviewed: it is
cognitive load but not on meaning, use, relationships, mathematically incorrect to state that a finite
and dealing with unfamiliar situations. number of numerical terms indicates a unique
underlying generator.
35 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

• Students need multiple experiences over time to better understood and applied. Similar
understand: the role of negative numbers and the development in the United Kingdom has not been
negative sign; the role of division as inverse of possible due to an over-prescriptive curriculum
multiplication and as the fundamental operation and frequent testing which forces a focus on
associated with rational numbers; and the meaning technical manipulation.
of equating algebraic expressions.
• Textbooks which promulgate an ‘arithmetic with
• Teachers of key stage 3 need to understand how letters’ approach should be avoided; this approach
hard it is for students to give up their arithmetical leads inevitably to the standard, obvious errors and
approaches and adopt algebraic conventions. hence turns students off algebra and mathematics
in favour of short-term gains.
• Substitution should be used purposefully for
exemplifying the meaning of expressions and • Symbolic manipulators, graph plotters and other
equations, not as an exercise in itself. Matching algebraic software are widely available and used to
terms to structures, rather than using them to allow people to focus on meaning, application and
practice substitution, might be more useful. implications. Students should know how to use
these and how to incorporate them into
• The affordances of ICT should be exploited fully, in mathematical explorations and extended tasks.
the learner’s control, in the teacher’s control, and in
shared control, to support the shifts of • We need to be free to draw on research and
understanding that have to be made including explore its implications in the United Kingdom, and
constructing objects in order to understand this may include radical re-thinking of the algebra
structure. curriculum and how it is tested. This may happen as
part of the ‘functional maths’ agenda but its
• Teachers should encourage the use of symbolic foundations need to be established when students
manipulation, using ICT, as a set of tools to support are introduced to algebra.
transforming expressions for mathematical
understanding.

For research
For policy • Little is known about school learning of algebra in
• The requirements listed above signal a training the following areas.
need on a national scale, focusing solely on algebra
as a key component in the drive to increase • The experiences that an average learner needs, in
mathematical competence and power. educational environments conducive to change, to
shift from arithmetical to algebraic thinking.
• There are resource implications about the use of
ICT. The focus on providing interactive whiteboards • The relationship between understanding the nature
may have drawn attention away from the need for of the representations in order to use them to
students to be in control of switching between understand functions, and understanding the
representations and comparisons of symbolic situations as an aid to understanding the
expression in order to understand the syntax and representations and how to use them.
the concept of functions. The United Kingdom may
be lagging behind the developed world in exploring • Whether teaching experiments using functional,
the use of CAS, spreadsheets and other software multi-representational, equation or generalisation
to support new kinds of algebraic thinking. approaches have an impact on students’ typical
notation-related difficulties. In other words, we do
• In several other countries, researchers have been not know if and how semantic-focused approaches
able to develop differently-sequenced curricula in to algebra have any impact on persistent and well-
which students have been able to use algebra as a known syntactic problems.
way of expressing general and abstract notions as
these arise. Manipulation, solution of equations, • How learners’ synthesise their knowledge to
and other technical matters to do with symbols understand quadratic and other polynomials, their
develop as well as with formal teaching, but are factorisation and roots, simultaneous equations,
36 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

inequalities and other algebraic objects beyond Endnotes


elementary expressions and equations.
1 The impor tance of inverses was discussed in the paper on
natural numbers
• Whether and how the use of symbolic
2 In the paper on r ational numbers we talk more about the
manipulators to transform syntax supports
relationship between fractions and rational numbers, and we
algebraic understanding in school algebra. often use these words interchangeably.
3 The advantage of this is that spotting lik e terms might be easier,
• Using algebra to justify and prove generalities, but this can also mask some other characteristics such as
rather than generate and express them. physical meaning (e.g. E = mc2) and symmetry e.g. x2y + y2x.
4 This should be contr asted with the problems young learners
• How students make sense of different metaphors have with expressing relations using n umber, described in our
for solving equations (balance, doing-undoing, paper on functional relations. Knowing that relations are
themselves number-like objects does not necessar ily mean we
graphical, formal manipulation).
have to calculate them.
5 This is discussed in detail in our paper s on whole numbers and
rational numbers and outlined here .
6 A very common mnemonic to remind people to do: brackets,
‘of ’, division, multiplication, addition and subtr action in that
order. It does not always work.
7 If n people all shak e hands with each other, how many
handshakes will there be?

Acknowledgements
This chapter was produced with the help of Nichola
Clarke who did much of the technical work.
37 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

References

Ainley, J. (1996) Purposeful Contexts for Formal Baker, B., Hemenway, C., Trigueros, M. (2001) On
Notation in a Spreadsheet Environment. Journal of transformations of basic functions. In Chick, H.,
Mathematical Behavior, 15(4) 405-422 Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and Vincent, J. (eds.)
Ainley, J., Nardi, E., and Pratt, D. (1999). Constructing Proceedings of the 12 th ICMI study conference: The
Meaning for Formal Notation in Active Graphing, future of the teaching and lear ning of algebra.
in I. Scwank (Ed.) Proceedings of the Fir st pp.41-47 University of Melbourne, Australia, Dec
Conference of the European Society f or Research in 9-14, 2001.
Mathematics Education, vol. 1 Forschungsinstitut Banerjee, R. and Subramaniam, K. (2004) ‘Term’ as a
fuer Mathematikdidaktik, Osnabrueck, bridge concept between algebra and arithmetic.
Alibali, M., Knuth, E., Hattikudur, S., Mcneil, N. and Paper presented at Episteme inter national
Stephens, A. (2007) A Longitudinal Examination conference December 13-17, International
of Middle School Students’ Understanding of the Centre, Dona Paula, Goa
Equal Sign and Equivalent Equations Mathematical Bastable, V. and Schifter D. (2008) Classroom stor ies:
Thinking and Learning, Volume 9, Issue 3, 2007, examples of elementar y students engaged in
221 – 247 early algebra. In J. Kaput, D. Carraher and M.
Amit, M. and Fried, M. (2005) Multiple Blanton (eds.) Algebra in the ear ly grades. 165-
representations in 8 th grade algebra lessons: 184. New York: Erlbaum
Are learners really getting it? In Chick, H. L., and Bednarz, N. and Janvier, B. (1996). Emergence and
Vincent, J. L. (Eds.). (2005). Proceedings of the development of algebra as a problem solving
International Group for the Psychology of tool: Continuities and discontinuities with
Mathematics Education 2-57 to 2-64 arithmetic. In N. Bednarz, C. Kieran and L. Lee, L.
Melbourne: PME (Eds.), Approaches to Algebra (pp. 115 136).
Amit, M. and Neria, D. (2007) ‘‘Rising to the Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic
challenge’’: using generalization in pattern Publishers.
problems to unearth the algebraic skills of Bednarz, N., Kieran C., Lee, L. (1996) Approaches to
talented pre-algebra students ZDM Mathematics algebra: perspectives for research on teaching. In
Education 40:111–129 Bednarz, N., Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.) Approaches to
Anghileri J., Beishuizen, M. and van Putten, C. (2002) algebra: perspectives for research on teaching. pp.3-
From informal strategies to structured 14 Dordrecht: Kluwer
procedures: Mind the gap! Educational Studies in Bell, A. (1996) Algebraic thought and the role of
Mathematics 49(2) 149-170. manipulable symbolic language. In Bednarz, N.,
Arcavi A. (1994) Symbol sense: The informal sense- Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.) Approaches to algebra:
making in formal mathematics, For the Learning of perspectives for research on teaching. 151-154
Mathematics, 14 (3) p24-35. Kluwer, Dordrecht
Arzarello, F., Bazzini, L., Chiappini, G. (1994) The Bills, L. (2007) Stereotypes of liter al symbol use in
process of naming in algebraic problem solving, in senior school algebra. In E.Pehkonen (ed.)
J. Ponte and J. Matos (Eds.) Proc. PME XVIII, Lisbon, Proceedings of the annual conference of the
II, p40-44. Internatioanl Group for the Psychology of
Mathematics Education. (2) 73-80. Lahti:
University of Helsinki.
38 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Blanton, M and Kaput, J (2005) Char acterizing a Carraher, D. Brizuela, B. and Schliemann A. (2000)
classroom practice that promotes algebr aic Bringing out the algebr aic character of arithmetic:
reasoning. Journal for Research in Mathematics Instantiating variables in addition and subtr action,
Education, 36 (5) 412-446, pp. 35-25 in T. Nakahara and M. Koyama (Eds.), Proceedings
Bloedy-Vinner, H. (1994) The analgebraic mode of of PME–XXIV, Vol. 2. Hiroshima, Japan, p 145-152.
thinking: the case of the parameter. In J.Ponte and Carraher, D., Brizuela, B. M., and Earnest, D. (2001).
J. Matos (eds.) Proceedings of the annual The reification of additive differences in early
conference of the Inter national Group for the algebra. In H. Chick, K. Stacey, J. Vincent, and J.
Psychology of Mathematics Education. (2) 88-95. Vincent (Eds.), The future of the teaching and
Lisbon, University of Lisboa. learning of algebra: Proceedings of the 12th ICMI
Boero, P. (2001). Transformation and anticipation as Study Conference (vol. 1). The University of
key processes in algebr aic problem solving. In Melbourne, Australia.
Sutherland, R., Rojano, T., Bell, A. and Lins, R.(eds.) Carraher, D., Mar tinez, M. and Schliemann, A. (2007)
Perspectives on School Algebra. pp. 99-119 Early algebra and mathematical gener alization
Dordrecht, Kluwer. ZDM Mathematics Education 40, 3-22
Booth, L. (1984) Algebra: Children’s strategies and Carraher, D., Schliemann, A., and Brizuela, B. (2001).
errors, a report of the strategies and errors in the Can young students operate on unknowns?
secondary school project, NFER-Nelson, London. Proceedings of the XXV Conference of the
Boulton-Lewis, G. M., Cooper, T. J., Atweh, B., Pillay, H., International Group for the Psychology of
Wilss, L., and Mutch, S. (1997). The transition from Mathematics Education, Utrecht, The Netherlands.
arithmetic to algebra: A cognitive perspective. In Vol. 1, 130-140
E. Pehkonen (ed.) Proceedings of the International Collis, K. (1971) A study of concrete and f ormal
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education , reasoning in school mathematics. Australian Journal
21(2), 185-192. of Psychology. 23, 289-296.
Brenner, M. E., Mayer, R. E., Moseley, B., Brar, T., Duran, Cooper, T. and Warren, E. (2007) The effect of
R., Reed, B. S. and Webb, D. (1997) Learning by different representations on Years 3 to 5 students’
understanding: The role of multiple ability to generalize. ZDM Mathematics Education
representations in learning algebra. American 40 29-37
Educational Research Journal, 34(4), 663-689. Davydov V. (1990) Types of generalisation in
Britt, M. and Irwin, K. (2007) Algebraic thinking with instruction: Logical and psychological prob lems in the
and without algebraic representation: a three-year structuring of school curricula. Reston, VA: NCTM.
longitudinal study. ZDM Mathematics Education Dettori, G. Garutti, R. Lemut, E. (2001) From
40, 39-53 arithmetic to algebraic Thinking by using a
Brown, L. and Coles, A. (1999) Needing to use spreadsheet, in R. Sutherland, T. Rojano, A. Bell and
algebra – a case study. In O Zaslavsky (ed.) R. Lins (Eds) Perspectives on School Algebra, p 191-
Proceedings of the 23rd t annual conference of the 208, Kluwer, Dordrecht.
International Group for the Psychology of Dickson, L. (1989a) Area of rectangle. In D. Johnson
Mathematics Education, Technion; Haifa, 2, 153-160. (ed.) Children’s Mathematical Frameworks 8 – 13:
Brown, L. and Coles, A. (2001) Natural algebraic a study of classroom teaching . 88-125, Windsor:
activity. In Chick, H., Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and NFER-Nelson.
Vincent, J. (eds.) Proceedings of the 12 th ICMI study Dickson, L. (1989b) Equations. In D. Johnson (ed.)
conference: The future of the teaching and lear ning Children’s Mathematical Frameworks 8 to 13: a
of algebra. pp.120-127 University of Melbourne, study of classroom teaching. pp. 151-190 Windsor:
Australia, Dec 9-14, 2001. NFER- Nelson
Bruner, J. (1966). Toward a Theory of Instruction. Dougherty, B. (1996) The write way: a look at jour nal
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press writing in first-year algebra. Mathematics Teacher
Carpenter, T. and Levi, L. (2000) Developing 89, 556-560
conceptions of algebraic reasoning in the pr imary Dougherty, B. (2001) Access to algebra: a process
grades. Research Report, Madison WI: National approach. In Chick, H., Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and
Center for Improving Student Learning and Vincent, J. (eds.) Proceedings of the 12th ICMI study
Achievement in Mathematics and Science . conference: The future of the teaching and lear ning
www.wcer.wisc.edu/ncisla/publications of algebra. pp. 207-212 University of Melbourne,
Australia, Dec 9-14, 2001.
39 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Drijvers, P. (2001) The concept of par ameter in a Fujii, T. and Stephens, M. (2008) Using number
computer algebra environment. In Chick, H., sentences to introduce the idea of var iable. In.C.
Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and Vincent, J. (eds.) Greenes and R. Rubenstein (eds.) Algebra and
Proceedings of the 12 th ICMI study conference: The Algebraic Thinking in School Mathematics . 70th
future of the teaching and lear ning of algebra. p p. Yearbook. pp. 127-140. Reston,VA: NCTM.
221-227 University of Melbourne, Australia, Dec Furinghetti, F. and Paola, D. (1994). Parameters,
9-14, 2001. unknowns and variables: a little difference?, in
English, L. and Sharry, P. (1996) Analogical reasoning Proceedings of the XVIII Inter national Conference for
and the development of algebraic abstraction. the Psychology of Mathematics Education, p p. ? ?
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 30(2), 135-157. University of Lisbon, Portugal.
Even, R. (1998). Factors involved in linking Goldin, G. (2002) Representation in mathematical
representations of functions. Journal of learning in English, L (ed.) Handbook of
Mathematical Behavior, 17(1), 105-121. international research in mathematics education
Falle, J. (2005) From ar ithmetic to algebra: novice pp.197-218, Mahwah NJ, Erlbaum.
students’ strategies for solving equations. Gray, E. and Tall, D. (1994). Duality, ambiguity and
Proceedings of annual conference of Mathematics flexibility: A proceptual view of simple ar ithmetic,
Education Research Group of Australasia. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 26
Downloaded from (2), 115-141
http://www.merga.net.au/search.php April 2008 Greenes, C. and Rubenstein, R. (2007) (eds.) Algebra
Filloy E. and Rojano, T. (1989) Solving equations: the and algebraic thinking in school mathematics . 70th
transition from arithmetic to algebra. For the Yearbook. Reston,VA: NCTM.
Learning of Mathematics, 9(2) 19-25. Hart, K. (Ed.) (1981) Children’s understanding of
Filloy, E. and Suther land, R. (1996), Designing mathematics 11-16, Murray, London..
curricula for teaching and lear ning algebra, in A. Heid, M. (1996) Computer Intensive Algebra:
Bishop, K. Clements, C. Keitel, J. Kilpatrick and C . Reflections on a functional approach to beginning
Laborde (Eds.) International Handbook of algebra. In Bednarz, N., Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.)
Mathematics Education Part 1, Chapter 4, Kluwer, Approaches to algebra: perspectives for research on
Dordrecht, p 139-160. teaching. Kluwer, Dordrecht
Filloy, E. Rojano, T. and Robio, G. (2001) Propositions Herscovics, N. and Linchevski, L. (1994) A cognitive
concerning the resolution of ar ithmetical-algebraic gap between arithmetic and algebra, Educational
problems, in R. Sutherland (Ed.) Perspectives on studies in mathematics, Vol. 27, pp. 59-78.
Algebra, Kluwer, Dordrecht, p155-176. Hirschhorn, D. (1993) A longitudinal study of
Foxman, D., Ruddock, G., Joffe, L., Mason, K., Mitchell, P., students completing four years of UCSMP
and Sexton, B . (1985) A Re view of Monitor ing in mathematics Journal for Research in Mathematics
mathematics 1978 to 1982, Assessment of Education 24 (2), 136-158
Performance Unit, Depar tment of Education and Hitt, F. (1998) Difficulties in the articulation of different
Science, London. representations linked to the concept of function.
Friedlander, A. and Tabach, M. (2001) Developing a Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 17(1), 123-134.
curriculum of beginning algebra in a spreadsheet Hollar, J. and Norwood, K. (1999) The effects of a
environment. In Chick, H., Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and graphing-approach intermediate algebra
Vincent, J. (eds.) Proceedings of the 12th ICMI study curriculum on students’ understanding of function
conference: The future of the teaching and lear ning Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 30
of algebra. Pp.252-257. University of Melbourne, (2) 220-226.
Australia. Jacobs, V., Franke, M., Carpenter, T., Levi, L., Battey, D.
Fujii, T. and Stephens, M. (2001) Fostering an (2007) Professional development focused on
understanding of generalisation through numerical children’s algebraic reasoning in elementar y
expressions: the role of quasi-var iables. In Chick, school. Journal of Research in Mathematics
H., Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and Vincent, J. (eds.) Education 38(3) 258-288
Proceedings of the 12th ICMI study conf erence: The Janvier, C. (1996) Modeling and the initiation into
future of the teaching and lear ning of algebra. algebra. In Bednarz, N., Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.)
Pp.258-264.University of Melbourne, Australia. Approaches to algebra: perspectives for research on
teaching. pp. 225-238. Kluwer, Dordrecht
40 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Johnson, D. (1989) (ed.) Children’s Mathematical Kirshner David (1989)The Visual Syntax of Algebra
Frameworks 8 – 13: a study of classroom teaching . Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
Windsor: NFER-Nelson. 20(3) 274-287
Kaput, J. (1998) Transforming algebra from an engine Krutetskii, V. (1976). The psychology of mathematical
of inequity to an engine of mathematical po wer abilities in school children. Chicago: University of
by ‘algebrafying’ the K–12 cur riculum. In National Chicago Press.
Council of Teachers of Mathematics and Küchemann, D. (1981) Algebra, in K. Har t (Ed.)
Mathematical Sciences Education Board (Eds.). The Children’s Understanding of Mathematics 11-16, p
nature and role of algebra in the K–14 cur riculum: 102-119 Murray, London..
Proceedings of a National Symposium (pp. 25–26). Lamon, S. (1998) Algebra: meaning through
Washington, DC: National Research Council, modelling, in A. Olivier and K. Newstead (Eds)
National Academy Press. 22nd Conference of the Inter national Group for
Kaput, J. (1999). Teaching and learning a new algebr a. the Psychology of Mathematics Education, vol. 3,
In E. Fennema and T. A. Romberg (Eds.), pp.167-174. Stellenbosch: International Group
Mathematics classrooms that promote for the Psychology of Mathematics Education.
understanding (pp. 133-155). Mahwah, NJ:Erlbaum. Lampert, M. (1986) Knowing, doing and teaching
Kerslake, D. (1986) Fractions: Children’s strategies and multiplication. Cognition and Instruction 3 ,305-342.
errors. Windsor: NFER-Nelson. Lee, L. (1996) An incitation into algebraic culture
Kieran, C. (1981) Concepts associated with the through generalization activities. In Bednarz, N.,
equality symbol. Educational Studies in Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.) Approaches to algebra:
Mathematics 12, 317-326 perspectives for research on teaching. pp. 87-106.
Kieran, C. (1983) Relationships between novices’ Kluwer, Dordrecht
views of algebraic letters and their use of Lehrer, R., Strom, D. and Confrey, J. (2002) Grounding
symmetric and asymmetric equation-solving metaphors and inscriptional resonance: children’s
procedures. In J. Bergeron and B. Herscovics emerging understanding of mathematical
(eds.) Proceedings of the International Group similarity. Cognition and Instruction 20(3) 359-398.
for the Psychology of Mathematics Education Lima, R. and Tall, D. (2008) Procedural embodiment
pp.1.161-1-168, Montreal: Universite de and magic in linear equations. Educational Studies
Montreal. in Mathematics 67(1) 3-18
Kieran, C. (1989) The early learning of algebra: a Linchevski, L., and Herscovics, N. (1996). Crossing the
structural perspective. In S. Wagner and C . Keiran cognitive gap between arithmetic and algebra:
(eds. ) Research issues in the learning and teaching Operating on the unknown in the context of
of algebra. pp. 33-56, Reston, VA, NCTM. equations. Educational Studies in Mathematics,
Kieran, C. (1992). The learning and teaching of 30,1, 39-65.
algebra, in D.A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of Linchevski, L. and Sfard, A. (1991) Rules without
Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning, reasons as processes without objects: The case of
Macmillan, New York, p390-419. equations and inequalities. In F. Furinghetti (ed.)
Kieran, C. and Saldanha, L. (2005) Computer algebr a Proceedings of the 15th International Group for the
systems (CAS) as a tool f or coaxing the Psychology of Mathematics Education. 2, 317-324.
emergence of reasoning about equations of Lins, R.L. (1990). A framework of understanding what
algebraic expressions. In Chick, H. L., and Vincent, algebraic thinking is. In G. Booker, P.Cobb and T.
J. L. (Eds.). (2005). Proceedings of the Inter national Mendicuti (eds.) Proceedings of the 14th
Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education Conference of the Inter national Group for the
pp.3-193-3-200 Melbourne: PME Psychology of Mathematics Education, 14, 2, 93-
Kieran, C. and Sfard, A. (1999), Seeing through 101. Oaxtepec, Mexico.
symbols: The case of equivalent expressions, Focus MacGregor, M. and Stacey, K. (1993) Cognitive
on Learning Problems in Mathematics, 21 (1) p1-17 Models Underlying Students’ Formulation of
Kieran, C., Boileau, A.and Garancon, M. (1996) Simple Linear Equations Journal for Research in
Introducing algebra by means of a technology- Mathematics Education 24(3) 274-232
supported, functional approach. In Bednarz, N., MacGregor, M. and Stacey, K. (1995), The Effect of
Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.) Approaches to algebra: Different Approaches to Algebra on Students’
perspectives for research on teaching. Pp. 257-294 Perceptions of Functional Relationships,
Kluwer, Dordrecht Mathematics Education Research Jour nal, 7 (1)
p69-85.
41 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

MacGregor M.and Stacey K. (1997) Students’ Radford, L. (2007) Iconicity and contraction: a
understanding of algebraic notation 11-15, semiotic investigation of forms of algebraic
Educational Studies in Mathematics. 33(1), 1-19 generalizations of patterns in different contexts
Mason, J. and Sutherland, R. (2002), Key Aspects of ZDM Mathematics Education 40 83-96
Teaching Algebra in Schools, QCA, London. Redden, T. (1994) Alternative pathways in the
Mason, J., Stephens, M. and Watson, A. (in press) transition from arithmetical thinking to algebraic
Appreciating mathematical structure for all. thinking. In J. da Ponte and J. Matos (eds.)
Mathematics Education Research Jour nal. Proceedings of the 18 th annual conference of the
Moss, J., R. Beatty, L. Macnab.(2006) Design for the International Group for the Psychology of
development and teaching of an inte grated Mathematics Education vol. 4, 89-96, University of
patterning curriculum. Paper present to AERA Lisbon, Portugal.
(April 2006.) Reed, S. (1972) Pattern recognition and
Nemirovsky, R. (1996) A functional approach to categorization. Cognitive Psychology 3, 382-407
algebra: two issues that emerge. In Bednarz, N., Reggiani, M. (1994) Generalization as a basis f or
Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.) Approaches to algebra: algebraic thinking: observations with 11-12 year
perspectives for research on teaching. pp. 295-316. old pupils Proceedings of PME XVIII, IV, p97 - 104.
Kluwer, Dordrecht Resnick, L. B., Lesgold, S., and Bill, V. (1990). From
Nickson, M. (2000) Teaching and learning protoquantities to number sense. In G. Booker, J.
mathematics: a teacher’s guide to recent research Cobb, and T. N. de Mendicuti (Eds.), Proceedings of
and its applications. Cassell London the Fourteenth Psychology of Mathematics
NMAP, National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008) Education Conference (Vol. 3, pp. 305-311). Mexico
downloaded April 2008 from: City, Mexico: International Group for the
http://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel Psychology of Mathematics Education.
/index.html Rivera, F. and Becker, J. (2007) Middle school children’s
Noble, T., Nemirovsky, R., Wright, T., and Tierney, C. cognitive perceptions of constr uctive and
(2001) Experiencing change: The mathematics of deconstructive generalizations involving linear figural
change in multiple environments. Journal for patterns ZDM Mathematics Education 40 65-82
Research in Mathematics Education, 32(1) 85-108. Robinson, K., Ninowski, J. and Gray, M. (2006)
Noss, R., Healy, L., Hoyles, C. (1997) The construction Children’s understanding of the ar ithmetic
of mathematical meanings: Connecting the visual concepts of inversion and associativity, Journal of
with the symbolic. Educational Studies in Experimental Child Psychology 94, pp. 349–362.
Mathematics 33(2), 203-233 Rothwell-Hughes, E.(1979) Conceptual Powers of
Nunes, T. and Bryant, P. (1995) Do problem situations Children: an approach through mathematics and
influence children’s understanding of the science. Schools Council Publications.
commutativity of multiplication? Mathematical Rowland, T. and Bills, L. (1996) ‘Examples,
Cognition, 1, 245-260. Generalisation and Proof ’ Proceedings of British
O’Callaghan, B. (1998) Computer-Intensive Algebra Society for Research in Learning Mathematics
and Students’ Conceptual Knowledge of London Institute of Education, pp. 1-7
Functions, Journal For Research in Mathematics Ryan, J. and Williams, J. (2007) Children’s Mathematics
Education, 29 (1) p21-40. 4 -15: learning from errors and misconceptions
Piaget, J. (1969) The mechanisms of perception. Maidenhead Open University Press.
London: RKP Sáenz-Ludlow, A.and Walgamuth, C. (1998) Third
Piaget, J., & Moreau, A. (2001). The inversion of Graders’ Interpretations of Equality and the Equal
arithmetic operations (R. L. Campbell, Trans.). In J. Symbol Educational Studies in Mathematics , 35(2)
Piaget (Ed.), Studies in reflecting abstraction (pp. 153-187.
69–86). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.Powell, A. B. Schwartz, J. and Yerushalmy, M. (1992) Getting
and Maher, C. A. (2003). Heuristics of Twelfth students to function in and with algebr a. In G.
Graders’ Building Isomorphism. In Pateman, N., Harel and E. Dubinsky (eds.) The concept of
Dougherty, B. and Zilliox, J. (eds.) Proceedings of function: Aspects of epistemology and pedagogy.
the 27th conference of the inter national group for MAA Notes, 25, 261-289. Washington DC: MAA.
the psychology of mathematics education 4, pp. 23- Sfard, A. and Linchevski, L. (1994) . The gains and the
30. Honolulu, Hawaii: University of Hawaii. pitfalls of reification: The case of algebr a.
Educational Studies in Mathematics , 26, 191-228.
42 PaperSUMMARY
6: Algebraic
– PAPER
reasoning
2: Understanding whole numbers

Siegler, R. S., and Stern, E. (1998). Conscious and Vlassis, J., (2002) The balance model: Hindrance or
unconscious strategy discoveries: a microgenetic support for the solving of linear equations with
analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology 127, one unknown. Educational Studies in Mathematics
377-397. 49(3), 341-359.
Simmt, E. and Kieren, T. (1999). Expanding the Warren, E. and Cooper, T. (2008) Patterns that
cognitive domain: the role(s) and consequence of support early algebraic thinking in the elementary
interaction in mathematics knowing. In F. Hitt and school. In.C. Greenes and R. Rubenstein (eds.)
M. Santos (Eds.) Proceedings of the Twenty First Algebra and Algebraic Thinking in School
Annual Meeting Psychology of Mathematics Mathematics. 70th Yearbook. pp.113-126,
Education North American Chapter, pp.299-305. Reston,VA: NCTM.
Stacey, K. (1989) Finding and Using P atterns in Linear Wertheimer, M. (1960) Productive thinking. New York:
Generalising Problems, Educational Studies in Harper
Mathematics, 20, p147-164. Wong, M. (1997) Numbers versus letters in algebraic
Stacey, K., and MacGregor, M. (2000). Learning the manipulation: which is more difficult? In
algebraic method of solving problems. Journal of E.Pehkonen (ed.) Proceedings of the annual
Mathematical Behavior, 18(2), 149-167. conference of the Inter natioanl Group for the
Steele, D. (2007) Seventh-grade students’ Psychology of Mathematics Education. (4) 285-290.
representations for pictorial growth and change Lahti: University of Helsinki.
problems 97-110 ZDM Mathematics Education Yeap, B-H. and Kaur, B. (2007) Elementar y school
40 97-110 students engaging in making gener alisation:a
Sutherland R. and Rojano T. (1993) A Spreadsheet glimpse from a Singapore classroom ZDM
Algebra Approach to Solving Algebra Problems, Mathematics Education 40, 55-64
Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 12, 353-383. Yerushalmy, M. (1997) Designing representations:
Thomas, M. and Tall, D. (2001) The long-term Reasoning about functions of two variables.
cognitive development of symbolic algebr a. In Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
Chick, H., Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and Vincent, J. (eds.) 28 (4) 431-466.
Proceedings of the 12 th ICMI study conference: The
future of the teaching and lear ning of algebra
pp.590-597. University of Melbourne, Australia,
Dec 9-14, 2001.
Tsamir, P., and Almog, N. (2001). Students’ strategies
and difficulties: The case of algebraic inequalities.
International Journal of Mathematics Education in
Science and Technology, 32, 513-524.
Tsamir, P., and Bazzini, L. (2001). Can x=3 be the
solution of an inequality? A study of Italian an
Israeli students. In M. van den Heuvel-Panhuizen
(Ed.), Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting for
the Psychology of Mathematics Education,
Utrecht: Holland. (Vol IV, pp. 303-310).
van Ameron. B. (2003) Focusing on inf ormal
strategies when linking arithmetic to early algebra.
Educational Studies in Mathematics, 2003, vol. 54,
no. 1, p. 63-75..
Vergnaud, G. (1997) The nature of mathematical
concepts. In T. Nunes and P. Bryant (eds.) Learning
and teaching mathematics: an international
perspective. pp. 5-28 Hove: Psychology Press
Vergnaud, G. (1998). A comprehensive theory of
representation for mathematics Journal of
Mathematical Behavior, 17(2), 167-181
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-73-3

www.nuffieldfoundation.org
7
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving
and integrating concepts
By Anne Watson, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Summary of Paper 7 3
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: Modelling, problem-solving and
integrating concepts 8
Paper 1: Overview
Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and References 35
whole numbers
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities About the author
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical Anne Watson is Professor of Mathematics
representation Education at the University of Oxford.
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation
in mathematics
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning About the Nuffield Foundation
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
concepts charitable trust established in 1943 by William
Paper 8: Methodological appendix Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant being. We fund research and pr actical
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while experiment and the development of capacity
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics to under take them; working across education,
in secondary schools. science, social science and social policy. While
most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which responsive grant programmes we also
has been published separately as Introduction and undertake our own initiatives.
summary of findings.

Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published


together as Summary papers.

All publications are available to download from


our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Summary of paper 7:
Modelling, problem-
solving and integrating
concepts

Headlines
We have assumed a general educational context • In many mathematical situations in secondary
which encourages thinking and problem-solving mathematics, students have to look for relations
across subjects. A key difference about mathematics between numbers and variables and relations
is that empirical approaches may solve individual between relations and properties of objects, and
problems, and offer directions for reasoning, but do know how to represent them.
not themselves lead to new mathematical knowledge
or mathematical reasoning, or to the power that
comes from applying an abstract idea to a situation. How secondary learners tackle
new situations
In secondary mathematics, the major issue is not
how children learn elementary concepts, but what In new situations students first respond to familiar ity
experiences they have had and how these enable or in appearance, or language, or context. They bring
limit what else can be lear nt.That is why we have earlier understandings to bear on new situations,
combined several aspects of secondar y mathematics sometimes erroneously. They naturally generalise from
which could be exemplified by par ticular topics. what they are offered, and they often over-generalise
and apply inappropriate ideas to new situations. They
• Students have to be fluent in understanding methods can learn new mathematical concepts either as
and confident about using them to know why and extensions or integrations of earlier concepts, and/or
when to apply them, but such application does not as inductive generalisations from examples, and/or as
automatically follow learning procedures. Students abstractions from solutions to problems.
have to understand the situation as well as being able
to call on a familiar repertoire of ideas and methods.
Routine or context?
• Students have to know some elementary concepts
well enough to apply them and combine them to One question is whether mathematics is lear nt
form new concepts in secondary mathematics, but better from routines, or from complex contextual
little is known from research about what concepts situations. Analysis of research which compares ho w
are essential in this way. Knowledge of a range of children learn mathematics through being taught
functions is necessary for modelling situations. routines efficiently (such as with computer ised and
other learning packages designed to minimise
• Students have to learn when and how to use cognitive load) to lear ning through problem-solving
informal, experiential reasoning and when to use in complex situations (such as through Realistic
formal, conventional, mathematical reasoning. Mathematics Education) shows that the significant
Without special attention to meanings many difference is not about the speed and retention of
students tend to apply visual reasoning, or be learning but what is being learnt. In each approach
triggered by verbal cues, rather than to analyse the main question for progression is whether the
situations mathematically. student learns new concepts well enough to use
4 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

and adapt them in future lear ning and outside International research into the use of ICT to pro vide
mathematics. Both approaches have inherent new ways to represent situations and to see
weaknesses in this respect. These weaknesses will relationships, such as by comparing spreadsheets,
become clear in what f ollows. However, there are graph plotters and dynamic images appear to speed
several studies which show that those who develop up the process of relating representations through
mathematical methods of enquir y over time can isomorphic reasoning about covariation, and hence
then learn procedures easily and do as well, or the development of understanding about
better, in general tests. mathematical structures and relations.

Problem-solving Application of earlier learning


and modelling
Knowing methods
To learn mathematics one has to lear n to solve Students who have only routine knowledge may not
mathematical problems or model situations recognize that it is relevant to the situation. Or they
mathematically. Studies of students’ problem solving can react to verbal or visual cues without reference
mainly focus in the successful solution of to context, such as ‘how much?’ triggering
contextually-worded problems using mathematical multiplication rather than division, and ‘how many?’
methods, rather than using problem solving as a always triggering addition. A fur ther problem is that
context for learning new concepts and developing they may not understand the underlying relationships
mathematical thinking. To solve unfamiliar problems they are using and how these relate to each other.
in mathematics, a meta-analysis of 487 studies For example, a routine approach to 2 x 1/3 x 3/2
concluded that for students to be maximall y may neither exploit the meaning of fractions nor the
successful: multiplicative relation.
• problems need to be fully stated with supportive
diagrams Students who have only experience of applying
• students need to have previous extensive generic problem-solving skills in a r ange of
experience in using the representations used situations sometimes do not recogniz e underlying
• they have to have relevant basic mathematical skills mathematical structures to which they can appl y
to use methods used in the past. Indeed, given the well-
• teachers have to understand problem-solving documented tendency for people to use ad hoc
methods. arithmetical trial-and-adjustment methods wherever
these will lead to reasonab le results, it is possible
This implies that fluency with representations and that problem-solving experience may not result in
skills is impor tant, but also depends on how clearly learning new mathematical concepts or w orking
the problem is stated. In some studies the difficulty is with mathematical str uctures, or in becoming fluent
also to do with the under lying concept, for example, with efficient methods.
in APU tests area problems were difficult with or
without diagrams.
Knowing concepts
To be able to solve problems whose wording does Students who have been helped to lear n concepts,
not indicate what to do, students have to be able and can define, recognise and exemplify elementar y
to read the problem in two ways: firstly, their ideas are better able to use and combine these
technical reading skills and under standing of ideas in new situations and while learning new
notation have to be good enough; secondly, they concepts. However, many difficulties appear to be
have to be able to interpret it to under stand the due to having too limited a r ange of under standing.
contextual and mathematical meanings. They have Their understanding may be based on examples
to decide whether and ho w to bring informal which have irrelevant features in common, such as
knowledge to bear on the situation, or, if they the parallel sides of par allelograms always being
approach it formally, what are the variables and parallel to the edges of a page . Understanding is
how do they relate . If they are approaching it also limited by examples being similar to a
formally, they then have to represent the prototype, rather than extreme cases. Another
relationships in some way and decide how to problem is that students ma y recognize examples of
operate on them. a concept by focusing too much on visual or verbal
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

aspects, rather than their proper ties, such as • have experience of mulling problems over time in
believing that it is possib le to constr uct an order to gain insight.
equilateral triangle on a nine-pin geoboard
because it ‘looks like one’. With suitable environments, tools, images and
encouragement, learners can and do use their
Robust connections between and within ear lier general perceptual, comparative and reasoning
ideas can make it easier to engage with new ideas, powers in mathematics lessons to:
but can also hinder if the ear lier ideas are limited and • generalise from what is offered and experienced
inflexible. For example, learning trigonometry involves • look for analogies
understanding: the definition of triangle; right-angles; • identify variables
recognizing them in different orientations; what angle • choose the most efficient variables, those with
means and how it is measured; typical units for most connections
measuring lines; what ratio means; similarity of • see simultaneous variations
triangles; how ratio is written as a fr action; how to • understand change
manipulate a multiplicative relationship; what ‘sin’ • reason verbally before symbolising
(etc.) means as a symbolic representation of a • develop mental models and other imagery
function and so on. Thus knowing about ratios • use past experience of successful and unsuccessful
can suppor t learning trigonometry, but if the attempts
understanding of ‘ratio’ is limited to mixing cak e • accumulate knowledge of operations and situations
recipes it won’t help much. To be successful students to do all the above successfully
have to have had enough exper ience to be fluent,
and enough knowledge to use methods wisely. Of course, all the tendencies just descr ibed can also
go in unhelpful directions and in par ticular people
They become better at problem-solving and tend to:
modelling when they can: • persist in using past methods and applying
• draw on knowledge of the contextual situation to procedures without meaning, if that has been their
identify variables and relationships and/or, through previous mathematical experience
imagery, construct mathematical representations • get locked into the specific situation and do not, by
which can be manipulated further themselves, know what new mathematical ideas
• draw on a repertoire of representations, functions, can be abstracted from these experiences
and methods of operation on these • be unable to interpret symbols, text, and other
• have a purpose for the modelling process, so that representations in ways the teacher expects
the relationship between manipulations in the • use additive methods; assume that if one variable
model and changes in the situation can be increases so will another; assume that all change is
meaningfully understood and checked for linear; confuse quantities.
reasonableness.

Knowing how to approach


mathematical tasks
To be able to decide when and ho w to use informal
or formal approaches, and how to use pr ior
knowledge, students need to be able to think
mathematically about all situations in mathematics
lessons. This develops best as an all-encompassing
perspective in mathematics lessons, rather than
through isolated experiences.

Students have to:


• learn to avoid instant reactions based in superficial
visual or verbal similarity
• practice using typical methods of mathematical
enquiry explicitly over time
6 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

RECOMMENDATIONS

Research about mathematical Recommendations for teaching


learning

Learning routine methods and lear ning Developers of the cur riculum, advisory schemes of work and
through complex exploration lead to teaching methods need to be a ware of the impor tance of
different kinds of knowledge and cannot understanding new concepts, and avoid teaching solely to pass
be directly compared; neither method test questions, or using solely problem-solving mathematical
necessarily enables learning new concepts activities which do not lead to new abstr act understandings.
or application of powerful mathematics
ideas. However, those who have the habit Students should be helped to balance the need f or fluency
of complex exploration are often able to with the need to work with meaning.
learn procedures quickly.

Students naturally respond to familiar Teaching should take into account students’ natural ways of
aspects of mathematics; try to apply prior dealing with new perceptual and v erbal information, and the
knowledge and methods, and generalize likely misapplications. Schemes of work and assessment should
from their experience. allow enough time for students to adapt to new meanings and
move on from ear lier methods and conceptualizations.

Students are more successful if they Developers of the cur riculum, advisory schemes of work and
have a fluent reper toire of conceptual teaching methods should give time for new experiences and
knowledge and methods, including mathematical ways of working to become familiar in sev eral
representations, on which to dr aw. representations and contexts before moving on.

Students need time and multiple experiences to develop a


repertoire of appropriate functions, operations,
representations and mathematical methods in order to solv e
problems and model situations.

Teaching should ensure conceptual under standing as well as


‘knowing about’, ‘knowing how to’, and ‘knowing how to use’.

Multiple experiences over time enable Schemes of work should allow for students to have multiple
students to develop new ways to work on experiences, with multiple representations over time to
mathematical tasks, and to develop the develop mathematically appropriate ‘habits of mind’.
ability to choose what and ho w to apply
earlier learning.

Students who work in computer- There are resource implications about the use of ICT. Students
supported multiple representational need to be in control of switching betw een representations and
contexts over time can understand and comparisons of symbolic expression in order to under stand the
use graphs, variables, functions and the syntax and the concept of functions. The United Kingdom may
modelling process. Students who can be lagging behind the developed world in exploring the use of
choose to use available technology are spreadsheets, graphing tools, and other software to suppor t
better at problem solving, and have application and authentic use of mathematics.
complex understanding of relations, and
have more positive views of mathematics.
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Recommendations for research

Application of research findings about problem-


solving, modelling and conceptual lear ning to current
curriculum developments in the United Kingdom
suggests that there may be different outcomes in
terms of students’ ability to solve quantitative and
spatial problems in realistic contexts. However, there
is no evidence to convince us that the new National
Curriculum in England will lead to better conceptual
understanding of mathematics, either at the
elementary levels, which are necessar y to learn
higher mathematics, or at higher levels which provide
the confidence and foundation for fur ther
mathematical study. Where contextual and
exploratory mathematics, integrated through the
curriculum, do lead to fur ther conceptual learning it
is related to conceptual lear ning being a r igorous
focus for curriculum and textbook design, and in
teacher preparation, such as in China, Japan,
Singapore, and the Nether lands, or in specifically
designed projects based around such aims.

In the main body of Paper 7: Modelling, solving


problems and learning new concepts in secondar y
mathematics there are several questions for future
research, including the following.
• What are the key conceptual understandings for
success in secondary mathematics, from the point
of view of learning?
• How do students learn new ideas in mathematics
at secondary level that depend on combinations of
earlier concepts?
• What evidence is there of the characteristics of
mathematics teaching at higher secondary level
which contribute both to successful conceptual
learning and application of mathematics?
8 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

Modelling, problem-
solving and integrating
concepts

Introduction
By the time students enter secondar y school, they the ‘pure’ and ‘applied’ aspects of lear ning at
possess not only intuitive knowledge from outside secondary level, and use research from both
mathematics and outside school, but also a r ange of traditions to devise some common implications
quasi-intuitive understandings within mathematics, and overall recommendations for practice.
derived from earlier teaching and from their
memory of generalisations, metaphors, images,
metonymic associations and str ategies that have Characteristics of learning
served them well in the past. Many of these typical secondary mathematics
understandings are described in the previous
chapters. Tall and Vinner (1981) called these We justify the broad scope of this chapter by
understandings ‘concept images’, which are a r agbag indicating similarities between the learning of the new
of personal conceptual, quasi-conceptual, perceptual concepts of secondar y mathematics and learning how
and other associations that relate to the language of to apply mathematics to analyse, express and solve
the concept and are loosel y connected by the problems in mathematical and non-mathematical
language and observable artefacts associated with contexts. Both of these aspects of lear ning
the concept. Faced with new situations, students will mathematics depend on interpreting new situations
apply whatever familiar methods and associations and bringing to mind a reper toire of mathematical
come to mind relatively quickly – perhaps not concepts that are understood and fluent to some
realising that this can be a r isky strategy. If ‘doing extent. In this review we will show that learning
what I think I know how to do’ leads so easily to secondary mathematics presents core common
incorrect mathematics it is hardl y surprising that difficulties, whatever the curriculum approach being
many students end up seeing school mathematics taken, which need to be addressed through pedagogy.
as the acquisition and application of methods, and a
site of failure, rather than as the development of a In all teaching methods, when presented with a new
repertoire of adaptable intellectual tools. stimulus such as a symbolic expression on the board,
a physical situation, or a statement of a complex ill-
At secondary level, new mathematical situations are defined ‘real life’ problem, the response of an
usually ideas which ar ise through mathematics and engaged learner is to wonder:
can then be applied to other areas of activity; it is
less likely that mathematics involves the formalisation What is this? This entails ‘reading’ situations,
of ideas which have arisen from outside exper ience usually reading mathematical representations or
as is common in the primary phase. Because of this words, and interpreting these in conventional
difference, learning mathematics at secondar y level mathematical ways. It involves perception, attention,
cannot be understood only in terms of overall understanding representations and being able to
cognitive development. For this review, we decipher symbol systems.
developed a perspective which encompasses both
9 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

What is going on here? This entails identifying Part 1: What learners have to
salient features including non-visual aspects, be able to do in secondary
identifying variables, relating par ts to each other, mathematics
exploring what changes can be made and the eff ects
of change, representing situations in mathematical In this chapter we describe what learners have to
ways, anticipating what might be the pur pose of a be able to do in order to lear n new concepts, solve
mathematical object. It involves attention, visualisation, problems, model mathematical situations, and engage
modelling, static and dynamic representations, in mathematical thinking.
understanding functional, statistical and geometr ical
relationships, focusing on what is mathematicall y
salient and imagining the situation or a Learning ‘new’ concepts
representation of it.
Extension of meaning
What do I know about this? This entails recognising Throughout school, students meet familiar ideas used
similarities, seeking for recognisable structures in new contexts which include b ut extend their old
beyond visual impact, identifying variables, proposing use, often through integr ating simpler concepts into
suitable functions, drawing on reper toire of past more complex ideas. Sfard (1991) descr ibes this
experiences and choosing what is lik ely to be useful. process of development of meaning as consisting of
Research about memory, problem-solving, concept ‘interiorization’ through acting on a new idea with
images, modelling, functions, analysis and analogical some processes so that it becomes familiar and
reasoning is likely to be helpful. meaningful; understanding and expressing these
processes and their effects as manageable units
What can I do? This entails using past exper ience to (condensation), and then this new str ucture
try different approaches, heuristics, logic, controlling becomes a thing in itself (reified) that can be
variables, switching between representations, acted on as a unit in future .
transforming objects, applying manipulations and
other techniques. It involves analogical reasoning, In this way, in algebra, letters standing for numbers
problem-solving, tool-use, reasoning, generalisation and become incorporated into terms and expressions
abstraction, and so on. which are number-like in some uses and yet cannot
be calculated. Operations are combined to descr ibe
Thus, students being presented with the task of structures, and expressions of str uctures become
understanding new ideas dr aw on past exper ience, if objects which can be equated to each other. Variables
they engage with the task at all, just as they would if can be related to each other in ways that represent
offered an unfamiliar situation and ask ed to express relationships as functions, rules for mapping one
it mathematically. They may only get as far as the f irst variable domain to another (see the ear lier chapters
step of ‘reading’ the stimulus. The alternative is to on functional relations and algebr aic thinking).
wait to be told what to do and treat ev erything as
declarative, verbatim, knowledge. A full review of Further, number develops from counting, whole
relevant research in all these areas is bey ond the numbers, and measures to include negativ es,
scope of this paper, and much of it is gener ic rather rationals, numbers of the form a + b √n where a
than concerned with mathematics. and b are rationals, irrationals and transcendentals,
expressions, polynomials and functions which are
We organise this Paper into three par ts: Part 1 number-like when used in expressions, and the uses
looks at what lear ners have to be able to do of estimation which contradict earlier shifts towards
to be successful in these aspects of secondar y accuracy. Eventually, two-dimensional complex
mathematics; Part 2 considers what learners actually numbers may also have to be under stood. Possible
do when faced with new complex mathematical discontinuities of meaning can ar ise between discrete
situations; and Part 3 reviews what happens with and continuous quantities, monomials and
pedagogic intervention designed to address typical polynomials, measuring and two-dimensionality, and
difficulties. We end with recommendations for future different representations (digits, letters, expressions
research, curriculum development and practice. and functions).

Graphs are used first to compare values of various


discrete categories, then are used to express two-
10 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

dimensional discrete and continuous data as in • how ratio is written as a fraction;


scatter-graphs, or algebraic relationships between • how to manipulate a multiplicative relationship;
continuous variables, and later such relationships, • what ‘sin’ (etc.) means as a symbolic representation
especially linear ones, might be fitted to statistical of a function and so on.
representations.
New concepts therefore develop both through
Shapes which were familiar in pr imary school have extension of meaning and combination of concepts.
to be defined and classified in new ways, and new In each of these the kno wledge learners bring to
properties explored, new geometric configurations the new topic has to be adaptab le and usable, not
become impor tant and descriptive reasoning based so strongly attached to previous contexts in which
on characteristics has to give way to logical it has been used that it cannot be adapted. A
deductive reasoning based on relational proper ties. hierarchical ‘top down’ view of learning mathematics
Finally, all this has to be applied in the three- would lead to thinking that all contr ibutory concepts
dimensional contexts of everyday life. need to be fully understood before tackling new
topics (this is the view tak en in the NMAP review
The processes of lear ning are sometimes said to (2008) but is unsuppor ted by research as far as w e
follow historical development, but a better analogy can tell from their document). By contrast, if we take
would be to compare lear ning trajectories with the learners’ developing cognition into account we see
conceptual connections, inclusions and distinctions of that ‘full understanding’ is too vague an aim; it is the
mathematics itself. processes of applying and extending prior knowledge
in the context of working on new ideas that
Integration of concepts contribute to understanding.
As well as this kind of extension, there are new ‘topics’
that draw together a range of earlier mathematics. Whichever view is taken, learners have to bring
Typical examples of secondar y topics are quadratic existing understanding to bear on new mathematical
functions and trigonometry. Understanding each of contexts. There are conflicting research conclusions
these depends to some extent on under standing a about the process of bringing existing ideas to bear
range of concepts met ear lier. on new stimuli: Halford (e.g. 1999) talks of conceptual
chunking to describe how earlier ideas can be dr awn
Quadratic functions: Learning about quadratic on as packages, reducing to simpler objects ideas
functions includes understanding: which are initially formed from more complex ideas,
• the meaning of letters and algebraic syntax; to develop further concepts and argues for such
• when letters are variables and when they can be chunking to be robust before moving on. He focuses
treated as unknown numbers; particularly on class inclusion (see Paper 5,
• algebraic terms and expressions; Understanding space and its representation in
• squaring and square rooting; mathematics) and transitivity, structures of relations
• the conventions of coordinates and graphing between more than two objects, as ideas which are
functions; hard to deal with because they in volve several levels
• the meaning of graphs as representing sets of of complexity. Examples of this difficulty were
points that follow an algebraic rule; mentioned in Paper 4, Understanding relations and
• the meaning of ‘=’; their graphical representation, showing how relations
• translation of curves and the ways in which they between relations cause problems. Chunking includes
can change shape; loss of access to lower level meanings, which may be
• that for a product to equal zero at least one of its useful in avoiding unnecessary detail of specific
terms must equal zero and so on. examples, but can obstruct meaningful use.
Freudenthal (1991, p. 469) points out that automatic
Trigonometry: Learning this includes knowing: connections and actions can mask sources of insight,
• the definition of triangle; flexibility and creativity which arise from meanings. He
• about right-angles including recognizing them in observed that when students are in the flo w of
different orientations; calculation they are not necessarily aware of what
• what angle means and how it is measured; they are doing, and do
• typical units for measuring lines; not monitor their work. It is also the case that m uch
• what ratio means; of the chunking that has tak en place in ear lier
• similarity of triangles; mathematics is limited and hinders and obstructs
11 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

future learning, leading to confusion with so that choices are based on visual, linguistic and cues
contradictory experiences. For example, the which might be misleading (see also earlier chapter
expectation that multiplication will make ‘things’ bigger on number) rather than mathematical meaning. As
can hinder learning that it only means this sometimes examples: the prototypical parallelogram has its
– multiplication scales quantities in a var iety of ways. parallel edges horizontal to the page; and x2 and 2x
The difficulties faced by students whose under standing are confused because it is so common to use ‘x = 2’
of the simpler concepts lear nt in primary school is as an example to demonstr ate algebraic meaning.
later needed for secondary mathematical ideas, are
theorised by, among others, Trzcieniecka-Schneider Abstraction of relationships
(1993) who points out that entrenched and limited A fur ther way to meet new concepts is through a
conceptual ideas (including what Fischbein calls process of ‘vertical mathematisation’ (Treffers, 1987)
‘intuitions’ (1987) and what Tall calls ‘metbefores’ in which experience of solving complex problems
(2004)) can hinder a student’s approach to unfamiliar can be followed by extracting general mathematical
examples and questions and create resistance , rather relationships. It is unlikely that this happens natur ally
than a willingness to engage with new ideas which for any but a few students, yet school mathematics
depend on adapting or giving up strongl y-held notions. often entails this kind of abstr action. The Freudenthal
This leads not only to problems understanding new Institute has developed this approach through
concepts which depend on ear lier concepts, but also teaching experiments and national roll-out over a
makes it hard for learners to see how to apply considerable time, and its Realistic Mathematics
mathematics in unfamiliar situations. On the other Education (e.g. Gravemeijer and Doorman, 1999)
hand, it is impor tant that some knowledge is fluent sees mathematical development as
and easily accessible, such as number bonds, • seeing what has to be done to solve the kinds
recognition of multiples, equivalent algebraic forms, of problems that involve mathematics
the shape of graphs of common functions and so • from the solutions extracting new mathematical
on. Learners have to know when to apply ‘old’ ideas and methods to add to the repertoire
understandings to be extended, and when to give • these methods now become available for future
them up for new and different understandings. use in similar and new situations (as with Piaget’s
notion of reflective abstraction and Polya’s
Inductive generalisation ‘looking back’).
Learners can also approach new ideas b y inductive
generalisation from several examples. English and Gravemeijer and Doorman show that this approach,
Halford (1995 p. 50) see this inductive process1 as which was developed for primary mathematics, is
the development of a mental model which fits the also applicable to higher mathematics, in this case
available data (the range of examples and instances calculus. They refer to ‘the role models can play in a
learners have experienced) and from which shift from a model of situated activity to a model f or
procedures and conjectures can be generated. For mathematical reasoning. In light of this model-
example, learners’ understanding about what a linear of/model-for shift, it is ar gued that discrete functions
graph can look like is at first a generalisation of the and their graphs play a key role as an inter mediary
linear graphs they have seen that have been named between the context problems that have to be
as such. Similarly, learners’ conjectures about the solved and the formal calculus that is developed.’
relationship between the height and volume of water
in a bottle, given as a data set, depends on reasoning Gravemeijer and Doorman’s observation explains
both from the data and from general knowledge of why, in this paper, we are treating the lear ning of
such changes. Leading mathematicians often remark new abstract concepts as related to the use of
that mathematical generalisation also commonly problem-solving and modelling as f orms of
arises from abductive reasoning on one generic mathematical activity. In all of these examples of new
example, such as conjectures about relationships learning, the fundamental shift lear ners are expected
based on static geometrical diagrams. For both these to make, through instruction, is from informal,
processes, the examples available as data, instances, experiential, engagement using their existing
and illustrations from teachers, textbooks and other knowledge to formal, conventional, mathematical
sources play a crucial role in the process. Learners understanding. This shift appears to have three
have to know what features are salient and generalise components: construction of meaning; recognition in
from them. Often such reasoning depends on new contexts; playing with new ideas to b uild fur ther
metonymic association (Holyoak and Thagard, 1995), ideas (Hershkowitz, Schwar tz and Dreyfus, 2001).
12 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

It would be wrong to claim, however, that learning Summary


can only take place through this route, because • Learners have to understand new concepts as
there is considerable evidence that lear ners can extensions or integrations of earlier concepts, as
acquire routine skills through progr ammes of inductive generalisations from examples, and as
carefully constructed, graded, tasks designed to deal abstractions from solutions to problems.
educatively with both right answers and common
errors of reasoning, giving immediate feedback • Robust chunking of earlier ideas can make it easier
(Anderson , Corbett, Koedinger and Pelletier, 1995). to engage with new ideas, but can also hinder if the
The acquisition of routine skills without explicit earlier ideas are limited and inflexible.
work on their meaning is not the f ocus of this
paper, but the automatisation of routines so that • Routine skills can be adopted through practise
learners can focus on str ucture and meaning by to fluency, but this does not lead to conceptual
reflection later on has been a successful route f or understanding, or ability to adapt to unfamiliar
some in mathematics. situations, for many students.

There is recent evidence from controlled tr ials • Learners have to know when and how to bring
that learning routines from abstract presentations earlier understandings to bear on new situations.
is a more efficient way to learn about under lying
mathematical structure than from contextual, • Learners have to know how and when to shift
concrete and stor y-based learning tasks (Kaminski, between informal, experiential activity to formal,
Sloutsky and Heckler, 2008). There are several conventional, mathematical activity.
problems with their findings, for example in one
study the sample consisted of undergraduates for • There is no ‘best way’ to teach mathematical
whom the under lying arithmetical concept being structure: it depends whether the aim is to become
taught would not have been new, even if it had fluent and apply methods in new contexts, or to
never been explicitly formalised for them before. learn how to express structures of given situations.
In a similar study with 11-year-olds, addition
modulo 3 was being taught. For one group a
model of filling jugs with three equal doses was Problem-solving
used; for the other group abstr act symbols were
used. The test task consisted of spurious The phrase ‘problem-solving’ has many meanings
combinatorics involving three unrelated objects 2. and the research liter ature often fails to mak e
Those who had been taught using abstract distinctions. In much research solving word problems
unrelated symbols did better, those who had been is seen as an end in itself and it is not clear whether
taught using jug-filling did not so well. While these the problem introduces a mathematical idea,
studies suggest that abstr act knowledge about formalises an informal idea, or is about translation of
structures is not less applicab le than exper ience words into mathematical instr uctions. There are
and ad hoc knowledge, they also illuminate the several interpretations and the ways students learn,
interpretation difficulties that students have in and can learn, differ accordingly. The following are the
learning how to model phenomena main uses of the phrase in the literature.
mathematically, and how familiar meanings
(e.g. about jug-filling) dominate over abstract 1 Word problems with arithmetical steps used to
engagement. What Kaminski’s results say to introduce elementary concepts by harnessing
educationists is not ‘abstract rules are better’ but informal knowledge, or as situations in which
‘be clear about the lear ning outcomes you are learners have to apply their knowledge of
hoping to achieve and do not expect easy operations and order (see Paper 4, Understanding
transfer between abstract procedures and relations and their gr aphical representation).
meaningful contexts’.
These situations may be modelled with concrete
materials, diagrams or mental images, or might dr aw
on experiences outside school. The purpose may
be either to lear n concepts through familiar
situations, or to lear n to apply formal or informal
mathematical methods. For example, upper primary
13 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

students studied by Squire, Davies and Br yant that this is about representing the changed lengths
(2004) were found to handle commutativity much in terms of the old lengths, and that these lengths
better than distributivity, which they could only do have to be multiplied to understand what happens
if there were contextual cues to help them. For to the area. She might ascr ibe some arbitrary
teaching purposes this indicates that distr ibutive numbers to help her do this, or some letters, or
situations are harder to recognise and handle, and a she might realise that these are not really relevant
mathematical analysis of distributivity suppor ts this – but this realisation is quite sophisticated.
because it entails encapsulation of one operation Alternatively she might decide that this is an
before applying the second and recognition of the empirical problem and generate several numerical
importance of order of oper ations. examples, then using inductive reasoning to give a
general answer.
2 Worded contexts which require the learner to decide to
use standard techniques, such as calculating area, time, 4 Exploratory situations in which there is an ill-defined
and so on. Diagrams, standard equations and gr aphs problem, and the learner has to mathematise by
might offer a bridge towards deciding what to do. identifying variables and conjecturing relationships,
For instance, consider this word problem: ‘The area choosing likely representations and techniques.
of a triangular lawn is 20 square metres, and one Knowledge of a range of possible functions may
side is 5 metres long. If I walk in a str aight line from be helpful, as is mental or gr aphical imagery.
the vertex opposite this side, towards this side, to
meet it at right angles, how far have I walked?’ The In these situations the problem might have been
student has to think of ho w area is calculated, posed as either quasi-abstract or situated. There may
recognise that she has been giv en a ‘base’ length and be no solution, for example: ‘Describe the advantages
asked about ‘height’, and a diagram or mental image and disadvantages of raising the price of cheese rolls
would help her to ‘see’ this. If a diagram is given at the school tuck shop b y 5p, given that cheese
some of these decisions do not ha ve to be made, prices have gone down by 5% but rolls have gone
but recognition of the ‘base’ and ‘height’ (not up by 6p each’. Students may even have posed the
necessarily named as such) and kno wledge of area entire situation themselves. They have to treat this as
are still crucial. a real situation, a real problem for them, and might
use statistical, algebraic, logical or ad hoc methods.
In these first two types of problem, Vergnaud’s
classification of three types of m ultiplicative problems 5 Mathematical problems in which a situation is
(see Paper 4, Understanding relations and their presented and a question posed f or which there is
graphical representation) can be of some help if they no obvious method. This is what a mathematician
are straightforwardly multiplicative. But the second means by ‘problem’ and the expected line of attack
type often calls for application of a standard f ormula is to use the f orms of enquir y and mathematical
which requires factual knowledge about the situation, thinking specific to mathematics. For example:
and understanding the derivation of formulae so that ‘What happens to the relationship betw een the
their components can be recognised. sum of squares of the tw o shor ter sides of
triangles and the square on the longer side if w e
3 Worded contexts in which there is no standard allow the angle between them to var y?’ We leave
relationship to apply, or algorithm to use, but an these kinds of question f or the later section on
answer is expected. Typically these require setting mathematical thinking.
up an equation or f ormula which can then be
applied and calculated. This depends on Learning about students’ solution methods for
understanding the variables and relationships; these elementary word problems has been a major f ocus
might be found using knowledge of the situation, in research on learning mathematics. This research
knowledge of the meaning of oper ations, mental focuses on two stages: translation into mathematical
or graphical imagery. For example, consider the relations, and solution methods 3. A synthesis can be
question, ‘One side of a rectangle is reduced in found in Paper 4, Understanding relations and their
length by 20%, the other side in increased b y 20%; graphical representation. It is not always clear in the
what change takes place in the area?’ The student research whether the aim is to solv e the original
is not told exactly what to do, and has to develop problem, to become better at mathematising
a spatial, algebraic or numerical model of the situations, or to demonstrate that the student can use
situation in order to proceed. She might decide algebra fluently or knows how to apply arithmetic.
14 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

Students have to understand that there will be sev eral account for about a quar ter of the er rors of lower
layers to working with worded problems and cannot achieving students. The initial access to such prob lems
expect to merely read and know immediately what is therefore a separate issue before students have to
to do. Problems in which linguistic str ucture matches anticipate and represent (as Boero (2001) descr ibes
mathematical structure are easier because they onl y the setting-up stage) the mathematics they are going
require fluent replacement of words and numbers by to use. Ballew and Cunningham (1982) with a sample
algebra. For example, analysis according to cognitive of 217 11-year-olds found that reading and
load theory informs us that problems with fewer computational weaknesses were to blame for
words, requiring fewer operations, and where the difficulties alongside interpretation – but they may
linguistic structure matches the mathematical str ucture have underestimated the r ange of problems lurking
closely, are easier for learners to solve algebraically within ‘interpretation’ because they did not probe
(Kintsch, 1986), but this is tautologous as such any fur ther than these two variables and the links
problems are necessarily easier since they avoid the between reading, understanding the relations, and
need for interpretation and translation. Such deciding what to compute were not analysed.
interpretation may or may not be related to Verschaffel, De Cor te and Vierstraete (1999)
mathematical understanding. This research does, researched the problem-solving methods and
however, alert us to the need f or students to learn difficulties experienced by 199 upper pr imary
how to tackle problems which do not tr anslate easily students with nine word problems which combined
– simply knowing what to do with the algebr aic ordinal and cardinal numbers. Questions were
representation is not enough. In Paper 4, carefully varied to require different kinds of
Understanding relations and their gr aphical interpretation. They found, among other
representation, evidence is given that rephrasing the characteristics, that students tended to choose
words to make meaning more clear might hinder operations according to the relative size of the
learning to transform the mathematical numbers in the question and that choice of f ormal
relationships in problems. strategies tended to be er roneous while informal
strategies were more likely to be cor rect.
Students might star t by looking at the numbers Interpretation therefore depends on under standing
involved, thinking about what the var iables are and operations sufficiently to realize where to apply them,
how they relate, or by thinking of the situation and recognizing how variables are related, as well as
what they expect to happen in it. Whether the reading and computational accur acy. Success also
choice of approach is appropr iate depends on involves visualising, imagining, identifying relationships
curriculum aims, and this obser vation will crop up between variables. All these have to be employed
again and again in this chapter. It is illustr ated in the before decisions about calculations can be made .
assumptions behind the work of Bassler, Beers and (This process is descr ibed in detail in Paper 2 for the
Richardson (1975). They compared two approaches case of distinguishing between additive and
to teaching 15-year-olds how to solve verbal multiplicative relations.) Then learners have to know
problems, one more conducive to constructing which variable to choose as the independent var iable,
equations and the other more conduciv e to grasping recognise how to express other var iables in relation
the nature of the problem. Of course, different to it, have a reper toire of knowledge of operations
emphases in teaching led to diff erent outcomes in the and functions to dr aw on, and think to dr aw on
ways students approached word problems. If the aim them. Obviously elementary arithmetical skills are
is for students to constr uct symbolic equations, then crucial, but automatisation of procedures only aids
strategies which involve identification of variables and solution if the str uctural class is properly identified in
relationships and understanding how to express them the first place. Automatisation of techniques can
are the most appropr iate. If the aim is f or learners to hinder solution of problems that are slightly different
solve the problem by whatever method then a more to prior experience because it can lead to o ver-
suitable approach might be for them to imagine the generalisation and misapplication, and attention to
situation and choose from a r ange of representations language and layout cues rather than the str uctural
(graphical, numerical, algebraic, diagrammatic) possibly meaning of the stated problem. For example, if
shifting between them, which can be manipulated to learners have decided that ‘how many…?’ questions
achieve a solution. always indicate a need to use m ultiplication (as in ‘If
five children have seven sweets each, how many do
Clements (1980) and other s have found that with they have in total?’) they may find it hard to answer
elementary students reading and comprehension the question ‘If 13 players drink 10 litres of cola, how
15 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

many should I buy for 22 players?’ because the activity. However, it is too simplistic to sa y ‘applying
answer is not a str aightforward application of facts is easy’. In this study, fur ther analysis suggests
multiplication. The ‘automatic’ association of ‘how that the questions posed may not have been
many’ with a multiplication algorithm, whether it is comparable on a str uctural measure of difficulty,
taught or whether learners have somehow devised it number of variables and operations for example,
for themselves, would lead to misapplication. although comparing ‘level of difficulty’ in different
question-types is not robust.
Learners may not know how and when to br ing
other knowledge into play; they may not have had In a well-replicated result, the APU sample of
enough experience of producing representations to 15-year-olds found area and per imeter problems
think to use them; the problem may offer a equally hard both in abstr act and diagrammatic
representation (e.g. diagram) that does not for them presentations (Foxman et al. 1985). A contextual
have meaning which can match to the situation. If question scored 10% lower than abstract versions.
they cannot see what to do, they may decide to tr y The only presentation that was easier for area was
possible numbers and see what happens. A difficulty ‘find the number of squares in…’ which vir tually tells
with successive approximation is that young learners students to count squares and par ts of squares. In a
often limit themselves to natural numbers, and do teaching context, this indication of method is not
not develop facility with fr actions which appear as a necessarily an over-simplification. Dickson’s study of
result of division, nor with decimals which are students’ interpretation of area (in four schools)
necessary to deal with ‘a little bit more than’ and ‘a showed that, given the square as a measuring unit,
little bit less than’. An area which is well-known to students worked out how to evaluate area and in
teachers but is under-researched is how learners then went on to formalise their methods and even
shift from thinking about onl y about natural numbers devise the rectangle area f ormula themselves (1989).
in trial-and-adjustment situations.
The research findings are therefore inconclusive about
Caldwell and Goldin (1987) extended what was shifts between concrete and abstract approaches
already well-known for primary students into the which can develop in the normal conditions of school
secondary phase, and found that abstract problems mathematics, but the role of pedagogy indicates that
were, as for primary, significantly harder than more might be done to suppor t abstract reasoning
concrete ones for secondary students in gener al, but and hypothesising as important mathematical practices
that the differences in difficulty became smaller for in secondary school.
older students. ‘Concrete problems’ were those
couched in terms of material objects and realistic Hembree’s meta-analysis (1992) of 487 studies of
situations, ‘abstract’ problems were those which problem-solving gives no surprises – the factor s that
contained only abstract objects and/or symbols. They contribute to success are:
analysed the scripts of over 1000 students who took • that problems are fully stated with supportive
a test consisting of 20 prob lems designed along the diagrams
concrete-abstract dimension in addition to some • that students have previous extensive experience in
other variables. Lower secondary students using the representations used
succeeded on 55% of the concrete prob lems and • that they have relevant basic mathematical skills to
43% abstract, whereas higher secondar y students use
succeeded on 69% of concrete and 66% of abstr act. • that teachers who understand problem-solving
Whether the narrowing of the gap is due to methods are better at teaching them
teaching (as Vygotsky might suggest) or natur al • that heuristics might help in lower secondary.
maturation (as some inter pretations of Piaget might
suggest) we do not know. They also found that Hembree’s analysis seems to say that learners get to
problems which required factual knowledge are the answer easiest if there is an ob vious route to
easier than those requir ing hypotheses for secondary solution. While Hembree did a great ser vice in
students, whereas for primary students the reverse producing this meta-analysis, it fails to help with the
appeared to be tr ue. This shift might be due to questions: How can students lear n to create their
adolescents being less inclined to enter imaginar y own representations and choose between them?
situations, or to adolescents knowing more facts, or it How can students lear n to devise new methods to
may be educative due to the emphasis teachers put solve new problems? How can students lear n to act
on factual rather than imaginative mathematical mathematically in situations that are not full y defined?
16 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

How do students get the exper ience that makes diagram which represents the var iables in the situation
them better at problem-solving? An alternative and then, perhaps, solve an equation or answer some
approach is to view problem-solving as far from other related question based on their model4. Thus
clear-cut and instead to see each prob lem as a modelling presents many of the oppor tunities and
situation requiring modelling (see next section). obstacles described under ‘problem-solving’ above but
the emphasis of this section is to f ocus on the
identification of variables and relationships and the
Summary translation of these into representations. Carpenter,
To solve problems posed for pedagogic purposes, Ansell, Franke, Fennema and Weisbeck (1993) show
secondary mathematics learners have to: that even very young children can do far more
• be able to read and understand the problem sophisticated quantitative reasoning when modelling
• know when they are expected to use formal situations for themselves than is expected if w e think
methods of it solely as application of known operations,
• know which methods to apply and in what order because they bring their knowledge of acting in similar
and how to carry them out situations to bear on their reasoning.
• identify variables and relationships, choosing which
variable to treat as independent A typical modelling cycle involves representing a
• apply appropriate knowledge of situations and realistic situation in mathematical symbols and then
operations using isomorphism between the model and the
• use mental, graphical and diagrammatic imagery situation, manipulate variables either in the model or
• choose representations and techniques and know the situation and obser ve how such transformations
how to operate with them re-translate between the model and the situation. This
• know a range of useful facts, operations and duality is encapsulated in the ideas of model-of and
functions model-for. The situation is an instantiation of an abstr act
• decide whether to use statistical, algebraic, logical model. The abstract model becomes a model-for being
or ad hoc methods. used to provide new insights and possibilities f or the
original situation. This isomorphic duality is a more
general version of Vergnaud’s model described in
Modelling Paper 4, Understanding relations and their gr aphical
representation. For learners, the situation can provide
In contrast to ‘problem-solving’ situations in which the insight into possibilities in the mathematics, or the
aim and purpose is often ambiguous, modelling refers mathematics can provide insights into the situation. For
to the process of expressing situations in con ventional example, a graphical model of temperature changes
mathematical representations which afford can afford prediction of future temper atures, while
manipulation and exploration. Typically, learners are actual temperature changes can afford understanding
expected to construct an equation, function or of continuous change as expressed by graphs.

Figure 7.1: Typical modelling cycle with two-way relationship between situation and representations.
17 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Research literature in this area gives primacy to and images of covariation, working mainly with
different features. We are limited to looking at undergraduates. The task is to work out how one
teaching experiments which are necessar ily influenced variable varies in relation to another variable. Their
by par ticular curriculum aims. Either the research findings have implications for younger students,
looks at the lear ning of functions (that is extending because they find that their students can constr uct
the learners’ reper toire of standard functions and and manipulate images of how a dependent var iable
their understanding of their features and proper ties) relates to the independent var iable in dynamic
and sees modelling, interpreting and reifying functions events, such as when variation is positional, or visually
as components of that lear ning (e.g. O’Callaghan, identifiable, or can be seen to increase or decrease
1998), or the research sees skill in the modelling relative to the dependent variable, but the rate at
process as the goal of lear ning and sees knowledge which it changes change is harder to imagine . For
of functions (their types and beha viour) as an our purposes, it is impor tant to know that
essential component of that. In either approach there adolescents can construct images of relationships, but
are similar difficulties. O’Callaghan, (1998) using a O’Callaghan’s work shows that more is required f or
computer-intensive approach, found that while this facility to be used to dev elop knowledge of
students did achieve a better understanding of functions. When distinguishing between linear and
functions through modelling than compar able quadratic functions, for example, rate of change is a
students pursuing a traditional ‘pure’ course, and were useful indicator instead of some par ticular values, the
more motivated and engaged in mathematics, they turning point or symmetr ical points, which may not
were no better at reifying what they had lear nt than be available in the data.
the traditional students. In pre- and post-tests
students were asked to: model a situation using a Looking at situations with a mathematical
function; interpret a function in a realistic situation; perspective is not something that can be directl y
translate between representations; and use and taught as a topic , nor does it arise naturally out of
transform algebraic functions which represent a school mathematical learning. Tanner and Jones
financial situation. Students’ answers improved in all (1994) worked with eight schools introducing
but the last task which required them to understand modelling to their students. Their aim was not to
the role of var iables in the functions and the relation provide a vehicle to learning about functions but to
between the functions. In other words, they were develop modelling skills as a f orm of mathematical
good at modelling but not at knowing more about enquiry. They found that modelling had to be
functions as objects in their o wn right. developed over time so that learners developed a
repertoire of experience of what kinds of things to
MacGregor and Stacey (1993) (281 lo wer-secondary focus on. Trelinski (1983) showed that of 223
students in free response f ormat and 1048 similar graduate maths students only 9 could constr uct
students who completed a multiple-choice item) suitable mathematical models of non-mathematical
show that the relationship between words, situations situations – it was not that they did not kno w the
and making equations is not solely one of tr anslating relevant mathematics, but that they had never been
into symbols and cor rect algebra, rather it involves expected to use it in modelling tasks before. It does
translating what is read into some kind of model not naturally follow that someone who is good at
developed from an existing schema and then mathematics and knows a lot about functions
representing the model – so there are tw o stages at automatically knows how to develop models.
which inappropriate relationships can be introduced,
the mental model and the expressions of that So far we have only talked about what happens when
model. The construction of mental models is learners are asked to produce models of situations.
dependent on: Having a use for the models, such as a problem to
• what learners know of the situation and how they solve, might influence the modelling process. Campbell
imagine it , Collis and Watson (1995) extended the findings of
• how this influences their identification of variables, Kouba’s research (1989) (repor ted in Paper 4) and
and analysed the visual images produced and used by four
• their knowledge of possible ways in which variables groups of 16-year-olds as aids to solving prob lems. The
can vary together. groups were selected to include students who had
high and low scores on a test of vividness of visual
What is it that students can see? Car lson and imagery, and high and low scores on a test of
colleagues (2002) investigated students’ perceptions reasoning about mathematical operations. They were
18 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

then given three problems to solve: one involving development of repertoire and deliberate
drink-driving, one about cutting a painted cube into pedagogy.
smaller cubes and one about three people consuming
a large bag of apples by successively eating 1/3 of what
was left in it. The images they developed differed in Functions
their levels of generality and abstraction, and success
related more to students’ ability to operate logically For learners to engage with secondar y mathematics
rather than to produce images, but even so there was successfully they have to be able to decipher and
a connection between the level of abstraction afforded interpret the stimuli they are offered, and this
by the images, logical operational facility and the use of includes being orientated towards looking for
visually based strategies. For example, graphical relations between quantities, noticing structures,
visualisation was a successful method in the dr ink- identifying change and gener alising patterns of
driving problem, whereas images of three men with behaviour. Kieran (1992) lists these as good
beards sleeping in a hut and eating the apples w ere approaches to early algebra. They also have to
vivid but unhelpful. The creation of useful mathematical know the difference between statistical and algebraic
images needs to be lear nt. In Campbell’s study, representations, such as the difference between a
questions were asked for which a model was needed, bar char t and an algebr aic graph.
so this purpose, other than producing the model itself,
may have influenced the modelling process. Models Understanding what a function is, a mapping that
were both ‘models of ’ and ‘models for’, the former relates values from one space into values in another
being a representation to express str uctures and the space, is not a straightforward matter for learners. In
latter being related to a fur ther purpose (e.g. van den Paper 4, Understanding relations and their gr aphical
Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2003). Other writers have also representation, evidence that the exper ience of
pointed to the positive effects of purpose: Ainley, Pratt transforming between values in the same space is
and Nardi (2001) and Fr iel, Curcio and Bright(2001) all different from transforming between spaces is
found that having a purpose contributes to students’ described, and for this paper we shall move on and
sense-making of graphs. assume that the pur pose of simple additive, scalar
and multiplicative functions is under stood, and the
task is now to understand their nature, a range of
Summary kinds of function, their uses, and the ways in which
• Modelling can be seen as a subclass of problem- they arise and are expressed.
solving methods in which situations are
represented in formal mathematical ways. Whereas in early algebra learners need to shift
from seeing expressions as things to be calculated
• Learners have to draw on knowledge of the to seeing them as expressing str uctures, they then
situation to identify variables and relationships and, have to shift fur ther to seeing functions as relations
through imagery, construct mathematical between expressions, so that functions become
representations which can be manipulated further. mathematical objects in themselves and numerical
‘answers’ are likely to be pair s of related values
• There is some evidence that learners are better at (Yerushalmy and Schwar tz, 1993). Similarly equations
producing models for which they have a fur ther are no longer situations which hide an unkno wn
purpose. number, but expressions of relationships between
• To do this, they have to have a repertoire of two (or more) var iables. They have also to
mathematical representations, functions, and understand the difference between a point-wise
methods of operation on these. view of functional relationships (as expressed b y
tables of values) and a holistic view (reinf orced
• A modelling perspective develops over time and especially by graphs).
through multiple situational experiences, and can
then be applied to given problems – the processes Yerushalmy and Gilead, in a teaching exper iment
are similar to those learners do when faced with with lower-secondary students over a few years
new mathematical concepts to understand. (1999) found that knowledge of a r ange of functions
and the nature of functions was a good basis f or
• Modelling tasks do not necessarily lead to solving algebraic problems, par ticularly those that
improved understanding of functions without the involved rate because a gr aph of a function allows
19 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

rates to be obser ved and compared. Thus functions fore (Goldin, 2002). Graphical representations
and their graphs suppor t the focus on rate that emphasise linearity, roots, symmetry, continuity,
Carlson’s students found difficult in situations and gradient; domain; ordered dataset representations
diagrams. Functions appeared to provide a bridge emphasise discrete covariation and may distract
that turned intractable word problems into students from star ting conditions; algebraic
modelling tasks by conjecturing which functions representations emphasise the structure of relations
might ‘fit’ the situation. However, their students could between variables, and the family of functions to
misapply a functional approach. This seems to be an which a par ticular one might be related. To
example of the well-known phenomenon of over- understand a function fully these have to be
generalizing an approach beyond its appropriate connected and, fur ther, students have to think about
domain of application, and arises from students features which are not so easy to visualiz e but have
paying too much attention to what has recentl y been to be inferred from, or read into, the representation
taught and too little to the situation. by knowing its proper ties, such as growth rate
(Confrey and Smith, 1994; Slavit, 1997). Confrey
Students not only have to learn to think about and Smith used data sets to in vite unit-by-unit
functional relationships (and consider non-linear comparison to focus on rate-of-change, and deduced
relationships as possibilities), which have an input to that rate is different from ratio in the ways that it is
which a function is applied gener ating some specific learnt and understood (1994). Rate depends on
output, but they also need to think about relations understanding the covariation of variables, and being
between relations in which there is no immediate able to conceptualise the action of change , whereas
output, rather a structure which may involve several ratio is the compar ison of quantities.
variables. Halford’s analysis (e.g.1999) closely follows
Inhelder and Piaget’s (1959) theories about the
development of scientific reasoning in adolescence . Summary
He calls these ‘quaternary’ relationships because they To understand the use of functions to describe
often relate four components appearing as two pairs. situations secondary mathematics learners have to:
Thus distributivity is quaternary, as it involves two • distinguish between statistical and algebraic
binary operations; propor tion is quaternary as it representations
involves two ratios. So are r ates of change, in which • extend knowledge of relations to understanding
two variables are compared as they both var y in relations between relations
relation to something else (their functional relation, • extend knowledge of expressions as structures to
or time, for example). This complexity might expressions as objects
contribute to explaining why Carlson and colleagues • extend knowledge of equations as defining
found that students could talk about co variation unknown numbers to equations as expressing
relationships from graphs of situations but not rates relationships between variables
of change. Another reason could be the opacity of • relate pointwise and holistic understandings and
the way rate of change has to be read from gr aphs: representations of functions
distances in two directions have to be selected and • see functions as a new kind of mathematical object
compared to each other, a judgement or calculation • emphasize mathematical meaning to avoid over-
made of their r atio, and then the same process has generalising
to be repeated around other points on the gr aphs • have ways of understanding rate as covariation.
and the ratios compared. White and Mitchelmore
(1996) found that even after explicit instr uction
students could only identify rates of change in simple Mathematical thinking
cases, and in complex cases tr ied to use algebr aic
algorithms (such as a given formula for gradient) In this section we mention mathematical problems –
rather than relate quantities directl y. those that arise in the explor ation of mathematics
rather than problems presented to lear ners for them
One area for research might be to f ind out whether to exercise methods or develop ‘problem-solving’
and how students connect the ‘method of skills. In mathematical problems, learners have to use
differences’, in which r ates of change are calculated mathematical methods of enquir y, some of which are
from tables of values, to graphical gradients. One of also used in word problems and modelling situations,
the problems with understanding functions is that or in learning about new concepts. To learn
each representation brings cer tain features to the mathematics in this context means tw o things:
20 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

to learn to use methods of mathematical enquir y faster at grasping the essence of a mathematical
and to learn mathematical ideas which ar ise in situation and seeing the str ucture through the
such enquiry. particular surface features. They generalised more
easily, omitted intermediate steps of reasoning,
Descriptions of what is entailed in mathematical switched between solution methods quickly, tried to
thinking are based mainly on Polya’s work (1957), get elegant solutions, and were able to reverse trains
in which mathematical thinking is described as a of thought. They remembered relationships and
holistic habit of enquir y in which one might dr aw principles of a problem and its solution r ather than
on any of about 70 tactics to make progress with the details and tended to explain their actions r ather
a mathematical question. For example, the tactics than describe them. Krutetskii’s methods were clinical
include make an analogy, check a result, look for and grounded and dependent on case studies within
contradictions, change the problem, simplify, his sample, nevertheless his work over many years
specialise, use symmetr y, work backwards, and so led him to form the view that such ‘abilities’ were
on. Although some items in Polya’s list appear in educable as well as innate and drew strongl y on
descriptions of problem-solving and modelling natural propensities to reason spatially, perceptually,
tactics, others are more likely to be helpful in purel y computationally, to make verbal analogies, mental
mathematical contexts in which facts, logic, and associations with remembered exper iences, and
known proper ties are more impor tant than merely reasoning. Krutetskii, along with mathematicians
dealing with current data. Cuoco, Goldenberg and reporting their own experiences, observed the need
Mark (1997) have devised a typogr aphy of aspects to mull, that is to leave unsolved questions alone for
of mathematical habits of mind. For example, a while after effortful attempts, to sleep, or do other
mathematicians look at change, look at stability, things, as this often leads to fur ther insights when
enjoy symbolisation, invent, tinker, conjecture, returning to them. This commonly observed
experiment, relate small things to big things, and so phenomenon is studied in neuroscience which is
on. The typography encompasses the per spectives beyond the scope of this paper, but does have
which exper ts bring to bear on mathematics – that implications for pedagogy.
is they bring ideas and relationships to bear on
situations rather than merely use current data and
specific cases. Both of these lists contain dozens of Summary
different ‘things to do’ when faced with mathematics. • Successful mathematics learners engage in
Mason, Bur ton and Stacey (1982) condensed these mathematical thinking in all aspects of classroom
into ‘specialise-generalise; conjecture-convince’ which work. This means, for example, that they see what is
focuses on the shifts betw een specific cases and varying and what is invariant, look for relationships,
general relationships and proper ties, and the curtail or reverse chains of reasoning, switch
reasoning shift between demonstrating and proving. between representations and solution methods,
All of these reflect the processes of mathematical switch between examples and generalities, and
enquiry under taken by experienced mathematicians. strive for elegance.
Whereas in modelling there are clear stages of
work to be done , ‘mathematical thinking’ is not an • Mathematical ‘habits of mind’ draw on abilities or
ordered list of procedures, rather it is a way of perception, reasoning, analogy, and mental
describing a cast of mind that views an y stimulus as association when the objects of study are
an object of mathematical interest, encapsulating mathematical, i.e. spatial, computational, relational,
relationships between relationships, relationships variable, invariant, structural, symbolic.
between proper ties, and the potential f or more
such relationships by var ying variables, parameters • Learners can get better at using typical methods of
and conditions. mathematical enquiry when these are explicitly
developed over time in classrooms.
Krutetskii (1976) conducted clinical inter views with
130 Soviet school children who had been identif ied • It is a commonplace among mathematicians that
as strong mathematicians. He tested them mulling over time aids problem-solving and
qualitatively and quantitatively on a wide r ange of conceptualisation.
mathematical tasks, looked for common factors in
the way they tackled them, and found that those
who are better at mathematics in gener al were
21 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Part 2: What learners do when • obscure the intended mathematical generalisation


faced with complex situations • invite ad hoc rather than formal solution methods
in mathematics • confuse students who are not skilled in deciding
what ‘outside’ knowledge they can bring to the
In this section we collect research findings that situation.
indicate what school students typicall y do when
faced with situations to model, solve, or make Clearly students (and their teacher s) need to be
mathematical sense of. clear about how to distinguish between situations in
which everyday knowledge is, or is not, preferable to
formal knowledge and how these relate. In Boaler’s
Bringing outside knowledge to comparative study of two schools (1997) some
bear on mathematical problems students at the school, in which mathematics was
taught in exploratory ways, were able to recognise
Real-life problems appear to invite solutions which are these differences and decisions. However, it is also
within a ‘human sense’ framework rather than a true that students’ outside knowledge used
mathematical frame (Booth 1981). ‘Wrong’ appropriately might:
approaches can therefore be seen not as er rors, but • enable them to visualise a situation and thus
as expressing a need for enculturation into what does identify variables and relationships
and does not count in mathematical problem-solving. • enable them to exemplify abstract relationships
Cooper and Dunne (2000) show that in tests the as they are manifested in reality
appropriate use of outside knowledge and ways of • enable them to see similar structures in different
reasoning, and when and when not to br ing it into situations, and different structures in similar
play, is easier for socially more advantaged students to situations
understand than less advantaged students who ma y • be engaged to generate practical, rather than
use their outside knowledge inappropriately. This is formal, solutions
also true for students working in languages other • be consciously put aside in order to perform as
than their first, who may only have access to formal mathematically expected.
approaches presented in standard ways. Cooper and
Harries (2002) worked on this problem fur ther and
showed how typical test questions for 11- to 12-year Information processing
olds could be rewr itten in ways which encourage
more of them to reason about the mathematics, In this section we will look at issues about cognitiv e
rather than dive into using handy but inappropriate load, attention, and mental representations. At the
procedures. Vicente, Orrantia and Verschaffel (2007) start of the paper we posed questions about what a
studied over 200 primary school students’ responses learner has to do at f irst when faced with a new
to word problems and found that elaborated situation of any kind. Information processing theories
information about the situation was m uch less and research are helpful b ut there is little research in
effective in improving success than elabor ating the this area within mathematics teaching except in
conceptual information. Wording of questions, as well terms of cognitive load, and as we have said before
as the test environment, is therefore significant in it is not helpful for cognitive load to be minimized if
determining whether students can or can not solv e the aim is to lear n how to work with complex
unfamiliar word problems in appropriate ways. situations. For example, Sweller and Leung-Martin
Contrary to a common assumption that giving (1997) used four experiments to find out what
mathematical problems in some context helps combinations of equations and words were more
learners understand the mathematics, analysis of effective for students to deal successfull y with
learners’ responses in these research studies sho ws equivalent information. Of course, in mathematics
that ‘real-life’ contexts can: learning students have to be able to do both and all
• lead to linguistic confusion kinds of combinations, but the researchers did find
• create artificial problems that do not fit with their that students who had achieved fluency with
experience algebraic manipulations were slowed down by
• be hard to visualise because of unfamiliarity, social having to read text. If the aim is merel y to do
or emotional obstacles algebraic manipulations, then text is an extr a load.
• structure mathematical reasoning in ways which are Automaticity, such as fluency in algebr aic
different from abstract mathematics manipulation, is achievable efficiently if differences
22 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

between practice examples are minimized. in this area shows how learners can be directed
Automaticity also frees up working memory for towards different aspects by manipulating variables
other tasks, but as Freudenthal and others have (Runesson and Mok, 2004; Chik and Lo, 2003).
pointed out, automaticity is not al ways a suitable goal
because it can lead to thoughtless application of Theories of mental representations claim that
methods. We would expect a learner to read text declarative knowledge, procedural knowledge and
carefully if they are to choose methods meaningfull y conceptual knowledge are stored in different ways in
in the context. The information-processing tutors the brain and also dr aw distinctions between
developed in the work of Anderson and his verbatim memory and gist memor y (e.g. Brainerd
colleagues (e.g. 1995) focused mainly on and Reyna, 1993). Such theories are not much help
mathematical techniques and processes, but included with mathematics teaching and lear ning, because
understanding the effects of such processes. We are most mathematical knowledge is a combination of all
not arguing for adopting his methods, but we do three kinds, and in a typical mathematical situation
suggest that information processing has something both verbatim and gist memor y would be employed.
to offer in the achievement of fluency, and the At best, this knowledge reminds us that providing
generation of multiple examples on which the ‘knowledge’ only in verbatim and declarative form
learner can then reflect to under stand the patterns is unlikely to help lear ners become adaptable
generated by mathematical phenomena. mathematical problem-solvers. Learners have to
handle different kinds of representation and kno w
Most of the research on attention in mathematics which different representations represent different
education takes an affective and motivational view, ideas, different aspects of the same ideas, and afford
which is beyond the scope of this paper (see NMAP, different interpretations.
2008). However, there is much that can be done
about attention from a mathematical per spective.
The deliberate use of var iation in examples offered Summary
to students can guide their focus towards par ticular • Learners’ attention to what is offered depends
variables and differences. Learners have to know on variation in examples and experiences.
when to discern par ts or wholes of what is off ered
and which par ts are most cr itical; manipulation of • Learners’ attention can be focused on critical
variables and layouts can help direct attention. What aspects by deliberate variation.
is available to be lear nt differs if different relations
are emphasised by different variations. For example, • Automaticity can be helpful, but can also hinder
students learning about gradients of straight line thought.
functions might be offered exercises as follows:
• If information is only presented as declarative
Gradient exercise 1: find the gradients between each knowledge then learners are unlikely to develop
of the following pairs of points. conceptual understanding, or adaptive reasoning.

(4, 3) and (8, 12) (-2, -1) and (-10, 1) • The form of representation is a critical influence
(7, 4) and (-4, 8) (8, -7) and (11, -1) on interpretation.
(6, -4) and (6, 7) (-5, 2) and (10, 6)
(-5, 2) and (-3, -9) (-6, -9) and (-6, -8)
What learners do naturally that
Gradient exercise 2: obstructs mathematical
understanding
(4, 3) and (8, 12) (4, 3) and (4, 12)
(4, 3) and (7, 12) (4, 3) and (3, 12) Most of the research in secondar y mathematics is
(4, 3) and (6, 12) (4, 3) and (2, 12) about student errors. These are persistent over time,
(4, 3) and (5, 12) (4, 3) and (1, 12) those being found by Ryan and Williams (2007)
being similar to those f ound by APU in the late
In the first type, learners will typically focus on the 1970s. Errors do not autocor rect because of
methods of calculation and dealing with negativ e maturation, experience or assessment. Rather they
numbers; in the second type , learners typically are inherent in the ways learners engage with
gesture to indicate the changes in gr adient. Research mathematics through its formal representations.
23 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Persistence of ‘child-methods’ pervades mathematics same’. Indeed the more er rors a student made the less
at secondary level (Booth, 1981). Whether ‘child- systematic their errors were. This was in a multiple-
methods’ are seen as intuitive, quasi-intuitive, choice context, and we may question the assumption
educated, or as over-generalisations beyond the that students who make a large number of errors in
domain of applicability, the implication for teaching is such contexts are engaging in an y mathematical
that students have to experience, repeatedly, that reasoning. However, they also sampled wr itten
new-to-them formal methods are more widely calculations and justifications and found that errors
applicable and offer more possibilities, and that ear lier which looked as if they might be due to ‘more-more’
ideas have to be extended and, perhaps, abandoned. and ‘same-same’ intuitions were often due to other
If students have to adopt new methods without errors and misconceptions. Zazkis, however (1999),
understanding why they need to abandon ear lier showed that this intuition per sisted when thinking
ones, they are likely to become confused and ev en about how many factors a number might have, large
disaffected, but it is possible to demonstrate this need numbers being assumed to have more factors.
by offering par ticular examples that do not yield to
child-methods. To change naïve conceptualisations is Persistence of confusions between different kinds
harder as the next four ‘persistences’ show. of quantity, counting and measuring
As well as the per sistence of additive approaches
Persistence of additive methods to multiplication, being taught ideas and being
This ‘child method’ is worthy of separate treatment subsequently able to use them are not immediatel y
because it is so per vasive. The negative effects of connected. Vergnaud (1983) explains that the
the persistence of additive methods show up again conceptual field of intensive quantities, those
and again in research. Bednarz and Janvier (1996) expressed as ratio or in ter ms of other units
conducted a teaching exper iment with 135 12- to (see Paper 3, Understanding rational numbers and
13-year-olds before they had any algebra teaching to intensive quantities), and multiplicative relationship
see what they would make of word problems which development continues into adulthood. Nesher and
required several operations: those with multiplicative Sukenik (1991) found that only 10% of students
composition of relationships tur ned out to be m uch used a model based on under standing ratio after
harder than those which involved composing mainly being taught to do so f ormally, and then only for
additive operations. The tendency to use additiv e harder examples.
reasoning is also found in reasoning about r atios and
proportion (Har t, 1981), and in students’ Persistence of the linearity assumption
expectations about relationships between variables Throughout upper primary and secondar y students
and sequential predictions. That it occur s naturally act as if relationships are al ways linear, such as believing
even when students know about a var iety of other that if length is multiplied by m then so is area, or if the
relationships is an example of how intuitive 10th term in a sequence is 32, then the 100th must
understandings persist even when more formal be 320 (De Bock,Verschaffel and Janssens1998; Van
alternatives are available (Fischbein, 1987) Dooren, De Bock, Janssens and Verschaffel, 2004,
2007). Results of a teaching exper iment with 93 upper-
Persistence of more-more, same-same intuitions primary students in the Nether lands showed that,
Research on the interference from intuitive rules gives while linearity is persistent, a non-linear realistic context
varied results. Tirosh and Stavy (1999) found that their did not yield this er ror. Their conclusion was that the
identification of the intuitive rules ‘more-more’ and linguistic structure of word problems might invite
‘same-same’ had a strong predictive power for linearity as a first, flawed response. They also found that
students’ errors and their deduction accords with the a single experience is not enough to change this habit.
general finding that rules which generally work at A related assumption is that functions increase as the
primary level persist. For example, students assume independent variable increases (Kieran, Boileau
that shapes with larger perimeters must have larger and Garancon 1996).Students’ habitual ways of
areas; decimals with more digits m ust be larger than attacking mathematical questions and problems
decimals with fewer digits, and so on. Van Dooren, De also cause problems.
Bok, Weyers and Verschaffel (2004), with a sample of
172 students from upper secondar y found that, Persistence with informal and language-based
contrary to the findings of Tirosh and Stavy, students’ approaches
errors were not in general due to consistent Macgregor and Stacey (1993) tested o ver 1300
application of an intuitive rule of ‘more-more’ ‘same- upper-secondary students in total (in a r ange of
24 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

studies) to see how they mathematised situations. Persistent application of procedures.


They found that students tend first of all to tr y to Students can progress from a manipulativ e approach
express directly from the natur al language of a to algebra to understanding it as a tool f or problem-
situation, focusing on in equalities between quantities. solving over time, but still tend over-rely on
Engaging with the under lying mathematical meaning automatic procedures (Knuth, 2000). Knuth’s sample
is not a natur al response. Students wanted the of 178 first-year undergraduates’ knowledge of the
algebraic expression to be some kind of linguistic relationship between algebraic and graphical
code, rather than a relational expression. Many representations was superficial, and that they reached
researchers claim this is to do with translating word for algebra to do automatised manipulations r ather
order inappropriately into symbols (such as ‘there than use graphical representations, even when the
are six students to each professor, so 6s = p’) but latter were more appropriate.
Macgregor and Stacey suggest that the cause is more
to with inadequate models of m ultiplicative
relationships and ratio. However, it is easy to see that Summary
this is compounded by an unfortunate choice of Learners can create obstacles for themselves by
letters as shor thand for objects rather than as responding to stimuli in particular ways:
variables. For example, Wollman (1983) and Clement • persistence of past methods, child methods, and
(1982) demonstrate that students make this classic application of procedures without meaning
‘professor-student’ error because of haste , failure to • not being able to interpret symbols and other
check that the meaning of the equation matches the representations
meaning of the sentence , over-reliance on linguistic • having limited views of mathematics from their past
structure, use of non-algebr aic symbols (such as p experience
for professor instead of p for number of professors) • confusion between formal and contextual aspects
and other reasons. At least one of these is a • inadequate past experience of a range of examples
processing error which could be resolved with a and meanings
‘read out loud’ strategy for algebra. • over-reliance on visual or linguistic cues, and on
application of procedures
Persistence of qualitative judgements in modelling • persistent assumptions about addition, more-
Lesh and Doerr (2003) repor ted on the modelling more/same-same, linearity, confusions about
methods employed by students who had not had quantities
specific direction in what to do. In the absence of • preferring arithmetical approaches to those based
specific instructions, students repeat patterns of on meaning.
learning that have enabled them to succeed in other
situations over time. They tend to start on each
problem with qualitative judgements based on the What learners do naturally
particular context, then shift to additive reasoning, then that is useful
form relationships by pattern recognition or repeated
addition, and then shift to propor tional and relational Students can be guided to explore situations in a
thinking. At each stage their students resor ted to systematic way, learning how to use a typically
checking their arithmetic if answers conflicted rather mathematical mode of enquir y, although it is hard to
than adapting their reasoning by seeing if answers understand phenomena and change in dynamic
made sense or not. This repetition of naïve strategies situations. Carlson, Jacobs, Coe, Larson and Hsu.
until they break down is inefficient and not what the (2002) and Yerushalmy (e.g. 1997) have presented
most successful mathematics students do. consistent bodies of work about modelling and
covariation activities and their work, with that of Kaput
In modelling and problem-solving students confuse (e.g. 1991), has found that this is not an inherently
formal methods with contextual methods; they cling maturation problem, but that with suitable tools and
strongly to limited prototypes; they over-generalise; representations such as those available in SimCalc
they read left-to-right instead of inter preting the children can learn not only to understand change by
meaning of symbolic expressions. In word problems working with dynamic images and models, but also to
they misread; miscomprehend; make errors in create tools to analyse change. Carlson and her
transformation into operations; errors in processes; colleagues in teaching experiments have developed a
and misinterpret the solution in the problem context framework for describing how students learn about
(see also Ryan and Williams 2007). this kind of co-variation. First they learn how to
25 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

identify variables; then they form an image of how the learn mathematics. In Krutetskii’s study of such
variables simultaneously vary. Next, one variable has to students, to which we referred earlier, (1976) he
be held still while the change in another is observed. found that they exhibited what he called a
This last move is at the core of mathematics and ‘mathematical cast of mind’ which had analytical,
physics, and is essential in constr ucting mathematical geometric, and harmonic (a combination of the tw o)
models of multivariate situations, as Inhelder and aspects. Successful students focused on structure
Piaget also argued more generally. and relationships rather than par ticular numbers of a
situation. A key result is that memor y about past
In these supported situations, students appear to successful mathematical work, and its associated
reason verbally before they can operate symbolically structures, is a stronger indicator of mathematical
(Nathan and Koedinger, 2000). The usual ‘order’ of success than memor y about facts and techniques.
teaching suggested in most cur ricula (arithmetic, He did not find any common aspects in their
algebra, problem-solving) does not match students’ computational ability.
development of competence in which v erbal modes
take precedence5. This fits well with Swafford and Silver (1981) reconstructed Krutetskii’s claim that 67
Langrall’s study of ten 11-year-olds (2000) in which lower-secondary high-achieving mathematics students
it was clear that even without formal teaching about remembered structural information about
algebra, students could identify var iables and ar ticulate mathematics rather than contextual information. He
the features of situations as equations where they asked students to sor t 16 problems into groups that
were familiar with the under lying operations. Students were mathematically-related. They were then given
were asked to work on six tasks in inter views. The two problems to work on and asked to write down
tasks were realistic problems that could be afterwards what they recalled about the prob lems.
represented by direct propor tion, linear relations in The ‘writing down’ task was repeated the next day,
a numerical context, linear relations in a geometr ic and again about four weeks later. There was a
context, arithmetic sequences, exponential relations correlation between success in solving problems and
and inverse proportion. Each task consisted of a tendency to focus on underlying mathematical
subtasks which progressed from str uctured structure in the sor ting task. In addition, students
exploration of the situation, verbal description of who recalled the str ucture of the problems were the
how to find some unknown value, write an equation more successful ones, but others who had
to express this given certain letters to represent performed near average on the problems could talk
variables, and use the equation to f ind out something about them structurally immediately after discussion.
else. The ability to express their verbal descriptions The latter effect did not last in the f our-week recall
as equations was demonstrated across the tasks; task however. Silver showed, by these and other
everyone was able to do at least one of these similar tasks, that structural memory aided transfer of
successfully and most did more than one . The only methods and solutions to new, mathematically similar,
situation for which no one produced an equation was situations. A question arises, whether this is teachable
the exponential one. The study also showed that, given or not, given the results of the f our-week recall.
suitably-structured tasks, students can avoid the usual Given that we know that mathematical str ategies can
assumptions of linearity. This shows some intuitive be taught in gener al (Vos, 1976; Schoenfeld, 1979;
algebraic thinking, and that formal symbolisation can 1982, and others) it seems likely that structural
therefore be introduced as a tool to express awareness might be teachable, however this may
relationships which are already understood from have to be sustained over time and students also
situations. Of course, as with all teaching exper iments, need knowledge of a reper toire of structures to
this finding is specific to the teaching and task and look for.
would not automatically translate to other contexts,
but as well as supporting the teaching of algebr a as We also know something about how students
the way to express generality (see Paper 6 Algebraic identify relationships between variables. While many
reasoning) it contributes to the substantial practical will choose a var iable which has the most
knowledge of the value of star ting with what students connections within the problem as the independent
see and getting them to ar ticulate this as a foundation one, and tended also to start by dealing with the
for learning. largest values, thus showing that they can anticipate
efficiency, there are some who pref er the least value
It is by looking at the capabilities of successful as the star ting point. Nesher, Hershkovitz and
students that we learn more about what it takes to Novotna, (2003) found these tendencies in the
26 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

modelling strategies of 167 teachers and 132 15- how they see it, what they look f or and what they
year-old students in twelve situations which all had notice. Scheme-theory suggests that what they look
three variables and a compar ative multiplication for and notice is related to the ways they have
relationship with an additive constraint. This is a already constructed connections between past
relatively large sample with a high n umber of slightly- mathematical experiences and the concept images
varied situations for such studies and could pro vide a and example spaces they have also constructed and
model for fur ther research, rather than small studies which come to mind in the current situation. Thus
with a few highly varied tasks. generalisations intended by the teacher are not
necessarily what will be noticed and constr ucted by
Whatever the disposition towards identifying students (Steele and Johanning, 2004).
structures, variables and relationships, it is widely
agreed that the more you know, the better equipped
you are to tackle such tasks. Alexander and others Summary
(1997) worked with very young children (26 three- There is evidence to show that, with suitable
to five-year-olds) and found that they could reason environments, tools, images and encour agement,
analogically so long as they had the necessar y learners can and do:
conceptual knowledge of objects and situations to • generalise from what is offered and experienced
recognise possible patterns. Analogical reasoning • look for analogies
appears to be a natural everyday power even for • identify variables
very young children (Holyoak and Thagard, 1995) • choose the most efficient variables, those with most
and it is a valuable source of hypotheses, techniques, connections
and possible translations and transformations. • see simultaneous variations
Construction of analogies appear s to help with • observe and analyse change
transfer, since seeking or constr ucting an analogy • reason verbally before symbolising
requires engagement with str ucture, and it is • develop mental models and other imagery
structure which is then sought in new situations • use past experience
thus enabling methods to be ‘carried’ into new • need knowledge of operations and situations to do
uses. English and Shar ry (1996) provide a good all the above successfully
description of the processes of analogical reasoning: • particularly gifted mathematics students also:
first seeing or working out what relations are • quickly grasp the essence of a problem
entailed in the examples or instances being offered • see structure through surface features
(abductively or inductively), this relational str ucture • switch between solution methods
is extracted and represented as a model, mental, • reverse trains of thought
algebraic, graphical i.e. constructing an analogy in • remembered the relationships and
some familiar, relationally similar form. They observed, principles of a problem
in a small sample , that some students act ‘pseudo • do not necessarily display computational
structurally’ i.e. emphasising syntax hindered them expertise.
seeking and recognising relational mappings. A critical
shift is from focusing on visual or contextual
similarity to structural similarity, and this has to be
supported. Without this, the use of analogies can
become two things to lear n instead of one .

Past experience is also valuable in the inter pretation


of symbols and symbolic expressions, as well as what
attracts their attention and the inter-relation
between the two (Sfard and Linchevski, 1994). In
addition to past exper ience and the effects of layout
and familiarity, there is also a diff erence in readings
made possible by whether the student perceives
a statement to be oper ational (what has to be
calculated), relational (what can be expressed
algebraically) or structural (what can be gener alized).
Generalisation will depend on what students see and
27 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Part 3: What happens with includes: descriptive longitudinal studies of cohor ts


pedagogic intervention of students who have been taught in ways which
designed to address typical encourage mathematical enquir y and proof and
comparative studies between classes taught in
difficulties? through enquiry methods and traditional methods.
Most of these studies f ocus on the development of
We have described what successful and unsuccessful classroom practices and discourse, and how social
learners do when faced with new and complex aspects of the classroom influence the nature of
situations in mathematics. For this section we show mathematical knowledge. Other studies are of
how par ticular kinds of teaching aim to tackle the students being encouraged to use specific
typical problems of teaching at this lev el. This depends mathematical thinking skills, such as exemplification,
on repor ts of teaching exper iments and, as with the conjecturing, and proof of the eff ects of a focus on
different approaches taken to algebra in the ear lier mathematical thinking over time. These focused
paper, they show what it is possible for secondary studies all suffer to some extent from the typical
students to learn in par ticular pedagogic contexts. ‘teaching experiment’ problem of being designed to
encourage X and students then are obser ved to do
It is worth looking at the successes and new X. Research over time would be needed to
difficulties introduced by researchers and developers demonstrate the effects of a focus on mathematical
who have explored ways to influence lear ning thinking on the nature of long-ter m learning.
without exacerbating the difficulties described Longitudinal studies emphasise development of
above. We found broadly five approaches, though mathematical practices, but the value of these is
there are overlaps between them: focusing on assumed so they are outside the scope of this paper .
development of mathematical thinking; task design; However, it is worth mentioning that the CAME
metacognitive strategies; the teaching of heur istics; initiative appeared to influence the dev elopment of
and the use of ICT. analytical and complex thinking both within
mathematics and also in other subjects, evidenced in
national test scores r ather than only in study-specific
Focusing on mathematical thinking tests (Johnson, Adhami and Shayer, 1988; Shayer,
Johnson and Adhami, 1999). In this initiative teachers
Experts and novices see problems differently; and were trained to use mater ials which had been
see different similarities and differences between designed to encourage cycles of investigation:
problems, because experts have a wider repertoire problem familiarity, investigating the problem,
of things to look for, and more exper ience about synthesising outcomes of investigation, abstracting
what is, and is not, worthwhile mathematically. the outcomes, applying this new abstr action to a
Pedagogic intervention is needed to enable all further problem, and so on.
learners to look for underlying structure or
relationships, or to devise subgoals and reflect on the • Students can get better at thinking about and
outcomes of pursuing these as successful students analysing mathematical situations, given suitable
do. In a three-year course for 12- to 15-year-olds, teaching.
Lamon educated learners to understand quantitative
relationships and to mathematise exper ience by
developing the habits of identifying quantities, making Task design
assumptions, describing relationships, representing
relationships and classifying situations (1998). It is Many studies of the complexity of tasks and the
worth emphasising that this development of habits effect of this on solving appear to us to be the
took place over three years, not over a few lessons wrong way round when they state that prob lems
or a few tasks. are easier to solve if the tasks are stated more
simply. For a mathematics cur riculum the purpose
• Students can develop habits of identifying quantities of problem-solving is usually to learn how to
and relationships in situations, given extended mathematise, how to choose methods and
experience. representations, and how to contact big
mathematical ideas – this cannot be achiev ed by
Research which addresses development of simplifying problems so that it is obvious what to
mathematical thinking in school mathematics do to solve them.
28 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

Students who have spent time on complex Recent work by Swan (2006) shows how task
mathematical activity, such as modelling and prob lem- design, based on introducing inf ormation which
solving, are not disadvantaged when they are tested might conflict with students’ current schema and
on procedural questions against students who ha ve which also includes pedagogic design to enab le these
had more preparation for these. This well-known conflicts to be explored collabor atively, can make a
result arises from several studies, such as that of significant difference to learning. Students who had
Thompson and Senk (2001) in connection with the previously been failing in mathematics w ere able to
University of Chicago School Mathematics Project: resolve conflict through discussion with other s in
those given a curriculum based on problems and a matching, sor ting, relating and generating tasks. This
variety of exploratory activities did better on open- led directly to improvements in conceptual
ended and complex, multistage tasks, than understanding in a var iety of traditionally problematic
comparable groups taught in more conventional domains.
ways, and also did just as well on traditional
questions. Senk and Thompson (2003) went on to • Students can sort out conceptual confusions with
collect similar results from eight mathematics teaching others if the tasks encourage them to confront
projects in the United States in which they looked their confusion through contradiction.
specifically for students’ development of ‘basic skills’
alongside problem-solving capabilities. The skills they
looked for at secondar y level included traditional
Metacognitive strategies
areas of difficulty such as fr actions computations and
algebraic competence. Each project evaluated its Success in complex mathematical tasks is associated
findings differently, but overall the result was that with a range of metacognitive orientation and
students did as well or better than compar ative execution decisions, but mostly with deliberate
students in basic mathematical skills at the evaluating the effects of cer tain actions (Stillman and
appropriate level, and were better at applying their Galbraith, 1998). Reflecting on the effects of activity
knowledge in complex situations. Additionally, several (to use Piaget’s ar ticulation) makes sense in the
projects repor ted improved attainment for students mathematics context, because often the ultimate
of previously low attainment or who were ‘at risk’ in goal is to under stand relationships between
some sense. In one case , algebraic manipulation was independent and dependent variables. It makes
not as advanced as a compar ison group taught from sense, therefore, to wonder if teaching these
a traditional textbook but teachers were able to strategies explicitly makes a difference to learning.
make adjustments and restore this in subsequent Kramarski, Mevarech and Arami (2002) showed that
cohorts without returning to a more limited explicitness about metacognitive strategies is
approach. New research applying one of these important in success not onl y in complex authentic
curriculum projects in the United Kingdom is showing tasks but also in quite ordinar y mathematical tasks.
similar findings (Eade and Dickenson, 2006 a; Eade Kramarski (2004) went on to show that explicit
and Dickenson, Hough and Gough, 2006 b). A U.K. metacognitive instruction to small groups provided
research project comparing two similar schools, in them with ways to question their approach to
which the GCSE results of matched samples w ere graphing tasks. They were taught to discuss
compared, also showed that those who were taught interpretations of the problem, predict the outcomes
through complex mathematical activity, solving of using various strategies, and decide if their
problems and enquiring into mathematics, did better answers were reasonable. The groups who had been
than students who were taught more procedur ally taught metacognitive methods engaged in discussions
and from a textbook. The GCSE scripts showed that that were more mathematically focused, and did
the former group was more willing to tackle better on post-tests of gr aph interpretation and
unfamiliar mathematics questions as problems to be construction, than control groups. Discussion
solved, where the latter group tended to not attempt appeared to be a factor in their success. The value
anything they had not been taught explicitl y (Boaler, of metacognitive prompts also appear s to be
1997). Other research also supports these results stronger if students are ask ed to write about their
(Hembree, 1992; Watson and De Geest, 2005). responses; students in a randomized trial tried more
strategies if they were asked to write about them
• Students who spend most of their time on than those who were asked to engage in think-aloud
complex problems can also work out how to strategies (Pugalee, 2004). In both these studies, the
do ‘ordinary’ maths questions. requirement and oppor tunity to express
29 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

metacognitive observations turned out to be we question whether heur istics are useful for solving
important. Kapa (2001) studied 441 students in f our problems. The evidence suggests that they are (e.g.
computer-instruction environments which offered Webb, 1979 found that 13% of var iance among 40
different kinds of metacognitive prompting while students was due to heur istic use), yet we do not
they were working on mathematical questions: know enough about how these help or hinder
during the solution process, during and after the approaches to unfamiliar problems. For example, a
process, after the process, none at all. Those with heuristic which involves planning is no use if the
prompts during the process were more successful, situation is so unfamiliar that the students cannot
and the prompts made more diff erence to those plan. For this situation, a heuristic which involves
with lower previous knowledge than to other s. collecting possible useful knowledge together (e.g.
While this was an ar tificial environment with special ‘What do I know? What do I want?’ Mason, Bur ton
problems to solve, the finding appears to suppor t and Stacey , 1982) may be more useful b ut requires
the view that teaching (in the f orm of metacognitive some initiative and effort and imagination to appl y.
reminders and suppor t) is impor tant and that The ultimate heuristic approach was probably
students with low prior knowledge can do better if Schoenfeld’s (1982) study of seven students in which
encouraged to reflect on and monitor the eff ects of he elaborated heuristics in a multi-layered way, thus
their activity. An alternative to explicit teaching and showing the things one can do while doing
requests to apply metacognitive strategies is to mathematical problem-solving to be fr actal in nature,
incorporate them implicitly into the ways impossible to learn as a list, so that true
mathematics is done in classrooms. While there is mathematical problem-solving is a creative task
research about this, it tends to be in studies involving a mathematical cast of mind (Kr utetskii,
enquiring into whether such habits are adopted b y 1976) and range of mathematical habits of mind
learners or not, rather than whether they lead to (Cuoco, Goldenberg and Mark, 1997) rather than a
better learning of mathematics. list of processes.

• Students can sometimes do better if they are Schoenfeld (1979; 1982) and Vos (1976) found that
helped to use metacognitive strategies. learners taught explicit problem-solving strategies are
likely to use them in new situations compared to
• Use of metacognitive strategies may be enhanced: similar students who are expected to abstr act
in small group discussion; if students are asked to processes for themselves in practice examples. There
write about them; and/or if they are prompted is a clear tension here betw een explicit teaching and
throughout the work. the development of general mathematical awareness.
Heuristics are little use without kno wledge of when,
why and how to use them. What is cer tainly true is
Teaching problem-solving heuristics that if learners perform learnt procedures, then we
do not know if they are acting meaningfull y or not.
The main way in which educator s and researchers Vinner (1997) calls this ‘the cognitive approach fallacy’
have explored the question of ho w students can – assuming that one can anal yse learnt behavioural
get better at problem solving is by constructing procedures as if they are meaningful, when perhaps
descriptions of problem-solving heuristics, teaching they are only imitative or gap-filling processes.
these explicitly, and comparing the test performance
of students who have and have not received this Application of learnt heuristics can be seen as
explicit teaching. In general, they have found that merely procedural if the heur istics do not require
students do learn to apply such heuristics, and any interpretation that draws on mathematical
become better at problem-solving than those who repertoire, example spaces, concept images and so
have not had such teaching (e .g. Lucas, 1974). This on. This means that too close a procedur al approach
should not surprise us. to conceptualization and analysis of mathematical
contexts is merely what Vinner calls ‘pseudo’. There is
A collection of clinical projects in the 1980s (e.g. no ‘problem’ if what is presented can be processed
Kantowski, 1977; Lee, 1982) which appear to sho w by heuristics which are so specific they can be
that students who are taught problem-solving applied like algorithmically. For example, finding
heuristics get better at using them, and those who formulae for typical spatial-numeric sequences (a
use problem-solving heuristics get better at problem common feature of the U.K. curriculum) is often
solving. These results are not entirel y tautologous if taught using the heur istic ‘generate a sequence of
30 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

specific examples and look f or patterns’. No initial take into account the Nether lands research in which
analysis of the situation, its variables, and relevant the outcomes of ‘realistic’ activity are scaffolded
choice of strategy is involved. towards formality. The familiar phrase ‘use of real
world problems’ is vague and can include a range
On the other hand, how are students to lear n how of practices.
to tackle problems if not given ideas about tactics
and strategies? And if they are taught, then it is lik ely The impor tance of the difference in curriculum aims
that some will misapply them as they do an y learnt is illustrated by Huntley, Rasmussen, Villarubi, Sangtog
algorithm. This issue is unresolved, but working with and Fey (2000) who show, along with other studies,
unfamiliar situations and being helped to reflect on that students following a curriculum focusing on
the effects of par ticular choices seem to be useful algebraic problem solving are better at prob lem
ways forwards. solving, especially with suppor t of graphical
calculators, but comparable students who have
There is little research evidence that students taught followed a traditional course did better in a test for
a new topic using problems with the explicit use of which there were no graphical calculators available
taught heuristics learn better, but Lucas (1974) did and were also more fluent at manipulating
this with 30 students lear ning early calculus and they expressions and working algebraically without a
did do significantly better that a ‘normal’ group when context. In the Boaler (1997) study, one school
tested. Learning core curriculum concepts through educated students to take a problem-solving view
problems is under-researched. A recent finding of all mathematical tasks so that what students
reported by Kaminski, Sloutsky and Heckler (2006; ‘transferred’ from one task to another was not
2008) is that lear ning procedurally can give faster knowledge of facts and methods b ut a general
access to underlying structure than working through approach to mathematics. We described earlier how
problems. Our reading of their study suggests that this helped them in examinations.
this is not a rob ust result, since the way they
categorise contextual problems and formal • There is no unique answer to the questions of why
approaches differs from those used by the research and when students can or cannot solve problems –
they seek to refute . it depends on the type of problem, the curriculum
aim, the tools and resources, the experience, and
• Students can apply taught problem-solving what the teacher emphasises.
heuristics, but this is not always helpful in unfamiliar
situations if their learning has been procedural. How can students become more systematic at
identifying variables and applying operations and
One puzzle which arose in the U .S. Task Panel’s inverses to solve problems? One aspect is to be
review of comparative studies of students taught in clear about whether the aim is f or a formal method
different ways (NMAP, 2008) is that those who have of solution or not. Another is experience so that
pursued what is often called a ‘problem-solving heuristics can be used flexibly because of exposure
curriculum’ turn out to be better at tackling to a range of situations in which this has to be done
unfamiliar situations using problem-solving strategies, – not just being given equations to be solved; not
but not better at dealing with ‘simple’ given word just constructing general expressions from
problems. How students can be better at sequences; etc. The value of repeated exper ience
mathematising real world problems and resolving might be what is behind a f inding from Blume and
them, but not better at solving giv en word problems? Schoen (1988) in which 27 14-y ear-old students
This comment conceals three impor tant issues: firstly, who had learnt to programme in Basic were tested
‘word problems’, as we have shown, can be of a against 27 others in their ability to solv e typical
variety of kinds, and the ‘simple’ kinds call on different mathematical word problems in a pen and paper
skills than complex realistic situations; secondly, that environment. Their ability to wr ite equations was
according to the studies repor ted in Senk and no different but their ability to solve problems
Thompson (2003) performance on ‘other aspects’ of systematically and with frequent review was
mathematics such as solving word problems may not significantly stronger for the Basic group. Presumably
have improved, but neither did it decline; thirdly, that the frequent review was an attempt to replicate the
interpretation of these findings as good or bad quick feedback they would get from the computer
depends on curriculum aims6. Fur thermore, the panel activity. However, another Basic study which had
confined its enquiries to the U.S. context and did not broader aims (Hatfield and Kieren, 1972) implied
31 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

that strengths in problem-solving while using Basic United Kingdom, these positive results were also
as a tool were not universal across all kinds of found in the 1980s in the C AN project, with the
mathematics or suitable for all kinds of lear ning goal. added finding that students who could choose which
method to use, paper, calculator or mental, had
A subset of common problem-solving heuristics are better mental skills than other s.
those that relate specifically to modelling, and
modelling can be used as a prob lem-solving strategy. We need to look more closely at why this is, what
Verschaffel and De Cor te (1997) working with 11- normal obstacles to lear ning are overcome by using
year-olds show that rather than seeing modelling-of technology and what other forms of learning are
and modelling-for as two separate kinds of activity, a afforded? Doerr and Zangor (2000) recognized that
combination of the two, getting learners to frame handheld calculators offered speed and facility in
real problems as word problems through modelling, computation, transformation of tasks, data collection
enables learners to do as well as other groups in and analysis, visualisation, switching representations,
both ‘realistic’ mathematical problem-solving and with checking at an individual level but hindered
word problems when compared to other groups. communication between students. Graham and
Their students developed a disposition towards Thomas (2000) achieved significant success using
modelling in all situational problems. graphical calculators in helping students under stand
the idea of var iable. The number of situations,
• Students may understand the modelling process observation of variation, facility for experimentation,
better if they have to construct models of visual display, instant feedback, dynamic
situations which then are used as models for representation and so on contr ibuted to this.
new situations.
• Students who can use available handheld
• Students may solve word problems more easily technology are better at problem solving and have
if they have experience of expressing realistic more positive views of mathematics.
problems as word problems themselves.
We do not know if it is only in interactive computer
environments that school students can dev elop a
Using ICT deep, flexible and applicable knowledge of functions,
but we do know that the affordances of such ICT
Students who are educated to use a vailable environments allow all students access to a wide
handheld technology appear to be better prob lem variety of examples of functions, and gives them
solvers. The availability of such technology removes the exploratory power to see what these mean in
the need to do calculations, gives immediate relation to other representations and to see the
feedback, makes reverse checking less tedious, allows effects on one of changing the other. These
different possibilities to be explored, and gives more possibilities are simply not available within the
support for risk taking. If the pur pose of complex normal school time and place constr aints without
tasks is to show assessors that students can ‘do’ hands-on ICT. For example, Godwin and
calculations than this result is negativ e; if the purpose Beswetherick (2002) used gr aphical software to
is to educate students to deal with non-routine enhance learners’ understanding of quadratic
mathematical situations, then this result is positiv e. functions and point out that the ICT enab les the
learning environment to be str uctured in ways that
Evidence of the positive effects of access to and use draw learners’ attention to key characteristics and
of calculators is provided by Hembree and Dessar t variation. Schwarz and Her shkowitz (1999) find that
(1986) whose meta-analysis of 79 research studies students who have consistent access to such tools
showed conclusively that students who had sustained and tasks develop a strong reper toire of prototypical
access to calculators had better pencil-and-paper functions, but rather than being limited by these can
and problem-solving skills and more positive use these as levers to develop other functions, apply
attitudes to mathematics than those without. The their knowledge in other contexts and lear n about
only years in which this result was not f ound was the attributes of functions as objects in themselv es.
grade 4 in the United States, and we assume that
this is because calculator use ma y make students Software that allows learners to model dynamic
reluctant to learn some algorithmic approaches experiences was developed by Kaput (1999) and the
when this is the main f ocus of the cur riculum. In the integration of a range of physical situations,
32 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

represented through ICT, with mental modelling to construct a continuous graph. Additionally,
encouraged very young students to use algebr a to comparing the real movement, their descriptions of it,
pose questions, model and solve questions. Entering and graphs also enabled them to correct and adjust
algebraic formulae gave them immediate feedback their descriptions. Nemirovsky found that switching
both from graphs and from the representations of between these representations helped them to see
situations. In extended teaching exper iments with that graphs told a continuous story about situations.
upper primary students, Yerushalmy encouraged them Rather than expressing instances of distance at
to think in terms of the events and processes inherent particular times, the students were talking about
in situations. The software she used emphasised speed, an interpretation of rate from the graph rather
change over small inter vals as well as overall shape. than a pointwise use of it. Using a similar approach
This approach helped them to under stand with nine- and ten-year-olds Nemirovsky found that
representations of quantities, relationships among these students were more likely to ‘read’ symbolic
quantities, and relationships among the expressions as relationships between variables rather
representations of quantities in single var iable than merely reading them from left to right as children
functions (Yerushalmy, 1997). Yerushalmy claims that taught traditionally often do.
the shifts between pointwise and holistic views of
functions are more easily made in technological Students at all levels can achieve deep understanding
environments because, perhaps, of the easy availability of concepts and also lear n relevant graphing and
of several examples and feedback showing translation function skills themselves, given the power to see the
between graphs, equations and data sets. She then effects of changes in multiple representations, taking
gave them situations which had more than one input much less time than students taught onl y skills and
variable, for example the cost of car rental which is procedures through pencil-and-paper methods
made up of a daily rate and a mileage r ate. This kind (Heid, 1988; Ainley and her colleagues, e.g. 1994).
of situation is much harder to analyse and represent
than those which have one independent and one • Computer-supported multiple representational
dependent variable. To describe the effects of the first contexts can help students understand and use
variable the second variable has to be invariant, and graphs, variables, functions and the modelling
vice versa. In discussion, a small sample of students process.
tried out relations between various pairs of variables
and decided, for themselves, that two of the variables
were independent and the final cost depended on
both of them. They then tried to draw separate
graphs in which one of the var iables was controlled.
We are not claiming that all students can do this by
themselves, but that these students could do it, is
remarkable. This study suggests that students for
whom the ideas of var iables, functions, graphs and
situations are seen as connected ha ve the skills to
analyse unfamiliar and more complex situations
mathematically. Nemirovsky (1996) suggests another
reason is that students can relate diff erent
representations to understand the stor y the graph is
representing. He undertook a multiple representation
teaching experiment with 15- and 16-year-olds in
which graphs were generated using a toy car and a
motion detector. Having seen the connection between
one kind of movement and the graph, students were
then asked to predict graphs for other movements,
showing how their telling of the stor y of the
movement related to the gr aphs they were drawing.
Students could analyse continuous movement that
varied in speed and direction b y seeing it to be a
sequence of segments, then relate segments of
movement to time, and then integrate the segments
33 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Recommendations problem-solving skills; to understand the issues


within the context better etc.
For curriculum and practice • Students need help and experience to know when
to apply formal, informal or situated methods.
The following recommendations for secondary
mathematics teaching draw on the conclusions • Students need a repertoire of appropriate
summarized above. functions, operations, representations and
mathematical methods in order to become good
Learning new concepts applied mathematicians. This can be gained through
• Teaching should take into account students’ natural multiple experiences over time.
ways of dealing with new perceptual and verbal
information (see summaries above), including those • Student-controlled ICT supports the development
ways that are helpful for new mathematical ideas of knowledge about mathematics and its
and those that obstruct their learning. applications; student-controlled ICT also provides
authentic working methods.
• Schemes of work and assessment should allow
enough time for students to adapt to new
meanings and move on from earlier methods and For policy
conceptualisations; they should give time for new
experiences and mathematical ways of working to • These recommendations indicate a training
become familiar in several representations and requirement based on international research about
contexts before moving on. learning, rather than merely on implementation of a
new curricula.
• Choice of tasks and examples should be
purposeful, and they should be constructed to • There are resource implications about the use of
help students shift towards understanding new ICT. Students need to be in control of switching
variations, relations and properties. Such guidance between representations and comparisons of
includes thinking about learners’ initial perceptions symbolic expression in order to understand the
of the mathematics and the examples offered. syntax and the concept of functions. The United
Students can be guided to focus on critical aspects Kingdom may be lagging behind the developed
by the use of controlled variation, sorting and world in exploring the use of spreadsheets,
matching tasks, and multiple representations. graphing tools, and other software to support
application and authentic use of mathematics.
• Students should be helped to balance the need for
fluency with the need to work with meaning. • The United Kingdom is in the forefront of new
school mathematics curricula which aim to prepare
Applications, problem-solving, modelling, learners better for using mathematics in their
mathematical thinking economic, intellectual and social lives. Uninformed
• As above, teaching should take into account teaching which focuses only on methods and test-
students’ natural ways of dealing with new training is unlikely to achieve these goals.
perceptual and verbal information (see summaries
above), including those ways that are helpful for • Symbolic manipulators, graph plotters and other
new mathematical ideas and those that obstruct algebraic software are widely available and used to
their learning. allow people to focus on meaning, application and
implications. Students should know how to use
• Schemes of work should allow for students to have these and how to incorporate them into
multiple experiences, with multiple representations, mathematical explorations and extended tasks.
over time to develop mathematically appropriate
‘habits of mind’. • A strong message emerging about learning
mathematics at this level is that students need
• The learning aims and purpose of tasks should be multiple experiences over time for new-to-them
clear: whether they are to develop a broader ways of thinking and working to become habitual.
mathematical repertoire; to learn modelling and
34 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

For research Endnotes


• There are few studies focusing on the introduction 1 ‘Induction’ here is the process of devising plausib le
of specific new ideas, based on students’ existing generalisations from several examples, not mathematical
inductive reasoning.
knowledge and experience, at the higher secondary
2 They claimed that the post-test was contextual because objects
level.This would be a valuable research area.This
were used, but the relations between the objects were
relates particularly to topics which combine concepts spurious so the objects functioned as symbols r ather than as
met earlier in new ways, such as: trigonometry, contextual tools.
quadratics and polynomials, and solving simultaneous 3 There a little research on inter preting problems in statistical
equations. (There is substantial research about terms, but this is beyond the scope of this paper.
calculus beyond the scope of this paper.) 4 Modelling has other meanings as well in mathematics
education, such as the provision or creation of visual and tactile
• There are many studies on the development of models of mathematical ideas, but here we are sticking to what
modelling and problem-solving skills, but a valuable mathematicians mean by modelling.

area for research, particularly in the new U.K. 5 Also, as is recognised in the Realistic Mathematics Education
and some other projects, students are able to engage in ad hoc
context at 14–19, would be the relationship
problem solving from a young age.
between these and mathematical conceptual
6 Meta-analysis of the studies they used is bey ond the scope of
development which, as we have shown above, this review.
involves similar – not separate – learning processes
if it is to be more than trial-and-error.

• There is little research which focuses on the


technicalities of good mathematics teaching, and Acknowledgements
it would be valuable to know more about: use of
imagery, the role of visual and verbal presentations, This paper was produced with the help of Nusr at
development of mathematical thinking, Rizvi who did much of the technical work.
development of geometrical reasoning, how
representations commonly used in secondary
mathematics influence learning, and how and why
some students manage to avoid over-generalising
about facts, methods, and approaches.

• There is very little research on statistical reasoning,


non-algebraic modelling, and learning mathematics
with and without symbolic manipulators.
35 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

References

Ainley, J. (1994) Building on children’s intuitions about Bock, D. De; Verschaffel, L.; Janssens, D. (1998) The
line graphs. In J. da Ponte and J. Matos (eds.) Predominance of the Linear Model in Secondar y
Proceedings of the 18 th annual conference of School Students’ Solutions of Word Problems
the International Group for the Psychology of Involving Length and Area of Similar Plane Figures
Mathematics Education vol. 1, pp. 1-8, University Educational Studies in Mathematics 35(1) 65-83
of Lisbon, Portugal. Boero, P. (2001). Transformation and Anticipation as
Ainley, J., Pratt, D., and Nardi, E. (2001). Normalising: Key Processes in Algebraic Problem Solving. In
children’s activity to constr uct meanings for Sutherland, R., Rojano, T., Bell, A. and Lins, R.(eds.)
trend.’ Educational Studies in Mathematics , 45 Perspectives on School Algebra. pp. 99-119
(1-3), 131-146. Dordrecht, Kluwer.
Alexander, P., White, C. and Daugherty, M. (1997) Booth, Lesley R.(1981) Child-Methods in Secondar y
Analogical reasoning and early mathematics Mathematics Educational Studies in Mathematics
learning. Pp. 117-148. In English (ed.) 12(1) 29-41
Mathematical reasoning: analogies, metaphors and Brainerd, C. J., and Reyna, V. F. (1993). Memory
images. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. independence and memory interference in
Anderson, J. R., Corbett, A. T., Koedinger, K., and Pelletier, cognitive development. Psychological Review, 100,
R. (1995). Cognitive tutors: Lessons learned. The 42-67.
Journal of Learning Sciences, 4, 167-207. Caldwell, J., Goldin, G. (1987) Variables Affecting
Ballew, Hunter ; Cunningham, James W. (1982) Word Problem Difficulty in Secondar y School
Diagnosing strengths and weaknesses of sixth- Mathematics Journal for Research in Mathematics
grade students in solving word problems Journal for Education 18(3) 187-196.
Research in Mathematics Education 13(3) 202-210 Campbell, K.; Collis, K.; Watson, J. (1995) Visual
Bassler, O., Beers, M. and Richardson, L. (1975) Processing during Mathematical Problem Solving
Comparison of two instructional strategies for Educational Studies in Mathematics 28(2) 177-194
teaching the solution of verbal problems Journal for Carlson, M., Jacobs, S., Coe, E., Larsen, S. and Hsu, E.
Research in Mathematics Education 6(3)170-177. (2002) Applying Covariational Reasoning While
Bednarz, N. and Janvier, B. (1996). Emergence and Modeling Dynamic Events: A Framework and a
development of algebra as a problem solving Study Journal for Research in Mathematics 33(5)
tool: Continuities and discontinuities with 352-378
arithmetic. In N. Bednarz, C. Kieran and L. Lee, L. Carpenter, T.; Ansell, E.; Franke, M.; Fennema, E.;
(Eds.), Approaches to Algebra (pp. 115-136). Weisbeck, L. (1993) Models of Problem Solving: A
Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Study of Kindergarten Children’s Problem-
Publishers. Solving Processes Journal for Research in
Blume, G. and Schoen, H. (1988).Mathematical Mathematics Education 24(5) 428-441
Problem-Solving Performance of Eighth-Grade Chik, P. and Lo. M. (2003) Simultaneity and the
Programmers and Nonprogrammers Journal for Enacted Object of Lear ning. In F. Mar ton and A.
Research in Mathematics Education 19(2) 142-156 B. M. Tsui (Eds.), Classroom discourse and the space
Boaler, J. (1997) Experiencing School Mathematics: of learning (pp.89-110) Mahwah, New Jersey:
Teaching Styles, Sex and Setting. Open University Lawrence
Press: Buckingham, England
36 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

Clement, J. (1982) Algebra Word Problem Solutions: Friel, S., Curcio, F. and Bright, G. (2001) Making Sense
Thought Processes Underlying a Common of Graphs: Critical Factors Influencing
Misconception Journal for Research in Mathematics Comprehension and Instructional Implications
Education 13(1) 16-30 Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
Clements, M. (1980) Analyzing children’s errors on 32(2), 124-158.
written mathematical tasks. Educational Studies in Godwin,S. and Beswetherick, R. (2002) Reflections
Mathematics, 11 (1), 1-21 on the role of task str ucture in learning the
Confrey, J. and Smith, E. (1994) Exponential properties of quadratic functions with ICT - a
Functions, Rates of Change , and the Multiplicative design initiative. In S. Pope (ed.) Proceedings of the
Unit Educational Studies in Mathematics 26(3) British Society for Research into Lear ning
135-164 Mathematics day conference 16 November,
Cooper, B. and Dunne, M) (2000) Assessing Children’s Nottingham University, British Society for
Mathematical Knowledge: Social Class, Sex and Research into Learning Mathematics. pp. 43-48.
Problem-Solving, Maidenhead: Open University Goldin, G. (2002) Representation in mathematical
Press learning in English, L (ed.) Handbook of
Cooper, B. & Harries, T. (2002) Children’s Responses international research in mathematics education
to Contrasting ‘Realistic’ Mathematics Problems: (pp. 197-218) Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Just How Realistic Are Children Ready to Be? Graham, A. and ; Thomas, M. (2000) Building a
Educational Studies in Mathematics 49(1) 1-23 versatile understanding of algebraic variables with
Cuoco, A., Goldenberg, E. P., and J. Mark. (1997). a graphic calculator Educational Studies in
‘Habits of Mind: an organizing principle for Mathematics 41(3) 265-282
mathematics curriculum.’ Journal of Mathematical Gravemeijer, K. and Doorman, M. (1999) Context
Behavior, 15(4), 375-402. Problems in Realistic Mathematics Education: A
Dickson, L. (1989) Equations. In D. Johnson (ed.) Calculus Course as an Example Educational
Children’s Mathematical Frameworks 8 to 13: a Studies in Mathematics 39(1-3) 111-129
study of classroom teaching. (p.151-190) Windsor: Halford, G. (1999) The Development of Intelligence
NFER-nelson Includes the Capacity to Process Relations of
Doerr, H. and Zangor, R. (2000) Creating meaning f or Greater Complexity.in M. Anderson (ed.) The
and with the gr aphing calculator Educational Development of Intelligence pp. 193-213 Hove:
Studies in Mathematics 41(2) 143-163 Psychology Press
Eade, F. and Dickinson, P. (2006a) Exploring Realistic Hart, K. (Ed.) (1981) Children’s Understanding of
Mathematics Education in English Secondar y Mathematics 11-16, Murray, London..
Schools. Proceedings PME July 2006 Hatfield, L. and Kieren, T. (1972) Computer-Assisted
Eade, F., Dickinson, P., Hough S. and Gough S (2006b) Problem Solving in School Mathematics Journal for
Developing Mathematics through Contexts: year 2. Research in Mathematics Education 3(2) 99-112
Annual Repor t September 2006 Heid, M. (1988) Resequencing Skills and Concepts in
www.partnership.mmu.ac.uk/cme Applied Calculus Using the Computer as a Tool
English, L. and Halford, G. (1995) Mathematics Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
Education: Models and Processes. Mahwah, NJ: 19(1) 3-25
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Hembree, Ray (1992) Experiments and Relational
English, L. and Sharry, P. (1996) Analogical Reasoning Studies in Problem Solving: A Meta-Analysis
and the development of algebraic abstraction. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
Educational Studies in Mathematics, Vol. 30, No. 23(3) 242-273
2.135-157. Hembree, Ray; Dessar t, Donald J. (1986) Effects of
Fischbein, E. (1987) Intuition in science and Hand-Held Calculators in Precollege Mathematics
mathematics : an educational approach. Dordrecht; Education: A Meta-Analysis Journal for Research in
D. Reidel Mathematics Education 17(2) 83-99
Foxman, D., Ruddock, G., Joffe, L., Mason, K., Mitchell, Hershkowitz, R., Schwarz, B.and Dreyfus, T. (2001)
P., and Sexton, B. (1985) A Review of Monitoring in Abstraction in Context: Epistemic Actions. Journal
mathematics 1978 to 1982, Assessment of for Research in Mathematics Education , 32 (2)
Performance Unit, Depar tment of Education and 195-222
Science, London.
Freudenthal, H. (1991) Didactical Phenomenology of
Mathematical Structures, Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
37 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. van den (2003) The Didactical Kieran, C., Boileau, A.and Garancon, M. (1996)
Use of Models in Realistic Mathematics Introducing algebra by means of a technology-
Education: An Example from a Longitudinal supported, functional approach. In Bednarz, N.,
Trajectory on Percentage Educational Studies in Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.) Approaches to algebra:
Mathematics 54(1) 9-35 perspectives for research on teaching. Pp. 257-294
Holyoak, K. and Thagard, P. (1995) Mental Leaps: Kluwer, Dordrecht
Analogy in Creative Thought. Cambridge, MA: Kintsch, W. (1986) Learning from text. Cognition and
Bradford Book, MIT Press Instruction 3(2).
Huntley, M., Rasmussen, C., Villarubi, R., Sangtong, J. Knuth, E. J. (2000). Student understanding of the
and Fey, J. (2000) Effects of Standards-Based cartesian connection: An exploratory study.
Mathematics Education: A Study of the Core-plus Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
Mathematics Project Algebra and Functions 31(4), 500-508.
Strand Journal for Research in Mathematics Kouba, V. L. (1989). Children’s solution strategies for
Education 31(3) 328-361 equivalent set multiplication and division
Inhelder, B. & Piaget, J. (1959). The Growth of Logical problems. Journal for Research in Mathematics
Thinking From Childhood to Adolescence. NY: Basic Education, 20, 147-158.
Books Kramarski, B. (2004) Making sense of graphs: Does
Johnson, D.C., Adhami, M. and Shayer, M. (1998). metacognitive instruction make a difference on
Does CAME work? Summary repor t on phase 2 students’ mathematical conceptions and
of the Cognitive Acceleration in Mathematics alternative conceptions? Learning and Instruction
Education, CAME, project. In Proceedings of the 14, 593-619.
British Society for Research into Lear ning Kramarski, B., Mevarech, Z. and Arami, M. (2002) The
Mathematics Day Conference, Bristol, 15 Nov. 1997. Effects of Metacognitive Instruction on Solving
London: BSRLM, 26-31. Mathematical Authentic Tasks Educational Studies
Kaminski, J., Sloutsky, V. and Heckler, A. (2006) Do in Mathematics 49(2) 225-250
Children Need Concrete Instantiations to Lear n Krutetskii, j, V. (1976). The psychology of mathematical
an Abstract Concept? Proceedings of the XXVIII abilities in school children. Chicago: University of
Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Chicago Press.
1167-1172. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Lamon, S. (1998) Algebra: Meaning through
Kaminski, J., Sloutsky, V. and Heckler, A. (2008) The modeling. In A. Oliver and K. Newstead (eds.)
Advantage of Abstract Examples in Learning Proceedings of the 22 nd Conference of the
Math Science 320 (454-455). International Group for the Psychology of
Kantowski, M. (1977) Processes Involved in Mathematics Education, vol. 3, pp.167-174,
Mathematical Problem Solving Journal for Research University of Stellenbosch: Stellenbosch.
in Mathematics Education 8(3) 163-180 Lee, K. (1982) Four th Graders’ Heuristic Problem-
Kapa, E. (2001) A Metacognitive Support during the Solving Behavior Journal for Research in
Process of Problem Solving in a Computerized Mathematics Education 13(2) 110-123
Environment Educational Studies in Mathematics Lesh, R. and Doerr, H. (2003). Foundations of a
47(3) 317-336 models and modelling per spective on
Kaput, J. (1999) Teaching and Learning a New Algebra mathematics teaching, learning and problem
With Understanding Dartmouth: University of solving. In R. Lesh and H. Doerr (Eds.) Beyond
Massachusetts. constructivisim: A models and modeling per spectives
Kaput, J. J. (1991). Notations and representations as on mathematics problem solving, learning and
mediators of constructive processes. In E. von teaching.. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum.
Glasersfeld (Ed.), Radical Constructivism in Lucas, J. (1974) The Teaching of Heuristic Problem-
Mathematics Education (pp. 53-74). Netherlands: Solving Strategies in Elementar y Calculus Journal
Kluwer Academic Publishers. for Research in Mathematics Education 5(1) 36-46.
Kieran, C. (1992). The learning and teaching of MacGregor, M. and Stacey, K. (1993) Cognitive
algebra, in D.A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of Models Underlying Students’ Formulation of
Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning, Simple Linear Equations Journal for Research in
Macmillan, New York, p390-419. Mathematics Education, Vol. 24, No. 3.,217-232.
Mason, J. Bur ton L. and Stacey K. (1982) Thinking
Mathematically, Addison Wesley, London.
38 PaperSUMMARY
7: Modelling,
– PAPER
problem-solving
2: Understanding
and integrating
whole numbers
concepts

Nathan, M. and Koedinger, K. (2000) Teachers’ and Sfard, A. (1991). On the dual nature of mathematical
Researchers’ Beliefs about the Development of conceptions: reflections on processes and objects
Algebraic Reasoning Journal for Research in as different sides of the same coin. Educational
Mathematics Education 168-190 Studies in Mathematics, 22, 1-36.
Nemirovsky, R. (1996) Mathematical nar ratives. In Sfard, A. and Linchevski, L. (1994) The Gains and the
N. Bednarz, C. Kieran and L. Lee, L. (Eds.), Pitfalls of Reification: The Case of Algebra
Approaches to Algebra (pp.197-223) Educational Studies in Mathematics 26(2-3) 191-228
Dordrecht/Boston/London: Kluwer Academic Shayer, M., Johnson, D.C. and Adhami, M. (1999).
Publishers. Does CAME work (2)? Report on Key Stage 3
Nesher, P. and M. Sukenik (1991). The Effect of results following the use of the Cognitive
Formal Representation on the Lear ning of Ratio Acceleration in Mathematics Education, CAME,
Concepts. Learning and Instruction 1: 161 - 175. project in Year 7 and 8. In Proceedings of the
Nesher, P., Hershkovitz, S. and Novotna, Jarmila British Society for Research into Learning
(2003) Situation Model, Text Base and What Else? Mathematics Day Conference, St. Martin’s College,
Factors Affecting Problem Solving Educational Lancaster, 5 June London: BSRLM, 79-84.
Studies in Mathematics 52(2) 151-176 Silver, E. (1981) Recall of Mathematical Problem
NMAP, National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008) Information: Solving Related Problems Journal for
downloaded April 2008 from: Research in Mathematics Education 12(1) 54-64
http://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel Slavit, D. (1997) An Alternate Route to the
/index.html Reification of Function Educational Studies in
O’Callaghan, B. (1998) Computer-Intensive Algebra Mathematics 33(3) 259-281
and Students’ Conceptual Knowledge of Squire, S., Davies, C. and Bryant, P. (2004) Does the
Functions Journal for Research in Mathematics cue help? Children’s understanding of multiplicative
Education 29(1) 21-40 concepts in different problem contexts British
Polya, G. (1957) How to Solve It, 2nd ed., Princeton Journal of Educational Psychology, 515-532
University Press. Steele, D. and Johanning, D. (2004) A Schematic-
Pugalee, D. (2004) A Comparison of Verbal and Theoretic View of Problem Solving and
Written Descriptions of Students’ Problem Development of Algebraic Thinking Educational
Solving Processes Educational Studies in Studies in Mathematics 57(1) 65-90
Mathematics 55(1) 27-47 Stillman, G. and Galbraith, P. (1998) Applying
Runesson, U., and Mok, I. A. C. (2004). Discernment Mathematics with Real World Connections:
and the Question, ‘What can be learned?’ In F. Metacognitive Characteristics of Secondary
Marton and A. B. M. Tsui (Eds.), Classroom Students Educational Studies in Mathematics
discourse and the space of lear ning (pp. 63-87). 36(2) 157-195
Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Swafford, J. and Langrall, C. (2000).Preinstructional
Associates, Inc., Publishers. Use of Equations to Describe and Represent
Ryan, J. and Williams, J. (2007) Children’s Mathematics Problem Situations Journal for Research in
4 -15: learning from errors and misconceptions Mathematics Education 31(1) 89-112
Maidenhead Open University Press. Swan, M. (2006) Collaborative Learning in
Schoenfeld, A. (1979) Explicit Heuristic Training as a Mathematics: A Challenge to Our Beliefs and
Variable in Problem-Solving Performance Journal for Practices London: National Institute of Adult
Research in Mathematics Education 10(3) 173-187. Continuing Education.
Schoenfeld, A. (1982) Measures of Problem-Solving Sweller, J. and Leung-Mar tin, L. (1997) Learning from
Performance and of Problem-Solving Instruction. equations or words.
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 13 Instructional Science, 25(1) 37-70.
(1) 31-49 Tall, D. O., (2004), Thinking through three worlds of
Schwarz, B., Hershkowitz, R> (1999) Prototypes: mathematics, Proceedings of the 28th Conference
Brakes or Levers in Learning the Function of the International Group for the Psychology of
Concept? The Role of Computer Tools Journal for Mathematics Education, Bergen, Norway.
Research in Mathematics Education 30(4) 362-389 Tall, D.O. & Vinner S. (1981), Concept image and
Senk, S. L. & Thompson, D. R. (2003). Standards-based concept definition in mathematics, with special
school mathematics curricula: What are they? What reference to limits and contin uity, Educational
do students learn? Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Studies in Mathematics, 12, 151-169.
39 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Tanner, H. and Jones, S. (1994) Using Peer and Self- Vos, K. (1976) The Effects of Three Instructional
Assessment to Develop Modelling Skills with Strategies on Problem-Solving Behaviors in
Students Aged 11 to 16: A Socio-Constructive Secondary School Mathematics Journal for
View Educational Studies in Mathematics 27(4) Research in Mathematics Education 7(5) 264-275
413-431 Watson, A. & De Geest, E. (2005) Principled teaching
Thompson, D. and Senk, S. (2001) The Effects of for deep progress: improving mathematical learning
Curriculum on Achievement in Second-Year beyond methods and materials. Educational Studies
Algebra: The Example of the University of in Mathematics, 58 (2), 209-234.
Chicago School Mathematics Project Journal for Webb, N. (1979) Processes, Conceptual Knowledge,
Research in Mathematics Education 32(1) 58-84 and Mathematical Problem-Solving Ability Journal
Tirosh, D. and Stavy, R. (1999) Intuitive rules: A way for Research in Mathematics Education 10(2) 83-93.
to explain and predict students’ reasoning White, P. and Mitchelmore, M. (1996) Conceptual
Educational Studies in Mathematics 38(1-3) 51-66 Knowledge in Introductory Calculus Journal for
Treffers, A. (1987) Three dimensions. A model of goal Research in Mathematics Education 27(1) 79-95.
and theor y description in mathematics instr uction – Wollman, W. (1983) Determining the Sources of
The Wiskobas project. Dordrecht: Reidel Error in a Translation from Sentence to Equation
Trelinski, G (1983) Spontaneous mathematization of Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
rsituations outside mathematics Educational 14(3) 169-181
Studies in Mathematics 14(3) 275-284 Yerushalmy, M. (1997) Designing representations:
Trzcieniecka-Schneider, I. (1993) Some remarks on Reasoning about functions of two variables.
creating mathematical concepts Educational Journal for Research in Mathematics Education 28(4
Studies in Mathematics 24(3) 257-264. ), 431-466
Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., Janssens, D. and Yerushalmy, M. and Gilead, S. (1999) Structures of
Verschaffel, L. (2007) Pupils’ over-reliance on Constant Rate Word Problems: A Functional
linearity: A scholastic effect? British Journal of Approach Analysis Educational Studies in
Educational Psychology, 77 (2) 307-321. Mathematics 39(1-3) 185-203
Van Dooren, W.; De Bock, D.,Weyers, D. and Yerushalmy, M.. and Schwartz, J. (1993), Seizing the
Verschaffel, L. (2004) The predictive power of Opportunity to Make Algebra Mathematically and
intuitive rules: A critical analysis of the impact of Pedagogically Interesting, in T. Romberg, E.
‘More A-More B’ and ‘Same A-Same B’ Educational Fennema and T. Carpenter, (Eds.), Integrating
Studies in Mathematics 56(2/3) 179-207 Research on the Graphical Representation of
Vergnaud, G. (1983). Multiplicative structures. In R. Functions, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale,
Lesh and M. Landau (Eds.), Acquisition of New Jersey.
mathematics concepts and processes. New York: Zazkis, R. (1999).Intuitive rules in number theory:
Academic Press. example of the more of A, the more of B r ule
Verschaffel, L. and De Cor te, E. (1997) Teaching implementation. Educational Studies in
Realistic Mathematical Modeling in the Mathematics, 40(2)197-209
Elementary School: A Teaching Experiment with
Fifth Graders Journal for Research in Mathematics
Education 28(5) 577-601
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E. and Vierstraete, H. (1999)
Upper Elementary School Pupils’ Difficulties in
Modeling and Solving Nonstandard Additive
Word Problems Involving Ordinal Numbers
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
30(3) 265-285
Vicente, S., Orrantia, J. and Verschaffel, L. (2007)
Influence of situational and conceptual rewording
on word problem solving British Journal of
Educational Psychology, 77(4) 829-848
Vinner, S. (1997) The Pseudo-Conceptual and the
Pseudo-Analytical Thought Processes in
Mathematics Learning Educational Studies in
Mathematics 34(2) 97-129
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-74-0

www.nuffieldfoundation.org
8
Key understandings in
mathematics learning
Paper 8: Methodological appendix
By Terezinha Nunes, Peter Bryant, and Anne Watson, University of Oxford

A review commissioned by the Nuffield Foundation


2 PaperSUMMARY
8: Methodological
– PAPER appendix
2: Understanding whole numbers

About this review

In 2007, the Nuffield Foundation commissioned a Contents


team from the University of Oxford to review the
available research literature on how children learn Appendix to Papers 1 to 7 2
mathematics. The resulting review is presented in a
series of eight paper s: List of journals consulted for Papers 2 to 5 6

Paper 1: Overview List of journals consulted for Papers 6 and 7 6


Paper 2: Understanding extensive quantities and
whole numbers Reviews and collections used f or algebra 6
Paper 3: Understanding rational numbers and
intensive quantities Large-scale studies used for Papers 6 and 7 6
Paper 4: Understanding relations and their graphical
representation References for Appendix 7
Paper 5: Understanding space and its representation
in mathematics
Paper 6: Algebraic reasoning About the authors
Paper 7: Modelling, problem-solving and integrating Terezinha Nunes is Professor of Educational
concepts Studies at the University of Oxford.
Paper 8: Methodological appendix Peter Bryant is Senior Research Fellow in the
Department of Education, University of Oxford.
Papers 2 to 5 focus mainly on mathematics relevant Anne Watson is Professor of Mathematics
to primary schools (pupils to age 11 y ears), while Education at the University of Oxford.
papers 6 and 7 consider aspects of mathematics
in secondary schools.
About the Nuffield Foundation
Paper 1 includes a summar y of the review, which The Nuffield Foundation is an endowed
has been published separately as Introduction and charitable trust established in 1943 by William
summary of findings. Morris (Lord Nuffield), the founder of Morris
Motors, with the aim of advancing social w ell
Summaries of papers 1-7 have been published being. We fund research and pr actical
together as Summary papers. experiment and the development of capacity
to under take them; working across education,
All publications are available to download from science, social science and social policy. While
our website, www.nuffieldfoundation.org most of the Foundation’s expenditure is on
responsive grant programmes we also
undertake our own initiatives.
3 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Appendix to
papers 1 to 7

This review was conceived as standing between a starts with a much broader question cannot use the
research synthesis and a theoretical review. ‘Research same conception of how the literature search will
syntheses focus on empirical studies and seek to be carried out. The variables to be analysed are not
summarize past research by drawing overall conceived from the start and one of the aims of
conclusions from many separate investigations that addressing such a broad question is in fact to clar ify
address related or identical hypotheses. The research how mathematics learning could be conceptualised.
synthesis hopes to present the state of kno wledge
concerning the relation(s) of interest and to highlight Theoretical syntheses have broader aims, which are
important issues that research has left unresolved’ in some ways similar to the aims adopted in this
(Cooper, 1998, p. 3). In a theoretical review, the aim synthesis, but the current conception of theoretical
is to present theor ies offered to explain a par ticular syntheses can only be par tially adopted in this review.
phenomenon and to compare them in breadth, Although there are occasionally alternative views of
internal consistency, and the empir ical suppor t that how a par ticular aspect of children’s mathematics
they find in empirical studies. ‘Theoretical reviews will learning can be explained, the notion of cr itical
typically contain descriptions of critical experiments experiments to assess which theor y is more powerful
already conducted or suggested, assessments of cannot easily be met when we try to understand
which theory is most powerful and consistent with how children learn mathematics. The very conception
known relations, and sometimes reformulations or of what it is that one is tr ying to explain var ies even
interactions or both of abstr act notions from when the same words are used to descr ibe the focus
different theories.’ (Cooper, 1998, p. 4). of the research. In the second paper in this review ,
we try to show exactly this. There are two alternative
It was quite clear to us that a review that aims theories about children’s understanding of number in
to answer the question ‘how children learn developmental psychology but the phenomenon that
mathematics, ages 5 to 16’ could not be treated as they are tr ying to explain is not the same: Piaget’s
a straightforward research synthesis. The aim of a theory focuses on children’s understanding of
research synthesis is usually more restricted than this. relations between quantities and Gelman’s theory
For example, a research synthesis in education might on children’s counting skills. For older children, the
try to examine the effect of one var iable on another problem becomes even more complex because
(e.g. the effect of reading aloud on children’ s literacy there are alternative views of the nature and content
learning; Blok,1999; Bus, van IJzendoorn, and of mathematical learning, and the role of pedagogy
Pellegrini, 1995) or the conditions under which a makes the notion of cr itical experiment either
particular educational practice can be said to w ork impossible or inapplicable. This is true of all research
(e.g. the effect of phonological or mor phological into secondary mathematics and reflects a change
instruction on literacy learning; Bus, and van from seeing mathematics as the f ormalisation and
IJzendoorn, (1999); Ehri, Nunes, Stahl, and Willows, extension of children’s quantitative and spatial
2001; Reed, 2008). Such searches star t from development to seeing lear ning mathematics as
previously defined variables, the incorporation of coming to understand abstract tools which can
which in a study can easil y be identified in a search provide new formal and analytical perspectives on
through the literature. A review of the literature that the world.
4 PaperSUMMARY
8: Methodological
– PAPER appendix
2: Understanding whole numbers

We did not approach this synthesis as a systematic Exclusion criteria


review but as an attempt to summar ise and develop
some of the main ideas that are par t of research 1 There are domains of research, such as histor y of
and theory about how children learn mathematics. mathematics, mathematics teacher development,
Within this perspective, we defined some inclusion neuropsychological studies of adults with br ain
and exclusion criteria from the outset. damage who have developed mathematics
difficulties, and studies of mathematical abilities in
animals and infants, which have not been so far
connected to a theor y of how children learn
Inclusion criteria
mathematics between 5 and 16 years. These
1 Theoretical explanations regarding how children domains of research are excluded.
learn mathematics which have been suppor ted
by research. There are theoretical explanations 2 Research that focused on learning how to use
in the domain of mathematics lear ning which specific technologies rather than on how
were proposed without their author s providing technologies are used by students to lear n
systematic empirical evidence. We did not consider mathematics. There is a relatively large number
these latter theories in the review except as of publications on how students learn to use
frameworks to structure the approach in the particular tools that are relevant to mathematics
absence of other explanations. (e.g. calculators, number line, spreadsheets, LOGO
and Cabri). Considering our aim of under standing
2 Research about children’s mathematics learning in how children learn mathematics, we will only refer
the age range 5 to 11 was considered when it to research that uses these tools when the f ocus is
focused on the four domains defined as the focus on mathematics learning (e.g. using spreadsheets to
of this research: children’s understanding of natural help students understand the concept of variable).
and rational numbers, relations between quantities
and functions, and space and its representation. We did not use methodological cr iteria in the choice
These were considered the cor nerstones for of papers. Descriptive as well as experimental
further mathematics learning in the domains research, qualitative or quantitative studies were
of algebra, modelling and applications to higher considered when we went through the search. In
mathematical concepts; the focus of these two view of the brevity of the period dedicated to this
papers was on students aged 12 to 16. For algebra synthesis, we did exclude mater ials that could neither
the available research on learning focuses on be obtained by electronic means or in the libr aries
identifying typical errors, hence showing critical of the University of Oxford. There is, therefore, a bias
aspects of successful lear ning but not how that towards papers published in English language
learning might take place. Fur ther than this we journals, even though we could have read
looked at teaching exper iments showing how publications in three other languages.
students respond to different pedagogical
approaches designed to overcome these typical The search process was systematic . We used the
difficulties. For modelling we intended to follow British Educational Index as a star ting point for the
a similar approach but little was available except search of papers in the four chapters about children
small-scale teaching experiments. in the age r ange 5 to 11. Three searches were
carried out, one for natural and rational numbers,
3 Research published in books and book chapters, one for geometry and one for understanding
journals and refereed conference proceedings relations and functions. We included in these
which aim at under standing how children learn searches three sets of k ey-words, the first defining
mathematics. Considering the constraints of the domain of research (mathematics education and
time, the search in jour nals was limited to those other key words from the thesaur us), the second
available electronically and otherwise in the defining the topic area (e .g. natural number, rational
University of Oxford. A list of journals and their number and other options from the thesaur us), and
aims and scope is appended. The refereed the third defining the age par ameters (through
conference proceedings of the Inter national Group schooling levels). Theses and one-page abstr acts
for the Study of the Psychology of Mathematics were excluded from the output list of ref erences
Education will be the onl y proceedings included in at this point. The references were then checked
the review. for availability and to see whether they repor ted
5 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

research results and excluded if they were not algebraic procedures in familiar situations unless
available or did not repor t any research results. We this was linked specifically to the development of
repeated this search process using Psych-inf o, a data algebraic reasoning. Most of the studies w e used
base which includes psychological research, which base their claims to success on the complementar y
had been poorly represented in the previous data needs both to act fluentl y with symbolic expressions
base. Finally, this initial search was complemented b y and to understand them. We accessed 174 paper s
a journal by journal search of the titles listed at the plus 78 references in books in addition to the
end of this note . This search seemed to yield mostl y reviews and studies mentioned above. Of these,
repeated references so we considered this the end about 95 were read but not all are included as
of the process of search. We also consulted books reference. Some of these overlapped in their
and book chapters of works that are recognised in conclusions, or added nothing or only a little to the
the literature and previous syntheses presented in main references.
the Handbook of Research on Mathematics Teaching
and Learning. Two the Task Group Repor ts of the For Paper 7, Modelling, problem-solving and
National Mathematics Advisory Panel, USA, were integrating concepts, an initial search using U.S. and
also consulted: the repor ts on learning processes U.K. spellings gave very few relevant results. We
and on conceptual knowledge. These were used therefore broadened the search to include:
as sources of references rather than for their modelling, problem-solving, realistic, real-life, variable
conclusions. In the end, approximately 200 papers and word problems. This process was iter ative as
were downloaded and read by the authors. the search for explanations for what could be
However, not all of these paper s are cited in the inferred about students’ learning led us into other
chapters. The references used are those which did related areas. Later we did fur ther searches on
contribute to the development of the concepts and some other terms which emerged as impor tant:
empirical results used in the synthesis. linearity, linear assumption, equation. Finally, we
searched for papers which addressed how students
For algebra, we conducted a systematic search in learned combinations of concepts which b uild on
electronic journals in English for refereed research elementary concepts, such as tr igonometry. In all
articles using algebra as the keyword. Journals are we located over 3200 references using British
listed below. We did not define an age r ange since Education Index, ERIC and other sources.
we were interested in how algebraic understanding Fortunately many of these were not research-
develops throughout school, although this happens based, or used the ter ms in irrelevant ways, or
mainly in secondar y education. We also used addressed the focus in limited ways related to
refereed innovation studies, which show what it is young children. The final relevant list consisted of
possible for learners to do, given par ticular kinds of 125 papers and a journal special issue. We used
teaching or technology; this tells us about these papers to point to other sources. Most of
possibilities. We restricted our use of these to studies these papers were repor ts of teaching experiments.
for which the lear ning aims clearly relate to a broad Teaching experiments usually have a par ticular
view of algebra given above. For example, we did commitment to the nature of an aspect of
not include self-referential studies in which, for mathematics and how it is best lear nt. The
example, it is assumed that patter n-spotting is an experiment is constr ucted to see if students will
important aspect of algebr a, so teaching and lear ning be able to do X in cer tain circumstances, and X
pattern-spotting is researched, but we would for is measured as an outcome b ut in this process
example include a study of teaching patter n-spotting knowledge of how X is lear nt, and what can go
where students’ ability to use pattern-spotting for a wrong, can be found. In reading this literature we
higher level algebraic purpose was discussed as an found an overall coherence about students’ learning
outcome. We also used refereed studies of students’ of higher mathematics and the f inal version of the
typical errors and methods (see below). These tell us paper was constr ucted to show these similarities.
what needs to be lear nt and hence descr ibe the A list of jour nals accessed is included in this
development of algebraic understanding, but not appendix. There were only four reviews of research
how successful students lear n it. We also drew on used, two meta-analyses by Hembree, (1986; 1992)
significant overviews and compilations of research on used as summaries of literature and the U.S. Task
algebra. These reviews were used as gateways to Panel (NMAP 2008) was used as a gatewa y to
other research literature. We excluded studies which other sources.
focus only on shor t-term fluent performance of
6 PaperSUMMARY
8: Methodological
– PAPER appendix
2: Understanding whole numbers

List of journals consulted for Kaput, J., Carraher, D. and Blanton, M. (eds.) Algebra
Papers 2 to 5 in the early grades. New York: Erlbaum
Mason, J. and Sutherland, R. (2002), Key Aspects of
Teaching Algebra in Schools, QCA, London
British Journal of Developmental Psychology Nickson, M. (2000) Teaching and learning
British Journal of Educational Psychology mathematics: a teacher’s guide to recent research
Child Development and its applications. London: Cassell
Cognition and Instruction NMAP, National Mathematics Advisory Panel (2008)
Educational Studies in Mathematics downloaded April 2008 from:
Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science and http://www.ed.gov/about/bdscomm/list/mathpanel
Technology Education /index.html
International Electronic Journal of Mathematics ZDM The International Journal of Mathematics
Education Education volume 40 (2008)
International Journal for Mathematics and Lear ning
International Journal of Science and Mathematics
Education Large-scale studies used
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education
for Papers 6 and 7
Monograph
Journal of Educational Psychology Concepts in Secondary Mathematics and Science
Learning and Instruction Project (CSMS) (see Har t et al., 1981)
Diagnostic tests derived from clinical inter views
with 30 children age 11 to 16. In these inter views
List of journals consulted for the test items were trialled and revised, and students’
Papers 6 and 7 own methods and typical er rors were observed.
Common errors and methods were found across
schools which were not teacher-taught but had
British Journal of Developmental Psychology arisen through students’ own reasoning. The sample
British Journal of Educational Psychology for testing was from urban, rural and city areas
Child Development across England. It was selected from volunteer
Cognition and Instruction schools according to IQ distr ibutions in order to
Educational Studies in Mathematics represent the countr y as a whole. About 3000
International Journal of Mathematical Education in students took the Algebra test.
Science and Technology
Journal for Research in Mathematics Education Strategies and Errors in Secondary Mathematics
Learning and Instruction Project (SESM) focused on a small n umber of errors
Mathematical Thinking and Learning arising in the CSMS study. There used a large number
Proceedings of International Group for the of individual inter views and some teaching
Psychology of Mathematics Education experiments involving several classes of students.

Ryan and Williams


Reviews and collections Ryan and Williams randomly-sampled 13 000 English
school children from ages 4 to 15 using diagnostic
used for algebra tests designed to reveal typical errors and child-
methods, as CSMS, but with the express pur pose
Bednarz, N., Kieran C., Lee, L. (eds.) Approaches to of identifying progress made by students in
algebra: perspectives for research on teaching. mathematics. They found little progress made
Kluwer, Dordrecht between ages 11 to 14, and that many errors were
Chick, H., Stacey, K., Vincent, J. and Vincent, J. (eds.) similar to those found by Har t et al. 20 years earlier.
Proceedings of the 12th ICMI study conference: The See Ryan, J. and Williams, J. (2007) Children’s
future of the teaching and learning of algebra. Mathematics 4-15: learning from errors and
University of Melbourne, Australia, Dec 9-14, 2001. misconceptions, Maidenhead: Open University
Greenes, C. and Rubenstein, R. (eds.) Algebra and Press. More details of the tests can be found in
Algebraic Thinking in School Mathematics. 70th Mathematics Assessment for Learning and Teaching,
Yearbook. Reston,VA: NCTM. (2005) London: Hodder and Stoughton.
7 Key understandings in mathematics lear ning

Mollie MacGregor and Kaye Stacey


A series of pencil and paper tests were administered
to 2000 students from a representativ e sample of
volunteer schools in Years 7–10 (ages 11 –15) in 24
Australian secondary schools.

Assessment of Performance Unit (APU) test results


of 1979 (Foxman et al., 1981). These tests involved a
cohort of 12 500 students age 11 to 15 and w ere
designed to track development of mathematical
understanding by sampling across schools and regions.

Children’s Mathematical Frameworks study (CMF)


(Johnson, 1989), 25 classes in 21 schools in the
United Kingdom were tested to find out why and
how students between 8 and 13 cling to guess-and-
check and number-fact methods rather than new
formal methods offered by teachers.

References for appendix


Bus, A. G., and van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1999).
Phonological awareness and early reading: A
meta-analysis of experimental training studies.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 403-414.
Bus, A. G., van IJzendoorn, M. H., and Pellegrini, A. D.
(1995). Joint book reading mak es for success in
learning to read: A meta-analysis on
intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review
of Educational Research, 65, 1-21.
Blok, H. (1999). Reading to young children in
educational settings: A meta-analysis of recent
research. Language Learning, 49, 343-371.
Cooper, H. M. (1998). Synthesizing research : a guide
for literature reviews. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Publications.
Ehri, L. C., Nunes, S. R., Stahl, S. A., and Willows, D. M.
(2001). Systematic Phonics Instr uction Helps
Students Learn to Read: Evidence from the
National Reading Panel’s Meta-Analysis. Review of
Educational Research, 71, 393-447.
Reed, D. K. (2008). A Synthesis of Morphology
Interventions and Effects on Reading Outcomes
for Students in Gr ades K–12. Learning Disabilities
Research and Practice, 23, 36-49.
Published by the Nuffield Foundation, 28 Bedford Square, London WC1B 3JS
Telephone 020 7631 0566
Copyright © Nuffield Foundation 2009
ISBN 978-0-904956-75-7

ISBN: 978-0-904956-76-4
www.nuffieldfoundation.org

Potrebbero piacerti anche