Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

The dark side of social networking sites: An exploration of the


relational and psychological stressors associated with Facebook
use and affordances
Jesse Fox a,⇑, Jennifer J. Moreland b
a
The Ohio State University, 154 North Oval Mall, Columbus, OH 43210-1339, USA
b
The Research Institute, Nationwide Children’s Hospital, 575 Children’s Crossroad, WB5K10, Columbus, OH 43215, USA

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Research on social media typically focuses on its benefits; considerably less is known about the dark side
Available online 24 December 2014 of social networking sites. Focus groups of adult Facebook users (N = 44) uncovered narratives surround-
ing individuals’ negative psychological and relational experiences tied to the social networking site and
Keywords: its affordances (e.g., connectivity, visibility, accessibility, persistence, and social feedback). Thematic anal-
Social networking sites ysis rendered five themes regarding Facebook stressors: managing inappropriate or annoying content, being
Social comparison tethered, lack of privacy and control, social comparison and jealousy, and relationship tension and conflict.
Jealousy
Results demonstrate that although Facebook users often experience negative emotions, they feel
Relationship conflict
Privacy management
pressured to access the site frequently due to the fear of missing out and to keep up with relationship
Facebook addiction maintenance demands. Some participants reported privacy violations due to Facebook’s visibility, con-
nectivity, and persistence. These features also afforded constant social comparison to other network
members, which triggered jealousy, anxiety, and other negative emotions. Relational turbulence occurred
due to the public nature of conflict on Facebook. Many participants’ responses revealed overarching con-
tradictions: initially they claimed Facebook was inconsequential, yet later recounted significant stressful
or hurtful events associated with Facebook. Our findings indicate some methods may not uncover the
actual nature or scope of users’ experiences.
Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction ments and ‘‘likes.’’ Facebook also offers the convenience of net-
work-wide, group, and private communication channels through
Social networking websites (SNSs) have become an integral one interface. Further, Facebook has a mobile application, ensuring
medium for communicating within and about interpersonal rela- that users can access the site easily from their devices. Collectively,
tionships. Facebook is the most dominant SNS in the U.S. and over these affordances explain why Facebook has grown and main-
one billion people worldwide possess an active Facebook account tained a devoted user base globally.
(Facebook, 2014). Over two-thirds of U.S. Facebook users visit the Although considerable research has focused on the benefits of
site at least once per day and are connected to an average of 338 using SNSs such as increased social capital, social support, and
friends (Pew Research Center, 2014). relationship maintenance (e.g., Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007;
One reason Facebook is the most popular social networking site McEwan, 2013; Nabi, Prestin, & So, 2013), fewer studies have
is the scope of affordances it provides for users. The ability to examined the nature of negative outcomes for adult users. Given
connect with one’s offline network online—as well as make new that users expect positive outcomes and often visit SNSs for relax-
connections online—allows users to communicate easily with net- ation, entertainment, or social connection (Ku, Chu, & Tseng, 2013;
work members. Through the posting and sharing functions, social Park, Kee, & Valenzuela, 2009), users may not anticipate negative
information is easily distributed and stored among members; users experiences or interactions, making them more potent or hurtful.
can also provide feedback to this information in the form of com- At this time, some survey-based research has identified links
between Facebook use and diminished well-being (e.g., Chen &
Lee, 2013; Chou & Edge, 2012; Kross et al., 2013), as well as nega-
⇑ Corresponding author at: 3084 Derby Hall, 154 North Oval Mall, Columbus, OH
tive experiences based on unfriending and romantic relationship
43210-1339, USA. Tel.: +1 (614) 247 2348.
E-mail addresses: fox.775@osu.edu (J. Fox), jennifer.moreland@nationwide
dissolution (Bevan, Ang, & Fearns, 2014; Fox, Jones, & Lookadoo,
childrens.org (J.J. Moreland). 2013; Marshall, 2012; Tokunaga, 2014). Further, a recent content

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.083
0747-5632/Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176 169

analysis showed a prevalence of negative content posted to looking at attractive users’ profiles on Facebook, participants felt
Facebook (Shelton & Skalski, 2014). What is lacking is a deeper worse about their bodies than participants exposed to less attrac-
investigation into how and why users have negative experiences tive profiles. Male participants who viewed profiles of successful
on Facebook, particularly given users’ tendency to proclaim, ‘‘it’s males demonstrated a greater perceived discrepancy between their
just Facebook,’’ and thus does not affect them substantially (Fox, current career status and their ideal career status when compared
Warber, & Makstaller, 2013). As Mao (2014) noted, qualitative to males who viewed the profiles of less successful people
methods are necessary to elaborate on quantitative studies about (Haferkamp & Krämer, 2011).
technology and gain more insight into the breadth and depth of Facebook can also have a negative impact on romantic relation-
users’ changing experiences. It is important to determine the scope ships. Research has shown that Facebook can promote romantic
of these experiences so users and scholars are aware of both the jealousy (Utz & Beukeboom, 2011), which may explain why it is
benefits and drawbacks to participating in SNSs and can learn to a commonly cited source of relational conflict (Fox, Osborn, &
manage or guide the management of negative Facebook experi- Warber, 2014; Fox, Warber et al., 2013). Furthermore, after rela-
ences in a psychologically and relationally healthy manner. To this tionships terminate, Facebook can enable unhealthy surveillance
end, we designed an exploratory study to investigate the rich nar- of the ex-partner and delay emotional recovery (Fox, Jones et al.,
ratives surrounding users’ negative emotional experiences with 2013; Fox & Warber, 2014; Marshall, 2012).
Facebook, rooting our study in the context of dark side interper- To date, survey-based research demonstrates that adult
sonal communication. Facebook users can feel worse after using the site. Experimental
researchers have manipulated specific content, such as the attrac-
tiveness or success of other users, and determined that Facebook
2. Experiences with social networking sites
has the potential to promote negative affect. What is missing from
the literature is an exploration into the variety and scope of nega-
2.1. The light side of Facebook
tive experiences resulting from Facebook use. Thus, we ask:

Several benefits have been ascribed to SNS use. Relationships on


RQ1: What kind of negative psychological experiences do users
Facebook bring social capital, the benefits users receive from their
have with Facebook?
associations with other people (Ellison et al., 2007; Valenzuela,
RQ2: What kind of negative relational experiences do users
Park, & Kee, 2008). Facebook affords the ability to strengthen weak
have with Facebook?
ties, maintain existing relationships, and define otherwise ambigu-
ous relationships (McEwan, 2013; Valenzuela et al., 2008). Face-
2.3. Facebook affordances
book’s connectivity enables users to promote group identity and
in-group relationships, particularly for users in ethnoracial, gender
Social networking sites are defined by users’ ability to maintain
identity, or sexual orientation minority groups (Fox & Warber, in
a profile, connect with other users, and trace the networks of con-
press; Zhao, Grasmuck, & Martin, 2008).
nected users (boyd & Ellison, 2008). On Facebook, users are norma-
In addition to promoting social relationships, SNS use may have
tively associated with their real (or chosen) name and identifying
individual psychological benefits for users. Several studies have
information. Thus, Facebook’s primary purpose is to provide access
identified boosts in self-esteem from interacting with or modifying
to users’ offline networks—and the social information they pro-
one’s own Facebook profile (Gentile, Twenge, Freeman, &
vide—online (Reich, Subrahmanyam, & Espinoza, 2012). As a result,
Campbell, 2012; Gonzales & Hancock, 2011; Toma & Hancock,
the site has distinct social implications and functionality compared
2013) as well as from general Facebook use (Gonzales, 2014).
to other social media.
Research has also determined that connectivity on SNSs may pro-
Facebook’s social functionality is tied to its specific set of affor-
mote perceptions of social support for some users, which in turn
dances (Fox, in press; Treem & Leonardi, 2012). Affordances can
facilitates well-being offline (Nabi et al., 2013). Thus, Facebook
influence users’ positive and negative experiences with technology
and other SNSs have demonstrated the potential to provide consid-
(e.g., Mao, 2014). Perhaps the most notable affordance of SNSs is
erable social and psychological benefits to users.
connectivity or association, which enables network members to rec-
ognize each other’s presence and often view each other’s profile
2.2. The dark side of Facebook content through a direct connection or a common node (Treem &
Leonardi, 2012). On Facebook, these nodes are referred to as
Although Facebook may provide a variety of benefits, SNSs also ‘‘friends.’’ Although this connectivity enables convenient access
manifest a dark side and can have deleterious consequences for to one’s entire network and may expand the network by identify-
users. A survey by Bevan, Gomez, and Sparks (2014) revealed the ing second degree connections, it may also reveal associations that
more time spent on SNSs and the more SNSs a person used, the promote stress (e.g., seeing that a romantic partner is still friends
lower their quality of life. Chen and Lee (2013) found that Facebook with an ex-partner).
interaction is associated with reduced self-esteem, cognitive over- Visibility concerns the public or private nature of information
load, and feelings of distress. Kross et al. (2013) examined Face- presented online. Although social information or artifacts may
book use over time and found higher levels of Facebook use were not be easily accessible or publicized offline, Facebook enables easy
associated with a significant decrease in well-being. At its worst, and immediate sharing among the network (Treem & Leonardi,
Facebook is used as a conduit for cyberbullying, stalking, and 2012). One drawback is that such information may be negatively
online harassment (Fox, in press; Kwan & Skoric, 2013). skewed (e.g., an unflattering or inappropriate picture of oneself
Because of the visibility of other network members’ experi- posted by a friend). Visibility also enables the monitoring of others’
ences, habits, and preferences, social comparison is a common content without their awareness; for example, Facebook does not
activity on Facebook. Several studies have indicated that social inform a user who has viewed their page. Thus, users can informa-
comparisons made on SNSs can be detrimental (Feinstein et al., tion seek surreptitiously.
2013; Johnson & Knobloch-Westerwick, 2014; Lee, 2014). Chou Social feedback is the ability for others to interact directly with
and Edge (2012) found those who have used Facebook longer shared information and subsequently respond to the user
expressed they believed other people were happier and had better (Sutcliffe, Gonzalez, Binder, & Nevarez, 2011). Facebook allows
lives than they did. Haferkamp and Krämer (2011) found that after users to comment, share, and ‘‘like’’ posts, all of which let the user
170 J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176

know other network members viewed and interacted with posted recorded all focus groups to obtain both verbal and nonverbal
information. Further, although social feedback can be conveyed (e.g., nodding, gestures) cues for later analysis (Corbin & Strauss,
privately, most of it is visible to other users. If the social feedback 2008). Three moderators trained in focus group methodology and
is positive, such as earning ‘‘likes’’ after posting a picture of a new qualitative analysis conducted the focus groups. Given their
haircut, it is likely the user will have a positive reaction or experi- knowledge on the topic was required to probe participants on rel-
ence self-affirmation (Toma & Hancock, 2013). Facebook’s features evant themes, the authors served as moderators for the study
can also convey negative social feedback, whether through a nega- (Vaughn, Schumm, & Sinagub, 1996).
tive comment or by ‘‘liking’’ a post that conveys a user’s negative When participants arrived at the study location, the modera-
experience (e.g., ‘‘I lost my job today.’’) tor(s) greeted them and guided them through the consent process.
Persistence is tied to the digital nature of the text, pictures, and Moderators worked from a semi-structured guide to lead the focus
other content posted online. Because digital material is easily groups (see Appendix A). The semi-structured nature of the guide
saved, duplicated, and recirculated, information shared on encouraged the natural flow of conversation and further probes
Facebook may be accessible long after the initial post and difficult were employed in each group when necessary.
to remove permanently (Treem & Leonardi, 2012). Members of
family and friend groups typically intersect on Facebook and they
can share and store memories on the site. In the case of an unflat- 3.2. Participants
tering photo posted by a friend, however, it may be impossible to
delete every copy, ensuring that the photo will endure long after Participants (N = 44) were 17 men and 27 women who were tak-
the initial post. Thus, negative information may persist long after ing one or more classes at a large Midwestern university and ranged
initial posts and even after an individual attempts to delete in age from 19 to 52 (M = 23.36, SD = 6.79). Some were offered extra
content. credit in exchange for their participation, whereas others partici-
Accessibility is the capability of easily reaching content on an pated without compensation. They identified as White/European/
SNS (Fox, in press). Facebook optimizes its interface for mobile European-American (52.3%), Asian/Asian-American (20.5%),
platforms to maximize the time people spend with the site. Users Multiracial (6.8%), Latino/a (2.3%), Middle Eastern (2.3%), and seven
are not required to get to a desktop computer; rather they can con- did not report their race/ethnicity due to a clerical error. All those
veniently log on any time and from any place using a digital device reporting sexual orientation stated they were heterosexual. Only
with cellular service or an Internet connection. Some scholars have one participant reported she did not currently use Facebook, but
noted downsides to constant accessibility such as ‘‘Facebook addic- had been a regular user until six months prior. Her data was
tion,’’ a constant need for gratification, and the tendency to become retained in the analyses as she effectively responded to questions
distracted from work or face-to-face interaction (Masur, Reinecke, regarding her prior use. Otherwise, all participants possessed an
Ziegele, & Quiring, 2014; Pang, 2013; Turkle, 2011). active Facebook account. They reported having their profile an aver-
Given these affordances, social networking sites like Facebook age of 3.55 years (SD = 1.90) and spent an average of 1.56 h
enable users to initiate, promote, and exacerbate many facets of (SD = 1.63) each day actively using Facebook (i.e., not just logged
the dark side of relationships (Fox, in press). At this time, however, in, but using the interface). This use is notably higher than Face-
limited research has delved into the relationship between Face- book’s recent report that users spend 40 min a day on the site
book’s affordances and negative experiences with the site. Thus, (Brustein, 2014).
we pose a third research question:

RQ3: What affordances of Facebook are associated with nega-


3.3. Analysis
tive experiences?

Twelve same-sex groups ranging in size from three to five par-


3. Method
ticipants were analyzed. We conducted same-sex groups because,
as Lindolf and Taylor (2011) note, ‘‘a homogeneous group is usually
We employed focus groups to uncover themes in the current
more willing to speak openly’’ (p. 186). We engaged in an open
study concerning emotional responses and stressors related to
coding process and then completed in vivo coding (Corbin &
Facebook use. We conducted focus group analysis for two primary
Strauss, 2008) to identify the terms participants chose to describe
reasons. First, few studies exist that explicate the nature of stress-
their experiences (e.g., creeping, TMI, friend sluts). Through itera-
ors associated with Facebook. Thus, a method allowing for suffi-
tions of the data, a constant-comparative method was applied to
cient freedom and depth of probing was needed. Second, social
identify, elaborate, and clarify categories (Corbin & Strauss,
networks are inherently social contexts designed for interaction,
2008). Emergent categories were examined within and across
and focus groups more closely reflect this dynamic multivocality
groups to determine salience and recurrence. After a training pro-
compared to other methods (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). Focus groups
cess, the first and second authors individually coded each group
provide rich, in-depth information and narratives often not
and identified common themes across groups to achieve investiga-
obtained using more closed-ended methods, such as surveys or
tor triangulation (Lincoln & Guba, 1985). Common themes were
experiments. Further, focus groups encourage the expression of
identified within each of the 12 focus groups.
thoughts, emotions, and experiences (Miles & Huberman, 1994).

3.1. Procedure
4. Results
Focus groups were conducted in two sessions. The inclusion of a
second wave of data enabled triangulation via member validation Following the coding process, five themes concerning the dark
and negative case analysis (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). Furthermore, side of Facebook emerged: managing inappropriate or annoying
we established diachronic reliability, as we found no noted differ- content, being tethered to Facebook, perceived lack of privacy and
ences in participant testimonies from one group of sessions to the control, social comparison and jealousy, and relationship tension.
other (Miles & Huberman, 1994). All groups were conducted in the The role of Facebook’s affordances manifested in the discussion
same location to maintain continuity. The research team video of each of these themes.
J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176 171

4.1. Managing inappropriate or annoying content connected to friends no matter the place or time. As one female
participant noted, ‘‘I feel like I couldn’t not have a Facebook though
In terms of minor negative experiences, a number of subthemes because all my friends have Facebook.’’ Such social pressure often
emerged concerning participants’ aggravation with others’ per- came coupled with feelings of guilt and dissatisfaction. Because
ceived misuse of Facebook or Facebook’s automated features. Reac- Facebook is available on mobile devices, it affords constant acces-
tions to this inappropriate content ranged from annoyance (e.g., sibility, which means users can reach it at all times and also that
due to continuous negative posts from a sulking friend) to shock other network members expect them to be able to reach it at all
(e.g., from hearing important news from a close friend via an imper- times. Several participants agreed they were unhappy with the
sonal channel like Facebook) to disgust (e.g., from seeing lewd, social pressure from social network members to comment imme-
offensive, or otherwise inappropriate content posted by friends). diately on friends’ posts and pictures or post their own pictures
Participants reported mild irritation from content they per- of recent events. One participant likened it to labor:
ceived as repetitive and pointless, whether user- or system-gener-
I think Facebook is one more thing that you have to like be
ated. Because Facebook affords both continuous and automatic
accountable for. . . ‘‘Hey, I posted on your Facebook wall. Why
posting, several participants were vexed by ongoing status updates
didn’t you see it?...Like, why didn’t you read my message?’’. . .[I]
about gaming achievements or score updates from live sporting
already have all these other things that I need to be doing and
events. The most frequently cited annoyance was Facebook’s auto-
keeping track of and Facebook is just one more thing you have
mated birthday notifications, which participants felt obligated
to be responsive to. . . it’s kinda like work, like you have to do it. . .
them to post birthday wishes on that person’s page. A female par-
ticipant reported: ‘‘You have to make sure to log on to their wall
that day and say ‘Have a great day!’’’ Another female participant Despite reports of negative emotions about feeling tethered to
agreed: ‘‘Right, or then people are upset. It’s totally the success the site, participants seemed unable to separate themselves from
of Facebook that we feel bad about not being on Facebook on that it. As a female participant noted, ‘‘You have to know what everyone
day. Isn’t that some bullshit?’’ Participants wanted to ignore these else is doing at all times.’’ Because Facebook affords constant
posts, but they realized doing so came at a social cost. The latter accessibility and updating, participants frequently cited a ‘‘fear of
quote points to how some participants even placed blame on missing out’’ on new social information that they felt was impor-
Facebook for feeling such pressure. tant (Przybylski, Murayama, DeHaan, & Gladwell, 2013). One male
Beyond mere annoyance, participants noted numerous instances participant cited this fear to explain the dissonance between his
of being shocked by the perceived inappropriateness of friends’ vis- continuing use of Facebook in spite of disliking it: ‘‘I don’t like
ible posts, comments, and photos. Although Facebook does post Facebook. I feel like I have to have it just because everyone else
rules regarding obscene content, it does not afford selective filter- has it. And I feel I’d be missing out on—I don’t even know what
ing. As one male participant noted, ‘‘There aren’t obviously clear I’d be missing out on.’’ It may be that spending more time on
cut boundaries as established by, like, Mark Zuckerberg as to what Facebook foments constant anticipation of important social infor-
you should be doing on Facebook. . .I just feel there are natural mation, even when the idea of what is important is nebulous.
boundaries that exist on how much you should be sharing.’’ Indeed, Further supporting the notion of being tethered, most partici-
participants often elaborated their standards regarding what they pants lamented spending too much time on Facebook, as it often
deemed appropriate self-disclosure on Facebook, but what was con- detracted from face-to-face interpersonal interaction, work, stud-
sidered acceptable varied between individuals and groups. Thus, ies, or sleep. One male participant related being late to class and
although many endorsed the idea of ‘‘natural boundaries’’ or that work often because of a need to check Facebook before he left
people ‘‘should know what’s okay and what’s not,’’ the discrepan- the house. A female participant confessed her schoolwork suffered
cies in between responses demonstrates that, similar to Fox and because she could not control her time on Facebook: ‘‘The paper
Anderegg (2014), norms and expectations for behavior on Facebook took me twice as long as it should have. . .I told myself it would
vary considerably. Such variation may be a source of conflict among be 15 minutes. . .It’s kind of a problem.’’ These participants demon-
friends. In terms of Petronio’s (1991) coordinated management of strate what Pang (2013) refers to as a ‘‘distraction addiction,’’
privacy theory, although participants report engaging in rule wherein users are so preoccupied with the information stream
development for themselves, they often avoid the process of from their devices that they have trouble functioning productively.
boundary coordination with others in the Facebook sphere. Instead, Together, these findings suggest why it may be difficult for users to
they choose to simply avoid offenders’ pages or block them from stay off of or stop using the site. Particularly, the lures of
their newsfeed. This passive response does not communicate the Facebook’s affordances and fears about missing out on social infor-
perceived violation to the offender, and thus the boundaries are mation may inform why many users report problematic behavior
not coordinated appropriately. and addictive tendencies regarding SNSs (Masur et al., 2014).
In addition to content-appropriateness, some participants noted
violations of context-appropriateness. Because Facebook affords 4.3. Privacy frustrations and perceived lack of control
mass posts to the network and connectivity does not distinguish
strong and weak ties, some participants feel SNSs are depersonal- A perceived lack of privacy on Facebook emerged as a popular
ized and an inappropriate way to share some information. Some theme that manifested in two ways. First, because of the affor-
participants mentioned astonishment over hearing significant dances of sharing, replicability, and connectivity, users must cope
news (e.g., a sibling’s pregnancy and a close friend’s engagement) with the inability to hide things from their existing Facebook net-
via Facebook. In these instances, participants were stunned and work. Second, because Facebook ultimately controls users’ privacy,
hurt that they learned about a major life event via the massperson- participants lamented the inability to hide things from the world
al channel of Facebook. Rather, they believed they should have (i.e., those outside one’s Facebook network) because of the lack
received personal, private contact from the source. of control of how Facebook itself shares information (e.g., making
material on Facebook searchable on the Internet).
4.2. Being tethered to Facebook In some cases, members outside of participants’ established
network gained access to their Facebook profile. A middle-aged
Similar to Turkle’s (2011) claims, participants reported feeling male participant experienced a surprise on Facebook: a teenage
pressure to continue being a part of the Facebook world and stay girl contacted him and claimed he was her father, a chronological
172 J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176

impossibility although he knew her mother at one time. Given the how they look on Facebook. I noticed that even with myself,
girl seemed troubled, he felt compelled to maintain contact, yet felt like, if I’m at an event it’s like, ‘I need to get a good picture.’
uncomfortable with the situation. After the girl got in trouble, her And I stop: ‘Do I really care? I’m having fun with the people
mother contacted the participant via Facebook asking what they I’m with, the world doesn’t need to know about it.’
should do with ‘‘their’’ daughter. At that point, this participant
Similarly, a male participant blamed Facebook (and its founder)
could no longer manage the stress of the Facebook connection:
for hegemonizing the tendency to socially compare:
‘‘That’s it for you. You’re blocked. Because it became obvious that
this woman had been living in some kind of delusion, and now . . .people are trying to constantly tweet their Facebook to
she had her daughter living it too. I was very stressed. And then express who they want to look like, not who they actually are,
my wife saw it [on Facebook], and she got really mad.’’ In the wake and constantly comparing themselves to other people. . . people
of the situation, this participant reported becoming very paranoid are just so, so worried about what other people think, when it
about people from his past contacting him. He strengthened his ultimately doesn’t matter. Facebook has this air of authority
privacy management on Facebook because ‘‘You just never know that it really doesn’t have. . .Mark Zuckerberg’s a business man
what’s going to jump up out of the past.’’ and so he’s gonna do whatever it takes to suck people in and
Although some privacy threats could arise from outside of the get their information.
network, most participants seemed more concerned with the
Social comparison became particularly salient to participants in
threat of privacy violation within their own network. One group
romantic contexts, wherein they would use Facebook’s affordances
brought up the Tyler Clementi case, wherein a roommate mali-
of persistent history and connectivity to self-compare with a roman-
ciously filmed his closeted roommate’s romantic encounter with
tic interest’s potential or former mates. Male participants especially
another man and then broadcast it over social media, outing
acknowledged comparing themselves to a romantic interest’s
Clementi and triggering him to commit suicide. The group reflected
ex-boyfriend and even the interest’s current male friends to assess
on the potential for long-term damage if someone were to be
their potential for success. At the other end of the relationship spec-
humiliated via Facebook, as information on Facebook is both visi-
trum, some women noted in the wake of a breakup, they pursued
ble and persistent. A male participant suggested that because of
downward social comparison with an ex’s new love interest to bol-
Facebook’s pervasiveness and network connectivity, that person
ster their own egos, even if it required considerable effort to do so:
would likely be left friendless:
I’ve had friends that have gone to crazy extents—they’ll find out
You’re pretty much [Facebook] friends with all your friends. You
mutual friends and ask them to sign on and will like look at
have a circle of friends on this planet, what are you going to do?
every single one of their albums...They want to find like a pic-
Make a new circle of friends? It’s your entire circle of friends.
ture where they [the new significant other] look unattractive
You don’t have secret reservoirs of friends. Again, it’s almost like
to make themselves feel better. [laughing]
you’re a celebrity where there is nowhere to run.
Although perhaps this social comparison was an attempt at
This comment reflects how a serious privacy violation would be
reducing uncertainty about the current or former relationship,
far-reaching and perhaps devastating, as Facebook’s connectivity
employing Facebook as a social comparison tool is known to elicit
could leave a user with no unaware ties and thus without a support
jealousy in users (Utz & Beukeboom, 2011). Furthermore, partici-
system.
pants also acknowledged the potential for seeing things on
Facebook that instigate feelings of mistrust about their current
4.4. Enduring social comparison and jealousy
romantic partner. Some participants noted that prior to the advent
of Facebook, they would not have been able to compare themselves
Similar to Haferkamp and Krämer (2011) and Chou and Edge
so readily to a partner’s former love interests.
(2012), our findings demonstrate individuals engage in various
manners of social comparison through Facebook. For participants
who started Facebook as an adolescent, the first basis of compari- 4.5. Fighting on and about Facebook: Relationship maintenance,
son (and competition) was the number of friends one had on the conflict, and deterioration
network: a quantifiable popularity contest. A female participant
noted, ‘‘[Getting Facebook friends] seems like such an arms race,’’ Given that Facebook networks typically mimic offline networks
and another in the same group agreed, ‘‘It was like a race to try (Reich et al., 2012), it is unsurprising that interactions occurring
to get so many friends.’’ Although participants mostly indicated offline may carry onto Facebook, and interactions happening on
that their ‘‘friend count’’ did not matter now, other comments indi- Facebook may transfer offline. Participants suggested that
cated that this measure may still serve as a heuristic cue, particu- Facebook could both exacerbate existing offline conflict and create
larly for outliers (i.e., judging ‘‘friend sluts’’ with thousands of new sources of conflict. As one female participant noted, ‘‘Facebook
friends, or those with very few connections). definitely causes drama.’’
Participants also reported comparing their lives to those of their One particularly rife area for this drama was romantic relation-
Facebook friends, which often resulted in feelings of jealousy or ships. Facebook’s affordances, such as the visibility of content to
dissatisfaction. One female participant discussed becoming preoc- the social network and the ability for relationship partners to link
cupied with others’ posts: ‘‘I feel like it consumes you. . .to the their profiles (i.e., go ‘‘Facebook official’’; Fox & Warber, 2013), pro-
point that you can’t live your normal life because you see everyone vided several areas where seemingly small issues took on a new
else’s. . . I think it makes you think your life is not as fun or as excit- and vicious life because of Facebook. A male participant noted
ing or interesting than other peoples’, which is sad because it’s not Facebook as a frequent source of interpersonal conflict:
true.’’ Likewise, a few participants noted how Facebook interac-
It’s kind of stressful sometimes because it becomes drama, it
tions seemed to change offline socializing because people are too
becomes conflict. The stupid little fights, and it comes up bigger
preoccupied with constantly documenting and uploading every
and bigger and bigger, just for these little reasons: oh, you
moment. A female participant lamented:
didn’t comment me back, or oh you didn’t post this picture
I think life is a lot less stressful when you’re not worried about up. I think every relationship is like that—my past two girl-
how it seems to other people. Everyone is so concerned about friends were like that the same way.
J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176 173

Another male agreed, noting he also had fights with his girl- are afforded by Facebook’s design. Although Facebook’s affor-
friend over their one-sided Facebook interaction: ‘‘She said our dances offer benefits for sharing social information, they also pres-
wall-to-wall was embarrassing, because it was like her her her ent downsides. Participants in this study noted visibility can work
her her, one me. Her her her her her, one me. I mean, it’s not that against users, as they see things they do not want to see.
I don’t care, it’s just, like, I have your number. I don’t need to write Furthermore, their friends often share things that users wish they
on your wall.’’ Although the male participant did not see a use for would not share, and the persistence of this information can haunt
the affordance of social feedback, his girlfriend’s use of the term users. Although Facebook is conveniently accessible through
‘‘embarrassing’’ indicates that she felt that his lack of response mobile devices, this also makes users feel tethered to the site.
had not just interpersonal but social significance due to its visibil- Because Facebook affords constant accessibility and updating,
ity. Similarly, one female mocked other women’s preoccupation users fear missing out on social information if they do not check
with their significant other’s social feedback on Facebook over the site regularly, creating more social labor for users. Participants
the course of a relationship: felt pressured by friends, relatives, and romantic partners to
engage in relationship maintenance on the site; accessibility cre-
[First, it’s] ‘Oh, he looks good in this picture, and I like his
ated an expectation that users would respond quickly to Facebook
clothes, and he’s friends with his brother, and they look so nice’
posts and messages. Connectivity can be great among friends and
and then six months later, it’s like, ‘I commented on his picture
relatives, but it can create problems when users do not want to
two days ago and he didn’t ‘like’ it. WHY.’ [throws up hands and
connect to others, want to terminate existing connections, or use
drops pen dramatically, others laugh]
this connectivity to enable unhealthy behaviors like monitoring
All of these comments indicate that although Facebook makes an ex-partner or engaging in social comparison. Despite these neg-
relationship maintenance convenient, its perceived accessibility ative experiences, however, all participants except one still main-
and the visibility of these interactions to the social network may tained their Facebook accounts. Future research should further
foster expectations. If partners’ Facebook use or expectations are explore how users weigh the psychological and relational costs
not compatible, this may create conflict and make relationship and benefits of their Facebook engagement.
maintenance on the site feel burdensome (Fox, in press). The use of focus groups in this study had many advantages, and
Participants noted the maintenance of family ties on Facebook one important finding emerged that probably would not have been
was an additional source of interpersonal strife. Several partici- evident using alternative methods. Namely, over the course of the
pants related face-to-face arguments resulting from not friending group discussions, the majority of participants revealed significant
or defriending their relatives on Facebook. For example, a female inconsistencies about their negative experiences with Facebook.
participant and her cousins were confronted at a family gathering These inconsistencies included: (1) suggesting other people caused
by their uncle’s new wife, who demanded to know why they had or experienced negative events on the site, whereas they them-
not friended her on Facebook. In other situations, the relative’s selves did not; (2) claiming Facebook was insignificant in their
online behavior is unpalatable. For example, a female participant lives, but later revealing instances in which Facebook had a consid-
defriended her sister-in-law because she found her posts vulgar; erable emotional impact on them; and (3) defending their own
thus, she did not want to be visibly linked to the sister-in-law. negative interactions on Facebook as uncontrollable, but attribut-
The first time she saw the sister-in-law face-to-face, she was ing others’ negative interactions to personality flaws. These con-
alarmed by her reaction: ‘‘We went to a wedding and she was like tradictions have significant implications for how SNS researchers
[makes angry face], ‘You defriended me on Facebook!’ And I was collect and interpret data about user’s experiences.
like, ‘I love you in real life. I don’t like you on the Internet.’’’ As this First, we noted a marked trend supporting the presence of the
incident demonstrates, the practice of defriending may be per- third person effect (Davison, 1983) regarding Facebook use. Although
ceived as a relational slight (Bevan, Ang et al., 2014), and this par- many participants downplayed their own negative interactions on
ticipant reported having to engage in relationship repair to salvage Facebook, they were quick to assert others posted negatively, cre-
their connection. ated drama on or about Facebook, or overreacted to Facebook con-
‘‘Facebook wars’’ or ‘‘comment wars’’ represent another form of tent. Later disclosures, however, revealed that, like their peers,
conflict participants cited wherein several discussion topics, par- these participants were responsible for negative content or events.
ticularly politics, could spark heated debates online through the A second related inconsistency was that many participants who
visible social feedback. Participants acknowledged the lack of initially claimed Facebook was inconsequential later shared stories
face-to-face conversation often resulted in the debates becoming in which Facebook had a significant impact on them. Early in the
uncivil, and most witnessed these fights continue offline. discussions, participants often decried other people ‘‘making a big
Alternatively, offline fights moved online. A male participant deal’’ out of Facebook or confusing it with ‘‘real life,’’ but later dem-
recalled an argument with a friend about gay marriage during a onstrated Facebook was consequential and had affected them off-
car ride. Although he thought the dispute ended, the friend later line. One participant said it was ‘‘ridiculous’’ to get upset over
posted about it: ‘‘We got into a big thing on Facebook. It was weird Facebook, but later relayed how she no longer talked to a close
having an argument over Facebook. Like, waiting for their response friend over comments the friend made on Facebook. Two men both
so you can say, ‘Ha ha, that’s crap.’ [mimics angry typing] It became claimed Facebook never played a role in their romantic relation-
this big 30-post tirade back and forth.’’ No matter the topic, ships, but later each discussed how their lack of communication
Facebook’s visibility and connectivity allow private conflicts to with their girlfriends on Facebook caused fights offline. The very
become public and also enable other network members to com- same participants who claimed that Facebook was not ‘‘real’’ all
ment on it or take sides, further fanning the flames and often pro- had stories to share about having negative emotions, offline con-
ducing offline relational consequences as well. flict, or even relational termination in part because of (if not wholly
attributable to) Facebook’s affordances. These findings reinforce the
5. Discussion and conclusion notion that although many people conceptualize them as separate
worlds, online and offline experiences are inextricably intertwined.
In this study, we sought to explore the breadth and depth of A third inconsistency is emerged when participants related inci-
participants’ negative emotional experiences with a social net- dents in which they were responsible for a negative event or drama
working site. This study revealed that SNS use can trigger many on Facebook. Their accounts often revealed the fundamental
minor and major negative emotional experiences, many of which attribution error (Ross, 1977). Although other Facebook users posted
174 J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176

drama because they were unstable or desperate for attention, when emerge and provided for a rich analysis of themes. Focus groups
participants themselves were probed, they claimed external circum- encouraged participants to converse and bounce ideas off one
stances (e.g., needing to respond to someone else’s misguided post) another in a safe environment, and indeed, over time they dis-
prompted their own behaviors (DeAndrea & Walther, 2011). Thus, closed more substantial and intense negative experiences. Quanti-
users demonstrate a blind spot when it comes to their own tative analyses concerning Facebook provide only one part of the
Facebook negativity, although they continue to judge others. picture; researchers should also seek out more descriptive data
These contradictions have significant implications for SNS to help shape a more comprehensive understanding of users’
researchers. First, they indicate that the breadth of users’ experi- online experiences.
ences are not truly captured by content analytical procedures or Future extensions of this work should address the limitations of
big data scrapes. For example, several of our participants reported this study. All participants in this study were enrolled in a college
posting positive messages (e.g., birthdays, congratulating people course; the commonality of these experiences across broader pop-
on life events) although they actually felt annoyed, offended, or ulations is unknown. Thus, the rich data provided by this study
hurt. Selective self-presentation is pervasive and undoubtedly may be complemented by further quantitative research with a
shapes the content of users’ posts, and taking these comments at nationally representative sample to examine how widely these
face value would not be an accurate reflection of users’ state of perspectives are held. Given Facebook’s global appeal—over 80%
mind. Second, participants appear to experience some dissonance of Facebook users are outside of the U.S. and Canada (Facebook,
in regards to Facebook. Given many spend considerable time on 2014)—cross-cultural studies would offer further insight and bases
the site, they may feel guilty, defensive, or embarrassed about neg- of comparison for relational experiences in different countries. Fur-
ative experiences with Facebook and may forget, ignore, minimize, thermore, our participants were skewed toward younger adults.
or avoid discussing the dark side. If researchers assess users’ atti- Although young adults are some of the heaviest users, SNS use
tudes or experiences in a single, straightforward survey, users has grown significantly in older populations and their experiences
may consciously or subconsciously provide positively-biased likely differ (Pew Research Center, 2014).
responses. Qualitative approaches or funneling techniques may With regard to methodology, scholars conducting future stud-
help mitigate bias in the way users describe their experiences with ies in this area may desire to employ male and female moderators.
social media. All focus group moderators in the current study were female,
Our findings also have practical applications for Facebook users. which may have influenced, further facilitated, or limited some
They suggest that many users are unaware of how Facebook may participants’ responding. Replications with sex-matched modera-
negatively affect their lives and do not necessarily manage nega- tors are advisable. Overall, these strengths and limitations point
tive experiences in a productive manner. First, our findings suggest to the need for further exploration of Facebook use and emotion-
users should become more aware of the time and effort they sink ally-charged, dark side interpersonal and masspersonal
into the site. As Pang (2013) suggests, contemplative computing interactions.
practices may help draw users’ attention to the time they spend In sum, this study revealed that although there are many posi-
with the site and make that time more efficient, thus helping users tive aspects to using social networking sites, users should be aware
feel less drained, annoyed by, or tethered to the site. Second, our of a dark side. Given every participant related a significant negative
findings suggest that although Facebook is convenient and accessi- relational and psychological experience tied to Facebook, manag-
ble, other channels may be more interpersonally appropriate for ing social media and its role in our relationships should be an
some communication, including arguments and important life essential part of media literacy education. Research should con-
announcements. Visibility to other network members may exacer- tinue to determine how we can maximize the benefits of social
bate conflict, and diminished nonverbal cues may make it difficult media while avoiding its drawbacks. Finally, users should remem-
to determine others’ emotional reactions during an argument. ber that social networking sites are tools, and as one male partici-
Although Facebook may be an acceptable venue for broadcasting pant noted: ‘‘Tools can make your life easier, or you can cut your
important announcements to weak ties, it is seen as too deperson- hand off. You just have to know how to use them.’’
alized to communicate these messages to strong ties. Third, given
many users felt that their privacy was violated on Facebook, our Appendix A. Semi-structured interview guide
findings suggest that users should be educated on how privacy is
practiced at the interpersonal, network, and corporate levels on When you share a positive life event or good news on Facebook,
every SNS they join. Users should be aware of the consequences what kind of positive or negative outcomes occur?
of the affordances of visibility, connectivity, persistence, accessibil- When you share a negative life event or bad news on Facebook,
ity, and social feedback as there are implications for both psycho- what kind of positive or negative outcomes occur?
logical and social well-being. Finally, users should be aware of the If you something good or bad happens to you, do you go on
detrimental effects of constant social comparison via SNSs. Users Facebook to rant or rave about it?
should acknowledge how SNSs afford selective self-presentation Have you ever posted something that you wished you could
(Walther, 1996) and avoid seeking out targets for comparison. take back?
Blocking these connections or removing them from visible feeds Have you ever deleted a post because it had an unintended
may help users control the impulse to self-compare and avoid impact, like hurting someone’s feelings?
the subsequent negative emotions. Do you think that, because of Facebook, you have a larger group
Given social media use continues to grow among younger and of friends you could rely on in a crisis (for example, if you
younger age groups, with a recent survey showing nearly 60% of needed someone to take care of you after breaking a leg, or if
children used SNSs by the age of 10 (Lange, 2014), it is essential you were having a hard time emotionally after a breakup)?
these social media literacy efforts start early. Existing patterns of Do you feel Facebook is a healthy or unhealthy practice overall?
SNS adoption suggest these efforts should not be restricted solely What kind of positive or negative emotional experiences have
to children, however (Pew Research Center, 2014); adolescents you had via Facebook during relationship initiation?
and adults of all ages should be clearly informed of how these sites What kind of positive or negative emotional experiences have
work and the potential costs and benefits of participation. you had within your romantic relationships on Facebook?
A few strengths and limitations to the current study are worth Overall, do you think Facebook is a healthy or unhealthy tool in
noting. The qualitative method allowed for individual narratives to romantic relationships?
J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176 175

Do you ever feel that Facebook has caused you stress or Haferkamp, N., & Krämer, N. C. (2011). Social comparison 2.0: Examining the effects
of online profiles on social-networking sites. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and
anxiety?
Social Networking, 14, 309–314. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0120.
Have you ever had physical repercussions from Facebook use? Johnson, B. K., & Knobloch-Westerwick, S. (2014). Glancing up or down: Mood
management and selective social comparisons on social networking sites.
Computers in Human Behavior, 41, 33–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.
09.009.
References Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., et al. (2013). Facebook
use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PloS One, 8(8),
e69841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841.
Bevan, J. L., Ang, P. C., & Fearns, J. B. (2014). Being unfriended on Facebook: An
Ku, Y. C., Chu, T. H., & Tseng, C. H. (2013). Gratifications for using CMC technologies:
application of expectancy violation theory. Computers in Human Behavior, 33,
A comparison among SNS, IM, and e-mail. Computers in Human Behavior, 29,
171–178. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.029.
226–234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.08.009.
Bevan, J. L., Gomez, R., & Sparks, L. (2014). Disclosures about important life events
Kwan, G. C. E., & Skoric, M. M. (2013). Facebook bullying: An extension of battles in
on Facebook: Relationships with stress and quality of life. Computers in Human
school. Computers in Human Behavior, 29, 16–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/
Behavior, 39, 246–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.021.
j.chb.2012.07.014.
boyd, D. M., & Ellison, N. B. (2008). Social network sites: Definition, history, and
Lange, M. (2014). 59 percent of tiny children use social media. New York Magazine
scholarship. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 210–230. http://
<http://nymag.com/thecut/2014/02/over-half-kids-social-media-before-age-
dx.doi.org/10.1111/j/1083-6101.2007.00393.x.
ten.html> Retrieved 31.07.14.
Brustein, J. (2014). Americans now spend more time on Facebook than they do on
Lee, S. Y. (2014). How do people compare themselves with others on social network
their pets. Business Week <http://www.businessweek.com/articles/2014-07-23/
sites? The case of Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 253–260. http://
heres-how-much-time-people-spend-on-facebook-daily> Retrieved 11.11.14.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.009.
Chen, W., & Lee, K. H. (2013). Sharing, liking, commenting, and distressed? The
Lincoln, Y. S., & Guba, E. G. (1985). Naturalistic inquiry. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
pathway between Facebook interaction and psychological distress.
Lindolf, T. R, & Taylor, B. C (2011). Qualitative communication research methods (3rd
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 728–734. http://
ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0272.
Mao, J. (2014). Social media for learning: A mixed methods study on high school
Chou, H. G., & Edge, N. (2012). ‘‘They are happier and having better lives than I am:’’
students’ technology affordances and perspectives. Computers in Human
The impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’ lives. Cyberpsychology,
Behavior, 33, 213–223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.002.
Behavior, and Social Networking, 15, 117–121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/
Marshall, T. C. (2012). Facebook surveillance of former romantic partners:
cyber.2011.0324.
Associations with postbreakup recovery and personal growth.
Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and
Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 15, 521–526. http://
procedures for developing grounded theory (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0125.
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communication. Public Opinion
Masur, P. K., Reinecke, L., Ziegele, M., & Quiring, O. (2014). The interplay of intrinsic
Quarterly, 47, 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/268763.
need satisfaction and Facebook specific motives in explaining addictive
DeAndrea, D. C., & Walther, J. B. (2011). Attributions for inconsistencies between
behavior on Facebook. Computers in Human Behavior, 39, 376–386. http://
online and offline self-presentations. Communication Research, 38, 805–825.
dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.05.047.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0093650210385340.
McEwan, B. (2013). Sharing, caring, and surveilling: An actor–partner
Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook ‘‘friends:’’
interdependence model examination of Facebook relational maintenance
Social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of
strategies. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 863–869.
Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1143–1168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0717.
j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x.
Miles, M. B., & Huberman, M. (1994). Qualitative data analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA:
Facebook (2014). Our mission <http://newsroom.fb.com/company-info/> Retrieved
Sage.
14.09.14.
Nabi, R. L., Prestin, A., & So, J. (2013). Facebook friends with (health) benefits?
Feinstein, B. A., Hershenberg, R., Bhatia, V., Latack, J. A., Meuwly, N., & Davila, J.
Exploring social network site use and perceptions of social support, stress, and
(2013). Negative social comparison on Facebook and depressive symptoms:
well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 721–727.
Rumination as a mechanism. Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 2, 161–170.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0521.
Fox, J. (in press). The dark side of social networking sites in romantic relationships.
Pang, A. S. K. (2013). The distraction addiction. New York: Little, Brown.
In B. K. Wiederhold, G. Riva, and P. Cipresso (Eds.), The psychology of social
Park, N., Kee, K. F., & Valenzuela, S. (2009). Being immersed in social networking
networking: Communication, presence, identity, and relationships in online
environment: Facebook groups, uses and gratifications, and social outcomes.
communities. Versita.
Cyberpsychology and Behavior, 12, 729–733. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/
Fox, J., & Anderegg, C. (2014). Romantic relationship stages and behavior on social
cpb.2009.0003.
networking sites: Uncertainty reduction strategies and perceived norms on
Petronio, S. (1991). Communication boundary management: A theoretical model of
Facebook. CyberPsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 685–691. http://
managing disclosure of private information between marital couples.
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2014.0232.
Communication Theory, 1, 311–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2885.
Fox, J., Jones, E. B., & Lookadoo, K. (2013). Romantic relationship dissolution on
1991.tb00023.x.
social networking sites: Social support, coping, and rituals on Facebook. Paper
Pew Research Center (2014). 6 new facts about Facebook <http://
presented at the 63rd Annual Conference of the International Communication
www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2014/02/03/6-new-facts-about-facebook/>.
Association, London, UK.
Przybylski, A. K., Murayama, K., DeHaan, C. R., & Gladwell, V. (2013). Motivational,
Fox, J., Osborn, J. L., & Warber, K. M. (2014). Relational dialectics and social
emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Computers in Human
networking sites: The role of Facebook in romantic relationship escalation,
Behavior, 29, 1841–1848. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014.
maintenance, conflict, and dissolution. Computers in Human Behavior, 35,
Reich, S. M., Subrahmanyam, K., & Espinoza, G. (2012). Friending, IMing, and
527–534. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.02.031.
hanging out face-to-face: Overlap in adolescents’ online and offline social
Fox, J., & Warber, K. M. (2013). Romantic relationship development in the age of
networks. Developmental Psychology, 48, 356–368. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Facebook: An exploratory study of emerging adults’ perceptions, motives, and
a0026980.
behaviors. CyberPsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 16, 3–7. http://
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0288.
attribution process. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 10, 173–220.
Fox, J., & Warber, K. M. (2014). Social networking sites in romantic relationships:
Shelton, A. K., & Skalski, P. (2014). Blinded by the light: Illuminating the dark side of
Attachment, uncertainty, and partner surveillance on Facebook.
social network use through content analysis. Computers in Human Behavior, 33,
CyberPsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 17, 3–7. http://dx.doi.org/
339–348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.08.017.
10.1089/cyber.2012.0667.
Sutcliffe, A. G., Gonzalez, V., Binder, J., & Nevarez, G. (2011). Social mediating
Fox, J., & Warber, K. M. (in press). Queer identity management and political self-
technologies: Social affordances and functionalities. International Journal of
expression on social networking sites: A co-cultural approach to the spiral of
Human-Computer Interaction, 27, 1037–1065. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
silence. Journal of Communication.
10447318.2011.555318.
Fox, J., Warber, K. M., & Makstaller, D. C. (2013). The role of Facebook in romantic
Tokunaga, R. S. (2014). Relational transgressions on social networking sites:
relationship development: An exploration of Knapp’s relational stage model.
Individual, interpersonal, and contextual explanations for dyadic strain and
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 30, 772–795. http://dx.doi.org/
communication rules change. Computers in Human Behavior, 39, 287–295.
10.1177/0265407512468370.
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.024.
Gentile, B., Twenge, J. M., Freeman, E. C., & Campbell, W. K. (2012). The effect of
Toma, C. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2013). Self-affirmation underlies Facebook use.
social networking websites on positive self-views: An experimental
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39, 321–331. http://dx.doi.org/
investigation. Computers in Human Behavior, 28, 1929–1933. http://dx.doi.org/
10.1177/0146167212474694.
10.1016/j.chb.2012.05.012.
Treem, J., & Leonardi, P. (2012). Social media use in organizations: Exploring the
Gonzales, A. L. (2014). Text-based communication influences self-esteem more than
affordances of visibility, editability, persistence, and association. Communication
face-to-face or cellphone communication. Computers in Human Behavior, 39,
Yearbook, 36, 143–189.
197–203. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.07.026.
Turkle, S. (2011). Alone together: Why we expect more from technology and less from
Gonzales, A. L., & Hancock, J. T. (2011). Mirror, mirror on my Facebook wall: Effects
each other. New York: Basic Books. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
of exposure to Facebook on self-esteem. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
13691180801999050.
Networking, 14, 79–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0411.
176 J. Fox, J.J. Moreland / Computers in Human Behavior 45 (2015) 168–176

Utz, S., & Beukeboom, C. J. (2011). The role of social network sites in romantic Vaughn, S., Schumm, J. S., & Sinagub, J. (1996). Focus group interviews in education
relationships: Effects on jealousy and relationship happiness. Journal of and psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Computer-Mediated Communication, 16, 511–527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/ Walther, J. B. (1996). Computer-mediated communication: Impersonal,
j.1083-6101.2011.01552.x. interpersonal, and hyperpersonal interaction. Communication Research, 23,
Valenzuela, S., Park, N., & Kee, K. F. (2008). Is there social capital in a social network 3–43. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365096023001001.
site?: Facebook use and college students’ life satisfaction, trust, and Zhao, S., Grasmuck, S., & Martin, J. (2008). Identity construction on Facebook: Digital
participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 14, 875–901. empowerment in anchored relationships. Computers in Human Behavior, 24,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1083-6101.2009.01474.x. 1816–1836. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.02.012.

Potrebbero piacerti anche