Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 2020 DOI: 10.1111/sjop.

12629

Personality and Social Psychology


Who shows which kind of humor? Exploring sociodemographic
differences in eight comic styles in a large Chilean sample
 MENDIBURO-SEGUEL1
ANDRES and SONJA HEINTZ2
1
Faculty of Education and Social Sciences, Andres Bello University, Santiago, Chile
2
Department of Psychology, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland

Mendiburo-Seguel, A. & Heintz, S. (2020). Who shows which kind of humor? Exploring sociodemographic differences in eight comic styles in a large
Chilean sample. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology.

The present study investigates mean-level differences in eight comic styles (fun, benevolent humor, nonsense, wit, irony, satire, sarcasm, and cynicism)
depending on the sociodemographic variables gender, age, education, political spectrum, and religious affiliation. A large and varied Chilean adult sample
(N = 1,272, 60.1% women; age M = 39.94, SD = 17.33) was recruited in face-to-face interviews and online testing. They completed self-reports of the
comic styles (the Comic Style Markers) and sociodemographic variables. Overall, small, but meaningful, differences in comic styles were found for the
different sociodemographic groups. Men scored higher than women in all comic styles except for benevolent humor, and six styles decreased with age.
Having lower education was associated with more cynicism, while the reverse pattern was found for satire and wit. Religious people showed lower scores
in four styles than non-religious people, and cynicism was higher in those identifying with left wing rather than right-wing attitudes. The present study thus
provides insights into who is more likely to engage in different kinds of humor in their everyday lives.
Key words: Humor, comic styles, individual differences, sociodemographics.
Andres Mendiburo-Seguel, Andres Bello University, Fernandez Concha 700, Las Condes, Metropolitan Region, Santiago, Chile. Tel: +56999497069;
e-mail: andres.mendiburo@unab.cl

INTRODUCTION Comic styles and comic style markers development


Humor is a ubiquitous phenomenon in our everyday life, and we The Comic Style Markers (CSM; Ruch et al., 2018a) attempt to
frequently experience how people differ in the kind of humor they complement further humor-related styles. The eight comic styles
prefer and show. Consequently, research on individual differences can be differentiated by being either “darker” or “lighter.” Darker
in humor comprises approaches from different points of view, for styles gather around the mockery family, and include sarcasm
example, related to its creation, its understanding, its psychological, (hurting others), cynicism (depreciating commonly acknowledged
physical, or social effects, and its manifestations (Martin & Ford, values, using mockery to highlight the weaknesses in the world
2018; Ruch, 2007). The study of styles is one of the main and disdaining any moral concept, which are considered
approaches when investigating humor as a trait (Craik, Lampert & ridiculous), satire (similar to sarcasm and cynicism in being
Nelson, 1996; Martin, Puhlik-Doris, Larsen, Gray & Weir, 2003; aggressive as well, but combined with attempts of kindness and
Ruch & Heintz, 2016; Ruch, Heintz, Platt, Wagner & Proyer, correcting others), and irony (which aims to create a mutual
2018a). The most recent approach, the Comic Style Markers (Ruch sense of superiority by stating things in ways that are different to
et al., 2018a), provides a promising approach to investigate their meaning, thus confusing those that are not “part of the
everyday humorous conduct in more detail by distinguishing eight group”).
comic styles (fun, benevolent humor, nonsense, wit, irony, satire, Lighter styles comprise a family that does not imply aggressive
sarcasm, and cynicism). They also provided initial elements and instead relates to benign and affiliative affect,
sociodemographic analyses in terms of gender, age, education, behaviors, cognitions, and goals. They include fun (promoting
nation, family status, and housing status. The present study extends good mood and camaraderie through joviality and lively and
these analyses by: (1) providing a more varied sample regarding harmless jokes), humor (showing understanding for the defects of
sociodemographic background; (2) investigating a new culture and the world and human mistakes, with a realistic, yet benevolent
language (Spanish-speaking Chileans); and (3) adding differences look), nonsense (presenting the ridiculous aspects of things that
in the political spectrum and religious denomination. do not make sense, enjoying contradictions, and considering
Styles are understood as features that predispose people to absurdities the funniest), and wit (enlightening through quick and
habitually behave in a certain way (Buss & Finn, 1987; Ruch & appropriate remarks and being able to establish links between
Heintz, 2016), which makes humor styles typical ways in which a ideas or thoughts).
person shows humor. The most comprehensive approach to
humor-related styles was proposed by Craik et al. (1996), who
distinguished five bipolar styles of everyday humorous conduct, Humor, sociodemographics, and attitudes
while the most widely used approach is based on Martin et al. Many studies report gender and age differences in humor, while
(2003), who distinguish four styles that capture well-being related only a few studies focused on other relevant variables, such as
functions of humor. education and political and religious attitudes. Regarding gender

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
2 A. Mendiburo-Seguel and S. Heintz Scand J Psychol (2020)

differences in humor, men were found to appreciate humor with participants with an apprenticeship (with a large effect). We thus
sexual contents more than women did (for a review, see Lampert expect differences in the education level mainly for wit, while
& Ervin-Tripp, 2007). Furthermore, men described themselves to the differences in the other comic styles are investigated
exhibit more aggressive humor (Martin et al., 2003) as well as exploratorily.
darker comic styles (i.e., irony, satire, sarcasm, and cynicism; While not much research has been conducted regarding the
Ruch et al., 2018a). These differences are in line with findings on political spectrum and humor, there have been a few studies that
gender differences in general traits (see Eagly & Wood, 2013; focused on the liberal-conservative continuum. For example,
Hyde, 2014) that consistently pointed to men having higher according to Ruch and Hehl (1988), the enjoyment of
scores in traits related to agency (e.g., aggression, risk-taking) and incongruity-resolution humor (i.e., humor with a clear punchline
women having higher scores in traits related to prosociality (e.g., that can be understood to solve the incongruity) is characterized
empathy, tender-mindedness). Furthermore, men were found to by conservative attitudes and conventional values, while nonsense
have higher scores than women in fun, nonsense, and wit (Ruch and incongruity are more likely to be enjoyed by people high in
et al., 2018a). In this study, we expect that men score higher than openness to experience and liberal attitudes. Also, the capacity to
women in all comic styles except for benevolent humor, and that enjoy humor has been observed to correlate negatively with right-
these effects are small in line with previous studies. wing authoritarism and a conservative orientation (Bonanno &
Regarding age differences in humor, studies tended to focus on Jost, 2006). We thus expect that people that position themselves
changes and correlations between age and behaviorally assessed on the right side of the left-right political spectrum would score
humor appreciation, comprehension, and creation (using joke and lower in nonsense, which entails playing with diverse and
cartoon tests), and less on how people describe their typical conflicting ideas and appreciating them.
everyday humorous activities (Martin & Ford, 2018). In general In terms of religiosity, Schweizer and Ott (2016) observed only
terms, it has been observed that humor production decreases, few correlations between Christian faith and humor appreciation.
perceived funniness increases, and humor comprehension does When practicing Christians were compared to atheists, the latter
not change with age (for a review, see Greengross, 2013). appreciated blasphemous and disparaging humor more and good-
Regarding the eight comic styles, Ruch et al. (2018a) observed in natured religious jokes less. Similarly, Ott and Schweizer (2018)
their samples of mostly young to middle-aged adults that irony, found that Christians were the least amused by jokes when
sarcasm, and cynicism tended to decrease with age, while compared to Muslims, Hindus, Atheists and Agnostics. Agnostics
benevolent humor, nonsense and wit tended to increase (all small were also the least likely to be offended. Regarding the eight
effects). These findings fit to the typically observed patterns of comic styles, Ruch et al. (2018a) observed that the character
increases of socially desirable traits (such as agreeableness, strength religiousness/spirituality was positively related to fun and
extraversion, and conscientiousness) in young adulthood followed benevolent humor, but correlated negatively with sarcasm and
by a plateau in middle adulthood (Costa, McCrae & L€ ockenhoff, cynicism. Overall, people with no religious affiliation are
2019). The variance in traits was found to increase in older age, expected to score higher in the mockery-related styles of sarcasm
which was linked to declines in physical and cognitive states for and cynicism than people with a religious denomination, as they
some participants (e.g., M~ottus, Johnson & Deary, 2012). As our might be more likely to openly voice criticism.
sample is more varied in terms of age and also includes older
adults, we investigate non-linear trends in the comic styles. We
expect increases in benevolent humor, nonsense and wit and Aims of the present study
decreases in irony, sarcasm, and cynicism from young to middle Most studies are limited by employing convenience and non-
adulthood. Changes from middle to older adulthood are representative samples. Specifically, women, younger participants,
investigated exploratorily as the comic styles might show and participants with a good education are usually
complex patterns in these age groups. overrepresented (as was the case in the study by Ruch et al.,
In terms of education, it can be expected that the 2018a), which limits the power and generalizability with which
sophistication of the comic styles will influence differences in sociodemographic differences in comic styles can be determined.
education level. Two prominent hypotheses have been put The present study thus extends the initial findings on the CSM by
forward: The cognitive congruency principle states that humor is investigating it in a more representative Chilean sample that
best comprehended and enjoyed if it matches one’s cognitive varied in terms of demographic variables (gender, age, and
level (e.g., McGhee, 1976); that is, it should neither be too education) as well as the political spectrum and religious
challenging nor too simple. The “expert skill hypothesis” states affiliation.
that a higher difficulty of comprehension is negatively related to
humor appreciation (Cunningham & Derks, 2005). These
METHODOLOGY
approaches would imply that more sophisticated comic styles
(i.e., benevolent humor, nonsense, wit, irony, and satire) would
Participants
be more often shown by participants with higher in comparison
to lower education. The simpler comic styles (fun, sarcasm, and In total, 1,272 people from Santiago de Chile took part on the study, all of
whom were selected through online and face-to-face sampling. Two
cynicism) would either show no education differences or would
subsamples of these participants were included by Mendiburo-Seguel and
be more enjoyed by participants with lower education levels. Heintz (2019) and Mendiburo-Seguel and Ford (2019); however, none of
Along these lines, Ruch et al. (2018a) found that wit was higher the analyses presented here have been published before, and the studies
in participants with a doctoral degree in comparison to had different aims (Table 1).

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol (2020) Sociodemographics and comic styles 3

Table 1. Sample description and cynicism (e.g., “I tend not to trust the sincerity of some intentions and
values, and often unmask them by cynical remarks”). People are asked to
Demographic variables n % Age (M) Age (SD) apply each statement to the way they experience and express humor and
answer them on a seven-point scale in which 1 means “strongly disagree,”
Mode of data collection 4 means “neither disagree nor agree,” and 7 means “strongly agree.”
Online 304 23.9 36.08 12.99 The CSM was translated from English to Spanish following the
Face-to-face 968 76.1 41.17 18.34 recommendations by Van de Vijver and Hambleton (1996) considering
Gender translation and back-translation. Two linguists helped with the adaptation
Women 757 60.1 39.41 17.03 of items to the Chilean culture. As shown in Table 2, the internal
Men 503 39.9 40.74 17.80 consistency (Cronbach’s alpha) for the Spanish version of the markers was
Educational level satisfactory. Mendiburo-Seguel and Heintz (2019) provided evidence of
Incomplete primary education 22 1.8 69.19 11.04 the construct validity of the Chilean version of the CSM, as similar
Complete primary education 29 2.3 53.33 19.45 correlations to other constructs (such as happiness) were found as in
Incomplete secondary education 62 5.0 49.07 21.53 previous studies using the original German version of the CSM (Ruch,
Complete secondary education 221 17.7 41.23 18.57 Wagner & Heintz, 2018b).
Incomplete technical education 52 4.2 38.64 14.68
Complete technical education 184 14.7 43.01 14.29
Incomplete university education 291 23.3 27.47 11.60 Procedure
Complete university education 332 26.6 42.70 15.45
Master, Doctorate, or equivalent 56 4.5 43.51 14.03 The face-to-face sample consisted of 968 people, all of which were
Religion (Yes/No) interviewed by trained pollsters at their houses through a multi-staged
Yes 683 54.4 43.72 17.59 sampling method considering cluster sampling (randomly selected blocks
No 573 45.6 35.46 15.95 from Santiago) to assure the representation of different profiles regarding
Political positioning socioeconomic status and a systematic sampling of houses on each of the
Extreme left 142 16.5 41.69 18.19 selected squares. The online sample consisted of 304 people who were
Left 181 21.0 34.77 15.30 contacted through an online panel provider and were paid for their
Center 388 45.1 40.47 18.83 participation.
Right 101 11.7 44.22 17.56 The study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of
Extreme right 49 5.7 49.76 19.80 the ethics committee of the Faculty of the first author’s University. People
Total 1,272 100.0 39.94 17.33 in the face-to-face sample were read an informed consent by the pollster,
containing the main aims of the study and asked to sign a sheet with their
Note: Sums within the sociodemographic variables can be <1,272 due to name in which it was stated that they read and understood the information.
missing values. People in the online sample were presented with the same information at
the beginning of the study and were asked to select “Yes” after the
question “After reading this information, do you agree to participate?” if
they agreed to participate in the study.
Instruments
Political spectrum. We used the following item from the Latin American
Public Opinion Project (LAPOP) “Americas Barometer” survey (Latin Data analysis
American Public Opinion Project, 2017): “Nowadays, when we speak of To establish the factorial validity of the Chilean CSM, confirmatory
political leanings, we talk of those leftists and those rightists. In other factorial analysis (CFA) was run considering recommendations of fit by
words, some people sympathize more with the left and others with the Schermelleh-Engel, Moosbrugger and M€ uller (2003) using the R package
right. According to the meaning that the terms “left” and “right” have for (R Core Team, 2019); lavaan (Rosseel, 2012). To correct for multiple
you, and thinking of your own political leanings, where would you place comparisons across the eight comic styles, a more conservative alpha level
yourself on this scale?” Response options varied from 1 (left) to 10 (right). of p < 0.006 (0.05/8) was chosen for all results. Gender differences and
As the cell sizes for each of the 10-point scale was heterogeneous, we differences in religious affiliation were investigated using independent
followed the same procedure as in the LAPOP (Zechmeister & Corral, t-tests, using Cohen’s d as the effect size (small |0.20|–|0.49|, medium
2010) and collapsed them into five categories of political positioning |0.50|–|0.79|, large>|0.80|) Differences in education and the political
(extreme left, left, center, right, extreme right). spectrum were investigated using MANOVAs and each of the comic
styles as criterion variables. Effect sizes for the MANOVAs were g2p
Religious affiliation. People were asked if they were part of a religion,
with options being “Catholicism,” “Orthodoxy,” “Protestantism,”
“Jehovah’s Witnesses,” “Islam,” “Judaism,” “Mormonism,” or “None/ Table 2. Means, standard deviations, reliability and confirmatory factor
Agnosticism/Atheism.” As most participants with a religious affiliation analysis results
indicated “Catholicism” (71.0%), a comparison among the different
denominations was not considered meaningful. We thus collapsed all Comic FL FL CITC CITC
participants who indicated a religious denomination into the religious styles M SD (Min) (Max) (Min) (Max) a
group, and all participants who indicated “None/Agnosticism/Atheism”
into the non-religious group. Fun 4.53 1.46 0.63 0.81 0.58 0.75 0.87
The Comic Style Markers (Ruch et al., 2018a) is a set of 48 items that Humor 5.05 1.09 0.36 0.65 0.30 0.54 0.72
measure eight different comic styles: Fun (e.g., “I am a funny joker”), Nonsense 4.61 1.54 0.52 0.83 0.47 0.76 0.87
benevolent humor (e.g., “I am a realistic observer of human weaknesses, Wit 4.54 1.34 0.66 0.77 0.61 0.70 0.87
and my good-natured humor treats them benevolently”), nonsense (e.g., Irony 4.24 1.35 0.52 0.77 0.46 0.65 0.80
“I like nonsensical humor”), wit (e.g., “I have the ability to tell something Satire 3.89 1.44 0.55 0.77 0.52 0.67 0.83
witty and to the point”), irony (e.g., “I can talk with close friends in a way Sarcasm 3.51 1.46 0.50 0.83 0.46 0.72 0.81
that only we would know what is the meaning of what we’re saying, while Cynicism 3.12 1.43 0.50 0.78 0.43 0.68 0.83
outsiders won’t sense it is merely irony”), satire (e.g., “I have a critical
attitude toward arrogant and unfair people, and my mockery serves to Note: FL = factor loadings, CITC = corrected item-total correlations,
establish equality and justice”), sarcasm (e.g., “Biting mockery suits me”), a = Cronbach’s alpha.

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
4 A. Mendiburo-Seguel and S. Heintz Scand J Psychol (2020)

(small 0.010–0.059, medium 0.60–0.139, large >0.140). To determine the cynicism declined with age (all small effects). Sarcasm
location of the effect, we first computed contrasts of each individual group (DR2 = 0.009, total effect R = 0.33, R2 = 0.107) and cynicism
with the mean of all other groups. Significant groups were then dummy
(DR2 = 0.012, total effect R = 0.23, R2 = 0.052) also had a
coded (group = 1, all other groups = 2) and compared using independent
t-tests. As the relationships between age and the different comic styles significant quadratic trend in addition to the linear trend. These
might not be linear, we tested linear, quadratic, and cubic functions using trends indicated that sarcasm decreased from 18 until around
the SPSS curvefit function, which were sequentially added in multiple 70 years and then tended to increase again, while cynicism
regression models. Effect sizes for the regressions were R2 (small 0.020– decreased until 60 years and then increased again; however, the
0.129, medium 0.130–0.259, large >0.260). Finally, to determine which
effect sizes of the quadratic trends were negligible. Finally, wit
sociodemographic variables explain unique variance in the comic styles,
we added all significant variables as predictors in standard multiple only had a significant cubic trend (R = 0.11, R2 = 0.012). This
regression analyses. All effect size guidelines are based on Cohen (1992). trend indicated that wit increased from 18 to 40 years, then
decreased until 70 years, and then increased again; however, the
effect size of this cubic trend was negligible.
RESULTS
Education. Comparisons between the nine educational groups
Internal consistency and factorial validity of the comic style were made using MANOVAs, with the comic styles as criteria.
markers in Spanish There were significant differences regarding education for irony,
satire, sarcasm, and cynicism. The education trends for all comic
The CFA showed an acceptable fit, v2(1052) = 3850.80
styles are shown in Table 4. Planned contrasts were used to
(p < 0.001), CFI = 0.90, RMSEA = 0.047 (90% confidence
compare each education level with the mean of all other levels
interval [0.045–0.048]), and SRMR = 0.046. Factor loadings for
to determine the location of the effect. This contrast revealed
each factor ranged between 0.36 and 0.83 (see Table 2), and
significant differences between participants with incomplete
factor correlations ranged from 0.30 (between humor and
primary education those with incomplete university education in
cynicism) to 0.79 (irony and sarcasm).
comparison to all other groups for the four comic styles. Follow-
up independent t-tests comparing these groups to all other
Intercorrelations of the scales groups showed that participants with incomplete primary
education had lower scores in irony (d = 0.18), satire
Table 3 shows the intercorrelations among the comic styles in the
(d = 0.25), sarcasm (d = 0.19), and cynicism (d = 0.20),
Spanish version. All of the correlations were statistically
while those with incomplete university education had higher
significant at the 0.006 level, and all of them were positive. As in
scores in these four comic styles (d = 0.13, 0.17, 0.26, 0.32,
the construction article (Ruch et al., 2018a), the highest
respectively). These education effects were thus negligible to
correlation was found between sarcasm and cynicism and satire
small.
(0.67 each), and the lowest correlations were found between
humor and sarcasm (0.31) and cynicism (0.23). Religion. Participants without religious affiliation had higher
scores than those with an affiliation in nonsense (d = 0.27;
Mnoaffiliation = 4.79, DT = 1.49; Mwithaffiliation = 4.43, DT = 1.57),
Comic style markers and demographic variables irony (d = 0.20; Mnoaffiliation = 4.32, DT = 1.27; Mwithaffiliation =
Gender. Gender comparisons revealed that men had significantly 4.12, DT = 1.40), satire (d = 0.25; Mnoaffiliation = 4.04, DT = 1.39;
higher scores than women in nonsense, satire, sarcasm, and Mwithaffiliation = 3.70, DT = 1.45), sarcasm (d = 0.39; Mnoaffiliation =
cynicism. Thus, five of the eight comic styles showed gender 3.75, DT = 1.35; Mwithaffiliation = 3.26, DT = 1.51), and cynicism
differences, of which three were small and two were negligible. (d = 0.49; Mnoaffiliation = 3.43, DT = 1.44; Mwithaffiliation = 2.81, DT
= 1.34). These differences in religious denominations were small.
Age. No significant age effects were found for benevolent humor
(all ps > 0.012). Significant linear trends were found for fun Political orientation. Comparisons between different political
(R = 0.22, R2 = 0.049), nonsense (R = 0.21, R2 = 0.044), positions were made using MANOVAs with the comic styles as
irony (R = 0.26, R2 = 0.066), satire (R = 0.21, R2 = 0.043), criteria. There was one significant difference regarding cynicism.
sarcasm (R = 0.31, R2 = 0.098), and cynicism (R = 0.20, Planned contrasts compared each political group with the mean of
R2 = 0.040). This showed that fun, nonsense, satire, sarcasm, and all the other groups to determine the location of the effect. This

Table 3. Intercorrelations of the Spanish version of the comic style markers

Comic styles Fun Humor Nonsense Wit Irony Satire Sarcasm

Humor 0.50*
Nonsense 0.44* 0.45*
Wit 0.61* 0.55* 0.39*
Irony 0.49* 0.43* 0.46* 0.55*
Satire 0.44* 0.44* 0.38* 0.49* 0.59*
Sarcasm 0.42* 0.31* 0.39* 0.41* 0.64* 0.67*
Cynicism 0.30* 0.23* 0.36* 0.33* 0.53* 0.60* 0.67*

Notes: N = 1,225–1,228. *p < 0.006.

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol (2020) Sociodemographics and comic styles 5

Table 4. Comic style means and standard deviations in different demographic groups

Fun Humor Nonsense Wit Irony Satire Sarcasm Cynicism

Variable No M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD M SD

Gender 1 4.45 1.50 5.04 1.12 4.47 1.58 4.44 1.40 4.15 1.37 3.80 1.49 3.40 1.49 2.95 1.42
2 4.66 1.40 5.09 1.06 4.81 1.47 4.70 1.23 4.36 1.31 4.03 1.36 3.68 1.40 3.37 1.41
t (df) 2.45 (1214) 0.83 (1214) 3.74 (1214) 3.49 (1112) 2.65 (1214) 2.85 (1083) 3.33 (1214) 5.03 (1213)
d 0.14 0.05 0.21* 0.21* 0.16 0.17* 0.19* 0.29*
Education 3 4.49 2.12 5.08 1.45 4.46 2.02 4.54 2.15 3.28 1.39 2.46 1.33 2.42 1.53 1.96 1.16
4 4.70 1.56 4.70 1.53 4.08 1.69 4.49 1.57 3.69 1.27 3.34 1.35 2.77 1.20 2.64 1.06
5 4.46 1.77 5.30 1.13 4.91 1.50 4.37 1.60 4.10 1.79 3.94 1.68 3.40 1.60 3.46 1.68
6 4.61 1.54 4.98 1.15 4.45 1.56 4.40 1.47 4.06 1.40 3.78 1.46 3.50 1.48 2.91 1.42
7 4.71 1.44 5.26 1.06 4.70 1.29 4.75 1.01 4.61 1.18 4.17 1.19 3.50 1.38 3.27 1.24
8 4.59 1.53 5.08 1.06 4.63 1.63 4.60 1.31 4.20 1.38 3.86 1.49 3.31 1.55 2.97 1.40
9 4.76 1.25 5.14 0.94 4.90 1.44 4.46 1.17 4.39 1.16 4.12 1.34 3.85 1.40 3.52 1.43
10 4.31 1.41 4.99 1.13 4.43 1.51 4.63 1.31 4.26 1.36 3.87 1.43 3.53 1.41 2.98 1.36
11 4.31 1.40 5.09 1.04 4.67 1.66 5.06 1.31 4.54 1.24 3.98 1.50 3.34 1.35 3.22 1.50
F (df) 2.17 (8, 1193) 1.44 (8, 1193) 2.88 (8, 1193) 1.97 (8, 1193) 3.67 (8, 1193) 4.33 (8,1193) 4.95 (8,1193) 6.85 (8,1193)
g2p 0.014 0.010 0.019* 0.013 0.024* 0.028* 0.032* 0.044*
Politics 12 4.48 1.55 5.00 1.05 4.67 1.68 4.57 1.38 4.24 1.30 4.02 1.45 3.70 1.37 3.49 1.56
13 4.55 1.41 5.12 0.96 4.79 1.48 4.40 1.33 4.29 1.22 4.06 1.31 3.79 1.22 3.66 1.39
14 4.57 1.52 4.98 1.11 4.49 1.57 4.45 1.33 4.20 1.38 3.78 1.47 3.57 1.45 3.17 1.37
15 4.59 1.47 4.98 1.06 4.59 1.44 4.61 1.24 4.26 1.33 3.84 1.29 3.53 1.47 3.12 1.33
16 4.18 1.62 4.60 1.13 4.21 1.69 4.33 1.53 4.00 1.58 3.48 1.49 3.21 1.50 2.86 1.56
F (df) 0.87 (4, 855) 2.32 (4, 855) 1.86 (4, 855) 0.75 (4, 855) 0.49 (4, 855) 2.63 (4, 855) 2.04 (4, 855) 5.83 (4, 855)
g2p 0.004 0.011 0.009 0.003 0.002 0.012 0.009 0.027*

Notes: 1 = women, 2 = men, 3 = incomplete primary, 4 = complete primary, 5 = incomplete secondary, 6 = complete secondary, 7 = incomplete
technical, 8 = complete technical, 9 = incomplete university, 10 = complete university, 11 = Master’s/doctoral degree, 12 = extreme left, 13 = left,
14 = center, 15 = right, 16 = extreme right. *p < 0.006.

contrast revealed significant differences between participants from though factorial validity, specifically the model’s obtained CFI,
the left political spectrum in comparison to all other political was slightly lower than the cut-off (0.90), this could be
groups. A follow-up independent t-test showed that participants interpreted as the result of the number of variables on each factor,
from the left political spectrum had higher scores in cynicism which could lead to a CFI below 0.95 even when having correct
compared to all other groups (d = 0.32), revealing a small effect. models (Kenny & McCoach, 2003). It has also been proposed
that a CFI of 0.90 is the lower acceptable value (Hu & Bentler,
1999).
Demographic variables as predictors of comic styles At a descriptive level, the markers for each style were very
Standard multiple regression analyses were run with all significant similar to the German and English versions: Benevolent humor
sociodemographic variables as predictors of each comic style. had the highest score, while sarcasm and cynicism had the lowest
Education and political spectrum were considered as dummy scores. In other words, people tended to score higher on the
variables based on the groups that had significant effects. No lighter styles and lower on the darker styles. Also, the comic
analyses were conducted for benevolent humor, as no styles correlations were similar as in previous studies, although
sociodemographic differences were found for this comic style, benevolent humor had positive correlations, instead of
and for fun, as age was the only significant predictor. As can be correlations around zero, with cynicism and sarcasm. This
seen in Table 5, the overlap was small for all comic styles, with indicates that people involved in humorous patterns tended to
the demographic variables explaining 1.2% (wit) to 11.1% engage in a wide range of styles (at least eight, in this case) or
(sarcasm) of the variance. Gender (men scoring higher) and age tended to not engage in these styles.
(comic styles declining with age) were the most relevant
sociodemographic aspects, which were significant for five of the
six comic styles. Satire was also predicted by having more than Comic styles and demographics
incomplete primary education, sarcasm and cynicism were When referring to the relationship of the comic styles with
predicted by not having a religious denomination, and cynicism demographic variables, the most novel contribution of the present
was predicted by being on the left political spectrum. Thus, each research should be noted: The consideration of a large sample that
comic style had a unique “sociodemographic signature.” is able to represent different groups, with most of the data (76%)
obtained through face-to-face surveys at the participants’ houses.
This way, it was possible to enhance conclusions regarding
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS sociodemographic differences (such as the educational level) in
Our results are consistent with those obtained by Ruch et al. the comic styles–one of the limitations described by Ruch et al.
(2018a) regarding the validity and reliability of the CSM. Even (2018a) in their study.

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
6 A. Mendiburo-Seguel and S. Heintz Scand J Psychol (2020)

Table 5. Multiple regressions (standardized beta weights) predicting the comic styles with the sociodemographic variables

Demographics Nonsense Wit Irony Satire Sarcasm Cynicism

Gender (1 = women, 2 = men) 0.11* 0.10* – 0.09* 0.09* 0.13*


Age 0.20* 0.07 0.26* 0.18* 0.29* 0.13*
Incomplete primary education (=1, others = 2) – – 0.04 0.10* 0.04 0.06
Incomplete secondary education (=1, others = 2) – – 0.05 0.01 0.01 0.06
No religious denomination (=1, others = 2) 0.06 – 0.02 0.05 0.08* 0.14*
Left political spectrum (=1, others = 2) – – – – – 0.11*
R 0.25* 0.12* 0.26* 0.25* 0.34* 0.33*
R2adj 0.059 0.012 0.066 0.060 0.111 0.101

Note: *p < 0.006.

Considering the above, the styles were related just as expected range of groups, even considering people that did not finish
to other variables such as gender and age, although some findings primary school. Indeed, we found small differences between the
must be pointed out. Consistent with Ruch et al.’s (2018a) educational groups regarding irony, satire, sarcasm, and cynicism,
observations, men tended to score higher in all comic styles with the lowest educated group (incomplete primary education)
(small effects), with the exception of benevolent humor. scoring lower than all other groups, and one of the best educated
Furthermore, the large age range in the present study allowed groups (incomplete university education; i.e., current university
fine-grained investigations of age differences in the comic styles students) scored higher than all other groups. Thus, against our
across the full adult life span. In line with previous findings, fun, expectations, wit did not show education differences, and the
irony, satire, sarcasm and cynicism were negatively related to age division was rather between lighter and darker styles than more or
(small effects). In contrast to previous findings, age was not less sophisticated styles. These findings show the important
related to benevolent humor and was negatively related to interaction between structure (sophistication) and content
nonsense, rather than the positive age correlations found by Ruch (mockery), which seems to jointly influence not only humor
et al. (2018a) . Thus, a downward trend could be observed appreciation (Cunningham & Derks, 2005; Heintz, 2020), but also
regarding the darker styles, but not an upward trend regarding the everyday humorous conduct.
lighter styles. Furthermore, sarcasm and cynicism showed Sarcasm and cynicism are styles that are related to the criticism
significant quadratic age trends, which indicated that the decrease of moral values and standards. In line with this conceptualization
lasted until late adulthood (60–70 years), but that the two comic and our expectations, the results showed that agnostics and
styles increased afterwards. Wit also showed a complex age trend, atheists tended to show these two styles more frequently that
as it increased from 18 to 40 years (which is in line with Ruch, participants with a religious denomination, which is probably
Heintz et al.’s findings), but then decreased until 70 years, and related to a critical approach towards moral positions that derive
then increased again. This shows the importance of investigating from religion. Similar results were previously found with sarcasm
older adults, as linear patterns observed in young and middle and cynicism and the character strength spirituality/religiousness
adulthood might not hold for older age groups (see M~ottus et al., (Ruch et al., 2018a). The same pattern was found for nonsense,
2012). irony, and satire. Irony and satire also entail mocking others,
Even though it could be tempting to relate these outcomes to which might be less acceptable for people adhering to a religion.
cultural differences (which can, nevertheless, be part of the The finding for nonsense could be explained by a higher tolerance
explanation), it is worth mentioning that our sample was more for ambiguities in non-religious people, as people with higher
heterogeneous in terms of age. Five of the comic styles scores in this comic style appreciate incongruities, rather than
(especially the darker ones) tended to diminish with age, with the solving them (Ruch & Hehl, 1983). As religiousness was found to
largest effects observed for sarcasm. However, not only the less relate to being more intolerant towards ambiguities (Budner,
socially desirable styles decreased, but also fun and nonsense, and 1962), this trait might explain the higher scores in nonsense for
benevolent humor did not increase. In addition, the quadratic and agnostics and atheists in the present study. More generally, the
cubic age trends for wit, sarcasm, and cynicism indicate that age pattern suggests that non-religious people engage more in playful
differences in the comic styles might be more complex than and mockery-related humor in their everyday lives, which is also
previously known. Thus, longitudinal studies are needed to track in line with previous findings on humor appreciation (Ott &
the developmental pattern of comic styles changes throughout Schweizer, 2018; Schweizer & Ott, 2016). As we mostly obtained
adulthood. This would also allow testing to what extent the comic small effects, future studies could investigate religious practice in
styles are malleable by socialization processes (e.g., solving addition to religious denomination as well as spiritual practices
important tasks in life such as work and family; for an overview, and beliefs to delineate which aspects of religion, religiosity, and
see Specht et al., 2014). spirituality might drive these effects.
When Ruch et al. (2018a) pointed out the limitations implied in The comparisons of the five political groups only yielded one
their samples, they specifically referred to the fact that they significant finding for cynicism, not for nonsense as initially
worked with well-educated people. In their case, subjects that expected. People who identified as liberals or closer to the left
held a doctoral degree scored significantly higher on wit than engaged more in cynicism than people situated in the extreme
those with an apprenticeship. Our design allows studying a wider left, center, or right wing of the political spectrum. Cynicism and

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol (2020) Sociodemographics and comic styles 7

disparaging political humor more generally have been previously how to run the survey. Given that the present study showed
implied in trusting politicians less (e.g., Fieschi & Heywood, similarities in several psychometric properties of the comic styles
2004; Mendiburo-Seguel, Vargas & Rubio, 2017), though (e.g., factor structure, patterns of correlations between the scales,
previous research did not find differences in party affiliation for reliability) to previous studies (which mostly used online samples),
political cynicism (Agger, Goldstein & Pearl, 1961). Thus, the biases introduced by the pollster effect are however deemed to
investigating cynicism as a comic style in relationship to other be minimal. Second, in the case of religion it should be considered
political attitudes, such as populism (Rooduijn, van der Brug, de that self-reported affiliation does not capture magnitude. This means
Lange & Parlevliet, 2017), as well as its malleability by that how actively a person participates in a religion probably has an
environmental factors, such as election news (Schuck, effect on our variables of interest, but we are not able to determine
Boomgaarden & de Vreese, 2013) would be interesting avenues it in this case. Third, the study needs replications, as the findings
for further research and applications. The lack of support for might not generalize well across different cultural contexts. Still, it
nonsense could stem from our investigation of appreciating and is the most comprehensive study to date on sociodemographic
showing nonsense in everyday life, while previous studies differences in the comic styles and humor more generally and likely
investigated the appreciation of specific jokes and cartoons provides robust findings due to the large sample size and the varied
(Bonanno & Jost, 2006; Ruch & Hehl, 1988). background of the sample. Fourth, the present study cannot
The regression analyses revealed a unique “sociodemographic determine why sociodemographic differences occurred in the comic
signature” for each comic style. Benevolent humor was unrelated to styles. This requires qualitative, longitudinal, and experimental
sociodemographic variables, which indicates that everyone was studies, for which the present findings can provide an impetus. For
equally likely to engage in this comic style. Fun and irony were example, future studies could derive hypotheses from evolutionary
related to being younger, and wit was higher in males rather than theories (e.g., Li, Griskevicius, Durante, Jonason, Pasisz & Aumer,
females. Nonsense was higher in men and younger people. Satire 2009) or test methodological influences, such as social desirability
was higher in men, younger people, and those with at least (see e.g., Cann & Matson, 2014).
complete primary education. Sarcasm was higher in men, younger
people, and those without religious denominations. Finally,
cynicism was higher in men, younger people, those without CONCLUSIONS
religious denominations, and those adhering to the left political The present study contributed to the validation and nomological
spectrum. In total, the sociodemographic variables explained up to network of eight comic styles and of individual differences in
11.1% of the variance (small effect), which indicates that they are humor more generally. First, it supported the factorial validity of the
one, yet not the only, predictors of comic styles. This shows that Comic Style Markers in confirmatory factor analyses, showing that
albeit certain groups tend to engage more in certain styles, the styles the measure can be used in another language (Spanish) and cultural
are ubiquitous for a people with many different backgrounds. setting (Chile). Second, the eight comic styles showed differential
It should also be considered that culture may have a role in these relationships with a range of sociodemographic variables, including
results. A study by Mendiburo-Seguel, Paez, and Martınez-Zelaya gender, age, education, religious affiliation, and the political
(2017) correlated data from different countries of the Humor Styles spectrum. The present findings are especially valid and
Questionnaire (Martin et al., 2003) with cultural dimensions. generalizable given the large sample employed. Overall, the present
Considering 26 countries, it was observed that Chile was in the study shows that both research and applications of humor need to
second quartile regarding aggressive humor, fourth regarding take into account the multidimensionality underlying individual
affiliative humor, and first regarding self-enhancing and self- differences in humor by differentiating at least eight comic styles.
defeating humor styles. In this case, such a profile was related
to higher femininity (high self-defeating and high self-enhancing),
lower power distance (higher self-defeating), and higher FUNDING
uncertainty avoidance (low affiliative). This raises the question of This study was funded by Grant No. 11160661 from the Chilean
how (or if) generalizations to other countries can be made from the National Fund for Scientific and Technological Development
data obtained in Chile. Luckily, in the last years joint efforts by (FONDECYT de Iniciaci on, CONICYT).
researchers from different countries (Heintz, Ruch, Aykan et al.,
2019; Heintz et al., 2018) have shown that certain types of humor
are empirically observed in different countries and cultures, but ETHICAL APPROVAL
also with evidence for regional differences. In these cases, the All procedures performed in studies involving human participants
considered theoretical model included two comic styles considered were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and
in this study (benevolent humor and corrective humor/satire), so national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration
evidence supports the idea that the behavior of comic styles in and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Chile could be similar to their behavior in other countries.

INFORMED CONSENT
LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDY Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants
First, the design of the study allowed the possibility of the pollster included in the study.
effect interfering with the results, at least in the face-to-face On behalf of all authors, the corresponding author states that
samples. To reduce this effect, pollsters were carefully trained on there is no conflict of interest.

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
8 A. Mendiburo-Seguel and S. Heintz Scand J Psychol (2020)

psychological well-being: Development of the Humor Styles


REFERENCES Questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 48–75.
Agger, R. E., Goldstein, M. N. & Pearl, S. A. (1961). Political cynicism: McGhee, P. E. (1976). Children’s appreciation of humor: A test of the
Measurement and meaning. The Journal of Politics, 23, 477–506. cognitive congruency principle. Child Development, 47, 420–426.
Bonanno, G. A. & Jost, J. T. (2006). Conservative shift among high- Mendiburo-Seguel, A. & Ford, T. E. (2019). The effect of disparagement
exposure survivors of the September 11th terrorist attacks. Basic and humor on the acceptability of prejudice. Current Psychology. Advance
Applied Social Psychology, 28, 311–323. online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00354-2
Budner, S. (1962). Intolerance of ambiguity as a personality variable. Mendiburo-Seguel, A. & Heintz, S. (2019). Comic styles and their relation
Journal of Personality, 30, 29–50. to the sense of humor, humor appreciation, acceptability of prejudice,
Buss, A. H. & Finn, S. E. (1987). Classification of personality traits. humorous self-image and happiness. Humor: International Journal of
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 432–444. Humor Research. Advance online, publication, https://doi.org/10.1515/
Cann, A. & Matson, C. (2014). Sense of humor and social desirability: humor-2018-0151.
Understanding how humor styles are perceived. Personality and Mendiburo-Seguel, A., Paez, D. & Martınez-Zelaya, G. (2017). Humor,
Individual Differences, 66, 176–180. cultura, personalidad y bienestar a nivel colectivo [Humor, culture,
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. personality and well-being at a collective level]. In J. Benavides (Ed.),
Costa, P. T. Jr, McCrae, R. R. & L€ockenhoff, C. E. (2019). Personality Psicologıa y Filosofıa del Humor [Psychology and Phylosophy of Humor]
across the life span. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 423–448. (pp. 333–348). Bogota: Ediciones Universidad Cooperativa de Colombia.
Craik, K. H., Lampert, M. D. & Nelson, A. J. (1996). Sense of humor and Mendiburo-Seguel, A., Vargas, S. & Rubio, A. (2017). Exposure to
styles of everyday humorous conduct. Humor: International Journal of political disparagement humor and its impact on trust in politicians:
Humor Research, 9, 273–302. How long does it last? Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2236. doi:https://
Cunningham, W. A. & Derks, P. (2005). Humor appreciation and latency doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02236
of comprehension. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, M~ottus, R., Johnson, W. & Deary, I. J. (2012). Personality traits in old
18, 389–403. age: Measurement and rank-order stability and some mean-level
Eagly, A. H. & Wood, W. (2013). The nature–nurture debates: 25 years of change. Psychology and Aging, 27, 243–249.
challenges in understanding the psychology of gender. Perspectives on Ott, K. H. & Schweizer, B. (2018). Does religion shape people’s sense of
Psychological Science, 8, 340–357. humour? A comparative study of humour appreciation among
Fieschi, C. & Heywood, P. (2004). Trust, cynicism and populist anti- members of different religions and nonbelievers. The European
politics. Journal of Political Ideologies, 9, 289–309. Journal of Humour Research, 6, 12–35.
Greengross, G. (2013). Humor and aging-a mini-review. Gerontology, 59, R Core Team (2019). R: A language and environment for statistical
448–453. https://doi.org/10.1159/000351005 computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
Heintz, S. (2020). Separating content and structure in humor appreciation: Retrieved 6 September 2019 from www.R-project.org.
A bimodal structural equation modeling approach. Journal of Rooduijn, M., van der Brug, W., de Lange, S. L. & Parlevliet, J. (2017).
Individual Differences, 41, 37–44. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/ Persuasive populism? Estimating the effect of populist messages on
a000301 political cynicism. Politics and Governance, 5, 136–145.
Heintz, S., Ruch, W., Aykan, S., Brdar, I., Brzozowska, D., Carretero- Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling
Dios, H. et al (2019). Benevolent and corrective humor, life and more. Version 0.5–12 (BETA). Journal of Statistical Software, 48,
satisfaction, and broad humor dimensions: Extending the nomological 1–36.
network of the BenCor across 25 countries. Journal of Happiness Ruch, W. (2007). The sense of humor: Explorations of a personality
Studies. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902- characteristic. Berlin: Mouton De Gruyter.
019-00185-9. Ruch, W. & Hehl, F.-J. (1983). Intolerance of ambiguity as a factor in the
Heintz, S., Ruch, W., Platt, T., Pang, D., Carretero-Dios, H., Dionigi, appreciation of humour. Personality and Individual Differences, 4,
A. et al (2018). Psychometric comparisons of benevolent and 443–449.
corrective humor across 22 countries: The virtue gap in humor goes Ruch, W. & Hehl, F.-J. (1988). Attitudes to sex, sexual behaviour and
international. Frontiers in Psychology: Personality and Social enjoyment of humour. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 983–
Psychology, 9, 92. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00092 994. https://doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(88)90132-8
Hu, L. & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in Ruch, W. & Heintz, S. (2016). The German version of the Humor Styles
covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new Questionnaire: Psychometric properties and overlap with other styles
alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary of humor. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 12, 434–455.
Journal, 6, 1–55. Ruch, W., Heintz, S., Platt, T., Wagner, L. & Proyer, R. T. (2018a).
Hyde, J. S. (2014). Gender similarities and differences. Annual Review of Broadening humor: Comic styles differentially tap into temperament,
Psychology, 65, 373–398. character, and ability. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 6.
Kenny, D. A. & McCoach, D. B. (2003). Effect of the number of Ruch, W., Wagner, L. & Heintz, S. (2018b). Humor, the PEN model of
variables on measures of fit in structural equation modeling. Structural personality, and subjective well-being: Support for differential
Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 10, 333–351. relationships with eight comic styles. Rivista Italiana di Studi
Lampert, M. D. & Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (2007). Exploring paradigms: The sull’Umorismo, 1, 31–43.
study of gender and sense of humor near the end of the 20th century. Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H. & M€ uller, H. (2003). Evaluating
In W. Ruch (Ed.), The sense of humor: Explorations of a personality the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and
characteristic (2nd edn, pp. 231–270). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. Methods of psychological
Latin American Public Opinion Project (2017). Chilean questionnaire research online, 8, 23–74.
V.18.0.6.0. Retrieved 12 March 2019 from, https://www.vanderbilt.ed Schuck, A. R., Boomgaarden, H. G. & de Vreese, C. H. (2013). Cynics all
u/lapop/chile/ABChi17-v18.0.6.0-Spa-170411_W.pdf around? The impact of election news on political cynicism in
Li, N. P., Griskevicius, V., Durante, K. M., Jonason, P. K., Pasisz, D. J. & comparative perspective. Journal of Communication, 63, 287–311.
Aumer, K. (2009). An evolutionary perspective on humor: Sexual Schweizer, B. & Ott, K. (2016). Faith and laughter: Do atheists and
selection or interest indication? Personality and Social Psychology practicing Christians have different senses of humor? Humor:
Bulletin, 35, 923–936. International Journal of Humor Research, 29, 413–438.
Martin, R. A. & Ford, T. E. (2018). The psychology of humor: An Specht, J., Bleidorn, W., Denissen, J. J., Hennecke, M., Hutteman, R.,
integrative approach (2nd edn). London: Academic Press. Kandler, C. & Zimmermann, J. (2014). What drives adult personality
Martin, R. A., Puhlik-Doris, P., Larsen, G., Gray, J. & Weir, K. (2003). development? A comparison of theoretical perspectives and empirical
Individual differences in uses of humor and their relation to evidence. European Journal of Personality, 28, 216–230.

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd
Scand J Psychol (2020) Sociodemographics and comic styles 9

van de Vijver, F. & Hambleton, R. K. (1996). Translating tests: Some “right” in Latin America], Perspectivas desde el Bar ometro de las
practical guidelines. European Psychologist, 1, 89–99. https://doi.org/ Americas, 38. Retrieved 15 January 2019 from, https://www.vanderb
10.1027/1016-9040.1.2.89 ilt.edu/lapop/insights/I0838es.pdf
Zechmeister, E. & Corral, M. (2010). El variado significado de “izquierda”
y “derecha” en America Latina [The varied meaning of “left” and Received 24 September 2019, accepted 12 February 2020

© 2020 Scandinavian Psychological Associations and John Wiley & Sons Ltd

Potrebbero piacerti anche