Sei sulla pagina 1di 4

Vol. 6, No.

1, April 2003

FORENSIC DO CUME NT EXAMINATION/Handwriting and ADHD : 39-4-030206/0304


ATTENTIONAL CONTROL OF HANDWRITING MOVEMENTS
The effects of conscious control on handwriting fluency.
by Oliver Tucha1, Geraldine M. Paul1, Klaus W. Lange1

REFERENCES:, Tucha, O., Paul, G. M., and Lange, K. W., "Attentional production of fluent handwriting movements. Furthermore, conscious
Control of Handwriting Movements," The International Journal of attention to visual feedback and also to mental control of graphomotor
Forensic Document Examiners, Vol. 6, No. 1, April 2003, pp. 1-4. output hampers the production of fluent handwriting movements.
However, that visual feedback is effectively used in automated
ABSTRACT: In the present study, the effect of conscious control on handwriting for the monitoring of stroke size was demonstrated in a
handwriting fluency of 16 healthy adults and 8 healthy children was second experiment.
assessed. Participants were asked to write a short sentence under different The experience of Mai and Marquardt in the treatment of patients
conditions. Conditions were: normal writing, writing with eyes closed, with writer's cramp [12,13], has led them to emphasize the potential of
neat handwriting, writing while visually tracking the pen tip and writing their findings for the treatment of handwriting disturbances. They
with closed eyes while mentally tracking the highest position in each letter. suggest, that the treatment of patients with writing disturbances, should
While no differences were found between normal handwriting and writing focus on smoothness and fluency of movements. The aim of training
with closed eyes, marked differences were observed between normal procedures should be to direct attention (conscious control) away from
writing and the other writing conditions. The present results indicate that the writing process, in particular from accuracy, uniformity and size of
automated handwriting movements are independent of visual feedback. handwriting.
Furthermore, conscious control of graphomotor output hampers the The aim of the present study was to assess the effect of conscious
production of fluent handwriting movements in both adults and children. control on handwriting fluency of healthy adults and healthy children.
The effect of attentional control on handwriting in children is of
KEYWORDS: Handwriting fluency, conscious, graphomotor. interest, since disturbances of handwriting movements and in
particular of handwriting fluency are common in children with
INTRODUCTIONS conditions, such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia
[14, 15].
In a number of experimental studies, the role of vision and visual
feedback in the spatial and temporal control of handwriting was assessed. 2. METHODS
These studies were concerned with handwriting performance of healthy
adults [1-6] and patients with neurological disorders [7]. 2.1 Participants and Procedure
After deprivation of vision, Smyth and Silvers [1] found no alterations
of movement time in healthy adults, but a considerable increase in the Sixteen right-handed students (eight female, eight male; mean age
number of errors in strokes and letters (omissions, repetitions, = 22.5 years; S.E. = 0.4 years) participated in the experiment. The
transpositions of letters and strokes). In contrast to these findings, a participants were asked to write the sentence "Ein helles grelles Licht"
significant increase of movement time and trajectory size has been ("A bright and glaring light") in cursive script under five different
observed in handwriting without vision [2-4]. Furthermore, a higher conditions. The conditions consisted of (A) normal writing, (B)
spatial variability was found in a no-vision handwriting condition than in writing with eyes closed, (C) neat handwriting, (D) writing while
a normal handwriting condition with vision [3]. However, in a more recent visually tracking the pen tip and (E) writing with closed eyes while
examination Marquardt and colleagues [6] found (in accordance with the mentally tracking the highest position in each letter. The sequence of
results of Smyth and Silvers [1]), no differences between normal conditions was randomly assigned to participants. Furthermore, eight
handwriting and handwriting with closed eyes with regard to peak velocity right-handed children (four female, four male; mean age = 12.6 years;
and overall speed of movement. In addition, the analysis of single strokes S.E. = 0.3 years) underwent the same procedure. Handedness was
under both conditions of handwriting revealed velocity profiles with a assessed on the basis of participants' responses on a short version of
smooth course which had only one peak and a bell shaped course [6]. the handedness questionnaire of Raczkowski, Kalat and Nebes [16,
These kinematic properties of handwriting movements denote fluent or 17].
automated movements [8-11].
Marquardt and colleagues [6] discuss their latter finding in terms of 2.2 Apparatus and Analysis
attentional control. As well as the two instructions mentioned above
(writing with eyes opened and writing with eyes closed), the participants A digitizing tablet (WACOM IV, Wacom, Neuss, Germany) was
were also requested to write a short sentence while visually tracking the used for registration of handwriting movements. The position of the
pen tip and while mentally tracking the pen tip. Under the latter two pen on the surface of the tablet can be localised with an accuracy of
experimental conditions of handwriting, numerous inversions of velocity 0.2 mm in both directions. Data processing was performed using a
profiles, indicating gross disturbances of handwriting fluency, were computer programme which was designed for the analysis of
observed. The authors conclude that visual feedback is not essential to the handwriting movements [18]. Kinematic data were calculated and
smoothed using nonparametric regression methods (kernel estimators)
11
Institute of Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg, 93040 devised by Marquardt and Mai [19]. For analysis, the mean number of
Regensburg, Germany. inversions of the direction of the velocity profiles of the letter
Address for correspondence: Professor K.W. Lange, Institute of combination "ll" was calculated. Data analysis focused on the vertical
Experimental Psychology, University of Regensburg, 93040 Regensburg, component of the strokes. The letter combination "ll" was chosen since
Germany, E-mail: oliver.tucha@psychologie.uni-regensburg.de, these letters represent a simple letter combination which is usually
klaus.lange@psychologie.uni-regensburg.de Fax:++49 - 941 - 943 4496. executed in script type. Moreover, the pen is not usually lifted from the
pad while these letters are written. The number of inversions of the

Copyright © 2003 Shunderson Communication, Inc. All Rights of Reproduction Reserved 1


The International Journal of Forensic Document Examiners

Figure 1: NIV of 16 healthy adult participants writing under five different conditions.

velocity profiles (NIV) were chosen, since automated and non-automated 1.87; p = .104). However, in comparison with movements during normal
handwriting movements can be distinguished from one another by profiles handwriting, the NIV was significantly increased when children were
of velocity [18]. Single strokes of automated movements lead to a smooth asked to write neatly (t = 7.21; p < .001), to write while visually tracking
course and have only one peak (NIV = 1) and a bell shaped course in their the pen tip (t = 8.43; p < .001) and when writing with closed eyes while
velocity profiles. Therefore, a greater dysfluency of handwriting is mentally tracking the highest position in each letter (t = 12.43; p < .001).
reflected in a higher NIV [8, 9, 11]. Statistical analysis was performed
using multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA). Post hoc analysis was 4. DISCUSSION
performed using t-tests for paired samples. An alpha level of .05 was
applied for statistical analysis. The present results indicate that automated handwriting movements
are independent from visual feedback. With regard to healthy adult
RESULTS participants, these findings correspond with observations of a former study
of Marquardt and colleagues [6] who used similar experimental
Adult Participants procedures. These authors also demonstrated that automated handwriting
movements could be performed without vision. Furthermore, the present
Multivariate analysis of variance revealed a significant difference findings suggest that conscious attention given to accuracy of handwriting
between writing conditions (F = 29.5; p < .001). While no differences (neatness), to visual feedback and also to mental control of graphomotor
were found between the NIV per stroke between normal handwriting and output results in less fluent handwriting movements. The participants'
writing with closed eyes (t-test for paired samples: t = 1.86; p = .083), attention to handwriting hampered the generation of automated
marked differences were observed between normal writing and the other movements. Automated processes are therefore not just independent of
writing conditions (Figure 1). In comparison with movements during conscious control as suggested by Näätänen [20], they are indeed
normal handwriting, the NIV was significantly increased when participants disturbed by conscious attention.
were requested to write neatly (t = 7.74; p < .001), to write while visually The examination of writing fluency of healthy children who had been
tracking the pen tip (t = 6.82; p < .001) and when they were asked to write requested to write under different experimental conditions revealed similar
with closed eyes while mentally tracking the highest position in each letter results. Conscious attention to visual feedback also resulted in marked
(t = 9.69; p < .001). deterioration of handwriting fluency, as indicated by an increased NIV.
These findings may explain the results of a previous study in which
Children improvements of qualitative aspects of handwriting, such as legibility and
accuracy, were observed in boys with ADHD following treatment with
The multivariate analysis of variance of the NIV in children revealed stimulant medication [15]. However, kinematic analysis of handwriting
a significant difference (Figure 2) between writing conditions (F = 33.5; movements revealed that these boys with ADHD on stimulant drug
p < .001). Subsequent post hoc analysis using t-tests for paired samples treatment performed less fluent writing movements than following the
indicated no differences in the NIV per stroke between normal withdrawal of the drug or than control participants. The less fluent
handwriting and writing with closed eyes (t-test for paired samples: t = handwriting processes of children with ADHD on medication may

2 Copyright © 2003 Shunderson Communications, Inc. All Rights of Reproduction Reserved


Vol. 6, No.1, April 2003

Figure 2: NIV of 18 healthy children writing under five different conditions.

therefore follow from the intention to write more neatly. [5]. Van Galen, G.P., Teulings, H.-L., & Sanders, J. (1994). On the
Mai and Marquardt demonstrated that training procedures directing interdependence of motor programming and feedback processing in
attention away from the writing process improved the handwriting of adult handwriting. In C. Faure, P. Keuss, G. Lorette, & A. Vinter (Eds.),
patients with writer's cramp [19, 13]. According to these findings, we Advances in handwriting and drawing. A multidisciplinary
suggest that the present findings may have implications for the treatment approach. (pp. 403-419). Paris: Télécom.
of handwriting disturbances of children, with conditions such as attention
deficit hyperactivity disorder and dyslexia [14, 15]. [6]. Marquardt, C., Gentz, W., & Mai, N. (1996). On the role of vision
in skilled handwriting. M.L. Simner, C.G. Leedham, & A.J.W.M.
REFERENCES Thomassen (Eds.), Handwriting and drawing research: Basic and
applied issues. (pp. 87-97). IOS Press.
[1]. Smyth, M.M., & Silvers, G. (12987). Functions of vision in the
control of Handwriting, Acta Psychologica, 65, 47-64. [7]. Smith, M.C., & Fucetola, R. (1995). Human Movement Science, 14,
109-123.
[2]. Van Galen, G.P., Smyth, M.M., Meulenbroek, R.G.J., & Hylkema,
H. (1989). The role of the short-term memory and the motor buffer [8]. Meulenbroek, R.G.J., & Van Galen, G.P. (1988). The acquisition
in handwriting under visual and non-visual guidance. In R. of skilled handwriting: Discontinuous trends in kinematic variables.
Plamondon, C. Y. Suen, & M. L. Simner (Eds.), Computer In A. Colley & J. Beech (Eds.), Cognition and action in skilled
recognition and human production of handwriting. (pp. 253-271). behaviour. (pp. 273-281). North-Holland: Elsevier Science
World Scientific Publ. Co. Publishers.

[3]. Van Doorn, R.R., & Keuss, P.J. (1992). The role of vision in the [9]. Meulenbroek, R.G.J., & Van Galen, G.P. (1989). The production
temporal and spatial control of handwriting. Acta Psychol Amst, of connecting strokes in cursive writing: Developing co-articulation
81, 269-286. in 8 to 12 year-old children. In R. Plamondon, C. Y. Suen, & M. L.
Simner (Eds.), Computer recognition and human production of
[4]. Van Doorn, R.R., & Keuss, P.J. (1993). Does the production of handwriting. (pp. 273-286). Singapore: World Scientific
letter strokes in handwriting benefit from vision? Acta Psychol Publishing.
Amst, 82, 275-290.
[10]. Eichhorn, T.E., Gasser, T., Mai, N., Marquardt, C., Arnold, G.,

Copyright © 2003 Shunderson Communication, Inc. All Rights of Reproduction Reserved 3


The International Journal of Forensic Document Examiners

Schwarz, J., & Oertel, W.H. (1996). Computational analysis of open


loop handwriting movements in Parkinson's disease: a rapid method
to detect dopamimetic effects. Movment Disorders, 11, 289-297.

[11]. Tucha, O., Aschenbrenner, S., & Lange, K.W. (2000). Mirror
writing and handedness. Brain Lang, 73, 432-441.

[12]. Mai, N., & Marquardt, C. (1994). Treatment of writer's cramp.


Kinematic measures as an assessment tool for planning and
evaluating training procedures. In C. Faure, P. Keuss, G. Lorette, &
A. Vinter (Eds.), Advances in handwriting and drawing. A
multidisciplinary approach. Paris: Télécom.

[13]. Mai, N., & Marquardt, C. (1995). Analyse und Therapie


motorischer Schrteibstörungen. Psychologische Beiträge, 37, 538-
582.

[14]. Sovik, N., Arntzen, O., & Thygesen, R. (1987). Writing


characteristics of "normal", dyslexic and dysgraphic children.
Journal of Human Movement Science, 13, 171-187.

[15]. Tucha, O., & Lange, K.W. (1999). Methylphenidate-induced


impairment of handwriting movements in hyperactive boys. In C.
G. Leedham, M. Leung, V. Sagar, & X. Xuhong (Eds.),
Proceedings of the 9th Biennial Conference of the International
Graphonomics Society. (Pp. 241-244). Singapore: Nanyang
University.

[16]. Chapman, L.J., & Chapman, J.P. (1987). The measurement of


handedness. Brain Cogn., 6, 175-183.

[17]. Raczkowski, D., Kalat, J.W., & Nebes, R. (1974). Reliability and
validity of some handedness questio nnai re ite ms.
Neuropsychologia, 12, 43-47.

[18]. Mai, N., & Marquardt, C. (1992). CS - Computer-assisted


movement analysis in handwriting. Operational manual. Munich:
MedCom Verlag.

[19]. Marquardt, C., & Mai, N. (1994). A computational procedure for


movement analysis in handwriting. Journal of Neuroscience
Methods, 52, 39-45.

[20]. Näätänen, R. (1992). Attention and brain function. Hillsdale:


Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

4 Copyright © 2003 Shunderson Communications, Inc. All Rights of Reproduction Reserved

Potrebbero piacerti anche