Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Early Development and Parenting, Vol.

3 (l),51-60 (1994)

Early Interactions and the


Socioemotional Development of
Deaf Infants
Lynne Sanford Koester
Gallaudet University, Washington, DC, U S A

Studies of the early face-to-face interactions and later attachment


behaviours between infants and their caregivers are reviewed, with
a particular focus on implications for deaf infants. Since the majority
of deaf infants are born to hearing parents, it is to be expected that
many parental responses will rely on vocalizations which are
inaccessible to the deaf infant. Are other sensory modalities
enhanced or used in a compensatorymanner by hearing parents so
as to make their communication with a deaf infant more effective?
Is the deaf infant’s heightened need for visual and tactile input
adequately met during normal interactions with their caregivers?
Does the deaf infant develop the same strategies for signalling
distress and coping with separation as does the hearing infant?
Although the research literature on this population is sparse, there
are beginning to be more reports addressingthese questions as early
identification becomes increasingly possible. Therefore, this paper
will cover issues of the role of eye contact and mutual coordination
in early interactions, compensatoryparenting behaviours from the
perspective of the ’intuitive parenting’ model, and precursors to
later attachment behaviours in deaf infants.
I Key words: Deaf infants, intuitive parenting, attachment behaviours.

’Deafness, by its nature, not only inhibits or interactions, relying on other sensory channels for
prevents access to sound and speech, it is also likely communication and respecting the child’s need to
to disrupt, in developmentally important ways, the attend sequentially to objects and social partners are
formation of relationships between children and just a few of the issues of importance when
those around them’ (Wood, 1989, p. 62). examining the dynamics of this unique interactive
The purpose of this article is to examine the system. Furthermore, as research with normally
research literature related to early social interactions hearing infants has shown, these are all relevant to
of infants and their caregivers, focusing primarily the larger picture of the infant’s socioemotional
on studies with deaf infants and on implicationsfor development and emergent sense of self.
their development. As Wood (1989)has stated, the
disruption of communication which typically occurs
when hearing parents have a deaf child affects
much more than the child’s language development MUTUAL INFLUENCES IN
alone. Coordination and timing of dyadic NORMAL INTERACTIONS
Developmental researchers, particularly those
Address for correspondence: University of Montana, studying infancy, have recognized for some time
Department of Psychology, Missoula, MT 59812-1041, USA. that individual members of an interacting system

CCC 1057-3593/94/01OO51-10 Received 17 November 1991


0 1994 by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Accepted 9 October 1992
52 L. S. Koester

mutually influence each other, and are continue to rely largely on vocalizations that are
consequently changed themselves (e.g. Brazelton inaccessible to the child (Jamieson, in press).
et al., 1974; Koester et al., 1987; Stern, 1974). As Without concurrent or supplementary use of other
Brazelton and Nugent (1987) have said, even in the modalities for reinforcing the infant’s visual
newborn period changes emanate from a ’reciprocal attention, responding to positive social bids or
feedback system’ inherent in the caregiver-child comforting distress, these otherwise appropriate
relationship. ‘We expect developmental prediction vocal responses by the parent are ineffective with
to come from the study of the processes of a severely or profoundly deaf infant. Since the
biobehavioral organization, based on a transactional visual and tactile channels are of particular
view in which development proceeds as infant and significance to a deaf infant, these are important
caregiver continually modify their own and each areas for study in relation to the infant’s early
other’s behavior through reciprocal interaction‘ adaptation to the social world.
(Brazelton and Nugent, 1987, p. 216).
Schaffer (1987) takes this transactional view one
step further by recognizing the interplay of THE ROLE OF EYE CONTACT
endogenous and exogenous factors as determinants IN EARLY INTERACTIONS
of the particular developmental course for a given
child. He asserts that each child brings to the world Eye contact and gaze aversion are thought to play
certain capabilities and limitations - a crucial roles in the mutual regulation process that
’developmental programme’ -which then both takes place during any adult-infant interactions,
cause developmental progression and influence the and are particularly important because they allow
social factors, such as parental responses, which the infant to be an active participant in modulating
impinge on the child. levels of arousal in response to external stimulation
Likewise, when parents respond appropriately, (Exline, 1982). Stern (1974), Brazelton (Brazelton et
they facilitate the child’s progression to the next al., 1974), Field (1978) and others have described the
phase along his or her developmental trajectory. As alternating periods of looking at and looking away
Schaffer argues, development must be viewed from the partner that seem to reflect the infant’s
within a social context: ‘. . . on the one hand there attempts to process stimulation and to regulate the
are social facilitating factors that help to bring about interactional sequence. As Field (1978) states:
new transitions, and on the other hand these
transitions produce social consequenceswhich will in ‘Mothers who are more active or “overstimulating” and
turnfacilitatefurther developments’(Schaffer, 1987, less sensitive or responsive to their infants’ pauses or
turning away during the conversation, are less able to
p. 241). On a similar note, Sameroff and Chandler elicit or hold their infants’ gaze. . . . One of the most
(1975) cautioned against assuming long-term important rules for the mother to learn is sensitivity to
negative outcomes as a result of inherent conditions the infant’s capacity for attention and inattention.’
in the child, urging instead that we consider the (P, 132)
many ways in which an infant‘s condition interacts
with the caregiving environment to produce Schaffer (1984) points out that the visual system
developmental outcomes. is a primary means of establishing contact and
The early parent-infant relationship involves communication with others, and that develop-
frequent face-to-face encounters, providing mentally it is already well adapted at birth to begin
researchers with a lens for observing the mutual to play this important role. Furthermore, its
influences or reciprocal effects most characteristic versatilityis enhanced by the fact that the infant can
of these interactions-and those which are most use a simple act-turning the head or closing the
important to their continued success. Behaviours eyes -to control the amount of stimulus input, to
such as eye contact and gaze aversion, ‘baby talk‘ seek or to avoid additional visual sensation.
and cooing or babbling, touching and comforting Alternationsbetween gazingat and gazing away are
(includingself-regulatorybehaviours by the infant) an intrinsic characteristicof the infant’s early social
and expressions of pleasure or distress occur on interactions, varying primarily in the timing and
both sides of the dyad and can be used as indices length of attention versus avoidance bouts; it is
of the mutuality, affect and responsiveness of the thought that these temporal factors are dependent
two partners. upon physiological constraints related to the
As has been shown in a study of deaf preschoolers, individual infant’s need for and tolerance of varying
however, hearing parents with deaf children may levels of arousal.
Interactions with Deaf lnfants 53

For deaf infants, maintaining visual contact is energy required for this task leaves little in reserve
crucial to dyadic communication; breaking this for attending to the social or object world.
contact is done at the risk of severing the Cues by the infant help to shape the contingent
interaction altogether, except when there is also responses of the adult, which in turn lead the infant
tactile contact. Therefore, an effective arousal- to believe that his or her behaviour has a positive
reducing behaviour such as gaze avert is only effect on the social relationship. Typically, these
available to deaf infants at considerable expense to mutual reactions and modifications of behaviour
the interactive system. Prior to achieving the occur so rapidly that it is only through m i c r o d y t ~ c
concept of object permanence, the act of turning techniques that researchers have identified the
away from the social partner by a deaf infant can contributions of each partner during early parent-
remove that partner from the child’s sensory and infant interactions. In some instances, however,
cognitive realm entirely, whereas a hearing child such as the presence of a sensory impairment, it
can still maintain auditory contact and awareness becomes possible to highlight the role of a specific
of the partner’s continuous presence despite component in the interactions, or to examine ways
frequent gaze aversions. As Koester and Trimm in which other interactive behaviours are affected
(1991) have found, 9-month-old hearing infants by the absence of one important ingredient such as
break eye contact more frequently during face-to- sight or sound.
face interactions than do deaf infants; even at that For example, several studies of deaf infants and
age, the deaf infants have apparently already either deaf or hearing parents have shown that the
learned that visual monitoring of the social mutual regulatory process is altered when
environment is the key to their access to whatever auditorylvocal communication is not available
communication may be available. (Gianino and Meadow-Orlans, 1987). Day and
Prezioso (1987) found that hearing mothers
experienced significant stress following the
diagnosis of a hearing loss in their infant, and that
COORDINATION OF this stress was translated into potentially
MOTHER-CHILD INTERACTIONS counterproductive interaction styles involving
Tronick (1989) has asserted that the long-term overprotectiveness and unnecessary simplification
outcomes of early interactions with infants depend of play behaviours. Meadow-Orlans et al. (1987)
largely on the balance of the infant‘s experiences observed that the greater positive affect expressed
of coordinated versus miscoordinated states within on the faces of deaf mothers appeared to substitute
the dyad. The infant who consistently experiences for that expressed vocally by hearing mothers with
success in efforts at ’interactive repairs’ develops their infants. Furthermore, 3-month-old infants
an expectation that his or her own actions will be (both deaf and hearing) paid closer attention to their
effective, and is therefore more likely to use similar mothers’ faces if the mother was deaf: that is, at this
strategies to reengage the mother when she is early age, they seemed to already understand the
temporarily unresponsive. Numerous investigations necessity of engaging the mother visually and
of normal dyads under experimental situations in responding to her visual cues in order to sustain an
which the mother is instructed to be non-responsive interaction.
(or present a ’still-face’)for a short time have found
that infants can also be remarkably resourceful in
communicating their needs and displeasure, and in
attempting to ‘repair’ this aversive situation (Carter COMPENSATORY
et al., 1990; Cohn and Tronick, 1983; Mayes and PARENTING BEHAVIOURS
Carter, 1990; Tronick et al., 1978). Studies of various at-risk infant populations have
By contrast, infants who have repeatedly shown that adults often try to compensate for the
experienced interactional failures without successful more limited capaaties of their partners (Brown and
transitions back to a coordinated state with the Bakeman, 1979; Field, 1977, 1984; Jones, 1980;
caregiver are more likely to build up negative affect, Wedell-Monnig and Lumley, 1980).
to become self-oriented (e.g. during the still-face) As Goldberg (1982) asks:
and to turn away or attempt to escape through
paceptual shutdown, thusbecomingnon-responsive ’While we seem predisposed toward successful adult-
in turn. In such a case, the infant’s primary concern infant interactions, what happens when somethinggoes
becomes the containment of negative affect; the wrong? One of the features of a successful system is
54 L. S. Koester

that there is more than one way to achieve a given amount of explained variance in the dependent
result; when one part of the system fails or functions measure of positive interactions at 18 months. No
improperly, corresponding adjustments are made such relationship was found for the hgaring infants,
so that the system as a whole can continue to although it was suggested that vocal responses
operate. . . .‘ (p. 40) would play a similar role for this group. This finding
confirms the importance of using response
Goldberg (1982) believes that parent-infant dyads modalities which are matched with the infant’s
incorporate many alternatives to the normally sensory capabilities.
optimal modes of interaction, such as varying their Similarly, MacTurk (1993) reports that for deaf but
bids for attention at times when their partners are not for hearing infants, social gaze (looking at the
unable (or unwilling) to respond immediately. The experimenter or mother during task performance)
interaction system may have limits to its capacity at 9 months is positively related to the infant’s
for coping with stress, but self-correcting mastery motivation at 12 months. It appears that
mechanisms often appear to help counteract threats deaf infants themselves develop strategies early in
to dyadic functioning. life to compensate for reduced sensory contact with
When a parent or infant has a hearing loss, one their environment.
of the most prominent features of early
interaction -that of vocalization -is either
unavailable or only partially accessible. While this INTUITIVE PARENTING
can lead to a distortion in the normal interactive
behaviour patterns, it is also possible for both One conceptual model which offers valuable
partners to adapt and compensate successfully by insights to further understanding of these issues is
relying on other communication modalities. As that of ’intuitive parenting’ (Koester et al., 1987;
Marschark (1992) states, such accommodation PapouSek and PapouSek, 1981, 1982, 1987). This
’serves to provide a reciprocal relationship between model proposes that many subtle, non-conscious
parent and (deaf) child and “bootstraps” parental behaviours are ideally suited to the needs
development in an effective, if somewhat different of the less mature infant partner, who must make
manner than that of hearing children. It is only by many adaptations to the demands of the social and
understanding those differences, however, that physical environment. Examples include such
we can hope to understand the psychological behaviours as the adult’s tendency to maintain a
functioning of deaf individuals’ (p. 22). close face-to-face interaction distance (appropriate
Recent investigations have shown that hearing to the infant’s limited visual focal range), the use
mothers provide more visual experiences to deaf of frequent repetition and simplified stimulation
than to hearing infants during face-to-face (also consistent with the infant’s learning needs and
interactions (Koester and Trimm, 1991), as well as capacities) and the use of ’motherese’ during
during object play (Meadow-Orlans and Steinberg, communication with a preverbal infant (thus
1991; Spencer, 1991). In addition, Koester (1991) making one’s infant-directed speech more salient
reports that even with 9-month-old infants these and easily attended to or identified by the infant)
maternal visual displays are well coordinated with (Fernald, 1989; Koester et al., 1989; PapouSek et al.,
the deaf infants’ visual attention, thus ensuring that 1985).
an important aspect of early dyadic communication Contrasts to the usual patterns of intuitive
is perceptually accessible even when the infant is parenting can clearly be found among dyads in
deaf. However, when visual gaze also involves joint which one or more of the partners has experienced
attention to objects, hearing parents may not be so a sensory loss such as deafness or blindness. It is
adept in coordinating their responses with a deaf important to remember, however, that although
child, as demonstrated by Spencer et al. (1992). such patterns may be altered to appear quite
In a longitudinal study of parents with deaf and different from those assumed to be the ‘norm’, there
hearing infants (MacTurk et al., 1993), multiple may be many other ways in which these partners
regression analyses were used to examine predictive achieve mutually satisfying and developmentally
relationships between interactive behaviours at appropriate interaction styles. In a case study of
infant ages 9 and 18 months. In this study, it was blind parents with a sighted infant, for example,
reported that the frequency with which mothers Adamson et al. (1977) report on the development
incorporated visual or tactile stimulation with their of reciprocal social interactions despite the persistent
9-month-old deaf infants sigruficantlyincreased the violation of natural face-to-face communication
Interactions with Deaf Infants 55

behaviours. In addition, the mother compensated whom such modifications may be initially just as
for the absence of visual information about her unfamiliar as the visual-gestural method of
baby’s state (particularly during feeding episodes) communication itself. For these parents, especially
by continually fingering the baby lightly. This those who decide to use sign language with their
provides an intriguing example of natural efforts on child, many of the intuitive behaviours which are
the part of a blind mother to use a non-visual closely linked to spoken communication may have
channel to monitor her infant’s behaviour. to be modified in order to accommodate to another
According to PapouBek and PapouSek (1982), modality. At the same time, new methods of
behavioural plasticity and compensatory adjustments communicating, of eliciting and maintaining
during social interchanges serve to protect both attention through visual and tactile channels and
partners against a variety of adverse factors of incorporating grammatical features into facial
stemming from either genetic or environmental expressions may have to be learned on a conscious
origins. level before they are easily incorporated into the
As Fraiberg (1974) has also observed, sighted hearing parent’s interactions with a deaf infant.
mothers with blind infants are often distressed by As mentioned previously, most studies of deaf
the lack of affective feedback due to the reduced children have relied on older populations, such as
facial expressiveness of the infant. However, she toddlers or preschoolers, largely due to the
also found that when parents were made aware of difficulties in obtaining samples of enough infants
how much excitement, tension and general affective for whom deafness has been diagnosed (Greenberg,
responsiveness could be seen in the same infant’s 1980; Greenberg and Marvin, 1979; Henggeler and
hands, they became much better able to ’read’ the Cooper, 1983; Henggeler et al., 1984; Lederberg et
signals from their infant and thus to derive greater al., 1986; Meadow et al., 1981).
mutual satisfaction from their interactions. Results for older children have been quite
consistent in demonstrating a greater use of
directiveness and control techniques, and less
harmonious relationships, in hearing parents of
EARLY INTERACTIONS deaf children (Lederberg, 1984; Lederberg et al.,
WITH DEAF INFANTS 1991; Meadow-Orlans and Steinberg, 1991;
We are just beginning to learn about the impact of Schlesinger, 1985; Schlesinger and Meadow, 1972;
deafness on the very early interactions between Spencer et al., 1992; Wedell-Monnig and Lumley,
infants and their parents, since most previous 1980). Explanations have varied from negative
studies of childhood deafness have been carried out maternal effect in response to the infant’s hearing
either with older children or with extremely small loss to a heightened effort to overcome the delayed
samples. Do interactions by a hearing parent with language development which often characterizes
a deaf infant appear ‘depressed’ in comparison to deaf children.
those with a hearing infant-are the partners’ Spencer et al. (1992) suggest that maternal
behaviours less smoothly synchronized or do they overcontrol may stem from the hearing mothers’
appear less mutually responsive?How do the stress concern about maintaining the deaf infants’
and emotional impact of a diagnosis of infant attention, and their desire to prolong eye contact.
deafness influence the early mother-infant The result is that the mother overrides any attempts
relationship, and are long-term effects (such as on by the child to explore the environment visually
socioemotional and communicative development) to (using such behaviours as physically turning the
be expected as a result? Are other sensory channels, infant‘s head back towards the partner, or moving
if used in a compensatory manner, interchangeable herself into the infant’s line of vision and
with hearing in these early interactions? inadvertently blocking the infant’s view of the
It has been reported that deaf mothers modify object of attention).
their sign language communication to an infant in Wood (1989) reports that deaf parents appear to
much the same way as hearing adults alter their know intuitively how to time their communicative
vocal speech in the form of motherese (Erting et al., efforts with a deaf child in order to make optimal
1990). That is, similar to motherese, sign language use of the infant‘s own attention strategies. By
by a deaf adult to an infant is simplified, more contrast, ‘hearing parents tend to be more intrusive
repetitious and less complex than with an adult. and overtly didactic, sometimes trying, literally and
However, most deaf infants (more than 90%) have physically, to orchestrate the infant’s role in the
hearing parents (Rawlings and Jensema, 1977), for interaction to ensure his attention’ (p. 67). For
56 L. S. Koester

example, Nienhuys and Tikotin (1983) report on than a parallel fashion. Visual attention to a social
their observations of one deaf and one hearing partner may be a crucial component of the deaf
infant between the ages of 6 and 12 months; they child’s early interactiverepertoire, but may also alter
found that the mother of the deaf infant tended to the expected sequencing or pacing of the
’lead’ the interactions, compared to the mother of interaction. ’Whenever it is necessary to negotiate
the normally hearing child who was described as the meaning of a new symbol or a symbolic
more effectively following her infant’s cues. expression, . . . the deaf child faces problems of
Not surprisingly, some of these same studies divided attention, the need to employ the visual
suggest that the deaf children themselves are more modality to monitor both acts and objects of
passive, less attentive and less responsive than communication‘ (Wood, 1989, p. 67).
hearing children; nevertheless, it is difficult to In a longitudinal comparison of deaf and hearing
determine which is the cause and which the effect. infants, Koester and Spencer (1992) found that the
That is, a passive child might respond to an overly amount of time deaf infants spent looking away
directive caregiver by withdrawing even more, just during early interactions (age 9 months) was
as the caregiver might respond to a child who is negatively related to their later communicative efforts
relatively non-reactiveby trying even harder to eliat (age 18 months). The opposite was true for hearing
a response- efforts which are then interpreted infants, suggesting that ‘gaze avert’ behaviour has
by observers as intrusive when viewed from different meaning for these two groups. That is,
the perspective of typical or ‘normal’ interactive when a hearing infant turns away to look at
behaviours. something in the surroundings, the caregiver often
In a related study, Meadow-Orlans and her responds with vocal narration or labelling of objects.
colleagues (1987) reported greater neutral affect in In contrast, the act of looking away from the partner
3- and 6-month-old infants whose mothers were by a deaf child effectively cuts off access to
deaf. This was seen as an indication that infants of communicationabout the new focus of interest. By
deaf mothers adapt to the greater amount of visual diminishing opportunities for joint attention,
information to which they must attend in the explanation and scaffolding, this simple gaze
absence of maternal vocal input. ‘If there is more avert may have long-term consequences for the
happening visually for these infants-on the development of language skills by the deaf child.
mother’s face, hands, and with the body- they may Deaf infants with hearing parents are faced with
become accustomed quite early to focusing intently a difficult challenge, that of interacting with a
in order to process the additional volume of caregiver whose usual mode of communication is
information provided in the modality that requires via a sensory channel not accessible to them. Thus
them to attend visually’ (Meadow-Orlans et al., they may experience higher levels of stress and
1987, p. 3). negative affect due to diminished reciprocity or
Since parental vocalizations are thought to be an synchrony with the partner. It is therefore to be
integral feature of dyadic interactions even with a expected that the tolerance levels of these infants
prelingual child, their absence or inaccessibility for would frequently be exceeded, forcing them to
a deaf infant may dramatically alter the nature of resort to behaviours such as self-comfort, protest,
the early parent-infant relationship. Behaviours gaze aversion or scanning the environment-rather
such as turn-taking, eliciting and maintaining than those behaviours which invite continued
attention and expressing pleasure or disapproval engagement with the social enviornment. Evidence
may all be highly dependent upon voice quality and of this is seen in the finding that deaf 9-month-olds
vocal patterns when hearing partners are involved; with hearing mothers engage in sigruficantlyfewer
when a parent or infant is deaf, however, similar efforts to signal, or re-engage, the mother when she
interactive patterns must be developed through is unresponsive than do their hearing agemates
alternative modalities if a comparable level of (Koester and Trimm, 1991).
satisfaction is to be achieved.
As stated earlier, Wood (1982,1989)has suggested
that deafness prevents the child from simultaneously PRECURSORS TO ATTACHMENT:
exploring the world visually and receiving the vocal FACE-TO-FACE INTERACTIONS
narrative that often accompanies such activity. Thus
for the deaf infant, visual attention to an object or A number of studies have examined the relationship
a person, and communication about that object or between the quality of infants’ attachments and
person, must typically occur ir. a sequential rather their earlier experiences of face-to-faceinteractions
lnteractions with Deaf lnfants 57

with the attachment figure (Blehar et al., 1977; to develop secure attachments. It has also been
Egeland and Farber, 1984; Isabella and Belsky, 1991; shown that deaf children with deaf parents
Isabella et al., 1989; Langhorst and Fogel, 1982; (presumably dyads with high communicative
Lewis and Feiring, 1989). It is assumed that competence) have attachment patterns similar to
sensitive, reciprocal and contingent interactions those found in hearing children with hearing
with the caregiver over the first year enhance the parents (Meadow et al., 1983).
probability of an infant being securely attached by As Lederberg and Mobley (1990) point out, the
the end of that year. As Blehar et al. (1977)report, development of attachment takes place continuously
mothers of infants who became anxiously attached over the infant’s first year in the context of face-to-
in their study ’more frequently initiated face-to-face face and other proximal interactions. Since the
interaction with a silent, impassive face and more infant is not completely mobile during these early
often failed to respond to their babies’ attempts to months, many of these interactions involve close
initiate interaction than did mothers of infants who physical contact with the caregiver. Therefore, the
were later judged to be securely attached’ (p. 190). mother’s ‘body language’ or non-vocal responses
In contrast, mothers of babies later classified as are often accessible to the deaf infant even in the
securely attached had been more contingent and absence of vocal reassurances.
facilitative of interaction in the previous Lederberg and her colleague found no significant
observations. Similarly, Egeland and Farber (1984) differences in the attachment classifications of deaf
found that securely attached infants had mothers and hearing toddlers (mean age 22 months), and
who were sensitive to their infants‘ needs, and who little effect of hearing status on their more global
encouraged reciprocity in earlier feeding and play interaction ratings. Attachment was related to
situations. toddler behaviours primarily in the sense that
In a recent paper comparing infants’ responses secure children initiated more of the interactions
to the still-face procedure at 4 months and their and engaged in more shared affect with their
subsequent attachment classificationsat 1year, Ross mothers. They concluded that ‘. . . sensitive
et al. (1991) suggest that this assessment of infant caregivers can adapt to a variety of special needs
coping strategies offers a better predictor of of their infantsltoddlers in such a way as to make
attachment than do either maternal or infant their children feel secure in their care‘ (Lederberg
behaviours during normal face-to-face situations. and Mobley, 1990, p. 1602).
They found that infants who were later rated as Similarly, Koester and MacTurk (1991) report no
secure had elicited more and displayed more sigruficant differences in attachment ratings for deaf
positive affect during the still-face than those who and hearing infants at 18 months. However,
were later rated as avoidant. although the overall classifications failed to
differentiate the two groups, the specific behaviours
contributing to the classifications did reveal
differences. For example, when reunited after
ATTACHMENT RELATIONSHIPS separation from their mothers, the hearing infants
IN DEAF CHILDREN sought more contact and maintained closer
There are reasons to expect that deafness might proximity than did the deaf infants. These two
have negative consequences for attachment, such behaviours involve such actions as looking,
as: (1)there is often poor communication between greeting, smiling, approaching, gesturing to be
infant and caregiver; (2) parental vocal feedback is picked up, reaching towards and touching the
inaccessible to the child, therefore reciprocal mother; they may therefore be thought of as parallel
interactions are infrequent and difficult to maintain; to the social eliciting behaviours which evolve
(3) stress is greater in families of children with during the infants’ early face-to-face interaction
special needs, and increased stress is associated experiences.
with insecure attachment (Vaughn et al., 1979). An increased incidence of avoidant behaviours
The degree to which attachment is affected by was also found among the deaf infants, suggesting
early deafness has been shown to be mediated by that their efforts to cope with the stress of separation
other variables such as communication proficiency. are more internalized than for hearing infants
Greenberg and Marvin (1979), for example, found (Koester and MacTurk, 1991). In terms of
that deaf preschoolers with poor communication attachment theory, avoidance is conceptualized as
skills were often insecurely attached, while those anger which is being turned inwards, whereas
with good communication skills were more likely resistance is seen as a more outward expression of
58 L. S . Koester

the same emotion. At age 9 months, these same important insights into the ways in which they
deaf and hearing infants were observed during intuitively accomplish these goals and thereby
face-to-face interactions with their mothers (Koester, contribute to the optimal development of infants
1991). Deaf infants responded to the stress of the growing up in a predominantly visual world.
still-face situation by resorting more frequently to
self-comfort than to the ‘other-directed’behaviours
observed in the hearing infants. In fact, hearing
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
infants in general (not just during the still-face)were
more likely to signal to their mothers, and to use Preparation of this manuscript was supported in
non-verbal behaviours to elicit social responses from part by the Gallaudet Research Institute, by grants
her, compared to deaf infants in the same situation. from the Division of Maternal and Child Health,
The higher rates of avoidant behaviour by deaf US Bureau of Health Care Delivery and Assistance
infants during the Strange Situation (Koester and (Grant MCJ-110563) to Gallaudet University, and
MacTurk, 1991) suggest an increased risk of from the Social and Behavioral Sciences Resarch
disturbed relationships between these infants and Program of the March of DimelBirth Defects
their hearing mothers. One cause may be the Foundation (Grant # 12-FY91-268). The assistance
difficulty in establishing an effective and contingent of Dr Kathryn Meadow-Orlans in reviewing an
system of communication that will be accessible to earlier draft of this paper is gratefully acknowledged.
the deaf infant. However, the mediating effect of
other forms of contingent responsiveness may help
to overcome these obstacles and in effect save the REFERENCES
relationship, as evidenced by the deaf infants‘
generally secure attachment ratings. Adamson, L., Als, H., Tronick, E. and Brazelton, T. B.
(1977).The development of social reciprocity between
a sighted infant and her blind parents. Journal of The
American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 16, 194-207.
SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Blehar, M.C., Lieberman, A. F. and Ainsworth, M. D. S.
(1977).Early face-to-face interaction and its relation to
Selective literature on early interactions and the later infant-mother attachment. Child Development, 48,
social-emotional development of infants has been 182-194.
discussed in this paper, with an emphasis on those Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B. and Main, M. (1974).The
findings of particular relevance to deaf infants and origins of reciprocity: the early mother-infant interaction.
In M. Lewis and L. Rosenblum (Eds), The Effect of the
their parents. Although research on this population Infant on its Caregiver. New York: Wiley, pp. 49-77.
is still sparse, advanced audiological technology is Brazelton, T. B. and Nugent, J. K. (1987). Neonatal
now making earlier diagnosis possible and therefore assessment as an intervention. In H. Rauh and H.-Ch.
increasing the availability of deaf subjects at a much Steinhausen (Eds), Psychobiology and Early Development.
younger age than previously. As a result, recent Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 215-229.
studies have begun to show some important Brown, J. V. and Bakeman, R. (1979). Relationships of
differences in interactive behaviours, particularly human mothers with their infants during the first year
of life. In R. W. Bell and W. P. Smotherman (Eds),
when a deaf infant is born to hearing parents. Both Maternal Influences and Early Behavior. Jamaica, N Y :
parents and infants in these dyads compensate at Spectrum.
an early age for the absence of auditory stimulation, Carter, A. S., Mayes, L. C. and Pajer, K. A. (1990). The
and the modality which most readily takes over for role of dyadic affect in play and infant sex in predicting
this purpose is that of vision. Whether or not the infant response to the still-face situation. Child
amount of compensation which apparently takes Development, 61, 764-773.
Cohn, J. F. and Tronick, E. Z. (1983). Three-month-old
place without instruction is sufficient for the infants’ reaction to simulated maternal depression. Child
cognitive and social needs of the infant has not yet Development, 54, 185-193.
been determined. It is quite possible that the more Day, P. S. and Prezioso, C. (1987).Anxious mothers and
subtle qualities which contribute to synchrony and at-risk infants: the influence of mild hearing impairment
mutuality between two partners-characteristics on early interaction. Society for Research in Child
such as contingency responding and maintenance Development, Baltimore, MD.
Egeland, B. and Farber, B. (1984). Infant-mother
of joint attention- are more difficult to establish attachment: factors related to its development and
when the usual vocal channel of communication is changes over time. Child Development, 55, 753-771.
not available. Further research which incorporates Erting, C. J., Prezioso, C. and Hynes, M. 0. (1990).
deaf parents as comparison groups should provide The interactional context of deaf mother-infant
Interactions with Deaf Infants 59

communication. In V. Volterra and C. Erting (Eds), From Koester, L. S. (1991). Face-to-face interactions between
Gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children. Berlin: hearing mothers and their deaf or hearing infants. In
Springer-Verlag, pp. 97-106. K. P. Meadow-Orlans, R. H. MacTurk, P. E. Spencer
Exline, R. V. (1982). Gaze behavior in infants and children: and L. S. Koester, Interaction and Support: Mothers and
a tool for the study of emotions? In C. E. Izard (Ed.), Deaf Infants (Grant #MCJ-110563). Washington, DC:
Measuring Emotions in lnfants and Children. New York: Gallaudet Research Institute.
Cambridge University Press, pp. 164-177. Koester, L. S. and MacTurk, R. H. (1991). Precursors of
Fernald, A. (1989). Intonation and communicative intent attachment relationships in deaf and hearing infants.
in mothers’ speech to infants: is the melody the Poster presented at International Society for the Study
message? Child Development, 60, 1497-1510. of Behavioural Development, Minneapolis, MN.
Field, T. M. (1977). Effects of early separation, interactive Koester, L. S., PapouSek, H. and PapouSek, M. (1987).
deficits, and experimental manipulations on infant- Psychobiological models of infant development:
mother face-to-face interaction. Child Development, 48, influences on the concept of intuitive parenting. In H.
763-771. Rauh and H.-Ch. Steinhausen (Eds), Psychobiology and
Field, T. M. (1978). The three R’s of infant-adult Early Development, Vol. 45, Advances in Psychology series.
interactions: rhythms, repertoires, and responsivity . North HollandlElsevier, pp. 275-287.
Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3(3), 131-136. Koester, L. S., PapouSek, H. and PapouSek, M. (1989).
Field, T. M. (1984). Early interactions between infants and Patterns of rhythmic stimulation by mothers with three-
their postpartum depressed mothers. lnfant Behavior and month-olds: a cross-modal comparison. International
Development, 7, 517-522. Journal of Behavioral Development, 12(2), 143-154.
Fraiberg, S. (1974). Blind infants and their mothers: an Koester, L. S. and Spencer, P. E. (1992). Early interaction
examination of the sign system. In M. Lewis and patterns and later communicative performance of deaf
L. A. Rosenblum (Eds), The Effect of the lnfant on its infants. Poster presented at the 100th Annual
Caregiver. New York: Wiley, pp. 215-232. Convention of the American Psychological Association,
Gianino, A. and Meadow-Orlans, K. P. (1987). Stress and Washington, DC.
self-regulation in six-month-old deaf and hearing infants Koester, L. S. and Trimm, V. M. (1991). Face-to-face
with deaf mothers. Poster presented at the biennial interactions with deaf and hearing infants: do maternal
meetings of the Society for Research in Child or infant behaviors differ? Paper presented at the
Development, Baltimore, MD. Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child
Goldberg, S. (1982). Some biological aspects of early Development, Seattle, WA.
parent-infant interaction. In S. G. Moore and C. R. Langhorst, B. and Fogel, A. (1982). Cross validation of
Cooper (Eds), The Young Child: Reviews of Research, Vol. microanalytic approaches to face-to-face play. Paper
3. Washington: NAEYC, pp. 35-56. presented at the International Conference on Infant
Greenberg, M. T. (1980). Social interaction between Studies, Austin, TX.
deaf preschoolers and their mothers: the effect of Lederberg, A. (1984). Interaction between deaf
communicationmethod and communicationcompetence. preschoolers and unfamiliar hearing adults. Child
Developmental Psychology, 16(5), 465-474. Development, 55, 598-606.
Greenberg, M. T. and Marvin, R. S. (1979). Attachment Lederberg, A., Chapin, S., Rosenblatt, V. and Vandell,
patterns in profoundly deaf preschool children. Mem’ll- D. (1986). Ethnic, gender, and age preferences among
Palmer Quarterly, 25(4), 265-279. deaf and hearing preschool peers. Child Development,
Henggeler, S. W. and Cooper, P. (1983). Deaf child- 2, 375-386.
hearing mother interaction: extensiveness and Lederberg, A. R. and Mobley, C. E. (1990). The effect of
reciprocity. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 8, 83-95. hearing impairment on the quality of attachment and
Henggeler, S. W., Watson, S . M. and Cooper, P. F. (1984). mother-toddler interaction. Child Development, 61,
Verbal and nonverbal maternal controls in hearing 1596-1604.
mother-deaf child interaction. Journal of Applied Lederberg, A. R., Willis, M. G. and Frankel, K. H. (1991).
Developmental Psychology, 5, 319-329. A longitudinal study of the effects of deafness on the
Isabella, R. A. and Belsky, J. (1991). Interactional early mother-child relationship. Poster presented at the
synchrony and the origins of mother-infant attachment: Biennial Meetings of the Society for Research in Child
a replication study. Child Development, 62, 373-384. Development, Seattle, WA.
Isabella, R. A., Belsky, J. and von Eye, A. (1989). The Lewis, M. and Feiring, C. (1989). Infant, mother, and
origins of infant-mother attachment: an examination mother-infant interaction behavior and subsequent
of interactional synchrony during the infant’s first year. attachment. Child Development, 60, 831-837.
Developmental Psychology, 25, 12-21. MacTurk, R. H. (1993). Social and motivational develop-
Jamieson, J. R. (in press). Teaching as transaction: ment in deaf and hearing infants. In D. J. Messer (Ed.),
Vygotskian perspectives on deafness and mother-child Mastey Motivation:Children’s Investigation, Persistence and
interaction. Exceptional Children. Development. London: Routledge, pp. 125-137.
Jones, 0. H. M. (1980). Prelinguistic communication skills MacTurk, R. H., Meadow-Orlans, K. P., Koester, L. S.
in Down’s syndrome and normal infants. InT. M. Field, and Spencer, P. E. (1993). Social support, motivation,
S. Goldberg, D. Stern and A. M. Sostek (Eds), High- language, and interaction: a longitudinal study of
Risk Infants and Children:Interactions w’th Adults and Peers. mothers and deaf infants. American Annals of the Deaf,
New York: Academic Press, pp. 205-226. 138(1), 19-25.
60 L. S. Koesfer

Marschark, M. (1992).Origins and interactions in social, Presented at the Biennial Meetings of the Society for
cognitive, and language development of deaf children. Resarch in Child Development, Seattle, WA.
In M. Marschark and E. Clark (Eds), Psychological Sameroff, A. J. and Chandler, M. J. (1975).Reproductive
Perspectives on Deafness. Edison, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 7-26. risk and the continuum of caretaking casualty. In
Mayes, L. C. and Carter, A. S. (1990).Emerging social F. D. Horowitz, M. Hetherington, S. Scarr-Salapatek
regulatory capacities as seen in the still-face situation. and G. Siege1(Eds), Revim of Child Development Research
Child Development, 61, 754-763. (Vol. 4 ) . Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Meadow, K. P.,Greenberg, M. T. andErting, C. (1983). Schaffer, H. R. (1984).Face-to-face interactions. The Child’s
Attachment behavior of deaf children with deaf parents. Entry into a Social World. London: Academic Press,
Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 22, pp. 44-78.
23-28. Schaffer, H. R. (1987). The social context of
Meadow, K. P., Greenberg, M. T., Erting, C. and psychobiological development. In H. Rauh and H.-Ch.
Carmichaei, H. (1981).Interactions of deaf mothers and Steinhausen (Eds), Psychobiology and Early Development.
deaf preschool children: comparisons with three other Amsterdam: Elsevier, pp. 239-255.
groups of deaf and hearing dyads. American Annals of Schlesinger, H. S. (1985).Deafness, mental health and
the Deaf, 126, 454-468. language. In F. Powell, T. Finitzo-Hieber, S. Firel-Patti
Meadow-Orlans, K. P.,MacTurk, R. H., Prezioso, C. T., and D. Henderson (Eds), Education of the Hearing-
Erting, C. J. and Day, P. S. (1987).Interactions of deaf Impaired Child. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press.
and hearing mothers with three- and six-month-old Schlesinger, H. S. and Meadow, K. P. (1972).Sound and
infants. Paper presented at the Biennial Meetings of Sign: Childhood Deafness andMental Health. Berkeley, CA:
the Society for Research in Child Development, University of California Press.
Baltimore, MD. Spencer, P. E. (1991).Mother-infant communication at
Meadow-Orlans, K. P. and Steinberg, A. (1991).Effects twelve and eighteen months. In K. P. Meadow-Orlans,
of infant hearing loss on mother-infant interactions at R. H. MacTurk, P. E. Spencer and L. S. Koester,
eighteen months. In K. P. Meadow-Orlans, R. H. lnferaction and Support: Mothers and Deaf Infants (Grant
MacTurk, P. E. Spencer and L. S. Koester, Interaction and # MCJ-110563).Washington, DC: Gallaudet Research
Support: Mothers and Deaf Znfants (Grant # MCJ-110563). Institute.
Washington, DC: Gallaudet Research Institute. Spencer, P. E., Bodner-Johnson, B. A. and Gutfreund,
Nienhuys, T. G. and Tikotin, J. A. (1983). Pre-speech M. K. (1992).Interacting with infants with a hearing
communication in hearing and hearing-impaired loss: what can we learn from mothers who are deaf?
children. Journal of British Association of Teachers of the Journal of Early Intervention, 16, 64-78.
Deaf, 6, 182-194. Stern, D. (1974).Mother and infants at play: the dyadic
PapouSek, H. and PapouSek, M. (1981).How human is interaction involving facial, vocal, and gaze behaviors.
the human newborn, and what else is to be done? In In M. Lewis and L. Rosenblum (Eds), The Effect of the
K. Bloom (Ed.), Prospective Zssues in Znfancy Research. Znfant on its Caregiver. New York: Wiley, pp. 187-213.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 137-155. Tronick, E. Z. (1989). Emotions and emotional
PapouSek, H. and PapouSek, M. (1982). Infant-adult communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44(2),
social interactions: their origins, dimensions, and 112-119.
failures. InT. M. Field, A. Huston, H. C. Quay, L. TrolI Tronick, E. Z., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S. and
and G. E. Fmley (Eds), Review of Human Development. Brazelton, T. 8. (1978). The infant’s response to
New York: Wiley. entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-
PapouSek, H. and PapouSek, M. (1987). Intuitive face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child
parenting: a dialectic counterpart to the infant’s Psychiatry, 17, 1-13.
precocity in integrativeCapacities. In J. D. Osofsky (Ed.), Vaughn, B., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A. and Waters, E.
Handbook of Infant Development (2nd edn). New York: (1979). Individual differences in infant-mother
Wiley, pp. 669-720. attachment at twelve and eighteen months: stability and
PapouSek, M., PapouSek, H. andBomstein, M. H. (1985). change in families under stress. Child Development, 50,
The naturalisticvocal environment of young infants: on 971-975.
the significance of homogeneity and variability in Wedell-Monnig,J.and Lumley, J. (1980).Child deafness
parental speech. In T. Field and N. Fox (Eds), Social and mother-child interaction. Child Development, 51,
Perception in Infants. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 766-774.
Rawlings, 8. W. and Jensema, C. J. (1977). Two Studies Wood, D. J. (1982). The linguistic experiences of the
of the Families of Hearing-Zmpired Children. Series R, prelingually hearing-impaired child. Journal of the British
Number 5.Washington, DC: Gallaudet College, Office Association of Teachers of the Deaf, 6(4), 86-93.
of Demographic Studies. Wood, D. J.(1989).Social interaction as tutoring. In M.
Ross, S., Cohn, J. F. and Campbell, S. B. (1991).Infant Bornstein and J. Bruner (Eds), Interaction in Human
response in the still-face predicts attachment security. Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 59-80.

Potrebbero piacerti anche