Sei sulla pagina 1di 20

A Ten-Year Chronicle of Student

Attitudes Toward Foreign Language


in the Elementary School
AUDREY L. HEINING–BOYNTON THOMAS HAITEMA
School of Education Fike High School
The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill 500 Harrison Drive
Chapel Hill, NC 27599-3500 Wilson, NC 27893
Email: ahb@email.unc.edu Email: thaitema@nc.rr.com

This article reports the results of 2 studies conducted over a 10-year period that researched
student attitudes toward early foreign language learning. These studies are unique because of
the long time frame in which the students were followed, and the large data sets collected at
the elementary school level. Surveys of students in the Foreign Language in the Elementary
Schools (FLES) programs examined the attitudes of all children in 2 school systems in North
Carolina: one suburban and one urban. The students studied either French or Spanish. The
number of responses to the survey questions ranged from 22,549 to 52,227 for a 4-year period.
The results indicated that boys and girls had positive attitudes when responding to 2 questions
about enjoyment of their FLES classes and teachers. The girls were positively inclined, and the
boys were neutral, in their desire to continue with foreign language study in the next grade.
Two items about the use of foreign language outside the school venue and comprehension of
foreign language teacher input revealed negative attitudes for both genders.
As a follow-up to the quantitative study, qualitative data about attitudes toward foreign
language speakers, foreign cultures, and their own education with respect to foreign language
study were gathered through structured interviews. The participants were the same students
who had completed the original surveys 10 years earlier. In corroboration of the quantitative
data, a qualitative analysis revealed that, for a majority of the students, foreign language study
was viewed positively, as were foreign language speakers and their cultures.

PUBLIC EDUCATION DEPENDS ON PUBLIC specific programs can be vital to continued finan-
support, and that support at the local, state, and cial support. In the world of education, the stu-
federal levels has been tied increasingly to ac- dent is an important member of the consuming
countability. Educational programs must respond public.
to the public’s desire to know if the funds they This article reports on two studies carried out to
allocate have been well spent. The public as well ascertain the chronology and development over
as school district personnel shape their notions of time of student attitudes toward learning lan-
program efficacy in concert with program evalua- guages in addition to English. The first study was
tion and high stakes testing. How educational pro- designed to follow and document a large popu-
grams are perceived depends on the participants’ lation of students in two school districts in North
and other stakeholders’ attitudes toward them. Carolina that offer either Spanish or French, or
A positive attitude can translate into increased both, at the elementary school level. The second
support for the program, whereas a negative atti- study was a follow-up study that focused on a group
tude may often lead to the program’s termination. of the students who were part of the original study
Thus, gauging the attitude of the public toward and were currently studying a world language in
high school. The purpose of both studies was to
document attitudes over time, and the research
The Modern Language Journal, 91, ii, (2007) questions for both studies are related.
0026-7902/07/149–168 $1.50/0 The purpose of the quantitative elementary

C 2007 The Modern Language Journal
study was to determine and document student
150 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
attitudes toward foreign languages (FLs) in the summaries of events and occurrences that per-
Foreign Language in the Elementary Schools mit a person to make quick decisions about the
(FLES) instruction over time. The specific re- significance of what he or she has witnessed or
search questions were: heard. What actions a person may take in a given
1. How do students’ attitudes toward FL study situation depend, to a great extent, on previously
change over time? held attitudes. As Maio and Olson (2000) noted,
2. How does the desire of students to continue attitudes aid in this decision making and simplifi-
with FL study change over time? cation by dividing the world into easily identifiable
3. How do the students’ attempts to use the and manipulated categories: things I like, dislike,
language outside of class change over time? or have no opinion about. In the area of cogni-
4. How do students feel their linguistic compe- tion, attitudes define the self. A person is the sum
tence changes over time? total of his or her attitudes. Thus, it is difficult
5. Are there gender differences with regard to for people to give up attitudes, even in the light
FL study, and if so, do they change over time? of overwhelming evidence that his or her percep-
tion of what has occurred is wrong. In addition,
The purpose of the qualitative follow-up high attitudes can guide the actual perceptual and cog-
school study was to answer the following three nitive processes.
research questions: Fazio (2000) offered yet another reason for the
1. How do former FLES students feel that their persistence of attitudes. In addition to imposing
early FL instruction affected their perceptions of coherence on and making sense of the world, at-
FL speakers? titudes provide cognitive simplicity, thus fulfilling
2. How do former FLES students feel that their the human need for affect. That is, by categorizing
early FL instruction affected their perceptions of the universe into things that one likes, dislikes, or
foreign cultures? is indifferent to, a person is able to maximize inter-
3. How do former FLES students feel that their actions with objects, situations, or people that en-
early FL instruction affected their perceptions of hance affective fulfillment and to minimize those
their own FL education? interactions that do not.
Other researchers define attitudes in terms of
REVIEW OF PREVIOUS SCHOLARSHIP functions that may be seen as complementary
to Zimbardo and Leippe’s (1991) classification.
The importance of attitudes in second lan- Fazio (2000) viewed attitudes as having an object
guage studies cannot be underestimated. Gardner appraisal function that is dependent on the ac-
(1985), for instance, cited research indicating that cessibility of the attitude object in the individual’s
the more positive students’ attitudes toward the mind. This accessibility, in turn, depends on prior
object of study, the higher their achievement. The experience with the object. Access to the attitude
lack of literature chronicling changes in student will be slower in first-time encounters than in sub-
attitudes toward FL study necessitates a review of sequent encounters. Indeed, if the object, or one
literature on attitude change for other subjects or similar to it, has never been encountered before,
fields, such as politics. then an attitude may be formed for the first time,
and subsequent encounters will crystallize or set it
Functions of Attitudes in the mind. Studies examining the issues of atti-
tude formation and crystallization are cited in the
Attitudes are not merely opinions that often next section.
influence decisions we make about everyday mat- Herek (2000) provided a useful dichotomy for
ters. They represent psychological constructs that the functionality of attitudes. He proposed that at-
have attracted the attention of researchers for titudes have instrumental or symbolic functions.
decades. According to Zimbardo and Leippe An instrumental function is based on an individ-
(1991), attitudes have three main components: ual’s self-interest in strictly utilitarian terms. It is
affective, behavioral, and cognitive. In the affec- founded on an object’s direct usefulness to the
tive realm, attitudes influence perceptions and person. A symbolic function views the attitude ob-
thought. Often, the way everyday life events are ject as a symbol that reflects deep-seated values
perceived depends on the affective portion of the and prejudices. It is indicative of social adjustment
attitudes that the viewer brings to the event. In the and is closely related to identity affirmation, ex-
areas of behavior, attitudes exist in order to sim- actly what makes a person have the personality
plify everyday existence. They provide evaluative that he or she has.
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 151
Attitude Change During the School Years attitudes toward specific subjects were somewhat
mixed.
The trinity that composes attitude—affect, be- Social studies received the lowest rating from all
havior, and cognition—has been examined in a the students at every grade level; student attitudes
number of studies that have looked at, among were extremely negative. Physical education and
other areas, the formation and subsequent sta- art were among the most positively viewed, fol-
bility of attitudes, attempts to predict academic lowed by science, math, and reading. All subject
behavior from attitudes, and the changes in atti- matters showed a decline in the students’ estima-
tudes toward various subjects over the academic tion, with positive attitudes declining significantly
careers of the study participants. These studies in all but science and reading. For these two sub-
have considered these changes as a function of jects, attitudes declined, but the change was not
age and gender. These studies will be reviewed statistically significant. The major declines in at-
and their implications for the present study eluci- titudes occurred between the sixth and seventh
dated. The almost complete lack of similar stud- grades and between the first and second grades.
ies on FL makes it necessary to use longitudinal Apparently, FL was not offered in this school sys-
studies from other subject areas that may allow tem at the time of the study because it did not
inferences to be drawn for FL. appear as a separate subject.
Eagly and Chaiken (1993), in a comprehensive Other researchers have observed the general
discussion of attitudes, lamented the lack of infor- decline in attitudes of elementary students toward
mation on the actual formation of attitudes in nat- school. Davies and Brember (2001) surveyed atti-
uralistic settings. They commented that this lack tudes of almost 800 children toward school in gen-
of information results from the inherent difficulty eral in a longitudinal study. The participants were
in determining at what point an attitude appears surveyed using a Smiley-face Likert scale in the
in a real-life scenario versus in the artificial envi- second and sixth grades. The authors reported a
ronment of the laboratory. An extensive review of significant decline in attitudes for both males and
pertinent literature revealed that in the interven- females. Boys started out significantly more nega-
ing years, little has changed in the knowledge of tive in their attitudes but were almost overtaken by
attitude formation in naturalistic settings. the girls over the course of the study. In an earlier
Two other studies used a multifactor model to study, Davies and Brember (1994) had detected a
determine which factors had the greatest effect downward trend in attitudes between the second
on attitudes toward specific subjects. Factor analy- and fourth grades in a study of 167 students. They
sis suggested that home environment, motivation, examined changes in young children’s attitudes
and attitude toward science in the previous grade toward school in general and toward certain ac-
accounted for the majority of the present attitude tivities in particular between the ages of 6 and 8.
toward science (Reynolds & Walberg, 1992a). In They also found that, although children overall
an analysis of their results, Reynolds and Walberg had a positive attitude toward school, there was a
suggested that any attempt to bolster achievement decline in attitude toward specific subjects or to-
among science students would have to be mul- ward activities related to them. Girls declined in
tipronged because the interconnectedness of all positive attitudes toward math, whereas boys in-
the factors means that changing one factor will creased slightly. Boys maintained a slightly greater
influence all the others. In a subsequent study, preference for science, and the girls a preference
Reynolds and Walberg (1992b) determined that for reading.
the same factors affected high school students’ at- Donato, Tucker, Wudthayagorn, and Igarashi
titudes toward mathematics. The conclusion they (2000) examined the attitudes of a small cohort
drew from their studies was that achievement in- of students (N = 17) who were involved in a
fluences attitude rather than the reverse, at least Japanese FLES program. They found, contrary
in high school. to the studies just cited, that the more years a
An older study by Haldyna and Thomas (1979) student had been in the Japanese language pro-
focused on the attitudes of elementary school gram, the more positive the attitude. This cohort
students toward school in general and toward of students achieved moderate success in speaking
certain subjects, in particular. They used a cross- ability while attending the Falk Laboratory School
sectional design to examine the attitudes of over at the University of Pittsburgh (Donato, Antonek,
3,000 students in first through eighth grades. The & Tucker, 1996) and appeared to be atypical to
children showed a definite decline in positive at- the previously cited studies.
titudes toward school overall that corresponded Shrigley (1990), in a meta-analysis of attitudes
to their progression through the grade levels, but and behavior, uncovered studies indicating that
152 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
attitude precedes behavior, and other studies that dren studying French and Spanish in elementary
showed the reverse. He concluded that attitude school over a 4-year period. The follow-up study
and behavior are correlates that mutually influ- is qualitative and explores the attitudes of 13 high
ence each other. He postulated that positive or school students who were among the original par-
pleasant experiences could lead to the devel- ticipants in the quantitative study.
opment of positive attitudes, whereas negative This article reports the two studies separately.
experiences would provoke a negative reaction. At the end, conclusions are drawn that relate
He also posited that if a person approaches an the answers to the research questions of both
object with a positive preformed attitude, he or studies.
she would likely have a positive experience, and
vice versa. Shrigley presented a “chicken or the ELEMENTARY SCHOOL STUDY
egg” scenario in which the influence can be in ei-
The two FLES programs used in this study, both
ther direction depending on the prior conditions
content based, have been functioning since the
at the time of the interaction with the attitudinal
late 1980s. Content-based FLES programs teach
object.
subject matter through the second language as
It is interesting that most researchers find that
a reinforcement of what the students are learn-
students become increasingly disenchanted with
ing in their regular classroom. These programs
school and often with particular subjects. The
have offered a second language from kinder-
males almost always have a more negative atti-
garten through fifth grade. The languages are ei-
tude, but in the case of the Davies and Brember
ther French or Spanish.
(2001) study, the females became almost as neg-
ative. Also, the study by Smith (1990) indicated Design of the Study
that attitudes, at least toward scholastic pursuits
such as reading, can remain relatively stable over A survey was conducted in these school systems
long periods. for 6 consecutive years from 1990 to 1995. The in-
Other studies have uncovered a negative cor- strument used was the FLES Program Evaluation
relation between student attitudes and years of Inventory (FPEI), which was created and used pre-
study. Attitudes tend to become more negative viously (Heining–Boynton, 1990).
as students spend more years studying a subject.
Haldyna and Thomas (1979) detected a negative Design and Development of the FPEI Questionnaire.
trend on the part of the majority of the students There are five different FPEI forms that target
that they studied. Similarly, Davies and Brem- each of the major stakeholders in FLES programs:
ber (1994, 2001) noted that both males and fe- students, parents, FLES teachers, classroom teach-
males showed marked declines in attitudes toward ers, and school administrators. The instruments
school in general and toward certain subjects in incorporate questions that correspond to 10 FLES
particular. Kush and Watkins (1996) reported that program issues: 6 of historical concern and 4 that
male and female students showed a significant were considered of contemporary concern at the
decline in attitudes toward reading between the time of the creation of the surveys. The histori-
fourth and seventh grades. Finally, Smith (1990) cal issues were qualified teachers; goals and ob-
reported that in his 40-year longitudinal study of jectives; pedagogy; articulation with middle and
attitudes toward reading, these attitudes tended high school; homework, grades/evaluation; and
to remain stable over extended periods. parent support. Contemporary issues at the time
These cited studies shed light on what might of the study were acceptance of FLES teachers
be expected for changes in attitude of FLES stu- by colleagues, workload, at-risk students, and stu-
dents. Because there were no long-term longitudi- dent satisfaction (Heining–Boynton, 1990). Many
nal studies dealing with elementary FL students, it of these concerns, of course, remain today.
was necessary to infer possible results from earlier A team of 25 FLES teachers, administrators,
studies on different academic areas. and consultants developed the pilot survey of the
FPEI for students. It consisted of four questions
OVERVIEW OF THE PRESENT STUDIES answered by marking a smiling or frowning face
on the students’ answer sheets after the teacher
The purpose of the present research is to report read the prompt (see Appendix A). In June of
the attitudes of students over an extended period 1989, 7,038 kindergarten and first-grade students
toward learning a language in addition to English in District A responded to the pilot version of the
in the elementary school. This research blends, in student FPEI (Heining–Boynton, 1990).
effect, two studies. The first study is quantitative Each year up to 1995, the last year of this
and comprises a large data set of thousands of chil- study, the same cohorts of students were surveyed
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 153
following the same procedures as in the pilot of the population, this study undertook to ex-
study. The surveys were distributed at the end of amine every member of the population of possi-
the school year so that the students had the ben- ble respondents. Only children who were absent
efit of a full year of study on which to base their on the days the surveys were administered were
responses. excluded. Therefore, practically speaking, there
Teachers were actively involved in the prepa- were no sampling errors.
ration of the student surveys. Some years, the Three Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were cal-
teachers felt that they wanted to find out if cer- culated to estimate the reliability of the FPEI. The
tain changes they had made to the program had questions from the surveys were divided accord-
been beneficial in the opinion of their students. ing to their domains. The first group of three
Therefore, they added items to some of the stu- items dealt with the children’s general appreci-
dent surveys to acquire feedback pertinent to mak- ation of FL study. These items were “Do you like
ing improvements to the program. For example, foreign language class?” “Do you like the foreign
in the fourth grade, teachers began introducing language teacher?” and “Is the foreign language
reading and writing. The survey used for fourth class fun?” The alpha for these three items was .76.
grade reflected that addition and asked the stu- The second group comprised the questions on
dents to respond to the prompts “I can write some writing, reading, and interesting activities in class.
words in the foreign language” and “I can read The alpha coefficient for this set of three was .50,
some words in the foreign language.” The fifth- which is moderately low. The lower value may be
grade survey had the prompt “Most of the time an artifact of the wording of the prompts and the
I understand what my foreign language teacher yes/no answer format. The final group of survey
is saying.” These prompts did not correspond to items included the prompts asking for feedback
any prompts in the other grade levels and thus on continuation of FL study, understanding what
are not reported and were not used in the lon- the teacher said, and using FL outside class. The
gitudinal analysis. They were intended to provide alpha of .50 was not unexpected given that these
information to the FLES practitioner. three questions were not highly interrelated, but
The other prompts and questions in the surveys were nevertheless grouped for this calculation.
addressed the same concerns and topics, but the
prompt order changed in some of the grade levels. Description of FPEI Survey Populations. District
Thus, a prompt that was number 3 in the third- A public schools have students from a population
grade survey may have been number 5 in the fifth- that ranges from urban to suburban. The overall
grade survey. Reliability analysis was conducted to racial composition of the schools did not change
determine the presence of any bias in the order of over the years of the FPEI study period, with the
the questions and the effect of the varied wording following ethnic makeup remaining fairly con-
of some prompts. No evidence of bias appeared stant: Caucasian, 69%; African American, 27%;
in either case, and the analysis suggested that the and Asian American, Hispanic, and Native Amer-
prompts were querying the same domain. ican, 4%. The percentage of students receiving
Face and content validity were established for free or reduced lunches remained constant at
the FPEI by examining the items and determining 25% of the total student body. The student pop-
that they indeed reflected the intent of the instru- ulation increased, which led to a change in the
ment, which was to determine attitudes toward FL number of elementary schools from 54 in 1990 to
instruction. No extraneous items could be consid- 80 in 2002 to 93 in 2006.
ered irrelevant to the research. That practition- District B schools experienced some growth in
ers knowledgeable in the field of FLES wrote the student population, but the racial composition by
items adds to the face validity. The clarity of the percentage remained constant over the study pe-
items that were answerable in a dichotomous man- riod, with the following makeup: Caucasian, 73%;
ner also contributes to this validity. African American, 18%; and Asian American, His-
As defined by Eagly and Chaiken (1993), con- panic, and Native American, 9%. The percentage
struct validity is the determination of whether an of students receiving free or reduced lunches re-
attitude measure actually does what was intended. mained constant at 18% of the total student body.
In the FPEI, the questions are based on certain District B went from six elementary schools in
generally accepted notions about the nature and 1990 to eight in 2003 and to nine in 2006.
functioning of attitudes. An examination of the The two districts in the study are not represen-
survey items indicated that they demonstrated tative of all schools in the United States. Never-
construct validity. theless, there is no reason to believe that what we
A final indication of the instrument’s validity is found would be different in other districts nation-
the fact that rather than just sampling a portion wide.
154 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
The elementary school study focused on the array of choices, such as in a traditional Lik-
attitudes of elementary school students toward ert scale of 1–5. The dichotomous nature of the
studying an FL. The analysis was exclusively of a responses may be a factor in the low spoilage rate
secondary nature. Data for five questions to which of the surveys. Responses were coded by the re-
a very large sample of elementary school students searchers with yes having a value of 1, and no a
responded formed the basis of analysis. Accord- value of 0. The mean of the responses is equal to
ingly, the findings, although confirming what was the proportion of affirmative answers. The scores
noted in the previous review of the literature, 0 and 1 represent, respectively, absence and pres-
should be taken as a modest attempt to give sta- ence of an attribute. The computed proportion
tistical meaning to well-established trends in the assumes a latent continuous measure in the pop-
teaching of FL in particular. ulation. This computed proportion (the mean)
is the best unbiased estimator of the attitude be-
Administration of the FPEI Survey. During the
ing measured with the assumption that an average
period 1990 to 1995, the FPEI was distributed
value of the score truly represents the group as a
annually in District A and District B. The chil-
whole in a single quantifiable figure. In addition,
dren surveyed were in kindergarten through fifth
the students were asked to identify themselves as
grade. The data for second through fifth graders
male or female.
are presented in this report. The surveys were con-
Two matters need to be emphasized. First, con-
ducted annually toward the end of the school year.
sidering that the data correspond to cross sec-
The dates varied each year.
tions of the student population in two different
The number of responses varied from year to
educational districts, we determined whether dif-
year in each district. Only those students present
ferences were observed over time. Second, we
on the day the survey was administered were
tested the hypothesis that boys and girls differ
counted. This number surpassed the number of
in attitudes toward FL learning and interest. In
participants needed for the analysis; therefore, it
addition, when the sample was split by gender,
was not necessary to administer a follow-up survey.
the intention was to determine whether the mean
Response rates exceeded 95% based on total en-
scores for boys differed from the mean score for
rollments. Overall, there was an observed decline
girls in any given year of the study. Other studies
in sample size for all groups (male and female)
have shown that females are more likely to have
over time.
better attitudes toward school, teachers, and the
Many of the same children responded to the
subject matter (Davies & Brember, 2001; Haldyna
survey each year, whereas others left the districts,
& Thomas, 1979).
and still others entered and were then subse-
Owing to very large sample sizes, all year and
quently administered the survey. Data indicating
gender differences, except one, were statistically
the exact percentage of turnover were not avail-
significant at the alpha = .05 level. Statistical sig-
able and, hence, could not be tabulated during
nificance is not indicated in the tables. The sole
the survey years. The initial purpose of the study
nonsignificant gender difference is discussed in
was to provide anonymous feedback to the teach-
the text. We focus on the significant findings as
ers, as well as to collect group data; only school
we present the results.
identifiers were used and are not germane to this
To highlight gender differences in attitudes to-
study.
ward FL learning, a means test was conducted. An
The number of questions in each grade level
alternative statistical approach would have been
survey varied. Considerably fewer children in the
simply to analyze the association of the dependent
total sample responded to certain questions that
variable (Do you like foreign language?) to years and
were included only in the fourth- and fifth-grade
gender. Cross-tabulations and chi-square could
forms. Other items were present in each grade-
have been used to detect the presence of an associ-
level survey, which accounted for the highest
ation. However, that approach is less useful than
response count of 52,227. For questions that were
the means test because all that can be stated in
not included at each grade level, the cumulative
measuring association is its existence. The anal-
response count was considerably less.
ysis used in this research makes possible clearer
Statistical Methods. Very few of the survey re- interpretations of the observed change in prefer-
sponse sheets from any of the survey periods were ences over time.
unusable. The answers to the prompts in the FPEI
surveys were set up as either yes or no responses. Treatment of the Data. Data for District A and
This format was used to take into account the District B are presented in separate sections, each
difficulty that very young children have in decid- with cohorts that range from 3- to 4-year peri-
ing between what may be considered a confusing ods. The decision to present data for the two
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 155
districts separately was made on the basis of two TABLE 1
considerations. First, the number of participants Means Over Time for Cohort 1 District A, 1992–
surveyed in the District A cohorts, 13,000 or more 1995, Grades 2 to 5
in some instances, would mask any possible differ- Boys Girls
ences in the smaller District B cohorts, N = 1,800 Do you like
or less. Second, there were programmatic differ- N Ma SD FL class? N Ma SD
ences in the delivery of services to the students.
1930 .84 0.37 1992 1829 .90 0.30
District B schools had more FLES contact time 1761 .75 0.43 1993 1852 .82 0.38
than District A schools. 1259 .65 0.48 1994 1308 .70 0.46
1232 .51 0.50 1995 1311 .59 0.49
Results 6182 .71 0.46 Total 6300 .77 0.42
Is the FL teacher fun?
What follows are the results of documenting the 1930 .91 0.29 1992 1829 .95 0.23
attitudes of children toward studying French and 1761 .90 0.30 1993 1852 .93 0.25
Spanish in elementary school. For each cohort, 1259 NA NA 1994 1308 NA NA
we report mean responses to the five questions 1232 .75 0.43 1995 1311 .83 0.38
over time followed by mean responses by gender. 6182 .87 0.34 Total 6300 .91 0.29
The results are interpreted based on the research Continue with FL next year?
questions. Appendix A links each FPEI item to the 1930 NA NA 1992 1829 NA NA
appropriate research question. 1761 .69 0.46 1993 1852 .77 0.42
1259 .53 0.50 1994 1308 .61 0.50
1232 .64 0.48 1995 1311 .73 0.48
Cohort 1 District A, 1992–1995, Second to Fifth 6182 .63 0.48 Total 6300 .71 0.45
Grade. Table 1 summarizes student responses in Use FL at home or with friends?
Cohort 1 District A, which was first surveyed in 1930 NA NA 1992 1829 NA NA
second grade in 1992 and last surveyed as fifth 1761 .39 0.49 1993 1952 .51 0.50
graders in 1995. Data are presented for both gen- 1259 NA NA 1994 1308 NA NA
ders and each year of data collection, as well as 1232 .31 0.46 1995 1311 .41 0.49
totals for both genders and all years. The value 6182 .36 0.48 Total 6300 .36 0.48
NA corresponds to not asked due to the absence Understand what FL teacher says?
of the question for particular years. 1930 NA NA 1992 1829 NA NA
Statistics by gender for the 1992–1995 Cohort 1761 .54 0.50 1993 1952 .60 0.49
1259 NA NA 1994 1308 NA NA
1 District A, appear in Table 2. For all students,
1232 .48 0.50 1995 1311 .48 0.50
regardless of their grade, females had a more pos- 6182 .52 0.50 Total 6300 .59 0.49
itive attitude than boys for each of the pertinent
items. Girls also had a higher mean score for each Note. NA = This item was not asked for this cohort
item than boys. in this year.
a
Yes = 1; No = 0.
For Cohort 1 District A, the data indicated that
for every question, girls scored higher than boys.
For mean scores across time, there was a down-
ward trend. For girls, the item “Do you like for- a drop of 8%; for girls (.93 to .83), it is a drop of
eign language class?” declined from .90 to .59 in 5%. The third item, “Are you going to continue
4 years. The observed decline for boys was from with the foreign language the next year?” showed
.84 to .51. In relative terms, both boys and girls a drop in the score followed by an increase in the
expressed a loss of liking of FL of about 17% observed value. Both girls and boys exhibited a
in 4 years. Research for other subject areas in- similar wave-type pattern. Because there was no
dicates that student attitudes also decline as stu- information for the 1992 year for this cohort, one
dents become older and are promoted to higher cannot see a definite trend for this item. A higher
grades. Davies and Brember (1994), for instance, score in the final year of measure, corresponding
determined that despite positive attitudes toward to fifth graders, seems to indicate a greater aware-
school, specific subjects were perceived in an in- ness on the part of the students of the importance
creasingly negative light. of learning an FL.
The second item, “Is the foreign language The fourth item, “Sometimes I use the foreign
teacher fun?” also showed a downward trend. language at home or with friends” appeared in the
Both genders scored higher than they did for the third- and fifth-grade surveys. There was a decline
previous item, which indicated a particular liking in the proportion for both boys and girls. Intu-
of the teacher. Over time, however, the enthusi- itively, this finding is unexpected because there is
asm declined. The decline for boys (.90 to .75) is an expectation of increased usage of FL due to the
156 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
TABLE 2
Means by Gender for Cohort 1 District A, 1992–1995, Grades 2 to 5
Gender N Ma SD t df Sig (2-tailed)

Do you like the FL class? Boys 6157 .71 0.45 –7.53 124321 0.000
Girls 6276 .77 0.42
Is the FL teacher fun? Boys 4914 .87 0.34 – 7.155 9888 0.000
Girls 4976 .91 0.29
Do you want to continue Boys 4231 .63 0.48 –8.191 8679 0.000
with FL next year? Girls 4450 .71 0.45
Sometimes I use the FL at Boys 2983 .36 0.48 –8.179 6140 0.000
home or with friends. Girls 3159 .47 0.50
Most of the time I Boys 2979 .52 0.50 –5.658 6123 0.000
understand what the Girls 3146 .59 0.49
teacher is saying.
a
Yes = 1; No = 0.

larger number of years of contact. The decline is Cohort 3 District A, 1993–1995, Second to Fourth
negligible. Grade. Cohort 3 District A (first surveyed in second
The fifth item, “Most of the time I understand grade in 1993 and last surveyed as fourth graders
what the foreign language teacher is saying” was in 1995) follow the same downward trends of Co-
more stable than the other four items. More or horts 1 and 2. As Table 5 shows, over time, as
less both girls and boys fluctuate over time with
similar proportions, with the noted higher aver-
age for girls, as it occurs with the other four items TABLE 3
surveyed. The lower proportion for this item may Means Over Time for Cohort 2 District A, 1992–
indicate comprehension difficulties as the instruc- 1994, Grades 3 to 5
tion becomes more complex. Boys Girls
For all cohorts, the strongest observed decline Do you like
related to Question 1. It was also the only ques- N Ma SD FL class? N Ma SD
tion with complete data for 4 years measured for
1657 .81 0.39 1992 1652 .85 0.36
District A. The data confirm that students lose in-
1810 .67 0.47 1993 1740 .72 0.45
terest in FL as they move to higher grades. 1279 .57 0.50 1994 1265 .61 0.49
4746 .69 0.46 Total 4657 .73 0.44
Cohort 2 District A, 1992–1994, Third to Fifth Is FL teacher fun?
Grade. Table 3 shows differences over time for Co- 1657 .94 0.24 1992 1652 .96 0.19
hort 2, which was first surveyed in the third grade 1810 NA NA 1993 1740 NA NA
in 1992 and last surveyed in the fifth grade in 1994. 1279 .79 0.41 1994 1265 .82 0.38
As noted previously, a consistent decline in liking 4746 .87 0.33 Total 4657 .90 0.30
the FL teacher was evident for both genders. In Continue with FL?
relative terms, the drop was about the same for 1657 .74 0.44 1992 1652 .79 0.41
1810 .57 0.49 1993 1740 .61 0.49
boys (.81 to .57) and girls (.85 to .61), that is,
1279 .54 0.50 1994 1265 .62 0.49
about 13%.
4746 .61 0.47 Total 4746 .67 0.46
For the remaining four items, and allowing for
Use FL at home/friends?
missing data for some years, there persisted a con- 1657 .42 0.49 1992 1652 .79 0.41
sistent pattern of decline in interest. The students’ 1810 NA NA 1993 1740 NA NA
attraction to the teacher dropped, as well as the in- 1279 .30 0.46 1994 1265 .40 0.49
terest in continuing with FL. The items question- 4746 .37 0.48 Total 4657 .48 0.50
ing FL usage and comprehension, respectively, Understand FL teacher?
also declined with little exception. 1657 .57 0.50 1992 1652 .64 0.48
Table 4 shows the same general pattern ob- 1810 NA NA 1993 1740 NA NA
served for other cohorts for District A as well 1279 .47 0.50 1994 1265 .57 0.50
as for District B. Female students scored higher 4746 .53 0.50 Total 4657 .61 0.49
than boys for all five items in the survey. The dif- Note. NA = This item was not asked for this cohort
ferences were all significant at the .001 level of in this year.
confidence. a
Yes = 1; No = 0.
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 157
TABLE 4
Means by Gender for Cohort 2 District A, 1992–1994, Grades 3 to 5
Gender N Ma SD t df Sig (2-Tailed)

Do you like the FL class? Boys 4731 .69 0.46 –4.662 9367 0.000
Girls 4638 .73 0.44
Is the FL teacher fun? Boys 2931 .87 0.33 –3.265 5834 0.001
Girls 2905 .90 0.30
Do you want to continue Boys 4731 .63 0.48 –4.983 9374 0.000
with FL next year? Girls 4645 .67 0.47
Sometimes I use the FL at Boys 2934 .37 0.48 –8.983 5842 0.000
home or with friends. Girls 2910 .48 0.50
Most of the time I Boys 2918 .53 0.50 –6.152 5825 0.000
understand what the Girls 2909 .61 0.49
teacher is saying.
a
Yes = 1; No = 0.

the students grew older and advanced to higher These differences can perhaps best be explained
grades, both girls and boys exhibited an already as resulting from district characteristics. District B
seen downward tendency for Item 1, the liking of is a smaller school system, with a more homoge-
FL class. Girls had a much higher starting point nous population and greater achievement levels.
(.91) than boys (.86). The relative drop in liking
FL was about 11%.
For the remaining items in the survey, it is diffi- TABLE 5
cult to draw conclusions about changes over time Means Over Time for Cohort 3 District A, 1993–
because for these items there are missing points 1995, Grades 2 to 4
resulting from the absence of the question dur- Boys Girls
ing the surveys. In general, students of both gen- Do you like
ders liked their teachers (Item 2) very much. But N Ma SD FL class? N Ma SD
their inclination to continue with FL dropped,
1883 .86 0.34 1993 1808 .91 0.28
and their usage of the FL, as well as their compre-
1411 .72 0.45 1994 1425 .82 0.38
hension of it, dropped to mean levels observed in 1384 .63 0.48 1995 1394 .69 0.46
other cohorts. 4678 .75 0.43 Total 4627 .82 0.38
Table 6 shows that the same earlier pattern of Is FL teacher fun?
differences between girls and boys were repeated 1883 .93 0.25 1993 1808 .94 0.23
for this cohort. The differences were statistically 1411 .88 0.32 1994 1425 .92 0.27
significant at the alpha = .05 level. For all five 1384 NA NA 1995 1394 NA NA
items, girls had higher means than boys. 4678 .91 0.29 Total 4627 .93 0.25
Continue with FL?
Cohort 4 District B, 1993–1995, Second to Fourth 1883 NA NA 1993 1808 NA NA
1411 .67 0.47 1994 1425 .76 0.43
Grade. When one examines the trends over time
1384 .55 0.50 1995 1394 .59 0.49
(Table 7), it is clear that as the members of Cohort
4678 .61 0.49 Total 4627 .68 0.47
4 District B (first surveyed in second grade in 1993
Use FL at home/friends?
and last surveyed as fourth graders in 1995) grew 1883 NA NA 1993 1808 NA NA
older and advanced to higher grades, a pattern 1411 .39 0.49 1994 1425 .53 0.50
similar to that observed for District A is evident. 1384 NA NA 1995 1394 NA NA
Item one, “Do you like foreign language class?” 4678 .39 0.49 Total 4627 .53 0.50
revealed disparities for girls and boys. The latter Understand FL teacher?
group displayed similar downward trends as those 1883 NA NA 1993 1808 NA NA
observed in District A, whereas for the girls, move- 1411 .59 0.49 1994 1425 .65 0.48
ment was in the opposite direction. 1384 NA NA 1995 1394 NA NA
Item 2, “Is the foreign language teacher fun?” 4678 .59 0.49 Total 4627 .65 0.48
showed differences for boys and girls in opposite Note. NA = This item was not asked for this cohort
directions. Boys tended to like the teachers as they in this year.
grew older; the opposite was the case for the girls. a
Yes = 1; No = 0.
158 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
TABLE 6
Means by Gender for Cohort 3 District A, 1993–1995, Grades 2 to 4
Gender N Ma SD t df Sig (2-Tailed)

Do you like the FL class? Boys 4660 .75 0.43 –7.990 9264 0.000
Girls 4606 .82 0.38
Is the FL teacher fun? Boys 3290 .91 0.29 –3.622 6517 0.000
Girls 3229 .93 0.25
Do you want to continue Boys 2781 .61 0.49 –5.188 5589 0.000
with FL next year? Girls 2810 .68 0.47
Sometimes I use the FL at Boys 1407 .39 0.49 –7.600 2826 0.000
home or with friends. Girls 1421 .53 0.50
Most of the time I Boys 1409 .59 0.49 –3.584 2830 0.000
understand what the Girls 1423 .63 0.48
teacher is saying.
a
Yes = 1; No = 0.

Item 3, “Do you want to continue with the Items 4 and 5, owing to just one time period
foreign language next year?” exhibited the same when the questions were asked, are to be taken as
downward trend as in District A. Both girls and a single measure of opinion regarding the use and
boys showed a decline in interest. comprehension of FL. The observed proportions
were just as low as those observed for District A.
Table 8 shows an already noted difference be-
TABLE 7
tween girls and boys. For all five items, the girls
Means Over Time for Cohort 4 District B, 1993– scored higher than the boys. The item with the
1995, Grades 2 to 4 smallest difference was “Do you want to continue
with foreign language next year?” The most im-
Boys Girls portant fact derived from Table 8 is the higher
Do you like
N Ma SD FL class? N Ma SD receptivity of girls to FL instruction.
Cohort 5 District B, 1993–1995, Third to Fifth
362 .76 0.43 1993 351 .82 0.39
175 .69 0.47 1994 206 .78 0.41 Grade. Table 9 illustrates differences over time for
237 .62 0.49 1995 246 .94 0.24 Cohort 5 District B (first surveyed as third graders
774 .70 0.46 Total 803 .78 0.24 in 1993 and last surveyed as fifth graders in 1995).
Is FL teacher fun? An already noted decline in the interest for FL was
362 .84 0.36 1993 351 .91 0.29 once again evident for both girls and boys in their
175 .93 0.25 1994 206 .98 0.14 liking of the world language they were studying.
237 .89 0.31 1995 246 .72 0.24 In this cohort, the boys’ attitudes dropped more
774 .88 0.33 Total 803 .94 0.24 than those of girls.
Continue with FL? For the remaining items surveyed, the most no-
362 NA NA 1993 351 NA NA ticeable difference compared to other cohorts was
175 .66 0.47 1994 206 .73 0.45
the substantial drop in the interest in continuing
237 .58 0.49 1995 246 .64 0.48
with FL study for the year 1994. Both boys and girls
774 .62 0.49 Total 803 .68 0.47
showed minimal interest in pursuing the subject at
Use FL at home/friends?
362 NA NA 1993 351 NA NA the end of the fourth grade. This minimal interest
175 .37 0.48 1994 206 .54 0.50 in pursuing FL study by both genders must be due
237 NA NA 1995 246 NA NA to something specific and unusual that occurred
774 .37 0.48 Total 803 .54 0.50 in District B that year. When compared with other
Understand FL teacher? cohorts for the same district or similar cohorts in
362 NA NA 1993 351 NA NA the other district, the low mean score observed in
175 .63 0.48 1994 206 .75 0.44 District B seemed out of the ordinary. As with all
237 NA NA 1995 246 NA NA previous cohorts, Cohort 5 girls had higher means
774 .63 0.48 Total 803 .75 0.44 than boys (Table 10). Even with smaller samples,
Note. NA = This item was not asked for this cohort all five items surveyed showed statistically signifi-
in this year. cant differences for boys and girls at the .05 level
a
Yes = 1; No = 0. of confidence.
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 159
TABLE 8
Means by Gender for Cohort 4 District B, 1993–1995, Grades 2 to 4
Gender N Ma SD t df Sig (2-tailed)

Do you like the FL class? Boys 755 .70 0.46 –3.430 1538 0.001
Girls 785 .78 0.42
Is the FL teacher fun? Boys 761 .88 0.33 –4.004 1546 0.000
Girls 787 .94 0.24
Do you want to continue Boys 381 .62 0.49 –1.803 798 0.072
with FL next year? Girls 419 .68 0.47
Sometimes I use the FL at Boys 175 .37 0.48 –3.471 378 0.001
home or with friends. Girls 205 .54 0.50
Most of the time I Boys 175 .63 0.48 –2.407 379 0.017
understand what the Girls 206 .75 0.44
teacher is saying.
a
Yes = 1; No = 0.

What follows are the answers to the research RQ 1, How Do Student Attitudes Toward FL Study
questions for the elementary school study. Ap- Change Over Time? The FPEI has two items that
pendix A identifies each survey item and the re- examine student attitudes toward FL classes and
search question that it answers. teachers. Data in the tables indicate that student
attitudes indeed change over time but in largely a
downward direction characteristic of both boys
TABLE 9 and girls. This change was statistically signifi-
Means Over Time for Cohort 5 District B, 1993– cant, although responses overall were positive.
1995, Grades 3 to 5 The overall positive attitude could be a result of
the FLES teachers’ use of age-appropriate ped-
Boys Girls agogical techniques, such that the students re-
Do you like
N M a
SD FL class? N Ma SD mained interested and excited about learning the
FL.
129 .81 0.39 1993 137 .80 0.40 It is interesting that studies on other subjects
175 .66 0.48 1994 154 .78 0.42 indicate a serious loss of enthusiasm when stu-
144 .63 0.49 1995 162 .75 0.43
dents (both genders) become negative in their
448 .69 0.46 Total 453 .78 0.42
attitudes; for example, reading (Kush & Watkins,
Is FL teacher fun?
1996) social studies (Haldyna & Thomas, 1979),
129 .96 0.19 1993 137 .98 0.15
175 NA NA 1994 154 NA NA or math for females (Davies & Brember, 1994).
144 .83 0.37 1995 162 .92 0.27 Student attitudes toward FLES seem to remain in
448 .89 0.31 Total 453 .95 0.23 the positive range on most measures.
Continue with FL?
129 .70 0.46 1993 137 .71 0.46 RQ 2, How Does the Desire of Students to Continue
175 .13 0.34 1994 154 .16 0.36 With FL Study Change Over Time? The prompt
144 .38 0.49 1995 162 .61 0.49 from the FPEI survey that corresponds to this re-
448 .37 0.48 Total 453 .49 0.50 search question asks “Do you want to continue
Use FL at home/friends? with the foreign language next year?” The group
129 .38 0.49 1993 137 .49 0.50
statistics in the current study indicated that the
175 NA NA 1994 154 NA NA
girls had more desire to continue FL studies than
144 .32 0.47 1995 162 .42 0.50
448 .35 0.48 Total 453 .77 0.42 the boys, and the difference was statistically signif-
Understand FL teacher? icant. In all cohorts, the girls had higher means
129 .59 0.49 1993 137 .69 0.46 than the boys. These means, with the exception of
175 NA NA 1994 154 NA NA one cohort, were also in the positive range. Only
144 .63 0.48 1995 162 .83 0.37 the girls in Cohort 5 (District B, 1993–1995, grades
448 .61 0.49 Total 453 .77 0.42 3 to 5) showed a neutral attitude toward continu-
Note. NA= This item was not asked for this cohort in ing their FL experience. The girls in this cohort
this year. had neutral attitudes, whereas the boys had neg-
a
Yes = 1; No = 0. ative attitudes. The boys in all the other cohorts
160 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
TABLE 10
Means by Gender for Cohort 5 District B, 1993–1995, Grades 3 to 5
Gender N Ma SD t df Sig (2-Tailed)

Do you like the FL class? Boys 447 .69 0.46 –2.884 896 0.004
Girls 451 .78 0.42
Is the FL teacher fun? Boys 273 .89 0.31 –2.346 570 0.019
Girls 299 .95 0.23
Do you want to continue Boys 447 .37 0.48 –3.413 898 0.001
with FL next year? Girls 453 .49 0.50
Sometimes I use the FL at Boys 271 .35 0.48 –2.543 566 0.011
home or with friends. Girls 297 .45 0.50
Most of the time I Boys 272 .61 0.49 –4.143 568 0.000
understand what the Girls 298 .77 0.42
teacher is saying.
a
Yes = 1; No = 0.

exhibited neutral attitudes toward continued FL RQ 5, Are There Gender Differences in Attitudes
study. In sum, the loss of enthusiasm for FL study Toward FL Study, and if so, Do They Change Over
over the 4 years of the current study, although Time? The data yielded an affirmative response to
statistically significant, still resulted in generally both parts of this research question. For each of
positive attitudes for the girls and in neutral atti- the five prompts in the FPEI, the boys and girls
tudes for the boys. showed statistically significant differences. For in-
dividual cohorts, statistically significant gender
RQ 3, How Do the Students’ Attempts to Use the differences were evident for every prompt, except
Language Outside Class Change Over Time? Statis- one.
tics for the respondents in this study indicated As previous discussion has revealed, in almost
that the use of the FL outside class was, on all cases, the means for both the boys and the
the whole, something the students, especially the girls declined and indicated a loss of positive atti-
boys, viewed in a negative manner. Individual co- tude toward all aspects of FL learning examined.
horts had essentially the same distribution, with In some cases, the differences between the means
the boys consistently in the negative range, and were even more pronounced over time. The dif-
the girls more in the neutral range. ferences between the means were significant, at
That there should be such a negative percep- the alpha = .05 level. The boys tended to start with
tion of the use of a language other than English more negative attitudes and, generally speaking,
outside class might be attributable to the chil- remained more negative, even as the girls’ atti-
dren’s attitudes toward school in general, whereby tudes declined. In some instances, the girls almost
activities associated with school, such as FL use, equaled or overtook the boys on the final adminis-
might undergo the same loss of prestige and de- tration of the survey. In a few instances, the change
sirability as school does itself. Studies cited pre- in attitude for one gender was two or three times
viously, notably those by Haldyna and Thomas greater than that of the other. The final group
(1979) and Davies and Brember (1994, 2001), in- means were often in the positive range. Only one
dicate a general decline in overall attitude toward item, “Sometimes I use the foreign language at
school. Nevertheless, it seems paradoxical that for home or with my friends,” had final means for
the most part there should be such a positive atti- boys and girls in the lower neutral range. There-
tude toward the FL classes and teachers, but not fore, we can conclude that although the means
toward the use of the subject matter itself. declined significantly, the majority of individual
cohort means were either positive, or at worst,
RQ 4, How Do Students Feel Their Linguistic Com- neutral. In addition, although the declines were
petence Changes Over Time? In this study, the girls statistically significant in this large sample, most
had a more positive attitude than the boys. Final changes were, in fact, small.
means for individual cohorts indicated that the
male attitudes were in the neutral range, whereas HIGH SCHOOL FOLLOW-UP STUDY
the female attitudes were neutral in three of the
five cohorts and positive in the remaining two (Co- How do student perceptions of early language
horts 4 and 5). learning carry over to the high school years? As
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 161
a follow-up to the quantitative, 4-year study, 13 speakers of languages other than English, percep-
high school students who had participated in the tions of foreign cultures, and perceptions of stu-
elementary school study were interviewed about dents’ own need and use for a language other than
their perceptions of FL learning as adolescents. English.
The interviews were designed to answer the fol- To ascertain the themes of the responses,
lowing research questions: the researchers followed Patton’s (1990) sug-
gestion to identify regularities to be classified
1. How do former FLES students feel that their and place those regularities into categories. The
FL instruction affected their perceptions of FL categories must have two main characteristics:
speakers? internal homogeneity (similarity of the data)
2. How do former FLES students feel that their and external heterogeneity (clear differences be-
FL instruction affected their perceptions of for- tween categories). For the interview questions di-
eign cultures? rected toward the students’ attitudes, reviewing
3. How do former FLES students feel that their the transcripts revealed categories. Preliminary
FL instruction affected their perceptions of their classifications were done by the researchers and
own FL education? cross-checked by an independent reviewer. Ini-
tially, there was an 80% coincidence of designa-
Design of the Study
tions. Where variances existed, adjustments were
The high school follow-up study used open- made after discussion and mutual agreement. The
ended, taped telephone interviews with students final categories used to describe the attitudes of
instead of written questionnaires. The interviews the high school students toward FL speakers, for-
were done by the researchers. eign cultures, and their own foreign language
study were sympathetic or indifferent/negative; posi-
Development and Validity of the Open-Ended Ques- tive or pragmatic; intrinsic or value-added.
tionnaire. To create the questions for the open-
ended interviews, the researchers held focus Description of the Participants. For the follow-up
discussion groups with former FLES students. The study, the population comprised all students who
topics that emerged from the discussions were the had studied either French or Spanish in District
FLES classes themselves; teachers and classroom A and B while in the elementary school. The pro-
activities; use of the FL outside class; student at- cess for receiving approval to conduct research
titudes toward foreigners; travel to other coun- in Districts A and B was a lengthy one. Adminis-
tries; future plans for either FL use or study, or trative constraints prohibited District B from par-
both. The interview format was constructed based ticipating in the follow-up study. After District A
on the discussions in these focus groups (see granted approval to conduct the study, 180 con-
Appendix B). The interviews consisted of eight tact letters were sent to 13 teachers at 12 differ-
questions. The first three questions supplied de- ent high schools. A note to the classroom teacher
mographic data. Questions 4 through 6 solicited requested that the letters be given to those stu-
more detailed information through the use of ad- dents considered the most responsible and likely
ditional probes. to respond.
Face and content validity were established for As a result of the requests, 13 students (5 males
the interview questionnaire by examining the and 8 females) agreed to be interviewed. These
items and determining that they indeed reflected students were all in the 11th or 12th grade and
the intent of the instrument, which was to deter- between the ages of 16 and 18. They were enrolled
mine attitudes toward FL instruction. The items in all levels of an FL from Level 1 to Advanced
reflected those of the FPEI. No extraneous items Placement literature. Their levels of proficiency
could be considered irrelevant to the research. or achievement were not determined.
The 13 students were enrolled in FL courses,
Analytical Methods. For the follow-up interviews which automatically excluded any students who
with the high school students who had partici- either had not chosen to continue with their FL
pated in the elementary school study, a template studies or were not enrolled in an FL course
analysis (Crabtree & Miller, 1994) was used. This at the time of the interviews. Also not repre-
type of analysis uses prefigured categories that sented were rural students. Nevertheless, the stu-
are open to reconfiguration as the analysis of dents surveyed represent a cross section of high
the data proceeds. For the present study, these school students studying a language in addition
categories corresponded to three areas presum- to English at the high school level who began FL
ably affected by the study of FLES: perceptions of study in elementary school. As the results report,
162 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
they held differing attitudes toward a language TABLE 11
other than English. These findings confirm what Sympathetic or Indifferent/Negative Perceptions of
was reported in the review of previous research Foreign Language Speakers
on attitudes toward other subject areas (Davies & Student Sympathetic Indifferent/Negative
Brember, 1994, 2001; Haldyna & Thomas, 1979;
Reynolds & Walberg, 1992a, 1992b). Female 1 X
Female 2 X X
Female 3 X
Results
Female 4 X
The results of the high school follow-up study Female 5 X
Female 6 X
follow. First, the demographic data of the respon-
Female 7 X
dents are reported, followed by a reporting of the Female 8 X
data based on the three research questions for this
study. Male 1 X
Male 2 X
Male 3 X
Demographic Data. Eight females and 5 males
Male 4 X
participated in the follow-up study. Male 1 stud- Male 5 X
ied Spanish in elementary school and was study-
ing Spanish as well as Latin in high school. Male
2 studied French in elementary school and was I think that my foreign language study has been bene-
studying Spanish 4 in high school. Male 4 studied ficial to my overall attitude towards people who don’t
speak English. (Male 2)
Spanish in K–5 and was in French 3 at the time of
the study. Female 4 studied both French and Span-
He (the FLES teacher) came from a real poor part of
ish in elementary school and was in Spanish 1 in Puerto Rico so his description of it makes us not take
high school. All other students studied Spanish in for granted what we have because these people have
their elementary schools and were studying Span- it a whole lot worse. (Female 2)
ish in high school. In North Carolina, students
may select the language(s) they wish to study in
I’m definitely more open than those who haven’t had
high school (and middle school if FL is offered). Spanish ’cause I know that some of those people are
They are not required to continue with the FL kind’a negative towards the Hispanic people that are
they began in the elementary school. Appendix C in this country. (Female 3)
presents the demographic data of the 13 students
interviewed. Selected quotations categorized as indiffer-
ent/negative are:
RQ 1, How Do Former FLES Students Feel That
I don’t really think it’s an issue. I didn’t absorb a lot.
Their Early FL Instruction Affected Their Percep-
I’ve got to say that the class as a whole in elementary
tions of FL Speakers? In response to the questions was pretty useless. (Male 1)
on attitudes toward FL speakers, the results
were categorized as either sympathetic or indif- I don’t know that they (elementary foreign language
ferent/negative. A sympathetic response actively classes) really have (affected attitudes toward foreign
showed a positive attitude toward FL speakers. language speakers). I can hardly remember the classes
Responses coded as indifferent/negative were themselves. They were actually pretty useless. I don’t
neutral in their viewpoints toward the speak- think it really affected my attitudes really. (Male 4)
ers of languages other than English, with neg-
ative opinions toward their FLES classes. No Just because, I mean, with immigration and all just
indifferent/negative responses indicated any sort because it will keep increasing. I don’t think I would
be against Spanish. I wouldn’t go out of my way for it
of mistrust or dislike of FL speakers. Table 11
but I wouldn’t hold them (my future children) from
shows the categorization of the interviewees’ it either. (Female 2)
responses.
Female 2 expressed opinions that were clas- In sum, 9 of the 13 high school students inter-
sified in both of the categories. Overall, there viewed felt that their FLES classes provided them
were 7 females and 3 males whose responses were with a platform for developing positive attitudes
classified as sympathetic, and 2 females and 2 toward FL speakers. Those students who did not
males whose responses were classified as indiffer- share a positive view of FL study in elementary
ent/negative. Representative comments from the school did not express negative views of people
sympathetic designation include: from other cultures.
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 163
RQ 2, How Do Former FLES Students Feel That itself valuable; the other 6 viewed FL study as a
Their Early FL Instruction Affected Their Perceptions means to an end. The researchers gave the labels
of Foreign Cultures? The second focus of the follow- intrinsic and value-added to these two types of opin-
up interviews was to discover the students’ percep- ions. Table 13 presents the tally of these responses.
tions of foreign cultures. These responses were The following observations by several respondents
classified as either positive or pragmatic. A positive exemplify the intrinsic viewpoint about their own
response reflected an enthusiastic or affirmative education and FL study.
attitude toward foreign cultures. The pragmatic I liked how we would take a different country every
category was employed for answers from respon- week and learn facts about them and how the people
dents who did not enthusiastically or even pos- lived. And that was cool ’cause it made you think about
itively endorse a multicultural viewpoint but ac- what you already had too. (Male 5)
cepted the need to understand other cultures
I don’t know what I want to do yet but I know I want
and their languages. Table 12 reports the results
to do something to help people. And if I can talk
and representative comments from those who re-
to foreign people that would be cool. So if I could
sponded in a positive manner follow. communicate with them it would be good. (Female 6)
So it (FLES) helped me realize that we’re not so much I love Spanish to pieces . . . . I definitely aspire to raise
isolated countries but that we’re one big world and my children bilingual . . . . I can’t imagine my life with-
we’re all in this together. (Female 7) out Spanish. (Female 7)
When your perspectives are broadened like that Even though I’m not really that good I plan on trying
(through FLES) in elementary school the impact to learn more. (Female 8)
is just huge. It definitely made me more welcom-
ing of other peoples’ cultures and perspectives. The other respondents were not intrinsically
(Male 5) motivated to learn an FL. Several actively disliked
the thought of learning more but expressed the
One participant expressed the following repre- knowledge of benefits derived from acquiring a
sentative pragmatic perception: certain level of linguistic ability. Representative
quotations from the value-added responses by the
I think it is very important with the job market the way
it is. With the constant rise in Hispanic population and students include:
immigrants, legal or illegal, in the United States and It (foreign language) just makes you a smarter person.
it’s a matter of adapting, of us adapting whether we (Male 2)
want to or not. (Male 1)
I don’t think it (foreign language use) is necessary but
it can get you ahead. If you know a foreign language
RQ 3, How Do Former FLES Students Feel That
you can get a lot more jobs and it gives you more
Their Early FL Instruction Affected Their Perceptions opportunities. I will need to learn Spanish at some
of Their Own FL Education? Of the 13 respondents, point. I don’t really like the language, I think it just
7 students felt that the study of language was in sounds crude but I’ll have to learn it. (Male 4)

TABLE 12 TABLE 13
Positive or Pragmatic Perceptions of Foreign Intrinsic or Value-Added Perceptions of Their Own
Cultures Education and Foreign Language Study
Student Positive Pragmatic/Utilitarian Student Intrinsic Value-Added

Female 1 X X Female 1 X
Female 2 X Female 2 X
Female 3 X Female 3 X
Female 4 X Female 4 X
Female 5 X Female 5 X
Female 6 X Female 6 X
Female 7 X Female 7 X
Female 8 X Female 8 X
Male 1 X X Male 1 X
Male 2 X Male 2 X
Male 3 X Male 3 X
Male 4 X Male 4 X
Male 5 X Male 5 X X
164 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
SUMMARY AND SYNTHESIS It was actually really pointless. I was young and I didn’t
know much about the world but I knew that it was just
It is important to review the results from both really stupid. I mean if they couldn’t bring teachers
the elementary and the high school studies, and to in there to teach us all the time then it meant it was
draw overall conclusions from both studies. The pretty worthless. (Male 4)
results of the elementary school study of a hetero-
Children and adolescents are knowledgeable
geneous group of elementary students indicated
consumers. Male 4’s attitude toward FLES indi-
that the female students were more interested in
cates that he knew that it was not an integral part
FL study than the males. As the females took ad-
of his elementary curriculum, as opposed to sci-
ditional classes in the subject and became older,
ence, mathematics, or language arts. This fact led
their interest in FL declined. For both genders,
him at an early age to perceive from the limited
the liking of the teacher and the subject matter,
amount of contact time that learning a language
the continuation of study, and the external use of
other than English was less important than learn-
FL showed a steady decline.
ing other subjects from the core curriculum.
The FPEI survey can be divided into two groups
After 10 years had passed since the initial FPEI
of items. The first group surveys the general will-
student survey, how did the adolescent respon-
ingness to learn and accept the teacher as a facili-
dents feel about continuing FL study? Female 5
tator of new knowledge, and treats FL as input to
and Male 4 did not want to continue their study
the students’ knowledge base. The second group
of languages other than English; all others did.
of FPEI items surveys the students’ usage of the
acquired knowledge as output. The higher means I went into middle school Spanish for three years. Es-
for the willingness to learn measures observed for sentially I did level one in sixth and seventh grades and
all cohorts in both districts indicates a general ac- then I took level two in eighth grade. I went into high
ceptance of FL study, a desire to please, and an school at level three. I think that elementary school is
openness to something new. By contrast, the ob- what has really opened my eyes to Spanish and made
served lower means for the second group of items me think that, “Hey, I really enjoyed speaking it. It’s
indicate the difficulties that the students in lower really fun.” (Female 7)
grades had in using the FL in external settings.
Even though I’m not really that good I plan on trying
Because environment influences the application
to learn more. I’m planning on becoming a music
of the knowledge base, it might be expected that, education major. Even if I am put in a place where
with the exception of bilingual home settings, stu- there is a high level of Hispanic population it will be
dents would tend to develop a less enthusiastic a plus. (Female 8)
opinion of FL as an output, as something they can
use daily. Although they reported little use of the FL when
As the statistics show, the students became in- they were elementary school students, all but one
creasingly disenchanted with FL over the span of high school respondent indicated using the target
their elementary school career. Nevertheless, it is language outside the classroom.
important to remember that finding this statis-
The three years of (middle school) French really had
tically significant change was influenced by the
a positive effect on me. And one summer I was off in
nature of the sampling. Rather than surveying
England and I heard two women having a conversa-
only a portion of the possible participants, we sur- tion in French and I went over and talked with them.
veyed nearly all the students and excluded only And I didn’t embarrass myself. It really affected me
students absent on the day of the survey. This because I was so taken with it. (Male 1)
universal inclusion made the statistical tests ex-
tremely sensitive to even small changes in the We go to Mexican restaurants all the time so I like to
means. order in Spanish there. (Female 1)
The high school study was done 10 years af-
ter the students began their FL learning. How I remember that one time one of my parents needed
a translator and they called me. It was like the tenth
did their attitudes change over time? How closely
grade. Then I realized that all this is useful and it’s
did the perceptions that they reported as chil-
real. (Female 7)
dren mirror their perceptions as adolescents?
When asked “Did you like your FLES class?” 11 The 13 high school students all acknowledged
of the 13 high school respondents said “yes.” Al- an improvement over the course of their years
though there were more positive than negative studying another language. Nevertheless, it is
comments, Male 4’s responses are important to troublesome to note that some of them remem-
note. He said that: bered making little linguistic progress. Also of
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 165
concern is Male 3’s comment that expresses the SUGGESTIONS FOR FUTURE RESEARCH
opinion of many high school world language
learners. The two studies included in this report lay the
groundwork for a wide array of future research
possibilities. One study would be to determine
I think they (FLES classes) were pretty good. You know
the relationship of student attitudes to linguis-
we had them 2 days a week for 30 minutes at a time. I
mean it wasn’t a long amount of time we studied for
tic achievement. Another study would be to dis-
but I feel that for the amount of time we learned a lot. cover the attitudes and reasoning of high school
(Female 1) students who began FL study in the elementary
school, and then stopped studying an FL in high
I always felt like kind of confused ’cause I didn’t school.
know it very good. Like I didn’t know what the words Still another important study would be to ex-
meant that I was saying. We would just repeat what the plore the impact of teacher retention on student
teacher said. (Female 4) attitudes. During the same period when their stu-
dents responded to the FPEI, FLES teachers were
I felt that I was very talented in the Spanish language also surveyed using the FLES teacher form of the
just because I knew how to say the alphabet very fast. FPEI. Over the years, they consistently reported
We really didn’t go much beyond numbers and ran- challenging working conditions, such as having
dom words. (Female 8) to teach hundreds of students each week for short
I’d love to actually learn how to speak it, but our periods. Further research is needed to determine
teacher makes it so hard to do I’ll never be able to. to what degree difficult teaching conditions affect
(Male 3) the attitudes of students.
We have known for decades from educational
In conclusion, the analysis from the combined
psychology research that the teacher is one of
10 years of data indicates that there was indeed
the most important parts in the equation for
a positive impact attributable to FLES instruction
students’ success in and their attitudes toward
on a student’s perception of speakers of languages
school. Future research may help world language
other than English, foreign cultures, and how
teachers find the tools and resources that they
FL study impacts their education. These findings
need to nurture positive student attitudes that, as
support earlier quantitative research by Vollmer
Gardner (1985) maintained, will lead to higher
(1962), who observed that students who had re-
achievement.
ceived FLES instruction were almost twice as likely
to persist into higher levels of FL instruction in
high school as students who did not receive FLES CONCLUSION
instruction. We are at a point in this country where
The importance of these two studies is far reach-
ensuring that students commit to a long sequence
ing. First, the enormous size of the elementary
of language study is paramount, and beginning
school data compares with the largest educational
language study at the elementary school is one
studies conducted in other disciplines. One rea-
way of doing so.
son for collecting such a large data set was to
demonstrate that teaching world languages in ele-
LIMITATIONS OF THE STUDIES mentary school is highly similar to teaching other
core subjects such as science and mathematics. In
The elementary school study was limited to a addition, the combined studies chronicle the im-
statistical examination of the attitudes of the stu- portant fact that even young language learners see
dents. There was no attempt to determine the rela- the need to begin FL study in elementary school,
tionship of student attitudes to linguistic achieve- and that the programs must have high expec-
ment. tations, rigorous curricula, and strong teaching.
The high school study was limited to students These studies document that attitude formation
who were currently studying a world language toward language learning is a critical component
who had also been participants in the elemen- of early schooling.
tary school study in District A. Because of school Finally, these studies make it clear that as FL
district constraints, students from District B could educators, we need to harness the positive desire
not be included in the study, nor could students of children and adolescents to learn and to be
from District A who had chosen not to pursue the successful in language learning, as well as to facil-
study of an FL. itate their success, thus ensuring higher levels of
166 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)
language achievement. Perhaps this notion is ex- tudes, achievements, and instruction in the later
pressed best by several of the study’s high school years of FLES. Foreign Language Annals, 33, 377–
respondents. 399.
Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of atti-
I was excited. I mean I was always excited about having tudes. Fort Worth, TX: HBJ, Inc.
the teacher come and all. I have positive memories Fazio, R. H. (2000). Accessible attitudes as tools for ob-
about it and I just remember that I looked forward to ject appraisal: Their costs and benefits. In G. R.
the teacher coming. (Female 1) Maio & J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Func-
tions of attitudes (pp. 1–36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
I think it (FLES) helped me at the tender age of 10 Gardner, R. (1985). Social psychology and second language
to realize that there’s a lot more out there than Amer- learning: The role of attitudes and motivation. Lon-
ica, which not a lot of 10 year-olds realize. So it helped don: Edward Arnold.
me realize that we’re not so much isolated countries, Haldyna, T., & Thomas, G. (1979). The attitudes of el-
but that we’re one big world, and we’re all in this to- ementary school children toward school and sub-
gether. I just love it (foreign language) to pieces. I ject matters. Journal of Experimental Education, 48,
can’t imagine my life without it (foreign language). 18–23.
I definitely aspire to raise my children bilingual. Heining–Boynton, A. L. (1990). The development and
(Female 7) testing of the FLES program evaluation inventory.
Modern Language Journal , 74, 433–441.
Herek, G. M. (2000). The social construction of atti-
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS tudes: Functional consensus and divergence in the
U.S. public’s reactions to AIDS. In G. R. Maio &
J. M. Olson (Eds.), Why we evaluate: Functions of
This research was supported in part by funding attitudes (pp. 325–364). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
awarded to the first author from The University of North Kush, J. C., & Watkins, M. W. (1996). Long-term stability
Carolina at Chapel Hill (UNC–CH), and the Fund- of children’s attitudes toward reading. Journal of
ing for the Improvement and Reform of Schools and Educational Research, 89 , 315–320.
Teachers (FIRST), U.S. Department of Education. The Maio, G. R., & Olson, J. M. (2000). Why we evaluate:
authors would like to thank Dr. Catherine Zimmer of Functions of attitudes. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
The Odum Institute for Research in the Social Sciences, Patton, M. Q. (1990). Qualitative evaluation and research
UNC–CH, for her invaluable statistical assistance. methods. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Reynolds, A. J., & Walberg, H. J. (1992a). A structural
model of science achievement and attitude: An
extension to high school. Journal of Educational
REFERENCES Research, 84, 150–158.
Reynolds, A. J., & Walberg, H. J. (1992b). A structural
Crabtree, B., & Miller, W. L. (1994). The interview: From model of high school mathematics outcomes. Jour-
structured interview to negotiated text. In N. Den- nal of Educational Research, 84, 150–158.
zin and Y. S. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative Shrigley, R. L. (1990). Attitude and behavior are corre-
research (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. lates. Journal for Research in Science Teaching , 27 ,
Davies, J., & Brember, I. (1994). Attitudes to school and 97–113.
the curriculum in year 2 and year 4: Changes over Smith, M. C. (1990). A longitudinal investigation of
two years. Educational Review, 46 , 247–259. reading attitude development from childhood to
Davies, J., & Brember, I. (2001). The closing gap in atti- adulthood. Journal of Educational Research, 83,
tudes between boys and girls: A five year longitu- 215–219.
dinal study. Educational Psychology, 21, 103–115. Vollmer, J. H. (1962). Evaluation of the effect of foreign lan-
Donato, R., Antonek, J. L., & Tucker, G. R. (1996). Mon- guage study in elementary school upon achievement in
itoring and assessing a Japanese FLES program: the high school (ERIC Documentation Reproduc-
Ambiance and achievement. Language Learning , tion Service No. ED 038901).
46 , 497–528. Zimbardo, P. G., & Leippe, M. R. (1991). The psychology
Donato, R., Tucker, G. R., Wudthayagorn, J., & of attitude change and social influence. Philadelphia:
Igarashi, K. (2000). Converging evidence: Atti- Temple University Press.
Audrey L. Heining–Boynton and Thomas Haitema 167

APPENDIX A
FLES Program Evaluation Inventory (FPEI) Student Items

Note: The surveys depended on the language offered at the students’ school. The instructions indicated that the
children were to circle the option that best described their feelings. The options given were yes or no. After each item
is the corresponding research question.
Grades K–2 Survey Items
Do you like your (foreign language) class? RQ 1
Is your (foreign language) teacher fun? RQ 1
Is learning (foreign language) fun? RQ 1
Do you want to learn more (foreign language)? RQ 2
Grade 3 Survey Items
I like (foreign language). RQ 1
I like my (foreign language) teacher. RQ 1
Most of the time, I understand what my (foreign language) teacher is saying. RQ 4
This is my first year of (foreign language).
Sometimes I use (foreign language) at home or with my friends. RQ 3
I want to continue (foreign language) next year. RQ 2
Grade 4 Survey Items
I like (foreign language). RQ 1
I like my (foreign language) teacher. RQ 1
I can write some words in (foreign language). RQ 4
I can read some words in (foreign language). RQ 4
We do interesting things in (foreign language) class. RQ 1
This is my first year of (foreign language).
I want to continue (foreign language) next year. RQ 2
Grade 5 Survey Items
I like (foreign language). RQ 1
I like my (foreign language) teacher. RQ 1
Most of the time I understand what my (foreign language) teacher is saying. RQ 4
This is my first year of (foreign language).
Sometimes I use (foreign language) at home or with my friends. RQ 3
I am planning to take (foreign language) next year. RQ 2

APPENDIX B
Follow-Up Interview Questionnaire

1. Which elementary school did you attend?


2. What foreign language(s) did you study in elementary school?
3. How many years of each foreign language did you study?
4. How do you use the foreign language you have learned outside of class?
5. How do you think your attitudes toward people who speak another language have been influenced by your foreign
language study in the elementary school?
6. What do you recall about your foreign language instruction in elementary school?
7. Imagine if you someday have children and they are able to study a foreign language in elementary school. How
do you feel about that?
8. How do you think in the future you may use the foreign language you have been learning in your career?
168 The Modern Language Journal 91 (2007)

APPENDIX C
Demographic Data of Current High School/Former FLES Students
Elementary Grades High School Highest High
Participant Foreign FLES Foreign School Level
ID Language Offered Language of Attainment

Female 1 Spanish K–5 Spanish 3


Female 2 Spanish K–5 Spanish 4
Female 3 Spanish K–5 Spanish 4
Female 4 Spanish 3
French 4–5 Spanish 1
Female 5 Spanish K–5 Spanish 2
Female 6 Spanish K–5 Spanish 2
Female 7 Spanish 3–5 Spanish AP literature
Female 8 Spanish 2–5 Spanish 3
Male 1 Latin 2
Spanish 2–5 Spanish 1
Male 2 French K–5 Spanish 4
Male 3 Spanish K–5 Spanish 3
Male 4 Spanish K–5 French 3
Male 5 Spanish K–5 Spanish 5

Forthcoming in The Modern Language Journal


Matthew E. Poehner. “Beyond the Test: L2 Dynamic Assessment and the Transcendence of Mediated
Learning”

Heather J. Smith. “The Social and Private Worlds of Speech: Speech for Inter-and-Intramental Activity”

Ludmila Isurin. “Teachers’ Language: L1 Attrition in Russian–English Bilinguals”

Ellen Knell, Linda Siegel, Qiang Haiyan, Zhao Lin, Pei Miao, & Zhao Wei. “Early English Immersion
and Literacy in Xi’an, China”

Carol A. Fraser. “Reading Rate in L1 Mandarin Chinese and L2 English Across Five Reading Tasks”

Carrie N. Jackson. “The Use and Non-Use of Semantic Information, Word Order, and Case Markings
During Comprehension by L2 Learners of German”

Nan Jiang & Tatiana M. Nekrasova. “The Processing of Formulaic Sequences by Second Language
Speakers”

David P. Benseler. “Doctoral Degrees Granted in Foreign Languages in the United States: 2006”

Potrebbero piacerti anche