Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2007, 40, 535–539 NUMBER 3 (FALL 2007)

FURTHER ANALYSIS OF ANTECEDENT INTERVENTIONS ON


PRESCHOOLERS’ COMPLIANCE
DAVID A. WILDER, KIMBERLY ZONNEVELD, CARELLE HARRIS,
ALONNA MARCUS, AND RENEE REAGAN
FLORIDA INSTITUTE OF TECHNOLOGY

Functional analyses were conducted to identify reinforcers for noncompliance exhibited by 3


young children. Next, the effects of three antecedent-based interventions—noncontingent access
to a preferred item, a warning, and a high-probability instructional sequence—were examined.
The high-probability instructional sequence was effective for 1 child. Antecedent interventions
were ineffective and extinction was necessary for the other 2 children.
DESCRIPTORS: extinction, functional analysis, high-probability sequence, noncompli-
ance, preschool children, warnings
________________________________________

Noncompliance with instructions is common antecedent intervention, the high-probability


in preschool settings (Crowther, Bond, & Rolf, (high-p) instructional sequence (Rortvedt &
1981) and may be particularly common when Miltenberger, 1994) was evaluated.
children are asked to terminate a preferred
activity (e.g., free play) or initiate a nonpreferred
METHOD
activity (e.g., clean-up). Cote, Thompson, and
McKerchar (2005) examined two common Participants and Setting
antecedent interventions to increase compliance Eddie (a 30-month-old boy), Ricky (a 42-
by preschoolers. One intervention involved month-old boy), and Timmy (a 40-month-old
a warning delivered prior to a transition. The boy) participated. Eddie and Ricky did not have
second involved providing the child with non- a psychiatric diagnosis or a developmental
contingent access to a toy during a transition. disability, and Timmy had been diagnosed with
Both interventions were ineffective when im- Fragile X syndrome. All 3 participants had age-
plemented alone, and extinction was necessary appropriate language skills and had been re-
to increase compliance. However, no functional ported by a preschool teacher or nanny to ignore
analysis was conducted as part of this study. instructions. Sessions were conducted in a small
Thus, although extinction was shown to be room at the children’s school (Ricky and
a necessary intervention component, it is not Timmy) or home (Eddie). Two to six sessions
known if the target behaviors were maintained were conducted per day, 2 to 3 days per week. A
by positive or negative reinforcement. In graduate research assistant, unfamiliar to the
addition, preference for the toys used during children, served as experimenter.
the second intervention was not formally
assessed. The current study addressed these Response Measurement and Definitions
limitations by conducting functional analyses of Data collectors recorded the occurrence or
noncompliance and stimulus preference assess- nonoccurrence of compliance (the child in-
ments prior to intervention. In addition, a third dependently completing or initiating the activity
described in the instruction within 10 s) on each
Requests for reprints should be sent to David A. Wilder, instructional trial. A second independent ob-
Florida Institute of Technology, School of Psychology, server recorded compliance during at least 50%
150 West University Boulevard, Melbourne, Florida
32901 (e-mail: dawilder@fit.edu). of sessions for all children. Interobserver agree-
doi: 10.1901/jaba.2007.40-535 ment was measured by comparing observers’

535
536 DAVID A. WILDER et al.

records on a trial-by-trial basis. An agreement was Each condition was presented as a trial. Each
defined as both observers recording an instance of trial consisted of a 2-min preinstruction period,
either compliance or noncompliance on a given the presentation of the instruction, and a 3-min
trial. Mean agreement was calculated by dividing postinstruction period. At least six trials (two
the number of agreements by the number of per each type of condition) were conducted per
agreements plus disagreements and multiplying day with brief breaks between each; 36 trials
by 100%. Agreement ranged from 94% to 100% were conducted in total. The order of trials was
for all participants during both the functional randomly determined.
analysis and the intervention evaluation. In the preferred activity condition, partici-
Data on integrity of the independent variable pants engaged with high-preference play materi-
were collected by recording whether or not the als identified via the preference assessment. After
therapist delivered the item during the non- 2 min, the therapist delivered the instruction to
contingent reinforcement condition, delivered turn off the video (Eddie) or give me (experi-
the warning during the warning condition, menter) the toy (Ricky and Timmy). If the child
delivered three high-p instructions during the complied, the experimenter said ‘‘thank you,’’
high-p condition, and used hand-over-hand and the child was free to engage with low-
guidance in the extinction conditions (Eddie preference play materials during the 3-min
and Ricky). Integrity was 100% for all sessions postinstruction period. If the child did not
for all participants. Interobserver agreement on comply, the therapist did nothing (i.e., did not
integrity was collected during at least 25% of turn off the video or remove the toy) for the
intervention sessions. Agreement was 100% for remainder of this 3-min period. This condition
all participants. tested for maintenance via positive reinforcement
because noncompliance resulted in continued
Procedure access to high-preference play materials.
Separate paired-stimulus preference assess- In the nonpreferred activity condition, low-
ments (Fisher et al., 1992) were conducted to preference play materials were available during
identify high-preference edible items and high- the preinstruction period. After 2 min, the
and low-preference play materials from an array therapist delivered an instruction to complete
of stimuli found in the children’s classroom or the low-preference task (i.e., come to potty or
home. The most preferred edible items for pick up papers). If the child complied, the
Eddie, Ricky, and Timmy were bread, candy, therapist said ‘‘thank you,’’ and the child was free
and a gummy bear, respectively. The most to interact with low-preference play materials in
preferred play materials for Eddie, Ricky, and the room for the remainder of the 3-min
Timmy were a video, a large action figure, and postinstruction period (typically 1.5 to 2 min).
a soft dart game, respectively. Low-preference If the child did not comply, the experimenter did
play materials were a book, crayons and paper, nothing (i.e., did not re-present the instruction
and a stuffed animal for Eddie, Ricky, and or guide the participant to comply) for the
Timmy, respectively. Finally, each child’s nanny remainder of the postinstruction period. This
or teacher was asked to nominate a task that was condition tested for maintenance via negative
not preferred by participants; teachers indepen- reinforcement because noncompliance resulted
dently chose picking up items off the floor for in avoidance of the nonpreferred activity.
Ricky and Timmy. Eddie’s nanny chose going to In the control condition, low-preference play
the potty. materials were available during the preinstruc-
Functional analysis. Three assessment condi- tion period. After 2 min, the experimenter
tions were presented in a multielement design. delivered an instruction to interact with the
FURTHER ANALYSIS OF ANTECEDENT INTERVENTIONS 537

high-preference play materials (i.e., turn on the Timmy. If the child did not comply with the
video [Eddie] or play with the high-preference instruction, the therapist did nothing (i.e., did
toy [Ricky and Timmy]). If the child complied, not guide the child to the potty; did not remove
the therapist said ‘‘thank you,’’ and the child the toy) for the remainder of the 3-min
had access to the high-preference material for postinstruction period (i.e., extinction was not
the remainder of the 3-min postinstruction in place).
period. If the child did not comply, the During the noncontingent reinforcement
therapist did nothing (i.e., did not turn on condition, immediately following the initial
the video or give the child the toy) for the instruction, the child was told that he could
remainder of the 3-min period. This control have a snack while performing the instructed
condition eliminated events designed to evoke activity, and the experimenter provided five
(low-preference task) and reinforce (contingent small pieces of his high-preference edible item.
access to high-preference play materials) non- Edible items were used instead of play materials
compliance in the preferred activity and non- because they were easier to consume while
preferred activity test conditions. complying with the instruction. During the
Intervention evaluation. Based on the func- warning condition, the child was informed that
tional analysis results, the nonpreferred activity he would have to end or begin another activity
condition (Eddie) and the preferred activity in 1 min (e.g., ‘‘in one minute, you have to give
condition (Ricky and Timmy) were used as the me the toy’’). During the high-p instructional
context for the treatment evaluation. Although sequence, the experimenter presented three
Timmy’s functional analysis suggested mainte- high-p instructions (i.e., ‘‘give me five,’’ ‘‘touch
nance via both positive and negative reinforce- your nose,’’ and ‘‘what color is it?’’) to the child,
ment, the positive reinforcement context was each 5 s apart. Five seconds after the third
chosen because it was more consistent with instruction, the target instruction (i.e., come to
problematic situations reported by his teacher. potty; give me the toy) was presented. Compli-
Three antecedent interventions (i.e., non- ance with high-p instructions produced praise.
contingent reinforcement, warning, and high-p All participants complied with all high-p
instructional sequence) were evaluated with instructions. During extinction (Eddie and
each child in reversal designs. In addition, Ricky), noncompliance with the initial instruc-
extinction was evaluated for Eddie and Ricky. tion resulted in the experimenter repeating the
Each session consisted of either five (Eddie and instruction after 10 s and using hand-over-hand
Ricky) or three (Timmy) trials, and each trial guidance to assist the participant in completing
consisted of a single instruction. Baseline the task; extinction was procedurally identical.
sessions were identical to the nonpreferred
activity condition (Eddie) or the preferred
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
activity condition (Ricky and Timmy) of the
functional analysis. During the three antecedent Figure 1 depicts the results of the functional
intervention phases, compliance resulted in analysis. Eddie displayed the lowest levels of
experimenter praise. All children stayed in the compliance in the nonpreferred activity condi-
session room during the 3-min postinstruction tion. Thus, it appeared that his noncompliance
period and then received a brief break, as in was evoked by the instruction to engage in
baseline. Because Eddie’s instruction involved a nonpreferred activity and was maintained by
going to the potty, breaks between his trials avoidance of that activity. Ricky displayed the
during baseline and intervention phases were lowest levels of compliance in the preferred
longer (15 min) than those for Ricky and activity condition. Thus, it appeared that his
538 DAVID A. WILDER et al.

Figure 1. Percentage of trials with compliance across the three conditions of the functional analysis for Eddie (top
left), Ricky (middle left), and Timmy (bottom left). Percentage of trials with compliance during each session across
baseline and intervention phases for Eddie (top right), Ricky (middle right), and Timmy (bottom right).
FURTHER ANALYSIS OF ANTECEDENT INTERVENTIONS 539

noncompliance was evoked by the instruction to extinction is frequently necessary to increase


terminate a preferred activity and was main- compliance in young children. It is interesting
tained by continued access to that activity. to note that, although the items used during
Timmy was compliant with few instructions noncontingent reinforcement were identified via
delivered in both the nonpreferred and preferred a formal preference assessment in this study, the
activity conditions. It appeared that his non- procedure was ineffective. Because individual
compliance was evoked by both types of antecedent interventions have been shown to be
instructions and was maintained by avoidance generally ineffective at increasing compliance in
of nonpreferred activities and access to preferred this and previous studies, one possibility for
activities. future research might be to combine several
Figure 1 also depicts the results of the antecedent interventions to increase their power.
treatment evaluation. Results were similar for One limitation of this study is that, during
Eddie and Ricky, who showed high levels of the nonpreferred activity condition, noncom-
compliance only when extinction was imple- pliance resulted in avoidance of a nonpreferred
mented. By contrast, Timmy complied at high task and continued access to low-preference
levels when the high-p instructional sequence play materials. Thus, negative reinforcement
was implemented; therefore, extinction was not was not isolated as a source of control. In
evaluated. addition, this assessment included a restricted
This study contributes to the literature on variety of tasks, and some experimental condi-
noncompliance by preschoolers in two ways. First, tions were evaluated only briefly.
it enables identification of the specific mecha-
nism responsible for the effects of extinction. In
Cote et al. (2005), extinction was shown to be a REFERENCES
necessary intervention component for all parti- Cote, C. A., Thompson, R. H., & McKerchar, P. M.
cipants. However, because no functional analysis (2005). The effects of antecedent interventions and
was conducted, it was unknown whether extinc- extinction on toddlers’ compliance during transitions.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 235–238.
tion improved compliance by eliminating escape Crowther, J. H., Bond, L. A., & Rolf, J. E. (1981). The
or positive reinforcement for noncompliance. In incidence, prevalence, and severity of behavior
the current study, the functional analysis enabled disorders among preschool-aged children in day care.
identification of the mechanism responsible for Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 9, 23–42.
Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian,
the effects of a procedurally identical extinction L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A com-
procedure across 2 participants. For Eddie, parison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers
extinction was effective because it eliminated for persons with severe and profound disabilities.
negative reinforcement for noncompliance; for Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491–498.
Rortvedt, A. K., & Miltenberger, R. G. (1994). Analysis of
Ricky, extinction was effective because it elimi- a high-probability instructional sequence and time-
nated positive reinforcement for noncompliance. out in the treatment of child noncompliance. Journal
Second, these results provide additional of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 327–330.
evidence that two commonly used antecedent
Received July 12, 2006
interventions (warning and noncontingent re- Final acceptance November 14, 2006
inforcement) are often ineffective and that Action Editor, Rachel Thompson

Potrebbero piacerti anche