Sei sulla pagina 1di 214

Directing Emotion: A Practice-led Investigation into the Challenge of

Emotion in Western Performance

Submitted by Jessica Marie Beck,


to the University of Exeter as a thesis for the degree of
Doctor of Philosophy in Performance Practice,
1 September 2011

This thesis is available for library use on the understanding that it is copyright material
and that no quotation from the thesis may be published without proper
acknowledgement.

I certify that all material in this thesis which is not my own work has been identified
and that no material has previously been submitted and approved for the award of a
degree by this or any other University.

Jessica M. Beck
Abstract

The intention of this thesis is to arrive at a better understanding of the nature of


emotion in Western performance using three specific theories/approaches to emotion in
performance – Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work, and Impulse and Awareness
Work. I utilize these approaches to examine, from the perspective of a director working
with actors, how they can be applied effectively to different dramaturgies and to
discover if these approaches can shed light on the elusive nature of emotion in
performance. In a series of case studies, I apply these methods and/or their core
principles to different dramaturgies, to investigate how each can address the ‘problem‘
of emotion with regard to text, characterization, imagination and the performance itself.
The Introduction highlights some of the problems and challenges around the topic of
emotion in performance and outlines the research project including a discussion about
practice-as-research and methodology.
Chapter One is in three sections. Section One explores the controversy
surrounding Diderot’s Paradoxe and how his polarization of ‘real’ emotion and
‘performed’ emotion has problematically influenced and dominated the discussion of
emotion in Western performance. Section Two outlines the underlying problems of
understanding the nature of emotion in everyday life, primarily the difficulties in
defining emotion and the predominance of Cartesian dualism in Western society. The
third part of the chapter explores the problem of emotion in Western performance, and
how Diderot’s Paradoxe continues to inform the conflicting ideologies of 20th century
theatre practitioners. Chapter Two introduces the theories/approaches to emotion that I
will be using to examine my research questions and is divided into four sections.
Sections one, two and three outline in turn Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work, and
Impulse and Awareness Work. Chapters Three, Four, and Five discuss the specific case
studies – Excerpts from Anton Chekhov’s Three Sisters, Samuel Beckett’s Play and
Footfalls, and Helena Enright’s Less than a Year.
This thesis is accompanied by five DVDs, which contain rehearsal footage as
well as the actual performances. When appropriate, the reader will be directed to the
DVDs for specific examples of the practice.

2
Table of Contents
Abstract ....................................................................................................................................2  

Table  of  Contents...................................................................................................................3  

Illustrations  and  Images .....................................................................................................7  

Accompanying  Materials ....................................................................................................9  

Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................... 11  

Introduction......................................................................................................................... 12  

The  Challenge  of  ‘Emotion’  in  Western  Performance ......................................................12  

The  Research  Project..................................................................................................................18  

Research  Questions  Guiding  this  Study................................................................................20  

Practice-­as-­Research ..................................................................................................................20  

The  Pilot  Study..............................................................................................................................22  

Methodology ..................................................................................................................................25  

Chapter  Preview ...........................................................................................................................26  

Chapter  One:  The  Question  of  Emotion  in  Western  Performance ..................... 30  

Section  I:  Diderot’s  Paradoxe ...................................................................................................31  

Paradoxe  Proposed ...................................................................................................................................33  

Paradoxe  Pondered...................................................................................................................................36  

Section  II:  The  ‘Problem’  of  Emotion  in  Life........................................................................40  

The  Notion  of  Emotion............................................................................................................................40  

Emotions  as  Biological  Inheritance ...................................................................................................41  

Emotions  as  Social  Constructs .............................................................................................................43  

Emotion  as  Perceptions  of  Bodily  Changes....................................................................................44  

Emotion  as  Result  of  Cognitive  Appraisal.......................................................................................46  

Distinguishing  Between  Emotions,  Moods,  and  Feelings ........................................................47  

The  Mind/Body  Split................................................................................................................................47  


3
Towards  a  Holistic  View  of  the  Mind,  Body  and  Emotions .....................................................48  

Section  III:  Key  Theories  on  Acting  and  Emotion  in  the  19th  and  20th  Centuries....50  

What  is  Acting?...........................................................................................................................................50  

Acting  Emotions.........................................................................................................................................51  

The  Style  of  Involvement .......................................................................................................................51  

The  Style  of  Detachment ........................................................................................................................54  

The  Style  of  Self-­‐Expression .................................................................................................................56  

Problems  with  this  Model/Towards  a  Psychophysical  Approach .......................................57  

The  Psychophysical  Process .................................................................................................................58  

Françoise  Delsarte  and  the  ‘Psychophysical’ ................................................................................60  

Section  IV:  Diderot’s  Paradoxe  and  the  Practitioners  of  the  Twentieth  Century ...61  

Paradoxe  and  Practitioners  of  the  Twenty-­‐first  Century .........................................................65  

Chapter  Two:  New  approaches  to  Acting  and  Emotion  in  the  20th  and  21st  

Centuries:  Alba  Emoting,  Emotional  Access  Work  and  Impulse  and  

Awareness ............................................................................................................................ 67  

Section  I:  Alba  Emoting ..............................................................................................................67  

The  Patterns ................................................................................................................................................71  

Learning  Alba  Emoting ...........................................................................................................................75  

In  Practice.....................................................................................................................................................76  

Section  Two:  Emotional  Access  Work ...................................................................................77  

The  Immediate  Exercises.......................................................................................................................80  

The  Contributory  Exercises ..................................................................................................................84  

In  Practice.....................................................................................................................................................85  

Section  III:  Impulse  and  Awareness  Work...........................................................................86  

In  Practice.....................................................................................................................................................91  

Chapter  Three:  Case  Study  One  -­  Investigating  the  Challenge  of  Emotion  in  

the  Rehearsals  and  Performance  of  Excerpts  from  Chekhov’s  Three  Sisters. 93  

4
Section  1:  Chekhov’s  Three  Sisters .........................................................................................93  

Chekhov,  Stanislavsky  and  the  Moscow  Art  Theatre.................................................................93  

Synopsis  of  Three  Sisters ........................................................................................................................97  

Section  II:  Methodology  and  Preliminary  Questions .......................................................98  

Section  III:  Rehearsals ............................................................................................................. 104  

Incorporating  Alba  Emoting .............................................................................................................. 104  

Incorporating  Emotional  Access  Work ......................................................................................... 113  

Incorporating  Impulse  and  Awareness  work ............................................................................ 123  

Section  IV:  Findings .................................................................................................................. 127  

Alba  Emoting ............................................................................................................................................ 127  

Emotional  Access  Work ....................................................................................................................... 128  

Impulse  and  Awareness  Work.......................................................................................................... 129  

More  Questions ....................................................................................................................................... 130  

Chapter  Four:  Case  Study  Three:  Play  and  Footfalls  by  Samuel  Beckett........132  

Section  I:  Introduction  to  the  Acting  ‘Problem’  of  Beckett.......................................... 132  

Methodology  for  Case  Study  Two:  Play  and  Footfalls............................................................. 135  

Section  II:  Play............................................................................................................................ 135  

Rehearsing  Play....................................................................................................................................... 137  

Alba  Emoting ............................................................................................................................................ 137  

Emotional  Access  Work ....................................................................................................................... 139  

Impulse  and  Awareness  Work.......................................................................................................... 140  

Physical  Restrictions............................................................................................................................. 143  

In  Performance........................................................................................................................................ 145  

Findings ...................................................................................................................................................... 147  

Section  III:  Footfalls.................................................................................................................. 151  

Rehearing  Footfalls ................................................................................................................................ 153  

Alba  Emoting ............................................................................................................................................ 154  

Emotional  Access  Work ....................................................................................................................... 154  


5
Physical  Restrictions............................................................................................................................. 157  

Performance  and  Findings ................................................................................................................. 159  

Section  IV:  The  ‘Affect’  of  Beckett’s  Plays  on  the  Actor ................................................ 160  

Chapter  Five:  Case  Study  Three:  Less  Than  a  Year  by  Helena  Enright............163  

Section  I:  The  Acting  ‘Problems’  of  Verbatim  Theatre.................................................. 163  

Section  II:  Methodology  and  Set-­up  of  Case  Study  Three............................................. 170  

Less  Than  a  Year  by  Helena  Enright ............................................................................................... 171  

Section  III:  Less  Than  a  Year  in  Rehearsals  and  Performance ................................... 176  

Alba  Emoting ............................................................................................................................................ 177  

Emotional  Access  Work ....................................................................................................................... 177  

Impulse  and  Awareness  work .......................................................................................................... 180  

In  Performance........................................................................................................................................ 182  

Section  IV:  Findings .................................................................................................................. 187  

Conclusion ..........................................................................................................................189  

Appendices .........................................................................................................................198  

Appendix  A .................................................................................................................................. 198  

Appendix  B .................................................................................................................................. 199  

Appendix  C................................................................................................................................... 202  

Appendix  D .................................................................................................................................. 203  

Bibliography ......................................................................................................................205  

6
Illustrations and Images

Figure: Page:

1. Frances Buckroyd as Natalya and James Bolt as Smirnov in 23


The Proposal. Photograph by Brian Astbury.

2. The Alba Emoting Effector Patterns. 72

3. Facciponti Bond’s AE Labeling System. 73

4. Beck instructs Alba Emoting. Photograph by Bernard Dubois. 74

5. Alan Cox and Ben Porter in Orwell: A Celebration at Trafalgar 77


Studios. Photograph by Dawn Cruttenden.

6. Astbury using Physical Metaphor with actors Carolina Ortega and 84


Matthew Blake. Photograph by Kezia Cole.

7. Students from the East 15 Contemporary Theatre Practice course 88


working with Ian Morgan. Photograph by Brian Astbury.

8. Matthew Flacks and Andrew Barron in All Alone by 92


Gene David Kirk. Photograph by Jessica Beck.

9. Eric Hetzler as Vershinin and Zofia Sozanska as Masha in 110


Three Sisters. Photograph by Jen Burton.

10. Joe Sellman-Leava as Andrey in Three Sisters. 111


Photograph by Jen Burton.

11. Lai SimSim as Irina and Jeremy West as Tusenbach in 113


Three Sisters. Photograph by Jen Burton.

12. Zofia Sozanska as Olga and Lai SimSim as Irina in Three Sisters. 116
Photograph by Jen Burton.

13. Eric Hetzler as Vershinin, Zofia Sozanska as Masha, and Lai SimSim 122
as Olga in Three Sisters. Photograph by Jen Burton.

14. Lauren Shepherd, Callum Elliott-Archer and Symmonie Preston in 137


Play. Photograph by Ben Borley.

15. Lauren Shepherd in Play. Photograph by Ben Borley. 145

16. Joe Sellman-Leava as the Inquisitor in Play. Photograph by Ben Borley. 150

17. Elizabeth Pennington in Footfalls. Photograph by Ben Borley. 153

18. Elizabeth Pennington in Footfalls. Photograph by Ben Borley. 162

7
19. Charlie Coldfield and Helena Enright in Less Than a Year 175
Photograph by Anna Johnson.

8
Accompanying Materials

Five accompanying DVDs are affixed to the inside of the back cover of this dissertation.
Listed below are the contents of each DVD, and the page number corresponding to their
place in the dissertation. The footage contains clips from rehearsals, documentation of
performances, and documentation of the academic presentations that accompanied each
case study. It is not necessary for the reader to watch all the footage, however the
chosen clips that may help for further clarification or understanding of the exercises.

DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: Page:

1. The Alba Emoting Patterns 105


2. Sozanska in the Alba Emoting Object Exercise 107
3. Alba Emoting Improvisation 108
4. Sozanska and Hetzler using 3a 110
5. Sellman-Leava Rehearsing 111
6. Astbury’s Physical Exercises 113
7. Sozanska and Lai after Anger/Energy Run 115
8. Lai and West after Physical Exercises 118
9. Hetzler and Sozanska after Physical Exercises 120
10. Physical Metaphor Exercises 120
11. Lai and West after Physical Metaphor 120
12. Hetzler and Sozanska after Physical Metaphor 121
13. Excerpt of Impulse and Awareness Exercise 124
14. Final Group Impulse Run 126

DVD Chapter 3, Disc 2:

15. Case Study 1 – Performance and Presentation 127

DVD Chapter 4:
1. Alba Emoting Mirror Work 138
2. Impulse and Awareness Ball Exercise 141
3. Excerpt from Impulse Run with Alba Emoting 142
4. Shepherd and the Urn 144
5. Alba/Impulse Run in Urns 144
6. Performance of Play – 27 April 2011 145
7. Dress Rehearsal of Play – 30 April 2011 146
8. Excerpt of Anger/Energy Run with Enright and Pennington 155
9. Pennington after Physical Metaphor 156
10. Under-reading with Pennington 158
11. Performance of Footfalls – 27 April 2011 159
12. Case Study 2 – Presentation 160

DVD Chapter 5, Disc 1:

1. Coldfield in Physical Exercises 179


2. Enright after Physical Exercises 179
3. Coldfield in Impulse Run 181
4. Case Study 3 – Presentation 182
9
5. Enright in Dress Rehearsal 182
6. Performance of Less Than a Year – 15 January 2011 183

DVD Chapter 5, Disc 2:

7. Performance of Less Than a Year – 16 January 2011 184


8. Prologue from 18 January 2011 185
9. Enright from 18 January 2011 185
10. Coldfield and Enright 19 January 2011 187
11. Coldfield and Enright 20 January 2011 187

10
Acknowledgements

This PhD would not have been possible without the guidance, support, and participation
of a wide variety of people in my life. From the bottom of my heart I would like to
thank the following people:

For excellent supervision and guidance: Professor Phillip Zarrilli, as well as Bella
Merlin who began the journey with us.

For generously sharing their innovations in actor training: Brian Astbury, Susana Bloch,
Grzegorz Bral, Laura Facciponti Bond and Ian Morgan.

For wonderful technical support: Jon Primrose, Chris Mearing, and Andy Yarwood.

For the friendly and supportive staff: Gayatri Simons, Christopher McCullough, David
Roesner, Peter Thomson, Mick Mangan, Jerri Daboo, Rebecca Loukes and Jane
Milling.

For participating as my guinea pigs/actors: James Bolt, Frances Buckroyd, Charlie


Coldfield, Callum Elliott-Archer, Helena Enright, Eric Hetzler, Lai Sim Sim, Sarah
Pearman, Elizabeth Jane Pennington, Symmonie Preston, Joe Sellman-Leava, Lauren
Margaret Shepherd, Zofia Sozanska, Jeremy West, and Alexander Warner.

Other support: Katrina Shewen, Fin Irwin, David Lockwood, Phil Hewitt, Gene David
Kirk, Trina Fischer and Juan Pablo Kalawski, Siggi Lindal, Rocco Dal Vera, Odette
Guimond, Bernard Dubois, Benjamin Borley, Kezia Cole, Anna Johnson, Maggie-Kate
Coleman, Miriam Ackerman, Joanna Mitchell, Chloe Whipple, Tom Angell, Rocco Dal
Vera and Tom Mansfield.

For amazing PhD-related conversations and book recommendations and support:


Richard Feltham, Lorraine Sutherland, Kelli Melson, Eric Hetzler, Natasha Lushetich,
Jason Price, Effrosyni Mastrokalou, James McLaughlin, Ioannis Souris, Sarah
Goldingay, Sarah Sigal, Sunhee Kim, Duncan Jamieson, Luke Beattie, James Clack,
and Lavinia Plonka.

And especially Helena Enright.

And for outstanding love and moral support: Ed and Marie Beck, Sarah Beck and
Christopher Lacey-Malvern.

Dedicated to Brian Astbury.

11
Introduction
The Challenge of ‘Emotion’ in Western Performance

The careers of many a scientist have been devoted to, if not devoured by,
the task of explaining emotions. Unfortunately, one of the most
significant things ever said about emotion may be that everyone knows
what it is until they are asked to define it. – Joseph LeDoux, The
Emotional Brain (1996: 23)

Neuroscientist Joseph LeDoux highlights a key problem in the quest for

understanding the nature of ‘emotion’. Despite the fact that the topic of emotion has

been debated and discussed throughout human history, by theologians, philosophers,

and more recently, neuroscientists, many unanswered questions about the nature of

emotion remain. Emotions are a common human experience. Neuroscientist Antonio

Damasio, author of The Feeling of What Happens (1999) states:

Without exception, men and women of all ages, of all cultures, of all
levels of education, and of all walks of economic life have emotions,
are mindful of the emotions of others, cultivate pastimes that
manipulate their emotions, and govern their lives in no small part by
the pursuit of one emotion, happiness, and the avoidance of
unpleasant emotions. (Damasio 1999: 35)

So if emotions are phenomena that are part of our everyday experience, why are they so

difficult to understand and define?

Emotion and theatre are often discussed simultaneously. But again, what is an

emotion? As Francis Sparshott points out in his essay on ‘Emotion and Emotions in

Theatre Dance’ as part of a collection of essays entitled Emotion and the Arts, the

frequency of ‘common usage’ of the word emotion ‘does not guarantee that there is an

identifiable entity or topic that could ground discussion’ (Sparshott 1997: 119).

Sparshott goes further, maintaining:

12
The bulk of everyday discussion of the emotions in art and life, as
valuable as it may be, is bedeviled by the assumption that an emotion is
somehow an ontologically established entity. One supposes that there
must be true things to be said about what emotions are, and what
emotions there are, just as one supposes that there must be true things to
be said about what dance is. (ibid.: 123)

In The Science of Acting, Sam Kogan, an acting teacher who trained at the Moscow

Institute of Theatre Arts under Stanislavsky’s student Maria Knebel, recounts his

bemusement when confronted by a student’s question, what is emotion? After scouring

Stanislavsky’s books and finding nothing clear, Kogan resigned himself to creating his

own definition, that ‘emotion is the bio-physiological result of a thought’ (Kogan and

Kogan 2010: 6). Kogan reflects: ‘My experience of the word ‘emotion’ taught me that if

I use a word, the meaning of which you don’t understand, it leaves you with confusion

which creates noise in your head which prevents you from thinking clearly’ (ibid.).

In theatre, emotion can be discussed from two perspectives, that of the audience

and that of the actor. There is the emotion one may feel as an audience member – a

response to a theatrical presentation; perhaps a response of empathy, when relating to

the story being told; anger, in response to issues being raised in the play; or frustration,

when the play is unsatisfactory (those who work in the theatre industry may be

particularly prone to this feeling). This is the affect of a play on the audience. Aristotle

noted the phenomenon of katharsis or ‘cleansing’ that could occur when watching a

tragedy, in which ‘through pity and fear effecting the proper purgation of such

emotions’ (Aristotle and Halliwell 2009: location 90). Other conditions that may

contribute to the affect of the play on the audience include all the elements of the

production from sound and lighting choices to the content of the play, to the

performances of the actors.

There is another aspect of emotion and theatre, however, which is the actor’s

task of portraying ‘emotion’ in performance – the affect of emotion on the performer.

13
As audience members respond uniquely to any given theatrical performance based on

their own personal life experiences, a director cannot necessarily predict or control the

response that individual audience members will have to a particular play. However, a

director can influence the actor’s performance (and the actor’s work with emotion) in

order to initiate a response from the audience. For that reason, my research will be

focused on how a director can facilitate the actor’s experience with emotion for the

purposes of performance.

The difficulties that arise when discussing the role of emotion in relation to the

actor’s task are many. One is the lack of an agreed definition for the term ‘emotion’.

Without a clear understanding of what emotion is, it is impossible to communicate

effectively about it. The term is usually left undefined and discussed in a vague manner

that opens the argument about how the actor is supposed to interact with emotion. Is the

actor reliving an emotion? Are they pretending to be experiencing an emotion? How can

they really be experiencing emotion if they are not really the character and are not really

having that experience? Should the actor be reliving an emotional experience from their

own lives? All these questions raise further ones, such as, how important is emotion to

the actor’s task? The idea of ‘emotion’ in acting theory has sparked continuous debate

within the acting world for centuries. A review of the historical and recent literature

from theorists to practitioners reveals that ‘emotions’ about ‘emotions’ run high. These

disputes will be discussed further in Chapter One.

In Joseph Roach’s investigation of the correspondence between Western

scientific and medical paradigms and Western acting theory, The Player’s Passion:

Studies in the Science of Acting (1993), Roach highlights the most influential debates

14
about acting theory throughout Western history1. Roach maintains that ‘at the center

of this ongoing controversy stands the question of emotion’ (Roach 1993: 11). Of

course, the ‘question of emotion’ is not one question, but many, that have been

vehemently debated throughout history in various fields. In Roach’s view, the most

prominent question of emotion was posed by the philosopher Denis Diderot (1713 –

1784) in Paradoxe sur le Comedien (1773, published 1830), in which he speculates on

the nature of emotion in performance and endeavors to identify who is the superior

actor, the actor who is sincerely experiencing emotion in performance or the actor who

remains detached from the emotion while performing? Diderot poses the question: ‘If

the actor were feeling [rather than merely playing] the part, wouldn’t it be virtually

impossible for him to act the same part twice in a row with the same passion and the

same success?’ [Gray’s emphasis](Diderot in Gray 2007: 250). Diderot’s paradox

continues to haunt Western acting theory even to this day, which I discuss in more

detail in Chapter One.

In a recent book The Philosophical Actor: A Practical Meditation for Practicing

Theatre Artists (2010), Donna Soto-Morettini closely analyses many common-usage

concepts in acting and ‘folk theory’ regarding performance. Soto-Morettini identifies

two prominent debates relating to emotion and the actor – the first is concerned with

‘the relationship of the actor’s own feelings to the feelings that the actor portrays’

(Soto-Morettini 2010: 13). The second debate focuses on ‘the relationship of the actor’s

own personality of the character the actor portrays’ (ibid.). Aside from investigating

issues of ‘truth’, Soto-Morettini raises some key questions about emotions that are

pertinent to this study, including:

1
The ‘problem of emotion’ as it has been configured in the West does not appear to be a ‘problem’ in
many non-Western cultures and performance traditions. This thesis focuses exclusively on the problem of
emotion in Western acting.
15
Are the feelings actors generate genuine feelings? If so, how does the
actor control these feelings?

How do actors ‘generate’ feelings?’

Is the difference between an actor who is feeling genuine feelings


and an actor with the ability to convince us that his/her feelings are
genuine simply one technique? How does that technique work, and
can it be taught? [original emphasis] (Soto-Morettini 2010: 115)

In this instance, Soto-Morettini is using the terms ‘emotion’ and ‘feeling’

interchangeably, though she later marks a distinction between the two terms. She

defines emotions as ‘something that happens first, sometimes below conscious level

[…] and feelings being the more immediately cognitive process that attends the

emotions and consciousness’ (ibid.: 117). Further discussion about the differences

between ‘emotion’ and ‘feeling’ will be addressed in chapter one.

Aside from Diderot’s paradox, haunting the discussion of emotion in

performance is the debate about whose emotion the actor should be expressing, the

character’s emotion or their own? Soto-Morettini begs the question ‘if we believe that

good acting involves the expression of emotion, then isn’t it MY emotion that’s being

stirred? And even if I fake it, isn’t it MY fake emotion that I’m portraying?’ [original

emphasis] (Soto-Morettini 2010: 86). This raises further questions, such as whose

emotion is being expressed? —the actor’s emotion or the character’s emotion? Robert

Gordon raises yet another paradox in his book The Purpose of Playing (2006),

explaining that ‘for the actor, the central paradox of acting is always the way in which

her real body is used to represent a virtual body’ (Gordon 2006: 2). A further

consideration is the question of aesthetics. Are we talking about recognizable emotion,

indicated emotion, or affective emotion? The kind of emotion required depends on the

demands and style of the specific play or theatrical piece. For this reason, this research

project will use different kinds of texts, specifically psychological realism, post-

dramatic theatre and documentary theatre.

16
In Acting (Re)Considered, Phillip Zarrilli acknowledges that:

Informing any “theory” of acting is a historically and culturally variable


set of assumptions about the body, mind, their relationship, the nature of
the “self,” the “inner” experience of what the actor does—often called
emotion or feeling—and the relationship between the actor and spectator.
(Zarrilli 1995: 635)

It is precisely this ‘inner’ experience, and the actor’s challenge of engaging with

‘emotion’ in rehearsals and performance that I will be investigating in this study.

Finding words to accurately describe the actor’s process is difficult, and when

translating from tacit knowledge to a verbal form, a gap emerges.

My attraction to the subject of emotion in relation to the actor in performance,

began with my first experience of what I would now describe as a ‘psychophysical’

approach to actor training at the London Academy of Music and Dramatic Art

(LAMDA) in 2002 with Brian Astbury, whose work will be discussed in detail in

Chapter Two. The word ‘psychophysical’ made its way into acting vocabulary primarily

through Stanislavsky. Bella Merlin, a professional actor and academic, refers to psycho-

physicality as ‘the dialogue between your body and your psyche’ (Merlin 2007a: 21).

Zarrilli deconstructs this term further:

Here, “psycho” does not mark the recent Western invention of


psychologies of the self/individual as in the compound “psychological,”
but rather refers to another meaning of the Greek psyche: the vital
principle—namely, the élan vital or the enlivening quality of the
(actor’s) breath/energy. (Zarrilli 2007: 636)

My previous acting background was in early American versions of Stanislavsky work.

This was almost entirely intellectual except for emotional memory exercises. A lot of

time was spent sitting around the table, discussing character motivation and psychology.

While these discussions were interesting, there was always a gap between those

discussions and the process of ‘acting’ per se. Although I could understand perfectly

what had been discussed, it did not readily help me when it came to the practice of

embodying the text or the role. Through Astbury’s ‘Emotional Access Work’, this ‘gap’

17
disappeared. From my perspective, ‘emotion’ seemed to be the missing link that filled

this gap, and ultimately led to this research project.

The Research Project


In this research project I will examine three approaches that have I begun to

integrate into my professional work – ‘Alba Emoting’ – developed by Chilean

neuroscientist Susana Bloch; ‘Emotional Access Work’ – developed by South African

theatre practitioner Brian Astbury; and ‘Impulse and Awareness Work’ – a theory of my

own in development using the selected principles of the training techniques of the

Polish theatre company Teatr Piesn Kozla (Song of the Goat). All three approaches

engage the physical or physiological aspects of emotion, and resonate with new

information as to how the mind and body function. Susana Bloch’s Alba Emoting is a

tool for actors to effectively induce the physiological changes that occur with an

emotion, devised from extensive and objective scientific research. Working as a

neuroscientist, Bloch and her research team were interested in the physiological changes

that take place when experiencing emotion, such as breath, heart rate, and changes in

muscle tonus. After various scientific experiments, Bloch identified six common

patterns that she considers to be basic emotions – anger, tenderness, fear, eroticism,

sadness and joy. Their research also revealed that if one can learn to activate the

voluntary aspects of the pattern together at the same time – breath, facial expression,

and postural attitude – one can physiologically activate that emotion. In a recent article

on Alba Emoting, I explain:

Although the connection between breathing and emotion is nothing new,


Bloch and her research team are among the first to explore this
relationship in the context of Western science, articulating phenomena
that many performers have been intuitively embodying for centuries.
(Beck 2010: 142)

18
Brian Astbury’s ‘Emotional Access Work’ lends itself more directly to a

rehearsal process as a whole. Astbury’s work is based on his theory that our (self)

conscious brain can be problematic in rehearsal and that the creative (sub) conscious

can be accessed through various exercises to overcome the intrusion of self-

consciousness in the actor’s process. Astbury’s techniques were derived from years of

working with actors in training, rehearsals, and performance. Astbury’s exercises draw

upon the work of process-oriented psychologist Arnold Mindell, Alexander Lowen’s

‘Bioenergetics’, Win Wenger’s ‘Image Streaming’, and the theories of neuroscientists

such as Antonio Damasio, Joseph LeDoux, and Michael Gazzaniga. Although many of

his techniques correlate with recent discoveries in neuroscience, this correlation is

mostly theoretical, based on Astbury’s observation and experience of more than twenty

years training actors.

And finally, I will be using a set of techniques that I call ‘impulse and awareness

work’. These are exercises that I use to increase the actors’ awareness of themselves,

their awareness in relation to one another, and to the space and environment. They are

also used to help the actors work off impulses from within, from one another, and the

surrounding environment including the space itself and any aural or visual stimuli. This

work is partly based on selected principles from the training of Teatr Piesn Kozla, a

Polish theatre company founded by Grzegorz Bral and Anna Zubrzycki, both formally

of the Gardzienice Theatre Association. Bral and Zubrzycki often refrain from

discussing ‘emotion’ directly, as Bral find that this can be problematic for a performer:

A performer shouldn’t really name and shouldn’t really use too many
definitions of what he or she does. A performer is somebody who has to
learn to flow – and has to plug into the stream because, if performance is
too analytical, you are already too late. (Zubrzycki and Bral 2010: 255)

In their training, however, they often talk about ‘making room for something to

happen’, which, in my experience from working with Bral, I take that ‘something’ to

mean ‘emotion’.
19
The goal of this research project is to arrive at a better understanding of the

nature of emotion in rehearsals and performance through these three approaches, and to

examine, from the perspective of a director, how they can be applied effectively to

different texts with actors from different backgrounds. In a series of case studies I will

apply these methods and/or their core principles to different dramaturgies, and

investigate how each can be applied to address the challenge of emotion in Western

performance.

Research Questions Guiding this Study


1) What, if anything, can be revealed about the nature of emotion in Western

performance when employing these three theories/approaches to emotion in

practice? Do these discoveries vary when these approaches are put into practice

on different dramaturgies with different demands on the actor?

2) What is the nature of the emotion being expressed? Whose emotion is it? The

actor’s or the character’s? Is there a difference?

3) Can these techniques assist a director in unifying an understanding of emotion

within a diverse cast? How much of the actor’s previous beliefs, training and

acting experiences affect their views on emotion in performance? How do these

previously held views/assumption interact with the three theories used?

And lastly, taking a cue from Soto-Morettini:

4) Are the feelings/emotions that actors generate ‘genuine’? If so, how does the

actor generate, control, and integrate these emotions in performance?

Practice-as-Research
Cartesian dualism may pose a challenge to the understanding of emotion and

acting, but it also affects how we perceive knowledge, as the traditional view places ‘the

mind as the sole locus of certain knowledge’ (Nelson 2009: 115). Robin Nelson points

out that ‘some practice-as-research projects that advance the idea of ‘embodied
20
knowledge’ pose a challenge […] to the privileging of mind over body in Western

intellectual tradition in respect of the locus of knowledge’ (ibid.). Despite this

challenge, practice-as-research is rapidly becoming an accepted form of inquiry within

the academy, though not without difficulty. Robin Nelson notes:

Practice-as-research is not characteristically data-based and the organic


nature of the creative processes means that the laying out of
methodologies in advance seems to beg the question of methodical
process more than it does in scientific research models (Nelson 2009:
113).

Brad Haesman and Daniel Mafe, authors of ‘Acquiring Know-How: Research Training

for Practice-led Researchers’ (2009), maintain that ‘the central problem is one of

research methodology’ (Haesman and Mafe 2009: 211). The type of research used for

this project is qualitative, which ‘often draws on individuals’ experiences of events,

processes and systems’ (McMillan and Weyers 2007: 124). Kathleen McMillan and

Jonathan Weyers maintain:

In an ideal sense, such investigations might be carried out without


preconception by allowing the participant to provide a completely,
unstructured and uninterrupted stream of thoughts with conclusions
drawn following examination of the information obtained. (ibid.)

Carole Gray, in her article ‘Inquiry Through Practice: Developing Appropriate Research

Strategies’ (1996) identifies two important aspects of practice-as-research:

[F]irstly the research which is initiated in practice, where questions,


problems, challenges are identified and formed by the needs of practice
and practitioners; and secondly, that the research strategy is carried out
through practice, using predominately methodologies and specific
methods familiar to us as practitioners. (Gray in Haesman and Mafe
2010: 213)

Chapter One: The Question of Emotion sets out the key questions, problems and

challenges as discussed by theatre practitioners regarding the nature of emotion in

Western performance. My research strategy is to explore these questions, problems and

challenges through rehearsals and performance, which are the ‘specific methods

familiar’ to theatre practitioners. In John Freeman’s book Blood, Sweat & Theory:

21
Research though Practice in Performance (2010), the author remarks that the recent

rise in the number of degrees in practice-as-research ‘is not to suggest that the linking of

research with performance is anything new’ (Freeman 2010: 151). He acknowledges:

The twentieth century saw numerous practitioners whose work was


highly practical in outcome at the same time as the investigative and
research qualities were clear to see…and not just in hindsight. These
researching practitioners – and we can think immediately of luminaries
such as Stanislavski, Meyerhold, Brecht, Grotowski, Barba and Brook –
created a legacy of intelligent and often intellectually vital work that did
much to shape their own time and ours. What differs now is the sheer
scale of practice-based research. (ibid.)

Following in the footsteps of the director/researchers that have come before me, this

study will investigate the ‘problem’ of emotion in Western performance through a

number of case studies.

The Pilot Study


Since practice-as-research, or practice-based research, is a relatively new

phenomenon within the academy, I chose to begin with a ‘pilot study’ to better

understand how to embark on practice-as-research rather than just ‘practice’. In the field

of qualitative research, a pilot study is usually considered ‘a preliminary study, often

conducted on a small scale’ which can give the researcher ‘a chance to work through

[one’s] approach to identify inconsistencies or weaknesses’ (McMillan and Weyers

2010: 127). In 2008 I conducted a pilot study to experiment with a practice-as-research

project. The texts used for this study were two of Chekhov’s one-act farces, The Bear

and The Proposal. Four actors participated in the project: James Bolt, Frances

Buckroyd, Sarah Pearman and Alex Warner. All the participants in this project were

actors who had previously approached me about being involved with the development

of my process and research. All four actors were early in their careers and had all

trained at accredited UK drama schools. The rehearsals and performances took place in

an empty school in Mill Hill East, North London. The project ran over fourteen days

22
and included three performances. Rather than offer a full account of this study, I will

highlight the difficulties and outcomes that informed how I have structured my later

case studies.

Figure 1: Frances Buckroyd as Natalya and James Bolt as Smirnov in The Proposal. Photograph by
Brian Astbury.

The most important developments from the pilot study that changed my

methodology include: 1) my method of gathering qualitative research material from the

participants, 2) the research questions, 3) documentation, 4) the participants, and 5) the

location and context of the projects.

My initial tool for gathering qualitative research from the participants was

through their journals – written responses to questionnaires and continual written

reflection about their experiences with the texts and the exercises. The data I was

gathering was insightful, yet sparse. I was reading their journals throughout the

rehearsal process, and would often have to ask the actors for further clarification about

points that they had made. During a post-show discussion after one of the performances,
23
I was impressed by how articulate the actors were when responding to questions about

the process. It occurred to me that actors are more comfortable talking about their

experiences rather than writing about them. One actor in the pilot study commented that

responding to the journal questions felt like ‘homework.’ For my later case studies, I

decided to conduct interviews with the actors throughout the study. Interviewing is

preferable for two reasons: the actors are more comfortable discussing their experiences

and I can also interject to ask for further clarification if necessary. Actors give clearer

responses to questions and provide more articulate accounts when interviewed rather

than when responding to questions in writing or in journal entries.

The research questions for the original pilot study consisted of my overall

questions for the project, rather than also considering the specific demands and

challenges of the particular dramaturgy. As a result, my focus was on documenting

every exercise or technique, rather than using the exercises or techniques that would be

the most beneficial to the rehearsal process, as I would in a professional setting.

Negotiating the demands of a specific text with the demands of the specific actors

involved is an important aspect of directing. In this case, I was using exercises for the

sake of using exercises in order to document them, rather than responding to the needs

of the actors and the text. Further to this point, the documentation of the project began

to take precedence over the project itself. My anxiety about making certain that every

aspect of the project was ‘captured’ on DVD inhibited my role as a participant-

researcher in the project. For the subsequent case studies, I made two important

changes: 1) I decided that the techniques that I would use would serve the play and the

actors rather than vice versa, and 2) resolved that the camera would be set up in the

corner of the room and forgotten about, or be used by my assistant director, so that my

full attention would be free to focus on working with the actors.

24
Initially, I felt it was important for my research that I use typical working

actors in London, as I would in the ‘real world’. But this came with challenges. Though

the actors were all interested in participating fully in the research project, they were still

‘jobbing actors’ in London. Throughout the fourteen-day process, the equivalent of

nearly two days of rehearsal was lost because the actors had auditions for other projects.

While all the actors were enthusiastic about being involved in the research project, the

reality is that each of the actors had the financial pressure of trying to afford to live in

London, and preferably to earn that money through their acting work. This confirmed

for me that it would be more beneficial to the research to work with actors in a

university setting – actors who had the time to reflect on their experiences more

thoroughly. Also, as practice-based research is meant to be shared and disseminated, the

ideal location for the performances would be within an academic community at the

University of Exeter. By the same token, rather than solely offering a post-show

discussion for the actors and the audience, I realized that it was important to also offer a

context for the research. Every case study, then, would have an accompanying

presentation.

Methodology
The primary objective of my research through practice is to shed light on the

nature of ‘emotion’ in Western performance using the three approaches outlined above

– Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work, and Impulse and Awareness Work – through

the practical application of these techniques to a rehearsal process using different

dramaturgies; specifically psychological realism, postdramatic theatre2, and Verbatim

Theatre. For this research, the following methodology was proposed.

2
A form of theatre with a ‘profoundly changed mode of theatrical sign usage’ (Lehmann
2006: 17). Hans-Thies Lehmann uses the term ‘postdramatic’ to cover a wide range of new
theatre forms that have emerged since the late 1960s in which the focus of the work is no
longer on the traditional dramatic text. Lehmann goes on to mention how ’authors like
25
Each rehearsal process and performance(s) is to be considered as a ‘case

study’. And each case study is a practical experiment that began with a set of research

questions around the nature of emotion in performance using different dramaturgies for

rehearsal process and performance. The case studies stand alone as individual

experiments with the theories and their relation to the specific dramaturgies of the

particular project. However, there may be some conclusions that result when

considering all three projects as a ‘collective case study’, which could potentially ‘lead

to better understanding, perhaps better theorizing, about a still larger collection of cases’

(Stake 2005: 437). Using qualitative methods such as interviewing and participant-

observation (as I directed each project), I obtained subjective data on the actors’

experiences with the three approaches, the dramaturgy, and their relation to the question

of emotion. As the case studies progressed, their formats developed. Case Study One:

Excerpts from Chekhov’s Three Sisters gave the actors the experience of one

performance; Case Study Two: Beckett’s Footfalls and Play had two performances; and

Case Study Three: Enright’s Less Than a Year had six performances, including a charity

performance at a local theatre. The way in which I integrated the approaches changed in

response to the dramaturgies and the needs of the actors involved. This thesis is

accompanied by four DVDs, which contain rehearsal footage as well as the actual

performances. When necessary, the reader will be directed to the DVDs for specific

examples of the practice.

Chapter Preview
Chapter One: The Challenge of ‘Emotion’ in Western Performance is in three

sections. Section I: Diderot’s Paradoxe explores the controversy surrounding Diderot’s

Samuel Beckett and Heiner Müller avoided the dramatic form […] because of its implied
teleology of history’ (ibid.: 39).

26
Paradoxe and how his polarization of ‘real’ emotion and ‘performed’ emotion has

problematically influenced and dominated the discussion of emotion in performance.

Section II: The ‘Problem of Emotion in Life’ outlines the underlying problems of

understanding the nature of emotion in everyday life, primarily the difficulties in

defining emotion and the predominance of Cartesian dualism in Western society.

Included in this chapter are some of the theories of philosophers, psychologists and

neuroscientists such as Aristotle, William James, Charles Darwin, Robert Solomon,

Paul Ekman, Antonio Damasio, as well as less frequently cited theorists including

Théodule-Augustin Ribot and Candace Pert. The latter part of the chapter explores the

problem of emotion in Western3 performance, and how Diderot’s Paradoxe continues to

inform the conflicting ideologies of 20th century theatre practitioners. The intention here

is to highlight theories of emotion from major Western theatre practitioners including

Konstantin Stanislavsky, Vsevolod Meyerhold, Jerzy Grotowski, Lee Strasberg, and

Bertolt Brecht, as well as less frequently cited practitioners such as Francois Delsarte

and Genevieve Stebbins.

Chapter Two: New Approaches to Acting and Emotion in the 20th and 21st

Centuries introduces the theories/approaches to emotion that I used to examine my

research questions and is divided into three sections. Sections one, two and three outline

Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work, and Impulse and Awareness Work

respectively.

Chapter Three presents Case Study One: Excerpts from Chekhov’s Three Sisters,

in which I incorporated the work of Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work and

Impulse and Awareness Work into a rehearsal process on a selection of excerpts from

3
Although I am using the term ‘Western’ performance, this study will not cover the entire gamut of
Western practitioners. Most of the practitioners mentioned in this chapter are from or have been
influenced by the Russian acting tradition (rather than French), as much of the acting training I have
experienced was Russian influenced.
27
Anton Chekhov’s Three Sisters. Chekhov’s writing is often discussed in relation to

emotion in performance. Bella Merlin suggests that Chekhov’s writing, in particular

The Seagull, ‘was the catalyst which provoked Stanislavsky into applying new laws to

the acting process in order that it too might be structured as an art form’ [original

emphasis] (Merlin 1999: 224).

Chapter Four presents Case Study Two: Beckett’s Play and Footfalls, in which I

used Samuel Beckett’s Play and Footfalls. Beckett’s plays are notoriously difficult to

perform, especially his later dramatic works. Challenges to his work include finding the

correct musicality or tempo-rhythm of the text, working with the physical restrictions

and specific instructions put forward by Beckett, and in some cases an absence of

psychological realism, among other things.

Chapter Five presents Case Study Three: Enright’s Less than a Year, in which I

engage with a form of documentary theatre often described as Verbatim Theatre, a form

in which the script is composed of edited transcripts from interviews with real people.

The play used for Case Study Three is Less than a Year by Irish playwright Helena

Enright and was created from the transcripts of interviews with a couple who lost their

eighteen-year-old daughter to cancer.

Finally, the conclusion brings together the findings of all three cases and offers

some potentially useful discoveries to engage with the challenge of emotion. Of course,

as Freeman acknowledges in Blood, Sweat and Theory, ‘any solutions sought and found

are more likely to relate to the specific set of circumstances created by the work in

question than to any globally significant responses’ (Freeman 2010: 59). While what

initially attracted me to Astbury’s work and eventually to this study was ‘emotion’,

since embarking on this research my views on the subject have changed. In retrospect I

believe that what I was drawn to was not necessarily the display of emotion as such, but

rather the fact that the entire organism of the actor was working together as a cohesive

28
entity – psychophysically. My journey to this conclusion will be documented

throughout the dissertation.

29
Chapter One: The Question of Emotion in Western Performance

As Joseph Roach explores in his book The Player’s Passion: Studies in the

Science of Acting, a relationship has always existed between the sciences – biological,

psychological, neurological – and the performing arts. Rightly or wrongly, the ‘question

of emotion’ has been essential to this interdisciplinary relationship and stands at the

forefront of heated theatrical debate. Historically, many directors and actors became

researchers out of necessity, turning to the science of the time to help them discover

better strategies to facilitate better performances from their actors or achieve better

performances from themselves. In Western performance today, the controversy

continues, and many directors and actors hold strong opinions varying from different

ways and means of eliciting emotion in performance to whether or not emotion is

important at all (See Blair 2008; Hornby 1992; Konijn 2000).

The historical (and current) controversy surrounding emotion and its role in

performance indicates that there is a ‘problem of emotion’ in Western performance. Or,

more specifically, quite a few ‘problems’ that can create challenges for both actors and

directors. Given the relationship between life and art, it is likely that these issues stem

from the controversy surrounding emotion in everyday life. After researching the field

of emotion science, I have identified two ‘problems’ that are particularly challenging.

The first is finding an accurate definition of emotion. The term ‘emotion’ is commonly

used in daily conversation, but what actually is it? The second major ‘problem’ is the

prominence of Cartesian dualism in Western society — the mind/body split.

This chapter is divided into four sections. Section I: Diderot’s Paradoxe

examines Diderot’s paradox, which is arguably responsible for placing the ‘question of

emotion’ at the forefront of theatrical debate. Section II: The ‘Problem’ of Emotion in

Life will look at the problems associated with understanding emotion in everyday life,

30
specifically focusing on the difficulties of defining emotion and the challenges that

arise because of the prominence of Cartesian dualism in Western thought. Section III:

Key Theories on Acting and Emotion in the 19th and 20th Centuries will explore some

theories of acting and their relationship with emotion, as well as discussing how certain

theatre practitioners have approached this challenge from the mid-19th century onwards.

The last section, Section IV: Diderot’s Paradoxe and the Practitioners of the 20th

Century, returns to Diderot and how his paradox impacted on the discussion of emotion

in the work of important theatre practitioners.

Section I: Diderot’s Paradoxe


In his book The Player’s Passion (1993), Joseph Roach follows the close

correlation between acting theory and the scientific understanding of the body

throughout history:

Conceptions of the human body drawn from physiology and psychology


have dominated theories of acting from antiquity to present. The nature
of the body, its structure, its outer dynamics, and its relationship to the
larger world that it inhabits have been the subject of diverse speculation
and debate. At the center of this ongoing controversy stands the question
of emotion. (Roach 1993: 11)

Of course, the ‘question of emotion’ is not one question, but many, that have been

vehemently debated throughout history in various fields. In Roach’s view, the most

prominent ‘question of emotion’ was posed by the philosopher Denis Diderot (1713 –

1784) in Paradoxe sur le Comedien (17734), in which Diderot speculates on the nature

of emotion in performance and endeavors to identify who is the superior actor; the actor

who is sincerely experiencing emotion in performance, or the actor who remains

detached from the emotion while performing. As mentioned in the introduction, Diderot

poses the question: ‘If the actor were feeling [rather than merely playing] the part,

4
Written in 1773, published in 1830.

31
wouldn’t it be virtually impossible for him to act the same part twice in a row with the

same passion and the same success?’ [Gray’s emphasis] (Diderot in Gray5 2007: 250).

Diderot addresses this question in the form of a dialogue between two speakers,

debating the virtues and pitfalls of acting and the role of emotion. His conclusion is that

‘unequal acting’ is the result of actors who ‘play from the heart’ (Diderot and Pollock

1883: 8) and that for a ‘sublime’ actor, ‘tears come from his brain’ (ibid.: 17). For if it

were otherwise, ‘the actor’s condition would be the most unhappy of conditions’

(Diderot in Gray 2007: 253).

Throughout Paradoxe Diderot introduces many original concepts, or at least,

according to Roach, articulates them in ‘modern form’ including: ‘emotion memory,

imagination, creative unconsciousness, ensemble playing, double consciousness,

concentration, public solitude, character body, the score of the role, and spontaneity’

(Roach 1993: 117). Among Diderot’s contributions of note include his call for actors to

maintain consistency in their performances – and emotion, in Diderot’s opinion – is an

obstacle to consistency. By the same token he also underlines the importance of study,

rehearsal and observation, insisting that ‘the actor who performs from reflection, from

study of human nature, from constant imitation of some ideal model, from imagination,

from memory, will be one and the same in all his presentations, always equally perfect’

(Diderot in Gray 2007: 250). He also recognizes the need for actor training, saying that

whomever ‘nature has designed to be an outstanding actor will excel in his art only

when long experience has been acquired, when the tumult of the passions has subsided,

when the head is calm and the spirit is under control’ (ibid.: 258). And finally, almost

conceding his point, but illustrating the importance of an actor’s work on integration

and connection to text, he states that while ‘pure, unadulterated nature alone may have

5
For this section I will be primarily working from John Gray’s 2007 translation of Diderot’s Paradoxe
sur le Comedien, included in his article ‘Diderot, Garrick, and the Art of Acting.’
32
some sublime moments,’ a truly great actor is one who ‘will have felt them, and

consequently conveyed them coolly and objectively’ (ibid.: 256).

Despite illuminating some of the complexities of the acting process, Diderot’s

paradox is often simplified to a firm conclusion which gives actors little choice – either

‘play from the heart’ resulting in ‘unequal acting’ (Diderot and Pollock 1883: 8), or

remain a ‘cool and calm spectator’ with ‘no sensibility whatever’ (Diderot in Gray: 249)

– and as a result limits the investigation of emotion to these two opposing sides. For

Diderot, it is ‘the absolute lack of sensitivity that makes for the best, truly sublime

actors’ (ibid.: 254). Perhaps unwittingly, Diderot shifted the focus of the debate about

acting to emotion, and the discussion continues today. But Diderot was not an actor

himself. Without having the experience of performing - tacit knowledge of the

embodied experience of acting – his argument is flawed. Yet his discourse has had a

major impact on how many theatre practitioners view emotion in performance.

Paradoxe Proposed

Diderot was particularly enamored with the English actor David Garrick (1717 -

1779), who was famous throughout the Western theatre world. Thomas Postlewait

maintains that Garrick ‘served to focus attention on acting as an emblem of a potential

(though not necessary) split between not only character and performer but art and life’

(Postlewait and McConachie 1989: 250). Often referred to as ‘Roscius’6 or ‘David

Shakespeare’, Garrick’s celebrity status gave him numerous opportunities to show off

his talent at private parties, and it was on these occasions that Diderot became

impressed with the English actor’s ability (Gray 2007: 244). In Paradoxe, Diderot cites

this example:

6
Famous Roman actor (126 -62 BC)

33
Garrick pokes his head between two uprights of a door, and in the
space of four to five seconds his face moves from wild ecstasy to
moderate joy, from that joy to calm, from calm to surprise, from surprise
to astonishment, from astonishment to sadness, from sadness to
dejection, from dejection to fear, from fear to horror, from horror to
despair, and ascends again from that last degree to the point where he
began his descent. Is it that he experienced in his soul all those
sensations, and managed to run that entire gamut of emotions in concert
with his face? I don’t believe so. (Diderot in Gray 2007: 262)

According to historian John Gray, Diderot was:

…convinced that [Garrick’s] propensity for acting out instantaneous


emotional changes illustrated beyond doubt his long-held view that a
great actor was one who could simulate any or every mood to order
without upsetting himself in the process. (Gray 2007: 245)

Diderot supposed that in order to do this, the actor must remain unmoved, and Paradoxe

was intended to prove just that.

But Diderot was not the only author to take his inspiration from Garrick, and in

fact, it was the anonymous publication of a pamphlet entitled Garrick or English Actors

in 1769 that prompted Diderot’s own work (Benedetti 2005: 83). The pamphlet, traced

back to Antoine Fabio Sticotti (d. 1772), was actually an embellished translation of

John Hill’s The Actor (1755), which emphasized the importance of emotionality in

performance (Gray 2007: 245;Worthen 1984). Diderot was not a fan of this pamphlet,

and in personal correspondence to his close friend Baron von Grimm, Gray reveals that

Diderot thought the author of the pamphlet was ‘a scoundrel’, but that it motivated him

to write ‘a little piece’ of his own (Gray 2007: 246).

Diderot’s Paradoxe closes with an appeal to his muse, Garrick, to confirm the

philosopher’s view:

I take you as witness, English Roscius, celebrated Garrick, you, who, by


the unanimous consent of all existing nations, are recognized as the
leading actor in their acquaintance, pay homage to the truth: haven’t you
told me, although you have strong feelings, that your acting would be
feeble if, whatever passion or character you had to project, you weren’t
able to evaluate yourself by thought, rather than feelings, even to the
stature of a Homeric ghost with whom you were trying to identify
yourself? When I objected to you that it was not according to yourself
that you were playing, confess how you replied; didn’t you admit that
34
you were keeping yourself well under control, and that you only
appeared so amazingly believable on stage because you were incessantly
showing the audience an imaginary being who was not you? (Diderot in
Gray 2007: 266).

Despite the fact that Diderot was convinced that his work was ‘totally and irrefutably

exemplified by Garrick’s acting’ (Gray 2007: 245), there is no indication that Garrick

ever formally acknowledged his contemporary’s viewpoint. Gray found evidence that

Garrick had been sent a copy of the manuscript by Jean Baptiste Antoine Suard in the

winter of 1776, who asks for Garrick to respond with notes on Diderot’s manuscript ‘as

promised’ (ibid.: 247). In a further letter from the following summer, Suard again

appeals to Garrick, pressing him to reply: ‘Send them [your observations] to me, I beg

you promptly; I am [being] urged to know your opinion on the question’ (qtd. in Gray

2007: 247). Gray found no record of any response on Garrick’s part, though he offers

what may have been Garrick’s ‘refutation of much of Diderot’s thesis’ (Gray 2007:

267). In Paradoxe, Diderot speaks very highly of the actresses La Clairon and

Dumesnil, and it turns out that Garrick was not as much of a fan of their craft as Diderot

supposed. In a personal letter to a friend, Garrick criticizes La Clairon for being too

detached from feeling:

Madm. Clairon is so conscious and certain what she can do, that she
never (I believe) had the feelings of the instant come upon her
unexpectedly.—but I pronounce that the greatest strokes of Genius, have
been unknown to the Actor himself, ‘till Circumstances and the warmth
of the Scene has sprung the Mine as it were, as much to his own
Surprize, as that of the Audience—Thus I make a great difference
between a great Genius, and a good Actor. (qtd. in Gray 2007: 267)

Garrick is also admitting here that a great actor may not know exactly how they achieve

‘the greatest strokes of Genius’. Perhaps Garrick’s reluctance to corroborate Diderot’s

assertion was one of the reasons Paradoxe was not published until 1830, forty-six years

after Diderot’s death.

While Garrick may have abstained from the discussion, Roach’s view on the

importance of Diderot’s question of emotion is justified. Since the publication of


35
Paradoxe, many actors, directors and philosophers have engaged in this dialogue,

even twisting Diderot’s essay to support their own arguments (See Roach 1993; Archer

1888; Coquelin et al. 1915; Strasberg in Diderot and Pollack 1957; and Konijn 2000).

Diderot’s conclusion sparked a debate that continues to dominate Western acting theory

and, as Roach maintains, ‘to this day many acting theorists, knowingly or

unknowingly, formulate their views in response to perspectives introduced in the

Paradoxe’ (Roach 1993: 117). Whether or not the recreation of emotion is the basis of

acting, Diderot’s Paradoxe brought emotion to the forefront of theatrical debate and

created a sharp division—anti-emotionalists vs. emotionalists.

Paradoxe Pondered

It is important to note that Diderot himself was not an actor, and there is no

evidence to suggest that he had the opportunity to experience acting in performance,

which drew criticism from his antagonists, in particular William Archer (1856 – 1924)7.

In the introductory chapter of his 1888 survey of actors called Masks or Faces?, Archer

claims that Diderot ‘founded his doctrine on slender evidence’ (Archer 1888: 2), and

indeed criticizes the philosopher for his lack of professional expertise:

A few anecdotes, of doubtful interpretation, are all that he advances in


support of it, and [Baron von] Grimm expressively tells us that for years
before he formulated his theory he had gone but rarely to the theatre.
‘Able as he was,’ a distinguished actress writes to me, ‘Diderot, both in
his Paradoxe and elsewhere, spoke without that intimate knowledge
which only actors of the highest order can possess.’ (Archer 1888: 2-3)

Archer sets out to disprove Diderot, complaining of his use of ‘false logic’ and ‘empty

paradox-mongering’ (ibid.: 4). Archer – a theatre critic and self-confessed amateur

psychologist – addresses Diderot’s question by interviewing professional actors directly

about their experiences. The responses to Archer’s questionnaire vary greatly of course;

7
For this section I will be primarily working from the 1883 publication of Diderot’s Paradox of Acting,
as this would have been Archer’s reference point.
36
but through his respondents he systematically picks apart Diderot’s argument. Archer

summarizes the points Diderot makes about ‘sensibility’ in order to unravel the paradox.

Archer describes Diderot’s use of ‘sensibility’ as:

i. A tendency to do without study and to rely on momentary


inspiration.
ii. A tendency to become incarnate in your personage, to live
in it and in it alone, to feel all its emotions and endure all
its agonies.
iii. A tendency to somnambulistic absorption in the business
of the scene, making consciousness for the moment one
and indivisible.
iv. A tendency to express your own moral nature, instead of
assuming and exhibiting the character created by the
playwright. (Archer 1888: 35)

These ideas, matched with an additional definition where Diderot refers to ‘sensibility’

as ‘that disposition which accompanies organic weakness […] delicacy of the nerves,

which inclines one to […] loss of self control, to exaggeration, to contempt, to disdain,

to obtuseness to the true, the good, and the beautiful, to injustice, to madness’ (Diderot

and Pollock 1883: 56), completely destroy the paradox for Archer. From this he

surmises that:

Sensibility, then, is a morbid habit of mind and body, which must


interfere, not with acting alone, but with all healthy art whatsoever. […]
Hysteria, surely, is a much apter name for the disease. Substituting this
term, then, we read Diderot’s thesis as follows: -‘The great actor must
not be hysterical.’ Agreed. But where is the paradox? (Archer 1888: 36)

These are just a few examples of the many sharp criticisms of Diderot’s Paradoxe that

Archer offers in Masks or Faces?

Archer declares himself an emotionalist, but it is not a term he chooses lightly.

Archer admits:

After a careful search for less cumbrous expressions, I have been forced
to fall back upon the terms ‘emotionalist’ and ‘anti-emotionalist’ to
indicate the contending parties in this dispute. They are painfully
clumsy; but the choice seemed to lie between them and still clumsier
circumlocutions. (Archer 1888: 11)

37
Archer goes on to discuss his findings and ideas about emotion in much greater detail

than Diderot’s two-character dialogue. Archer describes what he calls ‘simple’ emotions

as those which ‘tend to express themselves directly and unmistakably in changes of the

physical organs’, and include ‘grief and joy (with all their subdivisions), rage, terror,

and shame’ (Archer 1888: 37). ‘Complex’ emotions, on the other hand, do not share

these obvious outward changes, but are instead more ‘attitudes of the mind than

individual emotions’, and Archer mentions some examples such as ‘love and hatred,

jealousy and envy.’ (ibid.: 37). Archer theorizes that the complex emotions can

sometimes express themselves through the form of the simple ones; that love will

manifest itself in any of those emotions from grief to joy, and hatred sometimes through

anger. ‘Thus,’ concludes Archer, ‘the physical effects of the simple emotions may be

regarded as the raw material of expression; whence it follows that the reproduction of

these physical effects must be the very groundwork of the actors art’ (ibid.: 37-38).

Drawing upon examples from history, from the Greeks, to Cicero and Quintilian, all of

whom Archer classes as emotionalists, he enlists their support in his argument for actors

‘feeling’ emotion. However, as Roach points out, Archer seems to be confused in parts,

his ‘vague duality’ alternating ‘between working from the inside out through mental

concentration and from the outside in through physical’ (Roach 1993: 181).

In Paradoxe, Diderot concludes that ‘between him who counterfeits sensibility

and him who feels there will always be the difference between an imitation and a

reality’ (Diderot and Pollack 1883: 99). Archer’s ultimate conclusion is ‘[a]cting is

imitation; when it ceases to be imitation it ceases to be acting and becomes something

else’ [original emphasis] (Archer 1888: 196). Both are alluding to the difference

between emotions ‘onstage’ and emotions in ‘life’.

38
Both philosophers had their champions in the professional acting world. For

Diderot, his main advocate was Constant Coquelin (1841 – 1909). In his essay, Art and

the Actor, Coquelin says of Paradoxe:

I hold this paradox to be literal truth: and I am convinced that one can
only be a great actor on condition of complete self-mastery and ability to
express feelings which are not experienced, which may never be
experienced, which from the nature of things never can be experienced.
(Coquelin et al. 1915: 56)

Coquelin was a firm believer in expressing feelings without experiencing them. It

appears that to the anti-emotionalists, experiencing an emotion in performance is

inherently the same as experiencing an emotion in everyday life, and it is for this idea in

particular that Archer and others vehemently attacked Paradoxe. Coquelin used Diderot

to uphold his views on the actor’s craft as literal art, likening their work to that of a

painter, poet or sculptor. He wanted the actor’s work to be seen as artistic

representation, rather than imitation, asserting that ‘the actor needs not to be actually

moved. It is as unnecessary as it is for a pianist to be in the depths of despair to play the

Funeral March of Chopin’ (ibid.: 60-61).

Archer’s advocate was the actor Henry Irving, for whom experiencing the

emotion was a priority. In an introduction for the 1883 publication of Diderot’s

Paradoxe, for Irving it was clear that the actor ‘who combines the electric force of a

strong personality with a mastery of the resources of his art, must have a greater power

over his audiences than the passionless actor who gives a most artistic simulation of the

emotion he never experiences’ (Irving in Diderot and Pollack 1883: xvii). But the

emotionalists did not believe that an actor should become completely absorbed in their

experience of emotion, as Diderot would have us believe. They recognized the ability to

‘experience’ an emotion, as the character, without becoming hysterical, forgetting

oneself, or indeed going ‘mad’. Irving emphasizes that ‘it is necessary to this art that

the mind should have, as it were, a double consciousness, in which all the emotions

39
proper to the occasion may have full sway, while the actor is all the time on the alert

for every detail of his method’ (ibid.: xv – xvi). What Irving and the emotionalists

understood was dual consciousness, a phenomenon Diderot recognized but believed to

be mutually exclusive with experiencing emotion. Toward the end of Masks or Faces?,

Archer admits ‘the real paradox of acting…resolves itself into the paradox of dual

consciousness’ (Archer 1888: 150).

Section II: The ‘Problem’ of Emotion in Life

The Notion of Emotion

In order to address Diderot’s paradox in the twenty-first century, I will examine

different approaches to emotion in everyday life. First by asking, what is an emotion?

Philosophers have been asking this question for centuries, followed by scientists, then

psychologists and, more recently, neuroscientists. With improving technology to help

understand our functioning brains—positron emission tomography (PET scans),

functional magnetic resonance imagery (fMRI), and the like, this question of what is an

emotion must be close to being answered. A new field has emerged – affective

neuroscience – the ‘investigation of the neural basis of emotion and mood’ (Bear et al.

2001: 564) – dedicated to understanding the relationship between emotion and the brain.

Surely, there must be a consensus among the experts on what, exactly, an emotion is?

Unfortunately, a unanimous agreement on a definition of emotion has yet to be found.

Defining emotion is difficult; firstly because even today emotion scientists

disagree on a definition, and secondly because different fields of study – such as

philosophy, psychology, and neuroscience – approach the subject of emotion from very

different angles, using different research methodologies and often analyzing isolated

components of emotion. The specificity of individual studies of emotion often results in

40
contradictory information. Neuroscientist Elaine Fox, in her book Emotion Science

(2008), points out ‘there is no general agreement in emotion science on how emotion

should be defined’, nor is there an agreement on ‘basic emotions’, a term Fox

encourages us not to take ‘too literally’ (Fox 2008: 84).

Rather than present a chronological history of emotion theory, it will be clearer

to separate key theorists by the framework that they use to investigate emotion. For a

clear introduction to the current field, Fox divides the study of emotion into four distinct

perspectives that are still considered today8 - researchers who believe that: 1) emotions

are biologically given, 2) emotions are socially constructed, 3) emotions are the result of

bodily changes, and 4) emotions are the result of cognitive appraisal. I am choosing to

highlight four frameworks for investigating emotion that may be useful in this study.

Within each framework I mention the important theorists and their views of emotion,

including ideas on basic emotions and how emotions are induced.

Emotions as Biological Inheritance

The view that emotions are biologically given was first presented by Charles

Darwin in The Expression of Emotion in Man and Animals (1872). His main argument,

based on detailed observations of animal and human behavior, is that emotions are

innate (or inherited genetically) and are a key factor in evolution and the preservation of

species. Darwin asserts ‘the study of the theory of expression confirms to a certain

limited extent the conclusion that man is derived from some lower animal form, and

supports the belief of the specific or sub-specific unity of the several races,’ (Darwin et

al. 1999: 241) and that ‘expression in itself, or the language of the emotions, as it has

sometimes been called, is certainly of importance for the welfare of mankind’ (ibid.).

8
Historical approaches to emotion that are no longer valid, such as the Elizabethan theory of humours or
Descartes’ ‘animal spirits’, will not be discussed. Psychoanalytic views, such as those of Freud and
Lacan, will also not be included here as they do not address the physiology of emotion.
41
Darwin’s radical views that the entirety of the human race share common expressions

of emotion, and that we share many of those same expressions with animals as well,

were both contentious notions for Victorian society, already scandalized by his

publication of Origin of the Species (1859) and Descent of Man (1871). Certain

expressions, such as crying and blushing, he found unique to the human race,

suggesting that these were later developments on our evolutionary path. In this

biological approach, emotions are seen as ‘adaptations to significant problems,

especially problems that were common for our ancestors’ (Fox 2008: 4) and their

function allows ‘a range of different processes to be coordinated in order to solve an

immediate and urgent problem’ (ibid.). Darwin’s work has been criticized for being

mostly based on personal observation and several of his theories have turned out to be

false. But the main gist of Darwin’s argument has been supported by the more recent

research of psychologist Paul Ekman.

Ekman originally set out to refute Darwin, hypothesizing that emotions were

culturally created. However, after studying native tribes in Papua New Guinea, Ekman

came to the conclusion that Darwin was correct in his assumption that the expression of

emotions is universal. Ekman views emotion as a:

…process, a particular kind of automatic appraisal influenced by our


evolutionary and personal past, in which we sense that something
important to our welfare is occurring, and a set of physiological changes
and emotional behaviors begins to deal with the situation. (Ekman 2003:
13)

With the biological approach comes the assumption of a set of basic emotions. Again,

these can differ depending on the researcher and there is no agreement on basic

emotions among neuroscientists. Ekman identifies five: anger, fear, happiness, sadness,

and disgust. While Darwin did not produce a definitive list, he also includes tenderness

and eroticism.

42
Emotions as Social Constructs

The view that emotions are socially constructed has been popular primarily with

anthropologists and cultural theorists. Emotions are considered to be ‘the products of a

particular culture and they are produced by that culture in order to help define its values

and assist members of a society to negotiate particular social roles’ (Fox 2008: 4). In

this view, emotions are learned behaviors, unique to different societies. There is

evidence of different cultures having unique emotions particular to their societies. One

example is the Japanese emotion amae, which in Japan is considered to be a basic

emotion and ‘refers to the pleasant feeling that arises from a sense of togetherness,

especially when this emerges from complete dependence on another person’ (ibid.: 6).

A famous proponent of this view of emotions was the anthropologist Margaret Mead.

Her research in Samoa led her to believe that emotional expressions are not universal,

but unique to specific cultures. Ekman, a student of Mead’s, later found her theory to be

incorrect. Ekman found while some cultures may develop additional and unique

expressions or gestures, we all share a set of universal emotions. Ekman views this idea

that emotions are socially constructed as an outdated framework, not because it lacks

validity, but because this assumption can work with the idea that emotions are

biologically given. In the first framework presented, the idea is that emotions are

adaptations that help the organism survive. Adapting emotions to suit cultural situations

is crucial to help organisms survive in their socially constructed world. After a

symposium on ‘Feelings and Emotions’ held in Amsterdam 2004, emotion theorist Nico

Frijda and his associates Anthony S.R. Manstead and Agneta Fischer concluded:

In recent theorizing, the old controversy between a biological and a


cultural view is being abandoned and replaced by the idea that emotions
are both biologically and culturally determined. The debate is shifting
from the question of whether emotions are biological or cultural to that
of the extent which, and the ways in which, biological givens and
cultural influences determine emotions. (Manstead et al. 2004: 465)

43
Culture, however, has a major impact upon gender association with emotion and what

Ekman calls ‘display rules’. Display rules are ‘culture-specific prescriptions about who

can show which emotions, to whom, and when’ (Ekman 1993: 384). Different display

rules may ‘explain how cultural differences may conceal universals in expression’

(ibid.).

Emotion as Perceptions of Bodily Changes

American psychologist William James (1842 – 1910) first introduced the view

of emotions as perception of bodily changes in what became known as the James-Lange

Theory of emotion (Carl Lange, a Danish psychologist, independently posed the same

theory) (Solomon 2003: 65). In his aptly titled essay “What is an Emotion?” [1884],

James states:

My thesis…is that the bodily changes follow directly the PERCEPTION


of the exciting fact, and that our feeling of the same changes as they
occur IS the emotion. Common sense says, we lose our fortune, are sorry
and weep; we meet a bear, are frightened and run; we are insulted by a
rival, are angry and strike. The hypothesis here to be defended says that
this order of sequence is incorrect, that the one mental state is not
immediately induced by the other, that the bodily manifestations must
first be interposed between, and that the more rational statement is that
we feel sorry because we cry, angry because we strike, afraid because we
tremble. [original emphasis] (James in Lange et al. 1922: 13)

Contrary to previous emotion theories, for both James and Lange the physical

manifestations are the cause of emotion; or at least, that is how their theory is largely

interpreted. The James-Lange theory was continually attacked even up until the 1960s,

by scientists such as Walter Cannon (1871 – 1945) in 1927 (See Cannon 1987) and

Stanley Schachter (1922 – 1997) and Jerome Singer (See Schachter and Singer 1962).

Most of the complaints were that ‘the theory neglects the cognitive, behavioral, and

other, more sophisticated aspects of emotion and fails to account for the many subtle

distinctions between similar emotions’ (Solomon 2003: 66). This may be true to a

certain extent – James did not elaborate on his theory further and was widely criticized

44
for his example of the ‘bear in the woods.’ Psychologist Phoebe Ellsworth defends

James, maintaining that in his example of the bear ‘we find the roots of the

oversimplified notion that emotion is nothing but physiological arousal, when all James

meant to say was that bodily feedback was a necessary condition for emotion’ [my

emphasis] (Ellsworth 1994: 228). James states this point more clearly later in his essay

when he writes: ‘emotion disassociated from all bodily feelings is inconceivable’

(James in Lange et al. 1922: 18). Though James does not produce a definitive list of

emotions, he does site a few examples of ‘standard’ emotions, which include surprise,

curiosity, rapture, fear, anger, lust, and greed. He deems these as the names of ‘the

mental states with which a person is possessed’ (ibid.: 12). James continues:

The bodily disturbances are said to be the “manifestation” of these


several emotions, their “expression” or “natural language”; and these
emotions themselves, being so strongly characterized both from within
and without, may be called the “standard emotions”. (ibid.: 12-13)

James’s work has been an important influence on many theatre practitioners

including Stanislavsky, and more recently, British theatre director Katie

Mitchell.

Neuroscientist Antonio Damasio is also considered to share this viewpoint and

is often discussed alongside James. However, it should be noted that Damasio is also

firmly in the first camp that asserts that emotions are biologically given. For Damasio,

the biological function of emotion is twofold: the first function being ‘the production of

a specific reaction to the inducing situation’ (Damasio 1999: 53), and the second

function being ‘the regulation of the internal state of the organism such that it can be

prepared for a specific reaction’ (ibid.: 54). Damasio’s somatic-marker theory is an

extension of James’ hypothesis, but further developed. According to Damasio ‘the

process does not stop with the bodily changes that define an emotion’ (ibid.: 132).

Better articulating what Ellsworth believes James was trying to convey, Damasio

explains that ‘the cycle continues, certainly in humans, and its next step is the feeling of
45
the emotion in connection to the object that excited it, the realization of the nexus

between object and emotional body state’ [original emphasis] (ibid.). Damasio divides

emotions into three categories: background emotions, primary emotions, and social

emotions.

Emotion as Result of Cognitive Appraisal

Aristotle also acknowledged the role of emotion in the body. Aristotle,

developed a theory of emotion, as well as a set of principles for rhetoric. Dr. Robert C.

Solomon (1942 – 2007), a leading figure for the International Society for Research on

Emotions (ISRE), finds that Aristotle’s ideas are widely ignored by emotion theorists,

despite his untimely concordance with many contemporary views on the subject

(Solomon 2003: 5)9. In Rhetoric, Aristotle defines emotion as ‘that which leads one’s

condition to become so transformed that his judgment is affected, and which is

accompanied by pleasure and pain’ (Aristotle in Solomon 2003: 6). Here we see early

beginnings of a recurring theme where emotion is pitted against logic, a theme that is

still common in Western society today. ‘Judgment’ is a quality to be valued, and

‘emotion’ a potential impairment. Aristotle also associates the experience of emotion as

either pleasurable, painful, or indeed both (Benedetti 2005: 11). In On the Soul,

Aristotle observes that ‘most of the soul’s conditions—anger, courage, desire, and any

sensation—neither act nor are activated without the body’ (Aristotle in Solomon 2003:

9). It is very important to note that Aristotle acknowledges the importance of the body’s

participation in the experience of emotion, as this idea will be snubbed by later

theorists. He also admits, however, that ‘the act of thinking probably belongs to the

soul alone’ (ibid.). For Aristotle, there are three conditions on the dependence of which

emotion will appear: 1) ‘the individual state of mind’; 2) ‘the object of his feeling’; and

9
Solomon’s book What is an Emotion? includes recent translations from Rhetoric and On the Soul, from
which I will be quoting.
46
3) ‘the circumstances in which the feeling arises’ and further to these, ‘without a

knowledge of all three, it is impossible to arouse emotion’ (Benedetti 2005: 11).

Aristotle identifies ten emotions, some individual and some connected, including anger,

calm, friendship and enmity, fear and confidence, shame, favor, pity, indignation, envy,

and jealousy (ibid.: 10).

Distinguishing Between Emotions, Moods, and Feelings

A tool that may be helpful in the discussion of emotion is to distinguish between

emotions, moods and feelings. While there is no agreement on a definition of emotion,

most theorists generally consider emotions to be ‘discrete and consistent responses to an

internal or external event that has a particular significance for the organism’ [original

emphasis] (Fox 2008: 16). A ‘feeling’ can be broken down into two particular

categories. Bennett and Hacker, in The History of Cognitive Neuroscience, describe

‘feelings’ as a number of phenomena including ‘sensations, tactile perceptions,

appetites and affections’ (Bennett and Hacker 2008: 164). More often, however,

emotion scientists refer to ‘feeling’ as ‘the subjective representations of emotions’ (Fox

2008: 17). Damasio explains ‘the term feeling should be reserved for the private, mental

experience of an emotion, while the term emotion should be used to designate the

collection of responses, many of which are publicly observable’ (Damasio 1999: 42).

The Mind/Body Split

Another major challenge is an ingrained idea in most of Western society that the

mind and body are separate. In Passions of the Soul (1649), Rene Descartes proposes

his influential assertion that: ‘the mind by which I am what I am, is wholly distinct from

the body, and is even more easily known than the latter, and is such, that although the

latter were not, it would still continue to be all that it is’ (Descartes: location 421).

Although this paradigm is slowly shifting, our Cartesian inheritance is still profound,

47
embedded even in our language. Lakoff and Johnson’s book Philosophy in the Flesh:

The Embodied Mind and its Challenge to Western Thought (1999) offers a very detailed

and convincing argument using cognitive science to address and refute the philosophies

that have shaped our society. One of the challenges today is to re-embody the

disembodied mind:

The embodied mind is part of the living body and is dependent on the
body for its existence. The properties of the mind are not purely mental:
They are shaped in crucial ways by the body and brain and how the body
can function in everyday life. (Lakoff and Johnson 1999: 565)

This view challenges Descartes’ claim that the mind and body are separate. In

Descartes’ Error (1994) Damasio states:

Surprising as it may sound, the mind exists in and for an integrated


organism; our minds would not be the way they are if it were not for the
interplay of body and brain during evolution, during individual
development, and the current moment. The mind had to be first about the
body, or it could not have been. (Damasio 1994: xx)

Also, because of our inherited philosophical views, emotion and reason are often pitted

against each other. This view is not only incorrect, but research has shown that emotion

is a required component of decision-making (See Damasio 1994). Again here,

Descartes’ division of mind and body has had an impact. Lakoff and Johnson remark

that ‘these beliefs, in the popular imagination, have led to the dissociation of reason

from emotion and thus to the downplaying of emotional and aesthetic life in our culture’

(Lakoff and Johnson 1999: 401).

Towards a Holistic View of the Mind, Body and Emotions

Neuroscientist Candace B. Pert argues for a biochemical basis for emotion.

Pert’s research discovered small chemical reactions that occur when chains of amino

acids (called neuropeptides) bind with receptors on the surface of cells. These

neuropeptides may actually be a chemical substrate of emotion. Neuropeptides were

originally given the prefix ‘neuro’ because they were first discovered in high

48
concentration in the limbic system of the brain (the traditional ‘seat of emotions’).

Subsequently, however, neuropeptides have now been found throughout the entire

body, including a heavy concentration on the lining of the intestines, which may give

more weight to the expression to ‘gut-feeling’. Studies have even shown that

‘excitement and anger increase gut motility, while contentment decreases it’ (Pert 1997:

188). Since the discovery of neuropeptides outside of the limbic system, Pert believes

that the mind cannot be constricted to the brain and that, in fact, the mind is merely an

exchange of information occurring all throughout the body. Pert uses her research to

argue for a holistic view of mind and body, especially in relation to emotion.

Philosopher Robert Solomon also recognizes the need for a holistic view of

body and mind when discussing emotion. Solomon believes ‘the enormous range of

emotions suggests that no single claim or analysis will suit all emotions, or emotions as

such’ [original emphasis](Solomon 2004: 13). Solomon maintains that every emotion

has five aspects:

1) behavioral (including everything from facial expressions and verbal


expressions (“Damn!”) and reports (“I love you”) to elaborate plans for
action);
2) physiological (hormonal, neurological, neuromuscular);
3) phenomenological (everything from “physical” sensations to ways of
seeing and describing the “objects” of one’s emotions and “meta-
emotions”);
4) cognitive (including appraisals, perceptions, thoughts, and reflections
about one’s emotions); and
5) the social context (from the immediacy of interpersonal interactions to
pervasive cultural considerations).
(ibid.)

This is a more holistic view of emotion than some of the other emotion scientists.

Solomon recognizes that these five aspects are ‘often interwoven (e.g., behavioral and

physiological, phenomenological, cognitive, and cultural), and they should not be

construed as competing conceptions of emotion’ (ibid.). The various views on emotion

presented by emotion theorists in this section may be useful to considering the views of

theatre practitioners on emotion in performance.


49
Section III: Key Theories on Acting and Emotion in the 19th and 20th Centuries

What is Acting?

Describing exactly what acting is, or what an actor does, in concrete terms, is

very difficult. An inherent conflict exists between the embodied experience of acting

and the translation of that experience into words. The tacit knowledge that an actor has

of their process cannot be easily transformed into written or verbal knowledge. This

contributes to the propagated ‘myth’ of acting, especially among the public. Acting

theorist Richard Hornby describes acting as ‘the least understood of the arts’ (Hornby

1983: 19). He elaborates on this by pointing out:

[A]lthough an educated layman is likely to have a fairly accurate idea of how a


painter paints, a novelist writes, or a musician rehearses, he will probably not
have the slightest idea of how a trained actor prepares a role. (ibid.)

Even within the theatre industry debate rages about how an actor should work on a role.

Much of this debate develops out of misunderstandings or different views on what an

actor actually does. Historically, actors have been considered to be ‘dissemblers’, and

words to describe what an actor does in performance have included ‘pretend’ and

‘represent’. Bruce McConachie questions the difference between Western performance

and reality: ‘[n]o one would call a sports event an “illusion,” but theatre artists and the

theatergoing public sometimes refer to events as illusory and even unreal’ (McConachie

2007: 566). He notes that while ‘the actors and machinery of the production may be real

enough, […] the fictional world onstage somehow trumps the fact of material actors

doing real things with other people and objects’ (ibid.). Finding words to accurately

describe the actor’s process is difficult, and when translating from tacit knowledge to a

verbal form, there is a lacuna, or gap. Given McConachie’s observation and the

previous discussion of the problems that come with Cartesian dualism, the view of

acting that this research will use is that of director/practitioner Phillip Zarrilli.

According to Zarrilli:

50
…acting should not be viewed as embodying a representation of a role
or character, but rather as a dynamic, lived experience in which the actor
is responsive to the demands of the particular moment within a specific
(theatrical) environment. (Zarrilli 2007: 638)

Acting Emotions

An important factor when considering emotion in performance after a discussion

of emotion science is highlighted in Rhonda Blair’s The Actor, Image, and Action:

Acting and Cognitive Neuroscience (2008). Blair points out that ‘“[e]motion” for

Stanislavsky is what Damasio, Bloch, and others would call “feeling” within the

contemporary neurocognitive paradigm’ (Blair 2008: 48). Dutch psychologist Elly

Konijn, in her book, Acting Emotions: Shaping Emotions on Stage (2000) classifies

three predominant viewpoints of acting and emotion that have influenced Western

theatre practice. Konijn classifies the three acting styles as 1) the style of involvement,

2) style of detachment, and 3) the style of self-expression (Konijn 2000: 36). As with

the frameworks for emotion science that Fox pointed out, these classifications are useful

for this study.

The Style of Involvement

In Konijn’s first classification, the style of involvement, ‘the emotions portrayed

on stage must seem as ‘real’ as possible’ (ibid.). Practitioners that Konijn includes in

this style are Konstantin Stanislavsky (1863-1938) and Lee Strasberg (1901-1982). In

Konijn’s view, in this style ‘the emphasis is seemingly placed on the private emotions

of the actor, but actually the emphasis is placed on the character-emotions. The actor’s

private emotions are in the service of the character’ (ibid.: 38). Konijn continues:

The starting point for the emotions in the performance are those of the
character, as presented in the dramatic text. The private emotions of the
actor are used to shape the inner model, forming a basis for the character
in performance. Shaping the inner model must meet the criteria of
‘truthfulness’, emotions must be recognizable as they appear in daily life.
Therefore the actor fills the model with his own emotional memories and
experiences. Moreover, the style of involvement presumes that private
emotions will be relived time and again in performance. (ibid.)
51
Stanislavsky pioneered the development of the rehearsal process and provided a

framework for training actors that has particularly influenced most, if not all, acting

theories in Western society. To improve his actor training system, Stanislavsky pulled

from the work of psychologists Théodule-Augustin Ribot, William James, and even

explored Yoga. Stanislavsky discusses emotion frequently, though never defines it, and

often uses the words ‘emotion’ and ‘feeling’ interchangeably. However, in An Actor’s

Work, Stanislavsky does make a distinction between what he describes as ‘actor’s

emotion’ and ‘genuine emotion, or genuine, artistic experiencing’ (Stanislavsky and

Benedetti 2008a: 31). In his view, ‘actor’s emotion’ is ‘an artificial stimulation of the

periphery of the body’ (ibid.), a staple of Coquelin’s School of Representation that

Stanislavsky detested. It is in this section that Stanislavsky introduces his idea of ‘I am

being’:

That is what we call in our terminology, the state of “I am being”. Its


secret is the fact that the orderly logic of physical actions and feelings led
you to the truth, the truth evoked belief and together they created “I am
being”. And what is “I am being”? That means, I am, I live, I feel, I think
as one with the role. (ibid.: 186)

The importance of this idea for Stanislavsky, is that ‘“I am being” leads to emotion, to

feeling, to experiencing’ (ibid.). Stanislavsky’s system arose from essentially pioneering

the development of rehearsal processes. In his view:

If emotion immediately responds to the call, that is an enormous piece of


luck. Then everything falls into place spontaneously, in a natural way. A
representation surfaces, an appraisal on it is formed, and the two together
activate the will. In other words, all the psychological inner drives start
to work because of feeling (ibid.: 279-280)

This description implies a ‘psychophysical’ connection, and throughout Stanislavsky’s

system he uses different techniques to achieve this integration. However, many

American acting teachers discuss techniques that are either ‘inside out’ or ‘outside in’.

This binary, however, does not fully account for the dynamics of psychophysical

processes. Stanislavsky was continually revising his system throughout his lifetime.
52
Problematic English translations10, as well as the subtle differences in meanings of

words across languages, have led to huge misunderstandings and misinterpretations of

the Stanislavsky system.

One technique that Stanislavsky experimented with was called ‘affective

memory’, a term he acquired from psychologist Théodule-Augustin Ribot (1893-1891),

in his study The Psychology of the Emotions (1897). In a chapter entitled ‘The Memory

of Feelings’, Ribot maintains ‘the impressions of smell and taste, our visceral

sensations, our pleasant and painful states, our emotions and passions, like the

perceptions of sight and hearing, can leave memories behind them’ (Ribot 1911: 141).

His research was questioning whether or not these perceptions, images, and sensations

‘of emotions formally experienced, [can] be revived in the consciousness

spontaneously, or at will, independently of any actual occurrence which might provoke

them?’ (ibid.). Ribot asked participants to recall a specific case in which they

experienced a specific emotion (such as anger or fear) and categorized the results into

three groups: intellectual memory, affective memory, and objective memory. Those

who could ‘recall the circumstances plus the revived condition of feeling’ experienced

‘true “affective memory”’ [original emphasis] (ibid.: 153). It is this technique that

Konijn is referring to, when she discusses Stanislavsky’s use of an actor’s ‘private

emotions’.

One of the most prominent misinterpretations of Stanislavsky’s system was the

appropriation of his name for Lee Strasberg’s Method. Strasberg never met

Stanislavsky, but was introduced to the system through the teachings of Richard

Boleslavsky. Strasberg preferred the term emotional memory to affective memory, but

used both. One of Strasberg’s students, Edward Dwight Easty, in his book On Method

10
For a thorough account of the problems with the translations of Stanislavsky’s work, see Carnicke’s
Stanislavsky in Focus (1998), Chapter 4: The Publication Maze.
53
Acting (1966), considers ‘[b]eing able to express emotion with verisimilitude is for

most actors the crux of their analysis and work on a role’ (Easty 1966: 51). Easty

defines affective memory as ‘the conscious creation of remembered emotions which

have occurred in the actor’s own past life and their application to the character being

portrayed on the stage’ [original emphasis] (ibid.: 52). According to Easty, in Method

Acting:

The actor has to use his own feelings, not somebody else’s; he has to use
his body, not another body. The most and the best that he can do is to
train his emotions to respond accordingly with the character’s emotions.
[original emphasis] (ibid.: 53)

Daniel Meyer-Dinkgräfe, author of Approaches to Acting: Past and Present (2006)

points out that in these cases:

The spectator is confronted with an emotion that outwardly might fit the
situation of a specific scene in the play. However, what the spectator is
not aware of is that the emotions do not originate in the sequence of
causal conditions in the play, but from potentially ‘arbitrary’,
unsequenced, unrelated, individual substitutes.
(Meyer-Dinkgräfe 2001: 50)

The Style of Detachment

In Konijn’s view, the style of detachment ‘rejects the principle of identification

of the actor with the character during performance’ (Konijn 2000: 39). Theatre

practitioners that Konijn puts in this category include Bertolt Brecht (1898-1956) and

Vsevolod Meyerhold (1874-1940). In this style, ‘the emotions of the characters are

‘shown’ or ‘demonstrated’ in a reproducible form, referring to emotions as they occur in

reality, but not identical to them’ (ibid.). A question that arises from this statement,

especially in relation to the style of involvement, is this: are these emotions considered

to be ‘shown’ or ‘demonstrated’ because they are not psychologically experienced by

the actor? Or rather, because they are not the actor’s ‘private emotions’? Konijn

continues:

54
Brecht’s emphatic rejection of the overlap of actor’s emotions with
character-emotions in performance was a reaction against the central role
that emotions play in Stanislavsky’s style of involvement. Brecht finds
Stanislavsky naïve in this respect. With Brecht, actors not only present
characters, but also explicitly present ‘themselves’, their actual beings on
stage and have opinions about the characters. By letting go of the
demand for identification or involvement and by rejecting the effort to
create the illusion of reality on the stage, the style of detachment is
clearly parallel to Diderot’s standpoint on acting. (ibid.: 39)

Brecht is explicit in his rejection of this type of acting/emotional involvement:

For the actor it is difficult and taxing to conjure up particular inner


moods or emotions night after night; it is simpler to exhibit the outer
signs which accompany these emotions and identify them. In this case,
however, there is not the same automatic transfer of emotions to the
spectator, the same emotional infection. The alienation effect intervenes,
not in the form of absence of emotion, but in the form of emotions which
need not correspond to those of the character portrayed. On seeing worry
the spectator may feel a sensation of joy; on seeing anger, one of disgust.
When we speak of exhibiting outer signs of emotion we do not mean
such an exhibition and such a choice of signs that the emotional
transference does in fact take place because the actor has managed to
infect himself with the emotions portrayed, by exhibiting the outer signs;
thus, by letting his voice rise, holding his breath and tightening his neck
muscles so that the blood shoots to his head, the actor can easily conjure
up a rage. In such a case of course the effect does not occur. (Brecht
1964: 94)

What Brecht is describing here may in fact be a different way of accessing emotion, or

rather, accessing emotion that is not the actor’s ‘own, private’ emotion. In an essay

entitled ‘The Alienation Effect’ Brecht calls for emotion to be expressed through

gesture:

[F]eeling, when it is called for, should be brought out; that is, it should
become gesture. The actor must find a sensuous outward expression for
the emotions of his role—an action, wherever possible, which reveals
what is going on inside. The emotion concerned must come out, must be
set free, so that it can be given shape and greatness. [original emphasis]
(Brecht 1974: 310)

From this description, it can be inferred that Konijn considers the involvement style of

using ‘private’ emotions, as emotions that are only experienced in the mind? Konijn

also includes Meyerhold in this category, once a member of the Moscow Art Theatre,

55
who left and created his own acting technique called Biomechanics. In many ways

Meyerhold is in agreement with Brecht:

The essence of human relationships is determined by gestures, poses,


glances and silences. Words alone cannot say everything. Hence there
must be a pattern of movement on the stage to transform the spectator
into a vigilant observer, to furnish him with that material which the two
people in conversation yielded to the third, the material which helps him
grasp the true feelings of the characters. (Meyerhold and Braun 1998:
56)

When discussing the acting methods that proceeded Biomechanics (Stanislavsky’s

included), Meyerhold states:

[T]he actor has always been so overwhelmed by his emotions that he has
been unable to answer either for his movements or his voice. He has had
no control over himself and hence been in no state to ensure success or
failure. Only a few exceptionally great actors have succeeded
instinctively in finding the correct method, that is, the method of
building a role not from inside outwards, but vice versa. By approaching
their role from the outside, they succeeded in developing stupendous
technical mastery. (ibid.)

Here Meyerhold very clearly specifies an ‘outward in’ approach, and promotes

‘technical mastery’. A benefit to this approach, in Konijn’s view, is that

‘[r]epeatability is guaranteed in the style of detachment by placing emphasis on

technical mastery over the portrayal of emotions, situations, and motives’ (Konijn 2000:

39).

The Style of Self-Expression

The third classification is the style of self-expression, in which ‘the expression

of the actor’s own authentic emotions is key’ (Konijn 2000: 41) Konijn includes

practitioners such as Jerzy Grotowski (1933-1999), Eugenio Barba (b. 1936) and Peter

Brook (b. 1925). In the category style of self-expression, Konijn explains:

The emotions the characters portray in performance are the emotions of


the actors themselves and must be as spontaneous and true as possible.
Improvisation is of paramount importance in these presentations – not as
a studio/rehearsal technique as with Stanislavsky and Strasberg – but as a
component of live performance with an audience. (ibid.)

56
Grotowski rarely spoke about emotion directly. However, in a recent publication of

‘A Reply to Stanislavsky’ (2008), Grotowski more openly discusses emotion in relation

to Stanislavsky. In Grotowski’s view, Stanislavsky was misled in his search for

emotion:

[Stanislavsky] searched for that famous “emotional recall.” He still


thought that a return to memories of various emotions meant basically
the possibility of returning to the emotions themselves. He was mistaken
here, in believing that emotions are subject to the will. But in life one can
verify that emotions are independent from the will. (Grotowski and
Salata 2008: 33)

However, Grotowski hails Stanislavsky’s later work, specifically the Method of

Physical Actions, as a ‘revelation’, maintaining ‘[e]motions are independent from our

will, and exactly because of this Stanislavsky in his last period of activity preferred to

put the accent in the work on what is subject to our will’ [my emphasis] (ibid.: 33).

Problems with this Model/Towards a Psychophysical Approach

Konijn’s model is consistent with commonly held views of approaches to

emotion in performance, but is also limiting. Similar to some of the problems related to

emotion science, dividing approaches to acting in these three categories can be

problematic, and in many ways this classification system is blending acting processes

with aesthetics. In today’s industry a contemporary actor may be performing with a

company such as Teatr Piesn Kozla, and also performing in Mother Courage at the

National, or in a play by Chekhov at the Donmar in the same year. What these

categories also do not necessarily help us understand is how the emotion is activated.

The style of involvement emphasizes emotional recall, but Stanislavsky’s system was

not limited to this technique. In which category would Konijn place Stanislavsky’s later

techniques of Method of Physical Actions/Active Analysis?11 However, this breakdown

11
For a detailed account of the differences between these techniques see Carnicke’s ‘Stanislavsky and
Politics’ 2010.
57
does accurately reflect how acting emotions are still commonly viewed. As Soto-

Morettini points out, ‘[w]e play ‘out of ourselves’ or we play ‘character’. And these two

‘traditions’ have inspired much debate among theorists’ (Soto-Morettini 2010: 45). The

way in which Konijn sets out her categories does not allow for proper consideration of

psychophysical processes. In which category, for example, would Michael Chekhov’s

work be included? Chekhov maintains that the actor ‘must consider his body as an

instrument for expressing creative ideas on the stage, must strive for the attainment of

complete harmony between […] body and psychology’ (Chekhov 2002: 1). This idea of

a psychophysical connection does not easily fit into Konijn’s model.

The Psychophysical Process

Ribot’s impact on Stanislavsky is profound despite that fact that Ribot is a

highly neglected emotion theorist in Western culture. A contemporary of James, his

1897 publication The Psychology of the Emotions builds on the theories of scientists

(predominantly James and Lange but also others such as Darwin), adding his own

research to further explore the nature of emotion. Roach admits that Ribot’s ‘service as

a disseminator of the James-Lange theory has been overlooked’ (Roach 1993: 192).

With regard to emotion, Ribot’s view is thus: ‘there would be a great advantage in

eliminating from the question every notion of cause and effect, every relation of

causality, and in substituting for the dualistic position a unitary or monastic one’ (Ribot

1911: 112). For Ribot, the occurrence of emotion cannot be observed as a linear

sequence of events, rather it is a simultaneous process happening in the body and mind

as a whole. Subsequently, Ribot was one of the first psychologists to use the term

‘psychophysiological’ (which Stanislavsky would later apply to the actor’s process as

‘psychophysical’). He goes on:

No state of consciousness can be dissociated from its physical


conditions: they constitute a natural whole, which must be studied as
such. Every kind of emotion ought to be considered in this way: all that
58
is objectively expressed subjectively by correlative states of
consciousness, classed by external observation according to their
qualities. (ibid.)

Ribot also recognized the problem of emotion as it is described through language. He

preferred the use of the term ‘emotion’ over ‘passion’ because ‘it has the advantage of

emphasising the motor element included in every emotion’ (ibid.: 92), but warns that

the term is only ‘a generic expression’ used to ‘designate the chief manifestations of the

affective life’ (ibid.: 20). Relating to his assertion of the unity between states of

consciousness and the physical, he reminds us that they exist only ‘in connection with

each other and being inseparable except as abstract concepts’ [my emphasis] (ibid:.

112). It is language that severs the connection. Ribot offers a list of ‘primitive’ emotions

that he defines as those which are ‘not reducible to other emotions’ (ibid.: 13) including

fear, anger, affection, the self-feeling or egoistic emotion, and sexual emotion. He does

not include joy or grief (ibid.: 15).

It is not entirely clear why Ribot’s theories are so little known or referred to,

though Sharon M. Carnicke, a Stanislavsky expert, suggests that in countries such as the

United States, the popularity of Freudian psychology may have overshadowed Ribot’s

theories (Carnicke 1998: 131). In Russia, however, the opposite occurred, particularly

due to the fact that, in contrast to Freud, ‘Ribot’s espousal of the physical

methodologies made him acceptable in post-revolutionary Russia […] where Marxist

materialism became the approved philosophical standard’ (ibid.). All of Ribot’s major

works were translated into Russian, and Stanislavsky ‘owned six of them replete with

marginal notes’ (ibid.: 132). The original English translations of Stanislavsky’s books

by Elizabeth Hapgood include no mention of Ribot, though his name does appear in the

German translations (ibid.: 72).

Stanislavsky was most influenced by Ribot in two particular areas: ‘affective

memory’ which was mentioned previously and the notion of the ‘psychophysical’. The

59
first area is the idea that emotion is ‘a monistic phenomenon, a total psychophysical

event with no causal relationship between mind and body’ (ibid.: 139). Stanislavsky

developed his system in order for the actor to ‘work’ on themselves and develop a

‘psychophysical’ relationship between body and mind, asserting that ‘in every action

there is something psychological, and in the psychological, something physical’

(Stanislavsky in Carnicke 1998: 139). Unfortunately, as with my experience of actor

training in the United States, the wider public is mostly unaware of Stanislavsky’s view

of unity between body and mind. Meyer- Dinkgräfe points out:

Ultimately, for the older Stanislavsky, mind (psychotechnique) and body


(method of physical actions) work together as a unified whole. However,
his followers focused on one or the other, either preferring to work with
the mind or with the body. It is only late in the twentieth century that the
notion of bodymind began to take hold in approaches to acting, mainly
influenced by non-Western acting paradigms. (Meyer- Dinkgräfe 2006:
47)

Meyer- Dinkgräfe is incorrect to presume that all Stanislavsky’s followers focused on

mind or body rather than a unified whole – students such as Michael Chekhov,

Meyerhold, and Maria Knebel – were all working with the understanding of the

psychophysical relationship. However, Meyer- Dinkgräfe’s observation is an accurate

description of many of the ‘Stanislavsky’ teachers I encountered in the United States. I

generally agree with the observation that ‘non-Western acting paradigms’ have

influenced ideas of the unified mind and body in acting in the late twentieth-century (as

far as the theatre industry), however, many years prior these ‘non-Western acting

paradigms’ were influences on practitioners such as Stanislavsky, Meyerhold, Brecht

and Grotowski, as well as the American Delsartean, Genevieve Stebbins.

Françoise Delsarte and the ‘Psychophysical’

The term ‘psychophysical’, as used in relation to the actor’s process, is often

credited to Stanislavsky. Although he developed the concept independently through his

study of Ribot, he was not the first Western theatre practitioner to coin the phrase or the
60
first to use its philosophy. The term initially appeared in print in 1892, in a book

called Dynamic Breathing and Harmonic Gymnastics: A Complete System of Psychical,

Aesthetic and Physical Culture by Genevieve Stebbins, a prominent American

Delsartean and actress (Ruyter 1999: 62). Stebbins defines ‘psycho-physical culture’ as:

A completely rounded system for the development of body, brain and


soul; a system of training which shall bring this grand trilogy of the
human microcosm into one continuous, interacting unison, so that
nothing shall be useless, nothing thoughtless, and consequently, nothing
that is vital is wasted. (Stebbins 1892: 57)

Stebbins’ training and philosophy was based on the teachings of the French actor and

singer, Françoise Delsarte (1811-1871). Dissatisfied with actor training in Paris,

Delsarte searched for concrete principles from nature that could be applied to acting

technique (Delsarte 1892: 391), aiming to ‘discover and then teach what he came to

believe were the scientific principles of expression in the arts’ (Ruyter 1999: 8). Rather

than using the word ‘emotion’, Delsarte uses ‘gesture’ instead, defining it as ‘the direct

agent of the heart […] the fit manifestation of feeling’ (Delsarte 1892: 466). Delsarte’s

Law of Correspondence actually anticipated Ribot’s discussion of the psychophysical,

stating that the ‘position or movement of the body as a whole, or its parts, is an integral,

two-way connection with states of mind, feelings, and intentions’ (Ruyter 1999: 77).

Today, Delsarte is a name known primarily to mimes and dancers. Little of his system

remains in a clear or understandable form.

Section IV: Diderot’s Paradoxe and the Practitioners of the Twentieth Century

Having kept an annotated copy of Diderot’s Paradoxe in Russian, Stanislavsky

was familiar with Diderot’s investigation and believed that the philosopher was

genuinely misunderstood:

61
[Diderot] says that you cannot experience the same feelings as you do
in life, he says that… you can live with actual born again feeling, he says
what we say, that you can live with affected feelings. Diderot was not
understood, in the same way that Tolstoy’s wonderful book is not
understood. (Stanislavsky in Whyman 2008: 46)

Despite his appreciation for Diderot, however, Stanislavsky was not an advocate of

Coquelin and his school of representational acting. In An Actor’s Work, Torstov,

Stanislavsky’s fictional alter ego, offers scathing criticism, disregarding their ideology

as that which creates ‘theatrically beautiful, or picture-postcard feelings,’ but ultimately,

‘when it comes to the expression of deep passions, it is either too showy or too

superficial’ (Stanislavski and Benedetti 2008: 26). In Coquelin’s school of

representation, the actor strives to create emotions that belong to a higher art form,

rejecting a naturalistic interpretation of emotions that one would experience in life.

Coquelin remarked that ‘emotion sobs and stammers, alters and breaks the voice,’

(Coquelin et al. 1915: 61) complaining that the actor ‘would cease to be audible. The

natural effect of passion is to destroy all self government; we lose our heads, and how

can we be expected to do well rather than ill, when we cease to know what we are

doing?’ (ibid.). While Coquelin advocated a technique that would eliminate the

necessity of experiencing emotions, Stanislavsky spent his career searching for

technique to support the actor’s exploration of emotion, not replace it. In response to

Coquelin’s theory, Stanislavsky insists ‘the subtlety and depth of human feelings will

not yield to mere technique’ (Stanislavski 2008: 26). Rose Whyman, a Stanislavsky

scholar with access to his office in Moscow, found that Stanislavsky’s notated copy of

Paradoxe ‘indicates that he continued to work with it after the revolution’ (Whyman

2008: 46).

Strasberg also had strong opinions on Diderot. In an introduction to a reprinting

of both Diderot’s Paradox of Acting and Archer’s Masks or Faces?, Strasberg describes

Diderot’s paradox as thus: ‘to move the audience the actor must himself remain

62
unmoved’ (Strasberg in Diderot and Pollock 1957), simplifying the duologue to a

statement that opposes the aims of the American Method. Like many before him,

Strasberg uses Diderot’s writings in such a way as to support his own views. Evoking

Diderot’s earlier writings and comments, Strasberg claims that at one time the

philosopher had held an emotionalist point of view to such a degree that even some of

his students ‘have doubted that the Paradox is actually his’ (ibid.). Strasberg cites a

personal letter, written by Diderot to the actress Mlle. Jodin, in which he insists:

An actor who has only sense and judgment is cold; one who has only
verve and sensibility is crazy. It is a particular combination of good
sense and warmth which creates the sublime person; and the stage as in
life he who shows more than he feels makes one laugh instead of
affecting one. Therefore never try to go beyond the feeling that you have;
try to find the true point. (qtd. in Diderot and Pollack, 1957: xi).

Strasberg, like Stanislavsky, also implies that Diderot was misunderstood, that Diderot

‘obviously’ knew more than his followers who took Paradoxe literally. Strasberg even

goes so far as to suggest that perhaps Diderot’s attack on emotional actors was a result

of his unsuccessful career as a playwright: ‘[Diderot’s] caustic remarks on the character

of his contemporary actors certainly indicate disappointment and bitterness’ (ibid.: xi).

But after consulting Felix Vexler’s Studies in Diderot’s Esthetic Naturalism, Strasberg

changes his perspective on Diderot midway through his introduction, insisting that

Paradoxe is actually Diderot’s ‘challenge to the actor to recognize the high nature of his

art, a plea that the discipline and control the flow of his imagination and feeling’ (ibid.

xii) and for the actor ‘to recognize his responsibility to the play and playwright, to the

“whole” of the theatre’ (ibid.).

In The End of Acting, Richard Hornby makes an interesting point about

Strasberg’s understanding of Diderot and an unacknowledged shift in twentieth century

thinking. From a modern perspective, since both Diderot and Coquelin are classed as

anti-emotionalists, they are often considered to be externalists. Before the twentieth

century, however, ‘dualists in acting theory tended to identify emotion with the actor’s
63
body; reason was inside, emotions outside’ (Hornby 1992: 111). Strasberg, however,

‘associated emotion with the actor’s mind’ (ibid.) and seized Diderot’s internalism to

support his opposing side, reversing ‘the traditional polarity’ (ibid.). Hornby also makes

an important comparison between Coquelin and Strasberg:

Coquelin, who insisted that the actor should feel nothing, and Strasberg,
who insisted on intense emotion, both erroneously believed that the
actor’s artistic creation takes place entirely inside a conscious, fully
developed concept, a “knowing that,” which is then merely realized
outwardly. There is no place in such a model for the outside to affect the
inside. Artistic creation flows in one direction only, from within to
without…The actor has indeed become a ghost in the machine. (ibid.:
115)

Stanislavsky, however, recognized the tendency for the ‘outside’ to affect the ‘inside’,

and for the purposes of his investigations into emotion was a student of science as well.

Among the many works he consulted included the writings of psychologists Alfred

Binet (1857-1911), James, and Ribot. All three had opinions on Diderot’s Paradoxe

and/or Archer’s rebuttal.

Binet conducted a survey of his own, interviewing nine professional French

actors—including Coquelin—whose testimonies refuted what Binet considered to be

Diderot’s ‘best argument’—the idea that ‘it is not possible for actors to observe

themselves and experience emotion sincerely’ (Whyman 2008: 46). Binet found that, on

the contrary, professional actors were able to experience emotion when performing and

still be observant of the task at hand. As for Diderot’s concern of losing oneself in a

role, Binet writes that ‘clearly exceptional circumstances are needed for the actor

wholly to forget his personality’ (Binet in McGuinness 2000: 106). In an article entitled

Reflexions sur le Paradoxe de Diderot (1897), Binet published his findings and

dismissed Diderot’s theory.

Although James and Ribot never commented on Diderot directly, they were

aware of William Archer’s survey of actors. Despite declaring himself an emotionalist,

Archer does offer accounts from actors on both sides of the argument, mentioning
64
Coquelin, among others, who claim to feel no emotion. In response to Archer’s

findings, James concedes that this ‘discrepancy amongst actors’ is possibly because:

The visceral and organic part of the expression can be suppressed in


some men, but not in others, and on this it is probably that the chief part
of the felt emotion depends. Coquelin and the other actors who are
inwardly cold are probably able to affect the dissociation in a complete
way. [original emphasis] (James in Lange et al. 1922: 116)

Ribot, however, was not satisfied with James’ answer, insisting that he should have

referred to research with hypnotized subjects which demonstrated that when ‘their limbs

are placed in the attitude of prayer, anger, menace, or affection (which amounts to a

suggestion conveyed by the muscular sense), the corresponding emotion is produced’

(Ribot 1911: 97). Archer’s survey was conducted around the time that Ribot and James

were researching and formalizing their own theories on emotion, but before their ideas

had permeated into acting theory directly.

Paradoxe and Practitioners of the Twenty-first Century

The purpose of offering this rather lengthy discussion of Diderot’s Paradoxe, is

to demonstrate how one philosopher’s theory (developed as an outside observer rather

than from the experience of performance) has profoundly impacted on how the role of

emotion in Western performance is perceived. Following on from Roach’s thesis that

acting theory takes its cue from science, many discoveries in biology, particularly in the

field of neuroscience, are beginning to challenge and change old paradigms in acting

theory that held particular sway throughout the twentieth century, including Diderot’s

paradox. Psychologist Elly A. Konijn draws upon Paradoxe to support her theory of

task-based emotions, while theatre practitioner and academic Rhonda Blair calls for a

reconsideration of Paradoxe to ‘finally and definitely set aside the tired acting binary of

‘inside out’ vs. ‘outside in.’’ (Blair 2008: 14). Other practitioners, though not

referencing Diderot directly, are investigating the question of emotion for themselves

working with researchers from other fields, such as director Phelim McDermott and
65
psychologist Arnold Mindell; director Mick Gordon and neuropsychologist Paul

Broks; and director Katie Mitchell, who received a NESTA grant to experiment with the

research of neuroscientist Antonio Damasio.

66
Chapter Two: New approaches to Acting and Emotion in the 20th and
21st Centuries: Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work and Impulse
and Awareness

The three theories or approaches to emotion that I will be using in this research

project include Alba Emoting – developed by neuroscientist Susana Bloch; Emotional

Access Work – developed by theatre practitioner Brian Astbury; and Impulse and

Awareness Work – a theory of my own in development using some of the principles of

the training techniques of Teatr Piesn Kozla. This chapter is divided into three sections.

The first section discusses Alba Emoting, a technique developed by Dr. Susana Bloch.

Bloch is a neuroscientist, not a director, who in her research on emotion, has worked

closely with theatre practitioners on a continual basis. With the technique of Alba

Emoting, one can voluntarily trigger the physiological changes that occur in emotion in

life. Bloch’s research is falsifiable, based on empirical evidence. While the use of Alba

Emoting in a rehearsal process is subjective and can vary, the patterns are developed

from solid research. Section II discusses ‘Emotional Access Work’ as developed by

theatre practitioner Brian Astbury. Astbury has devised his own theory of emotion,

much in the same way as Stanislavsky, through years of experimentation with actors

and researching in other subject fields. His theories are unfalsifiable, based on years of

experimentation followed by successful results. His notions are those of a master

teacher. Section III discusses ‘impulse and awareness work’, a theory of my own that is

still in development, and is informed by the principles of the training of the Polish

theatre company Teatr Piesn Kozla (Song of the Goat) and the work of Grotowski.

Section I: Alba Emoting


This is Alba Emoting. A story of a love affair between breathing and emotion. -
Susana Bloch (Bloch 2008a)

67
Alba Emoting is a development from the BOS method, a scientifically devised

technique of physiologically activating emotion, developed by scientists Susanna Bloch,

Pedro Orthous and Guy Santibañez-H. Inspired by the James-Lange Theory of Emotion,

the research team, based in Chile in the 1970s, set out to examine the physiological

changes that occur during everyday emotion by monitoring respiratory movements,

heart rate, arterial pressure and changes in muscular tonus in subjects who were reliving

emotional experiences from their lives under hypnosis. The scientists also studied these

physiological changes in actors, drawing upon intense emotional memories. The

research suggested ‘the existence of a unique association between particular bodily

changes and a corresponding subjective experience’ [original emphasis] (Bloch and

Lemignan 1992: 32). The team went on to identify six ‘basic’ emotions originally

labeled as happiness/laughter, sadness/crying, fear/anxiety, anger/aggression,

sex/eroticism, and tenderness. Bloch considers ‘basic emotions’ to be innate, emotions

that are necessary for survival are found in other mammals, as originally suggested by

Darwin. More recently, the patterns are known as joy, sadness, fear, anger, eroticism (or

sexual love), and tenderness (or tender love). While joy, sadness, fear and anger are

recognized as basic emotions by many other emotion theorists, tenderness and eroticism

are usually not. Darwin considered both tenderness and eroticism as basic emotions, and

Ribot also included tenderness.

Each emotion has a distinct pattern of physiological changes. Bloch and her

colleagues noted that while many of these changes occurring were controlled by

involuntary physiological mechanisms, others were not. Those changes that could be

controlled voluntarily included changes in breath, facial musculature and postural

attitude. By having the subjects voluntarily replicate the respiratory-postural-facial

configuration of a certain emotion, the natural physiological changes that normally

accompany the specific emotion would occur, without out the subject having to

68
experience the subjective side of the emotion. The scientists also created a seventh

pattern, which they called ‘step out’ – a pattern that incorporates minimal, if any,

elements of the six patterns. Subjects instructed to reproduce ‘emotional effector

patterns’ (combination of the breath, facial expression and postural attitude) would

experience the emotion intended, but would immediately be able to ‘neutralize the

biochemical arousal using the step out pattern’ (Rix 2001: 207). It occurred to Bloch

that this discovery would be particularly valuable to a specific group of practitioners –

actors.

Bloch continued to develop the BOS method with theatre director Horacio

Manoz Orellana and actors from the Teater Klanen in Denmark. It was during this

period of research that Bloch developed her own system for teaching their method,

which she calls Alba Emoting (partly for ‘alba’ meaning ‘dawn’ in Spanish, and partly

for a production of Lorca’s The House of Bernarda Alba with the Teater Klanen) (Rix

2001: 209). (The English word ‘emoting’ has negative connotations in the theatre world

and the American teachers of Alba Emoting are currently in discussion with Bloch

about changing the name to ‘Alba Technique’.) In Alba Emoting, it is said that one is

working with ‘pure’ emotions, but ‘pure’ in the Spanish sense of the word meaning

‘single’ or not mixed. The aim of Bloch’s system was to teach actors to physiologically

trigger emotions, but equally give them a tool to deactivate the emotion in the form of

‘step out’ as well. Having identified the respiratory-postural-facial configurations of the

six emotions, Bloch intentionally created the ‘step-out’ technique (that bore no

resemblance to the other identifiable emotion patterns) as a way to neutralize the

induced effects. Bloch and her team define emotion as:

A complex and dynamic functional state of the entire organism, triggered


by an external or internal stimulus, integrated in the central nervous and
neuroendocrine systems, involving simultaneously a particular group of
effector organs (visceral, humoral, neuromuscular) and a subjective
experience (feeling). [original emphasis] (Bloch and Lemignan 1992: 32)

69
Bloch goes on to describe emotions as being ‘phasic’ (momentary at higher levels) or

‘tonic’ (lower levels over a period of time). For example, the ‘phasic’ fear could be

shock, and ‘tonic’ fear, anxiety. The intensity of the emotional effector pattern is also a

factor to consider; low-level joy may be just the beginning of a titter, while a higher

level is uncontrollable belly laughter. Despite various publications, Alba Emoting is still

in the process of becoming more widely known and practiced. Bloch and her team

began publishing articles about their research in scientific journals (see Bloch et al.

1987; Bloch et al. 1992), and eventually in theatre journals (See Bloch 1993;Rix 1993).

Bloch recently published a book in English, The Alba of Emotions: Managing Emotions

through Breathing (2006) and a translation of her newest book Surfeando al Ola

Emocional: Reconozca las emociones basicas y comprenda sus emociones mixtas

(2008b) (Surfing the Emotional Wave: Recognize basic emotions and understand your

mixed emotions) should become available in English in 2012.

Alba Emoting has been subject to criticism. Elly Konijn maintains Bloch’s

research ‘failed to consider that physiological phenomena measured in actors might be

related to things other than the presumed arousal of character related emotions’ (Konijn

2000: 107), which Konijn considers to be essential in her own theory of task-based

emotions. Rhonda Blair, an actor and academic researching the relationship between

acting and cognitive neuroscience, criticizes Konijn’s study for not addressing ‘the

psychophysiology of emotions as Bloch does’ or ‘neurocognitive processes out of

which feelings and actions arise’ (Blair 2008: 49). Paul Ekman, the psychologist who

specializes in facial expressions, agrees in general with Bloch’s findings, and his own

unpublished research found that ‘when subjects make facial expressions respiration falls

into place’ (Ekman in Bloch et al. 1988: 202). But he does have disagreements ‘in

regard to their [Bloch et al.’s] choice of emotions and the specification of the particular

facial expressions which characterize each emotion’ (ibid.). Other common criticisms

70
include objection to the commercial trademark on Alba Emoting, the existence of a

self-monitored certification process, and the use of ‘real’ emotions in performance.

While Bloch’s research focused on the physiological changes that occur in everyday

human emotion, she makes a clear distinction between emotion in life and emotion in

performance. Bloch defines acting behavior as ‘behavior produced at will by an actor in

order to transmit gnostic and emotional information to an audience by word, gesture and

posture within an artistic framework’ (Bloch et al. 1987: 1). Emotion in performance

shares similarities with everyday emotion in its physiological components and effects

on an observer, but crucial differences exist in the stimuli that trigger them and the

accompanying subjective cognitive processes. For these reasons, Bloch believes that in

order to ‘appear “natural” or “true” on the stage, actors do not need to “feel” the

emotion they are playing but must produce the correct effector-expressive output of the

emotional behavior’ (ibid.: 15), which is consequently in keeping with Diderot’s

conclusion. Blair states:

The idea that emotions can exist apart from conscious content might be
difficult for actors to grasp at first, but this is fundamental to understanding
the current science, which defines emotions as body states, while feelings
are consciously registered ‘interpretations’ of these body states. (Blair 2008:
47)

This clear distinction between ‘feeling’ and ‘emotion’ is potentially useful when

working with actors. However, while the foundations of Alba Emoting are grounded in

concrete scientific research, using the technique in practice (in direct application to a

rehearsal process or indeed to a theatrical text) is more subjective and open to

interpretation.

The Patterns

While the principles behind Bloch’s research have remained constant, some of

the semantics have changed. The six patterns are now labeled anger, fear, sadness, joy,

71
tenderness and sexual love. As previously mentioned, Bloch sets forth three steps for

achieving physiologically activated emotion: 1) breath; 2) facial expression; and 3)

postural attitude. See Figure 2 for a breakdown of the emotional effector patterns.

Figure 2: The Alba Emoting Effector Patterns


Neutral Breath Breathing Pattern: An inhalation through the nose and exhalation through the
mouth on an even count.
Facial Expression: The eyes are focused on the horizon, the lips are slightly apart
and the face is relaxed.
Postural Attitude: Muscles relaxed and balancing on skeletal system.
Tenderness Breathing Pattern: A gentle inhalation and exhalation through the nose. The
exhale is slightly longer than the inhale and there may be a slight hold before
beginning the next breathing cycle.
Facial Expression: The mouth forms a slight smile, gently engaging with the eyes.
Postural Attitude: Relaxed, muscle tension released, and once into the pattern
there will be a small head tilt to the side.
Anger Breathing Pattern: Sharp, even inhalations and exhalations through the nose,
though the length may vary.
Facial Expression: The mouth is closed, the teeth are aligned and the eyes narrow
from the bottom lids.
Postural Attitude: Tense with a forward motion.
Eroticism* (or *There are two forms of this pattern, active and receptive
Sexual Love) Breathing Pattern: An inhalation and exhalation through the mouth, starting on an
even count, though at higher levels the pattern does not conform to strict rhythms.
Facial Expression: (Receptive) - The eyes are semi-closed as if “looking through
the lashes.” The eyes have a soft focus; the mouth is open with the corners of the
mouth pulling up into a slight smile and the head tilts back. (Active) - The head is
more upright, the eyes more focused, and the lower jaw moves forward.
Postural Attitude: Relaxed and open in the receptive form, and while still relaxed
in the active form, it has a more forward motion.
Fear Breathing Pattern: A quick, shallow inhale through the mouth using chest
breathing, occasionally taking in additional shallow inhales through the mouth
when necessary. There is no specific exhalation.
Facial Expression: Eyes are wide open, pushing out and forward to increase
peripheral vision, and the eyes dart around. The chin is slightly lowered to protect
the neck, the jaw is dropped and the mouth forms an “O” shape.
Postural Attitude: Tense with a backward motion.
Joy Breathing Pattern: A light quick inhalation through the nose, and stepped
exhalation through the mouth, until all the air is fully expelled. Reaching this point
of “below empty”, should activate saccades, involuntary muscle movement in the
lower abdomen.
Facial Expression: The mouth opens wide, with corners of the mouth pulling up
into a broad smile that engages the eyes.
Postural Attitude: Relaxed and loose.
Sadness Breathing Pattern: A series of short inhalations through the nose, and a long
exhalation through the mouth until “below empty.” Once the air is completely
expelled saccades should begin, resulting in a slight hold at the bottom of the breath
before starting the next cycle of inhalations.
Facial Expression: A downward glance with the eyes. The brow pulls up and
together in the center of the forehead between the eyebrows, and the mouth is
slightly open with the corners of the mouth pulling down, which may cause a quiver
of the bottom lip.
Postural Attitude: Relaxed but collapsed.

72
Laura Facciponti Bond (a certified Alba Emoting instructor and professor at

the University of North Carolina, Ashville) has also created a specific system of

numbering each of the patterns to aid in teaching (see figure 3). In her years of teaching

Alba Emoting, Bond found that students were continually frustrated with the ‘limitation

of syntax’, many feeling that ‘the process towards induction was blocked by the word

association with each pattern’. As I have trained with Bond more than any other

instructor, and assisted on her workshops, I use her system and will explain the patterns

in this way:

Figure 3: Facciponti Bond’s AE Labeling System

1 1a Tender Love
Nose
1b Anger
0 Breathing

Breath 2 2a Sexual Love


Mouth
(Neutral &
Balancing) Breathing
2b Fear

3 3a Joy
Nose &
Mouth
3b Sadness
Breathing

Alba Emoting has a specific certification process based on the total number of training

hours accumulated and skill, with 6 certification levels (only Bloch and Pedro Sándor

have a Certification Level 6). In three years of training, I have worked with seven

certified Alba Emoting instructors, including Bloch, and am a fully certified instructor

myself. I currently hold a Certification Level 4.

73
Figure 4: Beck instructs Alba Emoting. Photograph by Bernard Dubois.

Bond has also conducted training workshops that combine learning Alba Emoting

with the Feldenkrais Method, a form of somatic education developed by Moshe

Feldenkrais (1904 – 1984). With regard to emotion, Feldenkrais maintains:

I may feel joyful, angry, afraid, disgusted. Everyone can, on seeing me,
recognize the feeling I experience. Which comes first: the motor pattern
or the feeling? I would like to stress the idea that they are basically the
same thing. We cannot become conscious of a feeling before it is
expressed by a motor mobilization, and therefore there is no feeling so
long as there is no body attitude. [original emphasis] (Feldenkrais 2010:
94)

The first workshop of this kind was co-run with Lavinia Plonka (a mime and actor who

studied with Grotowski and Lecaq, and now a highly respected Feldenkrais practitioner)

in Mexico in 2007, and subsequently more intensives of this nature have taken place in

Montréal, Canada in 2008 and 2009 in conjunction with another Feldenkrais

practitioner (and Alba Emoting CL3) Odette Guimond. This pairing of Alba Emoting

and the Feldenkrais Method is significant. Yvan Joly, an internationally renowned

Feldenkrais trainer and psychologist, defines somatic education as ‘the field of practice
74
and knowledge belonging to a variety of methods that are interested in learning an

awareness of the living body (the soma) moving in the environment’ [original emphasis,

my translation] (Joly 2008: 33). After participating in two of the Montréal workshops,

Joly considers Alba Emoting to be a form of somatic education. In a testimonial about

the workshops Joly states:

For movement, The Feldenkrais Method promotes ideas like


reversibility, awareness, intentionality, degrees of freedom, availability
to move in any direction, availability to change the direction without
preparation, finding neutral […] Alba Emoting proposes exactly the
same ideas specifically applied to the realm of emotional movements.
(Joly 2009)

The combination of these two methods is attracting a variety of participants in addition

to actors, such as Feldenkrais practitioners, psychologists and ontological coaches. In

South America, however, Alba Emoting is a popular tool in itself, being utilized in such

varied fields as education, family therapy, psychotherapy, management and

communication (Bloch 2009).

Learning Alba Emoting

When learning Alba Emoting, one can expect to go through three stages. The

first stage is considered to be ‘robotic’, and that is the phase when one is first learning

the patterns. In this initial learning period, the subjects are encouraged to explore

activating the breath, facial expressions and postural attitude in a non-subtle way, so as

to begin to identify, control and feel the voluntary components of the effector patterns.

The second stage is ‘induction’, which is the time when the Alba pattern’s trigger one’s

own emotion. This is considered to be the ‘magic moment’ in Alba Emoting and is what

leads to the third and final stage, which is ‘integration’. After an actor has learned the

patterns to a high standard, they can activate the patterns in a very subtle way, almost

without thinking, which is preferable for performance (Rix 2001).

75
In Practice

Because of the extensive training required in order for actors to fully grasp and

understand the Alba Emoting patterns, I have not used the training directly in

professional setting. However, I have used my knowledge of the technique in order to

address specific problems that actors may have. In 2009 I was asked to consult on a

production of Orwell: A Celebration at London’s Trafalgar Studios, working

specifically on an excerpt from George Orwell’s 1984, the Ministry of Love torture

scene. Every time the character portraying O’Brien raised his hand, the character of

Winston would have to wince in intense pain until the torture was over. With my

knowledge of Alba Emoting, I was able to suggest to the actor playing Winston

different breathing patterns and postural attitudes to assist his portrayal. The actor

playing Winston thus gained a reliable physical tool to achieve the heightened state

required of the play, night after night. Similarly, when teaching at East 15 Acting

School, I was working with a young actor who had to become very angry in a scene.

Every time he attempted the scene he was unintentionally mixing in elements of sadness

(when inadvertent mixing of the emotions occurs in Alba Emoting it is called an

‘entanglement’). I simply asked him to release the tension in his brow, and his

performance immediately improved. Neither of these actors had any knowledge of Alba

Emoting, nor did they need to. With this research project, however, I trained the actors

in the technique in order to engage in a more thorough inquiry of Alba Emoting.

76
Figure 5: Alan Cox and Ben Porter in Orwell: A Celebration at Trafalgar Studios, 2009. Photograph
by Dawn Cruttenden

Section Two: Emotional Access Work


Emotion is a physical force – an energy field – which is communicated,
transmitted through the air. – Brian Astbury (Trusting the Actor 2011: location
1681)

‘Emotional Access Work’ is the name given to a system of rehearsal exercises

developed by South African theatre practitioner Brian Astbury. Astbury was married to

actress Yvonne Bryceland, and together they ran The Space Theatre in Capetown, South

Africa, along with the playwright Athol Fugard in the 1970s. Astbury’s Emotional

Access Work is based on his theory that our conscious brain can be problematic in

rehearsal and that the creative (sub- or pre-) conscious brain can be accessed through

various exercises to overcome the intrusion of self-consciousness in the actor’s process.

Freud originated discussions about the ‘subconscious’, but, as New Scientist writer Kate

Douglas points out in her article ‘The Other You’, ‘many neurobiologists avoid the

word “subconscious”, preferring “non-conscious”, “pre-conscious” or “unconscious” to

describe thought processes that happen outside consciousness’ (Douglas 2007: 42).
77
Astbury uses ‘preconscious’ as defined by Stanislas Dehaene and his colleagues as

defined in their article entitled ‘Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: a

testable taxonomy’ (2006). Dehaene et al, assert:

We propose to call preconscious (or potentially conscious, or P-


conscious) a neural process that potentially carries enough activation for
conscious access, but is temporarily buffered in a non-conscious store
because of a lack of top-down attentional amplification. […] As shown
by the attentional blink and inattentional blindness paradigms, even
strong visual stimuli can remain temporarily preconscious. They are
potentially accessible (if they quickly gain access to conscious report if
they were attended), but they are not consciously accessed at that
moment. (Dehaene et al. 2006: 207)

Astbury believes that, for an actor, accessing preconscious thought, rather than relying

on ‘thinking’ is extremely important. He explains:

[W]hat I mean by ‘thought’ or ‘thinking’ in this context is conscious


thought. The majority of this thinking is sub- or pre- conscious. That’s
good thinking. […] The kind where we sit and consciously think, ‘Now
I’ll do this,’ or ‘Now I’ll do that’ is ‘conscious thought’ and, as I
frequently tell my actors ‘Conscious thinking is seriously bad for an
actor.’ (Astbury 2011: location 1104)

Gilles Fauconnier and Mark Turner, in their book The Way We Think: Conceptual

Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities (2002), remark on this distinction

between conscious and preconscious thought, maintaining that ‘even after training, the

mind seems to have only feeble abilities to represent itself consciously what the

unconscious mind does easily’ (Fauconnier and Turner 2002: 18).

Working as an acting teacher in institutions such as Mountview, The London

Academy of Music and Dramatic Art (LAMDA), and East 15, Astbury has been

developing his system over the last twenty-some years. According to Astbury:

The methods I have evolved over the past two decades all have their
roots in the practical struggle, with many young actors, to find ways to
utilise the power of the subconscious without paralysing it with
conscious thought; to free intuition to do its proper work; to shift
emotional and physical blockages which stand in the way of
performance; to learn to trust this ‘being’ which is, after all, an essential
part of ourselves, the part that for every moment of our lives keeps us
alive and functioning properly. (Astbury 2011: location 193-199)

78
By ‘this being’, Astbury is referring to ourselves, or more specifically, our pre-

conscious selves free of conscious-controlling thought. Astbury’s techniques derived

from years of working with actors in training, rehearsals, and performance. When he

began teaching at LAMDA, Astbury noticed that young actors had particular trouble

approaching emotion:

Very early on I started to identify the moment when an actor is


confronted by an emotion – and the consequences of this problem:
tightened jaws, shallow breathing, losing lines, bodily tensions. I began
to look for exercises to help actors push past these blockages. (Astbury,
2011: location 415)

Astbury’s theory is that our natural ‘defences’ (or ‘blockages’ as described above) can

interfere with the actor at work. He refers to most of his exercises as ‘Left-Brain

Disabling Devices’, which attempt to overcome left-brain analytical dominance with

right-brain creativity. Neurologists have discovered that uncreative people ‘have

marked hemispheric dominance, with the left hemisphere suppressing creative states

and processes. By contrast, creative people are said to have less hemispheric

dominance’ (York 2004: 6). However, Astbury’s casual use of discussing split-brain

theory is used purely for ease of discussion in the rehearsal room. In actual fact, the two

hemispheres of the brain cannot be simplified this way. Chris McManus, author of

Right Hand, Left Hand insists:

However tempting it is to talk of right and left hemispheres in isolation,


they are actually two half-brains, designed to work together as a smooth,
single, integrated whole in one entire, complete brain. The left
hemisphere knows how to handle logic and the right hemisphere knows
about the world. Put the two together and one gets a powerful thinking
machine. Use either on its own and the result can be bizarre or absurd.
(McManus 2002: 183-184)

Astbury’s exercises draw upon the work of psychologist Arnold Mindell, Alexander

Lowen’s ‘Bioenergetics’, Win Wenger’s ‘Imaging Streaming’, and the theories of

neuroscientists such as Antonio Damasio, Joseph LeDoux, and Michael Gazzaniga.

Although many of Astbury’s techniques resonate with recent discoveries in


79
neuroscience, this correlation is mostly theoretical, and based primarily on Astbury’s

observation and experience of more than twenty years training actors.

According to Astbury, when working with actors it is only once the emotional

connections have been properly made and integrated that it becomes easier for the actor

to move into ‘the moment’. Astbury’s aims include: freeing the mind and body from

conscious control and allowing the actor to connect with a proper emotion, experience

that emotion, and then integrate the experience without conscious thought. For clarity, I

will divide Astbury’s work into two different categories: immediate and contributory.

The immediate exercises release blocks and establish a psychophysical connection

between the actor and the text, and are more directly related to rehearsing the specific

text. The contributory exercises also help release blocks within the actor, but are

additional means of preparation not necessarily connected to the text. The immediate

exercises include under-reading, anger/energy runs, physical exercises and physical

metaphor. The contributory exercises include image streaming, EMDR (Eye Movement

Desensitization and Reprocessing), dreaming bodywork and primal integrative

breathing. There techniques will be discussed in further detail in the following sections.

The Immediate Exercises

Under-reading

Astbury uses a process called ‘under-reading’, an integral component to

Emotional Access Work, to which Astbury was introduced by director Tessa

Schneiderman. The actors are read their lines by other actors or stage managers offstage

in a continuous, constant stream. Instead of the actor being tethered to a script in their

hand and reading lines by means of a ‘left-brain’ activity—which interprets, analyzes

and tells the conscious mind how words should be expressed—they receive the

information aurally. This activity sufficiently occupies their ‘conscious brain’ enough to

allow the ‘pre-conscious brain’ the freedom to make multiple subconscious decisions in
80
fractions of milliseconds. During the rehearsal period, it is also an inadvertent way of

allowing the actors try the text in a variety of ways without conscious decision-making.

On a very practical level, under-reading removes the block of the actor holding/reading

off a script, which frees the actor to move and to more efficiently communicate with

their acting partners, at an earlier point in the rehearsal room. Meyerhold was also very

opposed to actors reading off a page. He states:

When actors scan the lines with their eyes, they unwittingly begin to
declaim, as is natural for people who are reading a text. I want to snatch
the parts from the hands of the actors as quickly as possible and therefore
I hurry to move on to the staging of the scenes. I would even have the
actors speak with the aid of a prompter than follow the parts with their
eyes. (Meyerhold in Gladov 2004: 132)

Even as early as 1928, in John Dolman’s The Art of Play Production, Dolman

recognizes ‘two distinct methods of memorization’ (Dolman Jr. 1928: 245). Dolman

explains:

By the one method the act of memorizing is made a purely mechanical


process having no relation to the study of meaning; the two things are, so
to speak, carried on independently by two separate portions of the mind.
By the other method the words, actions, and meanings are memorized
coordinately, and all associations built up from the start. (ibid.: 245-246)

Under-reading allows the actors to learn the text ‘on their feet’, as it were, and thus the

‘words, actions, and meanings’ are therefore ‘memorized coordinately.’ Under-reading

is also an essential component for many of Astbury’s other exercises.

Anger/Energy Runs

Anger, or energy runs as they are sometimes referred to, begin with the actor

approaching the text with extreme anger. This anger is unrelated to the context of the

play. Anger runs take place with under-reading. According to Astbury, the anger runs

(as well as the physical exercises) were developed in order to ‘serve one major purpose:

to create a wave of emotion. On this the character can surf with the actor. It doesn’t

matter if the character’s emotion is not anger. Once the wave is in motion it can take

any form it wishes’ (Astbury 2011: location 2657). Anger runs may turn into energy
81
runs, where if the anger shifts into another emotion, the actor should ‘surf it’, rather

then staying with anger for the sake of the exercise.

Physical Exercises

Astbury also uses a variety of physical exercises that appear to allow for what he calls

‘imprinting’ emotion and establishing an ‘emotional muscle memory’. These exercises are

conducted while the actors are being under-read, and they include pushing against the wall,

squatting as if sitting on a chair against the wall, lying on their backs with legs vertical and

toes pulling down, sit-ups, press-ups, Grotowski’s ‘The Cat’ - stretching back while on the

knees, or any other exercise that requires energy. The point is to busy the defense

mechanisms and distract the conscious thinking brain so that the actor can make an emotional

connection with the material that is being absorbed by the brain aurally, as the text is being

under-read. These physical exercises tire the actor, and as we know from everyday life it is

when we are tired that we are most vulnerable to emotion. This principle holds the same for

the physical work of Grotowski. In Shomit Mitter’s book Systems of Rehearsal: Stanislavsky,

Brecht, Grotowski and Brook, he says about Grotowski’s work, ‘Often sequences of such

exercises are performed without interruption on the basis of the assumption that fatigue can

break down the resistance of the actor more effectively than conscious contrivance’ (Mitter

1992: 96). Astbury, somewhat facetiously calls these physical exercises ‘left-brain disabling

devices’.

In practice, the physical exercises enable the body and mind, or the voice and body,

to work together. If you can allow the text, while you’re speaking it, to be affected by your

physical state – sit-ups, pushing against the wall, etc – then in turn, when you speak the text

your body will respond accordingly. It is a process of building up a kind of muscle memory.

The following is a passage about Meyerhold working with Stanislavsky, which sums up very

well what Astbury is trying to achieve with his exercises:

Stanislavsky especially tormented me when I was rehearsing Baron


Tuzenbach in Three Sisters [1901]. For a long time I couldn’t get it right.
82
The task seemed to be so simple: to come out, go up to the piano, sit
down, and begin to speak. But no sooner had I started when Stanislavsky
would send me back again. In those moments I almost hated him. It must
have been ten times that we began again with my entrance. At first I
didn’t understand what he wanted from me. Now it is clear. Each time I
would come out, strike a pose and begin to recite the text. Finally
Stanislavsky climbed up on the stage, threw a piece of paper on the floor
and asked me to break up my text in the following way: after having said
three lines, I would see the paper, pick it up, continue to speak, then
unfold it and speak further. Everything immediately fell into place. The
oratory disappeared as if by magic. The natural gesture of picking up and
unfolding / the paper helped me to saturate with life what had seemed to
be a “soliloquy”: “When I studied at the military school…”

Another time Stanislavsky demanded tension and inner energy from me,
but I was as cold as a dog’s nose. Then he gave me a sealed bottle of
wine, telling me to open it with the corkscrew and at the same time say
my lines. The physical difficulty of opening the bottle and the energy
needed for doing it immediately awoke me. That was a purely pedagogic,
technical device, which I often use now. You’ve probably noticed.
Pavlov should have been told about that. He would have been interested.
If just once you can get something right in rehearsal, then it will always
go right because of conditioned reflex. (Meyerhold in Gladov 2004, 149-
150)

Astbury’s physical exercises operate in much the same way.

Physical Metaphor

Astbury’s physical metaphor operates on the same principle as the physical

exercises but is slightly more sophisticated in the exercise’s relation to the text. There

are still physical exercises, but this time the exercise are a metaphorical physicalization

of the character’s experience. An example, which will be described in more detail in

Chapter Three, would be restraining the actor who plays Masha in Chekov’s Three

Sisters as she says good-bye to Vershinin; or the character of Duckling in Timberlake

Wertenbaker’s Our Country’s Good, speaking her ‘If you live’ speech while attempting

to lift a limp actor to his feet. This is a creative aspect to Astbury’s work that challenges

the director to find physical exercises that operate as a metaphorical bridge between the

actor and the character.

83
Figure 6: Astbury using Physical Metaphor with actor Carolina Ortega (being restrained) and
actor Matthew Blake (under-reading). Photograph by Kezia Cole.

The Contributory Exercises

Image Streaming

Image streaming is a unique combination of highly creative visualization and

analytical thinking skills that allows the development of a strong connection between

the conscious and unconscious mind, producing an ongoing sense of expanding

creativity and enhanced intuition. The actor finds a quiet place and closes their eyes,

focusing on the first image they see in the mind’s eye. While describing what they see

to another person, or by recording what is said, they follow the stream of images. This

can be done in relation to the character the actor is playing. The actor should try not to

consciously interfere – but allow the images to come appear naturally and organically

(something that can be difficult). With image streaming, an actor can discover images

from your own subconscious and these images stay stronger in the mind’s eye than

84
choosing images from a book. Many actors are surprised how vivid the images – and

even sensory experiences – from an image stream can be. Astbury describes image

streaming as ‘a very powerful tool for giving a character access to the actor’s

imagination’ [my emphasis] (Astbury 2011: 1593), or to put another way, a tool for

giving the actor access to their preconscious imagination.

Preconscious Integration Devices

Image Streaming (See Wenger and Poe 1996), EMDR (See Shapiro 2001) and

Dreaming Bodywork (See Mindell 1985) are all techniques that, in the context of

Astbury’s work, I will call ‘preconscious integration devices’ that come from therapies.

The latter two techniques are aimed at enabling the mind to deal with entrenched

trauma—which has resulted in habituated physical and mental patterns—by releasing

these patterns and integrating them through metaphor and getting past useless defenses.

Primal Integrative Breathing is a method of circular breathing which enables the body to

deal with entrenched defenses which are no longer of any use, most of which are based

on early memories which for some people are the result of birth trauma and therefore

not understood by the conscious body defense systems—coming as they do from a

period when infants had neither words nor concepts in which to describe its feelings.

Primal Integrative Breathing is aimed at releasing emotion, muscular tension and voice

through breath. While I have experienced all of Astbury’s techniques in training, I did

not use EMDR, Dreaming Bodywork, or Primal Integrative Breathing for this research

project.

In Practice

I trained with Brian Astbury over three years, both at LAMDA and East 15

Acting School. At East 15, when playing Lynnette ‘Squeaky’ Fromme in Stephan

Sondheim’s Assassins, there was a scene, where, with the actor playing Sarah Jane

Moore, we had to become excited/angry to the point where we began to shoot wildly at
85
a bucket of Kentucky Fried Chicken. As we neared the performance date, we were

struggling with the energy required for that moment. The solution was to do an

anger/energy run with tennis balls at hand to throw at the wall at the points where we

would be shooting the gun. The energy and physical engagement we found in that

rehearsal stayed with us throughout the run of performances. Without trying to ‘force it’

the emotion required in that moment had become a muscle memory.

Astbury describes his techniques as ‘emotional access work’, however I believe

this title does a disservice to the exercises, because the exercises do more than just

access emotion. From my experiences, both participating as an actor and from using

these techniques as a director, the exercises have more to do with accessing a different

level of attention, awareness and energy, which culminate in integration. Toward the

end of Astbury’s book, he states: ‘[i]t’s all about energy. Set energy in motion and

things happen. The text, and what lives beneath its surface, will give that energy

direction and focus’ (Astbury 2011: location 2653-2660). ‘Energy work’ is perhaps a

more accurate description. Through Astbury’s exercises the entire organism begins to

work as a cohesive unit/whole, ready to respond fully to the theatrical situation.

Unfortunately, far too many training systems tend to encourage the use
of the conscious left brain and ignore the wonders of the right. The
result: dull, restricted, out-of-the-moment acting, able to deliver only one
thought process at that time, not the multi-leveled complexity of the most
ordinary of us human beings. (ibid.: location 1381)

Rehearsal footage of Astbury’s exercises can be found in the case study chapters.

Section III: Impulse and Awareness Work

While training at East 15 Acting School in 2004, I was first exposed to the ideas

of Grotowski through Ian Morgan, now with Teatr Piesn Kozla in Wraclaw, Poland.

Grotowski considered his training to be a via negativa, ‘not a collection of skills but an

86
eradication of blocks’ (Grotowski and Brook 1968: 17). Morgan introduced our

course to Grotowski’s exercises plastiques, a physical training intended to lead to

spontaneous flow from one’s natural body rhythm. The work was exhausting, but

produced within us, as actors, a state of intense focus. Much as Astbury’s work uses

physical exhaustion to disrupt the conscious controlling left hemisphere, and

Grotowski’s exercises work similarly. Discussing the exercises plastiques in an

interview with Richard Schechner, Grotowski stated:

There are certain points of fatigue which break the control of the mind, a
control that blocks us. When we find the courage to do things that are
impossible, we make the discovery that our body does not block us. We
do the impossible and the division within us between conception and the
body’s ability disappears. (Wolford and Schechner 1997: 42)

It was observing this very phenomenon when using this work that caused me to

experiment with the exercises plastiques and text. I started with an amalgamation of

Astbury’s use of under-reading and Grotowski’s plastiques and rivers, which I call an

‘impulse run.’ Using this work in conjunction with text helps to block conscious

thought and impulse runs and the physical exercises help the actor to access a state in

which they can allow preconscious decisions that have already been made to surface—a

paradoxical combination of relaxation and concentration.

87
Figure 7: Students from the East 15 Contemporary Theatre Practice course working with Ian
Morgan in 2004. Photograph by Brian Astbury.

Since that time I have experienced the training of Teatr Piesn Kozla (Song of the Goat),

and have been experimenting with using selected principles from the training of the

Polish theatre company founded by Grzegorz Bral and Anna Zubrzycki, both formerly

of the Gardzienice Theatre Association. The company has developed its own training.

The major principle guiding their work is learning by experiencing coordination,

harmony, polyphony, rhythm, dramaturgy, acrobatics, and singing body. Their training

is cumulative physical group training intended to maximize spontaneity, impulse, peak

physical ability and ensemble work. Bral, when describing the training, points out:

First of all, you have to understand that they way we work is not the way
people would normally work with a text – not through psychological
analysis, not through memory, not through an emotional analysis of the
text. (Zubrzycki and Bral 2010: 251)

Unlike Astbury and Bloch, Bral and Zubrzycki rarely use the term ‘emotion’ directly

when working with actors. Rather, the training is intended to ‘make room for something

to happen’, though the implication after several first-hand experiences working with the

88
company, is that the ‘something’ is in fact ‘emotion’. Part of the ethos of their training

is not to discuss or analyze what is happening within the actor. This is especially a

challenge of writing about practice. In reference to this difficulty, Bral states:

My fellow practitioners understand this, because it comes with the


understanding of practice: if you name something, if you give a simple
definition to something, you stop the process. You close the experience.
(ibid.: 254-255)

Although I have participated in several intensive trainings with Teatr Piesn Kozla, I am

not fully trained in their techniques as I am with the others. Also, the ethos of the

company discourages imitation. Instead Bral insists that what is important is for

practitioners to grasp the principles of the training, and to use them in your own way,

rather than repeating the exact exercises of the company. The most important principle

from Teatr Piesn Kozla is that of coordination, which Zubrzycki defines as ‘the

interdependency and interrelatedness of all of the tools that an actor has’ (ibid.: 250).

The ‘tools’ to which Zubzrycki is referring include: ‘the tone of your voice, of your

body, of their relation one to the other; you have your imagination, sensitivity, feelings,

senses; you have text, you have melody, you have rhythms’ (ibid.: 259). One of the

obstacles to achieving this coordination is our cultural view of a split between mind and

body. The Teatr Piesn Kozla training has been developed to overcome this division.

Bral maintains:

Coordination is the one thing that we can deal with. There is always a
direct flow between how we think, what we say, and what we do. But, in
the culture we live in – whatever the culture we live in – we learn to
divide ourselves. […] We are very disconnected from our impulses, from
the flow, from the true feelings that we have. (ibid.: 259)

It is with this in mind that I started experimented with my own exercises that enhance

an actor’s coordination. The exercises always vary, developing from the specific

demands of any given dramaturgy that I work with.

89
Impulse and Awareness Work

The exercises are used to increase the actors’ awareness of themselves, their

awareness in relation to one another, and in relation to the space. They are also used to

help the actors to work off impulses from within, from one another, and the surrounding

environment including the space itself and any aural or visual stimuli. These exercises

vary in relation to the actor’s task for a specific performance, and will be discussed

specifically regarding their use throughout the dissertation in reference to each case

study. Lorna Marshall, an acting teacher at the Royal Academy of Dramatic Art

(RADA) has developed her own exercises that work on a similar level. In her book The

Body Speaks: Performance and Expression, Marshall describes an actor’s task:

As well as sensing what needs to happen at any moment, you must be


able to avoid the automatic and habitual management of impulse. You
are able to follow any impulse immediately into direct and complete
manifestation without hesitation, fear or judgment. (Marshall 2001: 33)

Developing this ability to ‘sense’ and ‘follow’ impulses is the aim of this work. Bral

points out another ‘problem’ with discussing practice:

If you name something an ‘impulse’, you’ve actually lost it. If you focus
on something that is an impulse, in that very moment you’ve lost it. So,
in order to follow something that you call ‘impulse’, you must, in a way,
not recognize it. (Zubryzcki and Bral 2010: 255)

When I use impulse and awareness exercises within a rehearsal context, I try and refrain

from too much discussion. Instead, I build up the exercises over time so that the actors

have continual experiences with the work, and can develop their own sense of what is

useful and what is not throughout the process. ‘Impulse runs’ allow actors to explore the

text in an abstract way, free from any contextual restrictions of the script. One can begin

to develop a kinesthetic sense of space, spontaneity and response to space, sounds and

energy from the other actors, and that of themselves. Like Astbury’s physical metaphor,

these exercises are not do not follow a set of concrete rules, but must be determined by

the particular text on which one is working.

90
In Practice

Using impulse and awareness work in practice, I once directed a challenging

play called All Alone by Gene David Kirk. The play was arguably set inside the mind of

a schizophrenic paedophile or internet predator. There were two men, Man A and Man

B, possibly the two sides of one person. They never interacted directly (with text), but

would often pick up each other’s sentences, rude nursery rhymes, etc. The energy and

rhythms were very important to the piece. The actors and I started developing ‘impulse

runs’ using a starting point of the plastiques, to work up a sweat and a focus. I then

added under-reading of the text. The under-reading in combination of the physical

workout offered the actors the freedom to explore the text outside of the context of the

play, or rather, explore the text in relation to the energy of the other actor. All Alone

garnered a shared best actor award between the two actors – The Micheál Mac

Liammóir Award for Best Male Performance when it played at the Project Arts Centre,

Dublin, for the Dublin International Gay Theatre Festival in 2006. I also received the

New York International Fringe Festival Excellence Award for Outstanding Direction,

when All Alone appeared at the SoHo Playhouse, NYC, in 2007. From my experience

and perspective as a director on the project, the success of the production was a result of

the way in which we approached the rehearsal process. For the most recent review of

All Alone, see Appendix A.

91
Figure 8: Matthew Flacks and Andrew Barron in All Alone by Gene David Kirk (NYC production).
Photograph by Jessica Beck.

For a more thorough examination of these techniques, I am including this work

in the research project. As this is the newest technique to my repertoire, and one that has

not been passed down directly from an outside source, it is difficult to describe the work

more fully at this point. My understanding of the impulse and awareness work develops

further throughout the research and will be explained in more detail in the case studies.

92
Chapter Three: Case Study One - Investigating the Challenge of
Emotion in the Rehearsals and Performance of Excerpts from
Chekhov’s Three Sisters

This chapter presents the first of three case studies. Each case study is conducted

with the intention of gaining a clearer insight into the nature of emotion in Western

performance. Case Study One is an investigation of the challenge of emotion through

the rehearsals and performance of excerpts from Anton Chekhov’s Three Sisters. In an

attempt to address my research questions, I incorporated the work of Alba Emoting,

Emotional Access Work and Impulse and Awareness Work into a rehearsal process on a

selection of excerpts from Chekhov’s Three Sisters, using the English version adapted

by Irish playwright Brian Friel. This chapter is divided into four sections. Section I:

Chekhov’s Three Sisters explores the reasons for choosing Chekhov’s play. Section II:

Methodology and Preliminary Questions specifically addresses the set up of the case

study, including the methodology, and the actors’ preliminary views on emotion.

Section III: Rehearsals focuses on the rehearsal process and performance of the first

case study, while Section IV: Findings offers some concluding thoughts on the project.

Section 1: Chekhov’s Three Sisters

Chekhov, Stanislavsky and the Moscow Art Theatre

Anton Pavlovich Chekhov (1860 – 1904) was writing plays at a time when actor

training, and theatre itself, were both on the brink of becoming revolutionized, not only

in Russia, but also simultaneously across Europe and America. Coinciding with the

emergence of Naturalism was the recognition of the importance of actor training and the

need for more rigorous rehearsal processes. Theatre historian Rose Whyman points out:

As Chekhov’s work as a playwright was beginning to become known


towards the end of the 1880s, the main repertoire continued to be the
93
classics, comedies and melodrama but there was a growing recognition
for theatrical reform. (Whyman 2011: 34)

This ‘theatrical reform’ was taking shape in the form of Naturalism. Chekhov’s plays

are often considered to be examples of Naturalism, though more specifically Whyman

maintains that Chekhov’s plays are ‘part of a movement in ‘New Drama’, the

transitional stage from nineteenth century drama to modernism’ (ibid.: 35). Whyman

describes ‘Naturalist writers’ as those ‘[i]nfluenced by Darwinism [who] opposed

romanticism and attempted to portray scientifically the underlying forces determining

people’s behaviour, that is, heredity or environmental factors’ (ibid.). In The Wadsworth

Anthology of Drama, W.B. Worthen considers Naturalism (and Realism) to be ‘the first

dramatic modes to consider themselves not as expressing the dominant political and

ideological order, but as criticizing the values and institutions of middle-class society’

(Worthen 2004: 535). For Whyman, however, the plays of Chekhov ‘are “naturalist” to

the extent that they question how social environment contributes to the individual’s

development’ (Whyman 2011: 37), although she admits that ‘Chekhov himself rejected

being categorized as a naturalist writer and positioned himself as a realist’ (ibid.).

Vsevolod Meyerhold, one of the original actors in Stanislavsky’s company at the

Moscow Art Theatre (MAT), and a director in his own right, also rejected the idea that

Chekhov’s plays were naturalistic, insisting that ‘Chekhov’s art demands a theatre of

mood’ (Meyerhold in Braun 1998: 32). When describing the MAT’s original production

of Chekhov’s play The Seagull, Meyerhold maintains:

The atmosphere was created, not by the mise en scène, not by the
crickets, not by the thunder of horses’ hooves on the bridge, but by the
sheer musicality of the actors who grasped the rhythm of Chekhov’s
poetry and succeeded in casting a sheen of moonlight over their
creations. (ibid.)

Meyerhold is pointing out that the writing, and the actors’ responses to it, rather than

the naturalistic conventions added by Stanislavsky, were the key to the production’s

94
success, while simultaneously nodding to the artistry of Chekhov’s writing. As

Whyman points out:

While Chekhov’s writing has affinities with the aims of naturalist


writers, and is realist to the extent that his models were from life, in his
own time, his work was described as ‘poetic realism’, indicating that his
writing was informed by a unique artistic vision. (Whyman 2011: 37)

It is worth noting the distinction that while many of the elements of Chekhov’s writing

are realistic, there is still a heightened-poetic element apparent in his work.

As the style of theatre was shifting, so were the demands upon the actor; this

was a period when the concept of psychological realism in acting was beginning to

form, predominantly in the work of Stanislavsky. Peta Tait, in Performing Emotions:

Gender, Bodies, Spaces, in Chekhov's Drama and Stanislavski's Theatre, notes that

‘Chekhov’s major plays were written around the time that the Moscow Art Theatre

(MAT) rejected nineteenth century declamatory delivery with its stylised and

exaggerated gestures, its emotional hyperbole’ (Tait 2002: 89). Previously, playwrights

had been working with stock characters and archetypes. Chekhov, though still using

‘vestiges of these types’ introduced a new complexity and depth to his characters which,

according to Richard Hornby, leaves his characters slightly ‘odd, askew’ (Hornby 2009:

114). Equally, in regard to the emotions of the characters, there were stock physical

actions that were ‘clearly demonstrated, even ‘telegraphed’ to the audience through

external ‘signs’.’ (Allen 2000: 6). Whyman believes that:

Chekhov’s theatre writing forced reconsiderations of what ‘drama’ is as


well as new ideas about acting. One of his methods was to take
conventional forms and subvert them, aiming to challenge the audience’s
expectations. (Whyman 2011: 34)

Audiences were accustomed to receiving certain emotional ‘signals’. According to Tait,

Chekhov ‘rejected a theatre in which there was a prolonged, emotive delivery for more

transitory, flashes of emotion’ (Tait 2002: 109). Both Chekhov, through his writing, and

95
Stanislavsky, through his productions, were challenging existing notions of the

traditional theatre of the period. Chekhov was quoted as exclaiming:

Where – in streets and houses – do you see people tearing about, leaping
up and down, and clutching their heads? Suffering should be expressed
as it is expressed in life – i.e. not with your arms and legs, but by the
tone of voice, or a glance: not by gesticulating, but by grace. Subtle inner
feelings, natural in educated people, must be subtly expressed in an
external form. You will say – stage conditions. But no conditions justify
lies. (Chekhov in Allen 2000: 7)

Hornby suggests that Chekhov’s insistence of the importance of these ‘subtle inner

feelings’ creates another challenge for actors as ‘characters tend not to say what they

mean, but talk about the weather, the landscape, a book they are reading’ (Hornby 2009:

113). Tait agrees, and insists that Chekhov’s plays

…flaunted a traditional dramatic convention of having characters say


what they mean by suggesting that what the character says is not
necessarily what he or she means. Acting takes place in the space
between the character’s dialogue and intended meaning, and that can
change in numerous ways according to the emotional emphasis of the
delivery of the theatre text. (Tait 2002: 108)

This proved a challenge for ‘actors of the old school’ as Worthern notes, ‘because the

characters did not conform to traditional types and the action seemed so indirect and

inconsequential, lacking familiar dramatic rhythms and climaxes’ (Worthen 2004: 539).

Actor and scholar Bella Merlin even suggests that Chekhov’s writing, in particular The

Seagull, ‘was the catalyst which provoked Stanislavsky into applying new laws to the

acting process in order that it too might be structured as an art form’ (Merlin 1999:

224). Stanislavsky and his acting company had to find a new approach to this new kind

of play.

It is well documented, however, that Chekhov and Stanislavsky disagreed quite

frequently on the interpretation of Chekhov’s plays. Ivan Bunin, a close friend of

Chekhov recalls a conversation where Chekhov stated: ‘You tell me that people cry at

my plays…But you are not the only one who does…After all, I did not write them to

make people cry; it was Stanislavsky who made them so worthy of pity’ (Bunin and
96
Marullo 2007: 48). In his memoir, My Life in Art, Stanislavsky mentions that

Chekhov considered Three Sisters to be a ‘cheerful comedy, a farce’, while

Stanislavsky believed it to be a ‘serious drama about Russian life’ (Stanislavsky and

Benedetti 2008b: 374). Whether or not Chekhov was making that statement ironically is

unclear. However, with this particular case study, the important factor in choosing to

use excerpts from Chekhov’s Three Sisters, is that the play is well written and offers

psychologically realistic, rounded and complex characters in emotional situations. The

two case studies that follow this one engage more directly with the demands of the

specific scripts.

Synopsis of Three Sisters

Chekhov began writing Three Sisters in 1899 and the play was first produced at

the MAT in 1901. The play is about the Prozorov children—three sisters Olga, Masha

and Irina, and their brother Andrey. As a family, they were moved from Moscow to a

rural town. With their father’s recent death, it is unlikely that the family will be able to

return to Moscow. Gradually, their way of life begins to crumble. Andrey marries a

local girl, Natasha, who the sisters do not care for, and Natasha becomes the mistress of

their family home. Irina begins the play full of hope, dreaming of falling in love, but

becomes disillusioned with her life and job at the post office. Masha is in an unhappy

marriage with Kulygin, her former teacher, and ends up having an affair with Lieutenant

Colonol Vershinin. Olga, an unmarried schoolteacher, resigns herself to remaining a

spinster. Andrey, also unhappy, must face his failures in life (becoming neither a

professor or a musician) and the fact that Natasha is having an affair and it may even be

likely that their children are not his. The family is thrown into further turmoil when it is

revealed that military will be leaving the town taking Vershinin, and the only social life

for the sisters, with it. Baron Tusenbach, in love with Irina, resigns from the military to

stay with her (although Irina has consented to marry the Baron, she does not love him),
97
but is killed in a duel. Three Sisters contains many difficult emotional situations,

making the text ideal for exploration work of the three techniques being examined in

this case study.

Section II: Methodology and Preliminary Questions

Five actors participated in the first case study. They ranged in age from 20-44,

and their acting experiences varied from education to professional. The actors included:

Eric Hetzler, an American actor with the most professional experience, who had

recently completed his PhD at the University of Exeter; Jeremy West, also American,

and also with extensive professional acting experience who was completing his MFA in

Staging Shakespeare; Zofia Sozanska, a Polish national in her mid-20s, who had just

completed the MA in Theatre Practice and had trained with Phillip Zarrilli. Lai SimSim,

from Hong Kong, who was also training on the MA in Theatre Practice; and Joe

Sellman-Leava, from the UK, who was in the last year of his undergraduate degree in

Drama. Aside from Hetzler, who once played Kulygin in a professional production of

Three Sisters, none of the other actors had previous experience with Chekhov.

Before beginning the rehearsal process, I started by interviewing each actor

about their views on emotion. I asked the following questions:

1. How do you define ‘emotion’ in relationship to performance? How do you

define ‘emotion’ in relationship to everyday life? Would you say there are

differences, and if so, what are they?

2. How do you usually work on ‘emotion’ in the rehearsal process?

3. In your opinion, how important in ‘emotion’ to a) the rehearsal process and b)

the performance?

98
4. In your own process as a performer, in order to access ‘emotion’ do you use a)

past experiences, b) imagination, c) physiological triggers or d) other. Do you

use any of these approaches in combination? Please explain.

5. What is the biggest obstacle for you when exploring ‘emotion’ in the rehearsal

process or in performance?

The answers that I received from the five individuals illustrated five very

different perspectives. This was probably due to the extensive diversity of cultural

backgrounds, training backgrounds, and acting experience. The following is a short

excerpt from each interview:

Hetzler: Emotion is a very loaded term. If I start with talking about emotion in
life, it’s a response to the things that happen. It’s a very instantaneous
response to something that’s just occurred…it can be joyful, it can be
sad, it can be…anything like that, and I think in performance I would
define it very similarly, because emotion is a response to the thing that
happens. […] It’s simply the response…it’s just what comes out based
on the stimulus that you’re given. So that’s how I would describe it in
both…I don’t really see any difference. (Hetzler 2009b)

West: Emotion in performance is a by-product of conflicting intentions, or not


necessarily conflicting. Sometimes I suppose, intentions that are working
together, which you might see in a romantic situation on stage, would be,
the intentions are actually working together. But nonetheless, two
intentions together, and the emotion is the by-product of those intentions.
[…] In life, I don’t really think it’s that different. I would imagine that I
might define it in the same way. Emotion in life is, more of a driving
force, I suppose, because in performance we know the action from the
text before we go into this environment and pretend we don’t know
what’s coming but we do. (West 2009b)

Sozanska: Emotion is […] I could call it as an “inner flame”. Maybe I’m being too
poetic, but there is something burning inside that guides you, how to
react on certain circumstances. So that’s helped me to define emotions,
helped me to define where I am, in here, now, in the moment. So let’s
say if something happened, if something was burning so, by my reaction,
the building of the tension, my heart beating, and the cognitive process,
the way how I perceive what happened, that is emotion…my relation to
what’s going on outside…or inside as well. (Sozanska 2009a)

Lai: I found emotion in life enriched the emotions in performance, but they
are not exactly the same thing. In my performance, the emotions are
more controlled and refined. […] not just random things that you can
express just whatever you want. But in life you can have lots of emotion
coming out. In life [the emotion] is more free. (Lai 2009b)
99
Sellman-Leava:

I think that the main difference would be that like, obviously, in life you
experience [emotions] spontaneously […] and they can be unexpected so
you can just feel them, and the difficulty in performance is trying to
show that emotion. (Sellman-Leava 2009b)

Between the five actors, three had definite ideas about emotion, equating ‘emotion’ with

a response (to a situation), a by-product (of conflicting or coinciding intentions), and an

inner flame. The latter two avoided offering a concrete definition, but noted the

distinction between emotion in life and in performance, mainly that in life emotion is

spontaneous and ‘more free’.

The interviews also revealed that the biggest obstacles that these actors faced

previously, when working on emotion in performance, included: 1) worrying about

whether or not what they were presenting was cliché; 2) finding it difficult to follow a

director’s instructions regarding certain emotions; 3) self-consciousness; 4) the extra

emotions or nerves associated with performing in front of an audience; 5) learning to

trust the company (in terms of being vulnerable around others); and 6) getting ‘off

book’ or learning the lines. The techniques that the actors preferred to use when

approaching emotion included: 1) imagination; 2) imagination in order to create a past

history; 3) work with the body to create rhythms; 4) butoh-like techniques; and 5) and a

variety of techniques depending on the type of dramaturgy or style of play. For the most

part though, none of the actors had a fixed technique for approaching emotion in

rehearsal or performance, but rather used a combination of approaches. West made the

following observation, saying ‘I don’t consider myself a Strasberg actor, a Meisner

actor, a Stanislavsky actor, a Uta Hagen actor…I am an actor who utilizes different

things, whatever the moment needs’ (West 2009a). Hetzler, when asked about his

approach to emotion in rehearsal, responded that he doesn’t have one, explaining:

…the emotion is the character’s response to the situation, and my job as


the actor is to be a vehicle for that character, and so any response should
100
be a truthful one, based on the stimulus I’m given as that character in
that moment. (Hetzler 2009b)

For Hetzler, the experience rehearsing with the other actors, in combination with the

script, should be sufficient for eliciting any required emotion for performance.

In question 4, I mentioned the use of ‘past experiences’, to which most of the

actors had a negative response for a number of reasons. West felt negatively about the

‘emotional hangover’ that could occur from using emotion from a past experience in his

own life, explaining:

I prefer to stay away from that, just because, in my experience, often


times that residual emotion will go off stage with you, and I prefer to
leave my emotion onstage with the character—or the character’s emotion
I should say—I like to be able to walk off stage, drop it, so that I can
come into the next scene fresh, for whatever the moment is. (West
2009b)

For Sellman-Leava, the youngest actor in the group, finding an appropriate memory

from his own past experience proved difficult:

I used to try and…try by thinking of something sad…which is difficult


because I don’t have that much in my life which makes me like, that
bad…and also, I think it just gets in the way…cause you effectively have
to stop what you’re doing on stage to think, to dig up something from the
past, and then make yourself cry and I just don’t think that’s practical.
(Sellman-Leava 2009b)

Similar to Sellman-Leava, both Hetlzer and Lai also mentioned the problem of

disconnecting from one’s scene partner in order to connect with a past experience.

There is a whole other bunch of people on stage with you, who are
depending on you to respond when we need you to respond and to keep
the energy flowing in the scene as a group or as a pair, and if you “check
out” because you have to, you know, be emotionally ready, well then
you’re not contributing to the work. You’re not a part of this group on
stage. As an actor you can actually feel the energy go, because suddenly
there’s a hole over there…[the actor is] deep inside their own self.
(Hetzler 2009b)

For Lai, the remembering of a past memory could inform her choices, but should not be

used directly:

And for the past experience I try not to use so much…because again, I
think a lot, just about myself, my experience, and then […] I’m not the
101
character at all if I’m feeling this way. But I think the past experience
will help me to understand, like common sense. I understand, for
example, if [the character] has a quarrel with her mother….I have a
quarrel with my mother as well, and I understand what feeling it is, but I
won’t recall the whole of my experience of quarrelling with my mother
to replace…the rehearsals. (Lai 2009b)

I mention these reflections because in comparison to any technique suggested, the use

of past memory triggered noticeably passionate responses from all but one actor,

Sozanska, who has never used the technique and preferred imagination or physical

work.

My specific research questions for this project are the same as those guiding the

overall research project which are: What, if anything, can be revealed about the nature

of emotion in Western performance when employing these three theories/approaches to

emotion in practice? Do these discoveries vary when these approaches are put into

practice on different dramaturgies with different demands on the actor? What is the

nature of the emotion being expressed? Whose emotion is it? The actor’s or the

character’s? Is there a difference? Can these techniques assist a director in unifying an

understanding of emotion within a diverse cast? How much of the actor’s previous

beliefs, training and acting experiences affect their views on emotion in performance.

How do these previously held views/assumption interact with the three theories used?

Are the feelings/emotions that actors generate ‘genuine’? If so, how does the actor

generate, control, and integrate these emotions in performance? And specifically in

relation to the first case study, how can I, as a director, incorporate these methods into

an effective rehearsal process and what role in that process do they serve?

I selected seven scenes, though only five scenes were selected for the

performance in order to keep the total running time at 30 minutes. The excerpts

included:

102
1) From Act II: the scene between Masha and Vershinin, when he confesses his

love for her and the affair really begins. With Sozanska as Masha and Hetlzer as

Vershinin.

2) From Act III: the scene between Irina and Olga (and Masha), when Irina

complains about her boredom and anger with life and Olga admits she would

marry any man. With Lai as Irina and Sozanska as Masha.

3) From Act III: a continuation from the previous scene, when Masha confesses her

love for Vershinin to her sisters. With Sozanska as Masha and Lai as Irina.

4) From Act III: a continuation from the two previous scenes, in which the Baron

enters looking for Irina. With West as the Baron, Lai as Irina, and Sozanska as

Olga.

5) From Act IV: Andrey’s monologue to his deaf servant Ferapont, pushing a child

in a pram while discussing his views on life and his relationship with Natasha.

With Sellman-Leava as Andrey.

6) From Act IV: the scene between Irina and the Baron, in which Tusanbach wants

some indication of her love for him, which she cannot give. He goes off to fight

a duel with Solyony in which he will lose his life. With West as the Baron and

Lai as Irina.

7) From Act IV: the scene between Masha and Vershinin, in which they have to

say goodbye. With Sozanska as Masha, Hetzler as Vershinin and Lai as Olga.

Throughout the chapter there will be opportunities to watch these scenes in rehearsal

and performance. When appropriate, the reader will be directed to the corresponding

DVD.

103
Section III: Rehearsals

Once the initial interviews were conducted, the rehearsal process began. The

rehearsal process ran over the course of three weeks, totaling around 75 hours. The

different techniques overlapped throughout the process, though for clarity I will discuss

each strand of work - Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work, and Impulse and

Awareness Work – separately.

Incorporating Alba Emoting

I began by introducing the Alba patterns first for two reasons: 1) the patterns are

specific and have a basis in physiology, rather than psychology (for example, rather

than having to depend on an individual’s unique and subjective idea of what ‘sadness’

may be, Alba offered concrete physiological steps); and 2) perhaps through them we

could create a common vocabulary. In order to use Alba Emoting in the rehearsal

process, it was first necessary to teach the actors the emotional effector patterns. Unlike

with the other techniques, the early ‘training’ process of Alba Emoting is completely

independent from the rehearsals of the text. After the initial interviews with the actors, I

began by teaching the Alba Emoting patterns over a period of six hours. This started

with the introduction of ‘neutral breath’ and the step-out technique, which, as

mentioned in Chapter 2, are the ‘safeguards’ for the Alba Emoting technique. The

neutral breath pattern in Alba Emoting is devoid of any of the same elements as the

emotional effector patterns, and thus acts as a method of ‘clearing’ any unwanted

emotion. Again, the six emotional effector patterns are tenderness (1a), anger (1b),

eroticism (2a), fear (2b), joy (3a), and sadness (3b). I prefer to use the number/letter

system that Alba Emoting instructor Laura Facciponti Bond created, rather than

referring to the patterns by name. To see an example of each pattern, please go to:

104
• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 1. The Alba Emoting Patterns (Duration:
00:01:58)

The actors’ initial experience with the emotional effector patterns is very

important as this allows them to recognize each pattern for themselves. My own

experiences when learning Alba Emoting were in workshops where the technique was

integrated with the Feldenkrais Method, and my process when teaching Alba Emoting

has been influenced accordingly. By introducing the patterns when the actors are lying

on the floor, they have the opportunity to explore the patterns, in their own space,

without the immediate self-conscious pressure of being observed. Once I introduced the

breathing pattern, I asked them to observe within themselves, or sense, whether or not

any other muscles wanted to move in accordance with the breathing pattern. This is an

opportunity for the actor to recognize what the pattern may be, to have a somatic

experience. Slowly then, I introduced the other elements of the pattern, such as the

facial expression and postural attitude. Often I find it necessary to repeat or rephrase

instructions if I see an actor using completely different muscles in the face, or if an actor

is seriously misinterpreting the instructions. However, I do not make corrections at this

early stage, as I want the actor to discover on their own their relationship with each

emotional effector pattern. After exploring the patterns on the floor, I then had the

actors explore the patterns in a sitting position as well. (When I say ‘explore’ the

patterns, I was continually repeating the instructions so they did not have to rely on

memory). I have found that actors may be able to better recognize or connect with the

emotional effector patterns in different positions. I wanted the actors to have the

experience of each pattern, both lying down on the floor and in a sitting position, before

we came together to discuss what each emotional effector pattern may be.

As explained previously, I do not name the pattern until the actors have

experienced it several times. Certain patterns are obvious to the actors from the first

105
encounter, while others can be more elusive, and that difference is down to each

individual. For this case study, it was only after I taught each pattern separately, and

then reviewed all six, did we discuss what each emotional effector pattern might be.

When discussing patter 3a (sadness), Hetzler had the following response:

I knew this one would be coming. And it’s something I’ve been thinking
about a lot. I started thinking about when was the last time I actually
cried. And it’s more than 30 years since […] So I was actually terrified,
because I don’t know if I even remember how, what will happen?
Especially if you’re working with emotion memory, God knows what
would’ve happened. So there was a little bit of trepidation, and as soon
as you mentioned the three inhalations I thought “oh shit, there it is”.
(Beck et al. 2009e)

However, for Hetzler, using the Alba Emoting pattern proved to be a safe way explore

tears:

For me, knowing that there was a pattern involved, made me feel safer.
It doesn’t feel like this personal emotional release that you get from that
situation. It’s more like, “okay, I’m just breathing a pattern, purely
psychophysical, and I can do this.” (ibid.)

I did not want the actors to focus on achieving an end-goal, such as laughter or tears

simply because I use the terminology ‘joy’ or ‘sadness’, but rather, to focus on the

somatic experience of each pattern in that moment. For this reason I refer to the

emotions as simply patterns rather than by name. In some cases, as with Hetzler, this

training is enabling the actor to reconnect or rediscover the dialogue between body and

mind through the emotional effector patterns without the psychological implications of

exploring ‘sadness’.

I had the actors revisit the Alba Emoting patterns at the start of each rehearsal.

Developing coordination between the three elements of each pattern—breathing pattern,

facial expression, and postural attitude—can take time. Differentiating muscles and

components of the patterns from one another can also takes time and practice. We

repeated the patterns every day at the start of rehearsal to reinforce the breath and

muscle coordination, until the actors’ were proficient in each pattern. Sometimes new

106
positions may ‘unlock’ integration with the pattern. Sometimes words or objects also

help this integration. For Sozanska, her first induction with pattern 3a (joy) came only

after exploring the pattern with an object, in this case, a sock. Sozanska reflected:

I remember the object. It felt very good. Suddenly this pattern was alive,
it wasn’t just a pattern […] it was so real for me. I didn’t need any
context to play with the object, the context it just appear [sic]. And my
laugh, my 3a [joy], or 2b [fear], was much more authentic, I could relate
to something. (Sozanska 2009c)

To watch a clip of Sozanska in the object exercise, please see:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 2. Sozanska in Alba Emoting Object Exercise


(Duration: 00:00:22)

Once the actors were comfortable enough with the three aspects of each pattern,

we began ‘sharing’ patterns. Sitting in chairs across from each other, the actors would

begin a pattern, and then begin to ‘mirror’ each other. They could begin to see in each

other the ways in which they may or may not be mixing emotional effector patterns. As

the actors developed a stronger coordination with the patterns, we were able to share

more often, having the actors interact with each other using opposing patterns. After this

point I begin to incorporate text from the play. This enabled the actors to experience

how the breathing/posture/facial expressions can affect the text.

Once the actors were experienced enough with sharing, objects, and text, they

were ready for what Bond refers to as ‘Alba opera’ – an improvisation using the

patterns, objects, and text. There is usually a fixed guide – for example, the actor is

limited to a particular piece of text, or the patterns are called out for the actor. I take this

one step further and combine it with the impulse and awareness work, which will be

discussed further in the Impulse and Awareness section. The Alba improvisation

exercise proved to be important for the actors who were still struggling with the

physical coordination of the three components. West found the process more organic,

commenting after the exercise that ‘it was actually a lot easier for me to go further into
107
some of the patterns. […] SimSim [Lai] and I were in one pattern, and her change into

another pattern prompted my change, which then prompted another’ (Beck et al.

2009d). Lai reported that within the Alba improvisation exercise she was surprised at

how changing between the Alba Emoting patterns triggered lines and images from the

text, rather than vice versa which is more typical an experience:

That one was very very interesting. The experience was so…how should
I describe it?…from the patterns. For example, you just name a pattern,
we did it, then images, or text, from Three Sisters comes up suddenly. So
it’s not, start from the text and then we have the emotion and da da da da,
it’s from the emotion. There are things coming automatically. […] It just
so surprised me, why I come up with this text, with this certain patterns
or emotion? [sic] (Lai 2009d)

For an example of the Alba improvisation exercise, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 3. Alba Emoting Improvisation (Duration:


00:00:58)

In two cases the experience of Alba Emoting changed the actors previously held

views on emotion, with West and Sellman-Leava. In an interview, West reflected:

I’m surprised that so much of the emotion we feel in life, on stage, is


physiological versus this other ethereal sort of placement that we never
know exactly where it is. Because people talk about love being in your
heart, fear in your stomach, whatever, people say “oh, that really got me
in the gut”, or whatever. It’s just amazing to think how much of it is
actually in the face and in the breath. (West 2009c)

Though the introduction of Alba Emoting caused a shift in West’s views on emotion, he

was still unsure of the technique as an actor’s tool midway through the process:

While I can greatly respect it as a scientific truth, the human physiology,


how much of it is useful or needed for the individual actor as a pre-
performative training method? I’m still discovering for myself. So I do
sort of dissect the two areas. One may have me completely convinced,
but the other one is still…I do have some reservations to be perfectly
honest. Not because I don’t think it’s not effective, obviously, but for me
as an individual actor, I have some reservations about is this going to be
more effective than other training methods that I’ve studied before.
(ibid.)

108
There was an initial sense from a few members in the group that the techniques I was

exposing them to were somehow in ‘competition’ with the techniques and tools they

used previously. This was not my intention, and as the rehearsal process progressed

these attitudes began to shift.

Aside from the general training of Alba Emoting, the technique was applied to

the rehearsal process and/or performance in four distinct ways. One approach was to

integrate the use of the Alba Emoting patterns into the impulse runs, which will be

discussed in more detail in the section on impulse and awareness work. The other three

uses included direct placement in the script for performance, using the Alba Emoting

patterns as specific vocabulary for a common understanding, and for the actors’ own

preparation for performance.

The following is an example of how I used the Alba Emoting technique in a

direct, prescriptive way when working on the scene between Masha and Vershinin in

Act II. The scene starts mid-sentence, as the couple return to the house from an event.

Because the actors needed the appropriate energy, I had them begin with the pattern of

3a (joy). In his follow up interview after the performance, Hetzler reported this as a

useful application of the technique:

The Alba, it helped with the initial energy of the scene, doing the 3a,
because I think there is always that tension of trying to jump right into a
scene that is that heightened without having anything before that. If you
are doing the whole play, you’ve worked to that scene, and your mind
and body are ready, from the character’s perspective to go to this next
level, because you’ve had the initial entrance, and the meeting of Masha,
[…] so, having the 3a was I think very helpful just getting us up, so that
we could get on our feet and do the scene with the level of intensity that
was required for the two of them, having come all the way from where
ever it was, and they are alone for the first time, and they are really
excited about it. (Hetzler 2009a)

To watch a clip of Sozanska and Hetzler using Alba Emoting to begin the scene in

rehearsal, please go to:

109
• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 4. Sozanska and Hetzler using 3a (Duration:
00:00:37)

Figure 9: Eric Hetzler and Zofia Sozanska. Photograph by Jen Burton.

Alba Emoting was also used as a specific vocabulary, particularly with Sellman-

Leava, who was playing Andrey. Sellman-Leava had a tendency to play his monologue

with an overriding presence of 3a (sadness), which was not entirely intentional. I used

the Alba Emoting patterns to increase his awareness of the emotions he was displaying.

By becoming more aware of what he was doing, Sellman-Leava was then in a position

to consciously change the emotions he was displaying. In Sellman-Leava’s follow-up

interview, he maintained:

…you [Beck] were able to say to me for example, what pattern are you
predominantly using? And that was a better way, for me […] because I
think if you would’ve said to me, and we hadn’t done any Alba work,
“you’re too sad for the whole thing”, that would have been true, but it
wouldn’t have been as easy to kind of rectify it. […] because with Alba,
I was able to know where physiologically I was going wrong, rather than
“okay, I’m too sad, I need to think…” (Sellman-Leava 2009a)

110
This was a surprising discovery for me. I had not realized just how useful Alba

Emoting could be for the actor to specifically address the physiological components of a

pattern, rather than have to rethink their views on the portrayal of a character. To watch

a clip of Sellman-Leava working on Andrey’s monologue, please see:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 5. Sellman-Leava Rehearsing (Duration:


00.05.27)

Figure 10: Joe Sellman-Leava as Andrey. Photograph by Jen Burton.

I also encouraged the actors to experiment with Alba Emoting in their

performance, if they were comfortable using the technique. West became more

confident with Alba Emoting as the rehearsal process continued. The following is

West’s account of how he was able to use the Alba Emoting patterns in the

performance:

I experimented a bit with the Alba patterns, but not to the full effect,
partially because of the way it was staged, I didn’t want to be sitting on
the side and start going into a pattern which might possibly pull focus
from the performance that was going on stage. So what I was doing,
111
essentially I was toning down everything and almost internalizing it…
the facial expressions – I was using 3b [sadness] – and the facial
expressions I was using were probably not drastic enough that it could
have been seen, perhaps, by someone from the outside, but there was
enough for the physiological triggers, I think, to allow me to go to a
certain preparatory state. Again, not using the full pattern, I wasn’t
attempting to put myself fully in that emotive state, but hinting at it, so
that when I walked on stage I almost had a charge of that emotion, so I
could very easily go into it, as opposed to walking on in a neutral state
and then having this limited time within the text to go from neutral to
this position. It was difficult because of that constraint, had I been able to
be offstage, I think, separate from the audience I might have been able to
fully do this with the patterns. But I found, and experimenting with the
patterns, mostly breathing, just as I begin to feel the pattern take a certain
hold, and then have a reaction, I would then clear the pattern with neutral
breath, and then immediately go back into the breathing pattern, so that
was almost, in my mind, almost constantly sort of pushing myself to the
edge of that and pulling myself back. So that my hope, and what I think,
was the achieved goal, was to place myself emotionally as if a very
precariously placed vase on the edge of a table where the scene and the
scene partner are responsible to cause the jarring that will knock the vase
onto the floor to break. I didn’t want to be broken before I walked into
the scene, I wanted the Baron to be so easily affected by Irina that this is
what I experimented with, and it seemed to work, it seemed to put me
into this position, again, of being precariously perched so that when she
would say those words I was allowed, as the scene partner, to actually be
affected by them much quicker and much deeper than had I walked on
just in a neutral state. (West 2009a)

West’s account is significant because he was able to incorporate Alba Emoting into the

preparation of his performance, even with the staging constraints of the performance

setting.

112
Figure 11: Lai SimSim and Jeremy West. Photograph by Jen Burton.

Incorporating Emotional Access Work

As mentioned previously in Chapter 2, the contributory exercises in Astbury’s

Emotional Access Work, operate together in a series of stages: 1) under-reading the

scene, 2) anger run, 3) energy run, 4) physical exercises 5) return to the scene with

under-reading. We did this with each scene. For an example of some of these stages,

please see:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 6. Astbury’s Physical Exercises (Duration:


00:01:18)

When moving through the physical exercises the scene is under-read repeatedly.

However, it is then my responsibility to orchestrate the under-reading. That is to say, if I

observe the actor beginning to have a connection with the text, I will have the under-

reader repeat that line continuously so that the actor can explore or deepen that

connection. When I perceive that this connection has been explored adequately, I have
113
the under-reader move on with the text. The exercises are usually brought to an end

when the actor has peaked. This is not an exact science, but rather based on my intuition

from my experience training with Astbury. As a caveat I should mention that I do not

always get the timing right; however, there does seem to be a natural crescendo. After

this crescendo, I bring the actors down to a whisper, cease the physical exercises and

allow the scene to wind down to a natural close.

I used this process for most of the scenes and monologues with the exception of

the scenes between Irina and Olga, and Olga and Masha from Act III. When it came to

working with Sozanska and Lai, there was no need to move beyond the energy run into

the physical exercises. When they started the energy run, they were immediately

making connections to each other and the text, responding with their whole selves.

Perhaps this was due to the fact that both actors shared a common actor training and had

worked together previously. In this instance, it was not necessary to push them further

at this stage. After the energy run, both actors responded positively to the experience,

citing two distinct aspects of the work. Sozanska reported on her surprise of the choices

that she made in the energy run:

It’s so unexpectable [sic]…so out of any…some phrase, or some


reaction, I would never consider, I would never plan it. She surprised me
so many times, it’s like being in a real story, I mean it.[…] It’s so… you
really want to react. (Beck et al. 2009f)

Lai was interested in the textual discoveries that she made about the characters through

the physical exploration:

But what we did just now, I found the relationship as sisters, much more
complicated than I did when I just read the script. Because when I read
it, it’s supposed to be, lots of love, […] but when I read it there wasn’t
hatred and jealousy, and I think with the anger put on that, I think it’s so
cruel, it sounds so nice “oh darling”, but it’s not, it’s exactly the
opposite, “I really hate you.” (ibid.)

Sozanska also made some discoveries about the character of Olga and her relationship

with Irina. In Act III, when Irina is describing herself as becoming ‘thin and ugly and

114
old’ (Friel 2000: 63), Sozanska reflected, ‘when I’m Olga, I think [Irina’s] talking

about me. When she’s talking about who she will become, it’s me, it’s me’ (ibid.). The

experience also changed her initial impression of the character of Olga:

For me, as Olga, Olga is very supple, [but] actually Olga is very fragile
at that moment, and before I look at Olga as someone very tough and
cold, and maybe she’s hiding something inside, the disappointment, or
bitterness, but here, she is so fragile, exposed to every word that [Irina]
say[s]. (ibid.)

To watch the scene between Sozanska and Lai after the anger/energy run, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 7. Sozanska and Lai after Anger/Energy Run


(Duration: 00:08:12)

115
Figure 12: Zofia Sozanska as Olga and Lai SimSim as Irina. Photograph by Jen Burton.

116
It is discoveries like these that the actors find when exploring the text on their

feet, that interests me most as a director. Many of the exercises from Astbury’s

Emotional Access Work, as well as the impulse runs in the Impulse and Awareness

Work, can be used similarly to Stanislavsky’s active analysis. In the drafts section of An

Actor’s Work on a Role, Stanislavsky discusses the discoveries that he and another actor

made while exploring a scene on their feet using active analysis. Stanislavsky reflects:

We needed logic and sequence in our actions and feelings, we had to


discover the truth in the [sic], create belief, the “I am being”, etc. But, to
do that, we didn’t sit at the table, with our heads in a book, we didn’t
divide the play into bits, pencil in hand, we got up on stage and did
things, looked into the facts, into our own nature to find what was
helpful. (Stanislavsky and Benedetti 2010: 68)

This may come as a surprise to those who associate Stanislavsky’s work with that of

sitting around a table, ‘dividing the play into bits’. He continues:

In other words, we did not analyse our actions coldly, with our heads,
theoretically but approached them practically, using life, human
experience, habits, our artistic and other kinds of flair, intuition, the
subconscious, etc. We looked for what we needed to fulfil physical and
other kinds of actions. Nature came to our aid and guided us. Look into
this process and you will find it was an inner and outer analysis of
ourselves as human beings in the life of the role. This process is nothing
like the cold, cerebral study of a role which actors usually take at the
beginning stages of work. [original emphasis] (ibid.).

While Stanislavsky is specifically discussing his technique of active analysis, there are

similarities between his work and Astbury’s in this way.

The next scene to be rehearsed in this way was the scene between Irina and the

Baron before he leaves to have a duel with Solyony. West and Lai responded positively

to the experience. According to West:

I was quite surprised actually, how quickly, and how easily accessible
everything was after we’d done the [physical exercises]. Yeah, I was
quite surprised how sort of everything was on the surface and you could
really… it’s like you are still in control, at least I never felt like there was
a moment where I was being led by my emotions, I was able to choose
the direction I wanted to go with the text, and how I interpreted the text,
at least at this particular phase in rehearsal, but everything seemed to be
sort of on the surface and it was like guiding a ship in the water, where
you don’t need much, you don’t have to force anything, you don’t have
117
to push. […] It was freeing, because I could really listen to what my
scene partner was giving me to react to, and trying to really be focused in
the moment without having to think about technical stuff. (Beck et al.
2009g)

For an example of the final run of this scene after the exercises, please see:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 8. Lai and West after Physical Exercises


(Duration: 00:09:40)

With the scene between Masha and Vershinin, the differences in the actors’

previous training backgrounds and philosophies came to a point of tension. Sozanska

found her first experience with the physical exercises useful:

I think that the exercises are very powerful. […] because on one side, it’s
activating my body, [….] I feel like I’m engaging fully with what I’m
doing. But also, it breaks that psychological barrier, by actually relating
to the pain, to the tension in my body. […] It’s excellent for an actor.
(Beck et al. 2009a)

Hetzler, however, disagreed:

On the one hand I see the usefulness if you are dealing with actors who
don’t have a sense of letting go, breaking through that psychological
barrier that we sometimes set for ourselves, especially on something as
intimate as this. […] For me personally, I don’t have that need anymore,
I’m not worried about it because I just get there. (ibid.)

Sozanska challenged Hetzler on his immediate dismissal of the physical exercises,

asking ‘Don’t you think that it helps you explore wider possibilities for you and the

character? And you act as you wouldn’t act with your experience. It’s like playing with

possibilities’ (ibid.). But Hetzler insisted:

The possibilities are always there, and this is what I’m saying now, is
that I’ve reached a point where I am free to go and do whatever happens.
That kind of, self-censorship I had when I started has gone away over the
years. Where I can come in and go and do it. I really don’t think about it
anymore. I do almost no homework. (ibid.)

This was Hetzler’s initial impression of the physical exercises, which only changed

slightly by the end of the process. It was in this moment that I perceived that these

118
exercises could be regarded as an affront to an actor’s existing process. Hetzler then

raised a very important concern:

The thing that concerns me is, yeah, you get these really cool things that
happen, but they are so fleeting, because they happen with so much
physical intensity and emotional intensity, my concern for the actor
trying to find them again. Because you’re going to go away, and then
come back again, and there’s been this moment, and everyone wants it,
and go “Oh my God”, and you yourself might go “that was just an
amazing breakthrough”, but now you’ve got to bring it back. And in
performance, we’re never going to do that. […] I just wonder if there’s a
fear that comes from “can I get this again?” (ibid.)

I responded to Hetzler by explaining my view of the physical exercises, that they are ‘an

abstract exploration of all of the stuff that feeds into the complex natures of some of

what’s going on in the relationships between characters. The idea is not to look for it

again’ (ibid.) That is the answer that Astbury offers when asked the same question, and

an answer that I understood from my experience as an actor working with these

techniques. However, in this case I realized how ambiguous that response can be for an

actor, especially in contrast to the very specific vocabulary of the Alba Emoting

technique. Hetzler did admit that ‘possibly what could have damaged [him] going into

this’ (ibid.), was his internal ‘gaze’. He explained: ‘I found myself, throughout all of

this, examining it. Examining my reactions to it and having that extra set of eyes going

“oh, that’s really cool, let’s explore that the more”’ (ibid.) In a later interview Sozanska,

commented on the experience of the exercises with Hetzler:

He hold back, I felt he hold back a lot [sic]. He didn’t choose to go with
me, and that was, wow, a big discovery. And even now when I’m doing
with Eric the impulse run, it’s the same [sic]. [...] When I was playing
with SimSim [Lai], this energy would be shared. It would come from us
rather than just from me. (Sozanska 2009b)

From my perspective as a director, I would agree with Sozanska’s observation. If I

sense that an actor is ‘resisting’ the exercises (i.e. trying to control their delivery of the

text), whether consciously or unconsciously, I do not ‘push’ them further with the

physical exercises. Usually, and in the case of Hetzler, they begin to relax as the

119
rehearsal process continues. To watch the scene between Hetzler and Sozanska after

the exercises please go to:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 9. Hetzler and Sozanska after Physical Exercises


(Duration: 00:07:30)

In the second week, I began using Astbury’s physical metaphor, which works

similarly to the physical exercises, but using a physical representation of the character’s

experience. To watch a brief example of these exercises go to:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 10. Physical Metaphor Exercises (Duration:


00:01:50)

For the scene from Act III between Irina and the Baron (which was not included in the

actual performance), West was instructed to physically try and hold on to Lai, while she

was instructed to escape. West reported that:

…just repeating a certain line or a certain word, just physically holding


[Lai], it became exhausting for me […] so there is all this kind of
frustration and such being built up emotionally. […] I think perhaps it’s
applied well [here] because of their extreme difference in their
relationship. Irina is not in love with him and he is 100% in love with
her, and so that kind of contrast I think lends itself well to that sort of
physical exertion and pulling away. (West 2009d)

The idea behind the physical metaphor exercise is that the experience and memory of

that experience will be useful or inform the performance of the scene. Lai found:

…in that last run, I still had the memory, every line, every sentence that
he said it’s like he was binding me. And [I was] trying to escape […]
“What should I do?” He’s coming closer and it’s so frustrating, and I
suppose it is frustrating for Irina as well. (Beck et al. 2009c)

To watch the run to which Lai is referring, please see:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 11. Lai and West after Physical Metaphor
(Duration: 00:03:46)

120
In the final scene, when Masha must say goodbye to Vershinin, Sozanska was

restrained by other members of the group, but given the objective to try and get to

Hetzler, who was standing just out of her reach. Sozanska found the exercise to be

‘strong’ (Sozanska 2009c), and added ‘I think they can be strong for anyone who will

open to them [the exercises]’ (ibid.). But when discussing the exercise further, Sozanska

raised an important point. For Sozanska:

…it was such an intensive experience, that I couldn’t really connect it to


Masha. Actually, it was so strong that I […] forgot about Masha. Even I
would repeat the sentence. It’s so real, that situation, and the adrenaline
and the tension, so that, I feel like me. (ibid.)

This was a potential insight into the question of whether or not the ‘emotion’ involved

belongs to the actor or the character, which will be discussed in more detail in Section

IV. To watch the scene between Hetzler and Sozanska after the exercise go to:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 12. Hetzler and Sozanska after Physical


Metaphor (Duration: 00:03:54)

121
Figure 13: Eric Hetzler, Zofia Sozanska as Masha and Lai SimSim as Olga. Photograph by Jen
Burton.

Another Astbury technique that I used in the rehearsal process was image

streaming, which made little impact on the group in regard to emotion. However, Lai’s

image stream is worth mentioning because she addresses an important point. Lai’s

image stream about Irina was very dark, including a gruesome wedding ceremony in

which Irina begins to remove her own fingers, and Lai admitted: ‘I’m surprised myself

with how I connect to this character. And definitely I found it related to me, and why I

have this connection’ (Lai 2009c). Lai then referred to past memories and how

sometimes a director would ask her how she connected to a character and ‘found it a bit

weird to share’ (ibid.) because the connection was ‘private’ (ibid.). With image

streaming, however Lai felt comfortable sharing her experience because:

…it’s just my imagination. It’s my imagination in this way, and it’s so


strong, and I find it really helps me to understand [Irina] as well. Because
the image for me, of the cupcake, of the wedding, the finger, pulling off
the fingers, is so strong, and actually when I rehearse these things keep
coming up. (ibid.)

122
Sozanska, who has had training in butoh, was interested in exploring ‘what would

happen if [you] took the images and did a butoh-like exercise’ (Sozanska 2009b),

finding the exercise ‘excellent to catch some images to play with’ (ibid.). However,

Sozanska did note a difficulty with the technique reporting, ‘I felt very limited to what I

could say because of my language limitation’ (ibid.). As Sozanska’s first language is

Polish, there were some images that she could not accurately describe in English, so in

order to overcome this she would simplify the description. I suggested that if she

wanted to explore the technique further, she should record herself in her original

language, and then listen to what she discovers, rather than translating into English for

the sake of the actor writing down her image stream.

The technique of under-reading was used in conjunction with the physical

exercises and physical metaphor, but stirred strong responses from the actors as a

technique unto itself. Hetzler found under-reading to be a way of ‘short-circuiting a

week of rehearsal’ (Beck et al. 2009a), and expanded further in an interview that ‘you

have both of your hands, which is fantastic. What you can’t do any other way’ (Hetzler

2009c). Sozanska reported that ‘under reading helped me to connect better with my

partner. It takes the weight from thinking about the lines and how I should say

something’ (Sozanska 2009b). West reflected that under-reading allowed him to

…explore emotional relationship and scene partner connection, which I


greatly enjoy, being in the moment and being able to take whatever wave
of emotion, or impulse, or connections, that you have with your scene
partner, however valid they are for the scene, that we have these
workshops where you can just go with it, and someone else is feeding
you the lines, so it’s very freeing. And it allows you to discover things
about relationships, for me, I should say, it allows me to discover things
about a relationship and scene play and possible options for
performance. (West 2009c)

Incorporating Impulse and Awareness work

The aim of using the Impulse and Awareness work in rehearsals for excerpts

from Three Sisters, was to develop a stronger connection between the actors who were
123
scene partners, in order to develop a ‘trust’ with one another, so that they were

comfortable and attuned to respond to their emotional impulses in performance. The

impulse and awareness sessions were specifically placed throughout the rehearsal

process in order to gradually build up to an ‘impulse run’, which also incorporates

under-reading and the Alba Emoting patterns. The first few exercises initially focused

on the actors’ attention, first on their own internal energy, the relationship with their

scene partner, and then opened out to include their awareness of the space. I each

instance I guided the actors through each exercise. For a transcript of an early impulse

run, please see Appendix B. Hetzler described an early impulse exercise as ‘really

intense’ (Beck et al. 2009b). He continued:

It starts out simple with breathing, and mirroring, and exercises we’ve all
done – part of acting 101 – but when you get in sync like that, and I think
the lack of eye contact […] you focus in even more, and I think you get a
much deeper connection. Because then when you do start to make
contact it’s almost impossible to break unless you have a really good
reason to. (ibid.)

To watch an excerpt of the exercise to which Hetzler is referring, please see:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 13. Excerpt of Impulse and Awareness Exercise


(Duration: 00:01:43)

The impulse and awareness exercises can also serve as a form of active analysis. In this

particular exploration West had the following experience:

In some of the quicker moments, […] the quick impulses that [Lai]
would have, would naturally make me want to withdraw, instead of
responding back, I think because of where I’m discovering that the
character is in his relationship to her, but I almost get the feeling that he
has the kind of admiration for her that she could yell at him and beat him
in the face and do whatever she wanted to, and he would just stand there
and take it because he loves her so much. (ibid.)

West’s observation, in conjunction with the impulse work, prompted a similar

revelation about the play for Hetzler:

I think is something in the way the Chekhov wrote the men in this play,
because they are, for all of Vershinin’s love for [Masha], he still, until
124
she does something, is kind of stuck. And then poor Andrey here… so
there is that kind of backing off. I felt that a lot too, until we started
getting a little deeper into it, and then we were a lot more in sync. (ibid.)

Hetzler refers to ‘poor Andrey’ because in the impulse work Sellman-Leava had to

work on his own, with an imaginary partner, and then tried to connect with the other

actors who were working in partners. I asked him what the experience was like, and he

responded, ‘difficult, because I was sort of having a connection to one person and then

have to go to another. And it was the two girls, it wasn’t the men really. I didn’t feel

connected to them, sorry.’ (ibid.). The impulse and awareness work was contributing to

the actors’ understanding of the relationships between their characters in the play.

Sozanska reflected that ‘when [Sellman-Leava] started crying I really felt like [his]

sister… all this love story developing, [referring to her relationship with Vershinin] but

my brother is crying’ (ibid.). At which point, Hetzler added:

The funny thing was, I couldn’t get mad at him, and I couldn’t get mad
at [Sozanska], so I had to walk away and kind of just stay with [her],
keep my connection, but thinking “God, that’s sad.” [Sellman-Leava]
looked really pathetic. (ibid.)

The impulse and awareness work was developed over the course of the rehearsal

process, and the actors grew stronger as a company, responding to one another, as the

process further developed. Sozanska commented that

…within the repetition it was more interesting as it become more


complex and we were more engaged in what we are doing and somehow
free ourselves and what we are doing, as a group. So I could see, and I
could sense the development of our group. (Sozanska 2009c)

Lai also noted a development in the group dynamic:

We’re getting together. There’s understanding, but we haven’t talked


about it. […] When I pose a question there will be an answer from
someone, for example my question is one action, or one gesture, or one
text [sic]. And I will get response from the other actors, which is
corresponding, really answering “answering”. That is very strong.
Particularly in the group impulse run. Before that day, I feel not so
together. (Lai 2009d)

125
The impulse and awareness exercises were building to an impulse run, in which the

Alba Emoting patterns could be used and the actors’ lines were under-read. As impulses

can be emotions, the actor could choose the patterns because the patterns engage both

body and mind, they are not limited to being ‘conscious’ decisions, as they become

organic experiences. I will also include sounds or music from the text. In the case of

Three Sisters, I used The Blue Danube by Johann Strauss II, as this piece of music is

featured in the play, as well as the sound of shots firing (from the duel between Solyony

and the Baron), as additional stimulus. Sozanska described the final group impulse run

as ‘excellent’ (Sozanska 2009c), maintaining:

I think we reached the highest level that we could reach as this group. I
think it was good, it was very playful. There is lots of playfulness, which
is always good, because then you search for more, and to give yourself
more opportunity. […] being with the group, it’s so supportive at the
same time because you get so much energy from them, so much
inspiration. You can come back to a pattern, I would come back to the
pattern, an Alba pattern, like the 3b [sadness] or 3a [joy], but it is not the
only way I would let my engine run and run, it rather would be the
connection with the partners which is great, always great. (ibid.)

To watch the final group impulse run, please see:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 1: 14. Final Group Impulse Run (Duration:


00:53:08)

However, Sozanska cited an important concern:

…it was so strong, the exercise, such intuitive work that [it] might be a
problem to come back to that experience. I don’t remember so much.
There are flashes, and my memory in some general feeling, but I even
don’t want to analyze the experience, it is just an experience. I’d like to
believe that it is somewhere in my body memory which will maybe help
me to play Masha. […] is it real that we have this body memory? It’s an
open question. By doing this kind of work can we go and play the scene,
such a realistic play like Chekhov, there’s so much psychology there.[…]
(ibid.)

Sozanska’s question will be discussed further throughout the dissertation. Hetzler found

the impulse work to be ‘very helpful’ (Hetzler 2009a), reporting that ‘it got me more

126
physically engaged sooner’ (ibid.). The impulse work did not appeal to West, in

particular, who posed another important question about the character and the actor:

Where is the separation? Are we talking about impulse of character or


are we talking about impulse of the actor, because this dichotomy is
always in existence on stage. We want to try as much as we can to be the
character in the scene. (West 2009d)

This is related to Sozanska’s earlier question about whether the ‘emotion’ in the

physical exercises belongs to her, or the character.

Section IV: Findings

The performance took place on September 25th, 2009 in Thornlea Studio 1 at the

University of Exeter, and was accompanied by a short presentation about the rehearsal

process and the techniques used. To watch the full performance and presentation, please

go to:

• DVD Chapter 3, Disc 2: 15. Case Study 1 - Performance and


Presentation (Duration: 01:14:59)

My conclusions on the first case study are explained in regard to the questions raised by

each technique, followed by additional questions that developed from the research that

informed the second case study.

Alba Emoting

The actors were able to grasp the Alba Emoting patterns reasonably well

considering the limited time we had in which I could teach them. However, some of the

actors were still at a point where they have to consciously focus on the Alba patterns,

which limited the extent to which the technique could be applied practically to the

performance. In time, the patterns will eventually become integrated and will not

127
require the actors to ‘think’ so much. But until that point, how useful is Alba in the

rehearsal process? As a director, it becomes a question of whether you take the time

necessary to train the actors you are using to a proficient level of Alba Emoting, work

with a company that is already trained in Alba, or simply use the knowledge of Alba to

help actors with particular moments by suggesting the appropriate breathing pattern,

facial expressions, or postural attitude to achieve a desired effect. Hetzler posed this

question in his concluding interview:

There is the “Well, do you teach [Alba Emoting] to them as a process as


you’re going, so every day you do maybe an hour or half hour of that, or,
if someone just has a block do you give them a breathing pattern? And in
some respects it might work really well from the beginning, because
when you are doing a sort of impulse runs after you’ve worked on the
text for a bit, and suddenly you’ve got this other grounding in these
patterns where they might recognize something occurring. (Hetzler
2009a)

To explore this question further in the second case study, I chose two plays. Based on

the requirements of the selected plays, one cast had extensive Alba Emoting training

over the period of four weeks, while the other only had an introduction to the technique.

Emotional Access Work

Overall, the actors responded positively to the emotional access work. With the

physical exercises, however, Hetzler reflected that, in his case, ‘some of [the exercises]

came too soon, because we hadn’t done enough text work to know who these people

were yet’ (ibid.) and suggested keeping the exercises, ‘but [to] maybe bring them in a

little later’ (ibid.) This was in contrast to Sellman-Leava, the youngest member of the

group, who found the emotional access work to be:

…the most useful out of all of [the techniques], just because I’ve never
worked that way and I think they are a really useful ways to connect with
text immediately. […] Astbury’s work, you know, it was straight away
from day one, we were able to just get, I don’t mean results as in, to
shortcut to something, because that’s certainly not what happened, but,
we were able to find something immediately that would have normally
taken weeks of rehearsal. (Sellman-Leava 2009a)

128
It occurred to me that perhaps by immediately using the Astbury work, there is a

danger of making an assumption that the actors automatically need it, rather than

specifically using the work to address the demands of the text and the needs of the actor.

As a director, I use Astbury’s work to explore exciting possibilities of how the actors

can play with the text. However, the way in which Astbury as named his exercises,

especially in the case of ‘left-brain disabling devices’ may impact on how an actor

receives the work. The title implies that there is a ‘problem’ that needs to be dealt with.

The most important concern raised in regard to Astbury’s emotional awareness work

was the question: is it repeatable? Can the emotional experiences that surfaced in

rehearsal be incorporated into performance for each performance required? These

questions will be considered in the third case study.

Impulse and Awareness Work

The impulse runs were yielding fascinating discoveries and beautiful acting

moments, but these became lost when returning to rehearsals the following day. There

would be a gap, or a warm-up time for the actors to re-adjust to the stage space that was

to be used in the performance, from the freedom of using the entire studio. In retrospect,

rather than finishing each day with an impulse run, I should have returned to each scene

in its performance context. This would have been useful in order to bridge the gap

between the abstract explorations found in the impulse runs and the reality of the space

in performance context. This was reflected in the feedback from the actors as well. Lai

observed how ‘when we come to the performance setting I found […] a gap between the

work, the abstract work, and the performance’ (Lai 2009a). Other questions raised

included the distinction between ‘character’ and ‘actor’, with regard to impulses, as well

as emotion, which will be explored further in the following case studies.

129
More Questions

Not all of my research questions could be answered through the first case study

on its own. However, the question about whether or not these techniques can assist a

director in unifying a common understanding of emotion within a diverse cast did yield

an answer: they did not. In this case study, the question itself was irrelevant. Despite

differing views on ‘emotion’ we were still able to achieve a cohesive working

relationship. But it had nothing to do with a shared understanding of emotion. Their

individual opinions on emotion played no part whatsoever in terms of how we related as

a company, or to the text. The question should be altered to ask: how can these

techniques assist a director in unifying a common working practice within diverse cast?

And further to that, is a definition for emotion even necessary if it does not affect the

rehearsal process?

There were several challenges in using excerpts from a play, rather than a

complete production. One was stylistic – the actors wore blacks rather than period

costumes, and we used a non-realistic set, which resulted in an ambiguity in the style of

the performance. Had we been beholden to produce the play in its entirety, there would

have been more period constraints, unless as a director I chose to take the play out of its

original historical context. In the instance, I did not make a specific directorial choice.

In this way I was not working with these excerpts of Three Sisters in the same way that

I would work on a script in the professional world. I decided that for the second case

study the actors should work on a play in a definite performance context without

ambiguity. Related to this, the actors should explore a full character journey, rather than

a fragmented one from lifted scenes. Lai commented on this as being a concern:

We’re not doing the whole play. So when it comes to the whole play, to
create the character, would […] with the work so far, there are so many
opportunities for exploration, then how we get them to one person who is
consistent, […] a character, full and complete, in a whole play? (Lai
2009d)

130
Taking all these concerns into consideration led me to the conclusion that the

demands of the text and the needs of the actor should lead the rehearsal process entirely,

rather the techniques themselves. For the second case study, I chose to use two short

plays, in precise performance context: Play and Footfalls by Samuel Beckett.

131
Chapter Four: Case Study Three: Play and Footfalls by Samuel
Beckett

This chapter presents the second case study, an investigation into the challenge

of emotion in performance through Samuel Beckett’s Play and Footfalls. Phillip

Zarrilli, a director and author of Psychophysical Acting, has done extensive work on

Beckett’s plays. Zarrilli writes:

What Beckett’s plays demand of the actor is not the creation of


characters nor the realization of conventional dramatic action as it
happens (unthinkingly) in the moment. Beckett makes special demands
on the actor—an attentiveness to an indeterminate necessary—actions
and words whose meanings cannot be foreclosed. (Zarrilli 2009: 115)

The purpose of this case study is to discover if any of the approaches to emotion

that I have chosen to research may be useful to the performer in achieving this

‘attentiveness’ and addressing the challenges of performing Beckett. This chapter is

divided into four sections. Section I: Introduction to the Acting ‘Problem’ of Beckett

explores the acting challenges of Beckett’s work and the methodology for the second

case study. Section II: Play specifically addresses Play, from rehearsals and

performance and subsequent findings. Section III: Footfalls focuses on the same for

Footfalls. Section IV: Summary offers some concluding thoughts on the ‘affect’ of

Beckett’s plays on the actor.

Section I: Introduction to the Acting ‘Problem’ of Beckett

The plays of Samuel Beckett (1906 – 1989) are notoriously difficult to perform,

especially his later dramatic works. Jonathan Kalb, in his book Beckett in Performance

insists that ‘[Beckett’s] dramas are not about experiences; they are those experiences

themselves’ [original emphasis](Kalb 1989: 3-4). The short plays I chose to work with

132
are two of Beckett’s later pieces Play and Footfalls. Specific challenges to these texts

include the absence of psychological realism, working with the physical

restrictions/instructions put forward by Beckett, and finding the correct

musicality/tempo-rhythm of the text.

One of the primary challenges for many actors approaching Beckett is that his

characters are not necessarily driven by psychological motivations. Irina Jun, a Polish

actor who worked with director Antoni Livera on several Beckett plays, stated in an

interview that ‘Beckett should not be approached psychologically’ (Ben-Zvi 1990: 48)

explaining that ‘the actor is not expected to feel what he plays but to perform very

accurately what he is supposed to do’ (ibid.). Zarrilli, in his article “Acting at the nerve

ends’: Beckett, Blau, and the Necessary’, discusses some of the problems that arise

when working on Beckett: ‘[f]or students primarily trained in an American,

Stanislavkian-based realist approach to character acting, it is extraordinarily difficult to

“let go” of the seeming necessity of reaching conclusions about the subtext of each

action so that it is played motivationally’ (Zarrilli 1997: 106).

It is reported by many actors who worked with Beckett that he himself had little

concern for given circumstances, subtext or character motivations which were typical of

many naturalistic plays of the time. Nancy Illig, a German actress who appeared in

Play, recalls a rehearsal with Beckett where a cast member tried to elicit some

background information:

The actor of the Man desperately threw this question out into the
darkness of the auditorium: “Why am I dead?” The author seemed
startled. He made various suggestions. Maybe because of a traffic
accident? Or suicide? But mightn’t he have died a natural death in bed?
Obviously this question was not a relevant one for Beckett. […] The
only thing important to Beckett was the situation: we were all three dead.
(Ben-Zvi 1990: 24)

For actors who rely on delving into the psychological motivations of their

characters Beckett can be extremely frustrating.

133
However, there is another way to look at this ‘problem’. As Kalb points out,

‘because Beckett is so reluctant to discuss the psychological motivations of his

characters […] his actors must work from external considerations inward’ [original

emphasis](Kalb 1989: 42-43). Kalb goes on to compare this phenomenon with the

James-Lange Theory of Emotion, where maintains that the physical changes can lead to

psychological ones:

The actor of a late role inhabits a given space, adopts a specific


physical attitude, as a self-sufficient activity because, for Beckett, that
physical predicament is the figure’s complete ontological condition.
Any psychological condition develops from the physical one – e.g.
kneeling but not sitting behind a cutaway urn and waiting for a
spotlight to prompt you to speech, or sitting on a high stool with your
head strapped to a backrest and completely masked except for your
mouth – and speech emanates from these bodily conditions, is a
vocalized aspect of them. (ibid.)

The physical ‘restrictions’ that Beckett puts forth in his scripts may actually help the

actors connect to the roles psychophysically. Another challenge, related to the physical

restrictions on movement, is how to keep the actor engaged with text. Zarrilli maintains

that ‘behind that apparent “inaction” is the blazing “flame” of an active, inner, vibratory

perceptivity’ (Zarrilli 1995: 197). The physical restrictions in Play and Footfalls will be

addressed in more detail later in the chapter.

Another potential obstacle to Beckett’s work is finding the appropriate

musicality or tempo-rhythm for each particular play. Many actors and directors who

have worked on Beckett discuss this element of his work. In an interview with Linda

Ben-Zvi, Billie Whitelaw, a renowned Beckettian actress emphasizes the importance of

finding the right score: ‘with these plays the music and tempos are essential. If you get

them right, everything else falls into place’ (Ben-Zvi 1990: 6). Irina Jun compares

Beckett’s plays to musical compositions, and also stresses that ‘an actor therefore has

to know and understand the work’s musical structure. It even seems to me that quite

often it is the way the text sounds that is more important than what it actually says or

134
means’ (ibid.: 48). Delphine Seyrig, a French actress, finds that the tempo Beckett sets

helps unravel the ‘abstract’ nature of his plays reporting that ‘when you work with

Beckett, you find yourself regretting not having this almost musical education. It’s a

concrete, real kind of work that is quite distinct from the question of interpretation’

(ibid: 20). Finding the appropriate tempo-rhythm is potentially very challenging.

Methodology for Case Study Two: Play and Footfalls

As with my other research projects, this case study will make no universal

claims about the nature of emotion in performance; rather, it is an attempt to arrive at

better understanding of how the director might approach the question and nature of

emotion in a specific Beckett text in rehearsals and in performance. The methodology

used in this case study is qualitative, based on interviews with the actors, and

observations and reflections throughout the rehearsal process and performances of

Beckett’s Play and Footfalls. Unlike the first case study, which involved melding Alba

Emoting, Emotional Access Work and Impulse and Awareness Work into a complete

rehearsal process to elicit ‘emotion’, the techniques in this case study were primarily

reductive, experimenting with how they might be used to aid the actor working on

Beckett’s plays which are often described as ‘colorless.’ Also, I wanted to explore how

Beckett’s very specific instructions may affect the actor on a psychophysical level. For

that reason, unlike the first case study, I did not impose a process as such. For this

specific case study, I kept a running assessment of the problems/challenges that the

actors encountered in rehearsal, and then used the techniques in an attempt to overcome

them.

Section II: Play


Beckett’s Play was written between 1962 and 1963 in English, with its first

performance in German at the Ulmer Theatre in 1963. In Play, Beckett has three actors

135
encased up to their necks in urns recounting their different sides of an extra-marital

affair from beyond the grave. There are three identified characters: w1, w2 and M.,

which presumably stand for Woman 1, Woman 2 and Man. There is also a spotlight,

which is often considered to be another role sometimes referred to as the ‘Inquisitor’ or

‘Interrogator’. Through the text it is implied that Woman 1 is Man’s wife, and Woman

2, his mistress. The three characters, in broken thoughts, recount their experiences with

the affair.

One of the most noticeably difficult tasks for the performers is that Beckett calls

for a ‘rapid tempo throughout’ (Beckett 2006: 307) as well as a repeat of the entire play,

with several options for variations including further increasing the tempo and the

addition of a breathless quality to the voices. His instructions to the actors performing

the roles consist of ‘faces impassive throughout’ (ibid.) with the further instruction that

their voices should be ‘toneless except where an expression is indicated’ (ibid.). There

are only three places in the script where Beckett does indicate expression, which include

the directions ‘vehement’, ‘hopefully’ (ibid.: 316), and ‘wild laughter’ (ibid.: 317).

Throughout the play Beckett indicates five different levels of volume on a scale from A

to E; A being ‘normal voice’ and E being completely ‘unintelligible’ (ibid.: 307).

Volume level C, which is in the middle of the range he describes as ‘faint’ and ‘largely

unintelligible’ (ibid.). In addition to these demands, Beckett is clear that the actors

speech is ‘provoked by a spotlight projected on faces alone’ (ibid.) and that the actors’

‘response to the light is immediate.’ (ibid.) He calls for a ‘single mobile spot’ that

‘should swivel at maximum speed from one face to another’ (ibid.: 318), expressive of a

‘unique inquisitor,’ rather than assigning a separate fixed spotlight to each individual

face. And last but not least, the actors should be encased in urns about a yard high,

either standing below stage level if trap doors are available, or kneeling throughout.

136
Beckett adds, ‘[t]he sitting posture results in urns of unacceptable bulk and is not to be

considered.’ (ibid.: 319).

Rehearsing Play

The actors for this project included Symmonie Preston as Woman 1, an

American MFA student of the Staging Shakespeare course; Lauren Shepherd as Woman

2, a Canadian also studying Shakespeare; Callum Elliott-Archer, a British third year

undergraduate studying drama; and Joe Sellman-Leava as the Inquisitor, also a third

year undergraduate who participated in the first case study as Andrey in Three Sisters.

None of the actors involved had any previous experience with Beckett.

Figure 14: Lauren Shepherd, Callum Elliott-Archer, and Symmonie Preston in Play. Photograph by
Ben Borley.

Alba Emoting

The second case study took place over four weeks. This allowed more time for

intensive Alba Emoting training for the actors involved in Play. As Beckett insists that

the three actors’ faces should remain ‘impassive throughout’ (Beckett 2006: 307), my

decision to train the actors in Alba Emoting for this process was twofold. Firstly by
137
increasing their awareness of the facial-postural-respiratory effector patterns, I hoped

the actors would be able to use Alba Emoting to eliminate any unwanted patterns that

might emerge when rehearsing Beckett’s text. Secondly, I suspected that specific

understanding of the breathing patterns could also be useful in the repeat of Play, in

which Beckett asks for a ‘breathless quality in voices from beginning of Repeat 1 and

increasing to end of play’ (ibid.: 320). Therefore the rehearsals for Play began with an

intensive week of Alba Emoting training. It was important that the actors had an in-

depth understanding of the technique, in order to apply the technique effectively to the

text. At the end of each day in this first week, we read through the text. The actors

would attempt to read the text ‘tonelessly’ as Beckett prescribes.

One of the first indicators that Alba Emoting would be an important tool in the

realization of Play, was at the beginning of the second week when we started doing

‘mirror-work’ with the Alba Emoting patterns. After the initial learning stage of Alba

Emoting, working in front of a mirror can be very useful for the actors, allowing them

to ‘see’ the patterns manifestations for themselves. To watch an example of the ‘mirror-

work’ please see:

• DVD Chapter 4: 1. Alba Emoting Mirror Work (Duration: 00:00:36)

I asked the actors to speak some of their text in different Alba Emoting patterns, in

which they could observe the pattern in themselves. For example, Shepherd observed

that when she laughed, she was mixing the 3a (joy) pattern with 2b (fear). I then asked

them to speak the text ‘tonelessly’ as they had been throughout the week. The actors

were surprised to discover how ‘emotive’ their attempt at ‘impassive’ and ‘toneless’ had

been. Then I asked them to speak the chorus using the ‘neutral’ pattern. Preston

observed ‘neutral is so very far away from what we’re doing when we’re doing it’

(Beck et al. 2010a) and noticed:

138
[T]here’s definitely some 3b [sadness] coming in when we lower our
volume […] When we lower the volume, we lower our gaze, we all tend
to drop our shoulders a little bit more on the second time when we lower
our volume. […] And it does then start affecting the speech pattern as
well. (ibid.)

The most common patterns to mix in with their ‘neutral’ text were anger, fear, and

sadness. Some elements of the patterns were a result of the psychology inherent in the

text, and others a result of the pace of the text. Sellman-Leava, who was an observer in

this process, remarked that once the actors had gone through this process:

You could see the difference. A huge difference. […] And you were all
very present […] as performers, but there was a real distance that wasn’t
there before. It was like the words were just coming out, they were just
flowing. […] but … In the neutral the second time, after having gone
through the other patterns. […] It was almost like there was a separation,
but you were engaged as performers. You could see that it helped. (ibid.)

Alba Emoting, in the case of rehearsing Play, became a kind of via negativa.

Grotowski’s concept of via negativa is a ‘technique of elimination’, which rids ‘the

organism of its resistance to the psychophysical process of playing a role’ (Slowiak and

Cuesta 2007: 20). In this case, Alba Emoting was used to rid the organism of the

unwanted physiological traces of the emotional effector patterns. In Shepherd’s final

interview, she maintained:

[A]ll the alba emoting that we did, made me more aware of things that I
was […] layering on to something that wasn't there, just by my natural
body movements […]that gave me the tool to recognize that I was doing
something. (Shepherd 2010a)

The actors were effectively able to use the technique to remove any unwanted

physiological components of emotional effector patterns.

Emotional Access Work

I used very little of Astbury’s Emotional Access Work for the rehearsal process

of Play, as the other techniques were more pertinent to the actors’ needs in the

timeframe that we had. In this instance, I did not feel that under-reading would be

helpful for embodying the text, given the technical demands of delivery of the text,
139
which includes repetitions and cut offs. I did use the sequence of physical exercises on

a section of the script in the second week of rehearsal. Interestingly, in contrast to

Preston’s remark about the group impulse work, Preston pointed out that the physical

exercises allowed for an important ‘disconnection’, a contrast to how the physical

exercises operated in the first case study. When describing the exercise from

Bioenergetics in which the actors put their feet in the air and pull their toes down,

Preston remarked:

[The exercises] allowed us, for the first time, to sort of disconnect from
the personal relationships between those people, and the back story of
those people, and to have it become about the words – although the
feelings will always be there, there was a certain amount of release that I
think is key to doing these three people. And after that I think there was a
marked difference in our approach. And I do think it was because we
were able to make that disconnection through the process of going
through that. And it was crucial to the success of the piece. (Preston
2010)

Impulse and Awareness Work

The second week was focused primarily on Impulse and Awareness Work. In

Play, the actors would have to respond to external stimuli, the spotlight of the

Inquisitor, changes in lighting, changes in volume, and rhythm, so the impulse and

awareness exercises reflected these demands. Included in the text is a ‘chorus’ in which

Beckett indicates very specific overlapping rhythms (for an example see Appendix C).

Because Beckett indicates five different levels of volume in the text (on a scale from A

to E, A being loudest), the actors had to build a sense of coherency with one another.

One exercise had the actors on the floor with their eyes closed. Speaking the text from

the chorus, the actors had to adjust their volume in accordance with a change in the

overhead lights (as, in the script, the intensity of the Inquisitor’s light changes with the

intensity of the volume of the actors). The purpose was to develop and attune their

awareness of each other’s volume and respond to the changes in lighting; skills they

would need for the performance of Play.


140
The most important impulse work was a set of exercises developed to assist

the actors with the challenge of working together as a company, and the order of the

lines. The technical requirements of Play almost demand that the actors, as well as the

Inquisitor, learn the entirety of the script, which is why I chose to use an actor to operate

the spotlight, rather than a technician. I started with a ball game, in which Sellman-

Leava (the Inquisitor) would throw the ball to the actor who was supposed to speak, and

the actor could not speak until he or she caught the ball. If a mistake was made in the

order of the text, the actor would throw the ball back to Sellman-Leava, rather than

speak. The point of this exercise was to help the actors learn their lines, and the order of

things, while working together as a company of four. It was also intended to condition

the actors to respond to the Inquisitor, rather than pre-empt their speech. Again, Beckett

insists that ‘their speech is provoked by a spotlight projected on faces alone’ (Beckett

2006: 307) and ‘the response to light is immediate’ (ibid.). For an example of this

exercise please see:

• DVD Chapter 4: 2. Impulse and Awareness Ball Exercise (Duration:


00:00:40)

For the most part, the actors found this exercise essential to the rehearsal

process. Elliott-Archer commented that unlike a typical rehearsal, where a mistake

would just be noted, the ball game was useful for highlighting problems:

[W]ith the ball it’s such a big gesture, you have thrown it to the wrong
person, you have taken a big break. This person has got the wrong thing,
so they throw it back and it’s like […] “okay, we need to work on this
particular section.” (Elliott-Archer 2010b)

Preston actually acknowledged that this exercise contributed to the actors’ task of

‘maintaining that score, and that pace, and showing you where it is that you may not

know exactly how long your rest is’ (Preston 2010). This exercise, however, requires a

different kind of concentration, and Shepherd found the ball game extremely frustrating.

141
Although Shepherd was the first actor to be off book, and arguably knew her lines

more thoroughly than the rest at an early stage, she found it very difficult to cope with

this exercise, maintaining:

I really dislike that activity. I can understand its benefits, I can see its
benefits, but because I personally, just…I know my entire body tenses
up, I consciously am working against it, trying to make myself calm,
relaxed and open for everyone else and receptive to everyone else, that I
can’t pay attention to the actual activity of what we should be doing. So
I’m not making any connections because I’m so conscious of not
seeming closed off, because that doesn’t help anyone else. So I end up
focusing on making sure that everyone else is getting what they need,
and I’m not going to get what I need because the activity doesn’t quite
work for me. (Shepherd 2010b)

I suggested that Shepherd should try and use either neutral breath from Alba Emoting,

or the tenderness pattern, which lowers the heart rate. As the rehearsal process

continued, Shepherd became more confident and comfortable with the exercise. Other

uses of impulse work included impulse runs in which Sellman-Leava used a flashlight

to signal the speech of each actor, and also impulse runs that incorporated Alba

Emoting. Commenting on the impulse work overall, Preston identified two areas in

particular in which the group work contributed:

I think it helped to cement us more as a group, [and] to create some of


those relationships between husband and wife and lover, to sort of just
have that since we are all trained in such a psychological fashion. It’s
something that we could relate to easily and, therefore, then work
together. (Preston 2010)

While I had anticipated that the impulse and awareness could build a group relationship,

I had not suspected that the impulse/alba runs would contribute to filling in the

psychological ‘gaps’ that Beckett leaves for his actors. To watch an excerpt of one of

the impulse runs, using the Alba Emoting patterns, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 4: 3. Excerpt from Impulse Run with Alba Emoting


(Duration: 00:02:40)

142
Physical Restrictions

Beckett asks that the actors either stand or kneel within their urns. As a

company, we chose to have the actors kneeling. From the beginning of the rehearsal

process, even before the actors were off book, we would rehearse with them in a

kneeling position. We used music stands to hold their scripts, and pillows or yoga

blocks to support their knees. I would always alternate between running the play with

the actors kneeling or on their feet to alleviate the physical strain as much as possible.

The urns that the actors would be using for the performance were ready to use in

rehearsal by the third week. Elliott-Archer reported:

[R]ehearsing in [the urn] was incredibly difficult. […] You could feel the
time, and you could feel your knees, and you’re constantly trying to
work out where the best position is […] But when we did the […] actual
performance, I was kind of focused on other things […] I barely even
noticed that I was on my knees. Whether that is because of the audience,
or the fact that […] you’ve got to be focused and you've got to be
present. So during the performance it was absolutely fine. I could have
stayed in there for a lot longer. During rehearsals it was very difficult to
keep up the stamina. (Elliott-Archer 2010a)

It was also at this point in the process that we discovered the ‘fear’ that actors can

experience from the demands placed on them by Beckett’s text. In Psychophysical

Acting, Zarrilli shares an account from Jenny Karminer, an actor who had worked with

Zarrilli on The Beckett Project, specifically Play:

Throughout the rehearsal and performance of Play, my experiences


ranged from elation to sheer terror to claustrophobia to utterly
inexplainable [sic] emptiness…The terror I felt while in the urn was
virtually all-consuming. One night I wanted to get out so bad I thought I
would have to scream and stop the show, but somehow I just kept going:
I opened my mouth and the right words poured out. Every night I lived
in fear of forgetting my lines, and I dropped sweat as the light was on
Karen or Jeff, paralyzed with panic. (Karminer in Zarrilli 2009: 143)

Shepherd’s experience with the urn was similar to that of Karminer. Shepherd was

unable to complete a performance in the urn for four days of rehearsal, describing the

experience of being in the urn as ‘hot and claustrophobic’ (Shepherd 2010a). At first

Shepherd attributed this difficulty to illness, having experienced flu-like symptoms. The
143
Inquisitor’s light would shine on Shepherd and she would freeze, (in the pattern of

fear from an Alba Emoting perspective), and then stop the run and abandon the urn. The

first time this occurred, we finished rehearsal for the day. For an example of this, please

see:

• DVD Chapter 4: 4. Shepherd and the Urn (Duration: 00:00:36)

When Shepherd was unable to complete a run the following day as well, I needed to

take action to resolve the issue.

Firstly, we had a half hour of ‘fooling around’ in the urns, taking silly pictures

of the actors. Sellman-Leava and I also took our turns spending time in the urns as well.

I usually try to avoid psychologically manipulative directing tactics, but in this case I

needed Shepherd to spend time in the urn in a relaxed state. When we returned to do a

proper run of the play, I thought we had achieved success. Towards the end of that run,

however, Shepherd froze once again. To try and assist Shepherd in her task of

performance I decided to use the techniques in order to address this problem, and I

asked the actors to do an Alba/Impulse run while in the urns. Because the impulse runs

last longer than a performance, Shepherd was in the urn for a period of forty-five

minutes (the running time of Play in performance is approximately fifteen minutes). To

watch an example of an Alba/Impulse run in the urns, please see:

• DVD Chapter 4: 5. Alba/Impulse Run in Urns (Duration: 00:01:01)

After four days of unsuccessful attempts of running the play straight through,

and two days before the first performance, Shepherd was finally able to remain in her

urn for the duration of the play. Shepherd later described her relationship with her urn

as developing ‘from hatred to love’ (Shepherd 2010a) and added ‘the more and more I

think about it, the more and more […] I'm convincing myself that I gave myself the flu,

144
because I was having issues’ (ibid.). For Shepherd, the Alba/Impulse run was the

single most important part of the process:

I honestly don't think I would've been able to do it properly as much as I


have the heart to do it properly if I hadn't had that run. It was really
important for me because I was having such trouble. It was really
helpful. (ibid.)

Figure 15: Lauren Shepherd in Play. Photograph by Ben Borley.

In Performance

Play had two performances, the first on Tuesday, April 27th and the second on

Friday, April 30th 2010, as part of the academic presentation. To watch the Tuesday

performance, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 4: 6. Performance of Play - 27 April 2010 (Duration:


00:12:46)

The actors were nervous for their first performance in front of an audience and

consequently started the play too fast, at speed required for the second half of the play.

As a result, there was a slight hitch in the first half, when Preston paused in the light,

145
and Sellman-Leava, as the Inquisitor, moved to another actor. Reflecting on the

experience, Preston remarked:

It was so fast, that when we got to the point where I had the problem, I
literally had not yet taken the breath, because the lines… there’s usually
a significantly longer amount of time, so I went to breathe in and I
wasn’t done breathing, and the camera was on me, the light was on me.
And then it went away, and I was desperate to get it back because of all
the parts that we could skip, that was one that was actually fairly…there
was a good deal of exposition in that particular line. (Preston 2010)

Luckily, the actors were able to regain their composure and carry on. Now aware of a

tendency to ‘rush’ in the presence of an audience, the actors were more readily prepared

for Friday’s performance. Preston pointed out the stress that can occur from being in the

urn:

When it goes wrong, there is a little bit of panic that sets in from being in
that position, where you can’t bring your shoulders back and you can’t –
where you’re sort of breathing in a [more] shallow place than you
normally would. (Preston 2010)

With this in mind, all three of the actors consciously used Alba Emoting patterns,

whether neutral breath or tenderness, to keep themselves calm and focused while in

their urns as the audience took their seats. The performance on Friday ran smoothly.

Unfortunately, due to operator error, the Friday night performance was not recorded. To

watch Friday’s dress rehearsal, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 4: 7. Dress Rehearsal of Play - 30 April 2011 (Duration:


00:12:46)

All of the actors involved in Play commented on physiological changes that

occurred in the performances. The following are a few of the actors’ accounts:

Preston: It felt like my heart was in my throat and it was racing, racing, racing. I
think we all felt the racing quality of it, and we were consciously trying
to slow it down, and it wasn’t until we got halfway through section 5 that
we were able to successfully do that. (Preston 2010)

Elliott-Archer: It was a little bit nerve-wracking actually. […] It’s safe to say that like
for the first half of the first round, I was, my heart was going, and I tried
to, you know…I relaxed into it and I calmed down and focused on the
146
light, but it was a bit nerve-wracking. I thought it went really well
though. (Elliott-Archer 2010a)

Shepherd: [W]e came in and got and in our urns and everything, and people started
coming in and I was fine, and then the lights went out. And all of a
sudden like, my heart just… It was absolutely an unreal experience […] I
felt like my body was pulsating, like my heart was going to come out of
my chest. And somehow, I think it was to do with the adrenaline, we
were all so ridiculously fast. […] and so you are thinking a mile a
minute, and your lips are going a mile a minute, and […] I actually felt
like I was moving forward and you're completely stationary. It was just
strange. It was a very strange feeling. (Shepherd 2010a)

This led me to consider the affects of performance conditions on the performers

themselves, which will be discussed in more detail in the conclusion. But the actors also

experienced a ‘buzz’ (Elliott-Archer 2010a) from performing Play. Sellman-Leava

described this feeling as ‘a nice sense of achievement. I think because it's such an

exercise in presence, I feel there is more of a shared experience than probably a lot of

other shows that I've done’ (Sellman-Leava 2010). Elliott-Archer remarked ‘Getting it

right was extremely satisfying. And getting it wrong was seriously frustrating’ (Elliott-

Archer 2010a). As Shepherd pointed out, ‘It's an incredible play of multitasking’

(Shepherd 2010a).

Findings

Alba Emoting was extremely useful in rehearsing Play for several reasons.

Because Alba Emoting essentially isolates concrete physical elements of emotional

patterns, the actors were able to differentiate between patterns and able recognize when

unwanted patterns were mixing in unintentionally. The actors could observe in

themselves when certain elements of patterns such as fear, anger and sadness were

sneaking into their ‘impassive’ faces, or even affecting their breathing. With this new

awareness, the actors could deliberately eliminate those tendencies. This also helped

prevent them from being swayed by the emotion implicit in their lines of text. Preston

147
reflected, ‘In terms of the delivery overall, I don’t know if I could do [Play] without

having done the Alba and do it as successfully’ (Preston 2010). Preston also noted:

I think it’s also been very helpful in terms of when you’re in it, doing it,
having done the Alba, to then notice more, “my brow was doing
something” or “my eyes aren’t on the horizon”, it becomes much more
obvious to the performer that that’s what they’re doing. (ibid.)

In this way, as with the first case study, Alba Emoting supplied us with an instant and

concrete vocabulary.

The biggest surprise about using Alba Emoting in relation specifically to

Beckett’s work is how it was used in another way—to keep the actors ‘present’ or

‘calm’. Sellman-Leava, who had the very difficult task of controlling the swiveling

spotlight, often used the Alba pattern ‘neutral breath’, which is a seventh pattern created

to neutralize the effects of the other six. He used neutral breath to keep focused and

relaxed, especially on occasions where he would make a mistake. Neutral breath

allowed him to stay present and prevent any emotional patterns resulting from

frustration or disappointment to interfere with continuing the difficult task at hand.

Sellman-Leava recounted:

Neutral breath has been really useful […] in terms of being present
enough to essentially be another performer with the three that speak, […]
to be present enough that there’s a connection when you move the light,
because there’s an impulse – it’s the impulse for them to speak, so it’s
not just moving the light when they speak, it’s actually – there’s a
connection. Neutral breath is really useful because it allows me to be
present. It helps me concentrate. If I get off track, or panic, if I go into
[…] 2b [fear] then it helps bring [me] back. (Sellman-Leava 2010)

Preston used very low-level 2a (eroticism) when the spotlight was off her in order to

release tension in the jaw, so she could enunciate at rapid speed, and to more

importantly to be able to take in enough breath before it was her time to speak.

I absolutely, when I’m doing it well, and I’m able to maintain the score
properly, I am breathing in 2a in between, in order to get enough air. It’s
imperative. Otherwise, if I don’t do that, you are going to see a lot of 1b
or 3b creeping in, you’re going to see - the jaw tightens quite a bit and
you’re going to get a lot more anger, a lot more…I don’t get much 2b,

148
other than […] when it went horribly – fear, total fear. Panic, panic,
panic. (Preston 2010)

What also helped Shepherd achieve with her ‘urn issues’ was employing Alba Emoting

as a technique to control the involuntary physiological reactions that she was having to

the confinement of the urn and the fear of failure. Rather than simply apply the neutral

breath pattern from Alba Emoting, Shepherd actually began to use the 1a (tenderness)

pattern in order to decrease her heart rate. Elliott-Archer also employed the neutral

breath pattern in performance, commenting for Play, an actor must ‘remain absolutely

calm and just trust that when you open your mouth something’s going to come out’

(Elliott-Archer 2010a).

The group impulse and awareness work proved very important in developing a

group trust and connection as well as preparing the actors for the task of performance

through practice and repetition, especially for Sellman-Leava, who remarked:

[O]ne of the best things about last night is that we were really together as
a company. There was, I think, a mistake, or one or two mistakes, though
what was really nice, even though none of us are looking at each other
[…] there was a really nice sense, a real connection between the four of
us. There was no sense of like, “I'm really lost.” There was no […] deer
in headlights moment […] There was no panicking. […] we know if
there’s been a mistake but we also know how to move on, and recover it,
and not overcompensate for it, but just keep going. So that was really
nice, to know that that’s possible, that sort of connection is possible even
if you're not all sharing the same space as we are not, even if we’re not
all moving, we are all static, and not even looking directly at each other
but there still is that energetic connection. (Sellman-Leava 2010)

Elliott-Archer also attributed this ‘energetic connection’ to the impulse and awareness

work:

I think that really showed yesterday, all the work that we been doing,
you know with Joe and the ball, and sort of knowing who is next and sort
of like being able to string a few lines together, rely on a few things has
really really paid off. (Elliott-Archer 2010a)

Regarding the final performance, Sellman-Leava added, ‘last night we were able to stay

on the same track, and that's a place that we've got to through all of the group work that

we been doing’ (Sellman-Leava 2010).


149
Figure 16: Joe Sellman-Leava as The Inquisitor in Play. Photograph by Ben Borley.

150
Section III: Footfalls
Footfalls was written in 1975 and first performed at the Royal Court in 1976.

The play features the character of May, who paces in a dim corridor of light, and

Woman’s Voice, which comes from ‘dark upstage’ (Beckett 2006: 399). The

implications from the text indicate that the voice is possibly the voice of May’s mother.

Beckett gives very precise instructions as to how May should pace as she converses

with the Woman’s Voice:

Strip: downstage, parallel with front, length nine steps, width one metre, a little off

centre audience right.

L ____r l r l r l r l r __ R

 l r l r l r l r l

Pacing: starting with right foot (r), from right (R) to left (L), with left foot (l) from L to

R.

Turn: rightabout at L, leftabout at R.

Steps: clearly audible rhythmic tread.

(ibid.: 399)

As the play progresses, May’s posture goes lower and lower, and the lights grow

dimmer and dimmer. Billie Whitelaw, who originated the role of May, describes

playing the role as ‘physically excruciating’ (Ben-Zvi 1990: 9) saying ‘as one gets

lower and lower, to stand in that position becomes almost intolerable; it is almost as if

one is curling round slowly within, into oneself, until finally one disappears, spiraling

inward, inward’ (ibid.). In Beckett’s notes from the Royal Court production that he

151
directed, he writes, ‘the walking should be like a metronome, one length must be

measured in exactly nine seconds’ (Brater 1987: 71). In the text, Beckett advises that

the actors’ voices should be ‘both low and slow throughout’ (Beckett 2006: 399).

Whitelaw’s advice to students working with Beckett begins with discussing ‘the

courage it takes to go slow, not afraid of being achingly boring, reciting like a

metronome, if need be’ (Ben-Zvi 1990: 10). Another challenge with this piece is

working out what the story is about—while the storyline of Play is relatively clear,

Footfalls is intentionally ambiguous. While in rehearsals for the Royal Court production

of Footfalls, Whitelaw asked Beckett if May were dead. His only answer for her was:

‘Let’s just say you’re not really there.’ (qtd. in Brater 1987: 60).

The actors involved in Footfalls included Elizabeth Pennington, from the UK,

who had just completed two years training with Phillip Zarrilli on the MFA in Theatre

Practice, and Helena Enright, an Irish actor/director/writer whose training consisted of

practical experience, both professional and amateur, in Limerick, Ireland. Pennington

played the character of May, and Enright played Woman’s Voice. Pennington had some

previous experience with Beckett on her course, as did Enright during her MA in Cork

when Zarrilli ran guest workshops. For both of them, this would be their first experience

performing Beckett’s work.

152
Figure 17: Elizabeth Pennington in Footfalls. Photograph by Ben Borley.

Rehearing Footfalls

In comparison to the rehearsal process of Play, rehearsing Footfalls was

relatively simple. The first task when rehearsing Footfalls was to mark out the path that

Pennington would walk. Following Beckett’s prescriptive directions was more difficult

than we had initially thought. May’s continuous pacing consists of nine steps, ending in

a rightabout turn at Left or a leftabout turn at Right (what Woman’s Voice refers to as

the ‘wheel’) and should result in a ‘clearly audible rhythmic tread’ (Beckett 2006: 399).

While Beckett leaves extremely specific instructions, he can still be unclear. We had to

take his Left and Right to mean Stage Left and Stage Right in order to adhere to all the

directions throughout the play. This is the opposite side of the stage from the Royal

Court production that was directed by Beckett himself. Beckett actually changed the

direction of his turns in his own production, and in many cases took liberties with his

own instructions.

153
Alba Emoting

While both the actors were given basic training in Alba Emoting, the technique

was not as essential to this rehearsal process as it was for Play. Enright reported that the

Alba Emoting patterns helped with ‘identifying certain emotions in this story that aren’t

necessarily psychologically obvious’ (Enright 2010). Enright employed a low level 2a

(eroticism) and 1a (tenderness) mix to help her find the voice of the mother and to also

strengthen her relationship to the character of May:

Like for example, tilting my head back, which is 2a, which is kind of
pleasure and eroticism, gave that sense of me being kind of… And
mixing it with a bit of 1a, tenderness, and tilting the head, and having it
back enabled me to put a strain on the voice, but also evoked a kind of
tender feeling. That tenderness. Because I was, I sort of tell the story first
[…] and that helped me kind of with that mother-daughter relationship.
Because we don't have a direct relationship on stage, it kind of helped
with that. (Enright 2010)

Pennington did not use the patterns in her performance, reporting that ‘for me, the Alba

was, it was there but it wasn't there’ (Pennington 2010a). However, there was a

recognition of components of the patterns that were occurring naturally within

Pennington’s performance:

Sometimes I would identify certain, you know, directions of the eyes, or


direction of lips, things like that, and I would wonder, because I was
automatically doing it when speaking certain lines, I would think about
why it was doing it. So that was interesting. (ibid.)

Though Alba Emoting was not as prominent in this rehearsal process, the technique did

inform our vocabulary, just as it had for Play and Three Sisters.

Emotional Access Work

The Emotional Access Work was used regularly in this process. The anger and

energy runs between Enright and Pennington offered an exploration into the

complicated relationship that the character of May and her mother might have had.

Enright found the anger and energy runs ‘quite useful, just in terms of unlocking, or not

in a psychological way, just in terms of getting [the text] into the body and just letting it
154
be free’ (Enright 2010). In a similar vein, Pennington found that the exercises

prevented her from falling into a strictly monotone delivery of the text:

[The physical exercises] made [the text] come out differently by using
different areas of the body or different psychologies behind it. So it was
good not to get stuck too early on, because even in the first read-throughs
I heard that, you know I was using the same intonations and things.
(Pennington 2010a)

The exercises also contributed to the actors’ sense of the story. At a certain point in one

of the anger runs, Pennington was stationary against the wall and Enright stormed up to

her counting (which are the lines from the text). Enright remarked that it ‘felt as if I was

trapping [Pennington] there, and that those steps, counting them, was trapping [her]’

(Beck et al. 2010b). To watch the moment Enright is describing please go to:

• DVD Chapter 4: 8. Excerpt of Anger/Energy Run with Enright and


Pennington (Duration: 00:00:35)

Another insight that Enright discovered came at the end of the physical exercises when

the actors were asked to ‘whisper’ their text.

It was nice to play with the different feelings that were coming out from
the text, that wouldn’t necessarily be there if you just kept reading it. In
the whisper one, when I was on my side, I got very much a [sense of]
tenderness, mixed with a kind of sadness. It was kind of an…almost
wanting to reach out to [Pennington]. (ibid.)

Enright also found the experience of the ‘legs in the air’ exercises, as the first step

towards discovering the placement of her voice for the role:

I found the stress in my voice when my legs were up in the air, and I
thought of, kind of, the age of the woman and how […] how it affects the
voice, the exertion of just living when you’re ninety years of age, if
you’re bedridden. (ibid.)

We also used Alba Emoting to help find a good placement for Enright’s voice.

I also used a Physical Metaphor exercise in which, Enright was limp, as a

lifeless body, while Pennington attempted to keep her upright, metaphorically trying to

keep the mother alive. Pennington described the experience to Enright, saying ‘It was

155
like you died, and I was just trying to keep you going. I didn’t know what else to do.

By the end I just wanted to hold you’ (Beck et al. 2010c). After the exercise of lifting

Enright was complete, I asked Pennington to simply repeat her text, just in a sitting

position. Pennington commented that ‘I just let the words drop through me. Like I

didn’t try to put anything on them. […] It was the most powerful one for me’ (ibid.). To

watch the run of Pennington’s speech to which she is referring, go to:

• DVD Chapter 4: 9. Pennington After Physical Metaphor (Duration:


00:01:13)

In her final interview, Pennington pointed out an aspect of using the emotional

access work that I had not previously considered:

The [emotional access work] […] was a great way to break the ice with
me and Helena working together, and starting to have a dynamic onstage,
even though we don't speak to each other, I was very very aware of her
and the words that she was saying, and I felt very comfortable working
with her the whole way through, and yourself [Beck] […] And I think
doing those initial workshops break those boundaries down, get you
looking at the text without the pressure of having to say the lines from
memory. (Pennington 2010a)

Similar to the group impulse work used in Play, the shared experience of the sequence

of physical exercises contributed greatly to the development of the working relationship

between Enright, Pennington and myself.

As the text of Footfalls is already full of vivid imagery, I decided to use image

streaming. Despite Beckett’s aversion to offering psychological motivations or

background details about the characters, Enright highlighted a point that I believe to be

true for many of the actors involved in this case study, maintaining ‘for me it's

important even if there isn't a story to understand the story […] I have to find some

meaning somewhere,’ (Enright 2010), and added that this is true for her even when ‘it's

not necessarily that the characters psychologically driven’ (ibid.). Image streaming

156
assisted the actors in gaining a sense of story, and for Enright the exercise added a

‘depth’ (ibid.) to her experience of the characters:

Well I got images initially of a bedroom, which is kind of light, a very


dark gray area, with light coming through, and I think that was directly
influenced from the text. And gave that kind of sense of the mother being
in the bed, like, to kind of care for her. But there was also a sense of kind
of a hauntedness [sic], that there were kind of things left untouched. […]
I saw a wedding ring on the dressing table, there were kind of cobwebs,
which these two people, or one of them, or both, could be ghosts. There
was a ghostly feel. (Enright 2010)

Pennington used images from the exercise to help her better understand and relate to her

character:

[T]he image of her walking outside her mother's bedroom, the pacing in
the corridor I think is quite an easy one to accept, it feels like the mother
potentially could be in the bedroom very ill, on her deathbed. Or she
could have died, and the daughter is still going. Or she's picking up this
routine despite the mother being gone, she doesn't know what else to do
because she's always been contained in this little house. And that's not to
add too much psychology to it, because it's quite an abstract situation
anyway, but there is a humanness to it that you can understand and relate
to. (Pennington 2010a)

Image streaming enabled the actors to connect to the story through a familiar process of

relating to characters and their situations, which was important to them even though

those connections may not be apparent to the audience once in performance.

Physical Restrictions

Footfalls requires the actor playing May to undertake a prescribed and measured

journey back and forth in a dim corridor of light. Given this, under-reading, just on its

own as a technique, was useful in that it allowed Pennington to begin rehearsing ‘on her

feet’ from the first rehearsal. Pennington described the use of under-reading as ‘just

fantastic’ (Pennington 2010a), for both learning lines and allowing freedom to explore

May’s walk from the beginning of the rehearsal process. To watch a clip of under

reading being used in this way, please go to:

157
• DVD Chapter 4: 10. Under-reading with Pennington (Duration:
00:01:13)

I have not outlined a specific section addressing the use of impulse and

awareness work for this process, because the exercises in the particular case were more

subtle than previously and were linked extensively to the demands of Beckett’s script.

The actors had to develop an awareness and connection with each other in relation to

the conditions of the performance space. We were fortunate to have the both the

performance space and Beckett’s prescriptive lighting state set more than a week before

the actual performance. This allowed the actors to develop a connection with each other

and the space, as it would be in performance, very early on in the rehearsal process. In

one exercise, I used a metronome to regulate tempo as the pace of the play slowed

down. But for the most part, the actors developing their awareness under the

performance conditions of Footfalls was one of the most important tools. In

Pennington’s case, just experiencing the prescribed footfalls produced a psychophysical

effect on her. The physical pacing caused Pennington to feel what she described as

‘sadness’ and in some cases ‘fear’.

I get tears every time I do it, and it's not that I'm crying, but there is
something coming out to me. […] for me, it's this containment, you
know, because you are holding your body so tight, and the shifting of
focus is very simple, and the eyes don't blink very much, and
everything's just under the surface, and you are so controlled, the
emotion has to come out somewhere, and I think it comes out of the
eyes. And also because you are not really lit, it's almost like it's safe to
squeeze it out. (Pennington 2010a)

The actors also had to adapt to the physical distance between them, as Enright had to be

in upstage darkness. The distance between the two performers in those performance

conditions also had a profound impact on Pennington:

[T]he mother's words are most poignant for me. When I'm just standing
there listening to the voice, I feel I'm listening to her talk about me, and
then the conversation, I feel like I'm interacting with her at that point.
That's the most emotional section for me. (ibid.)

158
Performance and Findings

Footfalls had two performances, the first on Tuesday April 27th, and the second

on Friday April 30th 2010. To watch the Tuesday performances please go to:

• DVD Chapter 4: 11. Performance of Footfalls - 27 April 2010 (Duration:


00: 33:58)

Both performances ran smoothly, although Enright made a small line mistake in the first

performance, which she described as ‘quite scary’ (Enright 2010). Enright explained:

[B]ecause, I didn't look at Liz, so I'm relying an awful lot on aural


[senses], to hear the steps. […] So as she's walking, I'm conscious of her
walking, and I was coming back having to say steps in time with her,
count the steps, and my brain just went into “will you never have done
revolving at all”, and as I said it I had a momentary “no!” […] it was like
so important not to drop focus. (Enright 2010)

Enright also discussed the challenge of playing Woman’s Voice as ‘very intense’

(ibid.), adding:

[T]here is a pressure once you kind of find that voice to be able to


maintain that voice throughout the piece. Because when you go silent
again, there is a fear sometimes that that voice won't come out again.
(ibid.)

Enright was able to effectively keep ‘the voice’ that we had found in rehearsal for the

performance, using the head tilt and deep breathing that she found useful with Alba

Emoting.

Pennington felt that both the performances went well from her perspective,

which I agree with. Pennington further reflected:

[W]ith the audience came a level of focus which I was pleased was there,
because it's not always there, you can be ready to perform but then
something kind of pulls you out of it, so last night it was good. It
worked. And I didn't really notice people shifting, so I must have been,
in the zone. (Pennington 2010a)

All of the actors involved in this case study achieved a level of focus or being ‘in the

zone’ in their performances. Elliott-Archer, also had a similar experience to Pennington:

159
[I]t's kind of strange like you have to get into a certain kind of focused
Zen in which like, it is focusing about the timing and the musicality and
the rhythm, the diction, your enunciation of it, and you know, you barely
recognize the words that you’re saying. (Elliott-Archer 2010a)

After the second performances of Play and Footfalls, I gave a presentation on the

rehearsal process. To watch the presentation after the performances, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 4: 12. Case Study 2 - Presentation (Duration: 00:37:33)

The findings for the process of Footfalls are not as clear as those for Play.

Before the performance, but a week after working with the lighting, Pennington

remarked:

I feel like following Beckett to the word, not trying to put anything else
on to it, it’s actually all there for me. […] I feel like I’m just channeling
it. I’m not trying to do anything with it, I’m just trying to keep my voice
low, keep the pace slow, remember where the pauses are, and when I do,
it works. Sometimes if the pauses aren’t long enough it’s not as
powerful. So I feel really proud of it but I don’t feel like I have a right to
be proud of it, because I don’t feel like it’s necessarily my doing. I feel
like the writing’s really strong. And I didn’t really see that before.
(Pennington 2010b)

I am in agreement with Pennington here. While we found some of the techniques very

useful to the process of realizing a production of Footfalls, simply following Beckett’s

instructions for the lighting, the rhythm of the text, and the movement of the pacing,

were the most important factors for the success of the performances.

Section IV: The ‘Affect’ of Beckett’s Plays on the Actor

Phillip Zarrilli, in his book Psychophysical Acting, insists ‘Beckett appears to

require the actor to overtly do less; however, what Beckett demands is that the actor

does more’ (Zarrilli 2009: 123). I found this to be especially true for the actors involved

in this case study. Previously, I was trying to examine ‘emotion’ in rehearsals and
160
performance independently of any ‘emotion’ that the actor may have to the rehearsal

process or the performance itself. This project has revealed that these phenomena are

more interlinked than I had considered. In his article ‘Playing Play’, W. B. Worthen

comments that ‘Beckett casts his actors as automata, sharply limiting the bodily

expression that locates the actor’s authorizing “presence” within the performance. […]

instead of affirming a Stanislavskian “I am,” their performance nearly reduces them to

“not I.”’ (Worthen 1985: 406). When asked if the experience of performing Beckett had

affected the actors as individuals, Elliott-Archer responded:

It does have a phys - an emotional response. And when you kneel and it's
like, it's time, and that light comes on […]. You can feel the light on your
shoulders, and the light sitting on there, it’s very intense. (Elliott-Archer
2010a)

It is interesting to note here that Elliott-Archer began to say ‘physical’ response, and

then chose the word ‘emotional’. The words that appeared most frequently in the actors’

testimonies included ‘intensity’, ‘fear’ and ‘exhaustion’. As Pennington reflected:

[W]hen you finish, you are exhausted. You don't realize how tired you
are when you are out there because of the adrenaline, but there is an
energy that keeps you going, and the need to keep walking as well is
quite useful. But when you stop, I mean, I think holding the body as tight
as I do, whether it is conscious or not, because like, I can't let it go,
because it doesn't have the same impact on the voice or the emotion I
feel, so it's draining when you stop. But I think because it is so
monotonous and lonely and isolated and contained, it feels like there is
an emptiness inside you. (Pennington 2010a)

161
Figure 18: Elizabeth Pennington in Footfalls. Photograph by Ben Borley.

162
Chapter Five: Case Study Three: Less Than a Year by Helena
Enright

The purpose of this chapter is to discuss the rehearsal process and performances

of Less Than a Year by Helena Enright. This research project is the third of three case

studies intended to gain a clearer insight into the nature of emotion in Western

performance, specifically in relation to a particular dramaturgy. In this project, I

incorporated Alba Emoting, Emotional Access Work and Impulse and Awareness Work

throughout the rehearsal process on a documentary play, specifically using a Verbatim

text. The play used in this case study is Less than a Year by Irish playwright Helena

Enright and was created from the transcript of an interview with a couple who lost their

teenage daughter to cancer. Section I: The Acting ‘Problems’ of Verbatim Theatre

discusses the acting problems that arise from working with this particular kind of text.

The second section addresses the set-up and methodology while Section III: Less Than

a Year in Rehearsals and Performance analyses the rehearsal process and

performances. The final section offers concluding thoughts on the case study.

Section I: The Acting ‘Problems’ of Verbatim Theatre

The term Verbatim Theatre is attributed to Derek Paget (1987). After a

discussion with playwright Rony Robinson, Paget defines Verbatim Theatre as ‘a form

of theatre firmly predicated upon the taping and subsequent transcription of interviews

with ‘ordinary’ people’ (Paget 1987: 317). More recently, Hammond and Stewart in

Verbatim Verbatim (2008) clarify that ‘the term verbatim refers to the origins of the text

spoken in the play’ (Hammond and Steward 2008: 9). Elaborating further on Paget’s

original definition, they add:

163
The words of real people are recorded or transcribed by a dramatist
during an interview or research process, or are appropriated from
existing records such as the transcripts of an official enquiry. They are
then edited, arranged or recontextualized to form a dramatic
presentation, in which actors take on the characters of the real
individuals whose words are being used. (ibid.)

In some cases, the actors involved are actually part of the process of

interviewing the 'real' subjects, while other times the interviews are conducted solely by

the writer or director, or a combination of both. Current practitioners often associated

with Verbatim Theatre include: Robin Soans (author of Life After Scandal [2007],

Talking to Terrorists [2005], The Arab-Israeli Cookbook [2004]), David Hare (The

Permanent Way [2003]), Alecky Blythe (The Girlfriend Experience [2008], Come Out

Eli [2004]), Anna Deavere Smith (Twilight [1992], Fires in the Mirror [1992]), Emily

Mann (Anulla Allen [1997], Execution of Justice [1986]), Jessica Blank and Eric Jenson

(The Exonerated [2005]) as well as directors Max Stafford-Clark (Out of Joint) and

Nicholas Kent (Tricycle Theatre).

Verbatim Theatre is a form of documentary theatre. One of the practitioners

often associated with developing an early documentary form is Erwin Piscator. In an

essay on ‘Objective Acting’, Piscator observes that this new theatre ‘required a new

actor’ (Piscator 1974: 304). He continues:

I could no longer use the classic declamatory actor—in love with his
voice and uninterested in what he said, but only in how he said it.
Neither could I accept the Chekhovian actor, hypnotizing himself behind
the “fourth wall.” (ibid.)

While Verbatim Theatre differs slightly from the form from which it developed,

Piscator’s observation is still relevant. A recurring theme in the literature surrounding

Verbatim Theatre is the challenges that it presents to the performer.

Few sources specifically focus on the challenges of acting Verbatim Theatre,

with the exception of a few reflections by actor Bella Merlin who performed in Hare's

The Permanent Way (2007, 2008) and several interviews in Playing for Real: Actors on

164
Playing Real People (Cantrell and Luckhurst 2010). However, within the budding

literature on Verbatim Theatre in general, many clues and observations about the

potential challenges of performance have emerged. Several points that I have identified

include: 1) A different structure or ‘shape’ than traditional plays; 2) Direct address with

the audience; 3) Requires ‘not-acting’ rather than ‘acting’; 4) Performances can be

creatively restrictive; 5) Use of vernacular speech, rhythms, ‘ums’ and ‘errs’ 6) Finding

the ‘essence’ of the character rather than mimicking; and 7) Use of emotion in the

performance. In addition to these technical challenges are the added ethical

implications/responsibility of the actor playing a ‘real person.’ While I will attempt to

discuss each of these challenges individually, it must be noted that many of these issues

are intertwined.

Robin Soans, an actor as well as a Verbatim playwright, describes Verbatim

Theatre as ‘a particularly nerve-wracking form of theatre for the actors’ (Soans in

Hammond and Stewart 2008: 21). After working as an actor on Waiting Room Germany

(1995) at the Royal Court, Soans’ observations on the differences between acting in

Verbatim and non-Verbatim plays are as follows:

Most performances have a geographical as well as an emotional and


psychological shape: ‘This is where I amble to the sideboard, laugh in a
cavalier fashion, and pour myself a brandy.’ There was none of that here.
In Waiting Room Germany we spent most of the time sitting on chairs at
the front of the stage talking to the audience. (ibid.)

With Verbatim Theatre having a particularly unique (or even being devoid of)

'geographical, emotional and psychological shape', many actors find the genre

particularly difficult, especially the convention of direct address with the audience.

Soans concludes that in Verbatim Theatre, ‘the principal skill required of the actor

remains that of a storyteller, and his or her key relationship is with the audience’ (ibid.).

In Playing for Real: Actors on Playing Real People, Diane Fletcher, who played

Claire Short in Called into Account (2007), describes the experience as ‘terrifying’

165
(Cantrell and Luckhurst 2010: 71) and maintains that this kind of work requires an

incredible amount of concentration: ‘[i]f you let your concentration waver at all, you

were lost and you couldn't act your way out of it. You couldn't invent; you mustn't

invent.' (ibid.) Chipo Chung, an actor in Talking to Terrorists (2005), explains that one

of the challenges of using direct address (as opposed to conventional 'fourth wall'

drama) is that 'as you're not acting opposite someone, you don't have their energy to use

in a dramatic exchange' (ibid.: 57-58). Finding and maintaining the energy, focus and

discipline required for Verbatim Theatre is similar, as Soans points out, to the task of a

storyteller. According to storyteller Michael Parent, the storyteller 'remains a vehicle for

the story and the audience remains a depository for it, and the storyteller in particular

must not allow his/her artistry or ego to get in the way of it' (qtd. in Wilson 2006: 84).

Among Verbatim Theatre practitioners, there is an emerging debate about the

actor’s task. Nicholas Kent insists that ‘for actors it’s not like being in an ordinary play’

(Kent in Hammond and Stewart 2008: 155) and this is commonly reported throughout

the literature. Kent attributes this difference to the content of the work, as most

Verbatim plays tend to tackle a specific event, social or political phenomenon, or

moment in history:

[The actors] know they’re taking part in something that is to some extent
‘history’, so they come with such a commitment to the truth and the
project that the minute anyone sees anyone else acting, everyone knows
– so no one acts; it’s like there’s an unwritten pledge that in no way will
anyone do anything for effect. So the atmosphere is very restrained.
(ibid.: 155-156).

In Kent’s observations, two points stand out in particular; a distinction between acting

and 'not' acting, and the idea that performances must be 'restrained'.

This idea of ‘restraint’ is discussed frequently by actors involved in Verbatim

Theatre. Chung describes documentary theatre as a form that 'calls for more restraint in

one's acting’ and refers to Talking to Terrorists as ‘in many ways the most restrictive

play I have ever been in’ (Cantrell and Luckhurst 2010: 57). According to Chung:
166
…the less outward theatricality you display, the more you serve the
representation of the person you are playing. It is harder work, less fun
than fiction definitely, and very exacting. There's a very high level of
focus and concentration (ibid.: 58).

Actor Alwyne Taylor, who appeared in When Can I Have a Banana Again (1982) at the

Derby Playhouse, warns:

You can give too much to a speech. People say something very serious
and moving – they’re not crying! They might be crying inside. But it’s
meant – it comes from the gut. You give too much and the audience
doesn’t have to work, or has to shy back…There’s an awful lot to
learn…particularly how to underplay. [original emphasis] (qtd in Paget
1987: 332)

The other idea that recurs in actors’ experiences with Verbatim is the idea that 'less is

more'. Chung, under the direction of Max Stafford-Clark, reveals 'the general acting

note was less, less, less, to make it as super-real as possible. There was no room for

thespian enthusiasm and largesse' (Cantrell and Luckhurst 2010: 58). Merlin’s

observation of her colleagues in The Permanent Way (also directed by Stafford-Clark)

was that ‘the more simple the acting style and the less cluttered the physical vocabulary

or the vocal colouring, the more deeply moving the performances could be’ (Merlin

2007b: 48). This raises the question then of how difficult is it for an actor to do ‘less’,

to adopt a ‘simple’ acting style, and, paradoxically, ‘not’ act?

Another challenge with Verbatim Theatre is the language that is used in the

texts. Unlike a wholly imagined play where a playwright is dictating the rhythm of the

piece, in a Verbatim text (although edited and crafted by a writer) the language and

rhythm are dictated by the interviewees whose testimonies have provided the source

material. Robinson believes ‘there has to be in the process for actors a realization of the

differentness of the material’ (qtd in Paget 1987: 330) and it is necessary for the actors

to have ‘recognition that the way in which people talk, the repetitiveness, the stumbling,

the oddity, is something that if they want to get…to make the verbatim stuff work on

stage, they’ve got to acknowledge’ (ibid.) Different Verbatim practitioners address this

167
problem in different ways. Alecky Blythe, founder of the theatre company Recorded

Delivery, also believes that ‘it is the actor’s instinct to perform: to heighten, to try to

make their lines ‘more interesting’ in an effort to project their character and make the

person they are playing seem real.' (Blythe in Hammond and Stewart 2008: 81-82). To

overcome this tendency, Blythe uses the recorded testimonies in performance so that

the actors are actually listening to the real person through a headset as they deliver the

text in performance. For Blythe, relying on the actor's 'instinct to perform' when

working with everyday speech 'is not the best approach, because everyday speech is

often more mundane and ‘everyday’ than anyone dares to invent. This is what gives it

the ring of truth’ (ibid.: 82). Soans purposefully chooses to retain many of the ‘ums and

ers, the stutters and repetitions' (Soans in Hammond and Stewart 2008: 41) in the play

to ‘highlight’ when ‘interviewees becomes very emotional’ (ibid.), a potentially useful

clue for actors trying to navigate his scripts.

Yet another challenge that arises from this type of work is ‘impersonation’,

which according to Paget ‘is very far from the aims of Verbatim Theatre’ (Paget 1987:

330). Many practitioners claim that mimicry is not part of the process. Stafford-Clark

insists that ‘you’re not doing impersonations of these people, but you are trying to

capture their spirit in some way’ (Hare & Stafford-Clark in Hammond and Stewart

2008: 65). Paul Makeham, an actor in the Australian production of Aftershocks, is clear

that the actors involved made ‘no attempt to mimic the story-tellers – the actors’ job

here is more to demonstrate than to impersonate' (Makeham 2005: 77). How then, is the

actor supposed to achieve this?

Anna Deavere Smith believes she can ‘learn to know who somebody is, not from

what they tell me, but from how they tell me’ (qtd. in Martin 2002: 338). Smith

prepares for her performances by rehearsing with the original recordings from the

interviews although she does not use the recordings in performance as Blythe does.

168
Through repetition she maintains that the words ‘make an impression on [her] body

and eventually on [her] psyche’ (ibid.). Her ultimate goal is to ‘become possessed, so to

speak, of the person’ (ibid.).

Jessica Blank and Eric Jensen have found a similar power in the actual words

without using the audio recordings to aid the actors. While working on The Exonerated,

they witnessed a strange phenomenon:

...the actors started to “channel” the real people whose words they were
reading. […] We were shocked to find that, even without any outside
input, the actors spoke and looked and moved almost exactly like the
exonerated folks whose words they were reading. Different actors read
the roles each day, the actors themselves as diverse in looks and
mannerisms as you could imagine. But as soon as they picked up the
pages and started reading, they all talked and moved just like Kerry or
Robert or Brad. The first couple days, we ascribed this phenomenon to
chance. But as we saw it happen over and over, without any special
effort on the part of the actors, we realized it was coming out of the
words. (Blank and Jensen 2005: 187-188)

It was this discovery that made the authors acknowledge ‘just how much our

psychology is contained in our speech’ (ibid.: 188).

The question of emotion in Verbatim Theatre also appears frequently in the

literature. Merlin, when writing about her experience playing the Second Bereaved

Mother in Hare's The Permanent Way maintains ‘the challenge to us as actors […] was

to present the “emotion recalled in tranquility” without the playing style seeming

inappropriately effortless or internally unconnected’ (Merlin 2007b: 46). Taylor’s

observation is consistent with Merlin’s idea of 'emotion recalled in tranquility', insisting

that ‘when people get upset in an interview, it’s not adrenalin-upset, like actors – it’s

emotional memory' (qtd in Paget 1987: 332). It is interesting that Taylor uses a

reference to actors when discussing ‘adrenalin-upset’, another implication that in

relation to this work the emotion can easily be overplayed. Chung, based on her

experiences in both Talking to Terrorists and Fallujah (2007), notes how 'in

documentary in general, you have to use a much more controlled emotional spectrum’

169
(Cantrell and Luckhurst 2010: 58). Her observation suggests that when working with

this type of text actors must recognize a different kind of ‘emotion’ and the idea that it

needs to be ‘controlled’. Merlin also makes another intriguing observation of her

experience performing:

I, as an independent individual, had to 'get out of the way' of the


character's words; there was no room for my own emotional response.
To replicate technically my observation of the mother's breakdown –
when to hesitate, when to falter, when to crack my voice – was the
required acting strategy. Any emotion, any tears, had to be her emotions,
her tears, and not my pity suffered on her behalf: it was a subtle, but
significant nuance. [original emphasis] (Merlin 2008: 134)

Merlin here is implying that working on a Verbatim text required a different use of

‘emotion’ than her experience of working on other play texts. Later in the article she

even describes her acting as ‘Brechtian’ (ibid.). Does Verbatim Theatre require a certain

‘distancing’ from the performer that other genres do not? This case study will attempt to

address some of these issues.

Section II: Methodology and Set-up of Case Study Three

Aside from shedding light on my primary research questions about the nature of

‘emotion’ in performance, this specific case study also attempts to answer some

secondary questions based on the experiences of the particular actors involved, i.e. how

they interact with excerpts from a particular text, and their experiences with three

particular techniques as introduced to them by a particular director/researcher. The

research questions guiding this particular case study include the following: What do

these three theories/training techniques reveal about the nature of emotion in Western

performance when put into practice specifically on a Verbatim text in relation to its

demands on the actor? How do these revelations compare to the other dramaturgies in

the previous case studies? And how can these techniques be used to aid the actors as

170
they face the challenges presented to them by Verbatim Theatre, not only in rehearsal,

but also over the course of a run of six performances?

Again, as with my other research projects, this case study will make no

universal claims about the nature of emotion in performance; rather, it is an attempt to

arrive at a better understanding of how the director might approach the question and

nature of emotion in a specific Verbatim text in rehearsals and in performance. The

methodology used in this case study is qualitative, based on interviews with the actors,

and our observations of the rehearsal process and performance, which will be filmed

and documented, and evaluated and reflected upon throughout the process and the

performances. In the first case study, I used excerpts from Three Sisters by Chekhov.

While a useful study for my research, having the actors only work on sections of the

script made it difficult to calculate how these techniques would work on a full character

journey. In the second study, I used two short plays (Beckett's Footfalls and Play) in

their entirety rather than excerpts to build on from the first research project. In this third

case study there are six performances of the play. The aim of this was to allow the

actors to have the experience of exploring what happens to the 'emotion' over the course

of a run of performances as well as performing in different performance spaces.

Less Than a Year by Helena Enright

The play Less Than a Year by Helena Enright was written for Enright’s master’s

degree in Drama and Theatre Studies at the University of Cork in 2005. The play was

created from the transcripts of recorded interviews conducted by Enright in 2004 with

an Irish couple whose teenage daughter died of cancer. While ‘[t]he play centres on the

true story of a young teenager who died of a form of cancer called Ewing’s Sarcoma’,

Enright reminds us that ‘[t]he story is told in the words of the parents and is their

version of what happened from the time of their daughter’s diagnosis to her death’ [my

emphasis] (Enright 2005: 49). I point this out because in the theatre industry there has
171
been much debate about the idea of ‘truth’ in Verbatim Theatre. From my perspective,

Verbatim Theatre allows the audience to take the place of the interviewer and listen to

the stories presented, as they were told to the original interviewer. As for truth, the

‘truth’ present in Verbatim Theatre is that the words spoken really were

spoken…whether or not the ‘real’ person being interviewed is telling the ‘truth’ is

another matter.

In her article ‘‘Letting it breathe’: Writing and performing the words of others’

(2011), Enright maintains that the actor’s ‘primary task [is] to embody the words

previously spoken by real people so that they [can] speak to [the audience] without

losing their original significance and intent.’ [original emphasis] (Enright 2011a: 187).

In order to help the actors achieve this, Enright transcribes the interviews as

meticulously as possible:

I listen carefully to the words the person speaks paying particular


attention to their phrasing – their ‘punctuation’ of their words. I treat this
punctuation like a form of musical notation in order to indicate on paper
to the actors a sense of the rhythm of the person’s speech. This often
requires my listening to the recordings several times in order to
determine how I can best illustrate this rhythm on paper. (ibid.: 186)

Enright’s careful notation is exemplified in an excerpt of the text from Less Than a

Year:

Mother So we went home anyway,


weeks were passing by anyway
and Emma wasn’t gettin any better.
She was starting to get weak at this stage and she had a little job.
She was after doin fifth year, and she had a little job in a bakery
and she said Mammy I’m giving that job up
and she was sitting on the couch
and this is about eight weeks later.
I said look it I’m not happy.
The lump is not goin (I said to myself)
and girls from work were giving me all stuff to rub on it.
So anyway am, I rang the local surgery
and I said did you ever get any results back
from Wexford hospital about Emma,
that swelling she had.
And they said no they never heard a word.
That was on a Monday.
172
I rang again on the Tuesday
and I said I’m getting worried about this.
This is not goin and it’s starting now to change completely
from a soft lump to a hard lump and she’s in pain.
What in the name of God am I goin to do?
They said that they would ring up Wexford hospital.
On the 13th of June, twelve o clock
he (Father) rang me at work to see what the story was.

Father She’s told me there’s something serious.


We have to go to the hospital
and make an appointment.
And we are to go as soon as
we get the appointment.

This excerpt also highlights the challenges of the venacular, including awkard phrasing

and repetitions.

Less Than a Year was first produced professionally by Island Theatre Company

in Limerick in 2006 with actors Seamus Moran and Joan Sheehy as the parents. Moran

describes his experience working on the play as ‘a fascinating process because people

don’t, [sic] their speech patterns are totally erratic. That’s been very difficult to master

and to learn […] everybody’s speech patterns are unique to themselves’ (qtd. in Enright

2011: 189). Sheehy, who played Mother, described her experience as ‘one of the

toughest […] if not the toughest challenge I have faced as an actor in over twenty years’

(ibid.). Though Sheehy added:

The language gives you everything you need to tell the story and find the
character […] the rhythms of speech, the repetition, the oddness of
expression is so rich and particular that you have to immerse yourself in
that and run with it and not impose a character or style of performance.
More than anything I’ve ever done I had to trust the language and kill
my instincts to embellish or act or strive for significant or emotional
moments. (ibid.)

The purpose of this case study is to establish whether or not any of these techniques can

aid the actors in their task of performance, while still respecting this notion of ‘trusting

the language’.

173
The actors involved in this case study were Charlie Coldfield, a professional

actor based in the South West of England, playing Father, and the playwright, Helena

Enright, playing the role of the Mother. Enright had previous experience with Verbatim

Theatre, acting in a production of Walking Away (2006), a testimony piece about

domestic violence also written by Enright. This was Coldfield’s first experience with

Verbatim Theatre.

I should note that as a playwright, Enright chooses to describe her writing as

‘theatre of testimony’ rather than Verbatim Theatre. Enright offers two reasons for this,

maintaining that ‘firstly, the term provides more of a sense of where, how and why the

words originated and secondly, that the term allows for more creative space with regard

to interpretation’ (Enright 2011b: 33). Although Enright makes this distinction for her

work, Less Than a Year would also be considered a Verbatim text.

174
Figure 19: Charlie Coldfield and Helena Enright in Less Than a Year. Photograph by Anna Johnson.

175
Section III: Less Than a Year in Rehearsals and Performance

Due to scheduling requirements, the rehearsal process for this case study was

relatively short, lasting just over two weeks. Our first step was a close and careful read-

through of the script. Aside from a prologue in which the actors introduce and offer

context about the story they will be telling, Less Than a Year is an hour-long section of

an interview with the Irish couple. I chose to divide the play into sections to give the

actors ‘anchor points’ and to assist the actors with the ‘shape’ of the play. Because the

script is taken from the transcript of an actual interview, the parents did not follow a

strictly linear narrative. The parents jump back and forth in the timeline, as well as go

off on tangents as memories and moments occur to them. By sectioning the play, and

adding subtle blocking in-between each section, the actors had a more solid structure to

rely on than what is offered by the text.

The vernacular speech, governed by the idiosyncrasies of the individuals who

offered their testimony rather than the dictated rhythm of the playwright, made this

material incredibly difficult for the actors to learn. The mother’s speech is littered with

‘so anyways’, ‘in the meantime’, ‘and I said’ and ‘then he said’ and so forth. Her turns

of phrase are more often than not, unusual and in some places awkward. The script is

also full of repetition of similar phrasing, false stops and starts, self-correction, ‘ams’,

‘uhs’. But in some places, this jumbled information transforms in to poetic rhythms. An

example would be the Mother saying:

Mother: I came home anyway and I got alright.


Because the hospital had finished, the chemotherapy was over,
and Emma was in remission.

At each rehearsal we used under-reading so that the actors would be able to absorb the

text aurally. The reader will recall that under-reading is where, instead of having a

script in the actors’ hands, the lines are fed to them by another reader, allowing them

176
freedom to begin to connect physically with their partner and the text. However, at the

end of each rehearsal we would return to the script to read through the material. In this

way the actors were learning the lines in two ways. Enright, whose character had a

significant majority of the text found the under-reading very useful ‘to allow [Enright]

to get up straight away and to move around with [the text], and just play with it and get

a sense of it’ (Coldfield and Enright 2011). Although she also added that she ‘needed to

go back to the script at times to actually map the journey’ (ibid.), so the combination of

both processes of reading and under-reading was crucial.

Alba Emoting

The rehearsal process began with basic Alba Emoting training, although from

the outset I did not plan to use Alba Emoting as rigorously as I had in the previous case

studies. Much of the literature surrounding Verbatim Theatre calls for ‘emotion recalled

in tranquility’ (Merlin 2007b: 46), so therefore, I felt a basic Alba Emoting training

would suffice. I did not plan to have the actors actively use the technique on stage;

instead I wanted them to use Alba Emoting so they could recognize the patterns in

themselves. Enright found the patterns useful for ‘allowing the sadness to happen at

times’ (Coldfield and Enright 2011). Enright explained:

I found with the Alba, it was – not if I knew there was a sad part coming
up – sometimes in just, in that moment, it was useful to just kind of
collapse the shoulders and expel the air and allow myself to do that.
(ibid.)

Coldfield used Alba Emoting in order to set himself ‘in a frame of mind’ (ibid.). As

with the previous case studies, Alba Emoting continued to inform our vocabulary

throughout the process.

Emotional Access Work

Based on my previous directing experiences, I had suspected that Astbury’s

Emotional Access Work might be the predominent tool that I would use in rehearsals
177
for Less Than a Year. The two techniques that proved to be the most useful were

under-reading and Astbury’s physical exercises. We did find early on that when simply

reading the text, there was a tendency for the actors to find a very slow, worthy rhythm,

almost a one-toned sadness. Coldfield commented on this distinction:

I thought it was really useful for not getting stuck in a rut of saying
things in a certain way. I think that really came out in the difference of
how it sounded when we under-read in comparison to when we were just
reading it off the page which we did a couple of times.
(Coldfield and Enright 2011)

When we replaced the scripts with under-reading, the natural storytelling was able to

come through. This technique also proved useful for the actors absorbing the text.

Enright, who had a majority of text in the play later remarked, ‘if I hadn’t had it up on

its feet in those two weeks, and had the script with me all the time, it would have been a

lot more difficult for me to come off book so quickly’ (ibid.).

Astbury’s physical exercises are intended to help remove any blocks, but also

physically engage the body and open up possibilities as to how the scene could be

played. Astbury affectionately calls these exercises ‘Left-Brain Disabling Devices’.

This is a reference to split brain theory, but Astbury only uses it as handy rehearsal

room terminology, as the brain is more complicated than that. As he says in his book,

Trusting the Actor, ‘[b]oth areas of the brain need to work in balance with each other.

Unfortunately, far too many training systems tend to encourage the use of the conscious

left-brain and ignore the wonders of the right (Astbury 2011: location 1379). His work

is aimed at trying to bypass the conscious controlling self through physical exercises

that often lead to a form of emotional release, which both Enright and Coldfield

experienced in this rehearsal process in the form of crying/sobbing, similar to grief. The

actor’s experiences were triggered through a combination of physical responses and

repetition of sections of the text—something that came as a surprise both of them.

Coldfield recounts his experience with the physical exercises:

178
I think I obviously broke quite quick, cause […] you know, that sort of
physical grueling element, you know, versus the fact that you suddenly
just, you can’t, you know, you can’t control it. And I think I said earlier
when crying, you feel out of control but not in a way that you feel like
vulnerable, like we didn’t feel vulnerable, you know. You could just get
up and walk out and you know […] you totally feel like you’re your own
boss still, but at the same time you feel out of control but happy to be out
of control because, because that’s just the state you’re in. (Coldfield and
Enright 2011)

Coldfield is referring here to the fact that once the physical exercises had finished and

rehearsal had ended, no residual emotion remained. To watch a clip of Coldfield during

the physical exercises, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 1: 1. Coldfield in Physical Exercises (Duration:


00:00:34)

Enright also had a very strong physiological response to the text and the

exercises, specifically working on a section describing a trip that the family took to the

shrine at Lourdes in France. Again, we went through all the steps from an anger run

through the physical exercises, and ended with Enright sitting up and simply telling the

story. During the physical exercises, especially when her legs were up in the air, and

later when Enright was on her side whispering the text, Enright experienced what she

described as ‘convulsions’ (Beck et al. 2011). After the physical exercises, Enright

encountered an emotional connection with the speech about Lourdes. To watch a clip of

Enright after the physical exercises, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 1: 2. Enright after Physical Exercises (Duration:


00:01:31)

The following is Enright’s reflection on the experience:

[W]hen I was actually lying down there, in it […] I was hysterical, I was
sobbing, I was going through it. I wasn’t going through it like [the
parents] did, but that’s the intensity of the feeling. But when I sat up and
tried to tell [the story], it was as if that power was behind it, and I knew I
could go there. But that’s what wasn’t the purpose, the purpose was to
tell you, so it was interesting. Because I actually felt as if I…not as if I
179
had that experience, but I had the emotion of that experience, if that
makes sense, to back up the telling of it. So it did feel connected. (ibid.)

Enright made a substantial point, that in this case, the emotional access work allowed

her to directly and physiologically connect with ‘the emotion of the experience’ that the

parents had when they traveled to Lourdes. The experience of the exercises added a

‘power’ that informed the telling of the story. At the end of the discussion, Enright

turned to me and said, ‘Jess, I did not ever think that you would ever get that out of me.

Nobody, no other director, has got that level out of me before. Ever.’ (ibid.). The

physical exercises can be very powerful for the actor in rehearsal, and equally powerful

for those watching the exercises. But as Hetzler queried in the first case study, are these

performances repeatable? This will be discussed in the section on the performances.

Impulse and Awareness work

For this case study, the impulse and awareness exercises were very subtle, and

mostly consisted of ‘impulse runs’ in which the actors could be ‘on their feet’ and tell

the story without the performance restrictions. In the actual performance, the actors

were mostly seated and speaking directly to the audience. In the impulse runs the actors

were free to speak to each other, or themselves, and could move around the space and

use the Alba Emoting patterns if they so chose. In one impulse run I joined in, sitting on

the sofa to act as a point of focus for their story-telling. Enright appreciated the impulse

work as a means of getting energy into the body and the text ‘because it’s very hard

when you’re just sitting here, not to let the voice sink. […] You’ve got to keep still, and

the energy about it and its quite difficult. […] those exercises are quite useful’

(Coldfield and Enright 2011). Coldfield found the exercises to ‘a good way of finding

things […] relating to some of the words that you’re saying. Suddenly [the words] ring

true, and you’re trying it all different ways (ibid.). In one impulse run, Coldfield had a

180
particularly strong connection with a section of the script in which he describes going

to the pub with his daughter. To watch a clip of this section, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 1: 3. Coldfield in Impulse Run (Duration:


00:00:53)

Coldfield reflected:

I remember being in that position, and it, and it, sort of making sense,
[…] something in the sort of intimacy of it, I suppose. Yeah. It does put
you in mood…in the mood for things…and you know, if you experiment
with something like, you suddenly find, “ooh”, that’s the tone, the
rhythm. (ibid.)

Enright pointed out a potential ethical dilemma. During the impulse runs, we were using

the text, and at certain points the actors were speaking text that was actually spoken by

the daughter, Emma. At one point towards the end of the play, the character of Mother,

repeating the words of her dying daughter, says ‘Mammy, this is not my home.’ Enright

reported that using this text in the impulse work felt ‘strange’ (ibid.), reporting:

[I]t really struck me, especially when I was saying lines that the mother
was saying the daughter had said. That when we were playing with these
lines, within the safety net of the studio, the rehearsal space, it just felt a
bit odd, it jarred with me. It didn’t mean that I didn’t play with it,
because […] anytime we played with [the lines] I wasn’t being
disrespectful in a sense, even if I was like shouting something that she
might have been saying, you know, about dying, or “I’m not in my own
bed.” But it just jarred with me a little bit. (ibid.)

There was a morbid feeling when Enright mentioned this, but ultimately she concluded

that, as we were using the exercises in order to enhance the performances in the play, it

was not a high priority ethical concern. Enright concluded:

I think in the safety [of the rehearsal room] and that because what we
were trying to do was tell the story as best as possible, and sort of, you
know, as honestly as possible, you need to be able to have that space to
go there and find different things in the physicality, and absorb it that
way […] I really do. I like that kind of work. (ibid.)

181
As this did ‘jar’ with Enright, I have not included a clip of this moment, but will add

that I also believe that we were respectful of the original material in the public showing

of it.

In Performance

We had six performances of Less Than a Year between the 15th and 20th of

January, 2011. Five performances were held at the University of Exeter and one

performance took place at The Bike Shed Theatre in Exeter. The first performance on

Saturday January 15th was accompanied by a formal presentation. To watch the

presentation please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 1: 4. Case Study 3 - Presentation (Duration:


00:26:16)

This was the first time the actors had the experience of performing in front of an

audience, and Enright described the performance as ‘pretty scary’ (Coldfield & Enright

2011), but also ‘electric’ (ibid.). In the dress rehearsal before the performance, I asked

the actors to ‘mark’ through it, meaning for them not to give full performances, but just

go lightly through the text and blocking. Instead, the actors gave a strong performance

in which Enright had a very strong emotional connection with a particular section of the

script describing the first time the daughter wore a wig. To watch a clip of this moment,

please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 1: 5. Enright in Dress Rehearsal (Duration:


00:00:48)

Enright reflected:

[E]ven though we were just supposed to mark it...again, the impulse to


tell the story comes out. And I just kind of went with it, and got quite
emotional in places I hadn’t expected to…and then I didn’t expect to
then leave those places out in the actual performance. (Coldfield &
Enright 2011)
182
As Enright mentioned, this section of the script was unintentionally left out of the actual

Saturday performance. While Enright was aware that a few sections of the text had been

missed, it was not until after the performance that she realized it those sections left out

were the very same sections that she experienced a strong emotional connection in the

dress rehearsal. I can only speculate as to why this occurred. Working from Astbury’s

theory of defenses, Astbury maintains ‘your defences are endlessly cunning in their

protection, their desire to keep you from the experience of pain, or anger, or fear, or any

one of the multitude of feelings that makes us human beings’ (Astbury 2011: location

2859 – 2866). It is possible that because Enright had experienced a strong emotional

connection to that particular section of text only a few hours before the performance,

Enright’s defenses, operating below her conscious awareness, prohibited her from

experiencing that feeling again so soon.

Reflecting on performing in front of an audience for the first time, Enright

remarked:

[T]hat was quite terrifying doing it in front of the audience having, there
was no way you could call for line, no nothing. […] And even though I
had forgotten those lines and different things, it was quite a confidence
builder that we could get ourselves out of it, not lose connection, and get
through to the end. […] and once that was done it was like ‘phew’.
(Coldfield & Enright 2011)

Enright also added, ‘There certainly was a buzz – I could feel a very intense listening

from the audience that I haven’t felt – didn’t feel in other performances’ (ibid.) To

watch the first performance, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 1: 6. Performance of Less Than a Year - 15


January 2011 (Duration: 01:06:35)

The performance at the Bike Shed Theatre was held on January 16th as a

fundraiser for the Bone Cancer Research Trust, the charity that is supported by the

mother who shared her story. To watch the charity performance, please go to:
183
• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 2: 7. Performance of Less Than a Year - 16
January 2011 (Duration: 01:11:28)

In a five-star review, a critic from UK Theatre Web described the play as ‘stunningly

effective’. (To read the full review, please see Appendix D). From my perspective, this

was one of the best of the six performances, as the actors were nearly word perfect in

regard to the text. The actors also coped well with performing the play in a new space.

Enright maintained:

I was very aware of the technical elements in the theatre. But I just got
on with telling the story, I just thought “oh, we’ve got to go through this
and tell them, it’s their first time hearing it” […] and trust it and yeah,
and I did feel then that kind of electric kind of sense to it. […] And I
think I was buzzing on a bit of a confidence from that performance the
day before, knowing that I’d messed up in a couple of places, got us out
even though I did miss some elements of the script, but, they still got it,
we still told the story, the essence of the story was fine so I thought no
matter what happens I can do that I can get out of it fine. So yeah, it did
have that kind of adrenaline pumping through it. (ibid.)

The adrenaline that Enright mentions carried through to the next performance, on

Monday January 17th when Less Than a Year returned to the studio space in Thornlea at

the University of Exeter. Enright added that she and Coldfield were, ‘still on a bit of a

high from the night before’ (ibid.) Coldfield’s experience of this performance is as

follows:

I thought Monday went well, I thought it was a good one. I thought it


had, that maybe we’d reached it, that there was almost like a slight, it
wasn’t a dip, but like a plateau from Sunday, so, you know, it was a
combination of energy and a growing confidence, and so I thought it was
a very, kind of slick, you know, performance […] The emotion was still
there and I thought it was quite affecting for the audience. (ibid.)

In essence, the first three performances were all highly commendable, from my point of

view as a director. It was not until the fourth performance that the actors experienced an

unpleasant performance.

The performance on Tuesday, January 18th is what I would describe as ‘the

second night slump’. A familiar concept to those working in the theatre industry, this

occurs when the first performance of a play goes very well and is full of energy, and
184
then in the second performance a distinct dip occurs in both energy and concentration.

Although this was our fourth performance, it was our second consecutive performance

in the same space. The actors began to stumble from the prologue. To watch a clip of

this, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 2: 8. Prologue from 18 January 2011 (Duration:


00:01:47)

Coldfield recounted that he ‘got spooked from the beginning in the prologue and I don’t

know why’ (ibid.). Describing the experience, Coldfield stated:

I just paused in the middle of a sentence, which was really weird, and I,
and my mind just went completely blank… it was probably for hardly
any amount of time…but…and I don’t know why I did it, but I felt really
spooked and I then I felt really uncomfortable. […] after that I really
didn’t make any mistakes, but just the way I said things and things were
off […] I didn’t feel connected to it at all really. I mean a little bit at the
end I felt connected. But then it was almost like…you know, you worry,
you try to take the connection you have and then force it through and you
can’t. (ibid.)

This problematic start had repercussions throughout the performance, at one point even

causing Enright to ‘dry up’ or forget where she was in the script. To watch a clip of this

moment, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 2: 9. Enright from 18 January 2011 (Duration:


00:01:39)

Enright recalled:

I don’t know what happened. I think when I went blank, and cause I, I
just went completely blank on that point, and thought …I just, and
panicked…there was a washing machine going on inside like…I just
couldn’t…and I kind of knew where I was, but I just, just lost it. […]
And I knew I had to talk about the doctor and the bone marrow but […]
it was terrifying, absolutely terrifying. (ibid.)

Once Enright recovered, both actors admitted to ‘overcompensating’. The audience still

responded positively to the performance, but for Coldfield, Enright and myself, the

performance clearly lacked the energy and engagement that we had experienced over
185
the first three performances. When it came to the final scenes, both actors were

consciously searching for the emotional moments. Enright described her experience of

the end of the play as feeling ‘fake’ (ibid.). I gave Enright and Coldfield the advice that

Astbury often gives his actors, which is not to look for the emotion; do not expect it. In

his book, Astbury describes an actor who faced the same challenge to which he

responded ‘stop expecting the speech to work, to stop trying to orchestrate the emotion.

“Let it surprise you.” (Astbury 2011: location 2403). Elaborating further on this

phenomenon, Astbury states:

It’s the same old story: start to think consciously and everything
vanishes. Being “in the moment” means being in a semi-trance. Any
conscious decision hauls you out of this state. While in that blessed state
the pre-conscious brain just purrs ahead making its lightning-fast
decisions before the spoil-sport conscious brain and its defences can
sabotage them. (Astbury 2011: location 2403)

Of course, achieving this state is not always easy. Fortunately, the Tuesday performance

was the only time Enright and Coldfield encountered this problem. It was also an

important experience, and Enright added:

[I]n a way, it was good to have that experience as well, because it’s like
“whoa, whoa” […] and going back into Wednesday made us much more
determined to trust it, and attack it, and for me, it was about making sure
I had the energy to do it. Because I think, for me, I felt my voice very
strange on the Tuesday night. My voice felt disconnected from me. It
went back to that ‘worthy’ kind of voice that I had when we did that
reading of it. […] So it was like “okay, let’s get this energy behind the
words again and back in to that.” (Coldfield and Enright 2011)

The performance on Wednesday January 19th was a welcomed return for the

actors. As Enright reported:

Wednesday night’s felt really nice. […] I was delighted. I was happy
because I cried. And it’s funny, I didn’t ever feel…I felt more in a state
when I hadn’t cried, or connected, because it’s not a…it’s not an
upsetting cry. It’s probably more upsetting for the people watching it.
We are performing, we’re aware of that, we…they’re aware that we’re
performing, but I think [the audience] are not expecting to be moved as
much as they are, and that’s where it gets them. (ibid.)

Coldfield commented on the emotion that came through his performance as well:
186
I think because [the emotion], because it catches you, you don’t plan it,
you don’t plan where it catches you…in the middle of a sentence…on a
couple of them it just caught me and it just took me time…there was
silence…and I just….because I couldn’t carry on speaking, because I had
to just, allow it out and then get back into it. (ibid.)

To watch a clip of both Coldfield and Enright connecting to emotion in this

performance, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 2: 10. Coldfield and Enright 19 January 2011


(Duration: 00:03:17)

The final performance took place on Thursday, January 20th 2011. The actors

completed what I considered to be a very connected and engaging performance.

However, the absence of tears in the penultimate scene made both actors question their

performances after the show. Enright commented:

I felt very connected to most of it. Just that little bit at the end […] I
don’t know….and it’s cause the tears hadn’t come and I found myself
judging myself going…why aren’t they coming? Why aren’t they
coming? […] And as the lines were coming out, “but [the tears] are not
here, they’re not here.” (ibid.)

Coldfield interrupted, ‘But it still felt like a good performance’ (ibid.) to which Enright

agreed. In the case of the final performance, Enright’s conscious search for the emotion

did not interfere with a believable and engaged telling of the story, perhaps because she

did not force the tears, but rather, was aware of their absence without interfering. To

watch a clip of the penultimate seen from the final performance, please go to:

• DVD Chapter 5, Disc 2: 11. Coldfield and Enright 20 January 2011


(Duration: 00:01:08)

Section IV: Findings

Many of the findings from the third case study have been mentioned throughout

the chapter, with the most effective technique for this production being the emotional
187
access work. But this case study, especially with a run of multiple performances, has

raised more questions. From my perspective as the director, I felt that five out of the six

performances were connected and energized. Only in four out of those five

performances did the actors experience the physiological affect of tears. This is not a

problem for me, as I believe that if the performances are connected an actor does not

have to cry in order to move an audience. But the final performance raised a question

for Enright:

I started to wonder, if we are to tell [the story] as honestly as possible, if


it’s feasible that we might cry at different points during [the play] and
the emotion can get us, [then] what happened when the emotion doesn’t
get us? If I was to force a cry last night, which I could have, then that’s
not really as honest in telling it. (Coldfield and Enright 2011)

Enright was right not to ‘force’ tears on the last night, but she did not need to, as the

play worked well regardless. This leads me to conclude that the energy and focus are

more important than the experience of emotion itself, and that only when the energy

and focus are maintained can the emotion come through in performance.

188
Conclusion

Diderot concluded that ‘on the stage, with what we call sensibility, soul, passion,

one may give one or two tirades well and miss the rest’ (Diderot and Pollack 1883: 95),

viewing emotion as an enemy of the actor. After conducting three case studies

investigating the nature of emotion in Western performance, I do not agree. Due to the

nature of this practice-as-research project, I cannot make any universal claims about the

nature of emotion in Western performance. What I can do, however, is offer the

findings of my research in relation to the context of the particular case studies with the

particular actors involved.

The research questions guiding this study were as follows:

1) What, if anything, can be revealed about the nature of emotion in Western

performance when employing these three theories/approaches to emotion in

practice? Do these discoveries vary when these approaches are put into practice

on different dramaturgies with different demands on the actor?

2) What is the nature of the emotion being expressed? Whose emotion is it? The

actor’s or the characters? Is there a difference?

3) Can these techniques assist a director in unifying an understanding of emotion

within a diverse cast? How much of the actor’s previous beliefs, training and

acting experiences affect their views on emotion in performance. How do these

previously held views/assumption interact with the three theories used?

4) Are the feelings/emotions that actors generate ‘genuine’? If so, how does the

actor generate, control, and integrate these emotions during performance?

As I began to work through these questions, I realized that in order to arrive at the

answer to the fundamental question about the nature of emotion in performance, it is

clearer to work through the questions in the reverse order.

189
Are the feelings/emotions that the actors generate ‘genuine’?

Drawing from my experience with the case studies, I have found that yes, the

feelings/emotions that actors generated during performance were ‘genuine’. As a

technique in its own right, Alba Emoting has been tested and proven to trigger the

physiological changes that occur during an emotion. In Three Sisters, Hetzler and

Sozanska used pattern 3a (joy) to achieve laughter. When the actors engaged in the

pattern, involuntary muscles in the abdomen were triggered. So when the actors were

engaged in the emotional effector patterns, they were experiencing ‘genuine’

physiological changes. I also found this to be true for the Emotional Access Work. In

Less Than a Year, both Coldfield and Enright were actually crying. The actors were

experiencing ‘genuine’ physiological changes such as tears, sniffling, and redness

appearing in the face. This was also true of Pennington in Footfalls. I am highlighting

crying here, not to favor one particular emotion over another, but because it is one of

the easier emotions in which one can perceive the physiological changes from an

outside perspective.

How does the actor generate, control, and integrate these emotions in
performance?

This question can be answered differently for each of the approaches. With the

Alba Emoting patterns, an actor can generate the particular desired emotion using the

correct configuration of the breathing-facial-postural elements of the emotional effector

pattern. This can be used in rehearsal to simply develop awareness – as with Sellman-

Leava in Three Sisters; to remove unwanted emotion – as with the cast of Play; or used

directly in performance – as with Hetzler and Sozanska in Three Sisters. In each of

these cases, the emotions were controlled by the actors regulating the intensity of the

patterns, or by choosing to employ neutral breath to clear them. Integration in

performance can happen a number of ways. 1) The patterns can be used directly as they

190
were in the case of Hetzler and Sozanska; 2) The patterns can be used in immediate

preparation for performance, an example being West’s use of the pattern 3b (sadness)

just before his scene in Three Sisters; or 3) The patterns can be used to calm nerves or

keep focus, and the patterns were used in this way by every cast member in Play.

With Astbury’s Emotional Awareness Work, his goal is for the actor to have a

connection, or emotional experience with the text, in rehearsal. This is achieved

through various physical exercises as discussed throughout the dissertation. When it

comes to integrating these emotions into performance, the actor must trust that these

emotional connections will automatically integrate into their performance, and must not

try and force the emotion. This may sound unreliable, especially when moving from a

very clear system of accessing emotion such as Alba Emoting. In practice, however,

Astbury’s techniques appear to achieve his goals effectively. In Less Than a Year, it

was when Enright consciously tried to cry, that the emotion did not surface. When

Enright trusted that the emotion would appear, it did.

With regard to the Impulse and Awareness work, the exercises, in rehearsal, are

intended to develop of kinesthetic sense of self and environment, to prepare the actor to

be able to spontaneously respond to impulses from within themselves and the theatrical

environment. The physical engagement, fatigue and surrounding stimuli such as the

other actors, rhythms and sounds, allow the actor to generate emotion in response to the

immediate situation. Integration occurs through continuous training and repetition,

which was the case with the cast of Play.

Can these techniques assist a director in unifying an understanding of emotion


within a diverse cast? How much of the actor’s previous beliefs, training and
acting experiences affect their views on emotion in performance? How do these
previously held views/assumption interact with the three theories used?

191
These questions are more difficult to answer. In each case study, the actors

involved had very different views on emotion prior to the rehearsal process. By the end

of each rehearsal process, the actors were mostly in agreement, specifically viewing the

experience of emotion in performance as more of physical/physiological process than a

psychological one. The introduction of the Alba Emoting training may have

inadvertently led them to this new way of thinking about emotion. The Alba Emoting

training offered the actors an immediate somatic experience with emotion, as well as a

concrete vocabulary. This instant vocabulary of the emotional effector patterns was one

way in which the actors and I unified our understanding of emotion. Alba Emoting

removes the subjective discussions about emotion and replaces those discussions with

precise breathing-facial-postural components that can be isolated and altered if

necessary.

The actors’ diverse views on emotion, from their previous experiences and

trainings, were reflected in their initial interviews, but did not seem to affect how they

responded to the approaches that I introduced into the rehearsal process. Interestingly,

the North American actors tended to regard emotion as more of a psychological

phenomenon; the actors who trained with Phillip Zarrilli regarded emotion as a type of

energy; and the British actors regarded emotion as a phenomenon that may or may not

occur in performance (but that they hoped would occur). However, their views

presented no conflict within the rehearsal process. Their opinions on the topic of

emotion proved irrelevant to the working process in rehearsal.

What is the nature of the emotion being expressed? Whose emotion is it? The
actor’s or the character’s? Is there a difference?

From my observations and interviews with the actors, the nature of the emotion

expressed in performance was ‘genuine’ emotion, which the actors experienced on a

psychophysical level as a result of specific rehearsal processes and in the context of a


192
performance situation. At no time throughout the rehearsal process did any of the

actors mention using their own ‘private’ emotions. This raises the classic question of

whose emotion is it? Psychologist Paul Ekman’s research may be able to resolve this

paradox. Ekman outlines nine paths for ‘accessing or turning on our emotions’ (Ekman

2003: 37). The first path is through our automatic-appraising mechanisms, which can

make a ‘decision or evaluation that brings forth emotion [which] is extremely fast and

outside of awareness’ (ibid.: 21). Automatic-appraising mechanisms, which Ekman

abbreviates as autoappraisers, are ‘on alert for two kinds of triggers’ (ibid.: 23), the

first of which are ‘events that are important to the welfare or survival of all human

beings’ (ibid.), and the second are ‘variations on those themes that develop in each

person’s experiences’ (ibid.: 24). This pathway to emotion is most familiar to our

human experience. Ekman points out that ‘[w]hile emotions are most often triggered by

automatic appraisers, that is not the only way in which they can begin’ [my emphasis]

(ibid.: 31). Ekman’s pathways include: 1) the automatic-appraising mechanisms, 2)

reflective appraisal that then click on autoappraisers, 3) memory of a past emotional

experience, 4) imagination, 5) talking about a past emotional event, 6) empathy, 7)

others instructing us about what to be emotional about, 8) violation of social norms, and

9) voluntarily assuming the appearance of emotion. From this list, we can consider the

historical views of theatre practitioners through a cognitive lens, and how they were

possibly accessing these other pathways to emotion with their techniques. Stanislavsky

experimented with the memory of a past emotional experience, imagination, empathy

and voluntarily assuming the appearance of emotion. Both Brecht and Meyerhold

rejected the memory of a past emotional experience and empathy, but embraced the

path of voluntarily assuming the appearance of emotion. As a result of my research, I

would also add a tenth category to Ekman’s list: that emotions can be triggered by

193
responses to physical exercises, exertion or exhaustion (as we have seen with

Astbury’s physical exercises, but also true for the work of Grotowski).

Because these other pathways are less common to every human experience, it is

easy to see how this question of whose emotion is it – the actor’s, or the character’s? –

began to emerge. Is there a difference between the two? Yes, in the stimuli that trigger

them. The actor, using their whole self as a vehicle for performance, is generating

emotion through a different pathway and in a different context than they would be when

experiencing emotion in their own lives.

What, if anything, can be revealed about the nature of emotion in Western


performance when employing these three theories/approaches to emotion in
practice?

After working on these three case studies, I am convinced that the actor can

experience ‘genuine’ emotion in performance without having to use their own private

emotional experience. Not one of the actors I worked with mentioned using their ‘own’

or ‘private’ emotions in performance, which perhaps was due to the methods I chose to

employ. While the actors’ bodies were experiencing patterns of emotional behavior,

they were psychophysiologically focused on the task at hand – the performance. After

one of the performances of Less Than a Year when Enright experienced tears, an

audience member refrained from approaching her immediately afterwards. Enright

remarked on the experience:

Somebody commented to me after a performance where I had cried and


got quite emotional that they weren’t sure what state I’d be in to talk to
them afterwards. I was fine the minute I’d finished […] Cause it wasn’t a
weeping or wailing for yourself […] and yet it wasn’t that detached that
it was so technical either. (Coldfield and Enright 2011).

I also found that it is not necessary for the actor to psychologically ‘become’ the

character. Again, drawing from Less Than a Year, Enright and Coldfield were aware

194
that they were not the couple that lost their daughter to cancer. They were equally

aware that they were in a performance. As Enright commented:

I would never say that I believed it was my daughter that I was talking
about that I was crying, and I wasn’t. To do that I think would be
disrespectful to the piece and to the story and to the real people […] and
I just tried to tell the story each night as honestly as I could in that
particular moment…in that zone. (Coldfield and Enright 2011)

Brecht would agree with Enright on this point:

At no moment must [the actor] go so far as to be wholly transformed into


the character played. The verdict: ‘he didn’t act Lear, he was Lear’
would be annihilating blow to him. He has just to show the character, or
rather he has to do more than just get into it; this does not mean that if he
is playing passionate parts he must himself remain cold. It is only that
his feelings must not at bottom be those of the character, so that the
audience’s may not be at bottom be those of the character either. The
audience must have complete freedom here. [my emphasis] (Brecht
1964: 193-194)

The idea of conceptual blending may be useful here. As an audience member, we can

watch a play and follow (or perhaps) be moved by the story and at the same time be

conscious of the fact that there are actors on the stage in front of us. In The Way We

Think: Conceptual Blending and the Mind’s Hidden Complexities, Fauconnier and

Turner maintain ‘[d]ramatic performances are deliberate blends of a living person with

an identity’ (Fauconnier and Turner 2002: 266). They go on:

In principle, actors are linked to characters by virtue of performing in the


real world actions that share physical properties with actions performed
by the characters in a represented world. […] While we perceive a single
scene, we are simultaneously aware of the actor moving and talking on a
stage in front of an audience, and of the corresponding character moving
and talking within the represented story world. (ibid.)

When children are playing, they are able to simultaneously exist in their ‘play acting’ as

well as commentate on the circumstances. Actors have this same ability.

The idea of a ‘character’s’ emotion is related. From the practice discussed here,

especially after Less Than a Year, I would conclude that the notion of ‘character’—

except when reading a play—is not useful in the rehearsal room. The actor is using

195
oneself to psychophysically realize a role. The idea of a ‘character’ existing

simultaneously is confusing. During the first case study, West made the comment:

…it begins to get to an ethereal point, I’m not quite comfortable, as if the
character exists in the ether, and just needs to possess my body. But what
I will say - because the character is going to be slightly different for
everybody - so what I will say is that I have discovered some
possibilities for the way that I, given my physiology and emotional
capabilities could possibly play this character. (West 2009c)

From the practice undertaken in this research project, it is more useful to consider the

‘character’ as a meeting of the particular actor and the text in a theatrical moment.

Which is why there are so many wonderful options.

Do these discoveries vary when used put into practice on different dramaturgies
with different demands on the actor?

After working on three very different dramaturgies, I found ‘emotion’ integral to

every script, in one way or another. What initially attracted me to Astbury’s work and

eventually to this study was ‘emotion’ (or so I thought). After this study, my views have

changed. In retrospect I believe that what I was drawn to was not necessarily the display

of emotion, but the fact that the entire organism of the actor was working in sync –

psychophysically. Based on my experiences, I consider emotion to be a by-product of

psychophysical connection—of the body/mind working together. This is a result of the

actors’ full engagement with the text in the theatrical moment—a quality and

connection that are necessary for any performer in any style. It is important for an actor

to become familiar with themselves. Emotion is important to investigate because – like

it or not – our capacity for emotional response is often governed by invisible shackles of

our culture. Our personal experience of emotion may be limited because of display rules

– both cultural and gender based.

196
If, as theatre makers, we desire to create work that is both ‘affective’ and

‘effective’, we are charged with a greater task: exploring not what it means to be

human, but what it is to be human. In this pursuit of understanding we are both ill-

equipped and yet, at the same time, our best resource. ‘Ill-equipped’ in the sense that we

must overcome our engrained habitual ways of thinking and experiencing as well as

overcoming the divide between mind and body. As Moshe Feldenkrais maintains, ‘We

have no real basis for thinking of the duality except the habit of thought’ (Feldenkrais

2010: 95). We are our own ‘best resource’ in that we have all the innate knowledge

within ourselves, and it is available to us if we can begin to recognize that knowledge

and cultivate it. I close with a quotation from the actor Henry Irving, from his preface to

Diderot’s Paradoxe sur le Comedien in 1883:

Perhaps it will always be an open question how far sensibility and art can
be fused in the same mind. Every actor has his secret. He might write
volumes of explanation, and the matter would still remain a paradox to
many. It is often said that actors should not shed tears, that real tears are
bad art. This is not so. If tears be produced at the actor’s will and under
his control, they are true art; and happy is the actor who numbers them
amongst his gifts. The exaltation of sensibility in art may be difficult to
define, but it is none the less real to all who have felt its power.

(Irving in Diderot and Pollack 1883 xix - xx)

197
Appendices
Appendix A
Review for All Alone from nytheatre.com review archive
By: Robert Weinstein (August 15, 2007)

The chat room in Theatre503 and Post Script Theatre's production of All Alone is a dark
and dirty place, a place where debauched minds throw on false personalities to lure
innocent people out of their isolation and commit despicable acts of violence and
cruelty. These reprehensible acts and impulses transform the chat room from a place to
connect into a landscape of perversion where the worst of human nature can take root
and flourish. The events depicted in the show are not always easy to watch or
comfortable to experience, but All Alone is a forceful, confusing, and infuriating piece
of theatre that deserves to be seen for the risks it dares to take and for the ones it fails to
achieve.

All Alone tells the story of two men and an incredibly disturbing chat room session.
Man A begins the show straddling what turns out to be a dead woman. He wears white
underwear, masturbates frequently, and enjoys creating violent variations on nursery
rhymes. Man B starts the show in underwear, too, but his are black and far more stylish.
He brushes his teeth for close to 15 minutes and when he finally rinsed, I wondered
what deteriorating effect it had on his enamel. While never implicitly stated, both men
are actually two sides of the same person, with Man A acting as a kind of Id and Man B
the Ego. Man A seems to provide the impulse for Man B's actions (Man B gets the
actual chatting duties) and the results of Man B's actions provide reactions in Man A
which provide more impulses. The roles reverse from time to time but this circular
relationship propels the piece forward and seems to push the boundaries of their
collective perversion toward points of no return.
And where is the Superego during all of this? That particular personality part lies dead
on the stage: desecrated, danced with, and spat on.

Gene David Kirk's script is full of inventive wordplay and rhythmic shifts. Jessica
Beck's direction allows her actors—Andrew Barron, Matthew Flacks and Maggie-Kate
Coleman—the freedom necessary to experience the effects of their characters' basest
instincts and the freedom to delve into what the show's postcard describes as "the
deepest horror of consciousness." But the freedom and exploration come at a price: All
sense of character disappears periodically, as do the sense of space and time. Characters
sometimes act but fail to react, or seem to react to unknown stimulus. And the relentless
pace rarely stops to let any of the characters and the production breathe. The result
threatens and sometimes succeeds in distancing the audience right out of
comprehension.

All Alone is a powerful and unflinching look at the truly ugly aspects of loneliness and
isolation. But as I walked away from the theatre, I wished that Beck had forced more
focus onto the proceedings and allowed the more humane aspects of the characters, the
hopeful selves that became these damaged creatures, to have their say.

http://www.nytheatre.com/showpage.aspx?s=9 [accessed May 13, 2011]


198
Appendix B

Included here is a transcript of how I guided an early impulse and awareness exercise.
These instructions were given over the course of 32 minutes. (Transcript from
September 12, 2009)

Beck:

So in the space, with your partner, stand about 6-8 feet apart.

And Joe, you’re going to have an imaginary partner, but you are also going to
try to keep an awareness of everyone else in the space.

Don’t make eye contact, but see if you can take in your whole partner, without
eye contact.

Just pay attention to your breathing, and the breathing of your partner. Or your
imaginary partner.

Now see if you can sync your breathing, so that you’re breathing in the same
rhythm.

And just start to notice anything that may be happening with your partner. Any
gentle swaying?

And just very slowly, start to mirror each other.

And imagine you’re invisible partner, but also keeping awareness of others in
the room, Joe.

And gently start to respond to any impulse that you may notice. And as you are
still mirroring your partner, it should be difficult to tell which partner initiated
the impulse.

And begin shifting weight from one foot to the other. Start to get in sync with
what you do.

Just explore making a connection with your partner, still without eye contact.

Then now allow yourself to be bolder with the movements, and if you fall out of
sync you can always re-sync.

And Joe, you can now let your imaginary partner disappear, and open your
awareness completely to the others in the room. Now they are not going to be
aware of few, but see if you can pick up on some of their movements, their
breathing.

Now, you no longer have to mirror your partner, but begin to move in
relationship to your partner, to what they are doing, which could be mirroring or
could be completely different. But you’re still moving with a bond.

199
And Joe, how can you connect with them, not by moving into their space, but by
picking up on their energy? Let their movements provoke your movements.

And just for yourself, see if any of these particular movements invoke a word, or
a line from the play, just for yourself.

Now gently start to move away from your partner, but see if you can keep the
connection.

And begin to move away from each other in the room, to walk around the room,
though keeping the connection.

And [Joe], you are free to shadow anyone.

Observe the connection. Does anything summon a word or line from the play? If
so, you can use it for yourself whenever you have an impulse to.

And beginning to move at more of a pace, with a sense of urgency, though


keeping that contact with your partner, Or trying to make contact Joe, if you
don’t have one.

And whether you come together or separate, try to keep the connection.

And if you ever feel that you’ve lost the connection to your partner, you can
come back together and re-establish it.

If you haven’t already, you are now welcome to make eye contact [with your
partner]. But try to keep that total awareness of them, without looking at them,
as well.

And [Joe], you can make eye contact with someone perhaps, and maybe entice
them away from their partner. Without being intrusive, see if you can seek a
connection. Perhaps someone will break a connection to form one with you. Or
perhaps they will maintain the connection and form a new one with you.

While maintaining that connection, start to open your awareness to the other
people in the room. Do any of their movements stir an impulse within you?

If at any time you need to vocalize, you can. Are there any words or phrases that
come to mind? Any lines? You may use them.

And we’re now to build up a little more of a pace, so that when there are
moments of stillness, they are definite choices.

And now if there’s ever a moment where you feel you have something to say to
the other person, you can use that line, that phrase, that word.

It could be said with a whisper, it could be said with the eyes.

(I start playing music on the CD player)

200
Now you also have the music, for your impulses to work off of.

Begin to use your text when you need to.

Don’t be afraid to speak to each other.

Find one of your lines and say something to that person. Regardless of the line.

Continue to explore the connection.

(music ends.)

And then slowly find a connection with your partner, wherever they are.

Move closer to them.

And sync your breathing.

And then slowly find your own space.

And then just relax.

201
Appendix C

CHORUS:

w1 Yes strange darkness best and the darker the worse


w2 Yes perhaps a shade gone I suppose some might say
M Yes please one assumed all out all the pain

w1 till all dark then all well for the time but it will come
w2 poor thing a shade gone just a shade in the head
M all as if never been it will come [Hiccup.] pardon

w1 the time will come the thing is there you’ll see it


w2 [Laugh - - - - - - - - ] just a shade but I doubt it
M no sense in this oh I know none the less

w1 get off me keep off me all dark all still


w2 I doubt it not really I’m all right still all right
M one assumed peace I mean not merely all over

w1 all over wiped out—


w2 do my best all I can—
M but as if never been—

(Beckett 2006: 319)

202
Appendix D

Review of Less Than a Year


Date Reviewed: 16th January 2011
Rating: *****
By Robert Isles

Helena Enright's enthralling Theatre of Testimony piece directed by Jessica Beck

This is not the sort of theatre that falls into the category of "light entertainment", in fact
arguably it is not entertainment at all but nor does it seek to educate, well not explicitly.
What is does do, is deliver a story that will touch pretty well every member of the
audience in one way or another, promote discussion of what is often seen as the
undiscussable and, in my opinion, help people (or prepare people) to face similar life
challenges.

If that sounds a bit of a wild claim let me explain... this is a Theatre of Testimony piece,
by which we mean that the words the actors speak are those of a real life couple
describing the worst of all possible things, the slow painful death of a daughter.

Unlike documentary the flow of the story is not interrupted by "interpretation" or other
views, nor is this really a re-enactment ... it is just the re-animation of the raw and
powerful words and emotions of an ordinary couple describing how they failed to cope
with their daughter's condition and how they were failed, repeatedly and almost
unbelievably badly, by much of the medical profession.

What I found most moving in this piece, written by Helena Enright from discussions
and interviews with the family, is that while they didn't direct blame anywhere they
were angry with the way they were failed, and in the way that they themselves failed
their own daughter. The father and mother are played by Charlie Coldfield and Helena
Enright and we see them, in their own words delivered falteringly and completely
believably, through the 10 months from their daughter becoming ill to her death. Jessica
Beck's direction has a light touch allowing the story to be told without distraction (a
distinct improvement over TV docu-style!) and allowing the emotions to be felt without
the story becoming depressing or maudlin.

This is not a sob-fest, it is not even a depressing piece, somehow, out of this terrible
situation the sheer normality and simplicity of these people's emotions makes it a
fascinating and compelling story to watch. Indeed it is that very normality and the
matter-of-fact delivery that makes the real emotions around the death itself so
stunningly effective - the emotional journey of these people is so powerfully depicted
by their attempts to be strong that the few times that we see them crack produce a
completely unforced sympathetic response from the audience.

My own father's death from sarcoma (not bone in his case) and my mother's attempt to
give him (and us) one last holiday which resulted in an air ambulance trip home and his
death shortly afterwards meant that this piece spoke very deeply to me ... but listening
to the audience afterwards it was clear that everyone felt some connection with the
203
piece ... cancer will hit one in three, no family won’t have had to go through
something that this story touches on, even if the sufferer survives ....

This is a short run of a powerful piece and I for one am very glad I had a chance to see
it ...

http://www.uktw.co.uk/index.php?pg=7&story=E8831295220073
[Accessed 18 January 2011]

(Lehmann 2006)

204
Bibliography

ALLEN, D. 2000. Performing Chekhov, London ; New York, Routledge.

ARISTOTLE & HALLIWELL, S. 2009. Poetics. Cybraria LLC (2 July 2009).


Amazon.co.uk. Kindle Edition.

ASTBURY, B. 2011. Trusting the Actor. Amazon Digital Services (3 May 2011)
Kindle Edition.

BEAR, M. F., CONNORS, B. W. & PARADISO, M. A. 2001. Neuroscience :


Exploring the Brain, Baltimore, Md., Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

BECK, J. M. 2010. Alba Emoting and Emotional Melody: Surfing the Emotional Wave
in Cachagua, Chile. Theatre, Dance and Performance Training, 1, 141-156.

BECK, J. M., COLDFIELD, C. & ENRIGHT, H. 2011. Group Discussion after


Emotional Access Work. [Dvd Recording]. University of Exeter. 08 January
2011.

BECK, J. M., ELLIOTT-ARCHER, C., PRESTON, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J. &


SHEPHERD, L. 2010a. Group Discussion after Alba Emoting Mirror Work.
[Dvd Recording]. University of Exeter. 09 April 2010.

BECK, J. M., ENRIGHT, H. & PENNINGTON, E. 2010b. Group Discussion after


Physical Exercises. [Dvd Recording]. University of Exeter. 14 April 2010.

BECK, J. M., ENRIGHT, H. & PENNINGTON, E. 2010c. Group Discussion after


Physical Metaphor with Enright and Pennington. [Dvd Recording]. University
of Exeter. 14 April 2010.

BECK, J. M., HETZLER, E., LAI, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J., SOZANSKA, Z. &
WEST, J. 2009a. Group Discussion on Emotional Access Work with Hetzler
and Sozanska. [Dvd Recording]. University of Exeter. 11 September 2009.

BECK, J. M., HETZLER, E., LAI, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J., SOZANSKA, Z. &
WEST, J. 2009b. Group Discussion on Impulse and Awareness Exercise. [Dvd
Recording]. University of Exeter. 12 September 2009.

BECK, J. M., HETZLER, E., LAI, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J., SOZANSKA, Z. &
WEST, J. 2009c. Group Discussion on Physical Metaphor with Lai and West.
[Dvd Recording]. Univeristy of Exeter. 16 September 2009.

BECK, J. M., HETZLER, E., LAI, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J., SOZANSKA, Z. &
WEST, J. 2009d. Group Discussion on the Alba Opera. [Dvd Recording].
University of Exeter. 17 September 2009.

BECK, J. M., HETZLER, E., LAI, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J., SOZANSKA, Z. &
WEST, J. 2009e. Group Discussion on the Introduction of Alba Emoting. [Dvd
Recording]. University of Exeter. 08 September 2009.
205
BECK, J. M., LAI, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J. & SOZANSKA, Z. 2009f. Group
Discussion on Emotional Access Work with Sozanska and Lai. [Dvd
Recording]. University of Exeter. 09 September 2009.

BECK, J. M., LAI, S., SELLMAN-LEAVA, J., SOZANSKA, Z. & WEST, J. 2009g.
Group Discussion on Emotional Access Work with Lai and West. [Dvd
Recording]. University of Exeter. 10 September 2009.

BECKETT, S. 2006. The Complete Dramatic Works, London, Faber and Faber.

BEN-ZVI, L. 1990. Women in Beckett : Performance and Critical Perspectives,


Urbana, University of Illinois Press.

BENEDETTI, J. 2005. The Art of the Actor : The Essential History of Acting, from
Classical Times to the Present Day, London, Methuen.

BENNETT, M. R. & HACKER, P. M. S. 2008. History of Cognitive Neuroscience,


London, Blackwell Publishing.

BLAIR, R. 2008. The Actor, Image, and Action : Acting and Cognitive Neuroscience,
London ; New York, Routledge.

BLANK, J. & JENSEN, E. 2005. Living Justice : Love, Freedom, and the Making of the
Exonerated, New York, Atria Books.

BLOCH, S. 1993. Alba Emoting: A Psychophysiological Technique to Help Actors


Create and Control Real Emotions. Theatre Topics, 3 (2), 121-145.

BLOCH, S. 2006. The Alba of Emotions: Managing Emotions through Breathing,


Santiago, Ediciones Ultramarinos PSE.

BLOCH, S. 2008a. Personal Interview [Interview]. Cachagua, Chile with J. M. Beck. 21


December 2008.

BLOCH, S. 2008b. Surfeando La Ola Emocional: Reconozca Las Emociones Basicas Y


Comprenda Sus Emociones Mixtas, Santiago de Chile, Uqbar Editores.

BLOCH, S. 2009. Personal Communication [Email]. 5 December 2009.

BLOCH, S. & LEMIGNAN, M. 1992. Precise Respiratory-Posturo-Facial Patterns Are


Related to Specific Basic Emotions. Bewegen & Hulpverlening, 1, 31-40.

BLOCH, S., ORTHOUS, P. & SANTIBANEZ-H., G. 1987. Effector Patterns of Basic


Emotions: A Psychophysical Method for Training Actors. Journal of Social and
Biological Structures, 10, 1-19.

BLOCH, S., ORTHOUS, P. & SANTIBANEZ-H., G. 1988. Commentaries on 'Effector


Patterns of Basic Emotions'. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 11,
201-211.

206
BRATER, E. 1987. Beyond Minimalism : Beckett's Late Style in the Theater, New
York, Oxford University Press.

BRECHT, B. 1964. Brecht on Theatre : The Development of an Aesthetic, Methuen.

BRECHT, B. 1974. The Alienation Effect. In: COLE, T. & CHINOY, H. K. (eds.)
Actors on Acting: The Theories, Techniques, and Practices of the Great Actors
of All Times as Told in Their Own Words. New York: Crown Publishers.

BUNIN, I. A. & MARULLO, T. G. 2007. About Chekhov : The Unfinished Symphony,


Evanston, Ill., Northwestern University Press.

CANNON, W. B. 1987. The James-Lange Theory of Emotions: A Critical Examination


and an Alternative Theory. The American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 100, No.
3/4, Special Centennial Issue, 567-586.

CANTRELL, T. & LUCKHURST, M. 2010. Playing for Real : Actors on Playing Real
People, New York, Palgrave Macmillan.

CARNICKE, S. M. 1998. Stanislavsky in Focus, Los Angeles, USA, Harwood


Academic Publishers.

CHEKHOV, M. 2002. To the Actor, London, Routledge.

COLDFIELD, C. & ENRIGHT, H. 2011. Final Interview. [Interview]. University of


Exeter with J.M. Beck. 21 January 2011.

COQUELIN, C., ALGER, A. L., JAMES, H. & HENRY JAMES COLLECTION


(LIBRARY OF CONGRESS) 1915. Art and the Actor, New York,, Printed for
the Dramatic Museum of Columbia University.

DAMASIO, A. R. 1994. Descartes' Error : Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain,
New York, Putnam.

DAMASIO, A. R. 1999. The Feeling of What Happens : Body and Emotion in the
Making of Consciousness, New York, Harcourt Brace.

DARWIN, C., EKMAN, P. & PRODGER, P. 1999. The Expression of the Emotions in
Man and Animals, London, HarperCollins.

DEHAENE, S., CHANGEUX, J.-P., NACCACHE, L., SACKUR, J. & SERGENT, C.


2006. Conscious, Perconscious, and Subliminal Processing: A Testable
Taxonomy. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 10 (5), 204-211.

DELSARTE, F. A. N. C. 1892. Delsarte's Own Words, New York,, E. S. Werner.

DESCARTES, R. The Works of Rene Descartes. Amazon Media EU S.à r.l. [Kindle
Edition].

DIDEROT, D. & POLLOCK, W. H. 1883. The Paradox of Acting, London,, Chatto &
Windus.

207
DIDEROT, D. & POLLOCK, W. H. 1957. The Paradox of Acting. By D. Diderot ...
(Translated by Walter Herries Pollock.) Masks or Faces? By William Archer,
Etc, pp. xiv. 240. Hill & Wang: New York.

DOLMAN JR., J. 1928. The Art of Play Production, New York, Harper & Brothers
Publishers.

DOUGLAS, K. 2007. Subconscious: The Other You. The New Scientist, Volume 196,
Issue 2632, 1 December 2007, 42-46.

EASTY, E. D. 1966. On Method Acting, N ew Y ork ,, Allograph Press.

EKMAN, P. 1993. Facial Expression and Emotion. Am Psychol, 48, 384-92.

EKMAN, P. 2003. Emotions Revealed : Understanding Faces and Feelings, London,


Weidenfeld & Nicolson.

ELLIOTT-ARCHER, C. 2010a. Final Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with


J.M. Beck. 28 April 2010.

ELLIOTT-ARCHER, C. 2010b. Second Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter


with J.M. Beck. 15 April 2010.

ELLSWORTH, P. C. 1994. William James and Emotion: Is a Century of Fame Worth a


Century of Misunderstanding? Psychol Rev, 101, 222-9.

ENRIGHT, H. 2005. Staging Cancer: A Research Project in Documentary Theatre.


Masters, University of Cork.

ENRIGHT, H. 2010. Final Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M. Beck.
28 April 2010.

ENRIGHT, H. 2011a. 'Letting It Breathe': Writing and Performing the Words of Others.
Studies in Theatre and Performance, 31 (2), 181-192.

ENRIGHT, H. 2011b. Staging Testimony: A Practice-Led Investigation into the Role of


the Playwright/Director in Verbatim Theatre. PhD, University of Exeter.

FAUCONNIER, G. & TURNER, M. 2002. The Way We Think: Conceptual Blending


and the Mind's Hidden Complexities, New York, Basic Books.

FELDENKRAIS, M. 2010. Image, Movement, and Actor: Restoration of Potentiality,


Interview with Richard Schechner and Helen Schechner. In: BERINGER, E.
(ed.) Embodied Wisdom: The Collected Papers of Moshe Feldenkrais. Berkeley,
California: North Atlantic Books.

FOX, E. 2008. Emotion Science : Cognitive and Neuroscientific Approaches to


Understanding Human Emotions, Basingstoke ; New York, Palgrave
Macmillan.

FREEMAN, J. 2010. Blood, Sweat & Theory, Oxfordshire, Libri Pub.

208
FRIEL, B. 2000. Three Sisters: A Version of the Play by Anton Chekhov, New York,
Dramatist Play Service Inc.

GLADOV, A. 2004. Meyerhold Speaks Meyerhold Rehearses, London, Routledge.

GORDON, R. N. 2006. The Purpose of Playing : Modern Acting Theories in


Perspective, Ann Arbor, University of Michigan Press.

GRAY, J. 2007. Diderot, Garrick, and the Art of Acting. The Age of Johnson, 19, 243 -
273.

GROTOWSKI, J. & BROOK, P. 1968. Towards a Poor Theatre. Preface by Peter


Brook, Holstebro: Odin Teatret Forlag.

GROTOWSKI, J. & SALATA, K. 2008. Reply to Stanislavsky. TDR: The Drama


Review, 52, 31-39.

HAESMAN, B. & MAFE, D. 2009. Acquiring Know-How: Research Training for


Practice-Led Researchers. In: SMITH, H. & DEAN, R. T. (eds.) Practice-Led
Research, Research-Led Practice in the Creative Arts. Edinburgh: Edinburgh
University Press.

HAMMOND, W. & STEWARD, D. 2008. Verbatim, Verbatim : Contemporary


Documentary Theatre, London, Oberon.

HETZLER, E. 2009a. Final Reflections. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.


Beck. 27 September 2009.

HETZLER, E. 2009b. Preliminary Questions. [Interview]. University of Exeter with


J.M. Beck. 07 September 2009.

HETZLER, E. 2009c. Reflections on Week One. [Interview]. University of Exeter with


J.M. Beck. 14 September 2009.

HORNBY, R. 1983. Understanding Acting. Journal of Aesthetic Education, 17, no. 3,


19-37.

HORNBY, R. 1992. The End of Acting : A Radical View, New York, NY, Applause
Theatre Books.

HORNBY, R. 2009. Playing Chekhov. The Hudson Review, 62 (1), 113-119.

JOLY, Y. 2008. Educación Somática: Reflexiones Sobre La Práctica De La Conciencia


Del Cuerpo En Movimiento, Mexico D.F., Plaza y Valdes.

JOLY, Y. 2009. Personal Communication [Email]. 5 December 2009.

KALB, J. 1989. Beckett in Performance, Cambridge England ; New York, Cambridge


University Press.

KOGAN, S. & KOGAN, H. 2010. The Science of Acting, London ; New York,
Routledge.
209
KONIJN, E. 2000. Acting Emotions : Shaping Emotions on Stage, Amsterdam,
Amsterdam University Press.

LAI, S. 2009a. Final Reflections. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M. Beck. 27
September 2009.

LAI, S. 2009b. Preliminary Questions. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.


Beck. 07 September 2009.

LAI, S. 2009c. Reflections on Week One. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.
Beck. 14 September 2009. .

LAI, S. 2009d. Reflections on Week Two. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.
Beck. 21 September 2009.

LAKOFF, G. & JOHNSON, M. 1999. Philosophy in the Flesh : The Embodied Mind
and Its Challenge to Western Thought, New York, Basic Books.

LANGE, C. G., JAMES, W. & HAUPT, I. A. 1922. The Emotions, Baltimore,,


Williams & Wilkins Company.

LEDOUX, J. E. 1996. The Emotional Brain : The Mysterious Underpinnings of


Emotional Life, New York, Simon & Schuster.

LEHMANN, H.-T. 2006. Postdramatic Theatre, Abingdon, England ; New York,


Routledge.

MAKEHAM, P. B. 2005. Interview with Paul Makeham, Original "Aftershocks" Cast


Member. In: STINSON, M. & WALL, D. (eds.) Dramactive (Book 2). McGraw-
Hill.

MANSTEAD, A. S. R., FRIJDA, N. H. & FISCHER, A. 2004. Feelings and Emotions :


The Amsterdam Symposium, Cambridge, Cambridge University Press.

MARSHALL, L. 2001. The Body Speaks, London, Methuen.

MARTIN, C. 2002. Anna Deavere Smith, Part I: The Word Becomes You. In:
ZARRILLI, P. B. (ed.) Acting (Re) Considered: A Theoretical and Practical
Guide. 2nd ed. London: Routledge.

MCCONACHIE, B. A. 2007. Falsifiable Theories for Theatre and Performance Studies.


Theatre Journal, 59, 553-577.

MCGUINNESS, P. 2000. Maurice Maeterlinck and the Making of Modern Theatre,


Oxford, Oxford University Press.

MCMANUS, C. 2002. Right Hand, Left Hand: The Origins of Asymmetry in Brains,
Bodies, Atoms and Cultures, London, Weidenfeld & Nicolson, Ltd.

MCMILLAN, K. & WEYERS, J. D. B. 2007. How to Write Dissertations & Project


Reports, Harlow, Pearson Prentice Hall.
210
MERLIN, B. 1999. Which Came First: The Systerm or 'the Seagull'? New Theatre
Quarterly, 15, 218-227.

MERLIN, B. 2007a. The Complete Stanislavsky Toolkit, London, Nick Hern.

MERLIN, B. 2007b. The Permanent Way and the Impermanent Muse. Contemporary
Theatre Review, 17 (1), 41-49.

MERLIN, B. 2008. Acting Hare: The Permanent Way. In: BOON, R. (ed.) The
Cambridge Companion to David Hare. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press.

MEYER-DINKGRÄFE, D. 2001. Approaches to Acting : Past and Present, London ;


New York, Continuum.

MEYERHOLD, V. & BRAUN, E. 1998. Meyerhold on Theatre, London, Methuen.

MINDELL, A. 1985. Working with the Dreaming Body, Boston, [Mass.] ; London,
Routledge & Kegan Paul.

MITTER, S. 1992. Systems of Rehearsal : Stanislavsky, Brecht, Grotowski and Brook,


London ; New York, Routledge.

NELSON, R. 2009. Modes of Practice-as-Research Knowledge and Their Place in the


Academy. In: ALLEGUE, L., JONES, S., KERSHAW, B. & PICCINI, A. (eds.)
Practice-as-Research in Performance and Screen. Baskingstoke: Palgrave
Macmillan.

PAGET, D. 1987. 'Verbatim Theatre': Oral History and Documentary Techniques. New
Theatre Quarterly, 3, 317-336.

PENNINGTON, E. 2010a. Final Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.


Beck. 28 April 2010.

PENNINGTON, E. 2010b. Second Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with


J.M. Beck. 23 April 2010.

PERT, C. B. 1997. Molecules of Emotion : Why You Feel the Way You Feel, New York,
NY, Scribner.

PISCATOR, E. 1974. Objective Acting. In: COLE, T. & CHINOY, H. K. (eds.) Actors
on Acting: The Theories, Techniques, and Practices of the Great Actors of All
Times as Told in Their Own Words. New York: Crown Publishers.

POSTLEWAIT, T. & MCCONACHIE, B. A. 1989. Interpreting the Theatrical Past :


Essays in the Historiography of Performance, Iowa City, University of Iowa
Press.

PRESTON, S. 2010. Final Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M. Beck.
28 April 2010.

211
RIBOT, T. A. 1911. Psychology of the Emotions, London, The Walter Scott Publishing
C., LTD.

RIX, R. 1993. Alba Emoting: A Preliminary Experiment with Emotional Effector


Patterns. Theatre Topics, 3, 139-145.

RIX, R. 2001. Alba Emoting: A Revolution in Emotion for the Actor. In: WATSON, I.
(ed.) Performer Training: Developments across Cultures. London: Harwood
Academic Publishers.

ROACH, J. R. 1993. The Player's Passion : Studies in the Science of Acting, Ann
Arbor, University of Michigan Press.

RUYTER, N. L. C. 1999. The Cultivation of Body and Mind in Nineteenth-Century


American Delsartism, Westport, Conn. ; London, Greenwood Press.

SCHACHTER, S. & SINGER, J. E. 1962. Cognitive, Social, and Physiological


Determinants of Emotional State. Psychological Review, Vol. 69, No. 5, 379-
399.

SELLMAN-LEAVA, J. 2009a. Final Reflections. [Interview]. University of Exeter with


J.M. Beck. 27 September 2009.

SELLMAN-LEAVA, J. 2009b. Preliminary Questions. [Interview]. University of


Exeter with J.M. Beck. 07 September 2009.

SELLMAN-LEAVA, J. 2010. Final Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with


J.M. Beck. 28 April 2010.

SHAPIRO, F. 2001. Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing : Basic


Principles, Protocols, and Procedures, New York ; London, Guilford Press.

SHEPHERD, L. 2010a. Final Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.


Beck. 28 April 2010.

SHEPHERD, L. 2010b. Second Interview. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.


Beck. 15 April 2010.

SLOWIAK, J. & CUESTA, J. 2007. Jerzy Grotowski, London ; New York, Routledge.

SOLOMON, R. C. 2003. What Is an Emotion? : Classic and Contemporary Readings,


New York, Oxford University Press.

SOLOMON, R. C. 2004. On the Passivity of the Passions. In: MANSTEAD, S. R.,


FRIJDA, N. & FISCHER, A. (eds.) Feelings and Emotions: The Amsterdam
Symposium. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

SOTO-MORETTINI, D. 2010. The Philosophical Actor : A Practical Meditation for


Practicing Theatre Artists, Bristol, Intellect.

212
SOZANSKA, Z. 2009a. Preliminary Questions. [Interview]. University of Exeter with
J.M. Beck. 07 September 2009.

SOZANSKA, Z. 2009b. Reflections on Week One. [Interview]. University of Exeter


with J.M. Beck. 14 September 2009.

SOZANSKA, Z. 2009c. Reflections on Week Two. [Interview]. University of Exeter


with J.M. Beck. 21 September 2009.

SPARSHOTT, F. 1997. Emotion and Emotions in Theatre Dance. In: HJORT, M. &
LAVER, S. (eds.) Emotion and the Arts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

STAKE, R. E. 2005. Qualitative Case Studies. In: DENZIN, N. K. & LINCOLN, Y. S.


(eds.) The Sage Handbook of Qualitative Research. Thousand Oaks; London:
Sage Publications.

STANISLAVSKY, K. & BENEDETTI, J. 2008a. An Actor's Work : A Student's Diary,


London ; New York, Routledge.

STANISLAVSKY, K. & BENEDETTI, J. 2008b. My Life in Art, London ; New York,


Routledge.

STANISLAVSKY, K. & BENEDETTI, J. 2010. An Actor's Work on a Role, London,


Routledge.

STEBBINS, G. 1892. Dynamic Breathing and Harmonic Gymnastics: A Complete


System of Physical, Aesthetic and Physical Culture, New York, Edgar S. Werner
& Company.

TAIT, P. 2002. Performing Emotions : Gender, Bodies, Spaces, in Chekhov's Drama


and Stanislavski's Theatre, Aldershot ; Burlington Vt., Ashgate.

WENGER, W. & POE, R. 1996. The Einstein Factor : A Proven New Method for
Increasing Your Intelligence, Rocklin, CA, Prima Pub.

WEST, J. 2009a. Final Reflections. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M. Beck.
27 September 2009.

WEST, J. 2009b. Preliminary Questions. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.


Beck. 07 September 2009.

WEST, J. 2009c. Reflections on Week One. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.
Beck. 14 September 2009.

WEST, J. 2009d. Reflections on Week Two. [Interview]. University of Exeter with J.M.
Beck. 21 September 2009.

WHYMAN, R. 2008. The Stanislavsky System of Acting : Legacy and Influence in


Modern Performance, Cambridge, UK ; New York, Cambridge University
Press.

WHYMAN, R. 2011. Anton Chekhov, London, Routledge.


213
WILSON, M. 2006. Storytelling and Theatre: Contemporary Storytellers and Their Art,
Basingstoke, Palgrave Macmillan.

WOLFORD, L. & SCHECHNER, R. 1997. The Grotowski Sourcebook, London ; New


York, Routledge.

WORTHEN, W. B. 1984. The Idea of the Actor : Drama and the Ethics of
Performance, Princeton ; Guildford, Princeton University Press.

WORTHEN, W. B. 1985. Playing "Play". Theatre Journal, 37 (4), 405-414.

WORTHEN, W. B. 2004. The Wadsworth Anthology of Drama, Boston, Mass. ;


London, Thomson/Wadsworth.

YORK, G. K. 2004. The Cerebral Localization of Creativity. In: ROSE, F. C. (ed.)


Neurology of the Arts: Painting, Music, Literature. London: Imperial College
Press.

ZARRILLI, P. B. 1995. Acting (Re)Considered : Theories and Practices, London ; New


York, Routledge.

ZARRILLI, P. B. 1997. Acting "at the Nerve Ends": Beckett, Blau, and the Necessary.
Theatre Topics, 7, 103-116.

ZARRILLI, P. B. 2007. An Enactive Approach to Understanding Acting. Theatre


Journal, 59.4, 635-647.

ZARRILLI, P. B. 2009. Psychophysical Acting: An Intercultural Approach after


Stanislavski, London, Routledge.

ZUBRZYCKI, A. & BRAL, G. 2010. Song of the Goat Theatre: Finding Flow and
Connection. New Theatre Quarterly, 26, 248-260.

214

Potrebbero piacerti anche