Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218

www.elsevier.com/locate/bodyimage

Adult attachment and body satisfaction


An exploration of general and specific relationship differences
Nita Mary McKinley *, Leigh Anne Randa
Interdisciplinary Arts and Science, University of Washington, 1900 Commerce Street,
Campus Box 358436, Tacoma, WA 98466, USA
Received 27 September 2004; received in revised form 27 April 2005; accepted 27 April 2005

Abstract

The relationship between general and specific adult relationship attachment and body satisfaction was examined in a
community sample of 133 women. Participants completed measures of body image (body surveillance, body-as-object esteem,
and body-as-process satisfaction) and general attachment (anxiety and avoidance) and then were primed for a romantic partner
or close friend relationship, after which they completed specific measures of attachment and body satisfaction. General
attachment results replicated previous research. Specific attachment results demonstrated distinct differences both from general
attachment and between romantic partner attachment and close friend attachment. Attachment independently predicted body
satisfaction both for general and close friend measures; however, in romantic relationships, only body surveillance indepen-
dently contributed to body satisfaction.
# 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Body image; Adult attachment behavior; Body surveillance

Introduction that attachment theory may provide insights into the


association of body image to interpersonal relation-
The way we feel about our bodies is central to our ships (Cash & Fleming, 2002). Although there is a
self concept (Cash & Pruzinksy, 2002) and rooted in burgeoning body of research showing a connection
our interactions with others (Kearney-Cooke, 2002). between attachment and eating disorders (see Ward,
Attachment theory, originally developed to explain the Ramsay, & Treasure, 2000, for a review), very little
bond between mothers and infants, has been usefully research has examined the relationship between
extended to understanding romantic attachments in attachment and body satisfaction or other elements
adulthood (Fraley & Shaver, 2000). Given the of body image (Cash & Fleming, 2002; Cash,
importance of relationships to body image, it is likely Theriault, & Annis, 2003). In this study, we examined
the relationship of attachment to body satisfaction in
* Corresponding author. Tel.: +1 253 692 4543;
fax: +1 253 692 5718.
adult women and whether that relationship varied as a
E-mail address: nmmckin@u.washington.edu function of the attachment figure’s role, either a close
(N.M. McKinley). friend or romantic partner.

1740-1445/$ – see front matter # 2005 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.04.003
210 N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218

Body image and significant others two continuous dimensions: anxiety, monitoring the
relationship and being sensitive to rejection or threats
Although body image is influenced by interactions to security, and avoidance, avoiding closeness with
with peers, and even strangers, theorists believe that the attachment figure. Secure attachment would
significant others play a major role in the development correspond to low levels of both dimensions. Indivi-
of body image (Kearney-Cooke, 2002; Tantleff-Dunn duals can have both a general attachment style that
& Gokee, 2002). Life stage may determine which is chronically accessible, and relationship-specific
significant others are most important, with parents attachment styles (Baldwin, Keelan, Fehr, Enns, &
contributing to body image most strongly in childhood, Koh-Rangarajoo, 1996; Mikulincer, Gillath, & Shaver,
friends contributing in adolescence, and romantic 2002). That adult attachment types are predictive
partners playing the most important role in adulthood of relationship quality has been widely replicated
(Tantleff-Dunn & Gokee, 2002). Research supports the (Brennan & Shaver, 1995). Exploring whether attach-
idea that romantic relationships are importantly related ment style and body image are related is likely to help
to body image in adulthood. For example, Hoyt and us more fully understand the importance of relation-
Kogan (2001) found that body satisfaction was related ships to body image.
to satisfaction in dating relationships and sex lives for There is a small body of evidence that attachment
both women and men undergraduates. Friedman et al. style is related to eating problems and weight concern.
(1999) found that marital status per se was not related In a review of this literature, Ward et al. (2000) found
to body satisfaction, but marital satisfaction was. that insecure attachment is related to eating problems
Close friendships may also be important to body in both clinical and non-clinical populations. There
image. In their review of research on friendship across are few studies, however, examining the relationship
the lifespan, Sherman et al. (2000) stated that there has of adult attachment to body satisfaction (Cash &
been less attention to friendships during middle Fleming, 2002; Cash et al., 2003). In a recent study,
adulthood compared to childhood, adolescence, and however, Cash et al. (2003) examined both general
old age, but that friendships in adulthood continue to adult attachment and romantic attachment in college
be an important contributor to psychological well- students. They found for general attachment style that
being. Because close friendships are voluntary, they secure attachment was positively related to body
may be especially important in feelings of self worth satisfaction; in romantic attachment, greater anxiety
(Fehr, 1996). There is some research demonstrating was related to lower body satisfaction, but avoidance
the importance of friendships to body image in was unrelated to body satisfaction. This is consistent
adolescence. For example, Paxton et al. (1999) found with Brennan and Shaver (1995), who found their
that body image concerns of adolescent girls were eating and body satisfaction measures to be related to a
more closely related to the concerns of other girls in preoccupied attachment style, which implies a relation-
their friendship cliques than to the concerns of their ship with anxiety, but not avoidance. It may be that
more general peer group. We found no research that those who are high on attachment anxiety attend more
examined close friendships and body image in adults. to socially relevant cues such as appearance, while
avoidance of closeness is less likely to cue a person to
attend to her or his appearance.
Attachment and body image Other individual traits may also cue a person to
attend to her or his appearance more or less and thus
One variable that may be important to understand- may be important to understanding the relationship
ing relationships and body image is adult attachment between body image and attachment. For example,
style. Attachment style refers to the ways individuals Cash et al. (2003) found that appearance schema, a
organize their connections to important others construct that measures the importance an individual
(Bowlby, 1969). Although attachment was originally places on appearance, was positively associated with
thought of as categorical (i.e., secure, preoccupied, both the anxiety and avoidant dimensions of attach-
fearful, and dismissing), Fraley and Shaver (2000) ment for women and with the anxiety dimension for
argued it is more accurately conceptualized as men. Another variable that may cue attention to
N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218 211

appearance is body surveillance, which is the tendency more sensitive to interpersonal appearance concerns
to view one’s body from the perspective of an outside than would body-as-process evaluations.
observer and which predicts negative body experience
for women (McKinley & Hyde, 1996). Body 1. Consistent with previous research (Cash et al.,
surveillance is presumably adopted to avoid potential 2003), we expected general attachment anxiety to be
negative judgment of others. Given the importance of related to body-as-object satisfaction and to
appearance for women’s romantic relationships uniquely predict this variable even with body
(Feingold, 1990), surveillance may function in a surveillance controlled. We expected no relationship
similar way as attachment anxiety as a way to monitor between body-as-object satisfaction and general
for threats of rejection. Evans and Wertheim (1998) attachment avoidance. We also did not expect to find
found a related construct, public self consciousness a relationship between either general attachment
(Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975), which measures a variable and body-as-process satisfaction.
person’s attention to the self as seen by others, 2. Because one’s romantic partner is likely to be the
mediated the relationship between a combined most chronically accessible attachment figure
intimacy measure (including attachment style) and (Baldwin et al., 1996), for specific romantic partner
eating, weight, and shape concerns. Controlling for the attachment style, we expected relationships with
tendency to adopt this view of the body would be body satisfaction similar to those for the general
important for understanding whether attachment style attachment style. We were unsure what to expect for
is more specifically related to body satisfaction over close friendship attachment style, but expected that
and above the individual trait of body surveillance. these relationships might be different from those for
the general or romantic partner conditions.
3. We expected that the greater the difference
The current study between general and specific attachment style
the greater would be the difference in general and
The purpose of this study was to clarify further the specific body satisfaction, further supporting the
relationship between adult attachment and body importance of the relationship between attachment
satisfaction. Particularly we were interested in style and body satisfaction.
exploring whether this relationship differed when 4. Again, because one’s romantic partner is likely to
participants were reporting on their general attach- be the most chronically accessible attachment
ment style or on a specific attachment to either a figure (Baldwin et al., 1996), we expected these
romantic partner or a close friend and whether these differences between general and specific attach-
relationships held with body surveillance controlled. ment style would be greater for close friendships
To our knowledge, no researcher has examined than for romantic partners.
attachment styles and body image for specific others,
either romantic partners or close friends.
After completing questionnaires about their gen- Method
eral attachment style and body image, participants
were primed for either the close friend or the romantic Participants
partner condition using a methodology similar to
Baldwin et al. (1996); then the participants completed Participants were recruited from a small commuter
the specific attachment and body image measures. university and from various workplaces, including a
Two dimensions of body satisfaction were used: body- motorcycle sales company and a state government
as-object, which includes evaluations of body parts office. Of the 133 total, 76 (57%) women were
(waist, chest, hips, and so on), and body-as-process, recruited from campus and 57 (43%) women were
which includes evaluations of body functions (energy recruited from workplaces. The mean age was 30.5
level, agility, health, and so on) (Franzoi, 1995). years (SD = 13.58; range 18–94 years). Seven women
Because body-as-object focuses more on appearance, were African American (5.3%), two women were
we presumed body-as-object evaluations would be Asian American (1.5%), 112 were European American
212 N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218

(84.2%), three were Hispanic (2.3%), four were Native strongly agree, with a mid-point of neither agree or
American or American Indian (3.0%), three were disagree. Dimension scores were an average of non-
biracial or multiracial (2.3%), and two women reported missing items. Higher scores indicate greater anxiety
their ethnicity as ‘‘other’’ (1.5%). Sixty-four (48.1%) and greater avoidance. Standard instructions were used
women were married, 12 (9.0%) were not married but to complete the ECR-R for the general condition.
living with partner, 20 (15.0%) were dating one person Instructions written for the specific condition asked
exclusively, five (3.8%) were dating more than one participants to remember the conversation with the
person, 32 (24.1%) were not currently in a romantic prime target and the feelings experienced (see below)
relationship, and none of the women reported never prior to responding to the measures. The order of items
having been in a romantic relationship. was varied for the two administrations. Internal
consistencies for both the general and specific versions
Materials were excellent, ranging from .90 to .96 in this sample.

Participants filled out demographic information, Body satisfaction


including age, current romantic relationship status, The Body Esteem Scale (BES; Franzoi & Shields,
and ethnicity. The survey then consisted of a general 1984) measures satisfaction with different aspects
section, consisting of the Body Surveillance scale, of the body. Franzoi (1995) had undergraduates
general attachment scales, and general body satisfac- classify these aspects as how one looks (17 items;
tion scales, and a specific section, consisting of specific body-as-object: BES-Object) and as what one does (12
attachment scales and specific body satisfaction scales. items; body-as-process: BES-Process). Participants
responded to items on a five-point scale from have
Body surveillance strong negative feelings to have strong positive feeling.
The Body Surveillance scale (McKinley & Hyde, For each of these two subscales, the mean of the non-
1996) measures the extent to which one views one’s missing items was calculated such that higher scores
body as an outside observer (‘‘I rarely worry about indicate greater body satisfaction. Standard instructions
how I look to other people’’). The scale consists of were used for the general condition. Instructions written
eight items and uses a seven-point scale, ranging from for the specific condition asked the participants to
strongly disagree to strongly agree with a midpoint of remember the primed target and their conversation
neither agree nor disagree. The Body Surveillance (see below) prior to completing the measure. Internal
Scale has been shown to be internally consistent and consistencies for all scales were excellent for this
temporally reliable (McKinley & Hyde, 1996). Scores sample, ranging from a = .85 to .98.
were calculated by summing item responses and
dividing by the number of non-missing items, with Procedure
higher scores indicating more surveillance. Internal
consistency for this sample was good: a = .86. Surveys for each of the two conditions, close
friendship and romantic partner, were randomly
Attachment style ordered prior to distribution. Undergraduate partici-
The Experiences in Close Relationships-Revised pants were recruited in classes or were approached in
(ECR-R; Fraley, Waller, & Brennan, 2000) scale, small groups in public areas of campus. Surveys were
developed via factor analysis of existing adult attach- taken home, and participants returned them via
ment scales, measures attachment style on two campus mail. Off-campus participants were recruited
dimensions: avoidance (ECR-Avoidance: discomfort via a contact person within the company or office.
with closeness and dependency, 18 items, ‘‘I feel Participants took surveys home and either returned
comfortable depending on others.’’); and anxiety (ECR- them to their contact person or returned them in a
Anxiety: concern with rejection and abandonment, 18 postage-paid envelope.
items, ‘‘I rarely worry that people won’t like me In Part I (General Condition), participants com-
anymore.’’). Participants responded to statements using pleted scales according to standard instructions.
a seven-point scale, ranging from strongly disagree to Participants were then primed by written instructions
N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218 213

to evoke a specific attachment figure, either a close Only surveys completed by women were analyzed
friend or romantic partner. The priming instructions (N = 133). Gender of primed attachment figure was
for the Close Friend were written to ensure that the related to priming group, x2 (1) = 88.86, p < .001. For
close friend fit the definition of an attachment figure the close friend condition, 51 (86%) primed attach-
(see Fraley & Shaver, 2000). Priming directions for the ment figures were women, six (10%) were men, and
Close Friend were: the sex was not give for two (3%) primed attachment
figures. For the romantic partner condition, four (5%)
Think of a close friend who is not your romantic
primed attachment figures were women, 64 (88%)
partner. This person is someone you enjoy being with,
were men, and the sex was not give for five (7%)
someone you want to talk to or be with when you have
primed attachment figure.
a problem, or a friend who has played a central role in
your life. Think about how this person has affected
your life. If there is not currently a close friend in your
Results
life, think of a close friend from your past.
Priming directions for the Romantic Partner were: To determine whether there were any differences in
age, the general attachment scales, body surveillance,
Think of a romantic partner you are emotionally
or the general BES scales as a function of priming group
involved with. Someone you cuddle with, and share
or recruitment site, a 2 (prime group)  2 (on- or off-
intimate moments with. Think about how this person
campus) MANOVA was conducted. The multivariate
has affected your life. If there is not currently a
effect for site was significant, Pillai’s F (6,122) = 5.45,
romantic partner in your life, think of a romantic
p < .001. Univariate tests showed that the off-campus
partner from your past.
group was significantly older (M = 40.62 years,
Immediately following these instructions, partici- SD = 1.64) than the on campus group (M = 29.34
pants were asked to write down the person’s initials, years, SD = 1.38), F (1,127) = 27.72, p < .001. There
indicate the person’s gender, and then to say the were no other differences between the sites. There was
person’s name to herself in order to increase the also a significant multivariate effect for prime group,
accessibility of their cognitive working model for this Pillai’s F (6,122) = 2.26, p < .05, however, univariate
person. Participants were then asked to imagine a tests only revealed a marginally significant effect for
situation involving separation from the specific person age, F (1,127) = 3.66, p < .10 and no other significant
to further increase the likelihood of activating univariate effect. The romantic partner group was
attachment behavior (Mikulincer et al., 2002). The marginally older (M = 37.03 years, SD = 1.40) than the
imagery instructions were close friendship group (M = 32.93 years, SD = 1.62).
The multivariate interaction was not significant, Pillai’s
Imagine that you are having a conversation with this
F (6,122) = 2.12, ns. Due to differences in age, age was
person before you leave on a trip alone. You will be out
controlled in subsequent analyses.
of touch for 2 weeks. Take a moment to really see this
close friend [romantic partner] in your mind. Imagine
Zero-order correlations
how they look. Imagine what you are thinking and
feeling.
Table 1 shows the correlations between the
Participants then completed Part II (Specific variables. For general measures, ECR-Anxiety scale
Condition) which included the specific measures. was negatively related to both BES-Object and BES-
A total of 300 surveys were distributed to women Process. In addition, there was a small significant
and men on campus and 111 were returned (37% negative relationship between ECR-Avoidance and
response rate). One hundred surveys were distributed BES-Process. For specific measures, both ECR-
off campus and 83 were returned (83% response rate). Avoidance and ECR-Anxiety had small negative
This difference in return rate may be the result of the relationships with both BES-Object and BES-Process.
contact person recruiting individuals from the work- Body surveillance was negatively related to general
places versus the more impersonal classroom setting. and specific measures of BES-Object, but was not
214 N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218

Table 1
Zero-order correlations
Variable Mean (SD) 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9
1. Age 34.48
(11.60)
2. Body surveillance 4.13
(1.20) .33 ***
3. ECR 1: Avoidance 3.40
(1.04) .10 .09
4. ECR 1: Anxiety 2.93
(1.16) .20 * .38*** .45***
5. BES OBJECT 1 3.42
(.64) .12 .23** .17 .40 ***
6. BES PROCESS 1 3.62
(.70) .28 ** .13 .20* .26 ** .71***
7. ECR 2: Avoidance 2.00
(1.03) .04 .13 .37*** .35 *** .19* .17
8. ECR 2: Anxiety 2.23
(1.38) .14 .16 .31* .45 *** .17 .10 .64 ***
9. BES OBJECT 2 3.53
(.71) .05 .20* .14 .31 *** .89*** .62*** .20 * .17*
10. BES PROCESS 2 3.68
(.75) .25 ** .09 .26** .27 ** .72*** .85*** .19 * .19* .72 ***
Note: N = 130–132.
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

related to BES-Process for women. Surveillance was explained. In the final step, only ECR-Anxiety and age
positively related to general ECR-Anxiety. significantly predicted BES-Object. Greater anxiety
and age were associated with less positive BES-
Attachment style and body satisfaction Object. For general BES-Process, the ECR measures
accounted for an additional 5% of the variance, with
To test whether ECR-Anxiety and ECR-Avoidance ECR-Anxiety and age both being significant con-
added significant additional variance to body satisfac- tributors to the final 17% of the variance accounted for.
tion over and above body surveillance and age, a series Again, greater anxiety and age were associated with
of hierarchic regressions were run for general and less positive BES-Process.
specific measures (Table 2). For each regression For close relationships for BES-Object, ECR-
predicting either BES-Object or BES-Process, age and Anxiety and ECR-Avoidance accounted for a signi-
body surveillance were entered on the first step to ficant additional 14% of the variance, for a total of
control for age differences between the groups and 19% of the variance accounted for. Only age
individual differences in body surveillance, followed contributed significantly negatively in the final step.
by the ECR-Anxiety and ECR-Avoidance measures. This may have been due to the strong correlation
For the general measures, ECR-Anxiety and ECR- between the two attachment scales in specific
Avoidance explained a significant additional 10% of relationships. Two additional exploratory regressions
the variance in BES-Object over and above age and were run: one with ECR-Avoidance only and one
surveillance, for a total of 19% of the variance ECR-Anxiety only. In both of these the attachment
N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218 215

Table 2
Predicting body esteem from relationship quality for women
Variable General (N = 130) Close friend (N = 56) Romantic partner (N = 71)
Step 1 (b) Step 2 (b) Step 1 (b) Step 2 (b) Step 1 (b) Step 2 (b)
Body-as-object
Age .22 * .26 ** .31 * .29 * .07 .09
Body surveillance .31 ** .17 .06 .01 .37 ** .34**
ECR: Anxiety .40 *** .21 .12
ECR: Avoidance .06 .25 .03
R2 adjusted .09 ** .19 *** .05 .19 ** .10 * .09*
DR 2 .09 ** .10 *** .05 .14 ** .10 * .00
Body-as-process
Age .36 *** .38 *** .40 ** .40 ** .28 * .30*
Body surveillance .25 ** .14 .01 .07 .38 ** .36**
ECR: Anxiety .26 ** .34 * .13
ECR: Avoidance .03 .10 .02
R2 adjusted .12 *** .17 *** .13 ** .27 *** .13 ** .12*
DR 2 .12 ** .05 ** .13 ** .14 ** .13 ** .00
*
p < .05.
**
p < .01.
***
p < .001.

scale contributed significantly to the final equation. ECR-Anxiety, ECR-Avoidance, BES-Object, and
For close relationships for BES-Process, ECR- BES-Process. Thus, negative difference scores meant
Anxiety and ECR-Avoidance added a significant higher scores for specific versus general scores and
additional 14% of the variance, for a total of 27% positive difference scores meant lower specific versus
accounted for. Only age and ECR-Anxiety contributed general scores. The BES-Object and BES-Process
significantly in the final step, with greater ECR- were highly skewed (skew = 7.84 and 2.17,
Anxiety and age associated with lower BES-Process. respectively), and transformations of the variables
For romantic partnerships for BES-Object, the did not result in a normal distribution. For this reason,
ECR variables did not add significant variance to the participants were divided into three separate groups,
model. Only surveillance contributed significantly and those whose BES improved in specific relationships,
positively to BES-Object. For BES-Process, the ECR those whose BES was worse in specific relationships,
variables did not add significant variance; both and those who had no difference. Age was included as
surveillance and age contributed uniquely and a covariate in each analysis, but did not contribute
negatively to the final step. As with close friendships, significantly and thus was dropped in the following
exploratory analyses were run with each of the ECR analyses.
variables separately because of potential multicolli- Two 2 (close friend or romantic partner)  3
nearity, and the results were the same as the analyses (difference in BES) MANOVAs with body surveil-
with the ECR variables entered in the same equation. lance as a covariate and difference in ECR-Avoidance
and ECR-Anxiety as the dependent variables were run
Predicting differences in body satisfaction from with either difference in BES-Object or difference in
attachment style BES-Process as the independent variable. For BES-
Object, the multivariate effect for the covariate was
To test whether differences in attachment style only marginally significant, Pillais’ F (2,121) = 2.41,
were related to differences in body satisfaction from p < .10. There was a significant effect for relationship
general to specific for close friends and for romantic type, Pillais’ F (2,121) = 5.32, p < .01. Only the
relationships, difference scores were calculated by univariate effect for difference in ECR-Anxiety
subtracting specific scores from general scores for was significant, F (1,122) = 10.33, p < .01, h2 = .08.
216 N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218

On average, there was less anxiety in specific versus relationships still were obtained when body surveil-
general relationships, but this difference in anxiety lance was controlled. A major contribution of this
was greater for close friendships (M = 1.02) than for study is the use of a more general adult population
romantic relationships (M = .23). The multivariate that included both undergraduates and community
effect for difference in BES group was also significant, participants.
Pillais’ F (4,244) = 2.57, p < .05. Univariate effects As expected, these results partially replicate Cash
were significant for difference in ECR-Avoidance, et al.’s (2003) study. General attachment anxiety was a
F (2,122) = 3.31, p < .05, h2 = .05. Although ECR- zero-order predictor of body satisfaction, as well as a
Avoidance was lower for specific relationships com- unique predictor of body-as-object satisfaction, with
pared to general relationships on average across BES body surveillance and age controlled. More positive
groups, a lower BES-Object in specific relationships body satisfaction was associated with lower levels of
was associated with a smaller difference in ECR- anxiety. Also, as predicted, general attachment
Avoidance (M = 1.53, 1.50, and .93 for increased BES, avoidance did not uniquely predict any body
no change, and decreased BES, respectively). Those satisfaction variable for women with body surveil-
whose BES-Object scores were higher, the decrease in lance and age controlled. This supports the hypothesis
avoidance was greater than those whose BES-Object that attachment anxiety, which represents sensitivity
scores were lower in the specific relationships, t to rejection, is more important in predicting negative
(97) = 2.24, p < .05. None of the other mean differ- body experience than is attachment avoidance, which
ences were significant. The multivariate interaction represents avoidance of closeness. It may be that
between relationship type and BES-Object difference women who are high on attachment anxiety use body
was not significant, Pillais’ F (4,244) = .64, ns. surveillance as a strategy to control rejection. This
For the BES-Process analysis, the multivariate strategy in turn leads to poorer body image. This
effect for relationship type was significant, Pillais’ F hypothesis is supported by the positive relationship
(2,121) = 5.66, p < .01. Only the univariate effect between attachment anxiety and body surveillance in
for difference in ECR-Anxiety was significant, F these data and research showing body surveillance
(1,122) = 11.36, p < .01, h2 = .09. Similar to the precedes negative body experience (e.g., Tiggemann &
analysis for BES-Object, ECR-Anxiety was lower Lynch, 2001). On the other hand, women who feel
on average for specific relationships than for general negatively about their bodies may both engage in
relationships, but this difference in anxiety was greater greater body surveillance and may also be higher in
for close friendships (M = 1.14) than for romantic attachment anxiety because of the importance of
relationships (M = .32). Neither the effect for change appearance for social relations (Cash & Fleming, 2002).
in BES-Process nor the interaction term were signi- Contrary to our predictions, both general attach-
ficant, Pillais’ F (4,244) = .12, ns, and Pillais’ F ment anxiety and avoidance also predicted body-as-
(4,244) = .63, ns, respectively. process satisfaction and anxiety predicted body-as-
process with body surveillance and age controlled.
Although body-as-process has been hypothesized to
Discussion be less sensitive to interpersonal contexts (Franzoi,
1995), for these women, fear of rejection may have
The purpose of this study was to examine the resulted in less positive evaluations of body function,
relationship of attachment style to body satisfaction in or less positive evaluations of body function may have
women. We expected to replicate Cash et al.’s (2003) led to greater fear of rejection.
study of attachment and body satisfaction. We also The analyses comparing the general variables with
extended this research by determining whether the those for specific significant others demonstrated
relationship between attachment style and body that the relationships between attachment and body
satisfaction differed as a function of whether the satisfaction differ in specific relationships compared
participants were reporting on their general attachment to general attachment style. Consistent with our
style or on attachment to a specific other, either a hypotheses, analyses of difference scores from general
romantic partner or a close friend, and whether these to specific relationships found greater differences for
N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218 217

attachment anxiety for close friendships than for There was no relationship between differences in
romantic relationships, although the effect sizes were body-as-process satisfaction and differences in attach-
small. This may be due to the characteristics of these ment style. For body-as-object, feeling more positive
two types of relationships. On the other hand, given about their bodies with a particular person appeared
that the gender of the primed attachment figure was to decrease women’s use of avoidance within that
confounded with the relationship type, with close relationship. Of course, it could also be that relation-
friends most likely to be women and romantic partners ships that encourage closeness also encourage more
most likely to be men, it may be that women positive feelings about the body than one would feel in
experience more fear of rejection in relationships with general. It is surprising given the evidence in these
men than with women. data and other studies (e.g., Cash et al., 2003) that
Regression analyses, however, did not support the attachment anxiety differences were unrelated to
hypotheses that romantic partner attachment style differences in body satisfaction. This suggests attach-
should be more similar to general attachment style ment anxiety is more useful for predicting levels of
than close friend attachment style. For romantic body satisfaction in general, while differences in
partnerships, only body surveillance uniquely con- attachment avoidance from general to specific relation-
tributed to body-as-object satisfaction and body-as- ships is more useful in predicting differences between
process satisfaction, whereas in the general regression general body satisfaction and body satisfaction in
analysis, body surveillance did not uniquely predict specific relationships.
body satisfaction. The analysis for women’s close As noted above, a major limitation of this study is
friendships was more similar to the general analysis, that the sex of the primed attachment figure was
with attachment anxiety being the only unique confounded with the relationship type. Future research
predictor for body-as-process satisfaction and both should include more diverse samples in terms of
attachment dimensions adding significantly to the sexual orientation and distribution of gender in close
variance for body-as-object satisfaction. The fact that friendships to examine whether different findings are a
the results for these analyses differ demonstrates the function of the relationship type or the sex of the
importance of examining attachment and body image primed attachment figure. Also, the use of categorical
in the context of specific others. Even though the variables for the difference scores rather than
presumption has been that attachment to romantic continuous variables, while necessary due to skew-
partners may the most chronically accessible (Baldwin ness, was likely to reduce power for finding effects.
et al., 1996) and the analyses of difference scores This research is also limited in that the self-report data
support this contention (as there was less difference are correlational, and thus, the direction of causation
between general and specific attachment for romantic cannot be determined. It seems entirely possible that
partners than for close friends), these regressions higher attachment anxiety decreases body satisfaction
analyses suggest that factors other than attachment, or body dissatisfaction increases attachment anxiety or
such as individual levels of body surveillance, may be that both of these occur. While this study included a
more important in understanding body image in the wider population sample than studies that include
context of specific romantic relationships. only undergraduates, it was limited in that it included
As predicted, differences in attachment style from primarily European American, heterosexual partici-
general to specific were associated with differences in pants, which limits its generalizability.
body-as-object satisfaction for women, although the This study demonstrates that examining differences
effect sizes were quite small, perhaps due to loss of in the general and specific relationship attachment
power by converting a continuous variable to a styles is important to understanding the relationship
categorical one. Women who felt less positive about between women’s relationship attachment and body
their body-as-object in specific relationships had satisfaction. Our data show that for specific romantic
smaller decreases in attachment avoidance in those partnerships, attachment did not uniquely predict body
relationships compared to their general attachment than satisfaction; whereas, it did predict body satisfaction
did women who had no difference or whose body-as- for specific close friendships and for general attach-
object evaluation improved in specific relationships. ment. In addition, the study overall shows there are
218 N.M. McKinley, L.A. Randa / Body Image 2 (2005) 209–218

significant connections between attachment style and research paradigms. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 59, 981–993.
body image in adults and suggests that further research
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and
in this area is warranted. Particularly, future studies private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of
may want to use longitudinal analyses to test whether it Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522–527.
appears that attachment style shapes body experience or Fraley, R. C., & Shaver, P. R. (2000). Adult romantic attachment:
whether body experience shapes attachment style, or Theoretical developments, emerging controversies, and unan-
whether a third factor, such as body surveillance shapes swered questions. Review of General Psychology, 4(2), 132–154.
Fraley, R. C., Waller, N. G., & Brennan, K. A. (2000). An item
both attachment style and body satisfaction. Answers to response theory analysis of self-report measures of adult attach-
these questions would be important both in suggesting ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(2), 350–
solutions for improved body satisfaction and/or more 365.
secure relationship attachments. Franzoi, S. L. (1995). The body-as-object versus the body-as-
process: Gender differences and gender considerations. Sex
Roles, 33, 417–437.
Franzoi, S., & Shields, S. (1984). The body esteem scale: Multi-
Acknowledgements dimensional structure and sex differences in a college popula-
tion. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48, 173–178.
We wish to thank Kristi Arevena, Natalie Dean, Friedman, M. A., Dixon, A. E., Brownell, K. D., Whisman, M. A., &
Wilfley, D. E. (1999). Marital status, marital satisfaction, and
Erin Fisher, and Colleen Keeton-Sims for data body image dissatisfaction. International Journal of Eating
collection and data entry, and Jeannie Jacobson and Disorders, 26(1), 81–85.
Bronwyn Pughe for help with editing. Hoyt, W. D., & Kogan, L. R. (2001). Satisfaction with body image
and peer relationships for males and females in a college
environment. Sex Roles, 45, 199–215.
Kearney-Cooke, A. (2002). Familial influences on body image
References development. In T. F. Cash, & T. Pruzinksy (Eds.), Body image:
A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice (pp. 99–
Baldwin, M. W., Keelan, J. P. R., Fehr, B., Enns, V., & Koh- 107). New York: Guilford Press.
Rangarajoo, E. (1996). Social-cognitive conceptualization of McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body
attachment working models: Availability and accessibility consciousness scale: Development and validation. Psychology
effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), of Women Quarterly, 20, 181–215.
94–109. Mikulincer, M., Gillath, O., & Shaver, P. R. (2002). Activation of
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books. the attachment system in adulthood: Threat-related primes
Brennan, K. A., & Shaver, P. R. (1995). Dimensions of adult attach- increase the accessibility of mental representations of attach-
ment, affect regulation, and romantic relationship functioning. ment figures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(3), 267–283. 83(4), 881–895.
Cash, T. F., & Fleming, E. C. (2002). Body image and social Paxton, S. J., Schutz, H. K., Wertheim, E. H., & Muir, S. L. (1999).
relations. In T. F. Cash, & T. Pruzinksy (Eds.), Body image: Friendship clique and peer influences on body image concerns,
A handbook of theory, research, and clinical practice (pp. 277– dietary restraint, extreme weight-loss behaviors, and binge eat-
286). New York: Guilford Press. ing in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108,
Cash, T. F., & Pruzinksy, T. (2002). Understanding body images: 255–266.
Historical and contemporary perspectives. In T. F. Cash, & T. Sherman, A. M., de Vries, B., & Lansford, J. E. (2000). Friendship
Pruzinksy (Eds.), Body image: A handbook of theory, research, in childhood and adulthood: Lessons across the lifespan.
and clinical practice (pp. 3–12). New York: Guilford Press. International Journal of Aging and Human Development,
Cash, T. F., Theriault, J., & Annis, N. W. (2003). Body image in an 51(1), 31–51.
interpersonal context: Adult attachment, fear of intimacy, and Tantleff-Dunn, S., & Gokee, J. L. (2002). Interpersonal influences on
social anxiety. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 23, body image development. In T. F. Cash, & T. Pruzinksy (Eds.),
89–103. Body image: A handbook of theory, research, and clinical
Evans, L., & Wertheim, E. H. (1998). Intimacy patterns and relation- practice (pp. 108–116). New York: Guilford Press.
ship satisfaction of women with eating problems and the med- Tiggemann, M., & Lynch, J. E. (2001). Body image across the life
iating effects of depression, trait anxiety and social anxiety. span in adult women: The role of self-objectification. Develop-
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 44, 355–365. mental Psychology, 37, 243–253.
Fehr, B. (1996). Friendship processes. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Ward, A., Ramsay, R., & Treasure, J. (2000). Attachment research
Feingold, A. (1990). Gender differences in effects of physical and eating disorders. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 73,
attractiveness on romantic attraction: A comparison across five 35–51.

Potrebbero piacerti anche