Sei sulla pagina 1di 8

Journal of Abnormal Psychology Copyright 1992 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1992, Vol. 101, No. 1,192-199 0021-843X/92/S3.00

Hypnosis, Reporting Bias, and Suggested Negative Hallucinations


Nicholas P. Spanos, Cheryl A. Burgess,
Patricia A. Cross, and Geoffrey MacLeod
Carleton University, Ottawa,
Ontario, Canada

We examined the role of reporting bias in hypnotic negative hallucinations by using a paradigm in
which reporting bias was assessed independently of perceptual change. In Experiment 1, highly
hypnotizable subjects reported significant loudness reductions when tested for hypnotic deafness.
Later, however, these subjects biased their reported loudness reductions in the absence of percep-
tual change, and their reporting bias scores were almost as large as their hypnotic deafness reports.
Subjects also biased their ratings of strategy use. In Experiment 2, ratings of blindness given in
response to a hypnotic negative visual hallucination suggestion were significantly correlated with
reporting bias scores obtained in this paradigm. Although hypnotic blindness and hypnotic deaf-
ness correlated significantly, the partial correlation between these variables was nonsignificant
when reporting bias scores were statistically controlled. Theoretical implications are discussed.

For more than a century, hypnosis has been associated with response to deafness suggestions despite behaving in a "non-
reports of dramatic, suggestion-induced changes in memory deaf" manner during delayed auditory feedback (Jones &
and perception. In most cases, however, the only available in- Flynn, 1989).
dexes of suggestion-induced experiential change have been ei- A second and more fundamental difficulty with the compli-
ther verbal report or other behaviors under subjects' direct vol- ance hypothesis stems from the lack of a methodology with
untary control. Some researchers have used indirect indexes of which to assess reporting bias independently of perceptual expe-
perceptual and memory change. For example, response to de- rience. For instance, reports of reduced pain or hearing after
layed auditory feedback has been used to index hypnotic deaf- suggestion may to some extent reflect both actual perceptual
ness (e.g., Sutcliffe, 1961). The results of these studies have typi- change and demand-induced reporting bias. In the absence of
cally indicated no evidence for perceptual or memory change some means for separating biased responding from perceptual
on the indirect indexes despite subjects' reports of experiencing change, investigators have been free to accept or reject sugges-
suggested changes. tion-induced biased responding as a hypothesis on the basis of
One interpretation of such findings is that hypnotic sugges- personal preference rather than data.
tions rarely if ever produce actual changes in perception or Recently, we developed a paradigm that enabled suggestion-
memory, but instead induce subjects to compliantly bias their induced reporting bias to be assessed directly and indepen-
reports in line with suggested demands (Sutcliffe, 1961; Wag- dently of experiential changes during hypnotic deafness
staff, 1981,1986). Compliant responding is biased responding,
(Spanos, Burgess, & Perlini, in press) and hypnotic analgesia
and it may be said to occur when subjects report experiences
(Spanos, Perlini, Patrick, Bell & Gwynn, 1990). For instance,
that they did not have or exaggerate experiences that they did
with respect to hypnotic deafness, the paradigm involved ad-
have, in terms of situational demands (Orne & Scheibe, 1964;
ministering an easily audible tone on three successive trials. A
Wagstaff, 1981). Response biases of this kind may reflect either
10-s waiting period followed termination of the tone on each
demand-induced lying or demand-induced retrospective rein-
trial, and after the waiting period subjects rated the loudness of
terpretation of experience. In either case, demand-induced re-
sponse biases reflect misdescriptions motivated by the desire to the preceding tone. The first trial was a baseline, and the sec-
meet role expectations (Spanos, 1991; Wagstaff, 1981). ond was preceded by a hypnotic suggestion for deafness. After
Although the compliance hypothesis is an old one, it has the second trial, the hypnotic suggestion was canceled and sub-
failed to gain general acceptance for at least two reasons. First, jects were administered the third tone. After termination of
a number of investigators have suggested that indirect indexes Tone 3, but before subjects made their loudness ratings, differ-
of perceptual and memory functioning do not validly reflect ent procedures were followed for experimental and control sub-
subjects' experiences. According to this hypothesis, for exam- jects. Control subjects simply waited 10 s and then rated the
ple, subjects might show at least partial hearing reduction in loudness of Tone 3, as they did on previous trials. In contrast,
during the waiting period between the termination of the Tone
3 and the loudness rating, experimental subjects were informed
that they had probably slipped into hypnosis and heard the last
This research was supported by a grant from the Natural Sciences
and Engineering Research Council of Canada. tone less loudly than they heard the baseline tone. This instruc-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to tion was designed to produce reduction in the rated loudness of
Nicholas P. Spanos, Department of Psychology, Carleton University, Tone 3 independent of subjects' actual perceptual experience of
Ottawa, Ontario K1S 5B6, Canada. Tone 3.
192
HYPNOSIS, COMPLIANCE AND HALLUCINATION 193

It is important to keep in mind that up to the termination of more loudness than they had on the hypnotic deafness trial
Tone 3, control subjects and experimental subjects were admin- (Tone 2). The increase in loudness reported by these subjects
istered identical procedures and, therefore, presumably experi- from Trial 2 to Trial 3 is open to several interpretations. One
enced equivalent levels of loudness during presentation of Tone possibility is that subjects would have reported an increase in
3. Consequently, any reduction in the rated loudness of this tone loudness across these trials even if they had been administered
by experimental subjects relative to controls must reflect a re- a hypnotic deafness suggestion before each trial. Alternatively,
porting bias rather than a difference in perceptual experience. the higher loudness reported after the demand instruction
Control subjects tested in this paradigm reported no significant (Tone 3) than after the suggestion (Tone 2) may indicate that
differences in the loudness of Tones 1 and 3. Therefore, for the demand manipulation was less potent than hypnotic sug-
experimental subjects, reduction in Tone 3 loudness ratings rela- gestion at lowering loudness ratings.
tive to Tone 1 loudness ratings served as an index of the degree One purpose of the present study was to assess these alterna-
to which the demand instruction led subjects to bias their Tone tive hypotheses by testing three groups of highly hypnotizable
3 loudness reports. subjects on three tone trials. Subjects in Groups 1 and 2 were
To investigate hypnotic analgesia, we (Spanos, Perlini, et al., administered, respectively, the control sequence and the de-
1990) replaced the tone with a pain stimulus and replaced the mand instruction sequence described earlier. Those in Group 3
suggestion for deafness with a suggestion for pain reduction. were also administered a Trial 1 baseline tone and a hypnotic
Thus the two studies (Spanos et al., in press; Spanos et al., deafness suggestion before Tone 2. However, instead of cance-
1990) in which we have employed this paradigm have yielded ling the hypnosis and suggestion after Trial 2, we gave these
the following findings. Subjects administered the demand in- subjects another deafness suggestion before Trial 3. This design
struction exhibited significant levels of reporting bias relative to enabled us to determine whether the increase in rated loudness
controls. In fact, the degree of Trial 3 reporting bias was usually that occurs from Trial 2 (deafness suggestion) to Trial 3 (de-
about half as large as the ratings of deafness and analgesia given mand instruction) reflects differences in the potency of the
after the hypnotic suggestion on Trial 2. It is important to note suggestion and instruction manipulations or is a result of re-
that the degree of Trial 3 reporting bias correlated strongly with peated exposure to the tone after either suggestion or demand
Trial 2 ratings of deafness and analgesia. In other words, people instruction.
who reported high levels of hypnotic deafness or analgesia on Hypnotic suggestions for perceptual change often explicitly
Trial 2 tended to be the same people who exhibited large re- instruct subjects to use cognitive strategies to bring about the
porting biases on Trial 3. requisite experiences. Furthermore, subjects who report the per-
In our paradigm, response to the Trial 3 demand instruction ceptual experiences called for often report using the strategies
correlated with hypnotizability to the same extent that hypnoti- contained in the suggestion or other strategies of their own
zability correlated with Trial 2 ratings of deafness and analge- making (e.g., Spanos, Radtke-Bodorik, Ferguson, & Jones,
sia. Furthermore, when level of Trial 3 reporting bias was statis- 1979).
tically controlled, the correlations between hypnotizability and After Trial 3 all subjects in the present experiment completed
deafness scores and hypnotizability and analgesia scores were four increasingly explicit questionnaires that asked them to de-
much reduced and sometimes no longer statistically signifi- scribe their experiences on Trial 3. This procedure allowed us
cant. In short, these findings suggest that demand-induced re- to determine whether subjects given the demand instruction
porting bias plays an integral role in hypnotic responding and would bias their postexperimental reports by reporting the use
that much of the reporting change that follows hypnotic sugges- of cognitive strategies and whether any such biasing effects were
tions appears to be explicable in terms of reporting bias rather a function of the amount of information supplied to subjects by
than in terms of actual changes in subjective experience. the questionnaires that purported to assess their experience.
The present study included two experiments aimed at ex-
tending the findings thus far obtained with this paradigm. Ex-
periment 1 was concerned with both the internal validity of the Method
paradigm and the influence of reporting bias on open-ended
Subjects. Forty-five Carleton University undergraduates volun-
subjective reports as well as on perceptual ratings. In Experi- teered to participate in a one-session study on suggestion and auditory
ment 2 we assessed response to suggestions for negative visual perception. These subjects had previously been scored as highly hyp-
hallucination as well as for deafness, and examined the role of notizable (scores of 5-7) on the objective dimension of the Carleton
strategy use and reporting bias at influencing reports of nega- University Responsiveness to Suggestion Scale (CURSSrO; Spanos,
tive visual hallucination. Radtke, Hodgins, Stam, & Bertrand, 1983). All subjects received
course credit for their participation.
Apparatus and materials. Three identical repetitions of a 30-s, 60-
Experiment 1 dB, 1000-Hz pure tone were prerecorded on a BASF magnetic tape and
In our earlier study on hypnotic deafness and reporting bias presented to subjects over Sony DR-27 headphones from a Philips
(Spanos et al., in press), both experimental and control subjects D8270 tape recorder. Half the subjects received the tone in the left ear,
and half received it in the right ear. On each trial, 10 s after termination
were administered a hypnotic deafness suggestion before Tone of the tone, subjects verbally rated its loudness using a 21-point cate-
2, but received no instructions before Tone 3. As already de- gory rating scale. This scale contained alternatives ranging from no
scribed, controls returned to baseline levels of reported loud- sound (Q) to intensely loud (20).
ness on Tone 3, whereas those administered the demand in- Procedure. All subjects were individually tested by the same female
struction reported less loudness than they had on baseline, but experimenter. Subjects were randomly assigned to one of three condi-
194 SPANOS, BURGESS, CROSS, AND MACLEOD

tions, with the restriction that there be an equal number of subjects (n = main effects for group at Trials 1 and 2 failed to approach signif-
15) in each condition. Before their baseline trial, subjects were in- icance. In other words, subjects in the three groups reported
structed to give their loudness ratings after the termination of the tone, equivalent levels of loudness on both the baseline trial and
when the experimenter tapped the back of their hands (10s after stimu- Trial 2.
lus termination). Subjects were requested to close their eyes and were
Within-group simple effects analyses revealed that subjects
presented with the baseline auditory test trial.
After the baseline loudness reports, a 10-min hypnotic induction
given the reinforced hypnotic deafness suggestion, F(2, 84) =
procedure (adapted from Barber, 1969) was administered to all 45 sub- 33.58, p < .001, subjects given the demand instruction, F(2,
jects. This was followed by a suggestion for deafness that informed 84) = 35.64, p < .001, and the control subjects, F(2,84) = 35.53,
subjects that hypnosis would enable them to use their imaginal and p < .001, reported significant changes in the loudness of the
attentional processes to control sensory input and that on the next trial tone across the three trials. Post hoc comparisons indicated
they were to use these abilities to reduce the loudness of the tone. After that subjects given the reinforced hypnotic deafness suggestion
providing their Trial 2 loudness ratings, subjects were administered reported the tone as significantly less loud on Trials 2 and 3
their respective treatments. than on the baseline trial. The latter two means failed to differ
Fifteen subjects (the hypnosis group) were given a reinforced hyp- significantly. Subjects administered the demand instruction
notic deafness suggestion that informed them that they were to con-
also reported significantly lower loudness on Trial 2 (suggested
tinue to relax and drift deeper into hypnosis. This was followed by the
same deafness suggestion administered on Trial 2. After their Trial 3
deafness) and on Trial 3 (demand instruction) than on the base-
loudness reports, these subjects were awakened and the deafness sug- line trial. However, the Trial 3 loudness ratings of these subjects
gestion was canceled. were slightly but significantly higher than their Trial 2 loudness
After making their Trial 2 ratings, the remaining subjects (n = 30) ratings. For the control subjects, Trial 2 loudness reports were
were administered a wake-up procedure and cancellation of the deaf- substantially lower than those of either the baseline trial or
ness suggestion. For these subjects, Trial 3 was preceded with the in- Trial 3. Furthermore, the Trial 3 ratings of these subjects did
struction that this last trial would be "just like Trial 1." In the 10 s after not differ significantly from their baseline ratings.
termination of the Trial 3 tone, but before making their loudness rat- In summary, all subjects reported equivalent and substantial
ings, half of these subjects (n = 15) received the following demand hearing loss in response to the deafness suggestion on Trial 2.
instruction: However, subjects in the three conditions exhibited different
People who are exposed to the tone more than once tend to drift patterns of response from Trial 2 to Trial 3. Subjects who re-
back into hypnosis, and this greatly reduces the intensity of the ceived hypnotic deafness suggestions on both Trials 2 and 3
sound that they hear. You probably drifted into hypnosis on this reported equivalent hearing loss on these two trials. Subjects
last trial, and for this reason, heard very little of the tone. given the demand instruction also reported significant Trial 3
The other half (the control group) were not given any instruction before loudness reduction, but to a less degree than that exhibited on
making their final loudness ratings. Trial 2. In contrast, Trial 3 loudness reports for the control
At the end of the third trial (after their loudness ratings), subjects in subjects did not differ from their baseline trial reports.
all conditions were given three open-ended questionnaires designed to Reported deafness and cognitive strategies. A one-way AN-
assess their reported subjective experience of the Trial 3 tone. On the OVA performed on the cognitive strategy scores obtained from
first questionnaire subjects described everything they had experi- the open-ended questionnaires was significant, F(2,42) = 5.22,
enced on the last trial (Trial 3). On the second questionnaire subjects p < .01. Subjects who received the reinforced hypnotic deafness
provided more details about what they had experienced on the last suggestion on Trial 3 attained significantly higher cognitive
trial. On the third they reported what, if anything, they had done on strategy scores (M = 2.20, SD = .94) than did subjects given the
the last trial in order to hear the tone less loudly. Subjects who reported
demand instruction (M = 1.47, SD = 1.06) and the control sub-
using a strategy on the first questionnaire received a score of 3, those
who first reported strategy use on the second questionnaire were
jects (M = 1.00, SD = 1.07). Those in the latter two groups did
scored 2, and those who first reported strategy use on the third ques- not differ significantly in cognitive strategy scores.
tionnaire were scored 1. Subjects who failed to report strategy use on A one-way ANOVA performed on subjects' scale ratings of
any of the questionnaires were scored 0. Two independent judges who strategy use was significant, F(2, 42) = 6.84, p < .01. Post hoc
were unaware of subjects' treatment assignment agreed on their strat- comparisons revealed that both the reinforced hypnosis sub-
egy score 89% of the time. jects (M = 6.53, SD =1.51) and the demand instruction subjects
After completing these open-ended questionnaires, subjects were (M = 6.60, SD = 1.24) rated themselves as using cognitive strate-
administered a 9-point Likert-type scale on which they rated the extent gies to a greater extent than did the control subjects (M = 4.60,
to which they had used imaginal or distraction techniques in order to SD = 2.16). The strategy use ratings of demand instruction
reduce the loudness of the Trial 3 tone. The scale alternatives ranged
from not at all (1) to all the time (9).
Table 1
Results Mean Loudness Ratings by Group
Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial3
Reductions in reported loudness. A 3 X 3 (Group [hypnosis,
demand, or control] X Trial [1,2, or 3]) mixed analysis of vari- Group M SD M SD M SD
ance (ANOYA) performed on the loudness ratings yielded a
significant Group X Trial interaction, F(4,84) = 15.84, p < .001. Hypnosis 14.27 3.49 10.40 4.66 9.27 5.02
Demand Instruction 13.47 4.02 8.07 2.71 10.87 4.57
This interaction was analyzed further in terms of simple main 10.80 3.99 15.87 3.78
Control 14.93 3.13
effects, and the relevant means are shown in Table 1. The simple
HYPNOSIS, COMPLIANCE AND HALLUCINATION 195

subjects failed to differ significantly from the ratings of subjects study to administer deafness suggestions that did not provide
in the hypnosis condition. cognitive strategies. If subjects who received these suggestions
continued to report greater strategy use on open-ended ques-
tionnaires and greater loudness reductions on Trial 3 than sub-
Discussion jects given a demand instruction, support would be provided
Our findings replicate those of earlier studies (Spanos et al, for the hypothesis that strategy use produces effects on percep-
in press; Spanos et al., 1990) and provide strong support for the tual reports above and beyond the effects of reporting bias.
hypothesis that reports of suggestion-induced perceptual alter-
ation proffered by highly hypnotizable hypnotic subjects re- Experiment 2
flect, to a substantial degree, reporting biases rather than ac-
tual changes in perceptual processing. In addition, the present Negative visual hallucinations are suggestion-induced re-
findings indicate that the increase in loudness reported from ports of an inability to see a target stimulus. Negative visual
Trial 2 (hypnotic deafness) to Trial 3 (demand instruction) by hallucination suggestions are among the most difficult items
subjects given the demand manipulation cannot be explained on standardized hypnotizability scales (Hilgard, 1965) and are
in terms of repeated exposure to the tone. Subjects who were usually "passed" by only highly hypnotizable subjects. Al-
given the hypnotic suggestion before Trial 2 and again before though relatively little empirical work has focused on negative
Trial 3 continued to report the same low levels of loudness on visual hallucinations, some evidence indicates that reporting
both trials. These findings indicate that hypnotic suggestions bias may play a prominent role in this phenomenon.
were slightly but significantly more potent at inducing reports Spanos, Flynn, and Gabora (1989) gave highly hypnotizable
of deafness than was the demand instruction employed in this subjects the suggestion that, on opening their eyes, they would
research. These findings might mean that Trial 2 loudness re- see only a blank piece of paper. In fact, the number 8 was dis-
duction scores reflect a small perceptual alteration component played prominently on the paper. Subjects who reported seeing
that was added to a reporting bias component. Alternatively, nothing on the paper were interviewed by a second experi-
these findings might simply mean that hypnotic suggestions menter, who implied to them that seeing nothing was the re-
contained stronger demands to bias perceptual reports than sponse typically given only by fakers, whereas seeing a figure
did our demand instruction. Regardless of which hypothesis that gradually faded over 1 min was typical of truly hypnotized
eventually proves true, the present findings clearly support people. Subjects were then given the opportunity to draw what
Wagstaff's (1981,1986) contention that suggestion-induced re- they had seen on the paper at successive points after they had
porting bias is a central component in hypnotic responding. opened their eyes. Under these circumstances, 14 out of 15 sub-
Subjects given the deafness suggestion before Trial 3 re- jects who had initially insisted that they saw nothing on the
ported more use of cognitive strategies in their open-ended re- paper now drew a number 8 as the figure seen when they first
ports than did either control subjects or subjects given the de- opened their eyes. In other words, when demands to report
mand instruction. As already mentioned, only subjects given having "seen nothing" were lifted, almost all of these highly
the Trial 3 suggestion maintained equivalent levels of reported hypnotizable subjects acknowledged through their drawings
loudness reduction across Trials 2 and 3. One interpretation of that they had seen the target figure they had earlier denied
these findings is that strategy use led to the greater-than-de- seeing.
mand loudness reductions in subjects given the Trial 3 sugges- These findings do not mean that subjects in the Spanos et al.
tion. On the other hand, these findings could indicate that (1989) experiment failed to experience any perceptual distor-
subjects who received the Trial 3 suggestion were particularly tion after the suggestion. By employing simple ocular strategies
likely to indicate strategy use because only these subjects had such as unfocusing the eyes, or averting gaze from the target
been given a suggestion that explicitly instructed them to use stimulus, subjects can easily produce blurring and other distor-
strategies on Trial 3. The open-ended questionnaires, by them- tions of visual perception. Subjects in the Spanos et al. experi-
selves, contained few cues that strategy reporting was required. ment and in other experiments on negative hallucination may
Consequently, most subjects given the demand instruction may well have employed such strategies to induce perceptual distor-
have been unaware that such reporting was called for. However, tions. Nevertheless, Spanos et al.'s findings suggest that, regard-
when these subjects were given the final Likert-type scale that less of any such distortions in perception, subjects clearly exag-
explicitly asked them to rate the extent of their strategy use, gerated their experiences in line with suggested demands by
their ratings on this dimension were as high as those of the stating that they could see nothing on the paper.
subjects given the Trial 3 deafness suggestion. In other words, Although hypnotic subjects are exposed to explicit demands
subjects were quite willing to bias their reports of strategy use as to gear their reports in line with the requests of suggestions,
well as their loudness reports. However, such biasing was un- they are also exposed to implicit demands to report honestly
likely to occur in the demand instruction condition until these and accurately about their experiences (deGroot & Gwynn,
subjects were provided (via an explicit questionnaire) with the 1989; Spanos, 1986). Furthermore, hypnotic subjects are likely
requisite information about the responses called for. Such infor- to differ substantially in the extent to which they are sensitive to
mation was provided by the suggestion for those subjects who norms for honest reporting. Consequently, such subjects are
received a suggestion before Trial 3. Consequently, for subjects likely to exaggerate and distort their experiences to different
in this condition, open-ended reports of strategy use and the degrees in response to experimental demands.
relationship between such reports and reported loudness re- Although the Spanos et al. (1989) study indicated that nega-
ductions remain ambiguous. It would be of interest in a future tive hallucinators exaggerate their experiences in terms of sug-
196 SPANOS, BURGESS, CROSS, AND MACLEOD

gested demands, it provided no means of assessing individual were asked to circle as many as they had used in their attempt to make
differences in the degree of such reporting bias. Moreover, nei- the word disappear. These strategies were listed as follows:
ther Spanos et al. nor other researchers have assessed the cogni-
1. I unfocused my eyes.
tive strategies that subjects employ in order to distort their per- 2. I crossed my eyes.
ceptual experiences and define themselves as "not seeing." 3. I looked above, below, or away from the word, rather than
In Experiment 2, we assessed the strategies for "not seeing" looking right at it.
that subjects reported in response to a negative visual hallucina- 4. I created a visual image to cover or block the word.
tion suggestion and examined the relationship between the use 5. I tried to imagine a different word.
6. I tried to think of other things, so as not to notice the word.
of such strategies and the degree to which subjects reported 7. Other (specify).
blindness. In addition, the role of reporting bias in negative
visual hallucination responding was assessed by testing sub- Subjects were tested in a second individual session within 2 weeks of
jects in the deafness paradigm described in Experiment 1 and the first. During the second session, 34 subjects were administered the
examining the relationship between bias scores obtained in that deafness paradigm with the demand instruction after Tone 3 in the
paradigm and blindness scores obtained in response to a nega- same manner as in Experiment 1. The remaining 15 subjects were ad-
tive visual hallucination suggestion. We anticipated that report- ministered the control sequence of the paradigm. After their loudness
ratings all subjects were administered two open-ended questionnaires
ing bias in the deafness paradigm would correlate significantly taken from Experiment 1 (Questionnaires 1 and 3) that assessed re-
with reports of blindness after the suggestion for negative visual ported strategy use during the Trial 3 tone. Subjects were scored 2 if
hallucination. they reported strategy use on the first questionnaire, 1 if they reported
strategy use on the second questionnaire, and 0 if they failed to report
Method strategy use on either questionnaire. Two independent judges agreed
on these ratings 92% of the time.
Subjects. Forty-nine Carleton University undergraduates volun-
teered to participate in a study of perception and hypnosis. These
subjects had previously tested as either medium (scores of 3 or 4) or Results
high (scores of 5-7) on the CURS&O (Spanos et al., 1983). All subjects Compliance and control. A Hotelling's T2 analysis that com-
received course credit for their participation. pared the scores of Session 2 experimental and control subjects
Procedure. All subjects were tested individually by the same male on the CURSS dimensions, open-ended blindness reports,
experimenter in two sessions. In the first session subjects were asked to
blindness scale self-ratings, and judges' blindness strategy use
close their eyes and were administered the 10-min hypnotic induction
procedure used in Experiment 1. This was followed by a suggestion for ratings failed to approach significance. Thus subjects in the
blindness that informed subjects that they were to become actively demand instruction and control conditions were equivalent on
involved in the suggestion and were to use whatever strategies they these measures.
could to make a word disappear from a piece of paper. Subjects were Session 2 experimental effects. A 2 X 3 (Condition [demand
then instructed to look at an 8.5 in. X 11 in. sheet of white paper that instruction or control] X Trial [1, 2, or 3]) mixed ANOV\ on
was held approximately 50 cm in front of their eyes. On the paper was loudness ratings yielded a significant interaction, F(2, 94) =
printed the word DINOSAUR in letters 20 mm in height. The sugges- 3.88, p < .025, that was analyzed further in terms of simple
tion instructed subjects to notice that the word was fading more and main effects. As shown in Table 2, the simple main effects for
more, until it completely disappeared and the paper was blank. They condition at Trials 1 and 2 failed to approach significance, indi-
were then asked to describe what they saw on the paper. Following cating that subjects in the demand instruction and control con-
Bryant and McConkey (1989), subjects who reported seeing all or part
of the word DINOSAUR were given a prompt that encouraged them to ditions of Session 2 reported equivalent levels of loudness on
try harder and to use whatever strategies they could to make the word these two trials. However, a significant simple main effect was
fade and disappear. After the prompt, these subjects were again asked found for condition at Trial 3, F(l, 70) = 6.28, p < .025. Subjects
what they saw on the paper. After their verbal reports, a wake-up pro- who received the demand instruction reported significantly
cedure was administered and the blindness suggestion was canceled. lower Trial 3 loudness ratings than did the nondemand (con-
Blindness scores were computed as follows. Subjects who in their ini- trol) subjects.
tial descriptions reported seeing nothing on the paper were given a Analyses within conditions revealed that significant changes
blindness score of 2. Subjects who initially reported seeing all or part of in the loudness of the tone across trials were reported by sub-
the word but reported complete blindness after the prompt received a jects given the demand instruction, F(2,94) = 30.14, p< .001,
score of 1. Finally, subjects who failed to report blindness despite the and also by subjects not given the demand instruction (con-
prompt received a score of 0.
Subjects were next administered a 5-point scale asking them to rate
the extent of their blindness. Scale alternatives ranged from not at all
(0) to completely (4). After completion of this hypnotic blindness self- Table 2
rating, subjects wrote responses to a one-page, open-ended question- Mean Loudness Ratings for Demand
naire that asked them what, if anything, they had done in order to "not Instruction and Control Subjects
see" the word. This questionnaire was scored for cognitive strategy use
by two independent judges who were unaware of subjects' experimen- Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3
tal condition and level of hypnotizability. Judges scored each subject as
reporting strategy use (1) or not reporting strategy use (0). Judges Subjects M SD M SD M SD
agreed 92% of the time, and discrepancies were resolved through dis- 11.44 3.82
Demand Instruction 13.29 3.94 9.47 3.32
cussion. Control 14.20 3.65 10.20 4.02 14.40 4.42
Finally, the subjects were given a list of seven possible strategies and
HYPNOSIS, COMPLIANCE AND HALLUCINATION 197

trols), F(2, 94) = 20.42, p < .001. Post hoc comparisons (least word (Strategy 4); and the remaining subject simply reported "I
significant difference) indicated that subjects given the demand blanked my mind."
instruction reported significant loudness reductions on Trial 2 A t test comparing Session 2 demand instruction and control
(suggested deafness) and on Trial 3 (demand instruction). How- subjects on auditory strategy scores obtained from the open-
ever, Trial 3 reported loudness reductions were significantly ended questionnaires failed to approach significance. As in Ex-
smaller than those of Trial 2. The nondemand subjects re- periment 1, subjects who received the demand instruction (M=
ported significantly lower loudness on Trial 2 than on Trial 1 or .74, SD = .57) reported no greater use of cognitive strategies to
3. Loudness ratings on Trials 1 and 3 failed to differ signifi- reduce the loudness of the Trial 3 tone than did control subjects
cantly.
Reporting bias in "blind" and"not blind"subjects. Analyses of Correlations. Table 4 shows the correlations among the de-
relationships between Session 2 reporting bias and response to pendent variables assessed in Sessions 1 and 2 for subjects given
the Session 1 negative visual hallucination suggestion included the demand instruction in Session 2. Difference scores were
only those subjects who were administered the demand in- used to compute Session 2 deafness scores (baseline-Trial 2)
struction in Session 2. For each of these subjects, a Session 2 and reporting bias scores (baseline-Trial 3).
reporting bias score was obtained by subtracting their Trial 3 In Session 2, the correlation between Trial 2 deafness scores
(demand instruction) loudness score from their Trial 1 score. and Trial 3 reporting bias scores was substantial and indicated
We anticipated that subjects who reported complete blind- that to a large extent subjects who reported high levels of hyp-
ness in Session 1 would report greater Session 2 reporting bias notic deafness were the same persons who, on the next trial,
then subjects who reported little or no blindness in Session 1. biased their reports in terms of experimental demands. Interest-
This hypothesis was tested in an analysis that included only ingly, the extent to which subjects biased their reports on Trial 3
those subjects who rated themselves as exhibiting complete also correlated significantly with the extent to which they re-
blindness (self-rating of 4, n = 11) or little or no blindness (self- ported use of cognitive strategies on that trial. In other words,
rating of 1 or 0,« = 9) in Session 1. As predicted, subjects who subjects who biased their reports of hearing reduction to a rela-
reported complete blindness (M = 3.73, SD = 2.65) exhibited tively large degree also tended to be attuned enough to experi-
substantially and significantly greater Session 2 reporting bias mental demands to generate reports of strategy use on the basis
than those who did not report blindness (M= .33, SD = 3.24), of the minimal cues provided by the open-ended question-
/(18) = 2.58 p < .05. naires.
Strategies for blindness and deafness. On the basis of their In Session 1, the correlation between open-ended reports of
Session 1 open-ended reports, 40 subjects (82%) were rated as blindness and self-ratings of blindness was very high and sup-
using one or more strategies in an attempt to "not see" the ports the notion that these two indexes assessed the same re-
target word. However, in response to the seven-item list of strat- sponse dimension. Neither of these Session 1 blindness indexes
egies, all subjects reported using at least one strategy. The per- correlated significantly with the extent of strategy use reported
centages of subjects who chose each listed strategy are given in by subjects in response to the negative visual hallucination sug-
Table 3. Twelve subjects (24%) chose Strategy 7 on the strategy gestion. On the other hand, both Session 1 blindness indexes
checklist, indicating that they had used strategies other than correlated significantly with Session 2 deafness ratings and re-
the available choices. Closer examination revealed that 8 of porting bias scores. More important, the partial correlations
these subjects (67%) reported ocular fixation on only a limited between deafness ratings and open-ended reports of blindness,
aspect of the stimulus (i.e., staring fixedly at a letter or portion r(31) = .03, and between deafness ratings and self-ratings of
of the word DINOSAUR); 2 subjects (17%) described sensations blindness, r(31) = .22, were not significant when the effects of
experienced during the suggestion, such as a blurriness of vi- reporting bias scores were statistically controlled. Reporting
sion, rather than reporting a specific strategy; 1 subject reiter- bias scores, like blindness scores, did not correlate significantly
ated that he had visualized a piece of white paper covering the with the extent of subjects' strategy use in response to the Ses-
sion 1 suggestion. Taken together, these findings indicate that
reporting bias influenced response to suggestions for negative
Table 3 visual hallucination as well as response to deafness suggestions.
Number and Percentage of Subjects Moreover, the correlation between reports of suggested blind-
Who Reported Using Each Strategy ness and deafness was mediated by subjects' tendency to bias
their perceptual reports in terms of suggested demands.
No. of
Strategy subjects %
General Discussion
1. I unfocused my eyes. 33 67
2. I crossed my eyes. 7 14 The findings of Experiment 2 concerning suggested deafness
3. I looked above, below, or away from the word, replicate those of Experiment 1 as well as those of Spanos et al.
rather than looking right at it. 29 59 (in press) and indicate that demand-induced reporting bias
4. I created a visual image to cover or block the
word. 13 27 plays an important role in the phenomenon of hypnotic deaf-
5. I tried to imagine a different word. 4 8 ness. The results of Experiments 1 and 2 further indicate that
6. I tried to think of other things, so as not to compliance pressures lead subjects to bias their reports of audi-
notice the word. 6 12 tory strategy use as well as their reports of reduced hearing.
7. Other (specify). 12 24
When assessment procedures explicitly implied that the report-
198 SPANOS, BURGESS, CROSS, AND MACLEOD

Table 4
Correlations Among Session 1 and Session 2 Dependent Variables in Experiment 2
Variable 1

1. Suggested deafness .79** .39* .31* .42** .04


2. Reporting bias .35* .37* .38* .02
3. Deafness strategies .03 .03 .11
4. Judges' blindness ratings .82** .04
5. Self-ratings of blindness .04
6. Blindness strategies

*p<.05. **p<.01.

ing of strategy use was appropriate and provided a simple rating self-presentation concerns and their resultant motivations to
scale for expressing such reports, subjects exposed to a demand bias their perceptual reports in terms of situational demands.
inducement but no deafness suggestion reported as much strat- Although suggested deafness scores in Session 2 correlated
egy use as subjects given hypnotic suggestions that explicitly significantly with suggested blindness scores in Session 1, these
requested strategy use. When open-ended questionnaires pro- correlations dropped to nonsignificance when the effects of
vided few cues concerning the reporting of strategies, subjects reporting bias were statistically controlled. These findings indi-
exposed to the demand instruction rarely reported strategy use. cate that the relationship between suggested blindness and sug-
Nevertheless, subjects who did report strategy use under these gested deafness in the present experiments was not mediated
low-cue conditions tended to be those who were most willing to by some general ability on the part of highly hypnotizable sub-
bias their perceptual reports. Thus subjects who were willing to jects to alter their perceptual experiences in line with sugges-
bias their perceptual reports were also willing to bias their re- tions. Instead, the common feature tying together responses in
ports of strategy use. Moreover, those who exhibited the largest these two situations was the tendency of subjects to bias their
biases in perceptual reporting also appeared to be particularly perceptual reports in terms of contextual demands.
sensitive to relatively subtle social cues (i.e., the wording of A number of investigators (Bryant & McConkey, 1989; Za-
open-ended questions) from which they derived and used infor- mansky, 1989) have contended that suggestion-induced reports
mation to better shape their self-presentations as responsive of blindness and deafness proffered by hypnotic subjects can be
hypnotic subjects. treated as accurate descriptions of private experience and have
Of course, it is possible to argue that subjects reported their developed theories of negative hallucination based on the valid-
experience of hypnotic deafness accurately on Trial 2 and only ity of such reports. The results of the present experiments, along
biased their responses when given the demand instruction after with those of earlier studies (Spanos et al, 1989; Spanos et al.,
Trial 3. If this were the case, however, there would be no reason 1990; Spanos, 1991), call these contentions into serious ques-
to expect a correlation between Trial 2 deafness reports and tion. Instead, our findings suggest that reports of hypnotic
Trial 3 bias scores. The fact that this correlation was both signifi- blindness and deafness should be treated skeptically unless cor-
cant and substantial suggests that Trial 2 hypnotic deafness roborated with objective indexes. In relation to this, theories of
scores reflect, to a large extent, reporting bias rather than per- negative hallucination that are based on the veridicality of such
ceptual change. reports are likely to be premature. On the other hand, our re-
The findings of Experiment 2 indicate that the tendency of sults strongly support Wagstaff 's (1981) contention that compli-
subjects to bias their reports in a hypnotic deafness situation ant responding plays a central role in hypnotic responding.
reflects a general tendency to bias reports across hypnotic test Although most investigators are rarely explicit when dealing
situations. The negative visual hallucination suggestion en- with the issue, they appear to reject the hypothesis that much
couraged subjects to become actively involved in distorting hypnotic behavior reflects reporting bias. Nevertheless, this re-
their perceptions, and subjects' strategy reports indicated that jection has not been based on convincing data or theory con-
all or almost all of them employed strategies (usually oculomo- cerning the limitations of this hypothesis. In fact, with few ex-
tor strategies) to create such distortions. Nevertheless, such ceptions (Orne, 1959; Sarbin & Coe, 1979; Wagstaff, 1981,1986)
strategy use was unrelated to the degree of blindness that sub- investigators have failed to spell out the implications of biased
jects reported. In contrast, the extent to which subjects biased responding for their theories of hypnosis. The present findings
their ratings in the deafness paradigm did correlate signifi- suggest that reporting bias deserves more serious consideration
cantly with blindness scores. This pattern of findings suggests than it has heretofore received in theoretical formulations of
that subjects used oculomotor and other strategies in an at- hypnotic responding.
tempt to induce perceptual distortion in response to the nega-
tive hallucination suggestion. Although strategy usage may
well have produced some degree of perceptual alteration, such References
usage was unrelated to subjects' reports of blindness. Instead,
the extent to which subjects defined whatever perceptual alter- Barber, T. X. (1969). Hypnosis: A scientific approach. New York: Van
ations they experienced as "blindness" was a function of their Nostrand Reinhold.
HYPNOSIS, COMPLIANCE AND HALLUCINATION 199

Bryant, R. A., & McConkey, K. M. (1989). Hypnotic blindness, aware- Spanos, N. P., Flynn, D. M., & Gabora, N. (1989). Suggested negative
ness and attribution. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98,443-447. visual hallucinations in hypnotic subjects: When no means yes. Brit-
deGroot, H. P, & Gwynn, M. I. (1989). Trance logic, duality and hid- ish Journal of Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis, 6, 63-67.
den observer responding. In N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hyp- Spanos, N. P., Periini, A. H., Patrick, L., Bell, S, & Gwynn, M. I. (1990).
nosis: The cognitive-behavioral perspective (pp. 187-205). Buffalo, The role of compliance in hypnotic and nonhypnotic analgesia.
NY: Prometheus. Journal of Research in Personality, 24, 433-453.
Hilgard, E. R. (1965). Hypnotic susceptibility. New York: Harcourt, Spanos, N. P., Radtke-Bodorik, H. L., Ferguson, J., & Jones, B. (1979).
Brace & World. The effects of hypnotic susceptibility, suggestions for analgesia and
Jones, W. D., & Flynn, D. M. (1989). Methodological and theoretical the utilization of cognitive strategies on the reduction of pain. Jour-
considerations in the study of "hypnotic" effects in perception. In nal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 282-292.
N. P. Spanos & J. F. Chaves (Eds.), Hypnosis: The cognitive-behavioral Spanos, N. P., Radtke, H. L., Hodgins, D. C, Stam, H. J., & Bertrand,
perspective (pp. 149-174). Buffalo, NY: Prometheus. L. D. (1983). The Carleton University Responsiveness to Suggestion
Orne, M. T. (1959). The nature of hypnosis: Artifact and essence. Jour- Scale: Normative data and psychometric properties. Psychological
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 277-299. Reports, 53, 523-535.
Orne, M. T., & Scheibe, K. E. (1964). The contribution of nondepriva- Sutcliffe, J. P. (1961). "Credulous" and "skeptical" views of hypnotic
tion factors in the production of sensory deprivation effect: The phenomena: Experiments on esthesia, hallucination and delusion.
psychology of the "panic button." Journal of Abnormal and Social Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 62,189-200.
Psychology, 68, 3-12. Wagstaff, G. F. (1981). Hypnosis, compliance and belief New York: St.
Sarbin, T. R., & Coe, W C. (1979). Hypnosis and psychopathology: Martin's Press.
Replacing old myths with fresh metaphors. Journal of Abnormal Psy- Wagstaff, G. F. (1986). Hypnosis as compliance and belief: A sociocog-
chology, 88, 506-526. nitive view. In P. Naish (Ed.), What is hypnosis?(pp. 59-84). Philadel-
Spanos, N. P. (1986). Hypnotic behavior: A social psychological inter- phia: Open University Press.
pretation of amnesia, analgesia and trance logic. Behavioral and Zamansky, H. S. (1989). Discussion commentary on "Suggested nega-
Brain Sciences, 9, 449-467. tive visual hallucinations in hypnotic subjects." British Journal of
Spanos, N. P. (1991). A sociocognitive approach to hypnosis. In S. J. Experimental and Clinical Hypnosis, 6, 67-70.
Lynn & J. R. Rhue (Eds.), Hypnosis theories: Current models and
perspectives (pp. 324-361). New \brk: Guilford Press.
Spanos, N. P., Burgess, C. A., & Periini, A. H. (in press). Compliance Received December 6,1990
and suggested deafness in hypnotic and nonhy pnotic subjects. Imagi- Revision received May 14,1991
nation, Cognition and Personality. Accepted May 18,1991 •

Potrebbero piacerti anche