Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Building and Environment


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/buildenv

Sharing lighting control in an open office: Doing one's best to avoid conflict T
a,c,∗ a,c a,b b a
Tatiana Lashina , Sanae Chraibi , Marija Despenic , Paul Shrubsole , Alexander Rosemann ,
Evert van Loenena,b
a
Eindhoven University of Technology, Eindhoven, the Netherlands
b
Philips Research, High Tech Campus 34, 5656 AE, Eindhoven, the Netherlands
c
Signify, High Tech Campus 7, 5656 AE, Eindhoven, the Netherlands

A R T I C LE I N FO A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Personal control for office workstation specific lighting was studied already for several decades, whereas this
Personal control form of lighting control for multi-user offices is a relatively young field of research. The proliferation of open
Lighting control offices in the last decade makes it vital to understand the benefits and drawbacks of personal lighting control in
Open office multi-user spaces.
Multi-user office
This paper presents the results of two field experiments that explored the experience of conflict and the social
Experimental study
dynamics among open office users to whom personal lighting control was offered. The study data revealed that
WELL standard
in multi-user spaces, individuals are self-conscious of the presence of others and deploy different strategies in
order to avoid conflict due to control of lighting. The paper discusses the implications these findings have for the
design of multi-user lighting control.
This study shows that individuals feel the nuisance of having no control over lighting stronger after they lose
it, than the satisfaction gains felt when they initially got control, known as a loss aversion bias. This has im-
plications for promoting beneficial effects of personal lighting controls in open office environments.

1. Introduction consisted of 2 field experiments: the first experiment (experiment 1)


was conducted in 2013 and the second experiment (experiment 2) was
To be in control gives a pleasant feeling. Not having an influence conducted in 2014. The current paper reports the outcomes of both
over events, circumstances of one's life or the environment people live experiment 1 and experiment 2 with respect to effects on social dy-
in, creates an uncomfortable feeling. Different studies show that control namics among the participants, the perceived frequency and degree of
in general and specifically control over lighting is something in- conflict, the observed phenomena of conflict avoidance and loss aver-
dividuals like to have as an antidote to not feeling in control [1,2]. A sion. The results of experiment 1, regarding the influence of personal
common practice related to thermal building controls is to mount pla- control on lighting quantity and quality perception, are published in
cebo controllers to give an illusion of control in order to decrease Ref. [7]. The results of the experiment 2 with respect to effects on
complaints [3]. Different studies [4,5], showed that, when controls are lighting quantity and quality and on lighting electric energy con-
available, the frequency of using them is relatively low especially sumption will be published in a separate paper.
compared to sensor input based adjustments. At the same time, office
users are typically very positive towards having personal controls in 1.1. Benefits of personal control for lighting
case they might need to make a change [4,6]. In the study of Moore
et al. office users' self-rated importance of lighting control was eval- Personal lighting control has been studied already for more than
uated [1]. The study showed a high mean score of 4.2 for the im- two decades as a means for saving energy and improving user comfort.
portance of being able to control lighting, rated on a 5-point scale There is a fundamental advantage provided by personal lighting con-
ranging from 1 - unimportant to 5- important. trols since they offer means to adjust lighting conditions to individual
The study of personal lighting control in multi-user open office preferences. Several studies have demonstrated a large diversity of in-
context investigated in this paper, was executed at Philips Research and dividual illuminance preferences covering a broad range from 80 lx to


Corresponding author. Department of the Built Environment, De Zaale, Groene Loper, building 6, Vertigo VRT 6.08, P.O. Box 513, 5612 AJ, Eindhoven, the
Netherlands.
E-mail addresses: tatiana.lashina_1@philips.com, t.lashina@tue.nl (T. Lashina).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2018.10.040
Received 17 August 2018; Received in revised form 21 October 2018; Accepted 22 October 2018
Available online 23 October 2018
0360-1323/ © 2018 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

1500 lx for workers in comparable working environments doing similar conflict, some personalities avoid using controls while others continue
tasks [8–12]. to use them, suggesting dominant personalities. Moore et al. observed
Boyce and colleagues conducted the study in an individual office that control decisions were not taken in order to reach consensus but
laboratory in US and showed that offices with control had higher rat- rather taken by dominant individuals [1].
ings of lighting quality and comfort, and experimental tasks were rated Despite higher satisfaction of users in offices with controls, the
as less difficult [11]. In another field experiment in US [13], 180 par- study [1] showed that 30% of those users expressed dissatisfaction with
ticipants experienced one of 4 different lighting conditions for one day. controls, which the authors associated with poor usability and inability
The study showed that individual control was associated with improved of users to make use of available controls. It was suggested that one of
comfort, and participants who experienced dimming control were more the reasons for not using controls was to avoid conflict with other office
motivated to sustain working on a difficult task and showed improved users. The offices in this study that did offer lighting control, were
performance with respect to attention. The same study also showed that designed with different lighting control group sizes that varied from 1
personal controls led to higher lighting quality evaluations and, among luminaire per group influencing at least 2 desks to 6 luminaires per
the tested lighting installations, the one that included personal control group. The study showed that in cases with smaller luminaire control
had the highest percentage of users satisfied [13]. Veitch et al. de- groups, the level of conflict experienced by users was smaller, the
monstrated that people who perceived their office lighting as being of controls were more frequently used, more energy was saved and the
higher quality rated the space as more attractive, reported more plea- users were more satisfied. The study also showed that locally situated
sant mood, greater well-being, improved motivation and vigilance [14]. controls were more beneficial than wall-mounted controls. Local con-
Veitch showed in a later study that availability of individually-con- trols resulted in lower luminaire output, controls being used more
trollable lighting leads to more favorable office appraisals and higher frequently, greater system awareness by the users, a greater perceived
levels of environmental satisfaction [15]. With respect to the effects on degree of control and less conflict experienced.
mood some studies did show a positive effect of personal control on
mood [12] while others failed to replicate this effect [16]. 1.4. Motivation for the study
Most of the previously mentioned studies considered a situation
where the individually adjustable lighting primarily was influencing a The current study focuses on the user experience of personal
participant's own desk. The testing conditions of some of these studies lighting control in the multi-user shared control context of an open
considered a private office where the user could dim lighting in the office. The goal is to explore the interplay between the known benefits
entire office. Other studies considered a cubicle where a luminaire was of controls demonstrated for workstation-specific lighting and possible
positioned above the user's desk and the user was able to control task drawbacks due to a prospect of conflict among multiple users of an
lighting at own desk. As such, these were conditions of truly personal open office.
control where the user would be controlling the light level at own desk One goal when designing the study was to offer lighting controls in
separate from other users. an open office setting in the best way possible. To achieve that, learn-
ings of the previous studies were used that included open offices having
1.2. Open office different control arrangements [1,18–22]. The test bed design aimed to
minimize the lighting control group size and it offered locally situated
While most studies of personal control considered the situation of controls instead of wall switches, since both aspects were reported to
workstation specific lighting, in the last decade private office lighting promote user satisfaction with lighting conditions. Another goal was to
arrangements have become increasingly rare. At the same time, open explore the social dynamics that would occur in the test office after the
offices accommodating multiple users are starting to dominate [17]. controls were distributed among the participants. The objective was to
The lighting for an open office is often designed as a regular grid of observe whether conflict would occur and among how many in-
luminaires in order to satisfy local regulations and design guidelines for dividuals, including the frequency and degree of conflict experienced.
lighting quantity and uniformity in the most efficient way. During the The study was to explore whether the conflict avoidance behavior re-
lighting design phase, the furniture layout is often not known or not ported in previous studies would manifest itself and to what degree.
being considered, since office layouts are likely to change throughout One of the questions was whether people would seek consensus and
the lifespan of a lighting installation. As a result, the luminaires grid in look for a middle ground to set the light level respecting each other's
most cases does not match the desk arrangements, whereby it does not needs.
allow offering lighting in a workstation-specific manner. This lighting
design leads to a situation where one luminaire affects a cluster of desks 2. Materials and methods
and one desk is illuminated by several luminaires.
A large scale study that specifically addressed the context of an open 2.1. Office layout
office was conducted in the UK from 1998 to 2000 [1,18–22], involving
14 open office buildings and more than 400 participants. Half of the For the field study, an open office was selected with a desk layout
examined open offices were equipped with personal dimming controls typical in the European context. This layout is known as an open
and another half did not have controls [22]. This study reported a bullpen office or a low partitioning office layout. This office layout is
higher level of satisfaction with lighting quantity among the users in gaining in popularity worldwide due to its costs efficiency [23]. In the
offices with lighting controls compared to those without controls. open bullpen arrangement desks are lined up in clusters of 2 rows on
each sides of a partitioning. The rows often stand perpendicular to the
1.3. Presence of others building windows façade (Fig. 1). Desks in such open offices stand next
to each other which decreases a perception of a workplace ownership
Although controls are highly desired [2], the presence of other of- [2,24]. This is in contrast to cellular offices and cubicles where desks
fice users does affect the use of such controls. It was shown that users in have much more separation creating enclosed private office cells.
private offices are more likely to use controls whereas users of open
offices tend to rather deploy psychological coping strategies, like 2.2. Participants
avoidance to use controls, despite discomfort [2]. The earlier men-
tioned UK study reported a strong correlation between the avoidance of There were differences between the two samples of participants of
using controls due to fear of conflict and dissatisfaction with the degree experiment 1 and 2. The 14 participants of experiment 1 were part of
of control [1]. The same study also showed that in a situation of the same administrative group, mostly senior employees with the

2
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

luminaire line were combined into control groups as indicated by bold


vertical rectangles in Fig. 2. In this arrangement luminaires 3 and 4
belonged to zone 1, luminaires 5 and 6 to zone 2 and so on. Each lu-
minaire control group was assigned to the closest row of desks below it,
creating 6 groups of 2–3 users per luminaire control group. Four lu-
minaires adjacent to the walls on both sides of the office were excluded
from the control groups. Their light output level was fixed at 60% to
maintain sufficient wall illuminance and prevent a possibility of having
dark walls in the office. Well-illuminated office walls were shown to
positively influence overall space appraisal and helped avoid sharp
contrast that could occur due to incoming daylight [25].

2.5. Light controllers

In order for the participants to experience lighting control during


the study each study participant received a personal controller on their
individual desk. Prior to that, each participant got an individual short
explanation of the purpose of using the controllers, security measures
Fig. 1. Test office impression from Ref. [7]. built-in in case an iPod would be taken away, and the actions to take in
case any technical problems would occur. The explanation of how the
controllers were assigned to the control zones and how they were
majority being between 30 and 65 years old (mean = 48.6, SD = 9.49),
controlling the luminaires in the space was not provided, since the in-
including 3 females and 11 males. These participants already knew
tention was for the participants to discover it on their own.
each other well. In experiment 2, the 14 participants belonged to sev-
Controllers were implemented as an application running on a
eral departments, and were a mix of senior employees as well as stu-
handheld smart device sitting in a docking station on each participant's
dents, also less familiar with each other. The age ranged from 25 to 65
desk. The application displayed a slider for dimming lighting in the
years old (mean = 44.3, SD = 11.58), including 1 female and 13 males.
whole range from off up to the maximum luminaire output as illu-
strated in Ref. [7]. Controllers belonging to the same control group
2.3. Installation were acting in an identical manner. A user action made on any of the
controllers of the same control group would set both luminaires of that
To conduct the field study, an open office space in one of the Philips control group at the user selected dimming level. After that all con-
office buildings in Eindhoven was chosen. In order to prepare the test trollers would display the new set value on the user interface.
office space for the purpose of the study, an existing lighting installation During the study, every participant had an assigned desk and every
was used. The office was initially equipped with 16 Philips TL5 49W desk controller was hard-coded to be connected to a particular control
lamps that were modernized by introducing Philips DALI dimmable zone. The presence-based lighting control in the test bed was im-
ballasts and combined light and presence sensors (Philips PLOS-CM- plemented for the whole space, so that when the first person would
KNX) with a 30 min time delay at every luminaire. The advantage of enter the space the whole space would light up. A presence control
using conventional luminaires was in the familiarity of the study par- would turn the lights off 30 min after the last person would vacate the
ticipants with this type of office lighting. If instead new LED based space.
luminaires would have been used, it could introduce a confounding
effect induced by a different color temperature or lighting distribution. 2.6. Blinds

2.4. Luminaires control groups The test bed area allowed for a good admission of daylight into the
space. The office was located at the southern façade of the building, on
The open office arrangement, as introduced in Section 2.1, makes it the fourth floor offering an unobstructed view due to the absence of
impossible to offer lighting controls in a truly personal workstation either high trees or neighboring high buildings. Daylight in the space
specific manner. Following the common practice of offering lighting created a challenge for adequate management of direct sunlight, i.e.
control in open offices, the luminaires in the test bed were combined glare. In the office, both internal and external blinds were available for
into control groups. Considering the benefits of smaller control size office occupants to deal with glare and regulate daylight intensity in-
groups as demonstrated by Moore et al. [1], the smallest possible side the space. Internal blinds were manually controllable motorized
control group size was implemented. Smaller control groups lead to Somfy blinds, controlling four clusters of window blinds separately
fewer users per control group, which makes it easier for members of the using four provided remote controls. The external blinds offered two
same group to reach a consensus. The top view of the test office layout modes of control: automatic and manual control using the physical
is shown in Fig. 2. As it can be seen the typical mismatch between the controllers mounted on the windowsill.
lighting grid and the desks grid was present in the office layout of the
test office. The office had two lines of luminaires, each line comprised 2.7. Conditions in experiment 1 and 2
of 8 luminaires. The desks were arranged in 3 clusters.
With the intent to give the participants equal access to control the This section explains the designs of experiment 1 and 2. Prior to
lights, the middle desks situated in between the two lines of luminaires conducting both experiments their protocols were reviewed and ap-
introduced a challenge. If controls would be distributed per luminaire proved by Philips Research internal ethics committee. All participants
to every user, the users sitting in the middle in between the luminaires signed an informed consent after familiarizing themselves with the
lines could feel less entitled to change the light level of the luminaires study conditions that included explanation of the intent of collecting
above their neighbors on their left and their right. Their neighbors’ and processing the data and the duration of keeping the data, voluntary
desks were located below the luminaires creating a more obvious link nature of the participation in the study, etc. The study participants were
between their desk and a luminaire. To resolve this asymmetry and give kept as naïve as possible about the actual purpose of the study to avoid
the participants equal sense of control, every two luminaires from each response bias. This was done by stating that the study aimed at

3
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

Fig. 2. Top view of the test office floor plan.

exploring their experience of the office space in a broad sense including control”.
aspects such as temperature, air quality, lighting and noise. In 2014, the second experiment was conducted, using the same test
In the first experiment, data was collected from the start of office and with the same modernized lighting installation. The office
September till mid-December of 2013. The study was designed as a space at the time of the study consisted of 16 workstations. However,
within-subject experiment with an ABBA reverse counterbalancing of two office workers could not participate in the study. In comparison to
conditions (Fig. 3) and involved 14 administrative office workers. The experiment 1, there were two additional desks, which are marked with
experiment was initiated with a reference “no control” - condition 1 in a diagonal stripes pattern in Fig. 2 (bottom right).
which the participants did not yet get the controllers. All luminaires In experiment 1, automatic luminaire output regulation based on
were set at their full output delivering on average 500 lx on the desk incoming daylight was explicitly excluded. Daylight regulation could
surface, which was measured when it was dark outside and all blinds potentially act as a confounding variable diluting the main purpose of
closed. Three full weeks of data were collected while the participants the experiment. After experiment 1 was completed, the intention was to
experienced this fixed light level condition. After the baseline condi- repeat the experiment but now to include the daylight regulation, since
tion, the iPod controllers were brought in and attached to the respective most systems in the field are daylight regulated. Having the daylight
docking stations on every participant's desk, initiating 7 weeks of the regulation would improve the ecological validity of the study.
“control” conditions 2 and 3. Conditions 2 and 3 were identical in terms Experiment 2 was designed to measure lighting quantity and quality
of their set-up. At the start of each “control” condition, the luminaire satisfaction, experience of conflict and preference for “control” or “no
light output was initially set at a default level of 60%, which corre- control” by letting the participants experience 5 different conditions
sponded to on average 300 lx on the desks. After a user would change (Fig. 4). Condition 1 and condition 2 were “no control” conditions where
this level, the system would remember it until the next change made by light output of the luminaire was regulated to maintain 500 lx and 300
a user. This system behavior was labelled as a “memorizing” personal lx average illuminance on the desk, respectively. Conditions 3, 4 and 5
control mode. were “control” conditions, where similar to experiment 1, the partici-
For the analysis of the data, the first week of the “control” condition pants received iPod dimming controllers on each individual desk. In
2 was excluded. Due to the novelty of the controllers, users played with condition 3 at the beginning of the day, the system would initially go
light levels in the first week substantially more than in the rest of the back to default 60% luminaire output level. The users could thereafter
“control” period. Finally, the “no control” condition was repeated again change the luminaire output to their liking. This system behavior
during the last 3 weeks of the experiment 1 by removing the iPods. This was labelled as “forgetting”. In condition 4 the last dimming level that
was the last condition 4. This study design helped to balance the number was selected by a user would be restored the next day, and this con-
of sunny and cloudy days occurring in “no control” and “control” dition was called “memorizing” (similar to the “control” conditions 2
conditions. At the same time, the ABBA design helped to counter- and 3 of experiment 1). In condition 5 the user was controlling the set-
balance the order effect occurring after switching from the “no control” point of the lighting system. The set-point defined the desk illuminance
to the “control” condition by reversing the order back to the “no the system was maintaining by regulating a corresponding luminaire's

Fig. 3. The order of the experimental conditions in experiment 1.

4
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

Fig. 4. The order of experimental conditions in experiment 2.

output in response to available daylight. Each condition of experiment Fig. 5.


2 consisted of 4 weeks, from which the first week was meant for the The degree of conflict was evaluated on a 7-point scale from 1- no
participants to adjust to the new study condition. The data was ana- conflict at all to 7 – very high conflict. The degree of the experienced
lyzed for the subsequent 3 weeks of each condition. conflict was assessed by the participants in experiment 1 with a mean of
During the conditions of the study short and extended surveys were 1.71 in Condition 2 and with a mean of 1.50 in Condition 3. In experi-
administered to collect subjective responses of the participants. Short ment 2 the respective mean values for the degree of conflict were 1.50
surveys were completed on a weekly basis and included questions as- for Condition 3, 1.36 for Condition 4 and 1.29 for Condition 5. The mean
sessing perceived lighting quantity, quality and glare. Extended surveys degree of conflict per participant in both experiment 1 and 2 is shown
in addition to the short surveys questions included extra questions. In in Fig. 6.
“control” conditions, extra questions assessed the perceived level of Some participants did indicate they tended to avoid using the con-
control and the level and degree of conflict due to the use of the lighting trols- 2 out of 14 in Condition 2 and 3 out of 14 participants in Condition
controls. The explanation of the questions used in the surveys can be 3 (experiment 1). The two participants who indicated to avoid using
found in Ref. [7]. controls in both condition 2 and 3 where the same people. Participants
had different reasons for this avoidance:

3. Results
• Fear of upsetting other occupants
3.1. Frequency and degree of conflict experienced • Changing a light level when others were present almost always led
to remarks (though rarely to complaints)

To explore the nature of conflict experienced between the partici- • The maximum light output maintained in the zone was the preferred
setting
pants during the study, the data on the frequency of conflict and the
degree of experienced conflict due to lighting controls with other users • Once the level was set it was ok and people did not complain
of the space, was collected via extended surveys during the “control”
conditions. The frequency of conflict was evaluated by the participants 3.2. Conflict avoidance
on a 7-point scale from 1 – never to 7- frequently. In experiment 1, the
users assessed the frequency of conflict with a mean of 2.64 in Condition When personal controls in the open office were introduced to offer
2 and with a mean of 2.0 in Condition 3. The respective mean values for the participants lighting control in a multi-user setting, it was hy-
the frequency of conflict in experiment 2 were 2.14 in Condition 3 and pothesized that the participants would discuss their preferences in an
1.64 in both condition 4 and condition 5. The means of the frequency of open way with their neighbors. It was expected that the participants
conflict for each participant for both experiment 1 and 2 are shown in would agree on a light level that everybody would find acceptable,

Fig. 5. Mean frequency of conflict in experiment 1 (left) and experiment 2 (right) from Ref. [26].

5
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

Fig. 6. Mean degree of conflict in experiment 1 (left) and experiment 2 (right) from Ref. [26].

within their control group, in view of potential individual preferences


differences. After the first series of interviews, it was discovered that the
participants never discussed their preferences with their neighbors. The
same response was given by the participants in all subsequent inter-
views of both experiment 1 and 2 during the “control” conditions. More
surprisingly, most participants knew well what their neighbor's pre-
ferences were. The picture that was revealed to us describes various
strategies people deployed to do their best in order to avoid conflict
with their neighbors.
The analysis of the face-to-face interviews revealed patterns of be-
havior that demonstrated various implicit strategies to avoid conflict
with neighbors. A number of participants shared to mainly make light
changes when their neighbors were not around, like early in the
morning when their neighbors did not yet arrive. This is also reflected
in the logged user interface data of experiment 1, showing the dis-
Fig. 8. Distribution of the number of changes throughout the day in conditions
tribution of the number of changes throughout the day in conditions 2
3, 4 and 5 (experiment 2).
and 3 (Fig. 7). As shown in Fig. 7, nearly half of the total changes were
made early in the morning before 9:00 a.m. In experiment 2 most
changes occurred between 9:00 a.m. and 12:00 a.m. (Fig. 8). At other or the neighboring zones were away. The participants shared that when
moments throughout the day the participants indicated to prefer their neighbors would arrive at their desk they would often not notice a
making changes when other neighbors sitting in the same control zone change.
Another strategy described by participants was to change the light
level by moving the slider relatively slow or in multiple small steps, so
that it would not be noticed by the neighbors present in the office. After
the controllers were introduced, the participants experimented more
extensively with the controllers. The number of control actions was
initially relatively high and decreased from slightly more than 4
changes per user per day during the first week of experiment 1 to
around one change, and stabilized at that level for subsequent weeks as
shown in (Fig. 9). During the phase of getting familiar with the con-
trollers, participants noticed that moving the slider relatively fast led to
their neighbors noticing the change, which was accompanied with
neutral remarks like, ‘Something happened to the lighting.’ On the
contrary, when changes were made by moving a slider relatively slow,
nobody made any remarks.

3.3. Incidence of conflict


Fig. 7. Distribution of the number of changes throughout the day (experiment
1). In the current paper the submissive and dominant behaviors of the

6
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

Fig. 9. Daily number of control actions per user (experiment 1).

users are discussed and were derived based on the survey data and face- users said that the ideal way of interacting with the lighting system
to-face interviews. In Ref. [26] data analysis techniques are described would be to communicate the personal light level preference initially to
based on sensor data and user actions logs that enabled exploration of the system, where after the system should maintain this light level
submissive and dominant behaviors using objective data sources. automatically. Several other participants indicated that they expected
In both experiment 1 and 2 neighboring participants with con- limited required interaction with a lighting system. These users ex-
flicting light level preferences were identified. This happened in the pected a certain level of intelligence from the system that would enable
control zone 3 in the experiment 1 and in the control zones 1 and 3 in the system to get some limited user input to learn user's preferences and
experiment 2 in both experiments having 6 control zones in total then maintain the preferred lighting conditions for the user with only
(Fig. 2). The classification of the zones for both experiments based on sporadic corrections from the user if needed. The further explored in-
objective and subjective measurements is given in Ref. [26]. In ex- telligent semi-automatic form of deriving user preferences by applying
periment 1, zone 3 included 3 users, 1 of those (user ID 4) preferred a machine learning is published separately in Ref. [26] and was also
relatively higher light level whereas 2 others preferred a lower one. The explored in Ref. [27].
participant with a higher light level preference expressed in the surveys Control actions were not always made to cater for own interests.
to be unsatisfied with the light quantity and quality. This participant Behaviors were observed where participants, for example, changed the
also expressed a preference for an office without lighting controls when light level in the afternoon because their neighbors preferred more light
asked at the end of the study to make a choice between an office with in the afternoon to compensate for the afternoon slump (experiment 1,
“control” or without. Although unsatisfied with the lighting condition, zones 5 and 6). In yet another example, a participant (experiment 2,
this participant did not discuss the situation with the neighbors from user ID 14) said when other neighbors in the control zone would be
the same zone and chose not to change the light level in the control present, the participant would deliberately change the light level to a
zone when set by other zone users. medium slider position. This strategy was explained as a way to respect
In experiment 2, a similar behavior was observed in zone 1, ac- other people's preferences by avoiding selecting any of the two ex-
commodating two users. One of the users (user ID 0) shared through the tremes.
surveys to be dissatisfied with the light quantity and quality survey. In
the interviews that user shared that his zone neighbor (user ID 1) 3.4. Preference for “control” or “no control”
preferred a lower light level than his personal preference. After the
controllers were introduced, his zone neighbor had set his preferred After having experienced “control” and “no control” conditions, the
light level, accompanied by the statement - ‘This is the way I like the participants were asked to make a hypothetical choice between having
light level to be.’ Aware of the conflicting preferences, the more sub- a desk in an office with controls as experienced during the study or a
missive user did not use the light controller to adjust the lighting for the desk in an office without controls. In experiment 1, one user (user ID 4)
remainder of the study, despite of his own dissatisfaction with the zone opted for an office without controls whereas in experiment 2 three users
lighting. (user IDs 5, 11 and 14) opted for “no control” (Fig. 10). In both studies,
These two cases demonstrate a situation in which users have rather users who opted for a “no control” office were either users who de-
different light level preferences compared to their neighbors. To avoid monstrated a submissive way of dealing with opposing lighting pre-
conflict, the dissatisfied users chose neither to change the light level nor ferences or users that were engaged in an implicit conflict.
to discuss the situation with their neighbors. This behavior was labelled
as a submissive behavior since the dissatisfied users submit their own
3.5. Loss aversion
comfort to the will of their neighbors neglecting their own needs.
In experiment 2, two users within zone 3 (user IDs 5 and 6) had
Participants were asked once a week to evaluate the light quantity
opposite light level preferences did not get involved in a submissive
on their desk, screen, and due to daylight. The assessments of light
behavior. Being aware of the rather different light level preferences, the
quantity were converted into a dissatisfaction scale, where the extremes
users chose to set their preferred light level when their neighbor was
of too little (1) and too much (7) are translated into dissatisfied (3), the
away from the office. Both users were aware of the situation and de-
just right (4) is translated into satisfied (0), and the steps in between
scribed it using similar details, but surprisingly, even these users chose
likewise. The descriptive statistics of the “no control” (Cnc) and the
not to discuss the situation openly with each other but were engaged in
“control” (Cuc) dissatisfaction data of experiment 1 is shown in boxplots
an implicit conflict. These two users were using their controllers more
in Fig. 11.
frequently than what was typical for users of other zones during both
Statistical analysis using the non-parametric Wilcoxon Signed Ranks
studies, and they both felt they were forced to use the controllers more
test shows a significantly lower dissatisfaction with light quantity on
often than they would otherwise prefer. In an interview one of these
the desk (p = 0.029) as well as on the screen (p = 0.047) in the user

7
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

condition, raised substantially higher compared to condition 3 demon-


strating a significant difference with both conditions 2 and 3 for quantity
of light on the desk. As such a larger effect on the dissatisfaction with
the lighting quantity on the desk was observed when controls were
removed than when they were introduced.
In experiment 2, the dissatisfaction with light quantity and quality
was analyzed by aggregating data for 2 “no control” conditions and 3
“control” conditions. Due to explorative nature of the study, α = 0.1
was used and the data showed a significant effect indicating a lower
light quantity dissatisfaction on the desk (Wilcoxon signed rank test
with p = 0.092) and higher light quality assessment (p = 0.096) in the
“control” conditions. It is important to notice that in experiment 2 no
measurements were conducted after the controllers were removed. As
such there was no effect of loss aversion to increase the dissatisfaction
in the “no control” conditions and yet again the data showed a similar
Fig. 10. A choice for "no control" versus "control" in a) experiment 1 and b) trend as in experiment 1.
experiment 2.
The participants gave a median score of 4 on a 7-point scale to the
importance of controlling the lighting over their desk before they had
controls in experiment 1. The importance increased to 6 after the par-
ticipants experienced the controls and it stayed at 6 in both conditions 2
and 3 (Fig. 12). In the last “no control” condition 4, the importance of
having control scored higher than in the initial “no control” condition 1.
As one participant commented in an interview: “Before I did not know
what difference it would make to have controls. After I have tried them,
I do not want to be without any more”.

4. Discussion

4.1. Conflict avoidance

Although previous research shows that some users avoid using


controls out of conflict avoidance considerations, this study revealed
that even dominant individuals avoid open discussion of preferences
with their neighbors and do not proactively search consensus. A sur-
prising outcome of the study is that none of the 28 participants openly
discussed their preferences with neighbors to find a mutually accep-
table light level.
Fig. 11. Boxplots of dissatisfaction with light quantity in “no control” and
The study explored what strategies users deployed to avoid conflict.
“control” conditions, ranging from 0-satisfied to 3-dissatisfied.
One strategy observed was making a change in absence of neighbors.
Another strategy was making a change by moving the slider slowly or
Table 1 using small incremental steps, so others would not notice the light
Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test results for "control" versus "no control". output change.
Dissatisfaction with quantity of light (0 = satisfaction, In several cases, strategies were proactively deployed by users to
3 = dissatisfaction) remain on good terms with their neighbors, like selecting a less extreme
dimming level when others would arrive or setting the level their
On desk On screen Daylight
neighbors preferred in the afternoon.
Z −2.179(a) −1.988(a) −1.246(a) Interestingly, similar strategies were observed with respect to
Asymp. Sig. (2- 0.029 0.047 0.213 dealing with daylight by means of blinds control. Individual daylight
tailed) level sensitivities and preferences showed to be diverse, resulting in a
variety of, sometimes conflicting, daylight requirements. Participants
a. Based on positive ranks.
Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test. who were less sensitive to high daylight levels were less demanding for
blinds to be completely closed on bright, sunny days. Some of them
“control” situations (Table 1). even had an explicit desire for blinds to be completely open at the lower
Looking at the 4 conditions of experiment 1 separately, as explained end to allow for an outside view. Other participants indicated experi-
in Section 2.7 and illustrated in Fig. 3, the descriptive statistics for the encing a quite severe disturbance from daylight. For these participants
lighting quantity dissatisfaction data is given in Table 2 including the blinds position was often not ideal due to other office users who
median scores and standard deviation (SD). preferred the blinds not being completely closed. Some also indicated
Table 3 gives the results of the Wilcoxon Signed Ranks significance that they did not want to completely close the blinds, to make sure their
testing for 4-subconditions. The data shows a small difference in the colleagues would not sit “in the dark”.
median scores between condition 1 (“no control”) and condition 2 During the study the external blinds were monitored via a webcam
(“control”) and this difference showed to be insignificant. The second installed in a building facing the building façade where the test bed was
“control” condition 3 did show a significant difference with the first “no running. The webcam images revealed that the blinds had a tendency to
control” condition 1 for quantity of light on the desk. It can be noticed be closed more on sunny days and to be opened more on cloudy days.
that the mean dissatisfaction score in condition 4, second “no control”

8
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

Table 2
Median dissatisfaction scores with quantity of light for the 4 sub-conditions.
Dissatisfaction with quantity of light (0 = satisfaction, 3 = dissatisfaction)

On desk On screen Daylight

Condition 1 (n = 47) 0.49 (SD = 0.688) 0.43 (SD = 0.617) 1.09 (SD = 1.080)
Condition 2 (n = 42) 0.43 (SD = 0.630) 0.38 (SD = 0.661) 0.90 (SD = 1.055)
Condition 3 (n = 41) 0.24 (SD = 0.489) 0.27 (SD = 0.549) 0.76 (SD = 0.994)
Condition 4 (n = 41) 1.10 (SD = 1.068) 0.71 (SD = 0.844) 0.76 (SD = 0.943)

Table 3 swap a desk in case of a conflict. The use of lighting controls in a flex
Wilcoxon Signed Ranks Test results for the 4 sub-conditions. desk needs further exploration.
Comparison Dissatisfaction with quantity of light (0 = satisfaction,
It was observed that even in cases in which individuals did experi-
between 3 = dissatisfaction) ence conflict (even relatively frequently) the degree of experienced
conflict was hardly ever above average. There are two possible ex-
On desk On screen Daylight planations to that. The test office chosen for this study faced southward
Conditions 1-2 Z −0.237b −0.773b −0.847b
such that daylight often exceeded the contribution of artificial lighting.
Sig. 0.812 0.440 0.397 In view of sufficient daylight and other environmental factors, dis-
Conditions 1-3 Z −2.240b −1.583b −1.561b satisfaction with the light level possibly was not critical enough to cause
Sig. 0.025* 0.113 0.118 a high degree of conflict. Another possibility is that the participants
Conditions 1-4 Z −1.260c −0.510c −1.513b
were giving a more socially desirable response to the degree of conflict
Sig. 0.208 0.610 0.130
Conditions 2-3 Z −2.271b −1.089b −0.855b question. This possibility is supported by results showing that the
Sig. 0.023* 0.276 0.393 participants who experienced a conflict situation tended not to opt for
Conditions 2-4 Z −1.917c −1.635c −1.282b an office with controls. This aspect could be further explored by de-
Sig. 0.055 0.102 0.200 ploying objective measurements rather than subjective.
Conditions 3-4 Z −2.505c −1.807c −0.183b
Sig. 0.012* 0.071 0.855

4.3. Loss aversion

Experiment 1 allowed to make a comparison between the subjective


ratings observed after the controllers were introduced and after the
controllers were removed. The results show a much smaller change in
the lighting quantity satisfaction scores observed after the controllers
were given to the participants than after they were removed from the
desks. This effect resembles what is known in behavioral economics as a
loss aversion. Loss aversion demonstrates the human propensity to ex-
perience a loss psychologically in a much more powerful way than a
gain. In other words, it looks like after the participants got the con-
trollers initially it mattered to them much less than when the controllers
were removed at the end of experiment 1. The differences between the
ratings given to the importance to control lighting in conditions 1 and 4
of experiment 1, support this observation. At the same time, in ex-
periment 2, no measurements were conducted after the controllers were
removed. Despite that fact, the data of experiment 2 also shows a higher
dissatisfaction with the lighting quantity and quality in “no control”
compared to when the participants did have “control”. The responses to
the question which office the participants would prefer also showed the
Fig. 12. Ratings of the importance to control lighting. majority preferring lighting controls in both experiments.

4.2. Incidence of conflict 5. Conclusions

By means of in-depth interviews and surveys it was explored in 5.1. Conflict avoidance
which situations conflicts did occur among the neighbors. The study
revealed the chosen behaviors of study participants they applied to the The study shows that most people, including dominant individuals,
control of light in view of the preference differences: the submissive are very conscious of a potential conflict with neighbors and use dif-
behavior and the implicit conflict behavior. ferent strategies to avoid conflict. This finding has implications for the
As mentioned in 2.2 the two samples of participants of experiments design of multi-user control solutions in buildings. After the user in-
1 and 2 were different. The participants of experiment 1 were more itiates an environmental change with a user interface, a system re-
familiar with each other than the participants of experiment 2. This sponse that leads to rapid, clearly noticeable parameter change is not
could be one of the reasons why several more cases of conflict were appreciated due to its disruptive effect on other users of the space.
observed in experiment 2. As a consequence, more participants opted Based on the study data, choices that enable users to make environ-
against controls and the participants were less active using the con- mental changes in a way that is hard to notice by others would promote
trollers than in experiment 1. the use of controls. A suggested strategy is to introduce a mode on the
The participants of the study were using individually assigned UI in which a user action would always lead to a gradual change of
desks. Flex unassigned desks would offer more flexibility to possibly light, to make it barely noticeable to other users of the space.

9
T. Lashina et al. Building and Environment 148 (2019) 1–10

5.2. Loss aversion environment and control systems in daylit offices: a literature review, Energy Build.
38 (2006) 728–742, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.enbuild.2006.03.001.
[5] A.D. Galasiu, G.R. Newsham, Energy savings due to occupancy sensors and personal
The observations made in experiment 1 showed a stronger effect on controls: a pilot field study, 11th Eur. Light. Conf. (Lux Eur. 2009, pp. 745–752.
dissatisfaction with lighting quality after the controllers were removed [6] G.R. Newsham, Research matters – comparing individual dimming control to other
compared to when they were introduced. This observation is discussed control options in offices, NRCC 8 (2007) 50090 http://irc.nrc-cnrc.gc.ca.
[7] S. Chraibi, T. Lashina, P. Shrubsole, M. Aries, E. van Loenen, A. Rosemann,
in Section 4.3 as an instance of the loss aversion phenomenon. The Satisfying light conditions: a field study on perception of consensus light in Dutch
study data supports previous findings indicating that even in multi-user open office environments, Build. Environ. 105 (2016) 116–127, https://doi.org/10.
office spaces the majority of users perceive lighting personal control in 1016/j.buildenv.2016.05.032.
[8] J.A. Veitch, G.R. Newsham, Exercised control, lighting choices, and energy use: an
a beneficial way. The loss aversion effect observed suggests, however, office simulation experiment, J. Environ. Psychol. 20 (2000) 219–237, https://doi.
that the nuisance of having no control over lighting will be felt stronger org/10.1006/jevp.1999.0169.
after people lose it than the satisfaction gains when people first get it. In [9] P.R. Tregenza, S. romaya, D. S, H. L, T. B, Consistency and variation in preferences
for office lighting, Light. Res. Technol. 6 (1924) 205–211.
this respect, a personal control rather belongs to a category of hygiene
[10] L. Halonen, J. Lehtovaara, Need of individual control to improve daylight utiliza-
factors, than a ‘wow’ effect creator. tion and user's satisfaction in integrated lighting systems, Publ. Int. L Eclair. Cie,
New Delhi, India, 1995, pp. 200–203.
5.3. Overall conclusions [11] P.R. Boyce, N.H. Eklund, N.S. Simpson, Individual lighting control: task perfor-
mance, mood, and illuminance, J. Illum. Eng. Soc. (2000) 131–142.
[12] G.R. Newsham, J.A. Veitch, C. Arsenault, C. Duval, Effect of dimming control on
In multi-user spaces, people are generally self-conscious of the office worker satisfaction and performance, Proc. IESNA Annu. Conf. (2004) 19–41
presence of other individuals. System design needs to facilitate making doi:10.1.1.200.4892.
[13] P.R. Boyce, J.A. Veitch, G.R. Newsham, C.C. Jones, J. Heerwagen, M. Myer,
environmental changes in a manner that is least disruptive to other C.M. Hunter, Lighting quality and office work: two field simulation experiments,
space users. Although offering control enhances satisfaction and the Light. Res. Technol. 38 (2006) 191–223, https://doi.org/10.1191/
majority of users prefer control over no control, there is always a risk of 1365782806lrt161oa.
[14] J.A. Veitch, G.R. Newsham, P.R. Boyce, C.C. Jones, Lighting appraisal, well-being
creating conflict due to opposing preferences. Solutions that help re- and performance in open-plan offices: a linked mechanisms approach, Light. Res.
solve conflict in multi-user context need to be further explored. By Technol. 40 (2008) 133–151.
demonstrating the loss aversion effect, it is shown that despite all [15] J.A. Veitch, C.L. Donnelly, A.D. Galasiu, G.R. Newsham, D.M. Sander,
C.D. Arsenault, Office Occupants' Evaluations of an Individually-controllable
benefits personal control is rather a hygiene factor. In order to bring the Lighting System, (2010), https://doi.org/10.4224/20374060.
benefits of personal control to office users its application needs support [16] P.R. Boyce, J.A. Veitch, G.R. Newsham, C.C. Jones, J. Heerwagen, M. Myer,
from certification and regulation bodies like WELL, since personal C.M. Hunter, L. Bedocs, Occupant use of switching and dimming controls in offices,
Light. Res. Technol. 38 (2006) 358–378, https://doi.org/10.1177/
control would have hard time promoting itself.
1477153506070994.
[17] Gensler, Workplace Survey United States - a Design and Performance Report,
Declarations of interest (2008).
[18] T. Moore, D.J. Carter, A. Slater, A study of opinion in offices with and without user
controlled lighting, Light. Res. Technol. 36 (2004) 131–146, https://doi.org/10.
None. 1191/13657828041i109oa.
[19] T. Moore, D.J. Carter, A.I. Slater, A qualitative study of occupant controlled office
Acknowledgements lighting, Light. Res. Technol. 35 (2003) 297–317, https://doi.org/10.1191/
1365782802lt047oa.
[20] T. Moore, Long-term patterns of use of occupant controlled office lighting, Light.
This research was performed within the framework of the strategic Res. Technol. 1 (2003) 43–59, https://doi.org/10.1191/1477153503li061oa.
joint research program on Intelligent Lighting between TU/e and [21] T. Moore, D.J. Carter, A.I. Slater, Conflict and Control : the use of locally ad-
dressable lighting in open plan office space, Proc. Chart. Inst. Buidling Serv. Eng.
Philips. The team thanks Sjoerd Mentink, Jos van Haaren, Greg Nelson (2000).
and Ben Versantvoort for support and making this research possible. [22] T. Moore, D.J. Carter, A.I. Slater, A field study of occupant controlled lighting in
offices, Light. Res. Technol. 34 (2002) 191–205.
[23] A. Marmot, J. Eley, Office Space Planning Designing for Tomorrow's Workplace,
References
McGraw Hill, 2000.
[24] P.M. Bluyssena, M.B.C. Aries, van P. Dommelen, Comfort of workers in office
[1] T. Moore, D.J. Carter, A.I. Slater, User attitudes toward occupant controlled office buildings: the European HOPE project, Build. Environ. 46 (2011) 280–288.
lighting, Light. Res. Technol. 34 (2002) 207–219, https://doi.org/10.1191/ [25] J.A. Veitch, Psychological processes influencing lighting quality, J. Illum. Eng. Soc.
1365782802lt048oa. 30 (2001) 124–140, https://doi.org/10.1080/00994480.2001.10748341.
[2] W. O'Brien, H.B. Gunay, The contextual factors contributing to occupants' adaptive [26] M. Despenic, S. Chraibi, T. Lashina, A. Rosemann, Lighting preference profiles of
comfort behaviors in offices - a review and proposed modeling framework, Build. users in an open office environment, Build. Environ. 116 (2017) 89–107, https://
Environ. 77 (2014) 77–88, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2014.03.024. doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2017.01.033.
[3] S. Plous, The Psychology of Judgment and Decision Making, Mcgraw-Hill Book [27] H.B. Gunay, W. O'Brien, I. Beausoleil-Morrison, S. Gilani, Development and im-
Company, 1993. plementation of an adaptive lighting and blinds control algorithm, Build. Environ.
[4] A.D. Galasiu, J.A. Veitch, Occupant preferences and satisfaction with the luminous 113 (2017) 185–199, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.buildenv.2016.08.027.

10

Potrebbero piacerti anche