Sei sulla pagina 1di 2

Current Biology

Magazine

Correspondence acoustic domain, amplitude, have been Piha


Bell Type 1
relatively overlooked, and little is known Bell Type 2

Extremely loud about their potential impact on sexual


selection and mate assessment [7].
0 5 10 15
Sound Pressure (Pa)
20

mating songs at

Duration (s)
1.3
The few studies published on sexual 1.2
1.1
selection and song amplitude have 1.0
close range in white focused on species with moderate
0.9
0.8
0.7
0 5 10 15 20
bellbirds song amplitudes, in the range of
70–90 dB SPL [8]. It is reasonable to
0.95

expect, however, that constraints on 0.90

Duration (s)
Jeffrey Podos1,* and Mario Cohn-Haft2 amplitude will be more evident, and
0.85
associated impacts on signal usage
Sexual selection in many animal and assessment more likely, in species 0.80
species favors the evolution of with exceptionally loud songs.
0.75
elaborate courtship traits. Such traits Cotingas and their close relatives, 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24
might help signalers convey, and the manakins, are renowned for Maximal Sound Pressure (Pa)
receivers discern, information about intense sexual selection, lek polygyny
signaler quality; or they might be and the evolution of elaborate
favored by perceptual or aesthetic courtship ornaments and displays.
preferences for elaborateness or Recent studies in manakins have
beauty [1–3]. Under either scenario we characterized high-performance
expect sexual trait elaboration to be components in males’ mating dances
countered by proximate constraints and mechanical sounds, showing
rooted in animals’ morphology, how these components are enabled
physiology and phylogenetic history by morphological and physiological
[3,4]. During expeditions to a montane specializations [4]. As for vocal
rainforest in the Brazilian Amazon, amplitude, while screaming pihas are Figure 1. Sound pressure levels of piha and
bellbird songs, as they vary by song type
we obtained amplitude-calibrated known to be exceptionally loud [5],
and in relation to song duration.
measures of mating songs in two male bellbirds (Procnias spp.) have Top panel: Average (grey) and maximal (white)
species of cotingas, the white bellbird been said to sing “what are probably sound pressure levels, converted from noise-
(Procnias albus) and the screaming the loudest of all bird calls” [9]. Yet and 1 m distance-corrected Leq dB(A) (re. 20
piha (Lipaugus vociferans). The as far as we know, bellbird song µPa) values, for a sample of the three song
screaming piha sings the loudest amplitudes have not been previously types analyzed. Means, ranges and quartiles
are shown. Screaming piha and white bellbird
songs of any passerine bird previously quantified.
Type 1 songs overlap in maximal sound pres-
documented [5]. However, we find that We tracked, observed and recorded sure, yet of the two, bellbird Type 1 songs
white bellbirds are >9 dB louder, and birds with a calibrated sound level achieve substantially higher average sound
thus achieve roughly triple the sound meter that samples amplitude values pressures, reflecting these songs’ explosive
pressure levels of pihas. Mechanical 50 times per second, suitable for amplitude onsets and sustained high amplitude
constraints on amplitude, and thus capturing rapid vocal amplitude levels (see Figure S1, lower panels). Both meas-
ures are even higher in bellbird Type 2 songs,
limits on the reach of sexual selection, modulations. Our analyses focused
non-overlapping with the other two types, with
are revealed by trade-offs between on two amplitude parameters: Leq maximal sound pressure levels more than three
maximal sound pressure and song (equivalent continuous sound level), times greater than in pihas. Second panel:
duration. We find that song amplitude a root-mean-squared average value, Song duration plotted against maximal sound
in bellbirds is context-dependent: and Lpeak, which captures maximal pressure for the three song types (piha songs
when a female was on the display transient amplitude values. Screaming = orange squares, bellbird Type 1 songs = blue
circles, bellbird Type 2 songs = brown trian-
perch, a male bellbird sang only his pihas across our site sang a single
gles). Durations in the bellbird Type 2 songs
louder song type, swiveling his body shared song type, while white are substantially briefer than for the other two
mid-song to face the female head bellbirds sang two shared song types, types, consistent with a potential respiratory
on. We know of no other species in one relatively common (Type 1) and constraint on song production. Third panel: The
which such high-amplitude vocal the other relatively rare (Type 2; ~one same data for the bellbird Type 2 songs only,
signals are directed to receivers in in every six songs; Figure S1, plotted on a per-individual basis (differenti-
ated by color/shape). As with the between-type
such close proximity. We propose that Video S1). For each song analyzed
data, three of the four birds individually exhibit
bellbird females balance an interest we constructed calibrated amplitude decreasing duration with increasing amplitude.
in sampling males at close range with profiles, corrected all amplitude The one bird that does not show this trade-off
a need to protect themselves from profiles to account for background sings with lower overall performance, i.e. with
hearing damage. noise, and then extrapolated lower maximal sound pressures and shorter
Birds are known to encounter amplitude values to those expected durations. This exception is consistent with
the supposition that trade-offs tend to occur
performance constraints when singing 1 m from the source, by applying
only when display phenotypes are pushed to
songs with rapid modulations in a function that accounts for sound maximal values [6]. Bottom panel: male white
timing and frequency [6]. By contrast, energy dissipation via spherical bellbird singing a Type 2 song. Photo: Anselmo
performance constraints in a third spreading. d’Affonseca.

R1068 Current Biology 29, R1055–R1069, October 21, 2019 © 2019 Elsevier Ltd.
Current Biology

Magazine

Screaming pihas in our sample spherical spreading (Figure S2). This ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
achieved maximal vocal amplitudes outcome reveals a strong influence
This research was supported by a Fulbright
roughly equivalent to values reported of distance, and accordingly a lesser
fellowship to J.P., with M.C.-H. as project
previously [5]: 106.9 ± 0.6 (mean role for other factors, in determining
sponsor in Brazil. For their contributions to
± SD) dB(A) Leq, and 116.1 ± 0.6 signal amplitudes available to cotinga
bird sightings, observations and photography
dB(A) Lpeak, n = 3 birds, all dB values receivers. The reliability of amplitude we thank Arthur Gomes, Ramiro Melinski,
re. 20 µPa. White bellbirds were as a proxy for distance highlights Maristele Oliveira, Marina Maximiano, Anaís
somewhat louder than pihas for their these signals’ potential utility to Prestes, Else Schlerman, Vitor Piacentini,
Type 1 songs (108.9 ± 2.3 dB(A) Leq cotingas for long-distance signaling Luciano Naka, Thiago Laranjeiras, Gabriel
and 116.7 ± 1.0 dB(A) Lpeak, n = 8 and localization across their leks [7,9]. Leite, and Anselmo d’Affonseca. Logistical
birds), and definitively louder for their On several occasions we observed support for both field and laboratory
Type 2 songs (116.6 ± 3.6 dB(A) Leq female white bellbirds joining males on components of the project was provided
and 125.4 ± 1.7 dB(A) Lpeak, n = 7 their display perches [9]. In this context, by the Instituto Nacional de Pesquisas da
birds). This latter song type is, as far the male first adopted a head-down/ Amazônia (INPA). Sue Anne Zollinger, Henrik
as we know, the highest amplitude tail-down posture, back towards the Brumm, Rindy Anderson, Katie Schroeder,
vocalization yet documented for any female, wattle fully distended. He then Amy Strauss, Laura Hancock and three
bird, with maximal sound pressure sang only his higher amplitude (Type 2) anonymous reviewers provided helpful
levels exceeding those in the song, swiveling dramatically mid- feedback on earlier manuscript versions.
screaming piha by about three-fold song to face the female head-on for Research conducted under UMass IACUC
(Figure 1, upper panel). the song’s second note (see also [9]). protocol 2018-0095.
The extreme nature of the vocal Females in these interactions always
display performances documented retreated as or just before the male AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
here imply that pihas and especially sang, yet still experienced songs at
bellbirds sing at or near their maximal very close range, sometimes within four Both authors contributed equally to the field
performance capacities. If so, we meters or less. work and to writing the manuscript. J.P.
might expect to observe trade-offs It is curious that one of the world’s conducted the analyses and prepared the
among display attributes as they loudest birds sings only its highest- figures.
approach their upper values [4,6]. amplitude song type in such close-
We here identify one such trade- range communication. Animals REFERENCES
off: maximal amplitude is seen to normally reserve loud calls for
vary negatively with song duration, communication over long distances, 1. Prum, R.O. (2017). The Evolution of Beauty:
both across song types (Figure 1, and some species are known to How Darwin’s Forgotten Theory of Mate Choice
Shapes the Animal World - and Us (Doubleday
middle panel) and individual bellbirds’ vocalize more softly when receivers are Press).
renditions of song Type 2 (Figure 1, nearby [7]. It is also unclear why female 2. Ryan, M.J. (2018). A Taste for the Beautiful: The
Evolution of Attraction (Princeton NJ: Princeton
lower panel). We hypothesize that as white bellbirds willingly stay so close University Press).
birds up-regulate respiratory air flow to males as they sing. At four meters, 3. Byers, J., Hebets, E., Podos, J. (2010).
to elevate song amplitude [10], they females would experience potentially Female mate choice based upon male motor
performance. Anim. Behav. 2010, 771–778.
deplete their respiratory tidal volumes damaging effective dB values at the 4. Miles, M.C., Goller, F., Fuxjager, M.J. (2018).
more quickly, leading to shorter- ear, of ~104 dB(A) Leq and ~113 Physiological constraint on acrobatic courtship
behavior underlies rapid sympatric speciation in
duration songs. This constraint could dB(A) Lpeak. Presumably these risks bearded manakins. Elife 7, e40630.
presumably limit further evolutionary are offset by benefits females gain in 5. Nemeth, E. (2004). Measuring the sound
elaboration in amplitude even in the assessing prospective mates. More pressure level of the song of the Screaming
Piha, Lipaugus vociferans: One of the loudest
face of strong and persistent sexual specifically, as they move around birds in the world? Bioacoustics 14, 225–228.
selection. display perches during courtship 6. Podos, J., Lahti, D.C. and Moseley, D.L. (2009).
Vocal performance and sensorimotor learning in
The effective or realized [9], female bellbirds might actively songbirds. Adv. Study Behav. 40, 159–195
amplitude of any vocal signal, that balance an interest in assessing males 7. Brumm, H., Naguib, M. (2009). Environmental
is the amplitude experienced by the at close range while trying to limit acoustics and the evolution of bird song. Adv.
Study Behav. 40, 1–33.
receiver, is influenced not just by hearing damage. Overall, the findings 8. Nemeth, E., Kempenaers, B., Matessi, G.,
signaler performance but also by how we present for white bellbirds well Brumm, H. (2012). Rock sparrow song reflects
male age and reproductive success. PLoS One
the signal propagates through the illustrate sexual selection’s power to 7, 9.
environment. Some factors that can drive evolution not just in displays 9. Snow, D. (1982). The Cotingas (Oxford
influence acoustic signal propagation themselves, but also in neural and University Press).
10. Suthers, R.A, Goller, F., Pytte, C. (1999). The
include distance to receivers, the behavioral mechanisms that govern neuromuscular control of birdsong. Philos.
structure and density of intervening display assessment. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B. 354, 927–939.
vegetation, abiotic noise such as from
wind or rain, and sound interference SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION 1
from other animals [7]. For the three University of Massachusetts, Department of
Biology, 611 North Pleasant St, Amherst, MA
song types in our sample we find Supplemental Information including 01003 USA. 2Instituto Nacional de Pesquisas
that amplitudes declined precisely experimental procedures, two figures and one da Amazônia (INPA), Coleções Zoológicas,
and steadily with distance, in tight video can be found with this article online at Av. André Araújo 2936, Manaus 69067 Brazil.
concordance with the prediction of https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2019.09.028. *E-mail: jpodos@bio.umass.edu

Current Biology 29, R1055–R1069, October 21, 2019 R1069

Potrebbero piacerti anche