Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Teaching and Teacher Education


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/tate

Emotional experience, expression, and regulation of high-quality


Japanese elementary school teachers
Rika Hosotani*, Kyoko Imai-Matsumura
The Joint Graduate School in Science of School Education, Hyogo University of Teacher Education, 942-1 Shimokume, Kato, Hyogo 673-1494, Japan

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: The present study investigates the emotional experience, expression, and regulation processes of high-
Received 11 June 2010 quality Japanese elementary school teachers while they interact with children, in terms of teachers’
Received in revised form emotional competence. Qualitative analysis of interview data demonstrated that teachers had various
24 March 2011
emotional experiences including self-elicited negative emotions. The major expression patterns were
Accepted 25 March 2011
identified as direct staging and the suppression of emotions. Teachers considered emotion expression in
front of children as a skill, and their emotion regulation processes involved considering various purposes,
Keywords:
appropriately using emotion expression, and ideal teacher images. The findings suggest that high-quality
Emotion regulation
Emotion expression
teachers effectively use emotional competence in teaching.
Emotional competence Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
Elementary school teacher
Emotional labor

1. Introduction will be enormously beneficial both for children’s development and


for teachers’ professional development.
1.1. Teacher’s emotional interaction
1.2. Teacher’s emotional experience and emotion expression
It is not enough for school teachers to have only academic
knowledge and good instruction skills. Research has indicated that Research on teachers’ emotions in teaching has attracted
teachers’ emotional interaction with children is also an important considerable attention recently (Hargreaves, 1998, 2000, 2001;
issue, since good emotional support from teachers affects the Isenbarger & Zembylas, 2006; Lasky, 2000; Nias, 1996; Schutz,
teacherechild relationship and plays a significant role in children’s Cross, Hong, & Osbon, 2007; Sutton, 2004; Sutton, Mudrey-
favorable adjustment to school (Hamre & Pianta, 2001, 2005; Camino, & Knight, 2009; Sutton & Wheatley, 2003; Zembylas,
Hamre, Pianta, Downer, & Mashburn, 2008; Mashburn et al., 2005a, 2005b). Indeed, teachers have various emotional experi-
2008; Turner et al., 1998; Turner, Meyer, Midgley, & Patrick, 2003; ences including positive and negative ones at school, as do children
Turner, Meyer, & Schweinle, 2003). Moreover, teachers’ quality (Hargreaves, 2001; Sutton & Wheatley, 2003). Then, how do good
emotional support plays a critical role for at-risk children who teachers emotionally interact with children? Good teaching is
display early social and/or academic problems in their school supposed to be charged with positive emotions (Hargreaves, 1998).
adjustment. For example, Hamre and Pianta (2005) found that at- However, teachers, as human beings, inevitably feel anger or frus-
risk children placed in first-grade classrooms benefited from strong tration while interacting with children. How will the teacher’s
emotional support in terms of achievement and teacherestudent emotional experience be dealt with while interacting with chil-
relationships. Thus, the quality of teachers’ emotional approach to dren? Emotions have motivational and regulatory functions and
children greatly influences children’s education. Therefore, an continually influence cognition and action (Izard, 1991; Izard, Stark,
understanding of how high-quality teachers interact with children Trentacosta, & Schultz, 2008). Are teachers’ own emotions an
impediment to teaching? Or do they use their emotions to advan-
tage while teaching? Some teachers may be able to deal effectively
with emotional arousal while others may not.
* Corresponding author. Tel./fax: þ81 795 44 2192.
Teaching is considered to entail a great deal of emotional labor
E-mail addresses: d08703h@hyogo-u.ac.jp (R. Hosotani), matumura@hyogo-u.ac.jp (Hargreaves, 1998, 2000; Isenbarger & Zembylas, 2006; Schutz
(K. Imai-Matsumura). et al., 2007). Emotional labor was defined in occupational

0742-051X/$ e see front matter Ó 2011 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.tate.2011.03.010
1040 R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048

psychology as “the effort, planning, and control needed to express interaction with children. (In this paper, we defined high-quality
organizationally desired emotion during interpersonal trans- teachers as teachers who have a certain level of teaching ability
actions” (Morris & Feldman, 1996, p. 987). Teachers may make including the ability of classroom management and academic
a great effort to express desired emotions as teachers. Are teachers’ instruction sufficient for supervising student teachers’ internships
emotion expressions associated with teaching? Teaching that is and conducting lessons that serve as models for other teachers.)
favorable for child development seems to take place in a classroom
whose climate is full of emotional warmth and child-centered 1.4. Current study
(Hamre & Pianta, 2005; Hamre et al., 2008) but do teachers in such
classrooms prevent their negative emotions such as anger or The first research question explored in this study is what high-
sadness from being expressed in their teaching? When a teacher quality Japanese elementary school teachers feel during interaction
tries to construct a caring community in a classroom as an approach with children. Internal emotional experiences provide individuals
to classroom management (Danforth & Boyle, 2006), does the with important information about their environment and situation,
teacher empower children with an emotionally neutral expression serving as a significant source of judgments and actions (Schwartz
or with a certain emotional expression? & Clore, 2003). Moreover, one can utilize emotions to facilitate
It is expected that the teacher’s way of expressing emotions may thinking and live adaptively (Izard et al., 2008; Salovey et al., 2008).
influence his or her manner of instruction in the class, classroom By investigating directly the emotional experience of high-quality
management, and handling of children’s misbehavior. Teacher’s teachers while interacting with children, we will be able to reveal
ability at emotion regulation and expression while interacting with their genuine emotional experiences which cannot be observed
children may have significance for teaching outcomes. In addition, from outside. Then, how their emotional experience is utilized in
because it is internationally acknowledged that teachers’ failure to their teaching, or how they exert emotional competence can be
manage children’s misbehavior can result in those teachers’ stress, considered.
and in extreme cases, burnout (Chan, 2006; Lewis, Romi, Qui, & The second research question of this study is what are high-
Katz, 2005), it is of great significance to investigate teachers’ quality Japanese teachers’ emotion expressions like in front of
emotion expression while interacting with children. children. Emotion expression toward others reflects the individ-
ual’s emotional competence (Saarni, 1999; Salovey et al., 2008).
1.3. Emotional competence Teachers’ emotion expression in front of children may be different
from the emotion that is actually experienced by the teacher
We try to formulate teachers’ emotional experience and because of emotion regulation processes. Moreover, high-quality
expression relating to teaching in terms of teachers’ emotional teachers may have developed strategies of emotion regulation for
competence. Mayer and Salovey defined emotional intelligence as teaching. Therefore, the present study investigates emotion
the ability to perceive and express emotions, to understand and use expression patterns of high-quality teachers and their inner
them, and to manage emotions, with an assumption that a person’s emotion regulation processes, by which expressional patterns
level of ability contributes substantially to the individual’s intel- should be produced, and then discusses the association between
lectual and emotional well-being and growth, and social relations high-quality teachers’ emotional competence and teaching.
(Mayer & Salovey, 1997; Salovey, Detweiler-Bedell, Detweiler- In one related study with American middle school teachers,
Bedell, & Mayer, 2008). Managing emotions means to monitor Sutton (2004) focused on the investigation of teachers’ emotion
and manage emotions in oneself and others to promote emotional regulation goals and strategies, mainly in terms of coping. We
and intellectual growth and well-being; it thus includes working considered emotion regulation from a rather broad emotional
on and regulating others’ emotions (Salovey et al., 2008). Saarni competence perspective, which includes managing one’s and
also conceptualized eight skills of emotional competence including others’ emotions, and focused on high-quality teachers’ emotion
the ability to differentiate inner emotional state and outer regulation while interacting with children. This study should
expression and to understand that one’s emotional-expressive contribute to understanding how high-quality teachers invest their
behavior may have an impact on others (Saarni, 1999). The ability to emotional competence in their classroom teaching.
manage others’ emotions plays a significant role in impression In sum, the present study considers teachers’ emotional expe-
management and persuasion, and those who can effectively regu- rience and emotional interaction in terms of their emotional
late others’ emotions are better able to act prosocially and to build competence and investigates how high-quality teachers exert
and maintain solid social networks (Salovey et al., 2008). Thus, in emotional competence while interacting with children. First, the
human contacts, the way a person regulates and expresses present study aims at identifying the variations of emotional
emotions, which we focus on in this study, reflects one aspect of experience that high-quality Japanese elementary school teachers
emotional competence (Goleman, 1998; Mayer, Roberts, & Barsade, have during interaction with children. Second, it investigates the
2007). variations of emotion expression patterns that high-quality
In teaching, emotions are things that teachers may use every teachers use in front of children. Finally, it examines their inner
day. Teachers’ emotional intelligence has been statistically emotion regulation processes, by which emotion expression
surveyed in relation to burnout in countries such as Hong Kong patterns should be generated, and discusses the association with
(Chan, 2006) and the UK (Brackett, Palomera, Mojsa-Kaja, Reyes, & education.
Salovey, 2010). However, to the best of our knowledge, no study has Teachers’ emotional experience may vary according to their
investigated teachers’ emotions and their utilization in terms of cultural setting. For example, Markus and Kitayama (1991)
exerting emotional competence. demonstrated that many Asian cultures, including Japanese, have
What emotional competence do high-quality teachers exert? distinct conceptions of individuality that emphasize harmonious
What are the emotional experiences of those teachers when interdependence with others. By contrast, within American culture
interacting with children in the first place? As a significant first step individuals are more likely to seek to maintain their independence
toward exploring teachers’ emotional competence in relation to from others by attending to the self and by discovering and
their contact with children, we investigated the emotional experi- expressing their unique inner attributes (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).
ences, expression and inner regulation processes, which bring It has been suggested that those differences in self-construal can
about expression, of high-quality Japanese teachers in their influence the nature of individual experiences, including emotional
R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048 1041

ones (Markus & Kitayama, 1991). Emotional display rules are also sufficient for supervising student teachers’ internships and con-
influenced by cultures (Saarni, 1999). However, does interest in ducting lessons that serve as models for other teachers in open
teachers’ emotional experience and expression related to teaching classes. The teachers who participated in this study supervised the
while interacting with children differ according to their cultural training of interns for one month more than twice a year at an
setting? Issues regarding teachers’ manner of interacting with elementary school affiliated with a university of teacher education
children, including strategies for teaching and handling of children’s in Japan. The teachers supervised the interns’ preparation of lesson
misbehavior, are of common interest worldwide. For example, plans, observed their lessons and gave practical advice for
studies have examined teachers’ behavior for building positive improving their teaching skills, and evaluated their performance
teacherestudent relationships in the United States (Newberry, and reported it to the university. In addition, the teachers had
2010; Newberry & Davis, 2008), teachers’ misbehavior in Romania experience in conducting open classes at a conference attended by
(Sava, 2002) and Australia (Riley, Lewis, & Brew, 2010), classroom approximately 1000 elementary school teachers from all over
management strategies of teachers who were successful in creating Japan. Most of the participants originally belonged to prefectures in
a positive working atmosphere in the Netherlands (Tartwijk, Brok, the west of Japan, but were selected by the local education boards
Veldman, & Wubbels, 2009), the relation between teachers’ class- because they were considered high-quality teachers, and therefore,
room management strategies and emotional consequences in Hong were transferred for some years to the current elementary school.
Kong (Leung & Lam, 2003), and teachers’ discipline styles and Thus, because their background teaching experience took place in
children’s misbehavior in Australia, China, and Israel through different prefectures including cities and rural areas and was not
a survey (Lewis et al., 2005). In addition, teachers’ approach toward the same, participants in this study can be considered as high-
children’s behavior is an important issue for pre-service teachers quality teachers drawn from various districts in Japan.
(Stoughton, 2007). Therefore, we believe that the implications The first author, the interviewer, had no vested interest in
regarding teachers’ emotional competence and teaching strategies the participants. The interviewer explained the study, asked for the
presented by this study will attract the interest of educators participation of the teachers, and obtained the consent from the
worldwide. Additionally, the present study, which was conducted in participants before the interview. In 2008, 17 teachers (T1eT17)
a non-Western society, has taken a significant step toward were individually interviewed by the first author (phase 1). In the
expanding international research on teachers’ emotions. following year, we added data collection (T18eT24, phase 2) to
enrich data and further develop categories. In total, the first author
2. Method interviewed 24 teachers (8 females). The average age of the
teachers was 33.8 years old (range: 24e41), and the average years
Data collection and analysis are as described below. Since there of teaching experience were 10.2 years (range: 2e18). Except for
is currently little information about elementary school teachers’ one music teacher who taught upper grade music, all participants
emotion regulation processes in the literature, qualitative analysis were class teachers (the number of students was approximately 35)
using an interview procedure was employed to examine it. and taught most subjects themselves. At least three teachers from
each grade from the first grade (age 6) to the sixth grade (age 12)
2.1. Data collection participated in the interview.

The present study repeated data collection and analysis in two 2.1.2. Semi-structured interview
phases (phase 1 and phase 2). In qualitative analysis, it is essential Semi-structured interviews were conducted with teachers
for the quality of the study to develop categories in sufficient depth based on a protocol developed by Sutton (2004). Before the inter-
(Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Strauss & Corbin, 1998). To construct view, the participants were assured that their cooperation was
classification schemes for qualitative analysis, a grounded theory voluntary, personal information would be protected, and the
approach was used (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). The grounded theory collected data would be used only for research; their willingness to
approach suggests conducting data collection and analysis in continue was confirmed. The core questions of the interviews,
a repetitive manner to develop concepts and identify relationships which were asked of all participants, are shown in Table 1. Teachers
between concepts until concepts are saturated (Corbin & Strauss, mainly talked about interactions that occurred in the previous
2008). To ensure sufficient development of concepts and qualita- month or two with their current students.
tive analysis, we first collected phase 1 data and analyzed them to Interviews were individually conducted in a quiet and empty
produce categories, and then added phase 2 data and further room in the elementary school. All the interviews were recorded
analyzed them. As described later, because no new categories were with a voice recorder with participants’ consent. Interviews lasted
identified even after adding phase 2 data, a certain level of satu- an average of 24 min (range: 15e58 min). All interviews were
rability was confirmed in our final category sets. transcribed, and qualitative analysis of the transcription was con-
Interviews allow participants to speak from their own ducted in the manner described in the following section, using
perspective, and the reflective component of emotional experience MAXQDA, which is software designed to assist in data coding.
is the most accessible aspect of emotion and can be most readily
related to the organization and guidance of cognition and action
(Izard, 1991). Because the aims of this study are to identify the Table 1
emotional experience and expression that mark high-quality Core questions of the semi-structured interview.

teachers and to investigate the inner processes that lead to 1. “When you think about teachers’ emotions and classroom teaching, what
expression, obtaining participants’ subjective information about comes to mind?”
their emotions seemed to be appropriate for investigating relations 2. “Could you look at the list and describe the emotions you feel during your
interactions with class students?”
among their emotions, perception, and behavior.
dThe list included anger, fear, sadness, joy, disgust, surprise, and
love/affection.
2.1.1. Participants 3. “On what occasions did you feel [emotions stated]?”
In the present study, high-quality teachers are defined as 4. “How did you deal with [emotions stated]?”
5. “Do you regulate, control, express, or fake other emotions while interacting
teachers who have a certain level of teaching ability including the
with children?”
ability of classroom management and academic instruction
1042 R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048

2.2. Procedure of qualitative analysis Table 2


Percentage of teachers who reported each emotional experience.

The following paragraphs describe the steps in the analysis of Emotional experience N (%)
the transcribed data. The analysis using the grounded theory Anger
approach involves cycles of close examination of the interview data Children-elicited anger 18 (75)
followed by formulation of concepts and classification schemes, Self-elicited anger 2 (8)
based on theoretical comparison (Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Larson & Sadness
Brown, 2007; Strauss & Corbin, 1998). Children-elicited sadness 6 (25)
The first step of the analysis involves investigation of the data Empathetic sadness 2 (8)
Self-elicited sadness 7 (29)
for the purpose of preliminary coding. Transcribed data were cut
into small sections according to content. All those sections were Fear
Children-elicited fear 3 (13)
coded individually and put into preliminary categories that repre-
Heavy responsibilities 2 (8)
sent shared properties. Most categories of emotional experience
and expression patterns were generated at this point. We decided Disgust
Children-elicited disgust 3 (13)
to focus on the basic emotions (anger, sadness, fear, disgust, and
Self-disgust 3 (13)
joy) posited by Izard (2007). We also identified three themes to
explore as discrete emotional experiences, discrete emotion Joy
Children-elicited joy 18 (75)
expression patterns, and emotion regulation processes regarding Pleasant daily interactions 6 (25)
direct staging and suppression. Sections in which teachers talked Empathetic joy 5 (21)
about the process for expressing emotions and points for consid- Successful teaching 5 (21)
eration that preceded emotion expression were analyzed as parts of Love/affection 24 (100)
emotion regulation processes. After coding all sections of the Surprise 23 (96)
interview data and making preliminary categories, we then
reviewed and refined categories through theoretical comparison to
see similarities and differences (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). As for number and percentages of teachers who reported each emotional
categorization, we adopted a category only when there were experience.
examples of such from at least two participants.
The final step involved theoretical analysis of the relationships 3.1.1. Anger
and linkages within and across categories (Corbin & Strauss, 2008; Qualitative analysis identified anger experiences as follows:
Larson & Brown, 2007). After we obtained category sets from phase children-elicited anger and self-elicited anger. Children-elicited
1 data, we analyzed phase 2 data and confirmed that no new anger, reported by 18 teachers (75%), was identified as anger felt
categories were identified. Therefore, we considered that the because of children’s not caring for peers, not following instruc-
categories were saturated (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). One of the tions, or not doing their best, or because of children’s dangerous
methods to validate the theoretical scheme is to tell the analytic actions. Simultaneously, self-elicited anger was identified by two
findings to respondents and other samples and request that they teachers (8%). Self-elicited anger was felt because they thought it
comment on how well it seems to fit their cases (Corbin & Strauss, was their own fault, consequently, that the children were insuffi-
2008). According to Corbin and Strauss, credibility and applicability ciently motivated to do their best.
are important for quality findings in qualitative research. To achieve
credibility, we repeated data collection and analysis using methods 3.1.2. Sadness
of theoretical comparison, enriched data, and then confirmed the Qualitative analysis identified sadness experiences as follows:
saturation of the obtained categories. Applicability indicates that children-elicited sadness, empathetic sadness, and self-elicited
the findings fit the area from which they were derived, are readily sadness. Children-elicited sadness was felt when children displayed
understandable, and will be used (Corbin & Strauss, 2008). We unfavorable behaviors or attitudes such as failing to maintain the
explained our analytic results to three experienced elementary discipline that they had been able to before. Some teachers repor-
school teachers, who had more than 15 years’ teaching experience ted that they felt children-elicited sadness after they felt anger.
and have experience in training interns. One of them was an Empathetic sadness was experienced as empathy for children’s
informant in the interviews. They understood our findings well and own sadness. Self-elicited sadness, similar to self-elicited anger,
the findings rang true to their experience, supporting the was felt because some teachers sensed that they had not fully
substantial applicability of our study. educated children. One teacher explained self-elicited sadness as
follows:
3. Results and discussion
It’s not about kids being bad. It comes back to me in the end.
When I noticed kids were not able to do this and that yet, I felt
In this section, we describe our findings and discuss the varia-
sorry. I could have guided them differently. Simply, I felt sorry
tion of teachers’ emotional experience, expression patterns, and
and I felt sad about that (T2, l.14).
regulation processes. The citations of teachers’ quotes have been
placed in parentheses (teacher’s code, the number of the line where
the quote starts in the transcript). 3.1.3. Fear
Fear experience was infrequent. Qualitative analysis classified
3.1. Variation of teachers’ emotional experience fear experiences as follows: children-elicited fear and heavy
responsibilities. Children-elicited fear was experienced when
To investigate teachers’ emotional competence, it is first neces- teachers noticed children’s misbehavior or physically dangerous
sary to identify teachers’ emotional experience. Teachers’ responses acts. One teacher said that when he found a child engaging in
about their emotional experience during interaction with children mischief, he felt fear and anxiety about the child’s future (T10, l.13).
were prompted by a list of emotion words. Table 2 shows categories Another teacher said that when children acted dangerously, she felt
of emotional experience derived from qualitative analysis and the anger as well as fear because of imagining the risk (T7, l.24). Some
R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048 1043

teachers reported fear due to acknowledging a teacher’s heavy Moreover, when teachers display strong anger, they are likely
responsibilities such as the teacher’s strong influence on children. aware of the objective of calm guidance. The following is a comment
reported by another teacher about how she guided children after
3.1.4. Disgust showing strong anger because of children’s not caring for others:
Disgust experience was also relatively infrequent. Qualitative
I show anger by raising my voice. If I have to show anger to one
analysis identified disgust experiences as follows: children-elicited
child, we would first move aside. When I show anger to kids, I do
disgust and self-disgust. Children-elicited disgust was disgust felt
so very emphatically. But I never lose my temper. I first give
because of children’s unfavorable behaviors or attitudes including
them it “like thunder,” then I loosen things up. I would ask them
bad manners or a too cynical attitude in some early adolescent girls.
“Do you know why you are being scolded?” or “Why did you do
Some teachers reported that children-elicited disgust led to anger or
this?” I do not stay angry for long (T17, l.51).
sadness. In contrast, self-disgust, similar to self-elicited anger and
self-elicited sadness, was identified as disgust which was felt when Suppression of anger was the second major response reported
some teachers reflectively thought that their communication with by 10 teachers (42%). Teachers reported that they consciously
children might have been inadequate. One teacher stated as follows: suppressed their anger through regulation processes because they
I sometimes felt self-disgust. I asked myself “Did I stunt the were reluctant to express their real anger without controlling it.
children’s growth with my words?” or I thought “I should have Some teachers said that they would first suppress their anger
listened to the kids more.” (T8, l.26.) because they thought they should first listen to children (e.g., T6,
l.15). One teacher said as follows:

3.1.5. Joy If you just say “Do it!” to kids, it won’t help. When they don’t do
Qualitative analysis identified joy experiences as follows: chil- something, maybe there are reasons. So, I usually suppress my
dren-elicited joy, pleasant daily interactions, empathetic joy, and anger first (T7, l.46).
successful teaching. Children-elicited joy, reported by 18 teachers
However, four teachers (17%) reported that they sometimes lost
(75%), was identified as joy felt because of children’s achievement or
autonomy. One teacher described children-elicited joy as follows: their temper and expressed their genuine anger toward children.
Extinguishing anger, reported by two teachers (8%), was iden-
I felt joy when kids went beyond my expectations. I felt joy tified as a way to extinguish anger through immediate reflection on
when kids were thinking and doing on their own. It was an event which might otherwise arouse anger in teachers. Teachers
a moment when they didn’t need my guidance. It is not often reported that anger was extinguished right after they changed their
you get to see such a moment, but when I see it, I would sense anger attribution and recognized the anger as their problem and
their development and feel joy (T22, l.9). not the children’s problem. Teachers realized that it was not worth
real anger because children were immature. One teacher explained
Some teachers reported that they felt joy because of pleasant
daily interactions with children. Empathetic joy was felt when this as follows:
teachers felt happy because of the children’s own joy or their When I felt anger, it was often anger over not being able to control
amusing playing. Some teachers reported that they felt joy because things as I wanted. In reality, I might be pressed because of work
of having taught successfully such as when they gave a good class. and did not focus on what I was facing then.I might have got
However, the joy may not necessarily be self-approbation because angry at the moment, but immediately after that, I was able to
of one’s own success, as one teacher indicated in the following: analyze it, get rid of my anger, and be myself again (T16, l.35).
I felt joy when I taught well in a class. It happened when what
the children wanted to learn and what I wanted to teach 3.2.2. Variation of sadness expression
matched very well. It brought me a feeling of unity, and I felt joy Qualitative analysis identified three ways to express (or not to
because of it (T20, l.25). express) teachers’ sadness: direct staging, showing empathetic
sadness, and suppression.
Five teachers (21%) reported direct staging of sadness, that is, to act
3.2. Emotion expression patterns in front of children or show deep sadness to children whether their real emotion was sad
or not. One teacher said that he sometimes drew a crying face on the
As mentioned above, teachers’ various emotional experiences blackboard and said to children, “This picture is me, crying. I’m sad,”
were identified. In the following sections, we will first overview so that he could convey sadness to younger children when the whole
variations of expression patterns for each emotion. class was being too noisy (T13, l.72). Direct staging of sadness seemed
to be performed more by teachers of lower grades. Another teacher
3.2.1. Variation of anger expression explained an example of the direct staging of sadness as follows:
Through the analysis, four ways in which teachers expressed (or
When children failed to do something, to follow a class rule for
did not express) anger were identified: direct staging, suppression,
losing temper, and extinguishing anger. Thirteen teachers (54%) were example, although they were able to do so the day before, I said
to the class “I’m sad.” I didn’t feel sad actually, but I said so to the
clearly aware of their direct staging of anger. Direct staging of anger
was identified as a purposeful display to children of a strong kids instead of saying “No!” to them (T17, l.156).
emotion of anger, whether the teacher’s real emotion was anger or
Suppression of sadness, reported by four teachers (17%), was
not. Teachers reported that they consciously control the intensity
also identified. Especially, teachers who reported self-elicited
of the expressed anger. One teacher explained the direct staging of
sadness said that they did not show their sadness to children and
anger due to children’s dangerous acts or due to their speaking ill of
that they got past the feeling quickly, saw the situation as a possible
others as follows:
source of improvement, and contemplated their next move.
It was when I was not actually angry that I displayed anger to Some teachers talked about efforts that they made to show
kids. When I hit my desk, made a loud noise and scolded them empathetic sadness when a child was feeling sad over a personal
severely, my real emotion was likely not genuine anger (T1, l.33). matter.
1044 R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048

3.2.3. Suppression of fear and disgust suggests that many teachers considered emotion expressions in
Suppression was the only category that was clearly identified for front of children as skills, and that they consciously decide to stage
fear and disgust expression. There were no teachers who said that or suppress their emotions according to the situation. This indicates
they consciously expressed fear or disgust in front of children. that teachers understand that their emotion expression influences
Teachers who mentioned fear or disgust reported that they first children, that is, teachers are aware of their emotion management
tried to suppress and hide the emotions from children. They tried as a skill related to teaching and consciously utilize it.
not to show their fear or disgust, probably because they did not Then, with what purpose and through what processes do high-
want to send the signal that their security was being violated or quality teachers stage or suppress emotions in front of children?
signal rejection (Izard, 1991). One teacher said, “Even if I feel disgust We qualitatively analyzed teachers’ data and identified some
over kids’ behavior, I don’t want them to notice it, because I value categories regarding the purposes and processes of direct staging
being around them as a teacher (T10, l.67).” Some teachers who and suppression of emotions. Table 3 shows the obtained cate-
reported self-disgust talked about efforts to motivate themselves to gories. We describe each category in the following sections.
deal with the situation. For example, one teacher said that although
he sometimes felt down after kids left school, he made it a rule to 3.3.1. Purpose of direct staging
get over it by the next morning (T21, l.82). 3.3.1.1. To evoke children’s emotion. Direct staging of emotion
involves enhancement of the intensity of the felt emotion as well as
3.2.4. Variation of joy expression conscious production of an emotional state. Qualitative analysis
Through qualitative analysis, four ways in which teachers indicated that teachers did direct staging of emotion to evoke
expressed (or did not express) joy were identified: direct staging, children’s emotions. In other words, teachers expected children to
suppression, natural expression, and intentional spreading. Eleven understand by feeling the teacher’s emotion at heart and by being
teachers (46%) clearly reported that they showed their joy by emotionally responsive. One teacher reported utilizing the direct
consciously exaggerating or dramatizing words or by making joyful staging of emotions as follows:
facial expressions regardless of their real emotions. One teacher
When I see my classroom not making progress or plodding,
said as follows:
I sometimes use and convey my emotion as a shot in the arm for
Well, as I grew older, I realized that I am acting. There is a big the children. It happens not only with anger but also with joy.
difference between me as a teacher in the classroom and my I think I sometimes need to express joyful emotion, telling the
natural self. When I express my joy to a kid, saying, “Wow, you kids “I like it”.to raise their morale (T5, l.53).
did it! Wonderful!” I might be thinking that “Oh, it’s been
At the same time, teachers’ direct staging of emotions appeared
a while since he came to talk to me. I guess he has some good
to function to convey teachers’ values, rules, or evaluation to chil-
news to share with me,” and then I act in a way that shows that I
dren. One teacher talked about the purpose of direct staging of
am happy to talk with him too (T16, l.66).
anger as follows:
Suppression of genuine joy, reported by eight teachers (33%),
Although I personally didn’t think it made me mad, I took
was also identified. One teacher said that even when children
advantage of it and decided to share the concern with the whole
achieved something and he felt sincerely happy, he sometimes
class. I raised my anger level, making a face and raising my voice
suppressed his joy and suggested the children could go further
(T21, l.60).
(T18, l.33). He said he did so because he thought there was still
considerable room for growth. Another teacher said that he sup- Another teacher reported the necessity of appealing to chil-
pressed his joy, kept a cool face, and gave instructions as usual, dren’s emotions by conveying the teacher’s emotion, not logic, as
although he was “smiling inside (T1, l.60)” when he found the first follows:
graders thinking independently and acting before they were told
I always show anger over life-threatening issues, behaviors that
what to do.
involve risk of injury, and insult to others. It’s not enough to talk
Natural expression of joy was identified by six teachers (25%).
calmly in order to make kids realize that those things are really
They reported that they expressed their joy naturally without
bad and unacceptable. I think I need to show, or fake, emotions
noticing it. Intentional spreading of joy, reported by four teachers
to teach them (T23, l.60).
(17%), was identified as a way in which teachers purposefully
spread a staged joy to the whole class. This seemed to happen after
teachers acknowledged a child’s significant achievement. One Table 3
teacher said as follows: Categories identified for the processes of direct staging and suppression of emotions.

When a kid does something good, I sometimes tell it to other Category


kids. I would say “Isn’t it great that he did it?” so that the kid can Purpose of direct staging
hear it. The kid can hear my comments indirectly. I express joy To evoke children’s emotion
through a joyous facial expression and words (T15, l.61). To convey approval to children

Purpose of suppression
To get children ready to listen
3.3. Emotion regulation processes dIneffectiveness of losing temper
Not wanting children to know teacher’s emotion
As demonstrated above, we identified some emotion expression To encourage children to develop abilities further
patterns that teachers used in front of children at school. Among dConflict

them, direct staging and suppression appeared to be major and Appropriate use of direct staging and suppression
completely different expression patterns. It should be noted that, Children’s personalities and developmental stages
Teacherechild relationship
indeed, sixteen teachers (67%) clearly reported that how they
express emotions was decided “on a case-by-case basis (e.g., T3, Ideal teacher images
l.31),” or that they “used various ways (e.g., T4, l.36),” or more Emotionally expressive teacher
Calm teacher
precisely, that emotion expression is “a skill (e.g., T2, l.155).” This
R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048 1045

Teachers likely conveyed their values, rules, or evaluation not 3.3.2.3. To encourage children to develop their abilities further. The
only with negative emotions but also with positive emotions. teacher needs to use foresight in appropriately setting goals for
Intentional spreading of joy also suggests similar functions. One children, and in guiding them. In teachers’ suppression of joy, we
teacher talked about the direct staging of joy as follows. found some cases where teachers suppressed their joy and priori-
tized guidance to encourage children to go further in developing
For good things, I sometimes express my emotions on purpose.
their abilities, even when they felt children-elicited joy or empa-
When I find what I wanted to see or what I expected, I convey
thetic joy. Moreover, some teachers reported that they felt conflict
the information “these are good.” (T2, l.42)
when suppressing joy and giving guidance to children. An example
of that was a conflict between teaching children and sharing joy
3.3.1.2. To convey approval to children. Many teachers reported that with children, reported by a teacher who reminisced about seaside
they tried to express positive emotions such as joy as much as training. According to him, some children succeeded in swimming
possible by using words, facial expressions, or physical contact. as far as 600 m out to sea; they were excited and joyful about it.
Moreover, some teachers reported that they sometimes “exagger- However, the children had displayed bad attitude before swim-
ated (e.g., T8, l.45)” their positive emotions to make an impression. ming. Then, he, as a class teacher, suppressed his joy and warned
One teacher said “I consciously express positive feelings a great deal the children about their behavior. The teacher discussed this as
because I want to give children approval as much as I can (T4, l.70).” follows.
Furthermore, some teachers reported that when a child was feeling
I don’t know if emotion control (suppression of joy) is a good
sad, they consciously express empathetic sadness to the child.
thing. I believe that I should express joy when I am with children
These findings suggest that teachers were willing to express
who are feeling sincerely happy. At the same time, I try to control
“approval” to children on a daily basis.
myself because I think I should do my job as a teacher before
sharing joyful emotions. I don’t know if that is good (T3, l.87).
3.3.2. Purpose of suppression
3.3.2.1. To get children ready to listen. When teachers suppressed
children-elicited negative emotions and calmly guided children, 3.3.3. Appropriate use of direct staging and suppression
teachers likely expected to prepare the children to listen to them. Direct staging and suppression are completely different styles of
In other words, teachers expected to “make children listen calmly emotion expression. For example, direct staging of joy and
(T1, l.213)” and make them understand cognitively. Teachers suppression of joy are opposite behaviors. However, both behaviors
reported “When I think of how kids can be helped to listen to me, are conducted in the hope of encouraging children’s development.
my anger takes second place (T1, l.215),” or “I would not express Then, how do teachers discern appropriate use of direct staging and
my emotions especially when I really get mad (T3, l.40).” One suppression? Qualitative analysis demonstrated that teachers
teacher reported that when children did misbehave, she first considered children’s personalities and developmental stages and the
suppressed her anger and “let children talk (T20, l.44),” and then teacherechild relationship before they decided on whether they
she calmly guided them. Another teacher also said that he first would do direct staging or suppression. For example, one class
suppressed his anger because he wanted to make children teacher of fifth graders said that he considered whether the child
understand by giving reasons why their behavior was wrong (T19, wanted approval before expressing his joy because some children
l.16). Thus, teachers likely suppress their real emotions and talk to did not like to be overly acknowledged for work on which they had
children calmly when they expect children to listen and under- expended little effort or for which they had shown no real enthu-
stand what they say. siasm (T14, l.58). Another teacher said that he carefully took into
In addition, ineffectiveness of losing temper was also identified as consideration the child’s emotional state, the situation the child
a notion of teachers. Six teachers (25%) reported that expressing was in, as well as the relationships with the child and with its
real anger without regulation was their mistake because it would parents. Then he decided “to what extent” he could speak force-
just “provoke children to protest (T13, l.78),” “fail to appeal to kids fully, before showing anger toward the child. He also said that “it is
(T12, l.140),” or “fail to make them understand (T19, l.15).” Sutton dangerous” to stage anger without establishing relationships with
(2004) also demonstrated that the ineffectiveness of yelling was children (T22, l.54).
acknowledged by American middle school teachers. Such a notion
may help teachers suppress their genuine anger. 3.3.4. Ideal teacher images
Two ideal teacher images were identified in the interview data:
3.3.2.2. Not wanting children to know teacher’s emotion. Teachers emotionally expressive teacher and calm teacher. Such ideal teacher
sometimes suppressed their emotions because they did not want images may also influence emotion regulation processes. An
children to know the teachers’ emotion. When teachers sup- emotionally expressive teacher was identified as the ideal teacher
pressed fear or disgust, which they never wanted kids to notice, or who expresses both positive and negative emotions reflecting
suppressed self-elicited emotions, they likely stayed calm and human characteristics. In the present study, one teacher talked
reacted to children as usual. In addition, teachers’ self-elicited about his ideal teacher image as follows. It should be noted that the
negative emotions functioned as a “source of improvement (T14, teacher consciously tried to be emotional.
l.52).” As for suppression of joy, some teachers did not express joy
Recently, I have come to the conclusion that teachers should be
in front of children but relished it in private. One teacher said as
more open in expressing their emotions regardless of whether
follows:
they are positive or negative. For example, it is okay for a teacher
I privately relished small pleasures because of things that others to say “No! You can’t do it!” angrily to convey that the teacher is
may not notice but I do, such as children’s subtle changes or really angry. For joy, surprise, and other emotions too .But it’s
incremental development. I think that kind of joy may be even on purpose. I mean, I’m trying to be emotional on purpose (T14,
more precious. I may express joy when children achieve l.28).
something or when they show apparent big changes, but those
The other ideal teacher image, the calm teacher, was identified
don’t happen so often. I experience deep joy when I find a tiny
as the image of an ideal teacher who always tried to be calm and
little change in children (T22, l.30).
refrain from expressing negative emotions in front of children. One
1046 R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048

teacher said that he “seldom expressed anger in a loud voice” clear intentions. Teachers use direct staging of emotions when they
because he thought “how teachers express emotions would influ- want to evoke and appeal to children’s emotion beyond logic.
ence children (T19, l.120).” Teachers also stage positive or empathetic emotions to convey
It is possible that those ideal teacher images influence how approval to children. Because expression of genuine emotions does
teachers regulate their emotions. For example, teachers who highly not effectively reach children, teachers try to suppress negative
value an emotionally expressive teacher may express and show emotions when children need to understand cognitively. Sometimes,
their emotions more often to evoke children’s emotions than teachers suppress joy to encourage children to develop their ability
teachers who always tried to be calm. further. Although teachers sometimes experience a conflict in the
As described above, qualitative analysis suggests that high- strategies that they have chosen, as T3 reported, high-quality
quality teachers’ emotion regulation processes involve various teachers use emotion expression skills and conduct elaborate deci-
purposes, decision on the appropriate use of expression patterns, sion-making with those intensions, considering children’s person-
and ideal teacher images. High-quality teachers may need to have alities and developmental stages and the teacherechild relationship.
not only emotion expression skills but also the ability to decide In the light of the caregiverechild relationship, teachers’ exag-
quickly whether to evoke emotion in children or to get them ready gerated showing of emotions may function to attract children’s
to listen by considering the children’s personalities and the attention, in the same way that multimodal motheresedadults
teacherechild relationship. communicating with infants with exaggerated vocal and gestural
communicationdassists in drawing an infant’s attention (Gogate,
4. General discussion Bahrick, & Watson, 2000). Although further research is needed,
teachers may express positive emotions more frequently and
The present study investigated what emotional experience exaggeratedly for lower grade children to convey approval, and
high-quality teachers have and how they express and regulate their suppress positive emotions more frequently for upper grade chil-
emotions while interacting with children in the light of teachers’ dren to encourage their reaching for higher goals.
emotional competence. Any emotion can have adaptive or malad- The present study suggests that good teachers are well aware of
aptive effects (Izard, 2007; Izard et al., 2008). Emotion researchers their emotion management and that they need the ability to decide
have included the ability to use emotions adaptively to promote how and which emotions they should express in front of children in
thinking in their definition of emotional competence (Izard et al., order to meet their goals of teaching; they also need to choose
2008; Salovey et al., 2008). Moreover, emotion utilization appropriate strategy through careful but quick consideration. It is
involves spontaneous as well as planned constructive actions and suggested that high-quality teachers not only have emotional
creative endeavors (Izard et al., 2008). The present study suggested availability but also effectively exert emotion management skills as
that self-elicited anger, self-elicited sadness, and self-disgust may one of their concrete teaching abilities.
serve as a motivation toward self-improvement for teachers. In terms of emotional labor, the present study strongly supports
Additionally, teachers’ positive emotional arousal elicited by their the notion that teaching requires a great deal of emotional labor
successful teaching may contribute to enhancing teachers’ self- (Hargreaves, 1998, 2000; Isenbarger & Zembylas, 2006; Schutz
efficacy (Tschannen-Moran & Woolfolk Hoy, 2007). et al., 2007) and emphasizes the necessity of having the ability to
The way one expresses emotion while interacting with others utilize emotions flexibly. Direct staging of anger may require much
reflects one’s ability to manage emotions (Saarni, 1999; Salovey labor to perform not only because there is emotional dissonance
et al., 2008). The present study examined for the first time how between an expressed emotion and a real emotion but also because
teachers tried to manage emotions of their own and others while the intensity of the required emotional display is high (Morris &
interacting with children. Generally, good teaching is supposed to Feldman, 1996). Furthermore, it is teachers, not administrators or
be charged with positive emotions (Hargreaves, 1998). Although the school, that decide on the spot what should be expressed and
this is not discussed in detail, all the teachers in this study have what should not. Many workplaces where maintaining good rela-
love/affection for children. Moreover, many teachers experienced tionships with customers is necessary generally require frequent
positive emotions and tried to express them as much as possible by expression of positive emotions (Hochschild, 1983; Morris &
their caring behavior (Isenbarger & Zembylas, 2006; O’Connor, Feldman, 1996). It may be characteristic of teachers’ profession-
2008). In some ways, teacherechild emotional interactions may alism that teachers have the skills to maintain good relationships
take the form of caregiverechild attachment (Wentzel, 2002). The with children as well as to express a variety of emotions including
caregiver’s emotional warmth, responsiveness, and emotional negative ones toward children. Teachers are able to safely express
availability, which are described as the caregiver’s supportive even negative emotions possibly because positive teacherechild
presence and acceptance of a broad range of emotions of the child, relationships act as a base, as T22 reported that staging anger
are important for building a solid relationship between the child without positive relationships is “dangerous”. Since the boundary
and the caregiver (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; between labor and caring may be blurred in teaching (Isenbarger &
Biringen & Robinson, 1991; Bowlby, 1980; Sorce & Emde, 1981). Zembylas, 2006), teachers’ positive relationships with children
According to the caregiverechild relationship analogy, teachers’ established by positive emotions and emotional availability can be
caring attitude such as willingness to empathize with children and considered to be built with emotional labor (Newberry & Davis,
express approval (in other words emotional availability) may help 2008). Although the negative impact of emotional labor, such as
in building secure relationships with children. stress, is often discussed, it also involves a positive aspect in the
However, the present study demonstrated that the emotion sense that emotional labor is rewarding (Isenbarger & Zembylas,
expression of high-quality Japanese elementary school teachers’ in 2006). Those who explained teachers’ emotion expression as
classrooms involves complex and various situational judgments a skill seemed willing to engage in emotion regulation, anticipating
rather than only positive emotional expression. Teachers intention- children’s development without being affected by the negative
ally expressed negative emotions as well, such as anger and sadness, aspects of emotional labor. A virtuous cycle may possibly exist,
toward children. In addition, teachers reported that they even where teachers execute emotional labor as educational profes-
consciously suppressed a positive emotion of genuine joy in front of sionals, establish positive relationships with children, and receive
children. Many teachers regarded their emotion expression as rewards in the form of children’s development, which in turn
“skills” related to teaching and they seemed to use those skills with pleases the teachers.
R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048 1047

There may be cultural influence on individuals’ emotional a certain level of teaching ability, including the ability of classroom
experiences. Markus and Kitayama (1991) suggested that those management and instruction. We think that the social desirability
with interdependent selves may experience more emotions that response bias was not influential because the teachers discussed
are derived primarily from referencing others, such as shame and seemingly unfavorable experiences, such as negative emotions,
guilt, than those with independent selves (Markus & Kitayama, mistakes, and conflicts, in the safe interview environment, where
1991). Teachers who participated in the present study never used confidentiality was protected.
the word “guilt,” but some self-elicited negative emotions might be Another possible limitation may be that the present study
similar to guilt. In Sutton’s work, self-elicited negative emotions examined only the self-reports of the participants. However, its aim
were not mentioned (Sutton, 2004). Further research will be was to examine the subjective, not objective, aspects of emotional
needed to discern whether teachers’ sensitivity to self-elicited experience and regulation, which lead to expression, and we have
negative emotions is influenced by cultural differences of self- performed substantial investigation regarding the aim through
concept or due to high-quality teachers’ high standards and goals repeated data collection and analysis. Although we need to be
that lead them to be demanding of themselves in their self-evalu- careful about making generalizations about the present study, since
ation (Lewis, 2008). it is an exploratory one, we do think that the findings and impli-
Japanese elementary schools emphasize educating the “whole- cations gained by it about high-quality teachers’ emotional
child,” that is, nurturing culturally valued qualities such as perse- competence will be a thrust for future research. The next stage of
verance, cooperation, and empathy along with the spectrum of the research, which will use other methodology, such as observa-
school activities (Hoffman, 2000). Therefore, teachers in this study tion, should examine the relation between teachers’ inner regula-
may have talked much about supporting children in self-regulating tion processes and observable emotion expression, as well as the
behavior. In addition, in Japanese classrooms, teachers and children relation between teachers’ emotion expression skills and their
tend to share an emotional unity (Hoffman, 2003). In the present influence on children and on relationships with children in actual
study, teachers’ expression of emotions such as anger, sadness, and school settings. We are now promoting studies in such directions.
joy to evoke children’s emotions was identified as a common In conclusion, the present study revealed various concrete
strategy that many teachers used to guide children. It is possible emotional experiences and emotion expression strategies that
that such a school culture influences teachers’ emotional experi- high-quality Japanese teachers used while interacting with chil-
ence and the manner in which they exert emotional competence. dren. It introduced an important concept regarding the way
However, as the present study suggests, we believe that it is teachers interact with children, that of teacher’s emotional
important to consider the perspective of teachers’ emotion competence, which may have been used implicitly by high-quality
management while interacting with children for improving child teachers. It also offered both theoretically and practically important
education not only in Japan but also in other countries. Indeed, the implications. High-quality teachers regard emotion expression in
manner in which teachers interact with children is a topic that has front of children as a skill and manage emotions through consid-
attracted the interest of researchers worldwide (Lewis et al., 2005; ering purpose, appropriate use, and ideal teacher images; it has
Newberry, 2010; Newberry & Davis, 2008; Riley et al., 2010; Sava, been suggested that high-quality teachers consciously use their
2002; Tartwijk et al., 2009). Aggression, characterized by yelling competence with emotion management as an effective practical
in anger and denigrating the students, is a possible albeit unfa- teaching tool. As this study has shown, accruing implicit knowledge
vorable kind of behavior of teachers, and such behavior is related to of the emotional competence that high-quality teachers may have
negative relationships with children and children’s misbehavior used will benefit teacher education. In teacher education, it may be
(Lewis et al., 2005; Riley et al., 2010; Sava, 2002). However, such necessary to not only teach academic instruction skills but also
a hostile attitude is a fundamentally different strategy from the promote awareness and skills to develop teachers’ emotion
direct staging of anger, through which high-quality teachers management. For example, the reflection method can be used in
consciously express anger to guide children, as reported in the teacher education courses to promote self-awareness of the level of
present study. It is possible that better teachers may use more emotional competence by having student interns reflect on their
sophisticated emotion expression strategies, whereas teachers who emotional interactions with children (Korthagen, 2004). In addi-
are insensitive to emotion management may behave in a manner tion, support to improve teachers’ competence with emotion
that is against children’s interests. A study conducted in the UK management can be provided to not only novice teachers but also
found that teachers try to suppress anger toward adults through long-careered teachers who are struggling with regard to their
cognitive effort, whereas anger toward children tends to be emotional interactions with children. Teachers who are aware of
expressed rather immediately, with less cognitive effort (Farouk, emotional knowledge and competence and use them effectively
2010).It is necessary to distinguish between teachers’ unregulated may be different in terms of children’s achievement or attitudes
emotion expression and high-quality teachers’ conscious emotion from teachers who are ignorant of them, depreciate emotional
expression. As Lewis et al. (2005) pointed out, teachers are interaction, and haphazardly interact with children. The findings of
professionals who need to respond in the best interests of the this study suggest that teaching based on good emotional compe-
children they teach. To foster such professionalism in teachers, it tence and the teacher education to promote and foster such
will be necessary to introduce and discuss the perspective of competence are needed.
teachers’ emotion management in teacher education.
A limitation of the study is the definition of high-quality
teachers. Since teachers’ quality should be considered on multiple References
levels, including their environment and beliefs as well as their
competence, it is difficult to answer “what is a good teacher” in Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M. P., Waters, E., & Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment:
A psychological study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
a simple way (Korthagen, 2004). In addition, teachers who are
Biringen, Z., & Robinson, J. L. (1991). Emotional availability in motherechild inter-
highly evaluated by others may not always be good teachers. actions: a reconceptualization for research. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,
Acknowledging the challenges posed in defining “high quality,” we 61(2), 258e271.
tried to define high-quality teachers on the basis of objective Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment. In Attachment and loss, Vol. 1. New York: Basic Books.
Brackett, M. A., Palomera, R., Mojsa-Kaja, J., Reyes, M. R., & Salovey, P. (2010).
performance, such as the experiences of supervising internships Emotion-regulation ability, burnout, and job satisfaction among British
and open classes, because both tasks cannot be performed without secondary-school teachers. Psychology in the Schools, 47(4), 406e417.
1048 R. Hosotani, K. Imai-Matsumura / Teaching and Teacher Education 27 (2011) 1039e1048

Chan, D. W. (2006). Emotional intelligence and components of burnout among Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence? In P. Salovey, &
Chinese secondary school teachers in Hong Kong. Teaching and Teacher Educa- D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications
tion, 22(8), 1042e1054. for educators (pp. 3e31) New York: Basic Books.
Corbin, J., & Strauss, C. A. (2008). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and Morris, J. A., & Feldman, D. C. (1996). The dimensions, antecedents, and conse-
procedures for developing ground theory (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage quences of emotional labor. Academy of Management Review, 21(4), 986e1010.
Publications. Newberry, M. (2010). Identified phases in the building and maintaining of positive
Danforth, S., & Boyle, J. R. (2006). Cases in behavior management (2nd ed.). Upper teacherestudent relationships. Teaching and Teacher Education, 26(8),
Saddle River: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall. 1695e1703.
Farouk, S. (2010). Primary school teachers’ restricted and elaborated anger. Cam- Newberry, M., & Davis, H. A. (2008). The role of elementary teachers’ conceptions of
bridge Journal of Education, 40(4), 353e368. closeness to students on their differential behavior in the classroom. Teaching
Gogate, L. J., Bahrick, L. E., & Watson, J. D. (2000). A study of multimodal motherese: and Teacher Education, 24(8), 1965e1985.
the role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Nias, J. (1996). Thinking about feeling: the emotions in teaching. Cambridge Journal
Development, 71(4), 878e894. of Education, 26(3), 293e306.
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with emotional intelligence. New York: Bantam Books. O’Connor, K. E. (2008). “You choose to care”: teachers, emotions and professional
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2001). Early teacherechild relationships and the identity. Teaching and Teacher Education, 24(1), 117e126.
trajectory of children’s school outcomes through eighth grade. Child Develop- Riley, P., Lewis, R., & Brew, C. (2010). Why did you do that? Teachers explain the use
ment, 72(2), 625e638. of legal aggression in the classroom. Teaching and Teacher Education, 2010(4),
Hamre, B. K., & Pianta, R. C. (2005). Can instructional and emotional support in the 957e964.
first-grade classroom make a difference for children at risk of school failure? Saarni, C. (1999). The development of emotional competence. New York: The Guilford
Child Development, 76(5), 949e967. Press.
Hamre, B. K., Pianta, R. C., Downer, J. T., & Mashburn, A. J. (2008). Teachers’ Salovey, P., Detweiler-Bedell, B. T., Detweiler-Bedell, J. B., & Mayer, J. D. (2008).
perceptions of conflict with young students: looking beyond problem behav- Emotional intelligence. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.),
iors. Social Development, 17(1), 115e136. Handbook of emotions (3rd ed.). (pp. 533e547) New York: The Guilford Press.
Hargreaves, A. (1998). The emotional practice of teaching. Teaching and Teacher Sava, F. A. (2002). Causes and effects of teacher conflict-inducing attitudes towards
Education, 14(8), 835e854. pupils: a path analysis model. Teaching and Teacher Education, 18(8), 1007e1021.
Hargreaves, A. (2000). Mixed emotions: teachers’ perceptions of their interactions Schutz, P. A., Cross, D. I., Hong, J. Y., & Osbon, J. N. (2007). Teacher identities, beliefs,
with students. Teaching and Teacher Education, 16(8), 811e826. and goals related to emotions in the classroom. In P. A. Schutz, & R. Pekrun
Hargreaves, A. (2001). Emotional geographies of teaching. Teachers College Record, (Eds.), Emotion in education (pp. 223e241). Burlington: Academic Press.
103(6), 1056e1080. Schwartz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2003). Mood as information: twenty years later.
Hochschild, A. R. (1983). The managed heart: Commercialization of human feeling. Psychological Inquiry, 14(3e4), 296e303.
Berkeley: University of California Press. Sorce, J. F., & Emde, R. N. (1981). Mother’s presence is not enough: effect of emotional
Hoffman, D. M. (2000). Pedagogies of self in American and Japanese early childhood availability on infant exploration. Developmental Psychology, 17(6), 737e745.
education: a critical conceptual analysis. The Elementary School Journal, 101(2), Stoughton, E. H. (2007). “How will I get them to behave?”: pre service teachers
193e208. reflect on classroom management. Teaching and Teacher Education, 23(7),
Hoffman, D. M. (2003). Childhood ideology in the United States: a comparative 1024e1037.
cultural view. International Review of Education, 49(1e2), 191e211. Strauss, C. A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and
Isenbarger, L., & Zembylas, M. (2006). The emotional labor of caring in teaching. procedures for developing grounded theory (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Teaching and Teacher Education, 22(1), 120e134. Publications.
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. New York: Plenum Press. Sutton, R. E. (2004). Emotional regulation goals and strategies of teachers. Social
Izard, C. E. (2007). Basic emotions, natural kinds, emotion schemas, and a new Psychology of Education, 7(4), 379e398.
paradigm. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(3), 260e280. Sutton, R. E., Mudrey-Camino, R., & Knight, C. C. (2009). Teachers’ emotion regu-
Izard, C. E., Stark, K., Trentacosta, C., & Schultz, D. (2008). Beyond emotion regula- lation and classroom management. Theory into Practice, 48(2), 130e137.
tion: emotion utilization and adaptive functioning. Child Development Perspec- Sutton, R. E., & Wheatley, K. F. (2003). Teachers’ emotions and teaching: a review of
tives, 2(3), 156e163. the literature and directions for future research. Educational Psychology Review,
Korthagen, F. A. J. (2004). In search of the essence of a good teacher: towards a more 15(4), 327e358.
holistic approach in teacher education. Teaching and Teacher Education, 20(1), Tartwijk, J.v., Brok, P.d., Veldman, I., & Wubbels, T. (2009). Teachers’ practical
77e97. knowledge about classroom management in multicultural classrooms. Teaching
Larson, R. W., & Brown, J. R. (2007). Emotional development in adolescence: what and Teacher Education, 25(3), 453e460.
can be learned from a high school theater program? Child Development, 78(4), Tschannen-Moran, M., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2007). The differential antecedents of
1083e1099. self-efficacy beliefs of novice and experienced teachers. Teaching and Teacher
Lasky, S. (2000). The cultural and emotional politics of teachereparent interactions. Education, 23(6), 944e956.
Teaching and Teacher Education, 16(8), 843e860. Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., Cox, K. E., Logan, C., DiCintio, M., & Thomas, C. T. (1998).
Leung, C., & Lam, S. (2003). The effects of regulatory focus on teachers’ classroom Creating contexts for involvement in mathematics. Journal of Educational
management strategies and emotional consequences. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 90(4), 730e745.
Psychology, 28(1), 114e125. Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., Midgley, C., & Patrick, H. (2003). Teacher discourse and sixth
Lewis, M. (2008). Self-conscious emotions: embarrassment, pride, shame, and guilt. graders’ reported affect and achievement behaviors in two high-mastery/high-
In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions performance mathematics classrooms. Elementary School Journal,103(4), 357e382.
(3rd ed.). (pp. 742e756) New York: The Guilford Press. Turner, J. C., Meyer, D. K., & Schweinle, A. (2003). The importance of emotion in
Lewis, R., Romi, S., Qui, X., & Katz, Y. J. (2005). Teachers’ classroom discipline and theories of motivation: empirical, methodological, and theoretical consider-
student misbehavior in Australia, China and Israel. Teaching and Teacher ations from a goal theory perspective. International Journal of Educational
Education, 21(6), 729e741. Research, 39(4), 375e393.
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: implications for cognition, Wentzel, K. R. (2002). Are effective teachers like good parents? Teaching styles and
emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224e253. student adjustment in early adolescence. Child Development, 73(1), 287e301.
Mashburn, A. J., Pianta, R. C., Hamre, B. K., Downer, J. T., Barbarin, O. A., Bryant, D., Zembylas, M. (2005a). Beyond teacher cognition and teacher belief: the value of the
et al. (2008). Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and children’s ethnography of emotions in teaching. International Journal of Qualitative Studies
development of academic, language, and social skills. Child Development, 79(3), in Education, 18(4), 465e487.
732e749. Zembylas, M. (2005b). Discursive practices, genealogies, and emotional rules:
Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R. D., & Barsade, S. G. (2007). Human abilities: emotional a poststructuralist view on emotion and identity in teaching. Teaching and
intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 507e536. Teacher Education, 21(8), 935e948.

Potrebbero piacerti anche