Sei sulla pagina 1di 8

VIDYA, v. 28, n. 1, p. 105-112, jan/jun, 2008 - Santa Maria, 2009.

ISSN 0104-270 X

TEACHING READING IN THE FOREIGN LANGUAGE CLASSROOM


ENSINAR LEITURA NA SALA DE AULA DE LÍNGUA ESTRANGEIRA

Carla Callegaro Corrêa Kader*

RESUMO ABSTRACT

Este ar tigo aborda os processos de leitura This ar ticle discusses bottom-up and top-
bottom-up e top-down, com ênfase nas down processes of reading, with emphasis
abordagens de ensino e aper feiçoamento da on teaching and enhancement approaches of
leitura na sala de aula de língua estrangeira, reading skills in the English classroom as a
com base nos estudos de Brown (2001), foreign language, based on studies by Brown
Goodman (1970), Nut tall (1996), entre (2001), Goodman (1970), Nutall (1996),
outros. Trata-se de uma pesquisa de cunho among others. It is a bibliographic research
bibliográfico que visa a destacar as fases de that emphasizes the phase of each process of
cada processo de leitura e as habilidades a reading and the skills related to them.
elas relacionadas.
Key words: processes of reading; (micro)
Palavras-chave: processos de leitura; (micro) skills; reading strategies.
habilidades; estratégias de leitura.

*
Professora do Colégio Agrícola de Frederico Westphalen (CAFW), Mestre em Estudos Linguísticos pela Universidade
Federal de Santa Maria.

105
INTRODUCTION a tex t. Nut tall (1996) compares bot tom-up
processes with the image of a scientist with a
This ar ticle focuses on reading as a com- magnifying glass or microscope examining all
ponent of general second language proficiency, the minute details of some phenomenon, while
but reading must be considered only in the per- top-down processing is like taking an eagle’s-
spective of the whole picture of interactive lan- eye view of a landscape below. The author
guage teaching. Significant findings affected the says that such a picture reminds us that field
approaches of teaching reading skills. We can independent and field dependent cognitive
emphasize some of them, such as: bot tom-up st yles are analogous to bot tom-up and top-
and top-down processing; schema theory and down processing, respectively.
background knowledge; the role of affect and According to Brown (2001), a half-century
culture and the power of ex tensive reading. ago, reading specialists might have argued that
Goodman’s work (1970) offered us the the best way to teach reading is through bot tom-
distinction between bot tom-up and top-down up methodology: teach symbols, grapheme-pho-
processing. In bot tom-up processing, readers neme correspondences, syllables and lexical
first recognize a multiplicity of linguistic signals recognition first, then comprehension would
(let ters, morphemes, syllables, words, phrases, derive from the sum of the par ts. More recent
grammatical cues, discourse markers, etc.) and research, on the other hand, has shown that
use their linguistic data-processing mechanisms a combination of top-down and bot tom-up
to impose some sor t of order on these signals. processing, or what has come to be called
These data-driven operations obviously require a “interactive reading”, is almost always a
sophisticated knowledge of the language itself. primar y ingredient in successful teaching
From among all the perceived data, the reader methodology because both processes are im-
selects the signals that make some sense, that por tant. In practice, a reader continually shif ts
cohere, that mean. from one focus to another, initially adopting a
When using previous knowledge and as- top-down approach to predict probable mean-
sumptions, it is called a top-down strategy, ing, then moving on to the bot tom-up ap-
because the reader goes down from more proach to check whether that is really what the
general knowledge and meanings to the spe- writer says (NUTTALL, 1996).
cific ones of the tex t.   Research has shown that the reader
Vir tually all reading involves a risk – a guess- brings information, knowledge, emotion,
ing game, in Goodman’s words (1970), be- experience and culture, that is, schemata
cause readers must, through a puzzle-solving to the printed word. Reading is only inciden-
process, infer meanings, decide what to re- tally visual. More information is contributed by
tain and not to retain, and move on. This is the reader than by the print on the page. That is,
where the top down reading process is im- readers understand what they read because they
perative because the reader draws on his or are able to take the stimulus beyond its graphic
her intelligence and experience to understand representation and assign it membership to an

106
appropriate group of concepts already stored in focused approaches to specific strategies for
their memories. Skill in reading depends on the intensive reading ought to be abandoned, but
efficient interaction between linguistic knowledge strengthens the notion that an ex tensive read-
and knowledge of the world. ing component in conjunction with other fo-
It is impor tant to emphasize that the love cused reading instruction is highly warranted.
for reading has propelled many a learner Language instructors are of ten frustrated
to successful acquisition of reading skills. by the fact that students do not automatically
Instruction has been found to be ef fective transfer the strategies they use when reading
when students’ self-esteem is high (DOLE, in their native language to reading in a lan-
BROWN and TRATHEN, 1996). The autonomy guage they are learning. Instead, they seem to
gained through the learning of reading strate- think reading means star ting at the beginning
gies has been shown to be a powerful motivator. and going word by word, stopping to look up
Similarly, culture plays an active role in moti- every unknown vocabulary item, until they
vating and rewarding people for literacy, es- reach the end. When they do this, students are
pecially because literate practices are learned relying exclusively on their linguistic knowledge,
within dynamic cultural systems that structure a bot tom-up strategy. One of the most impor-
roles and scripts (alphabetic, pictographic), tant functions of the language instructor, then,
privilege modes of reasoning, and of fer tools is to help students move past this idea and use
through which such practices may be carried top-down strategies as they do in their native
out. We cannot simply assume that cognitive fac- language. When language learners use reading
tors alone will account for the eventual success of strategies, they find that they can control the
second language readers (FITZGERALD, 1994). reading experience, and they gain confidence
A current issue in pedagogical research on in their ability to read in the foreign language.
reading is the ex tent to which learners will learn
to read bet ter in an atmosphere of enriched THE MICROSKILLS
surroundings or in an instructed sequence of
direct at tention to the strategies of ef ficient The use of microskills allows students of
reading. Ex tensive reading (KRASHEN, 1993) English as a foreign language to become ef-
is a key for students to gain in reading ability, ficient readers. Brown (2001) lists some im-
linguistic competence, vocabulary, spelling por tant microskills that learners should use in
and writing. Reading for pleasure and read- reading tasks: process writing at an ef ficient
ing without looking up all the unknown words rate of speed to suit purpose; recognize a core
were both highly correlated with overall lan- of words and interpret word order pat terns
guage proficiency (GREEN; OXFORD, 1995). and their significance; recognize grammati-
Krashen (1993) says that instructional cal word classes, pat tern, rules and elliptical
programs in readings should give strong forms; recognize that a par ticular meaning
consideration to the teaching of ex tensive may be expressed in dif ferent grammatical
reading. He does not suggest, of course, that forms; recognize cohesive devices in writ ten

107
discourse and their significance for inter- 6. use semantic mapping or cluster-
ing (grouping ideas into meaningful
pretation; recognize communicative func- clusters, helps readers to provide some
tions of writ ten tex ts, according to form and order to the chaos);
purpose; infer contex t that is not explicit 7. guess when you aren’t cer tain
(guess a meaning of a word, guess a
by using background knowledge; infer links grammatical relationship, guess a dis-
and connections between events, ideas, etc, course relationship, infer implied mean-
ing, guess about a cultural reference and
deduce causes and ef fects, and detect such guess content messages);
relations as main idea, suppor ting idea, new 8. analyze vocabulary (look for prefixes,
information, given information, generalization and look for suffixes, look for roots that are fa-
miliar, look for grammatical contexts that
exemplification; distinguish between literal and may signal information, look at semantic
implied meanings; detect culturally specific context for clues);
9. distinguish between literal and im-
references and interpret them in a contex t of plied meanings;
the appropriate cultural schemata; develop 10. capitalize on discourse markers to
and use a bat tery of reading strategies such process relationships.

as scanning, skimming, detecting discourse


markers, guessing meaning of words from According to Brown (2001, p. 315), stu-
contex t and activating schemata for the inter- dents should follow some principles for de-
pretation of tex ts. signing interactive reading techniques, such
For most second language learners who as: don’t overlook the impor tance of specific
are already literate in a previous language, instruction in reading skills (balance ex tensive
reading comprehension is primarily a mat ter and silent reading), use techniques that are
of developing appropriate, efficient comprehen- motivating, balance authentic tex ts, encourage
sion strategies. Some strategies are related the development of reading strategies, include
to bot tom-up procedures, and others to top- both bot tom-up and top-down techniques,
down processes. Following are ten such strat- subdivide the techniques into pre-reading,
egies, each of which can be applied as class- during-reading and af ter-reading phases.
room techniques (BROWN, 2001): He also advises the introduction of the topic
1. identify the purpose of reading before reading the text, the practice of skimming,
(clearly identify the purpose in reading scanning, predicting and activating schemata. The
something so you will know what you
are looking for); author emphasizes that students bring the best of
2. use graphemic rules and pat terns their knowledge and skills to a text when they have
to aid bot tom-up decoding, especially
for beginning level learners; been given a chance to ease into the passage.
3. use efficient silent reading techniques But af ter reading, besides comprehension
for rapid comprehension (for intermediate
to advanced levels);
questions, it should also be considered to work
4. skim the tex t for main ideas (quickly with vocabulary study, identifying the author’s
running one’s eyes across whole tex t purpose, discussing the author’s line of rea-
for its gist) for prediction;
5. scan the tex t for specific information soning, examining grammatical structures and
(quickly searching for some par ticular motivating students to a writing exercise.
piece or pieces of information in a tex t);

108
CLASSROOM ACTIVITIES students read what they have just said and
neutralize problems of unfamiliar content by
The main goal of English Foreign Language/ writing their own tex ts.
English Second Language (EFL/ESL) reading Another way to minimize inter ference
teachers is to minimize reading dif ficulties and from the tex t is to encourage narrow reading
to ma ximize comprehension by providing cul- (KRASHEN, 1981). Narrow reading refers to
turally relevant information. Goodman puts the the reading that is confined to a single topic
issue into focus when he says that even highly or to tex ts by a single author. Krashen (1981)
ef fective readers are severely limited in com- suggests that narrow reading and perhaps
prehension of tex ts by what they already know narrow input in general is more ef ficient
before they read. The author may influence the for second language acquisition. Students
comprehensibility of a tex t par ticularly for who read either a single topic or a single
specific targeted audiences. But no author can author find that the tex t becomes easier to
completely compensate in writing for the range comprehend. Readers adjust to the repeated
of differences among all potential readers of a vocabular y of a par ticular topic or to the
given tex t. (GOODMAN, 1979, p. 658) par ticular st yle of a writer. Fur thermore,
Considering Goodman’s point of view, repetitions of vocabular y and structure mean
the role of the teacher in the EFL/ESL reading that review is built into the reading.
classroom is to compensate the individual The third possibili t y of tex t facili tation
variation among readers, especially readers is to develop materials wi th local set tings
from dif ferent cultural backgrounds. One way and specialized low-frequency vocabular y.
to minimize this situation is to manipulate These materials might be newspapers,
either the tex t/or the reader, using bot tom- pamphlets, brochures, or booklets about
up or top down strategies or adopting the places of interest. English travel guides are
microskills proposed by Brown (2001). also good sources for the EFL/ESL reader.
Finally, Sustained Silent Reading (SSR)
THE TEXT is a good activit y for ESL readers. Through
this practice, students become self-directed
To ma ximize comprehension for the agents seeking meaning. To be ef fective,
beginning reader, the Language Experience however, an SSR program must be based
Approach (LEA) (RIGG, 1981) is an answer on selected tex ts that will be interesting to
to control vocabular y, structure and content. the readers and facilitated by the teachers.
The basic LEA technique uses the students’ Students select their own reading tex ts with
ideas and their own words in the preparation respect to content, level of dif ficult y and
of beginning reading materials. Students length. According to this approach, students
decide what they want to say and how to who choose their own tex ts are, in ef fect, also
say it, and then dictate to the teacher, who providing their own appropriate background
acts as a scribe. According to this approach, knowledge for understanding the tex t.

109
THE READER student’s ef for t will become more aware of
both the background knowledge and the cul-
In addition to the tex t control (CARREL; EI- tural problems that the readers bring to the
STERHOLD, 2000), it is impor tant to consider reading process and therefore he or she will
what can be done with the readers themselves. help students solve them and increase their
According to these authors, providing back- fluency in reading as a foreign language.
ground knowledge and previewing content for Ur (2001) creates a list of ten items that
the reader seem to be the most obvious strate- shows a contrast between the ef ficient reading
gies for the language teacher. In their opinion, process and the inef ficient reading process.
teachers should avoid having students read The author makes a list of reading tips that
material “cold”. Asking students to manipulate has a connection to Goodman’s, Brown’s,
both the linguistic and cultural codes (some- Carrel’s and Eisterhold’s reading orientation.
times linguistically easy but culturally dif ficult, Each item has an implication for teaching
and vice versa) can make them feel unmoti- reading in a foreign language classroom and
vated to reading or frustrated to the result of summarizes the reader’s and the tex t’s role
comprehending. during the reading process.
These readers need familiar content se- As listed above, Ur (2001) simplified the
lections and/or content preview as much as aspects teachers should obser ve to of fer
possible because they tend to do word-by- their students a variet y of materials which
word processing exclusively in a bot tom-up increase their interest and fluency in reading
processing mode, considering that they need as a foreign language.
more global, predictive processing in the top-
down processing mode. CONCLUSION
Carrel and Eisterhold (2000) also suggest
that illustrations may be appropriate for stu- Considering what was pointed out in this
dents with minimal language skills, especially ar ticle, we can of fer students dif ferent ways
because they provide the semantic content and strategies to increase their reading skills
component for low-level readers, making them and improve their receptive vocabulary and
free of focusing on vocabulary and structure grammar knowledge. We should not forget to
of the content. These authors advise teachers emphasize the use of dif ferent genres to sum
to carefully listen to what students say about up to the background knowledge of the learn-
the tex ts they are asked to read, because this ers to bet ter develop their reading skills.
will probably show their hidden comprehen- Thus, in achieving immediate goals in the
sion problems. EFL/ESL reading classroom, teachers must
Teachers should not respond to what the balance between the background knowledge
reader does (right/wrong) as much as to what presupposed by the tex ts the students read and
the reader is trying to do (Idem). A teacher the background knowledge the students really
who listens carefully and also answers to a possess (CARRELL; EISTERHOLD, 2000). This

110
Ef ficient Inef ficient

The language of the text is comprehen-


1. Language The language of the text is too difficult.
sible to the learners.

The content of the text is accessible to


The text is too difficult in the sense that
the learners; they know enough about
2. Content the content is too far removed from the
it to be able to apply their own back-
knowledge and experience of the learners.
ground knowledge.

The reading progresses fairly fast:


mainly because the reader has automa- The reading is slow: the reader does not
3. Speed tized recognition of common combina- have a large vocabulary of automatically
tions, and does not waste time working recognized items.
out each word or group of words anew.

The reader concentrates on the sig-


nificant bits, and skims the rest; may The reader pays the same amount of
4. Attention
even skip parts he or she knows to be attention to all parts of the text.
insignificant.
The reader takes incomprehensible vo-
The reader cannot tolerate incompre-
cabulary in his or her stride: guesses its
hensible vocabulary items: stops to
5. Incomprehensible meaning from the surrounding text, or
look every one up in a dictionary, and/or
vocabulary ignores it and manages without; uses
feels discouraged from trying to com-
a dictionary only when these strategies
prehend the tex t as a whole.
are insufficient.
The reader thinks ahead, hypothesizes, The reader does not think ahead, deals
6. Prediction
predicts. with the text as it comes.
The reader has and uses background
7. Background The reader does not have or use back-
information to help understand the
information ground information.
text.

The reader is motivated to read:


The reader has no particular interest in
8. Motivation by interesting content or a chal-
reading.
lenging task.

The reader is aware of a clear purpose


The reader has no clear purpose other
9. Purpose in reading: for example, to find out
than to obey the teacher’s instruction.
something, to get pleasure.

The reader uses different strategies for The reader uses the same strategy for
10. Strategies
different kinds of reading. all tex ts.

In: Ur, 2001, p.148.

111
balance may be achieved by manipulating FITZGERALD, J. How literacy emerges: foreign
ei ther the tex t and/or the reader variable, language implications. Language Learning
but teachers should not forget that the long- Journal 9, 32-35, 1994.
term goal should be to develop independent
GOODMAN, K. S. Reading: a psycholinguistic
readers outside the EFL/ESL classroom.
guessing game. In: Singer, H.; Ruddell, R.
This will make readers learn from the tex ts
B. (Eds.). Theoretical models and processes
they read for pleasure or not, for academic
of reading. Newark, DE: International Reading
purposes, reading for sur vival purposes or
Association, 1970.
for functioning in societ y at various levels.
In addition, the process of reading is a GREEN, J. M.; OXFORD, R. A closer look
highly interactive process between the stu- at learning strategies, L2 proficiency, and
dents and their prior background knowledge, gender. TESOL Quar terly 29, 1995: 261-297.
on the one hand, and the tex t i tself, on the
other (Idem). Therefore, ever y cul ture-spe- KRASHEN, S. The case for narrow reading.
cific inter ference deal t wi th in the class- TESOL Newslet ter, 15(6):23, 1981.
room presents an oppor tuni t y to build new ______. The power of reading. Englewood,
cul ture-specific schemata and, of course, CO: Libraries Unlimited, 1993.
new background knowledge to the readers
that can be used inside or outside the EFL/ NUTTALL, C. Teaching reading skills in a
ESL classroom. foreign language. Second edition. Ox ford:
Heinemann, 1996.
references RIGG, P. Beginning to read in English the LEA
way. In: Twyford, C. W.; Diehl, W.; Feath-
BROWN, D. H. Teaching by principles: and ers, K. (Eds.). Reading English as a second
interactive approach to language pedagogy. language: moving from theory. 81-90. Mono-
Second edition. New York: Longman, 2001. graphs in language and readings studies 4.
CARRELL, P. L.; EISTERHOLD, J. C. Schema Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 1981.
theory and ESL reading pedagogy. In: CAR- UR, P. A course in language teaching: practice
REL, P. L. et al. Interactive approaches to and theory. United Kingdom, Cambridge, 2001.
second language reading. United Kingdom:
Cambridge, 2000.

DOLE, J. A.; BROWN, K. J.; RATHEN, K.


The ef fects of strategy instruction on the
comprehension per formance of at-risk
students. Reading Research Quar terly 31:
62-88, 1996.

112

Potrebbero piacerti anche