Sei sulla pagina 1di 252

INFORMATION TO USERS

The most advanced technology has been used to photo­


graph and reproduce this manuscript from the microfilm
master. UMI films the text directly from the original or
copy submitted. Thus, some thesis and dissertation copies
are in typewriter face, while others may be from any type
of computer printer.

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the


quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print,
colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs,
print bleedthrough, substandard margins, and improper
alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send UMI a
complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these
will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material
had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

Oversize materials (e.g., maps, drawings, charts) are re­


produced by sectioning the original, beginning at the
upper left-hand corner and continuing from left to right in
equal sections with small overlaps. Each original is also
photographed in one exposure and is included in reduced
form at the back c: the book. These are also available as
one exposure on a standard 35mm slide or as a 17" x 23"
black and white photographic print for an additional
charge.

Photographs included in the original manuscript have


been reproduced xerographically in this copy. Higher
quality 6" x 9" black and white photographic prints are
available for any photographs or illustrations appearing
in this copy for an additional charge. Contact UMI directly
to order.

University Microfilms International


A Bell & Howell Information Company
300 North Zeeb Road, Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 USA
313/761-4700 800/521-0600

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Order Number 9019980

T h e to n a l style o f Sergei P rokofiev’s “R o m eo a nd J u lie t”

Stephenson, Kenneth Dale, Ph.D.


The University of Iowa, 1989

C o p y rig h t © 1 9 8 9 b y S te p h e n so n , K e n n e th D a le . A ll rig h ts reserved .

UMI
300 N. Zeeb Rd.
Ann Arbor, M I 48106

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
THE TONAL STYLE OF SERGEI PROKOFIEV'S

ROMEO AND JULIET

by

Kenneth Dale Stephenson

A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment


of the requirements for the Doctor of
Philosophy degree in Music
in the Graduate College of
The University of Iowa

December 1989

Thesis supervisor: Professor W. T. Atcherson

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Graduate College
The University of Iowa
Iowa City, Iowa

CERTIFICATE OF APPROVAL

PH.D. THESIS

This is to certify that the Ph.D. thesis of

Kenneth Dale Stephenson

has been approved by the Examining Committee


for the thesis requirement for the Doctor of
Philosophy degree in Music at the December
1989 graduation.

Thesis committee
Thesis supervisor

/ (/ >
n
Membejr)

Member

Member
Cl
Member

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Copyright by
KEN STEPHENSON
1989
All Rights Reserved

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
ABSTRACT

Prokofiev's Romeo and Juliet is tonal despite the

obvious stylistic differences between it and traditional

tonal music. Tonality is a sense the listener has

concerning the metrical placement and the pitch of an

approaching cadence. A regularity of phrase length gives

the listener a sense of the metrical placement of cadences

in this style, normally on the downbeat of the fourth

measure of a phrase. Information concerning the pitch is

provided (1) by cadence formulas, which involve a preference

for highly chromatic dominant sonorities, the notes of which

usually resolve by step or, frequently, by major third, and

(2) by the expectation that the key established at the

beginning of a melody, usually through the use of a diatonic

scale, should return by the time of the final cadence.

Between the beginning of a phrase and the end, however,

Prokofiev often indulges in colorful shifts to other,

distant scale areas, normally through the use of one

strategically placed altered scale degree, or through the

reharmonization of a common tone.

Prokofiev's melodic structures, while at times very

traditional, are often not: ascents as well as simple

oscillations are found. Regarding local-level melodic

ii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
tendencies, the major differences between this style and the

traditional tonal style are a higher level of chromaticism,

the resolution of many chromatic pitches in the non-

traditional direction, and a frequent use of perfect fifths

and perfect fourths between scale degrees other than 1 and

§. This style is suitable for accompaniment to a story both

because of the inherently rich possibilities for the

development of character and theme, and because of its goal-

directed nature.

iii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page
LIST OF TABLES............................................vii

LIST OF EXAMPLES.........................................viii

P R E F A C E .................................................. Xiv

CHAPTER

I. TONALITY................................... 1

Definition ............................... i
Signals of Metrical Placement of
Approaching Endings................ 10
Signals of the Pitch of Approaching
Endings....................... . . . . 20

II. PHRASE L E N G T H ............................ 29

Establishment of Meter ................... 30


Articulation of Phrases................ 34
Standard Phrase Length ................... 44

III. CADENCES................................... 59

Final C h o r d s ..................... ... 60


Dominant as Penultimate Chord......... 62
Nondominant Penultimate Chords........ 74
Half-Cadences and OtherCadence Figures. . 78
Antepenultimate Chords ................... 80

IV. KEY, SCALE, AND MODULATION........... . 83

Methods of Establishing K e y s ......... 84


Successions of Scales.................. 90
Relationships between Successive Scales. . 103
Modulations............................. 117

V. MELODIC STRUCTURE ........................... 125

Melodies with Descending Structures.. . . 126


Melodies with Nondescending Structures . . 132

iv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
VI. MELODIC TENDENCIES.......................... . 144

Individual Pitches ....................... 151


Consecutive-pair Patterns................... 158
Nonconsecutive Pairs ..................... 169
Intervals................................... 174

VII. D R A M A .......................................... 179

1. Introduction......................... 180
2. R o m e o ............................... 181
3. The Street Wakens, 4. MorningDance . . 183
5. The Q u a r r e l ......................... 186
6. The F i g h t ........................... 187
7. The Duke's Command, 8. Interlude. . . . 188
9.Preparations for the Ball (Juliet and
N u r s e ) ................................. 190
10. The Young J u l i e t ...................... 190
11. Arrival of the Guests.................. 193
12. Masks................................... 193
13. Dance of the Knights, 14. Juliet's
Variation............................... 195
15. M e r c u t i o ............................... 197
16. M a d r i g a l ............................... 197
17. Tybalt Recognizes Romeo, 18. Gavotte
(Departure of Guests.) .............. 199
19. Balcony Scene, 20. Romeo's Variation,
21. Love D a n c e ........................ 200
22. Folk D a n c e ............................. 205
23. Romeo and Mercutio, 24. Dance of the
Five Couples, 25.Dance withMandolins 206
26. Nurse, 27. The Nurse Delivers Romeo a
Note from J u l i e t ................... 207
28. Romeo at Friar Laurence's, 29. Juliet
at Friar Laurence's, 30. Public
Merrymaking, 31. Further Public
Festivities............................. 208
32. Meeting of Tybalt and Mercutio, 33.
Tybalt and Mercutio Fight, 34.
Mercutio Dies, 35. Romeo Decides to
Avenge the Death of Mercutio, 36.
Finale of the Second A c t ........... 209
37. Introduction, 38. Romeo and Juliet,
39. Romeo bids Juliet Farewell. . . . 212
40. Nurse, 41. JulietRefuses to Marry
Paris, 42. Juliet Alone,42.Interlude 213
44. At Friar Laurence's C e l l .............. 214
45. Interlude, 46. Juliet's Room, 47.
Juliet Alone ......................... 215
48. Morning Serenade, 49. Dance of the
Girls with Lilies................... 216

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
50. At Juliet's Bedside, 51. Juliet's
Funeral, 52. Juliet's Death.............217

CONCLUSION................................................220

APPENDIX A. LOCATION OF CADENCE FIGURES USED FOR


STUDY IN CHAPTER I I I ..................... 223

APPENDIX B. LOCATION OF MELODIES USED FOR STUDY IN


CHAPTER V I ............................... 225

SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY................ 226

vi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1. Frequency distribution of pitch classes in


melodies 1-37.................................... 152

2. Frequency distribution of pitch-classes by mode 154

3. Comparison of pc-probability distributions for


Schubert and Prokofiev.............. .... 155

4. Relative frequency of each pitch in Prokofiev


as compared with use by Schubert................. 158

5. Frequencies of consecutive pairs in Schubert


and Prokofiev.....................................160

6. Frequencies consecutive-pair combinations in


melodies 1-37.................................... 162

7. Frequencies of pc pairs with one note


intervening...................................... 170

8. Frequencies of pc pairs with zero or one note


intervening.......................................170

9. Frequencies of intervals formed by consecutive


notes, melodies 1-37............................. 175

vii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
LIST OF EXAMPLES

Example Page

1. Cadence formula common in third-mode chants. . 11

2. Cadence formula common in sixteenth-century


polyphony...................................... 12

3. Melodic direction leading toward cadence in


"America.” ................................... 13

4. Sixteenth-century passage in which no early


anticipation of cadenceis felt................. 15

5. Sixteenth-century hymn in which early


anticipation of cadenceis felt................. 16

6. Regular phrase length in Praetorius dance. . . 18

7. Root position tonic triad (marked with


asterisk) that is unstable..................... 19

8. Phrase in which the most dissonant chord is


also the most stable........................... 20

9. Passage in which tonic is sensed although no


tonic triad is present......................... 23

10. Chorale ending with sonority other than the


tonic triad.................................... 23

11. Sixteenth-century passage in which Ionian mode


is not sufficient for sensation of tonality. . 24

12. Eighteenth-century passage in which change of


scale brings about change of tonic. . . . . . . 25

13. Seventeenth-century passage in which dominant


is goal of first modulation.................... 27

14. Establishment of meter by dissonance pattern in


178:1-16....................................... 33

15. Establishment of meter by harmonic rhythm in


63:10-64:9..................................... 34

viii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
16. Melody that presents no measure-counting
problems in 18:10-19:8......................... 36

17. Melody that presents some measure-counting


problems in 9:1-11:1.................. 37

18. Articulation of phrases by caesura in 1:2-12. . 39

19. Articulation of phrases by change of idea in


116:6-117:12.......................... 41

20. Articulation of phrases by combination of


features in 50:1-8............................. 43

21. Renotation of 142:5-12 in 307:7-10. (The latter


transposed.) ................................. 46

22. Special significance as a result of expectation


of cadence in 122:1-123:1........... 50

23. Two versions of "Gavotte" cadence.............. 52

24. Irregular four-measure phrase in 60:1-4. ... 53

25. Five-measure phrase in 276:1-277:1............. 56

26. Eleven-measure phrase in 174:7-19.............. 57

27. Delayed resolution of tonic in a) 6:9-7:1 and


b) 103:8-9..................................... 61

28. Triad with added fourth scale degree as last


sonority in cadence in 67:9-10................ 61

29. Prominent §-to-l melodic motion in a) 8:11-12


and b) 107:15-17............................... 63

30. Various sonorities used as dominant


penultimates................................... 64

31. Use of the leading tone in cadential dominants


in a) 50:4 and b) 176:7-8...................... 65

32. Cadential dominant with neither the subtonic


nor the leading tone in 25:12-14.............. 65

33. Parallel motion by step over tonic bass in


2:11-12........................................ 67

34. Use of 2. in cadential dominant in 48:4......... 67

ix

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
35. Use of 5 in cadential dominants in a) 60:3-4
and b) 58:8.................................... 68

36. Use of & and 9 in cadential dominants in a)


145:7-8, b) 83:12, and c) 67:12-13............ 69

37. Use of 3 and 6 in cadential dominants in a)


206:4, b) 85:7-8, and c) 2:15-16.............. 70

38. Use of 10 in cadential dominants in a) 306:9-10


and b) Beginning: 3-4........................... 72

39. Use of 4 in cadential dominants in a) 111:7-8,


b) 16:4, and c) 121:4-6....................... 73

40. Leaps in all voices at cadence in 206:10. . . . 74

41. Major-third skip at cadences in a) 106:7-8, b)


203:6, and c) 50:2............................. 75

42. Infrequent cadential root relationships in a)


55:8-9, b) 363:10-11, and c) 3:13............. 76

43. Cadences that may be interpreted as authentic


or nonauthentic in a) 5:6-7, and b) 75:7-8. . . 77

44. Half cadences in a) 207:4 and b) 210:8......... 78

45. Dominant sonorities leading to first harmony of


phrase in a) 140:18-141:1, b) 2:17-3:1, c)
27:12-28:1, and d) 84:8-85:1.................. 79

46. Resolution of a dominant seventh used to


establish tonic in 110:16-111:8............. 85

47. Triad repetition used to establish tonic in a)


9:1-4, b) 76:1, and c) 247:1................ 86

48. Pedal point used to establish tonic in 26:1-4. 87

49. Emphasis on perfect fifth used to establish


tonic in 97:1-5................................ 88

50. Diatonic scale used to establish tonic in


Beginning: 1-1:1.............................. 89

51. Highly chromatic melody in 2:5-12.............. 92

52. Multiple scale shifts in quick succession in


6:1-7:1........................................ 97

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
53. Scale shift accomplished with one note in 49:8-
9; a) score, b) diagram of implied changes...101

54. Scale shift accomplished with one note in 50:7-


8; a) score, b) diagram of implied changes...102

55. Root relationships at scale shifts in a)


122:13-123:1, b) 49:8-9, c) 50:8, and d) 152:8-
153:1.............................................104

56. Cadential shift from scale on 1 in 151:10-


153:1.............................................105

57. Scale shift before authentic-cadence figure at


beginning of a phrase in 26:7-27:2............ 106

58. Scale shift before authentic-cadence figure at


beginning of a phrase in 73:1-74:1............ 106

59. Scale shifts before authentic-cadence figures


in transitional passage, measures 144:3-9. . . 108

60. First-phrase scale shifts moving through a


triad on 8 in 71:1-72:8.......................... 110

61. Two successive shifts to chord on 8 in 290:11-


291:4............................................. 113

62. Scale shift to 8 starting second phrase of a


melody in 213:11-214:8........................... 114

63. Scale shift to £ during second phrase of a


melody in 201:1-4................................ 115

64. Chromatic modulation in 60:5-11.................. 119

65. Modulation with no change of scale in 207:1-4. 120

66. Dramatic use of modulation in 360:1-362:5. . . 123

67. Traditional descending structure in 1:5-8. . . 127

68. Traditional descending structure in 1:13-20. . 127

69. Traditional descending structure in 151:5-


152:1............................................. 128

70. Descent from Z in Beginning: 1-4.................. 129

71. Interruption on 3 in 3:1-13...................... 130

xi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
72. Interruption on 3 in 18:5-19:8................131

73. Structural descent ending on § in 55:1-6. . . . 132

74. Structural descent in lowest line in 107:1-17. 133

75. Nontraditional structure in 6:3-7:1.......... 135

76. Nontraditional structure ii. 111:1-8.......... 136

77. Nontraditional structure in 87:10-89:10. . . . 138

78. Structure with reharmonized chord tones in


122:1-123:1................................. 140

79. Structure with reharmonized chord tones in


84:1-85:8................................... 142

80. Abundance of fourths and fifths in 151:1-152:1. 177

81. Ambiguous meter in 5:1-5...................... 182

82. Sunrise motive in 8:10-13..................... 183

83. Metrical comparison of 10:1-10 with 11:3-10. . 185

84. Modulating melody in 16:1-4...................186

85. Dissonances in 44:1-45:1..................... 189

86. Nurse's humorous character in 47:1-48:4. . . . 191

87. 93:1-7 as metrical transformation of 2:6-12.


(Latter transposed.) ......................... 196

88. Contrasting moods in 114:1-116:1. (Each three-


measure group corresponding to a single measure
at rehearsal 63 bracketed.)................. 201

89. Early shift and normal cadence in 133:5-8. . . 202

90. 139:1-7 as metrical transformation of 111:1-4.


(Latter transposed.) ......................... 203

91. Asymmetry within standard length in 142:4-12. . 204

92. Chromatic embellishment of vii°/V in 208:5-8. . 208

93. Minor mode as foreboding of death in 247:1-16. 210

xii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
94. Juxtaposition of two minor triads a third apart
as signal of death in 263:4-8.................. 212

95. Death and poison motives in 313:10-314:6. . . . 215

xiii

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
PREFACE

The choice of Prokofiev's tonal style as the topic for

this thesis stems from my'first experience of this music:

although it was like nothing else I had ever heard, it still

seemed to make sense to me in some indescribable way< The

subjects of my research, then, were at first largely my own

mind and my own listening processes. During the course of

this study, I realized that the forces that made the style

tonal were what had prompted a degree of intuitive

understanding so many years ago. My description of the

style, therefore, centers on those aspects that interest me

the most, that is, those that seem to me to contribute to

its tonal nature.

I begin with a definition of tonality, a word I use to

refer to a phenomenon I experience, and have experienced for

as long as I can remember clearly, when listening to certain

pieces of music, and certain pieces only. I experience it

when listening to Haydn, but not Josquin; when listening to

eighteenth-century hymns, but not chant; when listening to

American folk music, but not Indonesian music; when

listening to some Prokofiev, but not to Bart6k. I do not

deny the possibility that medieval monks did or that

present-day Indonesians do experience a somewhat analogous

xiv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
sensation, but it cannot be the same. The phenomenon I know

has at once to do with time and with pitch. It is a feeling

of knowing where in the progress of both those domains a

piece is at a given moment. It is the feeling that the

cadence of a period is the result of the key, not vice

versa, that the cadence is heard as resolution or not in

light of the key perceived possibly from the very start of

the period. I have evidence of two kinds that I am not

alone in having this experience. First, many people have

composed in such a way that their music not only elicits the

response, but toys with it in fascinating ways. Second,

other people have written or spoken of such a sensation,

although as a rule in terms that, in my opinion, emphasize

pitch over time to an excessive degree. I refer to the

sensation as tonality because I learned to do so at an early

point in my musical education. While many people use the

word in the same way, others have a broader use of the word,

applying it to all eight repertories listed above. I ask

readers of this latter group to bear with me as I use the

word in a way with which they may be uncomfortable.

After defining tonality, I give the reader an idea of a

form, the four-measure phrase, and then describe the

contents, filling the form first with broad strokes by

defining cadence patterns and the use of scales as pitch

sources, and then with more detail by means of a description

of melodic structure and pitch tendencies.

xv

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
I originally intended only to begin ray study with Romeo

and Juliet because of its many short, closed forms and the

ample opportunity they provided for the study of, for

instance, Prokofiev's cadences. It soon became clear,

however, that a thorough study of just the one piece would

be quite a large project. All examples after Chapter I are

taken from Romeo and Juliet, and the final chapter gives an

account of the way Prokofiev uses the features of this style

to contribute to the telling of that story.

I wish to express my thanks to my two successive thesis

advisors, Dr. Richard Hervig and Dr. W. T. Atcherson, for

their invaluable assistance and direction; to an unknown

teaching assistant from the University of Illinois who, in

the one presentation of his I was fortunate enough to

attend, explained the importance of the four-measure phrase

in the history of tonality; to Julie Sams who typed my

master's thesis without written acknowledgment only because

I was too embarrassed to have her type her own thanks; to my

Mom and Dad for the computers; and to my wife, Nancy, for

being patient and understanding, for working my way through

college, and for all that typing.

xvi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
CHAPTER I

TONALITY

Many of the works of Prokofiev, Romeo and Juliet among

them, are commonly deemed to be tonal. Most pieces have key

signatures, and many pieces' titles carry the name of a key.

Despite features associated with other music from the post-

tonal era such as dissonant harmonies, highly chromatic

melodies, and odd root relationships, the music still seems

to have tonality, in the sense of the word that is opposed

to that of modality or even pitch centricity. In attempting

a stylistic account of a piece of music by Prokofiev such as

Romeo and Juliet, one needs a definition of tonality that

captures its essential nature, independent of the stylistic

features particular to the Common-Practice Period.

Tonality has been defined in various ways. Many

writers, including Apel in the Harvard Dictionary and

Crocker in A History of Musical Style, basically agree with

Schoenberg's definition of tonality as "the art of combining

tones in such successions and harmonies or successions of

harmonies, that the relation of all events to a fundamental

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
tone is made possible”1 While Apel and Crocker do not

explain exactly what this relationship is, it is probable

that they have in mind a hierarchical relationship such as

the one described elsewhere by Schoenberg in which each tone

is said to be more or less closely related to the tonic and

thus more or less powerful in supporting the preeminence of

that pitch. Schenker has explained that these relationships

may be expressed as both local-level and large-scale

structures, and that the large-scale structures are the most

essential to the state of tonality of a piece.

Many writers agree that tonality has to do with such

relationships. But stylistic variety among pieces said to

have tonality, even as opposed to mere pitch centricity,

makes it impossible to determine precisely what these rela­

tionships are, giving reason to believe that the hierarchy

peculiar to the manifestation of tonality of the eighteenth

and nineteenth centuries may not constitute a necessary part

of tonality per se. Hindemith, in his Series 1, attempts to

provide an acoustically justified series in which, using C

as a model tonic, each of the other eleven tones is given a

specific position relative to the others according to its

"Willi Apel, Harvard Dictionary of Music. 2nd ed.


(Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press, 1969), s.v. "Tonality";
Richard L. Crocker, A History of Musical Style (New York;
McGraw-Hill, 1966), 356-57; Arnold Schoenberg et al,,
Schoenberg. ed. Merle Armitage (New York: G. Schirmer,
1937; reprint ed., Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1977), p.
280.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
3

distance, when used as a chord root, from the tonic pitch.2

But such a series does not represent necessary truths about

tonality since some of the orderings are contradicted by the

facts of actual pieces from the tonal literature. For

instance, in Series 1, the third degree of the major scale

is placed ahead of the second, whereas many pieces demon­

strate that the second degree is in fact the closer of the

two to the tonic. There seems no reason that other pitches

as well might not occupy opposite positions in certain tonal

styles, in some Western popular music of the 1960's and

1970's, for instance, the fourth degree is consistently used

as a closer relative of the tonic than the fifth.

Vincent Persichetti's conception of tonality allows

room for variations on the Common-Practice-Period norms,

presumably so that the category "tonal music" might include

more twentieth-century music. He describes tonality as a

structural concern involving the support of a tonic by any

two other pitches equally or nearly equally distant above

and below it.3 Thus, pitches do not possess tonal strength

because of intrinsically better intervals formed by them and

the tonic, but acquire strength simply by repeated use.

Persichetti's notion has not been widely accepted perhaps

'Paul Hindemith, The Craft of Musical Composition,


book 1: Theoretical Part, trans. Arthur Mendel, 4th ed.
(Mainz: Schott, 1970), p. 56.

'Vincent Persichetti, Twentieth-Centurv Harmony (New


York: W. W. Norton, 1961), pp. 248-49.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
4

because he gives up a priori acoustical justification for

the most closely related pitches, only to include structures

that have no empirical basis in the literature.

Concepts of tonality based on the architectural

structures of eighteenth- and nineteenth-century music are

inappropriate for a general definition because they exclude

many more recent pieces that are said to be tonal. A scheme

such as that of Persichetti, while broader, is not much

better. Perhaps the essence of tonality is not to be found

in architectural structures.

Architectural frameworks of this kind seem to me to be

based on a local-level phenomenon evoked by a familiarity

with relevant features of certain pieces of music. This

phenomenon involves the perception of individual sonorities

as filling certain roles in the temporal unfolding of the

music on the relatively local level of the phrase or the

period. When a succession of sonorities, be they single

notes or simultaneous combinations thereof, is perceived to

be part of a pattern considered standard, the listener

familiar with the pattern is able to project mentally its

continuation and completion. In traditional tonal music,

these patterns have at once to do with meter and with pitch,

so that the listener, near the beginning or middle of a

phrase, is able to project both the metrical placement and

the pitch of the supposedly approaching closing figure. As

an example, a hypothetical Western listener for some reason

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
5

unfamiliar with "Swanee River” will, by a familiarity with

scale forms, be able to determine quite early in the first

phrase the tonic pitch. Upon hearing the first phrase end

on the second scale degree and the second phrase begin the

same as the first, he can surmise that the second phrase

will cadence on the tonic pitch at the downbeat of the

fourth measure. While such a sense is usually referred to

in astronomical figures of speech, as Persichetti does when

he speaks of the "pull of the tonal center,"4 it is in fact

a subjective phenomenon, a factor in the listener's

interpretation of the rhythms and pitches, not a virtue of

the notes themselves.

Rather than imagining all the intervening steps between

the sonority heard at a given moment and the ultimate

resolution of the passage, the listener actually probably

only notes his familiarity with a possible, standard

continuation of the pattern. To say that a person listening

to a piece of tonal music is able to project the normal

continuation of a standard pattern is not to say that he

listens to the piece while making predictions about the

actual outcome of each passage. Rather, these projections,

normally unintentional and unnoticed, serve as references in

the context of which actual events are heard and by which

they are colored. Indeed, theorists such as Leonard B.

4Ibid., p. 248.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
6

Meyer and Hans Keller have argued that the essence of art

music lies in the contradiction between expectations and

actual musical events.5 Because such projections are made,

the role of a perceived sonority may be interpreted either

as the conclusion of such a pattern, in which case it sounds

stable, or as one of several hierarchically arranged steps

leading to that conclusion, in which case it sounds

unstable. In this thesis, the term "tonality" refers to a

sense the listener has of the degree of stability of a

sonority and, in the case of an unstable sonority, of the

amr ; of musical time required to reach a stable sonority.

Such a definition is not without difficulties.

Inherent in the subjective nature of the definition is the

possibility that a statement concerning the tonality of any

particular passage may not be relevant to the experience of

any listener other than the one making the statement.

Differences in physical, mental, or emotional state may

affect a single listener's ideas about or ability to make

projections, as well as his judgment of a passage's level of

conformity to such a projection. How much more might the

differences be between two different listeners, people of

different preferences, mental capabilities, or levels of

experience with a given style?

"Leonard B. Meyer, Music. The Arts, and Ideas


(Chicago; University of Chicago Press, 1S67), pp. 5-41
passim; Hans Keller, "The Nature of Musical Understanding,"
Musical Newsletter 4 (October 1973)-.13-19.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
7

A second difficulty has to do with human nature's

inclination to categorization. We are used to putting

labels such as "tonal," "modal," and "atonal" on pieces. A

subjective definition of tonality, however, makes the music

not the bearer of tonality, but a partial cause. Is a piece

of music tonal only when heard by the right person? In the

case of a rock song that seems to emphasize a certain pitch

as a tonal center but does not end with that pitch (or fades

out without ending on any pitch), is the piece tonal to a

person who expects a particular ending, even though that

ending is not an element of the style and indeed does not

occur in this particular case? Is it atonal because it does

not resolve?

A final area of difficulty, always present in

discussions about perceptions, is that of epistemological

skepticism. It is always possible to ask whether what one

perceives exists objectively, exists physically, or appears

the same to other observers, indeed whether other observers

exist. While the issues are certainly important to the

topic at hand, it is unnecessary and unfruitful to deal with

them every time one discusses something. In this thesis,

the objective existence of perceived objects and a basic

consistency of perception by separate observers is assumed.

Such a definition is not without difficulties. It

does, however, grant music its true nature, that of a

dynamic human activity, not a static artifact. Music is to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
be heard. Musical composition, in the traditional sense of

the word, involves the conscious, intentional arrangement of

sounds, whether in advance or in real time, in such a way

that the result is interesting to the composer as a

listener. (And in most cases it is to be supposed that the

composer hopes the result might be interesting to other

listeners as well.) If music says nothing else, it says to

the listener, even if the listener is only the composer

himself, '‘The composer wanted this to sound this way.”6 The

listener may be pleased with the choices made, uninterested,

unable to understand them, or convinced the composer was in

some way wrong in making them. These intentions, state­

ments, and responses are all part of the musical experience.

The truly subjective nature of tonality may be less

disturbing when viewed as an element of this communicative

process, as a part of the listener's response to a sound

composition, as a means of connecting various events in the

composition and of understanding, or attempting to

understand, the choice of events made by the composer. The

discussion of tonality necessarily risks difference of

‘Those who in recent times have composed in such a


fashion that the sounds produced in a performance are only
accidents of the process have not made music, in the strict
sense of the word. Such activity is indeed art, but the
art is of a mixed nature to which the term "music" does not
exactly apply. There is an element of poetry involved in
calling such art "music," and there is a strong element of
a possibly new art of which aesthetic arguments themselves
are the stuff.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
9

opinion. But we need not avoid discussion because of

difference of opinion, the instigator and sustainer of many

of the most interesting and enlightening conversations.

Tonality, then, is a subjective phenomenon. There is

usually, however, a rather high level of agreement among

knowledgable listeners concerning any given passage or

piece. As indicated above, the state of tonality of a piece

is dependent on certain features present in the piece, and

the understanding a listener has of these features as

stylistic norms. The fact that it is a group of common

features that contributes to this sense means that they may

be known by most listeners, and that the range of

interpretation of these features in any particular case is

probably quite small. A certain passage by Brahms, for

instance, might be interpreted as being in, say, D-flat

major or B-flat minor, but the same passage could probably

not be interpreted by a knowledgeable student of the score

as being in, for instance, c major or F-sharp minor.

Because of this high level of agreement, we may continue the

convenient practice of ascribing the condition of tonality

to the music itself, with the understanding that we speak

figuratively when we label music in this way.

While it is impossible to know when listeners began to

add the subjective layer of tonality to their perception of

music, it is possible to point to some relatively early

examples of music that support a tonal interpretation today.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
10

The features forming the basis of the tonal system of

western music begin occasionally to appear in combination in

the music of the middle ages, but gradually become standard

during the course of the sixteenth and seventeenth

centuries. Let us examine some of these features, beginning

with those that contribute to an anticipatory sense about

the metrical placement of endings, continuing then with

those relevant to the pitch of these endings.

Sl.qna_ls_o.f Metrical Placement of Approaching Endings

In order to have ideas about an ending, the listener

must know what constitutes an ending in a given style. An

ending may for various reasons be perceived as such after

the fact. For instance, the end of a group of notes might

be perceived as such by virtue of a relatively long temporal

separation between two notes, leaving the previous,

relatively proximate notes to be perceived as a group.

Because this grouping by proximity is a natural part of the

human perceptual process, this principle of closure might

apply to practically any style of music. Indeed this

principle plays a part in the formal structure of the chant,

secular monophony, and polyphonic music of the West up to

the sixteenth century. However, rhythmic caesura alone can

only alert the listener to the presence of closure as the

caesura is occurring, that is, after the last note of the

formal group has been sounded. More is needed if events

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
11

during a passage are to be heard as anticipatory of the

approaching end.

Something more is in fact provided by cadence formulas,

present in most of the early repertory mentioned above, and

in most later Western music as well. The presence of new or

unexpected material even at the end of a passage could make

that ending appear abrupt and unresolved. Perhaps because

these characteristics are felt to be incompatible with the

belief, more common in the past, that music should or does

represent the innate order of the universe, human soul, or

natural world, endings in many styles in the history of our

culture have been standardized. These common patterns at

endings, known to be endings by virtue of the rhythmic

caesura, are quickly recognized as signals of the imminence

of a degree of rhythmic closure. Thus, a listener, upon

hearing the initiation of one of these patterns, can

mentally project the continuation of the pattern and sense

the time of the approaching cadence. When the figure in

example l, a common cadential figure in third-mode chants,

Example 1. Cadence formula common in third-mode chants.

begins, the listener familiar with the function of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
12

figure senses the approach of the final note. Similarly, as

soon as the dissonance of the 7-6 suspension in example 2

Example 2. Cadence formula common in sixteenth-century


polyphony.

is heard, the listener familiar with this style can sense

that the seventh will resolve to a sixth on the next beat,

and that the sixth will in turn resolve to an octave one

beat after that.

Again, these resolutions may not necessarily occur in

the expected fashion. But the deceptive, evasive treatment

of a cadence formula will be heard in the context of the

expectations. Zarlino, for instance, speaks of such

evasions as modifications of cadences, although no cadence

occurs, because "the voices give the impression of leading

to a perfect cadence."7 This interpretation would apply

even though the evasions, were Zarlino/s advice to be

followed, might actually occur more frequently than what is

considered the normal consequence. The concern here is with

patterns that seem at the time to be leading toward certain

7Gioseffo Zarlino, The Art of Counterpoint, trans. Guy


A. Marco and Claude V. Palisca (New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press, 1968; reprint ed., New York: Da Cape,
1983), p. 151.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
13

goals, whether or not those goals actually arrive in any

particular case.

In both the cadence formulas described above, of

course, the pitch of the final note as well as its metrical

placement is anticipated, a matter to be discussed below.

Also present in both examples is a text, which might aid the

listener familiar with the text, or at least the language,

in sensing the approaching cadence. The increasing

acceptance of instrumental music during the sixteenth,

seventeenth, and eighteenth centuries, and its gradual

divorce from the voice and vocal models in part necessi­

tated, or at least accelerated, the emergence of tonality as

a purely musical means of giving music temporal direction.

Another factor often present when the approaching end

of a phrase can be anticipated is melodic direction. In the

second phrase of "America" ("God save the Queen"), a pattern

of descent toward the tonic pitch is established by the

series of pitches on successive alternate downbeats. (See

example 3.) Beginning on the fifth scale degree, the

Example 3. Melodic direction leading toward cadence in


"America."

f------------- J .'" T " — ------- 1 H ' l

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
14

pattern is broken after the third degree is reached. When

the return to the third degree is followed immediately by

the second, the metrical placement of the tonic pitch on the

next downbeat is sensed as almost inevitable.

While cadential formulas and melodic direction add some

degree of separation between the time an ending is first

sensed and the actual occurrence of the ending, the length

of this separation is still relatively small. The kind of

anticipation intended by the use of the term "tonality" in

this thesis is of a slightly larger scale. The key musical

feature to a response of this type is regularity of phrase

length. This regularity can be found in a least two kinds

of music in the sixteenth century: dances and strophic

chorale-texture hymns or songs.

In the Gombert passage given in example 4,6 not likely

to be considered tonal, no expectation of a cadence is

experienced until the dissonance of the suspension occurs,

in the eighth notated measure. The lack of metrical

regularity at any level higher than the tactus makes it

impossible to anticipate which tactus will bear the cadence.

And without a sense concerning the metrical placement of the

approaching cadence, the listener cannot anticipate its

pitch either. The sonority at the beginning of the seventh

“Nikolaus Gombert, "Super Flumina," measures 1-26, in


Historical Anthology of Music, revised ed., edited by
Archibald T. Davison and Willi Apel, vol. i (Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press, 1949), p. 118.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
15

Example 4. Sixteenth-century passage in which no early


anticipation of cadence is felt.

____________ Sm- y*rflumina B.-ln- l


a-nit, Bt- K- lo-m » ______ It-tic ft.

9u- ptr LLL~ * at p J *r


«ti«, a*. tV tt
h'h’£-1^, 4^
flumi-

[
44-A-- •w r w ^ r
i £lt-«{. mui, tiir— tedr rri
imut,
" rp
u-
A f "P-A. - I. iJ—■■J. iip
iJ .
1 M &i-bi-t#- nit, B*-bi L«mut £Uvimut,
<U- nt- «r, Z-
te-i-----r
t z : rt.ar-it- 1L l l L t" t. 1 LJ..f
dum.**
f LFl f / H
j*... i i A
1J J 4-J.J-J =j— h-=d = — ■
■1«d
umr€-arr-<u- «*- w , aCm. IrLl If

notated measure is not heard as a ii chord of some kind in a

G-Dorian tonality. Calling a harmony a "ii chord" means (1)

that the tonic pitch is known and (2) that what happens

after that harmony is heard in light of what normally

happens after a ii chord. The sonority here is heard only

as the result of lines that happen to sound an a-c-e

sonority at that point.

The hymn given in example 5, however, a hymn from the

Bohemian Brethren's KirchengesSnge of 1566/ is tonal: e-

flat is sensed as the pitch of ultimate stability at every

s,*Mit Freuden z-art," arr. Ruth E. Weyforight, English


text by W. Russell Bowie, printed in The Brethren Hymnal
(Elgin, IL: House of the Church of Brethren, 1951), no. 366.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
16

Example 5. Sixteenth-century hymn in which early


anticipation of cadence is felt.

W.RumU Bowie; b. 1M1 t. by Ruth E. W«ybd*hci>. 1900

j-j IjJj
1. Lord Christ, when first Thou cam’st to men,
"T
Up - on
1
■ cron they
2. O awe-ful love, which found no room i_
... e. . j .____ _j_
u_________________
3. New ad-vent of the love of Christ, Shall we a-g a in re -
UO wound-edhands of Je-sus,build In us Thy new ere-

r r 1

bound Thee, And mocked Thy sav - ing king-ship then


nied Thee. And, doomed to death, must bring to doom power which
fuse Thee. Till in the night of hate and war per- ish
war. Thy

J -I I j - I I

they crowned Thee: And still our wrongs may weave Thee
d - fed Thee, Till not a stone was left on s

- ver loss. We

thorns to pierce that stead-y brow. And robe of sor - row round Thee,
all a na-don s pride, o'er-thrown. Went down to dust be - side Thee1 ,
seek theking-dom of Thy peace, By which a - lone we choose Thee,
bring our hearts be-fore Thy cross. To fin-ish Thy sal - va - ticn. A - men.

J - a k U U .J. Jr-l Jr-Vi

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
17

moment in the piece, and the regularity of the groups of two

and of four measures enables anticipation of the metrical

placement of the cadences. Here the harmony on the first

beat of measure 3 is heard as a ii chord: it operates func­

tionally by preparing the V that in turn leads to the tonic

of the cadence. The pitch and time of the first cadence are

felt from the moment this harmony is sounded.

Similarly, many dances of the sixteenth and seventeenth

centuries appear tonal largely as a result of their regular

phrase lengths. In the Praetorius dance given in example

6,10 the unvarying use of four-measure phrases keeps the

approaching end of each phrase (or at least the point of

departure of a new phrase, as at measure 5) within the

listener's awareness at all times. And g-natural is the

expected and actual ultimate note of resolution. This is

true even in the second line, where f-naturals "spoil" the

Ionian-mode tone system.

These examples suggest that tonality is primarily a

metrical phenomenon. It is the presence of metrical

regularity— not a specific scale type, not the V-I cadence,

not the "suavity of the harmonic progressions" suggested by

10Michael Praetorius, Bransle, excerpts, in


Geaamta.us.g.abe..der-Husikalischen -Werke vQn.Michael
Praetorius. ed. Friedrich Blume, vol. 15 (Wolfenbiittel:
Georg Kallmeyer, 1929), pp. 22-23.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
18

Example 6. Regular phrase length in Praetorius dance.

Crocker11— that makes certain members of a group of pieces of

an otherwise somewhat similar style appear tonal. Caesura

is sufficient to create the sense of cadence, and regular

phrase length is sufficient to allow anticipation of the

cadence, the foundation of tonality.

The anticipation from early in a phrase of that

phrase's cadence colors all intervening sonorities with a

degree of imperfection, that word being used in the sense of

“Crocker, HistQgy Qf Musical style, p. 277.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
19

"incompleteness." Thus, while major triads on the fourth

and fifth degrees may be as consonant as the tonic triad,

they nevertheless sound secondary to it by virtue of their

function not as final harmonies, but as sonorities which

lead to that final harmony. (It is a hierarchical

relationship such as this, perhaps, that Crocker and Apel

have in mind in their epithets about tonality.) Two cases

are sufficient to illustrate the point. First, even the

tonic chord in root position can be heard as unstable if it

is perceived to occur in the middle of a phrase. (See

example 7.1J) Second, in some contemporary popular styles, a

Example 7. Root position tonic triad (marked with asterisk)


that is unstable.

chord will sound stable when its placement in the metrical

context is that of a cadential sonority, even when the chord

is as dissonant or more dissonant than other harmonies in

the phrase. (See example 8.) While music such as these

dances and the hymn loses a degree of subtlety as a result

“J. S. Bach, "Wie schon leuchtet der Morgenstern,"


measures 1-6, in 371 Harmonized Chorales, ed. Albert
Riemenschneider (New York: G.Schirmer, 1941), no. 86.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
20

Example 8. Phrase in which the most dissonant chord is also


the most stable.

of its abandonment of metrical irregularity, it gains a

level of richness not present in other music of the period.

Signals of the Pitch of Approaching Endings

Although I have described tonality as a fundamentally

metrical phenomenon, it is the domain of pitch that receives

most of the attention in most definitions and discussions of

the concept. (The use of the word "tonic" for the most

stable pitch in a key indicates the consensus that tonality

is primarily a matter of pitch.) While the importance of

pitch to tonality has been exaggerated to the point of

excluding meter altogether in most of these discussions,

pitch does play an important role. In the Western music of

the sixteenth through the early nineteenth centuries, scale

types and melodic and harmonic patterns were limited to the

point that tonality was able to develop as a sense

concerning the pitch as well as the metrical position of a

passage's approaching close.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
21

Here it is well to stress again the difference between

the projection of standard patterns to what would be their

normal conclusions and the prediction of actual musical

events. It is clear that the actual ending of a passage

need not correspond to what is anticipated. The ending

itself, a future event to someone listening to a passage,

has no power whatsoever over the listener's perceptions of

the passage. Suppose a man walking down a hall passes a

room and hears a voice from the room reciting at a steady

pace, ::P, Q, R, S, T," after which the man has passed far

enough that he cannot hear whether the voice is continuing.

He can nevertheless sense with some accuracy how much time

would pass until the letter "Z" would be said should the

pattern continue, whether or not that letter is actually

said. One need not hear the entire alphabet recited in

order to recognize a sequence of letters as a part of the

alphabet. After the possibility of the perception of

tonality is historically or personally established, all that

is essential to the listener's interpretation of a passage

as being tonal is that the passage conform, to some degree,

to a style that is considered standard tonal style. When

this is true, a passage is heard as tonal, no matter what

the ending.

This principle accepted, it can be seen that when

standard patterns of a tonal style (in classical tonality,

these are outlines of triads, emphasis on two pitches a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
fifth apart, etc.) are used, the tonic pitch, or standard

final pitch of resolution, of a piece may be known at almost

any moment. In fact, in order for a piece to be truly

tonal, this pitch must be known at an early point, and

sensed rather often during the piece. As Carl Dahlhaus

says, "It is not enough to settle on a final note: it must

be the centre of reference of the whole piece, so that, for

instance, the note A receives its 'colouring' or 'character'

in the mode on E primarily through its association with E."13

In the Brahms Intermezzo partially reproduced in example 9,14

b-flat is perceived as the tonic from the very first

measure, even though a B-flat harmony is not present until

the very end of the piece. In some instances, the identity

of the tonic pitch is quite clear throughout the piece, and

yet it is not the root of the final sonority. The very fact

that the ending of the chorale in example 10“ is heard as an

incomplete tonal ending and not a complete modal ending,

that it is heard as an exception to or comment on the

standards of a style, points to the nature of tonality as a

subjective sense concerning the standard direction suggested

by sonorities regardless of actual outcomes.

“Ihe-New -Grove Pi ct ionarvLofMusic and Musicians, ed.


Stanley Sadie, s.v. "Tonality," by Carl Dahlhaus.

14Johannes Brahms, Intermezzo, Op. 76, no. 4, measures


1=7, ed. Eugen D'Albert (Berlin: Uiistein, 1928), p. 16.

“Bach, "Komm, Gott Schdpfer," Choralesf no. 187.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 9. Passage in which tonic is sensed although no
tonic triad is present.

ft'

P ^ |R T ^ | p p |n7]r^

Example 10. Chorale ending with sonority other than the


tonic triad.

IH7. j
*
35 LLf— r ••r—
*

tr i
*
JLljj- n A A J . ^
=

p. pi
/ u a jt “u
u r
lr r ' l l~rxr

j .j. jO-A-
e/r f■r='T'r r

That the use of diatonic scales is not sufficient for

the establishment of a tonal style is made evident by the

vast number of diatonic chants and polyphonic pieces

considered modal. Not even the use of the so-called Ionian

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
24

mode, recognized by Glarean and Zarlino, is sufficient, as

is made evident by the Encina villancico reproduced in

example 11.16 Here one cannot sense the approach of the

Example 11. Sixteenth-century passage in which Ionian mode


is not sufficient for sensation of tonality.

rr 'ra*-—.r

U ' afciarfimndi-ifar.amntnartu- 1
ti pttur.M mrifc-Uttimj-tu.

cadence in measure 9 until the suspension in the previous

measure begins. However, in music from after the time

stylistic patterns associated with classical tonality have

been established, the presence of a particular diatonic

scale is enough to establish a single pitch as tonic. In a

piece in a major key, for instance, the change of scale

achieved by the raising of the fourth scale degree or the

lowering of the seventh is sufficient to suggest a new key

area. In the passage from the G-minor fugue of book one of

“Juan Encina, nPues que jamds," in


vol. 1, p. 100.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
25

Das Wohltemperierte Klavier found in example 12,17 the key of

Example 12. Eighteenth-century passage in which change of


scale brings about change of tonic.

^ ; . , * _

r i** *
: . T— r T .. . J

©
— ‘|- 3 u— r--- --- <*■*-=

— 1
1 2

f r 7crCv r 1 k'zJs

B-flat having been established in measures 11 and 12, the

introduction of e-natural in measures 13 and 14 is

sufficient to suggest f-natural as a new tonic, even though

a cadence is not expected at any particular time in this

continuous style, and even though no strong cadence on f

actually occurs. In a somewhat similar fashion, the

sounding of a single dominant-seventh chord can suggest the

presence of a key simply because each dominant-seventh

harmony can be found in only one diatonic pitch collection.

It is a curious situation that the very scales and

harmonies that provide one listening to music of an earlier

17J = s = Bach, Fugue no. 16, measures 10-15, Das


Wohltemperierte Klavier. book l, ed. Otto von Irmer
(Munich: Henle, 1950), p. 80.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
26

time only a local sense of direction, are able, in music

written after the establishment of tonality through metrical

regularity, to suggest to the listener at every moment what

part that moment might play in the unfolding of the piece.

As a result, in the eighteenth century, with means of

establishing tonality by pitch patterns alone within their

grasp, composers were able to once again add interest in the

metrical domain, with results such as the apparently

seamless preludes of J. S. Bach or the irregularity in many

of the first movements of Mozart. Metrical regularity,

then, acted as a catalyst, imbuing sonorities and

progressions with hierarchical coloring and a sense of

direction and then retiring for a time, leaving tonality,

for the most part, in the care of pitch relationships.

The patterns and structures of classical tonality are

based on a local-level sense of direction. Structures on a

larger scale based on these patterns come after the

establishment through metrical regularity of standard tonal

patterns and hierarchy. It is only in the late seventeenth

century, with pieces such as the Cazzati Sonata in example

13,18 that the dominant becomes the standard goal for

modulation and thus normally the second most important

structural component of a piece. The "large-scale

18Maurizio Cazzati, "Sonata prima La Pellicana,"


measures 1-15, in Historical Anthology, vol. 2 (Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press, 1950), p. 67.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 13. Seventeenth-century passage in which dominant
is goal of first modulation.

m i i
aanr-pgprpi

| r]h> M ten* J
if » T ( >v >
ft
yfr ~ • - f r f i1 cjjU t f fir 1

rfr rf~P m f~
ipbfrrp-U U-l v
=*
: Jy .j- m 1t •prrpf' pFTpf
E.iigr p u p
J’rf l

1^1-Ph.. 1 r i i i y j 3 p .j p p.r p.r


W ' C g J p - E i p M ir <]1

J _ J
fyr n- f y"'r T ~ 3*— f rr/tir
'trcirr PttT
M ^f-F. 4 = vri'JTip > .

^ h
o i.j*T3 fflrrr * Ti r f .f,P . . a . ... ..acctfPTi-
'EErEtf'w

^ f fT= f = j■f M pu ^ v -

Ikl*.. V K m — r-l r i^rtr Pr~ffr p.p r.r P'J 1 ii P H f e -1


«■1 ; J,r > r [ I’1 i
6 6 6 6 6 #r 1 fv\ v\T* 7 Jj
p

dissonance" spoken of by Charles Rosen in his Sonata Forms.19

while an essential feature of the tonal repertoire with

which he deals, is a later development of a much humbler

notion. What concerns us here is the more basic notion of

tonality, that is, the sense from early in a phrase or

period concerning both the time and the pitch of approaching

cadences, and the means by which the establishment of a

19See Charles Rosen, Sonata Forms, revised ed. (New


York: W. W. Norton, 1988), pp. 25, 129, and passim.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
28

tonal style can be achieved. When pitch patterns unfamiliar

to the listener are involved, it is metrical regularity that

is most able to bring about a new tonal style. It will be

seen that Prokofiev imposes metrical regularity on both

traditional and nontraditional patterns, creating a new

tonality and using the old at the same time.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
29

CHAPTER II

PHRASE LENGTH

While the subject of phrase length may be an important

part of the definition of any particular style, it is

especially important in the case of a tonal style. Tonality

has been defined partially as a sense concerning the

temporal distance of an approaching goal. This sense is

such that we need not think about an approaching goal in

terms as imprecise as "near" or "far." Rather, we are often

able to sense with great exactitude the amount of time

between a present moment and the arrival of a phrase ending.

A temporal sense as precise as this is dependent on a

measured perception of time. In tonal music, this measure­

ment is not conceived of in terms of units of clock time,

but rather in terms of units of the music itself. A point

of arrival might be anticipated, for instance, on the third

beat of the second measure of a phrase. "Beat," "measure,"

and "phrase" are all terms used to denote both musical and

temporal units.

While it is clearly convenient to think of time in a

piece of tonal music in terms of musical units, it is

important to note that it is in fact necessary to do so

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
30

since these units need not correspond to constant

measurements of clock time, being perceived only in

relationship to one another. The length of a measure is

defined as a certain multiple of the length of a pulse or

beat. Thus, in a passage in which the tempo is increasing,

any given measure will be shorter in clock time than the

last. However, in such a passage, the otherwise exact

repetition of a group of measures will be heard to have the

same musical length as the original statement. Time in

tonal music, then is conceived of in terms of these relative

units: a measure is commonly thought of as a certain number

of beats, and a phrase as a certain number of measures.

Therefore, in order to discuss the length of a phrase in a

piece, it is necessary to understand how that piece's meter

is established.

Establishment of Meter

For the purposes of this discussion, the term "meter"

refers to a regularly recurring pattern of strong and weak

beats. Such regularity is essential in a style in which

expectation of the precision described above is to play a

role. Meter, in this regular sense, may be established in

several ways. A regular series of dynamic accents is

perhaps the least subtle method, and is a primary way in

which meter is established in much dance music and popular

music of this era. A regularity of tonic or agogic accents

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
31

produced by the repetitions of a motive, or even by the

succession of similar figures, may also establish a meter.

Such was the case with music based on the rhythmic modes.

In the late fourteenth century and during the fifteenth

century, as the rhythms of each voice become less

repetitive, meter come to be more of a contrapuntal

phenomenon. An accented beat at the end of the fifteenth

century is one on which a suspension may occur, and in much

of the literature of the time, this possibility occurs

regularly every two or every three beats. In music of a

later time, harmonic rhythm is able to play a role in the

establishment of a meter. The C-major prelude from book I

of Das Wohltemoerierte Klavier and Chopin's E-flat-major

Prelude are clear examples.

Once a meter is established in any of these ways, the

listener projects the continuation of the pattern into the

immediate future of the music. Grosvenor Cooper and Leonard

B. Meyer seem to agree. Concerning rhythm, they explain,

Once a rhythm becomes established, it tends, if


possible, to be continued in the mind of the
listener— it tends, that is, to organize later
patterns in its own image. This is true even
where the natural grouping would seem to be
otherwise.1

Later, they imply that the same is true in the case of

Grosvenor Cooper and Leonard B. Meyer, The Rhythmic


Structure of Music (Chicago: University of Chicago Press,
1960), p. 13.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
32

morphological lengths and, presumably therefore, meter.2

This is not to say that an established meter is in fact

fixed for the entirety of a piece. But any hemiola, for

instance, or syncopation or outright change of meter will be

heard in comparison with, and will bear significance in

light of, the listener's projection of the originally

established meter.

If meter is not aurally established in the music of

Prokofiev in ways such as those described above, that is, if

the notated meter is only a means of reference or an aid to

performance, as it is in much of the music of Prokofiev's

contemporary Webern, for instance, then any discussion of

phrase length as a certain number of measures is irrelevant,

and the type of tonality discussed in the previous chapter

is not present here. However, all these means are in fact

employed by Prokofiev to the extent that most of the music

in question is clearly perceived as metrical. In Romeo and

Juliet, the use of dynamic accents as a basis for meter is

found, appropriately, in many of the scenes in which the

common folk are dancing. Excellent examples of this use are

found in number 3, "The Street Wakens," and the first eight

measures of number 4, "Morning Dance.11 Agogic accents and

the repetition of motives also contribute to the establish­

ment of meter in both examples. In the dance first heard at

2Ibid., p. 145.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
33

rehearsal 178, a pattern of dissonances somewhat similar to

that of Renaissance suspensions is found. (See example

14. )3 For the most part, however, the idea of dissonance

Example 14. Establishment of meter by dissonance pattern in


178:1-16.

M ,.i-
f f * #O d r r 4 *!
si n - M . --FU- 4 4
^ ^ 'I u hl J i 1

a*— *—-
3 ' *r ‘•Lir
LplpJ M> l
1 hrxr1

and its resolution as a determinant of meter is represented

3All examples are taken from Sergei Prokofiev, Romeo


and Juliet: Piano Score, Op. 64 (Melville, NY: Belwin Mills,
n.d.). Measures will be identified by numbers counted from
the appropriate rehearsal number, or from the beginning, as
indicated.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
34

in this music by the normal placement of harmonic reso­

lutions on downbeats. The minor triad on the first beat of

measure 178:14, for instance, acts as a resolution of the

dominant-seventh on the previous beat. This resolution is,

in part, what makes this beat a strong beat. This two-beat

pattern of dissonance and resolution is repeated twice

immediately afterward, confirming the duple meter estab­

lished by other means earlier. Harmonic rhythm is an impor­

tant factor in the establishment of meter in the melody at

rehearsal 64. (See example 15.)

Example 15. Establishment of meter by harmonic rhythm in


63:10-64:9.

ft t«apo

^ 'f ¥
Articulation of Phrases

The necessary conditions for tonality as defined in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
35

this treatise include not only regular meter but also a

standard phrase length. Therefore, in order to demonstrate

that the music of Romeo and Juliet is tonal, it is necessary

to show that phrases are indeed articulated in the style

and, then, that they usually consist of the same number of

measures. While it is generally agreed that the term

"phrase" designates a number of consecutive measures

perceived as a distinct unit on some hierarchical level, it

is not always easy to define the level for which the use of

the word is appropriate. I hope that any difference between

the reader's conception of the word "phrase" and mine will

be only a matter of the level on which the use of that term

is appropriate and will not hinder the following discussion.

Since the length of several phrases is to be

determined, an explanation of the method used in this

treatise to count measures is in order. A melody such as

the one in measures 19:1-8 presents no problems in this

regard. (See example 16.) Beginning on the downbeat of its

first measure and continuing to the very end of its last

measure, the melody is clearly 8 measures long. An

anacrusis, on the other hand, can slightly complicate the

situation. The bassoon melody at the beginning of number 3,

"The Street Wakens," begins on the second half of measure

9:4, after what most listeners would likely call a four-

measure introduction, and continues, past the entrance of

the violin, to the end of measure 9:12. (See example 17.)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 16. Melody that presents no measure-counting
problems in 18:10-19:8.

■J"- J jt!Th
(I * r n ]j
-f ^ ^ T¥ ’P»f'f
I L .-’ . - p j ’. L -

' X... 1
i ’I ' T T P
n l.r* *■
A t ----- V'-tT =1- T — rT I l"n=r= p| = —
^ i i

i ri t I r 1173 i
f-'-t--r=T~i
/ __

Calling the length of the phrase eight-and-a-half measures

might imply that the introduction should be called three-

and-a-half. The total length of the passage is, after all,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
37

Example 17. Melody that presents some measure-counting


problems in 9:1-11:1.

i A lleg retto J

ED
ft * i
,tjtt

■jp * ■ rr^ h —

Bjn
J

* J Cor.
mf F

i s m s Hpi
20

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
38

twelve measures. But doing so goes against the natural way

of stating the length of the introduction. A similar

problem results from the overlapping, mentioned above, at

the juncture of the first and second phrases. In order to

avoid any possible confusion of this nature, in this

treatise the number of measures in a passage will be

determined by counting only downbeats. Thus, the intro­

duction is said to be four measures long, and the first

phrase eight measures long. This method greatly simplifies

matters in all cases except those in which irregular meter

or an elision is involved.

A sequence of consecutive musical events will be

perceived as a temporal unit most commonly as a negative

result of some separation from other events immediately

before and after. The separation between two units may be

perceived to mark the end of the first unit, the beginning

of the second, or both. Perhaps the most easily understood

method of achieving the sense of separation is by rhythmic

caesura. A relatively long interval between attack points

in a melody, whether the result of a long note or of a rest,

can cause this sense both by serving as an end of previous

motion and by separating a melody into two groups of

respectively proximate notes.

This principle is at work in the melody shown in

example 18. The first measure of the melody proper, measure

1:5, consists of sixteenth notes and sixteenth rests. The

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
39

Example 18. Articulation of phrases by caesura in 1:2-12.

rests break the regular motion and cause the notes between

them to be perceived as groups, groups of three notes each.

A group this short may be called a motive. The eighth-note

motion of the next measure, although slower than that of the

previous measure, is consistent, and the four notes are

perceived as a group, not as four successive breaks in the

sixteenth-note motion. On the second beat of measure 1:8,

the lone dotted eighth represents a more significant point

of separation. (The two eighth notes on b-natural on the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
40

downbeat may be heard by some as the resting point since the

pitch-class remains the same for a quarter note's time; on

the other hand, the notes are placed in separate octaves.)

The four measures, measures 1:5-8, may be called a phrase, a

somewhat complete musical unit, the end of which is caused

by rhythmic caesura. The following four measures constitute

a second phrase, a quarter note on b (or more time if the

octave equivalence is accepted), on the downbeat of measure

1:12 halting the motion.

Prokofiev often achieves closure by taking advantage of

the traditional acceptance of certain melodic and harmonic

formulas and processes as cadential. In measures 1:7 and

1:8, for instance, the descending-fifth relationship between

the roots of two consecutive major triads, together with the

resolution of a major sixth to an octave by contrary motion,

contributes to the sense that one phrase is ending and, by

implication, that another is beginning.

Whenever closure of a phrase is perceived, any further

musical activity— assuming the phrase is not the last of a

piece— can only be interpreted as a new phrase. It is

possible, however, for a new phrase to begin without the

first having achieved closure. In this case, the first

phrase would sound incomplete if played alone. This notion

is incompatible with the somewhat common doctrine that a

phrase must end with a cadence, but is necessary if the term

"phrase" is to be used in the discussion of music other than

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
41

chorales and pieces in simple "song" forms. Number 17 in

Romeo and Juliet. "Tybalt recognizes Romeo," is a pastiche

of previously heard melodies, in which this type of

delineation occurs several times. At rehearsal 117, for

instance, although the eighth-note motion is not interrupted

and there is no resolution of harmony or melody, a division

is heard simply because a new idea begins. (See example

19.) A similar instance occurs eight measures later.

Example 19. Articulation of phrases by change of idea in


116:6-117:12.

ru n
ml
— , ru n
Anu
* j~
irrii ?
W- j1^
hj-(I
^ in ft
***• C-k' * * ItH lil

'L.OK.C l., Tl-W

;r\> —|fr-g-- -J - . m
m i m ^

|U
b m p

A change in almost any aspect of a piece may be heard

as the beginning of a new phrase even if the melodic idea is

not new. Prokofiev, as well as other composers, regularly

uses changes of instrumentation, changes of dynamics, or

changes of harmony (where the harmonic rhythm is slow) to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
42

delineate phrases. Repetition of an idea may also mark a

division in music, it may seem that two statements of an

idea would be grouped by similarity. But in fact, if the

first occurence of the idea represents a beginning point,

the second will as well.

Normally, these principles do not operate in isolation.

It is clear that a new phrase does not begin with every

harmonic change or motivic repetition. Thus, measure 50:4

is heard as the end of a phrase because of the combined

presence of the descending-fifth motion in the bass on beats

2 and 3, the concurrent resolution of dissonance, and the

relative temporal distance between the last note of the

melody in this measure and the first note in the next;

measure 5 begins a new phrase by virtue of the immediate

change in the dynamics level and the repetition of the

opening rhythm. (See example 20.)

All these means of division seem to appeal to the

natural human proclivity for hierarchically organizing

phenomena, and, perhaps in part because of their

naturalness, were used long before Prokofiev took advantage

of them. Thus, while Prokofiev/s music is in many ways

different from that of previous centuries, in this aspect it

is quite traditional, and Stravinsky has argued that only by

continuing a tradition can an artist create something truly

new. Tradition, he says, "appears as an heirloom, a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
43

heritage that one receives on condition of making it bear

fruit before passing it on to one's descendants."4

Example 20. Articulation of phrases by combination of


features in 50:1-8.

V iva ce J:

4Ig°r Stravinsky, Poetics of Music in the Form of Six


Lessons, trans. Arthur Knodel and Ingolf Dahl (Cambridge, MA:
Harvard University Press, 1947), p. 59.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
44

Standard Phrase Length

It has been argued in the previous chapter that a

regularity of phrase length is a necessary condition of the

emergence of a tonal style, and it was suggested that this

regularity may contribute to a sense of tonality even if the

pitch language is different from that of traditional Western

tonality. If Prokofiev's music can be said to exhibit a new

kind of tonality, it must be demonstrated that there is a

standard of phrase length at operation in the music.

In Romeo and Juliet. Prokofiev does indeed make use of

a standard phrase length. That standard, again a

representative of tradition, is four measures. The standard

is set at the very beginning of the ballet, the

"Introduction" beginning with two variations of a four-

measure phrase. The next melody, eight measures long, is

divided into two similar, four-measure phrases. The

following melody, in measures 1:13-20, exhibits a similar

form. This pattern having been established, the four-

measure phrase continues to predominate in most of the

numbers of the ballet. If a phrase closes itself, the

cadence is most likely to occur on the downbeat of the

fourth measure. If the phrase ends only because a change of

some kind suggests the beginning of a new phrase, that

change is most likely to occur on the downbeat of what would

have been the fifth measure.

In some cases, a phrase that on hearing seems to fit

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
45

the standard, actually lasts eight measures as notated, as

is the case with the first phrase of number 3, "The Street

Awakens," or only two, as is the case with the first four

phrases in number 10, "The Young Juliet." (Please refer

again to examples 17 and 20.) In the first case, were every

other bar line left out and the meter changed to 4/4, it is

doubtful that the metrical effect of the phrase would be

much different, since beats 1 and 3 in quadruple meter are

usually considered strong beats. Conversely, a two-measure

phrase, if not in triple meter, would not sound much

different if notated with a new barline added halfway

through each measure. The fact that renotating these two

phrases in this way would place the cadence of each on the

downbeat of the fourth measure explains the aural connection

between these phrases and those of the standard four

measures. Prokofiev himself confirms this connection by

renotating with a different number of barlines some of the

melodies in Romeo and Juliet when they return later in the

ballet. For instance, the eight-measure melody first found

in measures 142:5-12 is notated in measures 307:7-10 with

halved note values and lasts only half the original number

of measures. (See example 21.) The "andante" marking of

the first and the "adagio" marking of the second give the

two versions approximately the same apparent tempo.

There is some evidence to lead one to believe that a

phrase length of four measures, by virtue of its

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
46

Example 21. Renotation of 142:5-12 in 307:7-10. (The latter


transposed.)

hierarchically binary structure as two groups of two

measures each, seems more "normal,” to use the word selected

by Cooper and Meyer,5 than other lengths because it takes

advantage of a natural human tendency to perceive a uniform

series of pulses as grouped in twos. Carl Seashore

explains,

There are two fundamental factors in the


perception of rhythm: an instinctive tendency to
group impressions in hearing and a capacity for
doing this with precision in time and stress. The
subjective tendency is so deeply ingrained,
because of its biological service, that we irre­
sistibly group uniform successions of sound, such
as the tick of a clock, into rhythmic measure.
The supposed limping of a clock is often purely
subjective . . . . If a long series of quarter
notes were played with absolute uniformity in time
and stress, the listener would inevitably hear
them divided into measures and would actually hear

"Cooper and Meyer, Bh£tfamjg_Structur e , p. 155.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
47

the appropriate notes accented. Such is one of


nature's beneficent illusions.6

He explains the binary nature of this imposed grouping thus:

Our attention is periodic. All our mental life


works rhythmically, that is, by periodic pulsation
of effort or achievement with unnoticed
intermittence of blanks.7

The rhythmic measure, then, is simply taking


advantage of nature's supply of pulsating effort
of attention. And when the measure fits the
attention wave, it gives us a restful feeling of
satisfaction and ease.0

The point to be made is not that four measures is the

right length for a phrase regardless of style and that

phrases of other lengths are less satisfactory than those of

four (let us set aside Seashore's use of the word

"satisfaction" for the moment) or that they are only

modifications of them. Rather, it is that phrases of four

measures seem to appeal to this natural perceptual pattern,

appear regular, and promote the listener's projection of the

pattern into the future of the piece. That is, the

appearance of even a single two- or four-measure phrase can

promote the perception and expectation of metrical

regularity. Phrases which do not fit this hierarchical

6Carl E. Seashore, Psychology of Music (New York: McGraw-


Hill, 1938), p. 138. The reader may try a personal experiment
as a demonstration of the point concerning the "limping" of
a clock by trying to mentally switch the positions of "tick"
and "tock." Success would show that the apparent difference
in tone is subjectively imposed.

7Ibid., p. 140.

“Ibid., p. 141.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
48

binary pattern, far from being inferior, are indeed often

much more interesting precisely because of their

irregularity. Stravinsky's summary of the ideas of a

"philosopher-friend," Pierre Souvtchinsky, about musical

time supports this thought. It is interestingly reminiscent

of the last passage of Seashore quoted above. Stravinsky

explains,

Mr. Souvtchinsky . . . presents us with two kinds


of music: one which evolves parallel to the
process of ontological time, embracing and
penetrating it, inducing in the mind of the
listener a feeling of euphoria and, so to speak,
of "dynamic calm." The other kind runs ahead of,
or counter to, this process. It is not self-
contained in each momentary tonal unit. It
dislocates the centers of attraction and gravity
and sets itself up in the unstable; and this fact
makes it particularly adaptable to the translation
of the composer's emotive impulses. All music in
which the will to expression is dominant belongs
to the second type.9

Even if there were no inherent psychological reason for

the four-measure's phrase being natural, it would seem so to

members of our culture because of conditioning. Everyday

contact with nursery songs, folk songs, hymns, popular and

dance music, and the commonly familiar tunes of Mendelssohn,

Tchaikovsky and others, accustoms one to the statistical

standard of the four-measure phrase.10 A composer making use

Stravinsky, Poetics. p. 32.

10Of course, it is possible that this standard would never


have existed without the inherent psychological reason.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
49

of this standard length in a piece can take advantage of the

expectations and connotations it carries.

Because four measures is the most common length for a

phrase in the ballet, because such organization appeals to a

natural human mode of perception, and because of condition­

ing, expectation of four-measure phrases becomes an

important factor in the perception of much of the music in

this ballet. This is not to say that it is important in the

perception of all this music. If a passage begins with a

five-measure phrase, for instance, expectation of this kind

may simply be irrelevant. But if a listener hears evidence

of hierarchically binary rhythmic organization early in a

piece, he may expect it to continue. That is, a listener

may expect either a cadence on the fourth downbeat of a

phrase, or a change on what would be the fifth. Whatever

else happens may bear musical significance precisely because

it is a deviation.

Even when cadences do occur at the expected time, the

expectation may lend special significance to the pitches

used. The classical deceptive cadence provides a good

example. An example of another kind, one very representa­

tive of Prokofiev's style, is found in the opening melody of

the ’'Gavotte," number 18. (See example 22.) The contrast

in character, dynamics, melodic shape, etc. between the

first two phrases of the melody, measures 3-6 and 7-10,

causes the second phrase, from its first few notes, to be

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
50

Example 22. Special significance as a result of expectation


of cadence in 122:1-123:1.

A llegro J--i«
^ Cor., Areki

m
/-

* « M *

#1 P T F sp p p

T T j r fffl

heard as a departure from, rather than as a conclusive

response to, the first phrase. At the beginning of the

third phrase, measures 11-14, the return of the accents, the

increased dynamics, the similarity of the turn in measure 11

to the one in measure 2, the return of the rhythm of measure

5, and the occurrence of a second-inversion triad in the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
51

second measure of both the first and third phrases, cause

the third phrase to be heard as a return. Thus, the final

cadence of the melody is expected at the end of this phrase,

on the downbeat of the fourth measure, measure 123:1.

What is the expected pitch of this cadence? The

melody, despite all the brief encounters with other keys, is

clearly in D major. The descending fifth formed by the a-

naturals of the opening and the d on the downbeat of measure

122:3 establish the key. Plagal motion to d in measures 6

and 7, melodic arrival on d on the downbeat of measure 8,

harmonic arrival on d on the downbeat of measure 10, and the

ascending-fourth motion to d at the beginning of the third

phrase, confirm the priority of d. In order to bring full

closure to the melody, the cadence expected at the end of

this phrase should be on d.

Between the establishment of D-major at the beginning

of the phrase and the anticipated cadence, however, the

music takes a turn into C-sharp major. At this point the

phrase is ambivalent. It might return to D-major for the

closing cadence or it might not. In the second case there

are several possibilities. The cadence might occur at the

expected time in C-sharp or even some third key, or the

phrase night not cadence on the downbeat of the fourth

measure at all. The issue is pressed through the first half

of measure 13. On beat 4, it is finally clear that the

phrase will cadence in D-major. The projection of meter and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
52

the expectation that the cadence will be four measures long

are essential to the perception of this cadence. The c-

sharp in the melody on the penultimate beat resolves the

local melodic tendencies; only its metrical position causes

it to be heard as a penultimate note and not as the last

note in a deceptive cadence. (Compare the two versions

found in example 23.) The harmonization of the pitch with

Example 23. Two versions of "Gavotte" cadence.

AJ
r II S -t FT m
r*w[ I rT

an A-dominant-seventh only strengthens the fundamentally

metrical phenomenon.

A similar example is found in the melody at rehearsal

111. (Please refer to example 76, page 136.) Beginning in

measure 5 or 6, the listener begins to be aware of the

approach of the cadence on the downbeat of measure 8. The

melody having started in C major, the listener expects

closure in that key as well. Part of the interest of this

music is the conflict resulting from the distant

relationship of the anticipated pitch of resolution, c-

natural, and the current key, B-flat minor, and from the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
53

relatively short amount of time left to bridge this gap.

The anticipation of resolution on a particular beat, then,

is a significant factor in the effect of this melody, and is

the result of the acceptance of a particular standard of

phrase length.

Four measures is such a standard in this music that

even many metrically irregular phrases last four or eight

notated measures. Such a phrase is found at the beginning

of number 11, "The Arrival of the Guests," in measures 60:1-

4. (See example 24.) A clear authentic cadence occurs on

Example 24. Irregular four-measure phrase in 60:1-4.

ES E!*\l !
n rJTlrH °r 4
:r = * =
f to* tfjtlt o

what is notated as the downbeat of measure 4. What comes

before, however, does not sound like three measures of

triple meter. The first beat, because of the accent and

high dynamic level, presents itself as a downbeat. An

accent resulting from the beginning of melodic motion

possibly is heard on beat 3 of measure 1. The next apparent

downbeat occurs not on beat 1 of the next measure, but,

because of dynamic and tonic accents in both the melody and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
54

bass lines, on beat 2. This pattern of accents lends itself

to interpretation as duple or quadruple meter, but not the

notated triple meter. A dynamic accent in the melody on

beat 1 of measure 3 favors the duple-meter interpretation

On the very next beat, however, a dynamic accent in the bass

instruments and the tonic accent of an ascending tenth in

the melody break the pattern, so that the music leading to

the cadence is not clearly in any meter. Each of the next

two times the phrase appears, however, it follows a passage

clearly in triple meter, and is heard in light of the

projection of that meter.

To say that a standard phrase length exists in this

music is not to say that all phrases are of equal length.

There are a significant number of three-, five-, six-, and

seven-measure phrases, for instance. Some of these appear

to be modifications of four-measure phrases through elision

or extension. An example of an extension is found in the

opening melody of "Masks," number 12. (Please refer to

example 60, page 110.) The melody begins, in the key of B-

flat, in measure 71:5, after a four-measure introduction.

The first phrase, also four measures long, ends in measure

71:8 with a dotted-half-note a-natural in the melody. The

second phrase, after a brief excursion through D major,

returns to B-flat for a cadence on the fourth downbeat of

the phrase, in measure 71:12. The next melodic phrase does

not begin until two measures later, however, measure 71:13

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
55

consisting only of repetitions of a B-flat harmony of some

kind. Because the cadence occurs on the downbeat of the

fourth measure, this five-measure passage can be interpreted

as a four-measure phrase with a one-measure extension.

While many phrases of nonstandard length may be

regarded as modifications of four-measure phrases, some are

simply different from the standard. At rehearsal number

276, for instance, after nine consecutive four-measure

segments comes a five-measure phrase. (See example 25.)

There is no cadence on the fourth measure; in fact there is

no cadence at all. The musical idea simply continues for

five measures before the running sixteenth notes of the

previous passage return. After four more measures of

sixteenth notes, a three-measure phrase is heard.

A further example, the opening melody in the "Dance of

Five Couples," begins with an eleven-measure phrase,

measures 174:9-19. (See example 26.) Here there is a clear

cadence on the downbeat of measure 11, as the upper voices

resolve to an F-major triad above the f-natural pedal point.

When phrases of irregular lengths occur, they do not estab­

lish a temporary standard. Instead they are surrounded

either by phrases of four measures or by phrases of differ­

ent irregular lengths. An extreme example is provided by

number 47, "Juliet alone." Here the sequence of the number

of measures in each respective phrase is 3, 5, 4, 2 1/2, l,

2, 2, 1 1/2, 2, 3, 4, 3, 2, 4, 2, 2 3/4, 3 and 4.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
56

Example 25. Five-measure phrase in 276:1-277:1.

* I5Z2J Tr-W

' = ( % ■ * frb -
, - i — i------- .— A s - _ J ------
bi (bi r 'ir »....
T"i , r*ft‘tiQ

lJxJ- 't- tJ *u ?*ZJ LJ

s . ilii

As Souvtchinsky, through Stravinsky, suggests, it is

the metrically irregular passage which is most capable of

bearing an emotional interpretation. This is especially so

when heard in the context of a reinforced notion of the

standard of regularity, in Romeo and Juliet. Prokofiev

takes full advantage of the principle in order to express

the highly-charged emotional context of the last scenes of

the story. Much more than in the first three acts of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
57

Example 26. Eleven-measure phrase in 174:7-19.

ballet, irregular phrase length is quite common in the last

act and epilogue, where it is used in all the passages that

accompany Juliet's contemplation of the dreadful acts she is

deciding to commit: risking death by taking the sleeping

potion, leaving and frightening her family, being buried

alive, and, eventually, killing herself. (See especially

rehearsal numbers 314 and 315, where Friar Laurence first

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
58

gives Juliet the sleeping potion; number 47, "Juliet Alone,"

discussed above; and number 52, "Juliet's Death.") By

comparison, number 39, "Romeo Bids Juliet Farewell";

Juliet's last meeting with Paris, in number 41; and the

funeral, in the first half of number 51, all take place to

music consisting mostly of regular four-measure phrases.

A standard phrase length is a necessary condition of

the establishment of a tonal style. By establishing the

presence of such a standard early in the piece, Prokofiev is

able to play with the expectations that accompany it, both

in the tension created by last-minute tonicizations and in

the expressive use late in the piece of patterns perceived

to be irregular in the context of the established standard.

Prokofiev uses a length of four measures for this standard.

His preference for this length and his fascination with

submitting tonality to wild contortions within this regular

span may be reflected in his early preference of Schumann

over Mozart, a composer for whom he had little admiration

until quite late in his musical training.11

Having established the existence of phrases and some

facts concerning the rhythmic and metrical aspects of

phrases in this music, we may now turn to a study of matters

relating to pitch, beginning with cadences.

“Israel V. Nestyev, Prokofiev, trans. Florence Jonas,


foreword by Nicolas Slonimsky (Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press, 1960), pp. 22-23.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
59

CHAPTER III

CADENCES

At the end of the previous chapter, it was shown that,

in Romeo and Juliet, there is a basic difference of purpose

between phrases of four measures' length and those of other

lengths. This difference is normally corroborated by at

least one other technical difference: the irregular-length

phrases normally do not have cadences. In this chapter, we

will examine the cadences in Romeo and Juliet with the

intention of discovering whether there is a frequent

occurrence of such features as certain chord types, root

movement by certain intervals, or melodic function of

certain pitches.

Despite the great variety of chord types to be found in

these cadences, a variety which might suggest that the

harmonies here are merely the accidents of linear processes,

it is appropriate to speak of "chord types" and "root

movements" in this music. That Prokofiev thought of most of

his music as consisting primarily of a melody and

accompanying harmony is likely. When he speaks in his

various writings and lectures of inventiveness and

experimentation, he nearly always speaks of it in terms of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
60

melody and harmony. Novelty in the latter field was

especially important to him partly because of remarks, made

to him early in his life by Taneyev, that his harmonic

progressions were too simple.1

The following discussion is based on a study in which

seventy-nine distinct cadence types from the work were

identified. (Some cadence figures not found at the end of a

phrase were included in the study. See below, page 79.)

Statements of quantity such as "most of the cadences" or

"about 40 percent of the cadences" refer to the set of

cadences that includes one representative of each cadence

type. The location of each of these cadences is listed in

appendix A. Should one prefer to account for the multiple

instances of certain cadences, those repeated within a

section or in a reprise of a melody in a later section, some

of the figures could be significantly different.

Final Chords

The final sonority of each cadence is nearly always a

triad, normally complete but at times not. The triad is

usually a major triad: of the cadences examined, only twelve

end with a minor triad. In a few cases, the fifth is

missing from the final triad. In five instances, only one

note is present. In each of these five cadences, previous

Sergei Prokofiev et al., Autobiography. Articles.


Reminiscences. ed. Semyon Shlifstein, trans. R. Prokofieva,
(Moscow: Foreign Language Publishing House, n.d.), p. 20.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
61

material prompts a major-mode interpretation. In five other

cases, the triad is not fully resolved until one beat or one

half beat after the entrance of the root in the bass. (See

example 27 for two instances.) Only one cadence resolves to

Example 27. Delayed resolution of tonic in a) 6:9-7:1 and


b) 103:8-9.

a. b.

a sonority other than a triad. However, this sonority can

be interpreted as a major triad with an added fourth scale

degree (See example 28.) In any event, the cadence of which

Example 28. Triad with added fourth scale degree as last


sonority in cadence in 67:9-10.

it is a part is used motivically within a loosely

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
62

constructed phrase and does not serve to close any formal

unit. (On the other hand, a major triad with an added major

seventh serves as a one-measure codetta to number 3, "The

Street Wakens.”) In all the cadences, the root of the last

chord is found as the lowest sounding pitch. Thus, it is

seen that the familiar consonance of the root-position

triad, especially the major triad, is the fundamental

sonority in the style of the ballet, fundamental in the

sense, not that it is the most common sonority, but that,

because it is used as the final harmony in so many phrases,

it is the sonority that provides resolution for all others.

Dominant_as Penultimate Chord

Prokofiev is less traditional, and more varied, in his

choices for the penultimate sonorities in these cadences. A

kaleidoscopic array of chord types is found. However, in

about two-thirds of the cases, the sonority is likely to be

interpreted as a species of dominant harmony because of

emphasis on the dominant pitch in that harmony and on its

melodic resolution to the tonic pitch in the final chord.

(For now, let us assume that the final sonority is a tonic

harmony, if only for a point of reference through which to

designate other harmonies and pitches.) The dominant pitch

and its resolution are usually found in the bass, as shown

in example 29a. This placement in the lowest voice

strengthens the perceived tie between these cadences and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
63

Example 29. Prominent §-to-l melodic motion in a) 8:11-12


and b) 107:15-17.

a b,

traditional tonal cadences. At times, however, §-to-l

motion is so clearly present in the top voice that

interpretation of the penultimate chord as a V is still

quite natural. (See example 29b.) Of course, the metrical

placement of these chords, usually on the beat just prior to

an expected cadential downbeat, is also a factor in the

interpretation of their function, which is actually clear at

the moment of occurrence of the dominant harmony, before the

§ to 1 melodic motion is heard. This fact can be

illustrated by the passage in example 14, page 33, in which,

of three successive dominant-seventh chords, only the one on

beat 2 of the seventh measure will be heard filling a

dominant function.

Prokofiev, aware that metrical placement and melodic

motion from the dominant to the tonic are more than enough

to guarantee a successful cadence, makes use of many

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
64

colorful sonorities for the dominant harmony. A few are

given in example 30. since in this case there is no

Example 30. Various sonorities used as dominant


penultimates.

standard chord type, these sonorities are best considered

through a study of the properties of individual tones.

Of the eleven pitches that might possibly accompany the

dominant pitch, the leading tone is the most common, present

in nearly every case. It is normally approached from below

by half- or whole-steps, or from the tonic pitch above.

Rarely is it preceded by itself. As might be expected, when

found in the melody, the leading tone normally resolves to

the tonic pitch, while when in an inner line it is sometimes

followed by motion down to the fifth of the tonic harmony.

In both cases, the resolution is at times delayed by a chord

skip or passing tone. (See example 31.)

There are some cases where the leading tone is not

present. In measure 103:8, the leading tone is replaced by

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
65

Example 31. Use of the leading tone in cadential dominants


in a) 50:4 and b) 176:7-8.

a. b.

the subtonic, but is heard one beat later, over the tonic

bass, before finally resolving. (See example 27b.) In

measure 3 of the "Introduction," first it is the leading

tone that is heard above the dominant bass, then the

subtonic. Finally, on the second half of the third beat,

neither pitch is present. In measure 25:12, the seventh

scale degree is missing altogether. (See example 32.)

Example 32. Cadential dominant with neither the subtonic


nor the leading tone in 25:12-14.

Olloil
\p> fern
fr4 Bf fff-ftUft1
ffCor. ~ J0r= -- r f
f

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
66

Each other pitch of the chromatic scale is found as

part of a dominant harmony at some time in the score, the

second scale degree being the second most common pitch and

the fourth scale degree being the third most common. In

general, pitches from the diatonic major scale are preferred

slightly to the chromatic pitches, although the latter are

present in about 40 percent of the cadences involving a

dominant penultimate harmony. (From this point on, pitches

will be referred to by an underlined number corresponding to

the number of half-steps the given pitch lies above the

appropriate tonic pitch. In some cases, they will be

referred to by their position in a harmony.) As is the case

with the leading tone, il, these pitches are most often

approached by step (either half-step or whole-step) or by

the same note2. With one notable exception, to be explained

below, these pitches are also normally followed by step

(occasionally by the same note). Several times this scalar

continuation produces a tone dissonant to the final triad.

Normally in this situation, the motion by step continues

over the tonic bass, often more than one voice moving in

2In this thesis, in order for resolution by step to be


recognized, the two pitches must actually be a step apart
and not a seventh or a ninth. Otherwise, since each of the
twelve pitch-classes contains one member that lies no more
than a major second from one pitch or another in any given
major or minor triad, one would have to say that voice-
leading is always involved when the chord of resolution is a
triad. The point thus becomes moot.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
67

parallel motion, until the dissonance is resolved. (See

example 33. See also examples 27a and 29b.)

Example 33. Parallel motion by step over tonic bass in


2 :11-12.

The fifth of the dominant chord, or 2, is present in

about half the cadences in which that chord is involved. It

is normally approached by step, from above or below, or by

the same pitch. (See example 34.) It is occasionally

Example 34. Use of 2 in cadential dominant in 48:4.

m f FaS.

introduced freely. In nearly every instance, it is resolved

by step, in most cases moving down to the root of the tonic

triad. Occasionally it rises to the third of the triad. In

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
68

the most common pattern, 2. is found as part of descending

scalar pattern, A.-2-Q,.


The seventh of the chord, or 2, is also common. It is

usually preceded by the same pitch, that is, as a suspen­

sion, or by a leap, as an appoggiatura. (See example 35.)

Example 35. Use of 2 in cadential dominants in a) 60:3-4


and b) 58:8.

a. b.

It is at times approached from a half-step above or below.

Normally resolving down to the third of the tonic triad, it

sometimes rises to the fifth. Once, when in the lowest

part, it skips down to the tonic. (See example 35b.) In no

other instance is it resolved by skip or left unresolved.

Pitches £ and 2, somewhat rare in the penultimate

dominant harmony, both normally resolve down by step to 7,

the fifth of the tonic triad. (See example 36.) 2 is

followed in one case by a chromatic ascent to the tonic

pitch. (See example 36c.) In one instance, shown in

example 32, both 2 and 2 are present in the very dissonant

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
69

Example 36. Use of a and £ in cadential dominants in a)


145:7-8, b) 83:12, and c) 67:12-13.

*siy*
...
# ¥ f

dominant harmony and do not resolve directly by step. Each

of these two pitches is normally prepared either by step or

as a suspension.

1 occurs in a penultimate dominant harmony only two

times in the ballet. Since it resolves differently each

time, it is impossible to say what is its normal resolution.

(See examples 35b and 27b.) In each case, 2 is missing from

the dominant harmony. 1 may therefore be thought of as a

substitute for 2, as a lowered fifth.

Similarly, 1, occurring rather frequently, is usually

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
70

found as an upper substitute for 2• (See example 37.)

Example 37. Use of 2 and £ in cadential dominants in a)


206:4, b) 85:7-8, and c) 2:15-16.

a. b.

ftjjLjfcJB
Unlike most of the other pitches, 2 is often freely

introduced. It normally resolves up by half-step to 4, the

third of the tonic triad, although occasionally it remains,

as the third of a minor triad. (See example 37b.)

Pitch 2 also is present quite often in the dominant

chord in cadences. It resolves up to 7 in nearly every

instance, rather than resolving downward in the manner of

the seventh of a chord, as 2, with which it is sometimes

found, does. (Please refer again to examples 37a and c.)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
71

It is usually prepared in standard fashion. Interestingly,

about two-thirds of the dominant chords that contain 6 also

contain 3. Similarly, both pitches being found with near

equal frequency, about two-thirds of those with 3 also have

6. Since both pitches normally resolve up by half-step,

when present together at a cadence, they tend to move

together in parallel minor thirds. In fact, there is in the

work only one cadence involving the two pitches in which

they do not ascend together, a cadence in which the

penultimate chord is very dense and dissonant, obfuscating

the function of individual pitches. (Please refer again to

example 32. This cadence is repeated with only rhythmic

differences in measures 261:8-9.) With this same exception,

all instances but one involve the leading tone as well, so

that a complete major triad on the leading tone is heard

resolving upward in parallel motion to the tonic chord while

the bass moves from dominant to tonic. The close position­

ing of the members of the leading-tone triad at the top of

the entire sonority may suggest the interpretation of these

sonorities as bi-chordal combinations.

As might be expected, 0, when present in penultimate

dominants, remains as the root of the tonic triad. (Please

refer again to examples 32 and 36b.) It is present in only

two cases, however, so it is not possible to state that a

principle of normal resolution has been established.

Found four times, 10. is never present at the same time

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
72

with H , and may therefore be considered a substitute for

it. (See example 38.) It normally resolves up to the

Example 38. Use of IQ. in cadential dominants in a) 306:9-10


and b) Beginning:3-4.

a. b.

tonic, confirming its role as a substitute for the leading

tone. In example 27b, however, the leading tone occurs over

the tonic bass as a passing tone between 10 and Q. In one

case, shown as example 38b, 10 simply disappears before the

arrival of the tonic chord.

The remaining pitch, 4, serves as the only exception to

the normal rule that pitches in the dominant harmony resolve

by step or do not move at all.In all but one of its

several occurrences, it resolves down by major third to the

tonic pitch. (See example 39.) In measures 121:4-6, shown

as example 39c, it is found resolving down to the third of a

minor triad.

The prominent practices involved in the dominant-to-

tonic cadences are the following: 1) use of major or minor

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 39. Use of 1 in cadential dominants in a) 111:7-8,
b) 16:4, and c) 121:4-6.

b.

triads, or parts thereof, as final sonorities, 2) occasional

delay of the resolution of these final sonorities, 3) use of

a wide variety of penultimate sonorities, 4) very frequent

use of the leading tone in the penultimate sonorities, 5)

use of voice-leading for the resolution of tones, 6) rather

frequent occurrence of chromatic pitches in the penultimate

chord, 7) occasional use of structures that may be

interpreted as bi-chords, and 8) the resolution of pitch 4

by major third to the tonic pitch.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
74

Nondominant_Penultimate Chords

The practices listed above are found in somewhat

modified form in those cadences with nondominant penultimate

harmonies. For instance, the leading tone, while common in

the nondominant, appears in only about two-thirds of the

cases. Chromatic pitches are, on the other hand, present in

nearly every case. These pitches generally resolve by half­

step in the expected manner. The other possible half-step

resolution, from £ to £, occurs very infrequently.

Resolution by step, is the rule in these cadences. In only

a handful of instances do all the notes of a chord leap to

the notes of the final chord of the cadence. One case is

shown in example 40. (Passing tones in the bass fill in the

leap in that part.)

Example 40. Leaps in all voices at cadence in 206:10.

In many of the cadences with nondominant penultimates,

especially in those in which no resolution by step is

present, the major third as a skip of resolution is very

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
75

important. This skip may be a descent, as it is in the

authentic cadences, or an ascent. In most of these non-

authentic cadences, it is found in the bass of root-position

chords. (See example 41.) All involve harmonies on two of

Example 41. Major-third skip at cadences in a) 106:7-8, b)


203:6, and c) 50:2.

a. b.

the following: e-natural, a-flat, and c. The other root

movements occurring more than once are the traditional minor

second from 11 to 0, and the less traditional tritone. (See

examples 42a and b.) There is one case of a nondominant

structure likely to be interpreted as a bi-chord, in this

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
76

case a combination of two minor triads, one each on 2. and


11. (See example 42c.)

Example 42. Infrequent cadential root relationships in a)


55:8-9, b) 363:10-11, and c) 3:13.

Two cadences deserve special notice because of the

potential of interpreting them as either authentic or non-

authentic cadences. (See example 43.) In the first of

these, the sonority on the last eighth-note of measure 5:6

shares no notes with the previous sonority and, therefore,

may be heard as a separate harmony, a nondominant. On the

other hand, if the leap in the bass from c-natural to g-

natural is accepted as a chord skip in a dominant harmony, a

pattern occurring elsewhere in the ballet (please refer

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
77

Example 43. Cadences that may be interpreted as authentic


or nonauthentic in a) 5:6-7, and b) 75:7-8.

a. b.

again to example 39b), and the e-natural and g-sharp as a

passing tone and cambiata respectively, the whole beat may

be interpreted as supporting a dominant harmony. In the

second cadence, the tonic chord is expected on the downbeat,

where it occurs in an earlier parallel passage. (Please

refer again to example 27.) The diminished chord on 11

would, of course, work perfectly as a penultimate harmony.

However, the chromatic passing tones of the inner voices,

which occurred together with the tonic pitch in both melody

and bass on the cadential downbeat in the earlier passage,

are joined here by the dominant in the bass and a suspension

of the leading tone in the melody. Thus, while the pitches

of the last two sonorities form an authentic cadence typical

of Prokofiev, it is clear that the cadence is a variation on

a nonauthentic cadence.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
78

Half-Cader.c'ss and Other Cadence Figures

It was assumed above that the final chord of each

cadence was a tonic harmony. Actually there are a few half

cadences in the ballet, as well as a few cadence figures

that do not so much close a phrase as they lead, by means of

a dominant chord, to a new phrase that begins with a tonic

chord. In addition, there are cases in which the first

phrase of a period ends on a harmony from a foreign key or

scale. The practices involved in these last passages will

be dealt with in chapter IV.

All the half cadences but one occur in Friar Laurence's

theme, heard for the first time in number 28, "Romeo at

Friar Laurence's." In example 44a, the dissonance of the

Example 44. Half cadences in a) 207:4 and b) 210:8.

a. b.

final chord, especially the minor seventh, helps lead to the

interpretation of it as a dominant harmony. On the other

hand, the pure F-major triad of example 44b is heard as a V

and not as a I simply because the key of B-flat has been

firmly established previously. Curiously, the 11th chords

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
79

found as the final sonority in example 44a and as

penultimates in both cadences are not present in any other

cadences in the entire work.

In nearly all the cases in which a harmony leads to the

initial tonic harmony of the next phrase, the leading

harmony is either a chord traditionally accepted as a

dominant or such a chord with one change. (See example 45.)

Example 45. Dominant sonorities leading to first harmony of


phrase in a) 140:18-141:1, b) 2:17-3:1, c) 27:12-28:1,
and d) 84:8-85:1.

a. b.

c. d.

The minor changes involved include use of a major seventh

instead of a minor seventh, of a raised or lowered fifth,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
80

and of a minor third, it is because of the recognizability

of these figures as cadence figures that they were included

in the study.

Antepenultimate Chords

The antepenultimate harmonies of the cadences in Romeo

and Juliet are both more consistent and more varied than are

the penultimate harmonies. They are more consistent in that

the number of chord types is much lower. A vast majority of

these harmonies are the triads and seventh chords common to

traditional Western tonality. They are more varied in that

there is no one note of the chromatic scale which regularly

serves as the root of these harmonies. While 2 and 5,

common roots for antepenultimate harmony in traditional

tonal music, are sometimes found, 1, 6, 8, 10, and 11 also

occur quite frequently. The tonic chord, also common in

earlier music, is quite rare here.

One of the most recognizable traits of Prokofiev's

music is the sudden shift of keys, especially at the

dominant harmony in an authentic cadence. This technique is

associated with Prokofiev for good reason. A change of key

(or let us say scale) occurs at the penultimate harmony of a

phrase about half the time in this ballet. Of those

instances, only two involve penultimate chords other than a

dominant. Thus, a jump to a different key before a V-I

cadence is common in this ballet at least. It is the fact

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
81

that the antepenultimate chord is so often part of a key

different from that of the final two harmonies that accounts

for most of the variety in the roots used. Of the shifts to

a dominant, about four-fifths involve rather simple dominant

harmonies: triads or seventh chords, possibly with a raised

or lowered fifth. The normal simplicity of the two chords

involved in the shift makes the sudden change of scale quite

clear. The surprising-yet-expected touch created by the

normal metrical placement of these shifts— near the end of

the measure just before tonic arrival is anticipated— makes

these sudden jumps truly Prokofiev's.

Voice-leading is important in the motion to the penul­

timate harmony, as it is in the motion to the final harmony,

although possibly to a slightly lesser degree. Chromatic

voice leading, that is, half-step motion other than that

between notes of the diatonic scale of the prevailing key,

is present a little over half the time. As might be

expected, it is much more common when a change of key is

involved. Because the immediate goal is different, the

tendencies of the various tones in motion to the second-to-

last chord are slightly different from what they are in

motion to the last chord. 0. , quite common, normally goes

down to the leading tone, which in turn normally returns to

the tonic. 1 stays the same or moves up, never going down

to the leading tone, as it does traditionally in this

situation. 2.usually stays the same or moves up by step.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
82

2, 7, and H are rather rare and, hardly resolving the same

way twice, indicate no particular tendencies. 4, also

rather rare, usually goes up to 5. 2, one of the most

common pitches in the antepenultimate sonorities, does not

normally come down to Z or remain stationary, as it often

would in tonal music of previous times. Rather, it most

often moves up, more likely than not to 6. 6 and 8 both

normally move by half-step to 7. 9, very common, has no

particular tendency, moving up by whole- or half-step in one

case, staying in the next, and moving down in another. The

remaining pitch, 10, on the other hand, displays the

strictest tendency of all the tones, moving to H in every

instance but one.

In summary, cadences, and therefore formal units,

normally end with complete or implied root-position tonic

triads, usually major triads. Half cadences and other types

occur at times, but usually at the end of the first phrase

of a period. Most of the cadences ending on a tonic triad

have as penultimates harmonies with a dominant root. A wide

variety of structures is found among these dominant

sonorities, the members of which normally resolve by step.

The one notable exception to this rule is 4, which normally

resolves down by major third. Other roots are found,

notably pitches a major third above or below the tonic. We

now turn our attention from the end of the phrase to the

beginning and middle.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
83

CHAPTER IV

KEY, SCALE, AND MODULATION

The music of Romeo and Juliet features a metrical

regularity that invites anticipation of cadences at

particular points in time. Just before the anticipated

occurrence of such a cadence the root of the cadential

sonority may also be projected by the listener according to

his knowledge of cadence formulas. We now turn our

attention to ways in which Prokofiev establishes a key,

providing a tonal context that makes possible the even

earlier anticipation of the pitch of the final sonority.

The essence of tonality demands that the tonic pitch be

apparent early in a piece; if a tonic is not known until the

final sonority, the piece is not really tonal. We therefore

begin our discussion with methods of establishing a key at

the beginning of a melody or number. The early establish­

ment of a key has other consequences as well. For instance,

it invokes the perception of the tonal "colouring" of

sonorities throughout a given section in the manner de­

scribed by Dahlhaus. (See page 22.) In addition, it makes

possible the perception of certain events as modulations.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
84

Methods of Establishing Kevs

The course of music history has provided the western

composer with means for suggesting a key with just a few

sonorities, or even one. For instance, even the briefest

audiation of a dominant-seventh sonority can, under most

circumstances, suggest a key, as it does in the usual

introduction to "Happy Birthday." Prokofiev does not use

the dominant seventh to establish a key at the beginning of

any number in the ballet. However, there are a number of

instances in which a dominant seventh followed by the

standard resolution establishes the key of a new section in

the middle of a number. The chord of resolution normally

occurs on what is perceived as the first measure of a

group— the strong measure in a hypermeasure, to use Edward

T. Cone's terminology— and is perceived as a tonic, as is

the case at rehearsal 111. (See example 46.)

With less persuasiveness, a major or minor triad heard

as a first sonority can present itself as a tonic triad.

The argument becomes more convincing if the triad is

repeated or sustained. This method Prokofiev takes

advantage of several times in the work. (See example 47.)

For instance, the D-major triad outlined and repeated at the

beginning of number 3, "The Street Wakens," is heard as a

tonic triad (example 47a). Similarly, the E-minor triad

repeated at the beginning of number 13, "The Dance of the

Knights," is heard as a tonic (example 47b). As a third

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 46. Resolution of a dominant seventh used to
establish tonic in 110:16-111:8.

i^ii r -E— * | n uf |
m f ' Sf rtss. t

/ r H f o -T 3 «* J>-J-=
r r *«f t
___ _________
Oh. _______

L 1*I' r—
i,l 1i | 7 |
L O T 'y ^
. Iwt
f C .i^ l ___ T il 1
„ ----------------- ________ Cor., V-.i

« .jn -rn .rm


s: t ....

example, the repeated D-minor triad at the beginning of

number 32, "The Meeting of Tybalt and Mercutio," is heard as

a tonic sonority (example 47c). The term "tonic" may be

used here despite the presence of b-naturals, which suggest

a modal system, since tonality is not dependent on the

presence of any particular scale. Any of these triads, or

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
86

Example 47. Triad repetition used to establish tonic in a)


9:1-4, b ) 76:1, and c) 247:1.

a
g i A lle g re tto J :t>«

b c
Mode ra to

others like them, might, during the course of a number,

prove to have served some tonal function other than that of

a tonic triad, or even to have been merely the first in a

succession of tonally unrelated triads. That Prokofiev

consistently uses such triads as tonic harmonies, however,

is accepted by the listener early in the piece and

reinforces his natural inclination to interpret them as

tonic harmonies.

In such a style as this, even a single repeated pitch

can suggest a key. In Romeo and Juliet, a repeated opening

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
87

pitch may prove to be the tonic, as at the beginning of

number 4, "Morning Dance," but more often it turns out to be

the dominant, as in number 18, "Gavotte," and number 49,

"Dance of the Girls with Lilies." In any case, the listener

can expect one of two closely related keys when he hears a

single, repeated pitch at the beginning of a number.

When a piece does not begin with a simple repeated

triad or pitch, other factors that point to one pitch as

tonic are usually present. For instance, at the beginning

of number 6, "The Fight," it is a pedal point on c and the

consistent presence of C-major harmony on downbeats that

establish the tonic, even though the alternation of C-major

and D-major harmonies which takes place above it is not a

pattern traditionally found in C-major tonality. (See

example 48.) At the beginning of number 15, "Mercutio,"

Example 48. Pedal point used to establish tonic in 26:1-4.

[2i$]Presto J - . i m

shown in example 49, it is the outlining of a perfect fifth

that establishes the tonic. Emphasis is placed on a-flat as

the first and most often heard pitch, and on e-flat as the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 49. Emphasis on perfect fifth used to establish
tonic in 97:1-5.

resolution of both the f-flat cambiata in measure 1 and the

appoggiatura in measure 2. (The g-natural on the downbeat

has the rhythmic effect of an appoggiatura, even though it

is a major third away from its resolution.) The perfect

fifth formed by these two pitches prompts the interpretation

of a-flat as the tonic.

The opening of the ballet is unique in its means of

establishing a tonality. (See example 50.) By the third

harmony, that is, the downbeat of the first measure, c-

natural is clearly perceived to be tonic, although neither a

C-major harmony nor a G-dominant-7 has been sounded. The

main cause of the phenomenon is the use in these three

harmonies of every note of a diatonic scale, in this case

the C-major scale. While the pitch sequence in the upper

line, a-b-c, is not sufficient in itself to prompt the

interpretation of c as the tonic, its melodic stress on c

confirms the suggestion made by the use of the scale.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
89

Example 50. Diatonic scale used to establish tonic in


Beginning:l-l:l .

A n d a n te a s s a i

The first three harmonies of the ballet make to the

listener many suggestions concerning the style of the music

that is to follow, among them the following two notions.

First, they suggest that the diatonic scale is the primary

pitch source of the piece. This is not to say that most

melodies in the ballet consist of only diatonic pitches.

There are melodies with many chromatic pitches, melodies

with occasional modal inflections, and melodies of nearly

equal numbers of diatonic and chromatic pitches. But then

most of this is true of Mozart as well, and each of Mozart's

pieces includes at the beginning of each line a key

signature indicating, among other things, the seven pitch

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
90

classes (in the case of major-mode pieces, in any case)

which constitute the main pitch source for the piece. The

diatonic scale is the primary pitch source in Romeo and

Juliet in the sense that by occasional use of a pure

diatonic scale and by invoking traditional listening

patterns, a single diatonic scale seems to predominate at

almost every particular moment, chromatic pitches serving as

embellishments of the diatonic pitches. Second, these three

harmonies suggest that the diatonic scale can by its

presence alone establish a key. While at the beginning of a

number, a key is usually established by one of the methods

described above, the perception is usually corroborated by

the use of a diatonic scale, in most cases, a major scale.

In the middle of a number, on the other hand, the presence

of a diatonic scale, complete or implied, plays the primary

role in the establishment of a new key.

Successjpns of Scales

There are several examples of purely diatonic melodies

in Romeo and Juliet. Among these are the opening melody of

number 22, "Folk Dance," and the contemplative melody first

heard at rehearsal 55. There are also melodies that contain

only occasional chromatic notes, these functioning clearly

as embellishments of diatonic scale degrees. In the melody

beginning four measures after rehearsal 1, for example, the

one chromatic pitch, e-sharp, clearly serves as a neighbor

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
91

to f-sharp. (See example 18, page 39.) Others, such as the

melody in measures 2:6-13, have several chromatic pitches,

their relationship to the diatonic pitches always evident.

(See example 51.) Still other melodies seem to use several

diatonic scales, one after the other. In this type, most

pitches with accidentals are heard not as chromatic pitches

in relation to the original scale, but as diatonic pitches

of a secondary scale. The "Gavotte" melody, for instance,

uses pitches of the D-major scale for about two measures, B-

major for about one-and-a-half, D-major again, C-sharp-major

(with one chromatic embellishment: f-double-sharp), and D-

major one last time. (Please refer again to example 22,

page 50.) While an examination of the function of each

pitch, both diatonic and chromatic, will be taken up in a

later chapter, here we will examine the methods and patterns

used in scale succession.

A scale having been established at the beginning of a

number, a new scale can become predominant at two different

times: at the beginning of a period or in the middle of one.

The first instance is not at all unlike the situation with

changes of key that occur between strains of a Chopin waltz

or a Sousa march. This type of change may be properly

termed a modulation. The second instance represents the

single most recognizable feature of Prokofiev's tonal style.

Because of the small temporal scope of the changes of the

second type, they will be termed scale shifts, not

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
92

Example 51. Highly chromatic melody in 2:5-12.

timiU
■t-'prr*! x -i i i
— rni ii.
<J f l . T T ^J-id1vJ id-

waJL .F^l 1 m > n i 1 — p"i 1i ■!— r* i i i

j------ ------

vJT7 i J i Ji

modulations. It may be said that a little under half the

tonally closed melodies in the ballet contain scale shifts,

although it is impossible to arrive at an exact figure since

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
93

it is difficult at times to distinguish a scale shift from a

brief use of chromaticism.

The first change of scale in the piece, at the

beginning of measure 7, is a scale shift rather than a

modulation. (Please refer again to example 50.) Here, the

C-major scale is suddenly replaced by the B-major scale.

The method Prokofiev uses is a model for many of the scale

shifts and modulations in the rest of the piece. The first

clarinet, which is playing an ascending scale in measure 6,

plays, on the downbeat of measure 7, a d-sharp, one half-

step lower than the diatonic e-natural that might have been

expected. It is this half-step adjustment of a note in an

ascending scale that so often features in the scale shifts

in the work. The particular intervallic pattern of this

scale segment— beginning with the g-sharp in measure 6, the

pattern alternates half-step, whole-step, half-step,

whole-step, half-step— is often but not always found in

later instances.

A similar adjustment to a descending scale is also

present in this first scale shift. In measure 6, the second

clarinet descends through the C-major scale from f-natural

to b-natural. The a-natural that would follow on the

downbeat of measure 7 as the next pitch in the scale is

replaced by an a-sharp. Where the line is descending, the

downbeat presents a note higher than anticipated; where

ascending, a note lower than anticipated. The listener

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
94

quickly becomes accustomed to the practice so that, later in

the ballet, when this half-step adjustment is heard, he can

immediately assume that a shift of scales has taken place.

At the same time tnat the d—sharp and the a—sharp are

introduced, an f-sharp appears in the bass. Interestingly,

these very notes, all three foreign to the C-major scale,

are briefly heard simultaneously above the short g-natural

pedal in measure 3. Aside from the fact of the presence of

the g-natural, these notes are not heard in measure 3 as

representing a scale shift, the reason being that two of

them immediately resolve to pitches in the C-major scale

(the a-sharp somewhat mysteriously disappearing, as

described on page 65.) In measure 7, on the other hand,

these pitches are quickly perceived as diatonic pitches from

a new predominating scale both because the d-sharp and the

f-sharp do not resolve to notes in the C-major scale, and

because the melody that follows proves to be a transposition

of that heard in measure 3.1

Another method Prokofiev uses in the ballet to shift

scales is the harmonization of a diatonic melodic pitch with

a chord, usually a simple triad, foreign to the scale that

predominates up to that point. In the "Gavotte,” for

*A d-sharp in the cellos in measure 7 does happen to move


to e-natural, which is, incidentally, in the C-major scale.
Also, the a-sharp does resolve to b-natural here. Is it a
matter of subtle foreshadowing that prompted Prokofiev to
refrain from resolving the a-sharp in measure 3 to the very
pitch that would prove to be tonic five measures later?

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
95

instance, the a-g-f-sharp of the melody in measures 3 and 4

and the d-e-f-sharp of the bass in the same place are both

segments of the diatonic D-major scale, this key having been

established on the downbeat of measure 3. (Please refer

again to example 22, page 50.) This diatonic contrapuntal

frame, however, is harmonized by D-major, C-major, and B-

major triads, the last two foreign to the key of D-major.

The B-major triad might be heard as a V/ii, although the

parallel fifths leading to it and the fact that it is in

second inversion may be exceptional enough to cloud

traditional expectations. However, it soon becomes apparent

that the B-major harmony is not a chromatic chord in a

passage using the D-major scale, but rather the first

diatonic harmony in a brief passage, measures 4 and 5, using

the B-major scale. Similarly, at the downbeat of measure 6,

the b-natural in the melody, which is the expected

culmination of the rising B-major scale in measure 5, is

harmonized, not with a chord from B-major, but with the

subdominant harmony from the original scale, D-major. In

each of the cases described here, a single melodic pitch

acts as a pivot tone, much as it does in the common-tone

modulation of the previous century, being diatonic to both

the preceding and following predominant scales.

These two shifts actually combine both the methods

described: altered scale segments appear in inner voices in

each case. In the first shift, a-g-f-sharp in the upper

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
96

line is accompanied in parallel thirds by f-sharp-e-d-sharp

in the second line from the top. The d-natural that would

have served as the diatonic completion of this descending

scale segment is replaced by a note one half-step higher.

In the second shift, while the b-natural in the first

violins serves as a common-tone to the two scales in

question, the second violins play an ascending line in

parallel sixths to the melody, reaching a d-natural at the

downbeat, one half-step lower than the d-sharp that would

have occurred had no shift taken place. The last five notes

of the second violins' scale constitute, because of the

half-step adjustment, a sequence of alternating whole-steps

and half-steps.

Should it be possible for a few notes to represent and

establish the priority of a new scale without all the

pitches of that scale necessarily explicitly sounding, that

is, if a few pitches can be made to sound as stable elements

of a new scale rather than as foreign, unstable elements of

a previous scale, a quick succession of scale shifts would

in turn be possible. This in fact proves to be easily

accomplished. The final harmony of a phrase, for instance,

is heard as having a certain degree of stability, at the

local level at least, even if the chord is foreign to the

scale of the passage leading to it. Knowing scale shifts

occur in this music, a listener might interpret such a chord

as the representative of a new scale. The beginning of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
97

next phrase might then use the pitches of yet a third scale.

In this manner, two scale shifts will have been accomplished

in as many measures.

Multiple scale shifts in quick succession can be seen

in the melody at rehearsal 6. (See example 52.) The

Example 52. Multiple scale shifts in quick succession in


6:1-7:1.

n.,Arr«, rino

t 1H '1
t fj ir ,r
br hjf »rt t ~ ^ f.,^r
.j, m r r i
primary key and scale are established in the following way.

The first downbeat of the melody proper occurs two measures

after rehearsal 6. The ascending fourth motion in the bass

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
98

establishes e-natural as the tonic. The dominant harmony on

the pick-up beat differs in one detail from the traditional

dominant-seventh chord, as do so many of the dominant

harmonies in this ballet; the chord possesses not the

traditional minor seventh, but a major seventh, the raised

fourth degree of the E-major scale. This degree continues

to be raised in the measures following. Thus the

predominating diatonic scale at the beginning of this melody

is the E-Lydian scale.

This scale continues with no chromatic embellishments

until the fourth measure, the cadential measure of the first

phrase. Here the seventh degree of the scale, found in both

melody and bass, is harmonized with a C-minor triad. This

chord is foreign to the Lydian scale of the first three

measures. However, because it occurs on a cadential

downbeat, it possesses local-level stability. That is,

while the harmony does not provide tonal closure for the

period— this will come with the tonic chord at the end of

the second phrase— it is not heard as an embellishing chord

that is expected to resolve immediately. The d-natural and

the a-flat in the next beat are enough to define exactly

which diatonic scale is being represented; the scale with

three flats. The next measure brings another change of

scale; there are now four sharps. Although the scale of

three flats is firmly established, it is heard for the

duration of only one measure. Because it predominates for

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
99

such a short period of time, it is both unnecessary and

impossible to determine which note of the scale should be

called the tonic. The C-minor harmony could be analyzed as

the tonic chord in C-minor, for instance, or as the

submediant in E-flat major, or even as the mediant harmony

in A-flat Lydian. But it is none of these. The key does

not change, only the scale. In this style then, a C-minor

harmony can function as a part of the key of E. This

function, which will be defined more fully below, we may now

say is that of completing a half cadence.

An abrupt shift at the end of a melody is perhaps the

most familiar characteristic of the music of Prokofiev.

Such scale shifts can, in a manner similar to that described

above, be established very quickly. When a shift occurs on

the beat just before what is perceived to be a cadential

downbeat, the effect is almost instantaneous. The listener

practically immediately hears the sonority in question as a

penultimate to the projected cadential chord rather than as

a chromatic embellishing chord in relation to the previous

material. In the cadence to the famous "Gavotte11 melody

(please refer again to example 22), for instance, the A-

dorainant-seventh penultimate harmony is heard, even before

the D-major triad is sounded, as a diatonic member, in this

case the dominant, in the new scale rather than as a

chromatic embellishment in the C-sharp-major scale heard

just before. The listener's probable expectation of a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
100

return to the key established at the beginning of a period

makes a shift at this metrical position very convincing.

Because scale shifts are so easily perceived at this

metrical position, such a shift can be achieved with even a

single note. Thus, while it might at first seem that it

would be necessary to play all the notes of a scale, some

more than once, to establish its predominance in a passage

of music, it is in actuality possible for a single note to

imply all the other notes of a scale. Prokofiev often takes

advantage of the phenomenon, using one or more common tones

in the penultimate chord, so that one new note actually

seems to be doing all the work in accomplishing a scale

shift. In example 53a, for instance, the f-sharp does not

fit the E-flat triad preceding it. It is not heard as an

embellishment of E-flat, however. Because of its metrical

position, on the beat just before the fourth downbeat of the

phrase, it is perceived as a penultimate to, and by

implication as a dominant of, the sonority following. (It

is necessary to note that the melody preceding this codetta

is perceived as proceeding by two-beat measures, four per

phrase, and is notated thus when it first appears, at

rehearsal 9.) This projection turns out to be correct: b-

natural is indeed the next pitch. The f-sharp, then, while

being the only change explicitly sounded, bears by

implication other changes. When it is sounded, the listener

may imagine the b-natural, the e-natural, and the c-sharp as

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
101

Example 53. Scale shift accomplished with one note in 49:8-


9; a) score, b) diagram of implied changes.

a tempo

well, all notes foreign to the E-flat scale but diatonic

members of the B-major scale. (The other notes are common

to both scales if enharmonic equivalence is assumed.)

Example 53b, in which B-major has been enharmonically

respelled as C-flat-major, shows one possible interpretation

of the change, as an addition of four flats, the four

appropriate scale degrees being lowered by a half step.

Again, all of this is accomplished by a single note.

In example 54, several notes make up the penultimate

sonority. One note, b-natural, is held in common by both

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 54. Scale shift accomplished with one note in 50:7-
8; a) score, b) diagram of implied changes.

that chord and the one preceding. A second tone, e-natural,

while not a member of the preceding chord, does fit the B-

major scale of the preceding passage. Only the g-natural is

foreign to the B-major scale. However, Western ears are

familiar with the lowered sixth scale degree, as g-natural

would be in the key of B-major, as a common chromatic

embellishment. Why then is the penultimate harmony, e-g-b,

not heard as a minor iv chord in B major? Or why is the B-

major triad not heard as V of the next chord, an E-minor in

first inversion? It is the metrical position of the e-g-b

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
103

sonority that causes it to be heard as a penultimate

sonority, as a dominant. The emphasis placed on g-natural

because of its position as the bass note causes it to be

heard as the root of the chord, the dominant of C. Thus the

presence of a single pitch accomplishes a scale shift that

involves a change of no fewer than five pitches. (See

example 54b.)

Relationships between Successive Scales

The most common place for a scale shift is just prior

to an authentic cadence. A wide variety of root

relationships is found between the chords at these cadential

shifts. At times the root moves up a half-step to the

dominant as at measure 122:13. (See example 55a.) At other

times there is a third relationship— ascending as at measure

49:9, or descending as at measure 50:8— or even a tritone

relationship as at measure 152:8. (See examples 55b, 55c,

and 55d.) In fact, all eight of the roots possible in the

ante-penultimate position are used, one about as often as

another. (0, 2, and 5, are traditional choices and would

not constitute a shift. Excluding these pitches and 7, the

dominant, leaves eight possibilities.)

On the other hand, an examination of the tonics of the

two scales at each cadential shift reveals a preference for

certain "scale relationships," as they can be called. (We

use the term "tonic" here for lack of a better term, even

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
104

Example 55. Root relationships at scale shifts in a)


122:13-123:1, b) 49:8-9, c) 50:8, and d) 152:8-153:1.

rtlrl **"! h
m (
JBT J """ P

though we have said that a scale shift does not constitute a

change of key.) The secondary scale most often used just

prior to a cadential shift is that built on 1. The shift at

measure 152:8 provides the clearest example. (See example

56.) Here a V-I cadence in G-major is preceded by a passage

using the A-flat-major scale. The melodies at rehearsals

206 and 48 provide further examples. The next example of

the use of the scale on 1 just before a cadential shift, in

measures 178:14-15, is complicated slightly by the fact that

the scale on 1 is represented by a deceptive cadence

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
105

Example 56. Cadential shift from scale on l in 151:10-


153:1.

formula. (Please refer again to example 14, page 33.)

Incidentally, this example happens to represent one of the

few times a harmonic-minor scale, a scale which is not

diatonic in the sense that the major scale and all the modal

scales are, is used as a secondary scale in this work. Less

clear are two examples that each involve a V-I progression,

not as a cadence, but as a key-establishing figure at the

beginning of a phrase. These are found in measures 26:8 and

73:8. (See examples 57 and 58.) In each case, while the

predominance of the scale on 1 may or may not be established

in any given listener's mind, the dominant harmony of that

scale is heard briefly just before the dominant harmony of

the succeeding scale. In such a case, the ante-penultimate

chord might be construed as a chord built on the lowered

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
106

Example 57. Scale shift before authentic-cadence figure at


beginning of a phrase in 26:7-27:2.

-if iT . > > f = = / ^ t >« >e


1 1, 1,
1 ^ ^ ^ ifcj u 1

Example 58. Scale shift before authentic-cadence figure at


beginning of a phrase in 73:1-74:1.

GUcot-H# --- s• I—
r - 1 ^

j ij ij j
&r9
C-k. >
• \9 •f?
>
■ \=i»
> > ^ ^ ^ ^
Ok.

/ifi* r-iri ri n *i T ^ f ' ~ r ~ ? . r r » M rr r l.j j . j . j^1


\ ° - f i\iU ■J ----------. r - r„ w J
« *. . • ^ ^ ^ .**
rM'TfTf ■ r^ i* = a ^ .f— n-

T T T T ^ iBL^Jrd

jigi n i flurimPi1'?1P ^ n ft
'
‘tie. __ "1

sixth scale degree or as a German-sixth chord. In measure

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
107

26:8, however, the E-major triad, with the melodic

embellishments that accompany it, is part of a sequence of

dominant harmonies, and will be heard as such, thus implying

the presence of the A-minor scale just prior to a V-I

progression in A-flat. Interestingly, this is another of

the handful of cases in the work in which a harmonic-minor

scale is used as a secondary scale.

The second most common scale used in this position is

that built on H . The final shift in the "Gavotte" provides

an example that should be familiar to the reader by this

time: the C-sharp-major scale of measures 122:12-13 is

followed by V7-I in D-major. Other examples of a shift from

this scale are found at measures 48:6-7 and at measure 50:8.

Two other scales are used in this situation. The

harmonic-minor scale on 10 is found in the melody at

rehearsal 111, and the major scale on 4 just before the end

of rehearsal 49. (Please refer again to example 46, page 85,

and example 53, page 101.)

Two remaining examples may be considered, both

occurring in the passage in measures 144:5-7. (See example

59.) The passage is transitional rather than cadential, but

involves two authentic-cadence figures. The first one

establishes b-flat as the tonic momentarily; e-natural, the

tonic of the previously prevailing scale, is 6 in relation

to b-flat. One measure later, the b-flat is replaced as

tonic by g-natural. B-flat serves as 3 in relation to g.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
108

Example 59. Scale shifts before authentic-cadence figures


in transitional passage, measures 144:3-9.

t>\ Idale* T"


n a iA 4- xj— L .
i
♦ Pff S £L Ot '. .

The second most common place in a melody for a scale

shift is at the end of the first phrase in a period. As

described above, the new scale here is usually represented

by a single chord, on the downbeat, instead of by two

chords, as at a final cadence. There is a remarkable

consistency of relationships found among the scale shifts in

this metrical position. Nearly all the shifts involve at

some point a chord whose root is the lowered sixth scale

degree, or &, of the first scale. The third of the chord,

11 or 0 depending on the quality of the chord, is usually in

the melody. In this way the reharmonization of a diatonic

scale degree— as noted above, a key feature of many scale

shifts— is achieved.

In several melodies, the shift and the chord built on 2.

both occur right on the fourth downbeat. For instance, the

E-Lydian melody at rehearsal 6, discussed above, consists of

two phrases, the first of which ends with a C-minor triad,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
109

c-natural being a in E. (Please refer again to example 52.)

On the downbeat, H is in the melody as well as in the bass.

The period at rehearsal 152 also consists of two four-

measure phrases. (Please refer again to example 56.) The

first of these ends on an implied E-flat-major triad, e-flat

being £ in the original key of the period, G-major. E-flat

is in the bass while g-natural, £, is in the melody. As

a third example, the harmony in the fourth measure of the

"Gavotte" melody, while serving as IV in the original key of

D major, is built on £ of the immediately preceding scale,

B-major. (Please refer again to example 22.) B-natural, £,

is in the melody.

Whereas the three periods just described use triads on

£ for the cadence of the first phrase, other periods move

through a triad on £ to reach the cadential sonority. One

example is the opening B-flat melody in number 12, "Masks."

(See example 60.) This melody, which consists of two four-

measure phrases, is repeated immediately in a slightly

altered version. The first phrase of the first version

cadences on a rather dense chord that is likely to be

interpreted as a D-minor chord with added notes, while that

of the second version cadences on a similarly dense chord

which can be interpreted as a D-flat-major with additions.

The note in the melody is in the first case H , in the

second case, l£. But while the cadential sonorities of

these two statements differ, the chord on beat 3 of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 60. First-phrase scale shifts moving through a
triad on S in 71:1-72:8.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 60— continued.

CvTiV t ( i t ' H \t it1


.SfTtii,. f i i —

-L't4J It I
----
r^-
""{*£7I
iJ
T^"TUFiTifo
]i{J v v I— 1

■a*/~TTfn.^n~~'1~~7ii
iay *if ? 7 T »T =y * r.j^j .*
T,
#f- * '

^ f 3 1

— "■ fbr rfJ


i^lip Lf =t§^tSri § =@ r

»k T. » a. i r r r t ^

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
112

third measure of each, when the shift really begins to take

place, is essentially the same— a minor triad on f-sharp, 8,

with added tones. In each instance, the chord that follows,

on the beat just prior to the cadential downbeat, is the

dominant harmony of the cadential pitch.

The passage at rehearsal 111 similarly moves through a

triad on 8 just before the end of the first phrase. (Please

refer again to example 46, page 85.) This phrase, which

begins in C-major, cadences on a sonority which is most

likely heard as a G-flat-major triad in second inversion,

with a long appoggiatura on e-flat. It is the only phrase

in the ballet that cadences on a chord a tritone away from

the main tonic pitch. Just prior to this chord, however, is

the sonority that makes the passage like so many others, an

A-flat-major triad, a-flat being 8 in relation to c.

The passage at rehearsal 291 involves two shiftsto8

in a row. (See example 61.) When the melody begins to veer

away from B-flat major, in measure 291:3, the first move is

to a sonority with g-flat, 8, in the bass. At this point,

the e-flat in the melody is interpreted as an appoggiatura,

the implied harmony being a G-flat-major triad. However, as

this e-flat is resolved down by step, another e-flat appears

in the accompanying string parts. By the end of the

measure, the sonority is clearly an E-flat minor chord. The

subsequent move to a B-minor-seventh harmony in measure 4,

then, is heard as a move to 8 of E-flat minor.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
113

Example 61. Two successive shifts to chord on £ in 290:11-


291:4.

12911 Poco pin animato

4 . =
y 9 ^ ’
^ OJB
J^ , 1
if-i ____ ^ * cl ,n.

-f - 7 5 -si* d

* UA
...

ijisL t' ....... - le t

Not all scale shifts happen at or just prior to

cadences. In the tune at rehearsal 73, the first phrase

ends on an E-flat major chord, still heard as IV of the

original key of B-flat. (Please refer again to example 58.)

The second phrase, however, begins immediately with F-sharp-

minor harmony. The sonority is heard as a new beginning and

not as an embellishment of the harmonies immediately

preceding and following. While this harmony does not

exhibit a local-level embellishing function, however, as the

listener becomes familiar with the importance of chords

built on £, such harmonies begin to take on a structural

function. This melody's first shift of scale is

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
114

accomplished through means of a chord built on a, just as

those of all the melodies described above were. The only

difference is that in this melody the shift takes place at

the beginning of the second phrase instead of at the end of

the first phrase, £'s function in this style as the basis

of the first scale shift in a period is very similar to the

structural function ascribed in traditional tonality to the

dominant and, in the minor mode, the third scale degree.

In a similar fashion, the melody at 214, actually the

second part of a longer melody, begins its second half with

a chord built on 8. (See example 62.) At measure 5, the

Example 62. Scale shift to £ starting second phrase of a


melody in 213:11-214:8.

cello arpeggiates a C-major harmony, c-natural being £ in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
115

the key of E. While in this case the chord may be heard

fulfilling a traditional function, that of VI borrowed from

minor resolving to V through V/V, it is significant that, as

the most chromatic chord in the passage, the root is j£ in

the key. fi is in the melody on the downbeat.

The melody at rehearsal 201 provides a final example of

a scale shift involving a chord on £. (See example 63.)

Example 63. Scale shift to £ during second phrase of a


melody in 201:1-4.

The metrical placement of this shift is slightly unusual.

If the passage is viewed as consisting not of four measures

of 4/4 but of two four-measure phrases in 2/4, the shift is

seen to occur on the second beat of measure 6, the second

measure of the second phrase. The E-flat-dominant-seventh

chord leads directly to the A-flat-major triad on the next

downbeat, a-flat being a in relation to the tonic, c-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
116

natural. The return to the original scale one beat later is

remarkable. The c-sharp on beat 2 of measure 3 in the first

phrase is heard as an appoggiatura to d-natural, and the f-

natural in the bass is heard as the third of a D-minor

triad, the ii chord in C-major. C-sharp occurs at the same

metrical position in the second phrase. However, it is

heard there as the fourth scale degree of the preceding A-

flat-major scale, and the f in the bass as the third of a D-

flat-major triad, IV in A-flat. These pitches then are

resolved in C major exactly as they were in the first phrase

so that the shift back to the original scale is perceived

only retrospectively. Scale shifts in Prokofiev's music are

not usually characterized by such a degree of subtlety.

The relationship between a tonic and the pitch a major

third lower has been the object of attention twice so far.

First, it was seen as a somewhat common root relationship

between the two final chords at cadences. Second, it has

been seen as a very common root relationship at scale

shifts, especially when the main scale of a period first

gives way to a new scale. Whereas traditionally the chord

most often used as a penultimate at cadences, that is, the

dominant, is also the tonic chord of the most common

secondary key and the chord most often found at half

cadences, in Romeo and Juliet the dominant is rarely found

at half cadences or at scale shifts, either as the root of a

triad or as the tonic of a scale. Instead, it is the less

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
117

common penultimate, the lowered sixth scale degree in

traditional terms, or 8 in the terminology used in this

treatise, that normally fulfills these functions.

Other relationships are found, however. For instance,

at rehearsal 88, a melody in E-minor shifts up a minor

third, to G-minor at its fifth measure. (Please refer to

example 77, page 138.) At rehearsal 84 the shift, which

occurs at measure 6, slightly later than the usual time, is

from E-minor up four half-steps to A-flat-minor. (Please

refer to example 79, page 142.) While only something over

half the modulating melodies move to or through £, by

generalizing and saying third relationships are the most

common, nearly every melody can be said to fit the norm.

Modulations
Earlier a distinction was made between scale shifts and

modulations. Two aspects featured in the distinction:

whether the change occurs at the beginning of a melody or in

the middle, and for how long the change is effective. The

latter is actually dependent on the former. When a change

of scale occurs in the middle of a period, the listener may

expect the original scale of the period to return, as indeed

it does in nearly every case. However, when a change occurs

at the beginning of a new period in a collage of melodies

such as is often heard in this ballet, there need be no

expectation concerning further changes. Indeed, twenty-four

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
118

of the fifty-two numbers in the work start in one key and

end in another. Because of the need for a return in the

case of a scale shift, the change is usually not effective

for very long. A change at the beginning of a melody or

section, on the other hand, can normally be effective for a

much longer period of time. In such a case, the lack of a

need for return and the longer duration of the presence of

the scale lends independence to the scale. It thus

represents its own key, and any such change is heard as a

true modulation.

Often the modulations in Romeo and Juliet are much more

direct than the scale shifts, and therefore somewhat less

interesting. Three times in number 11, "Arrival of the

Guests," for instance, there is simply an ending in one key

and a beginning in another. This occurs at rehearsals 65,

66, and 67. Where a dominant-seventh chord precedes the

downbeat of a chromatic modulation as at measure 60:8, the

sound is similar to that of a scale shift, but the effect is

not the same? a new key really seems to be established, not

just a secondary scale area within a tonally closed melody.

(See example 64.)

What is of some interest, however, is the array of key

relationships exhibited by modulations. While the perfect

fourth is an uncommon relationship between scale areas

within a period, it is quite commonly found as the interval

between successive keys. On the other hand, the major third

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
119

Example 64. Chromatic modulation in 60:5-11.

and minor second, one or the other of which lies at the base

of most scale shifts, are found about as often as the

fourth. All other intervals are found, but much less often.

A typical scheme is that of number 10, "Young Juliet."

The opening eight bars, in C-major, are followed by a

somewhat transitional passage that begins in C and modulates

to F, motion of a perfect fourth. The next statement of the

main melody is in E-major, a minor second lower. At

rehearsal 53, a contrasting melody whose tonic, a-flat, is a

major third away from the previous tonic, intervenes. It is

followed by a third statement of the opening melody, this

time in A-major, representing a modulation up by half step.

A new contrasting melody, in C, enters at rehearsal 55. The

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
120

minor third between its tonic and that of the previous

passage is the only minor third between tonics in this

number. The passage goes through transitional material

around rehearsal 56, but ends in E-major, a major third

higher. The next melody, in F, is followed by the opening

melody, in C. It is interesting to note that a striking

number of the major-third relationships occur between the

pitches c, e, and a-flat. This was shown earlier to be so

in the case of cadences. Now it is evident that the

relationship is comnon in key relationships as well.

The case of Friar Laurence's first theme is unique in

the work in two respects. (See example 65.) First, it

Example 65. Modulation with no change of scale in 207:1-4.

And a n t e e s p r e s s iv o J:
2
[ OT] Ci.,

contains the only phrase in the ballet that moves to and

cadences on the dominant. Second, it achieves this

tonicization with no explicit change of scale. Thus,

although it is a brief move and happens in the middle of a

period, it cannot be called a scale shift. Even without a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
121

change of scale, however, the tonic seems to change

temporarily. How is a modulation achieved in such a brief

passage with no change of scale? The answer lies in the

counterpoint. The repetition of the B-flat harmony over the

course of three-and-a-half measures, far from making the

passage tonally static, rather removes from the harmony any

role in the tonal motion, much as a pedal point is normally

not considered partof the harmonies occurring above it.

All tonal motion is therefore a product of the motion of the

two lines.

The modulation begins on the downbeat of measure 3.

The major sixth heard here, c and a, is not perceived as the

second and seventh scale degrees in the main key of B-flat.

This is partly because no harmonic interpretation of these

two tones makes sense: neither the dominant harmony nor the

leading-tone harmony normally follows the mediant. Also

important, however, is the metrical placement. Unembel­

lished dominant-function harmonies do not normally occur on

downbeats in a passage in which harmonies change every beat.

Tonic chords in second inversion, however, do. The Western

listener then may interpret the c-natural in the bass as a

dominant, in F-major. One measure later, the interval

recurs, is resolved in t;he expected fashion to a fifth, and

followed by an octave on the new tonic. The harmony that

enters one half beat later contains the first e of any kind

in the passage. The fact that it is an e-flat and not an e-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
122

natural immediately returns to f its role as a dominant and

to b-flat its role as tonic.

The most dramatic use of modulation occurs in the last

number. (See example 66.) Here the beautiful melody first

heard at rehearsal 55, in "Young Juliet," is played four

times, alternately in the keys of C-major and A-flat-major.

A coda in C-major follows. Each phrase, beginning firmly in

a new key, sounds fresh and full of promise. In the C-major

statements, however, the melody cadences imperfectly, on the

third scale degree. And the A-flat statements do not end

with any cadence at all,but with a short, rather dissonant

extension that leads back to C major. The contrast between

the brave, sure beginning and the inconclusive end of each

phrase, paralleling the failure of Friar Laurence's plan to

bring resolution to the young couple's bold desires, is, in

part, what makes this final number so moving.

In summary, each period in Romeo and Juliet begins with

a firmly established key and uses, at almost every point, a

diatonic scale, usually a major scale, as its primary pitch

source. These predominating scales often change suddenly

within a period, most commonly at the cadential sonority in

the first phrase ofa period and at the penultimate sonority

in the last phrase. Thefirst type almost always involves a

chord built on £, while the second commonly involves scales

built on 1 or li. When a change of scales happens at the

beginning of a period, it has the effect of a true

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
123

Example 66. Dramatic use of modulation in 360:1-362:5.

13601

/li f. ' | M 3 = | ff-f y f-f :j F f ^ P $ j F-'f F


tS 1 r i ■ r
F "-T'1 t *— r— I f J
' m f cspress.

) J U l l ^ L i J ___ L u —
L*.. ... ,{. a . . . ----------£ —

m *
nkTTi f%\ a_.^Lr7* f E.,.. i =
g r X-rij= r f 1-f r m = ■f
/-■ j j i i-O f '*"/■' J' T ^-C.J■j-H- ■

t = ¥ T \ ki ’ bi-* r

j: 1 »r r ”

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
124

modulation. Major thirds, xainor seconds, and perfect

fourths are the most common scale relationships found at

these points. In the following chapter, we will add more

detail to our model of the construction of Prokofiev's

melodies by examining their structures.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
125

CHAPTER V

MELODIC STRUCTURE

Having discussed the way in which melodies are normally

constructed from a diatonic set of pitches or from a

sequence of such sets, we now turn our attention to the ways

individual pitches within a set are used, dealing first with

the short-range structural use of these pitches. While any

discussion of melodic structure depends on a reductive

analytical method of some kind, it is important to stress

that the reductions performed here are not necessarily

Schenkerian. Some of the melodies in Romeo and Juliet are

based on a scalar descent from 3 or §, just as Schenker

found to be true of most of the music of which he approved.

(Schenker's other possibility, a descent from § to i is not

found in Romeo and Juliet .) Others, however, have

fundamental lines that descend only to 3. or prolong 3 for

nearly the length of the period while a 3-2-1 descent occurs

at the very end in the lowest part. Still others have step

progressions that ascend rather than descend, or merely

oscillate between 8 and *1, while others yet cannot be

reduced to a step progression at all. In order to do

reductive analysis on such melodies, it is necessary to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
126

begin with the assumption that a pitch is made important by

having a strong metrical position, by being relatively

consonant, by receiving tonic or agogic accentuation, or by

occupying a position analogous to that of a note previously

deemed important. These pitches may form a step

progression, a structural phenomenon with a history longer

than even Schenker attested to. But often they do not.

With metrical regularity directing the music toward a goal,

traditional fundamental lines are unnecessary. Thus, while

the methods and symbology used in the following analyses may

be Schenkerian, the results are not always so.

Melodies with Descending Structures

Some of the melodies descend in one of the traditional

ways canonized by Schenker. These are generally the more

diatonic melodies and usually accompany the less emotionally

complex scenes. To illustrate, most of the melodies of the

"Introduction," in which the innocence and passion of youth

is depicted, have simple scalar descents as structures. The

second melody, for instance, is almost purely diatonic, and

is based on a repeated fundamental line. (See example

67.) The next melody, with only two chromatic pitches, is

built on a descent from the fifth scale degree. (See

example 68.) The melody at rehearsal 151, one of the folk

dances, is entirely diatonic and has as a structural line a

S-3-3-2-1 descent. (See example 69.) The opening melody of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
127

Example 67. Traditional descending structure in 1:5-8.

Example 68. Traditional descending structure in 1:13-20.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 69. Traditional descending structure in 151:5-
152:1.

i'f m u i n
(5) 4 3 2 1

J J

the ballet begins with a prolongation of 4 and continues

with a 3-2-1 descent. (See example 70.) This melody is

used later in the ballet, as at rehearsal 289, as a

consequent phrase in a two-phrase period, completing a

descent begun on §, but Prokofiev chooses to let it stand on

its own at the beginning of the work, opening the

"Introduction" with a prolongation of an unstable pitch.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
129

Example 70. Descent from 4 in Beginning:1-4

Many of the two-phrase periods in this work possess a

structure that deviates slightly from traditional schemes.

The most common pattern for periods in previous times

involves a phrase with a half cadence followed by a phrase

with a perfect authentic cadence. The fundamental line of

such periods often comprises a descent that is interrupted

at the second scale degree and then starts over. Because

Prokofiev rarely ends an antecedent phrase on the dominant,

he does not usually have even the opportunity to use such a

structure. His melodies may indeed exhibit an interrupted

descent as a structure, but the interruption is most likely

at the third scale degree. For instance, the fundamental

line of the last melody of the "Introduction," one of the

more chromatic of the descending-structure melodies,

descends to the third degree by the end of the first phrase

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
130

and then starts over to descend to the tonic at the cadence.

(See example 71.)

Example 71. Interruption on 3 in 3:1-13.

» 0 >
f if-*f -

§ 4 3 2 ^
— j .1 - ^ 4 --------
4 --:-

A second melody with an interruption on 3 is found at

rehearsal 18. (See example 72.) While the first phrase

actually ends on the tonic, the step-progression itself

stops at 3, the second phrase resolving the structural

dissonance by including 3 before 1.

In the case of the melody in example 71, the antecedent

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
131

Example 72. Interruption on 3 in 18:5-19:8.

(§» 4 2

phrase's isolated appearance starting in measure 2:6 and the

elision in measure 3:7 that clouds the beginning of the

consequent phrase leave the listener with the initial

impression that the first phrase is the complete melody,

that 3 is the goal of the melody. The melody first heard at

rehearsal 55 also ends on 3, at measure 55:6. (See example

73.) Unlike the melody from the "Introduction," however,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
132

Example 73. Structural descent ending on 3 in 55:1-6.

8 § 3

this phrase never proves, in all its appearances, to be an

antecedent in a period. It acts as though it were a

complete melody, although it is tonally unresolved, much as

the early stirring of love it depicts seems to Juliet to be

in need of resolution. Whether the resolution of Juliet's

desires ever comes is reflected by the development of this

phrase through the course of the ballet.

^elodl,e.s_with_N9iid.esc.eMi.ag Structures
Many of the melodies in Romeo and Juliet are not built

on a scalar descent. Of these, some are accompanied by a

descent to the tonic in lower lines, thus acting as a link

between the traditional and nontraditional types. Such a

melody is found at rehearsal 107 in the number titled

"Madrigal." Much of the melody emphasizes the third scale

degree, b-natural. (See example 74.) For instance, the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
133

Example 74. Structural descent in lowest line in 107:1-17.

A ndante tenero J =»* - ________ ^

n ^ f f T r T j U -.Iit F T

l ~i r r m r r
W . : t — P-
\ "
\ m p ttpreti.____ r
f
\
f ~ ~ it e s !
A —t------ f r " ~ — r f p-
T i. i i - J J J— — Lt H —

jd T T ij i

lim .
'^trliKr rr = = = — i

h u f p r *
10 Jl

first consonant sonority, on the second beat of the first

measure, doubles the third scale degree in the top (violin)

and middle (viola) lines. The middle line ends its

sequential ascent in the first phrase with a chromatic

embellishment of b-natural. In the second phrase, when the

top line finishes its motion, it is the middle line that

continues moving in eighth notes, at first turning around fa-

natural. Although the violas end the phrase with a descent

to the tonic, g-natural, the cellos, playing the lower line,

ascend at the same time, ending the phrase on the third

scale degree. In the chromatic third phrase, b-natural once

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
134

again acts as the goal of a sequence of interlocking thirds

in the viola line. When the fourth phrase begins much as

the first did, the listener begins to suspect the period is

to come to a close in the fourth measure; a scalar descent

from 5 to 1 in the upper line would be the traditional

method of bringing about this tonal closure. The cadential

figure in the melody, however, looks more like a bass line;

3-S-l. The bass line, in turn, contains the melodic

descent; b, a, and g are the last notes in the cello part.

A somewhat similar example is the period starting in

measure 6:3. (See example 75.) The descending step

progression is in the bass while the upper line, played by

the clarinet, unfolds a completely nontraditional structure.

The first long range step movement is from § to 5 at the

fourth downbeat. The step progression continues down a

third to the fifth scale degree, after which the seventh

degree returns as the penultimate note, leading back to the

tonic. This structure may be thought of as a scalar descent

from § to § followed by a return to 8, or as a simple lower

neighbor figure, with the seventh scale degree prolonged by

the descent down a third. Because of the emphasis placed on

as both the cadential pitch of the first phrase and the

penultimate note of the second phrase, the second

interpretation is preferable.

Of passages such as those just described in which a

traditional structural descent is found in the bass, it may

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
135

Example 75. Nontraditional structure in 6:3-7:1.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
136

be said that the upper line merely provides an accompaniment

to the true melodic structure. In passages in which there

is no scalar descent, even in the lowest line, the unusual

step progression of the melody must be considered the

primary melodic framework. A good example of this type is

the period first heard at rehearsal 111. (See example 76.)

Example 76. Nontraditional structure in 111:1-8.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
137

Here, although a step progression indeed serves as the

structural basis for the melody, the progression is not the

traditional descent, but rather an ascent. Much as the

previous example exhibited a structural progression

beginning on and moving away from the tonic, this melody

starts its ascent on 1, moving up through the lowered second

and third scale degrees to the fourth and, eventually, the

fifth degree, whereupon the melody descends through an

arpeggio, not a scale, to the tonic pitch. Such a structure

supposes the importance of the perfect fifth and of long-

range movement by step that underlies traditional

structures, but it utilizes these elements in a way not seen

in earlier times.

A further example of an unusual step progression is

found at rehearsal 88. (See example 77.) Once again, the

progression begins on and moves away from the tonic, jumping

back quickly once it reaches the dominant. The first step

of the progression is represented by the change in the top

note of the arpeggio figure, from e to d. The d-natural,

prolonged through reharmonization as the fifth of a g-minor

triad, is followed by a c-sharp and, at the end of the first

half of the period, a b-natural. At this point the

progression is interrupted in an almost classical sense:

after arrival on a nontonic pitch, the melody begins over

again, the b-natural serving both as the final structural

pitch of the first half and as an inner voice in the ensuing

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
138

Example 77. Nontraditional structure in 87:10-89:10.

----- ------ :v puaaoici tempo i;

* » ! # & =
f n i f f - i t f p
r

i ij'jlj'J W * 1
n < n ■■ P f f i. ----f --------- ^ T > . f a --- m ----

1 , f f = # = ■? .. r - g = = ■ 1 8 i f

1 .. t r i - - r |J 1 3 1 - q - p - y -1

^-iii ___ _ >_ => S3 C1.,V-«1 1.11

*
<1 mp

L x , V . - y i = =
j f |-f -r r ± = r £ 4 =

130 *fl - *6

ryjl=j
u r r jb . ■~ r..i_p. .
- ^ r fg Pi
*— *-- .---AJi: M - ' h a ,_::: ♦, -
A. £ £ £

fiitj-ff'liHiS’
. f r fi fit.............
1 M

rNfif^M==N#fJ

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 77— continued.

m. » :3 VS « :/ |* :| n .i M3 « :5 M/0

arpeggiation, which again places e-natural in the prominent

position. The classical scheme would dictate that the

melody begin its descent again, moving more quickly perhaps

the second time around so as to end on the tonic in the end.

The second half of this melody starts as though it would do

this, descending from 8 to i again. After the d-natural

arrives, however, f-sharp is prolonged through several

reharmonizations, and then the period simply returns to e-

natural. The second phrase thus seems to be a prolongation

of a neighbor-group figure, not of a scalar progression.

While many of Prokofiev's melodies are based on step

progressions, it is clear from examples such as the one just

described that the characteristic method of chromatically

shifting scales by reharmonizing chord tones does not always

support the possibility of step progressions. In the

"Gavotte" melody, for instance, perhaps the only step pro­

gression to be found is the motion from § to 5 and back to §

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
140

at the end. In a melody such as this, each section that

makes use of a single scale normally fills in or otherwise

prolongs a skip or skips between the root, third, and fifth

of a chord (usually tonic or dominant) representing that

scale. The final note is usually not heard in conjunction

with the chord in question, but reharmonized by a chord in

the scale of the next section. The "Gavotte” melody begins

by joining § and § in D-major with a passing tone. Then

when f-sharp arrives, it is reharmonized as § in B major.

(See example 78. For the original passage, please refer to

example 22, page 50.) The short passage using the B-major

Example 78. Structure with reharmonized chord tones in


122:1-123:1.

a * a

scale then ends on 1 of that scale. This note, b-natural,

is reharmonized as 6 in D major, an exception to the gener­

alization stated above concerning common-tone links. A

simple scalar melody using the D-major scale ensues,

followed by a transposition of the same melodic fragment

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
141

using the F-sharp-minor scale. The f-sharp that starts the

transposed appearance serves as the link between the two

scales, being 3 in the preceding scale and i in the new

scale. Similarly, a-natural serves as the link in the

return to the D-major scale. The method employed in the

final two shifts, from D-major to C-sharp-major and back

again, is not the use of a reharmonized common tone but the

half-step alteration of the concluding pitch of a scale

fragment, a method described in the previous chapter.

A melody with a similar type of structure is found at

rehearsal 84. (See example 79.) While there is a

descending step-progression in the inner voices, this

progression does not resolve to the tonic, e, and in fact

gets lost near the end of the second phrase, not

contributing to the closure of the period at all. The

structure of the melodic line is based on reharmonization of

pitches, not on step progressions. The opening b-natural is

reinterpreted in measures 6 and 7 as the third of an A-flat-

major triad and then as the root of a B-minor at the end of

measure 8. At the end of the second phrase, it is the fifth

of the A-flat-minor triad, e-flat, that is reinterpreted.

The metrical position of its reharmonization as the third of

a B-major triad, on the last beat of what is perceived as

the penultimate measure, imparts dominant function to the

sonority, an impression that is, of course, strengthened by

the fact that the harmony is the dominant in the original

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 79. Structure with reharmonized chord tones in
84:1-85:8.

Poco piu tranqulllo

j i j
* fTT V V7

W T Kp
dg

m* ^hi.J J ,*£ S H s!
5=^ tT T r*F T W f
W*l» *r

IS '
5

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
143

key. The structure of this melody, then, can be viewed as a

series of elaborated skips between b-natural and the pitch

four half-steps above it, followed by an e-natural. Each of

the first two times the upper note in the skip appears, it

occurs on a downbeat as the relatively stable fifth of an A-

flat-minor harmony. The last time, it occurs on the

penultimate beat as a leading tone, the third ofthe

dominant harmony in the main key of E-minor, andresolves to

the tonic pitch.

Having discussed the main categories of structure

employed by Prokofiev in Romeo and Juliet, we will now

examine immediate, local-level tendencies of scale degrees.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
144

CHAPTER VI

MELODIC TENDENCIES

The following discussion is based on a study in which

the notes of most of the melodies in Romeo and Juliet were

counted in various ways in order to identify normal patterns

in this style. It is at this point in the treatise, where a

large number of examples exist, that statistical methods

become appropriate and useful. This is not to say that they

will be precisely meaningful; at best, statistical

statements are only statements of probability. Here an

extra degree of uncertainty is added by the impossibility of

devising a truly random sample, or, in fact, of even

determining the exact contents of the entire population.

The difficulty is that the ultimate concern is not with

discovering patterns that occur more often in actuality, but

rather with those that seem to occur more often. If a given

melody is repeated ten times over the course of a work, each

pattern within that melody is itself heard ten times. Does

any such pattern indeed seem ten times as normal as one

found in a melody heard only once? Should the pattern occur

in other melodies as well, the repetition of the first

melody would seem in fact to confirm the notion that the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
145

pattern is in fact an integral part of the whole style. But

what of the comparison of a pattern that is found only in

the ten-times-repeated melody with another that is found in

five different melodies, each melody heard only once? The

recurrence of the second pattern in five different contexts

is likely to make it seem more normal than the pattern heard

in only one context, despite the fact that the latter is

heard twice as often. There can be no true resolution of

this problem since any two listeners may approach it in

different ways. Using V-I cadences and Tristan und Isolde

as examples, Leonard Meyer has pointed out that it is even

possible, to a listener familiar with the norms of a

particular body of literature, for a given pattern to seem

an integral part of the style of a certain work even if no

instances of it are heard in the work at all.

Despite these difficulties, people who analyze music

generally find satisfaction in discovering what normally

happens in a style. Thus the study is justified. Still,

there are arguments against using statistics in such a

study: such methods may seem at one moment cold and at

another inexact. As to the first criticism, any form of

note counting can only be a first step in meaningful

analysis. Subjective sense is always available, and indeed

necessary, for interpreting findings. As to the second

criticism, since the resolution of our subjective sense of

what seems more or less normal is not nearly so refined as

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
that suggested by the multi-decimal-place figures

statistical computations produce, inexact figures should be

enough for laying a basic foundation. Out of a total of

forty-four different melodies in Romeo and Juliet, thirty-

seven, those that begin and end in the same key, were

included in the reckoning for this study, each of them

counted only once. In a few cases, only major-mode melodies

were counted. The location of each of these melodies is

given in appendix B. Individual pitches (classification of

pitch determined by its intervallic relationship to the

prevailing tonic, hence the necessity for melodies that

begin and end in the same key), contiguous two-note

patterns, patterns of two notes separated by a third note,

and melodic intervals were counted. Distributions of the

tendencies were then subjected to chi-square tests to study

deviation from randomness, from other distributions provided

by the same study, and from distributions provided by a

similar study conducted by Joseph Youngblood on certain

nineteenth century art songs.1 In addition, Youngblood's

method of information-theory computation was applied to

these same distributions for further comparison with the

nineteenth-century style he examines.

The statistical test used in this study is known as the

chi-square test. This test is used to judge whether the

\Joseph E. Youngblood, "Style as Information," Journal


of Music Theory 2 (April 1958):24-35.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
147

difference between two nonnormal distributions is

significant or not. In other words, given a nonnormally

distributed set of probabilities (i.e., a set that does not

fit a bell curve) as a basis for expectation, the chi-square

test will determine, within a certain level of confidence,

whether a set of observed frequencies could have resulted

from those given probabilities. If not, a "significant"

difference exists, and a factor other than chance can be

assumed to have influenced the observed results.

The first step in this test is to subtract the observed

frequency of each possible outcome from its expected

frequency (the probability of that outcome multiplied by the

total number of events). The square of each difference is

taken and divided by the corresponding expected frequency.

The results are then totaled. This final figure is compared

with figures in a chi-square table, which can be found in

any statistics text. The table gives the minimum result

needed to assume significance of deviation with confidence

of a certain degree: 99%, 98%, 95%, etc. 99% confidence,

for instance, means that the difference could have arisen by

chance only one time in 100; the observer can be 99% sure

that other factors, not chance, caused the difference. The

table also gives different figures for varying degrees of

freedom (df). For our purpose, the degrees of freedom is a

number equal to the number of possible outcomes minus one.

The notion of degrees of freedom reflects the interdepend­

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
148

ence of the frequencies of the various outcomes when the

total frequency is given. If there are only two possible

outcomes in a certain system and the total frequency is

given, any result for one outcome determines the frequency

of the other; there is only one degree of freedom in the

system. If three outcomes are possible, the frequencies of

any two outcomes are independent of each other, but the

third is determined by the other two; there are two degrees

of freedom. Higher chi-square figures are needed with

higher degrees of freedom. Because the test involves

dividing the square of each difference by the expected

frequency, the fact that significance of deviation rises as

the total frequency rises is taken into account. (Anything

can happen once; probabilities take effect over multiple

occurrences.) In the same feature, however, lies the one

weakness of the test; any expected frequency less than about

2% of the total frequency may, since it is used as a

denominator, possibly lead to an overly large result.

As an example, let us imagine that for a given

location, two-thirds of the days are sunny, one-third

cloudy. The probability that any given day will be sunny is

thus .67, that it will be cloudy, .33. Over a 30-day

period, 20 sunny days would be expected, 10 cloudy days. An

observation of 19 sunny days and 11 cloudy days would not

seem unusual, and the chi-square test confirms that it is

not so. The difference between the expected frequency of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
149

sunny days and the observed frequency is 1. The square of

this difference is 1, and this divided by the expected

frequency is .05. The difference for cloudy days is also 1.

The square, 1 again, is divided by the expected number of

cloudy days, for a result of .1. The sum of the results is

.15. With one degree of freedom, a result of at least 3.841

is needed for even 95% confidence; the chi-square test

indicates that only chance was involved in the deviation.

Suppose, however, that 10 sunny days and 20 cloudy days were

observed. This would be quite an unusual weather pattern

for the location, as the chi-square test would affirm. The

difference squared is, in each case, 100. Dividing 100 by

the expected number of sunny days yields 5; dividing by the

expected number of cloudy days gives a result of 10.

Totalling these results yields a final chi-square figure of

15. Only 6.635 is needed for 99% confidence that a

significant difference exists.

The information-theory calculation is also a summation

of figures. For each outcome, the proportion of the

observed frequency to the total number of events is

multiplied by the log2 of that same proportion. (The log2 of

a number is that power to which 2 must be raised to yield

that number. 8 is 2 to the third power. Therefore the log2

of 8 is 3. 0.25 is 2 to the power of negative 2. There­

fore, the log2 of 0.25 is -2.) The absolute value of the

sum of these products yields the number of bits of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
150

information expressed by the system and is known as the

entropy of the system, represented by the letter H. (A bit

is the smallest amount of information, a binary choice, an

indication of the state of a situation where only two

possibilities exist. One bit of information is needed to

indicate one of two objects. Two bits are needed to

indicate one of four objects? thefirst bit narrows the

field in half, and the second canthen identify one ofthe

two remaining possibilities. Three bits are needed to

identify one of eight objects, etc.) Where the observed

frequencies are the same for every outcome, the entropy will

be the log2 of the number of outcomes. With four possible

outcomes, for instance, the result would be 2. Where the

frequencies are not equal, the theory states that less

information is expressed by each event. Another way

Youngblood explains it is that if a composition displays

more instances of one possibility than another, the composer

was less free to choose the rarer of the two. A lower

entropy figure indicates less freedom of choice, more

restrictions due to peculiarities of style. The entropy

figure of a nonequally distributed system may be divided by

the figure for an equally distributed system with the same

total number of events to yield the figure known as relative

entropy (Hr).

To demonstrate the information-theory or entropy test,

I will relay Youngblood's example: a composer is writing a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
151

piece for four drums in which he uses drum A 12.5% of the

time, drum B 12.5%, drum C 25%, and drum D 50%. The log2 of

.125 is -3; of .25, -2; and of .5, -1. (.125 * -3) + (.125

* -3) + (.25 * -2) + (.5 * -1) = -.375 + -.375 + -.5 + -.5 =

-1.75. The absolute value is taken just for the convenience

of having positive numbers as results. Thus H = 1.75.

Since H would equal 2 in a system with four equally probable

possibilities, Hr =1.75/2 = 87.5%.

This method has two main weaknesses. First, there is no

absolute standard for making judgments based on the results

of this test, as there is in the case of chi-square.

However, relative judgments may be made by comparing results

and deciding which of two systems, for instance, displays

more freedom of choice, i.e., which deviates from randomness

less. Second, since only relative frequency and not

absolute frequency is taken into account, the difference in

significance that accompanies greater frequencies is not

factored in. Getting "heads" only twenty-five times in one-

hundred coin tosses is viewed by this theory as no less a

product of chance than getting "heads" once in four tosses.

Still, the method may be useful in corroborating findings

from other tests.

Before any multi-note patterns can be examined, the

relative frequencies of pitches must be established. The

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
distribution of frequencies of each pitch class as occurring

in melodies 1-37 is shown in table 1.

Table l. Frequency distribution of pitch classes


in melodies 1-37.

PC Freq. Prob.
0 256 17.52%
i 30 2.05%
2 135 9.24%
3 56 3.83%
4 188 12.87%
5 103 7.05%
6 58 8.97%
7 263 18.00%
8 44 3.01%
9 144 9.86%
10 52 3.56%
11 132 9.03%
Total: 1461

The "PC” designation refers to the number of half steps

a given note lies above the tonic pitch of the melody in

which it occurs. Thus, there are 256 instances of the tonic

pitch in these melodies, 30 instances of the pitch one half­

step above the tonic, etc. (These pitch-classes will be

indicated in the text by the same nomenclature used in

chapter 3: an underlined integer, e.g. 3.) "Prob." refers

to the proportion of a given pc's frequency to the total

number of pitches, in this case 1461. A pitch chosen at

random from the melodies of this ballet then has an 18%

chance of being the dominant pitch, but is only about half

as likely to be the leading tone. The distribution is

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
153

clearly not random; the chi-square test yields a result of

584.86, where for 99% confidence with 11 degrees of freedom,

a result of only 24.725 is required. Thus, these frequen­

cies are significant aspects of the style. The dominant and

tonic, in that order, are the most common scale degrees,

followed by the third, the sixth, the second, the seventh,

and the fourth degrees of the major scale. As might be

expected, the chromatic pitches are less common. The

entropy of this distribution is 3.300. Since the H for

twelve classifications with equal probabilities is 3.585,

the Hr for this distribution is .921. Meaningless now, this

figure will prove somewhat useful when comparing other

distributions to this overall distribution.

Youngblood's study dealt only with melodies in major

keys. For this reason a division by mode of the melodies in

Romeo and Juliet is desired. Distributions for each mode

are given separately in table 2.

Not surprisingly, since it is the relative frequency of

4 and 3. which more than any other factor determines whether

a melody is in the major or minor mode, in each mode here

the appropriate third scale degree is found approximately

six times as often as its counterpart. What is surprising

is that the minor mode employs the leading tone less often

than does the major mode and employs the sub-tonic much more

often. Also interesting is the much higher frequency of

dominant pitches in the minor mode and the much lower

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
154

Table 2. Frequency distribution of pitch-classes by mode.

Major mode Minor mode


PC Freq. Prob. Freq. Prob.
0 211 18.19% 45 14.95%
1 26 2.24% 4 1.33%
2 107 9.22% 28 9.30%
3 31 2.67% 25 8.31%
4 184 15.86% 4 1.33%
5 92 7.93% 11 3.65%
6 46 3.97% 12 3.99%
7 185 15.95% 78 25.91%
8 23 1.98% 21 6.98%
9 126 10.86% 18 5.98%
10 17 1.47% 35 11.63%
11 112 9.66% 20 6.64%
Total: 1160 Total: 301

frequency of the subdominant. The use of 6 just under 4% of

the time in each mode is striking. As might be expected,

the entropy figures for these distributions, 3.220 for the

major mode (Hr =.898) and 3.189 for the minor mode (Hr =

.890), are lower than those for all the melodies together.

This is true because the strictures associated with each

mode are isolated in the dual breakdown. In other words,

the extra freedom indicated by the higher H value of the

combined distribution represents the freedom to choose one

of two modes.

We may now begin to compare the Prokofiev figures with

those obtained in Youngblood's study. The H values for the

three composers studied by Youngblood are as follows:

Schubert, 3.127 (Hr = .87); Mendelssohn, 3.03 (Hr = .846);

and Schumann, 3.05 (Hr = .85). Obviously, Prokofiev is more

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
155

free in his choice of pitches than is any one of the three

nineteenth-century composers. Schubert comes the closest,

probably because he is the most chromatic of the three. An

examination of differences between Prokoviev's choice of

pitches and Schubert's may disclose specific ways in which

the style of Prokofiev's music differs from an earlier tonal

style. In table 3, Schubert's frequency distribution, taken

from Youngblood, is given alongside Prokofiev's.

Table 3. Comparison of pc-probability distributions


for Schubert and Prokofiev.

Schubert Prokofiev (major)


PC Freq. Prob. Freq. Prob.
0 182 17.76% 211 18.19%
1 7 0.68% 26 2.24%
2 168 16.39% 107 9.22%
3 23 2.24% 31 2.67%
4 124 12.10% 184 15.86%
5 83 8.10% 92 7.93%
6 16 1.56% 46 3.97%
7 203 19.80% 185 15.95%
8 30 2.93% 23 1.98%
9 78 7.61% 126 10.86%
10 29 2.83% 17 1.47%
11 __82 8.00% -11.2. 9.66%
Total: 1025 Total: 1160

Although it is clear which pitches Prokofiev uses more

often than Schubert and which pitches less, the question at

hand is whether any of the differences are significant.

Assuming the proportions displayed for Schubert are typical,

that they can be expected, are the discrepancies between the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
156

two composers possibly a matter of chance, or do they

indicate significant differences in style? The chi-square

figure obtained by a comparison of the full distributions is

170.895. With only 24.725 or greater needed for 99% confi­

dence, it is clear that significant differences exist.

What, specifically, are they?

The first issue is the matter of chromaticism. Of the

1025 notes examined in the Schubert study, 920 (89.8%) are

diatonic, 105 (10.2%) chromatic. Of the 1160 notes in the

major-mode Prokofiev melodies, 1017 (87.7%) are diatonic,

while 143 (12.3%) are chromatic. The Prokofiev melodies

thus display more chromaticism. Is the difference signif­

icant? The chi-square test suggests that it is. Given the

Schubert percentages as the normal probabilities, the

Prokofiev figures yield a chi-square value of 5.733. Since

at 1 degree of freedom 5.412 is needed for 98% confidence

and 6.6535 for 99% confidence, we may say with a little more

than 98% confidence that the greater chromaticism in

Prokofiev is in fact a significant difference of style.

The chi-square test can also give an indication of

whether the difference in the frequency of any single pitch

is significant. When the distributions are regrouped into

two categories each, tonic pitches and nontonic pitches, the

chi-square test yields a result of .120. Since 3.841 is

needed for even 95% confidence, the difference is trivial.

Testing for a difference in the use of 1, however, yields a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
157

chi-square value of 39.649. Although chi-square figures are

suspect any time the expected frequency is under 2.0%, this

figure is so much higher than the 6.635 needed for 99%

confidence that it is still safe to say that Prokofiev uses

significantly more l's than does Schubert. Even compared to

Schumann, whose use of the pitch 1.5% of the time— a

proportion closer to Prokofiev's— makes the chi-square test

more sure, Prokofiev's use of 1 proves significantly higher.

The test yields a result of 4.25, over the 95% mark.

Of the twelve pitches, Prokofiev's low use of 2 yields

the greatest chi-square figure: 43.567. On the other hand,

the test for 3 yields one of the lowest: 1.203. Prokofiev

makes significantly more use of the third scale degree, 4.;

the chi-square value is 15.436. The smallest difference

between the two styles is in the use of 5; the chi-square

value is only .044. An abundant use of the raised fourth

scale degree, 6, appears to be one of the most significant

features of the style of Romeo and Juliet, as attested to by

the chi-square result of 41.228. A chi-square value of

10.837 shows that Prokofiev's use of the dominant, 7, is

significantly lower than Schubert's. His use of 8 is

somewhat lower, but a chi-square result of 3.466 suggests

the difference may not be significant. The use of 9 is

significantly greater in Prokofiev; the chi-square value is

17.578. A result of 7.590 reveals, surprisingly, that

Prokofiev's use of 10, a chromatic pitch, is significantly

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
158

lower. A result of 4.318 suggests that we may say with

something over 95% confidence that Prokofiev makes signif­

icantly more use of the leading tone. In short, the greater

use of chromatic pitches is accounted for by significantly

higher numbers in Prokofiev of pitches 1 and 6 (although use

of 10 is significantly lower), while the lower overall

percentage of diatonic pitches is accounted for by the

significantly less frequent use of pitches 2 and 7 (although

use of 4 and 9 is significantly higher). These tendencies

are summarized in table 4.

Relative frequency of each pitch in Prokofiev


as compared with use by Schubert.

(higher) 6 significantly higher


significantly higher 7 significantly lower
significantly lower 8 somewhat lower
(higher) 9 significantly higher
significantly higher 10 significantly lower
(lower) 11 somewhat higher

Consecutive-pair Patterns

Having examined the distribution of the frequency of

each pitch, we may now turn our attention to more specific

syntactical uses for each pitch. In order to do so we will

use another type of frequency table. This type of table

will indicate the number of times each of the 144 possible

pitch-class combinations occurs as a consecutive pair of

notes. These tables will be arranged in twelve-by-twelve

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
159

arrays, each line a distribution showing the number of times

a given pitch is followed by the tonic pitch, by a

representative of pc 1, by a representative of pc 2, etc.

Each of these distributions may be analyzed in the ways the

distribution of overall frequencies was analyzed, and they

may be compared to it and to each other.

Table 5 shows Youngblood's table of frequencies of

consecutive pairs in Schubert as well as entropy figures.

These figures have been recalculated and, in most cases,

differ slightly from the figures, calculated before even

pocket calculators were available, given by Youngblood. In

addition, the table gives the frequencies and entropy

measurements for melodies 1-37 in Romeo and Juliet.

In a comparison of the entropy measurements, it is

quickly observed that Prokofiev's style is freer than

Schubert's. In other words, more variety of patterns in

consecutive pairs exists in Prokofiev's music, as indicated

by the higher overall entropy level, 88.32% as opposed to

79.6% for Schubert. In fact, the level of freedom for each

of the twelve separate distributions is higher in the music

of Prokofiev. That is, after any given note, Prokofiev is

less likely than Schubert to defer to typical tendencies.

This is due in part to the general tendency to use more

chromatic pitches, but must be mostly attributed simply to a

general willingness to use almost any pitch after any other

pitch. This difference is most obvious in the case of 1, in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
160

Table 5. Frequencies of consecutive pairs


in Schubert and Prokofiev.

1st Followed by:


PC 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 H Hr

0 Schub 29 0 27 5 33 3 1 26 1 7 10 40 2.89 80.7%


Prok 34 14 23 14 43 8 1 33 7 13 4 37 3.19 89.1%

1 Schub 5 1 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 1.15 32.1%


Prok 6 0 5 1 2 5 0 3 0 5 0 2 2.82 78.7%

2 Schub 57 1 38 4 15 13 2 13 0 7 0 18 2.65 74.8%


Prok 38 2 3 13 26 7 4 14 0 7 2 18 2.93 81.8%

3 Schub 3 2 11 2 0 0 0 3 1 0 1 0 2.28 63.6%


Prok 7 2 7 7 12 4 2 11 1 0 1 2 3.06 85.3%

4 Schub 24 0 41 0 13 4 1 36 0 5 0 0 2.25 62.7%


Prok 15 1 47 4 23 19 6 29 6 22 0 11 3.03 84.6%

5 Schub 1 0 24 8 36 2 0 6 1 5 0 0 2.17 60.4%


Prok 7 2 6 3 36 7 12 14 3 10 2 1 2.96 82.5%

6 Schub 1 0 0 0 1 0 7 7 0 0 0 0 1.54 43.1%


Prok 2 0 2 1 2 12 8 20 3 3 2 3 2.83 78.9%

7 Schub 33 2 12 3 22 39 6 58 7 5 2 14 2.92 81.6%


Prok 57 1 14 5 25 28 20 44 13 27 14 11 3.22 89.8%

8 Schub 3 0 0 1 1 0 0 10 5 4 6 0 2.47 68.9%


Prok 5 1 0 4 1 4 1 8 1 12 5 2 3.00 83.7%

9 Schub 3 0 6 0 0 18 0 19 4 25 0 3 2.38 66.3%


Prok 18 2 12 2 9 5 0 43 5 13 12 23 2.99 83.3%

10 Schub 2 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 10 3 9 1 2.50 69.6%


Prok 4 3 8 2 1 2 0 4 3 7 1 17 2.96 82.5%

11 Schub 28 0 9 0 1 2 0 20 0 15 2 5 2.41 67.2%


Prok 55 2 7 0 5 1 3 19 2 23 9 5 2.58 72.1%

Entropy for total. distribution: Schub 5.71 79.6%


Prok 6.33 88.3%

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
161

which Schubert limits himself to three possibilities, while

Prokofiev is free to follow with one of eight different

pitches. The striking difference between the 32.1% relative

entropy in Schubert's case and the 78.67% in Prokofiev's

bears this out. The least difference seen is in the case of

pitches following 11.. In this situation, Prokofiev is

content to follow the traditional tendencies to continue

with the tonic pitch or with the sixth scale degree, a

judgment which will be corroborated later in the chapter by

chi-square tests.

Now that it has been established that Prokofiev is

freer in the use of each pitch than Schubert is, we wish to

know details concerning the use of each pitch. In partic­

ular, we wish to know whether tendency tones exist. That

is, are there tones that resolve in particular ways a

significant amount of the time? In order to answer this

question, it is necessary to do more than just determine

which notes follow a given pitch more often than others do.

The higher frequencies may merely be a result of the

generally high frequencies of those pitches. For instance,

although pitch 7 is followed most often by 7's and 0's, this

fact may be the result of the general abundance of 0's and

7's. We might also say that, while motion to 7 and motion

to 0 are tendencies of 7, this tendency is a general one and

not peculiar to the dominant pitch. For ease of reference,

a distribution for the Prokofiev melodies alone, with

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
162

entropy and chi-square figures as well, is given in table 6.

Table 6. Frequencies consecutive-pair combinations


in melodies 1-37.

1st Followed by:


PC 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 H Hr chi2

0 34 14 23 14 43 8 1 33 7 13 4 37 3.19 89.1% 60.239


1 6 0 5 1 2 5 0 3 0 5 0 2 2.82 78.7% 13.611
2 38 2 3 13 26 7 4 14 0 7 2 18 2.93 81.8% 49.419
3 7 2 7 7 12 4 2 11 1 0 1 2 3.06 85.3% 24.493
4 15 1 47 4 23 19 6 29 6 22 0 11 3.03 84.6% 78.814
5 7 2 6 3 36 7 12 14 3 10 2 1 2.96 82.5% 71.936
6 2 0 2 1 2 12 8 20 3 3 2 3 2.83 78.9% 55.831
7 57 1 14 5 25 28 20 44 13 27 14 11 3.22 89.8% 42.261
8 5 1 0 4 1 4 1 8 1 12 5 2 3.00 83.7% 34.753
9 18 2 12 2 9 5 0 43 5 13 12 23 2.99 83.3% 46.332
10 4 3 8 2 1 2 0 4 3 7 1 17 2.96 82.5% 53.823
11 55 2 7 0 5 1 3 19 2 23 9 5 2.58 72.1% 86.419

Entropy for total distribution: 6.33 88.3%

In general terms, the information-theory calculations

indicate that Prokofiev is least free in his use of pitches

11, 1, and 6. These tones are therefore probably tendency

tones. He is the most free in his use of 0 and 7. These

tones are probably not tendency tones. With this imprecise

tool we can be no more specific.

In order to determine more definitely the tendency

tones and their respective tendencies we must use the chi-

square test and compare each line in the consecutive-pair

table with the distribution of overall probabilities. Since

17.52% of the pitches are tonic, 17.52% of the total

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
163

frequency for each line is considered the expected frequency

for the tonic pitch in that line, etc. Surprisingly, given

the results of the entropy calculations, the only pitch we

may not say with confidence is a tendency tone is 1. With

11 degrees of freedom, 19.675 is needed for even 95%

confidence of significant difference. The figure of 13.622

calculated for pc 1 is not enough. The calculation for

every other pitch except 3 exceeds the 24.725 needed for 99%

confidence, and the result for 3. is very close. The reason

the two tests give contradictory results for 1 is its low

total frequency. Information theory indicates that the

distribution is not close to random, but it does not take

into account the fact that some very skewed distributions

can occur by chance when only a few cases are observed. The

sampling base is simply not large enough for us to come to a

definite conclusion in the matter.

We will now examine each line in detail to see what

specifically is peculiar about each distribution. Regroup­

ing each distribution twelve different ways— 0's and non-

O's, l's and non-l's, etc.— and comparing each result by

means of a chi-square test with a similar regrouping of the

proportions of table 1 will reveal just which differences,

whether higher or lower, are truly significant, thus showing

the specific tendencies of each pitch.

Pitch 0 is rather eccentric. The chi-square test for

its entire distribution yields a very high 60.239, and

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
164

indeed, upon examining the distribution of pitches following

it, one sees 4 following it as many as 43 times while 6

follows only once. Of course one expects a high number or

4/s and a low number of 6/s simply because of their relative

frequencies overall. Are these figures unusually exagger­

ated? 4 accounts for 12.87% of the pitches overall in the

37 melodies. The expected frequency of 4 in a distribution

whose total frequency is 231 is therefore just under 30

(theoretically, 29.7297). In a comparison of this figure

and the observed 43, the chi-square test yields a result of

6.804, over the 6.635 needed for 99% confidence; the

frequency is significantly high. The tonic pitch has a

tendency, then, to be followed by 4 an inordinate amount of

the time. 6 represents 3.97% of all the pitches in melodies

1 to 37. 3.97% of 231 is 9.1707. Thus, nine 6's are

expected, although only one is observed. The chi-square

test's result of 7.580 affirms that the number of 6's

actually observed is significantly low. The tonic has a

definite tendency not to be followed by the note a tritone

away. The most significant differences in the distribution

are the high number of l's (chi-square: 18.443) and of 11's

(chi-square: 13.703). These differences can be explained

perhaps by a preference in the style for conjunct motion.

Curiously, however, the number of 2's is not significantly

higher than the number expected (chi-square: .141). Other

significant differences are the low number of 5's (chi-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
165

square: 4.535) and of 9's (chi-square: 4.649). The chi-

square tests indicate that the high number of 0's and 7's

following the tonic pitch is the result of their relatively

high overall frequencies. Thus, movement following 0 shows

three definite positive tendencies and three definite

negative tendencies. The term "tendency tone" should,

however, be reserved for tones that show a single very-

strong positive tendency in resolution.

The distribution of notes following 1 shows only one

significant tendency, to move to j>. The chi-square result

of 4.597 allows us to say with something under 98%

confidence that this surprising finding cannot be purely a

product of chance. The high numbers of l's, 2/s, and £'s

can be attributed to overall frequency and, in the first two

cases, perhaps to a proclivity toward conjunct motion. But

the chi-square test cannot support the claim that anything

more than chance is necessarily involved.

The distribution for pitches following 2 contains three

significantly high numbers and three significantly low

numbers. Neighbors 0, 2, and 4 are inordinately high,

revealing a tendency for 2 to move by conjunct motion. The

pitch demonstrates a significant reluctance to repeat

itself, to move by tritone to 2, or, surprisingly, to move

to the dominant.

Pc 2 demonstrates three significant tendencies. The

most significant tendency (chi-square: 11.412) is for the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
166

pitch to repeat itself. Motion to 4, the pitch that most

frequently follows 3., can be said with just under 95%

confidence to represent a significant tendency. Like other

pitches, 3 has a tendency not to be followed by the pitch

six half-steps away, in this case, 9.

4 is the first candidate for the term "tendency tone."

Not thought of as such traditionally, in this style the

pitch shows a single, strong, positive tendency (chi-square:

58.997), namely, to move to 2. It has two negative tenden­

cies: it never moves by tritone to 10, and, surprisingly

considering its role in cadences, it moves to the tonic

pitch relatively rarely (32 cases expected, 15 observed).

5, which tends in traditional music to resolve down by

step, shows the same tendency here. Over one-third of the

notes that follow 5 in these melodies are 4/s (chi-square:

44.801). In this style, however, another tendency is

present. A chi-square figure of 15.938 demonstrates that

the number of 6's following 5's is significantly high. This

succession of 5 by the even more unstable 6 is truly non-

traditional. The low figures for the tonic pitch and for

11, a tritone away, mark significant negative tendencies.

The situation with 6 is very similar to that of 5; the

traditional resolution, up to 7, is present in this style,

as well as a second, nontraditional tendency, motion down to

5. Tie latter is 6's most significant tendency (chi-square:

16.466). Both tendencies involve conjunct motion. The

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
167

pitch also shows a significant tendency to repeat itself and

significant negative tendencies with regard to 4. and 0, the

latter being a tritone away.

Chi-square tests reveal seven tendencies for the

dominant pitch, 7, four significant with at least 95%

confidence, three nearly so. The positive tendencies are

motion to 5, 6, 7, and— the first nonconjunct positive

tendency— 0. The negative tendencies include motion by

tritone to 1, and motion to 2. Also among the negative

tendencies is movement to 11. In fact, the dominant pitch

moves to the chromatic 10 more often than to the generally

more frequent 11.

8 demonstrates two positive tendencies, motion to 9 and

motion to 10, and two negative tendencies, motion to 2 and

motion to 4. Traditionally the pitch may serve as either a

raised fifth, leading to 9, or a lowered sixth, leading to

7. While the first traditional tendency is represented in

this work, the second, probably traditionally the more

common, is not; the high number of 7's cannot be said with

confidence to be a result of anything more than the

generally high number of 7's. This discontinuation of a

traditional tendency is balanced by the new tendency to move

to 10.

The three positive tendencies demonstrated by 9, motion

to 7, 10, and JL1, all represent the general preference for

conjunct motion. The negative tendencies are motion to 4

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
168

and to 2. The chi-square test does not support the

statement that motion by tritone to 2 is significantly low.

10 may be added to the list of tendency tones. Its one

significant tendency is motion to H (chi-square: 35.411).

This tendency is directly opposed to the traditional

tendency for 12 to resolve down. Although this traditional

resolution is found, the number of 9's in this distribution

is apparently attributable only to their overall frequency,

not to any special tendency of 10. It shows significant

reluctance toward motion to 4.

11, the strongest of the traditional tendency tones,

like several others displays the traditional tendency plus

other nontraditional tendencies. The chi-square figure of

54.243 for the traditional motion to 2 is high, but

interestingly not as high as the figure for motion from 4 to

2. The nontraditional tendencies present are conjunct

motions to 9 and to 12- The pitch shows a negative tendency

to move to 3 or 4.

The study of consecutive pairs has revealed three

general principles concerning the style of Romeo and Juliet:

(1) that any pitch may be followed by practically any other

pitch, although (2) conjunct motion is preferred and (3)

motion by tritone is avoided. The first of these tendencies

represents a deviation from traditional tonal styles. With

regard to more specific matters, traditional tendency tones

2 and H preserve their traditional roles, although they

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
169

assume new secondary roles as well. This is also true of

the chromatic pitches 6 and, to some extent, 9. Chromatic

pitches 1 and 10, on the other hand, fill completely new

roles in this style.

Nonconsecutive Pairs

In traditional tonal music, a tendency tone need not

resolve immediately. Melodic embellishment may delay the

resolution by one or more notes. It is important then to

see if the immediate tendencies of any of the twelve pitches

continue to have strength at a distance of two notes. In

addition, other tendencies may be discovered. While it may

be desirable to examine pairs with two, three, or even more

intervening notes, this study is limited to pairs with only

one note intervening. A table giving the frequencies of

every combination is found in table 7. Table 8 combines

tables 6 and 7 by representing the number of instances in

which a pitch is found either directly after a given pitch

or two notes later. (If both notes following are of the

same pitch class, the tally is increased only by one.)

It might be expected that the information-theory

figures be higher when examining nonconsecutive pairs,

representing more freedom, and the chi-square figures lower,

representing less deviation from overall proportions. With

J2, this is certainly the case. The change in the chi-square

figure is especially dramatic. 13.625 is far below the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
170

Table 7. Frequencies of pc pairs with


one note intervening.

1st Followed by:


PC 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 H Hr chi2
0 39 5 20 13 33 18 10 33 8 29 3 12 3.29 91.7% 13.625
1 1 1 6 9 2 1 0 3 3 1 0 2 2.87 80.2% 74.647
2 24 3 7 0 16 10 4 26 2 11 6 18 3.11 86.8% 13.000
3 13 4 1 2 5 3 6 11 2 0 1 6 3.06 85.4% 25.985
4 41 3 15 2 24 7 7 33 2 20 0 26 2.98 83.2% 23=886
5 15 1 21 4 10 5 4 13 3 18 4 4 3.19 88.9% 27.501
6 8 1 3 1 7 3 7 9 3 9 3 4 3.32 92.5% 15.806
7 38 5 25 17 41 20 4 47 6 17 11 20 3.27 91.1% 16.443
8 5 3 1 2 2 3 2 5 5 1 10 5 3.30 92.0% 70.116
9 32 0 16 2 19 21 7 12 2 12 4 16 3.11 86.9% 29.662
10 8 0 5 2 2 3 0 12 3 4 9 4 3.07 85.7% 36.710
11 16 4 12 0 17 5 4 31 4 17 1 13 3.06 85.3% 17.151

Entropy for total. distribution: 6.48 90.4%

Table 8 Frequencies of pc pairs with zero


or one note intervening.

1st Followed by:


PC 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 H Hr chi2

0 58 18 43 25 68 24 10 62 14 39 7 47 3.32 92.7% 35.692


1 7 1 11 10 4 6 0 5 3 6 0 4 3.13 87.2% 45.839
2 52 5 9 13 41 17 7 38 2 17 8 36 3.14 87.7% 30.760
3 19 6 8 7 13 7 7 19 3 0 2 7 3.20 89.3% 24.307
4 51 4 61 6 42 26 12 56 8 39 0 35 3.11 86.8% 49.096
5 21 3 27 7 41 11 15 26 6 27 6 5 3.22 90.0% 41.951
6 9 1 5 2 8 15 13 25 6 12 5 7 3.25 90.8% 42.045
7 86 6 37 21 63 46 23 80 19 43 25 31 3.33 92.9% 17.840
8 10 4 1 4 3 6 3 11 6 13 15 7 3.31 92.4% 74.007
9 47 2 28 4 28 26 7 51 7 23 15 38 3.21 89.4% 22.429
10 12 3 13 4 2 5 0 16 6 10 10 21 3.21 89.5% 45.095
11 59 6 19 0 22 6 7 46 6 39 10 18 3.02 84.2% 38.512

Entropy for total distribution: 6.54 91.2%

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
171

19.675 needed for 95% confidence that significant overall

deviation from relative randomness exists. None of 0's

significant immediate tendencies are evident in the non-

consecutive chart. For instance, the chi-square test allows

confidence of close to 95% that the number of ll's is

significantly low in this situation, although motion to H

is one of the very strong tendencies displayed by the tonic

in direct motion.

The case of pc 1 is very interesting. Both the

information theory and the chi-square numbers are higher,

the latter dramatically so. The reason for this

contradictory situation is again the low total frequency.

It is difficult to make definite conclusions based on this

low number of total observations. The indications given by

chi-square tests, however, are that the number of fi/s is

low, the number of £'s high, and the number of 3/s

tremendously high (chi-square: 58.213).

Calculations show the nonconsecutive-pair distribution

for 2. both more free than the consecutive-pair distribution

and less deviant from general tendencies. Specific tests

reveal only one significant tendency: to move to 11. This

is probably a result of 2's tendency to move directly to 0

and 0's to move directly to 11.

3. shows little change in either deviation from overall

probabilities or freedom. The strong tendencies are

slightly different, however. 1 shows an unusual tendency to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
172

move, in the course of two notes, to 1 or to 6. The

tendency to avoid £ is still present.

4,'s strong tendency to move to 2. is replaced in this

situation with a tendency (the chi-square figure of 3.440

approaches the 95% confidence mark) to move to 0— possibly a

further indication of a preference for three-note, descend­

ing, diatonic scales— and to 11. The tendency to avoid the

tritone, 12, is still present.

2's tendency to move to 1 and l's tendency to move to 2

result in a significantly high number of 2's in the non-

consecutive-pair distribution for 2. 5 also exhibits a

strong tendency to move to 9.

In the nonconsecutive-pair study, £ shows a drastic

increase in freedom and decrease in deviation from overall

proportions. The only tendency significantly different from

overall tendencies is for the note to remain stationary.

Only the 0 line contains more 6's, and there are four times

as many 0's to begin with. It seems that once this rela­

tively rare pitch occurs, it wishes to remain.

The number of 3's found two notes after occurrences of

7 is significantly high, perhaps representing a preference

for a three-note descending figure in the minor mode. The

number of 6's can be said with almost 95% confidence to be

significantly low. Otherwise, the distribution represents

very nearly the proportions found generally in the melodies.

Information theory suggests that fi has less control

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
173

over pitches that follow two notes later than it has over

those immediately following. However, the chi-square test

shows that the distribution is drastically less like the

general distribution than is the consecutive-pair distri­

bution. What appears random to information theory appears

abnormal to the chi-square test since "randomness" should be

biased in this situation toward the tendencies of overall

proportions of the pitches. Especially unusual are the high

numbers of 10's and 8's. It seems that while scalar motion

leads away from a starting pitch in many cases, it does not

do so in the case of 8. The high number of 10's is probably

due to the chromatic tendency of 8 to move to 9 and for 9 to

move to 10. The chi-square test also indicates that the

number of l's is significantly high.

9 displays two strong tendencies in the non-

consecutive-pair chart: the number of 5's is significantly

high and the number of 7's is significantly low. The first

fact is probably a further instance of a preference for

descending diatonic scales. The pitch class represented

most often two notes after occurrences of 9 is 0. The

number is higher than expected, but the chi-square test does

not allow us to say with great confidence that the

difference is significant. It probably represents, however,

the product of the tendency of 9 to go to 11 and of 11. to go

to 0.

Although 10 shows a strong tendency to move directly to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
11 and 11 shows a strong tendency to move to 0, the three-

note combination 10-li-Q does not seem to be common; the

number of 0's two pitches after instances of 10 is actually

lower than the number suggested by general probabilities.

The only strong tendency displayed by 10 in this situation

is to return to 10.

The distribution for 11 displays two strong tendencies.

The number of 3's is significantly low, and the high number

of 7's, the most common pitch in this situation, is

significantly high. The second fact probably arises from

the preference for descending diatonic scales.

In summary, pitches do not generally exhibit a tendency

toward delay of their normal resolutions. That delayed

resolutions occur is unquestioned; they are simply not

significantly abundant. The greatest general tendency among

diatonic pitches is toward descent by diatonic third.

Chromatic pitches display more variety, although 6, 8, and

10 share a tendency to return two notes after they have

sounded.

Intervals

From observing the frequency distributions for

consecutive pairs, one would expect an examination of

intervals formed by consecutive pitches to reveal a high

number of major and minor seconds and a low number of

tritones. These two are indeed among the tendencies

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
175

indicated by a tally of such intervals. (See table 9.) It

Table 9. Frequencies of intervals formed by


consecutive notes, melodies 1-37.

No. of Frequency
half-steps ascending descending

0 100 --
1 195 147
2 134 190
3 88 72
4 87 57
5 88 60
6 5 7
7 29 38
8 8 19
9 7 15
10 3 2
11 5 9
12 26 17
13 0 1
14 2 3
15 1 2
16 1 0
17 0 1
18 0 0
19 1 1
20-23 0 0
24 1 1

will be seen that, in general, the smaller the interval, the

higher the frequency. There are, on the other hand, several

interesting exceptions to this general trend. For instance,

the number of repetitions is actually lower than the number

of the frequency of any kind of step. Another interesting

exception is that, while there are approximately the same

total number of half-steps as whole-steps (342 vs. 324),

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
176

there are significantly more descending whole-steps than

half-steps and significantly more ascending half-steps than

whole-steps. The consecutive-pair chart indicates that the

reason for the high number of descending whole-steps is

largely a result of the tendency of diatonic pitches 2, 4,

7, 9, and 11to descend more often to diatonic pitches than

to chromatic pitches. The chart also explains the high

number of ascending half-steps as the result of a

combination of the tendency for the leading tone to resolve

up by half-step with the tendencies of chromatic pitches 3,

6, 8, and 10 to resolve up rather than down.

In both ascending and descending motion, there are

fewer intervals of three half-steps than of two, fewer

intervals of four half-steps than of three. However, in

both directions, there are slightly more leaps of five half­

steps than of four. As expected from the study of

consecutive pairs, very few leaps of six half-steps in

either direction occur. But the number of leaps of seven

half-steps is still rather high. After this point, the

numbers drop sharply again.

The tally, then, shows that the number of perfect

fourths and perfect fifths is high considering the general

situation, in which larger intervals occur less often.

Although it is impossible to tell from Youngblood's chart

the difference between ascending fourths and descending

fifths, or between ascending fifths and descending fourths,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
177

or between these intervals and their octave equivalents, the

chart does show a total of 112 fourths and fifths, 10.93% of

the total intervals, while Prokofiev uses 218 fourths and

fifths and their octave equivalents, 15.31% of the 1424

intervals used in these melodies.

The high use of fourths and fifths is well represented

by the first melody in "Folk Dance." (See example 80.) The

Example 80. Abundance of fourths and fifths in 151:1-152:1.

Allegro giocoso J - u a

P P ftfltggitn-r

i rrrrTr

j'T P J l i jij

use of these so-called open intervals lends a simple,

elemental character to the music. Thus whether used in the

same way as in folk music or not, these intervals are able

easily to invoke in the listener thoughts of folk music, a

fact capitalized upon by other composers such as Copland.

It is no surprise that the octave is the most numerous

of the larger intervals, or that there are few intervals

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
178

larger than an octave. These tendencies have been true of

melodic style for generations. What is different from

traditional style, however, is the greater number of large

descending intervals compared to large ascending intervals.

In summary, the melodies of Romeo and Juliet, although

based on diatonic scales, contain a high number of chromatic

pitches. This high use of chromaticism is closely linked

with a willingness on Prokofiev's part to follow, on

occasion, any note with almost any other. On the other

hand, most tones show strong tendencies toward particular

resolutions, some traditional, some, especially in the case

of chromatic pitches, new. Conjunct motion is preferred. A

high incidence of fourths and fifths is observed, as well as

a low incidence of tritones, two features not common to much

chromatic music.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
179

CHAPTER VII

DRAMA

We now turn to a description of the way Prokofiev tells

a story with the music of Romeo and Juliet, noting the way

technical features of the music in each number contribute to

the expression of characters and themes in the story. Since

the ballet is based on Shakespeare's version of the story,

it is appropriate occasionally to draw parallels between the

text of his play and Prokofiev's music.1 Descriptions of

action taking place at particular moments are based on cues

scattered throughout the score and on a synopsis by George

Balanchine of the first Soviet production,2 choreographed by

Leonid Lavrovsky and presumably based on the original

libretto, on which Lavrovsky and Prokofiev collaborated.

While some of the following musical descriptions are

detailed enough to necessitate examples, the reader will

need access to a score in order to benefit fully from the

following synopsis.

Excerpts from the play are taken from Romeo and


Juliet, revised ed., edited by John E. Hankins, The Pelican
Shakespeare (New York: Penguin Books, 1970).

2This synopsis is found in Balanchine's New Complete


Stories of the Great Ballets, ed. Francis Mason (Garden
City, NY: Doubleday, 1968).

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
180

1, Introduction

The "Introduction" is full of the inconstancy,

innocence, and optimism of youth. Several uncomplicated

themes in several keys appear, one following the other

almost as quickly as Romeo's attraction to one young woman

is replaced by passion for another. The first three

melodies are nearly completely diatonic, reflecting the

simplicity that accompanies inexperience, although the

fourth, more sinuous and chromatic, makes pretensions toward

grandeur. While there is this hint of nascent maturity,

there is no foreboding of the tragedy to come. All is

resolved here as each melody finds its way to a tonic chord.

Even the first theme, beginning with a prolongation of pre­

dominant harmony, resolves safely within four measures,

serving as a model of the typical adolescent's belief that

any situation other than unrequited love in which he

suddenly finds himself will have a happy ending. If there

is any shadow in the number, it is the ironic shift down

from C major to B major just before the cadence of the

repeat of this opening phrase. This shift is answered,

however, by the last melody's eventual rise from B major to

C major.

Prokofiev's fondness for melodic perfect fourths and

fifths is displayed in the second melody of the

"Introduction." (Please refer to example 18, page 39.)

These intervals stand out in three ways. First, they are

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
181

abundant; they account for fourteen, or exactly one-third,

of the melody's forty-two melodic intervals. In addition,

they are often found between pitches related in ways other

than by succession; the perfect fifth between the first and

last notes of the opening motive, f-sharp and c-sharp, is an

example. Second, these leaps are usually followed by a step

in the same direction, contrary to tradition. Third, they

often occur between pitches other than the traditional

tonic-dominant pair, usually delaying the normal stepwise

resolution of the less stable tones. The first interval in

the third measure, for instance, leaves the sixth scale

degree, g-sharp, stranded momentarily; the f-sharp and the

a-sharp in the next beat serve as successors to g-sharp in,

respectively, ascending and descending step progressions.

2. Romeo

The first act begins with Romeo sulking over Rosaline's

refusal to return his love. As people in such a state often

find a strange comfort in the perpetuation of their misery

and thus willfully seek this perpetuation, this number seems

determined to go awry. The music resists the establishment

of meter, for instance. (See example 81.) The sequential

nature of the opening measures suggests the duple meter

indicated in the score. The b-natural at the end of measure

2, though, acting as a suspension, lends strength to the

second beat of that measure and, as a corollary, weakness to

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
182

Example 81. Ambiguous meter in 5:1-5.

[5] Andante J>o

S t s mm m m m m SfiFP

m m m
the next beat, notated as a downbeat. The very presence of

b-natural in place of the b-flat expected because of the

opening F-major harmony, as well as its downward resolution,

contributes to the quirkiness of the music.

Just as the music begins to wander, it is forcefully

and suddenly brought back in measure 7, much as Romeo might

stubbornly interrupt any spontaneous mental wandering to

return to his dark thoughts. The variation on the opening

measures that begins in measure 7 includes a brooding melody

played, suitably, by the low strings and bassoon. A second

variation includes a much less ponderous melody. After two

occurrences, however, the more positive train of thought

represented by this melody is terminated as the bass melody

returns once more. The four lovely final measures announce

the rising of the sun. (See example 82.) But, in the words

of Romeo's father,

. . . all so soon as the all-cheering sun


Should in the farthest East begin to draw
The shady curtains from Aurora's bed,
Away from light steals home my heavy son

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 82. Sunrise motive in 8:10-13.

j -ted f f f |» l l
>J >J jH ’ ;rl|

And private in his chamber pens himself,


Shuts up his windows, locks fair daylight out,
And makes himself an artificial night.
(I,i , 132-38.)

Romeo's sun rises in a later scene.

3. The Street Wakens. 4. Morning Dance

An essential element of the story is the unawareness on

the part of the lover's parents, and of the adult population

of Verona in general, of Romeo and Juliet's love and of the

impending disaster. This situation is an essential element

of the plot, of course, but is also important for the tone

of the story in that the tragedy is set in relief by the

portrayal of people for whom life is continuing merrily as

always. Later in the story, for instance, Old Capulet

restrains Tybalt's indignation and allows the presence in

his house of his hated enemy's son simply because a

disturbance would ruin the gaiety of the party. Both he and

Tybalt are totally unaware that Romeo's words reveal his

love for Juliet even though it is by the sound of his voice

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
184

that they recognize him. Prokofiev provides this element to

his telling of the story with the lively number 3 and the

boisterous number 4, at the same time providing an occasion

for ensemble dancing.

The opening melody of number 3 perfectly depicts the

simple, happy character of the folk of Verona. (Please

refer to example 17, page 37.) Since the tonic pitch occurs

on the downbeat of the second measure, it is clear that

expectation of the arrival of the tonic is not what makes

the passage tonal. Rather, it is the expectation of

standard phrase length, in conjunction with the tonic's

eventual arrival on the downbeat of a strong measure in

measure 9:11, that creates the sense of cadence or tonal

closure at that time; meter makes this passage tonal.

The meter having been established, Prokofiev indulges

in some slight metrical games. Notable among them is a

transformation of the second passage, measure 10:1-13. (See

example 83.) The melody being viewed on a two-bar-

hypermeasure level, the first instance of the opening motive

begins on a strong measure, the second on a weak measure.

As this pattern continues, the high d comes on a weak

measure, the fourth hypermeasure. The tonic pitch's

occurrence on a fourth downbeat initially suggests the

arrival of a cadence. There is, however, an extension; the

phrase lasts ten measures, or five hypermeasures. When this

phrase is repeated, beginning with pick-up notes to measure

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 83. Metrical comparison of 10:1-10 with 11:3-10.

r ir “I i
'±*L

11:3, the opening motive is heard only once. The omission

of an entire hypermeasure from this phrase puts the high d

on a strong measure this time; the phrase sounds short even

though its length is standard.

The "Morning Dance" adds a menacing note to the simple

character of the preceding number: both the introductory

material and the melody that begins at measure 5, a

variation on the melody from number 3, emphasize the minor

third and the tritone. When members of the house of Capulet

and of the house of Montague are on the street together,

tension is present, ready at any moment to flare up.

With the brash horn melody at rehearsal 16, Prokofiev

sets himself a compositional problem. (See example 84.)

The melody, while only four measures long, modulates up a

perfect fourth, creating, in Charles Rosen's words, a

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
186

Example 84. Modulating melody in 16:1-4.

P U i o , Ary«, Afckl

/!

T i n y . P i t t o , V- * . , C . k .

"large-scale dissonance."3 Prokofiev resolves this

dissonance in two ways. At the end of this number he simply

alters and extends the end of the phrase. A second method

is used later in the ballet.

5. The Quarrel

The menacing tone of the previous number is given full

development here as the potential for trouble between the

houses is realized in this quarrel on the streets. The

number opens with two statements of the same motive that

began number 4. The minor mode, introduced and used

occasionally in the previous number, is present throughout

the quarrel. When Prokofiev sets aside his preferences and

uses the minor mode, he usually has an expressive purpose

for doing so; the minor mode here represents the conflict

between the houses.

3See Charles Rosen, Sonata Forms, revised ed. (New York:


W. W. Norton, 1988), pp. 25, 129, and passim.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
187

Onomatopoeic representation of the quarrel is provided

by the often recurring tremolos and fast repeated notes,

features reminiscent of Monteverdi's stile agitato, and by

the quick exchanges of register and instrumentation that

occur in measures 20:3, 20:12-13, and elsewhere. The two-

sided nature of the altercation is represented by the

lengths of the various passages: an opening of two measures

is followed by a motive lasting two beats, a pair of motives

each comprising two notes, a passage two measures long, an

ascending passage of four measures, a pair of measures

filled with repeated notes, etc.

Whereas quarrelers often find trading insults fun, and

fighters often continue to seek blood without consciously

thinking why, those at the moment between the exchange of

insults and the exchange of blows experience great emotional

heat. The violent, dissonant cadence at the end of the

number perfectly captures the moment. This exceptional

cadence (see example 32, page 65; chapter 3 contains details

on the resolution of the pitches involved) is used only

twice, both times at violent moments essential to the cycle

of fate that is to bring Romeo and Juliet to their doom.

If the simplicity of passion is represented by the use

of four-measure phrases and the complexity of decision

making by the use of other lengths, then Prokofiev tells us

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
188

here that no thought is involved in belligerent hatred.

Once the fight begins, instinct takes over those involved,

and the four-measure standard takes over the music. No

structure inherent in the pitch patterns demands the change

at measure 5 to a new bass; the C-major and D-major

harmonies of the first four measures could have alternated

for only one measure or for one hundred. (See example 48,

page 87.) The standard phrase length is present for the

purpose of expressing the emotions of the characters. The

scurrying pace of the fight gives way at rehearsals 30 and

33. It is only here, where the belligerents have a chance

to catch a breath and think, that the four-measure pattern

is broken. The changes are slight: two measures are taken

away from the second phrase in rehearsal 33, and one measure

is added just before rehearsal 35.

7. The Duke's Command. 8. Interlude

The slow pace and loud, dissonant chords lend to this

number, in all respects reminiscent of the "Catacombs"

movement from Mussorgsky's Pictures at an Exhibition, an air

of authority, judgment, and death.

. . . What, ho! you men, you beasts,


That quench the fire of your pernicious rage
With purple fountains issuing from your veins!
On pain of torture, from those bloody hands
Throw your mistemp'red weapons to the ground
And hear the sentence of your movfed prince.

If ever you disturb our streets again,


Your lives shall pay the forfeit of the peace.
(I,i, 81-86, 94-95.)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
189

The Duke and his train exit the stage during the

"Interlude," marked "andante pomposo." One of the most

striking features of the number is its unusual dissonances.

(See example 85.) Mild dissonances are present as early as

Example 85. Dissonances in 44:1-45:1.

(T D A n d an te pom poso CL’l s t e s s o tem po)

1 y c o r .l i.s o a w / iLllt m r r p r L i U f i

iny i
i*tftfe4=!
1i ir iiij- Hffftfffr
L,lrT HsJ IL 1 l i - L --E .= fcJ- -i

.,0 ,1 , .s- ^i t elig> § g - g - fe ee [


t’ l l u r

T n . n .m sr m . n n
/Tr-«l ^ *#*■
a n b I p i.

-sfafei-s n e e.g cci » < o h


i* lllt rm t t - r r r t D J ~ =
/
fr, i;p___ i
J 53 3rw:
• ^ jLii H i

the second half of the first beat, where scale degrees 2 and

4 sound above newly reiterated scale degrees 1, 3, and 5.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
190

Sharper dissonances begin in measure 5 where b-natural

occurs in the trombones on the second beat while b-flat

occurs in the horns above it. A more extreme case occurs on

the second half of beat 2 in measure 45:1, where c-natural

sounds together with d-flat as well as with the b-natural in

the trombones. (The b-natural is sustained from the

previous measure, although the piano reduction does not show

this.) The slow pace of the number gives these dissonances

a stubborn character, contributing to the pomposity called

for by the performance instructions.

9. Preparations for_the Ball (Juliet and Nurse)

Although the number is subtitled "Juliet and Nurse," it

is the Nurse's character that is treated in this number.

The open fourth in measure 2 and the open fifth in measure

4, out of place in a passage of fuller sonorities; the heavy

accents in measure 48:1; the parallel octaves in 48:3 and 4;

the sudden scale shifts— all point to the Nurse's humorous,

awkward character. (See example 86.) Her enjoyment in

telling the same silly story of a fall Juliet took as a

child (I,ii, 38-57) is represented by the numerous

repetitions of the same few short passages.

10. The Young Juliet

Neither Romeo nor Juliet expects to fall in love with

each other; Romeo thinks he is in love already, and Juliet

is too young to have thought much about love. This number

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
Example 86. Nurse's humorous character in 47:1-48:4.

Andante assai. Scherzando

fArehi
rwrw

finds Juliet, who is not yet fourteen, scurrying around her

room playing games with her nurse. (Please refer to example

20, page 43.) The quick tempo, the playful cadences, the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
192

regular two-measure phrases, the running scales, and the use

of orchestra bells all point to Juliet's youth.

Inside the thirteen-year-old, however, womanhood is

springing. Juliet's mother enters at rehearsal 53 with one

of the themes from the "Introduction,11 a theme that is to be

associated in the next few scenes with Paris and with the

tradition of passionless courtship practices and arranged

marriage he represents. When Juliet's mother asks her what

she thinks of marriage, Shakespeare has her reply, "It is an

honor that I dream not of" (I, iii, 66); Prokofiev has her

remind her mother of her youth by repeating, at rehearsal

54, the music from the beginning of the number.

In both tellings, Juliet's mother points out that she

is indeed old enough to think about marriage. In the play,

the mother observes that she herself was a mother by the

time she was Juliet's age. In Leonid Lavrovsky's original

choreography, the mother leads Juliet to the mirror to make

her point. Juliet's budding maturity and readiness for love

are revealed at rehearsal 55. (Please refer to example 73,

page 132.) Everything up to this point has merely set the

stage; now characters begin to develop. Prokofiev brings to

the occasion the first truly odd-length melody. As does

Juliet's awareness of the inner stirring, the melody starts

tentatively. The listener has no idea what the pulse or

tempo are when the flute begins its long high c, and still

does not until two or three measures later. Regardless of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
193

this fact, the melody covers six downbeats, the last marking

an elision into the next phrase. Thus, with or without

prior knowledge of the score, the phrase does not seem to

fit the standard length. Prokofiev uses the odd length to

express the new complexity of Juliet's character.

11. Arrival of the Guests

This number is subtitled "Minuet," but the characters

of the several melodies cover a spectrum much wider than

that usually associated with minuets. The first section is

stately, rehearsal 61 is demure, rehearsal 63 is graceful,

rehearsal 66 is comically pompous, and rehearsal 67 is

dainty. The variety of the pageant is expressed even in the

tonal structures of the various themes. All the melodies

begin in one key and end in another except for the one in

rehearsal 66, which is too boorish to change keys. Many of

them have no real ending but either cadence imperfectly

(64:9) or simply lead into the next melody (61:16).

12.. Masks

In this number, the audience is introduced to Mercutio.

Although a choreographer may have him present on stage

during the earlier street scene, it is here that Prokofiev

reveals Mercutio's character as he and Benvolio decide to

put on masks and go to the Capulets' party. They try to

persuade Romeo to go, but he hesitates:

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
194

ROMEO
And we mean well in going to this masque,
But 'tis no wit to go.

MERCUTIO Why, may one ask?

ROMEO
I dreamt a dream to-night.

MERCUTIO And so did I.

ROMEO
Well, what was yours?

MERCUTIO That dreamers often lie.

ROMEO
In bed asleep, while they do dream things true.
(I, iv, 48-52)

Such repartee is represented in the clever rhythmic turn at

the beginning of the melody, the dense chords that make

humorously false the martial spirit called for by the

performance instruction "andante marciale," the subtle

difference between the two statements of the first melody,

and the wry scale shifts. (Please refer to example 60, page

110.) The melodic structure of the first melody, one in

which stable tones of one scale are repeatedly reinterpreted

as members of a new scale, provides a musical analogy to the

use of puns in the boys' dialogue.

The theme that is specifically associated with Mercutio

later in the ballet begins at rehearsal 73. (Please refer

to example 58, page 106.) The ugly youth who makes light of

learning and love is present in the crude repetitions of the

raised fourth scale degree in measure 2, the flippant leaps

in measure 3, and the cynical shift to the minor mode in

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
195

measure 5. The melody has no cadence here in its first

presentation; an ending becomes tragically necessary later.

Romeo thinks of Rosaline one last time as rehearsal 75

offers a reprise of the music from the first scene. After

this, however, the sunrise motive is heard again; Romeo's

outlook is about to change for the better.

13. Dance of the Knights. 14. Juliet's Variation

Knights dance to music whose claim to be able to create

a noble theme out of an arpeggio and a dotted rhythm

discloses the pretentious character of the dancers. (Please

refer to example 77, page 138 for a reprise of this melody.)

The E-minor tonality gives way to A-flat major at rehearsal

81, reinforcing the importance of major-third structural

relationships in general and the special relationship shared

by C, A-flat, and E in particular. After a slower interlude

and a final cadence, this boastful music is followed by

Juliet's dance with Paris, at rehearsal 84. (Please refer

to example 79, page 142.) Juliet's theme here begins with

the same E-minor and B-minor arpeggios that launched the

Knights' dance, but the rhythmic and metrical transfor­

mation, the slow staccato articulation, and the mysterious

glissandi in the violins make this music coy rather than

brash. E minor is juxtaposed with C major this time as one

of the refined love themes from the "Introduction11 serves as

Paris's interlude.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
196

In number 14, Juliet begins to transform these themes.

As she becomes more comfortable with the situation, her

youthful scales begin to intrude, and the refined nature of

the melodies begins to soften. Whether playing the shy

debutante or the sprightly young girl, Juliet is unprepared

for the change that is about to take place in her life.

With the music at rehearsal 93, Romeo enters theparty,

sees Juliet, and falls in love:

Did my heart love till now? Forswear it, sight!


For I ne'er saw true beauty till this night.
(I, v, 52-53)

Most of the rest of the number is devoted to the melody from

the end of the "Introduction," the most impassioned of the

love themes presented so far. The melody occurs first in a

quick waltz variation, portraying the excitement that

accompanies Romeo's first sight of Juliet. (See example

87.) At rehearsal 95, the melody returns to its original

Example 87. 93:1-7 as metrical transformation of 2:6-12.


(Latter transposed.)

version. Paris sees a threat to his plans brewing and, at

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
197

the end of the number, tries to stop Romeo's advances.

15. Mercutio

To aid his friend by distracting the other guests, to

grab some attention for himself, and to provide the ballet

with a break in a succession of love themes, Mercutio dances

in this number. His witty character is represented by the

quick tempo, the subtle hemiolas, the wide leaps, the short,

forceful gestures, and the chromaticism. The contrasting

section links E-minor with A-flat once again. The octatonic

scale suggested at many scale shifts (see page 93) is

present in its complete form in measures 102:9-10, lending

confidence to the notion that Prokofiev's occasional use of

the scale is not accidental.

1,
6. Madrigal

In this number, Romeo and Juliet dance together for the

first time. Surrounded by other people, they feel a tension

between their need to act modestly and the desire to express

their passion. In the play, this tension is present in the

sonnet they recite to each other:

ROMEO
If I profane with my unworthiest hand
This holy shrine, the gentle sin is this;
My lips, two blushing pilgrims, ready stand
To smooth that rough touch with a tender kiss.

JULIET
Good pilgrim, you do wrong your hand too much,
Which mannerly devotion shows in this;
For saints have hands that pilgrims' hands do touch,
And palm to palm is holy palmers' kiss.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
198

ROMEO
Have not saints lips, and holy palmers too?

JULIET
Ay, pilgrim, lips that they must use in prayer.

ROMEO
0, then, dear saint, let lips do what hands do!
They pray; grant thou, lest faith turn to despair.

JULIET
Saints do not move, though grant for prayers' sake.

ROMEO
Then move not while my prayer's effect I take.
Thus from my lips, by thine my sin is purged.

JULIET
Then have my lips the sin that they have took.

ROMEO
Sin from my lips? 0 trespass sweetly urged!
Give me my sin again.

JULIET You kiss by th' book.


(I, v, 93-110)

That Prokofiev thought of representing this text

specifically is suggested by the title, "Madrigal." He

portrays both the inner and the apparent emotions of the

pair by using two kinds of music in this number. The first

three melodies are quiet, mostly diatonic, and emotionally

restrained. There is a degree of tension in the first

melody with its mild diatonic dissonances, its chromatic

third phrase, and the sinuous interlocking thirds in its

middle line, but the simplicity of its outer lines and the

diatonicism with which it begins and ends keeps the tension

under control. (Please refer to example 74, page 133.)

This music specifically suggests love's sacred nature,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
199

expressed by the metaphors in the poetry cited above. The

other two melodies of the first part first occur in the

"Introduction,"1 themes which have previously represented

innocence and decorous love. This music has suited Paris's

bid for Juliet's hand, but the love that she and Romeo find

and the music that expresses it are to surpass in beauty and

intensity what has come before. At rehearsal 111, with a

melody marked "caloroso," the restraints fall loose, and the

young lovers' true feelings are made known. (Please refer

to example 46, page 85.) The wide range covered in just the

first two measures, the wide, slow leaps, the exotic scale

shifts, the at times sinuous chromaticism, and the

appoggiaturas and other accented dissonances all point to

the passion Romeo and Juliet feel for one another. A

reprise of one of the simpler melodies, at rehearsal 113,

brings the audience gently back from the realm of inner

emotions to an awareness of the surrounding situation.

1.7_.- Tvbalt_Recognizes Romeo. 18. Gavotte (Departure of


Guests.)

When Tybalt recognizes the son of the enemy, Montague,

he is ready to fight him; Mercutio, Old Capulet and others

hold him back. This conflict, as are others, is represented

in the number by the use of two different kinds of music.

Tybalt's rage is expressed by music from the "Quarrel" and

music from the "Dance of the Knights." The various motives

generally fill four measures each, representing the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
200

ingrained, instinctual nature of the hatred the men of the

two houses are taught to have for each other. The attempts

of others to calm Tybalt are set to music originally heard

at rehearsal 63, music whose phrases last five measures

each, a great contrast to the bold, square meter of Tybalt's

music. The meter is even less obvious in the variation

presented here since each beat of the original now takes up

one whole measure, and this only after an ambiguous

beginning. (See example 88.)

The turmoil having abated, the guests leave peacefully

in pageantry. The "Gavotte" from Prokofiev's Symphony No. 1

occurs here in expanded form. The interlude appears three

times. As a result, the main theme appears four times, each

time with variations, the most ironic of which occurs in the

last phrase, when the shift back to the D-major scale comes

early. (See example 89.) The very normal cadence that

results sounds very odd indeed in context. It is here that

the Nurse tells Juliet the name of the boy she has just

fallen in love with.

19. Balcony Scene. 20. Romeo's Variation, 21. Love Dance

But soft! What light through yonder window breaks?


It is the East, and Juliet is the sun!
(II, ii, 1-2)

The sunrise motive begins the "Balcony Scene;" Romeo's

night is over. His and Juliet's initial concern for names

and for proper behavior are evident in the use of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
201

Example 88. Contrasting moods in 114:1-116:1. (Each three-


measure group corresponding to a single measure at
rehearsal 63 bracketed.)

> >
i-hH ■-j^ — . -- -

■ 5- > c

'-fils]T-.ii i--
m m T@l
> £jy r=
/ -^1 •f
r Iraftgtu'iio itprtti:

r f -S - L 4^=
.M
ii
^ m ft ,
T— 1M -
---- J----
r

■ 41— •
I

J i " /
|» fe p d i d £ = d w

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
202

Example 89. Early shift and normal cadence in 133:5-8.

i ,i m t f |
■==»- mp r \ I
j jjir n s 1 ■ | 1=
1-
*• f--
.

restrained melodies from the first part of the "Madrigal."

The young lovers' desire to overstep the boundaries set by

society, on the other hand, is expressed by the throbbing

interruptions to these melodies, starting at rehearsal 136.

Once the more passionate music begins this time, the

innocent, restrained love themes of the "Introduction" are

silenced forever.

Prokofiev now follows one intense love theme with

another through the end of the first act. Number 20 begins

with a quick waltz variation of the "caloroso" music of

rehearsal 111 (see example 90), expanding on a side of

Romeo's character first revealed at rehearsal 93, where he

first sees Juliet. This number has an ABA form, the

interlude introducing a theme in the same fast triple meter,

a theme whose wriggling chromatic passing tones serve per­

fectly as a foil to the wide leaps of the surrounding music.

Number 21 introduces another new theme, at 142:5. (See

example 91.) This melody, while lasting eight measures,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
203

Example 90. 139:1-7 as metrical transformation of 111:1-4


(Latter transposed.)

places emphasis on the fourth downbeat with an appoggiatura

at the top of the melody's nearly three-octave range, thus

throwing off the symmetrical nature inherent in the eight-

measure length. As a further irregularity, the melody

begins in F find ends on Z. It is precisely irregularities

such as these that keep this long string of melodies moving

without being too regular .n its large-scale rhythm, and

that makes the section so successful.

Measure 143:11 witnesses a reprise of the melody

introduced in number 20. Since neither of its two phrases

cadences, Prokofiev is able to add interest here by merely

reversing the order of the phrases. The addition of

chromatically sliding parallel fifths at rehearsal 144

increases the melody's sensuousness. As a contrast to the

open-ended nature of the previous phrases, the series of

cadence figures discussed earlier (see page 107) increases

tension through its chromatic ascents, quickly shifting

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
204

Example 91. Asymmetry within standard length in 142:4-12.

P'J’3| E u j i rT1

scales, and a melodic rise of two octaves in as many

measures. None of the melodies being used seems enough to

express the height of emotion at this point; only one phrase

from each is heard. The climax is finally reached in

measure 145:4 as the previously noted appoggiatura in the

opening melody of this number is harmonized chromatically

with a half-diminished seventh, quite voluptuous in contrast

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
205

to the figure's original diatonic setting. The long section

quickly relaxes with a cadence at long last, the second half

of the "caloroso" music serving the purpose. The closing

melody of the "Introduction" and the sunrise motive, a

literal representation this time, provide a long, glowing

coda to the act.

A series of folk-style melodies makes up number 22.

Simple bass lines, diatonicism, use of the four-bar phrase,

and sequential treatment of simple motives are four of the

factors that give the music its relatively simple character.

In addition, emphasis on the perfect fifth and perfect

fourth— a result of a high incidence of these intervals

between consecutive pairs of melodic pitches, numerous

outlined triads in the melodies, use of the fifth as a

structural interval in bass lines, use of fifth

relationships between the keys of successive melodies, and

occasional use of quintal harmony— contributes to the folk­

like quality.

The first two periods of this number illustrate these

points. (Please refer to example 69, page 128, and to

example 56, page 105.) All phrases in these two periods,

including the introductory "phrase," last four measures.

The first melody is entirely diatonic; the second melody,

although comprising a section using the four-flat scale

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
206

between its G-major beginning and ending, uses no chromatic

pitches within these scale areas. The perfect fourth and

fifth are found between many consecutive pairs in the first

melody, notably in measures 151:7-9; the fifth is present in

each outlined triad in the sequential second melody. As the

D-major tonality of the first melody yields to G major in

the second (a fifth relationship), the D-major triad

stubbornly remains over the new g-natural bass, forming

quintally flavored harmony. As a final point, the simple

bass line of the second melody moves down by step (with

octave displacement in measure 52:4) to the fifth scale

degree, d, at which time it leaps back to the tonic. The

folk dances in the second act serve to provide more of the

ensemble dancing required for a successful spectacle, to

reinforce the idea of the general public's unawareness of

the impending tragedy, and to set the stage for the upcoming

street fight.

23. Romeo and Mercutio. 24. Dance of the Five Couples. 25_._
Dance with Mandolins

The sacred and the profane are juxtaposed in number 23

as Romeo's thoughts, expressed by the "Madrigal" melody, are

contrasted with the mocking humor of Mercutio's themes from

"Masks." Foreshadowing is supplied by the transformation of

the music from rehearsal 73 to the minor mode.

The folk dance that follows in number 24 displays an

unusual degree of sophistication, starting with the chro-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
207

matically sliding parallel sixths and the eleven-measure

form of the opening melody. (Please refer to example 26,

page 57.) Although other sections of the number use four-

bar phrases, chromaticism remains a distinctive feature of

most of the melodies, as in the chromatic sequence found at

rehearsal 176 and elsewhere. Two further examples of

relative sophistication, are found in rehearsal 178.

(Please refer to example 14, page 33.) The beginning of the

melody features an ingenious chromatic sequence that seems

to rise and fall at the same time. Near the end of the

melody, Prokofiev expands on one of Schubert's devices. The

reinterpretation of the second harmony in measure 13, a

seventh chord built on the lowered sixth scale degree, as a

dominant of the Neapolitan is a common traditional

procedure, although the use of a minor Neapolitan may be

new. But in the next measure, Prokofiev resolves this same

chord deceptively, so that a chord built on the lowered

sixth of the main key moves to a chord built on the lowered

sixth of the Neapolitan.

Chords generated through chromatic voice leading and

regular phrase length fill the sixteen-bar variations in the

"Dance with Mandolins."

26. Nurse, 27. The Nu£se_Jp.e.lJj&e.r.g_RQTOeo a Note.fr.Qm Juliet

The Nurse's music is given full development here.

Included is a clever musical pun discussed earlier (please

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
208

refer to example 63, page 115), suitable for a character

that indulges in wordplay such as

. . . I'll lay fourteen of my teeth—


And yet, to my teen be it spoken, I have but four-
She's not fourteen. (I, iii, 12-14)

In number 27, the sender of the note as well as the

emotional response of the receiver are made known by the

excited music from "The Young Juliet." The final cadence,

an E-minor triad followed by a B-flat-major triad, is

suggestive of the outcome of the marriage just agreed upon.

The tritone relationship is found at only one other cadence

in the ballet.

28. Romeo at Friar Laurence's. 29. Juliet at. Friar


Laurence's. 30. Public Merrymaking. 31. Further Public
Festivities
Friar Laurence is represented by two gentle melodies,

each moving mostly in slow quarters, as a chorale. The

lovely chromatic embellishment of a-vii°/V in measure 208:6

is especially effective in contrast with the totally

diatonic first melody. (See example 92.)

Example 92. Chromatic embellishment of vii°/V in 208:5-8.

3 if.
ri , 6
*," yr
-.■mi ¥ -taL. ----- —
rjMEO
JQL
w
1)0-

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
209

The music for the marriage ceremony, in number 29, uses

metrically irregular or ambiguous phrases; the effect of a

folk dance or street fight is not appropriate here.

Although the beautiful melody starting at rehearsal 213

consists of two eight-measure phrases, the regularity is not

clear since each phrase starts on the second beat of a

measure. No difference in harmony, rhythm, orchestration,

or texture marks the downbeat in either case.

The wedding is followed by a reprise of most of the

folk dances heard earlier. The large-scale dissonance of

the short modulating melody from the "Morning Dance" is

given its second resolution in this section, at rehearsal

228. Starting on D, the melody modulates, cadencing on G.

Then, after a short interlude, the melody starts again, this

time on A, and modulates to D. A simple I-IV-V-I

progression is the result.

32. Meeting of Tvbalt and Mercutio. 33. Tybalt and Mercutio


Fight. 34. Mercutio Dies. 35. Romeo Decides to Avenge the
Death of Mercutio. 36. Finale of the Second Act

The use of b-natural in rapid alternation with a D-

minor triad, whether thought of as a raised sixth that

resolves in the wrong direction or as half of an unresolved

tritone, tastes of impending disaster. The transformation

to minor, at 247:9, of the melody from "The Street Awakens,"

together with the addition of a dissonant, chromatic counter

subject indicates that the underlying tension that the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
210

public has tried to overlook is about to turn the festive

square of Verona into a place of death. (See example 93.)

Example 93. Minor mode as foreboding of death in 247:1-16.


T.al.V.le
A ciZl I I I. ^**~^****' e^—
- J-/j-jjj T B J T T j | j^ 1JJ
v.-r ■
C.b
8a«.,.Faic.
-i * ...r i r i . i i - r —f t » i—.— i i i i r r n - i
-=fi WA j f i U b =
Jcor. -r >• ' /
.P j - j j

, , ? ; ri > i . n ,
1 :» ............
f ’ ^ ’ r ^~]
/ C.ingl , Tr-ba cualord. - • ' > #•
i i i i i i n
.jmj-rpl

fi-j; pa u h £=
^ *ear
C r“. --f La
V ft
' - =' l
■2* f r >A - r K ..V >• sa-'

A new turn at rehearsal 248, full of violently unresolved

diminished fifths and fourths, is accompanied by the

arpeggiated motive from the "Dance of the Knights," showing

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
211

that Ingrained machismo and not reason or just retribution

is behind the quarrel.

In number 33, the exuberant music from Mercutio's dance

at the Capulet's party returns with a few alterations and

insertions; among them are metrical ambiguity in the opening

material resulting from a five-beat pattern superimposed on

the triple meter, and the series of dissonant chords at

measures 256:3-5 and elsewhere. This musical juxtaposition

shows that, while Mercutio has moments of trouble, his

cavalier attitude will not allow him to take seriously the

possibility of death. The recurrence of the violent cadence

from number 5 conveys the tragic consequences.

The slow minor-mode transformation of the melody from

"The Street Wakens" now, in number 34, represents the dying

Mercutio's staggering around, trying to fend off death. At

measures 263:6-7, we hear for the first time the motive of

death that will recur tragically often in coming scenes: the

juxtaposition of two minor triads a third apart over a slow

throbbing bass. (See example 94.) The return of the major

mode for the final reprise of Mercutio's theme, at rehearsal

265, underscores the poignancy of his futile struggle. How

different at this slow tempo seem the wide leaps at 265:5-7

that once sounded so joyously arrogant. When, at the

cadence, a B-minor triad is followed by an E-flat minor

triad, Mercutio is dead.

Romeo's decision to kill Tybalt in revenge and the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
212

Example 94. Juxtaposition of two minor triads a third apart


as signal of death in 263:4-8.

iIp #

i^PPPPPPP
A n k l toa aori.
ri
PP

subsequent duel is set to music from the first street fight.

The most significant difference is the string of violently

accented, dissonant sonorities at rehearsal 280; Romeo

achieves his goal. Hatred and inexorable fate are evident

in the loud, dissonant finale to Act 2.

37. Introduction. 38. Romeo and J u l i e t . 39. Romeo bids


Juliet Farewell

The "Introduction” to Act 3 is a reprise of "The Duke's

Command," reminding the audience that Romeo must, as a

result of his actions, live in exile for the rest of his

life or die. This judgmental music is contrasted with a

reprise, in number 38, of one of the melodies from the

wedding scene as the curtain rises on Romeo and Juliet,

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
213

together in the morning. Number 39 introduces a new melody,

at rehearsal 289. As do so many of the love themes, this

melody rises nearly two octaves in its opening gesture. The

first phrase ending on supertonic harmony, the opening

phrase of the entire ballet provides the perfect consequent.

The happy ending looked to by the original occurrence cf the

phrase is no longer a dream appearing from nowhere;

happiness, if but for a moment, is a reality. The rest of

this number is much like the balcony scene.

40. Nurse. 41. Juliet Refuses to Marrv Paris. 42. Juliet


Alone. 42. Interlude

As adolescents, neither Romeo nor Juliet can, or even

think to, look beyond circumstances to the future; as Romeo

departs, Juliet asks suddenly, "O, think'st thou we shall

ever meet again?" (Ill, v, 51) Unable to foresee

difficulty, the young couple must face it as it comes. To

previously introduced melodies, the Nurse, Juliet's parents,

and Paris enter in number 40, and Juliet is told she will

marry Paris. Not brave enough to admit the truth, Juliet

first tries to simply put the trouble off. She says,

I will not marry yet; and when I do, I swear


It shall be Romeo, whom you know I hate,
Rather than Paris. (Ill, v, 122-24)

To her parents, this response seems to be a complaint that

she is not old enough, an idea expressed in the music by the

use at the beginning of number 41 of a variation on music

from "Young Juliet". At rehearsal 303, Juliet's inner

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
214

thoughts are revealed as the music first heard when Juliet

realized her maturity is transformed to the minor mode: she

is old enough to marry, but has, to her now realized misery,

married a man not allowed in her house or, in fact, in her

city. Her mother exclaims, "I would the fool were married

to her grave!” (Ill, v, 141) As she and Old Capulet storm

out, the music of "The Dance of the Knights" reveals the

egotistical indignation felt by the Father and Paris.

Juliet is left alone for numbers 42 and 43. Various love

themes accompany her thoughts of Romeo as she, having

received no help even from the Nurse, decides to go to Friar

Laurence for help.

44_.__At Friar .Laurence's Cell

As Juliet explains her situation and Laurence begins to

realize what desperate measures are needed, even his melody

is transformed to the minor mode, at 313:3. The strikingly

different effects Prokofiev achieves through transformation

of his themes is reflective of a bit of wisdom spoken by

Friar Laurence himself:

0, mickle is the powerful grace that lies


In plants, herbs, stones, and their true gualities?
For naught so vile that on the earth doth live
But to the earth some special good doth give;
Nor aught so good but, strained from that fair use,
Revolts from true birth, stumbling on abuse.
Virtue itself turns vice, being misapplied,
And vice sometime 's by action dignified.
(II, iii,15-22)

With this very knowledge, Laurence is able to make a potion

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
215

that will cause Juliet to appear dead. At rehearsal 314, he

gives Juliet this potion. At 314:5, the upper strings play

a new motive in which can be heard the slowly bubbling

poison as well as Juliet's careful contemplation of its use,

while the low winds and strings play more new material, this

melody, with its outlining of the augmented second between

the unresolved leading tone and lowered sixth, representing

the hideousness of even feigned death. (See example 95.)

Example 95. Death and poison motives in 313:10-314:6.

Fa*., V-l

n— n rrn

Cl.*.
C.-fa*.
Tuba C-*„esp

Love themes at the end of this number indicate Friar

Laurence's assurances concerning the outcome of the affair.

45. Interlude. 46. Juliet's Room. 47. Juliet Alone

From this point until the final scene, the minor mode

predominates through the dark proceedings. "Dance of the

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
216

Knights" music in number 45 indicates the return of Paris.

Number 324 brings the return of the melody to which Juliet

grudgingly agreed to dance with Paris; here it accompanies

her false agreement to marry him. The second half of number

46 and all of number 47 are devoted mostly to the theme of

Juliet's mature love in its minor-mode form, and to the

poison and death motives. Paris and the Capulets satisfied,

they leave Juliet alone to contemplate and to perform the

dreadful act of drinking poison to fool her family into

thinking she is dead that she may safely leave the only home

she has known in order to live with an exile. The only

formal articulations in this contemplative and metrically

highly irregular passage are various juxtapositions of minor

triads. The last, at measures 333:4-6, involves a C-sharp-

minor triad moving to an F-minor triad over a throbbing bass

that keeps irregularly slowing down as Juliet's heart slows

to an imperceptible murmur.

48-.Morning_Serenade_,_ 49. Dance of the Girls with Lilies

These two numbers provide a contrast to the dark,

irregularly phrased music that begins at the scene of

Juliet's visit to Friar Laurence and extends to the final

scene. Both numbers move almost exclusively in four-measure

phrases that add up to simple symmetrical forms. The

"Morning Serenade" constitutes the longest major-mode

passage of this section. Its use of C-minor harmony as an

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
217

embellishing chord for the E-major tonic is especially

striking and provides the final instance of the special

relationship between those roots. Number 49, with its

harmonic progression generated by chromatic voice leading,

offers an example of a link between foreground and

middleground root relationships: several phrases beginning

in A minor eventually slip into G-sharp minor, and G-sharp

minor is the neighbor chord used to embellish A minor in the

first two measures of the melody.

50.. At Juliet's Bedside. 51. Ju l i e t s Funeral. 52. Juliets


Death

Part of the Nurse's music constitutes most of number

50. She tries to wake Juliet, and as she discovers what she

believes to be a dead body, the orchestra plays the slow

theme from "Young Juliet," which indicates that her readi­

ness for mature love has driven her to this act. The final

cadence juxtaposes A-flat and C as roots. The triads' major

quality, in contrast with the minor quality used at other

such cadences, makes the situation more poignant, as it did

when Mercutio's theme was played, at its last appearance, in

the major mode. The use of the major mode here also

suggests that good can come from the grim circumstances,

since Juliet's death is to serve as a means of ending the

feud between the two houses.

The death melody appropriately constitutes most of the

music of the funeral scene, in number 51. Its slow, regular

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
218

dotted rhythms serve perfectly for a funeral march. A love

theme at rehearsal 354 marks Romeo's presence. The death

melody at 355, forceful arpeggios at 356, harsh dissonances

at 357, and mysterious murmuring at 358 accompany the duel

between Romeo and Paris, the death of Paris, and Romeo's

commission of suicide.

Number 52 is entitled "Juliet's Death," and Romeo's

death takes place during the number entitled "Juliet's

Funeral." Prokofiev is correct in focusing attention on

Juliet here, since it is her character and not Romeo's that

really changes over the course of the story. Romeo's

suicide is the last rash act of a consistently rash boy, a

characterization confirmed by the strong, dissonant music

played during his death. How differently Prokofiev

accompanies Juliet's death! The juxtaposition of A-flat

major and C major that occurred at the nurse's discovery of

Juliet's feigned death returns as the theme of mature love

is transformed again to the major mode for four soaring

appearances. The full effect of the work is not appreciated

unless one sees, or at least visualizes, the gruesome action

taking place. "0 happy dagger!/ This is thy sheath; there

rust, and let me die." (V, iii, 169-70) The association of

the violent suicide of a thirteen-year-old girl with the

beautiful music of this last scene is very moving. A slow

coda accompanies the gathering of the two families and the

Prince in the sepulchre as Capulet and Montague reconcile.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
219

A tragic, ironic cadence with a tritone root relationship

closes the ballet.

Go hence, to have more talk of these sad things;


Some shall be pardoned, and some punished;
For never was a story of more woe
Than this of Juliet and her Romeo.
(V, iii, 307-310)

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
220

CONCLUSION

In general terms, a tonal style is one in which musical

events bear implications concerning future events in the

same composition. More specifically, a tonal style is one

in which these implications work together to point to the

time and pitch of the final sonority of a given formal unit.

Although matters of pitch receive most of the attention in

discussions of tonality, it is actually matters of time upon

which a tonal style must be based; it is only when the

metrical placement of a cadence can be anticipated that its

pitch can be sensed as well. The agent for establishing the

sense for the metrical placement of cadences was, in

history, a regularity of phrase length. Once the pitch

patterns within the framework of the regular phrase length

began to crystalize, composers became free to alter the

regularity of phrases without destroying the sense of

tonality. Thus, tonality can survive if the proper pitch

patterns alone are present. However, should a composer wish

to create using new pitch patterns a style that prompts

active anticipation of cadences, he would have to once again

provide the necessary metrical regularity.

Prokofiev is just such a composer. While he felt he

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
221

needed always to be seeking new musical patterns, his early

use of shocking dissonances later gave way, at about the

time of the composition of our model, Romeo and Juliet, to a

more accessible style that, by consensus, is tonal.

Prokofiev's use of the four-measure phrase allowed him to

create a sense of anticipation, while freeing him to write

within that framework progressions not quite like anything

heard before.

The use of particular pitch patterns at cadences, of

diatonic scales as pitch sources, of particular methods of

shifting from one scale to another, of (in some cases)

stepwise melodic structures, and of melodic tendencies of

the twelve individual tones provide the listener with means

by which to sense pitches of resolution in the style of

Romeo and Juliet. Granted, the patterns are in some cases

complex enough to require some practice and familiarity on

the part of the listener, even though the sense of temporal

anticipation is picked up very easily. While these patterns

contribute to the tonal nature of the style, they also, by

their peculiarity, make the style unique. The wide variety

of cadential dominant sonorities, the sudden scale shifts

just before the final sonority of a period's first phrase

and just before the penultimate sonority of a period's last

phrase, the occasionally nontraditional melodic structures,

the high level of chromaticism, and the unusual resolution

of chromatic pitches combine to make Prokofiev's some of

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
222

history's most easily recognized music. This tonal style is

eminently suitable for accompanying the telling of the story

of a Shakespearean play, especially that of Romeo and

Juliet, since in both the story and the music, events are

directed, by the Fates or by the listener's ear, through

ironic twists toward their inevitable conclusion.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
223

APPENDIX A

LOCATION OF CADENCE FIGURES


USED FOR STUDY IN CHAPTER III

Designations followed by (L) represent dominant-to-

tonic progressions that lead into a phrase without providing

closure to the previous phrase.

1. Beginning:3-4
2 . 1 :11-12
3. 2:11-12
4. 2:15-16
5. 2:17-3:1 (L)
6. 3:13
7. 4:3-4
8. 5:6-7
9. 6:9-7:1
10 . 8 :11-12
11. 9:10-11
12. 10:13-11:1
13. 16:4
14. 19:8
15. 19:44-45
16. 25:12-14
17. 26:8-27:1 (L)
18. 27:12-28:1 (L)
19. 48:4
20. 48:6-7 (L)
21. 48:8 (L)
22. 49:8-9
23. 50:2
24. 50:4
25. 50:8
26. 54:4
27. 54:7-8
28. 55:8-9
29. 58:7-8
30. 59:8-9
31. 60:3-4

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
224

32. 63:10-64:1
33. 64:8-9
34. 66:1-2
35. 66:4-5
36. 71:11-12
37. 73:8-74:1 (L)
38. 75:7-8
39. 77:2-3 (L)
40. 80:10
41. 84:8-85:1 (L)
42. 85:7-8
43. 86:3-4
44. 86:7-8
45. 90:13-14
46. 93:14-15
47. 95:8-9
48. 96:6-7
49. 103:8-9
50. 106:7-8
51. 106:10-12
52. 107:15-17
53. 111:7-8
54. 117:11-12
55. 121:5-6
56. 122:13-123:1
57. 125:8-9
58. 128:7-8
59. 137:11-138:1
60. 140:18-141:1
61. 142:11-12
62. 145:7-8
63. 152:8-153:1
64. 169:8-9
65. 174:18-19
66. 176:7-8
67. 178:15-16
68 . 201:2
69. 203:6
70. 203:7
71. 204:6
72. 206:8
73. 206:10
74. 207:4
75. 210:8
76. 254:6-7
77. 283:11-12
78. 289:2
79. 306:9-10

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
225

APPENDIX B

LOCATION OF MELODIES USED FOR STUDY IN CHAPTER VI

1. Beginning: 1-4
2. 1:5-12
3. 1:13-20
4. 2:6-13
5. 6:3-7 :1
6. 9:5-11:1
7. 18:5-19:8
8. 30:1-8
9. 44:5-8
10. 48:1-4
11. 50:1-8
12. 55:1-6
13. 60:1-4
14. 64:1-9
15. 67:1-4
16. 71:1-8
17. 73:1-4
18. 84:1-85:8
19. 88:3-89:10
20. 97:1-5
21. 106:1-8
22. 107:1-17
23. 111:1-112:8
24. 122:3-123:1
25. 125:2-9
26. 151:5-152:1
27. 152:2-153:1
28. 161:1-7
29. 174:9-19
30. 178:1-16
31. 201:1-4
32. 207:1-8
33. 208:1-209:8
34. 212:6-8
35. 213:5-214:8
36. 289:1-8
37. 315:3-6

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
226

SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY

Works Cited

Tonality

Apel, Willi. Harvard Dictionary of Music. 2nd ed. Cambridge,


MA: Belknap Press, 1969. S.v. "Tonality."

Crocker, Richard L. A History of Musical Style. New York:


McGraw-Hill, 1966.

Hindemith, Paul. The Craft of Musical Composition. Book l:


Theoretical Part. Translated by Arthur Mendel. 4th ed.
Mainz: Schott, 1970.

New. Grove_pictionarv of Music and Musicians. The. Edited by


Stanley Sadie. S.v. "Tonality," by Carl Dahlhaus.

Persichetti, Vincent. Twentieth-Century Harmony. New York:


W. W. Norton, 1961.

Rosen, Charles. Sonata Forms. Revised ed. New York: W.W.


Norton, 1988.

Schoenberg, Arnold, et al. Schoenberg. Edited by Merle


Armitage. New York: G. Schirmer, 1937; reprint ed.,
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1977.

Regular Phrase Length

Cooper, Grosvenor and Meyer, Leonard B. The Rhythmic


Structure of Music. Chicago: University of Chicago
Press, I960.

Seashore, Carl E. Psychology of Music. New York: McGraw-


Hill, 1938.

Stravinsky, Igor, poetics of Music in_the F_orm of Six


Lessons. Translated by Arthur Knodel and Ingolf Dahl.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1947.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
227

Information Theory

Youngblood, Joseph E. "Style as Information." Journal of


Music Theory 2 (April 1958):24-35.

Prokofiev and Romeo and Juliet

Balanchine, George. Balanchine's New Complete Stories of the


Great Ballets. Edited by Francis Mason. Garden City,
NY: Doubleday, 1968.

Nestyev, Israel V. Prokofiev. Translated by Florence Jonas.


Foreword by Nicolas Slonimsky. Stanford, CA: Stanford
University Press, i960.

Prokofiev, Sergei. Romeo and Juliet: Piano Score, Op. 64.


Melville, NY: Belwin Mills, n.d.

Prokofiev, Sergei et al. Autobiography. Articles.


Reminiscences. Edited by Semyon Shlifstein. Translated
by Rose Prokofieva. Moscow: Foreign Language Publishing
House, n.d.

Shakespeare, William. Romeo and Juliet. Revised ed. Edited


by John E. Hankins. The Pelican Shakespeare. New York:
Penguin Books, 1970.

Other Works on Prokofiev

English Language

Ashley, Patricia Ruth. "Prokofiev's Piano Music: Line,


Chord, Key." Ph.D. dissertation, University of
Rochester, Eastman School of Music, 1963.

Austin, William W. "Prokofiev's Fifth Symphony." Music


Review 17 (August 1956):205-20.

Biancolli, Louis. Serge Prpkofieff and His Orchestral Music .


New York: The Philharmonic-Symphony Society of New
York, 1953.

Brown, Malcolm Hamrick. "The Symphonies of Serge Prokofiev."


Ph.D. dissertation, Florida State University, 1967.

Dresser, Mary Anne. "Twentieth-Century Russian Cello


Sonatas." D.M.A. document, University of Texas at
Austin, 1983.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
228

Evans, Robert Kenneth. "The Early Songs of Sergei Prokofiev


and their Relation to the Synthesis of the Arts in
Russia 1890-1922.” Ph.D. dissertation, Ohio State
University, 1971.

Hanson, Lawrence and Elizabeth. Prokofiev; The Prodigal Son.


London: Cassell, 1964.

Kaufman, Rebecca Sue. “Expanded Tonality in the Late Chamber


Works of Sergei Prokofiev.” Ph.D. dissertation,
University of Kansas, 1987.

Me A1lister, Margaret Notman. "The Operas of Sergei


Prokofiev." Ph.D. dissertation, Cambridge University,
1970.

McAllister, Rita. "Sergey Prokofiev." In The New Grove


Russian Masters 2 . London: Macmillan, 1986.
A revision of the article by McAllister in The New
Grove Dictionary.

Martin, Rebecca Gena. "The Nine Piano Sonatas of Sergei


Prokofiev." D.M.A. project, University of Kentucky,
1982.

Moreux, Serge. "Prokofiev: An Intimate Portrait." Tempo. no.


11 (1949), pp. 5-9.

New Grove Dictionary of Music and, Musicians. The. Edited by


Stanley Sadie. S.v. "Prokofiev, Sergey," by Rita
McAllister.

Prokofiev, Sergei. Prokofiev by Prokofiev: A Composer/s


Memoir. Edited by David H Appel. Translated by Guy
Daniels. Garden City, NY: Doubleday, 1979.

________ . "Prokofiev Explains." Musical Times 89 (August


1948):233-34.

Prokofiev, Sergei et al. Sergei Prokofiev: Materials.


Articles. Interviews. Compiled by Vladimir Blok.
Translated by Kathleen Cook, Rose Prokofieva, et al.
[Moscow]: Progress Pulishers, 1978.

Robinson, Harlow. Seraei Prokofiev: A Biography. New York:


Viking, 1987.

Rogerson, Virginia C. "Political Influences on Twentieth-


Century Russian Composers and Their Compositions as
Observed in the Literature of Prokofiev, Shostakovich
and Khachaturian." M.A. thesis, California State

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
229

University, Fullerton, 1980.

Roseberry, Eric. "Prokofiev's Piano Sonatas." Music and


MUSi<?3.3ng 19 (1971) :38-42.

Samuel, Claude. Prokofiev. Translated by Miriam John.


London: Calder and Boyars, 1971.

Seroff, Victor. Sergei Prokofiev: A Soviet Tragedy. New


York: Funk and Wagnalls, 1968.

Smith, Elwood L. "The Fifth Symphony of Sergei Prokofiev."


Master's thesis, Eastman School of Music, 1957.

Vlahcevic, Sonia Klosek. "Thematic-Tonal Organization in the


Late Sonatas of Sergei Prokofiev." Ph.D. dissertation,
Catholic University, 1975.

Werth, Alexander. "The Real Prokofiev." Nation 176 (April 4,


1953):286.

Foreign Language

Aranovskii, Mark. Melos of S. Prokofiev. Leningrad: Muzyka,


1969.
In Russian.

Blok, Vladimir. "Prokofiev's Method of Composition: Problems


in the Organization of His Work Illustrated by
Materials from His Instrumental Music." M.A.
dissertation, Moscow Conservatory, 1973.
In Russian.

________ • Prokofiev's Music for Children. Moscow: Muzyka,


1969.
In Russian.

________ • S. S. Prokof'ev. Studies and Articles. Moscow:


Muzyka, 1972.
In Russian.

Kiseleva, E. I. "Auxiliary Tones in Prokofiev's Harmony." In


Theoretical Problems of 20th-Century Music I: A
Collection of Articles. Edited by Iuri Tiulin. Moscow:
Muzyka, 1967.
In Russian.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.
230

________ . "Polyharmony and Polytonality in the Work of S.


Prokofiev." In Problems of Music Theory; Book 2 . Edited
by Iuri Tiulin. Moscow: Muzyka, 1970.
In Russian.

Poszowski, Antoni. "The Features of the 'Prokof'ev


Dominant.'" Zeszvtv Naukowe Paristwowa Wyzsza Szkola

In Polish.

Streller, Friedbert. "Die friihen Sinfonien Sergej


Prokofjews: Ein Beitrag zu semantischer Analyse von
Musik." Ph.D. dissertation, Berlin, 1972.

Xolopov, Iuri. .Contemporary Traits in Prokofiev's Harmony.


Moscow: Muzyka, 1967.
In Russian.

________ • "Integral Development in the Harmonic Structures


of Prokofiev's Work." In Theoretical Problems of 20th-
.Csntury. Music I: A-Collection of Articles. Edited by
Iuri Tiulin. Moscow: Muzyka, 1967.
In Russian.

________ . "Russian Neoclassicism: Neoclassic Tendencies in


Prokofiev and Shostakovich." HudVeda 17 (1980):291-304.
In Czech.

Xolopova, Valentina. The problem of Rhvthm in the Work of


Composers_of the First Half of the 20th Century.
Moscow: Muzyka, 1971.
In Russian.

Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Potrebbero piacerti anche