Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

Personality and Individual Differences 123 (2018) 110–114

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The effects of money exposure on testosterone and risk-taking, and the MARK
moderating role of narcissism☆

Eric P. Stenstroma, , John B. Dinsmoreb, Jonathan W. Kunstmana, Kathleen D. Vohsc
a
Miami University, USA
b
Wright State University, USA
c
University of Minnesota, USA

A R T I C L E I N F O A B S T R A C T

Keywords: Although prior research has demonstrated that reminders of money influence motivations and behaviors, there
Money has been scant attention to whether money cues can alter physiological responses. An experiment testing male
Testosterone participants assessed whether being randomly assigned to handle money versus paper would change men's
Narcissism testosterone levels and affect financial risk-taking. Results showed that the effects of handling money on tes-
Risk-taking
tosterone levels and risk-taking depended on trait narcissism. Among men low in narcissism, handling money led
to a greater increase in testosterone levels from Time 1 (baseline) to Time 2 (post-manipulation) compared with
their counterparts in a neutral, non-money condition. Conversely, highly narcissistic men who were randomly
assigned to handle money exhibited a weaker increase in testosterone levels relative to men in the neutral
condition. The results of moderated mediation analyses suggested that money exposure affected financial risk-
taking through changes in testosterone levels. Men low in narcissism became more inclined to take risks through
an increase in testosterone levels, whereas men high in narcissism became more risk averse via a decrease in
testosterone levels.

1. Introduction testosterone levels, a hormone that has long been associated with status
(Archer, 2006; Geniole, Bird, Ruddick, & Carré, 2017; Mazur & Booth,
Prior research has established that reminders of money can affect 1998; Zilioli & Bird, 2017). Testosterone levels have been shown to rise
motivation, thought, and behavior (Vohs, 2015). For instance, thoughts after engaging in conspicuous consumption (Saad & Vongas, 2009) and
of money decrease prosocial behavior (Gasiorowska, Chaplin, after winning (versus losing) in a variety of competitions including
Zaleskiewicz, Wygrab, & Vohs, 2016; Vohs, Mead, & Goode, 2006) and athletics (Booth, Shelley, Mazur, Tharp, & Kittok, 1989), video games
increase task performance (Boucher & Kofos, 2012; Gasiorowska et al., (Carré, Campbell, Lozoya, Goetz, & Welker, 2013; Zilioli & Watson,
2016). Handling money renders people relatively impervious to social 2012), and games of chance (Apicella, Dreber, & Mollerstrom, 2014;
exclusion and physical pain (Zhou, Vohs, & Baumeister, 2009). McCaul, Gladue, & Joppa, 1992; see Zilioli & Bird, 2017, for a review).
The current work sought to test potential physiological changes Given that money can serve as a means of achieving status, and that
associated with being reminded of money in order to potentially help testosterone levels are positively associated with social status, we ex-
account for behavioral findings. Via random assignment to condition, pect handling money will result in a rise in testosterone levels.
some participants handled a large amount of money, whereas others Testosterone's links to status dovetail with an individual difference
handled slips of paper. Our main goal was to assess the effects of in the desire for status, trait narcissism, which we measured as a way to
handling money on changes in testosterone levels and a decision assess the status implications of handling money. Narcissism is char-
making correlate of testosterone, risk-taking. acterized by the motivation to self-enhance and gain recognition and
According to the Tool Theory of money motivation, money's psy- admiration by identifying and optimizing self-presentation events in
chological effects derive from its utility to achieve goals such as status one's social environment (Pincus et al., 2009). In response to an ag-
ascension (Kniffin, 2006; Lea & Webley, 2006). Drawing from Tool gression solicitation paradigm, narcissism predicts a greater rise in
Theory, we posit that handling money will elicit an increase in testosterone levels and more aggressive behavior in the form of white


This research was supported by a Rike Consumer Research Foundation Research Grant from Wright State University. The first author acknowledges the support he received from his
Richard T. Farmer Endowed Assistant Professorship and research grants from the Farmer School of Business, the Department of Marketing, and Miami University.

Corresponding author at: Farmer School of Business, Miami University, 800 E. High St, Oxford, Ohio 45056, USA.
E-mail address: stenstep@miamioh.edu (E.P. Stenstrom).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.035
Received 12 July 2017; Received in revised form 25 October 2017; Accepted 26 October 2017
0191-8869/ © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
E.P. Stenstrom et al. Personality and Individual Differences 123 (2018) 110–114

noise blasts directed at an ostensible opponent of a reaction-time game 2. Method


(Lobbestael, Baumeister, Fiebig, & Eckel, 2014).
The literature seems to point to two ways in which narcissism might 2.1. Participants and procedure
impact how testosterone levels change in response to handling money.
On one hand, some research suggests that handling money could lead to One hundred and nine men participated in exchange for a $15
a greater testosterone increase among narcissists compared to non- Amazon gift card and a chance to win additional money via a decision
narcissistic people. Narcissists are focused on their social rank and seek making task. We followed standard salivary testosterone collection
opportunities to improve their rank through enhanced self-presentation procedures (as recommended by Schultheiss & Stanton, 2009, Zilioli &
(Marshall, Lefringhausen, & Ferenczi, 2015; Sorokowski et al., 2015; Watson, 2014, Blascovich, Vanman, Mendes, & Dickerson, 2011, and
Weiser, 2015) and devaluing others (Krizan & Bushman, 2011; Pincus the Salimetrics lab that analyzed our saliva samples). After excluding
et al., 2009). In romantic relationships, narcissists are more likely to two participants for reporting an oral disease (Zilioli & Watson, 2014),
pursue short-term mating strategies (Schmitt et al., 2017), to seek ad- two for an insufficient amount of saliva which prevented testosterone
miration rather than intimacy, and to target romantic partners whose assaying, and one for not correctly completing the risk-taking task (who
beauty or social rank can enhance their status (Campbell, 1999). Con- selected not one but both of the options, rendering it impossible to
sidering that narcissists are highly motivated by status concerns, one calculate a total score), the final sample consisted of 104 participants
might expect that handling large amounts of money will lead to a (Mage = 20.63, SDage = 2.39).
greater increase in status and testosterone levels among highly narcis- Data collection took place twice, at the end of the spring and fall
sistic men relative to those low in narcissism. 2015 semesters. During the fall session we added a third condition
On the other hand, some literature suggests that handling money wherein participants handled small amounts of money (40 $1 bills),
could result in a decrease in testosterone levels among highly narcis- which falls outside of the scope of the current manuscript and is re-
sistic men. Narcissists have a grandiose self-concept (Morf & ported in the Supplementary Materials (the procedure and results are
Rhodewalt, 2001) and tend to be overconfident (Campbell, Goodie, & reported in Supplements S2 and S3, respectively). It should be noted
Foster, 2004). However, their grandiose ego is coupled with a highly that we had intended to report the two data collections as two ex-
vulnerable and fragile self-concept, producing a constant search for periments. Given that they used the same procedures and tasks, we
external affirmation (Morf & Rhodewalt, 2001). Narcissists' quest for opted to report them as one study in order to conduct higher powered
status and admiration leads to a tendency to make frequent social analyses and gain more accurate estimates (Cumming, 2013).
comparisons (Krizan & Bushman, 2011). Compared to those low in Participants were instructed to refrain from eating, drinking (except
narcissism, highly narcissistic people respond negatively to upward for water), or using nicotine for one hour prior to the scheduled start of
social comparisons (Bogart, Benotsch, & Pavlovic, 2004; Nicholls & the experiment (Zilioli & Watson, 2014). Sessions were conducted be-
Stukas, 2011). Narcissists who were instructed to handle large amounts tween 11:50 am and 5:30 pm. Upon arrival, participants were asked by
of money might be inclined to mentally simulate what it would be like the experimenter when they had last eaten, drank, and consumed ni-
to possess that money and compare it to their current access to money cotine in order to confirm that they had followed the fasting instruc-
or wealth. This comparison may act like a threat and engender a de- tions.
crease in testosterone levels. Given this mixed literature, we took an Next, students were asked to watch 10 min of relaxing nature videos
exploratory approach and remained agnostic towards how narcissism to induce a calm state. Participants were then instructed by the ex-
might influence the effect of money on testosterone levels. perimenter to rinse their mouths with water three times before
We assessed a form of decision making associated with status, watching another 10 min of calming videos. They were subsequently
narcissism, and testosterone, namely risk-taking. Narcissism has been asked to passively drool approximately 1.0 ml of saliva into a poly-
shown to be predictive of greater risk-taking (Campbell et al., 2004) propylene vial using a Salimetrics saliva collection aid to measure pre-
and gambling behavior (Lakey, Rose, Campbell, & Goodie, 2008). There manipulation testosterone levels. Saliva samples were immediately
is also considerable evidence suggesting that testosterone levels are stored in a freezer at − 20 °C.
positively associated with risk-taking. Although some research has Following the saliva sampling, participants were randomly assigned
found no association between testosterone and risk-taking (Derntl, to a money condition (a sorting task with 80 $20 bills) or a neutral
Pintzinger, Kryspin-Exner, & Schöpf, 2014; Zethraeus et al., 2009) or a condition (sorting 80 plain pieces of paper with the same size dimen-
non-linear association between these two factors (Stanton, Mullette- sions as $20 bills; Zhou et al., 2009; Supplement S1) in a between-
Gillman, et al., 2011), many studies have revealed a positive relation- subjects design (see Supplement S4 for sorting task details).
ship between risk-taking and various indicators of testosterone, from Participants subsequently completed a financial risk-taking task
prenatal to circulating (Apicella et al., 2008; Apicella et al., 2014; with real monetary outcomes (Holt & Laury, 2002; Supplement S5).
Stanton, Liening, & Schultheiss, 2011; Stenstrom, Saad, Nepomuceno, & They were presented with 10 lotteries, each with a safer option and a
Mendenhall, 2011; van Honk et al., 2004). Based on this literature, we riskier option. The number of risky options chosen was our measure of
expected that testosterone changes elicited by money exposure would risk-taking (0 to 10).
likely be positively associated with risk-taking. Fifteen minutes after the sorting task, a second saliva sample was
An experiment tested how handling money, versus slips of paper (as taken (Mehta & Josephs, 2006). We then measured narcissism using a
a non-money cue), impacts testosterone levels and subsequent risk- reduced 28-item version of the Pathological Narcissism Inventory
taking, potentially as a function of trait narcissism. We assessed tes- (Pincus et al., 2009; Supplement S6). Responses were given on a 5-point
tosterone levels before and after a money exposure manipulation. scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree) and scores were
Testosterone levels in men are generally more responsive to situational averaged (α = 0.89). Next, we measured the number of alcoholic
factors than are women's (Mazur & Booth, 1998; see Geniole et al., drinks consumed within the last 12 h, the number of caffeinated bev-
2017, for a meta-analysis), and therefore we tested only men. Money erages consumed that day, how much time they exercised that day, and
exposure was manipulated by having participants either handle $1600, the number of nicotine products used that day (van Anders & Goldey,
a large amount of currency (money condition), or bill-sized slips of 2010; Zilioli & Watson, 2014). We also collected basic demographic
paper (neutral condition; Supplement S1). information. Last, participants were given their earnings from the risk-
taking task.
Saliva samples were packaged with dry ice and shipped overnight to
Salimetrics (Carlsbad, CA) for analysis. There, each sample was as-
sayed, in duplicate, using a sensitive enzyme immunoassay for

111
E.P. Stenstrom et al. Personality and Individual Differences 123 (2018) 110–114

Table 1 change, β = − 35.48, t = 3.66, p < 0.001, 95% CI [−54.70,


Descriptive statistics. − 16.26]). Among low narcissism participants (−1 SD; PNI = 2.44),
being assigned to the money condition produced a larger testosterone
Both conditions Neutral condition Money condition
(N = 104) (n = 51) (n = 53) increase from Time 1 to Time 2 compared with being assigned to the
neutral condition (Mmoney = 13.16, Mneutral = − 3.32), β = 16.48,
M SD M SD M SD t = 2.19, p = 0.031, 95% CI [1.54, 31.42]. For more narcissistic par-
ticipants (+1 SD; PNI = 3.54), being assigned to the money condition
Testosterone 176.08 65.31 177.86 60.83 174.36 69.89
Time 1 (pg/ led to a weaker increase in testosterone levels compared to being as-
ml) signed to the neutral condition (Mmoney = 2.76, Mneutral = 25.34),
Testosterone 185.95 70.13 189.50 65.24 182.54 75.00 β = −22.59, t = 3.01, p = 0.003, 95% CI [−37.50, −7.68]. Within
Time 2 (pg/ the neutral condition, there was a significant positive association be-
ml)
tween narcissism scores and testosterone change, β = 26.03, t = 3.84,
Testosterone 9.88 28.74 11.64 29.81 8.18 27.84
change (pg/ p < 0.001, 95% CI [12.39, 39.67], R2 = 0.23. There was no significant
ml) association between narcissism scores and testosterone change within
Narcissism (PNI) 2.99 0.55 3.01 0.55 2.97 0.56 the money condition, β = − 9.45, t = 1.37, p = 0.177, 95% CI
Risk-taking 5.08 1.98 5.57 1.71 4.60 2.12
[−23.30, 4.39].

testosterone. The average intra-assay coefficients of variation was


7.34% and the inter-assay coefficients of variation averaged across low 3.3. Risk preferences
and high controls was 5.48%.
We tested whether testosterone change, money condition, trait
narcissism, or the interaction of condition and trait narcissism predicted
3. Results risk scores using the PROCESS macro for model 8 (Hayes, 2013;
N = 104). There was not a significant effect of testosterone change on
3.1. Manipulation check risk preferences, although it's worth noting that there was a positive
trend, β = 0.01, t = 1.75, p = 0.083, 95% CI [0.00, 0.03], and that a
Total sorting time did not differ significantly between conditions significant direct effect of the mediator on the dependent variable is not
(nneutral = 51; Mneutral = 356.04 s, SDneutral = 83.60; nmoney = 53; required to establish mediation (Zhao, Lynch, & Chen, 2010, p. 204).
Mmoney = 370.42, SDmoney = 73.48), t = 0.93, p = 0.353, 95% CI There was not a significant association between narcissism scores and
[−44.96, 16.21]. The equivalency across conditions suggests that any risk preferences, β = − 0.23, t = 0.65, p = 0.514, 95% CI [−0.92,
differences in outcomes were not a function of task difficulty. 0.46], nor was there a significant interaction between condition and
narcissism, β = 1.30, t = 1.78, p = 0.077, 95% CI [− 0.14, 2.75].
3.2. Testosterone change There was a significant main effect of condition on risk scores, such that
those in the money condition were more risk aversive than those in the
Mean Time 1 (baseline) and Time 2 (post-manipulation) testos- neutral condition, β = − 0.93, t = 2.48, p = 0.015, 95% CI [−1.68,
terone levels were 176.08 pg/ml (SD = 65.31) and 185.95 pg/ml − 0.19], R2 = 0.06.
(SD = 70.13), respectively (see Table 1 for descriptive statistics). Time
1 testosterone levels were not significantly associated with narcissism
scores, r = 0.048, p = 0.734 (see Table 2 for intercorrelations among 3.4. Moderated mediation
variables). There were no significant differences in Time 1 testosterone
levels as a function of experimental condition, t = 0.27, p = 0.786, Prior to testing for moderated mediation, we tested two simple
95% CI [− 22.03, 29.02]. Likewise, narcissism scores did not sig- mediation models (an indirect effect of money on risk preferences
nificantly differ between conditions, t = 0.44, p = 0.664, 95% CI through testosterone changes, and an indirect effect of narcissism on
[−0.17, 0.26]. Testosterone change was calculated using standard risk scores via testosterone changes), neither of which yielded a sig-
practices, by subtracting each participant's Time 1 testosterone level nificant indirect effect (Supplement S7). We subsequently tested a
from their Time 2 level (Apicella et al., 2014; Zilioli & Watson, 2014). moderated mediation model in which trait narcissism (Mod) moderated
We tested if trait narcissism moderated an effect of money condition the effect of money condition (X) on testosterone change (Med), and in
on testosterone change using the PROCESS macro for model 1 (Hayes, which this interaction helped predict risk-taking scores (Y). The
2013; version 2.15; continuous predictors were mean centered prior to PROCESS macro for model 8 (Hayes, 2013; Krishna, 2016; N = 104)
all regression analyses; N = 104). There was not a significant effect of showed a significant moderated mediation pattern (index of moderated
condition on testosterone change, β = − 3.05, t = 0.58, p = 0.566, mediation = − 0.44, SE = 0.23, 95% CI [− 0.95, − 0.09]).
95% CI [− 13.58, 7.47]), nor was there a significant association be- Among low-narcissism participants (− 1 SD; PNI = 2.44), being in
tween narcissism scores and testosterone change, β = 7.95, t = 1.64, the money, versus neutral, condition evinced a significant positive ef-
p = 0.104, 95% CI [−1.66, 17.55]. However, there was a significant fect on risk-taking through an increase in testosterone levels (condi-
interaction between condition and narcissism scores on testosterone tional indirect effect = 0.20, SE = 0.13, 95% CI [0.02, 0.54]). That is,
for participants low in narcissism, their increased risk-taking from
Table 2 being assigned to the money, versus neutral, condition was partly ex-
Intercorrelations among variables. plained by a rise in testosterone levels.
Among highly narcissistic participants (+ 1 SD; PNI = 3.54), being
Variable 1 2 3 4 5
in the money, compared to the neutral condition, revealed a significant,
1. Testosterone Time 1 (pg/ml) 1 negative effect on risk-taking through a decrease in testosterone levels
2. Testosterone Time 2 (pg/ml) 0.912⁎ 1 (conditional indirect effect = − 0.28, SE = 0.16, 95% CI: −0.84,
3. Testosterone change (pg/ml) − 0.046 0.367⁎ 1
− 0.06). That is, among participants high in narcissism, a drop in tes-
4. Narcissism (PNI) − 0.146 −0.074 0.152 1
5. Risk-taking 0.138 0.179 0.123 −0.024 1 tosterone levels helped to explain a pattern of greater risk aversion
among participants in the money, compared to neutral, condition.

p < 0.001.

112
E.P. Stenstrom et al. Personality and Individual Differences 123 (2018) 110–114

4. Discussion potential future research direction might be to explore how other


hormones might respond to money primes and/or interact with tes-
The findings of an experiment suggest that exposure to money elicits tosterone responses to impact risk preferences. For instance, given the
changes in testosterone levels and risk-taking in men, and that the di- studies showing that cortisol interacts with testosterone to predict risk-
rection of these effects depends on trait narcissism. Among men low in taking (Danese, Fernandes, Watson, & Zilioli, 2017), future research
narcissism, handling money resulted in a greater increase in testos- examining testosterone responses to status cues might benefit from also
terone levels compared with handling paper. To our knowledge, this measuring cortisol.
testosterone increase is the first direct evidence of a physiological re- Our findings add to the growing bodies of work investigating the
sponse to money exposure. Furthermore, considering the established role of money cues (Tong, Zheng, & Zhao, 2013), narcissism (Hyun,
association between testosterone and status (Mazur & Booth, 1998), our Park, & Park, 2016), and physiology in influencing consumer behavior
finding that handling money elicited a rise in testosterone levels among (Giddens, Schermer, & Vernon, 2009; Nepomuceno, Saad, Stenstrom,
non-narcissists may lend support to the Tool Theory of money moti- Mendenhall, & Iglesias, 2016; Röder, Brewer, & Fink, 2009). Although
vation, which posits that responses to money are derived from evolved our research showed that money exposure results in a rise in testos-
instincts such as status seeking (Kniffin, 2006; Lea & Webley, 2006). terone levels and subsequent financial risk-taking among men low in
Testosterone and risk-taking responses to status-related cues may have narcissism, it is unclear if these effects have downstream personal or
been selected for by promoting fitness in ancestral environments. social implications. Given that testosterone levels are positively asso-
For more narcissistic men, handling money surprisingly led to a ciated with status motives (Archer, 2006; Zilioli & Bird, 2017), re-
significantly weaker increase in testosterone levels relative to handling minders of money may motivate non-narcissistic men to strive for status
paper. Since narcissists have a vulnerable and fragile self-concept (Morf by signaling their competence and generosity to others (Anderson &
& Rhodewalt, 2001), frequently make social comparisons (Krizan & Kilduff, 2009) or by engaging in conspicuous consumption (Sundie
Bushman, 2011), and respond negatively to upward comparisons et al., 2011). Furthermore, considering the evidence linking testos-
(Bogart et al., 2004; Nicholls & Stukas, 2011), handling large amounts terone to aggression (Carré & Archer, 2018), an increase in testosterone
of money might have led to aversive upward status-related compar- levels in response to money exposure might also lead to aggression or to
isons. To the extent that this may have occurred, a weak testosterone other nefarious behavior such as cheating, lying, or sabotaging rivals in
response to handling large amounts of money may have been the result. pursuit of rank ascension. Accordingly, future research could explore if
Moreover, the moderated mediation results suggest the effects of money exposure among non-narcissists promotes prosocial or antisocial
money exposure on testosterone levels have downstream effects on behavior geared at gaining status.
decision making. Handling money, relative to handling paper, led to
heightened risk-taking through increased testosterone levels among Appendix A. Supplementary data
men low in narcissism. Considering the positive relationship between
testosterone and status motives (Archer, 2006; Zilioli & Bird, 2017), the Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://
rise in testosterone levels in response to handling money among non- doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.10.035.
narcissistic men might have motivated them to take financial risks in
hopes of gaining status through monetary rewards. In contrast, among References
more narcissistic men, handling money resulted in less risk-taking via
reduced testosterone levels. While these findings are in line with re- van Anders, S. M., & Goldey, K. L. (2010). Testosterone and partnering are linked via
search showing a positive relationship between testosterone changes relationship status for women and ‘relationship orientation’ for men. Hormones and
Behavior, 58(5), 820–826.
and risk preferences (Apicella et al., 2014), they are the first to suggest Anderson, C., & Kilduff, G. J. (2009). The pursuit of status in social groups. Current
that money exposure can lead to shifts in risk-taking. Future research Directions in Psychological Science, 18(5), 295–298.
could investigate if some of the established effects of money reminders Apicella, C. L., Dreber, A., Campbell, B., Gray, P. B., Hoffman, M., & Little, A. C. (2008).
Testosterone and financial risk preferences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(6),
on motives, thoughts, and behavior might also be mediated by testos- 384–390.
terone changes. For instance, given the research linking testosterone to Apicella, C. L., Dreber, A., & Mollerstrom, J. (2014). Salivary testosterone change fol-
status motives (Archer, 2006; Zilioli & Bird, 2017), and that perfor- lowing monetary wins and losses predicts future financial risk-taking.
Psychoneuroendocrinology, 39, 58–64.
mance may serve as a means of gaining status, researchers could test if Archer, J. (2006). Testosterone and human aggression: An evaluation of the challenge
the effects of money cues on task persistence and performance (e.g., hypothesis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(3), 319–345.
Gasiorowska et al., 2016) could be partly explained by shifts in tes- Blascovich, J., Vanman, E., Mendes, W. B., & Dickerson, S. (2011). Social psychophysiology
for social and personality psychology. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications.
tosterone levels.
Bogart, L. M., Benotsch, E. G., & Pavlovic, J. D. P. (2004). Feeling superior but threatened:
Limitations of the current work suggest additional avenues for fu- The relation of narcissism to social comparison. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,
ture research. Chief among these are the unexpectedly strong increase 26(1), 35–44.
in testosterone levels among more narcissistic men after handling slips Booth, A., Shelley, G., Mazur, A., Tharp, G., & Kittok, R. (1989). Testosterone, and
winning and losing in human competition. Hormones and Behavior, 23(4), 556–571.
of paper. We find this difficult to understand, but perhaps features of Boucher, H. C., & Kofos, M. N. (2012). The idea of money counteracts ego depletion
the experiment, from the vaguely competitive nature of the sorting task effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(4), 804–810.
to the inclusion of a lottery in the risk-taking task, may have unin- Campbell, W. K. (1999). Narcissism and romantic attraction. Journal of Personality and
Social Psychology, 77(6), 1254.
tentionally put them into a competitive mindset or led them to envision Campbell, W. K., Goodie, A. S., & Foster, J. D. (2004). Narcissism, confidence, and risk
winning, both of which have been shown to increase testosterone levels attitude. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17(4), 297–311.
(Archer, 2006; Zilioli & Bird, 2017). Another note is that all the ex- Carré, J. M., & Archer, J. (2018). Testosterone and human behavior: The role of in-
dividual and contextual variables. Current Opinion in Psychology, 19(1), 149–153.
perimenters in the current work were young women. Cross-sex inter- Carré, J. M., Campbell, J. A., Lozoya, E., Goetz, S. M., & Welker, K. M. (2013). Changes in
actions can increase testosterone levels (e.g., van der Meij, Buunk, van testosterone mediate the effect of winning on subsequent aggressive behaviour.
de Sande, & Salvador, 2008). However, once again, why these processes Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38(10), 2034–2041.
Cumming, G. (2013). The new statistics why and how. Psychological Science, 25(1), 7–29.
would occur only with more narcissistic men who frankly performed a Danese, G., Fernandes, E., Watson, N. V., & Zilioli, S. (2017). Testosterone and cortisol
fairly natural task, we are not certain. Future studies may investigate jointly predict the ambiguity premium in an Ellsberg-urns experiment. Frontiers in
how features of the experiment might influence the effects of money on Behavioral Neuroscience, 11, 1–9.
Derntl, B., Pintzinger, N., Kryspin-Exner, I., & Schöpf, V. (2014). The impact of sex
testosterone levels among narcissistic men. Additionally, future re-
hormone concentrations on decision-making in females and males. Frontiers in
search on whether money exposure elicits upward wealth comparisons Neuroscience, 8, 1–11.
could help explain why narcissists responded to handling money with a Gasiorowska, A., Chaplin, L. N., Zaleskiewicz, T., Wygrab, S., & Vohs, K. D. (2016).
weaker testosterone increase compared to handling paper. Another Money cues increase agency and decrease prosociality among children: Early signs of

113
E.P. Stenstrom et al. Personality and Individual Differences 123 (2018) 110–114

market-mode behaviors. Psychological Science, 27(3), 331–344. Röder, S., Brewer, G., & Fink, B. (2009). Menstrual cycle shifts in women's self-perception
Geniole, S. N., Bird, B. M., Ruddick, E. L., & Carré, J. M. (2017). Effects of competition and motivation: A daily report method. Personality and Individual Differences, 47(6),
outcome on testosterone concentrations in humans: An updated meta-analysis. 616–619.
Hormones and Behavior, 92, 37–50. Saad, G., & Vongas, J. G. (2009). The effect of conspicuous consumption on men's tes-
Giddens, J. L., Schermer, J. A., & Vernon, P. A. (2009). Material values are largely in the tosterone levels. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 110(2), 80–92.
family: A twin study of genetic and environmental contributions to materialism. Schmitt, D. P., Alcalay, L., Alves, I. C. B., Anderson, C. A., Angelini, A. L., Asendorpf, J. B.,
Personality and Individual Differences, 46(4), 428–431. ... Zupančič, A. (2017). Narcissism and the strategic pursuit of short-term mating:
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: Universal links across 11 world regions of the international sexuality description
A regression based approach. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. project-2. Psychological Topics, 26(1), 89–137.
Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Schultheiss, O. C., & Stanton, S. J. (2009). Assessment of salivary hormones. In E.
Review, 92(5), 1644–1655. Harmon-Jones, & J. S. Beer (Eds.). Methods in social neuroscience (pp. 17–44). New
van Honk, J., Schutter, D. J., Hermans, E. J., Putman, P., Tuiten, A., & Koppeschaar, H. York, NY: The Guilford Press.
(2004). Testosterone shifts the balance between sensitivity for punishment and re- Sorokowski, P., Sorokowska, A., Oleszkiewicz, A., Frackowiak, T., Huk, A., & Pisanski, K.
ward in healthy young women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 29(7), 937–943. (2015). Selfie posting behaviors are associated with narcissism among men.
Hyun, N. K., Park, Y., & Park, S. W. (2016). Narcissism and gift giving: Not every gift is for Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 123–127.
others. Personality and Individual Differences, 96, 47–51. Stanton, S. J., Liening, S. H., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2011). Testosterone is positively as-
Kniffin, K. M. (2006). Show me the status: Money as a kind of currency. Behavioral and sociated with risk taking in the Iowa gambling task. Hormones and Behavior, 59(2),
Brain Sciences, 29(2), 188–189. 252–256.
Krishna, A. (2016). A clearer spotlight on spotlight: Understanding, conducting and re- Stanton, S. J., Mullette-Gillman, O. D. A., McLaurin, R. E., Kuhn, C. M., LaBar, K. S., Platt,
porting. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 26(3), 315–324. M. L., & Huettel, S. A. (2011). Low-and high-testosterone individuals exhibit de-
Krizan, Z., & Bushman, B. J. (2011). Better than my loved ones: Social comparison ten- creased aversion to economic risk. Psychological Science, 22(4), 447–453.
dencies among narcissists. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(2), 212–216. Stenstrom, E., Saad, G., Nepomuceno, M. V., & Mendenhall, Z. (2011). Testosterone and
Lakey, C. E., Rose, P., Campbell, W. K., & Goodie, A. S. (2008). Probing the link between domain-specific risk: Digit ratios (2D: 4D and rel2) as predictors of recreational, fi-
narcissism and gambling: The mediating role of judgment and decision-making nancial, and social risk-taking behaviors. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4),
biases. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 21(2), 113–137. 412–416.
Lea, S. E., & Webley, P. (2006). Money as tool, money as drug: The biological psychology Sundie, J. M., Kenrick, D. T., Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Vohs, K. D., & Beal, D. J.
of a strong incentive. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(02), 161–209. (2011). Peacocks, Porsches, and Thorstein Veblen: Conspicuous consumption as a
Lobbestael, J., Baumeister, R. F., Fiebig, T., & Eckel, L. A. (2014). The role of grandiose sexual signaling system. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 664.
and vulnerable narcissism in self-reported and laboratory aggression and testosterone Tong, L., Zheng, Y., & Zhao, P. (2013). Is money really the root of all evil? The impact of
reactivity. Personality and Individual Differences, 69, 22–27. priming money on consumer choice. Marketing Letters, 24(2), 119–129.
Marshall, T. C., Lefringhausen, K., & Ferenczi, N. (2015). The big five, self-esteem, and Vohs, K. D. (2015). Money priming can change people's thoughts, feelings, motivations,
narcissism as predictors of the topics people write about in Facebook status updates. and behaviors: An update on 10 years of experiments. Journal of Experimental
Personality and Individual Differences, 85, 35–40. Psychology: General, 144(4), e86.
Mazur, A., & Booth, A. (1998). Testosterone and dominance in men. Behavioral and Brain Vohs, K. D., Mead, N. L., & Goode, M. R. (2006). The psychological consequences of
Sciences, 21(03), 353–363. money. Science, 314(5802), 1154–1156.
McCaul, K. D., Gladue, B. A., & Joppa, M. (1992). Winning, losing, mood, and testos- Weiser, E. B. (2015). # Me: Narcissism and its facets as predictors of selfie-posting fre-
terone. Hormones and Behavior, 26(4), 486–504. quency. Personality and Individual Differences, 86, 477–481.
Mehta, P., & Josephs, R. (2006). Testosterone change after losing predicts the decision to Zethraeus, N., Kocoska-Maras, L., Ellingsen, T., Von Schoultz, B. O., Hirschberg, A. L., &
compete again. Hormones and Behavior, 50(5), 684–692. Johannesson, M. (2009). A randomized trial of the effect of estrogen and testosterone
van der Meij, L., Buunk, A. P., van de Sande, J. P., & Salvador, A. (2008). The presence of on economic behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(16),
a woman increases testosterone in aggressive dominant men. Hormones and Behavior, 6535–6538.
54(5), 640–644. Zhao, X., Lynch, J. G., & Chen, Q. (2010). Reconsidering Baron and Kenny: Myths and
Morf, C. C., & Rhodewalt, F. (2001). Unraveling the paradoxes of narcissism: A dynamic truths about mediation analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(2), 197–206.
self-regulatory processing model. Psychological Inquiry, 12(4), 177–196. Zhou, X., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). The symbolic power of money re-
Nepomuceno, M. V., Saad, G., Stenstrom, E., Mendenhall, Z., & Iglesias, F. (2016). minders of money alter social distress and physical pain. Psychological Science, 20(6),
Testosterone & gift-giving: Mating confidence moderates the association between 700–706.
digit ratios (2D: 4D and rel2) and erotic gift-giving. Personality and Individual Zilioli, S., & Bird, B. M. (2017). Functional significance of men's testosterone reactivity to
Differences, 91, 27–30. social stimuli. Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology, 47, 1–18.
Nicholls, E., & Stukas, A. A. (2011). Narcissism and the self-evaluation maintenance Zilioli, S., & Watson, N. V. (2012). The hidden dimensions of the competition effect: Basal
model: Effects of social comparison threats on relationship closeness. The Journal of cortisol and basal testosterone jointly predict changes in salivary testosterone after
Social Psychology, 151(2), 201–212. social victory in men. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 37(11), 1855–1865.
Pincus, A. L., Ansell, E. B., Pimentel, C. A., Cain, N. M., Wright, A. G., & Levy, K. N. Zilioli, S., & Watson, N. V. (2014). Testosterone across successive competitions: Evidence
(2009). Initial construction and validation of the pathological narcissism inventory. for a ‘winner effect’ in humans? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 47, 1–9.
Psychological Assessment, 21(3), 365–379.

114

Potrebbero piacerti anche