Sei sulla pagina 1di 17

Journal of Counseling Psychology Copyright 1996 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1996, Vol. 43, No. 4, 510-526 0022-0167/96/$3.00

A Taxonomy of Difficulties in Career Decision Making

I t a m a r Gaff and M i n a K r a u s z S a m u e l H. O s i p o w
Hebrew University of Jerusalem Ohio State University

A general theoretical taxonomy of career decision-making difficulties, based on decision


theory, has been developed. To examine the proposed taxonomy empirically, a questionnaire
was constructed in which the various possible difficulties in the theoretical model were
represented by respective statements. The questionnaire was administered to a sample of 259
young Israeli adults who were at the beginning of their career decision-making process and
to an American sample of 304 university students. The observed relations among the 10
scales, which represent the 10 theoretical categories of difficulties, and those among the items
within 2 selected categories, were similar in the 2 samples and compatible with the expected
relations derived from the theoretical model. The implications for career counseling and
research are discussed.

The growing rate of change in the world of work in- Fouad, 1994; Meier, 1991; Tinsley, 1992). Indeed, research
creases the number of career transitions individuals make on career development and choice has devoted much atten-
during their lifetime. The quality of the career decisions tion to the categorization of various problems related to
made during these transitions is significant for both the indecision. However, some of these discussions have been
individual and society. Making a career decision is, how- purely theoretical, without any empirical testing (e.g.,
ever, a complex process. Although some people make such Campbell & Cellini, 1981; Miller, 1971), whereas others
decisions easily, at least apparently, others face difficulties have had mainly an empirical focus in attempting to develop
in making their career decisions, and many seek profes- various measures of career indecision. Furthermore, it ap-
sional help. One of the central aims of career counseling is pears that these two lines of research, the theoretical and the
to facilitate the career decision-making process of counsel- empirical, have been conducted independently of one an-
ees and, in particular, to help them overcome the difficulties other and by different groups of investigators.
they encounter during this process. Therefore, identifying In his introduction to the special issue on indecision in the
the unique difficulties that prevent individuals from reach- Journal of Vocational Behavior, Tinsley (1992, p. 211)
ing a decision is an essential step in providing them with the suggested that "efforts to relate research on career indeci-
help they need. sion to the decision making process and to develop a theo-
The construct of career indecision has been used to refer retical context for the construct of indecision will lead to
to the problems individuals may have in making their career important advances in our research on these constructs."
decision (for a review, see Slaney, 1988). Because of its The present article attempts to meet this challenge. Specif-
recognized significance, career indecision is one of the ically, the goal of this article is to present a new theoretical
central research issues of career psychology (Betz, 1992; model of difficulties in career decision making based on an
adaptation of decision theory to the context of career deci-
sions and to report an empirical test of this model in two
Itamar Gati and Mina Krausz, Department of Psychology, He-
different samples.
brew University of Jerusalem, Jerusalem, Israel; Samuel H. Osi-
pow, Department of Psychology, Ohio State University.
This research was supported by Grant 94-139 from the United
States-Israel Binational Science Foundation and carded out at the Career Indecision
National Council of Jewish Women Research Institute for Inno-
vation in Education of the Hebrew University. We thank Naomi Various theoretical approaches have been used to deal
Fassa for valuable discussions; Nitza Shitz, Leah Hass, Tzachi with career indecision, each emphasizing a different aspect.
Ashkenazi, Hadas Benari, Mary Hill, and Hadassa Keren for their For example, the psychodynamic approach (e.g., Bordin &
help in data collection and analyses; and Daniela Ben-Dor, Naomi Kopplin, 1973) tried to classify the individual's problems
Goldblum, Liat Kibari, Aluma Reiss, and Orit Trumper for their according to their internal sources instead of their observed
comments on an earlier version of this article. symptoms. The developmental approach (e.g., Osipow &
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Fitzgerald, 1996; Super, 1953) suggested that the typical
Itamar Gati, Department of Psychology, Hebrew University,
problems faced during the decision process correspond to
Jerusalem, Israel 91905, or to Samuel H. Osipow, Department of
Psychology, Ohio State University, 1885 Neil Avenue Mall, Co- the normal stages of career development, highlighting in
lumbus, Ohio 43210. Electronic mail may be sent via Interuet to particular the concept of vocational maturity (Crites, 1978).
Itamar Gati at msgati@pluto.mscc.huji.ac.il or to Samuel H. Osi- The vocational interests approach (e.g., Holland, 1985; Roe,
pow at sosipow@magnus.acs.ohio-state.edu. 1956; Salomone, 1982) proposed that insufficient crystalli-

510
DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 511

zation of interests is among the major factors preventing the of alternatives to select from, and there are many attributes
individual from reaching a decision. Because each of these or aspects that are considered in the comparison and eval-
theoretical approaches (as well as some others) has focused uation of the various alternatives. In addition to these char-
on a single major facet of the process of career decision acteristics, which are common to many decisions, career
making, their categorizations are limited to the particular decisions also have certain unique features. First, the num-
facet adopted and hence are neither comprehensive nor ber of potential alternatives is often fairly large (e.g., the
inclusive (Rounds & Tinsley, 1984). Furthermore, in many number of occupations, colleges, majors, or potential em-
cases, the empirical tests of these theoretical approaches ployers). Second, there is an extensive amount of informa-
have supported them only partially. tion available on each alternative. Third, a large number of
The empirical research on career indecision has focused aspects (e.g., length of training, degree of independence,
on developing various measures for examining individual type of relationship with people) is required to adequately
differences in career indecision. These measures include the characterize the occupations and the individual's prefer-
Career Decision Scale (CDS; Osipow, Carney, & Barak, ences in a detailed and meaningful way (e.g., Gaff, Garty, &
1976; Osipow, Carney, Winer, Yanico, & Koschier, 1987; Fassa, 1996; Gati, Osipow, & Givon, 1995; Katz, 1993;
Osipow &Winer, 1996); the My Vocational Situation Scale Lofquist & Dawis, 1978). Fourth, uncertainty plays a major
(MVS; Holland, Daiger, & Power, 1980); the Vocational role with respect to both the individual's characteristics
Decision Scale (VDS; Jones & Chenery, 1980); the Career (e.g., present and future preferences) and the nature of
Decision Profile (CDP, a revision of the VDS; Jones, 1989); future career alternatives (Gati, 1990; Gelatt, 1989).
the Behavioral Indecision Scale (BIS; Fuqua & Hartman,
1983); the Career Decision Making Self-Efficacy Scale
(CDMSE; Taylor & Betz, 1983); the Career Decision Di- Proposed T a x o n o m y
agnostic Assessment (CDDA; Bansberg & Sklare, 1986;
Larson, Busby, Wilson, Medora, & Allgood, 1994); the Assumptions Underlying the Proposed Taxonomy
Career Factors Inventory (CFI; Chartrand, Robbins, Morrill,
& Boggs, 1990); the Career Barriers Inventory (Swanson & Relying on decision theory, we first developed a model of
Tokar, 1991); and the Career Belief Inventory (CBI; Krum- an "ideal career decision maker." This term refers to a
boltz, 1991, 1994). Most of the research involving these person who is aware of the need to make a career decision,
measures has been carried out independently of theoretical is willing to make it, and is capable of making the "right"
conceptualizations (Tinsley, 1992). decision (i.e., a decision using an appropriate process and
The state of affairs reflected in this concise review of the most compatible with the individual's goals). The complex-
theoretical and empirical research has led us to the conclu- ity of the process of career decision making, as noted
sion that a new framework, which combines a theoretical earlier, makes it difficult for most people to be ideal career
analysis and empirical tests, may provide a unique and decision makers. We define any deviation from the ideal
significant contribution to research on career indecision. career decision maker as a potential problem that may affect
the individual's decision process in one of two possible
ways: (a) by preventing the individual from making a de-
Decision Theory and Career Decision M a k i n g cision or (b) by leading to a less than optimal decision.
The process of career decision making can be separated
Decision theory, on which the proposed taxonomy of into distinct components (e.g., Brown, 1990; Gati, Fassa, &
difficulties relies, has been playing an increasingly impor- Houminer, 1995; Katz, 1966; Pitz & Harren, 1980), each of
tant role in understanding the processes involved in career which presumably involves different kinds of difficulties.
decision making (e.g., Brown, 1990; Gati, 1986; Jepsen & Therefore, the various possible difficulties the individual
Dilley, 1974; Katz, 1966; Mitchell & Krumboltz, 1984; may face during the process of career decision making can
Neimeyer, 1988; Osipow, 1987; Osipow & Fitzgerald, be classified into distinct categories such that difficulties
1996; Phillips, 1994; Pitz & Harren, 1980; Walsh & Osi- with common features are included in the same category
pow, 1988). According to the normative theory of decision (Campbell & Cellini, 1981). Specifically, the classification
making, the best decision is the one that best helps to of the difficulties into categories was based on the following
achieve the decision maker's goals. These goals are repre- criteria: (a) belonging to the same stage or component of the
sented by the individual's preferences with respect to the process of career decision making; (b) having the same
various attributes of the alternatives under consideration. A assumed source; (c) having similarity in the hypothesized
rational career decision maker should choose the alternative possible impact of the difficulty (i.e., halting the process or
with the highest utility, where the utility of each alternative leading to a less than optimal decision); and (d) having
is a function of the perceived gap between the individual's similarity in the type of intervention needed to overcome it.
preferences and the alternative's characteristics in each of Furthermore, we assume that the individual may have a
these attributes. Utility theory is a normative model that single difficulty or a combination of them (Campbell &
may be regarded as a prescription for the best method for Heffernan, 1983) and that these difficulties may be located
making decisions (Baron, 1988; Brown, 1990). in one or several categories. However, we expect that the
Career decisions have the following features: There is an difficulties within each category will co-occur more often
individual who has to make a decision, there are a number than those from different categories. On the other hand,
512 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

problems from different categories can also be associated in specific categories of the proposed taxonomy are summa-
certain cases because, as Campbell and Heffernan (1983) rized in Figure 1.
pointed out, problem categories cannot and should not be
completely independent of each other.
The 10 Difficulty Categories
Features of the Proposed Taxonomy
Within the major category of Lack of Readiness, a dis-
The proposed taxonomy was developed through interac- tinction was made between two pairs of categories. One pair
tion and sequential interplay between theoretical consider- includes difficulties that are related to lack of motivation to
ations and empirical testing. Specifically, to ensure the engage in the career decision process and general indeci-
comprehensiveness of the proposed model, as well as its siveness concerning all types of decision making. The other
relevance for real-life contexts, we elicited descriptions of pair includes difficulties related to dysfunctional myths
career decision difficulties from 200 career counselees and (e.g., irrational expectations) about the process of career
10 expert career counseling psychologists. We compared decision making and lack of knowledge about the steps
these lists with the theoretical model to ensure that it in- involved in this process.
cluded all important and relevant difficulties and introduced
Within the major category of Lack of Information, a
a few changes in the set of difficulties included in the
distinction was made among three categories: lack of infor-
categories.
The proposed taxonomy itself is hierarchic (Fleishman &
mation about self, lack of information about occupations,
Quaintance, 1984), in which broad categories of difficulties and lack of information about ways of obtaining additional
are separated into categories and then subcategories based information. We assume that, because the last two catego-
on finer distinctions. Accordingly, during the diagnostic ries refer to external, objective information, they are more
process each difficulty of an individual can be classified as closely related to each other than to the first one (which
to its major category and then into finer categories and involves the individual).
subcategories (Campbell & Heffernan, 1983). Specifically, Within the major category of Inconsistent Information, a
in the proposed taxonomy the difficulties are divided into distinction was made among three categories: unreliable
three major categories. The first, Lack of Readiness, in- information, which includes difficulties related to unreliable
cludes four categories of difficulties that precede the en- or fuzzy information; internal conflicts, which include con-
gagement in making a specific career decision. Each of the flicts within the individual; and external conflicts, which
other two major categories, Lack of Information and Incon- include conflicts involving the influence of significant oth-
sistent Information, includes three categories of difficulties ers. Here too, we assume that the last two categories of
that arise during the actual process of career decision mak- conflict are more closely related to each other than to the
ing. The three major categories of difficulties and the 10 first.

CAREER DECISION HAKIN6 DIFFICULTIES

1
PRIOR TO BEGINNING THE PROCESS DURING THE PROCESS
I I I

J I
LACK OF LACK OF INCONSISTENT
READZNESS INFORMATION INFORMATION
DUE TO ABOUT DUE TO

r I

LACK OF INDECI- DYSFUNCTIONAL LACK OF SELF 0CCUPATZONS WAYS OF UNRELZABLE INTERNAL EXTERNAL
MOTIVATION SIVENESS MYTHS KNOWLEDGE OBTAINING INFORMATZON CONFLICTS CONFLICTS
ABOUT PROCESS INFOR/4ATZON
(STEPS)

Figure 1. The initial theoretical taxonomy of career decision-making difficulties.


DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 5 13

Specific Difficulties Within Categories theory are represented explicitly in the proposed taxonomy.
These distinctions are (a) between alternatives (i.e., careers)
The elaborated theoretical model, which includes 44 spe- and the characteristics (i.e., aspects) that can be used to
cific difficulties representing the various categories (and compare and evaluate them; (b) between specific missing
subcategories), is presented in the Appendix. Each of these information and the possible ways of obtaining that infor-
44 specific difficulties represents a distinct type of problem, mation; (c) between preferences and capabilities; and (d)
where these distinctions are based on both theoretical con- between lack of information about the present and lack of
siderations and apparent practical significance. It is impor- information about the future (explicitly representing the
tant to note that the degree of differentiation is not equal uncertainty involved).
across the 10 categories. Rather, the six categories associ-
ated with the difficulties that arise during the engagement in
the process were refined by a systematic division into sub- Implementation Issues
categories, whereas the four categories included in the ma-
We did not include in the proposed taxonomy problems
jor category of Lack of Readiness were not (for lack of
related to the implementation of the decision made because
theoretical rationale or practical need).
we believe that such problems typically have antecedents in
Specifically, within the category of lack of information
the process of career decision making. Thus, lack of imple-
about self, a distinction was made between lack of infor-
mentation is assumed to stem from the fact that the process
mation regarding preferences ("What do I want?") and lack
by which the decision was reached was problematic, and
of information regarding the individual's perceived capabil-
certain difficulties were not dealt with adequately. For ex-
ities ("What can I do?"). Each of these two subcategories
ample, lack of means to realize a plan may stem from the
was further divided into difficulties associated with lack of
fact that the issue of financial resources to implement the
information at present and difficulties associated with un-
decision was not dealt with adequately; lack of information
certainty about the future. Similarly, within the category of
about how to realize a preferred alternative may stem from
lack of information about occupations, a distinction was
lack of information about that alternative or additional
made between lack of information about the very existence
sources of information; delaying the implementation of a
of occupational alternatives and lack of information about
preferred alternative may stem from lack of confidence in
the characteristics of the alternatives. Each of these two
the process of career decision making, which, in turn, may
subcategories was further divided into lack of information at
be associated with lack of knowledge about the steps and
present and lack of information about the future. Within the
characteristics of this process.
category of lack of information about ways of obtaining
additional information, a distinction was made between
ways of obtaining information about the self (e.g., career Goals o f This Research
counseling services) and ways of obtaining information
about the occupations (e.g., career information centers). In this research we tested the hypothesis that the empirical
Within the category of unreliable information, a distinc- structure of career decision difficulties conforms to the
tion was made among three subcategories, according to the theoretical model. However, because of the complexity of
different kinds of unreliable information. Such unreliability the proposed model and its multilevel nature, we decided to
may be related to (a) the individual's preferences, (b) his or separate the test into two parts. First, we examined whether
her perceived capabilities, or (c) the occupational alterna- the empirical relations among the 10 categories conform to
fives regarded as relevant. Within the category of internal their theoretical structure. That is, we examined whether the
conflicts, a distinction was made between conflicts associ- 10 categories of difficulties are divided into the hypothe-
ated with incompatible preferences and those arising from sized three major categories (Lack of Readiness, Lack of
the gap between preferences and capabilities. The first sub- Information, and Inconsistent Information) and whether the
category was further divided into conflicts among preferred structure within each major category conforms to the ex-
aspects, conflicts among occupations, and conflicts between pected pattern of relations among them. Second, we exam-
an occupation and a desirable aspect (e.g., between the ined whether the structures of difficulties within two of the
desire to be a nursery school teacher and to have a very high categories--lack of information about the self and incon-
income). The second subcategory was further divided into sistent information--conform to their hypothesized internal
insufficient abilities and abilities higher than required. Fi- structure. These two categories were selected for three rea-
nally, within the category of external conflicts, a distinction sons: (a) We believe that dealing with difficulties in these
was made between conflicts among external sources (e.g., two categories is significant for the career counseling pro-
between two significant others) and conflicts between the cess; (b) their internal structure is systematic and well
individual and an external source. Each of these subcatego- defined; (c) the number of difficulties included in each of
ries of conflicts was further divided into conflicts expressed these categories (8 and 6, respectively) makes such a test
in terms of relevant aspects (i.e., what criteria or factors to possible (when only 3 difficulties are included in a category,
take into consideration in the decision) or in terms of its internal structure cannot vary much).
alternatives (i.e., what careers the individual should To conduct these tests, we developed a questionnaire that
choose). includes 44 statements corresponding to the 44 difficulties
Note that important distinctions that underlie decision in career decision making included in the theoretical model
514 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

and defined 10 scales corresponding to the 10 categories. whole questionnaire. Some of the items were revised on the basis
This questionnaire was administered in two different cross- of these analyses.
cultural contexts: an Israeli sample o f y o u n g adults w h o Using the revised questionnaire, we collected additional data
w e r e at the b e g i n n i n g o f their process of career decision from another group of approximately 200 young adults whose
characteristics were similar to those of the first group. We then
m a k i n g and an A m e r i c a n sample o f university students. The
analyzed their responses to the revised questionnaire in the same
use o f two culturally distinct samples enabled us to e x a m i n e
manner and made the necessary revisions in the questionnaire.
the generality and stability o f the p r o p o s e d t a x o n o m y . A f t e r This process was repeated four times (with different versions of
a discussion o f the results o f these two samples, the impli- the questionnaire and different groups of about 200 young adults)
cations for theory, research, and the career counseling prac- until the psychometric properties of the questionnaire were found
tice are explored. satisfactory and its internal structure had been consolidated.

Method Last R o u n d o f Data Collection


The questionnaire used in this study was developed especially Participants. Two different cross-cultural samples participated
for the purpose of testing the proposed taxonomy. Hence, before in the research: an Israeli sample and an American sample. The
using it to test the theoretical taxonomy, we had to carry out a Israeli sample consisted of 147 men and 112 women, ages 19 to 23
number of steps to ensure that it adequately represents the model years (Mdn and mode = 21, M = 20.68, SD = 0.74), who were
and that its psychometric properties are satisfactory. We begin by nearing the end of their military service. Ninety-eight percent of
briefly describing the development of the questionnaire, then turn the participants had 12 years of schooling. The questionnaires of
to our main goal of testing the model itself. The initial construc- two older participants (25 and 28 years) were not included in the
tions of the questionnaire as well as its pretests were carried out in analyses. The American sample included 304 students (186 men
Israel (in Hebrew), and only the last version was administered in and 105 women; 13 did not indicate gender) at a large midwestern
both Israel and the United States (after translation into English). university in the United States. The age range was 17 to 23 years
(Mdn and mode = 18, M = 18.70, SD = 1.10). Most participants
(74%) were freshmen, 18% were sophomores, and 6% were jun-
D e v e l o p m e n t o f the Career Decision Difficulties iors. Within the population from which the sample was drawn, the
Questionnaire ( C D D Q ) majority are Caucasian (71%); other ethnic groups include Asian
Americans (15%), African Americans (6%), Hispanic Americans
Initial construction of the questionnaire. On the basis of the (1%), Native Americans (1%), and others (those of mixed race and
theoretical taxonomy, we constructed a set of items, each of which non-Americans; 5%).
corresponds to one of the 44 difficulties in the theoretical model. CDDQ. The first page of the questionnaire included general
We decided to use only one representative item for each difficulty background information: age, sex, years of education, present job,
in order to prevent cognitive overload and motivational decrease the degree to which the participant is considering a career decision
(due to an overly long questionnaire). Then, to test the correspon- (very, little, not at all), whether the participant has some career
dence between the items in the questionnaire and the difficulties in alternative in mind, and his or her degree of confidence in that
the theoretical model, we asked five experienced career counseling alternative (on a 9-point scale). The following pages included 44
psychologists (3 MA and 2 PhD) to match each of the items in the items, each one corresponding to a particular difficulty. The par-
questionnaire with one particular difficulty from those included in ticipants were asked to rate (on a 9-point scale) the degree to which
the theoretical model. Mismatched items were revised, reworded, the difficulty represented by each item (e.g., "It is usually difficult
or replaced. Next, on the basis of the proposed 10 categories of for me to make a decision") described them (from 1 = does not
difficulties, we defined 10 scales, each of which is based on the describe me to 9 = describes me well). Finally, the participants
particular set of items representing the set of difficulties included were asked to rate the overall severity of their difficulties in
in that category. making a career decision (from 1 = not severe at all to 9 = very
Revisions of the questionnaire on the basis of empirical data. severe) and to list additional difficulties preventing them from
We asked 200 young adults, ages 20 to 22 years, who were at the making a career decision.
end of their obligatory military service and thus about to make Because the research questionnaire was developed in Israel (in
their first major career decision, to complete the questionnaire. Hebrew), it had to be translated into English for the American
Their responses were analyzed to ensure that the psychometric sample. The questionnaire was first translated in Israel by a native
properties of the questionnaire were satisfactory. Specifically, we Hebrew speaker and then examined by three other native Hebrew
first computed the mean and standard deviation of the responses speakers; all four are bilingual. Next, the translation was examined
for each item to ensure differentiation. Second, we computed the by a native English speaker who lives in Israel and after 20 years
score of each scale, where this score was defined as the mean of may be regarded as bilingual as well. Finally, the English version
the responses to the items included in that scale. Then we com- of the questionnaire was edited in the United States by a native
puted the item-scale correlation (excluding the item itself) and the English speaker.
item-other scales correlations (i.e., the correlations between the Procedure. The Israeli participants responded to the question-
item and the scores of the scales to which the item does not naire at the beginning of a 3-day workshop called "Orientation to
belong). Problematic items were located using the criteria pro- Civilian Life," organized by the Israeli Veterans Administration.
posed by Meir and Gati (1981). Third, we carried out a cluster In responding to the questionnaire, participants gave their consent
analysis of the intercorrelation matrix among the responses to the to use their responses in the research. Fewer than 5% did not return
44 items. Fourth, we assessed the scale reliabilities (Cronbach's the questionnaire. To allow us to assess test-retest reliability, we
alpha), as well as the reliability of the complete questionnaire. asked the participants to complete the questionnaire again on the
Finally, using retest data from the same group of participants, we third day of the workshop. Because at the end of the workshop the
examined the test-retest reliability of the items, the scales, and the soldiers would return to their original unit and could not be
DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 515

reached, we had only this short interval for the test-retest esti- of its applications to a variety of datasets, see Tversky and
mates. After responding the second time (n = 164), the partici- Hutchinson (1986).
pants were given an orientation lecture geared toward making
career decisions and were given an explanation for being asked to
fill in the questionnaire twice. The American participants re- Results
sponded to the questionnaire in groups of 15 to 25 students, as part
of the course requirement in introductory psychology. All partic- Because there were no systematic or meaningful differ-
ipants returned their questionnaires. ences between men and women in the scale scores, and the
Analyses. First, the psychometric properties of the question- pattern of correlations among the scales was highly similar
naires were examined, focusing in particular on the scales' means, for men and women (Spearman's r = .89 and .90 for the
standard deviations, scale intercorrelations, Cronbach alpha, and Israeli and the American samples), we report only the ag-
test-retest reliability. The cross-cultural comparison made it pos- gregated results.
sible to test the psychometric equivalence of the questionnaires
(Fouad, 1993). Second, to derive the empirical structure of the 10
scales and the empirical internal structure of the two selected Psychometric Properties of the Questionnaire
categories, we used cluster analysis. Cluster analysis is used to
partition the stimuli of a given data set into relatively distinct Israeli sample. The means, standard deviations, and re-
groups. One of its prevalent uses is to test a particular psycholog- liabilities of the scale scores in the Israeli sample are pre-
ical classification on the basis of prior knowledge or theory (Bor- sented in the left-hand side of Table 1. The table also
gen & Barnett, 1987).
presents the number of items per scale. The intercorrelations
In this study we used a clustering algorithm called ADDTREE
(Sattath & Tversky, 1977). ADDTREE is especially attractive among the scale scores in the Israeli sample are presented in
because it represents the proximity matrix in the form of an Table 2, above the diagonal. During the initial phases of
additive or "path length" tree, in which the variables are divided these analyses (as well as in those of the previous versions
into clusters and subclusters according to the proximity between of the questionnaire) we found that, unlike the structure in
them (as reflected in the degree of correlation between them). Figure 1, the category of lack of knowledge about the
Thus, the clustering analysis by ADDTREE of the intercorrelations process was much closer to the three categories in the major
among the 10 scales and the intercorrelations among the difficul- category of Lack of Information than to the three categories
ties within the two selected categories allowed us to directly in the major category of Lack of Readiness. Hence, in the
compare the empirical structures with the hypothesized theoretical following analyses this category was regarded as belonging
structures. Another advantage of ADDTREE is that, unlike many
to the difficulties of the major category of Lack of Infor-
other clustering programs available as part of standard statistical
packages (e.g., SAS, SAS Institute, 1982; SPSS x, SPSS, Inc., marion. As can be seen in Table 1, the standard deviations
1986), no decisions about the method or technique of clustering are of the scale scores show an acceptable variability, and the
needed (i.e., single linkage, complete linkage, average linkage, or correlations among the scale scores (Mdn = .37 and range
Ward's, 1963, minimum variance technique). For additional de- = .06-.78) indicate an adequate differentiation among
tails on ADDTREE, see Sattath and Tversky (1977); for the results them.

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Reliabilities of the Scales in the Israeli (n = 259)
and American Samples (n = 304)
American
Israeli sample sample
Number
Scale of items M SD a Retest M SD a
Lack of Readiness
Lack of Motivation 3 2.80 1.76 .66 .62 2.72 1.52 .53
Indecisiveness 4 3.80 1.85 .76 .72 4.52 1.73 .69
Dysfunctional Myths 3 4.52 1.51 .29 .51 5.11 1.57 .40
Lack of Information
Lack of Knowledge About the Process 3 4.04 2.28 .86 .64 3.63 2.08 .86
Lack of Information About Self 8 3.44 1.79 .87 .68 3.51 1.88 .91
Lack of Information About Occupations 4 4.62 2.37 .90 .62 3.96 2.15 .88
Lack of Information About Ways of
Obtaining Additional Information 2 3.52 2.19 .67 .50 3.22 1.87 .66
Inconsistent Information
Unreliable Information 6 3.12 1.68 .78 .67 3.34 1.72 .79
Internal Conflicts 7 3.42 1.69 .79 .66 3.57 1.59 .75
External Conflicts 4 2.28 1.75 .88 .67 2.64 1.90 .88
Lack of Readiness 10 3.71 1.23 .70 .67 4.16 1.02 .63
Lack of Information 17 3.84 1.78 .93 .74 3.60 1.78 .95
Inconsistent Information 17 3.04 1.53 .91 .72 3.27 1.45 .89
Total 44 3.49 1.36 .95 .80 3.60 1.32 .95
516 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

Table 2
lntercorrelations Among the CDDQ Scales in the Israeli (n = 259; Above DiagOnal) and American Samples
(n = 304; Below Diagonal) '
Inconsistent
Lack of Readiness Lack of Information Information Clusters
Scale 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14
1. Rm - - .34 .11 .27 .42 .23 .22 .38 .42 .37 .68 .37 .44 .52
2. Ri .29 -- .22 .46 .51 .28 .37 .35 .35 .28 .83 .49 .38 .58
3. Rd - . 0 5 .23 -- .16 .18 .06 .18 .14 .15 .14 .55 .17 .16 .28
4. Lp .46 .48 .05 -- .69 .63 .60 .55 .56 .36 .46 .84 .58 .77
5. Ls .45 .50 .09 .76 -- .63 .57 .69 .65 .48 .56 .91 .70 .88
6. Lo .41 .41 .02 .70 .73 -- .69 .55 .55 .32 .29 .86 .55 .74
7. La .28 .39 .01 .66 .66 .74 -- .53 .54 .37 .38 .77 .56 .72
8. Iu .40 .52 .08 .64 .76 .64 .65 -- .78 .62 .42 .70 .91 .84
9. Ii .46 .47 .10 .63 .68 .64 .59 .73 -- .67 .45 .68 .94 .86
10. Ie .32 .34 .08 .33 .38 .38 .35 .50 .44 -- .38 .46 .82 .67

11. R .57 .84 .55 .51 .54 .43 .36 .52 .53 .38 - -.52 .47 .67
12. L .47 .51 .06 .86 .95 .89 .80 .77 .72 .41 .54 -- .72 .92
13. I .47 .53 .11 .65 .74 .67 .65 .90 .89 .72 .57 .78 -- .90
14. Total .54 .64 .18 .81 .90 .82 .75 .87 .84 .58 .70 .94 .92 --
Note. CDDQ = Career Decision Difficulties Questionnaire; Rm = Lack of Motivation scale; Ri = Indecisiveness scale; Rd =
Dysfunctional Myths scale; Lp = Lack of Knowledge About the Process scale; Ls = Lack of Information About Self scale; Lo = Lack
of Information About Occupations scale; La = Lack of Information About Ways of Obtaining Additional Information scale; Iu =
Unreliable Information scale; Ii = Internal Conflicts scale; Ie =Extemal Conflicts scale; R = Lack of Readiness cluster; L = Lack of
Information cluster; I = Inconsistent Information cluster.

The reliability (Cronbach alpha) of the scales varies sig- category of Lack of Readiness was the lowest, .63 (which,
nificantly, as can be seen in Table 1. The reliability of the again, can be partially attributed to the unreliability of the
scale of Dysfunctional Myths (Rd) is very low, revealing Dysfunctional Myths scale), the reliability of the major
that a positive endorsement of a particular dysfunctional category of L a c k of Information was .95, and that of the
myth is not associated with the endorsement of other myths. Inconsistent Information was .89. The reliability of the
This finding was consistent across the various versions of whole questionnaire was .95.
the questionnaire and replicates those of Gati (1996b). Other The similarity between the Cronbach alpha reliabilities
scales had moderate-to-high reliabilities, ranging from .66 obtained in the two samples is reflected in a Spearman
for the scale of Lack of Motivation to .90 for Lack of rank-order correlation of .94 for the 10 scales. This simi-
Information About Occupations (the median scale reliability larity indicates that the variance in reliabilities is inherent to
was .78). The low reliability of Dysfunctional Myths con- the scales and not especially associated with the different
tributed to the relatively low reliability of the major cate- procedures and settings in which the two data sets were
gory of Lack of Readiness (.70); the reliabilities of the two collected. Furthermore, the rank order of the correlations
other major categories were much higher: Lack of Informa- between the 10 scales in the two studies was .90 (which is
tion = .93, and Inconsistent Information = .91. The reli- reflected in the similarity of Figures 2 and 3), indicating a
ability of the whole questionnaire was .95. The pattern of stable pattern of relations among them across cultures. Fi-
test-retest reliabilities was generally similar to that of Cron- nally, the rank-order correlation between the scale means
bach alpha, although the reliabilities were lower and the was .85, reflecting the similarity in the relative severity of
differences among the scales smaller (see Table 1). the difficulties in the two samples.
American sample. The means, standard deviations, and
reliabilities of the scale scores in the American sample are Empirical Structure of the 10 Scales
presented on the right-hand side of Table 1. The intercor-
relations among the scale scores in the American sample are Israeli sample. Figure 2 presents the empirical structure
presented in Table 2 below the diagonal. As in the Israeli of the 10 scales obtained by an A D D T R E E analysis of the
sample, the standard deviations of the scale scores of the intercorrelation matrix shown in Table 2. The clustering
American sample reflect an acceptable variability, and the structure in Figure 2 adequately summarized the empirical
correlations among the scales (Mdn = .46, range = - . 0 5 - relations among the scales: The linearly accounted-for vari-
.76) indicate an adequate differentiation among them. ance by the distances in the clustering structures was 94%.
The reliability of the scales varied significantly and in the The distance between any pair of scales in this clustering
same pattern as that of the Israeli sample: from .40 for the structure is represented by the sum of the horizontal seg-
scale of Dysfunctional Myths to .91 for the scale of Lack of ments on the shortest path connecting them. Thus, scales
Information About Self (the median scale reliability was within the same cluster are generally more closely related
.77). As with the Israeli sample, the reliability of the major than those belonging to different clusters.
DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 517

........................................ Motivation
R
.................................. Indecisiveness
..... i
........................................................... Dysfunctional myths
.......... Lack o f information about the s e l f

....
Lol ............... Lack o f knowledge about the process

..................... Lack o f information about occupations


........ i
. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . Lack o f information about ways o f o b t a i n i n g additional
information
.......... Unreliable information
II
................. Internal conflicts
--I
........................ External c o n f l i c t s

Figure 2. The empirical structure of the 10 difficulty categories--Israeli sample (n = 259). R =


Readiness; LoI = Lack of Information; II = Inconsistent Information.

As can be seen in Figure 2, the 10 scales are grouped into the intercorrelation matrix shown in Table 2. The clustering
three clusters corresponding to the three major categories: structure in Figure 3 adequately summarizes the empirical
Lack of Readiness, Lack of Information, and Inconsistent relations among the scales: The linearly accounted-for vari-
Information. Furthermore, there is a distinction between the ance of the data by the distances in the clustering structure
first category, which includes difficulties arising prior to is 98%.
the beginning of the process of career decision making, and As can be seen from Figure 3, the empirical structure of
the last two categories, which include difficulties arising the 10 scales in this sample is similar to that in the Israeli
during the decision-making process. The cluster of Lack of sample. First, as for the Israeli sample, there is a distinction
Readiness includes, as expected, the scales of Lack of between the difficulties that arise prior to beginning the
Motivation, Indecisiveness, and Dysfunctional Myths, al- process of career decision making and those arising during
though in a different internal arrangement. However, the this process. Second, the cluster of Lack of Readiness
scale of Lack of Knowledge About the Process joined the includes the scales of Lack of Motivation, Indecisiveness,
cluster of Lack of Information instead of the cluster of Lack and Dysfunctional Myths, and, again, the last two are paired
of Readiness. As stated, this finding also emerged in the together. Third, the cluster of Lack of Information includes
previous versions of the questionnaire. The cluster of Lack the same four scales as in the Israeli sample, with a similar
of Information includes, in addition to this scale, the scales internal structure. Specifically, Lack of Information About
of Lack of Information About Self, Lack of Information Occupations and Lack of Information About Ways of Ob-
About Occupations, and Lack of Information About Addi- taining Additional Information are paired with each other
tional Sources of Information, as suggested by the theoret- and separated from the other two scales--Lack of Knowl-
ical model. The third cluster, Inconsistent Information, in- edge About the Process and Lack of Information About
cludes, as hypothesized, the scales of Unreliable Self. It is important to note that Lack of Knowledge About
Information, Internal Conflicts, and External Conflicts. the Process is located in the cluster of Lack of Information
American sample. Figure 3 presents the empirical struc- rather than in the Lack of Readiness cluster, just as in the
ture of the 10 scales, obtained by the ADDTREE analysis of Israeli sample; in this both samples differ from the theoret-

............................... Motivation

_R__ - ............. Indecisiveness

....................................................... Dysfunctional myths

........................... External conflicts

..... Lack of information about the self

___L°I_ - ........ Lack of knowledge about the process

m m . . . . . Lack of information about occupations


/
. . . . . . . . . . Lack of information about ways of obtaining additional information
....... Unreliable information
--I
II ......... Internal conflicts

Figure 3. The empirical structure of the 10 difficulty categories--American sample (n = 304).


R = Readiness; LoI = Lack of Information; II = Inconsistent Information.
518 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

ical model. Finally, the scales of Unreliable Information and the difficulties in this category were grouped into three
Internal Conflicts are grouped in the major cluster of Incon- pairs: (a) inconsistency about preferences (about desirable
sistent Information. However, in contrast to what we ex- aspects, or preferred careers), (b) inconsistent information
pected, the scale of External Conflicts is located not in this about capabilities (abilities and personality traits), and (c)
major cluster, as in the Israeli sample, but rather in the Lack inconsistent information about the occupational alternatives
of Readiness cluster. (what careers there are and what they are like).
American sample. Figure 5a presents the empirical
Empirical Structure of Difficulties Within Categories structure of the eight difficulties within the category of lack
of information about self in the American sample. The
Israeli sample. Figure 4a presents the empirical struc- empirical structure supports the distinction between lack of
ture of the eight difficulties within the category of lack of information about preferences ("What do I want?") and lack
information about self, obtained by an ADDTREE analysis of information about capabilities ("What can I do?"). Fur-
of the intercorrelations between the responses to the eight thermore, in each of these clusters there is a pair of diffi-
items representing the eight difficulties related to this cate- culties representing lack of information at present (Difficul-
gory in the Israeli sample. As expected, the difficulties in ties 16 and 17, and 14 and 15, respectively; see the
this category were grouped into two subclusters correspond- Appendix) and a pair of difficulties related to lack of infor-
ing to the two subcategories: lack of information about mation concerning the future (Difficulties 20 and 21, and 18
preferences ("What do I want?") and lack of information
and 19, respectively). Thus, the internal structures of lack of
about capabilities ("What can I do?"). In each of the two
information about self in both studies are highly similar, but
clusters, the two difficulties representing lack of informa-
only that of the American sample is fully compatible with
tion at present are found together (Difficulties 16 and 17,
and 14 and 15, respectively; see Appendix), as proposed in the theoretically expected structure.
the theoretical model. However, the respective two difficul- Figure 5b presents the empirical structure of the six
ties concerning the future constitute a well-defined pair only difficulties associated with unreliable information in the
in the cluster of preferences (Difficulties 20 and 21). American sample. The structure reveals three pairs of dif-
Figure 4b presents the empirical structure of the six ficulties: (a) unreliable preferences, Co) unreliable informa-
difficulties related to the category of unreliable information tion about capabilities, and (c) unreliable information about
in the Israeli sample. Compatible with the theoretical model, career alternatives. This structure is nearly identical to that

a. Within the category o f lack o f information about the s e l f

Present .................................. Abilities


............. i
~-I .......... Future ................................................. Pe~ity
.................................. Abilities
. Future Personality t r a i t s

Present ................................... Occupations

Pr i ............... ! ............................. Aspects

I Future ........... Occupations


........................ i
......... Aspects

b. Within the category o f qnr~liable information

Cap ............................................................... Abilities


......... i
................................. Personali t y t r a i t s

Pr ........... Occupations
...................................... i
.............. Aspects

Occ .................. Existence


......................................................
.................. Characteristics

Figure 4. The structure of difficulties within categories--Israeli sample. Cap = Capabilities;


Pr = Preferences; Occ = Occupations.
DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 519

a. Within the category o f lack o f informatiQo ~bo~t the s e l f

Present ............................................. Abilities


. . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . i
___C)p______ - ................................... Personality traits

Future ................................ Abilities


........ i
............................................ Personality traits

Present .................................. Occupations


............. i
___Pr. . . . . . . -................................ Aspects

Future ............ Occupations


.................... i
....... Aspects

b. Within the category o f unreliable information

Cap ................................. Abilities


............................ i
............................. Personality t r a i t s

Pr --- Occupations
..................................... i
...... Aspects

Occ .................. Existence


.............. i
...... Characteristics

Figure 5. The structure of difficulties within categories--American sample. Cap = Capabilities;


Pr = Preferences; Occ = Occupations.

obtained in the Israeli sample data and compatible with the divided into more refined subcategories; these subcategories
expected theoretical structure. and the 4 other categories were broken down into 44 spe-
cific difficulties, each representing a distinct type of prob-
Discussion lem.
To test the proposed taxonomy, we constructed the
The central aim of this research was to develop and CDDQ, in which each of the 44 difficulties was represented
empirically test a taxonomy of difficulties in career decision by one respective item. On the basis of the theoretical
making. In the initially proposed taxonomy, outlined in model, 10 scales were defined for the questionnaire, each
Figure 1 and elaborated in the Appendix, the 44 difficulties including the set of items corresponding to the specific
were expressed in terms of concepts adopted from decision difficulties constituting the respective category. This ques-
theory and then adapted to the area of career decisions. The tionnaire was then administered to two samples from dif-
basic distinction in the proposed taxonomy was between ferent social-cultural contexts: an Israeli sample of young
difficulties arising prior to the beginning of the process of adults at the beginning of their process of career decision
career decision making and those arising during this pro- making and a sample of American university students. Two
cess. The major category of difficulties that arise prior to the levels of the proposed taxonomy were tested: the relations
beginning of the process, which was defined as Lack of among the 10 categories and the relations among specific
Readiness, included four categories: lack of motivation, difficulties that compose two selected categories.
indecisiveness, dysfunctional myths, and lack of knowledge The empirical pattern of relationships among the 10
about the process. The difficulties arising during the process scales, which represent the 10 difficulty categories in the
were divided into two major categories: Lack of Informa- theoretical model, was generally similar to the hypothesized
tion, which included three categories (lack of information pattern in both the Israeli and the American samples, with
about self, lack of information about occupations, and lack one clear deviation involving the scale of Lack of Knowl-
of information about ways of obtaining additional informa- edge About the Process. In the proposed taxonomy this
tion) and Inconsistent Information, which also included category was located in the major category of Lack of
three categories (unreliable information, external conflicts, Readiness because we assumed that this type of difficulty
and internal conflicts). Six of the 10 categories were further arises before the individual needs to make a particular
520 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

career decision and can be prevented by appropriate prep- such refinements seems to vary among the categories. Spe-
aration. Empirically, however, this category was consis- cifically, within some categories different difficulties re-
tently included in the cluster of Lack of Information in both quire distinct courses of action (e.g., lack of information
samples. Two reasons may account for this deviation from about preferences may require the help of a counselor,
the theoretical model. First, although knowledge of the steps whereas lack of information about abilities requires relevant
involved in career decision making may be acquired prior to tests). Within other categories, however, the same type of
beginning the process of career decision making, one need intervention is required for all difficulties (e.g., the various
not apply this knowledge until the process has actually dysfunctional myths may require face-to-face counseling).
begun. Accordingly, the participants probably felt a need for Indeed, the results of this study indicate that some elab-
knowledge of the steps involved in the process only when orations are justified. The empirical internal structure of the
they actually started the process of making a particular two selected categories, lack of information about self and
decision and not before. Second, this knowledge has a unreliable information, was almost identical to the proposed
cognitive basis (such as lack of information about the self or taxonomy. The initial organization of the difficulties within
occupations), whereas the other categories in the cluster of each of these categories was very similar in the two sam-
Lack of Readiness are more emotionally based. In light of ples, although the Israeli sample showed a slight deviation
this consistent and reasonable pattern of relations, we intro- from the theoretical model within one of the two subcate-
duced the relevant changes in the theoretical taxonomy; the gories of lack of information about self. The compatibility
revised taxonomy, which we now believe to be more ade- of the within-category structure of the difficulties with the
quate, is presented in Figure 6. theoretical model in these two categories, and the consis-
tency of the within-category structures in the two samples,
Differences Among the Difficulty Categories increase the likelihood of locating not only difficulty cate-
gories but also more refined subcategories. Future research
As can be seen from the list of difficulties in the Appen- may contribute to the further elaboration and refinement of
dix, certain parts of the taxonomy are refined more than the difficulties within other categories as well.
others. Two factors contributed to this variation in the Another difference among the 10 categories is the degree
degree of refinement. First, our theoretical knowledge var- of co-occurrence of the difficulties belonging to them. In
ies with respect to the different possible difficulties. For general, as can be seen from the medium-to-high reliabili-
example, although we have enough knowledge about lack ties of the 10 scales, the specific difficulties that compose
of information about the self to make two additional levels each category tend to correlate across individuals. The only
of within-category distinctions, for indecisiveness, as of yet exception to this rule was the scale of Dysfunctional Myths.
at least, we do not. Second, the practical significance of The unique feature of this category is the very low corre-

CAREER DECISION MAKING DIFFICULTIES

PRZOR TO BEGZNNZNG THE PROCESS DURZNG THE PROCESS


I
I
I I
LACK OF LACK OF INCONSZSTENT
READZNESS TNFORNATZON INFORNAT][ON
DUE TO ABOUT DUE TO

LACK OF INDECZ- DYSFUNCTZONAL THE CAREER S E L F 0CCUPATZONS ~AYS OF UNRELZABLE INTERNAL EXTERNAL
~OTZVATZON SZVENESS MYTHS DECZSZON 0BTAZNZNG INFORNATZON CONFLZCTS CONFLZCTS
MAKZNG PROCESS INFORHATZON
(STEPS)

Figure 6. The revised theoretical taxonomy of career decision-making difficulties.


DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 521

lation between the items representing the various dysfunc- quarter of college. It may be assumed that in this stage of
tional myths (range = .02-. 19 and .03-.26 in the Israeli and life they were just at the beginning of the process of sepa-
the American samples, respectively), which results in the ration from home and therefore might have been more
low scale reliability (.29 and .40, respectively). This pattern dependent on their families than the Israelis. This may have
shows that the endorsement of one myth was not related to contributed to the observed pattern that External Conflicts
the endorsement of others. As mentioned, a similar pattern were associated with the Lack of Readiness cluster in the
of low correlations between myths was also found by Gati American sample, but not in the Israeli sample.
(1996b). Nevertheless, in spite of the low consistency of the
Dysfunctional Myths scale, we decided not to delete or
change this category because of its prevalence and potential Implications
impact on the process of career decision making (Nevo,
1987). We strongly recommend, however, that the unique Theory. One of the questions raised by the empirical
nature of difficulties related to this category be further research on the various indecision measures, and on the
investigated. CDS (Osipow et al., 1987) in particular, concerns the di-
mensionality of career indecision. Two approaches have
been proposed for this issue. One considers indecision to be
Comparison Between the Two Samples multidimensional, whereas the other considers it a unidi-
mensional construct. The multidimensional approach sug-
The participants in the American sample differ from those gests that indecision has a number of well-defined dimen-
in the Israeli sample in many respects, including age (and sions, which, in the context of the CDS, have typically been
thus, presumably, maturity), language, and social-cultural- summarized by four factors: Diffusion, Support, Approach,
economic background. Nevertheless, the findings in these and External Barriers (e.g., Martin, Sabourin, Laplante, &
two samples almost replicated each other. Specifically, the Coallier, 1991; Shimizu, Vondracek, & Schulenberg, 1994;
patterns of reliabilities of the 10 scales in the two samples Shimizu, Vondracek, Schulenberg, & Hostetler, 1988; Von-
are very similar; the empirical structures of the 10 scales are dracek, Hostetler, Schulenberg, & Shimizu, 1990). In con-
similar in both the three main clusters and their internal trast, the unidimensional approach suggests that, due to the
structure; the structures of difficulties within the category of observed instability in the number and content of the factors
lack of information about self are very similar, and those underlying the various measures, there is not enough sup-
within the category of unreliable information are practically port for multidimensionality (e.g., Osipow, 1987; Savickas,
identical. Carden, Toman, & Jarjoura, 1992). This lack of consistency
However, the findings of the two samples differed in the and stability with regard to the dimensions underlying the
location of the scale of External Conflicts. In light of the various indecision measures can be attributed to measure-
great overall similarity, this difference is puzzling. To find ment errors and the lack of a theoretical framework (Tins-
out what caused it, we reexamined the specific items rep- ley, 1992). Indeed, when the question of dimensionality was
resenting the difficulties included in that scale and discov- examined for the rationally developed Career Factors In-
ered a small but apparently significant inaccuracy in the ventory (Chartrand et al., 1990), the results supported the
translation. In the Hebrew version the items mentioned lack hypothesized four-factor structure (Career Choices Anxiety,
of agreement between a significant other and the individual Generalized Indecisiveness, Need for Career Information,
regarding some aspect of the process, whereas in the En- and Need for Self-Knowledge). Note, however, that these
glish translation the word used was approval. Unlike agree- factors are quite different from those identified in the CDS.
ment, which implies symmetry between the involved parties The theoretical framework underlying the present re-
(i.e., the self and the significant other), approval implies search, as well as its empirical findings, suggest a new
asymmetry. Such asymmetry may imply that the individu- perspective concerning the dimensionality of career indeci-
al's position is inferior relative to that of the involved sion. Instead of trying to discover dimensions in the con-
significant other. Thus, in the American sample these dif- struct of career indecision, we have suggested a taxonomy
ficulties joined the cluster of Lack of Readiness, which is for the various difficulties constituting this construct. The
more closely related to issues of maturity, rather than In- assumption underlying this endeavor is that career indeci-
consistent Information cluster, which is more closely related sion is not a single type of problem with different symptoms
to incompatible preferences and conflicting opinions. but rather a group of problems that typically lead to the
Another possible explanation for this difference involves same final outcome (i.e., the inability to make a career
the difference in the characteristics of the participants in the decision). Accordingly, to better understand this construct,
two studies: The Israeli sample was composed of slightly it is necessary to investigate its components as well as the
older individuals (age 21), who were nearing the end of their relationships among them (i.e., its structure). The first step
2 or 3 years of compulsory military service, a period that in such an investigation was carded out in this research, and
typically has significant effects on the young people, and we regard the reported findings as promising. Specifically,
during which they usually become more independent and we found that the various difficulties associated with inde-
responsible. The majority of the American sample (74%), cision can be classified into 10 meaningful categories and
on the other hand, was composed of college freshmen (ages that some of these categories can be further classified into
18-19), who filled out the questionnaire during their first meaningful subcategories. Moreover, these categories and
522 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

subcategories were found to have a well-defined structure The relationship among the various measures of career
that is generally compatible with the theoretical model. In indecision has been the focus of several empirical investi-
light of this, we believe that future work on indecision gations (e.g., Fuqua & Newman, 1989; Tinsley, Bowman, &
should focus less on the question of dimensionality and York, 1989). Following this line of research, simultaneous
more on the development of a comprehensive, theoretically collection of data on the CDDQ and other career indecision
sound, and practically significant taxonomy. instruments, for example the CDS or the CFI, may make it
The proposed taxonomy of difficulties in career decision possible to identify the major aspects of indecision tapped
making is compatible with previous research on indecision. by the latter. For instance, data based on the last group of
For example, according to Shimizu et al. (1988), diffusion the American sample of participants (n = 19) reveal a high
(i.e., lack of confidence and structure), one of the four correlation (.71) between the total career indecision score on
factors in the CDS, is related to feelings of confusion, the CDS and the total CDDQ score. Further research should
discouragement, and lack of experience or information re- investigate this in a larger sample and also investigate the
garding career decision making. In the proposed model, empirical relations between the CDDQ scales and the four
these problems were included in the major category of Lack suggested factors of the CDS. This may shed light, albeit
of Readiness and were defined as lack of motivation, inde- indirectly, on the dimensionality of the CDS, an issue that
cisiveness, and dysfunctional myths. These difficulties ap- has been the focus of many studies, with inconclusive
pear to have an affective source and to be associated with results. Similarly, a comparison between the CDMSE (Tay-
low career maturity (Crites, 1978). Approach-approach lor & Betz, 1983) and the CDDQ may reveal what catego-
conflict, another factor in the CDS, is represented by three ries of difficulties are more closely associated with self-
difficulties in our category of internal conflicts: conflicts efficacy in career decision making in general and the five
among incompatible desirable aspects, conflicts among in- dimensions underlying the CDMSE in particular. In the
compatible desirable occupations, and conflicts between a same time, such investigations would contribute to the val-
desirable occupation and an incompatible desirable aspect. idation of the CDDQ and the taxonomy underlying it.
Lack of knowledge about self and lack of information about A different program of research on indecision has focused
occupations, two of the factors in the CFI (Chartrand et al., on the classification of subtypes of individuals according to
1990), are represented here by two categories and are fur- their problems in career decision making (e.g., Callanan &
ther divided, systematically, into subcategories. Problems Greenhaus, 1992; Larson, Heppner, Ham, & Dugan, 1988;
related to disapproval by significant others, one of the Lucas & Epperson, 1990; Rojewski, 1994; Savickas &
variables measured by the Career Barriers Inventory (Swan- Jarjoura, 1991). This line of research assumes that there is a
son & Tokar, 1991), or to authority orientation, a variable systematic variance in the sets of problem combinations
measured by one of the subscales of the CDDA (Bansberg across individuals; however, classifying individuals into
& Sklare, 1986), are represented here by the category of subtypes is based on the particular theoretical framework
external conflicts. As can be seen from these examples, the that underlies the taxonomy of the problems or difficulties.
proposed taxonomy reorganizes previously encountered dif- Future research should explore the contribution of the
ficulties into theoretically based categories, elaborates and present taxonomy of difficulties to the classification of
refines these categories, and includes additional difficulties subtypes of individuals.
that were not explicitly included in previous classifications. The categorization of the difficulties in the proposed
Moreover, although this taxonomy is cognitively oriented, it taxonomy can be linked to the different consequences (and
does encompass some affectively based difficulties (e.g., hence also interventions) that each difficulty may lead to.
those included in the categories of lack of motivation, Specifically, we assume that, in general, difficulties involv-
indecisiveness, and internal conflicts). ing lack of readiness may prevent the individual from be-
Future research. The proposed taxonomy was tested in ginning the decision-making process, difficulties involving
this study by analyzing the responses of deliberating young lack of information may stop the process once it has been
adults from two different cross-cultural samples. Although started, and difficulties involving inconsistent information
the two samples provide important evidence for the gener- (i.e., unreliable information that is not checked out or un-
ality of the taxonomy, it is important to replicate these tests resolved conflicts) may stop the process or lead to a deci-
in other samples with different characteristics, for example, sion that is less than optimal. The relative impact and
a sample of adult career decision makers (ages 40-60) and severity of each of these major categories, as well as that of
samples of various racial and ethnic minorities. Another each of the 10 categories, needs to be further studied with
way to examine the proposed taxonomy is to elicit career respect to both the process of career decision making and its
counselors' judgments about the difficulties of their coun- outcomes.
selees. Career counselors can be asked to rate the degree to Counseling. A proper diagnosis of counselees' prob-
which each difficulty or each of the 10 categories of diffi- lems in career decision making is necessary (although, of
culties is characteristic of a particular counselee. Then the course, not sufficient) to provide deliberating individuals
structure of difficulties can be derived from these ratings by with the help they need. As mentioned earlier, the purpose
procedures similar to those used in the present research and of this research was to develop and test a theoretical frame-
can also be compared with the theoretical model. This may work for difficulties in career decision making. Neverthe-
provide another perspective on the structure of difficulties less, it seems that the questionnaire developed to test the
and serve to validate the questionnaire. proposed taxonomy has the potential to become, after the
DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 523

necessary changes, an additional important tool for individ- career decisions. Locating and understanding the difficulties
ual career counseling. Specifically, the comprehensiveness faced by individuals during their process of career decision
and the theoretical basis of the CDDQ permits the assess- making thus have significant practical implications. The
ment of the individual's difficulties at three different levels results of this research may contribute to the facilitation of
of specificity: (a) three major categories (i.e., Lack of these career decisions and hence indirectly help to increase
Readiness, Lack of Information, and Inconsistent Informa- the quality of life in one of its more significant aspects,
tion); (b) 10 categories (represented by the 10 scales); and namely, work.
(c) specific difficulties (at the level of specific items). De-
pending on the counselor's judgment, all three levels can be References
assessed, or the assessment can focus on only the one or two
levels that seem most relevant. Bansberg, B., & Sklare, J. (1986). The career decision diagnostic
Thus, the questionnaire may be used, first, for an initial assessment. Monterey, CA: CTB/McGraw-Hill.
screening of clients who seek career counseling, by direct- Baron, J. (1988). Thinking and deciding. New York: Cambridge
ing the clients to the various intervention options available University Press.
Betz, N. E. (1992). Career assessment: A review of critical issues.
according to the CDDQ results. For example, those who In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of counseling
have difficulties in dealing with conflicts could be directed psychology (pp. 453-484). New York: Wiley.
to personal counseling; those who lack information about Bordin, E. S., & Kopplin, D. A. (1973). Motivational conflict and
their abilities, to further assessment; and those who lack vocational development. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 20,
information on educational or career options, to the occu- 154-161.
pational library. Second, the information from the CDDQ Borgen, F. H., & Barnett, D. C. (1987). Applying cluster analysis
administered before the first counseling session could pro- in counseling psychology research. Journal of Counseling Psy-
vide relevant data about the counselee that may help the chology, 34, 456-468.
counselor plan and guide the counseling process. Third, the Brown, D. (1990). Models of career decision making. In D. Brown
questionnaire may be used as a "needs assessment" instru- & L. Brooks (Eds.), Career choice and development (2nd ed.,
pp. 395-421). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
ment to collect information about the kinds of difficulties in
Callanan, G. A., & Greenhaus, J. H. (1992). The career indecision
career decision making that frequently occur in particular of managers and professionals: An examination of multiple
groups (e.g., 12th-grade students, college freshmen). This subtypes. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 41, 212-231.
may facilitate the design of interventions that best suit the Campbell, R. E., & Cellini, J. V. (1981). A diagnostic taxonomy of
needs of each group. Finally, it may serve, like other in- adult career problems. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 19,
struments that assess career indecision, to evaluate the ef- 175-190.
fectiveness of career interventions, by comparing the coun- Campbell, R.E., & Heffernan, J.M. (1983). Adult vocational
selees' responses before and after the intervention. behavior. In W. B. Walsh & S. H. Osipow (Eds.), Handbook of
However, all these important and useful applications should vocational psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 223-260). Hillsdale, NJ:
be implemented only after the CDDQ is transformed from a Erlbaum.
Chartrand, J.M., Robbins, S.B., Morrill, W.H., & Boggs, K.
research questionnaire aimed at testing a theoretical model
(1990). The development and validation of the career factors
into a validated clinical instrument. inventory. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 37, 490-501.
Computer-assisted career guidance systems. The diag- Crites, J. O. (1978). Theory and research handbook for the Career
nosis of difficulties is also very important for computer- Maturity Inventory. Monterey, CA: CTB/McGraw-Hill.
assisted career guidance systems. One of the problems as- Fleishman, E.A., & Quaintance, M.K. (1984). Taxonomies of
sociated with the use of these systems is that often the users human performance. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
do not use them effectively because they contain a great Fouad, N. A. (1993). Cross-cultural vocational assessment. Career
deal of information and many components, so that the user Development Quarterly, 42, 4-13.
"gets lost" (Gati, 1996a). Discovering the individuals' dif- Fouad, N.A. (1994). Annual review 1991-1993: Vocational
ficulties at the beginning of their dialogue with the comput- choice, decision-making, assessment, and intervention. Journal
of Vocational Behavior, 45, 125-176.
erized system should make it possible to direct them to
Fuqua, D.R., & Hartman, B.W. (1983). A behavioral index of
those components in the system that may provide the infor- career indecision for college students. Journal of College Stu-
mation or guidance they need or to recommend that they dent Personnel, 24, 507-512.
approach a career counselor in those cases in which the Fuqua, D. R., & Newman, J.L. (1989). An examination of the
computerized system cannot help (e.g., solving interper- relations among career subscales. Journal of Counseling Psy-
sonal conflicts). chology, 36, 487-491.
Gati, I. (1986). Making career decisions: A sequential elimination
approach. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 408-417.
Conclusion Gati, I. (1990). Why, when, and how to take into account the
uncertainty involved in career decisions. Journal of Counseling
Psychology, 37, 277-280.
The significance of this research is linked to the changes
Gati, I. (1996a). Computer-assisted career counseling: Challenges
in the world of work, a setting that is becoming increasingly and prospects. In M. L. Savickas & B. W. Walsh (Eds.), Hand-
dynamic. These changes imply an increase in the number of book of career counseling theory and practice (pp. 169-190).
career transitions individuals make during their lifetime and Palo Alto, CA: Davies-Black Publishing.
therefore in the frequency with which they must make Gati, I. (1996b). Towards career decision making. Unpublished
524 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

manuscript, Department of Psychology, Hebrew University of A1R-72 6/2035-3/71-TRJI). Silver Spring, MD: American Insti-
Jerusalem (in Hebrew). tutes for Research.
Gati, I., Fassa, N., & Houminer, D. (1995). Applying decision Mitchell, L. K., & Krumboltz, J. D. (1984). Research on human
theory to career counseling practice: The sequential elimination decision making: Implications for career decision making and
approach. Career Development Quarterly, 43, 211-220. counseling. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.), Handbook of
Gaff, I., Garty, Y., & Fassa, N. (1996). Using career-related aspects counseling psychology (pp. 238-280). New York: Wiley.
to assess person-environment fit. Journal of Counseling Psy- Neimeyer, G. J. (1988). Cognitive integration and differentiation
chology, 43, 196-206. in vocational behavior. The Counseling Psychologist, 16, 440-
Gati, I., Osipow, S. H., & Givon, M. (1995). Gender differences in 475.
career decision making: The content and structure of prefer- Nevo, O. (1987). Irrational expectations in career counseling and
ences. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 42, 204-216. their confronting arguments. Career Development Quarterly, 35,
Gelatt, H. B. (1989). Positive uncertainty: A new decision-making 239-250.
framework for counseling. Journal of Counseling Psychology, Osipow, S.H. (1987). Manual for the Career Decision Scale.
36, 252-256. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
Holland, J.L. (1985). Making vocational choices: A theory of Osipow, S.H., Carney, C.G., & Barak, A. (1976). A scale of
vocational personalities and work environments (2nd. ed.). educational-vocational undecidedness: A typological approach.
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 9, 233-243.
Holland, J. L., Daiger, D.C., & Power, P.G. (1980). My Voca- Osipow, S. H., Carney, C., Winer, J., Yanico, B., & Koschier, M.
tional Situation. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. (1987). The Career Decision Scale (rev. ed.). Odessa, FL: Psy-
Jepsen, D. A., & Dilley, J. S. (1974). Vocational decision making chological Assessment Resources.
models: A review and comparative analysis. Review of Educa- Osipow, S.H., & Fitzgerald, L.F. (1996). Theories of career
tional Research, 44, 331-349. development (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
Jones, L. K. (1989). Measuring a three-dimensional construct of Osipow, S.H., & Winer, J. (1996). Career assessment and the
career indecision among college students: A revision of the Career Decision Scale. Journal of Career Assessment, 4, 117-
Vocational Decision Scale--the Career Decision Profile. Jour- 130.
nal of Counseling Psychology, 36, 477-486. Phillips, S. D. (1994). Choice and change: Convergence from the
Jones, L. K., & Chenery, M. F. (1980). Multiple subtypes among decision-making perspective. In M.L. Savikas & R.W. Lent
vocationally undecided college students: A model and assess- (Eds.), Convergence in career development theories (pp. 155-
ment instrument. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 27, 469- 163). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
477. Pitz, G., & Harren, V. A. (1980). An analysis of career decision
Katz, M.R. (1966). A model for guidance for career decision making from the point of view of information processing and
making. Vocational Guidance Quarterly, 15, 2-10. decision theory. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 16, 320-346.
Katz, M.R. (1993). Computer-assisted career decision making: Roe, A. (1956). The psychology of occupations. New York: Wiley.
The guide in the machine. Hillsdale, NJ: Eflbaum. Rojewski, J. W. (1994). Career indecision types for rural adoles-
Krumboltz, J. D. (1991). Manual for the Career Beliefs Inventory. cents from disadvantaged and nondisadvantaged backgrounds.
Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 41, 356-363.
Krumboltz, J. D. (1994). The Career Beliefs Inventory. Journal of Rounds, J. B., & Tinsley, H. E. A. (1984). Diagnosis and treatment
Counseling & Development, 72, 424-428. of vocational problems. In S. D. Brown & R. W. Lent (Eds.),
Larson, J. H., Busby, D. M., Wilson, S., Medora, N., & Allgood, S. Handbook of counseling psychology (pp. 137-177). New York:
(1994). The multidimensional assessment of career decision Wiley.
problems: The career decision diagnostic assessment. Journal of Salomone, P.R. (1982). Difficult cases in career counseling: II.
Counseling & Development, 72, 323-328. The indecisive client. Personnel and Guidance Journal, 60,
Larson, L.M., Heppner, P.P., Ham, T., & Dugan, K. (1988). 496-500.
Investigating multiple subtypes of career indecision through SAS Institute. (1982). SAS user's guide: Statistics. New York:
cluster analysis. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 35, 439-446. Author.
Lofquist, L. H., & Dawis, R.V. (1978). Values as secondary to Sattath, S., & Tversky, A. (1977). Additive similarity trees. Psy-
needs in the Theory of Work Adjustment. Journal of Vocational chometrika, 42, 319-345.
Behavior, 12, 12-19. Savickas, M. L., Carden, A. D., Toman, S., & Jarjoura, D. (1992).
Lucas, M.S., & Epperson, D.L. (1990). Types of vocational Dimensions of career decidedness. Measurement and Evaluation
undecidedness: A replication and refinement. Journal of Coun- in Counseling and Development, 25, 102-112.
seling Psychology, 37, 382-388. Savickas, M. L., & Jarjoura, D. (1991). The Career Decision Scale
Martin, F., Sabourin, S., Laplante, B., & Coallier, J.C. (1991). as a type indicator. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38,
Diffusion, support, approach, and external barriers as distinct 85-90.
theoretical dimensions of the Career Decision Scale: Discon- Shimizu, K., Vondracek, F. W., & Schulenberg, J. E. (1994). Uni-
firming evidence? Journal of Vocational Behavior, 38, 187-197. dimensionality versus multidimensionality of the Career Deci-
Meier, S. T. (1991). Vocational behavior, 1988-1990: Vocational sion Scale. Journal of Career Assessment, 2, 1-14.
choice, decision-making, career development interventions, and Shimizu, K., Vondracek, F. W., Schulenberg, J. E., & Hostetler,
assessment. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 39, 131-181. M. (1988). The factor structure of the Career Decision Scale:
Meir, E.I., & Gati, I. (1981). Guidelines for item selection in Similarities across selected studies. Journal of Vocational Be-
inventories yielding score profiles. Educational and Psycholog- havior, 32, 213-225.
ical Measurement, 41, 1011-1016. Slaney, R. B. (1988). The assessment of career decision making. In
Miller, R. G. (1971). Development of a taxonomy of human per- W. B. Walsh & S. H. Osipow (Eds.), Career decision making
formance: Design of a systems task vocabulary (Tech. Rep. (pp. 33-76). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
DIFFICULTIES IN CAREER DECISION MAKING 525

SPSS, Inc. (1986). SPSSx user's guide, Edition 2. New York: MVS, Vocational Rating Scale, and Decision Rating Scale: Do
McGraw-Hill. they measure the same thing? Journal of Counseling Psychol-
Super, D. E. (1953). A theory of vocational development. Ameri- ogy, 36, 115-120.
can Psychologist, 8, 185-190. Tversky, A., & Hutchinson, W. J. (1986). Nearest neighbor anal-
Swanson, J. L., & Tokar, D. M. (1991). Development and initial ysis of psychological spaces. Psychological Review, 93, 3-22.
validation of the career barriers inventory. Journal of Vocational Vondracek, F. W., Hostetler, M., Schulenberg, J. E., & Shimizu,
Behavior, 39, 344-361. K. (1990). Dimensions of career indecision. Journal of Coun-
Taylor, K. M., & Betz, N. E. (1983). Applications of self-efficacy seling Psychology, 37, 98-106.
theory to the understanding and treatment of career indecision. Walsh, W.B., & Osipow, S. H. (Eds.). (1988). Career decision
Journal of Vocational Behavior, 22, 63-81. making. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Tinsley, H. E. A. (1992). Career decision making and career inde- Ward, J. H. (1963). Hierarchical grouping to optimize an objective
cision. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 41, 209-211. function. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 58,
Tinsley, H. E. A., Bowman, S.L., & York, D.C. (1989). CDS, 236-244.

Appendix

The 44 Difficulties Included in the Elaborated Theoretical Taxonomy


Prior to the beginning of the process
Lack of Readiness
Lack of Motivation (Rm)
1. Unwillingness to make a career decision
2. Work is not perceived as the most important thing in life
3. Feeling that time will lead to the "right" career choice
Indecisiveness (Ri)
4. A general difficulty in making decisions
5. A general need for conf'mnation and support for decisions
6. A general tendency to avoid commitment
7. A general fear of failure
Dysfunctional Myths (Rd)
8. The belief that entering a career will solve personal problems
9. The belief that there is an ideal career which can fulfill all aspirations
10. The belief that a career choice is a one-time thing and a life-long obligation
Lack of Knowledge About the Process of Career Decision Making (Lp)
11. About the steps involved in making a career decision
12. About the factors to take into consideration
13. About how to combine information concerning the self and career alternatives
Duringthe process
Lack of Information
Lack of Information About Self (Ls)
14. Lack of information about abilities
15. Lack of information about personality traits
16. Lack of information about preferred career alternatives
17. Lack of information about career-related preferences
18. Lack of information about abilities in the future
19. Lack of information about personality traits in the future
20. Lack of information about career alternatives that will be preferred in the future
21. Lack of information about career-related preferences in the future
Lack of Information About Occupations (Lo)
22. Lack of information about the variety of career or training alternatives
23. Lack of information about the characteristics of the career or training alternatives that interest the individual
24. Lack of information about the variety of future career or training alternatives
25. Lack of information about the future characteristics of the career or training alternatives
Lack of Information About Ways of Obtaining Additional Information (La)
26. Lack of information about ways of obtaining additional information about the self
27. Lack of information about ways of obtaining additional information about career and training alternatives

(Appendix continues on next page)


526 GATI, KRAUSZ, AND OSIPOW

Appendix (continued)
Inconsistent Information
Unreliable Information (Iu)
28. Unreliable information about abilities
29. Unreliable information about personality traits
30. Unreliable information about preferred career alternatives
31. Unreliable information about career-related preferences
32. Unreliable information about the existence of a particular career or training alternative
33. Unreliable information about the characteristics of career or training alternative(s)
Internal Conflicts (Ii)
34. Unwillingness to compromise
35. Several equally attractive career alternatives
36. Dislike of accessible career alternatives
37. Something in a preferred career alternative is undesirable
38. Preferences that cannot be combined in one career alternative
39. Abilities are insufficient for the requirements of the preferred career alternative
40. Abilities exceed those required in the preferred career alternative
External Conflicts (Ie)
41. Disagreement between a significant other and the individual concerning the desirable career alternative
42. Disagreement between a significant other and the individual concerning the desirable career-related characteristics
43. Disagreement between different significant others concerning the recommended career alternative
44. Disagreement between different significant others concerning the recommended career-related characteristics

Received March 20, 1996


Revision received May 28, 1996
Accepted May 30, 1996

Potrebbero piacerti anche