Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 1998 25; 765–769

Subjective assessment of temporomandibular joint sounds


J . F. P R I N Z Department of Anatomy, University of Hong Kong

SUMMARY If it could be shown that the human ear the human ear is a rather poor instrument for
was sufficiently sensitive to describe TMJ sounds, describing subtle differences in the position,
there would be no need to use sophisticated duration and latency of TMJ sounds, but is capable
electronic equipment to analyse the sounds. To of detecting small differences in frequency. It is
test this, the ability of normal listeners to therefore doubtful that the human ear can
distinguish the subtle changes in position, pitch, distinguish the reciprocal click associated with disc
duration and latency present in TMJ sounds is displacement with reduction from clicks due to
measured using triangle tests to determine the defects of form on the basis of their relative
just-noticeable differences. The results suggest that position in the envelope of movement.

be difficult to say if the sounds were louder than they


Introduction
were the year or even the week before. The same type
The human ear is a very sensitive detection system, of difficulty relates to the verbal description of sounds.
with a very large (140 dB) dynamic range and frequency For instance, TMJ sounds are usually described in
sensitivity ranging from below 20 Hz to over 20 kHz. terms relating to their aural characteristics. However,
Subjective assessment is the preferred assessment opportunities for confusion abound, since authors
method for comparing hi-fi audio equipment, since variously refer to high or low amplitude, hard or
small differences, which cannot be determined using soft, loud and soft, and fine or coarse sounds without
measuring instrumentation, can be perceived by the rigorously defining these terms.
ear. The pattern recognition capabilities of the human When listening to music in order to judge the quality
brain are unsurpassed; for instance, it is possible for a of a hi-fi system, several minutes may be needed to
person to be identified on the telephone from a few form a judgement of sound quality but TMJ sounds are
spoken words. However, discrimination of the very short – of the order of 30–40 ms, and only one or
amplitude of sounds is poor. For example, when walking two examples of the sound may be heard. Thus,
towards a radio playing music, the perceived loudness although some people claim to be able to hear subtle
remains almost constant, even though the power of the improvements in recorded music achieved, for instance,
signal from the loudspeaker follows an inverse square by using gold-plated connectors on their speaker leads,
relationship with the distance. It is difficult to quantify when listening to TMJ sounds the performance of the
features of a sound subjectively and auditory memory human ear does not achieve anything like these levels
for most types of sound is poor. Although it is possible of sensitivity.
to compare two sounds presented in a short space of Just as drawings such as a Necker cube (Fig. 1) where
time, this becomes progressively more difficult as the the orientation flips between two gestalts can induce
time interval between them increases. Thus, although visual illusions (Rucker, 1986), some sounds produce
it might be possible to assess the reproducibility of a auditory illusions. Two common illusions are: (1) when
series of TMJ sounds during a single session, it would a low amplitude sound precedes a high amplitude

© 1998 Blackwell Science Ltd 765


766 J . F. P R I N Z

Fig. 3. Watt’s soft crepitus. This 300 ms sample, taken from Watt’s
tape, is also perceived as a continuous sound, and was described
by Watt as a gentle rubbing sound.

sample shown in Fig. 2 in terms of its duration, but the


visual difference is obvious.
This potential conflict between the visual and aural
perception of Watt’s two types of crepitus is resolved by
the proposed classification scheme and it has been
argued that the three types of sound are the result of
different underlying mechanisms (Prinz, 1996; Prinz &
Ng, 1997; Prinz, 1998).
Fig. 1. The Necker cube. In clinical practice, TMJ sounds are currently usually
assessed using a stethoscope. In a research environment,
sonography, i.e. visual assessment of the sound
waveform, can also be used for description, with or
without computer assistance. These two methods are
not necessarily equivalent, i.e. the method used to
assess a sound can affect the perception of it. For
Fig. 2. Watt’s hard crepitus. Watt provided a tape recording of
instance, the hard crepitus described by Watt appears
TMJ sounds with his book (Watt, 1981). This 300 ms sample is
an example of what he termed hard crepitus – described as ‘foot- to the ear to be a long duration burst of continuous
steps walking on gravel’. This sound appears continuous to the sound, whereas visually it appears to consist of a series
ear. However, as can be seen, visually it consists of a series of of distinct pulses (Fig. 2), in contrast to the soft crepitus
three separate impulses. This sound would be described as a creak which appears to be continuous both visually and
in this study.
audibly (Fig. 3).
Observer reliability in detecting even the presence or
sound, the first sound is either not perceived at all, or absence of TMJ sounds using non-instrumental
is perceived as following the high amplitude sound measures is poor (Hardison & Okeson, 1990; Westling,
(Watt, 1981), and (2) when the interval (latency) Helkimo & Mattiason, 1992). Eriksson, Westesson &
between a series of short pulses becomes short, only a Sjoberg (1987) recorded TMJ sounds on tape and then
single long pulse is perceived. replayed them to observers who were asked to classify
Although TMJ sounds are typically divided into two each sound as either click, crepitus or silence. Even
groups, clicks and crepitus, Prinz & Ng (1997) suggested though Erikson used high quality equipment, observers
that a classification into three basic groups could be frequently mistook tape hiss for crepitus.
more appropriate. Single short-duration sounds were Psychophysical perceptions follow the Webber–
termed clicks, multiple short-duration sounds were Feschner law which states that the response to a
labelled creaks and all long-duration sounds were called stimulus follows a power law relationship (Cameron,
crepitus. Watt provided a tape recording of TMJ sounds 1975). With visual perception, differences of less than
with his book (Watt, 1981). Figure 2 shows a 300 ms 1% in brightness, colour and size can readily be detected,
sample of what he termed ‘hard crepitus’. This sound but auditory perception is far less sensitive (Pope, 1984).
appears continuous to the ear, but as can be seen, Two thresholds are important in hearing – the absolute
visually it consists of a series of three separate impulses, threshold, i.e. the level at which a stimulus can be
and could be interpreted as a series of three closely detected (0 dB), and the just-noticeable difference, i.e.
spaced clicks, or, as a (triple) creak. Figure 3 shows an the level at which an observer can detect that one
example of Watt’s soft crepitus, this is similar to the sound is louder than another.

© 1998 Blackwell Science Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 25; 765–769


SUBJECTIVE ASSESSMENT OF TMJ SOUNDS 767

Loudness is normally expressed in dB(A) relative to amplitude of the sound was constant for all of the
the threshold of human hearing adjusted to take account following three types of triangle tests:
of the ear’s frequency response (10–12 pa 5 0 dB(A) at (i) Single sounds where only the frequency of one
100 Hz). The smallest difference in loudness (i.e. the sound differed.
just-noticeable difference) that can be discriminated is (ii) Single sounds where only the duration of one
reported to be about 10% for music and speech sound differed.
(Cameron, 1975). However, no thresholds have been (iii) Triplets of 30 ms bursts of sound where only the
reported for brief click-like sounds, such as those from latency (i.e. the interval between the components of
the TMJ. the triplet) was varied between 10 and 300 ms.
Two factors determine the perceived origin of a sound: The three items for comparison in each test type were
firstly, the amplitude difference between the two ears presented in random order to 10 normal (on self-report)
and secondly, the phase relationship at the two ears. listeners who were asked to identify the odd one out.
Thus, a sound appears to originate from the loudest Each judge performed 100 triangle tests with an interval
side and from the side at which it appears first (Blauert, of 0·5 second between items. The sensory thresholds for
1982; Hashimoto et al., 1989, 1990). In normal listening, the smallest noticeable difference in duration, frequency
a sound is transmitted through the air with a velocity and latency were determined from the results. The
of 300 m/s, and the difference in the time of arrival at threshold for a just noticeable difference was set at
each ear, i.e. the phase relationship, is used to locate 60% (i.e. the level at which 60% of assessments were
the origin of the sound in space. However, TMJ sounds correct). Note that 33·3% of correct assessments can be
detected with a stethoscope, or with microphones, travel attributed to chance.
through the tissues of the head at velocities ranging
from 64 m/s to 5000 m/s (Franke & von Gierk, 1952; Aural assessment of TMJ sounds
Potts, Chrisman & Buras, 1983; Prinz, 1985, 1991,
The ability of five normal listeners to discriminate
1998). The human brain is not designed to resolve the
between the three types of TMJ sounds, single events
location of acoustic signals of this type. If the phase and
(clicks), multiple events (creaks) and bursts of
amplitude relationships conflict, it is the amplitude
continuous sound (crepitus), and to determine their
relationship that dominates the subjective assessment
position in the cycle (opening, middle or closing) and
of location (Blauert, 1982).
their location (unilateral left or right, or bilateral) was
tested. In addition, observers were asked to classify the
sounds as high, medium or low amplitude.
Materials and methods
To allow precise control of the sounds to be presented
When assessing TMJ sounds by ear, it is important to to the listeners, a set of test sounds were created. This
monitor the frequency of the sound, its duration and was done by splicing together digital samples from
the interval between events, since it is these factors actual TMJ sounds, recorded using microphones placed
which distinguish types of TMJ sound. The just- in the ears, that had been digitised at 44·1 kHz with 16
noticeable difference can be assessed using a triangle bit resolution (i.e. CD quality) using a Sound-Blaster
test. In this type of test, the observer is presented with 16 sound card*. For instance, a typical synthetic cycle
three items and asked to identify the odd one out. might consist of a tooth contact sound followed by
Where there is no discernible difference, the observer 300 ms of silence, a 300 ms sample containing a click,
is forced to guess. By varying the size of the difference 300 ms of silence and finally another tooth contact
between the test samples, the just-noticeable difference sound.
can be determined by observing the magnitude of the Three examples of each type of TMJ sound were used
difference at which a set proportion of the observers in constructing the cycles. The three clicks used were
correctly identify the ‘odd one out’ (O’Mahoney, 1988). of different durations (40 ms, 20 ms and 15 ms) but
A series of triangle tests were used to determine a were modified so that they had the same peak
typical listener’s sensitivity to duration, frequency and amplitude. The three examples of crepitus were formed
latency. Sounds were produced using the computer’s
internal loudspeaker to produce bursts of sound. The *Creative Inc., Singapore.

© 1998 Blackwell Science Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 25; 765–769


768 J . F. P R I N Z

from a single recording where the duration of the while for latency observers could detect differences of
crepitus was adjusted by editing the sample to create 30 ms (Fig. 6). Note that in this experiment the bursts
crepitant sounds of 50 ms, 100 ms and 150 ms in length. of sound generated by the loudspeaker had a sharp
Three creaks were likewise formed from a single actual attack and decay in contrast to the more gradual attack
recording, the interval between the events being and decay of clicks and creaks occurring in TMJ sounds;
adjusted by adding or removing silence between the hence discrimination of the individual bursts was easier
individual events. Thus, it was possible to create three in the artificial situation than when listening to actual
creaks with an inter-event spacing (latency) of 20 ms, TMJ sounds. There was a positive correlation of the
50 ms and 100 ms. number of correct assessments with both frequency
During playback, the amplitude relationship (balance) (r 5 0·48; P , 0·05) and duration (r 5 0·58; P , 0·05),
of the left and right channels was adjustable in steps. i.e. at higher frequencies and with longer duration, the
Ten 2 dB steps were used from a maximum left discriminatory ability of the listeners improved. There
(–10 dB) to maximum right (110 dB). The volume of were no significant differences between listeners.
each section of the cycle could also be precisely Ignoring the ‘don’t know’ responses in the evaluation
controlled in 255 steps. Synthetic cycles were of the TMJ sounds, 52% of the clicks, 82% of the
constructed by splicing together the samples of tooth creaks, 75% of the crepitus and 80% of the silent sound
contact sounds, silence, clicks, creaks and crepitus in sequences were correctly identified. All 250 mV sounds
different orders, based on a randomized Latin squares were classed by listeners as high amplitude. Those
design. In these tests, although the balance, amplitude having an amplitude between 150 mV and 200 mV
and character of the sounds varied, only three positions were generally classed as medium amplitude. Those
were presented, at 25, 50 or 75% of the cycle (early, below 100 mV were classed as low amplitude or silence.
middle and late). Observers performed poorly on locating the sounds
The synthetic cycles were played back from the in space, correctly classifying only 42% of the TMJ
computer via headphones. First, a series of three sounds as left, right or bilateral; however, this may have
examples of each sound type were presented to the been due to left–right confusion since ANOVA showed
listener as a training and calibration exercise. Following significant differences for listener. After a re-
a verbal description, each sound in the training set was classification of the sounds as central or non-central,
then repeated five times. The computer then presented performance improved to 77% and there were no
a series of 25 different synthetic sound sequences to longer any significant differences between listeners.
the listener, each sequence being repeated five times. Performance was poor in classifying sounds as early,
The listener was then asked to indicate the origin of middle or late, with only 49% of the sounds being
the sound (left, right or bilateral), the position in the correctly classified.
cycle (early, middle or late), a subjective evaluation of
the amplitude of the sound (high medium or low) and
Discussion
finally to categorize the sound as either a click, creak,
crepitus or silence. Listeners’ assessments were The results presented here cast some doubt on the
automatically recorded by a program, which allowed validity of the previous literature on TMJ sounds where
the listeners to code responses. For the assessment of data were collected using a stethoscope and/or
the origin of the sound, responses were also categorized palpation. Of course, in the clinical situation the dentist
as central or non-central since it appeared that several has a great deal more information than just sound.
of the listeners may have confused left and right when There are also visual and tactile sources. Although
responding. listeners recognized the difficulty of the triangle test,
they expressed some surprise when presented with the
results of their evaluations, most believing that they
Results
had performed better than was the case.
In the evaluation of the computer-generated sounds, In the tests using synthetic cycles, every cycle was
observers were very sensitive to frequency differences, constructed with identical tooth contact sounds at the
and were able to detect a difference of 2 Hz. For duration start and finish and all had the same overall duration
differences, the just noticeable difference was 60 ms (1 s). This test was in some ways simpler than the real

© 1998 Blackwell Science Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 25; 765–769


SUBJECTIVE ASSESSMENT OF TMJ SOUNDS 769

life situation where both cycle times and tooth contact the advantages of objective instrumental recording of
sounds are variable. Therefore, it might be expected TMJ sounds and automated classification by computer.
that listeners would perform better in this type of test
than they would in the clinical situation. References
In the categorization of the sounds into the three
BLAUERT, J. (1982) Binaural localisation. In: Binaural Effects in
groups (clicks, creaks and crepitus) subjects performed
Normal and Impaired Hearing (eds O.J. Pederson & T. Poulsen),
well, correctly classifying 72% of the examples Scandinavian Audiology. Supplement. 15.
presented to them, In a similar test using a visual CAMERON, O. (1975) Medical Physics. Springer Verlag, Berlin.
classification of the sounds (Prinz & Ng, 1997) 82% of ERIKSON, L., WESTESSON, P.L. & SJOBERG, H. (1987) Observer
a set of 42 TMJ sounds were correctly classified. performance in describing temporomandibular joint sounds.
Cranio, 5, 33.
Viewers performed better than listeners at classifying
FRANKE, E. & VON GIERK, H. (1952) The motions relative to the
sounds into the three groups, even though for the visual skull and their influence on hearing by bone conduction. Journal
assessment all of the sounds were different whereas for of Acoustic Society of America, 24, 142.
the auditory assessment, only a small subset of sounds HARDISON, J.D. & OKESON, J.P. (1990) Comparison of three clinical
were used throughout the tests. The ability of observers techniques for evaluating joint sounds. Cranio, 8, 307.
HASHIMOTO, K., TAKEHANA, S., ITO, Y., TAKENAKA, M., SUZUKI, N. &
to correctly identify the three groups of sounds suggests
ABE, T. (1990) A study of how to distinguish the side on which
that there are three (clicks, creaks and crepitus) rather
TMJ noise is occurring. Aichi Gakuin Daigaku Shigakkai Shi,
than just two (clicks and crepitus), natural groupings 28, 1151.
of TMJ sounds. Listeners were also surprisingly HASHIMOTO, K., TAKEHANA, S., TAKENAKA, M., YAMAMOTO, T., MITUYA,
consistent at classifying the sounds as high, medium or H. & SUZUKI, N. (1989) A study of how to distinguish the
low amplitude, even though no training was given as occurring side of TMJ noise multiple click. Nihon Ago Kansetsu
Gakkai Zasshi, 1, 11.
to this categorization.
O’MAHONEY, M. (1988) Sensory differences and preference testing.
Although listeners performed well at distinguishing In: Applied Sensory Analysis of Foods (ed. H.R. Moskovitz) p. 153.
the type of sound and its amplitude, they were poor at POPE, J.A. (1984) Medical Physics. Heinemann Educational
distinguishing a sound’s position (early, middle or late) Publications, London.
in the envelope of movement. In this test only 49% of POTTS, R.O., CHRISMAN, D.A. & BURAS, E.M. (1983) The dynamical
properties of human skin in vivo. Journal of Biomechanics, 16, 365.
classifications were correct and of these 33% could be
PRINZ, J.F. (1985) An instrument for the assessment of dental
attributed to chance. occlusion. UK patent No 8509307.
The difficulty experienced in classifying the position PRINZ, J.F. (1991) Computer Aided Gnathosonic Analysis
of sounds (early, middle or late) has important M.Med.Sci Thesis. University of Sheffield.
implications. The main diagnostic criteria used to PRINZ, J.F. (1996) Physical Mechanisms in the Pathogenesis of
differentiate defects of form from reciprocal clicking Temporomandibular Joint Sounds. PhD Thesis. The University
of Hong Kong.
(disc displacement with reduction) is that the defect of
PRINZ, J.F. (1998) Correlation of the characteristics of
form occurs in the same place on opening and closing temporomandibular joint and tooth contact sounds. Journal of
and the reciprocal click does not. These differences are Oral Rehabilitation, 25, 194.
small and the results suggest that it is doubtful that an PRINZ, J.F. & NG, K.W. (1997) Classification of TMJ Sounds. Archives
observer could distinguish the two types of sounds on of Oral Biology, 41, 631.
RUKKER, R. (1986) The Fourth Dimension and How to Get There.
this basis alone. However, both opening and closing
Penguin Books, London.
clicks due to defects of form are of similar frequency WATT, D.M. (1981) Gnathosonics and Occlusal Dynamics. Praeger,
whereas in reciprocal clicking the opening click is of New York.
higher frequency than the closing click and this WESTLING, L., HELKIMO, E. & MATTIASSON, A. (1992) Observer
difference in frequency can be detected by the human variation in functional examination of the temporomandibular
joint. Cranio, 6, 202.
ear.
The results presented here demonstrate the difficulties Correspondence: Dr J.F. Prinz, Department of Prosthetic Dentistry,
encountered by clinicians attempting to classify sounds London Hospital, Dental Institute, Turner St, London E1 2AD,
using a stethoscope alone and also serve to underline U.K. E-mail: jfprinz@mds.qmw.ac.uk

© 1998 Blackwell Science Ltd, Journal of Oral Rehabilitation 25; 765–769

Potrebbero piacerti anche