Sei sulla pagina 1di 15

J Neuroimmune Pharmacol

DOI 10.1007/s11481-015-9636-7

INVITED REVIEW

Novelty Seeking and Drug Addiction in Humans and Animals:


From Behavior to Molecules
Taylor Wingo1 & Tanseli Nesil 1 & Jung-Seok Choi1,2 & Ming D. Li1

Received: 9 August 2015 / Accepted: 13 October 2015


# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2015

Abstract Global treatment of drug addiction costs society Keywords Addiction . Drugs of abuse . Heritability .
billions of dollars annually, but current psychopharmacologi- Molecular connections . Novelty seeking
cal therapies have not been successful at desired rates. The
increasing number of individuals suffering from substance
abuse has turned attention to what makes some people more Introduction
vulnerable to drug addiction than others. One personality trait
that stands out as a contributing factor is novelty seeking. Globally, 243 million individuals suffer from drug addiction,
Novelty seeking, affected by both genetic and environmental costing society through increased health care costs, lost pro-
factors, is defined as the tendency to desire novel stimuli and ductivity, and crime (Burns 2014). Worldwide treatment of
environments. It can be measured in humans through ques- drug addiction costs more than $250 billion per year, but cur-
tionnaires and in rodents using behavioral tasks. On the be- rent psychopharmacological therapies have not reached the
havioral level, both human and rodent studies demonstrate desired success rates; an estimated 5 % of the world’s popu-
that high novelty seeking can predict the initiation of drug lation remains plagued with addiction, and this number is
use and a transition to compulsive drug use and create a pro- increasing at an alarming rate (Burns 2014). We argue that
pensity to relapse. These predictions are valid for several failed efforts to combat substance abuse are attributable to
drugs of abuse, such as alcohol, nicotine, cocaine, amphet- the inability to define what makes some individuals vulnerable
amine, and opiates. On the molecular level, both novelty seek- to addiction. Of various vulnerability factors, individual dif-
ing and addiction are modulated by the central reward system ferences in personality traits are an important risk factor for
in the brain. Dopamine is the primary neurotransmitter in- addiction. Although many personality traits have been impli-
volved in the overlapping neural substrates of both parame- cated in this trait, novelty seeking stands out as key.
ters. In sum, the novelty-seeking trait can be valuable for Research findings from studies on both human and animal
predicting individual vulnerability to drug addiction and for models of drug addiction indicate that novelty seeking can
generating successful treatment for patients with substance predict the risk of compulsive use of addictive drugs. Human
abuse disorders. studies have shown that novelty seeking is associated with
vulnerability to use and relapse of drug abuse (Zuckerman
and Neeb 1979; Cloninger 1987). High novelty seeking in-
* Ming D. Li
creases one’s risk of using addictive drugs (Piazza et al. 1989;
limd586@outlook.com Belin and Deroche-Gamonet 2012), and multiple studies have
found that individuals who display high novelty-seeking traits
1
use more drugs of abuse than do low novelty seekers (Piazza
Department of Psychiatry and Neurobehavioral Sciences, University
of Virginia, 450 Ray C Hunt Drive, Suite G-170,
et al. 1989; Koob 2000; Mahoney et al. 2015). Further, re-
Charlottesville, VA 22903, USA search has demonstrated a positive correlation between nov-
2
Department of Psychiatry, SMG-SNU Boramae Medical Center and
elty seeking and relapse rates for several drugs of abuse
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, Seoul National among addicts (Meszaros et al. 1999; Kahler et al. 2009;
University College of Medicine, Seoul, Republic of Korea Ismael and Baltieri 2014).
Novelty seeking as a predisposing personality trait has colleagues, sensation seeking is a Btrait defined by the seeking
been validated in animal models of multiple drugs of abuse of novel sensations, and the willingness to take physical, so-
and reflects a heritable tendency toward exploratory behavior cial, legal and financial risks for the sake of such experiences^
and desire for novel sensations (Cain et al. 2005). In rodents, (Zuckerman et al. 1993). Novelty serves as a signal that pro-
reactivity to novel stimuli correlates with drug self- vides motivation to pursue rewards. Individuals who seek
administration and drug-seeking behaviors (Piazza et al. novel experiences display overpowering motivational strength
1989; Blanchard et al. 2009; Flagel et al. 2010). After rats that leads to a decreased ability to control their actions, such as
are categorized as high or low responders on the basis of compulsive drug use. Individuals who are characterized as
behavioral reactivity to a novel environment, high-responder high sensation seekers often have high impulsiveness, explor-
rats consistently self-administer drugs such as amphetamine, atory excitability, extravagance, and disorderliness
cocaine, nicotine, and alcohol at higher rates than do low (Zuckerman and Neeb 1979; Cloninger 1987; Hiroi and
responders (Piazza et al. 1989; Hooks et al. 1991; Ambrosio Agatsuma 2005). High novelty seekers often display high
et al. 1995; Suto et al. 2001; Nadal et al. 2002; Belin and experience-seeking behaviors, boredom susceptibility, and
Deroche-Gamonet 2012; Manzo et al. 2014). greater rates of bipolar disorder (Zuckerman and Neeb 1979;
Molecularly, addiction and novelty seeking show an over- Zuckerman et al. 1993; Howard et al. 1997). These individuals
lap in the mesocorticolimbic reward pathway of the brain, exhibit higher than average rates of sociopathy, such as crim-
which are modulated by similar neurotransmitter systems inality and substance abuse (Zuckerman et al. 1993; Hiroi and
and neural circuits (Krebs et al. 2011). Exposure to novel Agatsuma 2005; Flagel et al. 2014).
stimuli activates the neural substrates in the brain that also Novelty-seeking behavior has been correlated with vulner-
mediate the rewarding and reinforcing effects of drugs of ability to psychopathological disorders and drug addiction in
abuse. Response to novelty is modulated by the dopaminergic humans (Zuckerman and Neeb 1979). Considerable evidence
and non-dopaminergic (e.g., serotonergic, GABAergic, gluta- has implicated novelty seeking as a behavioral feature in drug
matergic, and opioid) systems in the brain (Koob 2000). These addicts that could be innate (Zuckerman et al. 1993) or result
systems are involved in acquisition and maintenance of drug from drug abuse (Ersche et al. 2010). Defining the novelty-
addiction (Kalivas and Volkow 2011). However, it is less clear seeking phenotype in humans with psychiatric disorders, in-
whether novelty seeking mediates the development of com- cluding addiction, will give us more detailed information re-
pulsive drug use, or if this trait reflects a behavioral adaptation garding potential treatment. Consequently, researchers at-
induced by drugs of abuse. Further, individuals who display tempt to measure novelty seeking in order to predict vulnera-
high novelty seeking may be more prone to experimenting bility to these psychiatric diseases including addictions.
with drugs. Elucidating the molecular mechanisms underlying A parallel between the sensation-seeking trait in humans is
novelty-seeking behavior in drug-addicted patients will help novelty-seeking behavior among animals. In rodents, novelty
us to determine the unknowns in this debate. seeking has been defined as a preference for novel objects or
In this review, we focus on the findings from both human environments; high novelty-seeking rodents display enhanced
and animal studies and discuss the behavioral and molecular exploratory behavior toward novel situations, objects, or stim-
connections between novelty seeking and drug addiction. The uli (Piazza et al. 1989). In rodents, preference for novelty has
questions we address are: (1) what exactly is novelty seeking been correlated with higher sensitivity to the reinforcing ef-
as it is currently understood in the field? (2) what behaviors fects of many drugs of abuse, including amphetamine, co-
related to drug addiction can be predicted using novelty seek- caine, nicotine, and alcohol (Piazza et al. 1989; Hooks et al.
ing? (3) is the use of all addictive drugs correlated with nov- 1991; Ambrosio et al. 1995; Suto et al. 2001; Nadal et al.
elty seeking, or does only a particular group of drugs stand out 2002; Belin and Deroche-Gamonet 2012; Manzo et al.
with a strong correlation to this behavioral trait? (4) because 2014). Measuring novelty-seeking behavior in rodent models
novelty seeking can predict vulnerability to drug addiction, that mimic human psychiatric disorders including addiction
how can we test and quantify this behavior in humans and will help to elucidate the neurobehavioral details of psycho-
animals? (5) what are the molecular bases of novelty seeking pathological conditions.
and its connection to drug addiction? And, (6) what future
directions would generate valuable research for the field?
Measurement of Novelty Seeking in Humans

Definition of Novelty Seeking Novelty seeking is typically measured using questionnaires in


humans and behavioral tests in animals. In humans, novelty
Novelty seeking is one of the defining characteristics of a seeking is often measured alongside harm avoidance and re-
sensation-seeking personality in humans (Cloninger 1987; ward dependence, which are three heritable personality traits
Hiroi and Agatsuma 2005). As defined by Zuckerman and implicated in the vulnerability to addiction (Zuckerman and
Neeb 1979). Harm avoidance is a tendency to respond in- novelty-seeking scale from the TPQ is positively correlated
tensely to aversive stimuli and to learn to avoid punishment with the behavioral task of risk preference, as measured by a
and novelty. Reward dependence is a propensity to respond probabilistic decision-making task and negatively correlated
intensely to stimuli that produce a reward. In particular, indi- with the brain activation elicited by risk prediction in the
viduals who display high novelty seeking, low harm avoid- insula, striatum, and supplementary motor area (Wang et al.
ance, and high reward dependence may be at greater risk for 2015).
substance abuse (Zuckerman and Neeb 1979). Cloninger and
associates suggest that the three dimensions of harm avoid-
ance, novelty seeking, and reward dependence are correlated Measurement of Novelty Seeking in Animals
with high serotonergic activity, low dopaminergic activity, and
low noradrenergic activity, respectively (Cloninger et al. Several behavioral tests have been proposed for measurement
1991), although more recent studies have proposed different of novelty-seeking behavior in rodents (Table 2). The open
neurobiological explanations for these aspects of personality field arena test is the most commonly used behavioral para-
(Hooks et al. 1994; Tournier et al. 2013; Flagel et al. 2014). digm. Depicted in Fig. 1, this test measures locomotor activity
Three of the most commonly used assessments of novelty by providing an inescapable environment in which rodents are
seeking in humans are shown in Table 1. Briefly, the Tridi- placed in an apparatus. The following behavioral parameters
mensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) measures person- are recorded: time spent in the center versus the sides and
ality dimensions (harm avoidance, novelty seeking, and re- movements against a floor grid. However, the open field arena
ward dependence) in a 100-item, self-administered, true/false test forces animals to be in an enclosed environment, provok-
test format (Cloninger 1987; Cloninger et al. 1993). The TPQ ing a strong fear response (Cain et al. 2005). Rodents display
is intended to correspond closely to the underlying genetic elevated serum concentrations of corticosterone when ex-
structure of personality rather than to individual adaptations posed to inescapable novelty tasks such as the open field test
to experiences (Cloninger et al. 1991). A similar test, the (Cain et al. 2005). Anxiety-related behaviors outweigh explor-
Zuckerman Sensation Seeking Scale (SSS), specifically mea- atory behaviors during the test session. Therefore, this test
sures sensation seeking (Zuckerman 2007). The SSS contains may provide detailed information about anxiolytic drugs and
40 items in a forced-choice format and has four subscales. their effects on novelty-seeking behavior. Another disadvan-
Cloninger’s Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI) tage of this test is that locomotion can differ independently
measures a constellation of personality traits (Cloninger from neophilia (Brown and Nemes 2008). One test that over-
1987; Cloninger et al. 1993). The TCI contains 226 items in comes the aforementioned problems is the hole-board test,
a true/false format, encompassing seven subscales in two ma- which more directly measures novelty-seeking behavior by
jor categories. providing free-choice novelty. As shown in Fig. 2, this test
Other assessments of novelty-seeking behavior in humans requires that animals be placed in a hole-board, an enclosed
include the Zuckerman-Kuhlman Personality Questionnaire box with equally spaced holes (Boissier and Simon 1962).
(ZKPQ), a true/false test that measures impulsivity and sensa- Researchers measure dependent variables that include nose
tion seeking (Zuckerman and Neeb 1979), as well as other poking and head dipping, which directly represent exploratory
self-reporting questionnaires and surveys that quantify behavior independent of locomotor activity. Other behavioral
novelty-seeking behavior in various formats. The ZKPQ is tests used are the elevated plus maze and the light-dark box
designed to measure a variety of personality traits in addition test. These two tests are based on the innate aversion of ro-
to risk-taking behaviors (Zuckerman et al. 1993). This ques- dents to brightly lit environments; high novelty seekers would
tionnaire consists of 99 items in a true/false format, including spend more time in the open or light regions of each test than
five subscales plus items to detect inattention and socially would low novelty seekers (Hascoet et al. 2001; Crawford
desirable responding (Zuckerman et al. 1993). Finally, the et al. 2013). However, these two tests directly measure
Arnett Inventory of Sensation Seeking (Ahituv et al. 2007) anxiety- and stress-related behaviors and subsequent effects
is designed to assess sensation seeking, defined specifically on novelty-seeking behaviors. In the elevated plus maze test, a
as Bthe need for novel and intense stimulation^ (Arnett 1994). maze is elevated above the surface and generally consists of
The AIS contains 20 items on 4-point scales to which partic- four arms; two arms are considered Bopen^ because of full
ipants respond on the basis of agreement (Arnett 1994). The exposure, while the other two arms are considered Bclosed^
two subscales in this inventory are novelty and intensity. because of the wall enclosure (Crawford et al. 2013). The
There have been studies investigating correlations among as- light-dark box test consists of an apparatus that contains a
sessments of novelty-seeking behavior in humans. For exam- dark, enclosed compartment and a brightly illuminated
ple, Zuckerman and Cloninger (1996) reported a high corre- Baversive^ partition (Hascoet et al. 2001). In fact, researchers
lation between Zuckerman’s impulsive sensation-seeking who selectively breed rats for novelty-seeking behavior often
scale and TCI’s novelty-seeking scale. Moreover, the use these two tests. In short, the hole-board test may be more
Table 1 Common tests used to measure novelty seeking in humans

Measure Sub-measure Sub-measure component Number of questions

Tridimensional Personality Novelty seeking (NS) scale Exploratory excitability versus stoic rigidity 9
Questionnaire (TPQ) Impulsiveness versus reflection 8
Extravagance versus reserve 7
Disorderliness versus regimentation 10
Harm avoidance (HA) scale Anticipatory worry and pessimism 10
Fear of uncertainty 7
Shyness with strangers 7
Fatigability and asthenia 10
Reward dependence (RD) scale Sentimentality 5
Persistence 9
Attachment 11
Dependence 5
Zuckerman Sensation Thrill and adventure seeking (TAS) Desire to engage in sports or activities involving 10
Seeking Scale (SSS) speed and danger
Disinhibition (Dis) Desire for social and sexual disinhibition 10
Experience seeking (ES) Desire for experience through the mind and senses, 10
travel, and a non-conforming lifestyle
Boredom susceptibility (BS) Aversion to repetition, routine, and dull people 10
Temperament and Character Novelty seeking (Temperament) Exploratory excitability vs rigidity 11
Inventory (TCI) Impulsiveness vs reflection 10
Extravagance vs reserve 9
Disorderliness vs regimentation 10
Harm avoidance (Temperament) Anticipatory worry vs optimism 11
Fear of uncertainty vs confidence 7
Shyness vs gregariousness 8
Fatigability and asthenia vs vigor 9
Reward dependence (Temperament) Sentimentality vs insensitiveness 10
Attachment vs detachment 8
Dependence vs independence 6
Persistence (Temperament) Single scale 8
Self-directedness (Character) Responsibility vs blaming 8
Purposeful vs lack of goal direction 8
Resourcefulness vs apathy 5
Self-acceptance vs self-striving 11
Congruent second nature 12
Cooperativeness (Character) Social acceptance vs intolerance 8
Social disinterest 7
Helpfulness vs unhelpfulness 8
Compassion vs revengefulness 10
Pure-hearted vs self-serving 9
Self-transcendence (Character) Self-forgetful vs self-conscious 11
Transpersonal identification 9
Spiritual acceptance vs materialism 13

advantageous to researchers who are examining novelty- Genetic Effects on Novelty Seeking and Addiction
seeking behavior among naïve rodents. In contrast, re-
searchers may choose to utilize the open field arena test, the Although most personality traits have a heritability (i.e., the
elevated plus maze test, and the light-dark box test when proportion of phenotype variability that can be attributed to
studying selectively bred rodent lines. genetic variation) of only approximately 30 %, the heritability
Table 2 Common tests used to measure novelty seeking in rodents

Test Apparatus Measure Advantages Disadvantages Reference

Hole-board test Board that contains Head dipping, nose Measures emotionality Repeated exposure to novel (Boissier and
equally spaced poking, grooming, in addition to novelty environment can result in Simon 1962)
holes rearing seeking fewer head-dipping
behaviors
Open-field Arena with walls to Vertical, horizontal Locomotor response to a Behavior recorded can reflect (Welker, 1957)
arena test prevent escape movement, time mobile drug of can predict anxiety instead of novelty
in center versus time individual vulnerability seeking behavior
mobile in sides to addiction
Elevated Plus-shaped apparatus Time spent in open arms Locomotor activity can be Behavior recorded can reflect (Crawford et al. 2013)
plus maze containing 4 arms at versus time spent determined in addition to anxiety in contrast to novelty
right angles to each in closed arms novelty seeking behavior seeking/exploratory behavior
other; (2 open arms,
2 closed arms)
Light-dark Apparatus containing Time spent in light box Relatively inexpensive and Behavior recorded can reflect (Hascoet et al. 2001)
box test a small dark box versus time spent rapid test to determine anxiety or anxiogenic properties
(1/3rd of apparatus) in dark box novelty seeking behavior of drugs in contrast to novelty
and large, brightly seeking/exploratory behavior
illuminated box
(2/3rd of apparatus)

of novelty seeking was estimated to reach nearly 60 % in novelty-seeking behavior result in a consistent phenotype,
humans (Zuckerman et al. 1993). This estimate had been such that the phenotypic differences in response to novelty
quantified in non-human primate studies as well (Fairbanks can be predicted with >95 % certainty (Cummings et al.
et al. 2011; Greenwood et al. 2013), with an estimated value 2011). An HR line exhibits relatively high exploratory behav-
ranging from 0.35 to 0.43 (Fairbanks et al. 2011). Further, ior, whereas an LR line displays low affinity for a novel envi-
novelty seeking as a heritable behavioral trait has received ronment. Stead and colleagues used these selected lines to
support from selective breeding of High Responder (HR) determine the heritability of the novelty-seeking trait in
and Low Responder rats in the open field test (Stead et al. Sprague-Dawley rats; it proved to be 35.8 % in the first gen-
2006; Flagel et al. 2014). Eight generations of selection for eration. In other words, more than a third of the phenotypic

Video Camera HVS Image VP112 Digitizer Video Camera HVS Image VP112 Digitizer

Special soGware is used to track the rodent's position as it


moves from the center to side areas of the arena. Digitized
The head dip and nose poke behavior of rodents can be detected by
pictures are created within the soGware in relation to the rat’s
special motion sensors that are located at the bottom of each hole.
position to XY coordinates in the arena.The number of entries Sensor-detected nose pokes and head dips in each hole are then
to the center and side areas are recorded automatically and by transferred to the computer and are recorded. These novelty seeking
experimenters as the rat moves through the arena. related parameters are also manually recorded by experimenters. By using
the video camera system, the mobility of rats can be also monitored.
Fig. 1 Illustration of open field arena test, one of the most commonly
used behavioral paradigms to test novelty-seeking behavior in rodents. Fig. 2 Illustration of hole-board test, a free-choice novelty-seeking be-
This test provides an inescapable environment in which rodents are havioral task test for rodents. The apparatus is an enclosed box with
placed in an apparatus, with the following parameters being recorded equally spaced holes; the following behavioral parameters are measured
for each animal: time spent in the center versus the sides and movements during the test session: head dipping, nose poking, rearing, and grooming.
against a floor grid. Because of the enclosed environment, the open field Head dipping and nose poking represent exploratory behaviors that are
test provokes a strong fear response in rodents, as measured by elevated independent of locomotor activity. Compared with the open field arena
corticosterone concentrations. This test directly measures locomotion, test (see Fig. 1), the hole-board test more directly measures novelty-
which can vary independently of neophilia. As such, this test is utilized seeking behavior, because it has not been shown to elevate the cortico-
by researchers who are testing anxiety in addition to novelty-seeking sterone concentration. The hole-board test is advantageous to researchers
behavior in rodents studying novelty-seeking behavior in naïve rodents
variance within the founding generation’s offspring was attrib- dependence (Zuckerman and Neeb 1979; Khan et al. 2005;
utable to heritable factors. After the fourth generation, the rate Flagel et al. 2014). Studies have shown that high novelty
increased to more than 60 % (Stead et al. 2006). seekers use more alcohol and illicit substances than low nov-
Further, human and animal studies have strongly indicated elty seekers (Zuckerman and Neeb 1979; Cloninger 1987).
that the likelihood of abusing drugs is heritable (Han et al. Taken together, novelty seeking provides a broad spectrum
1999; Kendler et al. 2003; Agrawal and Lynskey 2008). Her- on which researchers can predict vulnerability to many types
itabilities have been determined for addiction to several types of psychiatric disorders. Animal models present a significant
of drugs, including alcohol, nicotine, amphetamine, cocaine, investigatory platform to determine the link between novelty-
and opiates. Walters showed that the upper limit for heritabil- seeking behavior and addiction.
ity of alcohol misuse in humans was 0.36 (Walters 2002). Li
and colleagues determined that the heritability of smoking
initiation among humans ranges from 0.37 to 0.55 and that Behavioral Connection Between Novelty Seeking
of smoking persistence is 0.46 to 0.59 (Li et al. 2003). Nesil and Addiction
and colleagues demonstrated that the heritability for high nic-
otine intake was 0.26 in rats after nine generations of selective Animal Studies
breeding (Nesil et al. 2013). Heritability for amphetamine self-
administration was estimated to be 0.74 among inbred rats The connection between novelty-seeking behavior and drug
(Meyer et al. 2010) and that for cocaine abuse in humans addiction has been widely studied in outbred and inbred rat
ranges from 0.42 to 0.79 (Agrawal et al. 2012). The heritabil- strains using multiple drugs of abuse. Individual differences in
ity for opiate dependence in humans is estimated to be 0.43 vulnerability to addiction were investigated in animal studies
(Weyandt 2005). beginning more than 20 years ago. Piazza and colleagues first
Taken together, these findings indicate that the propensity showed the correlation between novelty-seeking behavior and
for substance abuse reflects in part a genetic predisposition drug addiction in outbred Sprague-Dawley rats (Piazza et al.
that manifests as certain personality traits. These traits, such 1989). Vulnerability to amphetamine intake was predicted by
as novelty seeking, lead an individual to respond strongly to novelty seeking, measured by the open field arena test. Rats
novel stimuli, including drugs of abuse (Flagel et al. 2014). with high novelty-seeking scores were more likely to self-
administer amphetamine and at higher doses than low novelty
seekers (Piazza et al. 1989). A decade later, Bevins and col-
Novelty Seeking and Addiction leagues demonstrated that there are individual differences in
novelty-seeking behavior by using the novel place-preference
Vulnerability to substance abuse is multifaceted, modulated test in outbred rats. In this task, rats were grouped into high
by genetic, environmental, and neural components (Fig. 3). and low responders to novelty and were subsequently tested
Even so, high novelty seekers have a greater propensity to first for their responsively to amphetamine. The results showed
experiment with drugs. These individuals are thought to grav- that high responders had greater sensitivity to the activating
itate toward novel stimuli and respond rapidly to cues for effects of this drug (Bevins et al. 1997).
reward despite potential punishment, an indicator of drug ad- Considerable data have shown results consistent with those
diction (Flagel et al. 2014). To that end, novelty-seeking be- of these studies; cocaine, nicotine, morphine, and ethanol also
havior is positively correlated with vulnerability to externaliz- display the positive correlation between novelty seeking and
ing disorders such as drug dependence (Finn et al. 2002). High drug intake (Flagel et al. 2014). Belin and Deroche-Gamonet
novelty seekers have a propensity for earlier and more varied demonstrated that compulsive use of cocaine could be predict-
use of drugs in comparison with the general population ed by novelty seeking (Belin and Deroche-Gamonet 2012).
(Sutker et al. 1978; Cloninger 1987). These individuals often Piazza and colleagues showed that high novelty seekers of
report pleasure and curiosity as motivation for initial drug use outbred Sprague-Dawley rats began cocaine self-
and continue seeking and using drugs of abuse in order to administration after receiving low cocaine doses and a vertical
maintain these satisfying feelings (Sutker et al. 1978). In ad- shift in dose–response functions (Piazza et al. 2000). The
dition, novelty-seeking, combined with impulsivity is related study reported by Gulley and colleagues highlights the influ-
with craving and relapse in alcohol dependence (Evren et al. ence of a single cocaine exposure on subsequent behavioral
2012). Furthermore, maladaptive levels of novelty-seeking responses to the drug. In their study, rats were grouped into
behavior may mediate internalizing disorders, such as depres- high and low responders by response to initial cocaine expo-
sion, anxiety, and panic disorders, which also can lead indi- sure in a novel environment (Gulley et al. 2003). High novelty
viduals to initiate drug use in order to self-medicate (Khan seekers of outbred Sprague-Dawley rats also self-
et al. 2005). High novelty seeking is not a broad predictor of administered amphetamine at doses that did not induce self-
all psychiatric disorders, but it is highly predictive of drug administration in low novelty seekers (Piazza et al. 1989).
Fig. 3 Contributing factors to
drug addiction, portraying the
multifaceted vulnerability to
substance abuse and its relation to
novelty-seeking behavior. There
is no clear cause-and-effect rela-
tion between novelty-seeking be-
havior and drug addiction. Rather,
these two phenotypes synergisti-
cally influence each other, along
with an intricate network of con-
tributing factors including age,
genetic predisposition, use of ad-
dictive drugs, and environmental
factors

High novelty-seeking behavior also is predictive of the degree to develop HR and LR lines and to study novelty seeking in
of nicotine self-administration among outbred Sprague- the context of addiction (Stead et al. 2006; Davis et al. 2008).
Dawley rats (Suto et al. 2001). High novelty seekers self- Davis and colleagues confirmed the reliability of using these
administered nicotine more frequently and worked more to rat lines to examine novelty seeking, as HR rats responded
acquire the drug than did low novelty seekers (Suto et al. with significantly greater locomotor activity in a novel envi-
2001). Lewis, Fischer 344 (F344), NCI-Black Reiter (Cum- ronment (Davis et al. 2008). The HR rats also displayed lower
mings et al. 2011), and August-Copenhagen Irish (ACI) rats anxiety-like behaviors than did LR rats, indicating that HR
exhibit a positive correlation between locomotor response to a rats have greater exploratory behavior as well as less anxiety
novel environment and morphine self-administration (Davis et al. 2008). These results imply that these behavioral
(Ambrosio et al. 1995). Zheng and colleagues demonstrated traits work synergistically to increase drug self-administration
that the response to novelty in both juvenile and adult (Belin and Deroche-Gamonet 2012). Flagel and colleagues
Sprague-Dawley rats determined the magnitude of morphine’s found consistent results among selectively bred HR and LR
rewarding effects (Zheng et al. 2003). Finally, studies show a lines, in that HRs displayed an increase in drug-seeking be-
positive correlation between ethanol self-administration and havior, greater response to the rewarding effects of drugs such
novelty-seeking behavior (Nadal et al. 2002; Manzo et al. as cocaine, and a greater tendency to relapse after treatment
2014). Pelloux and colleagues recently demonstrated that nov- (Flagel et al. 2010). Additionally, after cocaine administration,
elty seeking strongly predicted the amount of ethanol consump- high novelty-seeking rats displayed greater locomotor activity
tion among Wistar rats (Pelloux et al. 2015). All in all, novelty- in a novel environment than did low novelty seekers (Flagel
seeking behavior among outbred rat lines can predict with high et al. 2010). These researchers concluded that selectively bred
consistency the intake rates of several drugs of abuse. HR rats exhibit Bbehavioral disinhibition^ and may represent
Recent studies using outbred Sprague-Dawley rats have one end of the continuum of addiction-vulnerable populations
suggested new insights into the treatment of addiction. High (Flagel et al. 2010). Rodent behavioral experiments
novelty seekers preferentially self-administer natural rewards confirming human findings provide further evidence of nov-
such as saccharin over addictive drugs such as cocaine elty seeking as a contributing factor in drug abuse.
(Vanhille et al. 2015). In turn, the vulnerability to addiction
as a result of high novelty seeking may be offset by providing Human Studies
non-drug rewards at early stages of drug use in order to pre-
vent the transition to compulsive drug abuse (Vanhille et al. Human studies have validated animal study findings that mal-
2015). For example, addicts may benefit from treatment that adaptive degrees of novelty-seeking behavior are correlated
incorporates exercise as a non-drug reward. with substance abuse. These abnormalities are also correlated
Another approach to clarifying the behavioral connection with age; a difference exists in novelty-seeking behavior be-
between novelty seeking and addiction is to breed rats selec- tween adolescents and adults. Not surprisingly, adolescents
tively on the basis of individual extents of locomotor response display higher levels of novelty-seeking behavior than do
to a novel environment (Stead et al. 2006). Stead and col- adults, so they are at higher risk for developing substance
leagues first proposed a study to breed rats selectively in order use disorders (Spear 2000; Bernheim et al. 2D0u1r3in)g.
adolescence, novelty seeking as a personality trait is strongly amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine in
predictive of drug use later in life, a fact that holds true for the ventral striatum. Leyton and colleagues reported that indi-
many types of drugs, such as depressants, psychostimulants, viduals with high novelty seeking showed greater
opiates, and hallucinogens (Andrucci et al. 1989). In a seminal amphetamine-induced dopamine release and amphetamine-
study published by Andrucci and colleagues, adolescent sen- induced drug wanting (Leyton et al. 2002b). Based on the
sation seeking was strongly correlated with drug use; specifi- aforementioned evidence, an individual displaying high nov-
cally, high Sensation Seeking Scores strongly predicted drug elty seeking can be vulnerable to developing addiction due to
use and not drug nonuse (Andrucci et al. 1989). This study increased sensitivity to the reinforcing effects of
indicated a positive correlation between sensation seeking and psychostimulants. This information may provide insight into
abuse of alcohol, amphetamines, barbiturates, caffeine, co- establishing prevention strategies aimed at reducing the inci-
caine, hallucinogens, marijuana, and tobacco (Andrucci et al. dence of high-risk behaviors.
1989). Cloninger and colleagues showed that in 431 children, In addition, high novelty seeking or sensation seeking is
novelty seeking was strongly predictive of early-onset alcohol associated with a greater frequency and amount of drug use, as
abuse (Cloninger 1987). Comeau and colleagues found con- well as with a higher risk of using addictive drugs and devel-
sistently that sensation seeking mediates a motivation for al- oping addiction (Hawkins et al. 1992). Individuals with high
cohol use among adolescents (Comeau et al. 2001). A study of sensation-seeking status showed greater sensitivity to the
2733 high school students demonstrated that Sensation Seek- psychostimulant-like effects of d-amphetamine on self-report
ing Scores positively correlated with recent use of alcohol and measures that were associated with the reinforcing effects of
tobacco (Schepis et al. 2008). Further adolescent studies have drugs (Kelly et al. 2006). Furthermore, novelty seeking is
highlighted the finding that sensation seeking is the best pre- correlated with the magnitude of amphetamine-induced in-
dictor of adolescents engaging in risky behaviors, including creases in extracellular dopamine in the ventral striatum. Ley-
drug use (Stautz and Cooper 2013). Sensation seeking peaks ton et al. (2002a, b) reported that high novelty seeking pre-
during the late adolescent years, so many individuals of this dicted greater amphetamine-induced dopamine release and
age initiate drug use; in turn, they experience enhanced re- desire for the drug. In view of the aforementioned evidence,
sponses to these drugs (Kandel et al. 1994; Spear 2000; an individual displaying high novelty seeking appears vulner-
Blanchard et al. 2009). Because drug use has already com- able to developing addiction because of his or her higher sen-
menced, during adulthood, although novelty-seeking behavior sitivity to the reinforcing effects of psychostimulants. This
may decrease, use continues. information may provide insight into effective prevention
Among adults, novelty-seeking behavior as part of a hu- strategies aimed at reducing the incidence of high-risk behav-
man personality increases vulnerability to addiction and re- iors, including drug addiction.
flects in part a psychobiological adaptation in the response There is some disagreement regarding whether novelty
to chronic substance exposure (Koob 2000; Belin and seeking can predict cessation outcomes. Carton and col-
Deroche-Gamonet 2012). In adults, novelty-seeking behavior leagues found that, consistent with other human studies,
can predict vulnerability to initiation of self-administration smokers scored highly on Sensation Seeking Scores, but that
and to compulsive drug use (Belin and Deroche-Gamonet sensation seeking predicted only initiation of smoking and
2012). Leyton and colleagues demonstrated a positive corre- was not related to cessation outcomes (Carton et al. 2000).
lation between novelty seeking and amphetamine-induced Conversely, other studies in human adults have confirmed
drug wanting (Leyton et al. 2002a). Mahoney and colleagues the idea that cessation outcome and relapse rates can be pre-
found that individuals with cocaine or methamphetamine use dicted by novelty seeking (Meszaros et al. 1999; Kahler et al.
disorders had significantly higher Sensation Seeking Scores 2009; Ismael and Baltieri 2014). For example, Meszaros and
across both drug groupings than did healthy controls colleagues demonstrated that novelty seeking was a strong
(Mahoney et al. 2015). A recent study of 8646 adult individ- predictor of relapse among detoxified alcohol-dependent pa-
uals using the TCI showed that novelty seeking was the trait tients (Meszaros et al. 1999). Kahler and colleagues found that
most associated with the use of alcohol, cannabis, and cocaine the level of sensation seeking was in fact negatively correlated
(Schneider et al. 2015). In addition, high novelty seeking or with smoking cessation success; high sensation seekers faced
sensation seeking is associated with an increased frequency reduced odds of cessation compliance and outcomes (Kahler
and amount of drug use, as well as an increased risk for using et al. 2009). Ismael and Baltieri found that novelty seeking
addictive drugs and developing addiction (Hawkins et al. was positively associated with craving intensity among
1992). Individuals with high sensation-seeking status showed cocaine-dependent outpatients in therapy (Ismael and Baltieri
greater sensitivity to the psychostimulant-like effects of d- 2014). On the other hand, both impulsivity and novelty-
amphetamine on self-report measures that were associated seeking might be related with craving and relapse in alcohol
with the reinforcing effects of drugs (Kelly et al. 2006). Fur- dependence. In particular, extravagance of novelty-seeking
thermore, novelty seeking is correlated with the magnitude of using TCI was related with relapse both directly and indirectly
via craving, whereas impulsivity was related with relapse only to dopamine^ (Blum et al. 2012). Opioid peptides are
indirectly via craving (Evren et al. 2012). neuromodulators that can be produced by the body; opiate
drugs mimic these peptides and are competitive antagonists
that bind to the opioid receptors (Koob 2000). The mu (μ)
Molecular Connection Between Novelty Seeking opioid receptor has been indicted in opiate addiction; for ex-
and Addiction ample, Matthes and colleagues showed that mice lacking this
receptor subtype do not respond the rewarding effects of mor-
The molecular connection between novelty seeking and ad- phine (Matthes et al. 1996). Serotonin, GABA, and glutamate,
diction on the cellular and neural network level has not been in addition to dopamine, have been linked to the reinforcing
fully elucidated. New findings indicate that the same neural effects of alcohol; Koob describes ethanol’s reinforcing effects
networks and neurotransmitters at the synaptic connections as an Borchestra^ of these combined neurotransmission sys-
modulate the response to both novel stimuli and addictive tems (Koob 2000). Also, GABA is a key modulator of both
drugs. Few studies assess the effects of these elements on ethanol and benzodiazepine effects; this hypothesis is support-
the gene sequence, extent of expression, protein structure, ed by the reversal of ethanol’s behavioral effects through
and the function of neurotransmitter systems within and be- GABAergic antagonists (Koob 2000). Kalivas and Volkow
tween neural networks. In fact, the response to both novel describe the glutamatergic projections from the prefrontal cor-
stimuli and addictive drugs clearly is modulated by the tex through the NAc as being key mediators in the develop-
mesocorticolimbic dopamine system of the brain. This path- ment of addiction (Kalivas and Volkow 2011). Drugs of abuse
way is mostly associated with stimulation of the reward sys- such as cocaine, nicotine, and heroin reduce the extracellular
tem. Naturally rewarding stimuli can enhance the activity of concentration of glutamate, the major excitatory transmitter in
dopaminergic neurons and lead to dopamine release in the the brain. In addition, ethanol antagonizes glutamatergic trans-
neural networks of the mesocorticolimbic system. Dopami- mission (Koob 2000; Kalivas and Volkow 2011). Studies with
nergic transmission between the ventral tegmental area human and animal models of alcohol addiction show that a
(VTA) and the nucleus accumbens (NAc) functions as the decrease in GABA inhibition in the central nervous system
main reward site of this neural network. Different neurotrans- (CNS) can raise the extracellular glutamate concentration dur-
mitter projections arising from several cortical and limbic re- ing withdrawal, resulting in hyperexcitability (Rossetti and
gions of the brain also modulate the activity of the central Carboni 1995; Faingold et al. 2000; Koob 2000; Cagetti
reward system as an integrated neural network. Both novelty 0et3al. 20 ; Gomez et al. 2012).
and drugs of abuse activate the central and integrated natural These neurotransmission systems have also been proposed
reward system by increasing the activity of dopamine fibers in to mediate novelty-seeking behavior. Cocaine increases dopa-
the VTA–NAc reward circuit (Bardo et al. 1996). mine release in the NAc and, to a greater extent, in high
Dopamine stands out as the central neurotransmitter regu- novelty-seeking than in low novelty-seeking rodents (Hooks
lating this reward circuit, directly connecting the VTA and the et al. 1991). Similarly, amphetamine induces dopamine re-
NAc. Other neurotransmitters, such as serotonin, glutamate, lease in human brains, and a high novelty-seeking score pre-
GABA, opioid peptides, and norepinephrine, are secondary to dicts greater amphetamine-induced dopamine release and fur-
dopamine, as they act in part to regulate the dopaminergic ther drug-seeking behavior (Leyton et al. 2002a). Serotonin is
system. Novel stimuli and some drugs of abuse such as co- a mediator of novelty-seeking behavior; high novelty seekers
caine directly affect dopamine release in the brain. Other ad- generally exhibit low responsiveness of the serotonergic sys-
dictive drugs such as alcohol act on these secondary neuro- tem (Netter et al. 1996). Evenden suggested that the associa-
transmitter systems, indirectly affecting dopamine release and tion between low tissue serotonin concentrations and low im-
altering the activity of the reward pathway in the brain pulsivity; serotonergic system underactivity may impact
(Cloninger 1987). novelty-seeking behavior in humans secondary to serotonin-
Understanding how certain drugs of abuse and novel stim- related impulsivity changes (Evenden 1999). Parallel findings
uli alter the neuronal and molecular circuitry of the natural have shown the same behavioral and molecular pattern in
reward system in the brain is critical for understanding the rodent studies involving median forebrain lesions. Rats with
molecular link between novelty seeking and drug addiction. decreased serotonergic activity show an increase in the rate of
Koob summarized the effects of several drugs of abuse, par- amphetamine self-administration (Leccese and Lyness 1984).
ticularly focusing on key neurotransmitters such as dopamine, In short, we know that drugs of abuse affect these neuro-
opioid peptides, serotonin, GABA, and glutamate (Koob transmission systems and that novelty-seeking behavior has
2000). Dopamine acutely mediates the rewarding effects of been linked to nearly all categories of addictive drug use.
psychostimulants such as cocaine, nicotine, and amphet- Thus, further studies that examine the role of neurotransmit-
amines (Koob 2000). Blum and colleagues stressed the impor- ters other than dopamine in mediating novelty-seeking behav-
tance of dopamine in addiction; in their words, Ball roads lead ior may provide greater insight into the underlying molecular
connection between the novelty-seeking personality trait and studies have shown that there exists a negative correlation
vulnerability to addiction. between DAT and novelty-seeking behavior, and this correla-
Because of its role as the primary neurotransmitter in the tion can affect the response to drugs of abuse, particularly
reward pathway of the brain, the dopaminergic system has nicotine and cocaine.
been more fully examined as a mediator of novelty-seeking A common approach to understanding the link between
behavior and substance abuse than have other neurotransmit- novelty seeking and addiction in animals is to identify brain
ter systems. Cloninger argued for a baseline difference in regions that are well established to be associated with both
mesolimbic dopaminergic signaling between high and low novelty seeking and addiction and then to examine genetic
novelty seekers, that individual variations in the brain’s similarities and differences in these regions between high
Bbehavioral activation system^ lead high novelty seekers to and low novelty seekers. Brain regions linked to individual
exhibit greater rewards from drugs of abuse (Cloninger 1987). differences in novelty seeking and addiction include the NAc
He suggested that low basal firing of midbrain dopaminergic (Flagel et al. 2010), VTA (Marinelli and White 2000), prefron-
neurons that project from the VTA to the NAc may be ob- tal cortex (PFC) (Bernheim et al. 2013), and hippocampus
served in high novelty seekers; in turn, they are more sensitive (Fumagalli et al. 2003; Flagel et al. 2010). Addiction studies
to any release of dopamine that would result from using a drug have further implicated the striatum (Toda 2012), arcuate nu-
of abuse. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that cleus (Koob 2006), ventral palladium (Mahler et al. 2014),
alcoholics typically have low levels of dopamine and its amygdala (See et al. 2003), and raphe nuclei (Kirby et al.
metabolites in cerebrospinal fluid samples (Cloninger 2011). High-resolution functional magnetic resonance imag-
1986). ing (fMRI) has demonstrated that distinct clusters within these
Conversely, other studies have found that rats exhibiting brain regions are modulated primarily by the anticipation of
novelty-seeking behavior are basally hyperdopaminergic reward and novelty (Krebs et al. 2011). Fig. 4 illustrates the
(Marinelli and White 2000; Nadal et al. 2002). Marinelli and neurotransmitter systems that modulate the response to re-
White (2000) demonstrated that high novelty-seeking rats ex- warding stimuli.
hibited higher basal firing rates in the VTA. Flagel and col- Several genes have been singled out as being associated
leagues used fast-scan cyclic voltammetry on rats selectively with a novelty-seeking phenotype. Allelic heterogeneity in
bred for novelty-seeking behavior and found that high novelty the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) has consistently been
seekers displayed a greater frequency of spontaneous dopa- associated with novelty seeking; Okuyama and colleagues
mine release in the NAc (Flagel et al. 2010). High novelty- first demonstrated that polymorphisms in the human DRD4
seeking rats also have been shown to display both a greater exon III could contribute to individual differences in novelty-
behavioral output response to an infusion of dopamine into the seeking behavior (Okuyama et al. 2000). Specifically, the
NAc and fewer dopamine receptor 2 (D2R) binding sites in −521 C/T polymorphism was significantly associated with
this region and the striatum (Hooks et al. 1994; Tournier et al. novelty seeking among adult males (n = 86) using the TCI.
2013; Flagel et al. 2014). Tournier and colleagues demonstrat- Multiple studies have confirmed the view that this polymor-
ed that high novelty-seeking rats had innately lower concen- phism mediates novelty-seeking behavior, including studies
trations of D2R in the substantia nigra (SN) and VTA than did with subjects displaying extreme phenotypes, animal models
low novelty-seeking rats; these low concentrations may result including gene knockouts, and studies investigating the evo-
in a specific pattern of dopaminergic signaling that underlies lutionary history of the DRD4 exon III polymorphism (Ronai
vulnerability to addiction (Tournier et al. 2013). However, et al. 2001; Bookman et al. 2002; Eichhammer et al. 2005;
these researchers conceded that the hyperdopaminergic activ- Golimbet et al. 2005; Ebstein 2006). Substance abuse has also
ity could be the cause or the result of these low concentrations been linked to DRD4; genome scans for genetic linkage to
of D2 receptor function. Flagel and colleagues stated that there drug addiction have shown a region on chromosome 11p
are more high-affinity D2 receptors in high than in low (D11S1984) that includes the DRD4 locus (Long et al.
novelty-seeking rats but that the total number of dopaminergic 1998). Recently, Thanos and colleagues found that DRD4
receptors may be constant (Flagel et al. 2014). Those investi- has an important role in novelty-seeking behavior in male
gators concluded that the D2high receptor proportion repre- mice and that the extent of such behavior positively correlated
sents individual differences in vulnerability to drug rewarding with alcohol consumption (Thanos et al. 2015).
effects; a larger proportion represents the most addiction- The val 158 met polymorphism of the catechol-O-
vulnerable individuals because of the hypersensitivity to do- methyltransferase gene (COMT) (rs4680), whose protein is
pamine release induced by certain drugs (Flagel et al. 2014). involved in the degradation of dopamine, has been implicated
The research on the mechanisms regulating dopamine uptake in the novelty-seeking trait (Hosak 2007; Montag et al. 2012).
and release has implicated the dopamine transporter as having Val158 allele carriers display more resilience in their tempera-
a key role in this action (Lim et al. 2012; Sabeti et al. 2002; ment (Kang et al. 2013), whereas Met158 carriers have a higher
Chefer et al. 2003; Gulley et al. 2003; Zhu et al. 2005). These risk of emotional dysregulation (Kempton et al. 2009). The
Dopamine (DA) GABA Opioid peptide (OP)

Glutamate (Glu) Serotonin (5-HT) Acetylcholine (ACh)

Fig. 4 Illustration of the neurocircuitry involved in the response to acetylcholine (ACh). A major inhibitory neurotransmitter of this reward
rewarding stimuli, including drugs of abuse and novelty. Dopamine system is GABA, connecting the VTA, NAc, PFC, thalamus, and ventral
(DA) plays a key role in this system, through excitatory projections di- palladium. Other brain regions implicated in the response to rewarding
rectly connecting the ventral tegmental area (VTA) with the nucleus ac- stimuli include the hippocampus, amygdala, raphe nuclei, striatum, and
cumbens (NAc) and prefrontal cortex (PFC). Other excitatory neurotrans- arcuate nucleus
mitters include glutamate, serotonin (5-HT), opioid peptides (OP), and

link between COMT and novelty seeking was shown by Chen the context of both addiction and novelty seeking have pro-
and colleagues, who demonstrated that males (n = 250) carry- vided contradictory evidence (Bolos et al. 1990; Lu et al.
ing the Val/Val genotype displayed lower novelty seeking 1996; Crettol et al. 2008), much remains to be elucidated.
(Chen et al. 2011). An earlier study by Hosak and colleagues Nonetheless, studies in both animals and humans looking at
found consistent results, in that 37 individuals who carried the the behavioral and molecular connections between novelty
Met/Met genotype displayed higher novelty seeking (Hosak seeking and drug addiction provide new insights into using
et al. 2006). this phenotype for creating preventative and curative measures
The serotonin receptor 2A gene (HTR2A) also mediates for addiction-vulnerable individuals.
novelty-seeking behavior (Heck et al. 2009; Nakamura et al.
2010). Heck and colleagues examined 17 genes involving
serotonergic and dopaminergic signaling that had been asso-
ciated with the novelty-seeking personality trait (Heck et al. Conclusions and Future Directions
2009). This gene was associated with novelty seeking; specif-
ically, two intronic SNPS, rs2770296 and rs927544, located The body of research in the field has provided solid evidence
within this gene correlated with novelty seeking (Heck et al. for association but not for causality between novelty seeking
2009). and addiction. If the correlation is truly causal, then individ-
The dopamine receptor 2 gene (DRD2) has been linked to uals who display underlying high novelty seeking will be
both substance abuse and novelty-seeking behavior (Blum more likely to experiment with drugs, transition to compulsive
et al. 1990; Compton et al. 1996; Lawford et al. 2000). Sub- drug use, and develop addictive behaviors. However, if addic-
jects with the DRD2 TaqI A1 allele have fewer dopamine D2 tion precedes high novelty seeking, then individuals who
receptors in the striatum and are basally hypodopaminergic compulsively abuse drugs may experience psychobiological
(Compton et al. 1996). Alcoholism and opiate dependence adaptations in the brain that increase novelty-seeking behav-
also have been linked to polymorphisms in DRD2 (Blum ior. Of course, the relation between novelty seeking and sub-
et al. 1990; Lawford et al. 2000). stance abuse could be a combination of input and output,
Taken together, these findings provide further evidence for whereby high novelty seekers are more likely to initiate drug
the molecular link between novelty seeking and drug addic- use and then experience adaptations that lead to even higher
tion. However, because various studies examining this gene in measures of novelty seeking.
Future research should include assessing the effect of age (2012) The genetics of addiction-a translational perspective. Transl
Psychiatry 2:e140
on novelty seeking and addiction. Because it is well
Ahituv N, Kavaslar N, Schackwitz W, Ustaszewska A, Martin J, Hebert
established that age affects novelty seeking in addition to sub- S, Doelle H, Ersoy B, Kryukov G, Schmidt S, Yosef N, Ruppin E,
stance abuse development, these studies would provide im- Sharan R, Vaisse C, Sunyaev S, Dent R, Cohen J, McPherson R,
portant insights into how to prevent and treat drug addiction. Pennacchio LA (2007) Medical sequencing at the extremes of hu-
man body mass. Am J Hum Genet 80:779–791
Human studies would provide good evidence for a relation;
Ambrosio E, Goldberg SR, Elmer GI (1995) Behavior genetic investiga-
animal models may demonstrate in a controlled setting the tion of the relationship between spontaneous locomotor activity and
causation between age and novelty seeking on drug the acquisition of morphine self-administration behavior. Behav
addiction-related behaviors. Furthermore, because the causal Pharmacol 6:229–237
Andrucci GL, Archer RP, Pancoast DL, Gordon RA (1989) The relation-
relation between novelty seeking and drug addiction is still not
ship of mmpi and sensation seeking scales to adolescent drug-use. J
entirely clear, researchers may wish to carry out animal studies Pers Assess 53:253–266
that demonstrate how novelty seeking and addiction develop- Arnett J (1994) Sensation seeking - a new conceptualization and a new
ment are connected to each other temporally. It may also be of scale. Personal Individ Differ 16:289–296
interest to examine the difference in self-administration be- Ballon N, Brunault P, Cortese S (2015) Sensation seeking and cocaine
dependence in adults with reported childhood ADHD. J Atten
tween stimulants and sedatives in high and low novelty Disord 19:335–342
seekers, as high novelty seekers might prefer cocaine to ben- Bardo MT, Donohew RL, Harrington NG (1996) Psychobiology of nov-
zodiazepines, for example, as reported by Blanchard and col- elty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behav Brain Res 77:23–43
leagues (Blanchard et al. 2009). Further studies elucidating the Belin D, Deroche-Gamonet V (2012) Responses to novelty and vulnera-
bility to cocaine addiction: contribution of a multi-symptomatic an-
link between various mental illnesses, novelty seeking, and imal model. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2
substance abuse may also enrich the field; for example, Bernheim A, Halfon O, Boutrel B (2013) Controversies about the en-
Dervaux and coworkers hypothesized that novelty seeking hanced vulnerability of the adolescent brain to develop addiction.
could be the glue between schizophrenia and alcohol depen- Front Pharmacol 4:118
Bevins RA, Klebaur JE, Bardo MT (1997) Individual differences in re-
dence (Dervaux et al. 2010). Ballon and colleagues found a sponse to novelty, amphetamine-induced activity and drug discrim-
link between childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ination in rats. Behav Pharmacol 8:113–123
(ADHD), later sensation seeking in adults, and eventual co- Blanchard MM, Mendelsohn D, Stamp JA (2009) The HR/LR model:
caine dependence (Ballon et al. 2015). Finally, as the popula- further evidence as an animal model of sensation seeking. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev 33:1145–1154
tion continues to age, focus within these paradigms may be Blum K, Chen AL, Giordano J, Borsten J, Chen TJ, Hauser M, Simpatico
turned onto the growing presence of debilitating diseases – T, Femino J, Braverman ER, Barh D (2012) The addictive brain: all
such as HIV-1 infection, Parkinson’s disease, and Alzheimer’s roads lead to dopamine. J Psychoactive Drugs 44:134–143
disease. Assessing the connection between novelty seeking Blum K, Noble EP, Sheridan PJ, Montgomery A, Ritchie T,
Jagadeeswaran P, Nogami H, Briggs AH, Cohn JB (1990) Allelic
and substance abuse in patients exhibiting these conditions association of human dopamine D2 receptor gene in alcoholism.
may provide valuable information regarding potential phar- JAMA 263:2055–2060
macologic and behavioral treatments. Regardless of the ap- Boissier JR, Simon P (1962) The exploration reaction in the mouse.
proach used in future research, novelty seeking will continue Preliminary note. Therapie 17:1225–1232
Bolos AM, Dean M, Lucasderse S, Ramsburg M, Brown GL, Goldman D
to be a key personality trait in individuals when predicting (1990) Population and pedigree studies reveal a lack of association
vulnerability to addiction. between the dopamine-D2 receptor gene and alcoholism. Jama-J
Am Med Assoc 264:3156–3160
Bookman EB, Taylor RE, Adams-Campbell L, Kittles RA (2002) DRD4
Acknowledgments This work was supported, in part, by US National promoter SNPs and gender effects on extraversion in African
Institutes of Health grants DA-012844 and DA-026356. Americans. Mol Psychiatry 7:786–789
Brown GR, Nemes C (2008) The exploratory behaviour of rats in the
Compliance with Ethical Standards
hole-board apparatus: is head-dipping a valid measure of neophilia?
Behav Process 78:442–448
Conflict of Interest The authors claim no conflict interest regarding
Burns L (2014) World drug report 2013 united nations office on drugs
this report.
and crime. Drug Alcohol Rev 33:216–216
Cagetti E, Liang J, Spigelman I, Olsen RW (2003) Withdrawal from
chronic intermittent ethanol treatment changes subunit composition,
reduces synaptic function, and decreases behavioral responses to
positive allosteric modulators of GABAA receptors. Mol
Pharmacol 63:53–64
References Cain ME, Saucier DA, Bardo MT (2005) Novelty seeking and drug use:
contribution of an animal model. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 13:
Agrawal A, Lynskey MT (2008) Are there genetic influences on addic- 367–375
tion: evidence from family, adoption and twin studies. Addiction Carton S, Le Houezec J, Lagrue G, Jouvent R (2000) Relationships be-
103:1069–1081 tween sensation seeking and emotional symptomatology during
Agrawal A, Verweij KJ, Gillespie NA, Heath AC, Lessov-Schlaggar CN, smoking cessation with nicotine patch therapy. Addict Behav 25:
Martin NG, Nelson EC, Slutske WS, Whitfield JB, Lynskey MT 653–662
Chefer VI, Zakharova I, Shippenberg TS (2003) Enhanced responsive- Finn PR, Mazas CA, Justus AN, Steinmetz J (2002) Early-onset alcohol-
ness to novelty and cocaine is associated with decreased basal do- ism with conduct disorder: go/no go learning deficits, working
pamine uptake and release in the nucleus accumbens: quantitative memory capacity, and personality. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 26:186–
microdialysis in rats under transient conditions. J Neurosci 23:3076– 206
3084 Flagel SB, Robinson TE, Clark JJ, Clinton SM, Watson SJ, Seeman P,
Chen C, Moyzis R, Dong Q, He Q, Zhu B, Li J, Li H, Lessard J (2011) Phillips PE, Akil H (2010) An animal model of genetic vulnerability
Sex modulates the associations between the COMT gene and per- to behavioral disinhibition and responsiveness to reward-related
sonality traits. Neuropsychopharmacology 36:1593–1598 cues: implications for addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 35:
Cloninger CR (1986) A unified biosocial theory of personality and its role 388–400
in the development of anxiety states. Psychiatr Dev 4:167–226 Flagel SB, Waselus M, Clinton SM, Watson SJ, Akil H (2014)
Cloninger CR (1987) Neurogenetic adaptive mechanisms in alcoholism. Antecedents and consequences of drug abuse in rats selectively bred
Science 236:410–416 for high and low response to novelty. Neuropharmacology 76 Pt B:
Cloninger CR, Przybeck TR, Svrakic DM (1991) The tridimensional 425–436.
personality questionnaire: U.S. normative data. Psychol Rep 69: Fumagalli F, Bedogni F, Maragnoli ME, Gennarelli M, Perez J, Racagni
1047–1057 G, Riva MA (2003) Dopaminergic D2 receptor activation modulates
Cloninger CR, Svrakic DM, Przybeck TR (1993) A psychobiological FGF-2 gene expression in rat prefrontal cortex and hippocampus. J
model of temperament and character. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50: Neurosci Res 74:74–80
975–990 Golimbet VE, Gritsenko IK, Alfimova MV, Ebstein RP (2005)
Comeau N, Stewart SH, Loba P (2001) The relations of trait anxiety, Polymorphic markers of the dopamine D4 receptor gene promoter
anxiety sensitivity, and sensation seeking to adolescents' motivations region and personality traits in mentally healthy individuals from the
for alcohol, cigarette, and marijuana use. Addict Behav 26:803–825 Russian population. Russ J Genet 41:789–793
Compton PA, Anglin MD, Khalsa-Denison ME, Paredes A (1996) The
Gomez H, Lluch JM, Masgrau L (2012) Essential role of glutamate 317 in
D2 dopamine receptor gene, addiction, and personality: clinical cor-
galactosyl transfer by alpha3GalT: a computational study.
relates in cocaine abusers. Biol Psychiatry 39:302–304
Carbohydr Res 356:204–208
Crawford CA, Der-Ghazarian T, Britt CE, Varela FA, Kozanian OO
(2013) Novelty-induced conditioned place preference, sucrose pref- Greenwood TA, Badner JA, Byerley W, Keck PE, McElroy SL, Remick
erence, and elevated plus maze behavior in adult rats after repeated RA, Dessa Sadovnick A, Kelsoe JR (2013) Heritability and linkage
exposure to methylphenidate during the preweanling period. Behav analysis of personality in bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord 151:748–
Brain Res 246:29–35 755
Crettol S, Besson J, Croquette-Krokar M, Hammig R, Gothuey I, Monnat Gulley JM, Hoover BR, Larson GA, Zahniser NR (2003) Individual
M, Deglon JJ, Preisig M, Eap CB (2008) Association of dopamine differences in cocaine-induced locomotor activity in rats: behavioral
and opioid receptor genetic polymorphisms with response to meth- characteristics, cocaine pharmacokinetics, and the dopamine trans-
adone maintenance treatment. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol porter. Neuropsychopharmacology 28:2089–2101
Psychiatry 32:1722–1727 Han C, McGue MK, Iacono WG (1999) Lifetime tobacco, alcohol and
Cummings JA, Gowl BA, Westenbroek C, Clinton SM, Akil H, Becker other substance use in adolescent Minnesota twins: univariate and
JB (2011) Effects of a selectively bred novelty-seeking phenotype multivariate behavioral genetic analyses. Addiction 94:981–993
on the motivation to take cocaine in male and female rats. Biol Sex Hascoet M, Bourin M, Dhonnchadha BAN (2001) The mouse light-dark
Differ 2:3 paradigm: a review. Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry
Davis BA, Clinton SM, Akil H, Becker JB (2008) The effects of novelty- 25:141–166
seeking phenotypes and sex differences on acquisition of cocaine Hawkins JD, Catalano RF, Miller JY (1992) Risk and protective factors
self-administration in selectively bred high-responder and low- for alcohol and other drug problems in adolescence and early adult-
responder rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 90:331–338 hood - implications for substance-abuse prevention. Psychol Bull
Dervaux A, Laqueille X, Bourdel MC, Olie JP, Krebs MO (2010) 112:64–105
Impulsivity and sensation seeking in alcohol abusing patients with Heck A, Lieb R, Ellgas A, Pfister H, Lucae S, Roeske D, Putz B, Muller-
schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry 1:135 Myhsok B, Uhr M, Holsboer F, Ising M (2009) Investigation of 17
Ebstein RP (2006) The molecular genetic architecture of human person- candidate genes for personality traits confirms effects of the HTR2A
ality: beyond self-report questionnaires. Mol Psychiatry 11:427–445 gene on novelty seeking. Genes Brain Behav 8:464–472
Eichhammer P, Sand PG, Stoertebecker P, Langguth B, Zowe M, Hajak G Hiroi N, Agatsuma S (2005) Genetic susceptibility to substance depen-
(2005) Variation at the DRD4 promoter modulates extraversion in dence. Mol Psychiatry 10:336–344
caucasians. Mol Psychiatry 10:520–522
Ersche KD, Turton AJ, Pradhan S, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW (2010) Hooks MS, Jones GH, Smith AD, Neill DB, Justice Jr JB (1991)
Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus sensation- Response to novelty predicts the locomotor and nucleus accumbens
seeking personality traits. Biol Psychiatry 68:770–773 dopamine response to cocaine. Synapse 9:121–128
Evenden JL (1999) Varieties sf impulsivity. Psychopharmacology 146: Hooks MS, Juncos JL, Justice Jr JB, Meiergerd SM, Povlock SL, Schenk
348–361 JO, Kalivas PW (1994) Individual locomotor response to novelty
Evren C, Durkaya M, Evren B, Dalbudak E, Cetin R (2012) Relationship predicts selective alterations in D1 and D2 receptors and mRNAs. J
of relapse with impulsivity, novelty seeking and craving in male Neurosci 14:6144–6152
alcohol-dependent inpatients. Drug Alcohol Rev 31:81–90 Hosak L (2007) Role of the COMT gene Val158Met polymorphism in
Faingold C, Li Y, Evans MS (2000) Decreased GABA and increased mental disorders: a review. Eur Psychiatry 22:276–281
glutamate receptor-mediated activity on inferior colliculus neurons Hosak L, Libiger J, Cizek J, Beranek M, Cermakova E (2006) The
in vitro are associated with susceptibility to ethanol withdrawal sei- COMT Val158Met polymorphism is associated with novelty seek-
zures. Brain Res 868:287–295 ing in Czech methamphetamine abusers: preliminary results. Neuro
Fairbanks LA, Bailey JN, Breidenthal SE, Laudenslager ML, Kaplan JR, Endocrinol Lett 27:799–802
Jorgensen MJ (2011) Environmental stress alters genetic regulation Howard MO, Kivlahan D, Walker RD (1997) Cloninger's tridimensional
of novelty seeking in vervet monkeys. Genes Brain Behav 10:683– theory of personality and psychopathology: applications to sub-
688 stance use disorders. J Stud Alcohol 58:48–66
Ismael F, Baltieri DA (2014) Role of personality traits in cocaine craving alcohol dependence on chromosomes 4 and 11 from an autosome-
throughout an outpatient psychosocial treatment program. Rev Bras wide scan in an American Indian population. Am J Med Genet 81:
Psiquiatr 36:24–31 216–221
Kahler CW, Spillane NS, Metrik J, Leventhal AM, Monti PM (2009) Lu RB, Ko HC, Chang FM, Castiglione CM, Schoolfield G, Pakstis AJ,
Sensation seeking as a predictor of treatment compliance and Kidd JR, Kidd KK (1996) No association between alcoholism and
smoking cessation treatment outcomes in heavy social drinkers. multiple polymorphisms at the dopamine D2 receptor gene (DRD2)
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 93:285–290 in three distinct taiwanese populations. Biol Psychiatry 39:419–429
Kalivas PW, Volkow ND (2011) New medications for drug addiction Mahler SV, Vazey EM, Beckley JT, Keistler CR, McGlinchey EM,
hiding in glutamatergic neuroplasticity. Mol Psychiatry 16:974–986 Kaufling J, Wilson SP, Deisseroth K, Woodward JJ, Aston-Jones
Kandel DB, Wu P, Davies M (1994) Maternal smoking during pregnancy G (2014) Designer receptors show role for ventral pallidum input
and smoking by adolescent daughters. Am J Public Health 84:1407– to ventral tegmental area in cocaine seeking. Nat Neurosci 17:577–
1413 U136
Kang JI, Kim SJ, Song YY, Namkoong K, An SK (2013) Genetic influ- Mahoney JJ, Thompson-Lake DGY, Cooper K, Verrico CD, Newton TF,
ence of COMT and BDNF gene polymorphisms on resilience in De La Garza R (2015) A comparison of impulsivity, depressive
healthy college students. Neuropsychobiology 68:174–180 symptoms, lifetime stress and sensation seeking in healthy controls
Kelly TH, Robbins G, Martin CA, Fillmore MT, Lane SD, Harrington versus participants with cocaine or methamphetamine use disorders.
NG, Rush CR (2006) Individual differences in drug abuse vulnera- J Psychopharmacol 29:50–56
bility: d-ampheta mi ne and sensation- seeking status. Manzo L, Gomez MJ, Callejas-Aguilera JE, Donaire R, Sabariego M,
Psychopharmacology 189:17–25 Fernandez-Teruel A, Canete A, Blazquez G, Papini MR, Torres C
Kempton MJ, Haldane M, Jogia J, Christodoulou T, Powell J, Collier D, (2014) Relationship between ethanol preference and sensation/
Williams SC, Frangou S (2009) The effects of gender and COMT novelty seeking. Physiol Behav 133:53–60
Val158Met polymorphism on fearful facial affect recognition: a Marinelli M, White FJ (2000) Enhanced vulnerability to cocaine self-
fMRI study. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 12:371–381 administration is associated with elevated impulse activity of mid-
Kendler KS, Jacobson KC, Prescott CA, Neale MC (2003) Specificity of brain dopamine neurons. J Neurosci 20:8876–8885
genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/ Matthes HW, Maldonado R, Simonin F, Valverde O, Slowe S, Kitchen I,
dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimu- Befort K, Dierich A, Le Meur M, Dolle P, Tzavara E, Hanoune J,
lants, and opiates in male twins. Am J Psychiatry 160:687–695 Roques BP, Kieffer BL (1996) Loss of morphine-induced analgesia,
Khan AA, Jacobson KC, Gardner CO, Prescott CA, Kendler KS (2005) reward effect and withdrawal symptoms in mice lacking the mu-
Personality and comorbidity of common psychiatric disorders. Br J opioid-receptor gene. Nature 383:819–823
Psychiatry 186:190–196 Meszaros K, Lenzinger E, Hornik K, Fureder T, Willinger U, Fischer G,
Kirby LG, Zeeb FD, Winstanley CA (2011) Contributions of serotonin in Schonbeck G, Aschauer HN (1999) The tridimensional personality
addiction vulnerability. Neuropharmacology 61:421–432 questionnaire as a predictor of relapse in detoxified alcohol depen-
Koob GF (2000) Neurobiology of addiction. Toward the Development of dents. The european fluvoxamine in alcoholism study group.
new Therapies. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 909: Alcohol Clin Exp Res 23:483–486
170–185 Meyer AC, Rahman S, Charnigo RJ, Dwoskin LP, Crabbe JC, Bardo MT
Koob GF (2006) The neurobiology of addiction: a neuroadaptational (2010) Genetics of novelty seeking, amphetamine self-
view relevant for diagnosis. Addiction 101(Suppl 1):23–30 administration and reinstatement using inbred rats. Genes Brain
Krebs RM, Heipertz D, Schuetze H, Duzel E (2011) Novelty increases the Behav 9:790–798
mesolimbic functional connectivity of the substantia nigra/ventral Montag C, Jurkiewicz M, Reuter M (2012) The role of the catechol-O-
tegmental area (SN/VTA) during reward anticipation: evidence from methyltransferase (COMT) gene in personality and related psycho-
high-resolution fMRI. NeuroImage 58:647–655 pathological disorders. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets 11:236–
Lawford BR, Young RM, Noble EP, Sargent J, Rowell J, Shadforth S, 250
Zhang XX, Ritchie T (2000) The D-2 dopamine receptor al allele Nadal R, Armario A, Janak PH (2002) Positive relationship between
and opioid dependence: association with heroin use and response to activity in a novel environment and operant ethanol self-
methadone treatment. Am J Med Genet 96:592–598 administration in rats. Psychopharmacology 162:333–338
Leccese AP, Lyness WH (1984) The effects of putative 5- Nakamura Y, Ito Y, Aleksic B, Kushima I, Yasui-Furukori N, Inada T,
hydroxytryptamine receptor active agents on D-amphetamine self- Ono Y, Ozaki N (2010) Influence of HTR2A polymorphisms and
administration in controls and rats with 5,7-dihydroxytryptamine parental rearing on personality traits in healthy Japanese subjects. J
median forebrain-bundle lesions. Brain Res 303:153–162 Hum Genet 55:838–841
Leyton M, Boileau I, Benkelfat C, Diksic M, Baker G, Dagher A (2002a) Nesil T, Kanit L, Li MD, Pogun S (2013) Nine generations of selection
Amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine, drug for high and low nicotine intake in outbred Sprague-Dawley rats.
wanting, and novelty seeking: a PET/[11C]raclopride study in Behav Genet 43:436–444
healthy men. Neuropsychopharmacology 27:1027–1035 Netter P, Hennig J, Roed IS (1996) Serotonin and dopamine as mediators
Leyton M, Boileau I, Benkelfat C, Diksic M, Baker G, Dagher A (2002b) of sensation seeking behavior. Neuropsychobiology 34:155–165
Amphetamine-induced increases in extracellular dopamine, drug Okuyama Y, Ishiguro H, Nankai M, Shibuya H, Watanabe A, Arinami T
wanting, and novelty seeking: a PET/[C-11]raclopride study in (2000) Identification of a polymorphism in the promoter region of
healthy men. Neuropsychopharmacology 27:1027–1035 DRD4 associated with the human novelty seeking personality trait.
Li MD, Cheng R, Ma JZ, Swan GE (2003) A meta-analysis of estimated Mol Psychiatry 5:64–69
genetic and environmental effects on smoking behavior in male and Pelloux Y, Costentin J, Duterte-Boucher D (2015) Differential involve-
female adult twins. Addiction 98:23–31 ment of anxiety and novelty preference levels on oral ethanol con-
Lim SS et al (2012) A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease sumption in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl).
and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 Piazza PV, Deminiere JM, Le Moal M, Simon H (1989) Factors that
regions, 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the global burden of predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration.
disease study 2010. Lancet 380:2224–2260 Science 245:1511–1513
Long JC, Knowler WC, Hanson RL, Robin RW, Urbanek M, Moore E, Piazza PV, Deroche-Gamonent V, Rouge-Pont F, Le Moal M (2000)
Bennett PH, Goldman D (1998) Evidence for genetic linkage to Vertical shifts in self-administration dose-response functions predict
a drug-vulnerable phenotype predisposed to addiction. J Neurosci seeking, activity, social interaction, and alcohol binge drinking in
20:4226–4232 adult mice. Synapse 69:356–364
Ronai Z, Szekely A, Nemoda Z, Lakatos K, Gervai J, Staub M, Sasvari- Toda S (2012) [The role of the striatum in addiction]. brain and nerve =.
Szekely M (2001) Association between novelty seeking and the-521 Shinkei Kenkyu no Shinpo 64:911–917
C/T polymorphism in the promoter region of the DRD4 gene. Mol Tournier BB, Steimer T, Millet P, Moulin-Sallanon M, Vallet P, Ibanez V,
Psychiatry 6:35–38 Ginovart N (2013) Innately low D2 receptor availability is associat-
Rossetti ZL, Carboni S (1995) Ethanol withdrawal is associated with ed with high novelty-seeking and enhanced behavioural sensitiza-
increased extracellular glutamate in the rat striatum. Eur J tion to amphetamine. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 16:1819–1834
Pharmacol 283:177–183 Vanhille N, Belin-Rauscent A, Mar AC, Ducret E, Belin D (2015) High
Sabeti J, Gerhardt GA, Zahniser NR (2002) Acute cocaine differentially locomotor reactivity to novelty is associated with an increased pro-
alters accumbens and striatal dopamine clearance in low and high pensity to choose saccharin over cocaine: new insights into the vul-
cocaine locomotor responders: behavioral and electrochemical re- nerability to addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology 40:577–589
cordings in freely moving rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 302:1201– Walters GD (2002) The heritability of alcohol abuse and dependence: a
1211 meta-analysis of behavior genetic research. Am J Drug Alcohol
Schepis TS, Desai RA, Smith AE, Cavallo DA, Liss TB, McFetridge A, Abuse 28:557–584
Potenza MN, Krishnan-Sarin S (2008) Impulsive sensation seeking, Wang Y, Liu Y, Yang LZ, Gu F, Li XM, Zha RJ, Wei ZD, Pei YK, Zhang
parental history of alcohol problems, and current alcohol and tobac- P, Zhou YF, Zhang XC (2015) Novelty seeking is related to individ-
co use in adolescents. J Addict Med 2:185–193 ual risk preference and brain activation associated with risk predic-
Schneider Jr R, Ottoni GL, de Carvalho HW, Elisabetsky E, Lara DR (2015) tion during decision making. Sci Rep-UK 5
Temperament and character traits associated with the use of alcohol,
Welker WI (1957) Free and forced exploration of a novel situation by rats.
cannabis, cocaine, benzodiazepines, and hallucinogens: evidence from
Psychol. Rep 3:95–108
a large Brazilian web survey. Rev Bras Psiquiatr 37:31–39
Weyandt LL (2005) The Physiological Bases of Cognitive and
See RE, Fuchs RA, Ledford CC, McLaughlin J (2003) Drug addiction,
Behavioral Disorders: Routledge.
relapse, and the amygdala. Ann N Y Acad Sci 985:294–307
Spear LP (2000) The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral mani- Zheng X, Ke X, Tan B, Luo X, Xu W, Yang X, Sui N (2003)
festations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24:417–463 Susceptibility to morphine place conditioning: relationship with
Stautz K, Cooper A (2013) Impulsivity-related personality traits and ad- stress-induced locomotion and novelty-seeking behavior in juvenile
olescent alcohol use: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev 33: and adult rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 75:929–935
574–592 Zhu J, Apparsundaram S, Bardo MT, Dwoskin LP (2005) Environmental
Stead JD, Clinton S, Neal C, Schneider J, Jama A, Miller S, Vazquez DM, enrichment decreases cell surface expression of the dopamine trans-
Watson SJ, Akil H (2006) Selective breeding for divergence in porter in rat medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurochem 93:1434–1443
novelty-seeking traits: heritability and enrichment in spontaneous Zuckerman M (2007) The sensation seeking scale V (SSS-V): still reli-
anxiety-related behaviors. Behav Genet 36:697–712 able and valid. Personal Individ Differ 43:1303–1305
Sutker PB, Archer RP, Allain AN (1978) Drug abuse patterns, personality Zuckerman M, Cloninger CR (1996) Relationships between Cloninger's,
characteristics, and relationships with sex, race, and sensation seek- Zuckerman's, and Eysenck's dimensions of personality. Personality
ing. J Consult Clin Psychol 46:1374–1378 and Individual Differences 21:283–285
Suto N, Austin JD, Vezina P (2001) Locomotor response to novelty Zuckerman M, Neeb M (1979) Sensation seeking and psychopathology.
predicts a rat's propensity to self-administer nicotine. Psychiatry Res 1:255–264
Psychopharmacology 158:175–180 Zuckerman M, Kuhlman DM, Joireman J, Teta P, Kraft M (1993) A
Thanos PK, Roushdy K, Sarwar Z, Rice O, Ashby Jr CR, Grandy DK comparison of 3 structural models for personality - the big 3, the
(2015) The effect of dopamine D4 receptor density on novelty big 5, and the alternative 5. J Pers Soc Psychol 65:757–768

Potrebbero piacerti anche