Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology Copyright 1990 by the American Psychological Association, Inc.

1990, Vol. 59, No. 2,337-343 0022-3514/9fyS00.75

Cognitive Processes in Perceived Social Support

Brian Lakey Patricia Bennett Cassady


Wayne State University University of North Carolina at Wilmington

This article describes 2 studies testing hypotheses that perceived social support operates in part as
a cognitive personality construct. Both studies found that perceived support manifested a pattern
of correlations more similar to cognitive variables than did support received from the environment
and that the relation between perceived support and psychological distress was reduced substan-
tially when the cognitive personality variables were controlled statistically. Study 2 also tested
hypotheses generated from schema theory that perceived support would be related to the interpre-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tation and recall of novel supportive behaviors. As predicted, low-perceived-support students inter-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

preted novel supportive behaviors more negatively than high-support students and remembered a
lower proportion of behaviors perceived as helpful.

The inverse relation between perceived social support and judging the supportive attempts of others as unhelpful, (b) en-
psychological distress is well documented, but how perceived hance the recall of past unhelpful received support, and (c)
support has its effects is largely unknown (S. Cohen & Wills, inhibit the recall of past helpful received support. Such pro-
1985; Heller, Swindle, & Dusenbury, 1986). Yet understanding cesses would increase the frequency of negative judgments of
these processes is critical if research on social support will ever interpersonal relationships, leading directly to psychological
be translated into effective treatment and prevention programs distress. This conception of perceived support was inspired by
(Heller et al, 1986; Kiesler, 1985). Historically, it has been as- Beck^ cognitive theory of depression (Beck, 1967, 1976). Ac-
sumed that significant others will provide supportive behaviors cording to this theory; interrelated negative beliefs about the
(e.g., advice or reassurance) to a person under stress, increasing self, the world, and the future are organized into depressive
the stressed individual's coping abilities and enabling him or schemas that lead to negatively biased information processing
her to avoid stress-related symptomatology (e.g, S. Cohen & and subsequent depression. Conceptualized as a cognitive vari-
Wills, 1985; Heller & Swindle, 1983; Thoits, 1985, 1986). Al- able, low perceived support would be considered an aspect of
though social support theory has emphasized the role of such the negative view of the world.
enacted or received support, most of the evidence for support
If low perceived support operates similarly to depressive self-
effects has been based on subjective self-report measures that
schemas, then perceived support should be associated with the
ask respondents to evaluate the quality of support available to
same sort of cognitive content and processes that have been
them (Heller & Lakey, 1985; Wetherington & Kessler, 1986).
found to covary with depression. Although the causal role of
Such measures of perceived social support are widely assumed
negative cognition in depression is unclear, it is well established
to reflect actual enacted support (Sarason, Sarason, & Shearin,
that dysphoric and depressed people manifest negative
1986), but the notion that these components are distinct con-
thoughts about themselves, their world, and their future (Coyne
structs that are only mildly related (Barrera, 1986; Heller &
& Gotlib, 1983; Segal & Shaw, 1986; Sweeney, Anderson, & Bai-
Lakey, 1985; Heller etat, 1986; Wetherington & Kessler, 1986)
ley, 1986). Consistent with hypotheses that depressive self-sche-
has become widely recognized. Thus, it appears that only a
mas influence information processing, studies have shown that
portion of perceived support effects can be explained as result-
depressed people exhibit negative biases in interpreting hypo-
ing from the supportive behaviors of others. Consequently, ad-
thetical stressful situations (Hammen & Krantz, 1976; Krantz
ditional conceptions of perceived support mechanisms are
& Hammen, 1979), interpersonal feedback (Gotlib, 1983), and
needed.
a variety of laboratory tasks (Kuiper, 1978; Rizley, 1978;
One alternative is that perceived support operates in part as a
Sweeney et al, 1986). Other researchers have found depressives
cognitive personality variable in which stable, organized beliefs
to show biases in recalling schema-congruent information (De-
about the quality of one's interpersonal relationships lead to
Monbreun & Craighead, 1977; Derry & Kuiper, 1981; Gotlib,
biased interpretation and recall of social interactions. In the
1983; Segal & Shaw, 1986).
case of persons with low perceived support, prior beliefs in the
inadequacy of their social relations would (a) bias them toward Recent research on person perception suggests that a link
exists between cognition about the self and cognition about
others. For example, people tend to rely on the same constructs
to describe other people that they use to describe themselves
Ralph W Swindle and anonymous reviewers provided valuable
comments on drafts. (Higgins, King, & Mavin, 1982; Hirschberg & Jennings, 1980;
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Lewicki, 1983). Consistent with the predictions of schema
Brian Lakey, Department of Psychology, 71 West Warren Avenue, theory, Higgins et al, (1982) found that people were more likely
Wayne State University, Detroit, Michigan 48202. to omit traits in reproducing descriptions of target people if

337
338 BRIAN LAKEY AND PATRICIA BENNETT CASSADY

those traits were not used in describing the self. Similarly, Mar- measure of enacted support was the Index of Socially Supportive Be-
kus. Smith, and Moreland (1985) found that students with mas- haviors (Barrera, Sandier, & Ramsey, 1981), a 40-item scale that asks
culinity self-schemas organized a schema-relevant film about a respondents to indicate the frequency with which they have received
target person into larger units and ascribed more masculine various supportive behaviors from others in the past month. This is a
widely used measure of received support with established reliability
traits to the person than did aschematics. Lewicki (1984) found
and validity (Sandier & Barrera, 1984).
that positive trait ratings were made more quickly about stimu-
Cognitive personality variables. Students completed measures of
lus people if the trait was seen as characteristic of the self. Ex-
three cognitive personality variables: self-esteem, dysfunctional atti-
tending this research to perceived social support, it suggests tudes, and control beliefs. Self-esteem was chosen for study because of
that the valence of one's thoughts about others should be closely the central role that cognition about the self plays both in Beck's cogni-
linked to the valence of thoughts about oneself. Furthermore, tive theory (1967,1976) and in the social cognition research relevant to
social support schemas should guide memory for and interpre- the present investigation (e.g., Markus et al, 1985). Dysfunctional atti-
tation of actual supportive behaviors in the same way that self- tudes were studied because they have been hypothesized to reflect
schemas are believed to influence thought about one^s own be- negative self-schema content and to act as a vulnerability factor for
depression (Segal & Shaw, 1986). Control beliefs were included in the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

havior.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

present investigation because prior research had suggested that they


are involved in social support processes (Lefcourt, Martin, & Saleh,
Study 1 1984; Sandier & Lakey, 1982). Participants completed the Rosenberg
(1979) self-esteem scale, Form A of the Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale
Study 1 was designed to test the viability of conceptualizing (Weissman & Beck, 1978; reviewed by Hammen & Krantz, 1985) and
perceived social support as a cognitive personality variable by the personal control items identified by Mirels (1970) in a factor ana-
comparing the pattern of correlations among measures of per- lytic study of Rotter's (1966) locus of control scale.
ceived support, cognitive personality constructs (self-esteem, Psychological distress. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI)
dysfunctional attitudes, and control beliefs), enacted support (Beck, 1967) and the Trait form of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
received from the environment, and psychological distress. If (Spielberger, Gorsuch, & Lushene, 1970) were administered. These
perceived support is more similar to cognitive variables than to scales have substantial evidence for their reliability and validity and
enacted support, then perceived support should behave more are frequently used in studies of social support.

like the former. Thus it was hypothesized that perceived sup-


port would be more strongly related to measures of cognitive
Results
personality variables than would enacted support. Further-
more, if perceived support is related to distress in the same Sex differences were examined as a preliminary step; no dif-
manner as the cognitive personality variables, then perceived ferences on any variable were observed. In addition, multiple
support would be more strongly related to measures of psycho- regression analyses were conducted to test for the existence of
logical distress than would enacted support, and controlling for interactions (J. Cohen & Cohen, 1983) by sex for any of the
individual differences in negative cognition should substan- study findings. None of these interactions approached signifi-
tially reduce the association between perceived support and cance, however; thus the results are reported for the combined
distress. sample.
To determine whether perceived support exhibited a pattern
Method of relations more similar to the cognitive personality variables
than to enacted support, simple correlations among the study
Participants and Procedure variables were calculated. Unlike enacted support, both per-
ceived support and the cognitive personality variables were di-
One hundred and one college students (70 women and 31 men) volun-
rectly related to both dysphoria and anxiety (Table 1). In addi-
teered to participate as part of a class demonstration of construct valid-
ity procedures in the first author^ junior-level personality course. Stu-
tion, although perceived support was significantly related to all
dents completed measures in groups of 25-35. Participation was not three cognitive personality variables, enacted support was not
required; data were gathered early in the semester before any of the significantly associated with any. Perceived support and en-
constructs included in this study were discussed in class. Data for this acted support did have some variance in common, however (r=
project were gathered across three semesters. .29, p = .003). For more conservative tests of the hypotheses
that perceived support would be more strongly related to the
Measures distress and cognitive variables than would enacted support,
the size of the correlations between the two support measures
Social support. Students completed two measuresof social support: and the criterion measures were compared using t tests (Walker
One reflected perceived support, and the other reflected enacted sup- and Lev, 1953; p. 257). As displayed in Table 2, perceived sup-
port received from the environment. The measure of perceived sup- port was significantly more strongly associated with all but one
port was the Interpersonal Support Evaluation List (S. Cohen & Ho-
of the distress and cognitive variables. The lone exception in-
berman, 1983; S, Cohen, Mermelstein, Kamarck, & Hoberman, 1985),
volved the comparison in predicting control beliefs, which ap-
chosen for its wide use in social support research and its established
proached statistical significance (p <. 10).
reliability and validity The scale consists of 48 items that ask respon-
dents to evaluate the adequacy of the social support available to them. To test the hypothesis that perceived support is related to
It was modified for the present study by providing a 4-point response psychological distress in the same way as cognitive personality
format with strongly agree and strongly disagree as extreme responses. variables, multiple regression analyses were conducted in
The reliability of the scale in the present sample was alpha =.91. The which perceived support was used to predict symptomatology
COGNITION AND SOCIAL SUPPORT 339

Table 1
Intercorrelations, Means, and Standard Deviations (N'= 101)

Measure M SD alpha

1. BDI — .84" .47** -.52** -.43** -.45** -.11 9.3 7.0 .81
2. Trait Anxiety — .56** -.66** -.40** -.51" -.12 41.8 11.1 .92
3. Dysfunctional
Attitudes — -.48** -.17 -.48** .01 82.6 12.4 .90
4. Self-Esteem .28** .40** .07 30.5 4.2 .83
5. Control Beliefs .23* .02 5.4 2.0 .60
6. Perceived Support — .29" 95.6 16.9 .91
7. Enacted Support — 91.3 25.4 .96

Note. BDI = Beck Depression Inventory.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

after the effects of the cognitive personality variables had been tudes and self-esteem loading less highly. However, Factor 2
entered first. Perceived support continued to predict dysphoria (eigenvalue = 1.67) appears to reflect primarily perceived social
(R2 change = .025, p = .043) and anxiety (R2 change = .025, p = support. A number of additional factor analyses were run with
.017) even when the cognitive variables had been controlled.1 various methods of extraction, rotation, and factor numbers in
Nonetheless, the strength of the association between perceived an attempt to force the perceived support variable into the Nega-
support and both dysphoria and anxiety was reduced substan- tive Affectivity factor. However in none of these analyses did
tially when the personality variables were controlled (r = .45 perceived support load on such a factor.
versus partial r - .20 for dysphoria and r = . 51 versus partial r=
.24 for anxiety). To determine whether enacted support could Discussion
account for the relation between perceived support and dis-
tress, multiple regression analyses were conducted in which the The results of Study 1 support the utility of conceptualizing
effects of enacted support were partialed out. However, control- perceived support as a cognitive personality variable, as per-
ling for the effects of enacted support made little difference in ceived support acted more similarly to such variables than to
the relation between perceived support and dysphoria (r - .45 enacted support received from the environment. Unlike en-
versus partial r = .44) or anxiety (r - .51 versus partial r = .50). acted support, perceived support displayed substantial associa-
Given that the measures of perceived support, negative cog- tions with high self-esteem and low dysfunctional attitudes. In
nition, and psychological distress were all highly intercorre- addition, similar to the cognitive variables, perceived support
lated, all of these measures may reflect the same underlying was associated with psychological distress, but enacted support
construct. Watson and Clark (1984) have proposed that mea- was not. Moreover, factor analyses revealed that perceived sup-
sures of psychological distress and negative cognition typically port was not a mere reflection of the negative affectivity con-
reflect the meta-construct of negative affectivity. To determine struct. The most compelling finding was that much of the asso-
whether perceived social support might merely be a reflection ciation between low perceived support and psychological dis-
of negative affectivity, a maximum likelihood factor analysis tress could be accounted for by individual differences in
with varimax rotation was conducted on the study variables. As negative cognition. This suggests that perceived support and
displayed in Table 3, two factors with eigenvalues exceeding 1 cognitive personality variables are linked to psychological dis-
were extracted, accounting for 29.8% and 23.9% of the vari- tress by similar processes. Nonetheless, these results do not
ance, respectively. Factor 1 (eigenvalue = 2.09) appears to reflect reveal what these processes might be.
the negative affectivity construct, with dysphoria and trait anxi- A cognitive conceptualization of perceived social support
ety loading most highly on this factor and dysfunctional atti- would predict that support schemas operate through influenc-
ing attention to, judgment of, and memory for support transac-
tions. Thus, much stronger evidence for a cognitive model of
Table 2 perceived support would be obtained if support appraisals were
Comparisons Between Perceived Support and Enacted Support shown to be related to such cognitive mechanisms. Study 2 was
in Predicting Distress and Cognitive Variables (N= 101) conducted to test such hypotheses. Specifically, compared to
high-perceived-support students, low-perceived-support sub-
Perceived Enacted jects were predicted to (a) evaluate novel supportive behaviors
Dependent variable support support /

Dysphoria -.45** -.11 3.14" 1


The p values for the effects of perceived support differed in the
Trait anxiety -.51" -.12 3.97"
Self-esteem .07 2.97"
equations predicting dysphoria and anxiety, even though the R2 change
.40"
Dysfunctional attitudes -.48" .01 4.69** values were identical, because the personality variables accounted for
Control beliefs .23* .02 1.81 more variance in anxiety (R2 = .56) than dysphoria (R2 = .41). Thus, the
error variance in anxiety was lower than that of dysphoria when the
*/><.05. *"p<.0l. perceived support-variable was entered.
340 BRIAN LAKEY AND PATRICIA BENNETT CASSADY

Table 3 send, 1980). This procedure is repeated six times, once for each hypo-
Maximum Likelihood factor Analysis With Varimax Rotation thetical stressful situation.
The SSERT provides three variables hypothesized to reflect cognitive
Variable Factor 1 Factor 2 social support processes. The first variable reflects respondents' per-
ceptions of the helpfulness of the supportive behaviors and represents
1. Trait anxiety .97 -.21 the sum of the helpfulness ratings across the 48 hems. The reliability of
2. Dysphoria .82 -.20
the helpfulness ratings in the present sample was alpha = 75. The other
3. Dysfunctional attitudes .51 -.32
-.64 .20 two variables represent recall processes and consist of the proportion
4. Self-esteem
5. Control beliefs .39 .11 of participants' negatively rated and positively rated supportive behav-
6. Perceived support -.33 .94 iors that are recalled.2
7. Enacted support -.06 .29 Other measures. In addition to the SSERT, participants completed
the same measures of dysphoria, self-esteem, and dysfunctional atti-
tudes described in Study 1. However, a different measure of perceived
support was used to enhance the generalizability of the results. This
was the Procidano and Heller (1983) scale, consisting of 40 items re-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

as less helpful, (b) recall a higher percentage of negatively rated


flecting perceptions of the supportiveness of both family and friends.3
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

supportive behaviors, and (c) recall a lower percentage of posi-


It was modified for the present research by including a 5-point scale
tively rated behaviors.
with strongly agree and strongly disagreeas extreme responses. This is a
In addition, this study attempted to replicate the results of widely used measure of perceived support with substantial evidence
Study 1, using a different measure of perceived social support. for its reliability and validity (Procidano & Heller, 1983).
To control for individual differences in memory ability, participants
were first administered a series of free-recall tasks. Each audiotaped
Study 2 list consisted of 20 nouns followed by a distractor task designed to
facilitate memory consolidation and to prevent rehearsal of the word
Method lists. The distractor task required students to read a short prose para-
graph and to mark each long e with a circle and each short e with a
Participants
slash. After 60 s, students were signaled to stop reading and to recall as
One hundred and one introductory psychology students volunteered many words from the audiotaped list as possible. The procedure was
to participate in the study in exchange for course credit. Fifty-nine repeated three times with different lists and paragraphs. The first two
students were women; and 41 were men. One student did not report his presentations were intended to familiarize the students with the task.
or her sex. Students participated in the study in small groups of 3-15. We calculated memory scores from the number of words recalled from
the third list.

Measures
Development of the Social Support Evaluation and Recall Task Results
(SSERT). The SSERT was constructed specifically for the present study
to assess individual differences in the evaluation and recall of support- To replicate the results of Study 1, the intercorrelations
ive behaviors. We modeled this measure after the Self-referent encod- among measures of dysphoria, self-esteem, dysfunctional atti-
ing task (Davis, 1979; Derry & Kuiper, 1981; Rogers, Kuiper, & Kirker tudes, and perceived support were examined. As in Study 1,
1977) and the Cognitive Bias Questionnaire (Hammen & Krantz, 1976; high perceived support was associated with fewer dysfunctional
Krantz & Hammen, 1979), both of which have been successfully ap-
attitudes (r = -.49, p < .001), higher self-esteem (r = .21, p =
plied to the study of cognitive processes in depression. The SSERT con-
.037) and less dysphoria (r = -.26, p = .008). As expected, peo-
sists of six paragraphs, each requesting the reader to imagine that he or
ple with low self-esteem and many dysfunctional attitudes expe-
she is describing a personal or academic problem to a friend or relative.
Each paragraph is accompanied by descriptions of eight hypothetical rienced more dysphoria (r = -.36, p < .001 and r= .35, p < .001,
supportive behaviors made by the friend or relative. We chose support- respectively). To determine whether the relation between per-
ive responses to be neither clearly supportive nor nonsupportive, to ceived support and dysphoria overlapped with the relation be-
allow for greater latitude in individual interpretation. For example, in tween negative cognition and symptoms, a hierarchical multi-
response to the situation in which the student experiences the breakup
of a romantic relationship, two supportive behaviors provided by a
parent are "Would you like to come home this weekend?" and "You'll
2
find someone else soon." After reading each paragraph, respondents Alpha was not computed for these scales because recall scores were
rate each supportive behavior for its helpfulness on a 5-point scale, based on different samples of supportive responses for each person.
ranging from very helpful to very unhelpful. Following these ratings, a This is because the particular recall score in which a given Hem is
60-s word association task is administered to prevent rehearsal of the included depends on the helpfulness rating provided by individual
responses. Experimenters present students with a tape-recorded list of participants. Thus, recall scores of positively- and negatively-rated sup-
30 nouns and adjectives and permit students to take 2 s to write down portive behaviors are based on different items and are not mere in-
the first word that comes to mind. On completion of the distractor verses of each other. The correlation between the two recall scores was
task, the hypothetical situation is presented again; students are asked r=-.02.
3
to recall as many of the previously rated supportive behaviors as they Consistent with Study 1, analyses that were based on subscales are
can. The paragraphs describing the stressful situation (but not the sup- not reported because there were no specific hypotheses concerning
portive behaviors) are presented again prior to recall because previous these scales. However, all of the findings reported in Study 2 hold for
research has shown that presentation of a thematic title facilitates both the Family and Friend subscales of the Procidano and Heller
schema activation in prose recall tasks(Dooling&Mullet,1973;Town- scales.
COGNITION AND SOCIAL SUPPORT 341

pie regression analysis was conducted in which BDI scores were that the slope of the relation between perceived support and
predicted from self-esteem, dysphoria, and perceived support, evaluations and recall did not differ as a function of sex.
respectively. Although self-esteem and dysfunctional attitudes
each made independent contributions to dysphoria (R = .45, General Discussion
F= 12.06, p < .001), the relation between perceived support
The main findings of these studies were that (a) perceived
and dysphoria was completely eliminated when the cognitive
support demonstrates a pattern of correlations more similar to
personality variables were controlled (r = -.26 versus partial
cognitive personality variables than to enacted support, (b)
r=.02).
much of the association between perceived support and psycho-
To determine the variables associated with biased interpreta-
logical distress can be accounted for by individual differences
tion of novel supportive behaviors, we calculated correlations
in negative cognition, (c) low perceived support is associated
between helpfulness ratings of the SSERT behaviors and dys-
with a bias toward perceiving supportive attempts as unhelpful,
phoria, perceived support, and the cognitive variables. Both
and (d) low perceived support is associated with a bias toward
low perceived support and high dysphoria were associated with
recalling fewer instances of helpful supportive behavior. These
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

judging the SSERT supportive behaviors as less helpful (r = .29,


last two findings could not be attributed to the effects of dys-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

p = .003 and r = —.27, p = .007, respectively), although neither phoria, as the relation between perceived support and helpful-
self-esteem (/•=—. 17, p = .097) nor dysfunctional attitudes (r = ness held when BDI scores were controlled statistically and dys-
.11, p = .299) predicted helpfulness ratings. To test whether phoria itself was unrelated to the recall of supportive behaviors.
perceived support predicted the perception of supportive be- Neither could self-esteem nor dysfunctional attitudes account
haviors independently of dysphoria, a multiple regression analy- for these effects, as these variables did not predict support per-
sis was conducted in which helpfulness was predicted by dys- ceptions or recall. Finally, the effects of perceived support could
phoria and then by perceived support. The results indicated not be explained as a mere reflection of negative affectivity
that perceived support scores predicted helpfulness ratings because of Study 1's finding that perceived support did not load
beyond dysphoria (R2 change = .053, F change (2, 98) = 5.82, highly on the Negative Affectivity factor.
p=.018). Although numerous investigations have demonstrated links
Multiple regression analyses also were conducted to deter- between support and distress, "social support research has not
mine whether perceived support would predict the recall of been theory driven, and so we still lack a basic understanding
positively and negatively perceived supportive behaviors. On of how social ties are health protective" (Heller, Swindle, &
the basis of schema theory, we predicted that low-perceived- Dusenbury, 1986, p. 466). The most important contribution of
support students would recall a greater proportion of negatively the present studies is that they provide clues as to how per-
rated behaviors and a smaller proportion of positively rated ceived support may influence psychological distress. These
behaviors as compared to high-support students. Because these studies support the hypothesis that perceived social support
predictions were made a priori, one-tailed tests of significance operates in part as a cognitive personality variable that influ-
were used. The memory-for-words variable was entered as the ences how supportive transactions with others will be inter-
first step, to control for individual differences in memory abil- preted and remembered. As such, this research creates a link
ity or performance motivation. This variable successfully pre- between social support research and work on cognitive pro-
dicted recall of positive (R = .20; F(l, 99) =4.16, p= .022) but cesses in emotional disorders (e.g. Beck, 1967,1976; Derry &
not negative (R - .10, F < 1) supportive behaviors. Perceived Kuiper, 1981; Gotlib, 1983; Krantz & Hammen, 1979). In the
support scores were entered as the second step and successfully same way that negative beliefs about the self may lead to dis-
predicted recall for positive supportive behaviors (R2 change = tress by negatively biasing information processing, persons
.048, Fchange (2,98) = 5.08, p = .013) but not negative behav- with low perceived support may be more distressed because of
iors (/•"= 1.18, m), indicating that persons with low-perceived- negative biases in evaluating and remembering the support that
support scores recalled fewer positive supportive behaviors they do receive. Of course, this does not mean that enacted
than those with high-support scores.11 To determine whether support does not play a role in the link between perceived sup-
the relation between perceived support and recall could be ac- port and disorder. Both Study 1 and previous research (e.g.
counted for by dysphoria or the cognitive variables, parallel Barrera, 1986) have documented relations between perceived
regression analyses were conducted with these variables en- support and recalled enacted support, although Study 1 indi-
tered as the second step. However, neither dysphoria, F(2,98) < cated that enacted support did not mediate the support-symp-
1, p = .35, dysfunctional attitudes, F(2,98) = 1.04, p = .31, nor tom relation. Instead, enacted support may operate indirectly
self-esteem, F(2, 98) = 2.56, p = .11, significantly predicted by contributing to the perception of support availability or by
recall of positively rated supportive behaviors. None of the vari- influencing coping behavior (Lakey & Heller, 1988).
ables in this study predicted recall of negatively rated support- Although perceived support showed important similarities
ive behaviors. to the cognitive personality variables, there were sufficient dif-
Finally, to determine whether the effects observed in these ferences to support the discriminant validity of the perceived-
data were conditional upon sex, multiple regression analyses
were conducted to test for Sex x Perceived Support interactions
in predicting both the evaluation and recall of supportive behav- 4
Perceived-support scores were significant predictors of recall of
iors (J. Cohen & Cohen, 1983). Neither of the interaction terms positively rated support behaviors regardless of whether memory
approached statistical significance (both Fs < 1), indicating scores were entered in the analysis.
342 BRIAN LAKEY AND PATRICIA BENNETT CASSADY

support construct. Unlike the cognitive personality variables, dardized laboratory stressful situations and providing them
perceived support (a) was related to enacted support, (b) formed with standardized supportive behaviors from confederates.
its own factor apart from distress, and (c) predicted specific Differences in how these supportive behaviors are interpreted
biases in processing support-related information. Thus, mea- and remembered could then be assessed.
sures of perceived support appear to reflect processes unique to In summary, this research investigated whether perceived
cognition about social relations that are not shared with other support operates in part as a cognitive personality variable.
measures of negative cognition. Unlike enacted support, perceived support correlated substan-
The possibility that perceived support operates akin to cog- tially with traditional cognitive personality variables, and much
nitive personality variables has implications for prevention pro- of the relation between perceived support and distress could be
grams designed to enhance social support. Most programs of accounted for by perceived support's similarity to these cogni-
this kind are oriented toward providing enacted support to peo- tive variables. Furthermore, low levels of perceived support
ple with low support levels. Perhaps such programs could en- were associated with negative biases in the evaluation of and
hance their effectiveness by including components to address memory for supportive behaviors. This research suggests the
the cognitive processes identified in the current research. As utility of further cross-fertilization between social support and
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

observed by Heller, Swindle, and Dusenbury (1986), "providing social cognition research.
support groups is not quite on target if the meaning of social
activity to the individual is not considered" (p. 468).
References
The results of the present research also mesh with recent
findings in social cognition. The finding that self-esteem and Barrera, M, Jr. (1986). Distinctions between social support concepts,
dysfunctional attitudes are closely related to perceived support measures, and models. American Journal ofCommunity Psychology,
is consistent with recent work suggesting that self-schemas are 14, 413-445.
involved in the judgment of others (Higgins, King, & Mavin, Barrera, M, Jr., Sandier, I. N, & Ramsey, T. B. (1981). Preliminary
studiesofascaleofsocialsupport:StudiesoncoIlegestudents. Amer-
1982; Lewicki, 1983; Markus, Smith, & Moreland, 1985). Simi-
ican Journal of Community Psychology, 9, 435-447.
larly, the finding that perceived support is associated with the
Beck, A. T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical
differential evaluation and recall of supportive behavior is con- aspects. New York: Harper & Row.
gruent with research suggesting that schemas influence how Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New
others are perceived, evaluated, and remembered (Bern, 1981; York: International Universities Press.
C. E. Cohen, 1980; Duncan, 1976; Howard & Rothbart, 1980; Bern, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex-
Lewicki, 1984; Lingle. Geva, Ostrom, Leippe, & Baumgardner, typing. Psychological Review, 88, 354-364.
1979; Markus & Zajonc, 1985; Taylor, Fiske, Etcoff, & Ruder- Cohen, C. E. (1980). Person categories and social perception: Testing
man, 1978). The similarity between the perceived support ef- some boundaries of the processing effects of prior knowledge. Jour-
fects observed in the present study and the findings of recent nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40, 441-452.
Cohen, J, & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation
social cognition research supports the utility of further cross-
analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaura.
fertilization between these areas.
Cohen, S, & Hoberman, H. (1983). Positive events and social supports
Finally, some important limitations of this research should as buffers of life change stress. Journal of Applied Social Psychology,
be noted. Although the identification of biases in the interpre- 13, 99-125.
tation and recall of social support was generated from a schema Cohen, S, Mermelstein, R, Kamarek, T, & Hoberman, H. (1985).
model of perceived social support, these data do not provide Measuring the functional components of social support. In I. G.
strong evidence that thought about social support is organized Sarason & B. R. Sarason (Eds.), Social support: Theory research, and
into cognitive structures or that these structures caused the ob- applications (pp. 73-94). The Hague, The Netherlands: Martinus
served effects (Higgins & Bargh, 1987; Segal, 1988). High- and Nijhoff.
low-perceived-support people may differ simply in the accessi- Cohen, S., & Wills, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the buffer-
ing hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310-357.
bility of positive versus negative support information, without
Coyne, J. C, & Gotlib, I. H. (1983). The role of cognition in depression:
these elements necessarily being linked. In addition, although a
A critical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 94,472-505.
major goal of this research was to help understand the pro- Davis, H. (1979). Self-reference and the encoding of personal informa-
cesses by which perceived support is linked to psychological tion in depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3, 97-111.
distress, the correlational design of the present studies prohibit DeMonbreun, B. G, & Craighead, W E. (1977). Distortion of percep-
any conclusions about causality. Establishing that the cognitive tion and recall of positive and neutral feedback in depression. Cogni-
mechanisms identified in this research play a causal role in tive Therapy and Research, 1, 311-329.
symptom development would require both prospective studies Derry, P. A, & Kuiper, K. A. (1981). Schematic processing and self-
showing that measures of these processes predict the develop- reference in clinical depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90,
286-297.
ment of future symptomatology and laboratory stress experi-
Dooling, D. J, & Mullet, R. L. (1973). Locus of thematic effects in
ments in which priming support schemas lead to differential
retention of prose. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 97,404-406.
perceptions of support and stress. Another limitation of the
Duncan, B. L. (1976). Differential social perception and attribution of
present research was the use of hypothetical, as opposed to real, intergroup violence: Testing the lower limits of stereotyping of
stressful situations and social support. Although investigators Blacks. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 590-598.
of social cognition often choose situations of lower external Gotlib, I. H. (1983). Perception and recall of interpersonal feedback:
validity to enhance experimental control, future research could Negative bias in depression. CognitiveTherapy and Research, 7,399-
enhance this validity somewhat by placing participants in stan- 412.
COGNITION AND SOCIAL SUPPORT 343

Hammen, C, & Krantz, S. (1976). Effects of success and failure on Procidano, M. E, & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social
depressive cognitions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 85,577-586. support from friends and family: Three validation studies. American
Hammen, C, & Krantz, S. E. (1985). Measures of psychological pro- Journal of Community Psychology, 11,1-24.
cesses in depression. In E. E. Beckham & W R. Leber (Eds.), Hand- Rizley. R. (1978). Depression and distortion in the attribution of cau-
book of depression: Treatment, assessment, and research (pp. 408- sality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 32-48.
444). Homewood, IL: Dorsey Rogers, T. B, Kuiper, N. A, & Kirker, W S. (1977). Self-reference and
Heller, K, & Lakey, B. (1985). Perceived support and social interaction the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personality and
among friends and confidants. In I. G. Sarason & B. R. Sarason Social Psychology, 35, 677-688.
(Eds.), Social support: Theory research and applications (pp. 287- Rosenberg, M. (1979). Conceiving the self New York: Basic Books.
300). The Hague, The Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff. Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus exter-
Heller, K, & Swindle, R. W (1983). Social networks, perceived social nal control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 80 (1,
support and coping with stress. In R. D. Felner, L. A. Jason, J. Mori- Whole No. 609).
tsugu, & S. S. Farber (Eds), Preventive psychology: Research and Sandier, I. N, & Barrera, M, Jr. (1984). Toward a multimethod ap-
practice in community intervention (pp. 87-103). New York: Perga- proach to assessing the effects of social support. American Journal of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

mon. Community Psychology, 12, 37-52.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Heller, K, Swindle, R. W, & Dusenbury, L. (1986). Component social Sandier, I. N, & Lakey, B. (1982). Locus of control as a stress modera-
support processes: Comments and integration. Journal of Consulting tor The role of control perceptions and social support. American
and Clinical Psychology 54, 466-470. Journal of Community Psychology, 10,65-80.
Higgins, E. T, & Bargh, J. A. (1987). Social cognition and social per- Sarason, I. G, Sarason, B. R, & Shearin, E. N. (1986). Social support as
ception. Annual Review of Psychology, 38, 369-425. an individual difference variable: Its stability, origins, and relational
Higgins, E. T, King, G. A, & Mavin, G. H. (1982). Individual construct aspects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 845-855.
accessibility and subjective impressions and recall. Journal of Person- Segal, Z. V (1988). Appraisal of the self-schema construct in cognitive
ality and Social Psychology, 43, 35-47. models of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 103,147-162.
Hirschberg, N., & Jennings, S. J. (1980). Beliefs, personality, and person Segal, Z. V, & Shaw, B. F. (1986). Cognition in depression: A reappraisal
perception: A theory of individual differences. Journal of Research of Coyne and Gotlibs critique. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 10,
in Personality, 14, 235-249. 671-693.
Howard, J. W, & Rothbart, M. (1980). Social categorization and mem- Spielberger, C. Q, Gorsuch, R. L, & Lushene, R. E. (1970). Manual for
ory for in-group and out-group behavior. Journal of Personality and the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychol-
Social Psychology, 38, 301-310. ogists Press.
Kiesler, C. A. (1985). Policy implications of research on social support Sweeney, E D, Anderson, K, & Bailey, S. (1986). Attributional style in
and health. In S. Cohen & S. L. Sy me (Eds), Social support and health depression: A m eta-analytic review. Journal of Personality and Social
(pp. 347-364). Orlando, FL: Academic Press. Psychology, SO, 974-991.
Krantz, S., & Hammen, C. (1979). Assessment of cognitive bias in Taylor, S. E., Fiske, S. T, Etcoff, N, & Ruderman, A. (1978). The cate-
depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 611-619. gorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping.
Kuiper, N. A. (1978). Depression and causal attributions for success Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778-793.
and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 236- Thoits, PA.(1985). Social support and well-being: Theoretical possibil-
246. ities. In I. G. Sarason & B. R. Sarason (Eds), Social support: Theory,
Lakey, B., & Heller, K. (1988). Perceived support, support from a research and applications (pp. 51-72). The Hague, The Netherlands:
friend, and social problem solving. American Journal of Community Martinus Nijhoff.
Psychology, 16, 811-824. Thoits, R A. (1986). Social support as coping assistance. Journal of
Lefcourt, H. M, Martin, R. A, Saleh, W E. (1984). Locus of control Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,416-423.
and social support: Interactive moderators of stress. Journal of Per- Townsend, M. A. R. (1980). Schema activation in memory for prose.
sonality and Social Psychology, 47, 378-389. Journal of Reading Behavior, 12, 49-53.
Lewicki, P. (1983). Self-image bias in person perception. Journal of Walker, H. M, & Lev, J. (1953). Statistical inference (p. 257). New York:
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 384-393. Holt.
Lewicki, P. (1984). Self-schema and social information processing. Watson, D, & Clark, L. A. (1984). Negative affectivity: The predisposi-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47,1177-1190. tion to experience aversive emotional states. Psychological Bulletin,
Lingle, J. H, Geva, N, Ostrom, T. M, Leippe, M. R, & Baumgardner, 96, 465-490.
M. H. (1979). Thematic effects of person judgments on impression Weissman, A. M,& Beck, A. T. (1978). Development and validation of
organization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 674- the Dysfunctional Attitude Scale: A preliminary investigation. Paper
687. presented at the meeting of the American Educational Research
Association, Toronto, Ontario.
Markus, H, Smith, J, & Moreland, R. L. (1985). Role of the self-con-
Wetherington, E, & Kessler, R. C. (1986). Perceived support, received
cept in the perception of others. Journal of Personality and Social
support and adjustment to stressful life events. Journal of Health and
Psychology, 49,1494-1512.
Social Behavior. 27, 78-89.
Markus, H, & Zajonc, R. E (1985). The cognitive perspective in social
psychology. In G. Lindzey, & E. Aronson (Eds), Handbook of social
psychology (3rd ed. Vol. 1, pp. I37-229). New York: Random House. Received February 26,1988
Mirels. H. L. (1970). Dimensions of internal versus external control. Revision received November 16,1989
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34, 226-228. Accepted December 21,1989 •

Potrebbero piacerti anche