Sei sulla pagina 1di 10

Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Computers in Human Behavior


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh

The emotional responses of browsing Facebook: Happiness, envy,


and the role of tie strength
Ruoyun Lin ⇑, Sonja Utz
Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien (Knowledge Media Research Center), Tübingen, Germany

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: On Facebook, users are exposed to posts from both strong and weak ties. Even though several studies
Available online 10 June 2015 have examined the emotional consequences of using Facebook, less attention has been paid to the role
of tie strength. This paper aims to explore the emotional outcomes of reading a post on Facebook and
Keywords: examine the role of tie strength in predicting happiness and envy. Two studies – one correlational, based
Facebook on a sample of 207 American participants and the other experimental, based on a sample of 194 German
Emotional contagion participants – were conducted in 2014. In Study 2, envy was further distinguished into benign and
Happiness
malicious envy. Based on a multi-method approach, the results showed that positive emotions are more
Social comparison
Envy
prevalent than negative emotions while browsing Facebook. Moreover, tie strength is positively
Tie strength associated with the feeling of happiness and benign envy, whereas malicious envy is independent of
tie strength after reading a (positive) post on Facebook.
Ó 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd. This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license
(http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).

1. Introduction and theoretical background 1.1. Psychological effects of social media usage

New communication technologies such as social media have The use of social media such as Facebook can cause both posi-
made social news more pervasive (Bazarova, 2012). Facebook con- tive and negative feelings, and the results of prior studies on the
tinuously keeps users updated with a variety of posts, and passive psychological effects of social media usage are quite mixed. From
consumption of news updates is the main Facebook activity that a long-term perspective, the use of social media offers benefits
people engage in (Wise, Alhabash, & Park, 2010). The majority of such as the possibility of developing and maintaining social capital
these updates are positive (Barash, Ducheneaur, Isaacs, & Bellotti, and social connectedness (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007;
2010; Utz, 2015). There is evidence for emotional contagion, show- Grieve, Indian, Witteveen, Anne Tolan, & Marrington, 2013;
ing happiness can spread through the news updates on online Sheldon, Abad, & Hinsch, 2011); Nevertheless, it may also lead to
social networks (Coviello et al., 2014; Kramer, Guillory, & negative outcomes such as social overload (Maier, Laumer,
Hancock, 2014). However, recent studies also indicate that expo- Eckhardt, & Weitzel, 2012), an over-optimistic perception towards
sure to positive posts on Facebook may induce envy and lead to others’ lives (Chou & Edge, 2012), and a decrease in life satisfaction
depression (Steers, Wickham, & Acitelli, 2014; Tandoc, Ferrucci, & (Kross et al., 2013). From a short-term perspective, the use of
Duffy, 2014) and reduced well-being over time (Kross et al., Facebook can evoke a feeling of flow, which is characterized by
2013; Verduyn et al., 2015). Given that Facebook has over 1.35 high positive valence and high arousal (Mauri, Cipresso, Balgera,
billion active users (Facebook, 2014) and there are on average Villamira, & Riva, 2011), and ‘‘joyful and fun’’ are the most common
1500 potential stories for users to check per visit (Facebook, positive feelings reported by users while using Facebook
2013), we are eager to understand how Facebook affects users’ (Krasnova, Wenninger, Widjaja, & Buxmann, 2013). Nonetheless,
emotions and identify relevant factors that will determine emo- the consumption of social news on Facebook can also trigger invid-
tional reactions. We argue that tie strength (relationship closeness) ious emotions such as jealousy and envy (Krasnova et al., 2013;
between the user and the poster is one important factor that Muise, Christofides, & Desmarais, 2009; Tandoc et al., 2014).
should affect emotional outcomes. Faced with mixed results from prior research on the psycholog-
ical effects of Facebook usage, it is important to differentiate
between interactive and non-interactive social media behavior
⇑ Corresponding author at: Leibniz-Institut für Wissensmedien (Knowledge
(Burke, Marlow, & Lento, 2010; große Deters & Mehl, 2013; Wise
Media Research Center), Schleichstr. 6, 72076 Tübingen, Germany. Tel.: +49 7071
979 347.
et al., 2010). Previous research has shown a consistent relation
E-mail address: r.lin@iwm-tuebingen.de (R. Lin). between using FB for interpersonal interaction and positive

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.04.064
0747-5632/Ó 2015 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.
This is an open access article under the CC BY-NC-ND license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
30 R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38

psychological outcomes (Burke et al., 2010; Hampton, Goulet, RQ2: What is the role of tie strength in explaining emotional
Marlow, & Rainie, 2012; Kim & Lee, 2011). However, it remains outcomes such as happiness and envy?
unclear as to what the psychological outcomes of non-interactive Facebook users often post about their positive life events, suc-
Facebook behavior are. cesses, and entertaining status updates (Utz, 2011, 2015), and
sometimes even present themselves in overly flatting ways
1.2. Psychological effects of browsing Facebook (Barash et al., 2010; Mehdizadeh, 2010; Qiu, Lin, Leung, & Tov,
2012). Especially after reading these types of posts, feelings of hap-
In this paper, we focus only on the psychological effects of pas- piness and envy are common emotional responses. Two mecha-
sive consumption. Previous research has revealed that the more nisms can explain this phenomenon: emotional contagion and
time individuals spent browsing Facebook, the more negative their upward social comparison. The feeling of happiness can be
mood was immediately afterwards, and this was mediated by a explained by the effects of emotional contagion (Cheshin, Rafaeli,
feeling of not having done anything meaningful (Sagioglou & & Bos, 2011; Hatfield, Cacioppo, & Rapson, 1993), i.e., the tendency
Greitemeyer, 2014). Users may experience the feelings of exhaus- for two individuals to emotionally converge; whereas the feeling of
tion and information overload when they are continuously brows- envy can be explained as a result of upward social comparison
ing social news (Maier et al., 2012). Also, being exposed to social (Festinger, 1954).
news on Facebook can evoke feelings of envy (Krasnova et al., In the following parts, we provide further information on these
2013). However, when it comes to momentary experiences, users two mechanisms and explain why tie strength could matter for the
often feel pleasant and positively aroused when browsing emotions of happiness and envy respectively.
Facebook (Mauri et al., 2011; Wise et al., 2010).
In an exploratory study (Krasnova et al., 2013), participants 1.3.1. Happiness: emotional contagion and the role of tie strength
were asked to report their overall feelings after their most recent Humans, as a species that are embedded in complex social net-
Facebook usage: 43.8% of the respondents reported at least one works, rely on the ability of sharing others’ emotions (empathy) to
positive emotional outcome (such as feeling joyful/fun, satisfied, engage in successful social interactions (Norscia & Palagi, 2011;
informed, excited, and relaxed), and 36.8% of the respondents Preston & de Waal, 2002). The ability to catch or experience other’s
reported at least one negative emotional outcome (such as feeling feelings leads to the phenomenon of ‘‘emotional contagion’’, which
bored, angry, frustrated, guilty, tired, sad, lonely, and envious). has been described as ‘‘the tendency to automatically mimic and
However, it is unclear as to whether different feelings are triggered synchronize expressions, vocalizations, postures, and movements
by different posts or whether a post can elicit several feelings. To with those of another person’s, and, consequently, to converge
the best of our knowledge, no research has been done that focuses emotionally’’ (Hatfield et al., 1993). Recent studies found that emo-
on momentary feelings of browsing Facebook on an individual mes- tions such as happiness can be transferred from one person to
sage level. To get a more comprehensive understanding on whether another not only in face-to-face communication (Hancock, Gee,
reading (positive) posts on Facebook leads to more positive emo- Ciaccio, & Lin, 2008; Hatfield et al., 1993; Neumann & Strack,
tions such as happiness or more negative emotions such as envy 2000), but also in computer-mediated-communication (Cheshin
and boredom, we examine the feelings respondents report per post et al., 2011; Coviello et al., 2014; Kramer, 2012). Therefore, observ-
on their Facebook News Feeds. Before we turn to the underlying ing others’ positive news on Facebook may lead to happiness via
processes, we examine the prevalence of positive and negative emotional contagion.
emotions. Our first research question is therefore: Previous research (in offline settings) demonstrates that empa-
RQ1: What are the most prevalent momentary emotional out- thy is more pronounced when the relationship between two indi-
comes of reading a post on Facebook? viduals within a dyad is closer (Beeney, Franklin, Levy, & Adams,
2011; Norscia & Palagi, 2011; Preston & de Waal, 2002). This is
1.3. Underlying mechanisms and the role of tie strength because kin relationships were extremely important to our ances-
tors’ survival. And thus, the ability to empathize with close others
More importantly, we are also interested in understanding the would have facilitated social interactions (Norscia & Palagi, 2011).
underlying mechanisms of the positive and negative emotions In addition, based on the Perception–Action Model for empathy,
caused by passive consumption of social news, and aim to examine individuals with higher similarity and familiarity are more likely
the role of relationship closeness in explaining those emotional to catch emotions from each other (Preston & de Waal, 2002).
outcomes (mainly happiness and envy). In a social network con- Therefore, we would expect a positive moderating effect of tie
text, relationship closeness is often intertwined with the expres- strength on emotional contagion even in the computer-mediated
sion of ‘‘tie strength’’ (Gilbert, 2012): A strong tie is usually a communication context (e.g., when reading a Facebook post).
close friend or family member, that with whom one shares an inti- More specifically, positive news may lead to happiness, and nega-
mate relationship; and a weak tie is usually an acquaintance that tive news may lead to sadness because of mood contagion, and the
one does not feel emotionally close to. As tie strength can also be contagious effect is stronger when the news comes from a strong
measured by relationship closeness (Marsden & Campbell, 1984), tie.
in this paper, we treat ‘‘tie strength’’ and ‘‘relationship closeness’’
as interchangeable. 1.3.2. Envy: social comparison and the role of tie strength
On Facebook, users can be exposed to posts from a variety of Envy, a pain caused by the good fortune of others, is another
different people, including acquaintances, colleagues, best friends, potential emotional outcome of encountering positive news on
and family members. Thus, the emotional outcomes of reading a Facebook (Krasnova et al., 2013), and it might be a reason why
post might not only depend on the content of the post, but may browsing Facebook can lead to depression (Tandoc et al., 2014;
also be influenced by the relationship between the poster and Verduyn et al., 2015). Hence, we are also interested in understand-
reader. For example, if your best friend posts good or bad news ing the processes underlying envy.
on Facebook you might react differently to this compared to seeing The concept of envy is often confused with jealousy, but clear
an acquaintance that you haven’t talked to in years posting the differences exist: Jealousy arises when one has something but is
same news. No research has been done to examine the role of tie afraid of losing it or has lost it to another person, while envy arises
strength in interpreting the emotional outcomes after reading a when another person has something that one does not have
post. Our second research question is therefore: (Parrott & Smith, 1993). Recent literature on envy further
R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38 31

established two different types of envy: benign envy and malicious


envy (Smith & Kim, 2007; Van de Ven, Zeelenberg, & Pieters, 2009).
Benign envy leads to a moving-up motivation (i.e., achieving the
desired attribute by improving one’s own situation), and malicious
envy leads to a pulling-down motivation (i.e., an intention to dam-
age the position of the superior other).
Envy generally arises from engaging in upward comparison(s)
(Nabi & Keblusek, 2014; Van de Ven, Zeelenberg, & Pieters, 2012),
i.e., when people compare themselves with better-off others
(Mussweiler, Rüter, & Epstude, 2004). Several researchers have
investigated the pre-conditions and underlying mechanisms for Fig. 1. Research model.

individuals to experience envy. In Festinger’s (1954) classic theory


of social comparison, he claimed that people have a desire to know
2. Exploratory and correlational examination (Study 1)
about their own opinions and abilities, and when the reference to
the physical world is not clear, people tend to compare themselves
2.1. Study overview and hypothesis
to others. In essence, he suggested that people select comparison
standards that are similar to themselves on the critical dimension,
In Study 1, we examined the momentary emotional states of
in order to get diagnostic information for self-evaluation (Festinger,
Facebook users after reading a post, and investigated the role of
1954). Similarly, Smith (2004) proposed four pre-conditions for
tie strength in predicting happiness and envy. We asked partici-
envy: being similar to the compared person, perceiving the situation
pants to report four recent posts from their News Feeds, as well
as self-relevant, the desired object is hard to achieve (or low per-
as their feelings after reading each post. As it is still unclear what
ceived control), and the feeling that the person does not deserve
the most prevalent momentary feelings are, an open research
the object (or one’s own inferior situation is perceived to be unde-
question (RQ1) was posed in this study.
served). The latter two factors were found to be the key appraisals
With regard to RQ2, we expected a positive moderating effect of
that distinguished benign from malicious envy: The more a situa-
tie strength on the relationship between the content of posts and
tion was perceived as fair and controllable, the more benign envy
the feeling of happiness based on the mechanism of emotional con-
rather than malicious envy was experienced, while malicious envy
tagion. Therefore,
was experienced when the situation was appraised as undeserved
for the compared person (Van de Ven et al., 2012).
H1. Tie strength positively moderates the relationship between
Even though previous research has addressed the role of per-
the content of the posts and the feeling of happiness.
ceived similarity with the compared person, it is still unclear what
role tie strength or relationship closeness plays in predicting the The role of tie strength in predicting envy is still ambiguous in
feeling of envy. Tesser (1988) argued in his Self-Evaluation such an exploratory setting. As indicated by Tesser’s (1988) theory,
Maintenance theory (SEM) that an upward comparison is most the effect of tie strength on feeling of envy is further moderated by
threatening when the superior other is psychologically close, and the compared domain. But it is difficult to know about which
social comparisons to strong ties may be a double-edged sword. domain the Facebook posts are involved with and whether this will
On the one hand, self-evaluation can be damaged when individuals be central or not to the users’ self-evaluation, therefore no predic-
are comparing themselves with a close friend who performs well in tions can be made with regard to the feeling of envy in this study.
a domain that is also important to them, which is called ‘‘compar-
ison process’’ in SEM. On the other hand, self-evaluation can be 2.2. Method
improved when the close others perform well in a domain that is
irrelevant to one’s self-definition, which is called ‘‘reflection pro- 2.2.1. Procedure and participants
cess’’ in SEM. Hence, based on the theories of social comparison An online questionnaire for active Facebook users was launched
and self-evaluation, we assume that the relationship closeness or in July 2014. Participants were asked to log into their own
tie strength also plays a role in predicting feelings such as envy Facebook accounts and browse the recent updates in their News
in the online communication context; envy should be more intense Feeds. If they had previously read the recent posts in their News
in response to posts of a strong tie (but only in a domain that is Feeds before, they were asked to recall their feelings at the time
important to self-definition). that they first read the posts; otherwise, they were asked to report
To sum up, no empirical research has been done which focuses their current feelings after reading those posts. Every respondent
on the role of tie strength/relationship closeness in interpreting or was asked to report the four most recent posts from four different
predicting the user’s emotions (mainly happiness and envy) after posters regardless of the source of the post (Facebook Friend vs.
reading a post on Facebook. In this paper, two studies, one correla- Facebook Page). We asked participants to report the posts from
tional and the other experimental, are presented, aiming to address Facebook Pages (in addition to Facebook Friends), because that
the research questions on momentary feelings after reading a post on information will be used in another separate paper. However, only
Facebook and the role of tie strength. The basic research model for the posts from Facebook Friends were included in this paper. We
both studies is presented in Fig. 1. We expected that tie strength also limited the reporting to four updates because we were con-
would positively moderate the effect of the content of Facebook cerned that too much repetitive reporting may lead to survey
posts on users’ emotions, such as strengthening the feelings of hap- fatigue.
piness or sadness; nevertheless, the role of tie strength in predict- In order to investigate the role of tie strength, we used the feel-
ing envy is dependent on the context and requires further ings of happiness and envy as the dependent variables; both the
investigation. More explanation on how tie strength matters in valence of post content (e.g., positivity and entertainment) and
the case of envy is presented in the introduction to the second the perceived relationship closeness between the participant and
study. Finally, because emotions after reading posts can also be the Facebook Friend were treated as independent variables.
influenced by individual characteristics such as prevailing mood A total of 207 respondents completed the questionnaire. A
and personality traits, we also measure these factors and include majority of them (92.3%) were American. Respondents were
them as control variables in our research. mainly recruited from a survey panel called ‘‘Tellwut’’; some were
32 R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38

recruited by a post on Reddit (http://www.reddit.com/r/ Table 1


SampleSize/). Respondents’ average age was 41.7 (SD = 14.6), and Descriptive statistics in Study 1.

36.2% of them were male. Most of the respondents (81.2%) Variables n Mean SD Min Max
reported that they visited Facebook daily, and, on average, for Happiness (DV) 587 5.18 1.64 1 7
1 hour per day. Connectedness 589 5.14 1.74 1 7
Informativeness 582 5.04 1.75 1 7
2.2.2. Measures Entertainment 583 4.63 1.87 1 7
Envy (DV) 574 2.27 1.78 1 7
Content of the posts. Participants were asked to copy and paste Jealousy 576 2.20 1.72 1 7
the original post if they felt comfortable doing so. Then, they were Annoyance 571 2.15 1.66 1 7
asked to rate whether the content of the post was negative or pos- Frustration 577 2.14 1.64 1 7
itive, boring or entertaining, superficial or intimate, and factual or Positive content (IV) 598 5.51 1.87 1 7
Relationship closeness (IV) 598 4.42 1.97 1 7
subjective on 7-point semantic differential scales.
Mood 598 4.91 1.25 1 7
Relationship closeness/Tie strength. The relationship between the Self-esteem 598 4.76 1.54 1 7
reader (the participant) and the poster (a Facebook Friend) was
measured by two items: ‘‘We have a close relationship/ friendship’’
and ‘‘I would categorize him/her as one of my strong ties’’ (on a
7-point Likert scale, where 1 = ‘‘strongly disagree’’ and Table 2
Correlation statistics in Study 1.
7 = ‘‘strongly agree’’). A numerical average was taken to present
the relationship between the poster and reader (Cronbach’s Pearson’s Correlations 1 2 3 4 5
a = 0.96). 1. Happiness (DV) 1.00
Emotions. Participants were asked to evaluate their emotions 2. Envy (DV) 0.13** 1.00
with several items after reading each post; ‘‘I feel pleasant’’ and 3. Positive content 0.69*** 0.09* 1.00
4. Relationship closeness 0.32*** 0.00 0.22*** 1.00
‘‘I feel envious’’ were used as key dependent variables for this
5. Mood 0.26*** 0.10* 0.16** 0.20*** 1.00
study. The answers were given on 7-point Likert scales 6. Self-esteem 0.12** 0.25*** 0.09* 0.10* 0.37***
(1 = ‘‘strongly disagree’’ and 7 = ‘‘strongly agree’’), and it was also
*
possible to select ‘‘does not apply’’, which was treated as a missing p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
value later. We also measured other momentary feelings such as ***
p < 0.001.
jealousy, frustration, annoyance, and connectedness for explora-
tory purposes.
Control variables. The mood state of the respondents before the the post and relationship closeness on the feeling of happiness,
questionnaire was measured as control variable. We also measured but left the prediction open for envy. In addition, we added prior
the respondents’ self-esteem with 6 items adapted from mood state and self-esteem as control variables. Two
Rosenberg’s (1965) scale (Cronbach’s a = 0.91), in addition to random-effects multi-level linear models are presented in
demographic information. Table 3. All variables were standardized, and therefore the effect
sizes are based on the unstandardized coefficients.
2.3. Results The first model was constructed to predict the feeling of happi-
ness, overall R2 = .54, Wald v2(5) = 398.37, p < .001. The results of
Among the 828 reported posts (4 cases per person), 598 posts Model 1 showed that both positive content, b = 0.62, z = 16.30,
were posted by a Facebook friend (as opposed to a Facebook page), p < .001, and relationship closeness, b = 0.09, z = 2.89, p = .004,
which is the focus of this study; the rest of the reported posts predicted the feeling of happiness, and there was a significant
(27.8%) were deleted in the following analysis. With regard to interaction effect between positive content and relationship
the likelihood of experiencing positive and negative emotions, closeness, b = 0.08, z = 2.22, p = .027, supporting H1. Specifically,
we treated an answer given between 5 and 7 (on a 7-Likert scale) respondents were happier after reading positive news from a
as experiencing that specific feeling. The descriptive results of the closer friend; similarly, the respondents were sadder after reading
emotional outcomes can be found in Table 1. Respondents felt negative news from a closer friend.
mostly positive feelings after reading the post: in 384 cases With regard to the second model, overall R2 = .08, Wald
2
(64.2%), the respondent felt pleasant after reading the post. Most v (5) = 35.32, p < .001, relationship closeness did not play a role
respondents agreed that they felt connected (66.4%), informed in predicting the feeling of envy: there was no main effect of
(63.7%), and entertained (53.7%) after reading a post. The feeling relationship closeness, b = 0.03, z = 0.68, p = .497, nor was there
of envy was found in 74 reported cases, which was 12.4% of the an interaction between relationship closeness and content,
total cases. In this study, being envious (12.4%) was the most fre- b = 0.02, z = 0.48, p = .628. Positive content was a significant predic-
quently reported negative feeling, compared with being jealous tor of envy, b = 0.16, z = 4.00, p < .001. In addition, Facebook users
(11.0%), annoyed (10.0%) and frustrated (9.7%). Overall, findings who have a higher self-esteem were less likely to feel envious,
from this study suggest that browsing Facebook is more likely to b = 0.25, z = 3.84, p < .001.
trigger various positive feelings as opposed to negative feelings
on an individual message level. When it comes to the content of 2.4. Discussion
the posts, the result shows that about 72.6% of the content from
the News Feeds were positive and 62.2% of them were The results of Study 1 showed that most of the posts on
entertaining. Facebook are positive and entertaining, which is in line with previ-
In the next step of the analysis, we examined the roles of con- ous findings (Barash et al., 2010; Utz, 2015), and positive emotions
tent and tie strength by running several multi-level linear models, are more prevalent than negative emotions when browsing
because the posts were nested in participants. A descriptive sum- Facebook. Tie strength was found to moderate the relationship
mary of important variables can be found in Table 1 and correla- between the content of the post and the feeling of happiness. As
tions between these variables are shown in Table 2. The feelings predicted by H1, the effect of emotional contagion was stronger
of happiness and envy were treated as dependent variables respec- when the tie strength is stronger; however, we did not find a mod-
tively. We expected an interaction effect between the content of erating effect of tie strength on the feeling of envy.
R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38 33

Table 3 posts, controlling for the content of the post and manipulating
Results of the random-effects multi-level linear models tested in Study 1 (unstan- tie strength. We focus specifically on scenarios that are positive,
dardized coefficient followed by z values based on robust standard errors).
such as posts about ‘‘travel and leisure’’ and ‘‘success’’. These types
Happiness Envy of positive posts can lead to upward comparisons and envy
Positive content 0.625 0.161 (Krasnova et al., 2013). In addition, most of the posts on
(16.30)*** (4.00)*** Facebook are quite positive and entertaining, as a result of impres-
Relationship closeness 0.095 0.031 sion management motives and privacy concerns (Barash et al.,
(2.89)** (0.68)
Positive content  Relationship closeness 0.078 0.016
2010; Utz, 2015).
(2.22)* (0.48) As previously discussed, emotions such as happiness are conta-
Mood 0.178 0.028 gious, and this effect is stronger for those who share a close rela-
(3.46)** (0.39) tionship. Therefore, we hypothesize a positive effect of
Self-esteem 0.045 0.258
relationship closeness on the feeling of happiness, in order to repli-
(1.08) (3.84)***
Constant 0.018 0.010 cate the findings of Study 1.
(0.47) (0.17)
N 587 574 H2. The closer the relationship, the happier a Facebook user will
Note. Random effects models were preferred based on the results of Hausman tests.
be after reading a positive post.
All variables were standardized before putting into the models.
*
p < 0.05. Research has shown that benign envy is an uplifting type of
**
p < 0.01. envy: people want to be closer to the comparison other, regard-
***
p < 0.001. less of the feeling of frustration (Van de Ven et al., 2009). In addi-
tion, Mussweiler et al. (2004) claimed that assimilation, i.e., the
As the positive emotions outweigh the negative emotions, self is assimilated toward a given standard, is more likely if the
browsing Facebook – at least on a message level (for a relatively target and the comparison other share a close relationship.
short amount of time) – is not as psychologically harmful as Based on the definition of benign envy and Mussweiler et al.’s
described in certain research (e.g., Konnikova, 2013, September (2004) concept of assimilation, we assume that a close and inti-
10). Users are happy after reading positive posts from their mate relationship between the reader and poster helps to develop
Facebook friends, and are even happier if the good news comes good will and the motivation of levelling-up; therefore, benign
from a strong tie; whereas envious feelings are more likely to be envy is more likely to happen when an envy-inducing post is
predicted by individual characteristics of the user such as low posted by a strong tie rather than a weak tie. Whereas for mali-
self-esteem, rather than relationship closeness. It seems that tie cious envy, we would expect an opposite effect: people are less
strength does not play a role in predicting envy. For negative news, likely to experience malicious envy, a pulling down motivation,
Facebook users experience more negative emotions when it comes towards their strong ties/best friends compared with other
from a strong tie than a weak tie. This phenomenon was recently less-intimate friends. As suggested by Tesser’s (1988) theory,
described as ‘‘cost of caring’’ in the context of Facebook individuals may do things, such as pulling down other’s perfor-
(Hampton, Rainie, Lu, Shin, & Purcell, 2015). However, one should mance, in order to reduce the decrease in self-evaluation.
notice that individuals have to bear the negative news from their Whether someone will get in way of other’s performance is
strong ties in real life anyway, as Facebook might not be the only dependent on the closeness of the individuals and the relevance
communicational channel for strong ties. of the compared domain. When the compared domain is not cen-
Here, we would also like to discuss the strengths and weak- tral to one’s self-definition, the reflection process is more impor-
nesses of this study. A strength of the study is that we did not tant than the comparison process (Tesser, 1988). We assume that
use a student sample, but a more heterogeneous sample. A limita- most of Facebook posts, such as a vacation picture, are not
tion is that it was a correlational study and therefore causality can- self-evaluation threatening, and less malicious envy is expected
not be assumed. For example, it is hard to say whether users feel when it comes from a close friend. Hence,
happy is merely because the post is shared by a strong tie, or it
is because positive posts are more likely to be posted by a strong H3. The closer the relationship, the more a Facebook user will
tie. Also, with regard to the null effects of relationship closeness experience benign envy.
on envy, this could either be explained by a floor effect (the mean
value for the degree of envy is relatively low), or that relationship
closeness is not influential when predicting envy. In order to fur- H4. The closer the relationship, the less a Facebook user will expe-
ther disentangle the puzzle, an experimental study is needed. rience malicious envy.
Moreover, previous research investigating the emotion of envy Although envy is a common emotion felt by most people, there
on social media has not addressed the difference between malicious are important individual differences in the tendency to feel it
and benign envy. In the next study, we differentiate between benign (Smith, Parrott, Diener, Hoyle, & Kim, 1999). For Facebook users
and malicious envy in the context of Facebook, and add with higher dispositional envy, we would expect that they are more
‘‘self-relevance’’ and ‘‘perceived control’’ as additional control vari- likely to engage in upward social comparisons and experience
ables. Several hypotheses are proposed with regard to the role of more (benign & malicious) envy after reading a positive post on
relationship closeness in predicting user’s happiness and envy after Facebook. Also, we included self-relevance and perceived control as
reading a post. control variables based on the social comparison literature. As
described earlier, four preconditions are relevant: perceived simi-
larity, self-relevant, perceived control, and perceived deserved-
3. Experimental examination of the role of tie strength (Study 2) ness/unfairness (Smith, 2004). However, perceived similarity is
highly correlated with tie strength, and perceived unfairness is
3.1. Study overview and hypotheses an item used in measuring malicious envy. Therefore, we only con-
trolled for self-relevance and perceived control when predicting
In order to replicate and extend the correlational findings from envy. For predicting feelings of happiness, we used dispositional
Study 1, we conducted an experiment with mocked-up Facebook happiness and mood as control variables.
34 R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38

3.2. Method have a new iPhone’’ (Cronbach’s a = 0.74 for vacation scenario,
and Cronbach’s a = 0.45 for iPhone scenario). These items were
3.2.1. Participants and procedure adapted from the scales used in Crusius and Lange’s (2014) study.
An online experiment was launched in Germany on Sep 22nd, Both benign and malicious envy were calculated by taking a
2014. Participants were mainly German students who use numerical average of the respective three items. Responses were
Facebook at least once a month, and they were recruited via a local measured on 7-point Likert scales ranging from ‘‘strongly disagree’’
panel. The experiment took about 20 min to complete, and for their to ‘‘strongly agree’’.
participation, respondents were entered into a lottery pool (with a Control variables. Again, we treated the mood of the respondents
20% chance of winning a 10 Euro Amazon voucher). In total, 194 and the demographical information of the respondents as control
participants completed the questionnaire. We deleted the cases variables. Furthermore, dispositional envy was measured with an
that failed the manipulation checks (see details in the end of pro- 8-item scale (Cronbach’s a = 0.85) developed by Smith et al.
cedure session) and those who finished the questionnaire and (1999) and dispositional happiness was measured with a 6-item
important questions too fast (by excluding 5% of cases with the scale (Cronbach’s a = .88) developed by Shiota, Keltner, and John
lowest response time for specific pages). Hence, 146 cases were left (2006). One sample item for measuring dispositional envy is ‘‘feel-
for the analysis. The average age was 25 years old (SD = 6.3). About ings of envy constantly torment me’’, and for dispositional happi-
16.4% of respondents were male. About 82% of respondents use ness is ‘‘I often feel bursts of joy’’.
Facebook at least once a day. The average daily usage time is about We also controlled for self-relevance and perceived control in this
40 min, slightly less than that of the American sample in Study 1. study. In the vacation scenario, we asked the participants ‘‘In gen-
In the online experiment, participants were first asked to report eral, how much would you like to travel to places like this?’’ as a
three of their Facebook friends (a strong tie, a weak tie, and a mid- measure of self-relevance and ‘‘Currently, how difficult is it for
dle tie) that they were not living with or romantically involved you to arrange such a vacation?’’ (reverse coded) as a measure of
with. We included the third group of middle ties in order to create perceived control. In the iPhone 6 scenario, self-relevance was
a continuum connecting the two ends, strong and weak ties, as measured by ‘‘In general, how much would you like to have an
proposed by Huszti, Dávid, and Vajda (2013). Since this study iPhone 6?’’, and perceived control was measured by the question
was launched after the summer vacation for German college stu- of ‘‘Currently, how difficult is it for you to buy an iPhone 6?’’
dents and slightly after the launch date of iPhone 6 (Sep 19th, (reverse coded). These two variables were measured with a scale
2014), we adopted two realistic scenarios based on the findings ranging from 0 to 10.
of Krasnova et al. (2013): one involved reading a post from a
Facebook friend who posted a vacation picture, and, the other sce- 3.3. Results
nario involved a picture of a newly bought iPhone 6. For each sce-
nario, we randomly assigned one of the reported Facebook friends 3.3.1. Descriptive results
to it. Latin square (3  2) was used to equally distribute different The descriptive results for Study 2 are shown in Table 4.
type of ties into two scenarios, and each participant received only Because the nature of these two scenarios is slightly different, we
two scenarios, with each scenario assigned with one type of tie. analyze them separately. As can be seen in the descriptive sum-
Therefore, the scenarios are a within-subjects factor and tie mary, the vacation scenario triggered more happiness,
strength was a within- and a between-subjects factor. t(146) = 5.02, p < .001, envy, t(146) = 8.51, p < .001, and benign
For each scenario, participants were asked to report some gen- envy, t(146) = 15.22, p < .001 than the iPhone 6 scenario, whereas
eral information about the assigned Facebook friend and then to malicious envy, t(146) = 0.38, p > .05, did not differ across scenar-
imagine that this person just posted a certain picture on ios. Perceived self-relevance was much higher in the vacation sce-
Facebook (a mocked-up post was shown to the participant). The nario than iPhone 6 scenario, t(146) = 17.65, p < .001, and
feelings after seeing the post were recorded accordingly. After fin- perceived control was slightly lower in the vacation scenario rather
ishing two scenarios, the participants were asked to evaluate their than the iPhone 6 scenario, t(146) = 4.14, p < .001. Most respon-
emotional closeness to the three Facebook friends that they had dents did not show interest in buying a new iPhone, neither did
reported in the beginning of the survey. We further used this ques- they envy other iPhone 6 owners. Again, the mean values for the
tion as a manipulation check, by excluding cases with a wrong
order in ranking the perceived relationship closeness for strong,
middle, and weak ties. Table 4
Descriptive statistics in Study 2.
3.2.2. Measures
Variables Scenario n Mean SD Min Max
Relationship closeness/Tie strength. The tie strength between the
poster and reader (participant) was a manipulated variable, but we Happiness Vacation 147 5.93 1.41 1.00 7.00
iPhone 147 5.05 1.78 1.00 7.00
also measured the relationship closeness by asking ‘‘to what extent Envy Vacation 147 3.11 1.87 1.00 7.00
do you feel emotionally close to XX’’ at the end of the question- iPhone 147 1.70 1.35 1.00 7.00
naire. The answers were given on a scale ranging from 0 to 100. Benign envy Vacation 147 3.64 1.58 1.00 7.00
Because this item is also used as the manipulation check, the range iPhone 147 1.60 0.82 1.00 4.67
Malicious envy Vacation 147 1.32 0.62 1.00 5.00
of the scale is highly extended.
iPhone 147 1.29 0.66 1.00 4.67
Emotions. Participants were asked to rate their feelings after Relationship closeness Vacation 147 47.90 36.41 0.00 100.00
reading the post in each scenario. They indicated to what extent iPhone 147 52.23 37.14 0.00 100.00
they agree or disagree with: ‘‘I feel happy for XX’’ and ‘‘I feel envi- Self-relevance Vacation 147 5.43 2.82 0.00 10.00
ous’’. In addition, malicious envy was measured with three items: iPhone 147 0.70 1.56 0.00 8.00
Perceived control Vacation 147 2.95 2.90 0.00 10.00
‘‘I felt malicious envy towards XX’’, ‘‘I had negative thoughts about iPhone 147 4.14 3.60 0.00 10.00
XX’’ and ‘‘It is unfair that XX can go on such a vacation/have an Mood 146 6.99 1.85 1.00 10.00
iPhone 6 while I cannot’’ (Cronbach’s a = 0.84 for vacation scenario, Dispositional envy 147 2.13 0.98 1.00 6.00
and Cronbach’s a = 0.67 for iPhone scenario). Benign envy was Dispositional happiness 147 4.56 1.17 1.00 7.00
Age 143 24.86 6.29 18.80 65.10
measured by 3 items: ‘‘I felt benign envy towards XX’’, ‘‘I admire
Male 147 0.15 0.36 0.00 1.00
XX’’, ‘‘I wanted to try harder to have such a vacation as well/ to
R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38 35

feeling of happiness were much higher than the mean values of Table 6
envy for both scenarios. This suggests that the positive emotion Results of the regression models on happiness tested in Study 2 (unstandardized
coefficient followed by t values based on robust standard error).
of happiness is more prominent than the negative emotion of envy
in both cases, even though we had chosen scenarios that were Happiness (vacation) Happiness (iPhone)
likely to trigger envy. Relationship closeness 0.022 0.013
(7.52)*** (3.27)**
Mood 0.080 0.135
3.3.2. Hypotheses testing (1.26) (1.55)
Table 5 shows the mean values for emotions (happiness, envy, Age 0.039 0.042
benign envy, and malicious envy) based on the manipulated condi- (4.25)*** (2.28)*
tions (strong tie, weak tie, and mid-tie group). ANOVAs were used Male 0.208 0.123
(1.17) (0.25)
to test the group differences. In line with H2 and H3, there was a
Dispositional happiness 0.058 0.150
significant effect of tie strength on the feeling of the happiness, (0.66) (1.09)
F(2, 144) = 27.36, MSE = 1.21, p < .001, g2 = .28, and benign envy, Constant 3.126 1.735
F(2, 144) = 10.52, MSE = 1.48, p < .001, g2 = .13, for the vacation sce- (6.25)*** (2.41)*
nario. The results of Scheffe post hoc test showed that the mean R2 0.38 0.15
N 142 142
values for happiness differed for all three groups (i.e., happiness
*
for strong ties > happiness for mid ties > happiness for weak ties). p < 0.05.
**
The post hoc test for tie strength on benign envy was slightly dif- p < 0.01.
***
p < 0.001.
ferent (benign envy for strong ties & benign envy for mid ties > be-
nign envy for weak ties) (see subscripts in Table 5). For the iPhone
6 scenario, no effect of tie strength was found. In addition, we Dispositional envy was positively associated with both benign
found no group differences in both scenarios for malicious envy, and malicious envy, which is also in line with our prediction.
thus H4 is not supported. Self-relevance was a significant predictor for benign envy but not
Furthermore, we ran linear regressions by using the continuous for malicious envy. That means users are more likely to experience
measurement of relationship closeness as a predictor and includ- benign envy when it is more self-relevant. Perceived control had a
ing the control variables. T-values were estimated by the robust slightly negative influence on the experience of malicious envy.
standard error in order to avoid the problem of heteroscedasticity. That means users with higher perceived control are less likely to
Table 6 presents the results of the linear regressions on the feeling experience malicious envy. Overall, the models on malicious envy
of happiness for both scenarios. In both scenarios, relationship revealed that malicious envy is highly related to personal traits,
closeness was a significant predictor of the feeling of happiness, regardless of tie strength or self-relevance.
for the vacation scenario, b = .55, t(136) = 7.52, p < .001, and for Similar to the results of the correlational study, tie
the iPhone 6 scenario, b = .27, t(136) = 3.27, p = .001. Therefore, strength/relationship closeness did not show a role in predicting
H2 is supported. In addition, age also positively predicted the feel- the overall feeling of envy after browsing Facebook. Again, feeling
ing of happiness. envious after reading a post was more likely to be predicted by
The regression models for the feeling of envy are presented in individual factors such as dispositional envy and age.
Table 7. In line with H3, relationship closeness predicted benign
envy in both scenarios: for the vacation scenario,
b = .30, t(136) = 5.17, p < .001 for the iPhone 6 scenario, and 4. General discussion
b = .23, t(136) = 2.90, p = .004; whereas for malicious envy, relation-
ship closeness did not show any effect for the vacation scenario, 4.1. Summary of research
b = .05, t(136) = 0.49, p = .622 or for the iPhone scenario,
b = .05, t(136) = 0.54, p = .591, therefore H4 is rejected. In this paper, we aimed to explore the momentary emotional
outcomes of browsing Facebook on an individual post level, and
Table 5 we were interested in whether the relationship closeness between
Descriptive results of the dependent variables by group (mean values followed by the poster and reader can predict those emotional outcomes. In the
standard deviation) and ANOVA tests in Study 2. first study, we found that, in general, positive emotions out-
Scenario Strong Mid Weak F(2, 144) Bartlett’s weighed the negative ones after reading a post on Facebook. Tie
tie tie tie test chi2 strength played a role in predicting the feeling of happiness after
group group group reading a post: the closer the relationship, the happier one felt
Happiness Vacation 6.78a 6.12b 5.00c 27.36*** 53.10*** after reading a positive post; and the sadder one felt after reading
(0.51) (1.11) (1.66) a negative post.
iPhone 5.47 4.84 4.80 2.25 0.14
The second study manipulated the tie strength between the
(1.75) (1.91) (1.61)
Envy Vacation 3.57 3.04 2.77 2.30 1.42 poster and reader. With two mocked-up positive (envy-inducing)
(1.81) (1.84) (1.91) scenarios, we re-tested whether this type of news may trigger
iPhone 1.72 1.47 1.93 1.43 11.89** more happiness if it comes from a strong tie rather than a weak
(1.39) (1.14) (1.50) tie. To the best of our knowledge, this is also the first study to dif-
Benign Vacation 4.22a 3.86a 2.90b 10.52*** 1.43
envy (1.33) (1.59) (1.51)
ferentiate malicious and benign envy in the context of Facebook
iPhone 1.71 1.59 1.48 0.90 3.68 use. The results in the second study not only confirmed the prelim-
(0.97) (0.73) (0.73) inary findings of the correlational study in an experimental setting,
Malicious Vacation 1.33 1.27 1.35 0.23 5.13 but also revealed that tie strength predicts benign envy, but not
envy (0.62) (0.45) (0.75)
malicious envy.
iPhone 1.33 1.23 1.33 0.39 2.64
(0.66) (0.58) (0.74)
4.2. Theoretical implications
Note. Means in the same row that do not share subscripts differ at p < .05 in Scheffe
multiple-comparison test.
**
p < 0.01. This paper provided some explanations for the mixed findings
***
p < 0.001. from prior research on social media usage (passive consumption),
36 R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38

Table 7
Results of the regression models on envy tested in Study 2 (unstandardized coefficient followed by t values based on robust standard error).

Envy Benign envy Malicious envy


Vacation iPhone Vacation iPhone Vacation iPhone
Relationship closeness 0.007 0.001 0.013 0.005 0.001 0.001
(1.80)  (0.49) (5.17)*** (2.90)** (0.49) (0.54)
Dispositional envy 0.633 0.320 0.209 0.167 0.251 0.158
(5.37)** (2.68)** (2.19)* (2.76)** (4.22)*** (2.64)**
Self-relevance 0.243 0.343 0.294 0.187 0.020 0.049
(4.78)*** (4.19)*** (7.95)*** (4.70)*** (1.26) (0.92)
Perceived control 0.112 0.059 0.081 0.010 0.023 0.023
(2.53)* (2.48)* (2.31)* (0.63) (1.78)  (1.74) 
Age 0.052 0.003 0.004 0.025 0.010 0.005
(4.19)*** (0.30) (0.33) (3.19)** (1.82)  (0.72)
Male 0.091 0.105 0.256 0.126 0.297 0.014
(0.24) (0.44) (0.80) (0.69) (1.34) (0.08)
Constant 0.615 0.304 0.559 0.133 0.761 0.955
(1.21) (0.71) (1.35) (0.43) (3.53)** (3.73)***
R2 0.38 0.26 0.49 0.23 0.23 0.11
N 143 143 143 143 143 143
 
p < 0.1.
*
p < 0.05.
**
p < 0.01.
***
p < 0.001.

and filled the research gap in the emotional outcomes of browsing are not central domains for one’s self-identification, and we found
Facebook on an individual message level. Krasnova et al. (2013) a lower degree of malicious envy than benign envy.
inferred that the invidious feeling of envy on social networking Furthermore, we would like to address the role of self-relevance
sites can lead to a decrease in users’ life satisfaction. They worried and the compared domain in predicting benign and malicious
that if too many negative feelings were triggered by envy-inducing envy. Self-relevance was a significant predictor of both envy and
posts (e.g., vacation pictures), users might quit using Facebook. benign envy, but not for malicious envy. Instead, dispositional envy
However, the present study found that positive feelings are more predicted malicious envy in our models. Taken together, this seems
prevalent than negative emotions, even when Facebook users were to indicate that malicious envy is rather a personality trait that is
presented with a vacation picture, which is the biggest cause of independent of situational factors such as tie strength or
envy on Facebook based on Krasnova et al.’s (2013) study. self-relevance; whereas emotions such as happiness and benign
Moreover, this paper filled the research gap on the role of tie envy are more situational dependent (there is an effect of tie
strength in predicting the emotional outcomes of browsing strength and self-relevance, but no effect of dispositional happi-
Facebook. We investigated the role of tie strength (i.e., the relation- ness). More fine-grained research is required to disentangle the
ship between the poster and reader) in interpreting or predicting relationships between the tie strength and self-relevance. Also,
user’s emotions of happiness and envy after reading a post. Based based on Tesser’s (1988) SEM model, there should be an interac-
on a sample of 207 American respondents and a sample of 194 tion effect between relationship closeness and domain-relevance
German students, we found that if positive news is posted by a on envy. In other words, whether a close relationship magnifies
strong tie, users are more likely to be happy for their friends and or dilutes the feeling of (malicious) envy also depends on the com-
experience only benign envy, which is a positive type of envy that pared domain. In this study, we only used scenarios, such as vaca-
has a levelling-up effect (at least in short-term). The finding on tion pictures, that are more likely to be a low relevant domain for
happiness contributes to the theory on emotional contagion, and, most Facebook users. High relevant domains are usually related to
consistent with previous research, suggests that emotional conta- skills and performance, but we should also keep in mind that
gion is stronger when the relationship between individuals is clo- whether a domain is relevant to self-definition is a very personal
ser. The finding with regard to benign envy also contributes to the and variable. Nevertheless, these issues are beyond the current
literature on social comparison and tie strength, by showing that research question, and we hope future research can be conducted
benign envy, rather than malicious envy, is more likely to be expe- to test these effects.
rienced when the relationship is closer.
With regard to the malicious envy, there was no effect of tie 4.3. Practical implications
strength. The null finding may be explained by a floor effect, as
the mean values for malicious envy were quite low in both scenar- 4.3.1. Psychological implications
ios (M = 1.33). It may be due partly to the scenario design. The results from our studies have implications for teachers and
Malicious envy in real life is relatively low, and more extreme parents who are worried about young adolescents’ social media
manipulations are required to trigger it. Future research could use. Because the positive effects of browsing Facebook outweigh
specifically focus on malicious-envy-inducing scenarios in order the negative effects, they do not need to worry too much about
to examine the role of tie strength in predicting malicious envy. the negative psychological effects as long as the users do not
Malicious envy is more likely to emerge when the envied object browse Facebook excessively (with an ‘‘appropriate’’ amount of
was perceived as undeserved (Van de Ven et al., 2012). However, usage time). Also, we found that self-esteem and dispositional envy
from a realistic point of view, not many Facebook users would played a significant role in predicting Facebook envy. For users
write posts in a way suggesting that their own achievement is who have a high dispositional envy or low self-esteem, we would
undeserved. The main goal of the present study was to examine suggest them do not obsessively use Facebook in a passive way.
the feelings that are induced by Facebook use by using selected With regard to the findings on the role of tie strength, one prac-
scenarios (vacation and iPhone) that are more representative of tical implication is that if Facebook users receive more posts from
actual Facebook posts. We assumed that these posts, in most cases, their close friends, they will experience more happiness. Further,
R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38 37

while individuals might experience envy towards their close between recalling feelings and reporting current feelings, as recall-
friend, it may be less detrimental as once expected (e.g., ing is not as accurate as reporting. In addition, there might have
Krasnova et al., 2013) since benign envy generally motivates indi- been too much priming of the tie strength in the experimental
viduals to self-improve. design, as we asked a few questions about the Facebook friend
before each scenario. Based on these limitations, future studies
4.3.2. Applied implications should adjust the experimental design (e.g., by measuring the feel-
Because most Facebook users cannot read all the potential sto- ing of envy in a third-person perspective, by using different items
ries from their Facebook friends and pages, a News Feed ranking for relationship closeness and emotions, or trying to avoid the
algorithm was created, adjusting the sequence of posts rather than effect of priming), and also retest the external and internal validity
using the chronological order (Facebook, 2013). However, it is still of this study. Finally, this paper only focused on the momentary
technically difficult to determine what kinds of stories are rela- feelings of browsing Facebook, future research is required to inves-
tively important for individual users and what kinds of posts can tigate the implications for long term use of Facebook on emotional
make users happier, rather than triggering negative feelings such outcomes.
as envy (as described in Chou & Edge, 2012). It is often the case
that, even reading the same status update can lead to different feel-
5. Conclusion
ings for different individuals.
The research showed that the emotional outcomes of browsing
In this paper, we examined the momentary feelings after read-
Facebook were significantly influenced by three components: the
ing a post on Facebook, and the role of relationship closeness in
content of the post, the personal traits of the Facebook browser,
predicting the feelings of happiness and envy. Over two studies,
and relationship between the poster and reader. Hopefully, these
with different methods (correlational and experimental) and dif-
findings can also offer some insights for the further improvement
ferent samples (American and German), we found that positive
of the Facebook News Feed algorithm. For example, the Facebook
emotions are more pronounced than negative emotions, and tie
algorithm can display more positive posts from strong ties to
strength moderates the feeling of happiness after reading a post
Facebook users who have a lower degree of dispositional envy. In
on Facebook, as well as the feeling of benign envy.
addition, future research can also retest these results in other
social media sites such as Twitter or LinkedIn. We hope that these
studies together can contribute to the construction of a better Acknowledgements
online-communication environment and eventually improve the
individual well-being of social media users. The authors would like to thank Nicole Muscanell for her valu-
able opinion and her efforts in proof reading. Also, we would like to
4.4. Limitations and future research thank Albert Schilling for helping conducting the studies. The
research leading to these results has received funding from the
Several limitations need to be addressed. First, the scenario of European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh
Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013)/ERC grant agreement
iPhone 6 may actually not be the best scenario for triggering envy
in a German sample. Most of the respondents rated this scenario no. 312420.
low in self-relevance, and very few participants reported any type
of envious feelings afterwards. This is also a potential reason for References
why most hypotheses were not supported by the ANOVA tests in
Barash, V., Ducheneaur, N., Isaacs, E., & Bellotti, V. (2010). Faceplant: Impression
the iPhone 6 scenarios, as the average self-relevance in the (Mis) management in Facebook status updates. In Proceedings of the fourth
iPhone scenarios was quite low. Future research could also exam- international AAAI conference on weblogs and social media.
ine the relationship between self-relevance and happiness. Second, Bazarova, N. N. (2012). Public intimacy: Disclosure interpretation and social
judgments on Facebook. Journal of Communication, 62, 815–832. http://
the reliabilities of the scales for malicious and benign envy were dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2012.01664.x.
relatively low (especially the scale of benign envy) in the iPhone Beeney, J. E., Franklin, R. G., Levy, K. N., & Adams, R. B. (2011). I feel your pain:
scenario in Study 2. More research is required to develop a scale Emotional closeness modulates neural responses to empathically experienced
rejection. Social Neuroscience. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
for measuring malicious and benign envy that is robust across var-
17470919.2011.557245.
ious domains (e.g. money, success, beauty, relationship, etc.). Third, Burke, M., Marlow, C., & Lento, T. (2010). Social network activity and social well-
both studies rely on self-reporting as a measure of emotions. There being. In Proceedings of the 28th international conference on human factors in
is a possibility that respondents might not be honest about report- computing systems – CHI ’10 (p. 1909). http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/1753326.
1753613.
ing their negative/bad feelings (social desirability). This could also Cheshin, A., Rafaeli, A., & Bos, N. (2011). Anger and happiness in virtual teams:
be another potential reason for why the hypothesis on malicious Emotional influences of text and behavior on others’ affect in the absence of
envy was not supported. Putting participants into a non-verbal cues. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 116,
2–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2011.06.002.
third-person’s perspective might be helpful for measuring mali- Chou, H.-T. G., & Edge, N. (2012). ‘‘They are happier and having better lives than I
cious envy. Nonetheless, our participants do honestly reveal a rel- am’’: The impact of using Facebook on perceptions of others’ lives.
atively high degree of envy and benign envy in the vacation Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 15(2), 117–121. http://
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0324.
scenario (see Table 5). Fourth, we did not use the same item for Coviello, L., Sohn, Y., Kramer, A. D. I., Marlow, C., Franceschetti, M., Christakis, N.,
measuring happiness across these two studies. The item of ‘‘I feel et al. (2014). Detecting emotional contagion in massive social networks. PLoS
pleasant’’ was used in the first study as a way to measure an overall One, 9(3), e90315. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0090315.
Crusius, J., & Lange, J. (2014). What catches the envious eye? Attentional biases
feeling, and the item of ‘‘I feel happy for the person’’ was used in within malicious and benign envy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 55,
the second study in order to be in line with the experimental con- 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.05.007.
text. The target of the emotions was slightly different between the Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook ‘‘friends’’:
Social capital and college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of
two studies. Last but not least, whereas the first study is relatively
Computer-Mediated Communication, 12, 1143–1168. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/
high in ecological validity, it is hard to generate any causality; j.1083-6101.2007.00367.x.
while the second study isolated other factors and controlled for Facebook (2013). News Feed FYI: A window into News Feed. <https://
the tie strength, the external validity is low. We did not distinguish www.facebook.com/business/news/News-Feed-FYI-A-Window-Into-News-
Feed>.
participants who had already seen the posts from those who saw Facebook (2014). Company info: Our mission. <http://newsroom.fb.com/company-
the posts first time in Study 1. There might be some biases info/>.
38 R. Lin, S. Utz / Computers in Human Behavior 52 (2015) 29–38

Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, Nabi, R. L., & Keblusek, L. (2014). Inspired by hope, motivated by envy: Comparing
117–140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/001872675400700202. the effects of discrete emotions in the process of social comparison to media
Gilbert, E. E. (2012). Computing tie strength. Dissertation Abstracts International, B: figures. Media Psychology, 17(2), 208–234. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Sciences and Engineering. 15213269.2013.878663.
Grieve, R., Indian, M., Witteveen, K., Anne Tolan, G., & Marrington, J. (2013). Face-to- Neumann, R., & Strack, F. (2000). ‘‘Mood contagion’’: The automatic transfer of
face or Facebook: Can social connectedness be derived online? Computers in mood between persons. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79,
Human Behavior, 29, 604–609. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.11.017. 211–223. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.2.211.
große Deters, F., & Mehl, M. R. (2013). Does posting Facebook status updates Norscia, I., & Palagi, E. (2011). Yawn contagion and empathy in Homo sapiens. PLoS
increase or decrease loneliness? An online social networking experiment. Social One, 6. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028472.
Psychological and Personality Science, 4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/ Parrott, W. G., & Smith, R. H. (1993). Distinguishing the experiences of envy and
1948550612469233. jealousy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 906–920. http://
Hampton, K. N., Goulet, L. S., Marlow, C., & Rainie, L. (2012). Why most Facebook dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.906.
users get more than they give. Pew Research Center. <http://pewinternet.org/ Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases.
Reports/2012/Facebook-users.aspx>. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/
Hampton, K., Rainie, L., Lu, W., Shin, I., & Purcell, K. (2015). Social media and the cost S0140525X02000018. discussion 20–71.
of caring. Pew Research Center. <http://www.pewresearch.org/about/>. Qiu, L., Lin, H., Leung, A. K., & Tov, W. (2012). Putting their best foot forward:
Hancock, J. T., Gee, K., Ciaccio, K., & Lin, J. M. (2008). I’m sad you’re sad: Emotional Emotional disclosure on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social
contagion in CMC. In Proceedings of the 2008 ACM conference on computer Networking. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2012.0200.
supported cooperative work (pp. 295–298). http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/1460563. Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
1460611. University Press.
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Emotional contagion. Current Sagioglou, C., & Greitemeyer, T. (2014). Facebook’s emotional consequences: Why
Directions in Psychological Science. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467- Facebook causes a decrease in mood and why people still use it. Computers in
8721.ep10770953. Human Behavior, 35, 359–363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2014.03.003.
Huszti, É., Dávid, B., & Vajda, K. (2013). Strong tie, weak tie and in-betweens: A Sheldon, K. M., Abad, N., & Hinsch, C. (2011). A two-process view of Facebook use
continuous measure of tie strength based on contact diary datasets. Procedia – and relatedness need-satisfaction: Disconnection drives use, and connection
Social and Behavioral Sciences, 79, 38–61. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/ rewards it. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 766–775. http://
j.sbspro.2013.05.056. dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0022407.
Kim, J., & Lee, J.-E. R. (2011). The Facebook paths to happiness: Effects of the number Shiota, M. N., Keltner, D., & John, O. P. (2006). Positive emotion dispositions
of Facebook friends and self-presentation on subjective well-being. differentially associated with Big Five personality and attachment style. The
Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 14(6), 359–364. http:// Journal of Positive Psychology. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17439760500510833.
dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0374. Smith, R. H., & Kim, S. H. (2007). Comprehending envy. Psychological Bulletin, 133,
Konnikova, M. (2013, September 10). How Facebook makes us unhappy. The New 46–64. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.46.
Yorker. <http://www.newyorker.com/tech/elements/how-facebook-makes-us- Smith, R. H., Parrott, W. G., Diener, E. F., Hoyle, R. H., & Kim, S. H. (1999).
unhappy>. Dispositional envy. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. http://dx.doi.org/
Kramer, A. D. I. (2012). The spread of emotion via Facebook. In Proceedings of the 10.1177/01461672992511008.
2012 ACM annual conference on human factors in computing systems – CHI ’12 (p. Smith, R. H. (2004). Envy and its transmutations. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach
767). http://dx.doi.org/10.1145/2207676.2207787. (Eds.), The social life of emotions (pp. 43–63). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
Kramer, A. D. I., Guillory, J. E., & Hancock, J. T. (2014). Experimental evidence of University Press.
massive-scale emotional contagion through social networks. Proceedings of the Steers, M.-L. N., Wickham, R. E., & Acitelli, L. K. (2014). Seeing everyone else’s
National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111, 8788–8790. highlight reels: How Facebook usage is linked to depressive symptoms. Journal
http://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1320040111. of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33, 701–731.
Krasnova, H., Wenninger, H., Widjaja, T., & Buxmann, P. (2013). Envy on Facebook: A Tandoc, E. J. C., Ferrucci, P., & Duffy, M. E. (2014). Facebook use, envy, and depression
hidden threat to users’ life satisfaction? In 11th International conference on among college students: Is Facebooking depressing? Seattle, Washington:
Wirtschaftsinformatik (pp. 1–16). International Communication Association.
Kross, E., Verduyn, P., Demiralp, E., Park, J., Lee, D. S., Lin, N., et al. (2013). Facebook Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior.
use predicts declines in subjective well-being in young adults. PLoS One, 8(8), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 21, 181–227. http://dx.doi.org/
e69841. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0069841. 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60227-0.
Maier, C., Laumer, S., Eckhardt, A., & Weitzel, T. (2012). When social networking Utz, S. (2011). Social network site use among Dutch students: Effects of time and
turns to social overload: Explaining the stress, emotional exhaustion, and platform. In Networked sociability and individualism. Technology for personal and
quitting behavior from Social Network Sites’ users. In Proceedings of European professional relationships. Hershey, PA: IGI Global.
Conference on Information Systems (ECIS), (pp. 1–12). Utz, S. (2015). The function of self-disclosure on social network sites: Not only
Marsden, P. V., & Campbell, K. E. (1984). Measuring tie strength. Social Forces, 63, intimate, but also positive and entertaining self-disclosures increase the feeling
482–501. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sf/63.2.482. of connection. Computers in Human Behavior, 45, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/
Mauri, M., Cipresso, P., Balgera, A., Villamira, M., & Riva, G. (2011). Why is Facebook 10.1016/j.chb.2014.11.076.
so successful? Psychophysiological measures describe a core flow state while Van de Ven, N., Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2009). Leveling up and down: The
using Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14, 723–731. experiences of benign and malicious envy. Emotion (Washington, DC), 9,
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2010.0377. 419–429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0015669.
Mehdizadeh, S. (2010). Self-presentation 2.0: Narcissism and self-esteem on Van de Ven, N., Zeelenberg, M., & Pieters, R. (2012). Appraisal patterns of envy and
Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking, 13(4), 357–364. related emotions. Motivation and Emotion, 36, 195–204. http://dx.doi.org/
http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0257. 10.1007/s11031-011-9235-8.
Muise, A., Christofides, E., & Desmarais, S. (2009). More information than you ever Verduyn, P., Lee, D. S., Park, J., Shablack, H., Orvell, A., Bayer, J., et al. (2015). Passive
wanted: Does Facebook bring out the green-eyed monster of jealousy? Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and
Cyberpsychology & Behavior, 12(4), 441–444. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/ longitudinal evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. http://
cpb.2008.0263. dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000057. No pagination specified.
Mussweiler, T., Rüter, K., & Epstude, K. (2004). The ups and downs of social Wise, K., Alhabash, S., & Park, H. (2010). Emotional responses during social
comparison: Mechanisms of assimilation and contrast. Journal of Personality and information seeking on Facebook. Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social
Social Psychology, 87, 832–844. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.6.832. Networking, 13, 555–562. http://dx.doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2009.0365.

Potrebbero piacerti anche