Sei sulla pagina 1di 13

See discussions, stats, and author profiles for this publication at: https://www.researchgate.

net/publication/227260477

Are Feminist Women Protected from Body Image Problems? A Meta-analytic


Review of Relevant Research

Article  in  Sex Roles · April 2009


DOI: 10.1007/s11199-008-9523-2

CITATIONS READS

80 1,370

2 authors:

Sarah K Murnen Linda Smolak


Kenyon College Kenyon College
47 PUBLICATIONS   3,881 CITATIONS    37 PUBLICATIONS   2,625 CITATIONS   

SEE PROFILE SEE PROFILE

Some of the authors of this publication are also working on these related projects:

A Social Constructivist Approach to Understanding the Relationship Between Masculinity and Sexual Aggression View project

Revision of the Levine & Smolak (2006) book entitled "The Prevention of Eating Problems and Eating Disorders: Theory, Research, and Practice" (Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum/Taylor & Francis) View project

All content following this page was uploaded by Sarah K Murnen on 31 May 2014.

The user has requested enhancement of the downloaded file.


Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197
DOI 10.1007/s11199-008-9523-2

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Are Feminist Women Protected from Body Image Problems?


A Meta-analytic Review of Relevant Research
Sarah K. Murnen & Linda Smolak

Published online: 7 August 2008


# Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2008

Abstract It was hypothesized that feminist women would The purpose of the present study was to examine the
experience positive body image due to a heightened ability strength of the association between the variables across the
to critique cultural pressures related to thinness. Samples available studies using the technique of meta-analysis.
from 26 studies (almost all North American) were compiled Meta-analysis yields a quantitative index of the size of the
for a meta-analysis to examine the association between association between variables as well as procedures to
feminist identity and measures related to body image and determine whether variability in the size of that relationship
eating problems. The largest number of effect sizes found across samples is associated with study characteristics in a
addressed the association between feminist identity and predicted manner.
body attitudes (k=28), and it was found that there was a Other meta-analytic research reviews support the idea
positive, significant association that was strongest when that the cultural experiences of girls and women can
older women participants were tested and when a more encourage body image dissatisfaction and eating disordered
purposeful sample (e.g., women studies students) was attitudes. For example, mass media exposure (Groesz et al.
questioned. There were also significant negative associa- 2002; Murnen et al. 2007), sexual abuse (Smolak and
tions between feminist identity and other measures related Murnen 2002), and agreement with traditional notions of
to eating problems. femininity (Murnen and Smolak 1997) have all been linked
to body image dissatisfaction and/or eating disorders across
Keywords Feminist identity . Body image . Eating disorders a number of studies. Body image dissatisfaction is so
common among women and girls in North America that it
is considered normative and its presence is linked to the
Introduction development of eating disordered attitudes and behaviors
(e.g., Thompson et al. 1999).
Various theorists have proposed that the acquisition of a The existence of high levels of body dissatisfaction and
feminist identity should help protect women from negative disordered eating attitudes in girls and women in Western
feelings about their bodies in that feminist women learn to cultures can be explained by a feminist framework which
recognize and resist oppressive cultural messages about the incorporates objectification theory. Many feminists have
importance of thinness. Researchers have empirically argued that in a patriarchal culture women are subjected to
examined the association between feminist identity and various means of subordination including the objectifica-
measures related to the body including measures of body tion and degradation of their bodies (e.g., Bordo 1993;
dissatisfaction. Most of this research has been conducted in Kilbourne 1999; Wolf 1991). Women are often defined as
North America. Studies have yielded conflicting results. their bodies; and their bodies are treated as objects that exist
for the sexual pleasure of men. In Western cultures this
objectification of women occurs through a variety of means
S. K. Murnen (*) : L. Smolak
including media portrayals and the sexual gaze of men
Department of Psychology, Kenyon College,
Gambier, OH 43022, USA (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997). Ward (1995) found that
e-mail: Murnen@kenyon.edu the most common theme among television shows popular
Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197 187

among North American adolescents is that women attract not “self-silence” they should receive protection from
men through their physical appearance. In a recent review eating problems (e.g., Smolak and Munstertieger 2002).
of the data she concluded that women were objectified in Rubin and colleagues found some support for these ideas
the media to a greater extent than men (Ward 2003). in a recent qualitative study of self-proclaimed feminists
Further, the ideal woman currently portrayed in North (Rubin et al. 2004). They held focus groups with 25
American media is unrealistically thin (e.g., Engeln- feminist and/or womanist North American college women
Maddox 2006) which can lead women to waste energy who were willing to discuss cultural beauty ideals. The
striving towards an impossible goal. women agreed that feminists were more likely to celebrate
According to objectification theory, cultural objectifica- bodily diversity among women and that a feminist
tion encourages self-objectification where women learn to perspective should lead to a conscious awareness of cultural
monitor their own bodies; this in turn can lead to body messages as well as the development of strategies to resist
shame, and is proposed to put women at risk for the cultural pressure. They believed that feminism promoted
development of eating disorders, depression, and/or sexual ways to reclaim the body from the objectifying gaze
problems (Fredrickson and Roberts 1997; McKinley and through “emancipatory resistance” which might occur
Hyde 1996). Correlational and experimental research through athletics, dance, “taking up space,” “moving with
supports a link between self-objectification and body image confidence,” and redefining beauty. Despite an awareness
dissatisfaction in adolescent girls and women (Smolak and of what a feminist perspective could offer women to help
Murnen 2007). In addition to self-objectification, women protect against the development of body image dissatisfac-
experience other disembodying experiences in a patriarchal tion in an objectifying culture, the women felt that they
culture such as sexual harassment, sexual abuse, and rape. were still susceptible to cultural messages about thinness.
Piran and Cormier (2005) wrote about the “disrupted Researchers have also studied this topic using quantita-
connection” that many women experience with their bodies tive techniques. While some studies find a moderate
in a patriarchal culture. They argued that a focus on how association between feminist identity and measures associ-
the body looks rather than feels can lead women to ated with body image (e.g., Tiggemann and Stevens 1999),
disconnect from their bodies and treat them in self-injurious others find small or no relationships (e.g., Cash et al. 1997).
ways through such means as smoking, cutting, and The purpose of the present study was to conduct a meta-
improper feeding. analysis of the correlational data that relate feminist identity
If a feminist perspective can help explain the data it can to measures related to body concerns. For example, many
also be used to try to help prevent body dissatisfaction and researchers have examined how feminist identity relates to
related problems. Various researchers have explored this body dissatisfaction, which is one index of body image. In
idea (e.g., Peterson et al. 2006). Although there are a the present data set there were several types of scales used
variety of definitions of a feminist, common to many (e.g., that measured body attitudes. These included scales assess-
Reid and Purcell 2004) is that a feminist recognizes that ing satisfaction with specific body parts, including items not
discrimination against women exists, experiences a sense of related to weight and shape, such as the Multidimensional
shared fate with women as a group, and wants to work with Self-Relations Body Questionnaire—Body Area Satisfaction
others to improve women’s status. There are several reasons Scale (MSRBQ-BASS, Brown et al. 1990); surveys that
why a feminist perspective might prevent women from focused on weight and shape dissatisfaction such as the
developing body-related problems. A feminist perspective Body Shape Questionnaire (BSQ, Cooper et al. 1987); and
might allow women to see that objectification and the thin the Body Shame scale of the Objectified Body Conscious-
ideal are sources of women’s oppression that should be ness Scale (McKinley and Hyde 1996) that includes
resisted. For example, feminist women should be more measures of the integration of body dissatisfaction and self.
critical of media portrayals of women and might seek ways Body dissatisfaction is closely related to the internaliza-
to avoid exposure to unhealthy or unrealistic depictions of tion of the thin ideal. Internalization of the thin ideal may
women. If feminist women experience sexist discrimination be considered a risk factor for the development of body
in other ways they might not personalize the experience, dissatisfaction (Thompson and Stice 2001). Similarly, both
but instead search for a cultural explanation which might body dissatisfaction and internalization of the thin ideal
prevent internalization of blame. Further, feminist women may be risk factors for disordered eating and eating
should generally question traditional feminine roles that are disorders (Stice 2002). Thus, these constructs can be
associated with appearance and thinness (Mahalik et al. conceptualized to exist on a continuum, moving from thin
2005); and might instead develop self-definitions based on ideal internalization to body dissatisfaction to disordered
other attributes such as intelligence or creativity that are eating attitudes and behaviors (e.g., Levine and Smolak
more empowering. Feminist women might possess stronger 2006). Data addressing the relationship between feminist
voices to speak their minds and to the extent that they do identity and “drive for thinness” scores measured by the
188 Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197

Eating Disorders Inventory (Garner et al. 1983); and scores measures. Finally, there are several measures of feminist
on the Eating Attitude Test (EAT) (Garner and Garfinkel, identity as well as of body image attitudes. Thus, variation
1982) which measures eating disordered attitudes were due to measurement was examined to see if it affected the
available for analysis. Finally, feminist attitudes have been size of the relationship. It was expected that feminist
related to internalization of the thin media ideal measured identity measures that required people to self-identify as a
by the Sociocultural Attitudes towards Appearance Ques- feminist would yield more variability and thus stronger
tionnaire (SATAQ; Heinberg et al. 1995). relationships than measures that did not require self-
Thus, several sets of effects were examined in the identification. While many people indicate that they agree
present study. The following hypotheses were proposed: with feminist attitudes, fewer proclaim themselves to be
feminists (e.g., Zucker 2004). No specific predictions about
1. Feminist identity should be associated with positive
how various body attitude measures (body parts dissatis-
attitudes about the body. Measures of body dissatisfac-
faction, overall weight dissatisfaction, body shame) would
tion, esteem, and shame represented the operationaliza-
relate to the size of the effect were proposed.
tion of body attitudes.
2. Feminist identity should be negatively related to “drive for
thinness” as measured by the EDI drive for thinness scale.
Method
3. Feminist identity should be negatively associated with
eating disordered attitudes.
Study Retrieval
4. Feminist identity will be negatively associated with
internalization of the thin media ideal. This association
Psychinfo, ERIC, and Dissertation Abstracts databases were
might be the strongest one found in the data set as
searched with the term “feminist” to cast a large net over the
theorists have proposed that feminist attitudes should
entire body of research related to feminism. Titles and
lead to a critical evaluation of external forces that
abstracts were examined to see if the article contained any
threaten women’s body satisfaction.
measure of feminist identity linked to an aspect of body image
It was expected that there would be variation in the size or eating problems. Further, Social Science Citation Index was
of the effect across studies, and that various factors would used to examine all studies that cited the articles that described
predict variation. The age of the participants was examined the development of all of the commonly used measures of
as a possible source of variation as at least one study found feminist identity. The abstracts of all of the articles citing at
differences due to age (Tiggemann and Stevens 1999). It is least one of these scales were examined closely to see if
possible that possession of a feminist identity is a stronger relevant data could be obtained. Specific Psychinfo author
predictor among women older than college age in that it searches were also conducted of various authors who seemed
might take time for a feminist identity to help women to have empirical or theoretical interest in this topic. The
develop resistance strategies and a strong sense of self that abstracts of all of the studies associated with the following
can help protect against societally based body disruption. authors were examined for possible data: Fischer, Henley,
The ethnicity of the sample was examined as well. In a Hyde, Liss, Moradi, Piran, Szymanski, Tiggemann, Tolman,
meta-analytic review of the data on ethnicity and body and Wittig. Finally, the reference pages of all articles and
dissatisfaction Grabe and Hyde (2006) found a small chapters were examined for additional references.
difference between white and black women in body Selection criteria did not include country of origin,
dissatisfaction with black women experiencing more satis- ethnicity, or age. However, the studies represented exclu-
faction. It is possible that the larger the percent of white sively Western nations. Twenty-one of the studies were
women in a sample the larger the effect size. There was conducted with participants from the United States; two
little diversity in the country of origin of the studies given studies were conducted with Canadian citizens (Affleck
that most were conducted in North America, so no 2000; Dionne et al. 1995); two studies questioned both US
differences by country of sample could be examined. About and Canadian participants (Legatt 2004; Ojerholm and
half of the studies in the analysis were dissertations, so it Rothblum 1999); and one study was conducted in Australia
was possible to examine whether effect size varied with (Tiggemann and Stevens 1999). Furthermore, in most
publication; it might be expected that there would be a studies a large percentage of the participants (at least
publication bias toward publishing significant results. There 75%) were Caucasian, with the exception of Affleck
was also variability in the sampling methods used across (2000); Brooks (2002); Cash et al. (1997); and Linnebach
studies. In some cases purposeful samples of lesbian (2005). The samples used in the analyses were almost
women or women specifically interested in the topic of equally divided between those with women in their 20s and
body image were chosen, which could lead to stronger those with women older than their 20s. There were no
effect sizes due to more variability on feminist identity available data from girls younger than college age.
Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197 189

Analysis identity development scale (FIDS) to capture all five stages


of development. Fischer and colleagues developed the
Because both variables under study were typically contin- Feminist Identity Composite (FIC), which combines items
uous rather than one being categorical, effect sizes were from the FIS and FIDS and is argued to have better
calculated as r rather than d values, using procedures psychometric properties (Fischer et al. 2000). All three scales
suggested by Shadish and Haddock (1994) in which the have been used in research to link feminist identity to body
Fisher z transformation of r is used as an unbiased estimate image attitudes and eating disordered attitudes, and most
of the population effect size. Each r value is weighted by authors included all of the stage score values in their results.
the sample size of the study. The statistical significance of It was expected that the higher stages of feminist identity
the overall effect size is tested by calculating a z score would be most correlated with body-related variables. Thus,
value. Cohen’s (1977) guidelines were used to evaluate the the synthesis score and/or the active commitment score was
size of the r value with values of r=.10 indicating a small used in the analysis. In some studies only one score was
effect, values of r=.30 indicating a moderate effect, and available, and in one study a combined score was used.
values of r=.50 indicating a large effect. If the r is A couple of researchers used Morgan’s (1996) Liberal
statistically significant, the homogeneity of r is calculated Feminist Ideology Scale (LFIS) which measures agreement
and its significance determined by a statistic that approx- with feminist goals and feminist ideologies. Some studies
imates a chi-square value. If there is significant heteroge- used Henley et al. (1998) Feminist Perspectives Scale (FPS)
neity in r across studies, the variability in r can be which measures agreement with a variety of feminist
examined to determine if it systematically associated with perspectives including conservative, liberal, radical, social-
various study characteristics using procedures analogous to ist, and cultural feminist; and women of color (Henley et al.
analysis of variance. 1998). A total score across these perspectives was used.
Fassinger’s (1994) scale was used in one study as was that
Predictor Variable: Measurement of Feminist Identity of Smith et al. (1975) which each measure agreement with
feminist attitudes. Finally, some researchers used self-
Feminist identity, the predictor variable, was operational- identification as a feminist as their measure of feminist
ized in various ways across the studies found. The most identity, sometimes measured on a one-item Likert scale.
typical way was to use one of the three “stage scale” Feminist identity scales vary in terms of whether or not
measures of feminist identity. These measures are based on one has to self-identify as a feminist to respond to the scale.
a model proposed by Downing and Roush (1985), patterned On Morgan’s (1996) scale, the scale of Henley et al. (1998),
after Cross’ (1971) model of racial identity development. In and all of the scales designed to measure stage of feminist
the Downing and Roush (1985) model of feminist identity, identity development, the word “feminist” does not appear
it is proposed that women pass through five stages of on the scale. Thus, one does not have to self-identify as a
development. At first women exhibit “passive acceptance” feminist to agree with attitudes on the scale. In contrast,
(PA) of traditional gender roles, believing that they are scales measuring agreement with feminist attitudes and
advantageous to women. If women are led to question questions asking one to self-identify as a feminist make it
traditional roles, perhaps through consciousness-raising or clear that the scale concerns feminist identity. Respondents
the experience of a “crisis,” they might move into the must think about whether or not they are feminists in
“revelation” phase (REV) where they realize the presence responding to these scales. This is important because
of discrimination against women. Women in this stage are although many people agree with egalitarian attitudes (e.g.,
often very emotional and might experience guilt for their Williams and Wittig 1997), fewer will label themselves a
participation in a sexist culture, and they exhibit dualistic feminist or evaluate the term “feminist” positively. About
thinking in which all men are seen as “the enemy.” In the one-quarter to one-third of Americans would proclaim
third stage, labeled Embeddedness/Emanation (EE), women themselves to be feminists (Huddy et al. 2000); and in
recognize the value of women’s contributions to society and college women these rates have ranged from 5% among
might want to associate mostly with other women. Women introductory psychology students at a Northeastern univer-
then might move into the Synthesis (SYN) stage where sity (Liss et al. 2004), to 15% among University of
feminist identity becomes integrated, and women no longer Connecticut psychology students (Liss et al. 2001), to as
engage in dualistic thinking. Finally, Active Commitment high as 67% of a small group of students from a liberal arts
(AC) is the stage that is associated with engaging in actions college (Liss et al. 2000). There are negative connotations
that support the feminist movement. Rickard (1989) associated with feminists, and those who adopt a feminist
developed the Feminist Identity Scale (FIS) which measures self-label have been found to have different attitudes than
the first four proposed stages of development, leaving out those who merely agree with egalitarian beliefs. For
AC. Bargad and Hyde (1991) developed the feminist example, Zucker (2004) found that self-identified feminists
190 Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197

were more likely to report experience with suffering from BSQ, Cooper et al. 1987; and the Body Esteem Scale,
sexism, and to report more activist behavior than those who Mendelson and White 1982); and the Body Shame scale on
were egalitarian but not feminist. Thus, we expected that the OBC (McKinley and Hyde 1996) which measures
women who self labeled as feminists would have more extreme dissatisfaction with the body that is believed to
positive body attitudes and less disordered eating than result from internalization of the thin ideal. For all of these
would those who simply agreed with egalitarian principles. scales the effect size was coded so that a higher r meant a
positive association between feminist identity and positive
Criterion Variables attitude toward the body.
There were also sufficient data to examine the association
The largest number of effect sizes was found relating between feminist identity and “drive for thinness,” measured
feminist identity to a measure of attitudes about the body. by the EDI scale (Garner et al. 1983); eating disordered
There were three different types of body attitude measures attitudes measured by the EAT (Garner and Garfinkel 1982);
including measures of satisfaction with body parts (e.g., and internalization of the thin media ideal measured by the
EDI body dissatisfaction subscale, Garner et al. 1983; the SATAQ (Heinberg et al. 1995). For all of these measures
Multidimensional Body-Self Relations Questionnaire— data were coded so that a higher score indicated a healthier
Appearance Evaluation, MBSRQ-AE, and MBSRQ-BASS, response; e.g., lower drive for thinness, lower eating
Brown et al. 1990); measures of weight dissatisfaction (e.g., pathology, and less internalization of the media ideal.

Table 1 Effect sizes related to feminist identity and body dissatisfaction.

Authors, year Pub Age Min Fem Samp Att Number Attr Dthinr EATr SATAQr

Affleck 2000 N Y M NF Y BPD 447 .002 .002 −.03 –


Beamer 1999 N Y N NF N BPD 181 .01 – – .25
Brooks 2002 N Y M NF N BPD 89 −.02 – −.01 –
Calandra 2001 N O N NF Y BPD 93 .04 – – –
Callow 2005 N O N NF Y BPD 202 −.01 – – –
Cash et al. 1997 Y Y M NF N BPD 122 .02 −.09 – –
Cogan 1999 Y O N F Y BPD 173 .22 .29 .143 –
Dionne et al. 1995 Y Y N NF N BPD 200 .15 – – –
Eisele 2007 N Y N NF N BPD 397 .05 – – –
Fingerat and Gleaves 2004 Y Y N NF Y BPD 202 .07 .19 – .05
Garner 1997 Y O N F Y WTD 3452 .17 – – –
Gorman 1999 N Y N NF N BPD 100 −.001 −.03 −.09 –
Guille and Chrisler 1999 Y O N NF Y n.a. 105 – – .20 –
Haines et al. 2008 Y O N F Y SHA 126 −.06 – – –
Hurt et al. 2007 N O N F Y SHA 282 .24 – .10 –
Leggatt 2004 N Y N F Y SHA 163 .29 – .20 .41
Myers and Crowther 2007 N Y N NF N WTD 195 −.04 – – .04
Linnebach 2005 N Y N NF N WTD 116 −.05 – – .03
Linnebach 2005 N Y M NF N WTD 95 .35 – – .23
Noffsinger-Frazier 2004 N O N NF Y SHA 345 .24 – .22 .25
Ojerholm and Rothblum 1999 Y Y N F Y WTD 409 .06 – – –
Rockwern and Murnen 2008 N Y N NF N SHA 61 .22 – .06 –
Sabik and Tylka 2006 Y Y N NF N WTD 256 .09 – .11 –
Slaviero 2006 N O N NF Y SHA 241 −.04 – −.08 .00
Snyder and Hasbrouck 1996 Y Y N NF N SHA 71 −.01 −.10 – .13
Tiggemann and Stevens 1999 Y Y N F N WTD 41 −.11 – – –
Tiggemann and Stevens 1999 Y O N F N WTD 69 .24 – – –
Tiggemann and Stevens 1999 Y O N F N WTD 46 .31 – – –
Tiggemann and Stevens 1999 Y O N F N WTD 23 −.27 – – –
Welsh 2007 N O N NF Y n.a. 80 – – −.10 –

Pub Whether study was published, y yes, n no; age refers to age of participants with Y younger and O older; Min indicates minority status of
sample with M significant proportion minority participants, N no significant proportion; FEM indicates whether feminist self-identification was
part of the scale with F feminist self-identification and NF no feminist self-identification; Sam indicates whether the sample was purposeful (Y
yes) or not (N no); Attr refers r values for body attitude scales with BPD body parts dissatisfaction, WTD weight dissatisfaction; and SHA body
shame; Dthinr r values for Drive for Thinness; EATr r values for EAT; and SATAQr r values for internalization of the media
Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197 191

Analysis of Heterogeneity of effect sizes relating feminist identity to drive for thinness
(k=6); to scores on eating disorders inventories (k=12); and
There were sufficient data in the body attitudes data set to to scores on the SATAQ (k=9). All of the effect sizes
conduct analyses of the sources of heterogeneity of the associated with the 26 studies are listed in Table 1.
effect size. Several variables from the studies were coded to
allow for an analysis of heterogeneity. The age of the Body Attitudes: Hypothesis 1
sample was coded such that samples of women in their 20s
were compared to samples containing women who were There were 28 effect sizes from 24 studies that assessed the
older than 20. The presence of minority women in the association between feminist identity and attitudes towards
sample was examined such that samples where the the body. For those studies that used a feminist identity
proportion of non-minority participants was greater than stage scale measure (the FIDS, FIS, or the FIC), a choice
75% were compared to samples where the proportion was needed to be made about which subscale best captured
less than or equal to 75%. Published studies were compared feminist identity. There were seven studies in which either a
to non-published studies. Some samples were purposely synthesis subscale or active commitment subscale could be
chosen because the women had a particular interest in the used as the measure of feminist identity. In the first set of
topic or were particularly likely to be feminist such as analyses the relationship of the synthesis subscale to body
women’s studies students or lesbians. These samples were attitudes was examined. In this analysis, the overall effect
compared to convenience samples that usually consisted of size was r=.123 which was statistically significant, z=
students from introductory psychology courses. Studies 11.04, p<.001, and heterogeneous across studies, X2 (27)=
where the word “feminist” was incorporated into the scale 79.82, p<.001. There was one study with a very large
were compared to those where this was not the case. sample size in the data set, the Garner (1997) Psychology
Finally, the measurement of body attitude used was Today study, for which readers were surveyed and asked
examined such that studies that measured dissatisfaction whether they self-identified as a feminist, and whether they
with body parts were compared to studies of weight were “extremely dissatisfied” with their weight. The size of
dissatisfaction and to studies of body shame. the r value for this study was r=.172, and this effect had a
large weighting factor in the analysis with N=3,452. Thus,
another analysis was done excluding this study and the
Results effect size found was smaller, but still significant. These
data are summarized in Table 2. A second set of analyses
There were 26 studies used in the analysis. All but two was done substituting the active commitment subscale for
studies contained data concerning the relationship between the synthesis subscale for the seven studies where this was
feminist identity and body attitudes, and two studies had possible. The effect size was smaller, but still significant.
more than one sample, so there were a total of 28 effects With the Garner (1997) study included it was r=.115, and
sizes aggregated for the relationship between feminist with it excluded it was r=.072. Table 2 displays these
identity and body satisfaction. There was a smaller number values and their statistical significance.

Table 2 Summary of r values.

Feminist identity correlated with… k Number r d z score Heterogeneity

Synthesis with body attitudes 28 8,197 .123 .247 11.04* 79.82*


Syn w/ Body Att excluding Garner 27 4,745 .085 .171 5.76* 64.46*
Active commitment with Body Att 28 8,197 .115 .232 10.36* 69.98*
AC w/ Body Att excluding Garner 27 4,745 .072 .144 4.87* 49.65*
Synthesis with drive for thinness 6 1,116 .072 .145 2.29* 18.98*
Synthesis with EAT scores 12 2,419 .082 .164 3.91* 24.82*
Synthesis with SATAQ internalization 9 1,660 .149 .301 6.06* 35.35*
BodyAtt w/ SATAQ group of studies 9 1,660 .084 .169 3.40* 21.21*

Positive r values indicate a positive association between variables such that the more feminist, the more positive the attitudes about the body, the
lower the drive for thinness, the lower the score on an eating disordered inventory, etc. Synthesis indicates that feminist identity was measured by
the synthesis scale on the FIS, FIDS, and FIC, when available. In another analysis, active commitment scores were used instead of synthesis
scores in these studies (k=5). In some analyses the Garner study was excluded since it had a large influence on the analysis, given a sample size of
3,452. In a subset of studies SATAQ scores measuring internalization of the media were available. The r value relating to feminist identity and
SATAQ was examined, as well as the r value between feminist identity and body satisfaction for this subset of studies.
*p<.05, statistically significant
192 Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197

An analysis of the heterogeneity in the effect sizes using 5.04, p=.025 such that the explicitly feminist measures
the synthesis data set was conducted (due to the larger r were over-represented among published studies compared
value than the active commitment data set). These data are to unpublished studies. There was also a small association
displayed in Table 3. In the first three sets of contrasts the between age of participants and how purposeful the sample
Garner study was excluded since it was not believed to be was such that older participants were over-represented in
relevant to the domains that were examined, thus it was not the more purposeful samples compared to the more general
desirable for this study to exert undue influence. Samples in samples, X2(1)=4.86, p=.026.
which the participants were in their 20s were compared to
samples in which the participants were older than 20. There Other Measures: Hypotheses 2–4
was a significant difference in the effect size such that it was
smaller for younger participants compared to older partic- With respect to the other body-related measures, there was
ipants. There was not a significant difference between studies a small but significant association between feminist identity
with a larger proportion of minority participants compared to and drive for thinness scores, r=.072, z=2.29, p<.05; and
studies with a smaller proportion. Similarly, there was no between feminist identity and scores on eating disorder
significant difference in the size of r in published studies inventories, r=.082, z=3.91, p<.01. There was significant
compared to those that were not published. heterogeneity in both of these data sets but an insufficient
For the next three sets of analyses, the Garner study was number of values (6 and 12, respectively) to analyze the
included because it was believed to be relevant to the variability in more detail.
domains that were examined. It was found that the effect There was a set of nine studies that used the SATAQ
size was significantly larger when the word feminist was (Heinberg et al. 1995) along with a feminist identity scale.
part of the measurement of the predictor, compared to when In these studies the association between feminist identity
it was not. Also, when a purposeful sample was chosen the and internalization of the media ideal was significant, with
effect size was significantly larger than when a convenience r=.149, z=6.06, p<.01. Across these same studies the
sample was selected. Finally, when body parts satisfaction relationship between feminist identity and body attitudes
was measured the effect size was smaller, compared to was smaller, r=.084, z=3.40, p<.05. There was significant
dissatisfaction with weight, and especially compared to heterogeneity in these two sets of effect sizes. All of these
body shame. data are summarized in Table 2.
Chi-square analyses were conducted to ensure that the
values of the coded variables (feminist identity measure,
age of participants, minority status of participants, nature of Discussion
publication, nature of sample, and body attitude measure)
were not associated with one another. There was a small The effect sizes yielded in the study were small, but
association found between feminist identification measure statistically significant. The largest number of effect sizes
used and whether or not the study was published X2 (1)= was found for the relationship between feminist identity

Table 3 Examination of variability in r values associated with feminist identity (synthesis) and body attitudes.

Contrast r k Number heterogeneity between

Participants age 20sa .057 17 2,945 9.46*


Participants older than 20s .113 10 1,800
Minority sample >75%a .092 4 753 1.26
Minority sample ≤75% .047 23 3,992
Not publisheda .062 15 2,851 3.49
Published .118 12 1,894
Other feminist scale .063 17 3,232 18.08*
Feminist label scale .161 11 4,965
Unusual sample .136 12 6,135 11.09*
Convenience sample .066 16 2,062
Dissatisfaction body parts .039 13 2,062 27.14*
Weight dissatisfaction .147 10 4,702
Body shame .214 5 977

*p<.05, statistically significant


a
Contrasts did not include Garner (1997) study
Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197 193

and body attitudes. In this analysis there was significant measure explicitly made reference to the word “feminism”
variability in the effect size across studies. The r values suggests that many of the samples likely had a restricted
were significantly larger when body shame was the number of women with strong feminist identities. With a
measure of body attitudes, when feminist self-identification larger group of feminists a stronger association might be
was incorporated into the measurement of feminist identity; uncovered.
when the sample was not a convenience sample of In addition, it is important to recognize the likely
introductory psychology students but instead a more complexity of the relationship between feminist identity,
purposeful sample such as those interested in the topic, body attitudes, and eating disordered attitudes and behav-
lesbian women, and/or women’s studies students; and when iors. Recent research has revealed some of this complexity.
the sample was of women older than college age. It is For example, Hurt et al. (2007) found that feminist self-
proposed that a consolidated feminist identity that likely identification did not directly relate to lower eating
exists among those who proclaim themselves to be pathology, but instead was mediated by self-surveillance
feminists, and who have had a significant amount of measured by the OBC and adherence to various feminine
experience with their feminist identity, helps protect against role norms. Sabik and Tylka (2006) found that feminist
extreme dissatisfaction with the body that can result when identity moderated the relationship between perceived
one internalizes unrealistic cultural ideals (as in the case of recent and lifetime sexist events and disordered eating.
body shame). Two sets of effect sizes—the relationship between
It was also found that feminist identity was associated feminist identity and body shame, and between feminist
with a lower drive for thinness, and with lower scores on identity and internalization of the media—were a bit
eating disorder inventories. There were not sufficient data stronger than other effects (although still small). These data
to examine variables that might be associated with the are consistent with feminist theory. Although the body
variability in these effect sizes. There were nine studies shame data were originally grouped with measures of body
where the SATAQ-internalization scale was administered attitudes, there are distinct differences in this scale
along with a body satisfaction scale. These data are compared to measures of body and weight dissatisfaction.
interesting because they show that the relationship between The scale measures the extent to which women have so
feminist identity and internalization of the media is stronger internalized cultural messages about thinness that it affects
than that between feminist identity and body attitudes. It is their conception of the self (McKinley and Hyde 1996).
likely the case that feminist women are more resistant to Women who report body shame are believed to have
media images that glorify slenderness, but this does not internalized the cultural messages that they should be thin,
completely parallel higher body satisfaction rates. that they should be able to control their weight, and that
Thus most of the data show only weak relationships they are not worthy individuals otherwise. Feminist identity
between feminist identity and feelings and thoughts related seems to provide some protection against this internaliza-
to the body. For example, the relationship between feminist tion. Similarly, feminist identity was negatively related to
attitudes and satisfaction with body parts, weight satisfac- internalizing the thin media ideal as measured by the
tion, drive for thinness, and eating disordered attitudes were SATAQ (Heinberg et al. 1995). It is likely that feminism
all small effect sizes. Perhaps there is not a strong helps women critically evaluate and perhaps avoid harmful
association between developing a feminist identity and a cultural messages. There is still a lot we need to know
sense of self independent of cultural pressures about the about feminist identity development in general (e.g., Hyde
body. After all, these pressures are very strong. Body image 2002). Indeed, none of the studies used in this meta-
dissatisfaction is so common among women and girls in analysis included adolescent or younger girls. Feminist
Western cultures to be considered normative, and females identity development could be fruitfully studied along with
experience more body dissatisfaction than males (Feingold other variables such as body attitudes and critical thought.
and Mazzella 1998; Ricciardelli and McCabe 2004). In a study of grade school girls it was found that some girls
Feminists might not be immune to these pressures. Rubin could reject comparing themselves to sexually objectified
and colleagues’ (Rubin et al. 2004) qualitative data are images of women and that this rejection was associated
consistent with this idea in that college-age feminist women with positive body esteem (Murnen et al. 2003).
reported that they could not completely resist socio-cultural The interpretation of these meta-analytic findings are
pressures about thinness. limited by several aspects of the studies included. The
On the other hand, the patterns of data revealed in this samples were narrow in terms of age of participants, their
analysis suggest that there are validity issues in the research country of origin, and ethnicity. Body image (Anderson-
that has been conducted. The fact that body attitude effect Fye 2008; Becker and Fay 2006) shows considerable
sizes were larger in more purposeful samples, in samples of variability across cultures in terms of the definition and
older women, and in studies where the feminist identity meaning of attractiveness. Investment in gender equality,
194 Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197

which may signal acceptability of feminism and feminist correlated with anger which was associated with negative
identity, similarly differs substantially (Eagly and Woods psychological functioning. More advanced stages of iden-
1999). Thus, it is reasonable to argue that the relationship tity should lead to the best outcomes. Thus, it would be
between body image and feminist identity may also vary useful to study the development of feminist identity along
cross-culturally. It will be important to develop culturally with variables such as body attitudes and critical thought.
sensitive measures of feminist identity. In addition, We should be careful to continue to develop scales that can
although the present analysis did not indicate differences capture the process of feminist identity in a valid manner.
in the feminist identity–body image relationships by There is still a lot we need to know about feminist identity
ethnicity, much work remains to be done in this area. Body development in general (e.g., Hyde 2002), and the most
image and the meaning of feminism may vary within US valid and reliable forms of measurement. In terms of other
ethnic groups (e.g., Grabe and Hyde 2006). In the USA, constructs, Engeln-Maddox and Miller (2008) recently
African-American women continue to demonstrate lower developed a scale that measures critical evaluation of media
levels of body dissatisfaction than do other ethnic groups. that could be used to measure the critical thought processes,
More generally, it is important to test the applicability of along with the SATAQ (Heinberg et al. 1995).
objectification theory in non-Western cultures. The present In addition to cultural analysis, a second potentially
data suggest that feminist attitudes might help prevent important aspect of feminism is its emphasis on collective
internalization of cultural sources of objectification such as action. If one can recognize oppression and act against it, a
exposure to the thin media ideal. A feminist orientation number of sexist barriers can be recognized and resisted.
might interrupt the process of self-objectification in that For example, feminism might help one to deal effectively
feminists might contextualize cultural experiences rather with sexist discrimination so that women can realize career
than personalize them. Although it is likely that objectifi- goals. Being successful in a career might help one
cation of women exists in all patriarchal cultures given that experience a strong identity that is fairly independent from
this is believed to be an important source of women’s appearance concerns. Similarly, if one has developed a
subjugation (Schur 1983), the nature of the objectification strong voice through their collective action, it should
might differ. In the most patriarchal cultures where women promote self-reliance rather than reliance on culturally
have little control over financial resources and other aspects defined definitions of femininity. Klonoff and colleagues
of their lives, objectification might be manifested in very (Klonoff and Landrine 1995; Klonoff et al. 2000) have
direct, personal ways such as control over women’s proposed a model of the positive effects of feminism and
sexuality. The relevance of self-objectification, feminist found support for the idea that feminist attitudes helped
attitudes, and body image dissatisfaction to understanding buffer women from the negative effects of sexist discrim-
such experiences is unclear. It is likely that the issue of ination. Zucker (2004) found that self-proclaimed feminist
personal control is very relevant to coping with direct women were more likely to report that they had experi-
objectification experiences, but the control would not come enced problems associated with sexism, and that they
from the development of a feminist identity if that was not engaged in more activist behavior than those who held
an available cultural construct. egalitarian attitudes but did not call themselves feminists.
Despite the limitations of the samples in the data set, the Third, feminism may empower women to act more in
findings offer some testable ideas about the developmental their own self interest than to blindly follow the dictates of
nature of the association between feminist identity and society that women should focus much attention on their
body-related constructs. There are at least three components bodies. Worell and colleagues (Worell 2001, 2006; Johnson
of feminism that might serve as protective factors. First, et al. 2005) have argued that interventions should go
feminist identity should lead to an elevation of critical beyond symptom treatment and should facilitate women’s
thought. According to the Downing and Roush (1985) positive, healthy development through empowerment. Their
model, the early stages of feminist identity involve construct of empowerment is multi-faceted and includes
“revelation” of the existence and effects of sexism. In this valuing women’s perspectives and focusing on context as
early stage women are likely to experience intense feelings the source of psychological problems. Desired outcomes of
including guilt and shame, but also the beginnings of empowerment-oriented intervention include greater decision-
critical thought. In later stages when affect has leavened the making and problem solving skills, confidence in one’s own
critical thought processes should be effective in preventing ability to gain support and love from others, self-nurturance,
internalization of oppressive cultural messages. There is and personal choice in life circumstances. These skills might
some support for the idea that early stages of feminist decrease the likelihood that women would development body
identity are associated with distress. Fischer and Good image and eating problems. Indeed, Johnson et al. (2005)
(2004) found that “revelation” scores measured by the reported that empowerment was associated with overall well-
Feminist Identity Composite (Fischer et al. 2000) were being and less symptom severity.
Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197 195

In sum, in the search for variables that might predict Cash, T. F., Ancis, J. R., & Strachan, M. D. (1997). Gender attitudes,
body satisfaction and help prevent eating disorders, feminist identity, and body images among college women. Sex
Roles, 36, 433–447.
feminist identity is worthy of further exploration. Compared Cogan, J. C. (1999). Lesbians walk the tightrope of beauty: Thin is in
to other meta-analytic findings, the association between but femme is out. Journal of Lesbian Studies, 3, 77–89.
feminist identity and body shame is one of the strongest Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical power for the behavioral sciences. (rev.
“protective” effects we have found in our meta-analyses; ed.). New York: Academic.
Cooper, P. J., Taylor, M. J., Cooper, Z., & Fairburn, C. G. (1987). The
for example, it is larger than that of participating in sports development and validation of the Body Shape Questionnaire.
that don’t emphasize leanness and eating disorder scores International Journal of Eating Disorders, 6, 485–494.
(Smolak et al. 2000). On the other side of the equation, the Cross, W. E. (1971). The Negro-to-Black conversion experience: Toward
effects associated with media use and body dissatisfaction a psychology of Black liberation. Black World, 20, 13–27.
Dionne, M., Davis, C., Fox, J., & Gurevich, M. (1995). Feminist
(Groesz et al. 2002; Murnen et al. 2007) and with sexual ideology as a predictor of body dissatisfaction in women. Sex
abuse and eating disorders (Smolak and Murnen 2002) are Roles, 33, 277–287.
similar in size. These data suggest that there are disruptive Downing, N. E., & Roush, K. L. (1985). From passive acceptance to
societal factors that threaten to harm girls’ and women’s active commitment: A model of feminist identity development in
women. The Counseling Psychologist, 13, 695–709.
connections to their bodies. It is proposed that the Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in
development of a feminist consciousness can help girls human behavior: Evolved dispositions versus social roles.
and women develop critical consciousness to resist and/or American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.
reinterpret sexist experiences. On a larger level, feminism Eisele, H. (2007). The impact of women’s studies classes on
feminism, self-efficacy, and weight concern. Poster presented at
can encourage people to join together in collective action to the annual conference of the Association for Women in
expose and eradicate forces that threaten to disrupt body Psychology; San Francisco, CA, March.
satisfaction and healthy eating. Engeln-Maddox, R. (2006). Buying a beauty standard or dreaming of
a new life? Expectations associated with media ideals. Psychology
of Women Quarterly, 30, 258–266.
Engeln-Maddox, R., & Miller, S. A. (2008). Talking back to the media
ideal: The development and validation of the critical processing
References
of beauty images scales. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32,
159–171.
Affleck, D. J. (2000). Women’s experiences of their bodies: Fassinger, R. E. (1994). Development and testing of the Attitudes
Understanding the connection between feminist consciousness, Towards Feminism and the Women’s Movement (FWM) Scale.
body image, and eating disorders. Dissertation Abstracts Psychology of Women Quarterly, 18, 389–402.
International (Accession no. AAINQ45632). Feingold, A., & Mazzella, R. (1998). Gender differences in body
Anderson-Fye, E. (2008). Cross-cultural issues in child and adolescent image are increasing. Psychological Science, 9, 190–195.
body image. In L. Smolak & J. K. Thompson (Eds.), Body Fingeret, M. C., & Gleaves, D. H. (2004). Sociocultural, feminist, and
image, eating disorders, and obesity in youth, (2nd ed.) psychological influences on women’s body satisfaction: A
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (in press). structural modeling analysis. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
Bargad, A., & Hyde, J. S. (1991). Women’s studies: A study of 28, 370–380.
feminist identity development in women. Psychology of Women Fischer, A. R., & Good, G. E. (2004). Women’s feminist consciousness,
Quarterly, 15, 181–201. anger, and psychological distress. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
Beamer, L. J. (1999). Psychological and sociocultural determinants of 51, 437–446.
a preoccupation with thinness in college women. Dissertation Fischer, A. R., Tokar, D. M., Mergl, M., Good, G. E., Hill, M. S., &
Abstracts International (Accession No. AEH9927604). Blum, S. A. (2000). Assessing women’s feminist identity
Becker, A. E., & Fay, K. (2006). Sociocultural issues and eating development: Studies of convergent, discriminant, and structural
disorders. In S. Wonderlich, J. Mitchell, M. de Zwaan, & H. validity. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 15–29.
Steiger (Eds.), Annual review of eating disorders, part 2—2006 Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T. A. (1997). Objectification theory:
(pp. 35–63). Oxford: Radcliffe. Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental
Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable weight: Feminism, Western culture, and health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206.
the body. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press. Garner, D. M. (1997). The 1997 Body Image Survey results.
Brooks, W. (2002). The relationship among feminist attitudes, perceived Psychology Today, p. 32, February
parental attitudes toward women, achievement orientation, and Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1982). The eating attitudes test: An
eating disordered behavior in late adolescent women. Dissertation index of the symptoms of anorexia nervosa. Psychological
Abstracts International (Accession no. AAI3042291). Medicine, 9, 273–279.
Brown, T. A., Cash, T. F., & Mikulka, P. J. (1990). Attitudinal body Garner, D. M., Olmstead, M. P., & Polivy, J. (1983). Development and
image assessment: Factor analysis of the Body-Self Relations validation of a multidimensional Eating Disorder Inventory for
Questionnaire. Journal of Personality Assessment, 55, 135–144. anorexia nervosa and bulimia. International Journal of Eating
Calandra, J. M. (2001). Body image in menopausal lesbian and Disorders, 2, 15–34.
heterosexual women. Dissertation Abstracts International Gorman, K. S. (1999). Early stages of feminist identity, avoidant coping,
(Accession no. AAI9997528). and anorexic tendencies in undergraduate women. Dissertation
Callow, J. L. (2005). Adult women’s experience of their bodies: The Abstracts International (Accession no. AAM9907697).
connection between perceived control, physical appearance, Grabe, S., & Hyde, J. S. (2006). Ethnicity and body dissatisfaction among
feminist ideology, and body image satisfaction. Dissertation women in the United States: A meta-analysis. Psychological
Abstracts International (Accession no. AAI3161369) Bulletin, 132, 622–640.
196 Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197

Groesz, L. M., Levine, M. P., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). The effect of Mendelson, B. K., & White, D. R. (1982). Relation between body-
experimental presentation of thin media images on body esteem and self-esteem of obese and normal children. Perceptual
satisfaction: A meta-analytic review. International Journal of and Motor Skills, 54, 899–905.
Eating Disorders, 31, 1–16. Morgan, B. L. (1996). Putting the feminism into feminism scales.
Guille, C., & Chrisler, J. C. (1999). Does feminism serve as a Introduction of a liberal feminist attitude and ideology scale
protective function against eating disorders? Journal of Lesbian (LFAIS). Sex Roles, 34, 359–390.
Studies, 3, 141–148. Murnen, S. K., Levine, M. P., Smith, J., & Groesz, L. (2007). Do
Haines, M. E., Erchull, M. J., Liss, M., Turner, D. L., Nelson, J. A., fashion magazines promote body dissatisfaction in girls and
Ramsey, L. R., et al. (2008). Predictors and effects of self- women? A meta-analytic review. Paper presented at the annual
objectification in lesbians. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, convention of the American Psychological Association; San
181–187. Francisco, CA, August.
Heinberg, L. J., Thompson, J. K., & Stormer, S. (1995). Development Murnen, S. K., & Smolak, L. (1997). Femininity, masculinity, and
and validation of the Sociocultural Attitudes Towards Appearance disordered eating: A meta-analytic review. International Journal
Questionnaire. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 17, 81– of Eating Disorders, 22, 231–242.
89. Murnen, S. K., Smolak, L., Mills, J. A., & Good, L. (2003). Thin, sexy
Henley, N. M., Meng, K., O’Brien, D., McCarthy, W. J., & women and strong, muscular men: Grade-school children’s
Sockloskie, R. (1998). Developing a scale to measure the responses to objectified images of women and men. Sex Roles, 49,
diversity of feminist attitudes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 427–437.
22, 317–348. Myers, T. A., & Crowther, J. H. (2007). Sociocultural pressures, thin-ideal
Huddy, L., Neely, F., & Lafay, M. R. (2000). The polls-trends: internalization, self-objectification, and body dissatisfaction: Could
Support for the women’s movement. Public Opinion Quarterly, feminist beliefs be a moderating factor? Body Image, 4, 296–308.
64, 309–350. Noffsinger-Frazier, N. A. (2004). Objectification theory and disordered
Hurt, M. M., Nelson, J. A., Turner, D. L., Haines, M. E., Ramsey, L. eating: The impact of feminist identification, internalization of
R., Erhcull, M. J., et al. (2007). Feminism: What is it good for? sociocultural standards of appearance, and sexual orientation.
Feminine norms and objectification as the link between feminist Dissertation Abstracts International (Accession no. AAI3137803).
identity and clinically relevant outcomes. Sex Roles, 57, 355– Ojerholm, A. J., & Rothblum, E. D. (1999). The relationship of body
363. image, feminism, and sexual orientation in college women.
Hyde, J. S. (2002). Feminist identity development: The current state of Feminism and Psychology, 9, 431–448.
theory, research, and practice. The Counseling Psychologist, 30, Peterson, R., Tantleff-Dunn, S., & Bedwell, J. (2006). The effects of
105–110. exposure to feminist ideology on women’s body image. Body
Johnson, D., Worell, J., & Chandler, R. (2005). Assessing psycho- Image, 3, 237–246.
logical health and empowerment in women: The personal Piran, N., & Cormier, H. C. (2005). The social construction of women
progress scale revised. Women & Health, 41, 109–128. and disordered eating patterns. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
Kilbourne, J. (1999). Deadly persuasion: Why women and girls must 52, 549–558.
fight the addictive power of advertising. New York: Free. Reid, A., & Purcell, N. (2004). Pathways to feminist identification.
Klonoff, E. A., & Landrine, H. (1995). The schedule of sexist events: Sex Roles, 50, 759–769.
A measure of lifetime and recent sexist discrimination in Ricciardelli, L. A., & McCabe, M. P. (2004). A biopsychosocial
women’s lives. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 19, 439–472. model of disordered eating and the pursuit of muscularity in
Klonoff, E. A., Landrine, H., & Campbell, R. (2000). Sexist adolescent boys. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 179–205.
discrimination may account for well-known gender differences Rickard, K. M. (1989). The relationship of self-monitored dating
in psychiatric symptoms. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, behaviors to level of feminist identity on the Feminist Identity
93–99. Scale. Sex Roles, 20, 213–226.
Leggatt, J. M. (2004). What happens when grrrls riot?: The Rockwern, B., & Murnen, S. K. (2008). Internalization of “Everyday”
relationship between feminism and eating pathology in adolescent Objectification Experiences, Feminist Attitudes, and Eating
girls. Dissertation Abstracts International (Accession no. Disordered Attitudes. Poster presented at annual meeting of the
AAINQ92547). Midwestern Psychological Association, April.
Levine, M. P., & Smolak, L. (2006). The prevention of eating Rubin, L. R., Nemeroff, C. J., & Russo, N. F. (2004). Exploring
problems and eating disorders: Theory, research, and practice. feminist women’s body consciousness. Psychology of Women
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Quarterly, 28, 27–37.
Linnebach, D. (2005). Culture’s not so great expectations: Does Sabik, N. J., & Tylka, T. L. (2006). Do feminist identity styles
feminist identity moderate women’s experiences with sexism and moderate the relation between perceived sexist events and
body image dissatisfaction? Dissertation Abstracts International disordered eating? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 30, 77–84.
(Accession no. AAI3141648). Schur, E. M. (1983). Labeling women deviant: Gender, stigma and
Liss, M., Crawford, M., & Popp, D. (2004). Predictors and correlates social control. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.
of collective action. Sex Roles, 50, 771–779. Shadish, W., & Haddock, C. K. (1994). Combining estimates of effect
Liss, M., Hoffner, C., & Crawford, M. (2000). What do feminists size. In H. Cooper, & L. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of research
believe? Psychology of Women Quarterly, 24, 279–284. synthesis (pp. 261–284). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Liss, M., O’Connor, C., Morosky, E., & Crawford, M. (2001). What Slaviero, D. L. (2006). An exploration of feminist ideology as a
makes a feminist? Predictors and correlates of feminist social protective factor for body image disturbance. Dissertation
identity in women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 124–133. Abstracts International (Accession no. 2007-99004-214).
Mahalik, J. R., Morray, E. B., Coonerty-Femiano, A., Ludlow, L. H., Smith, E. R., Ferree, M. M., & Miller, F. D. (1975). A short scale of
Slattery, S. M., & Smiler, A. (2005). Development of the attitudes towards feminism. Representative Research in Social
Conformity to Feminine Norms Inventory. Sex Roles, 52, 417–435. Psychology, 6, 51–56.
McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body Smolak, L., & Murnen, S. K. (2007). Feminism and body image. In V.
consciousness scale: Self-objectification, body shame, and Swami & A. Furnham (Eds.), The body beautiful (pp. 236–258).
disordered eating. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 186–216. London: Palgrave Macmillan.
Sex Roles (2009) 60:186–197 197

Smolak, L., & Murnen, S. K. (2002). A meta-analytic examination Ward, L. M. (1995). Talking about sex: Common themes about
of the relationship between child sexual abuse and eating sexuality in the prime-time television programs children and
disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 31, 136– adolescents view most. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 24,
150. 595–615.
Smolak, L., & Munstertieger, B. F. (2002). The relationship of gender Ward, L. M. (2003). Understanding the role of entertainment media in
and voice to depression and eating disorders. Psychology of the sexual socialization of American youth: A review of
Women Quarterly, 26, 234–241. empirical research. Developmental Review, 23, 347–388.
Smolak, L., Murnen, S. K., & Ruble, A. E. (2000). Female athletes Welsh, A. C. (2007). Feminism, career attitudes and disordered eating
and eating problems: A meta-analysis. International Journal of in college women. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the
Eating Disorders, 27, 371–380. American Psychological Association: San Francisco, CA,
Snyder, R., & Hasbrouck, L. (1996). Feminist identity, gender traits, August.
and symptoms of disturbed eating among college women. Williams, R., & Wittig, M. A. (1997). I’m not a feminist but...: Factors
Psychology of Women Quarterly, 20, 593–598. contributing to the discrepancy between pro-feminist orientation
Stice, E. (2002). Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: A and feminist social identity. Sex Roles, 37, 885–904.
meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 825–848. Wolf, N. (1991). The beauty myth: How images of beauty are used
Thompson, J. K., Heinberg, L. J., Altabe, M. N., & Tantleff-Dunn, S. against women. New York: W. Morrow.
(1999). Exacting beauty: Theory, assessment, and treatment of body- Worell, J. (2001). Feminist interventions: Accountability beyond
image disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psychological symptom reduction. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 335–
Association. 343.
Thompson, J. K., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internalization: Mounting Worell, J. (2006). Pathways to healthy development: Sources of
evidence for a new risk factor for body-image disturbance and eating strength and empowerment. In J. Worell, & C.D. Goodheart
pathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 181– (Eds.), Handbook of girls’ and women’s psychological health
183. (pp. 25–35). New York: Oxford.
Tiggemann, M., & Stevens, C. (1999). Weight concern across the life- Zucker, A. (2004). Disavowing social identities: What it means when
span: Relationship to self-esteem and feminist identity. International women say, “I’m not a feminist, but...”. Psychology of Women
Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 103–106. Quarterly, 28, 423–435.

View publication stats

Potrebbero piacerti anche