Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Accident Analysis and Prevention 33 (2001) 679– 684

www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

Sensation seeking, risky driving and behavioral adaptation


Brian A. Jonah *, Rachel Thiessen, Elaine Au-Yeung
Transport Canada, Road Safety Directorate, Place de Ville, Tower C, 330 Sparks Street, Ottawa, Ont., Canada K1A 0N5

Received 30 March 2000; received in revised form 13 July 2000; accepted 7 August 2000

Abstract

A study on the relationship between sensation seeking (SS) and risky driving, aggressive driving and behavioral adaptation is
reported on. College students completed a questionnaire concerning their driving behavior and level of sensation seeking. Results
indicated that high SS’s were significantly more likely than low SS’s to speed, not wear belts, drink frequently, drive after drinking,
perceive a low risk of detection for impaired driving, and perceive that they could drink more beer before being impaired. High
SS’s were also more likely to report aggressive driving habits. High SS’s were significantly more likely than low SS’s to say that
they would drive faster on highways and on wet roads and drive after drinking, if operating a vehicle equipped with anti-lock
brakes. The results are consistent with previous research. © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Keywords: Sensation seeking; Behavioural adaptation; Adventure seeking

1. Introduction tion (‘I like wild and uninhibited parties’) and one that
reflects a more cautious predilection (‘I prefer quiet
Ever since Tillman and Hobbs (1949) stated that ‘a parties with good conversation’). The SSS consists of
man drives as he lives’, there has been interest in the four subscales, thrill and adventure seeking (TAS),
driver’s personality as a potential underlying causal experience seeking (ES), boredom susceptibility (BS),
factor in driver behavior (cf. Beirness, 1996). One per- and disinhibition (Dis). It is important to note that
sonality trait that has received considerable attention is there are no items on the SSS, which refer to driving
sensation seeking. According to Zuckerman (1994), sen- behavior.
sation seeking (SS) ‘is a trait defined by the seeking of The possibility that SS underlies risky driving has
varied, novel, complex, and intense sensations and ex- been the subject of a considerable amount of research.
periences and the willingness to take physical, social, Jonah (1997) reported that of 40 studies reviewed in the
legal, and financial risks for the sake of such literature, all but four found a positive relationship
experiences’. Central to this trait is ‘the optimistic between SS and some aspect of risky driving (e.g.
tendency to approach novel stimuli and explore the impaired driving, nonuse of seat belts, speeding, follow-
environment’. ing too closely, traffic violations). The order of magni-
Sensation seeking is operationally defined in terms of tude of the relationship between SS and risky driving
scores on the sensation seeking scale (SSS) which was was in the 0.30 –0.40 range, depending on the measure
first published by Zuckerman et al. (1964). Form V of of driving behavior. This relationship has been ob-
the SSS is currently the most commonly used measure served among drivers from Canada, the United States,
of sensation seeking (Zuckerman, 1994). The 40 forced- Great Britain, the Netherlands, Sweden and Finland
choice items on this scale require subjects to choose and has been found for both men and women but is
between a statement which reflects a desire for sensa- weaker among the latter.
In discussing the implications of the relationship
* Corresponding author. Tel.: + 1-613-9981968; fax: +1-613-
between SS and risky driving, Jonah (1997) raised the
9902913. possibility that SS may not only influence the way
E-mail address: jonahb@tc.gc.ca (B.A. Jonah). drivers perceive and respond to risk while driving, but it

0001-4575/01/$ - see front matter © 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
PII: S 0 0 0 1 - 4 5 7 5 ( 0 0 ) 0 0 0 8 5 - 3
680 B.A. Jonah et al. / Accident Analysis and Pre6ention 33 (2001) 679–684

may also moderate how they adapt to changes in 2. Method


perceived risk within the driving environment. Jonah
postulated that ‘…high SS’s adapt their behavior to 2.1. Subjects
take advantage of such improvements in safety to a
greater extent than low SS’s and that consequently, The subjects were 120 male and 159 female students
they exhibit greater risky driving’. Is it possible that in psychology, design, marketing, or dental assistant
such behavioral adaptation is not a general phe- courses at two colleges in Montreal and Ottawa,
nomenon observed among all drivers, as purported by Canada. The average age of the sample was 25.03 years
Wilde (1994), but rather one that is pursued mainly by (S.D.= 6.30 years) with the youngest being 17 and the
those drivers who seek to optimize their level of risk oldest 60. All subjects were licensed and had driven
and the potential rewards from those risks? It could be within the last 12 months.
argued that high sensation seeking drivers would adapt
to perceived vehicle technology induced reductions in 2.2. Procedure
risk by taking greater risks. It is hypothesized that
sensation seeking may moderate behavioral adaptation The students voluntarily completed a two part ques-
to risk such that high SS’s are more likely to ‘spend’ tionnaire during class time with the concurrence of the
any safety benefit afforded by vehicle design improve- professor. The students were told ‘This questionnaire
ments by engaging in risky driving, while low SS’s are asks about your driving habits as well your opinions
more likely to ‘bank’ the safety benefit by not altering about driving and road safety’. They were also in-
their behavior, thereby enhancing their level of safety. formed that their responses would be kept confidential
Indeed, it may be the very drivers who would benefit and that they did not have to put their name on it.
the greatest from vehicle safety enhancements (i.e. high The first part of the questionnaire included questions
about distance traveled in the last 12 months, seat belt
SS), who are most likely to adapt their behavior to
use, preferred highway driving speed, perceived risk of
perceived reductions in risk.
motor vehicle collision injury, drinking behavior, driv-
Some support for this hypothesis has been reported
ing within 2 h of drinking, legally impaired driving,
by Ward et al. (1997) who evaluated driver response to
perceived likelihood of apprehension for impaired driv-
an adaptive cruise control (ACC) device which auto-
ing, drinking capacity before one’s ability becomes
mates speed and headway maintenance. High and low
impaired, drinking capacity before one’s blood alcohol
SS’s drove a vehicle along a set route on a highway
concentration is over legal limit, and collisions and
with and without the ACC device. Measures of arousal,
traffic violations during the past 2 years. Many of these
effort, situational awareness and driving behavior were
items had been used in previous research. A measure of
taken. The results indicated that the use of the ACC
aggressive driving was also included where drivers indi-
device resulted in higher measured peak speeds as well cated their agreement or disagreement with statements
as lower levels of arousal and effort being reported by about engaging in various aggressive driving behaviors
high SS’s compared with low SS’s. (e.g. swearing at other drivers, beating other drivers
There is considerable debate of late concerning ‘road from getaway, driving close behind other vehicles to get
rage’ and the aggressive driver (American Automobile driver to move over, etc.).
Association, 1997). While there have been studies of To measure behavioral adaptation, the subjects were
aggressive driving behavior and aggressive personality asked ‘If you know that you are driving a car equipped
traits of drivers, no attempt has yet been made to link with anti-lock brakes, which increase safety, how likely
SS with aggressive driving. Aggressive behavior toward are you to do each of the following — drive faster on
other drivers on the road may be another manifestation highways, drive faster on wet roads, drive closer behind
of high SS’s thirst for excitement. A driver who other vehicles, drive after drinking, not wear a seat belt,
screeches his tires as he races away from other drivers and speed up if the light turned yellow’. Subjects rated
at an intersection and then gives those left in his dust their likelihood as not at all likely, somewhat likely, or
an obscene gesture, may find their aggressive behavior very likely.
stimulating. The second part of the questionnaire was Form V of
The present study investigated the relationship be- Zuckerman’s SSS which consists of 40 forced-choice
tween SS and self-reported risky and aggressive driving, items (Zuckerman, 1994). The distribution of total sen-
as well as subjects’ likelihood of adapting to anti-lock sation seeking scores was split at the median so that
braking systems by driving in a more risky manner. subjects with total SS scores below 20 were classified as
Since most previous research on SS and risky driving low sensation seekers and those with scores 20 and
has used only male subjects, both men and women were higher were considered as high sensation seekers. There
included in this study to determine whether gender were roughly equal numbers of subjects categorized as
moderates these relationships. high and low SS.
B.A. Jonah et al. / Accident Analysis and Pre6ention 33 (2001) 679–684 681

The questionnaire was pretested with 20 subjects. drinks before their ability to drive was impaired, and
More details about the questions asked are provided in believe that the chances of an impaired driver being
the tables showing the results. caught by the police are low. There was a trend for high
A copy of the full questionnaire can be obtained sensation seekers to be more likely to report a traffic
from the first author. violation within the last two years (PB 0.12) but there
was no difference on collision involvement. There were
2.3. Analyses no interaction effects with gender, indicating that the
differences between high and low SS’s on these risky
The discrete data were analyzed using multiple  2 to driving measures existed for both men and women.
test for the effects of SS, gender and their interaction.
Rating scales were analyzed using an SS×Gender
3.2. Aggressi6e dri6ing habits
analysis of variance. It was decided to analyze the data
using median splits on SS rather than correlations so
The degree to which drivers agreed that they engage
that interactions with gender could be detected and
in various aggressive driving habits is exhibited in Table
plotted. The 0.05 level of significance was adopted for
2. High sensation seekers were more likely (PB0.05)
these analyses. For correlational analyses, Pearson
than low’s to agree that they swear at other drivers, like
product moment correlations were conducted. The 0.01
to beat other drivers at the getaway, think it is fun to
level of significance was adopted for these analyses.
weave through traffic, like passing other cars, find
driving at high speeds is exciting, like to outsmart other
drivers, and often lose their temper while driving. Addi-
3. Results tionally, there were marginally significant differences
(PB 0.10) on not being easily provoked or angered
High and low sensation seekers did not significantly when driving, never using the horn when annoyed and
differ on age, gender, annual kilometers traveled, col- making rude signs to other drivers.
lege attended or computer ownership (proxy measure There were significant SS by gender interactions on
for socio-economic status). Given the main interest of only two aggressive behavior items, passing and making
the study was SS, only significant the main effects of SS rude signs. For the passing behavior, there was no
and interaction effects of gender with SS are reported. difference between high and low SS’s among males but
among female drivers, high SS’s reported more aggres-
3.1. Risky dri6ing and risk perception sive passing than low SS’s. For making rude signs, the
opposite pattern emerged with no effect of SS among
Table 1 presents self-reported risky driving behavior females but high SS’s among males reporting more of
for high and low sensation seekers. High sensation this behavior than low SS’s.
seekers (SS’s) were significantly (P B0.05) more likely A principal components factor analysis performed on
than low sensation seekers to say that they do not the aggressive driving items identified two factors with
always wear seat belts, they would drive 120 km/h or eigenvalues greater than 1.0. The first factor included
faster on a highway if there was no speed limit, drink all the aggressive driving items except ‘Not easily pro-
two to seven times per week, drive while they thought voked’, ‘Never use the horn’ and ‘Never enter intersec-
that their blood alcohol concentration was over the tion on red light’ which loaded on the second factor.
legal limit, believe that they could drink five or more Composite aggression scales were created based on

Table 1
Risky driving, risk perception and sensation seeking

Measure Level of sensation seeking P

Low (N =145, %) High (N = 134, %)

Do not always use seat belts 4.9 13.4 0.02


Drive over 120 km/h on expressway if no limit 17.2 37.3 0.001
Drink 2-7 times per week 7.6 28.9 0.001
Drive within 2 h of drinking 28.3 39.5 0.07
Drive while over legal limit 3.5 11.1 0.04
Drink 5 plus beers in 2 h before driving ability impaired 8.9 26.0 0.01
Low chances impaired driver will be caught 23.9 50.8 0.001
Collision involvement last 2 years 20.0 26.1 n.s.
Traffic violation last 2 years 22.8 31.3 0.11
682 B.A. Jonah et al. / Accident Analysis and Pre6ention 33 (2001) 679–684

Table 2 items were averaged to create a composite measure of


Aggressive driving habits and sensation seeking
behavioral adaptation. An analysis of variance per-
Measure Level of sensation seeking P
formed on this composite scale revealed that high sen-
sation seekers were more likely than low sensation
Low (N =145) High (N= 134) seekers to indicate that they would drive in a risky
manner if they thought that the vehicle that they were
Swear at other drivers 2.50a 2.93 0.001 driving was equipped with ABS. Although the effect
Beat drivers at 1.75 2.10 0.001
getaway
was small, it was statistically significant (PB0.05).
Fun to weave through 1.66 1.98 0.001 There was no interaction effect with gender on the
traffic composite measure.
Like passing other 1.93 2.26 0.001
cars
Not easily 2.61 2.46 0.10 3.4. Risky dri6ing and SS Subscales
provoked/angered
when driving Correlations were conducted to determine how well
Driving at high speeds 2.14 2.42 0.003
the four subscales of SS, TAS, ES, Dis and BS, as well
is exciting
Like to outsmart 1.90 2.26 0.001 as the total SS scores correlated with the risky driving
drivers measures. These correlations appear in Table 4. Over-
Never use horn when 2.59 2.39 0.08 all, the Dis subscale was significantly (PB 0.01) corre-
annoyed lated with more (7) of the driving measures that any of
Make rude signs 1.66 1.82 0.09
the other SS subscales. The Dis subscale was the sub-
Often lose temper 2.30 2.53 0.02
Never enter 3.24 3.20 n.s. scale most strongly related to the alcohol consumption
intersection on red and driving measures and to the aggressive driving
light measures. The BS subscale was related to speeding,
Drive close behind 1.53 1.62 n.s. drinking, aggressive driving and behavioral adaptation.
Aggression factor 1 1.85 2.11 0.001
Neither the Total SS scores nor any of the subscales
Aggression factor 2 2.48 2.27 0.01
were significantly related to collision and violation
a
Means on a 1–4 rating scale, where 4 indicates greatest agree- frequency.
ment.

these two factors and the high SS’s expressed greater 4. Discussion
aggression on both scales than low SS’s (P B 0.01).
There were no interaction effects with gender on these The results of this study clearly demonstrate that
composite scales. high sensation seekers engage in more risky driving
than low sensation seekers. Although, the present re-
3.3. Beha6ioral adaptation sults are based on self-reports, they are consistent with
previous research which has observed the actual risky
The subjects’ likelihood that they would engage in
Table 3
various risky driving practices if they were driving an Likelihood of behavioral adaptation to anti-lock brakes and sensa-
ABS equipped vehicle is presented by their level of tion seeking
sensation seeking in Table 3. High sensation seekers
were significantly (P B0.05) more likely to say that they Measure Level of sensation seeking P
would drive faster on highways, drive faster on wet
Low (N =145) High (N =134)
roads, and drive after drinking. High SS’s were also
marginally more likely to say that they would not wear Drive faster on 1.23a 1.37 0.03
their seat belt (PB 0.09) and that they would drive highway
closer behind other vehicles (P B0.12). There was a Drive closer behind 1.03 1.17 0.12
significant SS by gender interaction only on the likeli- Drive after drinking 1.01 1.08 0.02
Don’t wear belt 1.01 1.06 0.09
hood of driving faster on highways. Male subjects Speed up on yellow 1.46 1.50 n.s.
exhibited no difference as a function of SS but among light
female subjects, high SS’s were more likely to say that Drive faster on wet 1.17 1.29 0.03
they would drive faster. roads
The behavioral adaptation items were factor ana- Composite behavioral 1.17 1.30 0.05
adaptation measure
lyzed using principal components and only one factor
emerged with an eigenvalue greater than 1.0. All six a
Means on a 1–3 rating scale, where 3 indicates very likely to
items loaded on this factor. The ratings on these six engage in behavior.
B.A. Jonah et al. / Accident Analysis and Pre6ention 33 (2001) 679–684 683

Table 4
Correlations between risky driving and sensation seekinga

TAS ES DIS BS Total SS

Seat belt use −0.02 −0.07 −0.06 −0.10 −0.09


Highway speed 0.22* 0.11 0.23* 0.22* 0.28*
Drinking frequency 0.23* 0.19* 0.48* 0.21* 0.41*
Chances being caught −0.11 −0.12 −0.12 0.02 −0.12
Drive within 2 h of drinking 0.19* 0.17* 0.28* 0.09 0.27*
Beers drunk before feeling impaired 0.19* 0.21* 0.28* 0.07 0.27*
Beers drunk before legally impaired 0.19* 0.21* 0.28* 0.07 0.26*
c Collisions in last 2 years 0.01 0.00 0.06 0.13 0.07
c Violations in last 2 years 0.09 0.02 0.05 0.14 0.10
Aggressive driving 1** 0.23* 0.09 0.38* 0.34* 0.37*
Aggressive driving 2** 0.06 0.02 0.22* 0.21* 0.21*
Likelihood of behavioral adaptation** 0.01 −0.01 0.13 0.18* 0.10

a
*, PB0.01; N =279; **, composite measures; TAS, thrill and adventure seeking; ES, experience seeking; DIS, disinhibition; BS, boredom
susceptibility.

driving of high and low sensation seekers (Jonah, 1997). ways or on wet roads, and driving after drinking. While
Although, there were a few sporadic SS by gender these results may reflect greater potential behavioral
interactions, there was no clear indication that the adaptation among high SS’s, these results are only
relationship between SS and risky driving was moder- suggestive since it was not known how subjects would
ated by gender. drive without ABS and hence the measure did not
The observation that high SS’s reported exhibiting really reflect a change in behavior. Further, the subjects
greater aggressive behavior while driving than low SS’s actual response to ABS was not measured only their
is a new finding. Aggressive driving such as weaving likelihood to engage in the behavior. However, if it
through traffic, frequent passing or driving too closely found that it is primarily the high SS’s who engage in
are also risky since this behavior can lead to collision behavioral adaptation, this should be a major concern
involvement. However, while behaviors such as swear- to road safety policy makers because it is the high SS’s
ing, making rude signs or honking the horn may be who are already more likely to take risks while driving.
aggressive they may pose relatively little risk to other Even if a measure improves the safety of 80 or 90% of
drivers. In contrast, impaired driving behavior, though the driving population, behavioral adaptation by the
not inherently aggressive toward other drivers, unless it remaining 10–20%, who may already be at greater risk
provokes aggressive activity such as following too of collision involvement given their risky driving, may
closely or weaving in and out of traffic, is clearly risky. eradicate the overall effectiveness of the measure. There
It may well be that these aggressive behaviors, whether is need for future research which permits high and low
they reflect either hostility or overt aggressive actions, sensation seekers to drive vehicles with and without
are part of the same stimulation seeking pattern of high ABS over extended periods of time to determine
SS drivers or they could reflect a component of atten- whether in fact, high sensation seekers are more likely
tion deficit disorder or impulsivity. Indeed, sensation to adapt their driving behavior than low SS’s. In addi-
seeking, impulsivity and attention deficit disorder may tion, research should look at behavioral adaptation to
all have the same latent origin. other vehicle safety improvements such as lane depar-
Overall, the Disinhibition subscale of SS was most ture warning systems or night vision systems. While this
strongly related to the risky driving, aggression and intelligent transport system technology is expected to
behavioral adaptation measures. Jonah (1997) reported improve safety, it may end up having the opposite
that in previous studies of risky driving and sensation effect, at least for the high-risk subpopulation of high
seeking, it was the TAS subscale which seemed to be SS drivers.
more strongly related to risky driving. However, in Future research should also investigate the relation-
some cases the magnitude of the correlations, while ship between sensation seeking and other characteristics
statistically significant at the 0.01 level, were relatively of drivers such as impulsivity and attention deficit
low and hence may not be very consequential. disorder. The manifest risky driving behaviors of these
In a hypothetical situation where the subjects were different types of drivers may in fact be based on the
told that they were driving a vehicle equipped with same latent fundamental characteristic. Furthermore,
ABS, which would be safer, high SS’s indicated that future research should be conducted on the biological
they would be more likely than low’s to engage in risky bases of risky driving behavior to determine the extent
driving behaviors, particularly faster driving on high- to which such behavior is biologically predetermined,
684 B.A. Jonah et al. / Accident Analysis and Pre6ention 33 (2001) 679–684

perhaps genetically, as has been suggested by Zucker- Beirness, D.J., 1996. The relationship between lifestyle factors and
man (1994). If a significant proportion of high SS collisions involving young drivers. In: New to the Road: Reducing
the Risks for Young Motorists International Symposium, June
drivers’ behavior is biologically determined, consider- 8 – 11. UCLA Brain Information Service/Brain Research Institute,
able thought should be devoted to how these drivers Los Angeles, p. 17.
should be dealt with. Should licensing authorities be Jonah, B.A., 1997. Sensation seeking and risky driving: a review and
screening driving license applicants for such high-risk synthesis of the literature. Accident Analysis and Prevention 29
characteristics? Can risky behavior which is biologically (5), 651 – 665.
Tillman, W.A., Hobbs, G.E., 1949. The accident prone automobile
based, be treated by medication? Can these drivers be driver: a study of the psychiatric and social background. Ameri-
trained to overcome their propensity to risky driving? can Journal of Psychiatry 106, 321 – 331.
Clearly, the results of the present research generate as Ward, N.J., Fairclough, S., Humphreys, M., 1997. A field evaluation
many questions as answers. of a research adaptive cruise control (ACC) system: automating
the driving task (Unpublished manuscript).
Wilde, G.J.S., 1994. Target Risk. PDE Publications, Toronto, Ont.,
Canada.
Zuckerman, M., 1994. Behavioural Expressions and Biosocial Bases
References of Sensation Seeking. University of Cambridge Press, Cambridge.
Zuckerman, M., Kolin, I., Price, L., Zoob, I., 1964. Development of
American Automobile Association, 1997. Aggressive driving: three a sensation seeking scale. Journal of Consulting Psychology 28,
studies. AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. Washington, DC. 477 – 482.

Potrebbero piacerti anche