Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

IALP Student Award 2010

Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 Published online: October 7, 2010


DOI: 10.1159/000319913

Speech Disfluency Types of Fluent and


Stuttering Individuals: Age Effects
Fabiola Staróbole Juste Claudia Regina Furquim de Andrade
Department of Physiotherapy, Communication Science and Disorders, Occupational Therapy, Faculty of Medicine,
University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil

Key Words Introduction


Stuttering ⴢ Fluency ⴢ Speech disfluency types
Over the years, much research has been completed ex-
amining the speech disfluencies of normally fluent indi-
Abstract viduals (children, adolescents and adults). Although the
Objective: The purpose of the present study was to quantify descriptions of speech disfluencies have been extensive,
and compare the typology of speech disruptions presented especially for children and adults, these investigations al-
by stuttering and fluent Brazilian Portuguese-speaking chil- most exclusively focused on understanding the speech of
dren, adolescents and adults. Subjects and Methods: Par- English-speaking individuals from Anglo-European cul-
ticipants were 150 individuals, with ages between 4.0 and tures. Because stuttering is a fluency disorder observed
49.11 years, who were divided into six groups: children who across languages and cultures, there is value in under-
stutter (n = 25), children who do not stutter (n = 25), adoles- standing the disfluent speech of individuals from cultur-
cents who stutter (n = 25), adolescents who do not stutter ally and linguistically diverse backgrounds.
(n = 25), adults who stutter (n = 25) and adults who do not A central issue for the diagnosis and treatment of stut-
stutter (n = 25). For each participant, the frequency of stut- tering is to understand that people who stutter, just like
tering-like disfluencies (SLD) and other disfluencies was cal- people who do not stutter, present quantitative and qual-
culated based on the video recordings of spontaneous itative variations in stuttering. This knowledge decreases
speech samples. Results: The results obtained for SLD were the chances that the clinician will be fooled into thinking
significant when comparing stuttering and fluent individu- that the client’s change during therapy is due to therapy
als. In children who stutter, adolescents who stutter, and itself when it may be nothing more than ‘natural’ varia-
adults who stutter, a higher frequency of SLD was found. tions in behavior. To avoid this, it is important to know
Other disfluencies did not differentiate the groups. Conclu- about the central tendency (average, median, or mode) of
sion: The age factor did not qualify quantitatively any of the a person’s stuttering [1].
groups, but indicated qualitative differences (in terms of the The differentiation of subtypes of speech disruptions
type of speech disfluencies) between groups. The results re- assumed a central role in the studies about stuttering.
vealed similarities when compared with studies carried out Van Riper [2] suggested that the number of speech disflu-
in different languages. Copyright © 2010 S. Karger AG, Basel encies (especially of certain types) is considered the most
important index of stuttering severity.
The types of speech disfluencies most frequently as-
Student Supervisor: Claudia Regina Furquim de Andrade. sociated with stuttering are: within-word disfluencies

© 2010 S. Karger AG, Basel Fabiola Staróbole Juste


1021–7762/11/0632–0057$38.00/0 Rua Cipotênea, 51
Fax +41 61 306 12 34 Cidade Universitária
E-Mail karger@karger.ch Accessible online at: São Paulo 05360-160 (Brazil)
www.karger.com www.karger.com/fpl Tel. +55 11 3091 8406, Fax +55 11 3091 7714, E-Mail fjuste @ usp.br
(i.e., prolongations, blocks and sound and syllable repeti- differentiate more and less typical behaviors and identify
tions) and monosyllabic repetitions [1, 3–5]. Between- stuttering in different populations.
word disfluencies (i.e., short monosyllabic whole-word The purpose of the present study is to (a) classify,
repetitions, polysyllabic whole-word repetitions, revi- quantify and compare the typology of speech disruptions
sions, interjections) occur in the speech of both stuttering presented by stuttering children, adolescents and adults,
and normally fluent individuals [6–8]. (b) compare the results with the corresponding fluent
Ambrose and Yairi [3] developed a weighted measure groups and (c) verify differences in the occurrence of SLD
for stuttering-like disfluencies (SLD) for this purpose. In and OD in the different age groups of Brazilian Portu-
an attempt to describe these speech disfluencies more guese-speaking individuals.
typically found in the speech of stuttering children, these
authors defined SLD as consisting of any one of the fol-
lowing types of disfluency: part-word repetitions (e.g., Method
‘b-but’, ‘thi-thi-this’), single-syllable word repetitions
Subjects
(e.g., ‘you you you’, ‘and and’), disrhythmic prolongations Subjects were 150 individuals, 108 male and 42 female, aged
(e.g., ‘mmmmy’, ‘coooookie’), blocks (e.g., ‘#toy’), and between 4.0 and 49.11; they were divided into six groups: children
broken words (e.g., ‘o#pen’). Ambrose and Yairi [3] de- who stutter (CWS, n = 25), children who do not stutter (CWNS,
scribed other kinds of disfluencies as ‘other disfluencies’ n = 25), adolescents who stutter (AWS, n = 25), adolescents who
(OD), for example, interjections (e.g., ‘um’), revision or do not stutter (AWNS, n = 25), adults who stutter (AdWS, n = 25)
and adults who do not stutter (AdWNS, n = 25).
abandoned utterances (e.g., ‘Mon ate/ Mon fixed dinner’, The groups of children (CWS and CWNS) consisted of 38 boys
‘I want/Hey look at that’) and multisyllable or phrase rep- and 12 girls, with ages between 4.0 and 11.11 years (mean age =
etitions (e.g., ‘because because’, ‘I want I want to go’). 5.7 years). The groups of adolescents (AWS and AWNS) consist-
Conture [1] and Yairi and Ambrose [4] suggested a crite- ed of 30 males and 20 females, with ages between 12.0 and 17.11
rion of 3% SLD per total number of words as one useful years (mean age = 15.3 years). The groups of adults (AdWS and
AdWNS) consisted of 40 males and 10 females, with ages between
means to help distinguish stuttering from nonstuttering 18.0 and 49.11 years (mean age = 27.6 years). All participants were
children. native speakers of Portuguese living in São Paulo, Brazil.
Several studies suggested that instances of stuttering
or SLD are more frequent in the speech samples of indi- Criteria for Participation
A participant was selected for the groups CWS, AWS or AdWS
viduals diagnosed as stutterers compared to individuals based on all of the following criteria: (a) presenting a fluency pro-
considered fluent speakers [1, 3–5, 9–12]. In general, the file outside the age reference values [15, 16], (b) receiving a score
mean frequency of SLD in the speech of stuttering chil- of 12 or more indicating stuttering (equivalent to at least ‘mild’
dren reported varies between 9.6 and 16.9%, while in nor- stuttering) on the Stuttering Severity Instrument 3 [17], (c) pre-
mally fluent controls the mean frequency varies between senting over 3% SLD per 100 syllables [1, 4].
A participant was selected for the groups CWNS, AWNS or
1.2 and 3.02% [4]. AdWNS based on the absence of the above-mentioned criteria for
Such age-related changes seem likely to make conver- selection of the groups CWS, AWS and AdWS. This means that
sation more effortful and to generate higher disfluency for a participant to be considered as a member of the groups
rates. Duchin and Mysak [13] reported no change in dis- CWNS, AWNS or AdWNS, he or she had to meet all of the fol-
fluencies across adults (21–30 years vs. 45–54 years vs. lowing criteria: (a) presenting fluency profile within the age refer-
ence values, (b) receiving the maximum of 10 points (equivalent
55–64 years vs. 75–91 years). Leeper and Culatta [14] also to ‘very mild’ stuttering) on the Stuttering Severity Instrument,
reported no change in the frequency of disfluencies be- (c) presenting 3% or less SLD per 100 syllables.
tween older adults (55–92 years) and young adults (25– A participant was excluded when he or she reported any neu-
35 years). These results regarding disfluencies suggest rological, psychiatric and/or communication disorders.
that they do not appear to be vulnerable to the aging pro-
Apparatus
cess. The participants were video recorded using a digital video
One must be cautious in generalizing findings de- camera, Sony DRC-SR62. All material was then transferred to the
scribing English-speaking individuals to other linguistic computer hard disk Dell Optiplex GX620.
and cultural groups. Some investigations of fluency in
Speech Sample
other populations have included studies of the German
Spontaneous speech samples – with a minimum duration of
and Spanish languages. By understanding expected char- 20 min – were analyzed. The samples for the six groups were ob-
acteristics in normally fluent individuals and speakers of tained using similar procedures. The younger speakers either
languages other than English, clinicians will be able to produced a spontaneous monologue on a topic of their choice, or

58 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 Staróbole Juste/Furquim de Andrade


a sample was elicited by prompts given by a therapist. Prior to the Table 1. Comparison between stuttering and fluent individuals:
monologue, the subjects were given suggestions as to topics, such SLD and OD occurrence
as family, school, sports, friends, hobbies, films, actual television
programs, etc. The recordings of the adults consisted of stretches Source SS DF MS F p value
of spontaneous monologue elicited by a researcher/speech thera-
pist. The topics the adults used were current employment, school- CWS ! CWNS
ing or hobbies. SLD 6,938 1 6,938 38.95 <0.001*
Again, as with the young children, the purpose of these con- OD 4.5 1 4.5 0.134 0.716
versations was to observe fluency and nonfluency produced in the AWS ! AWNS
longest phrases (approximately 20–30 utterances/child). For each SLD 3,996 1 3,996 64.11 <0.001*
child an observation of speech was made based on a sample of 100 OD 2.42 1 2.42 0.046 0.830
words out of the longest utterances, typically longer than 5 words AdWS ! AdWNS
per utterance. In fact, more than 100 words were produced. The SLD 8,090 1 8,090 43.5 <0.001*
mean duration of the time needed to obtain these speech samples OD 3.92 1 3.92 0.081 0.777
was about 20–25 min. Orthographic transcriptions were carried
out and the stuttering episodes were marked. SS = Sum of squares; DF = degrees of freedom; MS = mean
square.
Observation of Characteristics of SLD and OD
For each participant, the frequency of SLD and OD was calcu-
lated. The type of SLD was recorded as one-syllable word repeti-
tions, sound and syllable repetitions, sound prolongations, and
Table 2. Overall comparison among age groups: SLD and OD oc-
blocks. Multisyllable word repetitions, phrase repetitions, inter-
jections, revisions, and interrupted utterances were defined as currence
OD.
As mentioned above, direct observation of SLD and OD is SS DF MS F p value
based on a sample of 100 words (8150–250 syllables) obtained
from the longer utterances in the speech sample of a participant. CWS ! AWS ! AdWS
It was reasoned that this direct observation method would have SLD 876.9 2 438.5 1.571 0.215
the most immediate usability in a clinical diagnostic setting. This OD 10.99 2 5.493 0.082 0.921
procedure demands audiovisual observation skills of the observ- CWNS ! AWNS ! AdWNS
ing clinician. As will be presented below, the first author’s intra- SLD 1.627 2 0.813 0.166 0.847
judge and interjudge measurement reliability for assessing these OD 36.24 2 18.12 0.975 0.382
speech samples was determined (with all samples being audio-
videotaped at the time of testing).
Overall a corpus of approximately 15,000 words was available
for observation and analysis. All subsequently described mea-
sures of speech (dis)fluency were based on the first author’s judg- Table 3. Comparison between age groups: SLD and OD occur-
ments, an individual with many years of experience assessing and rence
treating stuttering.
Source SS DF MS F p value

SLD
Results Stuttering
CWS ! AWS 499.3 1 499.3 2.128 0.151
General Speech Disfluency Data CWS ! AdWS 32 1 32 0.098 0.766
AWS ! AdWS 784.1 1 784.1 3.203 0.080
A series of one-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) Fluent
were performed on the data with group (CWS vs. CWNS, CWNS ! AWNS 1.62 1 1.62 0.30 0.590
AWS vs. AWNS, AdWS vs. AdWNS) as between-subjects CWNS ! AdWNS 0.5 1 0.5 0.09 0.770
factor for the dependent measures SLD and OD as data AWNS ! AdWNS 0.32 1 0.32 0.09 0.760
presented a normal distribution (table 1). As expected, ta- OD
Stuttering
ble 1 shows that only the results obtained for SLD were CWS ! AWS 0.32 1 0.32 0.010 0.940
significant when comparing stuttering and fluent individ- CWS ! AdWS 9.68 1 9.68 0.018 0.670
uals (for children, adolescents and adults). Stuttering indi- AWS ! AdWS 6.48 1 6.48 0.08 0.780
viduals presented a higher frequency of this type of speech Fluent
disfluency. OD did not differentiate the groups, i.e. stut- CWNS ! AWNS 14.58 1 14.58 1.004 0.321
CWNS ! AdWNS 35.28 1 35.28 1.55 0.219
tering and fluent individuals presented similar results in
AWNS ! AdWNS 4.5 1 4.5 0.244 0.624
terms of the occurrence of this type of speech disfluency.

Speech Disfluency Types: Age Effects Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 59


Table 4. Number of SLD and OD for children, adolescent and adult groups

Type Children Adolescents Adults


CWS CWNS AWS AWNS AdWS AdWNS

SLD
Blocks 6.20 (6.39) 0 6.16 (5.35) 0 13.84 (16.42) 0
Prolongations 5.84 (7.40) 0.48 (0.82) 3.20 (3.23) 1.48 (1.58) 5.84 (7.67) 1.44 (1.19)
Sound repetitions 1.68 (1.89) 0.04 (0.20) 1.40 (2.96) 0.06 (0.23) 2.32 (3.36) 0.08 (0.26)
Syllable repetitions 5.36 (5.52) 0.20 (0.41) 4.24 (6.02) 0.04 (0.20) 2.76 (2.50) 0.20 (0.41)
One-syllable word repetition 7.60 (5.89) 2.40 (2.72) 5.36 (4.33) 0.96 (0.68) 3.52 (2.62) 1.20 (1.35)
Total SLD 26.68 (18.65) 3.12 (2.89) 20.36 (11.02) 2.48 (1.81) 28.28 (19.19) 2.84 (1.95)
OD
Multisyllable word repetitions 1.04 (1.24) 0.60 (0.65) 1.16 (1.21) 1.36 (1.55) 1.84 (2.01) 2.08 (1.68)
Phrase repetition 1.92 (1.73) 0.64 (0.91) 1.44 (1.23) 1.96 (1.24) 1.16 (1.80) 0.92 (0.91)
Interjections 5.96 (4.80) 8.56 (4.13) 10.00 (8.22) 8.32 (2.23) 10.36 (6.90) 9.68 (4.24)
Revisions 1.64 (1.55) 1.16 (1.37) 0.92 (1.08) 1.44 (1.47) 1.00 (0.82) 1.00 (1.08)
Interrupted utterances 0.24 (0.44) 0.44 (0.71) 0.20 (0.41) 0.20 (0.50) 0.08 (0.28) 0.20 (0.50)
Total OD 10.80 (6.68) 11.40 (4.76) 13.72 (9.71) 13.28 (3.20) 14.44 (8.36) 13.88 (5.17)

Data shown as mean with standard deviation in parentheses.

Two ANOVAs were performed on the data with group 19.576, p ! 0.001; AdWS: F(1, 48) = 8.070, p ! 0.001;
(CWS, AWNS, AdWS and CWNS, AWNS, AdWNS) as AdWNS: F(1, 48) = 17.714, p ! 0.001]. To further analyze
between-subjects variable and within-subjects variables these interactions, a series of ten one-way ANOVAs were
SLD and OD (table 2). Although no interaction was ob- performed for each group (tables 5–7).
served on such analyses, a series of one-way ANOVAs was Table 5 revealed that for CWS, there was no statisti-
performed to investigate possible subsumed interactions cally significant difference between blocks, prolongation
(table 3). Tables 2 and 3 show that, although differences and one-syllable word repetitions (blocks and prolonga-
among different age ranges exist, these differences were tion p = 0.855; blocks and one-syllable word repetitions
not considered statistically significant for fluent individ- p = 0.424; prolongation and one-syllable word repetitions
uals and stutterers. p = 0.357). These were the three most frequent types of
SLD for CWS (table 4). Indeed, for CWNS, the most fre-
Types of SLD Speech Disfluencies quent SLD was the one-syllable word repetition (table 4),
Table 4 displays the distribution of speech disfluencies which was significantly different from the other types of
according to their types for all groups. CWS, AWS and SLD (p ! 0.001 for prolongations, blocks, sound and syl-
AdWS showed all types of OD and SLD. The fluent groups lable repetitions, table 5).
(CWNS, AWNS and AdWNS) also presented all types of Table 6 shows that, as the results obtained for AWS, the
OD and SLD, except for blocks. analysis revealed no statistically significant difference on
the comparison between blocks, prolongation and one-
Types of SLD Speech Disruptions and Age Groups syllable word repetitions (blocks and prolongation p =
Six one-way ANOVAs with group factor (CWS, 0.731; blocks and one-syllable word repetitions p = 0.564;
CWNS, AWS, AWNS, AdWS, AdWNS) for dependent prolongation and one-syllable word repetitions p = 0.051).
measure SLD speech disfluencies (blocks, prolongations, These three SLD types were the most frequent ones for
sound and syllable repetitions and one-syllable word AWS (table 4). For AWNS, the most frequent SLD was the
repetitions) were performed. For all the groups, the one-syllable word repetition (table 4), which was statisti-
ANOVAs revealed a significant effect of SLD [CWS: cally significant (p ! 0.001 for prolongations, blocks,
F(1, 48) = 3.706, p = 0.007; CWNS: F(1, 48) = 15.381, p ! sound and syllable repetitions, table 6).
0.001; AWS: F(1, 48) = 4.239, p = 0.003; AWNS: F(1, 48) =

60 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 Staróbole Juste/Furquim de Andrade


Table 5. One-way ANOVAs for SLD types: children Table 6. One-way ANOVAs for SLD types: adolescents

Blocks Prolon- Sound Syllable Blocks Prolon- Sound Syllable


gation repetition repetition gation repetition repetition

CWS AWS
Prolongation F 0.034 Prolongation F 0.358
p 0.855 p 0.731
Sound repetition F 11.505 7.412 Sound repetition F 15.150 4.226
p 0.001* 0.009* p <0.001* 0.040*
Syllable repetition F 5.608 4.138 9.943 Syllable repetition F 4.132 7.372 4.479
p 0.022* 0.030* 0.003* p 0.040* 0.008* 0.040*
One-syllable F 0.649 0.866 22.920 13.621 One-syllable F 0.338 4.000 14.261 5.272
word repetition p 0.424 0.357 <0.001* <0.001* word repetition p 0.564 0.051 <0.001* 0.020*
CWNS AWNS
Prolongation F 8.512 Prolongation F 21.817
p 0.005* p <0.001*
Sound repetition F 1.000 6.753 Sound repetition F no 21.817
p 0.322 0.012* p variance <0.001*
Syllable repetition F 6.000 2.324 3.097 Syllable repetition F 1.000 20.329 1.000
p 0.018* 0.134 0.085 p 0.322 <0.001* 0.322
One-syllable F 19.416 11.387 18.673 15.956 One-syllable F 50.453 43.374 50.453 42.604
word repetition p <0.001* 0.001* <0.001* <0.001* word repetition p <0.001* 0.001* <0.001* <0.001*

Degrees of freedom for all analyses were 1, 48. Degrees of freedom for all analyses were 1, 48.

Blocks were the SLD most frequently observed for groups, the ANOVAs revealed a significant effect of OD
AdWS (table 4), and there was a statistically significant [CWS: F(1, 48) = 20.414, p ! 0.001; CWNS: F(1, 48) =
difference when compared with other disfluency types 74.768, p ! 0.001; AWS: F(1, 48) = 29.009, p ! 0.001;
(p = 0.032 for prolongations, p = 0.001 for sound repeti- AWNS: F(1, 48) = 114.999, p ! 0.001; AdWS: F(1, 48) =
tions, p = 0.002 for syllable repetitions and p = 0.003 for 40.112, p ! 0.001; AdWNS: F(1, 48) = 83.234, p ! 0.001].
one-syllable word repetitions, table 7). For AdWNS, the To further analyze these interactions, a series of ten one-
SLD most frequently observed were prolongation and way ANOVAs were performed for each group (ta-
one-syllable word repetitions (table 4), with no statistical bles 8–10).
difference between them (p = 0.509, table 7). For OD, however, no differences between stuttering
In summary, we observed that the SLD most frequent- and fluent groups were observed. For every group and all
ly observed for children and adolescents who stutter were age ranges, interjection was the most frequently observed
blocks, prolongations and one-syllable word repetitions. type of disfluency (table  4), which statistically differed
For adults, the SLD most frequently observed were blocks. from the other OD (p ! 0.001). Other types of disfluency
For fluent speakers, results indicate that, for children, the were rare (tables 8–10).
most frequent SLD were one-syllable word repetitions
and, for adolescents and adults, the most frequent SLD
were prolongations and one-syllable word repetitions. Discussion

Types of OD Speech Disruptions and Age Groups Although SLD is one of the most important parame-
Six one-way ANOVAs with group factor (CWS, ters for the diagnosis of stuttering [1, 3, 9, 18], it is ob-
CWNS, AWS, AWNS, AdWS, AdWNS) for dependent served that it is also present in the speech of fluent indi-
measure OD speech disfluencies (multisyllable word rep- viduals. Two factors seem to differentiate stutterers and
etitions, phrase repetitions, interjections, revisions and fluent speakers regarding speech disruptions: (a) quan-
interrupted utterances) were performed. For all the titative factors – the frequency of these disruptions in

Speech Disfluency Types: Age Effects Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 61


Table 7. One-way ANOVAs for SLD types: adults Table 8. One-way ANOVAs for OD types: children

Blocks Prolon- Sound Syllable Word Phrase Inter- Revision


gation repetition repetition repetition repetition jection

AdWS CWS
Prolongation F 4.874 Phrase repetition F 4.271
p 0.032* p 0.044*
Sound repetition F 11.861 4.418 Interjection F 24.620 15.674
p 0.001* 0.041* p <0.001* <0.001*
Syllable repetition F 11.130 3.644 0.275 Revision F 2.280 0.363 18.335
p 0.002* 0.062 0.602 p 0.138 0.550 <0.001*
One-syllable F 9.636 2.050 1.983 1.101 Interrupted F 9.249 22.165 35.211 18.870
word repetition p 0.003* 0.159 0.166 0.299 utterances p 0.004* <0.001* 0.001* <0.001*
AdWNS CWNS
Prolongation F 36.422 Phrase repetition F 0.032
p <0.001* p 0.858
Sound repetition F no 36.422 Interjection F 90.482 87.541
p variance <0.001* p <0.001* <0.001*
Syllable repetition F 6.000 24.176 6.000 Revision F 3.399 2.491 72.129
p 0.018* <0.001* 0.018* p 0.071 0.121 <0.001*
One-syllable F 19.636 0.422 19.636 12.500 Interrupted F 0.693 0.752 93.675 5.408
word repetition p <0.001* 0.509 <0.001* 0.001* utterances p 0.409 0.390 <0.001* 0.024*

Degrees of freedom for all analyses were 1, 48. Degrees of freedom for all analyses were 1, 48.

Table 9. One-way ANOVAs for OD types: adolescents Table 10. One-way ANOVAs for OD types: adults

Word Phrase Inter- Revision Word Phrase Inter- Revision


repetition repetition jection repetition repetition jection

AWS AdWS
Phrase repetition F 0.658 Phrase repetition F 1.588
p 0.421 p 0.214
Interjection F 28.325 26.546 Interjection F 35.149 41.656
p <0.001* <0.001* p <0.001* <0.001*
Revision F 0.547 2.535 30.020 Revision F 3.735 0.165 45.403
p 0.463 0.118 <0.001* p 0.059 0.687 <0.001*
Interrupted F 14.049 22.972 35.483 9.769 Interrupted F 18.735 8.836 55.445 28.466
utterances p <0.001* <0.001* <0.001* 0.003* utterances p <0.001* 0.005* <0.001* <0.001*
AWNS AdWNS
Phrase repetition F 2.280 Phrase repetition F 9.208
p 0.137 p 0.004*
Interjection F 164.023 155.177 Interjection F 69.412 102.027
p <0.001* <0.001* p <0.001* <0.001*
Revision F 0.035 1.820 165.505 Revision F 7.302 0.080 98.392
p 0.852 0.184 <0.001* p 0.009* 0.778 <0.001*
Interrupted F 12.662 43.263 315.375 15.862 Interrupted F 28.719 12.037 123.268 11.294
utterances p <0.001* <0.001* <0.001* <0.001* utterances p <0.001* 0.001* <0.001* 0.001*

Degrees of freedom for all analyses were 1, 48. Degrees of freedom for all analyses were 1, 48.

62 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 Staróbole Juste/Furquim de Andrade


speech (higher number for stutterers); (b) qualitative fac- However, qualitative differences between the types of
tors – the type of SLD observed (different for stutterers speech disfluencies were observed. For CWS and AWS,
and fluent individuals). the most frequently observed SLD types were blocks and
As might be expected from previous studies of En- one-syllable word repetitions. For adults, blocks were the
glish-, German- and Spanish-speaking individuals [1, most frequently observed SLD, which statistically dif-
3–5, 10–12, 19–22], SLD frequency differed between the fered from other speech disfluencies of this type.
stuttering and fluent groups. The occurrence of speech A possible explanation may be the fact that, for chil-
disruptions (SLD) suggests a greater deficit in the motor dren, speech disfluencies predominantly occurred in
processing of speech [1, 10, 18, 23]; therefore stuttering function words [24–27]. Among function words, the ar-
individuals, independently of their age, present a higher ticle was the grammatical class with the highest number
occurrence of SLD when compared with their fluent of speech disfluencies [25–27]. In Brazilian Portuguese,
counterparts [3, 12, 20–22]. The results of the present the articles, both definite (o, a, os, as) and indefinite (um,
study indicate that the vast majority of stutterers present uns, uma, umas), are monosyllabic words [28]. Moreover,
within-word prolongations or blocks. However, these dis- one can observe a high occurrence of monosyllabic words
fluencies were not observed in the speech samples of flu- in this language [28] when analyzing other function
ent individuals, regardless of their age. words (conjunctions, prepositions, pronouns and inter-
Regarding prolongations, a more specific analysis of jections). This fact may account for the large number of
characteristics is necessary. This analysis should differ- one-syllable word repetitions.
entiate the two types of prolongations observed in the Au-Yeung et al. [25] observed that disruptions in func-
speech samples of the participants: within-word prolon- tion words mainly occurred when function words pre-
gations and prolongations at the end of words. As previ- ceded content words in the speech of children – fluent
ously mentioned, within-word prolongations were exclu- speakers as well as stutterers. This fact validates the hy-
sively observed in speech samples of stutterers of all age pothesis of the present study that disruptions in function
groups. In a study carried out with German-speaking words are used as a postponing tactic when the subse-
children [22], the authors suggested that approximately quent content word is not ready for execution.
77% of the children who stutter exhibited some form of In subsequent studies [26, 29], the shift in the occur-
prolongation or blocks, but 0% of the normally fluent rence of disruptions from function to content words
children produced prolongations or blocks. According to along the age range, in fluent individuals and in stutter-
that study [22], while no one knows the precise origin of ers, was investigated. The results indicated that all chil-
prolongations/blocks (nor for that matter, any other stut- dren, fluent and stutterers, presented more disruptions
tered disfluency), Alm [23] suggested that stuttering may in function words. With increasing age, a difference be-
be related to difficulties with basal ganglia functioning, tween groups was observed: fluent individuals continued
difficulties thought to negatively influence timing cues to use the tactic of delaying the production of content
needed to realize speech-language production and that words, showing speech disfluencies in function words;
these temporal disruptions could contribute to the pro- stutterers, however, seemed to abandon this delaying tac-
duction of prolongations. Similarly, Conture [1] described tic and, from adolescence, began to produce more disrup-
prolongations from a temporal perspective, labeling them tions in content words, especially blocks.
as ‘cessations’ of speech production. The results of the present study indicate that fluent
In the present study, prolongations observed in the and stuttering individuals do not behave differently in
speech of fluent individuals occurred exclusively on the terms of the occurrence of this type of speech disruption.
last phoneme of the last syllable of the word. These pro- These findings agree with other studies carried out in dif-
longations seem to have the same purpose as hesitations, ferent languages [3, 12, 20, 21], i.e. when considering OD,
i.e. they are a strategy used to facilitate coarticulation be- differences between stuttering and fluent individuals are
tween the words [12]. When the speaker is articulating a not statistically significant. OD are common to all speak-
lexical unit, the motor programming of the subsequent ers and fundamentally reflect linguistic uncertainty and
unit is already available. For fluency to be achieved, the imprecision, or else, they intend to improve the compre-
mechanism used by the speaker can be the prolongation hension of the message [3, 12, 18].
of the end of the previous word [24, 25].
Results also indicated that SLDs were not quantitative-
ly different among the different age groups of stutterers.

Speech Disfluency Types: Age Effects Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 63


Conclusion pects that are observed in the speech of children, but are
no longer observed in the speech of adults.
The present study quantified and compared the speech These results show that the analysis of speech disflu-
disfluency types presented by stuttering and fluent chil- encies, both quantitative and qualitative, is useful for dif-
dren, adolescents and adults. The results indicated that ferentiating fluent individuals and stutterers. This analy-
SLD (speech disfluencies suggesting deficits in the motor sis is an important instrument for assessment and diag-
processing of speech) differentiated the groups of fluent nosis of stuttering in all age groups.
individuals and stutterers (more frequent in stutterers) in Finally, it is important to emphasize that, although the
all age ranges tested. OD (speech disfluencies related to results of the present study are similar to studies in other
linguistic processing of the message) did not differentiate languages (English, Spanish and German), generaliza-
the groups of fluent individuals and stutterers in any of tions and comparisons with other populations of Portu-
the age ranges tested. guese speakers are necessary. Thus, results of this present
No significant quantitative differences were observed study should be limited to the Brazilian Portuguese
among the groups of stutterers (children, adolescents and speakers described in this investigation.
adults). The differences observed between these groups
were qualitative ones, i.e., the type of disfluencies shown
in the speech – which seems to undergo a modification Acknowledgment
with age. This modification of speech disfluency types This research was supported by a grant from FAPESP
seems to be related to certain language development as- (07/03702-5).

References
1 Conture EG: Stuttering, Its Nature, Diagno- 12 Juste F, Andrade CRF: Tipologia das ruptu- 20 Carlo EJ, Watson JB: Disfluencies of 3- and
sis, and Treatment. Boston, Allyn & Bacon, ras de fala e classes gramaticais em crianças 5-years old Spanish-speaking children. J Flu-
2001. gagas e fluentes. Pró-Fono 2006;18:129–140. ency Disord 2007;28:37–53.
2 Van Riper C: The Nature of Stuttering. En- 13 Duchin SW, Mysak ED: Disfluency and 21 Natke U, Sandrieser P, Pietrowsky R, Kal-
glewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall, 1971. characteristics of young adults, middle-aged veram KT: Disfluency data of German pre-
3 Ambrose NG, Yairi E: Normative disfluency and older males. J Commun Disord 1987;20: school children who stutter and comparison
data for early childhood stuttering. J Speech 245–257. children. J Fluency Disord 2006;31:165–176.
Lang Hear Res 1999;42:895–909. 14 Leeper LH, Culatta R: Speech fluency: effects 22 Boey RA, Wuyts FL, Van de Heyning PH, De
4 Yairi E, Ambrose NG: Early Childhood Stut- of age, gender and context. Folia Phoniatr Bodt MS, Heylen L: Characteristics of stut-
tering: for Clinicians by Clinicians. Austin, Logop 1995;47:1–14. tering-like-disfluencies in Dutch-speaking
Pro-Ed, 2005. 15 Andrade CRF: Fluência; in Andrade CRF, children. J Fluency Disord 2007;32:310–329.
5 Yaruss JS, LaSalle LR, Conture EG: Evaluat- Béfi-Lopes DM, Wertzner HF, Fernandes 23 Alm PA: Stuttering and the basal ganglia cir-
ing stuttering in young children: diagnostic FDM: ABFW – Teste de Linguagem Infantil: cuits: a critical review of possible relations. J
data. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 1998;7:62–76. nas áreas de fonologia, vocabulário, fluência Commun Disord 2004;37:325–369.
6 Cordes AK, Ingham RJ: Stuttering includes e pragmática, ed 2. Barueri, Pró-Fono, 2004, 24 Wingate ME: The first three words. J Speech
both within-word and between-word disflu- pp 71–94. Hear Res 1979;22:604–612.
encies. J Speech Hear Res 1995;38:382–386. 16 Andrade CRF: Perfil da Fluência da Fala: 25 Au-Yeung J, Howell P, Pilgrim L: Phonologi-
7 Starkweather CW: Fluency and Stuttering. Parâmetro comparativo diferenciado por cal words and stuttering on function words.
Englewood Cliffs, Prentice-Hall, 1987. idade para crianças, adolescentes, adultos e J Speech Lang Hear Res 1998;41:1019–1030.
8 Yairi E: Disfluencies of normally speaking idosos (CD-ROM). Barueri, Pró-Fono, 2006. 26 Howell P, Au-Yeung J, Sackin S: Exchange of
two-year-old children. J Speech Hear Res 17 Riley G: Stuttering Severity Instrument for stuttering from function words to content
1981;24:490–495. Young Children (SSI-3), ed 3. Austin, Pro- words with age. J Speech Lang Hear Res
9 Ambrose NG, Yairi E: The role of repetition Ed, 1994. 1999;42:345–354.
units in the differential diagnosis of early 18 Andrade CRF: Abordagem neurolingüística 27 Dworzynski K, Howell P, Natke U: Predict-
childhood incipient stuttering. Am J Speech e motora da gagueira; in Ferreira LP, Béfi- ing stuttering from linguistics factors for
Lang Pathol 1995; 4:82–88. Lopes DM, Limongi SCO (eds): Tratado de German speakers in two age groups. J Flu-
10 Pellowski MW, Conture EG: Characteristics Fonoaudiologia. São Paulo, Rocca, 2004, pp ency Disord 2003;28:95–113.
of speech disfluency and stuttering behav- 1001–1034. 28 Rosa MC: Classes de palavras, tipos de sig-
iors in 3- and 4-year-old children. J Speech 19 Yairi E: Disfluency characteristics of child- nificado e questões relacionadas; in Rosa
Lang Hear Res 2002;45:20–34. hood stuttering; in Curlee RF, Siegel GM MC: Introdução à Morfologia. São Paulo,
11 Yairi E, Lewis B: Disfluencies at the onset of (eds): Nature and Treatment of Stuttering. Contexto, 2003, pp 91–114.
stuttering. J Speech Hear Res 1984; 27: 154– Needham Heights, Allyn & Bacon, 1997, pp 29 Au-Yeung J, Gomez IV, Howell P: Exchange
159. 49–78. of disfluency with age from function words to
content words in Spanish speakers who stut-
ter. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2003;46:754–766.

64 Folia Phoniatr Logop 2011;63:57–64 Staróbole Juste/Furquim de Andrade


Reproduced with permission of the copyright owner. Further reproduction prohibited without permission.

Potrebbero piacerti anche