Sei sulla pagina 1di 14

Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Published online: October 3, 2012

Diseases DOI: 10.1159/000341998

Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s


Disease: The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis
Angie A. Kehagia a Roger A. Barker b Trevor W. Robbins c, d
a
School of Psychology, University of St. Andrews, St. Andrews, b Cambridge Centre for Brain Repair,
Department of Clinical Neurosciences, c Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, and d Department of
Experimental Psychology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Key Words mediated fronto-striatal executive impairments and a de-


Acetylcholine ⴢ Catechol- O -methyl-transferase ⴢ Dementia ⴢ mentia syndrome with distinctive prodromal visuospatial
Dopamine ⴢ Dual syndrome hypothesis ⴢ Executive deficits in which cholinergic treatments offer some clinical
function ⴢ Fronto-striatal dysfunction ⴢ Noradrenaline ⴢ benefits. Copyright © 2012 S. Karger AG, Basel
Overdose ⴢ Parkinson’s disease

Abstract Introduction
Research into the heterogeneous nature of cognitive impair-
ment documented in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) Parsing the heterogeneous nature of cognitive impair-
has focused on disentangling deficits that vary between in- ment in Parkinson’s disease (PD) into ontologically
dividuals, evolve and respond differentially to pharmacolog- meaningful deficits entails establishing clear cognitive,
ical treatments, and relate differentially to PD dementia anatomical and neurochemical definitions for these def-
(PDD). We summarise studies conducted in our laboratory icits as independent and/or hierarchically related entities.
over the last 2 decades, outlining the incremental develop- In essence, our task has been that of interpreting neuro-
ment of our hypotheses, the starting point for which is our psychological variation between and within individual
early work on executive deficits mirroring fronto-striatal dys- patients and groups, at a single or multiple time points,
function. We present subsequent findings linking these def- employing pharmacological manipulations, neuroimag-
icits to a model of dopaminergic function that conforms to ing and analyses of genotypic variation to delineate the
an inverted curvilinear function. We review studies that in- nature of each of these deficits.
vestigated the range of dopamine-independent attentional The diversity of progressive neurodegeneration in PD
and visuospatial memory deficits seen in PD, demonstrating is secondary to the intracellular fibrillisation of ␣-synu-
that abnormalities in these domains more accurately predict clein, which is its major pathological hallmark, and ad-
PDD. We conclude with an exposition of the dual syndrome ditional neuropathological features may include vascular
hypothesis, which distinguishes between dopaminergically disease, Lewy bodies, neurofibrillary tangles and amy-

© 2012 S. Karger AG, Basel Trevor W. Robbins, CBE FRS FMedSci


1660–2854/13/0112–0079$38.00/0 Department of Experimental Psychology
Fax +41 61 306 12 34 University of Cambridge, Downing Street
E-Mail karger@karger.ch Accessible online at: Cambridge CB2 3EB (UK)
www.karger.com www.karger.com/ndd E-Mail twr2 @ cam.ac.uk
Fig. 1. Schematic diagram of the cortico-striatal circuitry and OFC/ACC = orbitofrontal cortex/anterior cingulate; vl-PFC =
possible causes of cognitive deficits in PD (superimposed blocks ventrolateral PFC; PPC = posterior parietal cortex; dlPFC = dor-
or arrows). Note possible effects on mesocortical dopamine (from solateral PFC; SMA/PMC = supplementary motor area/premotor
the ventral tegmental area); the serotoninergic (5HT) raphé nu- cortex; MD = mediodorsal; VA = ventral-anterior; VL = ventro-
clei; the noradrenergic (NA) projection to the cortex from the lo- lateral; GPi = globus pallidus, internal segment; SNr = substantia
cus coeruleus (LC) and the cholinergic input from the basal fore- nigra, pars reticulata; GPe = globus pallidus, external segment;
brain or substantia innominata (SI). Lewy bodies or associated stn = subthalamic nucleus; Nacc = nucleus accumbens; vm-
neurodegeneration could impair cortical functioning. Following CAUD = ventromedial caudate nucleus; tail-CAUD = tail of the
either depletion (block) or overactive (arrow) dopamine (D), sys- caudate; V Put = ventral putamen; dl-CAUD = dorsolateral cau-
tems could modulate cognitive function. ACh = Acetylcholine; date; PUT = putamen; SN = substantia nigra.

loid plaques more commonly seen in dementia. This im- coeruleus [3–5], dorsal raphé nuclei [6, 7] and cholinergic
pacts on the major neurotransmitter systems at uneven brainstem nuclei, particularly the basal nucleus of
rates and asymmetrically between individuals, rendering Meynert [8] (fig. 1).
analyses of cognitive impairment patterns particularly The evolution and heterogeneity of cognitive impair-
challenging: early nigrostriatal degeneration causes pro- ment in PD mirrors the complexity of the disease pro-
gressive loss of dopamine neurotransmission in a dorsal cess, in terms of the variable as well as the interacting
to ventral gradient within the basal ganglia [1], unevenly compromise of the catecholaminergic and cholinergic
affecting different fronto-striatal ‘loops’ [2] at different systems. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in the form
stages of the disease in all patients. Parallel deficits in the of executive and working memory deficits similar to
mesocorticolimbic dopamine systems originating in the those seen in frontal lobe-damaged patients is present in
midbrain also develop. Beyond causing pronounced do- up to 50% of cases; however, PD dementia (PDD), a dis-
paminergic disturbance, PD also detrimentally affects tinctive pattern of rapid cognitive decline leading to
the noradrenergic, serotoninergic and cholinergic sys- aphasia, apraxia and agnosia (thus resembling deficits
tems, by causing degeneration respectively of the locus seen in patients with temporal lobe damage and cortical

80 Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Kehagia /Barker /Robbins


     
CANTAB TESTS OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTION

Tower of London test of planning ID/ED Attentional Set Shifting

COPY
THIS

Fig. 2. Screen shots of three of the CAN- Spatial Working Memory


TAB tests of fronto-executive function; the
‘SoC’ version of the ToL test of planning;
the intra-dimensional (ID)/ED set shifting
task and the self-ordered spatial working
memory task – all configured to work
via touch-sensitive screen methodology.
These tasks were used in a study by Owen
et al. [21] to define fronto-striatal cognitive
deficits in PD. Reproduced with permis-
sion from Cambridge Cognition.

dementia), is only seen in perhaps as many as 10% of shown to be sensitive to frontal lobe lesions [23]. The tests
cases [9]. Indeed, PDD represents a superordinate aspect included the Stockings of Cambridge (SoC) test of plan-
of clinical heterogeneity due to its detrimental implica- ning (derived from the ToL test), intra-/extra-dimension-
tions for patient mortality and quality of life [10]. In this al (ED) set shifting, which is a test of shifting aptitude
review, we chart our progress over the last 2 decades us- between stimulus exemplars and higher-order perceptual
ing a quasi-chronological approach culminating in the dimensions, and the self-ordered Spatial Working Mem-
dual syndrome hypothesis, which captures the evolu- ory task of the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Au-
tion in our understanding of cognitive dysfunction in tomated Battery (CANTAB; fig. 2) [24]. These studies re-
PD. vealed planning and spatial working memory deficits as
a function of disease severity in the PD groups, which
paralleled those seen in patients with frontal lobe exci-
Dopaminergic Amelioration of Executive Deficits sions.
Mirrors Fronto-Striatal Function Critically, we [22, 25] demonstrated specific beneficial
effects of dopaminergic medication on planning and spa-
Early research into the cognitive impairment patterns tial working memory, but not visual recognition memory
caused by PD revealed deficits across the cognition spec- (fig. 3), visuospatial paired associates learning (PAL; a de-
trum, in terms of visuospatial memory and learning [11– layed cued recall test) or ED set shifting, more clearly de-
14] and, prototypically, executive function including fining earlier findings of general cognitive amelioration
planning, problem solving and attentional shifting, on as a result of dopaminergic pharmacotherapy [26, 27] as
tests such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and Tower specific improvements in executive function mirroring
of London (ToL) [15–20]. The PD dysexecutive syndrome fronto-striatal enhancement. These findings heralded a
exhibits similarities to the effects of frontal lobe damage, range of studies employing short-term withdrawal from
and its conceptualisation as a fronto-striatal deficit is well levodopa (L-DOPA), the dopamine precursor included in
exemplified by our early studies [21, 22], comparing the most therapeutic regimes. These studies extended dem-
performance of unmedicated and medicated PD patients onstrations of dopaminergic enhancement to other tasks
at different disease stages on a test battery previously sensitive to fronto-striatal dysfunction, including task

The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis in PD Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 81


Some beneficial effects of L-DOPA on cognition
in PD; a summary

ToL speed ToL accuracy


1.0
40

Proportions of ‘perfect’ solutions


‘On’ L-DOPA
0.8
‘Off’ L-DOPA
Initial thinking time (s) 30
0.6

20
0.4

10 0.2

0 0
2 3 4 5 2 3 4 5
a Difficulty (moves) b Difficulty (moves)

40 100
SWM Recognition memory
Between-search errors

30 90

Percentage correct
Lack of effect
20 80

10 70

0 60
2 3 4 6 8 Sim. 0 4 8 16
c Difficulty (boxes) d Delay (s)

Fig. 3. Effects of L-DOPA medication withdrawal in PD. Significant impairments are observed in ToL perfor-
mance, as measured by latency (a) and accuracy (b; see also fig. 2) and self-ordered spatial working memory
(SWM) performance (c; see also fig. 2), but not in visual recognition memory (d), which is impaired relative to
age and IQ-matched controls. Sim. = Simultaneous matching. Reproduced from Lange et al. [25], with permis-
sion from the publishers.

switching in the face of stimulus interference [28, 29] and Dopaminergic ‘Overdosing’ Leads to Impulsivity
verbal working memory at the manipulation rather than
the retrieval phase of the task [30]. On this verbal work- Withdrawal studies were instrumental in revealing
ing memory paradigm, we found that impaired but not the role of dopamine neurotransmission in the fronto-
cognitively intact PD patients demonstrated specific re- striatal circuitry in forms of cognitive impairment in PD
ductions in dorsal fronto-striatal blood oxygenation lev- by demonstrating not only benefits, but also pernicious
el-dependent signal change using functional magnetic effects, of dopaminergic medication. Such effects were re-
resonance imaging (fMRI) [31], further emphasizing the ported initially by Gotham et al. [32] in terms of deterio-
case that these deficits have a locus within a dopaminer- ration in conditional learning, alongside improvements
gic fronto-striatal network. in alternating fluency. In one of the earliest findings in
support of a putative dopaminergic ‘overdosing’ theory,
Swainson et al. [33] found that medicated PD patients

82 Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Kehagia /Barker /Robbins


     
learning [28, 33, 36] (fig. 5). Beyond striatal effects, the
Probabilistic reversal dopamine overdose hypothesis may also account for
100 PD On diminution in reward sensitivity sometimes seen in med-
PD Off icated PD patients by additionally implicating the meso-
Controls
80 limbic dopamine pathway, from which the ventral stria-
People passing (%)

tum and amygdala receive midbrain dopamine projec-


60 tions. Putatively, dopaminergic medication may cause an
overall increase in tonic dopamine that obscures phasic
40
dopamine dips evoked by error-correcting feedback [37].
20 An fMRI study of probabilistic reversal learning in PD
patients tested ‘on’ and ‘off’ L-DOPA implicated the nu-
0 cleus accumbens, but not the dorsal striatum or prefron-
acquisition reversal
tal cortex (PFC), as the site of this medication effect, pro-
Stage
viding support for this hypothesis [38]. A similar mech-
anism of action involving the ventral striatum may
account for the emergence of impulse control deficits
Fig. 4. Detrimental effects on probabilistic reversal learning fol-
lowing L-DOPA medication in PD, according to the number of and compulsive gambling associated with dopamine ago-
patients successfully passing a learning criterion. Note the ab- nist treatment [39], in particular pramipexole [40].
sence of drug effects in the learning stage. Reproduced from
Swainson et al. [33], with permission from the publishers.
Further Complications: Prefrontal Dopamine and
Cognitive Deficits in PD

showed a non-perseverative reversal deficit during prob- Given the tripartite division of the main midbrain do-
abilistic reversal learning similar to that seen in a group pamine pathways into the nigrostriatal, mesolimbic and
of frontal and temporal lobe lesion patients. In contrast, mesocortical systems, it would appear reasonable to as-
the L-DOPA withdrawn PD group surprisingly revealed sume that the mesocortical projection is additionally im-
unimpaired performance in the form of intact reversal plicated in aspects of cognitive impairment in PD, espe-
learning (fig. 4). In a replication of this result, Cools et al. cially in fronto-striatal deficits of working memory and
[28] demonstrated reversal deficits alongside improve- planning, consistent with preclinical evidence on the role
ments in task switching in patients tested on their normal of dopamine in normal cognitive function [41]. For in-
dopaminergic regime, and the opposite pattern following stance, improvements in spatial working memory and
withdrawal. These findings were extended to the Cam- planning conferred by L-DOPA are associated with de-
bridge Gambling Task, which is sensitive to orbitofrontal creased regional cerebral blood flow in the right dorsolat-
damage and where L-DOPA led to increased impulsive, eral PFC [42], suggesting that L-DOPA may enhance PFC
delay-averse behaviour in PD patients which was absent efficiency in PD. However, the locus of this enhancement
when the same patients were withdrawn from medication could well be in the caudate nucleus rather than the PFC
[29]. Both probabilistic reversal learning and the Cam- itself, with a consequent improvement of functioning
bridge Gambling Task rely on ventral fronto-striatal cir- arising due to dopaminergic enhancement within the
cuitry: the former is associated with blood oxygenation fronto-striatal ‘loops’ linking the cortex and striatum,
level-dependent signal change in the ventral striatum and rather than any isolated cortical or subcortical site.
inferior frontal cortex [34], and the latter with increases Moreover, instead of an obvious loss of cortical dopa-
in regional cerebral blood flow in the orbitofrontal cortex mine, 18F-fluorodopa positron emission tomography
[35]. Thus, considering the uneven gradient of dopamine (PET) studies have shown what appears to be a compen-
loss across the corticostriatal ‘loops’ in early PD, we hy- satory up-regulation in prefrontal cortical dopamine me-
pothesised that dopaminergic medication restores neu- tabolism in early (unilateral, Hoehn and Yahr stage I)
rotransmission in severely affected dorsal circuits, impli- compared to later (bilateral) stage PD [43, 44], possibly
cated in executive deficits of planning and working mem- reflecting the reciprocal relationship it often bears to the
ory, but putatively ‘overdoses’ relatively less affected activity of subcortical (mainly striatal) dopamine sys-
ventral circuits implicated in reward processing and tems [45, 46]. This up-regulation clearly complicates

The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis in PD Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 83


Fig. 5. a Schematic depiction of four of
the ’fronto-striatal loops’, ranging from a
‘motor’ loop involving the putamen and Motor Spatial Visual Affective
supplementary motor area through to an SMA
PMC
DL-PF PPC
VL-PF ST OFC HC
‘affective’ loop comprising the nucleus ac- SSC IT Cing BLA
cumbens and certain areas of the orbito-
frontal cortex, with two prominent ‘cogni-
tive’ loops in between (reproduced from
Head of Tail caudate Ventral
Lawrence et al. [36]). Also shown is the hy- Putamen
caudate V putamen striatum
pothesised gradient of dopamine depletion
for PD which is greatest in the dorsal stria-
tum and weakest in the ventral striatum.
SMA = Supplementary motor area; SSC = GPi SNpr GPi SNpr GPi SNpr
VP
somatosensory cortex; PMC = premotor cv cl dm r dm rm
cortex; PPC = posterior parietal cortex; DL-
PF = dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; VL- Gradient of dopamine loss
PF = ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; ST =
superior temporal cortex; OFC = orbito- VLo VAmc VApc MDpc MDmc VAmc MDmc
frontal cortex; Cing = cingulate cortex; HC
= hippocampus; BLA = basolateral amyg-
dala; V putamen = ventral putamen; GPi =
a
globus pallidus, internal segment; VP =
ventral pallidum; SNpr = substantia nigra
Effects of medication on dopamine levels in loops Gambling/
pars reticulata; cv = caudoventral; cl = cau- reversal learning
dolateral; dm = dorsomedial; r = rostral; rm
= rostromedial; VLo = ventrolateral thala- Too Task set switching/
mus; VA = ventral anterior thalamus; MD = high working memory
mediodorsal thalamus; mc = magnocellu-
Dopamine level

lar; pc = parvocellular. b Schematic render- Optimal


ing of typical forms of behaviour associated
with each loop after withdrawal of dopami-
nergic medication. Note on the extreme left,
tasks such as task set switching and spatial Too
working memory are mildly improved by low
dopamine medication. By contrast, on
gambling and reversal learning, tasks asso-
ciated with ventral frontal mediation, L- Motor Dorsolateral/ Lateral/ Inferotemporal
DOPA withdrawal significantly impairs b prefrontal orbitofrontal
performance. Reproduced with permission
from Swainson et al. [33] and the publishers.

analyses of the role of PFC dopamine in parkinsonian viduals, Val homozygosity compared with heterozygos-
cognition and could conceivably even mask impairments ity confers greater COMT efficiency, or more effective
stemming from striatal dopamine loss. Presumably, how- catabolic methylation of dopamine, thereby reducing
ever, a reduction in PFC dopamine levels eventually con- PFC dopamine levels. By contrast, Met homozygocity
tributes to the emergent cognitive deficit pattern as the renders COMT less efficient, leading to increased PFC
disease progresses from unilateral to bilateral and more dopamine levels (and putatively, enhanced PFC activity).
severe impairment. Although dopamine transporters play a predominant
These hypotheses are difficult to test using conven- role in the regulation of striatal dopamine, their relative
tional methods, but an opportunity to do so arose from absence in the PFC renders COMT activity in this region
the discovery of the catechol-O-methyl-transferase the major means of regulating dopamine neurotrans-
(COMT) polymorphism, for which a single methionine mission. A much simplified summary of the consider-
(Met) to valine (Val) substitution at residue 158 confers able literature stimulated by the findings of Weinberger
up to fourfold increase in enzymatic efficiency as a func- and colleagues [47, 48] in healthy individuals and pa-
tion of the number of Val alleles carried. In healthy indi- tients with schizophrenia holds that, in general, COMT

84 Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Kehagia /Barker /Robbins


     
1.5 p = 0.017

1.0

Change in ToL per year

Prefrontal function
0.5
Val/Val
SZ/ Early PD
0 control
–0.5 Disease
progression
–1.0
Met/Met
–1.5

–2.0
Val/Val Val/Met Met/Met b Dopamine level
a COMT genotype

Fig. 6. a Longitudinal analysis: 5-year fol-


10.5 10.5
low-up of patients with PD (n = 70) on per- r = 0.10 r = –0.21
formance on the ToL test of planning as a 10.0 10.0
function of COMT genotype. b Hypothe- 9.5 9.5
sised inverted U-shaped curve explaining 9.0 9.0
data in terms of the Yerkes-Dodson curve.
ToL

ToL
The performance of patients with PD is 8.5 8.5
contrasted with that of patients with 8.0 8.0
schizophrenia (SZ). Cross-sectional anal- 7.5 7.5
ysis of ToL planning performance in pa-
tients relatively early (!1.6 years; d) and 7.0 7.0
Val/Val Val/Met Met/Met Val/Val Val/Met Met/Met
later (61.6 years; c) in the course of PD
(n = 425), stratified by COMT genotype. c COMT genotype d COMT genotype
Fisher’s test: p = 0.001
Reproduced from Williams-Gray et al.
[52], with permission from the publishers.

polymorphism confers superior performance on tasks formance that included the dorsolateral PFC, frontopolar
sensitive to prefrontal function, such as working memo- and parietal cortex [51].
ry, as a function of the number of Met alleles carried. Met These findings were expanded upon in a separate co-
homozygotes outperform Val homozygotes [47, 48] in a hort study of 425 patients [52] which confirmed the orig-
pattern that conforms to an inverted U-shaped function, inal finding of SoC impairment in those patients with
analogous to the Yerkes-Dodson principle [49]. These more Met alleles relatively early in the course of the dis-
findings bear obvious implications concerning those as- ease (!1.6 years), compared with those later in the course,
pects of parkinsonian cognition mirroring prefrontal where the relationship was actually reversed (fig.  6c).
dopamine neurotransmission and raised the possibility These findings are readily interpretable in terms of a do-
of adopting a neurogenetic approach to further investi- paminergic optimum titrated according to the hypothet-
gating these deficits. ical inverse U-shaped function which is exceeded in ear-
In collaboration with Weinberger’s group, and in the ly PD: Met homozygosity may actually have detrimental
context of the Cambridgeshire Parkinson’s Incidence effects on SoC planning at a time when PFC dopamine is
from GP to Neurologist (CamPaIGN) cohort study which up-regulated, placing Val homozygotes, unusually, at an
we elaborate on later, 288 PD patients stratified by COMT advantage. If valid, this hypothesis predicts improve-
polymorphism were assessed, focusing in particular on ments for Met homozygote patients as their disease pro-
the CANTAB SoC test [50]. Surprisingly, Met homozy- gresses and the initially up-regulated PFC dopaminergic
gosity was associated with impaired planning accuracy in system eventually becomes compromised. Indeed, at the
PD, an effect especially magnified in male patients and 5-year follow-up of 70 of these PD patients, the initially
those on dopaminergic medication. A subsequent fMRI disadvantaged Met homozygotes actually exhibited dif-
study confirmed that Met-homozygote patients showed ferential improvement in planning performance over this
hypoactivation in regions underpinning planning per- time span compared with Val homozygotes (in whom no

The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis in PD Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 85


change was documented; fig. 6a) [52]. This observation tial shift) [58]. Further evidence from studies employing
implies that their initial deficit may well have been caused noradrenergic de-afferentation in the rat [59, 60] impli-
in part by excess PFC dopamine which was ameliorated cates noradrenaline in the ED shifting component of the
as the disease progressed, possibly as a function of pro- parkinsonian executive deficit [for review, see ref. 61]
gressive loss in mesocortical dopamine. which is consistent with the early and profound degen-
Collectively, this series of studies represents a most un- eration of the locus coeruleus in PD [3]. Indeed, improve-
usual case of symptom reversal with increasing disease ments in global cognition and wakefulness have been
progression, and, crucially, has offered perhaps a unique shown in an 8-week, placebo-controlled study of the nor-
form of endorsement of the inverted-U hypothesis adrenaline re-uptake inhibitor atomoxetine in PD pa-
(fig. 6b). This clearly supersedes earlier simplistic hypoth- tients [62], while a smaller uncontrolled trial, using how-
eses that dopamine loss in PD is responsible for produc- ever flexible dosing of atomoxetine, demonstrated spe-
ing the cognitive deficits associated with the disease, and cific improvements in executive function and attention in
that dopaminergic medication may restore many aspects PD [63]. Investigation of this noradrenergic hypothesis of
of cognitive functioning. Not only may ‘overdosing’ of higher-order cognition in PD is also currently underway
the mesolimbic circuitry occur, as proposed originally by in our laboratory.
Swainson et al. [33] and Cools et al. [28], but a relative ex- In addition to executive function, visual memory is
cess of PFC dopamine may additionally contribute to as- often [11–13, 55] but not always impaired in PD [64]. For
pects of cognitive impairment in PD. example, early work by Sahakian et al. [11] highlighted
Overall, these findings have highlighted new facets of visual pattern and spatial recognition memory deficits in
the PD dysexecutive syndrome and accommodate the a severely affected, dopaminergically medicated patient
variability in its manifestation as one which mirrors fron- group but not in more mildly affected unmedicated pa-
to-striatal modulation according to a Yerkes-Dodson- tients, while both groups were impaired on a task of vi-
like curvilinear function, subject not only to disease stage suospatial PAL. The latter deficits in PAL are significant
and dopaminergic medication, but also genetically deter- in that this particular task has been found to predict a
mined properties of dopamine neurotransmission. diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in a group of patients
with questionable dementia (MCI) recruited on the basis
of subjective memory complaints in a memory clinic [65].
Beyond Dopamine: Set Shifting and Memory In two follow-up studies, we addressed the role of dopa-
Deficits in PD minergic medication in recognition memory. Employing
L-DOPA withdrawal within subjects, Lange et al. [25]
Over the course of this research program focusing on demonstrated that withdrawal caused a deterioration in
the dopamine-dependent aspects of cognitive impair- spatial working memory and ToL performance, but had
ment in PD utilizing dopaminergic withdrawal, compar- no effect on tests of visual pattern (fig. 3) and spatial rec-
isons between medicated and pharmacologically naïve ognition memory, visuospatial associative learning as
patients, as well as neuroimaging and genetics, we have well as simultaneous and visual delayed-matching-to-
alluded to two emergent sets of findings which point to sample tasks. Consistent with this and using the same
orthogonal patterns of impairment in PD reflecting dif- tasks, Owen et al. [55] found no differences as a function
ferent pharmacological and neural substrates. of medication status between medication-naïve and
The first of these concerns ED set shifting, where pa- mildly medicated patients, reporting deficits only in the
tients are required to shift attention not between stimuli, more severely affected medicated group. These aspects of
a capacity which is known to be sensitive to striatal dopa- the PD mnemonic deficit stand in contrast to dopamine-
mine, but between higher-order perceptual dimensions dependent working memory, which relies on online, self-
or modalities (e.g. from lines to shapes). PD patients ex- ordered manipulation of stimulus representations. In-
hibit reliable deficits in ED shifting [53], but dopaminer- stead, these impairment patterns quite possibly reflect
gic withdrawal has no effect on this aspect of higher-or- cholinergic deficits in the frontal and temporal cortex, in
der attentional control [28, 30, 54–56], or on task switch- agreement with known pathology in the basal forebrain
ing between categorical stimulus judgments [57]. These cholinergic nuclei and ascending pathways to the frontal
results are in line with animal work showing that striatal and temporal cortices present even at early disease stages
dopamine depletion in the marmoset produces deficits in [66]. This interpretation gains support from observations
reversal learning but not ED shifting (at least for the ini- of memory deficits, particularly at short retention inter-

86 Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Kehagia /Barker /Robbins


     
p value
Age >72 years

p value
Age >72 –0.42
years
–0.27
Fig. 7. Predictive power of clinico-patho- ≥25
logical variables for dementia in PD; pen-
–0.38
tagon copying and semantic verbal fluen-
cy from the MMSE were the best predic- ToL <10
Beck Depression score >7 –0.38
tors. Reproduced from Williams-Gray et
al. [52], with permission from the publish-
ers. UPDRS = Unified Parkinson’s Disease
Rating Scale; NART = National Adult
Reading Test.

vals, indicating deficits during registration, following the first to assess the incidence of PD and parkinsonism,
long-term administration of anti-cholinergic medication and the extent and natural history of cognitive deficits in
in PD [67], as well as detrimental effects on memory the ensuing patient cohort [50]. We identified five sub-
caused by the muscarinic antagonist hyoscine (scopol- groups of patients at disease presentation, who were im-
amine) in a group of PD patients, but not in a matched paired on (i) ToL, indicating fronto-striatal deficits, (ii)
control group [68]. Treatment with another muscarinic visual pattern recognition memory, indicating temporal
antagonist, trihexiphenidyl, has also been associated with lobe dysfunction, (iii) both ToL and pattern recognition
PD impairments on a visual associative learning task of memory, as well as (iv) a cognitively preserved subgroup
transfer generalisation [69]. and (v) a group of patients who demonstrated marked
cognitive impairment and had an MMSE score !24, in-
dicating dementia.
Contributions from Longitudinal Cohort Studies to In further studies on this patient cohort, we employed
Understanding Clinical Heterogeneity a data-driven approach using cluster analysis to separate
patients into groups characterised by internal cohesion
Most studies investigating the nature of cognitive and external isolation [71], as well as longitudinal assess-
dysfunction in PD employ prevalent cross-sectional co- ment [9] to assess the evolution of cognitive impairment.
horts of patients, usually screened for the presence of Both approaches identified a cluster of patients with mild
dementia using the Mini-Mental State Examination cognitive deficits and rapid disease progression, and an-
(MMSE) [70]. Such studies are thus inherently biased in other cluster of cognitively impaired patients with a non-
terms of patient selection, which limits their potential to tremor dominant, akinetic motor phenotype at presenta-
investigate more severe cognitive impairment in the tion with dopamine-unresponsive gait disturbance, po-
form of dementia. tentially indicating the impact of cholinergic deficits due
Large cohort studies have been instrumental in efforts to degeneration of the pedunculopontine nucleus [72].
to systematically address this aspect of cognitive hetero- The CamPaIGN study produced a striking finding of sig-
geneity. Using a community-based epidemiological ap- nificant clinical value given its prognostic implications: it
proach in a population of approximately 700,000 over a identified a patient phenotype indicative of early transi-
25-month incidence period, the CamPaIGN study was tion to PDD, which was older (172 years) at presentation

The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis in PD Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 87


Color version available online
Fig. 8. Dual syndrome hypothesis (Keha-
Acetylcholine
gia et al. [61, 74]). The dopamine-sensitive
Dopamine
fronto-striatal profile of deficit in PD
(MCI) is contrasted with that seen in PD 4 Noradrenaline
dementia, which may respond to a greater 3 5
degree to cholinergic therapy. Pathways on 6 2
1
the brain cut-out are those compromised 7 8
by the disease and likely implicated in cog- PD
nitive impairment. Cholinergic pathways: dementia
(1) from the pedunculopontine nucleus to
the thalamus, (2) from the basal nucleus
of Meynert to neocortex; dopaminergic
PD MCI
pathways: (3) nigrostriatal: from the sub-
stantia nigra (pars compacta) to the stri-
atum, (4) mesolimbic: from the ventral PDD cognitive deficits
tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens, Early cognitive deficits
(5) mesocortical: from the ventral tegmen- Semantic fluency
tal area to the frontal cortex, (6) tubero- Auditory verbal learning
Rule shifting (WCST) Visuospatial skills
infundibular: from the hypothalamus to Planning (ToL)
the pituitary, and noradrenergic pathways: Verbal and visual memory
Attentional set shifting (EDS) Hallucinations
(7) from the lateral tegmental nucleus to Working memory
the amygdala and hippocampus, (8) from Feedback-based learning
the locus coeruleus to the hypothalamus, Delayed response inhibition
thalamus, amygdala, cortex and cerebel-
lum. WCST = Wisconsin Card Sorting
Test; EDS = ED shifting.

and performed poorly on two simple bedside tests of se- icit syndrome. We also showed that the H1/H1 MAPT
mantic fluency and copying overlapping pentagons from haplotype, but not COMT, independently predicted de-
the MMSE [9] (fig. 7). mentia onset.
Within the framework of the CamPaIGN study, and
in parallel to our neurogenetic investigations into COMT
polymorphism, we addressed genetic susceptibility fac- The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis
tors associated with cortical Lewy body pathology that
characterises PDD, namely ␣-synuclein and tau. Using Our approach to understanding the heterogeneity of
this candidate gene approach, Goris et al. [73] found a cognitive impairment in PD has employed a range of
robust association between the MAPT (microtubule-as- techniques in a large number of studies spanning 2 de-
sociated protein tau) gene and the rate of dementia in the cades. We have integrated these diverse findings along
incident cohort at the 3.5-year follow-up. In the Cam- with clinicopathological and diagnostic criteria in a com-
PaIGN cohort at the 5-year follow-up and an additional prehensive review [74], which permits a broad dichotomy
cross-sectional prevalent cohort, Williams-Gray et al. to be drawn between hypothetically independent but
[52] replicated the predictive utility of neuropsychologi- partially overlapping syndromes of MCI and dementia in
cal assessment of pentagon copying and semantic fluen- PD.
cy, reflecting temporal lobe function, but not phonemic The dual syndrome hypothesis differentiates between
fluency, sensitive to frontal lobe damage, in dementia. By two broad syndromes (fig. 8): (i) a profile of neuropsycho-
contrast, some of the ‘classic’ deficits within the fronto- logical deficit in non-demented PD patients with MCI
striatal cluster, for example, on SoC (i.e. ToL) planning, and a tremor-dominant phenotype on tests of planning,
were not predictive of progression to dementia, suggest- working memory and executive function reflecting fron-
ing at least two independent aspects of the cognitive def- to-striatal dysfunction, amenable to dopaminergic ame-

88 Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Kehagia /Barker /Robbins


     
lioration but susceptible to overdosing effects, and modu- Notwithstanding the extensive evidence supporting
lated by the effects of COMT polymorphism and disease the role of dopamine in the emergence of fronto-striatal
severity, and (ii) an akinetic subgroup with pronounced deficits, these may also be affected by cholinergic or by
gait disturbance demonstrating early deficits in visuo- other non-dopamine neurotransmitter loss due to the
spatial function and semantic fluency indicative of pos- pervasive and interacting patterns of neurodegenera-
terior cortical and temporal lobe dysfunction, who exhib- tion. Some degree of frontal cholinergic compromise
its rapid cognitive decline to dementia and in whom cho- must also contribute to executive dysfunction, and a par-
linergic treatment may offer some clinical benefit [75]. allel noradrenergic imbalance may contribute to a differ-
In addition to profound executive, attentional, mne- ent subset of attentional impairment. Overall, we submit
monic and visuospatial disturbance, PDD encompasses that the use of the terms ‘fronto-striatal’, ‘parietal’ and
fluctuating attention and a range of additional psychiat- ‘temporal’ lobe deficits is essentially a short-hand de-
ric symptoms, including hallucinations and depression. scription for deficits that may arise also from the discon-
Its pathological characteristics, in terms of increased nection of neural networks involving all of these struc-
Lewy body load in temporal regions and the presence of tures. Nevertheless, these convergent lines of evidence
plaques and vascular changes, would appear distinct represent strong grounds for the implication of different
from those of idiopathic PD. However, the nosology of neurocognitive systems in the profile of parkinsonian
PDD is controversial and it remains unclear whether it cognitive impairment, with some clear therapeutic im-
lies further along the disease severity continuum in rela- plications.
tion to PD or whether it represents a distinct clinical en-
tity (reviewed by Kehagia et al. [74]). Nonetheless, the
core features of PDD are thought to reflect widespread Conclusion
cholinergic dysfunction: acetylcholinesterase activity as
measured by PET was found to be lower in PDD com- In the course of deciphering cognitively and neuro-
pared to non-demented PD and Alzheimer’s disease pa- chemically specific entities within the broad and hetero-
tients throughout the entire cortex, and in particular in geneous constellation of the cognitive deficits caused by
the frontal, parietal and superior temporal cortex as well PD, we have operated within and across these levels of
as the hippocampus [76]. Moreover, consistent with the analysis, employing neuropsychological, pharmacologi-
detrimental effects of anti-cholinergic medication on vi- cal, imaging, genetic and epidemiological approaches to
suospatial memory in early PD reviewed above, cholines- test constantly evolving hypotheses. We have presented
terase inhibitors such as rivastigmine [75] are the most these, as they evolved, beginning with early conceptu-
efficacious in conferring moderate clinical benefits in alisations of the fronto-striatal syndrome and leading
PDD [see 74 for discussion of possible mechanisms]. onto dopamine-specific hypotheses pertaining to over-
Whilst we have proposed that these ‘syndromes’, dopa- dosing healthy circuits and the inverted curvilinear
minergic fronto-striatal impairments in PD on the one function that may characterise dopaminergic optima as
hand and the distinctive cholinergic visuospatial, memory a function of COMT and disease progression. Finally,
and psychiatric deficits of PDD on the other, may be dis- the dual syndrome hypothesis encompasses the broader
tinguishable, we nevertheless acknowledge some degree of range of dopamine-independent, cholinergic deficits
overlap. PD dementia cannot exist in isolation of signifi- and represents a starting point for future studies, espe-
cant disruption in dopamine neurotransmission, given cially given the gradual emergence of genetic risk factors.
that nigrostriatal degeneration underlies the diagnosis of For example, could visuospatial and memory deficits
PD in the first instance. While mesolimbic and mesocor- seen early in the course of PD represent prodromal de-
tical dopamine dysfunction also contributes in different mentia signs? Can the dual syndrome hypothesis lead to
ways to the executive impairment profile, it should be not- refined functional biomarkers? We are currently investi-
ed that the mesocortical dopamine system innervates ar- gating these questions in our ongoing ICICLE (Incidence
eas of the parietal and temporal lobes also [77, 78], and, as of Cognitive Impairment in Cohorts with Longitudinal
such, could conceivably contribute to posterior cortical Evaluation) study, which is longitudinally following two
deficits. However, against this possibility is the lack of independent incident cohorts from a population denom-
therapeutic response to dopaminergic medications for inator of 1 million. Combining anatomic-clinical, bio-
these typical parietal and temporal lobe impairments. chemical and genetic approaches with neuropsychology
and neuroimaging, we seek to identify putative biomark-

The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis in PD Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 89


ers and establish a panel of tests predictive of dementia Acknowledgements
that will ultimately lead to a powerful diagnostic algo-
The Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute is co-
rithm at the service of clinicians and research into dis- funded by the Medical Research Council and the Wellcome Trust
ease-modifying agents. Thus, our hypotheses aim to ex- (G00001354). We gratefully acknowledge support from the Med-
plain and predict in order to contribute to disease man- ical Research Council and Wellcome Trust, Parkinson’s UK,
agement tailored to the needs of the individual patient. Cure-PD and a National Institute for Health Research Biomedical
It is hoped that such knowledge will lead to improved Research Centre award to Addenbrooke’s Hospital and the Uni-
versity of Cambridge. A.A.K. was supported by the Isaac Newton
therapies and, with this, better quality of life for patients Trust.
with PD.

Disclosure Statement

T.W.R. consults for Cambridge Cognition.

References
1 Kish SJ, Shannak K, Hornykiewicz O: Un- 11 Sahakian BJ, Morris RG, Evenden JL, Heald 20 Brown RG, Marsden CD: An investigation of
even pattern of dopamine loss in the stria- A, Levy R, Philpot M, et al: A comparative the phenomenon of ‘set’ in Parkinson’s dis-
tum of patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s study of visuospatial memory and learning ease. Mov Disord 1988;3:152–161.
disease. Pathophysiologic and clinical impli- in Alzheimer-type dementia and Parkin- 21 Owen AM, James M, Leigh PN, Summers
cations. N Engl J Med 1988;318:876–880. son’s disease. Brain 1988;111(Pt 3):695–718. BA, Marsden CD, Quinn NP, et al: Fronto-
2 Alexander GE, DeLong MR, Strick PL: Par- 12 Loranger AW, Goodell H, McDowell FH, Lee striatal cognitive deficits at different stages
allel organization of functionally segregated JE, Sweet RD: Intellectual impairment in of Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1992; 115(Pt
circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Parkinson’s syndrome. Brain 1972; 95: 405– 6):1727–1751.
Annu Rev Neurosci 1986;9:357–381. 412. 22 Owen AM, Sahakian BJ, Hodges JR, Sum-
3 Cash R, Dennis T, L’Heureux R, Raisman R, 13 Mortimer JA, Pirozzolo FJ, Hansch EC, mers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW: Dopa-
Javoy-Agid F, Scatton B: Parkinson’s disease Webster DD: Relationship of motor symp- mine-dependent frontostriatal planning
and dementia: norepinephrine and dopamine toms to intellectual deficits in Parkinson dis- deficits in early Parkinson’s disease. Neuro-
in locus ceruleus. Neurology 1987;37:42–46. ease. Neurology 1982;32:133–137. psychology 1995;9:126–140.
4 Chan-Palay V, Asan E: Alterations in cate- 14 Mayeux R, Stern Y, Rosen J, Leventhal J: De- 23 Owen AM, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Polkey
cholamine neurons of the locus coeruleus in pression, intellectual impairment, and Par- CE, Robbins TW: Planning and spatial
senile dementia of the Alzheimer type and in kinson disease. Neurology 1981;31:645–650. working memory following frontal lobe le-
Parkinson’s disease with and without de- 15 Taylor AE, Saint-Cyr JA, Lang AE: Frontal sions in man. Neuropsychologia 1990; 28:
mentia and depression. J Comp Neurol 1989; lobe dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease: the 1021–1034.
287:373–392. cortical focus of neostriatal outflow. Brain 24 Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Sahakian
5 Zarow C, Lyness SA, Mortimer JA, Chui HC: 1986;109:845–883. BJ, McInnes L, Rabbitt P: Cambridge Neu-
Neuronal loss is greater in the locus coeru- 16 Morris RG, Downes JJ, Sahakian BJ, Evenden ropsychological Test Automated Battery
leus than nucleus basalis and substantia ni- JL, Heald A, Robbins TW: Planning and spa- (CANTAB): a factor analytic study of a large
gra in Alzheimer and Parkinson diseases. tial working memory in Parkinson’s disease. sample of normal elderly volunteers. Demen-
Arch Neurol 2003;60:337–341. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1988;51:757– tia 1994;5:266–281.
6 Brooks DJ, Piccini P: Imaging in Parkinson’s 766. 25 Lange KW, Robbins TW, Marsden CD,
disease: the role of monoamines in behavior. 17 Robbins TW, James M, Owen AM, Lange James M, Owen AM, Paul GM: L-dopa with-
Biol Psychiatry 2006; 59:908–918. KW, Lees AJ, Leigh PN, et al: Cognitive defi- drawal in Parkinson’s disease selectively im-
7 Scatton B, Javoy-Agid F, Rouquier L, Dubois cits in progressive supranuclear palsy, Par- pairs cognitive performance in tests sensi-
B, Agid Y: Reduction of cortical dopamine, kinson’s disease, and multiple system atro- tive to frontal lobe dysfunction. Psychophar-
noradrenaline, serotonin and their metabo- phy in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunc- macology (Berl) 1992;107:394–404.
lites in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res 1983; tion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994; 26 Loranger AW, Goodell H, Lee JE, McDowell
275:321–328. 57:79–88. F: Levodopa treatment of Parkinson’s syn-
8 Jellinger KA: Pathology of Parkinson’s dis- 18 Canavan AG, Passingham RE, Marsden CD, drome. Improved intellectual functioning.
ease. Changes other than the nigrostriatal Quinn N, Wyke M, Polkey CE: Prism adapta- Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972;26:163–168.
pathway. Mol Chem Neuropathol 1991; 14: tion and other tasks involving spatial abili- 27 Bowen FP, Kamienny RS, Burns MM, Yahr
153–197. ties in patients with Parkinson’s disease, pa- M: Parkinsonism: effects of levodopa treat-
9 Williams-Gray CH, Foltynie T, Brayne CE, tients with frontal lobe lesions and patients ment on concept formation. Neurology 1975;
Robbins TW, Barker RA: Evolution of cogni- with unilateral temporal lobectomies. Neu- 25:701–704.
tive dysfunction in an incident Parkinson’s ropsychologia 1990; 28:969–984. 28 Cools R, Barker RA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins
disease cohort. Brain 2007; 130(Pt 7):1787– 19 Cools AR: Basal ganglia and Parkinson’s dis- TW: Enhanced or impaired cognitive func-
1798. ease: neurobiological and pharmacological tion in Parkinson’s disease as a function of
10 Levy G, Tang MX, Louis ED, Cote LJ, Alfaro aspects in animals and man. Clin Neurol dopaminergic medication and task de-
B, Mejia H, et al: The association of incident Neurosurg 1984;86:178–195. mands. Cereb Cortex 2001;11:1136–1143.
dementia with mortality in PD. Neurology
2002;59:1708–1713.

90 Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Kehagia /Barker /Robbins


     
29 Cools R, Barker RA, Sahakian BJ, Robbins 42 Cools R, Stefanova E, Barker RA, Robbins 54 Slabosz A, Lewis SJ, Smigasiewicz K, Szymu-
TW: L-Dopa medication remediates cogni- TW, Owen AM: Dopaminergic modulation ra B, Barker RA, Owen AM: The role of
tive inflexibility, but increases impulsivity in of high-level cognition in Parkinson’s dis- learned irrelevance in attentional set-shift-
patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsy- ease: the role of the prefrontal cortex re- ing impairments in Parkinson’s disease.
chologia 2003;41:1431–1441. vealed by PET. Brain 2002; 125(Pt 3):584– Neuropsychology 2006;20:578–588.
30 Lewis SJ, Slabosz A, Robbins TW, Barker RA, 594. 55 Owen AM, Beksinska M, James M, Leigh PN,
Owen AM: Dopaminergic basis for deficits 43 Kaasinen V, Nurmi E, Bruck A, Eskola O, Summers BA, Marsden CD, et al: Visuospa-
in working memory but not attentional set- Bergman J, Solin O, et al: Increased frontal tial memory deficits at different stages of
shifting in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsy- [(18)F]fluorodopa uptake in early Parkin- Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 1993;
chologia 2005;43:823–832. son’s disease: sex differences in the prefron- 31:627–644.
31 Lewis SJ, Dove A, Robbins TW, Barker RA, tal cortex. Brain 2001;124(Pt 6):1125–1130. 56 Owen AM, Roberts AC, Hodges JR, Sum-
Owen AM: Cognitive impairments in early 44 Rakshi JS, Uema T, Ito K, Bailey DL, Morrish mers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW: Contrast-
Parkinson’s disease are accompanied by re- PK, Ashburner J, et al: Frontal, midbrain and ing mechanisms of impaired attentional set-
ductions in activity in frontostriatal neural striatal dopaminergic function in early and shifting in patients with frontal lobe damage
circuitry. J Neurosci 2003;23:6351–6356. advanced Parkinson’s disease. A 3D [(18)F] or Parkinson’s disease. Brain 1993; 116(Pt
32 Gotham AM, Brown RG, Marsden CD: dopa-PET study. Brain 1999;122(Pt 9):1637– 5):1159–1175.
‘Frontal’ cognitive function in patients with 1650. 57 Kehagia AA, Cools R, Barker RA, Robbins
Parkinson’s disease ‘on’ and ‘off’ levodopa. 45 Pycock CJ, Kerwin RW, Carter CJ: Effect of TW: Switching between abstract rules re-
Brain 1988;111(Pt 2):299–321. lesion of cortical dopamine terminals on flects disease severity but not dopaminergic
33 Swainson R, Rogers RD, Sahakian BJ, Sum- subcortical dopamine receptors in rats. Na- status in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsycho-
mers BA, Polkey CE, Robbins TW: Probabi- ture 1980;286:74–76. logia 2009;47:1117–1127.
listic learning and reversal deficits in pa- 46 Roberts AC, De Salvia MA, Wilkinson LS, 58 Collins P, Wilkinson LS, Everitt BJ, Robbins
tients with Parkinson’s disease or frontal or Collins P, Muir JL, Everitt BJ, et al: 6-Hy- TW, Roberts AC: The effect of dopamine de-
temporal lobe lesions: possible adverse ef- droxydopamine lesions of the prefrontal cor- pletion from the caudate nucleus of the com-
fects of dopaminergic medication. Neuro- tex in monkeys enhance performance on an mon marmoset (Callithrix jacchus) on tests
psychologia 2000;38:596–612. analog of the Wisconsin Card Sort Test: pos- of prefrontal cognitive function. Behav Neu-
34 Cools R, Clark L, Owen AM, Robbins TW: sible interactions with subcortical dopa- rosci 2000;114:3–17.
Defining the neural mechanisms of probabi- mine. J Neurosci 1994;14(5 Pt 1):2531–2544. 59 Tait DS, Brown VJ, Farovik A, Theobald DE,
listic reversal learning using event-related 47 Egan MF, Goldberg TE, Kolachana BS, Cal- Dalley JW, Robbins TW: Lesions of the dor-
functional magnetic resonance imaging. J licott JH, Mazzanti CM, Straub RE, et al: Ef- sal noradrenergic bundle impair attentional
Neurosci 2002;22:4563–4567. fect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on set-shifting in the rat. Eur J Neurosci 2007;
35 Rogers RD, Owen AM, Middleton HC, Wil- frontal lobe function and risk for schizo- 25:3719–3724.
liams EJ, Pickard JD, Sahakian BJ, et al: phrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2001; 98: 60 McGaughy J, Ross RS, Eichenbaum H: Nor-
Choosing between small, likely rewards and 6917–6922. adrenergic, but not cholinergic, deafferenta-
large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and 48 Mattay VS, Goldberg TE, Fera F, Hariri AR, tion of prefrontal cortex impairs attentional
orbital prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci 1999;19: Tessitore A, Egan MF, et al: Catechol O- set-shifting. Neuroscience 2008;153:63–71.
9029–9038. methyltransferase val158-met genotype and 61 Kehagia AA, Murray GK, Robbins TW:
36 Lawrence AD, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW: individual variation in the brain response to Learning and cognitive flexibility: fronto-
Cognitive functions and corticostriatal cir- amphetamine. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2003; striatal function and monoaminergic mod-
cuits: insights from Huntington’s disease. 100:6186–6191. ulation. Curr Opin Neurobiol 2010;20:1–6.
Trends Cogn Sci 1998;2:379–388. 49 Yerkes RM, Dodson JD: The relation of 62 Weintraub D, Mavandadi S, Mamikonyan E,
37 Schultz W: Getting formal with dopamine strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-for- Siderowf AD, Duda JE, Hurtig HI, et al: Ato-
and reward. Neuron 2002;36:241–263. mation. J Comp Neurol Psychol 1908; 18: moxetine for depression and other neuro-
38 Cools R, Lewis SJ, Clark L, Barker RA, Rob- 459–482. psychiatric symptoms in Parkinson disease.
bins TW: L-DOPA disrupts activity in the 50 Foltynie T, Brayne CE, Robbins TW, Barker Neurology 2010;75:448–455.
nucleus accumbens during reversal learning RA: The cognitive ability of an incident co- 63 Marsh L, Biglan K, Gerstenhaber M, Wil-
in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychophar- hort of Parkinson’s patients in the UK. The liams JR: Atomoxetine for the treatment of
macology 2007;32:180–189. CamPaIGN study. Brain 2004;127(Pt 3):550– executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s dis-
39 Bostwick JM, Hecksel KA, Stevens SR, Bow- 560. ease: a pilot open-label study. Mov Disord
er JH, Ahlskog JE: Frequency of new-onset 51 Williams-Gray CH, Hampshire A, Robbins 2009;24:277–282.
pathologic compulsive gambling or hyper- TW, Owen AM, Barker RA: Catechol O- 64 Flowers KA, Pearce I, Pearce JM: Recogni-
sexuality after drug treatment of idiopathic methyltransferase Val158Met genotype in- tion memory in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurol
Parkinson disease. Mayo Clin Proc 2009;84: fluences frontoparietal activity during plan- Neurosurg Psychiatry 1984;47:1174–1181.
310–316. ning in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J 65 Blackwell AD, Sahakian BJ, Vesey R, Semple
40 van Eimeren T, Ballanger B, Pellecchia G, Neurosci 2007;27:4832–4838. JM, Robbins TW, Hodges JR: Detecting de-
Miyasaki JM, Lang AE, Strafella AP: Dopa- 52 Williams-Gray CH, Evans JR, Goris A, mentia: novel neuropsychological markers
mine agonists diminish value sensitivity of Foltynie T, Ban M, Robbins TW, et al: The of preclinical Alzheimer’s disease. Dement
the orbitofrontal cortex: a trigger for patho- distinct cognitive syndromes of Parkinson’s Geriatr Cogn Disord 2004;17:42–48.
logical gambling in Parkinson’s disease? disease: 5 year follow-up of the CamPaIGN 66 Dubois B, Ruberg M, Javoy-Agid F, Ploska A,
Neuropsychopharmacology 2009; 34: 2758– cohort. Brain 2009;132(Pt 11):2958–2969. Agid Y: A subcortico-cortical cholinergic
2766. 53 Downes JJ, Roberts AC, Sahakian BJ, Even- system is affected in Parkinson’s disease.
41 Robbins TW, Arnsten AF: The neuropsycho- den JL, Morris RG, Robbins TW: Impaired Brain Res 1983;288:213–218.
pharmacology of fronto-executive function: extra-dimensional shift performance in
monoaminergic modulation. Annu Rev medicated and unmedicated Parkinson’s
Neurosci 2009;32:267–287. disease: evidence for a specific attentional
dysfunction. Neuropsychologia 1989; 27:
1329–1343.

The Dual Syndrome Hypothesis in PD Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 91


67 Cooper JA, Sagar HJ, Doherty SM, Jordan N, 71 Lewis SJ, Foltynie T, Blackwell AD, Robbins 75 Emre M, Aarsland D, Albanese A, Byrne EJ,
Tidswell P, Sullivan EV: Different effects of TW, Owen AM, Barker RA: Heterogeneity of Deuschl G, De Deyn PP, et al: Rivastigmine
dopaminergic and anticholinergic therapies Parkinson’s disease in the early clinical stag- for dementia associated with Parkinson’s
on cognitive and motor function in Parkin- es using a data driven approach. J Neurol disease. N Engl J Med 2004;351:2509–2518.
son’s disease. A follow-up study of untreated Neurosurg Psychiatry 2005;76:343–348. 76 Bohnen NI, Kaufer DI, Ivanco LS, Lopresti B,
patients. Brain 1992;115(Pt 6):1701–1725. 72 Lewis SJ, Barker RA: Understanding the do- Koeppe RA, Davis JG, et al: Cortical cholin-
68 Dubois B, Danze F, Pillon B, Cusimano G, paminergic deficits in Parkinson’s disease: ergic function is more severely affected in
Lhermitte F, Agid Y: Cholinergic-dependent insights into disease heterogeneity. J Clin parkinsonian dementia than in Alzheimer
cognitive deficits in Parkinson’s disease. Neurosci 2009;16:620–625. disease: an in vivo positron emission tomo-
Ann Neurol 1987;22:26–30. 73 Goris A, Williams-Gray CH, Clark GR, graphic study. Arch Neurol 2003; 60: 1745–
69 Herzallah MM, Mustafa AA, Misk AJ, Al- Foltynie T, Lewis SJ, Brown J, et al: Tau and 1748.
Dweib LH, Abdelrazeq SA, Myers CE, et al: alpha-synuclein in susceptibility to, and de- 77 Bentivoglio M, Morelli M: The organization
Depression impairs learning whereas anti- mentia in, Parkinson’s disease. Ann Neurol and circuitry of mesencephalic dopamine
cholinergics impair transfer generalization 2007;62:145–153. neurons and the distribution of dopamine
in Parkinson patients tested on dopaminer- 74 Kehagia AA, Barker RA, Robbins TW: Neu- receptors in the brain; in Dunnett DB,
gic medications. Cogn Behav Neurol 2010; ropsychological and clinical heterogeneity of Bjorklund A, Hokfelt T (eds): The Handbook
23:98–105. cognitive impairment and dementia in pa- of Chemical Neuroanatomy. Amsterdam,
70 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR: ‘Mini tients with Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neu- Elsevier, 2005, pp 1–107.
Mental State’: a practical method for grading rol 2010; 9:1200–1213. 78 De Keyser J, Ebinger G, Vauquelin G: Evi-
the cognitive state of patients for the clini- dence for a widespread dopaminergic inner-
cian. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189–198. vation of the human cerebral neocortex.
Neurosci Lett 1989;104:281–285.

92 Neurodegener Dis 2013;11:79–92 Kehagia /Barker /Robbins


     

Potrebbero piacerti anche