Sei sulla pagina 1di 598

The Oxford Handbook of Human Motivation

OX F O R D L I B R A RY O F P S Y C H O L O G Y

editor- in-chief

Peter E. Nathan

are a editors

Clinical Psychology
David H. Barlow

Cognitive Neuroscience
Kevin N. Ochsner and Stephen M. Kosslyn

Cognitive Psychology
Daniel Reisberg

Counseling Psychology
Elizabeth M. Altmaier and Jo-Ida C. Hansen

Developmental Psychology
Philip David Zelazo

Health Psychology
Howard S. Friedman

History of Psychology
David B. Baker

Industrial/Organizational Psychology
Steve W. J. Kozlowski

Methods and Measurement


Todd D. Little

Neuropsychology
Kenneth M. Adams

Personality and Social Psychology


Kay Deaux and Mark Snyder
OXFORD L I B R A RY OF PSYCHOLOGY

Editor in Chief peter e. nathan

The Oxford
Handbook of
Human Motivation
Edited by
Richard M. Ryan

1
1
Oxford University Press, Inc., publishes works that further
Oxford University’s objective of excellence in
research, scholarship, and education.

Oxford New York


Auckland Cape Town Dar es Salaam Hong Kong Karachi
Kuala Lumpur Madrid Melbourne Mexico City Nairobi
New Delhi Shanghai Taipei Toronto

With offices in
Argentina Austria Brazil Chile Czech Republic France Greece
Guatemala Hungary Italy Japan Poland Portugal Singapore
South Korea Switzerland Thailand Turkey Ukraine Vietnam

Copyright © 2012 by Oxford University Press, Inc.

Published by Oxford University Press, Inc.


198 Madison Avenue, New York, New York 10016
www.oup.com

Oxford is a registered trademark of Oxford University Press

All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced,


stored in a retrieval system, or transmitted, in any form or by any means,
electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording, or otherwise,
without the prior permission of Oxford University Press

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data


The Oxford handbook of human motivation / edited by Richard M. Ryan.
p. cm. — (Oxford library of psychology)
ISBN 978–0–19–539982–0
1. Motivation (Psychology) I. Ryan, Richard M.
BF503.O94 2012
153.8—dc23
2011018564

9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1
Printed in the United States of America
on acid-free paper
SHORT CONTENTS

Oxford Library of Psychology vii

About the Editor ix

Contributors xi

Contents xv

Chapters 1–564

Index 565

v
This page intentionally left blank
O X F O R D L I B R A R Y O F P S YC H O L O G Y

The Oxford Library of Psychology, a landmark series of handbooks, is published


by Oxford University Press, one of the world’s oldest and most highly respected
publishers, with a tradition of publishing significant books in psychology. The
ambitious goal of the Oxford Library of Psychology is nothing less than to span
a vibrant, wide-ranging field and, in so doing, to fill a clear market need.
Encompassing a comprehensive set of handbooks, organized hierarchically, the
Library incorporates volumes at different levels, each designed to meet a distinct
need. At one level are a set of handbooks designed broadly to survey the major
subfields of psychology; at another are numerous handbooks that cover impor-
tant current focal research and scholarly areas of psychology in depth and detail.
Planned as a reflection of the dynamism of psychology, the Library will grow
and expand as psychology itself develops, thereby highlighting significant new
research that will impact on the field. Adding to its accessibility and ease of use,
the Library will be published in print and, later on, electronically.
The Library surveys psychology’s principal subfields with a set of handbooks
that capture the current status and future prospects of those major subdisciplines.
This initial set includes handbooks of social and personality psychology, clini-
cal psychology, counseling psychology, school psychology, educational psychol-
ogy, industrial and organizational psychology, cognitive psychology, cognitive
neuroscience, methods and measurements, history, neuropsychology, personality
assessment, developmental psychology, and more. Each handbook undertakes to
review one of psychology’s major subdisciplines with breadth, comprehensiveness,
and exemplary scholarship. In addition to these broadly conceived volumes, the
Library also includes a large number of handbooks designed to explore in depth
more specialized areas of scholarship and research, such as stress, health and cop-
ing, anxiety and related disorders, cognitive development, or child and adolescent
assessment. In contrast to the broad coverage of the subfield handbooks, each of
these latter volumes focuses on an especially productive, more highly focused line
of scholarship and research. Whether at the broadest or most specific level, how-
ever, all of the Library handbooks offer synthetic coverage that reviews and evalu-
ates the relevant past and present research and anticipates research in the future.
Each handbook in the Library includes introductory and concluding chapters
written by its editor to provide a roadmap to the handbook’s table of contents and
to offer informed anticipations of significant future developments in that field.
An undertaking of this scope calls for handbook editors and chapter authors who
are established scholars in the areas about which they write. Many of the nation’s
and world’s most productive and best-respected psychologists have agreed to edit
Library handbooks or write authoritative chapters in their areas of expertise.
vii
For whom has the Oxford Library of Psychology been written? Because of its
breadth, depth, and accessibility, the Library serves a diverse audience, including
graduate students in psychology and their faculty mentors, scholars, researchers,
and practitioners in psychology and related fields. Each will find in the Library the
information they seek on the subfield or focal area of psychology in which they
work or are interested.
Befitting its commitment to accessibility, each handbook includes a compre-
hensive index, as well as extensive references to help guide research. And because
the Library was designed from its inception as an online as well as a print resource,
its structure and contents will be readily and rationally searchable online. Further,
once the Library is released online, the handbooks will be regularly and thoroughly
updated.
In summary, the Oxford Library of Psychology will grow organically to provide a
thoroughly informed perspective on the field of psychology, one that reflects both
psychology’s dynamism and its increasing interdisciplinarity. Once published
electronically, the Library is also destined to become a uniquely valuable interac-
tive tool, with extended search and browsing capabilities. As you begin to consult
this handbook, we sincerely hope you will share our enthusiasm for the more
than 500-year tradition of Oxford University Press for excellence, innovation, and
quality, as exemplified by the Oxford Library of Psychology.

Peter E. Nathan
Editor-in-Chief
Oxford Library of Psychology

viii oxford l ibrary of psych ology


A B O U T T H E E D I TO R

Richard M. Ryan
Richard M. Ryan, Ph.D., is a widely published researcher and theorist in the areas
of human motivation, development, and well-being, with over 250 articles, chap-
ters, and books. He is codeveloper (with Edward L. Deci) of Self-Determination
Theory, an internationally researched theory that has been applied in hundreds
of studies within areas such as development, education, work, relationships, med-
icine, psychical activity, and cross-cultural psychology. Ryan is also an award-
winning teacher and researcher who has given addresses in over 60 universities
worldwide. He is a Fellow of several professional organizations, including the
American Psychological Association and the American Educational Research
Association, and is an Honorary Member of the German Psychological Society.
He has been a visiting scientist at the Max Planck Institute, a James McKeen
Cattell Fellow, a Leverhulme Fellow, and recipient of other grants and awards.
Recent research interests include the effects of intrinsic and extrinsic life goals on
well-being; mindfulness and self-regulation; vitality; motivation in health care,
education, and virtual environments.

ix
This page intentionally left blank
CO N T R I B U TO R S

Henk Aarts Andrew J. Elliot


Department of Psychology Department of Clinical & Social Sciences
Utrecht University in Psychology
Utrecht, The Netherlands University of Rochester
Mark D. Alicke Rochester, NY
Department of Psychology Alexandra M. Freund
Ohio University Department of Psychology
Athens, OH University of Zurich
Anthony J. Amorose Zurich, Switzerland
School of Kinesiology and Recreation Ron Friedman
Illinois State University Department of Clinical & Social Sciences
Normal, IL in Psychology
Larry C. Bernard University of Rochester
Psychology Department Rochester, NY
Loyola Marymount University Shelly L. Gable
Los Angeles, CA Department of Psychological and Brain
Jenna Cambria Sciences
Department of Human Development University of California, Santa Barbara
University of Maryland Santa Barbara, CA
College Park, MD Guido H. E. Gendolla
Charles S. Carver Geneva Motivation Lab
Department of Psychology FPSE, Department of Psychology
University of Miami University of Geneva
Coral Gables, FL Geneva, Switzerland
Cecilia Sin-Sze Cheung Peter M. Gollwitzer
Department of Psychology Psychology Department
University of Illinois at New York University/University of Konstanz
Urbana–Champaign New York, NY/Konstanz, Germany
Champaign, IL Adam M. Grant
Ruud Custers The Wharton School
Department of Psychology University of Pennsylvania
Utrecht University Philadelphia, PA
Utrecht, The Netherlands Martin S. Hagger
Edward L. Deci School of Psychology and
Department of Psychology Speech Pathology
University of Rochester Curtin University
Rochester, NY Perth, Australia
Jacquelynne S. Eccles Marie Hennecke
Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
University of Michigan University of Zurich
Ann Arbor, MI Zurich, Switzerland
xi
E. Tory Higgins Kou Murayama
Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
Columbia University University of Munich
New York, NY Munich, Germany
Martin Grosse Holtforth Maida Mustafić
Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
University of Zurich University of Zurich
Zurich, Switzerland Zurich, Switzerland
Susan A. Jackson Gabriele Oettingen
Adjunct Senior Lecturer Psychology Department
School of Human Movement Studies New York University/University of Hamburg
The University of Queensland New York, NY/Hamburg, Germany
Brisbane, Australia Erika A. Patall
Flow Consultant and Writer Department of Educational Psychology
www.bodyandmindflow.com The University of Texas at Austin
Pelin Kesebir Austin, TX
Psychology Department Eva M. Pomerantz
University of Colorado at Colorado Springs Department of Psychology
Colorado Springs, CO University of Illinois at
Lindsay E. Kipp Urbana-Champaign
School of Kinesiology Champaign, IL
University of Minnesota, Twin Cities Thery Prok
Minneapolis, MN Department of Psychological and Brain
Woogul Lee Sciences
Division of Psychological and University of California, Santa Barbara
Quantitative Foundations Santa Barbara, CA
University of Iowa Tom Pyszczynski
Iowa City, IA Psychology Department
Nicole Legate University of Colorado at Colorado Springs
Department of Psychology Colorado Springs, CO
University of Rochester Lili Qin
Rochester, NY Department of Psychology
Chris C. Martin University of Illinois at
Department of Psychology Urbana–Champaign
College of William and Mary Champaign, IL
Williamsburg, VA Johnmarshall Reeve
Laura A. Maruskin Department of Education
Graduate School of Business Korea University
Stanford University Seoul, South Korea
Stanford, CA K. Ann Renninger
Johannes Michalak Department of Educational Studies
Department of Psychology Swarthmore College
University of Hildesheim Swarthmore, PA
Hildesheim, Germany Michael Richter
Mark Muraven Geneva Motivation Lab
Department of Psychology FPSE, Department of Psychology
University at Albany University of Geneva
Albany, NY Geneva, Switzerland

xii contributors
Tomi-Ann Roberts Stephanie Su
Department of Psychology Swarthmore College
Colorado College Swarthmore, PA
Colorado Springs, CO Robert E. Thayer
Richard M. Ryan Department of Psychology
Department of Psychology California State University,
University of Rochester Long Beach
Rochester, NY Long Beach, CA
Michael F. Scheier Todd M. Thrash
Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
Carnegie Mellon University College of William and Mary
Pittsburgh, PA Williamsburg, VA
Abigail A. Scholer Ellen L. Usher
Department of Psychology Department of Educational, School, and
University of Waterloo Counseling Psychology
Waterloo, ON, Canada University of Kentucky
Dale H. Schunk Lexington, KY
School of Education Patricia L. Waters
University of North Carolina at Greensboro Department of Psychology
Greensboro, NC Colorado College
Constantine Sedikides Colorado Springs, CO
Center for Research on Self and Identity Maureen R. Weiss
School of Psychology School of Kinesiology
University of Southampton University of Minnesota, Twin Cities
Southampton, UK Minneapolis, MN
Jihae Shin Allan Wigfield
The Wharton School Department of Human Development
University of Pennsylvania University of Maryland
Philadelphia, PA College Park, MD
Paul J. Silvia Rex A. Wright
Department of Psychology Department of Psychology
University of North Carolina at Greensboro University of North Texas
Greensboro, NC Denton, TX

con tr i butor s xiii


This page intentionally left blank
CONTENTS

Part One • Introduction


1. Motivation and the Organization of Human Behavior:
Three Reasons for the Reemergence of a Field 3
Richard M. Ryan

Part Two • General Theories of Human Motivation


2. Social Cognitive Theory and Motivation 13
Dale H. Schunk and Ellen L. Usher
3. Cybernetic Control Processes and the Self-Regulation of Behavior 28
Charles S. Carver and Michael F. Scheier
4. The Role of Death in Life: Existential Aspects of Human Motivation 43
Pelin Kesebir and Tom Pyszczynski
5. Too Much of a Good Thing? Trade-offs in Promotion and
Prevention Focus 65
Abigail A. Scholer and E. Tory Higgins
6. Motivation, Personality, and Development Within Embedded
Social Contexts: An Overview of Self-Determination Theory 85
Edward L. Deci and Richard M. Ryan

Part Three • Motivational Processes


7. Ego Depletion: Theory and Evidence 111
Mark Muraven
8. Flow 127
Susan A. Jackson
9. Implicit–Explicit Motive Congruence 141
Todd M. Thrash, Laura A. Maruskin, and Chris C. Martin
10. Curiosity and Motivation 157
Paul J. Silvia
11. Interest and Its Development 167
K. Ann Renninger and Stephanie Su

Part Four • Goals and Motivation


12. Achievement Goals 191
Kou Murayama, Andrew J. Elliot, and Ron Friedman
13. Goal Pursuit 208
Peter M. Gollwitzer and Gabriele Oettingen

xv
14. Unconscious Goal Pursuit: Nonconscious Goal Regulation
and Motivation 232
Henk Aarts and Ruud Custers
15. The Motivational Complexity of Choosing: A Review
of Theory and Research 248
Erika A. Patall
16. On Gains and Losses, Means and Ends: Goal Orientation
and Goal Focus Across Adulthood 280
Alexandra M. Freund, Marie Hennecke, and Maida Mustafić

Part Five • Motivation in Relationships


17. Self-Enhancement and Self-Protection Motives 303
Constantine Sedikides and Mark D. Alicke
18. The Gendered Body Project: Motivational Components
of Objectification Theory 323
Tomi-Ann Roberts and Patricia L. Waters
19. Relatedness Between Children and Parents: Implications
for Motivation 335
Eva M. Pomerantz, Cecilia Sin-Sze Cheung, and Lili Qin
20. Avoiding the Pitfalls and Approaching the Promises
of Close Relationships 350
Shelly L. Gable and Thery Prok

Part Six • Evolutionary and Biological Perspectives


21. Neuroscience and Human Motivation 365
Johnmarshall Reeve and Woogul Lee
22. Evolved Individual Differences in Human Motivation 381
Larry C. Bernard
23. Moods of Energy and Tension That Motivate 408
Robert E. Thayer
24. Effort Intensity: Some Insights From the Cardiovascular System 420
Guido H. E. Gendolla, Rex A. Wright, and Michael Richter

Part Seven • Motivation in Application


25. Motivation in Psychotherapy 441
Martin Grosse Holtforth and Johannes Michalak
26. Motivation in Education 463
Allan Wigfield, Jenna Cambria, and Jacquelynne S. Eccles
27. Advances in Motivation in Exercise and Physical Activity 479
Martin S. Hagger
28. Work Motivation: Directing, Energizing, and Maintaining
Effort (and Research) 505
Adam M. Grant and Jihae Shin

xvi contents
29. Youth Motivation and Participation in Sport and
Physical Activity 520
Maureen R. Weiss, Anthony J. Amorose, and Lindsay E. Kipp
30. Through a Fly’s Eye: Multiple Yet Overlapping Perspectives
on Future Directions for Human Motivation Research 554
Richard M. Ryan and Nicole Legate

Index 565

con ten ts xvii


This page intentionally left blank
PA RT
1
Introduction
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER

Motivation and the Organization of


1 Human Behavior: Three Reasons for
the Reemergence of a Field
Richard M. Ryan

Abstract
The fact that behavior is typically active, organized, and goal oriented represents one of the wonders
of animate nature. Nonetheless, the organization and integrity of behavior can be disrupted by social
contexts, implicit primes and motives, or by biological factors. There has been a strong resurgence
in empirical research on these topics, as well as recognition of the potency of psychological factors.
Three reasons for this resurgence of interest in the psychology of human motivation are reviewed in
detail: ( 1) the theoretical depth and interdisciplinary nature of the field; ( 2) methodological innovations
that have opened up new avenues of inquiry, and ( 3) the practical importance of motivation research
as a translational science and for improving individual and community wellness through empirically
supported interventions. Contributions within this volume are illustrative of all these factors, manifesting
interdisciplinary depth, sophisticated methods, and practical applicability.
Key Words: motivation, organization, goals, regnant causes, implicit motives

The most salient and noteworthy feature of the people’s motivation is explicit and conscious; at other
behavior of animate entities is that it is organized. times behavior is clearly energized and directed by
The actions of living things reflect a directed coor- nonconscious, implicit aims and attitudes. Finally,
dination of functions and processes toward specific whether motives are implicit or explicit, the behav-
ends. That behavior sequences are typically coher- ior organized by them will be variously successful.
ent and internally regulated, and thus demonstrate Effective motivation requires not only arousal or
equifinality and adaptability is one of the great won- energy but also guidance by an affective and cogni-
ders of our science. It is also the central focus of the tive system that, at least for most of us, is susceptible
field of motivation. to distraction or depletion. The authors represented
This Oxford Handbook of Motivation is concerned in this handbook collectively address all of these fac-
in particular with human motivation, with all the ets and dynamics of human motivation, grappling
complications that topic entails. Like that of other with the multiple ways in which the integral organi-
organisms, human behavior betrays an internal zation of motivated action is maintained, as well as
organization, actively operating within its environ- how akrasia, or motivational breakdowns, occur.
ment, and employing layered, interacting functions This timing of this Handbook is particularly
and processes. Humans are clearly motivated, goal- apt, given that human motivation is being more
directed, creatures. They seek out specific ends, rang- intensively studied today than ever before. Broad,
ing from concrete goals such as obtaining food and empirically based theories of motivation (many of
shelter to abstract ones such as developing a sense the major ones represented within this volume)
of meaning or attaining aesthetic ideals. Sometimes are again on the ascendance, influencing thinking


across disciplines, domains, and applications of the the power of motivated but nonconscious goals to
behavioral sciences. I say “again” because the field of entrain and direct behavior. Freund, Hennecke, and
motivation has seen some rises and falls in its brief Mustafić (Chapter 16) distinguish between process
history. and outcome-focused goals and the differential
In the early 20th century, motivational theories dynamics and influence of these goals across the life
were the major organizing forces within both experi- span. In all these cases motivation and goals are dis-
mental and applied psychologies. Theorists of moti- tinguished but interactive.
vation such as Tolman (1932) and Hull (1943) on Alongside these cognitive/goal theories, frame-
the behavioral side, and the formulations concern- works concerned with fundamental motivations have
ing motivation within psychodynamic camps (e.g., also rearisen in the past two decades to be among
Freud, 1962/1923; Hartmann, 1939) spawned con- the most actively researched topics in psychologi-
siderable empirical research that was integrated and cal science. These motivational theories replace, in a
interpreted through these paradigms. Yet following certain sense, the old drive theory accounts of Hull
White’s (1959) seminal review of the inadequa- and Freud with a different set of “drivers.” Rather
cies of both behavioral and psychodynamic drive than tracing motives to drive reduction these theo-
theories to explain active exploration, curiosity, and ries look to the evolved and acquired psychological
other phenomena associated with motivation, learn- needs and motives of individuals. Thus, within ter-
ing, and development, some major shifts happened ror management theory (TMT; see Kesebir & Pyszc-
within the discipline, and for many experimental- zynski, Chapter 4, this volume) the dynamic driver
ists, motivation faded as a focus of inquiry. of most behavior is anxiety reduction. People are
On the behavioral side, even before drive the- motivated to pursue cultural goals and projects that
ories were stumbling, the cognitive revolution help them feel esteemed and avoid awareness of vul-
was beginning to supplant them. Indeed, Hilgard nerability and mortality. Self-determination theory,
(1987) argued that cognitive approaches had pre- on the other hand, focuses on intrinsic motivations
sented a worldview in which questions of motiva- and the basic psychological needs that support them
tion as posed within drive theories were effectively as being fundamental to active behavior (see Deci
“dead.” In the cognitive tradition issues of moti- & Ryan, Chapter 6). Sedikides and Alicke (Chap-
vation could be addressed in terms of acquired ter 17) argue for self-esteem as a central motivational
valences or preferences, attributions, and expectan- force, driving behaviors across cultures. These and
cies, all used to predict the direction and persistence other broad theories within this book thus look to
of behavior. Indeed, I believe if Tolman were alive psychological needs as giving rise to cognitive goals
today he would feel vindicated in seeing the reli- and the actions they guide.
ance of behavioral theorists on those “hypothetical” As the examples illustrate, there is clearly a
intervening variables that stand between the envi- renewed energy surrounding the study of goals and
ronment and manifest behavior. motivation. There are many reasons for this, but
Without tracing the history of this movement, three are especially worth elaboration: (1) the theo-
it is no accident that this volume contains a very retical and multidisciplinary depth of motivational
significant set of contributions that derive from the questions; (2) the methodological innovations in
cognitive traditions within psychology, in particular both quantitative and experimental tools that have
the chapters on the topic of goals. As discussed by facilitated exploration of motivational phenomena;
Murayama, Elliot, and Friedman (Chapter 12, this and (3) the obvious practical and social importance
volume), goals can be defined as a form of regulation of motivation research, with its utility as a transla-
that guides behavior in the service of specific aims. tional, applicable science. Each shall be considered
Goals, they argue, help the individual to focus atten- in turn.
tion and to protect responses compatible with one’s
motives. This definition suggests how closely goals Reason 1: The Theoretical Depth
and motivation can be tied, insofar as goals are in and Interdisciplinary Nature of
many ways the servants of motives. For example, in Motivation Studies
Chapter 13, Gollwitzer and Oettingen demonstrate The study of motivation drills at core founda-
how explicitly set goals, especially when accompa- tional issues in the science. As stated earlier, what
nied by specific implementation plans, enhance the is most amazing about the behavior of organisms
likelihood that one’s intentions reach fruition. In is the fact that it is spontaneously organized: It is
contrast, Aarts and Custers (Chapter 14) marvel at both energized and directed. This is evident in what

 m otivation and the organiz ation o f h uma n b eh avi or


Tolman (1932) understood to be the purposive significant or functionally relevant to a problem,
nature of organisms, as they evidence effort, equifi- thus providing the most satisfying explanation of
nality, and adaptive intelligence toward specific ends. events. Many causally relevant analyses can be “cor-
The principles and mechanisms through which this rect” without being pertinent, or regnant in this
occurs, as well as the conditions that support or thwart sense. Indeed, rather than competing, each type of
these spontaneous capabilities, are critical problems explanation and analysis must coordinate, even as
for scientists at all levels of behavior analysis, from some rightfully predominate because of their prag-
physiological to cultural. Motivation is a problem matic utility or value.
unique to life scientists. Indeed it is the organized
nature of actions that separates the life sciences from The Unique Place of Psychological Theory
the physical sciences, where organized, purposive, Causal explanations can operate at the level of
behavior does not occur, and where entropy is the physical/material causes, as well as at the level of
dominant force (Mayer, 1997). Instead, in the life cognitive, emotional, and social constructs as theo-
sciences, and in the understanding of human behav- rized and measured with the tools of psychology.
ior, the core interest is in discovering the bases of the Although some scientists early in the 20th century
negentropic, coherent, and integrated efforts of indi- eschewed abstract or formal variables like those so
viduals as they pursue specific goals and outcomes. frequently used in psychology, most all contem-
Within this Handbook we see the problem of moti- porary philosophers of science embrace them and
vated, organized behavior viewed through multiple acknowledge their necessity (Curd & Cover, 1998).
perspectives, including evolutionary (see Bernard, Psychological models of motivation, which make
Chapter 22), physiological (e.g., Gendolla, Wright, up the bulk of the current volume, operate on the
& Richter, Chapter 24), neurological (Reeve & level of inferred constructs, intended to capture the
Lee, Chapter 21), cognitive (e.g., Carver & Scheier, forces at work in energizing and directing action.
Chapter 3), phenomenological/experiential (e.g., Causal models at this level of analysis can be a par-
Jackson, Chapter 8), and cultural (e.g., Sedikedies & ticularly important point of entry into describing
Alicke, Chapter 17), among others. At each level of and predicting motivated behaviors. If one wants
analysis there are basic scientific questions concern- to intervene in intentional behaviors (e.g., dietary
ing the processes that instigate and support versus habits, work practices, physical activity and exer-
disrupt or deplete motivational processes. In fact, the cise), knowing the types of feedback, significant
volume illustrates that motivation can be meaning- cognitions, meanings, and perceived social contexts
fully studied through multiple levels of description that support or thwart these behaviors provides con-
and causal models. siderable leverage. Because the sources of variance
Speaking of multiple levels or types of causal- accounting for molar behavior are so readily cap-
ity might give some scientists pause, particularly if tured by the constructs and “causes” studied by psy-
they view the issue of causation reductively or nar- chologists, they represent among the most regnant
rowly. But it is clear that when it comes to motiva- levels of analysis for many human behaviors.
tion there is rarely if ever a singular cause at work. James (1892) clearly recognized this special power
Rather, actions can be depicted best as outcomes of a and utility of psychological theory, describing it as a
set of determinative processes that can be described science of “practical prediction and control” which
through various levels of analysis and theoretical when realized would represent “an achievement com-
models. One level of inquiry does not supplant or pared with which the control of the rest of physical
have epistemological priority over the others, but nature would appear comparatively insignificant”
each has a different type of explanatory power and (p. 148). Ok, perhaps astrophysicists would not
relevance to specific concerns and questions. More- agree! Nonetheless, the extent to which psychological
over, motivation is itself a phenomenon that resists interventions can impact important behaviors, from
simple reductionism, because an inventory of com- health maintenance to learning, is impressive. It is
ponents and their functions does not by itself explain perhaps for this reason that psychological variables
their emergent orchestration and directedness. such as needs, goals, attributions, and perceptions
What shifts in scientific and practical discourse even supply the target or criterion variables upon
is not the plausibility, but the relevance, of differ- which other levels of analysis are often focused.
ent levels of analysis as explanations, making some In addition to considerations of prediction and
causal analyses more regnant than others (Ryan & control, the psychological analysis of actions is also
Deci, 2006). Regnant causes are those deemed most semantically meaningful in a way mere physical

rya n 
descriptions could never be. As Kauffman (2000) view, which depicts human nature as relatively open
underscored, “compared to a hypothetical ‘complete’ to social and cultural conditioning and learning.
physical description, the action-and-doing descrip- It also emphasizes the importance of feelings of
tion picks out the relevant features with respect to efficacy and competence, and how any factors that
the goals of the autonomous agent” (p. 126). Kauf- diminish that psychological experience undermine
mann further maintained that, once we are at the the subsequent probability of motivated action.
level of creatures that can have internal models of, Control theory is presented in Chapter 3 by Carver
and plans for, the future, we “seem to have arrived and Scheier. They would likely not, when speaking
at a level of organization in which action and goal technically, call their framework a theory of moti-
talk becomes essential” (p. 126). This is just to say vation, but rather a cybernetic model of behavior
again that reductionism is often a misplaced lan- regulation. Yet in the editor’s view, it needed to be
guage game, in which the most important features included here anyway. Their influential perspective
of a situation are obscured rather than highlighted. has generated more than three decades of careful
In this regard, psychological explanations are not research on goals and their successful, and unsuc-
only often the most causally regnant, they also often cessful, enactment. In terms of motivation, control
make the most sense among explanations. theory interprets goal-directed action as reflecting a
The fact that in this field we can plumb multiple hierarchy of feedback processes that regulate behav-
levels of analysis from the molecular to the social ior. In this model, affect and emotions are understood
and seek to coordinate them with psychological as both generated and intensified or dampened as an
phenomena reflects the dynamic nature and com- aspect of regulation, providing another set of feed-
plexity of motivation. The field thus befits the sci- back processes. This model leads to both expected
entific ideal of consilience (Wilson, 1999) in which and surprising predictions—among them that when
multiple levels of analysis mutually inform and we are feeling particularly good we are more likely
constrain the problems in focus. Because science is to reduce effort on a task and “coast.”
inherently systematic, and totalizing, coordination In Chapter 5 Scholer and Higgins discuss regula-
between levels of analysis, or consilience, is logically tory focus theory, first introduced by Higgins (1997),
demanded. Furthermore, in this reciprocal coordi- and consider two fundamental motivational sys-
nation the constraints, contours, and limits of pre- tems: the promotion system and the prevention
diction within any given level of analysis become system. The theory is introduced largely in terms
apparent. of individual differences—of the benefits and trade-
Theoretical depth leads to a richness and diversity of offs faced by people who are prevention oriented
frameworks. The volume opens with chapters summa- (i.e., vigilant and security focused) versus promo-
rizing what are among the most vibrantly researched tion oriented (i.e., eager and accomplishment seek-
and integrative theories of human motivation on the ing). The former are highly sensitive to change and
current stage. They collectively attest to the multiple more oriented to “oughts” and “shoulds”; the latter
deep psychological accounts of human motivation are more interested in change and growth, and are
that are supported by empirical research. Each of oriented toward pursuing ideals. These distinct ori-
these theories was in fact selected for this volume entations have different adaptive value as a function
because it represents a framework that is organizing of context, as Scholer and Higgins review, and each
significant scientific and scholarly inquiries around can mobilize approach or avoidance behaviors. The
the globe, and often in multiple disciplines. theory also assumes an underlying motivation for
For example social cognitive theory, as developed people to experience regulatory fit—that is, behavior
by Bandura (1986) and described in this volume that is consistent with their prevention or promo-
by Schunk and Usher (Chapter 2) emphasizes the tion orientation. Regulatory focus theory thus pres-
idea that human learning and behavior are largely ents intersecting principles that afford a specificity
shaped by social environments, including the reac- of predictions concerning people’s emotions and
tions and approval of others. As they observe and motivation in different situations.
interact within social-cultural contexts, individuals Terror management theory, presented here by
learn about their own efficacy as well as the contin- Kasebir and Pyszczynski, is a broad theory of human
gent consequences of specific behaviors. They then meaning and values derived from both existentialist
act in accordance with their beliefs about their capa- reflections on death anxiety and the work of Ernst
bilities and the expected outcomes of actions. Social Becker, who once argued that the task of a unified
cognitive theory is thus a broad and widely applied science should be “the incessant implementation of

 m otivation and the organiz ation o f h uma n b eh avi or


human well-being” (Becker, 1968, p. xiii). TMT thus his chapter has broad relevance to both theo-
argues that our personal goals and cultural activi- ries and practical models of motivation.
ties are mainly focused on self-esteem maintenance, In Chapter 10, Silvia tackles that most important
which in turn serves as a buffer from awareness of of motivational forces for development and learn-
mortality. Defense against the anxiety associated ing, namely curiosity. He discusses curiosity as both
with death is thus in the TMT view a principal an evolved feature of human nature, and as a moti-
driving force of symbolic and cultural activities, and vational process that is strongly affected by social
the generation of meanings and purposes. TMT has contexts and supports. Similarly, Renninger and Su
harnessed experimental techniques to assess attitudes take on the topic of personal interests—reviewing
and motivations following mortality salience events, both the development of those abiding passions
with results that suggest that people are indeed often and investments that define us as individuals, and
acting out of nonconscious defensive attempts to the factors that sustain them. Patall, in Chapter 15,
stave off existential threat. TMT challenges the view reviews and integrates the vast literature on choice
of humans as conscious and rational beings, show- as it relates to motivation. She looks at the evidence
ing instead that underlying ultimate concerns can that choice facilitates sustained motivation over time
in some individuals automatically activate complex, through enhancing commitment to actions; and
and sometimes defensive, behaviors and attitudes. how choice can entail costs, from cognitive load to
This Handbook also contains a chapter on self- cultural conflicts. Finally, in a quite unique chapter
determination theory (SDT). Although presented (Chapter 18) Roberts and Waters consider the issue
here by Ed Deci and myself, the theory represents of gender as it relates to motivation and interper-
the efforts of a diverse yet cohesive community of sonal relationships. They specifically are concerned
scholars from around the world with interest in this with objectification as an influence on women, and
perspective. SDT envisions an active, assimilative, its costs for both their motivated performance and
and dynamic human nature, supported or thwarted well-being. These topical reviews integrate an array
in its basic psychological needs. In fact, SDT posits of empirical findings on motivational processes and
a specific human nature, one that thrives under con- raise critical questions for continued research.
ditions of support for competence, autonomy, and In short, the theoretical chapters in this volume
relatedness, and yet becomes defensive, reactive, and represent some of the most important organizing
compliant under conditions of need deprivations or frameworks in the science of motivation today. Each
thwarts. The assumption of universal basic needs has of these explanatory frameworks shifts out a distinct
been both descriptively and experimentally genera- yield of predications, laws, and applications that
tive, addressing phenomena such as the undermin- are broadly influencing the scientific and applied
ing effect of controlling rewards, the characteristics communities. Looking across this collection, I am
that make an activity intrinsically motivated, the reminded here of the words of pioneer psychologist
processes that facilitate greater internalization and Robert S. Woodworth, who once stated about psy-
integrated regulation of extrinsic motivation, and chological schools of thought that: “Every school is
the reasons materialism leads to unhappiness. SDT good, though no one is good enough” (Woodworth,
has thus been broadly applied in domains from 1948, p. 255).
work, education, psychotherapy, and medicine to
sport, play, and entertainment. Reason 2: Methodological Innovations and
Outside of broad-based theories this volume also the Resurgence in Motivation Studies
contains reviews of theory and research on specific Although the romantic view of the development
motivational processes and phenomena that have big of new knowledge is that it is the product of individ-
implications. For example, Chapter 7 by Muraven ual insight and genius, many of the recent insights
addresses a phenomenon that has captured the inter- in the field of motivation were made possible less by
ests of dozens of experimental social psychologists individual genius and more by new and better tools
for over a decade—namely ego depletion. Muraven, for exploration. Explorers in a dark cave get farther
who is an originator of the ego-depletion concept when someone provides a better headlamp.
and model, examines the myriad factors associated Among these new tools, several deserve to be
with the self-control of behaviors that require effort highlighted as playing particularly strong roles in
and drain human energies. Ego-depletion effects advancing the science of human motivation: Sta-
bear on the multiple ways that the human inten- tistical advances in structural equation modeling,
tions and goal pursuits are vulnerable to akrasia, and multilevel modeling, and growth-curve analysis;

rya n 
experimental advances in the measurement and But beyond habits, research suggests that much of
priming of implicit motivational processes; and new the time our actions are being selected or sustained
interfaces linking biology and neuropsychology to based on motivational dynamics of which we are
psychological models of behavior. unaware. Our attitudes and motives can be, to dif-
ferent degrees, implicit. Of course, as Westin (1998)
Changes in Statistical Methods points out, this is something long clear within
One of the characteristic features of behavioral psychodynamic circles, but there is a new vigor in
science is its frequent use of statistical inference in experimental studies regarding this topic.
the development of laws and principles. Although Many of the methods underlying recent research
there are clearly limits to inductive-statistical expla- on nonconscious motivational processes build off
nations of events (see classic work by Hempel, of the idea of accessibility, in which reaction times
1965), the probabilistic and multidetermined nature are used to estimate how activated a motive or atti-
of human behavior makes such methods essential tude is for a person. Related to the issue of acti-
tools of behavioral science. Yet these statistical tools vation are priming methods, in which motives or
themselves have traditionally had limitations in attitudes are potentiated by exposure to, or “prim-
what they could describe, and what covariances and ing” of, strongly associated constructs, thereby
patterns could be detected. For example, the classi- enhancing the accessibility of, and thus the like-
cal ANOVA approach to data restricts our imagina- lihood of enacting, specific motives or goals (e.g.,
tion to what accounts for mean changes in a given see Aarts, Custers, & Holland, 2007). Activating or
variable, rather than trajectories, patterns, or intra- priming a motive or goal can set in motion a rich
individual variability in change. network of cognitive, affective, and behavioral pro-
Recent methodological advances in quantitative cesses that provide both energy and direction (i.e.,
analysis have thus lent new excitement to the field. motivation) outside of conscious awareness. In fact,
In particular, multilevel modeling methods (e.g., people’s behavior can frequently be prompted by
Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) have allowed investiga- goals primed by situational elements of which they
tors to look not only at how individuals differ from are not aware but that nonetheless make certain
one another in motives and goal, but also at how motives more accessible than others. Chapter 14 by
and why an individual waxes and wanes in various Aarts and Custers in this volume provides an excel-
motives and behavioral regulations across time or lent review of some of these methods, along with
situations. Most every classical question in the field considerable evidence that well-organized behav-
was originally posited as a “between persons” issue; iors not only can be, but frequently are, under
yet for most of us personally and practically the core “unconscious control.”
concern is at a “within-person” level of analysis, or This strong renewed interest in nonconscious
what leads to rises and falls in motivation within motivation has also opened up a dynamic new area
individuals over time, settings, or events. Describ- of investigation where we can look not just beyond
ing change over time, and what components of self-report, but at the interface of conscious (and
motivation remain stable or vary intra-individually reportable) and nonconscious motives, as Chapter 9
becomes increasingly critical as we examine trajec- by Thrash, Maruskin, and Martin in this volume
tories during or following critical events or planned reviews. As they point out, as methodological refine-
interventions. These new tools have thus allowed us ments have occurred, correlations between implicit
to at least begin to overcome the limitations of a and explicit measures of motives and attitudes have
cross-sectional psychology (see Lazarus, 2003) that increased, and these refinements have helped clarify
hampered the study of motivation for so long. more systematic individual and situational variations
in implicit/explicit discrepancies. Such discrepancies,
New Experimental Methods and the Study of in turn, appear to be related to both developmental
Implicit Motivation and proximal factors, and to predict well-being and
Current experimental methods are allowing motivational outcomes.
researchers to investigate previously underexplored At the same time as studies impress us with the
phenomena, including the ubiquitous influence of potential of nonconscious processes to organize inten-
nonconscious motivations. Clearly a great deal of tional behaviors, the same methods allow researchers
human behavior is not consciously driven. We have to demonstrate how individuals can exert tremen-
many habitual and overlearned behaviors that can be dous regulatory control over their own actions. Thus,
performed without intention or conscious control. research has shown, for example, how people high

 m otivation and the organiz ation o f h uma n b eh avi or


in mindfulness and autonomy (see Deci & Ryan, genetic influences. This is again congruent with the
Chapter 6, this volume), or in an implemental rather idea of consilience and the principle that all levels of
than deliberative phase of action (see Gollwitzer & analysis must be capable of coordination.
Ottengen, Chapter 13) are more resilient in the face of
depletion effects, threats, and challenges as they pur- Reason 3: Practical Importance of
sue goals. This is true even with respect to regulating Motivational Science as a Core
implicit processes, which some can manage through Translational and Applied Discipline
volitional processes (e.g. Legault, Green-Demers, Perhaps just as crucial to the resurgence of the
Grant, & Chung, 2007; Niemiec et al., 2010). Ironi- field of motivation as these scientific advances is a
cally, it seems, the very focus on the influence of non- renewed appreciation of its practical importance.
conscious motivations over behavior has made salient As any good dialectical materialist might have pre-
the specific strengths and resources that allow some dicted, it is probably more because motivation mat-
individuals to override such influences and more ters on the bottom line—for productivity at work,
effectively pursue consciously endorsed goals. learning in schools, and adherence within clinics—
than because it is of inherent intellectual or scientific
Toward a Life Science: Beyond Reductionism interest that it is at the forefront of our thinking.
to Coordinated Analyses Given that the most important societal goals require
Robust advancements in methods have also been human energy and commitment to be actualized,
evident in a new synergism between biological and motivation may in fact be the most critical applied
psychological inquiry. Methods such as functional topic of our field. Indeed, even for discoveries in
magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) allow us to link other sciences to be applied, motivation represents
brain processes with specifically activated motives a core translational science, because it addresses what
and inhibitions, clarifying the mechanics behind must occur for new knowledge, products, or inven-
behavioral dynamics (see Chapter 21 by Reeve and tions to be adopted and actively used.
Lee). More accurate physiological models of cardio- Chapters in this Handbook speak to myriad
vascular functioning allow better gauging of effort, important applications of motivation theory. Indeed,
and thus the study of its dynamics and determinants reviewed in this volume are chapters on topics where
(e.g., Gendolla, Wright, & Richter, Chapter 24). motivation is clearly a central concern, including
In the area of coping, assays of cortisone and other work (Grant & Shin, Chapter 28), education (Wig-
biologic indicators also allow us to better gauge field, Cambria, & Eccles, Chapter 26), psychotherapy
human reactivity, stress resources, and estimate the (Holtforth & Michalak, Chapter 25), and exercise
likelihood of goal success as a function of differ- and sport (Hagger, Chapter 27; Weis, Ambrose, &
ent sources of motivation. Finally, studies of how Kipp, Chapter 29). Moreover, because motivation
the physiological effects of diet and activity impact is so richly an interpersonal matter, also included
mood and motivation (see Chapter 23 by Thayer) is a section on motivation in relationships, which
show the import of biological factors on vitality and contains work on parenting (Pomerantz, Cheung,
functioning. and Qin, Chapter 19), close relationships (Gable
As with statistical enhancements, these observa- & Prok, Chapter 20), gender and objectification
tional advances in the biological sphere, especially as (Roberts & Waters, Chapter 18), and self-protection
they are linked with constructs of psychological inter- in the context of social comparisons (Sedikides &
est, have tremendous promise for refining theory. Alicke, Chapter 17). What one sees in each of these
The fact of the matter is that psychological processes review chapters is a generative framework that not
are themselves embodied. The different constructs only is advancing the basic science but is also help-
studied within social sciences must therefore map to ing to translate that science into practices that yield
distinct patterns of activation (Ryan, Kuhl, & Deci, better human outcomes from the workplace to the
1997). Such mapping is not an acceptance of physi- playground. These chapters, applied to everyday
calism, but rather reflects integrative science rather concerns and settings, make clear the extent to which
than reductionism, and helps pave the “two-way motivation theories and research are organizing and
street” that Reeve and Lee depict between neurosci- informing significant practical activities and inter-
ences and psychology. More important, it facilitates ventions in multiple fields of human endeavor.
tests of theory, harnessing biology to advance reg- The word motivated is not a complex term. It sim-
nant psychological models, providing new avenues ply means “to be moved.” Although human bodies
for examining covariations with external, social, and can be physically moved by many forces, it is those

rya n 
animating energies that organize purposive action Lazarus, R. S. (2003). Does positive psychology have legs? Psy-
that are illuminated by the authors in this volume. chological Inquiry, 14, 93–109.
Legault, L., Green-Demers, I., Grant, P., & Chung, J. (2007).
And they are shedding light on phenomena that are On the self-regulation of implicit and explicit prejudice:
not only of great practical concern to most of us but A self-determination theory perspective. Personality and
also represent one of the central scientific mysteries Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 732–749.
in our universe. Mayer, E. (1997). This is biology: The science of the living world.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino, P. J.,
References Breen, W. E., Levesque-Bristol, C., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). Being
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Holland, R. W. (2007). The non- present in the face of existential threat: The role of trait mindful-
conscious cessation of goal pursuit: When goals and nega- ness in reducing defensive responses to mortality salience. Jour-
tive affect are coactivated. Journal of Personality and Social nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 344–365.
Psychology, 92, 165–178. Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: models: Applications and data analysis methods. Newbury
A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Park, CA: Sage
Becker, E. (1968). The structure of evil. New York: Braziller. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Self-regulation and the
Curd, M., & Cover, J. A. (1998). Philosophy of science: The central problem of human autonomy: Does psychology need choice,
issues. New York: Norton. self-determination, and will? Journal of Personality, 74,
Freud, S. (1962). The ego and the id. New York: Norton. 1557–1585.
(Original work published 1923). Ryan, R. M., Kuhl, J., & Deci, E. L. (1997). Nature and
Hartmann, H. (1939). Ego psychology and the problem of adapta- autonomy: Organizational view of social and neurobiologi-
tion. New York: International Universities Press. cal aspects of self-regulation in behavior and development.
Hempel, C. G. (1965). Aspects of scientific explanation. New York: Development and Psychopathology, 9, 701–728.
Free Press. Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men.
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American New York: Century.
Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300. Westin, D. (1998). The scientific legacy of Sigmund Freud:
Hilgard, E. R. (1987). Psychology in America: A historical Toward a psychodynamically informed psychological sci-
survey. New York: Harcourt, Brace. ence. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 333–371.
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of
behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.
James, W. (1892). A plea for psychology as a ‘natural science.’ The Wilson, E. O. (1999) Consilience: The unity of human knowledge.
Philosophical Review, 1, 146–153. New York: Vintage.
Kauffman, S. (2000). Investigations. Oxford, England: Oxford Woodworth, R. S. (1948). Contemporary schools of psychology-
University Press. revised edition. New York: The Ronald Press.

 m otivation and the organiz ation o f h uma n b eh avi or


PA RT
2
General Theories of
Human Motivation
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER

Social Cognitive Theory and


2 Motivation

Dale H. Schunk and Ellen L. Usher

Abstract
Social cognitive theory is a theory of psychological functioning that emphasizes learning from the social
environment. This chapter focuses on Bandura’s social cognitive theory, which postulates reciprocal
interactions among personal, behavioral, and social/environmental factors. Persons use various vicarious,
symbolic, and self-regulatory processes as they strive to develop a sense of agency in their lives.
Key motivational processes are goals and self-evaluations of progress, outcome expectations, values,
social comparisons, and self-efficacy. People set goals and evaluate their goal progress. The perception
of progress sustains self-efficacy and motivation. Individuals act in accordance with their values and
strive for outcomes they desire. Social comparisons with others provide further information on
their learning and goal attainment. Self-efficacy is an especially critical influence on motivation and
affects task choices, effort, persistence, and achievement. Suggestions are given for future research
directions.
Key Words: social cognitive theory, vicarious processes, symbolic processes, self-regulatory processes,
goals, self-evaluations of progress, outcome expectations, values, social comparisons, self-efficacy

Introduction By interacting with others, people learn knowledge,


Motivation refers to the process whereby goal- skills, strategies, beliefs, rules, and attitudes. Through
directed activities are energized, directed, and their observations and interactions with others, indi-
sustained (Schunk, Pintrich, & Meece, 2008). Con- viduals also learn about the appropriateness, useful-
temporary cognitive theories of motivation postu- ness, and consequences of behaviors. People act in
late that individuals’ thoughts, beliefs, and emotions accordance with their beliefs about their capabilities
are central processes that underlie motivation. These and the expected outcomes of actions.
cognitive perspectives stand in contrast both to Although there are different social cognitive
early views that linked motivation with individual perspectives on motivation, this chapter focuses on
differences in instincts and traits and to behavioral Bandura’s (1977b, 1986, 1997, 2001) social cogni-
theories that viewed motivation as an increased or tive theory of psychological functioning. Bandura’s
continued level of responding to stimuli caused by theory underscores the key roles of vicarious, sym-
reinforcements or rewards. bolic, and self-regulatory processes in human learn-
In this chapter we provide an account of motivation ing and behavior. This social cognitive framework
from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Social often is employed by researchers to explore the
cognitive theory emphasizes the idea that much human operation and outcomes of cognitive and affective
learning and behavior occur in social environments. processes hypothesized to underlie motivation.


The next section describes the conceptual factor. With respect to the interaction of self-
framework of social cognitive theory to include efficacy and behavior, much research shows that
the key roles played by vicarious, symbolic, and self-efficacy influences achievement behaviors such
self-regulatory processes. Some key social cogni- as task choice, effort, persistence, and use of effec-
tive motivational processes are discussed, includ- tive learning strategies (person → behavior; Schunk
ing goals and self-evaluations of progress, outcome & Pajares, 2009). These behaviors also affect self-
expectations, values, social comparisons, and self- efficacy. As students work on tasks and observe their
efficacy. A separate section is devoted to discuss- learning progress, their self-efficacy for continued
ing self-efficacy, or one’s perceived capabilities for learning is enhanced (behavior → person).
learning or performing actions at designated levels The link between personal and environmental
(Bandura, 1977a, 1997), given its centrality to factors can be illustrated with students with learning
learning and motivation. We conclude the chapter disabilities, many of whom hold low self-efficacy for
with suggestions for future research. performing well (Licht & Kistner, 1986). Instruc-
tors in such environments may base their reactions
Conceptual Framework to these students on perceived attributes about the
This section discusses the conceptual framework of students (e.g., low skills) rather than on students’
social cognitive theory. Of particular importance are actual capabilities (person → social/environment).
the following: reciprocal interactions among personal, In turn, environmental feedback can affect students’
behavioral, and social/environmental factors; the dif- self-efficacy, such as when a teacher tells a student,
ferences between enactive and vicarious learning; the “I know you can do this” (social/environment →
distinction between learning and performance; and person).
the roles of vicarious, symbolic, and self-regulatory The link between behaviors and environmen-
processes in psychological functioning. tal factors is seen in many instructional sequences.
Environmental factors can direct behaviors, such as
Reciprocal Interactions when a teacher points to a display and says, “Look
A central tenet of Bandura’s (1977b, 1986, 1997, here,” which students do without much conscious
2001) social cognitive theory is that human behav- effort (social/environment → behavior). Students’
ior operates within a framework of triadic recipro- behaviors can alter their instructional environ-
cality involving reciprocal interactions among three ments. When teachers ask questions and students
sets of influences: personal (e.g., cognitions, beliefs, give incorrect answers, teachers are apt to reteach
skills, affect); behavioral; and social/environmen- the material rather than continue with the lesson
tal factors. This reciprocal network is illustrated in (behavior → social/environment).
Figure 2.1. Social cognitive theory reflects a view of human
These reciprocally interacting influences can be agency in which individuals are proactively engaged
demonstrated using self-efficacy as the personal in their own success and development (Schunk &

Behavioral
Factors

Personal Factors Social/Environmental


(cognitions, beliefs, skills, affects) Factors

Fig. 2.1. Reciprocal interactions in social cognitive theory.

 social cognitive theory and motivati on


Pajares, 2005). These beliefs allow individuals to distinction is not apparent with enactive learning
exert a large degree of control over their thoughts, because persons demonstrate what they have
feelings, and actions. In reciprocal fashion, people learned. But vicarious learning may not be demon-
affect and are influenced by their actions and envi- strated until sometime after the modeled behavior
ronments. But the scope of this reciprocal influ- occurs. Whether learning results in changed perfor-
ence is broader than individuals because they live in mances depends on factors such as learners’ moti-
social environments. Collective agency refers to peo- vation, interests, incentives to perform, perceived
ple’s shared perceived capabilities of accomplishing needs, physical conditions, social pressures, and
tasks as a group. As is true with individuals, groups competing activities.
also affect and are influenced by their actions and Students learn many new skills, strategies, and
environments. behaviors, only some of which they may demon-
strate at the time of learning. Because teachers are
Enactive and Vicarious Learning responsible for ensuring that students learn, they
In social cognitive theory, learning occurs enac- assess student learning in various ways (e.g., tests,
tively through actual doing and vicariously through quizzes, assignments, homework). The assumption
observing modeled performances (e.g., live, filmed, is that students will demonstrate what they have
symbolic; Bandura, 1977b). Enactive learning learned; however, this may not always happen. Able
involves learning from the consequences of one’s students who are motivated to be socially accepted
actions, which can inform and motivate. Actions by their peers may not demonstrate the full range of
convey information about the accuracy or appropri- their learning so that they appear more in line with
ateness of one’s behaviors. People rewarded for their their classmates’ competencies. Authentic assess-
actions typically understand that they are perform- ments that take various forms can help teachers
ing well, whereas punishments signal behavioral accurately gauge students’ learning.
inappropriateness. Individuals tend to be motivated
to learn and perform behaviors that they believe will Vicarious, Symbolic, and Self-Regulatory
have desirable consequences and to avoid learning Processes
behaviors that they believe will be punished. Bandura’s (1986) social cognitive theory stresses
Much human learning occurs vicariously and the idea that people possess capabilities that distin-
therefore does not require actual performance by guish them as humans and motivate them to strive
learners. Vicarious learning offers an efficient alter- for a sense of agency. Among the most prominent
native to learning via direct experience. Humans of these are vicarious, symbolic, and self-regulatory
would be hopelessly inefficient if their involvement processes.
were required for all learning. Vicarious learning
also saves people from undesirable consequences. Vicarious Processes
Observing or reading about safety techniques saves The capability for learning vicariously allows
individuals from acting in potentially dangerous individuals to acquire beliefs, cognitions, affects,
ways. As with enactive learning, observers are moti- skills, strategies, and behaviors, from observations
vated to learn actions that lead to successes. Peo- of others in their social environments. As noted
ple attend to successful models who demonstrate earlier, this capability saves people time over what
actions that they believe will benefit them (Schunk, would be required if all learning had to be demon-
1987). strated at the time of learning. This capability also
Learning of complex skills typically occurs both allows people to regularly shape their lives, because
enactively and vicariously. By observing teacher they select environmental features (e.g., individuals,
models, students may learn some aspects of a com- materials) to which they want to attend. Thus, stu-
plex skill. As students practice the skills, teach- dents who want to become teachers enroll in edu-
ers provide feedback and corrective instruction as cation programs and put themselves in situations
needed. Through observation, practice, and feed- where they can learn vicariously, such as by attend-
back, students learn skills and enjoy greater success. ing classes, observing and working with classroom
teachers, and reading books and other materials.
Learning and Performance The models in individuals’ environments serve as
Unlike older behavioral theories, social cognitive important sources of information and motivation.
theory distinguishes new learning from performance This section discusses the relevance of vicarious pro-
of previously learned actions (Bandura, 1977b). The cesses for learning and motivation.

s c h un k, us h er 
types of vicarious processes command better attention. Observers are more
Bandura (1986) distinguished three types of motivated to attend to models who have status and
vicarious processes: response facilitation, inhibition credibility, such as teachers. Task features can affect
and disinhibition, and observational learning (Table attention, such as when teachers use bright colors,
2.1). Response facilitation refers to modeled actions oversized features, and interactive materials. Atten-
that serve as social motivators for observers to act tion also is affected by observers’ beliefs about the
in the same fashion. Response facilitation effects functional value of the modeled behaviors. Modeled
are common in everyday life. An individual walk- activities that observers believe are important and
ing down a street who encounters a group of people likely to lead to desirable outcomes motivate them
looking in a store window may be motivated to stop to pay attention. Students’ attention should be
and look in the window. raised when teachers provide verbal markers, such
Response facilitation effects do not represent as when they announce that the material they are
learning because people already know how to per- about to cover will be on a test.
form the actions. The behaviors of others motivate Retention involves cognitively organizing,
observers’ actions. There is evidence that response rehearsing, coding, and transforming information
facilitation effects can occur without conscious for storage in memory. Relative to the other pro-
awareness (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). cesses of observational learning, social cognitive
Inhibition and disinhibition effects result from theory devotes less attention to this process. Theo-
models strengthening or weakening observers’ ten- rists and researchers in the information process-
dencies to act in given ways. Inhibition can happen ing tradition have addressed this aspect in depth
when models are punished for their actions, whereas (Matlin, 2009).
disinhibition can result when models perform The third process—production—involves trans-
threatening or prohibited actions without nega- lating cognitive conceptions of modeled actions
tive consequences. Classroom misbehavior may be into behaviors. Especially with complex behaviors,
disinhibited when students observe other students it often is the case that observers will learn only
misbehaving without being reprimanded by the some features. Learners refine their skills through
teacher; a sudden reprimand may inhibit further practice and feedback that may include additional
misbehavior. modeling.
Like response facilitation, inhibition and disin- Motivation is a key process in observational
hibition represent motivational effects on behav- learning because onlookers are more apt to attend
ior, not new learning. A difference between these to, retain, and produce those modeled actions that
two categories is that, whereas response facilitation they believe are important. People are selective;
involves behaviors that are socially acceptable, inhi- they do not learn or perform everything that they
bition and disinhibition typically involve actions observe. Rather, they attempt to learn those actions
that have moral or legal implications (e.g., breaking that they believe will lead to desirable outcomes and
rules) or involve strong emotions (e.g., fears). help them attain their goals, and they avoid those
Observational learning through modeling occurs actions that they believe will result in dissatisfying
when observers perform behaviors that they had not outcomes. As they observe the actions of others,
learned prior to exposure to the models (Bandura, people form expectations about different outcomes,
1969). Observational learning has four component which are based on their observations of models and
processes: attention, retention, production, and their own experiences. Their learning and perfor-
motivation. mances are based in part on these expectations.
Observational learning requires that observers
attend to relevant features so that they can be per- model characteristics
ceived. Certain features of models and situations Researchers have investigated the characteris-
tics of effective models. For example, perceived
similarity between models and observers can affect
Table 2.1. Types of Vicarious Processes modeling (Schunk, 1987). Similarity in important
ways serves as a source of information for determin-
• Response facilitation
ing behavioral appropriateness, forming outcome
• Inhibition/disinhibition expectations, and assessing one’s self-efficacy. Age
similarity between model and observer is impor-
• Observational learning
tant for gauging behavioral appropriateness but

 social cognitive theory and motivati on


less so for actual learning, which is enhanced more sessions. Cognitive-modeling children observed
by models whom observers believe are competent. an adult model explain and demonstrate division
When competence and age similarity do not match solution strategies while applying them to prob-
(e.g., younger model is more competent than a lems. Didactic-instruction children received written
same-age model), children are swayed more by the instructional material that explained and demon-
competent model. Peers can be effective models strated the operations. Compared with didactic
when children hold self-doubts about their learning instruction, cognitive modeling promoted division
or performance capabilities. Viewing a similar peer achievement and accuracy of perceived division
successfully perform a task may raise observers’ self- capabilities (i.e., self-efficacy was better aligned with
efficacy and motivate them to learn because they are actual skills).
apt to believe that if the model could learn, they can
as well (Schunk, 1987). Symbolic Processes
Model gender can influence modeling by convey- In addition to their capability for vicarious learn-
ing information about task appropriateness. In gen- ing, individuals possess the capacity for symbolic
eral, observing a same-gender peer model perform representations, which involve language, math-
a behavior without negative consequences conveys ematical and scientific notation, iconography, and
that the action is appropriate. Model gender is less cognition, and which help people adapt to and alter
important in learning academic skills and strategies their environments (Bandura, 1986). They use sym-
(Schunk, 1987). bolic processes to interpret actions and outcomes in
Researchers have also explored the effects of their lives and to guide their future actions. Because
exposing students to mastery and coping models of the human capacity to symbolize, people do not
(Schunk, 1987). Mastery models demonstrate fault- simply react to events in their lives but rather gen-
less performance from the outset. Coping models erate new courses of actions for solving problems.
initially have difficulty learning skills but through Symbolic processes also foster communications with
effort gradually improve their skills and eventually others (e.g., in person, on the phone, electronically,
perform as well as mastery models. Research shows in writing), which lead to further learning.
that children who have experienced previous learn-
ing difficulties may benefit more from observing Self-Regulatory Processes
coping models (Schunk & Hanson, 1985). Such Social cognitive theory assigns a prominent
children may perceive themselves as more similar in role to self-regulatory processes (Bandura, 1986;
competence to coping models, which can raise their Zimmerman, 2000). Self-regulation refers to the
self-efficacy and motivation for learning. processes that individuals use to personally activate
Viewing one’s own performances, or self-modeling, and sustain behaviors, cognitions, and affects, which
can facilitate learning and motivation. In a study by are systematically oriented toward the attainment of
Carroll and Bandura (1982), adults viewed models goals (Zimmerman, 2000). Prior to embarking on
performing a motor skill, then attempted to repro- a task, people set goals and determine which strate-
duce it. Performances of some learners were taped gies to use. They then regulate their behaviors to
and learners were allowed to watch this concurrent conform to their internal standards and goals. As
visual feedback while performing. Visual feedback they work on tasks, they assess their progress toward
given before learners had formed a mental model their goals and decide whether to continue or alter
of the skill had no effect on performance; however, their strategies. During breaks and when tasks are
once learners had formed such a mental model, the complete, they reflect on their experiences, seek-
visual feedback enhanced their production of the ing to make sense of them and to determine what
skill. The self-modeled feedback presumably helped their next steps should be. As they reflect on what
to reduce discrepancies between learners’ mental they have done, their beliefs that they have learned
models and actual performances. and made progress strengthen their self-efficacy and
Observational learning is enhanced when mod- motivate them to continue learning. We elaborate
eled displays contain explanations and demonstra- on these motivational processes next.
tions (cognitive modeling). Schunk (1981) compared
the effects of cognitive modeling with those of Motivational Processes
didactic instruction on children’s long-division The preceding sections show how observational,
self-efficacy and achievement. Children who lacked symbolic, and self-regulatory processes can have
division skills received instruction and practice over motivational effects on individuals. Among the most

s c h un k, us h er 
Table 2.2. Key Social Cognitive Motivational Processes influences the amount of effort that people expend.
In general, people work harder to attain goals per-
• Goals and self-evaluations of progress
ceived to be difficult than goals thought to be easier;
• Outcome expectations however, perceived difficulty and motivation do not
bear an unlimited positive relation to one another.
• Values
Goals that people believe are overly difficult do
• Social comparisons not motivate because people hold low self-efficacy
for attaining them. The opposite may also be true.
• Self-efficacy Although people may feel efficacious for attain-
ing goals perceived as very easy, these goals may
not motivate because people often procrastinate in
critical are goals and self-evaluations of progress, attempting them.
outcome expectations, values, social comparisons, Another distinction can be made between learn-
and self-efficacy (Table 2.2). These processes are ing and performance goals. A learning goal refers
covered in the following sections. to what knowledge, behavior, skill, or strategy stu-
dents are to acquire; a performance goal refers to
Goals and Self-Evaluations of Progress what task students are to complete. These goals can
Goals, or what people are consciously trying have differential effects on achievement behaviors
to attain, involve important symbolic and self- (Anderman & Wolters, 2006). Learning goals focus
regulatory processes that people use to instigate and students’ attention on processes and strategies that
sustain actions. Initially, people must make a com- help them acquire competence and improve their
mitment to attempt to attain goals because goals do skills. Focusing on knowledge and skill acquisition
not affect behavior without commitment (Locke motivates behavior and sustains attention to impor-
& Latham, 2002). As persons work on a task, they tant features. Students in pursuit of a learning goal
compare their current performance with their goals. are apt to feel self-efficacious for attaining it and be
Positive self-evaluations of progress strengthen self- motivated to expend effort, persist, and use effective
efficacy and sustain motivation. A perceived dis- learning strategies. Self-efficacy is substantiated as
crepancy between present performance and the goal they work toward their goal and assess their progress
may create dissatisfaction, which can raise effort. (Schunk, 1996).
Goals motivate people to expend effort necessary In contrast, performance goals focus attention
and persist at the task (Locke & Latham, 2002). on completing tasks. They may not highlight the
Greater effort and persistence typically lead to bet- importance of the processes and strategies underly-
ter performance. Goals also help to direct people’s ing task completion or raise self-efficacy for learn-
attention to relevant task features, behaviors to be ing. As students engage in a task, they may be less
performed, and possible outcomes, and they can likely to determine their progress by comparing
affect how people process information. their present and past performances. Performance
Although goals are important motivational pro- goals can lead to social comparisons with the work
cesses, their effects depend on their properties: spec- of others to determine progress. These comparisons
ificity, proximity, and difficulty. Goals that include can lower self-efficacy among students who experi-
specific performance standards are more likely to ence learning difficulties, which adversely affects
activate self-evaluations of progress and enhance motivation and learning.
motivation and learning than are general goals (e.g., Research supports these hypothesized effects
“Do your best;” Bandura, 1986). Specific goals indi- of learning and performance goals (Anderman &
cate the amount of effort needed to succeed, and Wolters, 2006). For example, Schunk (1996) con-
evaluating progress toward specific goals is straight- ducted two studies in which elementary children
forward. Goals also are distinguished by how far with low fraction skills received instruction and
they project into the future. Proximal, short-term practice on fractions over sessions. Children worked
goals enhance motivation and learning better than under conditions involving either a goal of learn-
do distant, long-term goals, because it is easier to ing how to solve problems or a performance goal
determine progress toward goals that are closer at of merely solving them. In the first study, half of
hand (Bandura & Schunk, 1981). the students in each goal condition completed
Goal difficulty, which refers to the level of task a self-evaluation at the end of each instructional
proficiency required as assessed against a standard, session in which they evaluated their progress in

 social cognitive theory and motivati on


learning to solve the types of problems covered dur- work lackadaisically or give up readily. This situa-
ing that session. The learning goal with or without tion can be demotivating; students may believe that
self-evaluation and the performance goal with self- positive outcomes will result but that they person-
evaluation led to higher motivation, self-efficacy, ally lack the self-efficacy to motivate themselves to
and achievement. In the second study, all students continue. For example, they may believe that if they
evaluated their learning progress at the end of the studied hard they would do well on the test, but
last instructional session. The learning goal led to they may doubt their self-efficacy to study hard.
higher motivation and achievement outcomes than Shell, Murphy, and Bruning (1989) obtained
did the performance goal. evidence of the influential role of outcome expec-
Schunk and Ertmer (1999) conducted two stud- tations. College students completed measures of
ies with college undergraduates as they worked on self-efficacy and outcome expectations for reading
computer projects over sessions. Students received a and writing, as well as reading and writing achieve-
goal of learning computer applications or a goal of ment tests. For the self-efficacy assessment, students
performing them. In the first study, half of the stu- judged their competencies for performing various
dents in each goal condition evaluated their learning reading and writing tasks. For the outcome expec-
progress midway through the instructional program. tation measure, students judged the importance of
The learning goal led to higher self-efficacy, self- reading and writing skills for achieving life goals,
judged progress, and self-regulatory competence such as getting a job, being financially secure, and
and strategy use. The opportunity to self-evaluate being happy.
progress promoted self-efficacy. In the second study, Self-efficacy and outcome expectations related
students in the self-evaluation condition assessed positively to achievement in both domains, although
their progress after each instructional session. Fre- the relations were stronger for reading than for writ-
quent self-evaluation produced comparable results ing. In both domains, self-efficacy related more
when linked with a learning or performance goal. strongly to achievement than did outcome expec-
These results suggest that infrequent self-evaluation tations, although the latter results were significant
of one’s progress complements learning goals, but and added to the prediction of achievement. This
multiple self-evaluations can outweigh the benefits study also found that self-efficacy and outcome
of learning goals and raise motivation and achieve- expectations in each domain related significantly to
ment outcomes. achievement in the other domain, which suggests
that improvements in students’ self-efficacy and
Outcome Expectations outcome expectations in one literacy area may gen-
Outcome expectations are beliefs about the eralize to other areas.
expected outcomes of actions. They can refer to
external outcomes, such as “If I study hard, I should Values
do well on the test.” They also can refer to inter- Values are individuals’ perceptions of the impor-
nal outcomes (e.g., “If I study hard, I will feel good tance and utility of learning and acting in given ways.
about myself ”), and to progress in learning (e.g., The role of values in motivation has been explored
“If I study hard, I will learn more”). People form extensively by achievement motivation researchers
outcome expectations about the likely consequences (Eccles, 2005; Wigfield & Eccles, 2002; Wigfield,
of given actions based on personal experiences and Tonks, & Eccles, 2004). Values enter prominently
observations of models (Bandura, 1986; Schunk & in a social cognitive account of motivation (Ban-
Zimmerman, 2006). Outcome expectations are a dura, 1997). People who value attaining a sense
source of motivation. Individuals act in ways they of agency believe that they can exert a significant
believe they will be successful and attend to models degree of control over important elements in their
whom they believe will teach them valued skills. lives and are motivated to do so.
Outcome expectations can sustain behaviors over Individuals act in ways to bring about the out-
long periods when people believe their actions will comes they value and avoid actions leading to out-
eventually produce desired outcomes. Students who comes that are inconsistent with their values. They
hold a sense of self-efficacy for succeeding and believe are motivated to learn when they deem that learn-
that their actions will result in positive outcomes are ing in a given area is important. Students who value
motivated to continue working even when progress mathematics may do so for various reasons, such as
occurs slowly. Conversely, those whose self-efficacy because they want to become mathematics teachers
is weaker may, when they encounter difficulties, or because they believe that mathematics has many

s c h un k, us h er 
uses in everyday life. Valuing mathematics may lead to the best learning. Schunk (1983b) showed that
them to take more mathematics courses and expend difficult goals raised children’s academic motivation
greater effort to succeed. more than easier goals, that persuasive self-efficacy
Investigations by achievement motivation feedback (e.g., “You can work 25 problems”) raised
researchers have shown that values and expectancy self-efficacy more than feedback indicating how
beliefs such as self-efficacy relate positively to stu- children’s performances compared to those of peers,
dents’ achievement. When both expectancy beliefs and that difficult goals plus persuasive feedback led
and values are used to predict achievement, expec- to the highest achievement.
tancy beliefs are significant predictors, whereas val- The effects of social comparisons on self-efficacy
ues are not. In contrast, values are better predictors and motivation depend on the abilities of the com-
of students’ intentions to take future courses and parison peers. Guay, Boivin, and Hodges (1999)
actual enrollment in those courses than are expec- found that the relation between children’s perceived
tancy beliefs (Wigfield & Eccles, 2002). Thus, values competence (analogous to self-efficacy) and achieve-
seem most important as contributors to individuals’ ment was stronger when best friends’ achievement
choices, which are key motivational outcomes. was low than when it was high. Students’ social
comparisons with close friends’ achievement may
Social Comparisons make students’ own performances look worse than
Given its emphasis on learning from the social they really are. In contrast, children may assess their
environment and reciprocal interactions among capabilities more accurately when they have low-
personal, behavioral, and social/environmental vari- achieving friends because they rely less on social
ables, social cognitive theory underscores the impor- comparison and more on objective assessments of
tance of social comparisons, which refer to the process their progress and performances.
of comparing ourselves with others (Wheeler & Suls, Thus, it seems that social comparisons can
2005). Although people often compare their perfor- enhance motivation but not necessarily self-efficacy
mances with objective standards, they also socially or learning. Social comparisons that focus students
evaluate their capabilities, especially when objective on the accomplishments of similar and average
standards are unclear or unavailable. Comparisons peers imply that they, too, are average and therefore
indicating that one is improving or more competent have no reason to feel highly self-efficacious. Self-
than others can raise self-efficacy and motivation; efficacy may decline when students socially compare
comparisons that result in negative self-evaluations themselves to high-achieving peers. Self-efficacy and
can diminish these outcomes. motivation may benefit more from providing stu-
The most accurate self-evaluations arise from dents with objective information indicating that
comparisons with others whom people believe they are making learning progress without referring
are similar to themselves in the particular ability to peers’ accomplishments.
or characteristic being evaluated. The more alike
observers are to models, the greater the probability Self-Efficacy
that similar actions by observers will produce com- Self-efficacy is a critical variable affecting learn-
parable results (Schunk, 1987). Model-observer ing and motivation (Bandura, 1997). This sec-
perceived similarity in competence can improve tion discusses how individuals develop and alter
learning (Braaksma, Rijlaarsdam, & van den Bergh, their self-efficacy, the consequences of self-efficacy,
2002). Observing similar others succeed can raise research on self-efficacy in achievement situations,
observers’ self-efficacy and motivate them to try the and collective self-efficacy.
task. Similarity may be especially influential with
persons who have experienced difficulties and pos- Sources of Self-Efficacy Information
sess self-doubts about performing well. People acquire information to assess their self-
Although social comparisons can motivate indi- efficacy from four primary sources: their mastery
viduals, their effects are not automatic. Among ele- experiences (interpretations of actual performances),
mentary school children, Schunk (1983a) found that vicarious (modeled) experiences, forms of social per-
providing children with social comparative informa- suasion, and physiological indexes (Bandura, 1997;
tion about how their performances compared with Table 2.3). One’s actual performances constitute the
those of others promoted their motivation but that most reliable information because they typically are
pursuing goals enhanced their self-efficacy. Giving interpreted as tangible indicators of one’s capabilities
children both goals and comparative information led (Schunk & Pajares, 2009; Usher & Pajares, 2008b).

 social cognitive theory and motivati on


Table 2.3. Informational Sources of Self-Efficacy for superior performance among classmates might
make external attributions (e.g., “I did well because
• Mastery experiences (interpretations of actual
performances)
the test was easy”) that leave self-efficacy relatively
unchanged. Whether a vicarious experience raises
• Vicarious (modeled) experiences or lowers self-efficacy depends on the models one
selects for comparison, how similar the models are
• Forms of social persuasion
perceived to be, the models’ attitudes, and dispari-
• Physiological indexes ties between the observers’ and models’ achievement
and progress.
Teaching practices can also increase the frequency
Successful performances raise self-efficacy, whereas with which students compare their performances to
failures may lower it, although an occasional failure those of others. Schools create comparative struc-
or success after many successes or failures should tures when they group students according to aca-
not have much impact. demic ability levels as measured by achievement test
The influence of actual performances on self- scores or similar criteria. Such practices can send
efficacy depends on numerous circumstantial factors students a public message of their (in)efficacy. And
such as task difficulty, effort expended, aid received, because exposure to multiple skilled models sustains
and preconceptions of one’s capabilities (Bandura, learning self-efficacy, students who find themselves
1997). Consequently, the cognitive interpretations among highly talented peers may reap long-term
of the results of one’s actions, not the actions them- self-efficacy benefits, whereas those surrounded by
selves, determine the influence of past performances less-skilled peers may harbor similar self-doubts.
on efficacy judgments. For example, meeting the Students who internalize personal standards may
minimum requirements for passing geometry may be less prone to making unfavorable comparisons
not boost the mathematics self-efficacy of a student (Pajares, 2006).
who holds extraordinarily high personal standards Because of the human capacity for symbolism
and who longs to be an engineer. For another stu- and forethought, people are capable of cognitively
dent, whose values and interests lie elsewhere, an generating events that can serve as guides for action.
average performance in geometry may boost self- Students are, therefore, partial creators of their mod-
efficacy and lead to continued motivation in math- eled experiences. Through cognitive self-modeling,
ematics classes. Failures can also serve an important people are able to visualize themselves confronting
function when they lead to better strategies that and overcoming challenges (Bandura, 1997). Envi-
make self-efficacy more robust. sioning one’s academic success can raise self-efficacy,
Individuals acquire much information about whereas imagining oneself failing lowers self-efficacy
their capabilities through social comparisons with and can ensure the feared failure. The fact that this
others (Bandura, 1997). Similarity to others is a mode of cognitive influence has not been extensively
cue for gauging one’s self-efficacy (Schunk, 1987). examined need not suggest its impotence in chang-
Observing similar others succeed can raise observ- ing self-efficacy. As William James remarked over a
ers’ self-efficacy and motivation when they believe century ago, “The reaction due to things of thought
that if others can perform well, they can too. But is notoriously in many cases as strong as that due to
a vicarious increase in self-efficacy can be negated sensible presences. It may be even stronger” (James,
by subsequent failure. Persons who observe similar 1905, p. 53).
peers fail may believe they lack the competence to The third source of self-efficacy information on
succeed, which can negatively affect motivation. which individuals rely comes from the persuasive
In their daily school environments, students messages of others (e.g., “I know you can do this”;
likely compare themselves to particular classmates Bandura, 1997). But social persuasions must be
who are engaged in similar learning activities. Sur- credible for people to believe that success is attain-
passing one’s peers builds self-efficacy, whereas infe- able. Although positive feedback can raise individu-
rior performances lower it. Despite these tendencies, als’ self-efficacy, the increase will not endure if they
the influence of peer models on one’s self-efficacy subsequently perform poorly (Schunk & Pajares,
cannot be reliably predetermined. A high-achieving, 2009). Factors that influence the persuasory punch
competitive student might get a self-efficacy boost of a message include source credibility, valence of the
from being outperformed by a classmate (Usher, message, and frequency. A youngster’s self-efficacy is
2009). On the other hand, a student who stands out likely to suffer more from disparaging remarks than

s c h un k, us h er 
from positive ones (Bandura, 1997). Students who The influence of these sources of self-efficacy
hear frequent messages from multiple sources that might also be multiplicative, in that two sources
they are incapable may come to believe that to be combine interactively. Students who have had few
the case. mastery experiences in a given domain may be more
People are more likely to attend to social mes- likely to rely on what others tell them than would
sages about their capabilities when they lack ade- students who have had ample opportunities for mas-
quate knowledge of what is required to succeed tery (Usher, 2009). Beliefs in one’s personal efficacy
in a particular domain. To be most effective and for learning might also follow a transformational
motivating, persuasive messages from others must experience. A meaningful individual encounter
be matched to the individuals’ current skill level. with a caring teacher might have a more profound
Students are quick to dismiss lofty praise or empty influence on one’s self-efficacy than a year’s worth of
inspirational mantras. Those who are most skilled at school. A disparaging remark can also leave a last-
building students’ self-efficacy couple positive feed- ing bruise on one’s sense of efficacy and undermine
back about students’ capabilities with scaffolded subsequent motivation. We now turn to the many
tasks that build mastery (Evans, 1989). outcomes that are influenced by these important
Individuals also can acquire self-efficacy infor- self-beliefs.
mation from physiological and emotional reactions
such as anxiety and stress (Bandura, 1997). Strong Effects of Self-Efficacy
emotional reactions provide cues about anticipated Within a social cognitive system of triadic recip-
success or failure. For example, a student who feels a rocality, self-efficacy is hypothesized to influence
crippling fear when heading to advanced chemistry behaviors and environments and in turn be affected
may interpret that fear as a sign of personal inef- by them (Bandura, 1986, 1997). Self-efficacy exerts
ficacy. When people experience negative thoughts its influence through cognitive, motivational, affec-
and fears about their capabilities (e.g., feeling ner- tive, and selection processes. Students who feel effi-
vous when thinking about taking a test), those affec- cacious about learning should engage in thoughts
tive reactions can lower self-efficacy. On the other and actions that improve their learning, such as
hand, positive affect or excitement in learning can setting goals, using effective learning strategies,
motivate. A student who feels energized by chal- monitoring their comprehension, evaluating their
lenging academic work likely enjoys a sense of self- goal progress, and creating effective environments
efficacy for succeeding. When people notice their for learning. In turn, self-efficacy is influenced by
stress abating (e.g., feeling less anxious while taking the outcomes of one’s behaviors (e.g., goal progress,
a test), they may experience higher self-efficacy for achievement) and by input from one’s environment
performing well. (e.g., feedback from teachers, social comparisons
As we noted earlier, informational sources related with peers; Schunk & Pajares, 2009).
to one’s abilities do not affect self-efficacy automati- Despite its benefits, self-efficacy is not the only
cally (Bandura, 1997). Individuals interpret the influence on behavior. No amount of self-efficacy
results of events, and these interpretations provide will produce a competent performance when indi-
the impetus for upward or downward shifts in viduals lack the needed skills to succeed (Schunk &
one’s self-efficacy (Schunk & Pajares, 2009). People Pajares, 2009). Discussed earlier was the impor-
weigh and combine information from the various tance of other motivating factors such as outcome
sources to form self-efficacy judgments. Many fac- expectations and values (Bandura, 1997; Wigfield
tors influence the ways in which students interpret et al., 2004). Even learners who feel highly effi-
and integrate this information when forming their cacious about their mathematical skills will not
self-efficacy and motivation-related beliefs. For some become mathematics majors in college if they do
individuals, the accumulation of informational not value a career as a mathematician, and they
sources enhances self-efficacy. Other people tend typically engage in activities that they believe will
to rely on information from one source more than result in positive outcomes and avoid actions that
from others. For example, in a study of the sources they believe may lead to negative outcomes. None-
of academic self-efficacy among middle school stu- theless, given requisite skills, positive values, and
dents, girls and African American students seemed outcome expectations, self-efficacy is a key deter-
more attuned to social persuasions when forming minant of individuals’ motivation, learning, self-
their self-efficacy than did boys and White students regulation, and achievement (Schunk & Pajares,
(Usher & Pajares, 2006). 2009).

 social cognitive theory and motivati on


Self-efficacy can have diverse effects in achieve- self-regulation, and life decisions in diverse contexts
ment contexts (Bandura, 1997; Pajares, 1996; (Bandura, 1997; Klassen & Usher, 2010; Multon,
Schunk & Pajares, 2009; Table 2.4). It can influ- Brown, & Lent, 1991; Pajares, 1997; Schunk &
ence various motivational outcomes, including Pajares, 2009; Stajkovic & Luthans, 1998).
choice of tasks, effort, and persistence. Individuals For example, much research shows that self-
are apt to select tasks and activities at which they efficacy correlates with motivation, learning, and
feel competent and avoid those at which they do achievement (Schunk & Pajares, 2009). Using
not. Self-efficacy can affect how much effort people meta-analysis, Multon et al. (1991) found that self-
expend on an activity, how long they persist when efficacy was related to academic performance and
they encounter difficulties, and their levels of learn- accounted for 14% of the variance. Stajkovic and
ing and achievement. People with high self-efficacy Luthans (1998) found that self-efficacy resulted in a
tend to set challenging goals, work diligently, per- 28% gain in work performance. Using path analy-
sist in the face of failure, and recover their sense sis, Schunk (1981) found that self-efficacy exerted a
of efficacy after setbacks. As a consequence, they direct effect on children’s mathematics achievement
develop competence. On the other hand, those and persistence. Pajares and Kranzler (1995) found
with low self-efficacy may set easier goals, expend that mathematics self-efficacy had a direct effect on
little effort to succeed, give up readily when they mathematics performance and mediated the influ-
experience difficulties, and feel dejected after they ence of mental ability. Self-efficacy for self-regulated
encounter failure, all of which negatively affect skill learning also predicts academic motivation, achieve-
acquisition. ment, and continuation in school (Caprara et al.,
Self-efficacy also influences one’s level of self- 2008; Usher & Pajares, 2008a).
regulation (Schunk & Pajares, 2009; Zimmerman Self-efficacy relates not only to task choice but
& Cleary, 2009). Those with higher self-efficacy for also to career choice (Betz & Hackett, 1983).
learning set challenging goals, employ what they Social cognitive career theorists have demonstrated
believe are effective strategies, self-monitor their that basic social cognitive variables, including self-
learning goal progress, make strategy adjustments efficacy, outcome expectations, and goals, help
and seek help as needed, and create an effective explain career decision making and development
work environment. As formal and informal learn- (Brown & Lent, 2006). As learners grow and are
ing environments become increasingly technologi- given more choices over their decisions and activi-
cal, one’s capabilities to minimize distractions and ties, they are apt to select activities that involve
find reliable information are at a premium. In turn, capabilities they believe they can develop and turn
these activities result in better performance and away from areas in which they have doubts. The
higher self-efficacy for continued improvement. We influence of self-efficacy on career development is
next highlight some specific research findings on the partly mediated by perceived effort and persistence
effects and sources of self-efficacy. (Schunk & Pajares, 2009).
The relation of self-efficacy to effort and persis-
Research Evidence tence is not always linear. In novel learning situ-
Researchers have explored the operation of self- ations, students initially do not possess skills and
efficacy in various domains (e.g., education, health, must expend effort and persist to succeed. As skills
business) and among individuals differing in age, develop, however, students should be able to attain
developmental level, and cultural background. This the same level of performance with less effort in
research has shown that self-efficacy is a strong a shorter time. When this does not happen, self-
predictor of individuals’ motivation, achievement, efficacy may decline. Thus, if an advanced student
believes she is capable in science but suddenly must
exert a herculean effort to pass physics, she might
Table 2.4. Effects of Self-Efficacy
begin to rethink her pursuit of a science-related
• Motivational outcomes (task choices, effort, career.
persistence) In addition to documenting the effects of self-
efficacy, researchers have examined the influence of
• Learning
the four hypothesized sources on self-efficacy devel-
• Achievement opment. Mastery experiences have been shown to
be the most powerful and consistent predictor of
• Self-regulation
self-efficacy across academic domains and age levels

s c h un k, us h er 
(Usher & Pajares, 2008b). Scaffolding instruction loosely knit, collective efficacy beliefs may not pre-
to provide for frequent successes offers learners dict outcomes well; rather, individual self-efficacy
many opportunities to build a sense of self-efficacy may be a better predictor. Conversely, in more tightly
in their capabilities. The relative predictive power knit units—such as many elementary schools—the
of the other three sources has been variable across collective efficacy beliefs of teachers may be a better
studies. For example, in their study of the sources predictor of the efforts of the faculty as a whole to
of academic and self-regulatory efficacy beliefs of affect student learning (Henson, 2002).
sixth-grade students, Usher and Pajares (2006) The same four sources are important for the
found that girls and African American students development of collective self-efficacy: performance
relied on the social persuasions of others when attainments, vicarious experiences, social persua-
forming their confidence, whereas this source was sion, and physiological indicators. Group members
not a significant predictor of boys’ or White stu- rely on what they know about the capabilities of
dents’ self-efficacy. Klassen (2004) also found that each group member, as well as the group’s collective
Indo-Canadian students reported greater reliance capacity, when evaluating what they can do together
on vicarious experiences and social persuasions than (Bandura, 1997). When members work together
did Anglo-Canadian students. Investigating the successfully to implement changes, learn from one
importance of social messages, whether transmitted another and from other successful groups, receive
through vicarious enactment or verbal persuasion, encouragement for change from supervisors and
for various groups of learners remains an impor- others, and work together to cope with difficulties
tant area of inquiry for understanding how efficacy and alleviate stress, their beliefs about what they can
beliefs take root. do will be raised (Goddard, Hoy, & Woolfolk Hoy,
Experimental research has shown that instruc- 2004). Individuals who believe in their group’s col-
tional and social processes that convey information lective capabilities will be more motivated to work
to students that they are making learning progress on the group’s behalf, implement innovative ideas,
and becoming more competent raise self-efficacy, and enact systemic change.
motivation, and achievement (Schunk & Pajares, Educational research shows that collective self-
2009). Other instructional strategies for building efficacy is important for teachers’ job satisfaction and
students’ self-efficacy include having students pur- motivation to remain in teaching. Researchers have
sue proximal and specific goals, using social models found that teachers’ collective self-efficacy bears a sig-
in instruction, providing social comparative infor- nificant and positive relation to their job satisfaction
mation indicating competence, and having students in various contexts (Caprara, Barbaranelli, Borgogni,
self-monitor and evaluate their learning progress & Steca; 2003; Klassen, Usher, & Bong, 2010). Rel-
(Schunk & Ertmer, 2000). A noncompetitive class- atively less research attention has been given to the
room climate can lower students’ anxiety, which can collective efficacy beliefs of students. Klassen and
lead to a more favorable evaluation of their own Krawchuk (2009) showed that the collective efficacy
capabilities (Bandura, 1997). beliefs of early adolescents working in small groups
became more strongly related to the group’s success on
Collective Efficacy an interdependent task over time. Perceived collective
Researchers have explored the operation of col- efficacy also depends on group members believing that
lective efficacy beliefs, or individuals’ beliefs about others are working on their behalf. Consistent with
their collective capabilities to learn or produce Bandura’s (1997) contention, however, even high self-
desired actions (Bandura, 1997). Collective efficacy efficacy will not lead to performance changes unless
perceptions are not simply the sum or average of the the environment in which groups function provides
self-efficacy of individual group members; rather, appropriate avenues for success.
they reflect individuals’ perceptions of the capabili-
ties of the group as a whole. In educational settings, Future Directions
collective teacher efficacy denotes the perceptions of Social cognitive theory offers a viable account
the faculty as a whole to influence student outcomes of motivation, and researchers continue to test its
(Goddard, Hoy, & Woolfolk Hoy, 2000; Henson, predictions. But there remain several questions that
2002). should be addressed by investigators. In particular,
The role of collective efficacy beliefs on group research is recommended on the benefits of mod-
motivation may depend on the level of organiza- eled observations, developmental appropriateness,
tional coupling (Henson, 2002). In units that are and cross-cultural relevance.

 social cognitive theory and motivati on


Benefits of Modeled Observations perceptions of the present situation. Such complex
Individuals learn new skills and strategies by cognitive processing may be beyond the capabilities
observing models. Modeled observations also moti- of young children, which can diminish the predic-
vate observers to improve their skills. In educational tive utility of self-efficacy. Furthermore, mentally
settings, use of models is apt to save instructional processing information conveyed by models can be
time as teachers or others can explain and demon- complex, as when models demonstrate problem-
strate skills and strategies to be acquired. This also solving strategies.
prevents students from learning inaccurately, as Children can learn from observing models and
might happen if there were less structure. make reasonably valid self-efficacy judgments (Ban-
Although models are important, their effects on dura, 1986). Models for children are effective when
students’ self-efficacy and motivation are weaker than their explanations and demonstrations are brief and
are those resulting from actual performance accom- restricted to specific skills. Self-efficacy assessments
plishments. Researchers might explore how best typically contain a restricted range of choices, and
to combine modeled demonstrations with learner children are given practice to ensure that they under-
practice to optimize motivational effects. In some stand the nature of the judgment process. Thus,
situations, relatively little practice may be needed, although social cognitive principles are assumed to
but more is likely when skills to be learned are com- apply to learners at different developmental levels,
plex. Such research would contribute to clarifying researchers might explore what constraints develop-
how learners weigh and combine sources of self- mental factors place on applying these principles.
efficacy information to arrive at self-efficacy judg- Longitudinal designs that track changes in
ments. For example, how is self-efficacy affected if learners’ self-efficacy and motivation over time
models perform successfully but students then have would help researchers understand the influence of
difficulty when they practice? This type of research developmental stages on efficacy appraisals. Such
also would have instructional implications because designs could also target changes in the efficacy-
it would suggest ways to effectively use instructional related information that students perceive. Multi-
time to promote self-efficacy and motivation. level modeling techniques could help document
Technological innovations might facilitate this how self-efficacy levels fluctuate among groups of
line of research. Computers and handheld devices students (e.g., between grades 3 and 5) and could
make it possible for modeled experiences to be at examine predictors of individual students’ self-
students’ fingertips. For example, researchers could efficacy growth trajectories. Despite the costs of
use video recordings of models at varying skills lev- conducting longitudinal research, such designs will
els to examine their influence on a diverse group be able to clarify important questions related to
of learners. An experimental design would enable developmental shifts in the sources and effects of
varying of model characteristics such as similarity, self-efficacy that have not been clearly answered by
proficiency, and degree of shared coping. Video cross-sectional designs.
playback of one’s own performances could enhance
students’ ability for cognitive self-modeling as well. Cross-Cultural Relevance
The changing nature and availability of technology Pajares (2007) called for a careful consideration
make possible new and diverse modeling opportu- of cultural context in the investigation of academic
nities. If such videos of modeled skills prove to be motivation in general and self-efficacy in particu-
effective, teachers could benefit from developing lar. Because the relation between self-efficacy, other
their own library of vicarious experiences for their motivation variables, and achievement varies in
students. important ways across cultural groups, research-
ers should use caution when generalizing research
Developmental Appropriateness results to other contexts. As Pajares (2007) noted,
Social cognitive theory emphasizes complex “Research findings must be carefully understood as
interactions among personal, social/environmen- being bounded by a host of situated factors” (p. 30),
tal, and behavioral factors. This complexity leads which limit what is known about a given variable.
to questions about the applicability of the theory Most social cognitive research relevant to moti-
to learners of all ages and developmental levels. vation has been conducted with individuals in
For example, when assessing self-efficacy, individu- Western societies (Klassen & Usher, 2010). Fortu-
als must weigh and combine information from the nately this situation is changing as researchers are
environment, their prior experiences, and their testing social cognitive ideas in settings globally.

s c h un k, us h er 
As a research topic, self-efficacy has much interna- based college majors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23,
tional appeal, with the resulting increase in research 329–345.
Braaksma, M. A. H., Rijlaarsdam, G., & van den Bergh,
in different cultures. While cross-cultural research H. (2002). Observational learning and the effects of model-
has yielded differences (McInerney, 2008), overall observer similarity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94,
the principles espoused by social cognitive theory 405–415.
have shown themselves to be cross-culturally rel- Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (2006). Preparing adolescents to
evant. Additional investigations will determine make career decisions: A social cognitive perspective. In
F. Pajares & T. Urdan (Eds.), Adolescence and education,
whether the motivational processes postulated Vol. 5: Self-efficacy beliefs of adolescents (pp. 201–223). Green-
by social cognitive theory operate consistently in wich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
diverse societies. Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., Borgogni, L., & Steca, P. (2003).
Efficacy beliefs as determinants of teachers’ job satisfaction.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 821–832.
Conclusion Caprara, G. V., Fida, R., Vecchione, M., Del Bove, G., Vecchio,
Social cognitive theory stresses learning from G. M., Barbaranelli, C., & Bandura, A. (2008). Longitudi-
the social environment. The conceptual focus of nal analysis of the role of perceived efficacy for self-regulated
Bandura’s (1986) social cognitive theory postulates learning in academic continuance and achievement. Journal
reciprocal interactions among personal, behavioral, of Educational Psychology, 100, 525–534.
Carroll, W. R., & Bandura, A. (1982). The role of visual moni-
and social/environmental factors. Social cognitive toring in observational learning of action patterns: Making
researchers have investigated the operation of vicari- the unobservable observable. Journal of Motor Behavior, 14,
ous, symbolic, and self-regulatory processes, in the 153–167.
various ways that individuals interact with their Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The Chameleon Effect:
environments and one another. The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.
A key point underlying social cognitive theory Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task value and the Eccles et al.
is that persons are motivated to develop a sense of model of achievement-related choices. In A. J. Elliot & C. S.
agency for being able to exert a large degree of con- Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp.
trol over important events in their lives. Among the 105–121). New York: Guilford Press.
influential variables affecting motivation are goals Evans, R. I. (1989). Albert Bandura: The man and his ideas—a
dialogue. New York: Praeger.
and self-evaluations of progress, outcome expecta- Goddard, R. D., Hoy, W. K., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2000). Col-
tions, values, social comparisons, and self-efficacy. lective teacher efficacy: Its meaning, measure, and impact on
Important questions remain to be addressed by student achievement. American Educational Research Journal,
researchers, which will further refine social cognitive 37, 479–507.
theory and expand its applicability to motivation. Goddard, R. D., Hoy, W. K., & Woolfolk Hoy, A. (2004). Col-
lective efficacy beliefs: Theoretical developments, empiri-
cal evidence, and future directions. Educational Researcher,
References 33(3), 3–13.
Anderman, E. M., & Wolters, C. A. (2006). Goals, values, and Guay, F., Boivin, M., & Hodges, E. V. E. (1999). Social compari-
affects: Influences on student motivation. In P. A. Alexander son processes and academic achievement: The dependence of
& P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology the development of self-evaluations on friends’ performance.
(2nd ed., pp. 369–389). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 564–568.
Bandura, A. (1969). Principles of behavior modification. New York: Henson, R. K. (2002). From adolescent angst to adulthood:
Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Substantive implications and measurement dilemmas in the
Bandura, A. (1977a). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of development of teacher efficacy research. Educational Psy-
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. chologist, 37, 137–150.
Bandura, A. (1977b). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, James, W. (1905). Varieties of religious experience. New York:
NJ: Prentice Hall. Longmans, Green, and Co.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: Klassen, R. M. (2004). A cross-cultural investigation of the
A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice efficacy beliefs of South Asian immigrant and Anglo non-
Hall. immigrant early adolescents. Journal of Educational Psychol-
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New York: ogy, 96, 731–742.
Freeman. Klassen, R. M., & Krawchuk, L. L. (2009). Collective motiva-
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspec- tion beliefs of early adolescents working in small groups.
tive. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26. Journal of School Psychology, 47, 101–120.
Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence, Klassen, R. M., & Usher, E. L. (2010). Self-efficacy in educa-
self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self- tional settings: Recent research and emerging directions. In
motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, T. C. Urdan & S. A. Karabenick (Eds.), Advances in motiva-
586–598. tion and achievement: Vol. 16A. The decade ahead: Theoretical
Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1983). The relationship of math- perspectives on motivation and achievement (pp. 1–33). Bing-
ematics self-efficacy expectations to the selection of science- ley, UK: Emerald Publishing Group.

 social cognitive theory and motivati on


Klassen, R. M., Usher, E. L., & Bong, M. (2010). Teachers’ Schunk, D. H., & Hanson, A. R. (1985). Peer models: Influence
collective efficacy, job satisfaction, and job stress in cross- on children’s self-efficacy and achievement. Journal of Educa-
cultural context. Journal of Experimental Education, 78, tional Psychology, 77, 313–322.
464–486. Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2005). Competence perceptions
Licht, B. G., & Kistner, J. A. (1986). Motivational problems of and academic functioning. In J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.),
learning-disabled children: Individual differences and their Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 85–104). New
implications for treatment. In J. K. Torgesen & B. W. L. Wong York: Guilford Press.
(Eds.), Psychological and educational perspectives on learning Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2009). Self-efficacy theory. In
disabilities (pp. 225–255). Orlando: Academic Press. K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically at school (pp. 35–53). New York: Routledge.
useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year Schunk, D. H., Pintrich, P. R., & Meece, J. L. (2008). Motiva-
odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. tion in education: Theory, research, and applications (3rd ed.).
Matlin, M. W. (2009). Cognition (7th ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education.
Wiley. Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2006). Competence
McInerney, D. M. (2008). The motivational roles of cultural and control beliefs: Distinguishing the means and ends. In
differences and cultural identity in self-regulated learning. P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educa-
In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and tional psychology (2nd ed., pp. 349–367). Mahwah, NJ: Erl-
self-regulated learning: Theory, research, and applications (pp. baum.
369–400). New York: Taylor & Francis. Shell, D. F., Murphy, C. C., & Bruning, R. H. (1989). Self-
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation efficacy and outcome expectancy mechanisms in reading and
of self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes: A meta-analytic writing achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81,
investigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 30–38. 91–100.
Pajares, F. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefs in achievement settings. Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. (1998). Self-efficacy and work-
Review of Educational Research, 66, 543–578. related performances: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin,
Pajares, F. (1997). Current directions in self-efficacy research. In 124, 240–261.
M. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and Usher, E. L. (2009). Sources of middle school students’ self-
achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 1–49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. efficacy in mathematics: A qualitative investigation of stu-
Pajares, F. (2006). Self-efficacy beliefs during adolescence: Impli- dent, teacher, and parent perspectives. American Educational
cations for teachers and parents. In F. Pajares & T. Urdan Research Journal, 46, 275–314.
(Eds.), Adolescence and education, Vol. 5: Self-efficacy beliefs Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2006). Sources of academic and self-
of adolescents (pp. 339–367). Greenwich, CT: Information regulatory efficacy beliefs of entering middle school students.
Age Publishing. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 31, 125–141.
Pajares, F. (2007). Culturalizing educational psychology. In Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008a). Self-efficacy for self-regulated
F. Salili & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Culture, motivation, and learn- learning: A validation study. Educational and Psychological
ing (pp. 19–42). Charlotte, NC: Information Age. Measurement, 68, 443–463.
Pajares, F., & Kranzler, J. (1995). Self-efficacy beliefs and general Usher, E. L., & Pajares, F. (2008b). Sources of self-efficacy in
mental ability in mathematical problem-solving. Contempo- school: Critical review of the literature and future directions.
rary Educational Psychology, 20, 426–443. Review of Educational Research, 78, 751–796.
Schunk, D. H. (1981). Modeling and attributional effects on Wheeler, L., & Suls, J. (2005). Social comparison and self-
children’s achievement: A self-efficacy analysis. Journal of evaluations of competence. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck
Educational Psychology, 73, 93–105. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp.
Schunk, D. H. (1983a). Developing children’s self-efficacy and 566–578). New York: Guilford Press.
skills: The roles of social comparative information and goal Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of
setting. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 76–86. competence beliefs, expectancies for success, and achieve-
Schunk, D. H. (1983b). Goal difficulty and attainment infor- ment values from childhood through adolescence. In A.
mation: Effects on children’s achievement behaviors. Human Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achieve-
Learning, 2, 107–117. ment motivation (pp. 91–120). San Diego, CA: Academic
Schunk, D. H. (1987). Peer models and children’s behavioral Press.
change. Review of Educational Research, 57, 149–174. Wigfield, A., Tonks, S., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Expectancy value
Schunk, D. H. (1996). Goal and self-evaluative influences dur- theory in cross-cultural perspective. In D. M. McInerney
ing children’s cognitive skill learning. American Educational & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Big theories revisited (pp. 165–198).
Research Journal, 33, 359–382 Greenwich, CT: Information Age Publishing.
Schunk, D. H., & Ertmer, P. A. (1999). Self-regulatory processes Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social
during computer skill acquisition: Goal and self-evaluative cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, &
influences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 251–260. M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13–39).
Schunk, D. H., & Ertmer, P. A. (2000). Self-regulation and San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
academic learning: Self-efficacy enhancing interventions. In Zimmerman, B. J., & Cleary, T. J. (2009). Motives to self-
M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Hand- regulate learning: A social cognitive account. In K. R. Went-
book of self-regulation (pp. 631–649). San Diego, CA: Aca- zel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school
demic Press. (pp. 247–264). New York: Routledge.

s c h un k, us h er 
CHAPTER

Cybernetic Control Processes and the


3 Self-Regulation of Behavior

Charles S. Carver and Michael F. Scheier

Abstract
This chapter describes a set of ideas bearing on the self-regulation of action and emotion that has been
given labels such as cybernetic and feedback control processes. The ideas have roots in many sources,
including the concept of homeostasis and attempts to create mechanical devices to serve as governors
for engines. With respect to motivation, these ideas yield a viewpoint in which goal-directed action is
seen as reflecting a hierarchy of feedback control processes and the creation and reduction of affect are
seen as reflecting another set of feedback processes. The portion of the model devoted to affect is of
particular interest in that it generates two predictions that differ substantially from those deriving from
other theories. The first is that both approach and avoidance can give rise to both positive and negative
feelings; the second is that positive affect leads to coasting, reduction in effort regarding the goal under
pursuit. The latter suggests a way in which positive affect is involved in priority management when
many goals are in existence at the same time. Recent interest in dual-process models, which distinguish
between top-down goal pursuit and reflexive responses to cues of the moment, has caused us to
reexamine some of our previous assumptions and to consider the possibility that behavior is triggered in
two distinct ways. The chapter closes with a brief consideration of how these ideas might be compatible
with other viewpoints on motivation.
Key Words: cybernetic, feedback loop, control theory, affect

This chapter describes several aspects of a view- systems) regulate some current condition so as to
point on the guidance of behavior that we have used stay “on course.” The idea that such systems under-
throughout our careers in psychology. This view- lie overt, intentional behavior as well as homeostatic
point has roots in several places. One of them is self-regulation is the theme of this chapter. We
the broad conception of homeostatic mechanisms, amplify on this idea shortly, but first we’ll provide a
mechanisms that regulate diverse aspects of the little more background.
body’s physiological functioning (Cannon, 1932). Cybernetic ideas had a brief heyday in motiva-
Another source is ideas about mechanical gover- tional psychology (broadly defined) in the 1950s
nors and computing machines (e.g., Ashby, 1940; through 1970s. Probably the best known example
Rosenblueth, Wiener, & Bigelow, 1943; Wiener, of this viewpoint was an engaging book by Miller,
1948). In the middle of the 20th century, Wiener Galanter, and Pribram (1960). This book intro-
(1948) coined the term cybernetic (from the Greek duced into the psychological lexicon the acronym
word meaning “steersman”) to characterize the over- TOTE, which stands for test-operate-test-exit, a
all functioning of this type of system. Cybernetic sequence of events that take place in a cybernetic
systems (whether mechanical, electronic, or living control system. Miller et al. were not the only


people to use cybernetic concepts during this period Feedback Control
(ideas with a similar character were proposed, for What are the elements of a cybernetic feedback
example, by MacKay, 1956, 1966; for review see control system? The term feedback control can seem
Miller et al., 1960), but Miller et al. received the quite forbidding. An easy point of entry into the
most attention from psychologists. To some extent logic behind it, however, is the goal concept, which
this may be attributable to the fact that the opera- is more intuitive. People have many goals, at varying
tion of a TOTE unit paralleled the operation of the levels of abstraction and importance. Goals energize
basic element of a computer. Computers (which and guide activities. Most goals can be reached in
were fairly new at the time) were starting to influ- many ways, leading to the potential for vast com-
ence people’s thinking about the nature of cogni- plexity in the organization of action. This is a view
tion. Thus, Miller et al.’s book was very much in the that is easy and familiar for most people, and it is
spirit of its time. part of the conceptual landscape of contemporary
Today when people use the word cybernetic, they psychology. From this view, the transition to think-
generally are referring either to robotics or to the ing about cybernetic control is relatively straight-
World Wide Web. It is also fairly common to asso- forward.
ciate the viewpoint we describe here with the disci-
pline of engineering, partly because of its heritage Feedback Processes
in devices that govern engines and partly because The basic unit of cybernetic control is the
of the usefulness of control theory in engineer- feedback loop. A feedback loop has four elements
ing applications. It’s important, though, to keep (MacKay, 1966; Miller et al., 1960; Powers, 1973;
in mind that these ideas have ties that extend well Wiener, 1948): an input function, a reference value,
beyond engineering. As noted earlier, they pertain a comparison process, and an output. Think of the
additionally to the homeostatic controllers of the input function as perception. The input function
body. They also pertain to diverse other complex brings in information of some sort about present cir-
systems in nature. cumstances. Think of the reference value as a goal. The
Thirteen years after Miller et al.’s (1960) intrigu- perceived input is compared to this value, to deter-
ing volume came another book that had a particularly mine whether a difference exists. A discrepancy that is
strong impact on our thinking. This book, written by detected by this comparison creates what is called an
William Powers (1973), was an extremely ambitious “error signal.” The output function is a response to any
undertaking. Powers set out to portray how human detected error (we treat the output here as equivalent
behavior might reflect a hierarchy of cybernetic con- to behavior, but sometimes the behavior is an internal
trol processes. That is, he tried to account for how signal rather than a physical movement).
the nervous system creates the physical movements If the comparison detects no discrepancy, the
by which intentions and even abstract values are output remains as it was. If the comparison detects
expressed in action. At center stage in his account was a discrepancy, the effect on output depends on
the feedback loop, the basic unit of cybernetic con- what kind of loop it is. There are two kinds. In a
trol. Powers set out to map several layers of postulated discrepancy-reducing loop (also called negative, for
feedback processes to aspects of the nervous system. negating), the output acts to reduce (or eliminate)
Perhaps even more than Miller et al. (1960), Powers the discrepancy. Homeostatic systems are examples
made a compelling case for the idea that the feed- of discrepancy-reducing systems. For example, if a
back construct was up to the challenge of accounting person’s internal temperature sensors detect that his
for the complexity of behavior. He focused not on body temperature is elevated above “normal,” pro-
one single loop, but on an interwoven network of cesses are engaged that serve to reduce body tem-
loops, dealing with regulation of diverse properties perature so that it returns to that reference value.
simultaneously. Specifically, sweat would be released, which cools
We adopted the Powers (1973) model as a con- the body as it evaporates. If the sensors detect a
ceptual heuristic (Carver & Scheier, 1981). It helped deviation below normal, rather than above, the out-
us interpret a literature in personality and social put would be shivering, which generates heat via
psychology in which we were immersed at the time muscle contractions.
(see Carver & Scheier, 2112). And it provided a ref- Discrepancy-enlarging feedback loops also exist,
erence point for us for the next 30 years. Indeed, in in which the output serves not to counter a discrep-
some ways it serves as the conceptual backbone of ancy but to enlarge it (these are also called positive feed-
this chapter. back loops). One might think of the reference value

c a rver, s c h ei er 
in this kind of loop as an “anti-goal.” Discrepancy- As noted earlier, discrepancy-reducing and
enlarging loops are generally believed to be less com- discrepancy-enlarging loops may work in concert,
mon in living systems than discrepancy-reducing and it is fairly easy to point to such compound
loops, because they are unstable. Unless overridden, structures in behavior. An avoidance loop tries to
they can enlarge discrepancies without end. distance from an anti-goal. But there may exist an
Some people believe that this kind of loop is approach goal that happens to be incompatible with
always problematic and dysfunctional (Powers, 1973). the anti-goal. If the person adopts that approach
Others believe that positive loops are an important goal, the tendency to avoid the anti-goal is joined
part of complex systems (DeAngelis, Post, & Travis, by the tendency to move toward the approach goal.
1986; Maruyama, 1963; McFarland, 1971), but that The approach loop pulls the behavior into its orbit.
in living systems (and other cases in which positive This pattern of dual influence describes what behav-
feedback is adaptive), the effect of this loop is limited ioral psychologists call active avoidance. In active
in some way or other. There may be a natural endpoint avoidance an organism confronting a feared stimu-
(e.g., sexual arousal prompts further increase in arousal lus picks a relatively safe location to escape to and
to the point of orgasm, which ends the increase). actively approaches that location.
Alternatively, the discrepancy-enlarging function may Social and personality psychology also have good
be constrained by a discrepancy-reducing function. examples of discrepancy-enlarging loops being con-
To put it differently, avoidance of one reference point strained by discrepancy-reducing loops. This pattern
can give way to approach of another reference point. seems represented in Higgins’s (1996) concept of
the ought self (Carver et al., 1999) and in Ryan and
Feedback Processes in Overt Behavior Deci’s (2000) concept of introjected values. In both
A cybernetic approach to motivation generalizes of these constructs, the initial impetus to behavior
these principles to behavioral goals, in which dis- is the desire to avoid social sanction of some sort.
crepancies are reduced by overt actions (Miller et al., Thus, the starting point is an effort to create dis-
1960; Powers, 1973; Toates, 2006). Negative feed- tance from an anti-goal. However, a good way to
back processes, as applied to overt behavior, represent avoid social sanction is to locate a socially approved
the engagement of effort to reach a valued goal, main- value that is different from (or even opposite to) the
tain a desired condition, or conform to some salient disapproved value, and move toward it. By homing
standard. Goal-directed behavior entails knowing in on the positive value, one simultaneously escapes
(at some level) the desired end one wants to reach, the feared or disliked value. Thus, both ought selves
knowing what the present condition is with respect and introjects represent positive values to conform
to that desired end, and being able to decide whether to, but the motivational dynamic underlying them
the present condition does or does not match the is more complex than the dynamic underlying other
desired end. It is also necessary, of course, to be able positive values.
to create actions that will cause the present condition
to change in appropriate ways. However, that ability Further Issues
would be of little help in itself if the other functions At least a couple more issues should be noted
were not also operating. before we move on. One of them concerns a common
In a way, this is the essence of what a cybernetic misconception about the nature of feedback processes.
view brings to the motivational table: It forces the The other concerns a somewhat disconcerting reality
realization that all of those functions are necessary about the nature of feedback processes.
for successful goal pursuit, not just the capacity to As was described earlier, homeostasis is a com-
act. It forces the realization that the action occurs in mon illustration of the feedback principle, because
service to changing the input (Powers, 1973). it is so easily understood. Another common illustra-
The principle of positive feedback can also be tion is the room thermostat, which senses deviations
applied to overt behavior. What might be called from a set point and engages devices that counter
“anti-goals” for behavior are conditions that one the deviations. Because of the common use of these
wants to avoid. An example would be a feared or illustrations, some people incorrectly infer that
disliked possible self (Carver, Lawrence, & Scheier, feedback loops can act only to create and maintain
1999; Markus & Nurius, 1986; Ogilvie, 1987), steady states. Some reference values (and goals) are
which one tries hard to not-be. Another example indeed static end states or stable preferred conditions
would be a scene of public humiliation, which most (e.g., to own one’s home, to arrive at the end of the
people will try to avoid. month with a balance above zero in one’s checking

 cyb ernetic control processes and th e s elf -reg ul ati on of b eh avi or


account). But other reference values are dynamic that are encountered at those points. Programs are
and evolving (e.g., experiencing the pleasures of a the level of the Powers hierarchy that most closely
month’s vacation, raising children to become good resembles Miller et al.’s TOTE construct, because
citizens). In such cases, the goal for action regula- of the sequencing of steps and subroutines that
tion is the process of traversing the changing tra- programs contain. There is some blurring between
jectory of the activity, not just the arrival at the levels, however. Programs can become quite famil-
endpoint. Feedback processes apply perfectly well iar, as a result of repetition. If they become familiar
to such moving targets (Beer, 1995). enough that they are executed all at a piece without
Although the feedback loop is an abstract con- much monitoring, they probably are no longer pro-
cept, it is not too hard to portray its elements con- grams but instead have become sequences.
ceptually. In some specific instances of feedback Programs are sometimes enacted in the service
control (e.g., in artificial electronic systems), it is of broader guiding principles. Principles are more
also easy to point to the physical existence of each abstract qualities. They can provide a basis for mak-
element. In other instances, however, doing this is ing decisions at choice points within programs,
harder. In particular, some feedback loops have no and they can suggest that particular programs be
explicit representation of a reference value. The sys- undertaken or be refrained from. The term principle
tem regulates around a value, but the value is not refers to the sorts of qualities that social psycholo-
represented anywhere as a goal (Berridge, 2004; gists often call values (Schwartz & Bilsky, 1990;
Carver & Scheier, 1999b, 2002). Schwartz & Rubel, 2005). What defines a principle
as such is its abstractness and broad applicability
Levels of Abstraction to diverse behaviors. Being a principle does not in
Let us return, though, to cases with explicit ref- itself imply anything about what behavior results.
erence points or goals, inasmuch as these cases are For example, one principle leads people to support
the focus of most of what we have to say. Goals vary affirmative action, whereas a different principle leads
quite considerably in how concrete or abstract they people to oppose it (Reyna, Henry, Korfmacher, &
are. You can have the goal of being a good citizen, Tucker, 2006).
but you can also have the goal of recycling—a nar- Even individual values are not the end of poten-
rower goal that contributes to being a good citizen. tial complexity and abstraction, though. Patterns of
To recycle entails other, more-concrete goals: plac- values coalesce to form the essence of a person’s sense
ing newspapers or bottles and cans into containers of desired (and undesired) self or a person’s sense of
and moving them to a pickup location. The fact that desired (and undesired) community. These proper-
goals have subgoals leads to the idea that goals form ties are very broad points of reference (goals).
a hierarchy (Powers, 1973; Toates, 2006; Vallacher
& Wegner, 1987). Abstract goals are attained by the Hierarchy of Processes in Action
very process of attaining concrete goals that help Powers (1973) argued that, in a hierarchical
define the abstract ones (Carver & Scheier, 1998, organization, high-level control loops “behave” by
1999a, 1999b, 2003). setting and changing reference values for loops at
Goals at different levels of abstraction have differ- the next lower level of control. Those loops, in turn
ent kinds of characterizations. Some kinds of relatively act by setting reference values for lower levels, and
low-level goals are defined by brief sequences of action: so on (Fig. 3.1). At the very lowest level, the output
for example, picking up a pen or walking across the is changes in muscle tensions. Thus, for a person to
room. Such sequences (Powers, 1973) are fairly simple act in a way that is intended to exemplify a particu-
(though each can also be broken down further into lar principle also requires the simultaneous involve-
subcomponents of motor control (e.g., Rosenbaum, ment of all layers of control lower than the principle
Meulenbroek, Vaughan, & Jansen, 2001). Sequences level.
have something of a self-contained quality about In his statement about hierarchical organiza-
them, and they require little monitoring once they are tion of feedback processes, Powers (1973) devoted
triggered. most of his attention to levels of abstraction that are
Such sequences can be organized into more elab- even more basic than sequences. As personality-
orate strings of actions, which Powers (1973) called social psychologists, we have not found those lower
programs. These strings of action are more planful. levels of much direct interest. On the other hand,
They often require choices to be made at various the argument that control of behavior relies on a
points along the way, which depend on conditions single principle instantiated at multiple levels of

c a rver, s c h ei er 
Fig. 3.1. Three-level hierarchy of Output 1 and
feedback loops. The output from Reference Reference 2 Output 2 and
the comparison in a given loop is C1 Reference 3
the reference value for the next lower C2
level, and so on. The final (motoric) C3
output creates a change in input
that is (at varying levels of
abstraction) relevant to all
Input Input Input Output
levels involved.

abstraction is a very interesting one, because it has a the incentives. Threats are avoided by systems that
high degree of parsimony. enlarge discrepancies between present conditions and
Knowledge of the nervous system has progressed the threats. The logic of feedback processes thus pro-
enormously since 1973, of course, and parts of the vides a way to think about this fundamental dichot-
picture that Powers created are doubtlessly contra- omy among motivations, a dichotomy that plays a
dicted by later evidence. However, the viability of key role in many other ideas about motivation.
the core idea that action reflects feedback processes
engaged simultaneously at multiple levels of abstrac- Feedback Processes and Affect
tion need not depend entirely on specific details. Motivation is partly about how people move
From the point of view of personality-social from one place to another. However, it is also partly
psychology, goals from the ideal self down through about the degree of urgency behind the action.
sequences can be thought of as common starting A sense of urgency or intensity implies the involve-
points for self-regulation. All of them serve as classes ment of affect, feelings that occur in the course of
of values to try to approximate or to deviate from. experience.
Any of them might be taken as the focal point for a What is affect? Where does it come from? Affect
given behavior (that is, the person could try to self- is positive or negative feelings. Affect is the core of
regulate at any of these levels). Once that value is the experience of emotion, though the term emotion
adopted, lower levels are engaged automatically by often incorporates connotations of physiological
the engagement of that one. Thus, it is easy to imag- changes that frequently accompany hedonic expe-
ine cases in which a person is behaving according to riences. A truism is that affect pertains to whether
a principle (e.g., a moral or ethical value), and it is one’s desires are being met (Clore, 1994; Frijda,
easy to imagine cases in which the person is behav- 1986, 1988; Ortony, Clore, & Collins, 1988). But
ing according to a plan or program. It is also easy, what is the internal mechanism by which feelings
however, to imagine cases in which the person is act- arise?
ing impulsively and spontaneously, without regard
to either principle or plan. In all of these cases, the Mechanism
physical movements involved are being managed Many different kinds of answers to this question
by systems automatically engaged by whichever have been offered, ranging from neurobiological
level of control is in charge. Later in the chapter we (e.g., Davidson, 1992) to cognitive (Ortony et al.,
reexamine this idea and consider some potentially 1988). We have proposed an answer that focused
important differences among these various levels of on what appear to be some of the functional proper-
abstraction. ties of affect (Carver & Scheier, 1990, 1998, 1999a,
1999b). In suggesting this answer, we used feedback
Approach and Avoidance control as an organizing principle. Now, however,
In some ways, the dual concepts of discrepancy- the control bears on a different quality.
reducing and discrepancy-enlarging loops map nicely We suggested that feelings arise as a consequence
onto the general form of approach and avoidance of a feedback loop that operates simultaneously
processes. Incentives are approached by systems that with the behavior-guiding loop and in parallel to
close discrepancies between present conditions and it. We regard its operation as automatic. The easi-

 cyb ernetic control processes and th e s elf -reg ul ati on of b eh avi or


est characterization of what this second process is positive, neutral, or negative for any given goal-
doing is that it is checking on how well the first directed action, depending on how well or poorly
process (the behavior loop) is doing. The input for the action seems to be attaining the goal.
this second loop thus is the rate of discrepancy reduc-
tion in the action system over time. (We focus first on Reference Criterion
discrepancy-reducing loops, then consider enlarg- What determines the criterion? There doubtlessly
ing loops.) are many influences. Furthermore, the orientation
Consider a physical analogy. Action implies that a person takes to an action can induce a differ-
change between states. Difference between states is ent framing that may change the criterion (Brendl
distance. The action loop thus controls the psycho- & Higgins, 1996). What is used as a criterion is
logical analog of distance. If the affect loop assesses probably quite flexible when the activity is unfamil-
the action loop’s progress, then the affect loop is iar. If the activity is very familiar, the criterion is
dealing with the psychological analog of velocity, likely to reflect the person’s accumulated experience,
the first derivative of distance over time. To the in the form of an expected rate (the more experience
degree that this analogy is meaningful, the input you have, the more you know what is reasonable to
to the affect loop should be the first derivative over expect). Whether “desired,” “expected,” or “needed”
time of the input used by the action loop. is most accurate as a depiction of the criterion rate
Input (how well you are doing) does not by may depend greatly on the context.
itself create affect; a given rate of progress has dif- The criterion can also change, sometimes readily,
ferent affective consequences in different contexts. sometimes less so. The less experience the person has
We argued that this input is compared to a reference in a domain, the easier it is to substitute one criterion
value (cf. Frijda, 1986, 1988), just as in other feed- for another. We believe, however, that change in rate
back loops. In this case, the value is an acceptable or criterion in a relatively familiar domain occurs rela-
expected rate of behavioral discrepancy reduction. tively slowly. Continuing overshoots result automat-
As in other feedback loops, the comparison checks ically in an upward drift of the criterion; continuing
for deviation from the standard. If there is a discrep- undershoots result in a downward drift (see Carver
ancy, the error signal causes a change in the output & Scheier, 2000). Thus, the system recalibrates over
function. repeated events. A (somewhat ironic) consequence
We think the error signal in this loop is manifest of such recalibration would be to keep the balance of
subjectively as affect, a sense of positive or negative a person’s affective experiences (positive to negative,
valence. A rate of progress below the criterion yields across a span of time) relatively similar, even if the
negative affect. A rate high enough to exceed the crite- rate criterion changes considerably.
rion yields positive affect. If the rate is not distinguish-
able from the criterion, there is no valence. In essence, Two Kinds of Action Loops, Two
the argument is that feelings with positive valence Dimensions of Affect
mean you are doing better at something than you So far we have addressed only approach loops.
need to, and feelings with negative valence mean you The view just outlined was that positive feeling
are doing worse than you need to (for detail see Carver exists when a behavioral system is making more
& Scheier, 1998, Chapters 8 and 9). The absence of than adequate progress doing what it is organized
affect means being neither ahead nor behind. to do. The systems addressed so far are organized
A couple of clarifications about what we do not to reduce discrepancies. Yet there seems no obvi-
mean to say here: We are not arguing for a delib- ous reason why the principle should not apply to
erative thinking through of whether rate conforms systems that enlarge discrepancies. If such a system
to the criterion rate. We assume that the testing is making rapid enough progress attaining its ends,
is continuous and automatic. Nor are we arguing there should be positive affect. If it is doing poorly,
for a deliberative thinking about what the affective there should be negative affect.
valence means. We assume that the meaning (i.e., That affects of both valences are possible seems
being ahead versus behind) is intrinsic to the affect’s applicable to both approach and avoidance. That
valence, which itself arises automatically. is, both approach and avoidance have the poten-
One implication of this line of argument is that tial to induce positive feelings (by doing well), and
the affects that might potentially exist regarding both have the potential to induce negative feelings
any given action should fall on a bipolar dimen- (by doing poorly). But doing well at approaching
sion. That is, it should be the case that affect can be an incentive is not quite the same experience as

c a rver, s c h ei er 
doing well at moving away from a threat. Thus, there urgent events. A person whose reactions are between
may be differences between the two positives, and those extremes responds quickly but without behav-
between the two negatives. ioral overreaction and oscillation.
Drawing on the work of Higgins (e.g., 1987, 1996), For biological entities, being able to respond
we have argued for two bipolar dimensions of affect, quickly yet accurately confers a clear adaptive advan-
one bearing on approach, the other on avoidance tage. We believe this combination of quick and
(Carver, 2001; Carver & Scheier, 1998). Approach- stable responding is a consequence of having both
related affect includes such positive affects as elation, behavior-managing and affect-managing control sys-
eagerness, and excitement, and also such negative tems. Affect causes people’s responses to be quicker
affects as frustration, anger, and sadness (Carver, 2004; (because this system is time sensitive); as long as the
Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009). Avoidance-related affective system is not overresponsive, the responses
affect includes such positive affects as relief, serenity, are also stable.
and contentment (Carver, 2009) and such negative Our focus here is on how affects influence behav-
affects as fear, guilt, and anxiety. ior, emphasizing the extent to which they are inter-
woven. However, note that the behavioral responses
Affect and Action: Two Facets of that are linked to the affects also lead to reduction
One Event in Time of the intensity of the affects, returning them to the
This two-layered viewpoint implies a natural set point. We thus would suggest that the affect
connection between affect and action. That is, if the system is, in a very basic sense, self-regulating (cf.
input function of the affect loop is a sensed rate of Campos, Frankel, & Camras, 2004). It is undeni-
progress in action, the output function of the affect able that people also engage in voluntary efforts to
loop must be a change in the rate of progress in that regulate their emotions (e.g., Gross, 2007; Ochsner
action. Thus, the affect loop has a direct influence & Gross, 2008), but the affect system does a good
on what occurs in the action loop. deal of that self-regulation on its own.
Some changes in rate output are straightfor-
ward. If you are lagging behind, you try harder. Affect Issues
Some changes are less straightforward. The rates of This view of affect differs from most other theo-
many “behaviors” are defined not by pace of physi- ries bearing on emotion in at least two ways. One
cal action but in terms of choices among potential difference concerns the idea of dimensional struc-
actions, or entire programs of action. For example, ture underlying affect (Carver, 2001).
increasing your rate of progress on a project at work
may mean choosing to spend a weekend working Two Underlying Bipolar Dimensions
rather than playing with family and friends. Increas- In some theories (though not all) affects are seen
ing your rate of being kind means choosing to do as having underlying dimensionality (e.g., Watson,
an act that reflects kindness, when an opportunity Wiese, Vaidya, & Tellegen, 1999). Our view has
arises. Thus, change in rate must often be translated this character. It holds that affect generated through
into other terms, such as concentration or alloca- approach has the potential to range from positive
tion of time and effort. (joy) through neutral to negative (anger, sadness);
The idea of two feedback systems functioning affect generated through avoidance also has the
jointly is something we stumbled into. It turns out, potential to range from positive (relief ) through
however, that this idea is quite common in control neutral to negative (fear, anxiety). Most dimen-
engineering (e.g., Clark, 1996). Engineers have sional models, however, are quite different from this
long recognized that having two systems function- one. They are unipolar. They ascribe affects with
ing together—one controlling position, one con- positive valence to an approach system and ascribe
trolling velocity—permits the device they control affects with negative valence to an avoidance system
to respond in a way that is both quick and stable, (e.g., Cacioppo, Gardner, & Berntson, 1999; Lang,
without overshoots and oscillations. Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990; Watson et al., 1999).
The combination of quickness and stability in There is at least some support for our view. There
responding is desirable in many of the devices engi- is evidence, albeit limited, that positive feelings of
neers deal with. It is also desirable in human beings. calmness and relief (as situationally relevant) relate
A person with very reactive emotions is prone to to avoidance motivation (Carver, 2009; Higgins,
overreact and oscillate behaviorally. A person who Shah, & Friedman, 1997). There is far more evidence
is emotionally unreactive is slow to respond even to linking sadness to failure of approach (for reviews,

 cyb ernetic control processes and th e s elf -reg ul ati on of b eh avi or


see Carver, 2004; Higgins, 1996). There is also a on other grounds by Izard (1977, p. 257; Izard &
good deal of evidence linking the approach system Ackerman, 2000, p. 258).
to the negative affect of anger (Carver & Harmon- Expending greater effort to catch up when
Jones, 2009). Although it is clear that diverse nega- behind, and coasting when ahead, are both pre-
tive feeling qualities coalesce with one another in sumed to be specific to the goal domain to which
mood states (Watson, 2009), the evidence does the affect is attached, usually the goal from which
not make that case with regard to situation-specific the affect arises in the first place. We do not argue
affective responses. that positive affect creates a tendency to coast in
This issue is important, because it has implications general, but with respect to the activity producing
for any attempt to identify a conceptual mechanism the positive feelings. We should also be clear that we
underlying creation of affect. Theories positing two are talking about the current, ongoing episode of
unipolar dimensions assume that greater activation action. We are not arguing that positive affect makes
of a system translates to more affect of that valence people less likely to do the behavior later on.
(or more potential for affect of that valence). If the Does positive affect lead to coasting? There is not
approach system relates both to positive and to nega- a great deal of evidence on this question, but there
tive feelings, however, this direct transformation of is some. To test the idea requires generating positive
system activation to affect is not tenable. A concep- affect (or creating the perception of being ahead of
tual mechanism is needed that naturally addresses one’s reference point) with respect to one behavioral
both valences within the approach function (and, domain and then measuring behavior in the same
separately, the avoidance function). The mechanism domain. Many studies have created positive affect
described here does so. in one context and assessed its influence on another
task or in another context (e.g., Isen, 1987, 2000;
Counterintuitive Effect of Positive Affect Schwarz & Bohner, 1996). However, that does not
A second issue also differentiates this model from test this question.
most other views (Carver, 2003; Carver & Scheier, We know of three sources of evidence. One study
2009). Recall our argument that affect reflects the found that professional basketball teams were more
error signal from a comparison in a feedback loop. likely to lose after a playoff victory than after a defeat
If this is so, affect is a signal to adjust rate of prog- (Mizruchi, 1991). Although this is consistent with
ress. This would be true whether the rate is above the coasting after winning, it is also highly ambiguous.
mark or below it—that is, whether affect is positive It is impossible to tell whether the pattern reflects
or negative. For negative feelings, this is fairly intui- coasting after success or renewed effort after failure
tive. The first response to negative feelings about or both. Less ambiguously, a series of three studies by
something is usually to try harder. If the person Louro, Pieters, and Zeelenberg (2007) found consis-
tries harder—and if more effort (or better effort) tent evidence that positive affect induces coasting,
increases progress—the negative affect diminishes but only when goal attainment was imminent.
or ceases. A more recent experience-sampling study had par-
For positive feelings, prediction is counterin- ticipants make a set of ratings pertaining to each of three
tuitive. In this model, positive feelings arise when goals, three times a day, for 21 days (Fulford, Johnson,
things are going better than they need to. But the Llabre, & Carver, 2010). The ratings included reports
feelings still reflect a discrepancy (albeit a positive of effort toward the goal during the previous time block,
one), and the function of a negative feedback loop perceived progress toward it during the previous time
is to keep discrepancies small. Such a system is orga- block, and expected progress in the forthcoming time
nized in such a way that it “wants” to see neither block. Multilevel modeling revealed that instances of
negative nor positive affect. Either quality (devia- progress exceeding expectation were followed by reduc-
tion from the standard in either direction) would tion in effort toward that goal in the next time period.
represent an “error” and lead to change in output Skepticism about the idea that positive affect
that would eventually reduce it. This view argues (or getting ahead) leads to coasting stems in part from
that people who exceed the criterion rate of prog- the fact that it is hard to see why a process would be
ress (and who thus have positive feelings) will auto- built into the organism that limits positive feelings—
matically tend to reduce subsequent effort in this indeed, dampens them. We see at least two bases for
domain. They will “coast” a little—ease back. This such an arrangement. The first lies in a basic biologi-
prediction derives from a consideration of feedback cal principle: It is adaptive not to spend energy need-
principles, but a similar argument has been made lessly. Coasting prevents this. Indeed, Brehm built

c a rver, s c h ei er 
a motivational theory around the argument that peo- a lower priority. Positive feelings regarding approach
ple engage only as much effort as is needed to accom- (happiness, joy) indicate that an incentive is being
plish a given task—and no more (e.g., Brehm & Self, attained and could temporarily be put on hold
1989; Wright & Kirby, 2001). because you are doing so well; thus, this goal can
A second basis for such an arrangement stems assume a lower priority (see Carver, 2003).
from the fact that people have multiple simultane-
ous concerns. Given multiple concerns, people do Priority Management and Feelings
not optimize their outcome on any one of them but of Depression
“satisfice” (Simon, 1953)—that is, they do a good One more aspect of priority management must
enough job on each concern to deal with it satisfac- be addressed, concerning the idea that some goals
torily. This permits them to handle the many con- are best abandoned. We have long held that suffi-
cerns adequately, rather than just any one of them. cient doubt about goal attainment yields a tendency
Coasting facilitates satisficing. A tendency to coast to disengage from effort, and even to disengage
with respect to some goal virtually defines satisficing from the goal itself. This is certainly a kind of pri-
regarding that particular goal. A tendency to coast ority shift, in that the abandoned goal now has an
also fosters satisficing for a broader set of goals, by even lower priority than it had before. But how does
allowing easy shift to other domains at little or no this case fit the ideas described thus far?
cost (see Carver, 2003, for detail). This case seems at first to contradict Simon’s
(1967) view that negative affect is a call for higher
Affects and Priority Management priority. But there is an important difference between
This line of argument brings up a broad function two classes of negative affect related to approach
that deserves further attention: the shifting from (Carver, 2003, 2004; in this discussion we disregard
one goal to another as focal in behavior (Dreisbach avoidance). Some of these affects coalesce around
& Goschke, 2004; Shallice, 1978). This basic and frustration and anger. Others coalesce around sad-
very important phenomenon is often overlooked. ness, depression, and dejection. The former relate to
People typically have many goals under pursuit an increase in priority, the latter to a decrease.
simultaneously, but only one has top priority at a Earlier in this section we characterized our view
given moment. People need to shield and main- as implying that approach-related affects fall on a
tain intentions that are being pursued (cf. Shah, bipolar dimension. However, the dimension is not
Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002), but they also need a simple straight line. Progress below the criterion
to be able to shift flexibly among goals (Shin & creates negative affect, as the incentive slips away.
Rosenbaum, 2002). Inadequate movement gives rise to frustration, irri-
The issue of priority management was addressed tation, and anger, prompting more effort to over-
very creatively many years ago by Simon (1967). He come obstacles and reverse the inadequate current
proposed that emotions are calls for reprioritization. progress. But efforts sometimes do not change the
He suggested that emotion arising with respect to situation. Indeed, a loss precludes movement for-
a goal that is out of awareness eventually induces ward. In this case, the feelings are sadness, depres-
people to interrupt their behavior and give that goal sion, despondency, and hopelessness. Behaviors also
a higher priority than it had. The stronger the emo- differ in this case. The person tends to disengage
tion, the stronger is the claim that the unattended from—give up on—further effort.
goal should have higher priority than the goal that In the first case, feelings of frustration and anger
is presently focal. are a call for an upgrade in priority, an increase in
Simon’s discussion focused on cases in which a effort, a struggle to gain the incentive despite set-
nonfocal goal demands a higher priority and intrudes backs. In the second case, feelings of sadness and
on awareness. By strong implication, his discussion depression accompany reduction of effort and a
dealt only with negative affect. However, there is downgrade in priority. As described earlier, both the
another way for priority ordering to shift: The focal upgrade and the downgrade have adaptive functions
goal can relinquish its place. Perhaps positive feel- in the appropriate situations.
ings also pertain to reprioritization, but rather than
a call for higher priority, they reflect reduction in pri- Shifts in the Theoretical Landscape:
ority. Positive affect regarding avoidance (relief or Two Modes of Functioning
tranquility) indicates that a threat has dissipated, no We now turn to an entirely different issue.
longer requires so much attention, and can assume During the last two decades, changes have occurred

 cyb ernetic control processes and th e s elf -reg ul ati on of b eh avi or


in how people view cognition and action. The to social psychologists. They proposed a model in
implicit assumption that behavior is generally man- which overt social behavior is a joint output of two
aged in a top-down, directive way has been chal- simultaneously operating systems that they termed
lenged. Questions have been raised about the role reflective and impulsive. Again, differences in the sys-
of consciousness in many kinds of action. Interest tems’ operating characteristics lead to differences in
has arisen in the idea that the mind has both explicit behavior. The reflective system anticipates the future,
and implicit representations. These various issues makes decisions on the basis of those anticipations,
have also influenced how we think about ideas we and forms intentions. It is planful and wide ranging
have been using. in its search for relevant information. It is restrained
and deliberative. The impulsive system acts sponta-
Two-Mode Models neously when its schemas or production systems are
Several literatures have developed around the sufficiently activated. It acts without consideration
idea that there are two somewhat distinct modes of for the future or for broader implications or conse-
functioning (Carver, Johnson, & Joormann, 2008). quences of the action. This depiction is very similar
In personality, Epstein (e.g., 1973, 1994) has long in some ways to the ideas of Epstein (1973, 1994)
advocated such a view. He argues that people experi- and Metcalfe and Mischel (1999).
ence reality through two systems. What he calls a Two-mode thinking has also been very influ-
rational system operates mostly consciously, uses log- ential in developmental psychology. Rothbart and
ical rules, is verbal and deliberative, and thus is fairly her colleagues have argued for the existence of three
slow. In contrast, the experiential system is intuitive temperament systems: two for reactive approach and
and associative in nature. It provides a quick and reactive avoidance, and a third termed effortful con-
dirty way of assessing and reacting to reality. It relies trol (e.g., Derryberry & Rothbart, 1997; Rothbart,
on salient information and uses shortcuts and heu- Ahadi, & Evans, 2000; Rothbart & Posner, 1985;
ristics. It functions automatically and quickly. It is see also Nigg, 2000). Effortful control concerns
considered to be emotional (or at least very respon- (in part) the ability to suppress approach when it
sive to emotions) and nonverbal. is situationally inappropriate. Effortful control is
The experiential system is presumably older and superordinate to approach and avoidance tempera-
more primitive neurobiologically. It dominates ments. The label effortful conveys the sense that this
when speed is needed (as when the situation is emo- is an executive, planful activity, entailing the use of
tionally charged). The rational system evolved later, cognitive resources beyond those needed to react
providing a more cautious, analytic, planful way of impulsively. This view of effortful control has sub-
proceeding. Operating in that way has important stantial resemblance to depictions of the delibera-
advantages, provided there is sufficient time and tive mode of the other two-mode models outlined
freedom from pressure to think things through. earlier.
Both systems are presumed to be always at work,
jointly determining behavior, though the extent Hierarchicality Reexamined
of each one’s influence can vary by situation and Thus, several sources of theory suggest that the
disposition. mind functions in two modes (indeed, the ones
A model in many ways similar to this was pro- described earlier are far from an exhaustive list).
posed by Metcalfe and Mischel (1999). Drawing on All promote the view that a deliberative mode of
decades of work on delay of gratification, Metcalfe functioning uses symbolic and sequential process-
and Mischel (1999) proposed that two systems influ- ing and thus is relatively slow; all suggest that a
ence self-restraint. One they called a “hot” system: more impulsive or reactive mode of functioning
emotional, impulsive, and reflexive. The other they uses associationist processing and is relatively fast.
called a “cool” system: strategic, flexible, slower, and Many of the theories suggest that the two modes are
unemotional. How people respond to difficult situ- semiautonomous in their functioning, competing
ations depends on which system is in charge. with each other to influence actions. Indeed, many
There are also several two-mode theories in social point to situational variables that influence which
psychology (Chaiken & Trope, 1999). The essence mode dominates at a given time.
of such a view has existed for a long time in the These kinds of ideas have influenced how we think
literature of persuasion. Strack and Deutsch (2004) about the hierarchy of control that was proposed
have recently extended this reasoning more broadly by Powers (1973). We said earlier that programs of
into the range of behavioral phenomena of interest action entail decisions. They seem to be managed

c a rver, s c h ei er 
top-down, using effortful processing. Planfulness, an Although the self-control situation is often
element of programs, is also a common characteriza- portrayed as pitting longer and shorter term goals
tion of behavior managed by the reflective system. against each other, a somewhat different view also
It seems reasonable to map program-level control onto seems plausible. The self-control situation may pit
the deliberative, reflective mode of functioning. the two modes of processing against each other.
In contrast to this deliberative quality, well- This would be consistent with the literature on self-
learned sequences occur in a relatively automatic control failure, which tends to portray such failures
stream once they are triggered. Sequences (along as involving a relatively automatic tendency to act
with lower levels of control) are necessarily called in one way, being opposed by a planful effort to
up during the execution of programs. However, per- restrain that act. The action being inhibited is often
haps sequences can also be triggered more autono- characterized as an impulse, a desire that is automat-
mously, without being specified by efforts toward ically translated into action unless it is controlled
a higher goal. Sequences may be triggered by the (often because the action is habitual). The restraint
activation of strong associations in memory. In such is presumed to be effortful and to depend on lim-
cases, the operating characteristics would seem akin ited resources. If the planful part of the mind is able
to those of the reactive mode of functioning. to attend adequately to the conflict, the person can
In the past we have often noted that the level of resist the impulse. If not, the impulse is more likely
control that is functionally superordinate can vary to be expressed. This portrayal seems quite conso-
by situations and persons (e.g., Carver & Scheier, nant with the two-mode models of functioning.
1998, 1999a). As we said earlier, it is easy to imag-
ine cases in which a person is behaving according The How Versus the What of Motivation
to a principle (e.g., a moral or ethical value), and The cybernetic approach to motivational issues
it is easy to imagine cases in which the person is is primarily about the structure and dynamics of
behaving according to a plan or program. It is also behavior rather than the content of behavior. It is
easy, however, to imagine cases in which the person a depiction of relations among processes that occur
is acting impulsively and spontaneously, without as people negotiate the psychological and behavioral
regard to either principle or plan. space of their lives. We think these principles are
In making this case in the past, our emphasis informative both about adaptive functioning and
generally focused simply on how sequences and pro- about problems in functioning. We also believe the
grams differed. Now we are inclined to wonder if ideas described in this chapter represent a viewpoint
this particular differentiation is not perhaps more that is compatible with many other theories that
important than we had realized. Perhaps we have are described in this book, standing alongside them
underappreciated the extent to which lower levels rather than in place of them. In that sense, these
of self-regulatory structures can be triggered autono- ideas may be less a “theory” than a “meta-theory,”
mously and their outputs enter the stream of ongo- a very general way of conceptualizing interwoven
ing action, without oversight from higher levels, and functions, a declaration of belief about how com-
potentially even in conflict with values at higher plex systems work.
levels. This seems an important question for further However, this is a viewpoint that is primarily
exploration. about the how of motivated behavior rather than the
what. It bears on control of actions that are selfish
Self-Control: Impulse and Restraint as well as control of actions that are well socialized.
The idea that conflicts exist between longer term Those actions differ not in their structure but in the
and shorter term goals is also part of a literature on content of the principles and programs (and per-
self-control and self-control failure (e.g., Baumeister, haps the self ) that exist in the persons who engage
Heatherton, & Tice, 1994). This literature focuses in the actions. This view thus is very different from
on cases in which a person is both motivated to act views of motivation that address (for example) what
and motivated to restrain that action. This is essen- specific core motives may underlie human growth
tially the same case as examined by work on chil- and optimal functioning (e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2000,
dren’s effortful control, and it is also the same logical 2001). It was never the explicit goal of the feedback
structure as is in the delay of gratification paradigm. approach to speak to those issues.
A difference is that in the self-control literature On the other hand, it is also possible to stretch
the intent often is to delay indefinitely rather than these ideas a bit more, to speak to at least some of those
temporarily. issues. It is inherent in a hierarchical organization of

 cyb ernetic control processes and th e s elf -reg ul ati on of b eh avi or


values that the values have some degree of compatibil- A person’s repertoire of goals changes in many ways
ity. If there is too much inconsistency among goals, over time. Some changes are very simple and restricted;
effort toward one of them enlarges discrepancies with other cases involve the adoption of goals that are
respect to another. This is bad enough when the goals very new.
are simply in competition for devotion of time and Sometimes the change is limited to shifting one’s
resources to their attainment (for example, when tak- level of aspiration. Goals that aren’t being attained
ing extra time to work on a project at the office takes are scaled back to be less demanding. Goals that are
time away from engagement with one’s family). It is attained too easily are raised to be more demanding.
even more problematic if the goals are intrinsically in Such changes allow the person to continue in the
conflict (for example, when taking a new job for one- same general domain of activity at a level that is both
self in a new town requires one’s spouse to accept an challenging and attainable. When such a change has
inferior new job). been made, however, the goal is not quite the same
To the extent that the biological blueprint of a as it was before.
human being incorporates species-wide imperatives, Another small step in the direction of new goals
goals at various lower levels of abstraction must be would be cases in which a person engages in an activ-
brought into at least some degree of compatibility ity for one purpose (e.g., going to a gym to work out,
with those imperatives. Precisely what species-wide with the goal of staying healthy), and inadvertently
imperatives are contained in that blueprint is a finds that the activity also satisfies a second purpose
question on which there is a good deal of debate. (making new friends). The activity thereby acquires a
Clearly the establishment of dominance hierarchies second kind of usefulness and becomes connected to
is one of them; relatedness to at least some other a different higher level goal than it was connected to
humans is another. before. This behavior has evolved a new link upward
The upshot of this set of issues is that a model in the goal hierarchy (Fig. 3.2). The activity itself
of hierarchical organization of the self and its goals (going to the gym and exercising) already was in place
appears to entail continuing pressure toward com- as a goal, and thus it is not new itself. But its broader
patibility among the values that define the self and implications are now different—perhaps quite differ-
one’s view of community. The attainment of lower ent—than they were. This change in a goal’s connect-
order goals is the process by which higher order edness to other aspects of the self structure also implies
goals are realized, all the way to the highest values newness.
the person has. In many cases, new activities are undertaken pre-
cisely because they have been pre-identified as poten-
Where Do New Goals Come From? tially relevant to a higher order goal in the person’s life.
The principle that lower order goals have links For example, a person who is high in openness, who
to higher order ones also has implications for how likes to explore diversity in life, may decide to take a
new goals arise and are adopted as reference values vacation tour of Asia, try scuba diving, or experiment
(for broader treatment, see Carver & Scheier, 1999b). with bicycle racing. In such cases, the new activity is

Fig. 3.2 Attainment of a goal at a


Ideal self relatively lower level of abstraction often
can contribute to more than one goal at
the next higher level. An example, also
discussed in the text, is that going to the
physical connection
Principles gym can contribute to the maintenance
health to others
of physical health, and it can also be a
way of making friends, thus enhancing
connection to others.
Programs go to gym

Sequences

c a rver, s c h ei er 
approached because it is identified as a possible means feedback loops are embedded in many different
to satisfy the desire (the goal) of exploration. kinds of systems, at many levels of abstraction (e.g.,
Exploration provides an easy illustration, but Ford, 1987; von Bertalanffy, 1968). The principle
it certainly is not the only higher level desire that of feedback control has been found useful in under-
can lead to new activities. Any time someone says, standing phenomena as diverse as weather systems,
“You ought to try this—I think you might enjoy it,” the stability of ecological systems, and homeostasis.
an inference is being made that the activity will sat- The argument that the same fundamental principle
isfy a broader desire the person has. Anytime people underlies even the regulation of overt action asserts
contemplate undertaking new activities, they are a rather astonishing link between human experience
considering how the activities might fit into their and other aspects of nature, parts of nature that
current patterns of preferences. could hardly be more different from human life.
In these examples a link is prespecified between The possibility that such a link is real is at least part
the “new” goal and an existing one. Sometimes, of the fascination.
though, an activity seems to come together without
much forethought or planning, and (when it occurs) Acknowledgments
is found to be enjoyable. In such cases, the person Preparation of this chapter was supported by grants from
may actively seek to identify the activity’s essence, the National Cancer Institute (CA64710), the National Science
Foundation (BCS0544617), and the National Heart, Lung,
so as to make the positive experience repeatable
and Blood Institute (HL65111, HL65112, HL076852, and
by intention. Thus, it becomes a new goal. That HL076858).
is, in order to make the experience repeatable, the
person encodes its nature in memory in a man- References
ner that renders it accessible to top-down use later Ashby, W. R. (1940). Adaptiveness and equilibrium. Journal of
on. In this sort of case, a bottom-up self-assembly Mental Science, 86, 478–483.
(component elements coming together without an Baumeister, R. F., Heatherton, T. F., & Tice, D. M. (1994).
explicit higher level reference value) leads to synthe- Losing control: Why people fail at self-regulation. San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
sis of a new reference value at the higher level. Beer, R. D. (1995). A dynamical systems perspective on
What makes an experience unexpectedly enjoy- agent-environment interaction. Artificial Intelligence, 72,
able? Finding an experience enjoyable, we suggest, 173–215.
means that engaging in the experience serves to Berridge, K. C. (2004). Motivation concepts in behavioral
move the person toward another goal that already is neuroscience. Physiology and Behavior. 81, 179–209.
Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation.
in place as part of the self. The person may have had Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131.
no idea beforehand that the new activity was going Brendl, C. M., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Principles of judging
to connect to that already incorporated value. But valence: What makes events positive or negative? Advances in
because it does connect, the experience of the new experimental social psychology, 28, 95–160.
activity creates positive affect. Thus, a new action, Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1999). The
affect system has parallel and integrative processing com-
as well as an old one, can fairly quickly acquire an ponents: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and
upward link to a higher order goal. A given principle Social Psychology, 76, 839–855.
(for example) can be fulfilled in myriad activities, Campos, J. J., Frankel, C. B., & Camras, L. (2004). On the nature
even activities that might at first not have seemed of emotion regulation. Child Development, 75, 377–394.
relevant to the principle. Cannon, W. B. (1932). The wisdom of the body. New York:
Norton.
Carver, C. S. (2001). Affect and the functional bases of behavior:
Closing Comment On the dimensional structure of affective experience. Person-
We have chosen a rather unusual construct to ality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 345–356.
be interested in for such a long time. We are, after Carver, C. S. (2003). Pleasure as a sign you can attend to some-
thing else: Placing positive feelings within a general model of
all, personality psychologists, and these ideas are
affect. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 241–261.
not exactly mainstream personality. We could have Carver, C. S. (2004). Negative affects deriving from the behav-
focused on goals and left it at that. But, no, we ioral approach system. Emotion, 4, 3–22.
keep dragging in the idea that goal-directed action Carver, C. S. (2009). Threat sensitivity, incentive sensitivity, and
involves feedback processes. Why? the experience of relief. Journal of Personality, 77, 125–138.
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-
The answer is fairly simple. Scientists in diverse
related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulle-
disciplines see feedback processes as among the tin, 135, 183–204.
basic building blocks of nature. Not of motivation, Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Joormann, J. (2008). Seroton-
but of nature. It was suggested many years ago that ergic function, two-mode models of self-regulation, and

 cyb ernetic control processes and th e s elf -reg ul ati on of b eh avi or


vulnerability to depression: What depression has in com- Epstein, S. (1973). The self-concept revisited: Or a theory of a
mon with impulsive aggression. Psychological Bulletin, 134, theory. American Psychologist, 28, 404–416.
912–943. Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the
Carver, C. S., Lawrence, J. W., & Scheier, M. F. (1999). Self- psychodynamic unconscious. American Psychologist, 49,
discrepancies and affect: Incorporating the role of feared selves. 709–724.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 783–792. Ford, D. H. (1987). Humans as self-constructing living systems:
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self- A developmental perspective on behavior and personality.
regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. New Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
York: Springer Verlag. Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, England:
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of Cambridge University Press.
positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psycho- Frijda, N. H. (1988). The laws of emotion. American Psychologist,
logical Review, 97, 19–35. 43, 349–358.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of Fulford, D., Johnson, S. L., Llabre, M. M., & Carver, C. S.
behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press. (2010). Pushing and coasting in dynamic goal pursuit:
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1999a). Themes and issues in Coasting is attenuated in bipolar disorder. Psychological
the self-regulation of behavior. In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Science, 21, 1021–1027.
Advances in social cognition (Vol. 12, pp. 1–105). Mahwah, NJ: Gross, J. J. (Ed.). (2007). Handbook of emotion regulation. New
Erlbaum. York: Guilford Press.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1999b). Several more themes, Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and
a lot more issues: Commentary on the commentaries. affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.
In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 12, Higgins, E. T. (1996). Ideals, oughts, and regulatory focus:
pp. 261–302). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Affect and motivation from distinct pains and pleasures.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Scaling back goals and In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology
recalibration of the affect system are processes in normal of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior
adaptive self-regulation: Understanding “response shift” phe- (pp. 91–114). New York: Guilford.
nomena. Social Science and Medicine, 50, 1715–1722. Higgins, E. T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997). Emotional
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Control processes and responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus as
self-organization as complementary principles underly- moderator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,
ing behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 515–525.
304–315. Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2003). Three human strengths. behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
In L. G. Aspinwall & U. M. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology social psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 203–252). San Diego, CA:
of human strengths: Fundamental questions and future direc- Academic Press.
tions for a positive psychology (pp. 87–102). Washington, DC: Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making.
American Psychological Association. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook of
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2009). Action, affect, and two- emotions (2nd ed., pp. 417–435). New York: Guilford.
mode models of functioning. In E. Morsella, J. A. Bargh, & Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum.
P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action Izard, C. E., & Ackerman, B. P. (2000). Motivational, orga-
(pp. 298–327). New York: Oxford University Press. nizational, and regulatory functions of discrete emotions.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2112). A model of behavioral In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.), Handbook
self-regulation. In P. A. M. Van Lange, A. W. Kruglanski, & of emotions (2nd ed., pp. 253–264). New York: Guilford
E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology Press.
(Vol. 1, pp. 505–525). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emo-
Chaiken, S. L., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999). Dual-process theories tion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review,
in social psychology. New York: Guilford. 97, 377–395.
Clark, R. N. (1996). Control system dynamics. New York: Louro, M. J., Pieters, R., & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Dynam-
Cambridge University Press. ics of multiple-goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social
Clore, G. C. (1994). Why emotions are felt. In P. Ekman & Psychology, 93, 174–193.
R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamen- MacKay, D. M. (1956). Toward an information-flow model of
tal questions (pp. 103–111). New York: Oxford University human behavior. British Journal of Psychology, 47, 30–43.
Press. MacKay, D. M. (1966). Cerebral organization and the conscious
Davidson, R. J. (1992). Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the control of action. In J. C. Eccles (Ed.), Brain and conscious
nature of emotion. Brain and Cognition, 20, 125–151. experience (pp. 422–445). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
DeAngelis, D. L., Post, W. M., & Travis, C. C. (1986). Positive Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psy-
feedback in natural systems (Biomathematics, Vol. 15). Berlin chologist, 41, 954–969.
and New York: Springer-Verlag. Maruyama, M. (1963). The second cybernetics: Deviation-
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effort- amplifying mutual causal processes. American Scientist, 51,
ful processes in the organization of temperament. Develop- 164–179.
ment and Psychopathology, 9, 633–652. McFarland, D. J. (1971). Feedback mechanisms in animal behav-
Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect ior. New York: Academic Press.
modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the Metcalfe, J., & Mischel, W. (1999). A hot/cool-system analysis
cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psy- of delay of gratification: Dynamics of willpower. Psychological
chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 343–353. Review, 106, 3–19.

c a rver, s c h ei er 
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans Schwartz, S. H., & Rubel, T. (2005). Sex differences in value
and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & priorities: Cross-cultural and multimethod studies. Journal of
Winston. Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 1010–1028.
Mizruchi, M. S. (1991). Urgency, motivation, and group perfor- Schwarz, N., & Bohner, G. (1996). Feelings and their moti-
mance: The effect of prior success on current success among vational implications: Moods and the action sequence.
professional basketball teams. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54, In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology
181–189. of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior
Nigg, J. T. (2000). On inhibition/disinhibition in developmen- (pp. 119–145). New York: Guilford.
tal psychopathology: Views from cognitive and personality Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forget-
psychology as a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological ting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal
Bulletin, 126, 220–246. shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
Ochsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2008). Cognitive emotion 1261–1280.
regulation: Insights from social cognitive and affective neu- Shallice, T. (1978). The dominant action system: An information-
roscience. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17, processing approach to consciousness. In K. S. Pope &
153–158. J. L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of consciousness: Scientific inves-
Ogilvie, D. M. (1987). The undesired self: A neglected vari- tigations into the flow of human experience (pp. 117–157).
able in personality research. Journal of Personality and Social New York: Wiley.
Psychology, 52, 379–385. Shin, J. C., & Rosenbaum, D. A. (2002). Reaching while cal-
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive culating: Scheduling of cognitive and perceptual-motor
structure of emotions. New York: Cambridge University Press. processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 131,
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. 206–219.
Chicago, IL: Aldine. Simon, H. A. (1953). Models of man. New York: Wiley.
Reyna, C., Henry, P. J., Korfmacher, W., & Tucker, A. (2006). Simon, H. A. (1967). Motivational and emotional controls of
Examining the principles in principled conservatism: The cognition. Psychology Review, 74, 29–39.
role of responsibility stereotypes as cues for deservingness in Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive deter-
racial policy decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- minants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology
chology, 90, 109–128. Review, 8, 220–247.
Rosenbaum, D. A., Meulenbroek, R. G. J., Vaughan, J., & Toates, F. (2006). A model of the hierarchy of behaviour,
Jansen, C. (2001). Posture-based motion planning: Applica- cognition, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition:
tions to grasping. Psychological Review, 108, 709–734. An International Journal, 15, 75–118.
Rosenblueth, A., Wiener, N., & Bigelow, J. (1943). Behavior, Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people
purpose, and teleology. Philosophy of Science, 10, 18–24. think they’re doing? Action identification and human behav-
Rothbart, M. K., Ahadi, S. A., & Evans, D. E. (2000). Tem- ior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.
perament and personality: Origins and outcomes. Journal of Von Bertalanffy, L. (1968). General systems theory. New York:
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 122–135. Braziller.
Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. (1985). Temperament and Watson, D. (2009). Locating anger in the hierarchical structure
the development of self-regulation. In L. C. Hartlage & of affect: Comment on Carver and Harmon-Jones. Psycho-
C. F. Telzrow (Eds.), The neuropsychology of individual differ- logical Bulletin, 135, 205–208.
ences: A developmental perspective (pp. 93–123). New York: Watson, D., Wiese, D., Vaidya, J., & Tellegen, A. (1999).
Plenum. The two general activation systems of affect: Structural
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory findings, evolutionary considerations, and psychobiologi-
and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop- cal evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,
ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. 820–838.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001). On happiness and human Wiener, N. (1948). Cybernetics: Control and communication in
potentials: A review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic the animal and the machine. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
well-being. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 141–166. Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Effort determination of
Schwartz, S. H., & Bilsky, W. (1990). Toward a theory of the cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applica-
universal content and structure of values: Extensions and tions in social psychology. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances
cross-cultural replications. Journal of Personality and Social in experimental social psychology (Vol. 33, pp. 255–307).
Psychology, 58, 878–891. New York: Academic Press.

 cyb ernetic control processes and th e s elf -reg ul ati on of b eh avi or


CHAPTER

The Role of Death in Life: Existential


4 Aspects of Human Motivation

Pelin Kesebir and Tom Pyszczynski

Abstract
The capacity for self-reflection, which plays an important role in human self-regulation, also leads people
to become aware of the limitations of their existence. Awareness of the conflict between one’s desires
(e.g., to live) and the limitations of existence (e.g., the inevitability of death) creates the potential for
existential anxiety. In this chapter, we review how this anxiety affects human motivation and behavior in
a variety of life domains. Terror management theory and research suggest that transcending death and
protecting oneself against existential anxiety are potent needs. This protection is provided by an
anxiety-buffering system, which imbues people with a sense of meaning and value that function to
shield them against these concerns. We review evidence of how the buffering system protects against
existential anxiety in four dimensions of existence: the physical, personal, social, and spiritual domains.
Because self-awareness is a prerequisite for existential anxiety, escaping self-awareness can also be an
effective way to obviate the problem of existence. After elaborating on how existential anxiety can
motivate escape from self-awareness, we conclude the chapter with a discussion of remaining issues
and directions for future research and theory development.
Key Words: terror management theory, experimental existential psychology, death anxiety, existential
anxiety, motivation

Unlike any other animal, we humans live our theory and research have revealed about existential
lives starkly aware that, despite our most fervent anxiety and its effects on human behavior and expe-
desires, death will sooner or later come to us. This rience. The main tenet of TMT is that the desire to
knowledge, combined with other uniquely human transcend the fragility of human existence by con-
sophisticated mental abilities, inevitably leads people struing oneself as a valuable contributor to a mean-
to ask questions about the meaning, value, and pur- ingful universe lies at the root of a diverse array of
pose of existence. Although writers and philosophers otherwise distinct human motives.
throughout the ages have pointed to the vital impact The research we will review in this chapter
of existential concerns on the human psyche, system- focuses on a uniquely human source of motivation.
atic empirical investigation of how existential con- Although other animals react with fear to clear and
cerns affect human motivation began only relatively present dangers that threaten their existence, only
recently. The purpose of terror management theory humans have the self-awareness that leads them to
(TMT; Greenberg, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1986) realize that death is inevitable. Like other evolu-
is to explain the role that awareness of the inevitabil- tionary advances, this awareness led to changes in
ity of death plays in diverse aspects of human life. the way motivational systems operated by building
In this chapter, we review what terror management on previous evolved adaptations. Thus, existential


motivation operates on other more basic motive of comparing the self’s current state to standards
systems—co-opting them to meet new needs and and the increased effort to reduce any discrepancies
changing the way other needs are pursued. We start are viewed as the most basic process through which
by considering how the emergence of self-awareness the self regulates its own actions. Self-awareness thus
changed the human condition. adds multiple layers of sophistication and flexibility
to the simple system of comparing and matching to
Self-Awareness: A Blessing and a Curse standards through which all self-regulating systems
Awareness of self is a tremendously adaptive cog- operate.
nitive capacity that exponentially increases the flex- One of the most important innovations that
ibility of the system through which humans regulate Carver and Scheier (1981) brought to their syn-
their behavior (Becker, 1971; Duval & Wicklund, thesis of self-awareness and self-regulation was their
1972; Leary, 2004; Pyszczynski & Greenberg, conceptualization of a hierarchy of standards that
1993). Self-awareness is a distinct type of conscious- integrated concrete physical actions and the even
ness that enables the human self to become an object more concrete biological, chemical, and electrical
to itself. Although some other species are capable changes through which these actions are accom-
of a rudimentary form of self-recognition, they lack plished, with the more abstract goals, identities,
the linguistic abilities to conceive of an abstract self and sense of self-worth that these actions (and
and use it to structure their experiences and behav- their lower level components) are oriented toward
ior (Mitchell, 2003). Self-awareness enables humans achieving. From this perspective, all behavior func-
to step back, reflect on their circumstances, weigh tions to simultaneously meet multiple hierarchically
multiple options for how to meet their needs and organized goals, and this organization gives coher-
the chances of each one succeeding, and then select ence and flexibility to human action. The standard
the option they believe will be most successful for at any given level of abstraction is simultaneously
achieving their goal. It greatly expands one’s options a behavior through which the standard at a higher
for how to behave and gives greater executive control level of abstraction is met. For example, writing a
to the self over one’s actions. Accompanied by other paper for a college class is a behavior through which
uniquely human capacities, such as language and the standard of getting a good grade in the class is
symbolic thought, causal thinking, and imagination, met; getting a good grade in the class is the behavior
reflexive self-awareness has been critical to the for- through which the more abstract standard of getting
mation of complex human society and culture as we a college degree is met; getting a college degree is the
know it today. As Leary argues, “Science, philoso- behavior through which the more abstract standard
phy, government, education, and health care would of getting a good job is met; getting a good job is the
all be impossible if people could not consciously self- behavior through which the more abstract standard
reflect” (2004, p. 12). of having a successful career is met; and having a suc-
Contemporary thinking about the role of self- cessful career is the behavior through which the even
awareness in human behavior was stimulated by more abstract goal of being a valuable person is met.
Duval and Wicklund’s (1972) objective self-awareness One could also move down the hierarchy to consider
theory. They pointed out that conscious attention can the component behaviors through which writing a
be directed either externally, toward the environment, paper, gathering information, reading articles to pro-
or internally, toward the self. Objective self-awareness vide that information, moving the focus of one’s eyes
theory posits that directing attention toward the self across the words on the page are accomplished, and
instigates a self-evaluative process, in which one’s cur- so on down to the biological and chemical reactions
rent state on whatever dimension is currently salient is that underlie these actions.
compared with salient standards for that dimension. Flexibility in behavior is provided by the fact that
The detection of discrepancies between current state there are usually multiple behaviors through which
and standards produces affect that motivates the per- any given standard can be met. For example, self-
son to either reduce any discrepancies or escape the esteem can be achieved by means of success in one’s
self-focused state. Research has been highly supportive career, relationships, community activities, or family.
of these basic propositions (for reviews, see Carver & And there are many ways to succeed in any of these
Scheier, 2002; Duval & Silvia, 2001; Pyszczynski & more specific endeavors, just as there are many par-
Greenberg, 1993). Carver and Scheier (1981; Carver, ticular routes through which any particular success
1979) integrated these ideas with a very general cyber- could be attained. Self-awareness sets in motion a
netic model of self-regulation in which this process variety of executive processes through which choices

 the rol e of death in life


among these multiple routes to goals at these vari- was qualitative and descriptive, with a particular
ous levels of abstraction are met. Of course this is emphasis on phenomenology. Notwithstanding the
a very complex system and we are able to provide rich insights these methods are capable of yielding,
only a brief overview here. For a more thorough pre- causal inferences regarding the effect of existential
sentation, see Carver and Scheier (1981; 2002). For realities on human motivation can be made only
present purposes, our goal is to make clear the cen- through rigorous experimental research. This is why
tral role and adaptive utility that self-awareness and terror management theory’s application of experimen-
hierarchical organization of standards and behavior tal methods to existential psychological questions has
play in human motivation and behavior. Put simply, been an invigorating contribution to existential psy-
self-awareness increases the human capacity for free- chology, resulting in the prolific subfield of social psy-
dom and willful self-determined behavior. chology known as experimental existential psychology
Although self-awareness opened the door to many (see Greenberg, Koole, & Pyszczynski, 2004; Pyszc-
new opportunities for humans, it also set the stage zynski, Greenberg, Solomon, & Koole, 2010).
for some uniquely human challenges. Perhaps the
most basic problem born from self-awareness was the Terror Management Theory
recognition of one’s limits, one’s perpetual vulner- Terror management theory was inspired by
ability, and one’s ultimate mortality. Human beings, cultural anthropologist Ernest Becker’s (1971, 1973,
compelled by their sophisticated mental abilities to 1975) attempts to integrate and synthesize what
be aware of their own existence, had to face the basic he viewed as the most important insights into the
conditions of life and their limitations against them. human condition provided by the social and natu-
The juxtaposition of what humans were born into ral sciences, as well as humanities. Building on the
and what they naturally desired created certain exis- work of thinkers as diverse as Freud, Rank, Mead,
tential dilemmas with which they had to contend. Fromm, Kierkegaard, Heidegger, and Sartre, Becker
Irvin Yalom (1980) delineated four ultimate concerns built on the premise that the idea of death is unbear-
and proposed that the individual’s confrontation able to a self-aware animal: “To have emerged from
with each of these “givens of existence” constituted nothing, to have a name, consciousness of self, deep
a major existential conflict. These four concerns are inner feelings, an excruciating inner yearning for
death, freedom, isolation, and meaninglessness. Human life and self-expression—and with all this yet to die”
beings wish to continue being, yet they are inevitably (1973, p. 87). To Becker, the terror inherent in this
finite (death). They wish for ground and structure, knowledge haunted humans like nothing else and
yet there is no universal design or plan for human life was a mainspring of human activity: “Of all things
other than that which humans create, leaving people that move man, one of the principal ones is his ter-
responsible for creating themselves and their world ror of death” (1973, p. 11). In his view, a major
(freedom). They wish for communion with others and function of individual character and societal institu-
to be part of something larger than themselves, yet tions was to deny one’s mortality and avert this ter-
they are born alone and ultimately die alone (isola- ror. He viewed human striving for a sense of value
tion). They desperately seek meaning, yet there is no and unshakable meaning as the primary defense
preordained, inherent meaning to the universe (mean- against the terror-inducing awareness of mortality,
inglessness). According to Yalom, each of these clashes and he conceptualized this striving as taking place
between the structure of existence and the wishes of within the context of the cultural worldviews to
the self-reflective human being spawns conscious and which people subscribe. To Becker, participating in
unconscious fears and motives. Existential psychol- and contributing to a cultural system that imbues
ogy is the branch of psychology that investigates how existence with order, purpose, and permanence pro-
these fears and motives affect humankind, and how vided the individual with a feeling of outliving or
they interact with the other needs and desires that are outshining death and the psychological equanimity
essential to human existence that this produces.
For most of the still brief history of psychology, TMT was initially developed to answer three
the existential subdiscipline was synonymous with fundamental questions about human nature: Why
existential psychotherapy; its concepts and theories do people need self-esteem? Why do people need to
were scattered in a piecemeal fashion within the exist- believe that out of the multitude of ways that people
ing literature; and it had little interaction with empiri- construe reality, theirs happens to be the one that is
cally oriented psychological science (Jacobsen, 2007). ultimately correct? And why are interpersonal and
The methodology of existential psychological research intercultural relations so frequently ridden with

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
conflict and violence? Becker’s ideas offered potential sections, we provide an overview of TMT findings
answers to these and many other questions. TMT that support this claim; however, we first describe
was an attempt to simplify Becker’s ideas and inte- the logic of the methods commonly employed in
grate them with existing knowledge within the fields TMT studies.
of social, personality, developmental, cognitive, and
motivational psychology in a way that would gener- The TMT Research Strategy
ate testable hypotheses about the functions of self- TMT research has been focused on three gen-
esteem and culture. TMT posited that knowledge of eral hypotheses that have been combined in vari-
inevitable mortality, when combined with the bio- ous ways to assess the basic propositions of the
logically rooted craving for life, creates a potential theory and applied to a diverse array of behaviors
for paralyzing terror. To function effectively in the and social problems to document the generality and
world, people must keep this terror at bay. Protec- generativity of the theory. The earliest TMT stud-
tion from this terror is provided by self-esteem and ies (Greenberg et al., 1990; Rosenblatt, Greenberg,
faith in one’s cultural worldview (Solomon, Green- Solomon, Pyszczynski, & Lyon, 1989) used the
berg, & Pyszczynski, 1991). These two psychologi- mortality salience hypothesis to assess the theory’s
cal entities function to buffer death-related anxiety. propositions, which has remained the most com-
Later research revealed close interpersonal relations mon approach to testing TMT. Indeed, according to
as an additional component of the anxiety-buffering a recent meta-analysis of 238 empirical TMT jour-
mechanism (Mikulincer, Florian, & Hirschberger, nal articles reporting 277 experiments, 83% directly
2003). tested this hypothesis (Burke, Martens, & Faucher,
TMT posits that awareness of the inevitability of 2010). The mortality salience (MS) hypothesis states
death is a powerful motivating force that influences that to the extent a psychological structure (e.g., self-
the human needs for meaning, self-esteem, and esteem, faith in one’s cultural worldview) provides
close relationships. The precursors of these motives protection against death anxiety, reminders of death
probably initially evolved because they solved prac- should intensify the need for this structure, and
tical problems of living that increased our ancestors’ therefore lead to more positive reactions to people
chances of passing on their genes by staying alive, and ideas that support that structure and more nega-
mating, and caring for their offspring. However, tive reactions to people and ideas that threaten it.
once human intelligence had evolved to the point In a typical MS study, the experimental group is
that awareness of death emerged, the need for pro- exposed to a reminder of death (mortality salience)
tection from the fear that this awareness created led and then compared to a control group that has not
people to develop systems of meaning and value been reminded of death on the variable hypothesized
that provided protection from this fear. From this to buffer against existential anxiety.
point on, people no longer simply needed mean- The most common mortality salience induction
ing systems that helped them procure the necessities technique entails asking participants two open-ended
of life—now, their meaning systems also needed to questions about their own mortality, as first utilized
help manage their potential for existential anxiety. by Rosenblatt et al. (1989). Specifically, participants
The value of accuracy and practical utility of the are asked to “Please briefly describe the emotions
meaning systems was usurped by the value of death that the thought of your own death arouses in you”
transcendence, and from this point on the pursuit and “Please jot down as specifically as you can what
of truth and protection were often in conflict with you think will happen to you as you physically die
each other. and once you are physically dead.” In the control
Well over 400 separate studies conducted in over condition, participants respond to similarly worded
20 countries have tested and supported hypotheses questions regarding a neutral (e.g., watching TV)
derived from TMT. These studies helped expand the or negative topic not related to death (e.g., dental
theory beyond its initial focus and applied it to top- pain). Other techniques to manipulate MS include
ics as varied as religion and spirituality, legal deci- having participants complete fear of death scales,
sion making, nostalgia, human sexuality, fascination watch car crash or Holocaust videos, read an essay
with fame, creativity, materialism, and psychopa- about cancer or the 9/11 attacks, exposing them to
thology. The fact that existential concerns have been subliminal death primes, and interviewing them in
shown to affect human behavior across so many front of a funeral home or cemetery. Findings have
domains suggests that existential anxiety is a central been highly consistent across these different mortal-
motivating force for the human psyche. In the next ity salience inductions.

 the rol e of death in life


In their meta-analysis, Burke and colleagues (2010) levels of anxiety in the neutral self-esteem condi-
found that MS manipulations yielded moderate-to- tion, it had no effect on anxiety in the self-esteem
large effects (r =.35, d =.75) on a wide range of attitu- boost condition. Follow-up studies by Greenberg
dinal, behavioral, and cognitive dependent variables. et al. (1993) replicated this finding with different
This effect size reaches the top quartile of effects for manipulations of self-esteem and threat and physi-
psychology in general and the 80th percentile for ological measures of anxiety. These studies showed
theories in social psychology (Lipsey & Wilson, 1993; that both experimentally elevated and disposition-
Richard, Bond, & Stokes-Zoota, 2003). The same ally high levels of self-esteem led to lower levels of
meta-analysis revealed that a longer delay between death-denying defensive distortions, which presum-
MS manipulation and the dependent variable assess- ably were decreased because of the anxiety-buffering
ment yields larger effect sizes. This finding highlights effect of high self-esteem. Still other studies com-
an important finding regarding how people react to bined the anxiety-buffer and mortality salience
reminders of death—death-related thoughts elicit hypotheses to show that bolstering self-esteem,
strongest defensive reactions when they are no longer faith in one’s worldview, or close personal relation-
in current focal attention, yet are still accessible. This ships eliminates the increase in defensiveness that
led to a distinction between the types of defenses that reminders of death otherwise produce (e.g., Florian,
people use to cope with conscious and nonconscious Mikulincer, & Hirschberger, 2002; Harmon-Jones
death-related thoughts (Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & et al., 1997).
Solomon, 1999). People deal with conscious thoughts A third, increasingly common approach to assess-
of mortality using proximal defenses that operate in a ing TMT is the death-thought accessibility (DTA)
relatively direct and rational fashion—for example, by hypothesis. The hypothesis states that to the extent a
reminding themselves of their excellent health or the psychological structure serves to protect against death
“longevity gene” running in their family, by resolv- anxiety, weakening this structure would increase, and
ing to eat better, to exercise more, to have more regu- strengthening it would decrease, the accessibility of
lar checkups, and so on. Nonconscious thoughts of death-related thoughts. According to a recent review
death, that is, thoughts that are highly accessible but (Hayes, Schimel, Arndt, & Faucher, 2010), there
not in current focal attention, lead to distal defenses are over 80 published studies that have made use of
that cope with the problem in a more indirect, sym- the DTA concept in the context of TMT. The vast
bolic manner. These distal defenses emerge only when majority of these studies assessed DTA through the
thoughts of mortality have faded to the fringes of word-fragment completion task. This task, originally
consciousness. Research showing that the removal of used by Greenberg et al. (1994), consists of word
delay and distraction tasks eliminates effects of MS on fragments, some of which can be completed in either
worldview defense and self-esteem striving supports death-related or death-unrelated ways (e.g., SK _ _ L
this dual process model of defense (Greenberg, Arndt, can be completed as skull or skill). DTA is operation-
Simon, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 2000). Hence, alized as the number of words completed in death-
delay/distraction tasks (e.g., scales of positive and neg- related ways. The successful use of this measure in
ative affect, word puzzles, anagram tasks) between the languages other than English, including Hebrew,
MS induction and measures of the dependent variable Chinese, French, and Dutch, attests to the construct
are essential for testing the MS hypothesis. validity and generality of the method. DTA studies
A second early approach to assessing TMT were essential to the development of TMT, because
was the anxiety-buffer hypothesis. According to they revealed that the anxiety buffer does not operate
this hypothesis, to the extent that a psychological only when death thoughts are activated by external
structure buffers anxiety, then strengthening that events (as studies testing the MS hypothesis show),
structure should lead to less anxiety in threatening but that they are continuously functioning to keep
situations and weakening it should lead to more anx- death-related thoughts beneath consciousness.
iety. In the initial test of the anxiety-buffer hypoth- In our view, the most convincing aspect of the
esis (Greenberg, Solomon et al., 1992), participants evidence for TMT is the high degree of consistency
were given bogus positive or neutral personality and convergence in findings across different meth-
profiles designed to either increase their self-esteem ods. Although in some cases it may be possible to
or have no effect on it. They then watched a graphic offer alternative explanations for specific findings,
video of death-related scenes or a neutral film, after we have yet to encounter an attempt to provide
which their state anxiety was assessed. Although an alternative account of the converging evidence
the death-related video led to significantly elevated provided by these diverse methods. We now discuss

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
evidence obtained with these and other methods the rest of the animal kingdom ironically also leads
that reveal the role of existential anxiety in energiz- to the realization that humankind is ultimately part
ing and directing human behavior. of nature and subject to the same ultimate fate of
death and decay. The knowledge that one is “up in
Evidence for the Motivational Role of the stars and yet housed in a heart-pumping, breath-
Existential Anxiety gasping body” (Becker, 1973, p. 26), the awareness
In presenting the findings on the diverse ways of one’s common fate with all creatures, explains
that the fear of death affects human behavior, we why people are often ill at ease with their own cor-
use a taxonomy widely used by existential psycho- poreality. Indeed, research has shown that remind-
logists (van Deurzen-Smith, 1984). According to ers of death intensify the desire to distance oneself
this framework, humans experience the world on from other animals and from one’s own body.
four basic dimensions, commonly referred to with For example, Goldenberg and colleagues (2001)
their German names: physical dimension (Umwelt), demonstrated that mortality salience leads to increased
personal dimension (Eigenwelt), social dimension preference for an essay that describes humans as dis-
(Mitwelt), and spiritual dimension (Überwelt). The tinct from animals over one that emphasizes human-
first three dimensions are drawn from the work of animal similarities. These researchers also found that
Swiss psychiatrist Ludwig Binswanger (1946). Based mortality reminders increase disgust reactions to
on the writings of authors such as Buber (1923), situations involving bodily products (e.g., “seeing a
Jaspers (1951), and Tillich (1952), existential psy- bowel movement left unflushed in a public toilet”)
chotherapist van Deurzen-Smith (1984) proposed and animals (e.g., “seeing maggots on a piece of
a fourth, spiritual dimension. According to van meat in an outdoor garbage pail”). In a similar vein,
Deurzen-Smith (1997), all these dimensions have viewing pictures of bodily wastes has been found to
their own paradoxes and tensions, their own human increase the accessibility of death-related thoughts
objectives and aspirations, as well as their own ideals (Cox, Goldenberg, Pyszczynski, & Weise, 2007).
and evils. They create a complex four-dimensional Other research has shown that intimations of the
field of forces that encompass the major aspects frailty of the human body, as in the case of elderly
of the human experience. The four dimensions are people (Martens, Greenberg, Schimel, & Landau,
obviously interrelated, with the self standing at 2004) or persons with physical disabilities (Hirsch-
the center of the person’s entire network of physi- berger, Florian, & Mikulincer, 2005), spontaneously
cal, social, personal and spiritual relations. Indeed, increase the accessibility of death thoughts.
these four dimensions overlap substantially with The urge to distance oneself from one’s body in
William James’s four constituents of the self—the the face of death thoughts acquires great practical
material self, social self, spiritual self, and the pure significance in the context of health behaviors. Ironi-
ego (1950). For organizational purposes, nonethe- cally, the salience of mortality thoughts often poses
less, we will treat them separately and discuss how a barrier to health-promoting behaviors that could
the human experience on each of these dimensions actually forestall death (Goldenberg & Arndt, 2008).
is affected by existential concerns. Research has shown, for example, that when mortality
is salient, reminders of creatureliness decrease wom-
The Physical Dimension en’s willingness to conduct breast self-examinations
The physical dimension is concerned with how (Goldenberg, Arndt, Hart, & Routledge, 2008).
people relate to nature and the material world around Interestingly, thoughts of death increase health-
them. Their relationship to their bodies, physical envi- promoting behavior when they are in current focal
ronment, concrete surroundings, and material posses- attention, but they decrease such behavior when
sions makes up this dimension (van Deurzen, 2002). they are on the fringes of consciousness. This is con-
How do existential motives shape human behavior sistent with the TMT distinction between proximal
and experience on the physical dimension? defenses, which deal with the problem of death in
a rational way and emerge when one is consciously
the problem of the body thinking about death, and distal defenses, which
Human beings are condemned to a dual exis- deal with the problem of death symbolically by
tence: They are half animal and half symbolic—to boosting one’s sense of meaning and value that
use Becker’s colorful metaphor, they are “gods with emerge when such thoughts are accessible but not in
anuses” (1973, p. 51). The capacity for self-reflection focal attention. For example, Routledge, Arndt, and
that distinguishes the human race so sharply from Goldenberg (2004) found that immediately after

 the rol e of death in life


reminders of death people were more interested in materialism
using a sunscreen that provided a high level of pro- Materialism, or the importance a person attaches
tection (to reduce their chances of skin cancer), but to worldly possessions (Belk, 1985), has frequently
after a delay and distraction, they were more inter- been recognized by scholars as a way to secure mean-
ested in sunscreen with a lower level of protection ing and transcend death. Irvin Yalom, for example,
(to get a better tan). wrote that accumulating material wealth can become
According to TMT, the great efforts individuals “a way of life which effectively conceals the mortal
and societies put in denying and disguising the body’s questions churning below” (1980, p. 121). Others
physicality are motivated, to a large extent, by a need contended that underlying the American ideology of
to escape the creaturely aspects of existence. This need affluence is the pursuit of secular personal immor-
is perhaps nowhere more apparent than in the domain tality through material means (Hirschman, 1990).
of human sexuality, which is a potent reminder of the It has also been suggested that achieving immortality
fundamentally animal side of human nature. After is a significant motivating force for collectors (e.g.,
all, as Cole Porter put it, “birds do it, bees do it, even Pearce, 1992).
educated fleas do it.” Supporting the argument that Research inspired by TMT provides empirical
sex is threatening when it is closely associated with support for this general line of thinking. Kasser
creatureliness, research found that when similarities and Sheldon (2000), for instance, demonstrated
between humans and animals were salient, remind- that participants primed with mortality thoughts
ers of death resulted in decreased attraction to the not only reported higher financial expectations for
physical, but not romantic (and hence uniquely themselves 15 years in the future but also became
human), aspects of sex (Goldenberg, Cox, Pyszczyn- greedier and less environmentally sensitive in a forest-
ski, Greenberg, & Solomon, 2002). These researchers management simulation. Another study (Mandel
also found that when participants were primed with & Heine, 1999) revealed that subtle reminders of
human-animal similarities, thinking about physical, mortality increase preference for high-status prod-
but not romantic, aspects of sex increased the acces- ucts such as Lexus automobiles or Rolex watches.
sibility of death-related thoughts. These findings sug- Rindfleisch, Burroughs, and Wong (2009) similarly
gest that construing human sex as indistinguishable showed that the strong connections materialistic
from animal copulation can be uncomfortable due to individuals form with their brands serve to buffer
the mortality concerns it arouses. against existential insecurity. These and other paral-
Confrontations with the natural world at its lel findings suggest that people often seek protection
wildest can induce a similar sense of discomfort. from existential anxiety in the sense of value and self-
Studies reveal that people have more death thoughts esteem provided by material objects.
in wilderness settings compared to cultivated nature
or urban settings, and that death reminders reduce The Personal Dimension
the perceived beauty of wild landscapes and increase The personal dimension refers to how individuals
the perceived beauty of cultivated landscapes (Koole relate to themselves (van Deurzen, 2002). It includes
& Van den Berg, 2005). This helps explain the appeal views about their identity, character, past experience,
of carefully mowed lawns and manicured gardens and future possibilities. In this section we examine
and the many hours that people devote to imposing how existential concerns affect human behavior and
unnatural order on their natural environment. experience on the personal dimension, particularly
Finally, it has also been found that mortality in the context of self-esteem and psychopathology.
thoughts increase people’s desire to fly, whereas
engaging in flight fantasies mitigates defensive reac- self-esteem
tions to mortality thoughts. These findings suggest Self-esteem refers to people’s evaluations of
that fantasies of flight can serve a terror manage- themselves, and it is almost axiomatic in social psy-
ment function by helping people to transcend phys- chology that people strive for positive self-esteem.
ical confines, albeit in imagination only (Solomon, The question of why people need self-esteem was, in
Greenberg, Pyszczynski, Cohen, & Ogilvie, 2009). fact, one of the original questions that begot TMT.
All in all, our review indicates that existential con- The theory posits that self-esteem functions to keep
cerns play a distinct role in humankind’s relation- death anxiety at bay (for a review, see Pyszczynski,
ships with their bodies and nature. Now we examine Greenberg, Solomon, Arndt, & Schimel, 2004).
how existential concerns affect our relationship with According to TMT, self-esteem is attained by meet-
material possessions. ing or exceeding the standards of value that are part

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
of one’s cultural worldview; it is the sense that one Research shows, for example, that in achievement-
is a valuable contributor to a meaningful universe. related tasks, participants reminded of their mor-
Although the standards upon which self-esteem is tality are more likely to attribute positive outcomes
contingent vary across cultures and individuals, the to internal, stable, and global causes and negative
underlying need for self-esteem is universal. outcomes to external, unstable, and specific causes
A large body of research supports the notion that compared to participants in a control condition
self-esteem provides a buffer against existential anxi- (Mikulincer & Florian, 2002). Furthermore, the
ety. In the first test of this hypothesis, as we have seen, accessibility of death-related thoughts induced by
Greenberg, Solomon and colleagues (1992) showed mortality reminders is mitigated when participants
that boosting participants’ self-esteem through bogus are given the opportunity to provide causal attribu-
positive feedback leads to lower levels of self-reported tions excusing their failure.
anxiety in response to graphic depictions of death, and The body of research reviewed here highlights
lower physiological arousal when anticipating painful the role of self-esteem in buffering existential anxi-
electric shocks. Other research revealed that both arti- ety. Humans struggle for a sense of identity and
ficially enhanced and dispositionally high self-esteem significance in the world, partly as a way to shield
are associated with lower levels of worldview defense themselves from death and its attendant anxieties.
and lower death-thought accessibility in response to This search for validation and value oftentimes takes
mortality reminders (Harmon-Jones et al., 1997), as the form of expanding oneself and merging with
well as lower levels of defensive distortions aimed at something larger than oneself. The family, nation,
denying vulnerability to early death (Greenberg et al. religion, science, or art can all serve as avenues for a
1993). Studies also demonstrate that death-thought person to find meaning and value in a vast arena that
accessibility increases when participants think about will not be shattered by one’s death. These avenues
their “undesired self” (Ogilvie, Cohen, & Solomon, for self-expansion provide the person with symbolic
2008) or when their self-esteem is directly threatened, immortality—the sense that one is a valuable part
such as when they are informed that their person- of something larger, more significant, and longer
ality is ill suited for their career aspirations (Hayes, lasting than one’s individual existence. In the words
Schimel, Faucher, & Williams, 2008). Conversely, of John Steinbeck, “After the bare requisites of liv-
having participants affirm their most important val- ing and reproducing, man wants most to leave some
ues reverses the effect of self-esteem threat on death- record of himself, a proof, perhaps, that he has really
thought accessibility (Hayes et al., 2008). existed. He leaves his proof on wood, on stone, or
In addition to evidence that self-esteem buffers on the lives of other people. This deep desire exists
death anxiety, research also shows that death remind- in everyone, from the boy who scribbles on a wall to
ers increase people’s striving for self-esteem. In one the Buddha who etches his image in the race mind”
dramatic illustration of this point, Israeli soldiers (1995, p. 49). Lifton (1979) has elaborated on the
engaged in more risky driving behavior after mor- various ways in which humans strive for symbolic
tality reminders, but only to the extent they derived immortality, the most common of which seem to be
self-esteem from their driving ability (Taubman Ben- living on through one’s progeny and through one’s
Ari, Florian, & Mikulincer, 1999). Further support works.
for the notion that existential anxiety increases striv- In line with the idea that symbolic immortality
ing for self-esteem in domains one is invested in is can help to manage the threat of death, research has
provided by studies showing that mortality salience found an inverse correlation between self-reports
improved strength performance among individuals of symbolic immortality and fear of personal death
invested in strength training, but it had no impact on (Florian & Mikulincer, 1998). In the same study, a
those not invested in strength training (Peters, Green- high sense of symbolic immortality also reduced par-
berg, & Williams, 2005). Similarly, mortality salience ticipants’ tendency to respond to mortality remind-
increased identification with one’s body and interest ers with increased worldview defense, suggesting
in sex among people high in body self-esteem, but a protective, anxiety-buffering role for symbolic
not among those with low body self-esteem (Golden- immortality. Interestingly, the desire for symbolic
berg, McCoy, Pyszczynski, Greenberg, & Solomon, immortality may at times even trump the desire for
2000). life. In a study reported in The Economist, more than
Existential anxiety also amplifies self-serving half of 198 Olympic-level American athletes said
biases, which are perhaps the most commonly that they would take a banned drug if they knew
researched manifestation of the need for self-esteem. that by taking it they would win every competition

 the rol e of death in life


for the next 5 years but then die from the substance’s enduring tendency to experience negative emotional
side effects (“Superhuman Heroes,” 1998). The case states and which is robustly associated with a broad
of suicide bombers is another illustration of how array of psychological disorders (Malouff, Thorstein-
the quest for symbolic immortality can paradoxi- sson, & Schutte, 2005), makes it more difficult for
cally lead to suicide (Kruglanski, Chen, Dechesne, individuals to manage death anxiety (e.g., Arndt &
Fishman, & Orehek, 2009). Solomon, 2003; Goldenberg, Pyszczynski, McCoy,
In this section, we have reviewed evidence show- Greenberg, & Solomon, 1999; Goldenberg, Rout-
ing that a personal sense of worth and significance ledge, & Arndt, 2009).
can effectively buffer anxiety. In the next section, we TMT has also recently been applied to the under-
discuss findings from the emerging literature on the standing of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) in
role of death anxiety in psychological disorders. the form of anxiety-buffer disruption theory (Pyszc-
zynski & Kesebir, 2011). Anxiety-buffer disruption
psychopathology theory posits that PTSD results from a breakdown
TMT argues that successful management of exis- in one’s anxiety-buffering system, which normally
tential anxiety is required for effective functioning provides protection from anxiety in general and
and psychological well-being. If that is the case, then death anxiety in particular. When the anxiety buf-
problems in managing this anxiety would be associ- fer stops functioning effectively due to a traumatic
ated with psychological disturbances. Existentially encounter, the individual becomes defenseless in the
oriented scholars have often argued that psycho- face of incapacitating fears and anxieties. As a conse-
logical disorders reflect extreme, graceless, or ineffi- quence, he or she is flooded with overwhelming anx-
cient ways of dealing with existential anxiety (Becker, iety, leading to hyperarousability, intrusive thoughts,
1971, 1973; Lifton, 1979; Yalom, 1980). Becker and avoidance behavior, the primary clusters of
(1973), for example, posited that mental illness PTSD symptoms. Recent research has supported the
results when people fail in their death-transcendence hypothesis that if PTSD involves a disrupted anxi-
goals. Psychiatrist Irvin Yalom similarly noted: ety buffer, PTSD-inflicted individuals would not
“Either because of extraordinary stress or because of respond to death reminders in the way that psycho-
an inadequacy of available defensive strategies, the logically healthier individuals with functional anxi-
individual who enters the realm called ‘patienthood’ ety buffers do. A study conducted in the aftermath
has found insufficient the universal modes of deal- of the 2005 Zarand earthquake in Iran, for example,
ing with death fear and has been driven to extreme showed that individuals with high PTSD symptom
modes of defense. These defensive maneuvers, often severity 2 years after the earthquake did not respond
clumsy modes of dealing with terror, constitute the to mortality reminders with typical cultural world-
presenting clinical picture” (1980, p. 111). view defenses (Abdollahi, Pyszczynski, Maxfield, &
Recent TMT studies provide empirical support Luszczynska, in press). Another study conducted
for the proposition that psychological disorders are with survivors of the Ivory Coast civil war (Chatard
associated with mismanaged death anxiety (for a et al., 2011) revealed that participants with high
review, see Arndt, Routledge, Cox, & Goldenberg, levels of PTSD symptoms did not respond to mor-
2005). Mortality reminders have been found to tality reminders with the typical immediate suppres-
exacerbate anxiety symptoms in those who suffer sion of death-related thoughts, while those with low
from anxiety disorders such as phobia and obsessive- PTSD symptom levels in the study did. These and
compulsive disorder (Strachan et al., 2007). In one similar studies (e.g., Edmondson, 2009; Kesebir,
study, clinically diagnosed spider phobics spent less Luszczynska, Pyszczynski, & Benight, in press) pro-
time looking at pictures of spiders presented on vide encouraging initial support for anxiety-buffer
a computer screen after mortality reminders, and disruption theory, though more research is of course
they also rated the spiders in the pictures as more needed. Although the well-functioning individuals
threatening. No such effect of mortality reminders who have been studied in the vast majority of TMT
was observed among nonphobic participants. In a studies have shown little signs of the abject terror
similar vein, following mortality salience, college of death posited by the theory, presumably because
students who scored high on a measure of contami- their anxiety-buffer systems are intact, this terror is
nation obsession and compulsive hand-washing easy to see in those whose anxiety buffers are mal-
used more water to wash their hands after they had functioning. Expanded use of TMT to understand
been soiled with gooey electrode gel. Other stud- psychological disorders seems a promising line of
ies have shown that neuroticism, which refers to an inquiry for the future.

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
The Social Dimension and values, and conversely, why they are generally
The social dimension refers to our relationships uncomfortable around, and at times hostile toward,
with other people, the culture we live in, and the those who do not.
groups that make the social fabric of daily life. The If cultural worldviews protect against the potential
need to belong to and affiliate is a powerful, fun- for terror inherent in the knowledge of one’s mortality,
damental, and extremely potent human motive then mortality reminders would intensify the need to
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Although this need hold on to one’s ingroup and worldview and defend
may have initially evolved because of the distinct them against the outgroup and rival worldviews.
evolutionary advantages that group living provides, In addition, threats to one’s ingroup and cultural
TMT argues that with the evolution of sophisti- worldview should increase death-related thoughts and
cated intelligence, it took on the existential function anxieties. Since the earliest days of TMT research,
of helping people manage death-related anxiety. an avalanche of studies has supported these prepo-
Here we present this literature in two major sec- sitions and demonstrated the role of these tenden-
tions; first we review how existential motivation cies in ingroup bias and outgroup hostility. The first
affects humans as they relate to the groups to which evidence of the role of death concerns in intergroup
they belong (and do not belong) and then we review conflict came from a study by Greenberg and col-
the role of existential motivation in close personal leagues (1990) which showed that after mortality
relationships. reminders, American Christians evaluated a fellow
Christian student more positively and a Jewish student
group belonging and worldview more negatively. Other studies found that mortality
validation salience increases preference for an author with pro-
According to TMT, faith in one’s cultural world- American views over an author with anti-American
view is a potent buffer against existential anxiety. views among American students (Greenberg et al.,
The term worldview comes from the German word 1994), and it increases criticism of an anti-Japan essay
Weltanschauung, meaning a view or perspective on writer among Japanese students (Heine, Harihara,
the world that encompasses one’s total outlook on Niiya, & 2002). Conversely, when participants heav-
life, society, and its institutions (Koltko-Rivera, ily invested in their Canadian identity were exposed to
2004). TMT defines cultural worldviews as per- material that derogates Canadian culture, they exhib-
sonally and culturally held assumptions and beliefs ited increased accessibility of death-related thoughts
about the nature of existence. Individuals construct (Schimel, Hayes, Williams, & Jahrig, 2007).
their own individualized worldviews as they go Further corroborating the existential function
through life by combining the beliefs and values served by the ingroup, a study conducted in Italy
of the individuals with whom they interact, the revealed that reminders of mortality increase peoples’
groups to which they belong, and the broader society identification with their ethnic identities as Italians,
that surrounds them. Cultural norms, moral values, their belief in the entitativity of this identity, and
and religious beliefs are among the most central their ingroup bias (Castano, Yzerbyt, Paladino, &
cultural worldviews. They are “theories of reality” Sacchi, 2002). These participants rated Italians as a
that explain what life is and how it should be lived. significantly more stable, coherent, and distinct eth-
As such, they imbue existence with meaning, pur- nic group after mortality reminders, while another
pose, structure, and permanence, thereby helping to study found mortality reminders to lead participants
control anxiety. to view their ingroup as more human (Vaes, Heflick,
Individuals are heavily invested in their world- & Goldenberg, 2010). Mortality reminders have
views and rely on them for navigating through life. been reported to intensify ingroup favoritism (e.g.,
Yet there is a problem with worldviews: It is impos- Castano et al. 2002; Tam, Chiu, & Lau, 2007), and
sible to definitively prove the accuracy or superiority this bias seems to occur even when the group alloca-
of one’s own worldview. As shared human construc- tion is based on minimally meaningful criteria such
tions, cultural worldviews depend on social consen- as aesthetic preferences (Harmon-Jones, Greenberg,
sus for sustenance (Hardin & Higgins, 1996). The Solomon, & Simon, 1996).
wide diversity of extant worldviews exacerbates peo- It is important to note, however, that according to
ple’s motivation to validate their own worldviews as TMT, ingroups provide existential protection only to
a protection against existential anxiety. This, from a the extent that they are a source of value and mean-
TMT perspective, is the central reason people are ing. The desire to affiliate with groups thus depends
attracted to those who share their cherished beliefs on the broader connotations and value of this

 the rol e of death in life


affiliation for the individual. In support of this idea, killing of thousands of civilians as collateral damage.
Harmon-Jones and colleagues (1996) found that Research has also shown that reminders of death lead
mortality primes did not increase ingroup bias when conservative Israelis to view violence against Pales-
group assignment was entirely random. Similarly, tinians as more justified (Hirschberger & Ein-Dor,
participants reminded of their own death exhibited 2006).
reduced identification with their college football team These findings imply that the psychological
after the team’s loss (Dechesne, Greenberg, Arndt, & protection that cultural worldviews provide against
Schimel, 2000); and Mexican American participants the reality of death often comes at the price of
primed with mortality showed decreased affiliation increased intergroup conflict and violence. Fortu-
with their ethnicity when they were exposed to a nately, the link between existential anxiety and
negative example of their ingroup by reading about intergroup conflict is neither automatic nor inevi-
a “Mexican drug cartel chief ” (Arndt, Greenberg, table. An early study showed that a chronically high
Schimel, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 2002). or temporarily heightened level of tolerance can
A perceived threat to one’s cherished beliefs can eliminate negative reactions toward dissimilar oth-
undermine the much-needed sense of meaning, ers induced by mortality primes (Greenberg, Simon
value, and existential security, propelling people et al., 1992). Since then, research has revealed that
to defend their worldview and even resort to vio- the effect of mortality reminders on reactions to
lence. This is why existential anxiety is not only threatening others depends on the particular norms
associated with an intensified need to validate one’s that are salient to the person. Jonas and colleagues
worldview and cling to one’s ingroup but also with (2008) demonstrated, for instance, that whereas a
a host of unsavory behaviors such as outgroup dero- mortality prime led people to become harsher toward
gation, stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. a moral transgressor when conservative values were
Reminders of death have been shown to increase made salient, a benevolence prime counteracted this
stereotypic thinking about an outgroup and pref- effect. Others found that the violence-promoting
erence for those who confirm one’s stereotypes effects of death anxiety can be attenuated or reversed
(Schimel et al., 1999), as well as punitive reactions when values such as compassion or shared human-
toward those who violate one’s moral/cultural values ity are salient (Motyl, Hart, & Pyszczynski, 2010;
(Rosenblatt et al., 1989). Rothschild, Abdollahi, & Pyszczynski, 2009). This
The outrage felt at worldview-threatening others line of research may suggest a promising direction
can also lead people to resort to violence, as exem- for those who wish to promote peace in the face of
plified by a study in which participants in a mortal- ongoing intractable war and violence.
ity salience condition administered a larger amount
of hot sauce to a person who disliked hot sauce and close personal relationships
disparaged their political ideology (McGregor et al., As Bowlby (1969) has pointed out, all infants are
1998). Furthermore, there is evidence that the anni- born with an attachment system oriented toward
hilation of worldview-threatening others can mollify maintaining proximity to significant others in times
death anxiety. Hayes, Schimel, and Williams (2008) of stress. To survive, children need caregivers who
found that while Christian participants responded will provide protection and ensure that their needs
with increased death-thought accessibility to a news are met. The attachment relationship to the care-
article reporting the Muslimization of Nazareth, giver helps children manage distress and feel secure,
informing them that many Muslims had died in a even before they possess the cognitive complexity to
plane crash on their way to Nazareth eliminated this develop a sense of self or a concept of death. In the
effect of worldview threat. Other studies revealed that last decade, TMT research has demonstrated that
existential fears can heighten the support for violence the anxiety-buffering role of interpersonal attach-
committed against worldview-threatening others. ments continues well into adulthood (Hart, Shaver,
Pyszczynski et al. (2006) documented that mortal- & Goldenberg, 2005; Mikulincer et al., 2003). Par-
ity reminders increased Iranian college students’ sup- ents continue to function as a safe haven in the face
port for martyrdom attacks against the United States. of death thoughts, as revealed by studies showing
A follow-up study by these authors found that that activating thoughts of one’s parent in response
reminders of death or 9/11 made politically conser- to mortality reminders reduces death-thought acces-
vative American college students more accepting of sibility and worldview defense (Cox et al., 2008).
extreme military action in the War on Terror, such Close personal relationships—be they with fam-
as the use of nuclear and chemical weapons, or the ily members, romantic partners, or friends—work

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
in concert with faith in one’s worldview and self- a sense of security, and the hope of transcending
esteem in a dynamic, interrelated system to provide death, thereby acting as a powerful balm against
protection against existential anxiety. existential fear.
In support of the anxiety-buffering function of
personal relationships, mortality reminders have The Spiritual Dimension
been found to increase people’s willingness to ini- The spiritual dimension entails the human
tiate social interactions and decrease their sensi- proclivity to connect with something greater than
tivity to rejection (Taubman Ben-Ari, Findler, & oneself, typically involving abstract, supernatural,
Mikulincer, 2002). Research has also shown that magical, or divine beings or entities. It encom-
mortality thoughts lead to reports of increased com- passes our beliefs, values, and ideals pertaining to
mitment to one’s romantic partner (Florian, Miku- these entities as well as the experiences and altered
lincer, & Hirschberger, 2002). Conversely, inducing states of consciousness that are often part of these
participants to think about their relationship prob- relationships. From an existential perspective, the
lems (Florian et al., 2002) or about fear of intimacy spiritual dimension functions to help people tran-
(Taubman Ben-Ari, 2004) increases the accessibility scend the limitations of human existence in general
of death-related thoughts. Consistent with the idea and mortality in particular. In this section, we will
that close personal attachments serve to buffer death review research on how concerns about mortality
anxiety, writing about one’s romantic commitment affect behavior on the spiritual dimension, particu-
has been demonstrated to eliminate the need to larly when it comes to the questions of meaning,
resort to worldview defense after mortality remind- religion, and spirituality.
ers (Florian et al., 2002). Human beings require meaning, both to navi-
Research also shows that individual differences in gate through the mundane tasks of daily life and
attachment style predict differences in how people to imbue their lives with purpose and transcendent
respond to existential threats. While correlational value (Frankl, 1963). To live without meaning, val-
studies document that securely attached individu- ues, or ideals is distressing (Yalom, 1980), and many
als report less fear of death than insecurely attached people are willing to live and die for their ideals
individuals (Mikulincer, Florian, & Tolmacz, 1990), and values. TMT posits that believing that things
experiments show that chronic attachment styles are as they are supposed to be—that the mundane
moderate terror management defenses. Mikulincer ways of life make sense, and that human existence
and Florian (2000), for example, found that mortal- fits into some overall meaningful pattern—provides
ity reminders led to harsher judgments about moral the coherence, structure, and security that pro-
transgressions among insecurely attached, but not tect people against death anxiety. Indeed, cultural
securely attached, individuals. In contrast, death worldviews and personal relationships can succeed
thoughts led to an increase in one’s sense of symbolic as existential anxiety buffers only to the extent they
immortality and in the desire for intimacy among provide the individual with this sense of meaning.
securely attached persons but not insecurely attached Supporting the notion that maintaining a mean-
persons. From this body of research, close personal ingful view of reality is essential for protection
relationships emerge as an integral part of the exis- against existential anxiety, research finds, for exam-
tential anxiety-buffer system—intimately related to ple, that reminders of mortality increase distaste for
self-esteem and worldview validation needs, but dis- apparently meaningless art, particularly among those
tinct from them. It is possible that the reliance on who dispositionally prefer unambiguous knowl-
interpersonal attachments as an existential defense edge (Landau, Greenberg, Solomon, Pyszczynski,
involves more automatic and biologically based & Martens, 2006). Similarly, Vess, Routledge,
mechanisms, while worldview defense is mediated Landau, and Arndt (2009) documented that death
by cultural-symbolic processes (Wisman & Koole, reminders bolster perceptions of life’s meaning
2003). among participants with a high personal need for
The social dimension constitutes an extraordi- structure—those who are inclined to prefer simple
narily important aspect of the human experience— and unambiguous interpretations of reality. Further-
heaven, as well as hell, is indeed other people. In more, death thoughts are found to lead people to
this section, we have reviewed the role that existen- imbue everyday actions with more meaning and to
tial concerns play on this dimension. As we have judge their current actions to be more meaningfully
seen, the groups to which we belong and the people connected to their long-term goals (Landau, Kosloff,
with whom we relate can provide meaning, value, & Schmeichel, 2010). The desire to see the world

 the rol e of death in life


as a just and orderly place (Lerner, 1980) can also anxiety-buffering effects of belief in afterlife is
be considered a manifestation of the fundamental provided by Dechesne and colleagues (2003), who
need for meaning, structure, and comprehensibil- found that exposure to scientific-looking evidence
ity. In line with this, Landau and colleagues (2004) about the existence of life after death eliminates
found that for participants high in need for struc- the typical increased worldview defense and striv-
ture, reminders of mortality increased preference for ing for self-esteem that is produced by death primes.
narratives that suggest a just world and a benevolent Mortality reminders have also been demonstrated to
causal order of events in the social world (see also intensify faith in supernatural agents. Norenzayan
Hirschberger, 2006). and Hansen (2006) found that after the activation
This body of research, taken together, suggests of death thoughts, North Americans, particularly
that thoughts of death intensify the desire to see those who were religiously affiliated, displayed
the world as a meaningful, structured, and ordered stronger belief in God and divine intervention, even
place, particularly for people who are predisposed to showing greater belief in spiritual entities associated
simpler interpretations of reality. Mortality thoughts with religious faiths other than their own.
also seem to intensify the need to find meaning Research also suggests that different orientations
on a larger scale, a so-called cosmic meaning—the to religious faith have different psychological con-
sense that “life in general or at least human life fits sequences. Whereas a fundamentalist orientation
into some overall coherent pattern” (Yalom, 1980, has been shown to be associated with a variety of
p. 423). In Becker’s words, “man cannot endure his socially undesirable tendencies, an intrinsic orienta-
own littleness unless he can translate it into meaning- tion appears to be especially effective in managing
fulness on the largest possible level” (1973, p. 196). death-related fears. Religious fundamentalism refers
The belief that there is some superordinate design to to the belief that there is one absolute truth and that
life and that each individual has some particular role all other belief systems are wrong and evil. A large
to play in this design can thus be an extraordinary body of research has found religious fundamental-
source of existential comfort. ism to be positively associated with racial prejudice
Historically, religions have been the major (e.g., Altemeyer, 2003; Altemeyer & Hunsberger,
sources of cosmic meaning, and despite the increase 1992; Laythe, Finkel, & Kirkpatrick, 2001), reli-
in popularity of atheistic worldviews (e.g., Dawkins, gious ethnocentrism (Altemeyer, 2003), and support
2006; Harris, 2004; Hitchens, 2007), this is true for for militarism (e.g., Henderson-King, Henderson-
the vast majority of people today as well. Religions, King, Bolea, Koches, & Kauffman, 2004; Nelson
typically, offer a comprehensive meaning schema, & Milburn, 1999). These attitudes are mediated
according to which the world and human life are by the absolutist authoritarian structure of the fun-
part of a divinely ordained plan. This plan includes damentalist’s belief system (Laythe et al., 2001).
stories about the origin of the universe, clear moral A rigid black-and-white orientation to truth is likely
guidelines, and theodicies that help people explain to make beliefs that deviate from one’s own espe-
and endure suffering—all of which make the inevi- cially threatening and thus encourage more vigorous
tability of death easier to handle. American histo- attempts to assert the correctness of those beliefs—
rian and philosopher Will Durant talked about the derogation of and violence toward those with differ-
“eternal hunger of mankind for supernatural con- ent beliefs are ways of bolstering confidence in the
solations” (1932, p. 36), and TMT argues that this veracity of one’s own beliefs.
hunger stems largely from existential anxieties, and Intrinsic religious orientation, on the other hand,
particularly the need to deal with the overwhelm- seems to have more benefits and few costs. Batson,
ing reality of death (for a comprehensive review of Schoenrade, and Ventis (1993) report that intrin-
the terror management function of religion, see Vail sic religious beliefs are associated with lessened
et al., 2010). death anxiety and heightened existential well-being.
Religions, unlike any other institutions, are Research also shows that people high in intrinsic reli-
capable of promising literal immortality to their giousness do not engage in some forms of worldview
believers—in the form of heaven, reincarnation, defense after reminders of mortality, and experience
or some other form of afterlife—which can be a lessened death-thought accessibility following mor-
powerful tool in mollifying death anxiety. Research tality salience if they are given a chance to affirm
shows, for example, that among those who believe their religious beliefs (Jonas & Fischer, 2006).
in an afterlife, reminders of death increase this belief Becker (1973) notes the distinctive human need
(Osarchuk & Tatz, 1973). Further support for the “to spiritualize human life, to lift it onto a special

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
immortal plane, beyond the cycles of life and death of meaning, an underlying reality that transcends
that characterize all other organisms” (p. 231). everyday life, and a sacralized, magical cosmos—a
While religions can effectively address this need for need that is, at least partially, driven by existential
some people, others prefer less clearly structured concerns. This concludes our discussion of how
forms of spirituality. Spirituality can be defined existential motivation influences the human experi-
as a “personal quest for understanding answers to ence on the four dimensions of living. Our review
ultimate questions about life, about meaning, and suggests that on all the four dimensions—the physi-
about relationship to the sacred or transcendent” cal, social, psychological, and spiritual—knowledge
(Koenig, McCullough, & Larson, 2001, p. 18). The of one’s mortality and accompanying existential
idea of the sacred is considered to be the distinctive concerns intensify people’s striving for special
core of spirituality (e.g., Pargament, 1999), and it meaning, value, and security. An inevitable fate of
has been frequently proposed that people fervently nonexistence, a realization that “our existence is but
desire to live in a “sacralized cosmos” (Eliade, 1959). a brief crack of light between two eternities of dark-
By providing a sense of transcendence, boundless- ness” (Nabokov, 1999, p. 9), is extremely difficult
ness, ultimate value and purpose, the sacred can to accept, which renders the quest for assurances
alleviate the pain accompanying one’s awareness of of invulnerability a primary human motive. As we
creatureliness, powerlessness, and ultimate finitude. have seen, a broad array of human behaviors—from
Supporting this notion, studies show that construing self-esteem striving to outgroup derogation, from
different aspects of the world (e.g., nature, children, materialism to spirituality—serve to provide pro-
music) in sacred terms can protect the individual tection against existential dread. The breadth and
against death anxiety and its possibly destructive depth of phenomena that have been subjected to
effects such as outgroup hostility and materialism research by TMT and shown to be affected by exis-
(Kesebir, Chiu, & Pyszczynski, unpublished data). tential concerns testifies to the prominent role that
The human predilection for a sacred, magical, existential motivation plays in human life. There
divinely inspired view of reality can also manifest remains, however, one last behavioral tendency that
itself in the affection for charismatic leaders, for can be induced by existential motivation we have
hero worshipping, and the fascination with celeb- not yet explored.
rities. The word charisma, for example, originates
from a Greek word meaning “divine gift,” or “talent Transcending Death by Escaping
from God,” and studies find that reminders of death Self-Awareness
intensify preference and support for charismatic As discussed earlier in this chapter, self-awareness
leaders who proclaim the superiority of one’s ingroup is a prerequisite for experiencing existential anxiety.
(Cohen, Solomon, Maxfield, Pyszczynski, & Green- In support of this claim, research shows that simple
berg, 2004). Cultural heroes, as well as famous peo- self-awareness manipulations, such as viewing one-
ple who represent individually and collectively held self in a mirror, increase the accessibility of thoughts
values, tend to be perceived as symbolically and lit- about both life and death (Silvia, 2001). This sug-
erally immortal, which might help their admirers to gests that escaping self-awareness would be one way
transcend death and insignificance by proxy. In sup- to obviate the problem of thoughts of death. Indeed,
port of the existential function of famous people, research has shown that participants induced to
Kesebir, Chiu, and Kim (unpublished data) demon- write about death spend less time on the task when
strated that after mortality reminders, participants they are made to feel self-aware (Arndt, Greenberg,
expect famous people to be remembered for a longer Simon, Pyszczynski, & Solomon, 1998). This indi-
time in the future, and this effect is qualified by how cates that self-awareness makes mortality thoughts
much the famous people represent cultural values. either more accessible or more threatening.
Similarly, the more a famous person was perceived According to self-awareness theory (Duval &
to represent her culture’s values, the less likely peo- Wicklund, 1972), self-focused attention triggers
ple thought that a plane she boarded would crash. evaluative processes in which people compare them-
These findings suggest that charismatic, heroic, or selves to whatever standards and values are currently
famous people might occupy a demigod status in salient. If they perceive themselves as falling short of
the eyes of their fans, and in so doing provide them these standards, they either change their behavior in
with meaning and existential stamina. the direction of the standards or attempt to resolve the
In this section, we have argued that humans distress this produces by trying to lose self-awareness
harbor a potent need for an all-encompassing sense (Carver & Scheier, 1981; Duval & Wicklund, 1972).

 the rol e of death in life


TMT suggests that self-awareness leads to comparison loss of self-awareness is still at a preliminary stage.
with standards, and to behavior aimed at reducing any Yet there are data showing that death-related stimuli
discrepancies that are detected, because self-awareness increase consumers’ desire to purchase higher quan-
can cause a leakage of existential terror. As a way to tities of food products and lead them to actually eat
buffer this terror, people strive to meet their standards higher quantities, particularly among those who
of value and acquire the self-esteem this brings (Pyszc- have low self-esteem (Mandel & Smeesters, 2008).
zynski, Greenberg, Solomon, & Hamilton, 1990), Similarly, Hirschberger and Ein-Dor (2005) found
which both require comparisons with standards to that eating a tasty snack eliminated the effects of MS
effectively accomplish. Consistent with this analysis, on defensive responses. There is also a plethora of
self-awareness has been shown to lead to a host of anecdotal evidence suggesting that after the 9/11
behaviors that are also induced by mortality remind- attacks, Americans resorted to drinking, gambling,
ers—behaviors more in tune with both personal and renting videos, watching television, and shopping
social standards of value (Diener & Wallbom, 1976; as a way to deal with the shock (Pyszczynski, Solo-
Scheier & Carver, 1988; Wicklund, 1975) or behav- mon, & Greenberg, 2003). The New York Times, for
iors aimed at maintaining self-esteem such as the example, reports three months after the event that
self-serving attributional bias (Duval & Silvia, 2002; according to liquor distributors, “the dramatic rise
Federoff & Harvey, 1976). in consumption of alcoholic beverages immediately
The human eagerness to lose self-awareness, after September 11 was a nationwide phenomenon”
escape consciousness, or enter a state of forgetful- (Burros, 2001). This could be interpreted as an
ness of existence can thus be a response to existential attempt on the Americans’ part to flee the massive
anxiety. The TMT analysis suggests that underly- existential insecurity produced by the 9/11 attacks.
ing the desire to escape self-awareness is something There is also indirect evidence for the idea that
even deeper than the wish to escape thoughts of existential anxiety can generate the urge to escape
one’s shortcomings or the modern culture’s empha- self-awareness: Studies show that among restrained
sis on and fascination with selfhood (Baumeister, eaters, self-esteem threats increase the amount of eat-
1991)—it is the need to evade confrontation with ing (Heatherton, Herman, & Polivy, 1991; Polivy,
the existential reality of death and the potential for Herman, & McFarlane, 1994), suggesting that a
terror this invokes. A variety of behaviors have been threat to one’s existential anxiety buffer might inten-
shown to reduce levels of self-awareness, including sify the desire to lose self-awareness.
alcohol consumption (Hull, 1981), binge eating Under what conditions would existential anxiety
(Heatherton & Baumeister, 1991), television view- drive people to shut off self-awareness, and under
ing (Moskalenko, & Heine, 2003), and sexual mas- what conditions would it lead to a more active striv-
ochism (Baumeister, 1988). Spiritual exercises such ing for meaning, value, and security? This question is
as meditation are also considered to lead to lower critical, considering that people’s attempts to escape
levels of self-awareness (Baumeister, 1991). A myriad self-awareness sometimes occur through extremely
of religious doctrines converge on the importance self-destructive means. Previous research suggests
of shedding the self and emphasize mystical prac- that avoidance of self-awareness occurs primarily
tices that help one lose self-consciousness—such as when people perceive the discrepancy between their
reaching Nirvana in Buddhism or fanaa in Sufism. current state and ideal state to be so high that it is
In principle, any absorbing activity can provide an unlikely to be reduced (Duval, Duval, & Mulilis,
effective means of escape. Csikszentmihalyi’s concept 1992). Drawing a parallel, we might predict that
of flow, which he characterizes as a process that pro- existential anxiety is most likely to lead to self-escapist
duces optimal human experience, similarly entails a behaviors when people perceive the gap between
loss of self-consciousness, a merging of action and their actual self and ideal self as hardly bridgeable,
awareness, and a transformation of one’s percep- when they are having extreme difficulties finding
tion of time (Csikszentmihalyi, 1975). It appears meaning in their lives or reconciling their worldviews
that avenues for escaping self-awareness can cover a with their life experiences. In other words, people
broad range from the most sadly self-destructive to would resort to escapism in the face of existential
the most spiritually exalted, and according to TMT, anxiety, when their anxiety buffers are— temporarily
they all help shield the individual from the existen- or chronically—not strong enough to provide pro-
tially problematic implications of self-awareness. tection. The most extreme, irreversible form of flight
Research testing the effects of mortality remind- from the self is suicide (Baumeister, 1990), and in
ers on the desire to engage in activities that promote our analysis, people would be more likely to commit

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
suicide when their anxiety buffers have stopped func- Over the years, TMT studies underwent a
tioning entirely and the ensuing terror is overwhelm- number of refinements and improvements in meth-
ing. An existence devoid of any meaning, value, or odology. This allowed us to obtain converging sup-
hope would turn self-awareness into an unbearable port for the predictions of the theory through a
state and might make suicide an appealing escape. variety of operationalizations and to broaden our
After all, ironically, dying seems to be the one certain understanding of the mechanisms involved in ter-
way to rid oneself from existential anxiety for good ror management. Open-ended items about mor-
(Greenberg, Solomon, & Arndt, 2008). tality, death anxiety scales, proximity to funeral
Escape from self-awareness might also be likely homes, and subliminal death primes, for example,
when people’s self-regulatory energies are depleted have all been shown to instigate terror management
and they lack the stamina needed for actively pursu- responses—a testament to the validity of the role
ing death transcendence. Gailliot, Schmeichel, and death concerns play in human motivation. When
Baumeister (2006) reported an inverse relationship it comes to the topic of methodology, however,
between strength of self-control and the accessibility of we should remember that a whopping majority of
death thoughts—people who are good at self-control TMT studies test the mortality salience hypothe-
seem to have lower levels of chronic death-thought sis; that is, they make mortality thoughts salient to
accessibility, while reminders of mortality are shown to assess their effects on the dependent variable (Burke
lead to poorer self-regulation. When we consider that et al., 2010). While this is a powerful and indispens-
lack of self-control would be associated with behaviors able tool to test hypotheses derived from TMT, the
aimed at escaping the self, the moderating role of self- theory would benefit from new and creative meth-
control in the relationship between death thoughts ods to explore the workings of terror management.
and the demand for losing self-awareness becomes In that sense, the increasingly common use of the
apparent: If death thoughts reduce the capacity for death-thought accessibility methodology in the lit-
self-control, they would be even more likely to lead erature (Hayes et al., 2010) is encouraging, though
to self-escapist behaviors. Discovering ways to prevent not sufficient. We encourage researchers to venture
the depletion of self-regulatory resources in the face of beyond the tried-and-true methods.
existential anxiety, or to replenish them, seems thus a Perhaps the most common criticism leveled
worthy goal for future research. against TMT has been that the effects obtained in
In sum, the capacity for existential anxiety is a response to mortality salience may not be unique to
consequence of self-awareness and the existential thoughts of death per se, but are due to some other
burden is felt most deeply when we are self-aware. aversive state elicited by death thoughts (e.g., nega-
In some instances, particularly when the anxiety tive affect, arousal) or some other threat inherent in
buffer is doing a poor job in counteracting exis- the knowledge of mortality (e.g., meaninglessness,
tential anxiety, people might choose to avoid the uncertainty, lack of control). Although some stud-
self-focused state as a way to make the problem of ies have found that threats to one’s meaning system
existence disappear. We have initial evidence on the or sense of certainty produce the same effects as
role of existential anxiety in prompting the desire to those produced by mortality reminders (e.g., Proulx
escape self-awareness; however, we believe that the & Heine, 2008; van den Bos, Poortvliet, Maas,
topic needs to be explored further, given the serious Miedema, & van den Ham, 2005), the bulk of
costs associated with destructive escape strategies. empirical evidence suggests that priming alternative
topics (e.g., meaninglessness, uncertainty, cultural
Remaining Issues and Future Directions values, failure, giving a speech in public, worries
In the preceding sections, we have presented a about life after college, social exclusion, general
myriad of studies demonstrating how existential anxiety, dental pain, general pain, paralysis) typi-
concerns—and particularly death anxiety—can cally fails to produce the same defensive responses
affect human behavior in diverse life domains. While as priming mortality thoughts (Greenberg et al.,
we believe that the preponderance of evidence puts 2008). Additionally, a recent meta-analysis finds
the role of existential anxiety as a motivational force that the effects produced by mortality salience
for the human psyche beyond dispute, questions follow the unique, signature time course—death
and unexplored areas, naturally, remain. We have thoughts have more influence on distal or symbolic
touched upon some of these issues earlier in our dis- TMT defenses after a delay—consistent with the
cussion, and before concluding, we wish to briefly dual process model described by Pyszczynski and
comment on a few others. colleagues (1999). Furthermore, a growing number

 the rol e of death in life


of studies, as we reviewed earlier, reveal that threats (for a detailed discussion of how these accounts
to one’s anxiety buffer make thoughts of death, interrelate, see Pyszczynski, Greenberg, Solomon,
but not thoughts of other threats, more accessible. & Maxfield, 2006).
We believe these findings constitute convincing evi-
dence affirming the specific role of death concerns. Conclusion
Although we attribute a unique quality to aware- “Death is immense/We all are his/with laughing
ness of death for the human motivational system, mouths/When we are in/the midst of life/he dares
by no means do we wish to intimate that threats to to weep/right in our midst” goes the End Poem by
meaning or uncertainty are without consequence. celebrated Bohemian-Austrian poet Rainer Maria
On the contrary, as we repeatedly noted, mean- Rilke. These lines constitute a literary tribute to the
ing and certainty are essential components of the role that knowledge of death plays in our lives, of
anxiety buffer. Indeed, TMT posits that elements which we are perhaps not sufficiently aware. In this
of the anxiety-buffering system are effective only chapter, we have tried to elucidate this role, based on
to the extent they are held with certainty or faith TMT research that emerged over the last 25 years.
and provide the person with a sense of meaning We have argued that the desire to transcend death
and value. We view existential anxiety as a complex is a powerful motive in human life. A broad array
force that is born from the clash of human desires of human behaviors seems to ultimately serve to
with the realities of existence. TMT construes the render death anxiety and accompanying existential
conflict between the biologically rooted desire for anxieties less accessible or less threatening.
immortality and the inevitability of death as leading Despite the problems that result from human
people to need certainty regarding whatever system awareness of the inevitability of death, many think-
of meaning they use to feel safe and secure. ers have argued that what gives life its depth and
One way of resolving this issue is to view threats intensity is its limited duration. We echo these
to meaning, structure, and certainty as threatening sentiments in suggesting that, though agonizing,
for both epistemic and existential reasons. Epistemi- heightened awareness of death—rather than a for-
cally, people need certainty about a structured and getfulness or denial of it—might ultimately lead
meaningful world because this provides a basis for us to happier, wiser, more authentic lives. The pic-
confident action. This generally motivates people to ture that emerged from terror management theory
seek accurate understandings that will help them research regarding the role of death in human life to
attain their goals that ultimately relate to evolved date has mostly been a dark, unappealing one. It is
proclivities for survival and reproductive success. possible, however, that people can turn the reality of
These motives, at least in rudimentary form, prob- death into a constructive, empowering force for their
ably exist in all animals (except perhaps the sim- lives. Research with people who had near encounters
plest ones). Although there may be rare instances in with death or those who experienced posttraumatic
which an inaccurate understanding of reality facili- growth attests to the tremendously positive, trans-
tates the attainment of concrete goals, accuracy is formative impact that death can have on some peo-
the general rule. Existentially, people need certainty ple. The next frontier of terror management theory
regarding well-structured meanings that help them might thus be a positive existential psychology—a
cope with the fears that result from their awareness research area that investigates how death thoughts
of the reality that existence is finite. This often, can become a source of strength and virtue rather
though not always, leads to a preference for fanci- than a source of dread and destruction.
ful wish-fulfilling beliefs that bear little relation to
objective reality and to avoidance of and disdain for References
anything that might challenge these beliefs. It may Abdollahi, A., Pyszczynski, T., Maxfield, M., & Luszczynska, A.
be, then, that the biases and defenses instigated by (in press). Posttraumatic stress reactions as a disruption in
anxiety-buffer functioning: Dissociation and responses to
epistemic and existential threats often take differ- mortality salience as predictors of severity of post-traumatic
ent forms, and this might help reconcile TMT with symptoms. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice,
other theories that emphasize the needs for meaning and Policy.
and certainty (Heine, Proulx, & Vohs, 2006; Lind Altemeyer, B. (2003). Why do religious fundamentalists tend to
& van den Bos, 2002). That said, we believe that be prejudiced? The International Journal for the Psychology of
Religion, 13, 17–28.
these accounts have more in common than not, Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism,
and despite their differences, they all shed light on religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. The Interna-
how existential anxiety motivates human behavior tional Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113–133.

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Schimel, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Castano, E., Yzerbyt, V., Paladino, M-P., & Sacchi, S. (2002).
Solomon, S. (2002). To belong or not to belong, that is the I belong, therefore, I exist: Ingroup identification, ingroup
question: Terror management and identification with gender entitativity, and ingroup bias. Personality and Social Psychol-
and ethnicity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, ogy Bulletin, 28, 135–143.
26–43. Chatard, A., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Selimbegović, L., Konan,
Arndt, J., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, P., & Van der Linden, M. (2011). Extent of trauma exposure
S. (1998). Terror management and self-awareness: Evidence and PTSD symptom severity as predictors of anxiety-buffer
that mortality salience provokes avoidance of the self- functioning. Trauma Psychology: Theory, Research, Practice,
focused state. Personality and Social Psychological Bulletin, 24, and Policy, doi: 10.1037/a0021085.
1216–1227. Cohen, F., Solomon, S., Maxfield, M., Pyszczynski, T., &
Arndt, J., Routledge, C., Cox, C. R., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2005). Greenberg J. (2004). Fatal attraction: The effects of mor-
The worm at the core: A terror management perspective on tality salience on evaluations of charismatic, task-oriented,
the roots of psychological dysfunction. Applied and Preventa- and relationship-oriented leaders. Psychological Science, 15,
tive Psychology, 11, 191–213. 846–851.
Arndt, J., & Solomon, S. (2003). The control of death and the Cox, C. R., Arndt, J., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Abdollahi, A.,
death of control: The effects of mortality salience, neuroti- & Solomon, S. (2008). Terror management and adults’
cism, and worldview threat on the desire for control. Journal attachment to their parents: The safe haven remains. Journal of
of Research in Personality, 37, 1–22. Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 696–717.
Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., & Ventis, W. L. (1993). Cox, C. R., Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., & Weise, D.
Religion and the individual: A social-psychological perspective. (2007). Disgust, creatureliness, and the accessibility of death
New York: Oxford University Press. related thoughts. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37,
Baumeister, R. F. (1988). Masochism as escape from self. Journal 494–507.
of Sex Research, 25, 28–59. Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San
Baumeister, R. F. (1990). Suicide as escape from self. Psychologi- Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass, Inc.
cal Review, 97, 90–113. Dawkins, R. (2006). The God delusion. London: Bantam.
Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Escaping the self: Alcoholism, spiritual- Dechesne, M., Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., & Schimel, J. (2000).
ity, masochism, and other flights from the burden of selfhood. Terror management and the vicissitudes of sports fan affili-
New York: Basic Books. ation: The effects of mortality salience on optimism and
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: fan identification. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30,
Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental 813–835.
human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Dechesne, M., Pyszczynski, T., Arndt, J., Ransom, S.,
Becker, E. (1971). The birth and death of meaning. New York: Sheldon, K. M., van Knippenberg, A., & Janssen, J. (2003).
Free Press. Literal and symbolic immortality: The effect of evidence
Becker, E. (1973) The denial of death. New York: Free Press. of literal immortality on self-esteem striving in response to
Becker, E. (1975). Escape from evil. New York: Free Press. mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Belk, R. W. (1985). Materialism: Trait aspects of living in 84, 722–737.
the material world. Journal of Consumer Research, 12, Diener, E., & Wallbom, M. (1976). Effects of self-awareness on
265–280. antinormative behavior. Journal of Research in Personality, 10,
Binswanger, L. (1946). The existential analysis school of thought. 107–111.
In R. May, E. Angel, & H. F. Ellenberger (Eds.), Existence Durant, W. (1932). On the meaning of life. New York: Ray Long
(pp. 191–213). New York: Basic Books. & Richard R. Smith.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment. New York: Duval, T., Duval, V., & Mulilis, J. (1992). Effects of self-focus,
Basic Books. discrepancy between self and standard, and outcome expec-
Buber, M. (1923). I and thou (Trans. W. Kaufmann). Edinburgh, tancy favorability on the tendency to match self to standard
Scotland: T&T Clark. and withdraw. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62,
Burke, B. L., Martens, A., & Faucher, E. H. (2010). Two decades 340–348.
of terror management theory: A meta-analysis of mortality Duval, T. S., & Silvia, P. J. (2001). Self-awareness and causal attri-
salience research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, bution: A dual-systems theory. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic
155–195. Publishers.
Burros, M. (2001, December 5). In a stressed city, no room at Duval, T. S., & Silvia, P. J. (2002). Self-awareness, probability of
the bar. The New York Times. Retrieved from http://www. improvement, and the self-serving bias. Journal of Personality
nytimes.com/2001/12/05/dining/in- a- stressed- city- no- and Social Psychology, 82, 49–61.
room-at-the-bar.html Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972) A theory of objective self-
Carver, C.S. (1979). A cybernetic model of self-attention awareness. New York: Academic Press.
processes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, Edmondson, D. (2009). From shattered assumptions to weakened
1251–1281. worldviews: Evidence of anxiety buffer disruption in individu-
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self- als with trauma symptoms. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation,
regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. University of Connecticut, Storrs.
New York: Springer. Eliade, M. (1959). The sacred and the profane. (Trans. W. R. Trask).
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Control processes San Diego, CA: Harcourt.
and self-organization as complementary principles under- Federoff, N. A., & Harvey, J. H. (1976). Focus of attention, self-
lying behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, esteem, and the attribution of causality. Journal of Research in
304–315. Personality, 10, 336–345.

 the rol e of death in life


Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1998). Symbolic immortality and on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural
the management of the terror of death: The moderating role worldview. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58,
of attachment style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- 308–318.
ogy, 74, 725–734. Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Simon, L., &
Florian, V., Mikulincer, M., & Hirschberger, G. (2002). The Breus, M. (1994). Role of consciousness and accessibility of
anxiety-buffering function of close relationships: Evidence death-related thoughts in mortality salience effects. Journal of
that relationship commitment acts as a terror management Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 627–637.
mechanism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, Greenberg, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., &
527–542. Chatel, D. (1992). Terror management and tolerance: Does
Frankl, V. E. (1963). Man’s search for meaning. New York: Simon mortality salience always intensify negative reactions to
& Schuster. others who threaten one’s worldview? Journal of Personality
Gailliot, M. T., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). and Social Psychology, 63, 212–220.
Self-regulatory processes defend against the threat of death: Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Arndt, J. (2008). A basic
Effects of self-control depletion and trait self-control on but uniquely human motivation: Terror management.
thoughts and fears of dying. Journal of Personality and Social In J. Y. Shah & W. L. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motiva-
Psychology, 91, 49–62. tion science (pp. 114–134). New York: The Guilford Press.
Goldenberg, J. L., & Arndt, J. (2008). The implications of death Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosenblatt, A.,
for health: A terror management model of behavioral health Burling, J., Lyon, D., . . . Pinel, E. (1992). Why do people
promotion. Psychological Review, 15, 1032–1053. need self-esteem? Converging evidence that self-esteem
Goldenberg, J. L., Arndt, J., Hart, J., & Routledge, C. (2008). serves as an anxiety-buffering function. Journal of Personality
Uncovering an existential barrier to breast self-exam behav- and Social Psychology, 63, 913–922.
ior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 260–274. Hardin, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality:
Goldenberg, J. L., Cox, C. R., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., How social verification makes the subjective objective.
& Solomon, S. (2002). Understanding human ambivalence In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook
about sex: The effects of stripping sex of meaning. Journal of of motivation and cognition. Vol. 3: The interpersonal context
Sex Research, 39, 310–320. (pp. 28–84). New York: The Guilford Press.
Goldenberg, J. L., McCoy, S. K., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Harmon-Jones, E., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Simon, L.
& Solomon, S. (2000). The body as a source of self-esteem: (1996). The effects of mortality salience on intergroup bias
The effect of mortality salience on identification with one’s between minimal groups. European Journal of Social Psychol-
body, interest in sex, and appearance monitoring. Journal of ogy, 26, 677–681.
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 118–130. Harmon-Jones, E., Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T.,
Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., McCoy, S. K., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & McGregor, H. (1997). Terror management
& Solomon, S. (1999). Death, sex, love, and neuroticism: theory and self-esteem: Evidence that increased self-esteem
Why is sex such a problem? Journal of Personality and Social reduces MS effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
Psychology, 77, 1173–1187. ogy, 72, 24–36.
Goldenberg, J. L., Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, Harris, S. (2004). The end of faith: Religion, terror, and the future
S., Kluck, B., & Cornwell, R. (2001). I am not an animal: of reason. New York: W.W. Norton.
Mortality salience, disgust, and the denial of human crea- Hart, J., Shaver, P. R., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2005). Attachment,
tureliness. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 130, 427–435. self-esteem, worldviews, and terror management: Evidence
Goldenberg, J. L., Routledge, C., & Arndt, J. (2009). Mammo- for a tripartite security system. Journal of Personality and
grams and the management of existential discomfort: Threats Social Psychology, 88, 999–1013.
associated with the physicality of the body and neuroticism. Hayes, J., Schimel, J., Arndt, J., & Faucher, E. H. (2010).
Psychology and Health, 24, 563–581. A theoretical and empirical review of the death-thought
Greenberg, J., Arndt, J., Simon, L., Pyszczynski, T., & accessibility concept in terror management research. Psycho-
Solomon, S. (2000). Proximal and distal defenses in response logical Bulletin, 136, 699–739.
to reminders of one’s mortality: Evidence of a temporal Hayes, J., Schimel, J., Faucher, E. H., & Williams, T. J. (2008).
sequence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, Evidence for the DTA hypothesis II: Threatening self-esteem
91–99. increases death-thought accessibility. Journal of Experimental
Greenberg, J., Koole, S., & Pyszczynski, T. (2004). Handbook of Social Psychology, 44, 600–613.
experimental existential psychology. New York: The Guilford Hayes, J., Schimel, J., & Williams, T. J. (2008). Fighting death
Press. with death: The buffering effects of learning that worldview
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1986). The causes violators have died. Psychological Science, 19, 501–507.
and consequences of a need for self-esteem: A terror manage- Heatherton, T. F., & Baumeister, R. F. (1991). Binge eating
ment theory. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public self and private as escape from self-awareness. Psychological Bulletin, 110,
self (pp. 189–212). New York, NY: Springer-Verlag. 86–108.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Pinel, E., Simon, L., Heatherton, T. F., Herman, C. P., & Polivy, J. (1991). The effects
& Jordan, K. (1993). Effects of self-esteem on vulnerability- of physical threat and ego threat on eating. Journal of Person-
denying defensive distortions: Further evidence of an anxiety- ality and Social Psychology, 60, 138–143.
buffering function of self-esteem. Journal of Experimental Social Heine, S. J., Harihara, M., & Niiya, Y. (2002). Terror manage-
Psychology, 29, 229–251. ment in Japan. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 5, 187–196.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Heine, S., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. (2006). The meaning main-
Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., & Lyon, D. (1990). Evidence tenance model: On the coherence of social motivations.
for terror management II: The effects of mortality salience Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 88–110.

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
Henderson-King, D., Henderson-King, E., Bolea, B., Koches, Landau, M. J., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T.,
K., & Kauffman, A. (2004). Seeking understanding or send- & Martens, A. (2006). Windows into nothingness: Terror
ing bombs: Beliefs as predictors of responses to terrorism. management, meaninglessness, and negative reactions to
Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology, 10, 67–84. modern art. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90,
Hirschberger, G. (2006). Terror management and attributions of 879–892.
blame to innocent victims: Reconciling compassionate and Landau, M. J., Johns, M., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T.,
defensive responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Solomon, S., & Martens, A. (2004). A Function of form:
ogy, 91, 832–844. Terror management and structuring of the social world.
Hirschberger, G., & Ein-Dor, T. (2005). Does a candy a day keep Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 190–210.
the death thoughts away? The terror management function Landau, M. J., Kosloff, S., & Schmeichel, B. (2010). Imbuing
of eating. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 179–186. everyday actions with meaning in response to existential
Hirschberger, G., & Ein-Dor, T. (2006). Defenders of a lost threat. Self and Identity, 10, 64–76.
cause: Terror management and violent resistance to the dis- Laythe, B., Finkel, D., & Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2001). Predicting
engagement plan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, prejudice from religious fundamentalism and right-wing
32, 761–769. authoritarianism: A multiple-regression approach. Journal
Hirschberger, G., Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (2005). Fear for the Scientific Study of Religion, 40, 1–10.
and compassion: A terror management analysis of emotional Leary, M. R. (2004). The curse of the self: Self-awareness, egotism,
reactions to persons with disabilities. Rehabilitation Psychol- and the quality of human life. New York: Oxford University
ogy, 50, 246–257. Press.
Hirschman, E. C. (1990). Secular immortality and the Ameri- Lerner, M. J. (1980). The belief in a just world: A fundamental
can ideology of affluence. Journal of Consumer Research, 17, delusion. New York: Plenum Press.
31–42. Lifton, R. J. (1979). The broken connection: On death and the
Hitchens, C. (2007). God is not great: How religion poisons every- continuity of life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
thing. New York: Twelve Books. Lind, E. A., & van den Bos, K. (2002). When fairness
Hull, J. G. (1981). A self-awareness model of the causes and works: Toward a general theory of uncertainty management.
effects of alcohol consumption. Journal of Abnormal Psychol- In B. M. Staw & R. M. Kramer (Eds.), Research in organi-
ogy, 90, 586–600. zational behavior (Vol. 24, pp. 181–223). Greenwich, CT:
Jacobsen, B. (2007). Invitation to existential psychology: A psychol- JAI Press.
ogy for the unique human being and its applications in therapy. Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psycho-
London: Wiley. logical, educational, and behavioral treatment: Confirmation
James, W. (1950). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: from meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 48, 1181–1209.
Dover Publications. Mandel, N., & Heine, S. J. (1999). Terror management and
Jaspers, K. (1951). Way to wisdom (Trans. R. Manheim). marketing: He who dies with the most toys wins. Advances in
New Haven and London: Yale University Press. Consumer Research, 26, 527–532.
Jonas, E., & Fischer, P. (2006). Terror management and religion: Mandel, N., & Smeesters, D. (2008). The sweet escape: The
Evidence that intrinsic religiousness mitigates worldview effects of mortality salience on consumption quantities for
defense following mortality salience. Journal of Personality low and high self-esteem consumers. Journal of Consumer
and Social Psychology, 91, 553–567. Research, 35, 309–323.
Jonas, E., Martens, A., Niesta, D., Fritsche, I., Sullivan, D., & Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., & Schutte, N. S. (2005).
Greenberg, J. (2008). Focus theory of normative conduct and The relationship between the five-factor model of personality
terror management theory: The interactive impact of mortal- and symptoms of clinical disorders: A meta-analysis. Journal
ity salience and norm salience on social judgment. Journal of of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 27, 101–114.
Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 1239–1251. Martens, A., Greenberg, J., Schimel, J., & Landau, M. J. (2004).
Kasser, T., & Sheldon, K. M. (2000). Of wealth and death: Ageism and death: Effects of mortality salience and perceived
Materialism, mortality salience, and consumption behavior. similarity to elders on reactions to elderly people. Personality
Psychological Science, 11, 348–351. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1524–1536.
Kesebir, P., Luszczynska, A., Pyszczynski, T., & Benight, C. C. McGregor, H., A., Lieberman, J.D., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S.
(in press). Posttraumatic stress disorder involves disrupted Arndt, J., Simon, L. & Pyszczynzki, T. (1998). Terror man-
anxiety-buffer mechanisms. Journal of Social and Clinical agement and aggression: Evidence that mortality salience
Psychology. motivates aggression against worldview-threatening others.
Koenig, H. G., McCullough, M. E., & Larson, D. B. (2001). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 590–605.
Handbook of religion and health. London: Oxford University Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2000). Exploring individual dif-
Press. ferences in reactions to mortality salience: Does attachment
Koltko-Rivera, M. E. (2004). The psychology of worldviews. style regulate terror management mechanisms? Journal of
Review of General Psychology, 8, 3–58. Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 260–273.
Koole, S. L., & Van den Berg, A. E. (2005). Lost in the wil- Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (2002). The effect of mortality
derness: Terror management, action orientation, and nature salience on self-serving attributions—Evidence for the func-
evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, tion of self-esteem as a terror management mechanism. Basic
1014–1028. and Applied Social Psychology, 24, 261–271.
Kruglanski, A. W., Chen, X., Dechesne, M., Fishman, S., & Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Hirschberger, G. (2003). The
Orehek, E. (2009). Fully committed: Suicide bombers’ existential function of close relationships: Introducing death
motivation and the quest for personal significance. Political into the science of love. Personality and Social Psychology
Psychology, 30, 331–357. Review, 7, 20–40.

 the rol e of death in life


Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attach- and anxiety: The hierarchy of terror. Anxiety Research, 2,
ment styles and fear of personal death: A case study of affect 177–195.
regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Koole, S. L.
273–280. (2010). Experimental existential psychology: Coping with
Mitchell, R. W. (2003). Subjectivity and self-recognition in ani- the facts of life. In S. T. Fiske, D. T. Gilbert, & G. Lindzey
mals. In M. R. Leary & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (5th ed., pp. 724–757).
and identity (pp. 567–593). New York: The Guilford Press. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Moskalenko, S., & Heine, S. J. (2003). Watching your troubles Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Maxfield, M.
away: Television viewing as a stimulus for subjective self- (2006). On the unique psychological import of the human
awareness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, awareness of mortality: Theme and variations. Psychological
76–85. Inquiry, 17, 328–356.
Motyl, M., Hart, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (2010). When animals Pyszczynski, T., & Kesebir, P. (2011). Anxiety buffer
attack: The effects of mortality salience, infrahumanization disruption theory: A terror management account of
of violence, and authoritarianism on support for war. Journal posttraumatic stress disorder. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping,
of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 200–203. 24, 3–26.
Nabokov, V. (1999). Speak, memory. New York: Everyman’s Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., & Greenberg, J. (2003). In the wake
Library. (Original work published 1951). of 9/11: The psychology of terror. Washington, DC: American
Nelson, L. L., & Milburn, T. W. (1999). Relationships between Psychological Association.
problem-solving competencies and militaristic attitudes: Richard, F. D., Bond, C. F., & Stokes-Zoota, J. J. (2003). One
Implications for peace education. Peace and Conflict: Journal hundred years of social psychology quantitatively described.
of Peace Psychology, 5, 149–168. Review of General Psychology, 7, 331–363.
Norenzayan, A., & Hansen, I. G. (2006). Belief in supernatural Rindfleisch, A., Burroughs, J. E., & Wong, N. (2009). The safety
agents in the face of death. Personality and Social Psychology of objects: Materialism, existential insecurity, and brand con-
Bulletin, 32, 174–187. nection. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 1–16.
Ogilvie, D. M., Cohen, F., & Solomon, S. (2008). The undesired Rosenblatt, A., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., &
self: Deadly connotations. Journal of Research in Personality, Lyon, D. (1989). Evidence for terror management theory I:
42, 564–576. The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who
Osarchuk, M., & Tatz, S. J. (1973). Effect of induced fear of violate or uphold cultural values. Journal of Personality and
death on belief in afterlife. Journal of Personality and Social Social Psychology, 57, 681–690.
Psychology, 27, 256–260. Rothschild, Z., Abdollahi, A., & Pyszczynski, T. (2009). Does
Pargament, K. I. (1999). The psychology of religion and spiri- peace have a prayer? The effect of mortality salience, com-
tuality? Yes and no. International Journal for the Psychology of passionate values and religious fundamentalism on hostility
Religion, 9, 3–16. toward out-groups. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
Pearce, S. M. (1992). Museums, objects, and collections: A cultural 45, 816–827.
study. Leicester, England: Leicester University Press. Routledge, C., Arndt, J., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2004). A time to
Peters, H. J., Greenberg, J., & Williams, J. M. (2005). Applying tan: Proximal and distal effects of mortality salience on sun
terror management theory to performance: Can reminding exposure intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle-
individuals of their mortality increase strength output? tin, 30, 1347–1358.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 111–116. Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1988). A model of behavioral self-
Polivy, J., Herman, C. P., & McFarlane, T. (1994). Effects of anx- regulation: Translating intention into action. In L. Berkowitz
iety on eating: Does palatibility moderate distress-induced (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21,
overeating in dieters? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, pp. 303–346). New York: Academic Press.
505–510. Schimel, J., Hayes, J., Williams, T., & Jahrig, J. (2007). Is death
Proulx, T., & Heine, S. J. (2008). The case of the transmogrifying really the worm at the core? Converging evidence that world-
experimenter: Affirmation of a moral schema following implicit view threat increases death-thought accessibility. Journal of
change detection. Psychological Science, 19, 1294–1300. Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 789–803.
Pyszczynski, T., Abdollahi, A., Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Schimel, J., Simon, L., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon,
Cohen, F., & Weise, D. (2006). Mortality salience, martyr- S., Wazmonsky, J., & Arndt, J. (1999). Stereotypes and ter-
dom, and military might: The Great Satan versus the Axis of ror management: Evidence that mortality salience enhances
Evil. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 525–538. stereotypic thinking and preferences. Journal of Personality
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1993). Hanging on and letting and Social Psychology, 77, 905–926.
go: Understanding the onset, maintenance, and remission of Silvia, P. J. (2001). Nothing or the opposite: Intersecting terror
depression. New York: Springer-Verlag. management and objective self-awareness. European Journal
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., & Solomon, S. (1999). A dual- of Personality, 15, 73–82.
process model of defense against conscious and unconscious Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., & Pyszczynski, T. (1991). A terror
death-related thoughts: An extension of terror management management theory of social behavior: On the psycho-
theory. Psychological Review, 106, 835–845. logical functions of self-esteem and cultural worldviews.
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Arndt, J., & In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
Schimel, J. (2004). Why do people need self-esteem? psychology (Vol. 24, pp. 93–159). San Diego, CA: Academic
A theoretical and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, Press.
130, 435–468. Solomon, S., Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Cohen, F., &
Pyszczynski, T., Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., & Hamilton, J. Ogilvie, D. (2009). Teach these souls to fly: Supernatural
(1990). A terror management analysis of self-awareness as human adaptation. In M. Schaller, A. Norenzayan, S.

kes eb i r, p ys zc z yn s ki 
Heine, T. Yamagishi, & T. Kameda (Eds.), Evolution, culture, Vail, K., III, Rothchild, Z., Weise, D., Solomon, S.,
and the human mind (pp. 99–119) Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2010). A terror manage-
Erlbaum. ment analysis of the psychological functions of religion.
Steinbeck, J. (1995). Pastures of heaven. New York: Penguin Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14, 84–94.
Classic. van den Bos, K., Poortvliet, M., Maas, M., Miedema, J., &
Strachan, E., Schimel, J., Arndt, J., Williams, T., Solomon, S., van den Ham, E. (2005). An enquiry concerning the
Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (2007). Terror mismanage- principles of cultural norms and values: The impact of uncer-
ment: Evidence that mortality salience exacerbates phobic tainty and mortality salience on reactions to violations and
and compulsive behaviors. Personality and Social Psychology bolstering of cultural worldviews. Journal of Experimental
Bulletin, 33, 1137–1151. Social Psychology, 41, 91–113.
Superhuman heroes. (1998, June 6). The Economist, pp. 10–12. van Deurzen-Smith, E. (1984). Existential therapy. In W. Dryden
Tam, K. P., Chiu, C. Y., & Lau, I. Y. (2007). Terror management (Ed.), Individual therapy in Britain. London: Harper and
among Chinese: Worldview defence and intergroup bias in Row.
resource allocation. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, van Deurzen-Smith, E. (1997). Everyday mysteries. London:
93–102. Routledge.
Taubman-Ben-Ari, O. (2004). Intimacy and risky sexual behav- van Deurzen, E. (2002). Existential counselling and psychotherapy
ior—What does it have to do with death? Death Studies, 28, in practice (2nd ed.). London: Sage.
865–887. Vess, M., Routledge, C., Landau, M., & Arndt, J. (2009). The
Taubman Ben-Ari, O., Findler, L., & Mikulincer, M. (2002) dynamics of death and meaning: The effects of death-relevant
The effects of mortality salience on relationship strivings and cognitions and personal need for structure on perceptions of
beliefs: The moderating role of attachment style. British Jour- meaning in life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
nal of Social Psychology, 41, 419–441. 97, 728–744.
Taubman Ben-Ari, O., Florian, V., & Mikulincer, M. (1999). Wicklund, R. A. (1975). Objective self awareness. In L. Berkowitz
The impact of mortality salience on reckless driving: A test (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 8,
of terror management mechanisms. Journal of Personality and pp. 233–275). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Social Psychology, 76, 35–45. Wisman, A., & Koole, S. (2003). Hiding in the crowd: Can mor-
Tillich, P. (1952). The courage to be. Glasgow, Scotland: Collins/ tality salience promote affiliation with others who oppose
Fontana. one’s worldviews? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Vaes, J., Heflick, N., & Goldenberg, J. (2010).”We are people”: 84, 511–526.
Ingroup humanization as an existential defense. Journal of Yalom, I. (1980). Existential psychotherapy. New York: Basic
Personality and Social Psychology, 98(5), 750–760. Books.

 the rol e of death in life


CHAPTER

Too Much of a Good Thing?


5 Trade-offs in Promotion and
Prevention Focus
Abigail A. Scholer and E. Tory Higgins

Abstract
Different kinds of motivational orientations provide distinctive ways of perceiving the world, dealing
with life’s inevitable slings and arrows, regulating challenges and opportunities, and creating success.
In this chapter, we explore these differences in the two motivational systems outlined in regulatory
focus theory: the promotion and prevention systems (Higgins, 1997). In particular, we discuss these
systems in terms of the trade-offs in each; what are the benefits and costs of a strong promotion focus?
What are the advantages and drawbacks of a strong prevention focus? We explore the trade-offs of each
system with regard to three significant aspects of self-regulation and motivation: emotional experiences,
the balance between commitment versus exploration, and performance. We conclude by discussing the
importance of constraints on these systems for effective self-regulation and by suggesting avenues for
future research.
Key Words: regulatory focus, motivation, self-regulation, emotion, commitment, performance

Air travel can provide not only logistical but fundamental motivational systems outlined in regu-
also intellectual challenges. When you tell your latory focus theory (Higgins, 1997): the promotion
seatmate that you study motivation, inevitably system and the prevention system. This chapter is
the question arises: “I’m having trouble motivat- a response to our seatmate (i.e., “It is not always
ing (substitute wife, son, employee). How can I get about increasing motivation”), albeit with perhaps
them more motivated?” Such is the typical concep- more nuance and complexity that one dares get
tion of motivation. More is better. The problem is into in Row 22.
always that people are lacking in amount. If the We begin by introducing the promotion and
maximum level of motivation can be achieved, all prevention systems. We then explore what kind of
will be right with the world. And you can never life an individual would have if each system were
have enough. totally unconstrained. In other words, what kind of
This chapter is dedicated to our fellow traveler. life would a purely promotion-focused individual
Is more motivation always better? The answer to face? What kinds of opportunities and challenges
this question, we believe, is consistent with what would a purely prevention-focused individual con-
we face (sometimes resignedly) in most aspects of front? We explore the trade-offs of the pure forms of
life: There are always trade-offs. Having a lot of a each system generally and the trade-offs in relation
good thing means having at least some of a bad to specific situations. For instance, more promotion
thing, too. Strength is intimately connected to may be useful when brainstorming a new ad cam-
weakness. Benefits come at some cost. This chapter paign (Friedman & Förster, 2001), but not so useful
is an exploration of such trade-offs within the two if overseeing the safety of one’s employees (Wallace,


Little, & Shull, 2008). We organize our discussion difference between “0” and “+1.” Promotion-focused
of trade-offs around three significant issues in moti- individuals are less sensitive to negative deviations
vation and self-regulation: emotional life, commit- from the status quo or neutral state, that is, the dif-
ment versus exploration, and performance. Lastly, ference between “0” and “–1” (Brendl & Higgins,
we discuss the importance of constraints on these 1996; Higgins, 1997; Higgins & Tykocinski, 1992).
systems in order to achieve optimal self-regulation. In other words, promotion-focused individuals are
We describe the ways in which the prevention and maximally sensitive to gains versus nongains. Impor-
promotion systems may constrain each other, as well tant gains are those related to their ideals, wishes, and
as how other motivational orientations (e.g., regula- aspirations.
tory mode; Higgins, Kruglanski, & Pierro, 2003) In contrast, the world of a prevention-focused
may also provide constraints on these systems. individual is a world filled with duty. An indi-
vidual who is chronically prevention focused has
Overview of Regulatory Focus Theory been socialized to see that what matters in life is
Building on earlier distinctions (Bowlby, 1969; maintaining satisfactory states by preventing bad
1973; Higgins, 1987; Mowrer, 1960), regulatory things from happening—ensuring the absence ver-
focus theory distinguishes between two coexisting sus presence of negative outcomes. Caretaker–child
motivational systems (promotion, prevention) that interactions that encourage the development of
serve critically important but different survival needs a prevention focus direct attention to security needs
(Higgins, 1997). The systems differ in what funda- and emphasize desired end states as oughts, duties,
mentally motivates (nurturance versus security) and and obligations (Higgins, 1987, 1997; Keller, 2008;
in what regulatory strategies are preferred (eagerness Manian et al., 1998; Manian et al., 2006). Conse-
versus vigilance). Given that each system addresses quently, prevention-focused individuals are con-
a significant survival need, it is not surprising that cerned with safety and responsibility and focus on
people need both systems to be maximally effec- the necessity of maintaining the absence of nega-
tive in the world. However, in any given moment, tive outcomes. This orientation is best served by
one system is likely to predominate over the other, using vigilant avoidance strategies in goal pursuit—
due to either chronic or situational differences in avoiding mismatches to desired end states and
accessibility. In this chapter, we imagine what life avoiding matches to undesired end states (Crowe
would be like if an individual were purely promo- & Higgins, 1997; Higgins et al., 1994; Liberman
tion or prevention focused by exploring the benefits et al., 2001; Molden & Higgins, 2005; Wang &
and costs of the extreme forms of each system. Lee, 2006). This sensitivity to the absence and
The world of a promotion-focused individual is presence of negative outcomes (nonlosses/losses)
a world filled with possibility for advancement. An is reflected in greater assigned significance to the
individual who is chronically promotion focused difference between “0” and “–1” than to the dif-
has been socialized to see that what matters in life is ference between “0” and “+1” (Brendl & Higgins,
making good things happen—seeking the presence 1996; Higgins, 1997; Higgins & Tykocinski, 1992).
versus absence of positive outcomes. Caretaker–child Important nonlosses are those related to duties,
interactions that support the development of a pro- oughts, and responsibilities.
motion focus direct attention to nurturance needs Importantly, although the promotion and pre-
and emphasize desired end states as ideals (Higgins, vention systems are concerned with the regula-
1987, 1997; Keller, 2008; Manian, Papadakis, Strau- tion of different needs, promotion and prevention
man, & Essex, 2006; Manian, Strauman, & Denney, orientations each involve the approach and avoid-
1998). Consequently, promotion-focused indi- ance systems of self-regulation—each involve both
viduals are concerned with growth, advancement, approaching desired end states (e.g., approaching
and accomplishment that are served by using eager nurturance or safety, respectively) and avoiding
approach strategies in goal pursuit—approaching undesired end states (e.g., avoiding nonfulfillment
matches to desired end states and approaching mis- or danger, respectively). In other words, although
matches to undesired end states (Crowe & Higgins, at the strategic level promotion and prevention
1997; Higgins, Roney, Crowe, & Hymes, 1994; relate differentially to eager approach and vigilant
Liberman, Molden, Idson, & Higgins, 2001; Molden avoidance strategies, at the system level each sys-
& Higgins, 2005; Wang & Lee, 2006). Advance- tem is involved in both approach and avoidance
ments that count are those that result in positive (Scholer & Higgins, 2008). These differences mean
deviations from the status quo or neutral state—the that some desired end states will be more valuable

 too m uch of a good th ing?


or relevant in one system versus the other (Higgins, or a failure and exactly how those triumphs and
2002). For instance, prevention-focused individuals tragedies feel—both the precise quality and the
may value the desired end state of an accident-free intensity—depends at least in part on whether they
production line more than promotion-focused indi- are experienced within the promotion versus pre-
viduals (Henning, Stufft, Payne, Bergman, Mannan, vention systems. Additionally, the preferred stra-
& Keren, 2009). Additionally, the same desired end tegic preferences of each system are sustained or
state can be presented in different ways by preven- disrupted by different affective states. In this sec-
tion- versus promotion-focused individuals. For tion, we explore the trade-offs in the emotional life
example, the same desired end state, such as hav- of a purely prevention-focused individual versus
ing a good marriage, may be represented as a duty a purely promotion-focused individual.
or responsibility for prevention-focused individuals
but as an ideal or aspiration for promotion-focused The Price of Happiness, The Cost of Calm
individuals. Furthermore, the fit (e.g., promotion Success and failure are defined differently within
eager) or nonfit (e.g., promotion vigilant) between the promotion and prevention systems, have dif-
an individual’s underlying goal orientation and use ferential significance, and have distinct emotional
of strategic means affects strength of engagement in signatures (Higgins, 1997, 2001). Success in a pro-
the goal pursuit activity beyond any direct implica- motion focus reflects the presence of a gain: the
tions of either the system or the strategy itself (regu- positive outcome of an advancement, an improve-
latory fit theory; Higgins, 2000). In other words, ment. In contrast, success in a prevention focus
the effectiveness of a given strategy depends not only reflects just a nonnegative state: the establishment
on the inherent properties of the strategy and task or maintenance of a satisfactory state. Thus, while
demands but also on whether the strategy sustains promotion success requires progress or advance-
or fits an individual’s underlying orientation. When ment from “0” to “+1,” prevention success requires
individuals experience regulatory fit by using strate- only maintenance of “0” such that a nonnega-
gic means that sustain their underlying orientation, tive, satisfactory state persists. Fundamentally, “suc-
they “feel right” about and engage more strongly in cess” in promotion requires positive change (gain),
what they are doing (Higgins, 2000, 2006). whereas “success” in prevention simply requires a
As noted earlier, promotion and prevention state or condition that is satisfactory. This differ-
orientations can arise either from chronic accessi- ence between requiring change (progress) versus
bility (personality differences) or from temporary requiring just a satisfactory state or condition con-
accessibility (situational factors). Consequently, reg- stitutes a basic asymmetry between promotion and
ulatory focus has been studied both as a personal- prevention.
ity variable with chronic strength or predominance Failure, too, is defined differently for promotion
of prevention or promotion orientations (e.g., and prevention. For promotion-focused individuals,
Cunningham, Raye, & Johnson, 2005; Higgins both “0” and “–1” are nongain, failure states. They
et al., 2001; Higgins, Shah, & Friedman, 1997; both represent a failure to make progress, a failure to
Lockwood, Jordan, & Kunda, 2002; Ouschan, advance forward from “0.” For prevention-focused
Boldero, Kashima, Wakimoto, & Kashima, 2007) individuals, however, only “–1” is experienced as
and as a situational variable involving priming ide- failure (Brendl & Higgins, 1996; Higgins, 1997).
als or oughts or framing goal pursuits as potential Not making progress is not a failure.
gains or nonlosses (e.g., Friedman & Förster, 2001; This means that if both a promotion-focused
Higgins et al., 1994; Liberman et al., 2001; Shah & individual and a prevention-focused individual are
Higgins, 1997; Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998). in a current state of loss (“–1”), acceptable move-
Because we believe that what ultimately matters in ment for a prevention-focused individual requires
terms of predicting behavior is the regulatory state reaching the satisfactory state of “0,” whereas “0”
that one is in, whether that arises from chronic holds no special meaning for a promotion-focused
or temporary accessibility (cf. Higgins, 1999), we individual. Instead, acceptable movement for a
review research that examines regulatory focus as promotion-focused individual means making prog-
both a measured and manipulated variable. ress beyond “0” towards “+1.” Similarly, if both a
promotion-focused individual and a prevention-
Trade-offs in Emotional Life focused individual have moved from “0” to a cur-
Few would argue with the claim that success rent state of “+1,” the promotion-focused person
feels better than failure. Yet what counts as a success would experience failure with a setback to “0”

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
because it would represent a nongain, a removal of failure is an extremely painful state precisely because
the previous progress, whereas the satisfactory state its low motivational intensity reflects having no
of “0” would still count as a success for a prevention- interest in engaging with life, a very painful psycho-
focused person. logical condition.
Molden, Lucas, Gardner, Dean, and Knowles The distinct quality of prevention failure impacts
(2009) provide intriguing evidence for how different how individuals anticipate and respond to failure.
kinds of social losses “count” as promotion versus For example, prevention-focused individuals appear
prevention failures. When asked to describe a time to be more susceptible to self-handicapping than
that they “did not belong,” prevention-focused indi- promotion-focused individuals (Hendrix & Hirt,
viduals were more likely to describe a time in which 2009), presumably because self-handicapping is
they were actively rejected (“–1” or an unsatisfac- a tactic for maintaining a current satisfactory state
tory state), whereas promotion-focused individuals (e.g., the belief that you have high ability). In addi-
were more likely to describe a time in which they tion, after experiencing an unfavorable outcome that
had been more passively ignored (no opportunity is represented as an unsatisfactory state, prevention-
to advance from “0” or a nongain). Similarly, Sas- focused individuals are more upset if the process
senberg and Hansen (2007) have shown that social yielding that outcome was fair than unfair (Cropan-
discrimination based on “–1” unsatisfactory states zano, Paddock, Rupp, Bagger, & Baldwin, 2008).
increases distress for prevention-focused, but not Cropanzano et al. (2008) suggest that because the
promotion-focused, participants. fair process does not allow one to easily attribute
In addition to differences in what counts as suc- failure to external causes, it is particularly threat-
cess or failure, the intensity of the experience also ening for prevention-focused individuals (see also
differs for promotion and prevention individuals. Brockner, 2010). As we’ll explore in more depth
For a prevention-focused individual who is sensi- later, however, prevention failure, while painful, can
tive to negative unsatisfactory states, “–1” failure is also energize the system. Promotion failure gener-
unacceptable in a way that it is not for a promotion- ally provides no such benefit.
focused individual, and it is experienced more As described earlier when discussing the plea-
intensely by prevention-focused than promotion- sures and pains of promotion and prevention,
focused individuals (Idson, Liberman, & Higgins, the quality of emotional response to success and
2000). In contrast, “+1” success is more meaning- failure also differs within the promotion and pre-
ful and experienced more intensely for promotion- vention systems. Success in the promotion system
focused than prevention-focused individuals (Idson, reflects the presence of a positive outcome (a gain
Liberman, & Higgins, 2000). This means that the or advancement) and results in cheerfulness-related
potential for positive emotional intensity would be emotions like happiness and joy. In contrast, suc-
greater in the promotion system than the prevention cess in the prevention system reflects the absence of
system, that is, a promotion gain of “+1” is more a negative outcome (maintaining a satisfactory
intense than a prevention nonloss of “+1,” whereas state) and results in quiescence-related emotions like
the potential for negative emotional intensity would peacefulness and calm. Failure in a promotion focus
be greater in the prevention system than the promo- reflects the absence of a positive outcome (nongain
tion system, that is, a prevention unsatisfactory state or nonadvancement) and results in dejection-related
of “–1” is more intense than a promotion nongain emotions like sadness and disappointment. Because
of “–1.” failure in a prevention focus reflects the presence of
It should be emphasized, however, that emo- a negative outcome (an unsatisfactory or dangerous
tional intensity is not the same as level of pleasure state), it results in agitation-related emotions like
or pain. The feeling of peace and calm from preven- anxiety and worry (Higgins, 1997; Shah & Higgins,
tion success is not as intense as the feeling of joy 2001). Consistent with these distinct emotional
and elation from promotion success, but this does sensitivities, individuals in a promotion focus are
not mean that the former is necessarily less pleasant faster at appraising how cheerful or dejected a given
than the latter. Similarly, the feeling of sadness and object makes them feel, whereas individuals in a
discouragement from promotion failure is not as prevention focus are faster at appraising how quies-
intense as the feeling of anxiety and worry from pre- cent or agitated an object makes them feel (Shah &
vention failure, but this does not mean that the for- Higgins, 2001). Furthermore, these distinct emo-
mer is necessarily less painful than the latter. Indeed, tional responses to failure mean that promotion and
the feeling of depression from severe promotion prevention individuals are differentially motivated

 too m uch of a good th ing?


by anticipating failure-related dejection versus Evidence of distinct patterns of intergoal inhi-
agitation. Whereas promotion-focused individuals bition supports the unique dynamics of emotional
are more motivated to perform well when imagin- vulnerabilities within each system. Shah, Friedman,
ing potential dejection, prevention-focused indi- and Kruglanski (2002) found that individuals selec-
viduals are more motivated to perform well when tively showed greater intergoal inhibition for goals
imagining potential agitation (Leone, Perugini, & that could alleviate emotional distress. Shah et al.
Bagozzi, 2005). found that when participants were depressed, they
A particularly significant type of failure that showed greater intergoal inhibition for one type of
people experience occurs when their actual selves focal goal—ideal goals—the goals that would result
are discrepant from their desired selves—whether in happiness and satisfaction if obtained. Similarly,
these desired selves are represented in the preven- when participants were anxious, they selectively
tion system (ought selves) or the promotion system showed greater intergoal inhibition when the focal
(ideal selves). In support of distinctive patterns of goal was an ought goal. In other words, participants
emotional response to this type of failure, several were more likely to shield and protect a goal from
studies have found that priming ideal (promo- competing goal alternatives when that goal could
tion) discrepancies leads to increases in dejection, alleviate their emotional stress if it were attained.
whereas priming ought (prevention) discrepancies One significant implication of the relation
leads to increases in agitation (Boldero, Moretti, between regulatory focus concerns and emotional
Bell, & Francis, 2005; Higgins, Bond, Klein, & responses to success and failure is that it creates the
Strauman, 1986; Strauman, 1989; Strauman & possibility that individuals may be thwarted by mis-
Higgins, 1987). Not surprisingly, the magnitude of aligned emotional expectancies. To the extent that
an emotional response to a discrepancy is related to individuals experience successful self-regulation as
that discrepancy’s magnitude, accessibility, relevance being about both achieving the desired end state
to a particular context, and importance (Higgins, (e.g., going to the gym three times a week) and
1999). Simply encountering an individual who achieving the desired affective state (e.g., happi-
resembles a parent can activate self-discrepancies ness), the impact of successes may be undermined
associated with that parent’s ideals or oughts for the if individuals expect promotion-related emotions
individual, producing dejected affect for parent- from prevention successes (and vice versa). Individ-
related ideal self-discrepancies and agitated affect for uals often have beliefs or hopes about how achieving
parent-related ought self-discrepancies (see Reznik a particular goal will make them feel (e.g., if I can
& Andersen, 2007; Shah, 2003). Additionally, being go to the gym three times this week, I’ll feel really
socially rejected (a prevention negative state) leads to happy). Someone who sets prevention goals and
increased anxiety and withdrawal, but being socially expects to be happy will be sorely disappointed.
ignored (a promotion nongain) leads to sadness and In addition, setting promotion goals does not
attempts to reengage (Molden et al., 2009). guarantee happiness either. The risk of aiming for
These differences in the emotional dimensions happiness is that individuals become vulnerable to
associated with each system result in characteristic depression (Strauman, 2002; Strauman et al., 2006;
possibilities and vulnerabilities within each system Vieth et al., 2003). Indeed, it is when the motiva-
(Brockner & Higgins, 2001; Higgins, 1987, 1997, tional system is particularly strong (when promo-
2001; Shah & Higgins, 2001). Both prevention- and tion really matters to you) that individuals are most
promotion-focused individuals experience a sense vulnerable to failures within the system (Higgins
of well-being when they successfully attain a goal. et al., 1997). Strauman (2002), in his self-regulation
Both prevention- and promotion-focused individu- theory of depression, proposed that the chronic
als experience displeasure when they fail. However, failure of promotion-focused individuals to meet
the emotional trade-offs within each system are dis- promotion goals is a causal factor in the onset of
tinct, as we explore in more detail later. Only pro- depression. While the potential highs in promotion
motion goals provide the possibility of happiness (in may be very high indeed (Idson et al., 2000, 2004),
the sense of joyful and ebullient). Only prevention the lows embody the very depths of desolation. Sev-
goals provide the possibility of calm (in the sense eral studies now support the link between failures in
of peace and serenity). However, within the promo- the promotion system and depression (Eddington
tion system, the price of happiness is vulnerability to et al., 2009; Jones, Papadakis, Hogan, & Strauman,
depression. Within the prevention system, the cost 2009; Miller & Markman, 2007; Papadakis, Prince,
of calm is vulnerability to anxiety. Jones, & Strauman, 2006; Strauman et al., 2006;

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
Vieth et al., 2003) and suicidal ideation (Cornette, which the client was under the mistaken impression
Strauman, Abramson, & Busch, 2009). that attaining prevention goals would lead to the
While promotion-focused individuals may be happiness and satisfaction that she dearly wanted.
particularly susceptible to depression, coping styles Part of the usefulness of the therapy for her (and for
and implicit beliefs about the nature of the failure other clients) appears to be learning that succeeding
may moderate the vulnerability (Cornette et al., or failing at promotion versus prevention goals has
2009; Jones et al., 2009; Papadakis et al, 2006). In distinct emotional consequences.
two studies, individuals who engaged in rumination Less work has been done to examine the link
and who had failures in the promotion system were between the prevention system and anxiety disor-
more likely to show depressive symptoms. Individu- ders (for a recent review, see Klenk, Strauman, &
als who had a more reflective coping style appeared Higgins, 2011). However, some empirical evidence
to be buffered from the link between promotion does suggest that chronic actual-ought discrepan-
failure and depression (Jones et al., 2009; Papadakis cies do predict certain patterns of anxiety (Scott &
et al, 2006). Additionally, individuals who believed O’Hara, 1993; Strauman, 1989; Strauman et al.,
that their promotion failures (actual-ideal discrep- 2001). For instance, Strauman (1989) reported that
ancies) were stable and unchanging were most likely social phobics had higher actual-ought discrepancy
to show a relation between promotion failure and scores relative to depressed or control participants.
suicidal ideation (Cornette et al., 2009). Like reflec- Furthermore, social phobics exhibited increased
tive coping, belief in transitory failure appeared to agitation in response to actual-ought discrepancy
provide a buffer against depression (Cornette et al., priming relative to depressed or control partici-
2009). Together, these studies suggest that it is the pants. Scott and O’Hara (1993) extended this work
“chronic and catastrophic” promotion failures that to show that university students diagnosed with any
are likely to push individuals toward depression one of a number of anxiety disorders (generalized
(Vieth et al., 2003, p. 249). anxiety disorder, panic attacks, agoraphobia, social
Self-system theory (SST) is a recently developed phobia, or obsessive-compulsive disorder) also had
structured psychotherapy to treat the depression higher actual-ought discrepancy scores than nonanx-
that is associated with individuals who have chronic ious or depressed students.
promotion goals and are failing (Vieth et al., 2003). There is no doubt that failure in both the pre-
SST incorporates many principles from other forms vention and promotion systems is painful, albeit
of therapy such as cognitive therapy, interpersonal in different ways. However, as noted earlier, failure
psychotherapy, and behavioral activation therapy. within the prevention system is not only painful but
However, SST uses these principles in service of also unacceptable in a way that promotion failure is
helping patients to identify their promotion and not, in the sense that prevention-focused individu-
prevention goals, their strategies for attaining them, als experience returning to a satisfactory “0” state
the obstacles they have encountered in goal pur- as being a motivational necessity. This has significant
suit, and how they can do things differently and/or implications for the actions that prevention-focused
more effectively. In a randomized trial comparing individuals are willing and motivated to take when
SST with cognitive therapy (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & in an unsatisfactory negative state. For prevention-
Emery, 1979), SST, for individuals with a poor pro- focused individuals, a state of “–1” is intolerable;
motion effectiveness, was found to lead to reduced they should be willing to do whatever is necessary to
symptoms for depression and decreased dysphoric get back to “0” or the status quo. “0” does not hold
responses to promotion goals compared to cognitive the same significance for promotion-focused indi-
therapy (Strauman et al., 2006). The effectiveness of viduals. While ultimately they are motivated to get
SST speaks to the importance of understanding the to “+1,” any progress away from “–1” is in service
trade-offs and vulnerabilities within a given motiva- of that end; the status quo (“0”) holds no special
tional system. meaning as the state they want to reach. Conse-
The success of SST also supports the idea that quently, when individuals are in an unsatisfactory
some awareness of the trade-offs within systems may state of “-1,” it is prevention-focused individuals,
also be beneficial. As part of the educational and rather than promotion-focused individuals, who
goal-setting aspects of the theory, therapist and cli- have been found to be especially motivated to take
ent discuss the implications of the different concerns risks that have the possibility of returning them to
of the promotion and prevention systems. Interest- the status quo (Scholer, Zou, Fujita, Stroessner, &
ingly, Vieth et al. (2003) describe a case study in Higgins, 2010). For prevention-focused individuals

 too m uch of a good th ing?


at “–1,” failure carries with it an increased likelihood Kluger, 2004). But there is also a trade-off of failure
of engaging in actions that are perceived as neces- for prevention-focused individuals. Because their
sary to restore “0,” even if they are risky. increased vigilance after failure reduces the numbers
of possible causes they consider for their failure,
Keeping the Engine Revved: Strategic they are more likely than promotion-focused indi-
Preferences and Life Experiences viduals to engage in self-serving attributions after
Success and failure not only result in different failure (Molden & Higgins, 2008).
emotional responses in the promotion versus pre- Notably, it is not the case that promotion-
vention systems, but they also have distinct impli- focused individuals simply give up after initial fail-
cations for the strategic inclinations that sustain ure; rather, they are likely to respond to failure in
each system and the motivational experiences that ways that protect their eagerness for future perfor-
are associated with these strategic inclinations (i.e., mances. For example, after failure feedback in an
eager and vigilant experiences). For promotion- ongoing performance situation, promotion-focused
focused individuals, failure is not only negative individuals show only slight decreases in expectan-
affectively, but it also reduces the strategic eagerness cies for future performance (Förster, Grant, Idson,
that sustains or fits the promotion system. In con- & Higgins, 2001). In addition, after failure they use
trast, success is both affectively positive and sustains tactics to maintain a positive self-evaluation, which
eagerness within the promotion system. supports the eagerness that serves their promotion
For prevention-focused individuals, on the other (Scholer, Ozaki, & Higgins, 2011). Promotion-
hand, failure poses no threat to the system’s preferred focused individuals are also more likely to generate
strategic orientation. While failure in the prevention additive (eager) counterfactuals when reflecting on
system is very emotionally negative, it increases the past failures (Roese, Hur, & Pennington, 1999),
strategic vigilance that fits prevention (Idson et al., and, indeed, when they engage in upward counter-
2004). Success, however, while emotionally positive factuals that sustain eagerness they perform better on
within the prevention system, has the potential to subsequent tasks (Markman, McMullen, Elizaga, &
disrupt strategic vigilance that sustains the system’s Mizoguchi, 2006). Promotion-focused individuals
optimal effectiveness. Vigilance is hard to maintain also protect themselves against negative feedback by
in a state of calm and quiescence. Thus, while the being generally optimistic (Grant & Higgins, 2003)
pure promotion-focused individual would be wise and having high self-esteem (Higgins, 2008). More-
to seek a life of half-full glasses in order to maintain over, there is some evidence that promotion-focused
eagerness, the pure prevention-focused individual individuals can be less distracted by negative feel-
would be better off seeing life’s glasses as half-empty ings after making an error, such as action-oriented
in order to maintain vigilance. The trade-offs, of promotion-focused individuals being buffered from
course, are that the promotion-focused individual the negative impact of speed-related errors on subse-
runs the risk of seeing good where there is none quent trials (de Lange & van Knippenberg, 2009).
while the prevention-focused individual runs the In contrast to promotion-focused individuals,
risk of seeing no good when it’s there. prevention-focused individuals, in order to main-
To the extent that the strategic vigilance of tain their vigilance, respond to failure by lowering
prevention-focused individuals can become ener- expectancies even more (Förster et al., 2001), and by
gized through failure, prevention-focused indi- maintaining relatively less positive self-evaluations
viduals should generally show better performance in ongoing performance situations (Scholer, Ozaki,
after failure feedback or when anticipating failure. et al., 2011). Prevention-focused individuals are also
In contrast, given that the strategic eagerness of more likely to generate subtractive (vigilant) coun-
promotion-focused individuals can become deflated terfactuals when reflecting on past failures (Roese
after failure, promotion-focused individuals should et al., 1999), and they perform better on subsequent
show worse performance after failure feedback. tasks when they employ counterfactuals that sustain
Indeed, Idson and Higgins (2000) found that vigilance (Markman et al., 2006). Unlike promo-
promotion-focused individuals showed a decline in tion pride, prevention pride is uncorrelated with
performance after failure feedback relative to success self-esteem (Higgins, 2008).
feedback, whereas prevention-focused individuals As noted earlier, these different strategic prefer-
showed the opposite pattern—better performance ences in promotion and prevention create unique
after failure feedback than after success feedback vulnerabilities within each system. The stronger the
(see also Idson et al., 2000, 2004; Van-Dijk & system, the more likely the individual is to embrace

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
the preferred strategy, leaving the individual even are the benefits and costs of a system that pushes
more vulnerable to the potential downsides. for commitment versus a system that embraces
Promotion-focused individuals may, at times, be exploration?
overly optimistic and overeager (even manic), when
a dose of realism would serve them well. Promotion- If It’s Not Broke, Don’t Fix It
focused individuals may be less attentive to failure A number of aspects of the prevention system
and areas that need improvement, which has the converge to make prevention-focused individuals
potential to reduce the effectiveness of learning. more likely to stay committed to a current course of
Promotion-focused individuals, for instance, are action and less open to change in general. Increased
more likely to develop illusions of control regarding prevention focus is associated with increased valu-
uncontrollable outcomes (Langens, 2007). While ation of security and decreased valuation of open-
these illusions of control can help buffer them against ness to change (Higgins, 2008; Leikas, Lönnqvist,
the harsh realities of the world (Taylor, Lerner, Sher- Verkasalo, & Lindeman, 2009; Vaughn, Baumann,
man, Sage, & McDowell, 2003), such illusions can, & Klemann, 2008). Prevention-focused individuals,
at times, be problematic. Eagerness carried too far concerned with duties and obligations, are particu-
simply leaves them untethered to reality. larly likely to construe goals and actions as neces-
Promotion-focused individuals are also vulner- sities (Shah & Higgins, 1997). To the extent that
able to the strategic nonfit of failure to their system. existing goals and loyalties are perceived as duties
Accumulated failures deliver such a punch of non- that must be upheld, prevention-focused individu-
fitness to the system from reduced eagerness that it als should cling more tightly to what they have (cf.
can begin to break down, producing the anhedonia Brickman, 1987). Furthermore, duties and obliga-
of depression (no interest in anything) discussed tions often involve responsibility to others. A num-
earlier (Strauman, 2002; Strauman et al., 2006). ber of studies have found support for an association
Prevention-focused individuals, on the other hand, between prevention focus and interdependent self-
may be overly attentive to negative signals, when construals, such that prevention-focused individu-
a dose of optimism would serve them well. They als are more likely to view themselves within the
may not give themselves or others enough credit for context of a broader social network (Aaker & Lee,
success and may be less likely to adopt those posi- 2001; Lee, Aaker, & Gardner, 2000) and are more
tive illusions that can buffer against a number of motivated by leadership styles that emphasize a
negative health outcomes (Taylor et al., 2003). Fur- sense of organizational duty and self-sacrifice (Choi
thermore, while strategic vigilance generally serves & Mai-Dalton, 1999). Additionally, the prevention
them well, taken too far it may be problematic, even individual’s acute sensitivity to loss and preference
to the extent of producing pathological generalized for vigilant strategies creates reluctance to take leaps
anxiety disorder (Higgins, 2006; Klenk et al., 2011). that might expose him or her to potentially greater
Thus, while strong promotion and prevention sys- losses (Crowe & Higgins, 1997; Idson & Higgins,
tems both provide many benefits, the strengths do 2000).
not come without the possibility of downsides in Several studies support the idea that prevention
life experiences as well. focus is associated with commitment to the status
quo (Chernev, 2004; Crowe & Higgins, 1997; Jain,
Trade-offs in Commitment Versus Lindsey, Agrawal, & Maheswaran, 2007; Liberman,
Exploration Idson, Camacho, & Higgins, 1999). For instance,
Effective self-regulation requires both an ability prevention-focused participants are more likely to
to stay the course (even when sometimes difficult) want to continue working on an interrupted task
as well as an openness to change course when nec- rather than begin a new one (Liberman et al.,
essary. Staying the course involves commitment, 1999). The endowment effect, in which people
whether that is commitment to a goal, individual, value an object more simply because they possess it,
or group. Openness to changing courses involves is uniquely associated with the prevention, but not
exploration, whether that is exploring other goals, promotion, system (Liberman et al., 1999). When
other products, or other relationships. The preven- prevention-focused individuals’ initial preference in
tion system, all else being equal, excels at com- a consumer choice paradigm is framed as the status
mitment. The promotion system, all else being quo, they are particularly likely to stick with their
equal, excels at exploration. In this section, we initial choice (Chernev, 2004). Prevention-focused
explore the trade-offs of each system in turn; what people’s commitment to “the way things are” is also

 too m uch of a good th ing?


reflected in their relative reluctance to adopt new 1997; Liberman et al., 2001; Molden & Higgins,
technology relative to promotion-focused people 2004), consistent with a worldview that is less open
(Herzenstein, Posavac, & Brakus, 2007). This pref- to change (Higgins, 2008; Vaughn et al., 2008). By
erence for the status quo appears to be due both to considering fewer possibilities, prevention-focused
enhanced sensitivity to potential losses (Liberman individuals are less likely to choose a wrong path or
et al., 1999) and increased motivation to minimize be tempted by alternate paths rather than doing just
possible regret if things do not go well (Chernev, what is necessary. For instance, when sorting objects
2004). prevention-focused participants organize the objects
To justify commitment to a chosen course of into fewer categories than promotion-focused par-
action (status quo or otherwise), prevention-focused ticipants (Crowe & Higgins, 1997). Prevention-
people may sometimes see the world as a zero-sum focused individuals generate fewer hypotheses when
game (i.e., if Product A is good, then Product trying to explain someone else’s behavior and are
B is bad). Disparaging alternatives and enhancing more likely to endorse only one (Liberman et al.,
a chosen path is one way to increase commitment to 2001). Prevention-focused people in relationships
that choice (cf. Gollwitzer, Heckhausen, & Steller, pay less attention to romantic alternatives than
1990). A study comparing the effect of different promotion-focused participants (Finkel, Molden,
comparative frames in advertising on prevention- Johnson, & Eastwick, 2009). Thus, by limiting the
versus promotion-focused participants illustrates paths that they consider, prevention-focused indi-
this well (Jain et al., 2007). Jain et al. (2007) com- viduals have a better chance of protecting commit-
pared the effectiveness of two possible comparative ments they have already made.
frames—positive versus negative. Positive compara- Prevention-focused individuals also consider
tive frames suggest that the advertised (target) brand fewer explanations for their successes and failures
is better than its comparison, whereas negative com- than promotion-focused individuals (Molden &
parative frames suggest that the comparison brand Higgins, 2008). Although prevention-focused indi-
is worse than the advertised (target) brand. The neg- viduals may be motivated to consider fewer alterna-
ative frame effectively marks the comparison brand tives in order to minimize the possibility of mistakes,
as unacceptable, suggesting that the target brand is a restricted option set can sometimes increase error
the safe, right one to choose. or bias. While prevention-focused individuals con-
Not only did prevention-focused participants sidered fewer explanations for successes and failures,
evaluate the target brand more positively in the these tended to be more self-serving (Molden &
negative frame condition, but their ratings of the Higgins, 2008).
target and comparison products were also negatively Part of the reason that prevention-focused indi-
correlated. In other words, as prevention-focused viduals may be less open to considering a number
participants endorsed the target brand, they were of alternatives is that prevention-focused individu-
more likely to disparage the comparison brand. als are relatively more content with “safe” options
Furthermore, the way in which prevention-focused that promise neither extreme highs nor lows (Zhang
participants approached the task suggested underly- & Mittal, 2007). When given a choice between an
ing vigilance against the perceived “other” (cf. Shah, enriched option (option with extreme values on its
Brazy, & Higgins, 2004); prevention-focused par- attribute—e.g., movie with great art direction but
ticipants were more likely to evaluate the compari- mind-numbing plot) versus an impoverished option
son brand first and remembered more advertised (option with average values on its attributes—e.g.,
information about the comparison brand relative to movie with average cinematography and average
the advertised brand (Jain et al., 2007). Consistent plot), prevention-focused individuals prefer the
with this logic, prevention-focused individuals are impoverished option (Zhang & Mittal, 2007).
also more likely to give more negative product eval- Because prevention-focused individuals weight the
uations, relative to promotion-focused individuals, negative aspects more heavily, an option with aver-
when presented with two-sided product endorse- age values wins out (Zhang & Mittal, 2007). This
ments (e.g., the juice is natural but expensive) than is also consistent with work that has shown that
with one-sided product endorsements (e.g., the prevention-focused individuals, unlike promotion-
juice is natural) (Florack, Ineichen, & Bieri, 2009). focused individuals, are not trying to maximize
When making decisions or comparing options, outcomes in the world. In other words, the classic
prevention-focused individuals are also likely to con- expectancy x value effect on goal commitment is not
sider relatively few alternatives (Crowe & Higgins, observed for prevention-focused individuals (Shah

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
& Higgins, 1997). When the world is construed unacceptable state. For instance, when individuals
in terms of duty and obligations, a relatively low have fallen below the status quo, as in a stock invest-
expectancy does not necessarily diminish com- ment paradigm, prevention-focus strength, but not
mitment for an important goal. If a goal is really promotion-focus strength, predicts a willingness
valuable, such as maintaining a certain GPA being to take risks that have the possibility of returning
experienced as a necessity, then expectancy becomes participants to the status quo (Scholer et al., 2010).
irrelevant. And, again, there is a trade-off. While Thus, when change allows an individual to avoid
this prevention-focused orientation can support losses, prevention-focused individuals should be
greater commitment to significant goals, the poten- especially motivated to take action.
tial downside is that preferences and choices might The dynamics discussed in the previous para-
not be optimized. graph highlight an important issue. Prevention-
Nonetheless, greater commitment to important focused individuals are not arbitrarily committed
goals does have a number of benefits. Prevention- to embracing the status quo and eschewing risk
focused individuals who are chronically or tempo- and change. It is not a love affair with the status
rarily concerned about health issues are more likely quo itself, but with what the status quo represents.
to engage in health care–taking behaviors, such as These preferences serve their underlying motiva-
monitoring their health or signing up for cancer tion to achieve security and act in accordance with
screenings (Uskul, Keller, & Oyserman, 2008). duties and obligations. When things are going well
Fuglestad et al. (2008) also found that prevention- and the world appears relatively safe, conservative
focused individuals were more successful at main- biases in action (i.e., avoiding errors of commission)
taining changes after successful initiation (weight support the prevention system (Crowe & Higgins,
loss and smoking cessation) than were promotion- 1997; Friedman & Förster, 2001). However, when
focused individuals. Because successful behav- things are not going well, the tactics that support
ior maintenance for changes like weight loss and the system may shift (Scholer & Higgins, 2008).
smoking cessation requires being vigilant against For instance, while erring on the side of misses in
backslides (Rothman, 2000), prevention-focused a signal detection paradigm supports prevention
individuals may be particularly equipped for these motivation when the targets are neutral or positive,
kinds of challenges. Indeed, prevention-focused this tactical approach is folly when the targets are
individuals outperform promotion-focused indi- negative. Under these circumstances, prevention
viduals under conditions in which they must resist focus is associated with a risky bias (i.e., avoiding
distraction in order to stay focused on a focal task errors of omission); missing a negative signal (e.g.,
(Freitas, Liberman, & Higgins, 2002). the potential mugger across the street) would be
It is important to note that commitment to duties a serious threat to safety (Scholer, Stroessner, &
and obligations sometimes means that prevention- Higgins, 2008).
focused individuals will actually initiate action or The concern with missing negative signals and
change more quickly than promotion-focused indi- a desire to “play it safe” has a number of upsides
viduals. If the current state is deemed to be an unac- for prevention-focused individuals. Prevention-
ceptable, unsatisfactory state or if change itself is focused individuals are more likely to fiercely
represented as a duty or responsibility, prevention- defend that to which they are committed, whether
focused individuals may be especially likely to take that is a favorite product, their goals, or their close
action. Necessities and duties cannot be put away relationships. Prevention-focused individuals are
for another day. Individuals who are told that a likely to vigilantly monitor against potential health
product can prevent something negative (versus threats and to maintain health changes because of
achieve something positive) remember more about their vigilance against potential slippage. Because
the product and are more likely to sign up to test prevention-focused individuals are less likely to
the product, as long as goal relevance is high (Poels even consider how green the grass is on another hill,
& Dewitte, 2008). Prevention-focused participants they are more likely to be content with the hill on
initiate work on important goals (e.g., applying for which they stand, which is a definite plus for their
a fellowship) earlier than promotion-focused par- marital partners.
ticipants (Freitas, Liberman, Salovey, & Higgins, However, putting aside the issue of marriage for
2002). Furthermore, because prevention-focused a moment, sometimes other hills do offer better
individuals are sensitive to loss, they will be moti- grass or better vistas. Prevention-focused individuals
vated to do whatever it takes to get out of a current may miss opportunities to improve their situation

 too m uch of a good th ing?


because they are content with “good enough”—they when given minimal or ambiguous information.
are content with “0” being satisfactory and “+1” not When the risks of a new product were made salient,
being necessary. Furthermore, perceptions of cur- promotion-focused individuals were no more likely
rent states can be amiss, and prevention-focused than prevention-focused individuals to buy the
individuals may be more likely to stay in suboptimal product (Herzenstein et al., 2007).
states that they’ve categorized as “0” even though, In search of the ultimate experience, promotion-
in fact, they are negative. For instance, returning focused individuals prefer extreme highs, even at the
to marriage, prevention-focused individuals may risk of some extreme lows, rather than a middling
be more likely to stay in bad or even abusive rela- experience. When given a choice between enriched
tionships, both out of a sense of duty and a belief versus impoverished options, promotion-focused
that the relationship is “good enough.” In sum, the participants chose options with extreme attribute
significant benefits of high commitment within the values (both positive and negative) rather than
prevention system do not come without a price. impoverished options with average attribute values
(Zhang & Mittal, 2007). The greater attractiveness
Always on the Make of enriched options appears to be due to the pro-
The promotion system pushes for exploration in motion system’s greater weighting of positive ver-
the service of advancement. The promotion indi- sus negative attributes. When positive attributes are
vidual, especially sensitive to gains, is aware of the weighted more heavily, the enriched option trumps
possible greener grass that might be just over the the impoverished one (Zhang & Mittal, 2007).
next hill. Furthermore, the preferred eager strategies Consistent with this, promotion-focused individu-
of promotion-focused individuals suggest a world als are the epitomy of the classic maximizer (Shah
of better possibilities and opportunities. Motivated & Higgins, 1997); that is, promotion-focus indi-
by the difference between “0” and “+1,” promotion- viduals make decisions and evaluate commitment to
focused individuals seek out many options in their goals using a value x expectancy calculation.
aim for the ultimate experience. Consistent with This desire for maximization is also observed
this view, openness to experience (cf. John & Srivas- in the promotion-focused individual’s consider-
tava, 1999) has been shown to positively correlate ation of multiple alternatives and options when
with the promotion system (Higgins, 2008; Vaughn making decisions. Promotion-focused participants
et al., 2008). Increased promotion focus is also neg- employ a greater number of categories when sorting
atively associated with values related to stability and objects relative to prevention-focused participants
tradition (Leikas et al., 2009). (Crowe & Higgins, 1997; Liberman et al., 2001;
The promotion individual’s eagerness to pursue Molden & Higgins, 2004). When considering mul-
all possible paths means that promotion-focused tiple options, promotion-focused individuals can
individuals are less likely to stay committed to the embrace one option without derogating others; in
status quo. Relative to prevention-focused individu- other words, finding positives in Object A does
als, promotion-focused individuals are more willing not mean that Object B is negative or even needs
to give up an activity they are working on or a prize to be discounted (Liberman et al., 2001). Unlike
they currently possess for a new activity or prize prevention-focused individuals, promotion-focused
(Chernev, 2004; Crowe & Higgins, 1997; Liber- individuals are more persuaded by positive com-
man et al., 1999). Promotion-focused individuals parative frames (frames that suggest the advertised
also value the desired end state of having all the lat- (target) brand is better than the comparison brand).
est and greatest technology more than prevention- Furthermore, their ratings of the target brand are
focused individuals (cf. Herzenstein et al., 2007; uncorrelated with their ratings of the comparison
Higgins, 2002) and are more likely to accept infor- brand. In other words, they can prefer one product
mation technology changes at work (Stam & Stan- while still acknowledging benefits in the other (Jain
ton, 2010). Promotion-focused individuals own et al., 2007).
more new high-tech products than prevention- Being able to see the good in multiple paths,
focused individuals and are more likely to buy however, can bring challenges to relationships. As
cutting-edge, but not conventional, products (Her- maximizers, promotion-focused individuals are more
zenstein et al., 2007). Promotion-focused individu- likely to ask themselves whether current relationships
als’ tendency to adopt new technology appears to be make the cut. Promotion-focused individuals report
driven by their likelihood of seeing possibilities and paying more attention to romantic alternatives and
opportunities, rather than lurking dangers and risks, being more proactive about pursuing them relative to

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
prevention-focused individuals. Even when in a long- individuals are less likely to miss opportunities
term relationship, promotion-focused individuals and more likely to extract all that they can from
show more positive evaluations of romantic alterna- what the world has to offer (Galinsky, Leonardelli,
tives than prevention-focused individuals. Although Okhuysen, & Mussweiler, 2005). Yet promotion-
it is generally the case that individuals evaluate focused individuals run the risk of always being
romantic alternatives less positively when they are on the make and never being satisfied with what
committed to their current relationship, this effect they have. At times, promotion-focused individu-
is attenuated for promotion-focused individuals als may have trouble committing to relationships,
(Finkel et al., 2009). While the promotion-focused goals, or objects because of the nagging possibility
individual might be more likely to initially see the that still more could be gained. Promotion-focused
good in someone, he or she is also more likely to turn individuals may also run into trouble because they
away and see the better in someone else. have not paid enough attention to negative signals.
Because promotion-focused individuals are more For instance, promotion-focused individuals may
likely to make decisions in accordance with potential minimize accrued losses by focusing on the gains.
gains, they may open themselves up to unintended Furthermore, because they are less concerned with
losses. Again, there is a trade-off. At times, an igno- the difference between “–1” and “0,” promotion-
rance or inattention to losses can be beneficial. For focused individuals may be less likely to take action
instance, promotion-focused individuals are more when things, in fact, are not satisfactory.
successful at initiating certain health changes such
as weight loss and smoking cessation, and Fuglestad Trade-offs in Performance
et al. (2008) suggest that this is because successful Both promotion and prevention-focused indi-
initiation of such behaviors is often motivated by viduals are motivated to perform well. As we’ve
the perception of substantial gains (Foster, Wadden, discussed previously, the systems are differentially
Vogt, & Brewer, 1997). Thus, promotion-focused sensitive to a number of factors that have the poten-
individuals may rise to the initiation challenge more tial to influence performance—different kinds of
eagerly than prevention-focused individuals. desired end states (nurturance versus safety), out-
Yet seeing the world through gains-colored comes (success versus failure), and strategies (eager-
glasses can also get promotion-focused individuals ness versus vigilance). In this section, we discuss
in trouble. When one is focused on possible gains additional differences between the systems that
(e.g., getting to enjoy this divine torte), it can be impact productivity and performance. While the
easy to miss the possible losses (e.g., not fitting promotion system values speed, the prevention sys-
into one’s favorite jeans tomorrow). For example, tem values accuracy (Förster, Higgins, & Bianco,
promotion-focused individuals who tend to be 2003). A promotion-focused individual is more
chronic thrill-seekers are more likely to engage in likely to see the big picture, whereas a prevention-
health-detrimental behaviors, such as using stimu- focused individual is more likely to see the dots of
lants to “push through” an illness (Uskul et al., paint (Förster & Higgins, 2005; Semin, Higgins,
2008). If good health is seen as just another posi- de Montes, Estourget, & Valencia, 2005). Promo-
tive outcome (and not a necessity), it may be more tion focus facilitates creativity, while a prevention
likely to be overridden by other, conflicting goals. focus facilitates performance on analytical tasks
Promotion-focused individuals may not as easily (Friedman & Förster, 2001; Seibt & Förster, 2004).
resist tempting distractions (Freitas et al., 2002). As we develop later, these differences have distinct
Sengupta and Zhou (2007) have also shown that advantages and disadvantages within each system.
impulsive eaters, relative to nonimpulsive eaters, are Furthermore, unlike some of the trade-offs that
more likely to show promotion system activation we’ve discussed in earlier sections, these trade-offs
upon exposure to a tempting food; this activation are often more closely tied to specific situations;
mediates the effect of impulsivity on choice of the for example, whether enhanced creativity will be
tempting food. a boon or a bust typically depends on the demands
Promotion-focused individuals hold the world of a given task.
on a string. It can be a beautiful world, full of hope, The promotion system is associated with greater
possibility, and promise. Any peak experience may creativity (Crowe & Higgins, 1997; Friedman
be topped tomorrow and the promotion-focused & Förster, 2001) and a tendency to engage in
individual believes that you should never stop look- more global processing (Förster & Higgins, 2005),
ing. Because of this worldview, promotion-focused including the use of more abstract language

 too m uch of a good th ing?


(Semin et al., 2005). Eager strategies coupled with and seeing the big picture that can support creative
openness to novel and diverse ideas facilitate cre- thought. For instance, prevention focus has been
ative performance on many kinds of tasks. For associated with increased perseverance on initially
instance, promotion-focused participants perform activated information, blocking the subsequent
better than prevention-focused participants on production of more novel responses (Friedman &
creative insight problems and on tasks that require Förster, 2001). But relative to promotion-focused
creative generation (e.g., generating creative uses individuals, prevention-focused individuals do bet-
for a brick). ter on tasks that require item-specific elaboration
The enhanced creativity of promotion-focused (Zhu & Meyers-Levy, 2007). Prevention-focused
participants appears to be due, at least in part, to individuals focus on the concrete components
the fact that promotion-focused participants are less of a visual scene; they are faster at identifying the
likely to be blocked by recently activated information smaller letters that make up a larger letter, whereas
that can interfere with novel production (Friedman promotion-focused individuals show the opposite
& Förster, 2001). In addition, a promotion-focused pattern (Förster & Higgins, 2005). A local process-
individual is more likely to see the forest beyond the ing approach supports a prevention-focused individ-
trees; global processing facilitates moving beyond ual’s concern with vigilantly maintaining security.
concrete details in order to see new possibilities To guard against possible danger and loss, it is nec-
(Förster & Higgins, 2005). Consistent with this, essary to be thoroughly aware of one’s surroundings
promotion-focused individuals do better on tasks (e.g., Has someone moved that vase to the left?)
that require relational elaboration (Zhu & Meyers- and be prepared for action (e.g., There might be an
Levy, 2007) and are better at “expanding the pie” intruder in the house). Additionally, local processing
in integrative negotiations (Galinsky et al., 2005). facilitates analytical thinking (Friedman, Fishbach,
But what happens when there are obstacles to carry- Förster, & Werth, 2003) and that may be one of the
ing out the creative task successfully and persistence reasons that prevention-focused individuals tend to
is needed despite the likelihood of success being perform better on those kinds of tasks (Friedman &
low? Once again there is a trade-off. The trade- Förster, 2001; Seibt & Förster, 2004). Additionally,
off is that promotion-focused individuals generate prevention-focused individuals are good at main-
more ideas than prevention-focused individuals, but taining, that is, committing to, the necessary con-
prevention-focused individuals persevere in the cre- straints involved in analytical reasoning.
ative project more than promotion-focused individ- The prevention system’s focus on the concrete
uals in the face of obstacles (Lam & Chiu, 2002). is also related to the prevention system’s emphasis
And there is another trade-off from a promo- on accuracy in performance (Förster et al., 2003).
tion focus as well. When tasks demand creativity, Most tasks require some combination of speed
being in a chronically or temporarily promotion- (there is some deadline at some point) and accuracy
focused state will serve one well. However, there are (work riddled with errors is generally unacceptable).
times when seeing the world more abstractly and Prevention-focused individuals weight accuracy
globally can be problematic. Important details and more heavily; a job well done is a job done without
errors can be missed. Sometimes the insight comes error (or at least minimized error). Across multiple
precisely from attention to concrete, logical con- studies, Förster et al. (2003) found that preven-
nections. Weighting the abstract more heavily can tion focus was associated with greater accuracy
even lead promotion-focused individuals to focus and slower performance. Furthermore, the closer
less on the concrete, pragmatic functions of prod- participants got to the goal, the more these effects
ucts they are considering (Hassenzahl, Schöbel, & were intensified. Prevention-focused participants
Trautmann, 2008). are also more likely, relative to promotion-focused
Prevention-focused individuals, on the other participants, to use a rereading strategy when
hand, focus on the concrete more than the abstract, they encounter confusing text (Miele, Molden, &
see the local rather than global features, and tend Gardner, 2009). When rereading provides the pos-
to perform worse on creative tasks and better on sibility of clarification, this strategy relates to bet-
tasks that require analytical processing (Förster & ter performance. When rereading cannot clarify,
Higgins, 2005; Friedman & Förster, 2001; Seibt however, this strategy is unrelated to performance,
& Förster, 2004; Semin et al., 2005). The vigilance suggesting that prevention-focused individuals will
of the prevention system against making mistakes sometimes be more likely to invest resources in
works against taking some of the risks, opening up, thoroughness that is not rewarded.

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
This motivational concern with accuracy is also what matters most is that a task is done right, even
reflected in the prevention system’s greater con- if that requires more time. When task complexity is
cern with safety (Henning et al., 2009; Van Noort, high, promotion focus is associated with increased
Kerkhof, & Fennis, 2008; Wallace & Chen, 2006; productivity performance and decreased safety per-
Wallace et al., 2008). Whether or not this focus on formance (Wallace et al., 2008). Being less con-
accuracy and safety is beneficial for performance cerned about safety can have potentially devastating
depends on the demands of the situation. In many impacts on overall production if a serious mistake is
situations, accuracy and thoroughness in perfor- made or a significant accident occurs.
mance is valued. At times, however, in order to
manage multiple demands, it is better to simply get Constraining the Systems
a task done adequately, rather than complete only Being “more” motivated, in terms of increased
part of it well. Additionally, while high produc- promotion or prevention system activation, is
tion and safe production can often coexist, there not unequivocally a good thing. While increased
are times when one must be sacrificed for the other. strength of either the promotion system or the
Wallace et al. (2008) found that under normal con- prevention system can have beneficial effects for
ditions, prevention focus was related to good safety well-being and self-regulation, one system is not
performance and was unrelated to productivity per- better than the other, nor does increased motiva-
formance. Under high task complexity, however, tion within a system come without costs. Rather,
the trade-offs between these concerns became hard as we’ve explored in this chapter, with the increased
to avoid, with prevention also becoming related benefits of more motivation come distinctive vul-
to decreased productivity performance (the classic nerabilities. Some weaknesses/costs exist regardless
quality vs. quantity trade-off). of an individual’s situation. For instance, in general,
In contrast to the prevention system, the promo- increased promotion focus is related to increased
tion system values speed in performance (Förster risk of depression (Strauman, 2002). Some costs
et al., 2003). The more quickly a task can be com- emerge only under specific conditions. For example,
pleted, the more quickly an individual can move on the prevention system’s concern with accuracy will
to the next potential gain. A job well done is a job be particularly problematic in situations that value
done quickly and efficiently. Promotion-focused speed or output quantity, not thoroughness or out-
individuals generally perform faster and with less put quality. Additionally, some vulnerabilities may
accuracy than prevention-focused individuals, with emerge when individuals are out of step with the
these effects intensifying the closer participants get dominant motivational orientation in their culture,
to a goal (Förster et al., 2003). Similarly, promotion as when well-being is reduced for individuals high
focus is associated with increased productivity per- in promotion focus who live in a culture such as
formance (Wallace & Chen, 2006; Wallace et al., Japan that is low in promotion as an aggregate (Ful-
2008). mer et al., 2010) or for individuals high in preven-
The promotion system is also associated with tion focus who live in a culture such as Italy that
increased reliance on affective information when is low in prevention. These individuals can “feel
making decisions and forming evaluations, which wrong” in and disengage from the situations within
may be due, at least in part, to the fact that affect- their culture that are a nonfit for them (cf. Higgins,
based heuristics tend to less effortful and faster 2008). In many different ways, then, it is clear that
(Pham & Avnet, 2009). Under the right conditions, the pure, unconstrained forms of each system pres-
this emphasis on speed can serve the promotion- ent challenges.
focused individual very well, sometimes not even Because of these challenges, constraints on
at the cost of accuracy. For instance, Förster et al. the systems are important, both for effective self-
(2003) found that, as predicted, promotion-focused regulation and optimal well-being (Higgins, 2011).
individuals were faster at finishing a proofreading Constraints allow for the systems to be kept in
task compared to prevention-focused individuals. check. Constraints allow for flexible responding,
This speed, however, was actually associated with such that promotion or prevention moments can
better performance for finding “easy” mistakes; while shine brighter, less tarnished by potential down-
promotion-focused individuals were less likely to sides. Idealism can be reigned in by reminders of
spot tricky or difficult errors, they were more likely duties or possible dangers. Performance can be opti-
to catch the obvious problems. At other times, how- mized when one balances the need for speed with
ever, the trade-offs are more evident. Sometimes a concern for accuracy. Constraints can come in

 too m uch of a good th ing?


a number of different forms. Constraints can come which system best serves particular task demands
from within regulatory focus (e.g., prevention sys- and to be able to flexibly switch between systems.
tem constraining the promotion system) and from Exploring what factors—both within an individual
other motivational systems (e.g., regulatory mode; and within environments—make it more or less
Kruglanski et al., 2000). Constraints can come from likely that the dual strength of the systems can be
within an individual (e.g., their own prevention and utilized is an important question that remains to be
promotion orientations interacting) or in dynam- explored.
ics that emerge in dyads or groups (cf. Bohns et al., Additionally, it is an open question how chronic
2011; Levine, Higgins, & Choi, 2000). Constraints and situationally induced temporary accessibility
can emerge from interactions between different of the systems may work together dynamically in
chronic tendencies or between chronic tendencies terms of constraining forces. For instance, how is
and situational presses. a chronic promotion-focused individual served by
Within dyads and groups, the promotion and a leadership style that induces prevention focus? On
prevention systems can exert valuable constraining the one hand, such a situation can be problematic
forces on each other. For instance, imagine a team if the individual is in a consistent state of regula-
that needs to create a new product idea and present tory nonfit (Higgins, 2000). On the other hand, if
it to a client. Team members who are promotion the organization’s primary objectives revolve around
focused will tend to be better at generating inno- security, creating a prevention-focused environ-
vative and creative ideas, but they may be likely ment at work may place important constraints on
to overlook potential problems or to miss errors promotion-focused employees who would otherwise
in their presentation. Their creative contributions be less naturally inclined to attend to such issues.
will result in a better output if they are balanced by As another example, adding a coworker’s preven-
prevention-focused members who are more likely tion concern with reducing errors of commission to
to thoroughly analyze proposals for possible chal- a personal promotion concern with reducing errors
lenges and errors. Thus, teams made up of mem- of omission could enhance someone’s decision-
bers with complementary regulatory focus may get making discriminability.
the benefits of each system with fewer of the costs Though the promotion and prevention systems
(cf. Bohns et al., 2011). Having a partner or team may place important constraints on each other,
member with a complementary regulatory focus constraints can also come from other motivational
may also allow individuals to engage strategically systems. For example, individuals also differ in the
in an activity in ways that fit both their orientation extent to which they are motivated by two dif-
and the orientation of their partner, such as cooking ferent aspects of self-regulation—initiating and
a meal together and having the prevention partner maintaining smooth movement from state to state
take on the vigilant tasks and the promotion partner (locomotion) and comparing and critically evaluat-
take on the eager tasks (Bohns et al., 2011). Given ing options (assessment), a distinction highlighted
that regulatory focus orientations can be manipu- in regulatory mode theory (Higgins et al., 2003;
lated within groups (e.g., Levine et al., 2000), it is Kruglanski et al., 2000). Locomotors prefer action
possible to create work environments that optimize over inaction, such that they would rather do almost
the benefits of each system (for a locomotion plus anything rather than nothing (Higgins et al., 2003).
assessment case of this, see Mauro, Pierro, Man- The locomotion system’s preference for action can
netti, Higgins, & Kruglanski, 2009). Exploring the potentially provide a useful constraint on the pre-
most effective ways to do this is an exciting avenue vention system’s desire for thorough and careful
for future research. analysis. Individuals who are chronically high in
Even within individuals, the promotion and pre- both prevention and locomotion may be better off
vention systems, because they are orthogonal, can than individuals who are chronically high in preven-
exert constraining influences on one another. In tion and assessment, for instance. This latter combi-
other words, individuals can be chronically strong in nation may be particularly problematic in creating
both the promotion and prevention systems. How- individuals who will carefully assess without end—
ever, less is known about what factors make such going over and over a decision without being able to
high-promotion/high-prevention individuals more take action (e.g., being “lost in thought”).
or less effective self-regulators. Simply being chroni- Though constraints on the systems are important,
cally strong in both systems may not be enough; it it is also important to recognize that sometimes what
may also be important to have the skills to identify matters in terms of effective performance or well-being

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
is not whether individuals are in a prevention- versus 2011). Future research will need to investigate how
promotion-focused state, but whether individuals promotion, prevention, locomotion and assessment
are in a state of regulatory fit, that is, pursuing goals motivations function together effectively. It is this
using means that fit their underlying motivational full organization of motivations, working together,
orientation (Higgins, 2000). For instance, while in that is critical.
some situations prevention-focused individuals may
be better at exhibiting self-control (e.g., resisting References
Aaker, J. L., & Lee, A. Y. (2001). ‘I’ seek pleasures and ‘we’ avoid
temptations) relative to promotion-focused individ- pains: The role of self-regulatory goals in information pro-
uals (Freitas et al., 2002), there are other situations cessing and persuasion. Journal of Consumer Research, 28,
in which both prevention- and promotion-focused 33–49. doi:10.1086/321946
individuals can be successful in exhibiting self-control Beck, A. T., Rush, J., Shaw, B., & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive
when they use strategic means that fit their underly- therapy of depression. New York: The Guilford Press.
Bohns, V. K., Lucas, G. M., Molden, D. C., Finkel,
ing orientation (Hong & Lee, 2008). Similarly, while E. J., Coolsen, M. K., Kumashiro, M., . . . Higgins, E.T.
in some situations promotion-focused individuals (2011). When opposites fit: Increased relationship well-
may be more open to change relative to prevention- being from partner complementarity in regulatory focus.
focused individuals (Higgins, 2008), being in a state Manuscript in preparation.
of regulatory fit may make both promotion- and Boldero, J. M., Moretti, M. M., Bell, R. C., & Francis, J. J. (2005).
Self-discrepancies and negative affect: A primer on when to
prevention-focused individuals more flexible and look for specificity, and how to find it. Australian Journal of Psy-
open to alternatives. For instance, Maddox, Filoteo, chology, 57, 139–147. doi:10.1080/00049530500048730
Glass, and Markman (2010) found that individuals Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol. 1: Attachment..
in regulatory fit showed better set shifting (aban- New York: Basic Books.
doning a current rule for a new applicable rule) on Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss. Vol. 2: Separation: Anxiety
and anger. New York: Basic Books.
a Wisconsin Card Sorting task relative to individuals Brendl, C. M., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Principles of judging
in nonfit. valence: What makes events positive or negative? Advances
in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 95–160. doi:10.1016/
Concluding Comments S0065–2601(08)60237–3.
On the one hand, both promotion- and Brickman, P. (1987). Commitment, conflict, and caring. Englewood
prevention-focused individuals can be successful in Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
life pursuits; on the other hand, both promotion and Brockner, J. (2010). A contemporary look at organizational justice:
prevention-focused individuals can experience diffi- Multiplying insult times injury. New York: Routledge.
Brockner, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Regulatory focus theory:
culties. Having a lot of either promotion or preven- Implications for the study of emotions at work. Organiza-
tion motivation does not guarantee a smooth ride; tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 86, 35–66.
rather, there are trade-offs to being strongly moti- doi:10.1006/obhd.2001.2972.
vated in either system. Having more motivation, Chernev, A. (2004). Goal orientation and consumer prefer-
then, is not always better. Having more motivation ence for the status quo. Journal of Consumer Research, 31,
557–565. doi:10.1086/425090.
simply means that one is likely to experience both Choi, Y., & Mai-Dalton, R. R. (1999). The model of follow-
the upsides and the downsides of a particular moti- ers’ responses to self-sacrificial leadership: An empirical test.
vational system. What those upsides and downsides The Leadership Quarterly, 10, 397–421. doi:10.1016/S1048–
are depends on whether an individual is in a pro- 9843(99)00025–9.
motion or prevention state. And how beneficial the Cornette, M. M., Strauman, T. J., Abramson, L. Y., & Busch, A. M.
(2009). Self-discrepancy and suicidal ideation. Cognition and
upsides are or how detrimental the downsides are Emotion, 23, 504–527. doi:10.1080/02699930802012005.
depends on the particular demands of the situation Cropanzano, R., Paddock, L., Rupp, D. E., Bagger, J., & Bald-
or task. What may matter most for effective self- win, A. (2008). How regulatory focus impacts the process-
regulation is having the right motivation that fits the by-outcome interaction for perceived fairness and emotions.
demands of a particular situation, and understand- Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 105,
36–51.
ing that even then, there can be trade-offs. Exploring Crowe, E., & Higgins, E. T. (1997). Regulatory focus and stra-
how to negotiate these trade-offs in order to maxi- tegic inclinations: Promotion and prevention in decision-
mize the benefits and minimize the costs will con- making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Pro-
tinue to be an interesting and significant question cesses, 69, 117–132. doi:10.1006/obhd.1996.2675.
going forward. As we hinted at earlier, we believe Cunningham, W. A., Raye, C. L., & Johnson, M. K. (2005).
Neural correlates of evaluation associated with promo-
that the answer to effective self-regulation will need tion and prevention regulatory focus. Cognitive, Affective
to go beyond promotion and prevention and add and Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 202–211. doi:10.3758/
locomotion and assessment to the picture (Higgins, CABN.5.2.202.

 too m uch of a good th ing?


Eddington, K. M., Dolcos, F., McLean, A. N., Krishnan, congruous thoughts and information. Journal of Personality
K. R., Cabeza, R., & Strauman, T. J. (2009). Neural corre- and Social Psychology, 59, 1119–1127.
lates of idiographic goal priming in depression: Goal-specific Grant, H., & Higgins, E. T. (2003). Optimism, promotion
dysfunctions in the orbitofrontal cortex. Social Cognitive pride, and prevention pride as predictors of quality of life.
and Affective Neuroscience, 4, 238–246. doi:10.1093/scan/ Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1521–1532.
nsp016. doi:10.1177/0146167203256919.
Finkel, E. J., Molden, D. C., Johnson, S. E., & Eastwick, P. Hassenzahl, M., Schöbel, M., & Trautmann, T. (2008). How
W. (2009). Regulatory focus and romantic alternatives. In motivational orientation influences the evaluation and choice
J. P. Forgas, R. F. Baumeister, & D. M. Tice (Eds.), Psychol- of hedonic and pragmatic interactive products: The role of
ogy of self-regulation: Cognitive, affective, and motivational regulatory focus. Interacting with Computers, 20, 473–479.
processes. The Sydney symposium of social psychology (pp. doi:10.1016/j.intcom.2008.05.001.
319–335). New York: Psychology Press. Hendrix, K. S., & Hirt, E. R. (2009). Stressed out over pos-
Florack, A., Ineichen, S., & Bieri, R. (2009). The impact of regu- sible failure: The role of regulatory fit on claimed self-
latory focus on the effects of two-sided advertising. Social handicapping. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45,
Cognition, 27, 37–56. doi:10.1521/soco.2009.27.1.37. 51–59. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.08.016.
Foster, G. D., Wadden, T. A., Vogt, R. A., & Brewer, G. (1997). Henning, J. B., Stufft, C. J., Payne, S. C., Bergman, M. E., Mannan,
What is a reasonable weight loss? Patients’ expectations and M. S., & Keren, N. (2009). The influence of individual dif-
evaluations of obesity treatment outcomes. Journal of Consult- ferences on organizational safety attitudes. Safety Science, 47,
ing and Clinical Psychology, 65, 79–85. doi:10.1037/0022– 337–345. doi:10.1016/j.ssci.2008.05.003.
006X.65.1.79. Herzenstein, M., Posavac, S. S., & Brakus, J. J. (2007). Adoption
Förster, J., Grant, H., Idson, L. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Suc- of new and really new products: The effects of self-regulation
cess/failure feedback, expectancies, and approach/avoidance systems and risk salience. Journal of Marketing Research, 44,
motivation: How regulatory focus moderates classic rela- 251–260. doi:10.1509/jmkr.44.2.251.
tions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 253–260. Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-Discrepancy: A theory relating self
doi:10.1006/jesp.2000.1455. and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340.
Förster, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). How global versus local Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American
perception fits regulatory focus. Psychological Science, 16, Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300. doi:10.1037/0003–066-
631–636. doi:10.1111/j.1467–9280.2005.01586.x. X.52.12.1280.
Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Bianco, A. T. (2003). Speed/ Higgins, E. T. (1999). Persons or situations: Unique explanatory
accuracy decisions in task performance: Built-in trade- principles or variability in general principles? In D. Cer-
off or separate strategic concerns? Organizational Behavior vone & Y. Shoda (Eds.), The coherence of personality: Social-
and Human Decision Processes, 90, 148–164. doi:10.1016/ cognitive bases of consistency, variability, and organization (pp.
S0749–5978(02)00509–5. 61–93). New York: Guilford Press.
Freitas, A., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. (2002). Regulatory Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit.
fit and resisting temptation during goal pursuit. Journal of American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230.
Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 291–298. Higgins, E. T. (2001). Promotion and prevention experiences:
Freitas, A., Liberman, N., Salovey, P., & Higgins, E. (2002). Relating emotions to nonemotional motivational states. In
When to begin? Regulatory focus and initiating goal pursuit. J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 121–130. 186–211). London: Psychology Press.
Friedman, R. S., Fishbach, A., Förster, J., & Werth, L. Higgins, E. T. (2002). How self-regulation creates distinct val-
(2003). Attentional priming effects on creativity. Cre- ues: The case of promotion and prevention decision making.
ativity Research Journal, 15, 277–286. doi:10.1207/ Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12, 177–191. doi:10.1207/
S15326934CRJ152&3_18. S15327663JCP1203_01.
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion Higgins, E. T. (2006). Value from hedonic experience and
and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and engagement. Psychological Review, 113, 439–460.
Social Psychology, 81, 1001–1013. doi:10.1037/0022–3514 Higgins, E. T. (2008). Culture and personality: Variability across
.81.6.1001. universal motives as the missing link. Social and Personal-
Fuglestad, P. T., Rothman, A. J., & Jeffery, R. W. (2008). Getting ity Psychology Compass, 2, 608–634. doi:10.1111/j.1751–
there and hanging on: The effect of regulatory focus on per- 9004.2007.00075.x.
formance in smoking and weight loss interventions. Health Higgins, E. T. (2011). Beyond pleasure and pain: How motivation
Psychology, 27, S260-S270. doi:10.1037/0278–6133.27.3. works. New York: Oxford University Press.
Fulmer, C., Gelfand, M., Kruglanski, A., Kim-Prieto, Higgins, E. T., Bond, R., Klein, R., & Strauman, T. (1986).
C., Diener, E., Pierro, A., & Higgins, E.T. (2010). On “feel- Self-discrepancies and emotional vulnerability. How
ing right” in cultural contexts: How person-culture match magnitude, accessibility, and type of discrepancy influ-
affects self-esteem and subjective well-being. Psychological ence affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51,
Science, 21(11), 1563–1569. 5–15.
Galinsky, A. D., Leonardelli, G. J., Okhuysen, G. A., & Higgins, E. T., Friedman, R. S., Harlow, R. E., Idson,
Mussweiler, T. (2005). Regulatory focus at the bargain- L. C., Ayduk, O. N., & Taylor, A. (2001). Achievement
ing table: Promoting distributive and integrative success. orientations from subjective histories of success: Promotion
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 1087–1098. pride versus prevention pride. European Journal of Social Psy-
doi:10.1177/0146167205276429. chology, 31, 3–23. doi:10.1002/ejsp.27.
Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Delib- Higgins, E. T., Kruglanski, A. W., & Pierro, A. (2003). Regu-
erative and implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward latory mode: Locomotion and assessment as distinct

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
orientations. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 35, Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1192–1199.
293–344. doi:10.1016/S0065–2601(03)01005–0. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.07.009.
Higgins, E. T., Roney, C., Crowe, E., & Hymes, C. (1994). Ideal Langens, T. A. (2007). Regulatory focus and illusions of
versus ought predilections for approach and avoidance: Dis- control. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 226–
tinct self-regulatory systems. Journal of Personality and Social 237. doi:10.1177/0146167206293494.
Psychology, 66, 276–286. Lee, A. Y., Aaker, J. L., & Gardner, W. L. (2000). The pleasures
Higgins, E. T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997). Emotional and pains of distinct self-construals: The role of interde-
responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus as pendence in regulatory focus. Journal of Personality and
moderator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, Social Psychology, 78, 1122–1134. doi:10.1037/0022–3514
515–525. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.72.3.515. .78.6.1122.
Higgins, E. T., & Tykocinski, O. (1992). Self-discrepancies and Leikas, S., Lönnqvist, J., Verkasalo, M., & Lindeman, M. (2009).
biographical memory: Personality and cognition at the level Regulatory focus systems and personal values. European Jour-
of psychological situation. Personality and Social Psychology nal of Social Psychology, 39, 415–429. doi:10.1002/ejsp.547.
Bulletin, 18, 527–535. doi:10.1177/0146167292185002. Leone, L., Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2005). Emo-
Hong, J., & Lee, A. Y. (2008). Be fit and be strong: Mastering tions and decision making: Regulatory focus moder-
self-regulation through regulatory fit. Journal of Consumer ates the influence of anticipated emotions on action
Research, 34, 682–695. doi:10.1086/521902. evaluations. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 1175–1198.
Idson, L.C., & Higgins, E.T. (2000). How current feedback doi:10.1080/02699930500203203.
and chronic effectiveness influence motivation: Everything Levine, J. M., Higgins, E. T., & Choi, H. (2000). Develop-
to gain versus everything to lose. European Journal of Social ment of strategic norms in groups. Organizational Behavior
Psychology, 30, 583–592. and Human Decision Processes, 82, 88–101. doi:10.1006/
Idson, L. C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2000). Distin- obhd.2000.2889.
guishing gains from nonlosses and losses from nongains: A Liberman, N., Idson, L. C., Camacho, C. J., & Higgins, E. T.
regulatory focus perspective on hedonic intensity. Journal of (1999). Promotion and prevention choices between stability
Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 252–274. doi:10.1006/ and change. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
jesp.1999.1402. 1135–1145. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.77.6.1135.
Idson, L., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E.T. (2004). Imagining Liberman, N., Molden, D. C., Idson, L. C., & Higgins,
how you’d feel: The role of motivational experiences from E. T. (2001). Promotion and prevention focus on alterna-
regulatory fit. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, tive hypotheses: Implications for attributional functions.
926–937. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 5–18.
Jain, S. P., Lindsey, C., Agrawal, N., & Maheswaran, D. (2007). doi:10.1037/0022–3514.80.1.5.
For better of for worse? Valenced comparative frames and Lockwood, P., Jordan, C. H., & Kunda, Z. (2002). Motiva-
regulatory focus. Journal of Consumer Research, 34, 57–65. tion by positive or negative role models: Regulatory focus
doi:10.1086/513046. determines who will best inspire us. Journal of Personality and
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five Trait taxon- Social Psychology, 83, 854–864. doi:10.1037/0022–3514
omy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In .83.4.854.
L. A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Maddox, W. T., Filoteo, J. V., Glass, B. D., & Markman,
Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). New York: Guil- A. B. (2010). Regulatory match effects on a Modified
ford Press. Wisconsin Card Sort Task. Journal of the International
Jones, N. P., Papadakis, A. A., Hogan, C. M., & Strauman, Neuropsychological Society, 16, 352–359. doi:10.1017/
T. J. (2009). Over and over again: Rumination, reflection, S1355617709991408.
and promotion goal failure and their interactive effects on Manian, N., Papadakis, A. A., Strauman, T. J., & Essex, M. J.
depressive symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, (2006). The development of children’s ideal and ought self-
254–259. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.12.007. guides: Parenting, temperament, and individual differences
Keller, J. (2008). On the development of regulatory focus: The in guide strength. Journal of Personality, 74, 1619–1645.
role of parenting styles. European Journal of Social Psychology, doi:10.1111/j.1467–6494.2006.00422.x.
38, 354–364. doi:10.1002/ejsp.460. Manian, N., Strauman, T. J., & Denney, N. (1998). Tempera-
Klenk, M., Strauman, T., & Higgins, E. T. (2011). Regula- ment, recalled parenting styles, and self-regulation: Testing
tory focus and anxiety: A self-regulatory model of GAD- the developmental postulates of self-discrepancy theory. Jour-
Depression comorbidity. Personality and Individual Differ- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1321–1332.
ences, 50, 935–943. Markman, K. D., McMullen, M. N., Elizaga, R. A., &
Kruglanski, A., Higgins, E.T., Pierro, A., Thompson, E., Atash, Mizoguchi, N. (2006). Counterfactual thinking and regula-
M., Shah, J., & Spiegel, S. (2000). To “do the right thing” tory fit. Judgment and Decision Making, 1, 98–107.
or to “just do it”: Locomotion and assessment as distinct Mauro, R., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., Higgins, E. T., & Kruglan-
self-regulatory imperatives. Journal of Personality and Social ski, A. W. (2009). The perfect mix: Regulatory complemen-
Psychology, 79, 793–815. tarity and the speed-accuracy balance in group performance.
Lam, T. W., & Chiu, C. (2002). The motivational function of Psychological Science, 20, 681–685. doi:10.1111/j.1467–
regulatory focus in creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 9280.2009.02363.x.
36, 138–150. Miele, D. B., Molden, D. C., & Gardner, W. L. (2009). Moti-
de Lange, M. A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2009). To err vated comprehension regulation: Vigilant versus eager meta-
is human: How regulatory focus and action orienta- cognitive control. Memory and Cognition, 37, 779–795.
tion predict performance following errors. Journal of doi:10.3758/MC.37.6.779.

 too m uch of a good th ing?


Miller, A. K., & Markman, K. D. (2007). Depression, regulatory Scholer, A. A., Stroessner, S. J., & Higgins, E. T. (2008).
focus, and motivation. Personality and Individual Differences, Responding to negativity: How a risky tactic can serve a vigi-
43, 427–436. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.12.006. lant strategy. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
Molden, D., & Higgins, E. (2004). Categorization under 767–774.
uncertainty: Resolving vagueness and ambiguity with eager Scholer, A. A., Zou, X., Fujita, K., Stroessner, S. J., & Higgins,
versus vigilant strategies. Social Cognition, 22, 248–277. E. T. (2010). When risk-seeking becomes a motivational
Molden, D., & Higgins, E. (2008). How preferences for eager necessity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99,
versus vigilant judgment strategies affect self-serving con- 215–231.
clusions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, Scott, L., & O’Hara, M. W. (1993). Self-discrepancies in clini-
1219–1228. cally anxious and depressed university students. Journal of
Molden, D. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Motivated thinking. Abnormal Psychology, 102, 282–287. doi:10.1037/0021–
In K. Holyoak and B. Morrison (Eds.), Handbook of think- 843X.102.2.282.
ing and reasoning (pp. 295–320). New York: Cambridge Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2004). Stereotype threat and performance:
University Press. How self-stereotypes influence processing by inducing regu-
Molden, D. C., Lucas, G. M., Gardner, W. L., Dean, K., & latory foci. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87,
Knowles, M. L. (2009). Motivations for prevention or pro- 38–56. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.87.1.38.
motion following social exclusion: Being rejected versus Semin, G. R., Higgins, E.T., de Montes, L. G., Estourget,
being ignored. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Y., & Valencia, J. F. (2005). Linguistic signatures of regu-
96, 415–431. doi:10.1037/a0012958. latory focus: How abstraction fits promotion more than
Mowrer, O. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: prevention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89,
Wiley. 36–45. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.89.1.36.
Ouschan, L., Boldero, J. M., Kashima, Y., Wakimoto, R., & Sengupta, J., & Zhou, R. (2007). Understanding impulsive eat-
Kashima, E. S. (2007). Regulatory Focus Strategies Scale: ers’ choice behaviors: The motivational influences of regu-
A measure of individual differences in the endorsement of latory focus. Journal of Marketing Research, 44, 297–308.
regulatory strategies. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 10, doi:10.1509/jmkr.44.2.297.
243–457. doi:10.1111/j.1467–839X.2007.00233.x. Shah, J., Brazy, P., & Higgins, E. T. (2004). Promoting us or prevent-
Papadakis, A. A., Prince, R. P., Jones, N. P., & Strauman, ing them: Regulatory focus and manifestations of intergroup
T. J. (2006). Self-regulation, rumination, and vul- bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 433–446.
nerability to depression in adolescent girls. Develop- Shah, J. (2003). The motivational looking glass: How significant
ment and Psychopathology, 18, 815–829. doi:10.1017/ others implicitly affect goal appraisals. Journal of Personality
S0954579406060408. and Social Psychology, 85, 424–439. doi:10.1037/0022–3514
Pham, M. T., & Avnet, T. (2009). Contingent reliance on the .85.3.424.
affect heuristic as a function of regulatory focus. Organiza- Shah, J., & Higgins, E. T. (1997). Expectancy × value effects:
tional Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108, 267–278. Regulatory focus as determinant of magnitude and direction.
doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2008.10.001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 447–458.
Poels, K., & Dewitte, S. (2008). Hope and self-regulatory doi:10.1037/0022–3514.73.3.447.
goals applied to an advertising context: Promoting Shah, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Regulatory concerns and
prevention stimulates goal-directed behavior. Journal appraisal efficiency: The general impact of promotion and
of Business Research, 61, 1030–1040. doi:10.1016/j. prevention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
jbusres.2007.09.019. 693–705. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.80.5.693.
Reznik, I., & Andersen, S. M. (2007). Agitation and despair Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance
in relation to parents: Activating emotional suffering in incentives and means: How regulatory focus influences goal
transference. European Journal of Personality, 21, 281–301. attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74,
doi:10.1002/per.628. 285–293. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.74.2.285.
Roese, N. J., Hur, T., & Pennington, G. L. (1999). Counter- Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forget-
factual thinking and regulatory focus: Implications for ting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal
action versus inaction and sufficiency versus necessity. Jour- shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1109–1120. 1261–1280. doi:10.1037/0022–3514.83.6.1261.
doi:10.1037/0022–3514.77.6.1109. Stam, K. R., & Stanton, J. M. (2010). Events, emotions, and
Rothman, A. J. (2000). Toward a theory-based analysis of behav- technology: Examining acceptance of workplace technol-
ioral maintenance. Health Psychology, 19, 64–69. ogy changes. Information Technology & People, 23, 23–53.
Sassenberg, K., & Hansen, N. (2007). The impact of regulatory doi:10.1108/09593841011022537.
focus on affective responses to social discrimination. Euro- Strauman, T. J. (1989). Self-discrepancies in clinical depression
pean Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 421–444. doi:10.1002/ and social phobia: Cognitive structures that underlie emo-
ejsp.358. tional disorders? Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 98, 14–22.
Scholer, A. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2008). Distinguishing levels of doi:10.1037/0021–843X.98.1.14.
approach and avoidance: An analysis using regulatory focus Strauman, T. J. (2002). Self-regulation and depression. Self and
theory. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoid- Identity, 1, 151–157. doi:10.1080/152988602317319339.
ance motivation (pp. 489–503). New York: Psychology Press. Strauman, T. J., & Higgins, E. T. (1987). Automatic activation
Scholer, A. A., Ozaki, Y., & Higgins, E. T. (2011). Inflating and of self-discrepancies and emotional syndromes: When cog-
deflating the self: Sustaining motivational concerns through nitive structures influence affect. Journal of Personality and
self-evaluation. Under review. Social Psychology, 53, 1004–1014.

s c h oler, h i g g i n s 
Strauman, T. J., Kolden, G. G., Stromquist, V., Davis, Vaughn, L. A., Baumann, J., & Klemann, C. (2008). Openness
N., Kwapil, L., Heerey, E., & Schneider, K. (2001). The to experience and regulatory focus: Evidence of motivation
effects of treatments for depression on perceived failure in from fit. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 886–894.
self-regulation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 693–712. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.11.008.
doi:10.1023/A:1012915205800. Vieth, A. Z., Strauman, T. J., Kolden, G. G., Woods,
Strauman, T. J., Vieth, A. Z., Merrill, K. A., Kolden, T. E., Michels, J. L., & Klein, M. H. (2003). Self-System
G. G., Woods, T. E., Klein, M. H., . . . Kwapil, L. (2006). Therapy (SST): A theory-based psychotherapy for depres-
Self-system therapy as an intervention for self-regulatory dys- sion. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10, 245–268.
function in depression: A randomized comparison with cog- doi:10.1093/clipsy/bpg023.
nitive therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, Wallace, C., & Chen, G. (2006). A multilevel integration of
74, 367–376. doi:10.1037/0022–006X.74.2.367. personality, climate, self-regulation, and performance.
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & Personnel Psychology, 59, 529–557. doi:10.1111/j.1744–
McDowell, N. K. (2003). Are self-enhancing cognitions 6570.2006.00046.x.
associated with healthy or unhealthy biological profiles? Wallace, J. C., Little, L. M., & Shull, A. (2008). The moderat-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 605–615. ing effects of task complexity on the relationship between
doi:10.1037/0022–3514.85.4.605. regulatory foci and safety and production performance.
Uskul, A. K., Keller, J., & Oyserman, D. (2008). Regulatory fit Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 13, 95–104.
and health behavior. Psychology and Health, 23, 327–346. doi:10.1037/1076–8998.13.2.95.
doi:10.1080/14768320701360385. Wang, J., & Lee, A. Y. (2006). The role of regulatory focus in
Van-Dijk, D., & Kluger, A. N. (2004). Feedback sign effect on preference construction. Journal of Marketing Research, 43,
motivation: Is it moderated by regulatory focus? Applied Psy- 28–38. doi:10.1509/jmkr.43.1.28.
chology: An International Review, 53, 113–135. doi:10.1111/ Zhang, Y., & Mittal, V. (2007). The attractiveness of enriched
j.1464–0597.2004.00163.x. and impoverished options: Culture, self-construal, and regu-
Van Noort, G., Kerkhof, P., & Fennis, B. M. (2008). The persua- latory focus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
siveness of online safety cues: The impact of prevention focus 588–598. doi:10.1177/0146167206296954.
compatibility of web content on consumers’ risk perceptions, Zhu, R., & Meyers-Levy, J. (2007). Exploring the cognitive
attitudes, and intentions. Journal of Interactive Marketing, 22, mechanism that underlies regulatory focus effects. Journal of
58–72. doi:10.1002/dir.20121. Consumer Research, 34, 89–96. doi:10.1086/513049.

 too m uch of a good th ing?


CHAPTER

Motivation, Personality, and Development


6 Within Embedded Social Contexts: An
Overview of Self-Determination Theory
Edward L. Deci and Richard M. Ryan

Abstract
Self-determination theory maintains and has provided empirical support for the proposition that all
human beings have fundamental psychological needs to be competent, autonomous, and related to
others. Satisfaction of these basic needs facilitates people’s autonomous motivation (i.e., acting with a
sense of full endorsement and volition), whereas thwarting the needs promotes controlled motivation
(i.e., feeling pressured to behave in particular ways) or being amotivated (i.e., lacking intentionality).
Satisfying these basic needs and acting autonomously have been consistently shown to be associated with
psychological health and effective performance. Social contexts within which people operate, however
proximal (e.g., a family or workgroup) or distal (e.g., a cultural value or economic system), affect their
need satisfaction and type of motivation, thus affecting their wellness and effectiveness. Social contexts
also affect whether people’s life goals or aspirations tend to be more intrinsic or more extrinsic, and
that in turn affects important life outcomes.
Key Words: self-determination, autonomy, motivation, control, autonomy support, social contexts,
intrinsic motivation, life goals, autonomous motivation, embedded social contexts

To be autonomous means to behave with a sense and creativity in their life activities, relationships,
of volition, willingness, and congruence; it means and life projects.
to fully endorse and concur with the behavior one Yet autonomy is not just an individual affair.
is engaged in. Autonomy—this capacity for and Across history, groups of people have struggled to
desire to experience self-regulation and integrity—is protect or gain autonomy, and to be free of coer-
a central force within both the life span develop- cive forces from their own dictatorial governments
ment of individuals and in the movement of history or from invasions by other collectives. They have
toward greater freedom and voice for citizens within fought, and often died, to be free of oppression, as
cultures and governments. well as to express and actualize their valued aims
In healthy individual development, people and ideals. These struggles continue today, with
move in the direction of greater autonomy. This respect to both totalitarian regimes and the con-
entails internalizing and integrating external trolling forces of wealth and power wherever they
regulations over behavior, and learning to effec- subjugate or disenfranchise individuals or cultural
tively manage drives and emotions. Additionally, subgroups.
it means maintaining intrinsic motivation and Although autonomy is clearly a central issue in
interest, which are vital to assimilating new ideas both individual and collective development and
and experiences. When people are more autono- wellness, it is nonetheless a complex construct,
mous, they exhibit greater engagement, vitality, manifest in different ways. Within self-determination


theory (SDT), the concept of autonomy is, at The majority of research within SDT has focused
different times, used to refer to a motivational state, on people’s proximal social contexts and the salient
to an enduring motivational orientation, and to people within them: parents, teachers, coaches,
a fundamental psychological need, depending on managers, friends, physicians, and partners, for
what problem is being addressed. Each of these example (e.g., Deci & Ryan, 2008). Yet it is not
more specific concepts relates to the formulations just these immediate social connections that affect
of autonomy and autonomy support within SDT, people’s development and functioning, because
and the purpose of this chapter is to discuss these each proximal interpersonal context is embedded in
multiple aspects of the construct and their mean- various other more distal ones. For example, class-
ings within theory and practice. room teachers create the interpersonal climate that
A central function served by the concept of affects the motivation of students on a daily basis,
autonomy within SDT is to differentiate types yet the classrooms are embedded within schools
of motivation with their corresponding qualities where key administrators also create broader cli-
of functioning. Many historical and contemporary mates, affecting the teachers’ motivation, goals, and
theories of motivation have treated motivation as a behaviors (Deci, Spiegel, Ryan, Koestner, & Kauff-
unitary concept, either by not specifying types of man, 1982; Pelletier, Séguin-Lévesque, & Legault,
motivation or by specifying types but then add- 2002). Schools in turn are embedded within school
ing them together to form total motivation (e.g., districts, and the key administrators of the districts
Bandura, 1996; Hull, 1943). Such theories have affect the behaviors, motivations, and experiences
sometimes been able to effectively predict amount of principals, and then onward down to teachers
of behavior, but they have been much less effective and students. District administrators, in turn, are
in predicting qualities of behaviors. SDT maintains impacted by local, state, and national government
that knowing whether people’s motivation is more policies, which themselves will tend to be either
autonomous or more controlled is far more important autonomy supportive or controlling.
for making predictions about the quality of people’s Ryan and Weinstein (2009) discussed an exam-
engagement, performance, and well-being than is ple of embedded effects, detailing how government
the overall amount or intensity of motivation. And policies concerning high-stakes testing have had
even more refined predictions can be made from the a coercive influence on educational administra-
subtypes of either autonomous or controlled moti- tors’ objectives and in turn on classroom practices.
vation, as we will explain in the pages ahead. This has resulted in more teaching to tests, and less
teacher and student autonomy, engagement, and
Motivation Within Embedded Contexts satisfaction at the bottom of this chain of embedded
Motivated individuals exist within social con- contexts. In short, increasingly distal contexts such
texts, and research indicates that contexts vary in as government policies can affect individuals (in this
the degree to which they support the individuals’ example, the students), primarily via mediation by
autonomy versus control their behaviors, thoughts, the important intervening contexts (viz., state gov-
and feelings. Furthermore, at any given time peo- ernments, district administrators, principals, and
ple are under the influence of numerous embedded teachers). This is true not only in relation to educa-
contexts (Bronfenbrenner, 1979; Connell & Well- tion; one sees similar embedded context affects in
born, 1991). Both proximal interpersonal contexts relation to work organizations, sport teams, health
(e.g., the behavior of people’s parents or managers) care practices, and in many other domains.
and distal contexts (e.g., the cultural norms and At the most distal levels of analysis, considerable
economic structures of their society) can variously research has examined how cultural contexts and
support or undermine intrinsic motivation and the values (e.g., individualism and collectivism) affect
integrative tendency, which together are the bases and characterize individual motivation and behav-
of autonomous behavior. Thus, SDT uses the qual- iors (e.g. Chirkov, Ryan, Kim, & Kaplan, 2003).
ity of the social contexts within which people exist, Typically, the research focuses only on how various
as well as the individuals’ own motivational states, cultural dimensions describe individuals within
orientations, and experiences of need satisfaction, a culture, but presumably much of the effect of the
to make predictions about such outcomes as the culture ripples through different embedded con-
quality of behaviors, emotional experiences, cog- texts at both distal and proximal levels. Parents
nitive structures, and psychological and physical and schools, for example, serve to transmit their
health. cultures’ values to the young people within those

 an overview of self-determination theory


cultures. Cultures and countries differ not only but were instead derived empirically. In other words,
in their ambient values, they also have economic we found it necessary to posit these needs as human
systems (e.g., capitalism, socialism) and political universals in order to provide meaningful interpreta-
systems (e.g., democracy, totalitarianism), which tions of various phenomena that had emerged from
impact individuals—from the everyday motiva- research projects—phenomena such as the under-
tions of workers, to the value systems and lifestyles mining of intrinsic motivation by tangible rewards
embraced by their citizens (e.g., Kasser, Cohn, (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 1999), contextual factors
Kanner, & Ryan, 2007; Vansteenkiste, Ryan, & promoting the internalization of extrinsic motiva-
Deci, 2008). Furthermore, countries typically cre- tion (Ryan & Connell, 1989), and goal contents and
ate laws that tend to be congruent with the eco- lifestyles affecting well-being (Vansteenkiste et al.,
nomic and/or political systems and that either 2008). Subsequent research was designed to test the
constrain or support the growth of competencies, existence and operation of these needs, and numer-
the abilities of citizens to affiliate and connect, and ous studies have shown, for example, that across cul-
the exercise of people’s autonomy, and in so doing tures (e.g., Chirkov et al., 2003), ages (e.g., Kasser
the laws affect the wellness and effectiveness of the & Ryan, 1999), and socioeconomic levels (Williams,
citizens. McGregor, Sharp, Levesque, et al., 2006) people who
The social contexts at each of these levels of experience greater satisfaction of the basic psycholog-
analysis can be examined with SDT concepts to ical needs also display greater psychological health.
investigate their effects on the behaviors, thoughts, In what follows, we first discuss the nature of
feelings, and well-being of the people within those autonomous and controlled motivation, focusing
contexts. Throughout this chapter we will review on differences in processes and outcomes that follow
research examining the effects of different levels of from the different kinds of motivation, reviewing
social contexts on individuals’ motivation, integrity, studies that have confirmed these differences across
and psychological health. many life domains—including home, school, work,
leisure, and health care—in multiple developmen-
Contexts and Basic Psychological Needs tal periods and cultural contexts. We begin with a
According to SDT, social contexts, whatever focus on the more state-like or domain-specific
their level, have their impact on individuals by experiences of autonomy and control, and we move
facilitating versus impairing satisfaction of basic on to the more enduring individual differences in
psychological needs. We define needs as organismic autonomy and control that are termed causality
necessities for health. Psychological needs are a orientations (Deci & Ryan, 1985). We then turn to
subset of these necessities that are essential for psy- different types of life goals or aspirations that have
chological growth, integrity, and wellness. We have distinct motivational and well-being consequences.
posited that people require three specific psycho- We also discuss social-contextual conditions at vari-
logical nutriments for healthy functioning: They ous levels as they affect the development and func-
need to feel competent in negotiating their external tioning of autonomous and controlled motivations
and internal environments; they need to experience and of intrinsic and extrinsic life goals. Finally, we
relatedness to other people and groups; and they need review studies that have assessed satisfaction of the
to feel autonomy or self-determination with respect basic psychological needs as mediators between
to their own behaviors and lives. To the extent that social contextual conditions and various outcomes
these needs are satisfied, people will develop health- such as performance and wellness, and we consider
ily and thrive, but to the extent that the needs are the relations of need satisfaction to motivations and
not satisfied, people will experience various psycho- goals.
logical detriments. Social contexts at each level of
proximity vary considerably in the degree to which Autonomous and Controlled Motivation
they facilitate versus impair satisfaction of the basic SDT has two important meta-theoretical
psychological needs, and SDT is concerned in part assumptions concerning the nature of people that
with an examination of the factors within contexts have played an important role in the theory’s devel-
that impact the degree to which basic psychological opment. First, people are assumed to be inherently
need satisfactions are afforded or frustrated. active and thus to proactively initiate engagement
It is worth noting that these three needs—for com- with their environments. Intrinsic motivation is the
petence, relatedness, and autonomy—were not sim- energizing basis for this activity. Second, people
ply assumed or formulated based on casual theorizing are assumed to have an evolved developmental

d ec i , rya n 
tendency toward integration and organization of people will often be eagerly engaged in activities
psychic material. This process includes taking in because they enjoy the process of accomplishing
or internalizing various types of information from some task or goal. At work or in sport, for example,
the external world (e.g., values, attitudes, contin- people may be very immersed in doing a task well
gencies, and knowledge), as well as integrating the and experience deep satisfaction of competence and
regulation of internal forces (e.g., drives and emo- autonomy needs as they do.
tions). These two fundamental assumptions are In contrast to intrinsic motivation, extrinsic moti-
extremely important for our discussion of autono- vation involves doing an activity because it leads to
mous motivation, because autonomous motiva- a separable consequence—the goal is separate from
tion comprises two broad categories of motivation: the activity itself. Carrots (rewards or accolades) and
intrinsic motivation, which is a manifestation of sticks (punishments or threats) are the classic extrin-
our active nature and is the prototype of autono- sic motivators. Extrinsic motivation, when driven
mous motivation; and well-internalized extrinsic by such classic contingencies, is often experienced
motivation, which develops because of the natu- as controlled—that is, people often feel pressured,
ral integrative tendency that is the basis of healthy through the seduction of rewards or the coercion of
development. Thus, nonintrinsic, socially transmit- threats, to do a task. Their behavior tends to become
ted motivations and regulations can become fully dependent on the contingencies, so they do not do
internalized and form the basis for autonomous or the behaviors if the contingencies are not opera-
self-determined extrinsically motivated behavior tive. To the extent that people do feel controlled by
(e.g., Ryan & Deci, 2003). extrinsic motivators, their need for autonomy will
Intrinsic motivation involves doing an activity be thwarted and some negative motivational, per-
because it is interesting and enjoyable. It is often said formance, and well-being consequences are likely to
that when people are intrinsically motivated, doing follow. Extrinsic motivation is not invariantly con-
the activity is its own reward. However, although trolled, however, and to account for this phenom-
it may be heuristically useful to think about it that enon, SDT has differentiated extrinsic motivation
way, a more precise way of defining intrinsic motiva- using the concept of internalization.
tion is in terms of the inherently satisfying internal
conditions that occur when doing an intrinsically Differentiation of Extrinsic Motivation
motivated behavior, thus helping to sustain it. These As noted, the classic example of being extrinsi-
inherent satisfactions (experienced directly as inter- cally motivated is acting in the pursuit of rewards
est and enjoyment) derive primarily from experi- or the avoidance of punishments. Within SDT
ences of competence and autonomy as well as, in we refer to this as external regulation, which is the
some cases, from relatedness. type of regulation emphasized in operant psychol-
One typically delightful example of intrinsically ogy (Skinner, 1953). External regulation is a highly
motivated behavior is children playing. In play, chil- controlling form of motivation. Here the focus is on
dren are often wholly absorbed in activities, experi- contingencies that are controlled by external agents,
encing a sense of interest and joy as they manipulate along with the resulting outcomes. However, Ryan,
objects and explore their environments. As this Connell, and Deci (1985) argued that people have
occurs, their basic psychological needs for compe- an inclination, as part of the inherent integrative
tence and autonomy are likely being met, as they process, to internalize the regulation of behaviors
self-organize their actions and experience effectance. that are valued by important others in their envi-
And through play, the children are learning. With ronments. Parents may convey to their children
adults as well, learning can be intrinsically moti- that they value an activity by doing it themselves,
vated; people sometimes learn simply because they and the children may thus internalize the value of
find the material or activity interesting. This is espe- the behavior. Ryan and colleagues further argued
cially important because studies have shown that, that although internalization is typically treated as
when children (Grolnick & Ryan, 1987) or college a dichotomous concept—that is, as being either
students (Benware & Deci, 1984) are intrinsically external to the person or internal (Sears, Maccoby,
motivated to learn, their learning tends to be deeper & Levin, 1957)—it is useful to recognize that val-
and more conceptual, and they tend to remember ues or behavioral regulations can be internalized to
it longer, than when the learning is extrinsically differing degrees.
motivated by grades or rewards. Similarly, accom- Specifically, Ryan et al. described three different
plishment can be intrinsically motivated; that is, types of internalization that differ in the degree to

 an overview of self-determination theory


which they represent full internalization and thus We have now specified three subtypes of autono-
the degree of autonomy of the resulting behaviors. mous motivation: identified and integrated forms
The authors used the term introjection (e.g., Perls, of extrinsic motivation, along with intrinsic motiva-
1973) to refer to the least autonomous form of tion. Typically, behaviors that are initially extrinsi-
internal regulation. Here, people have taken in a cally motivated are not transformed into intrinsically
behavioral regulation, but it has retained more or motivated behaviors, because they retain their
less the same form it was in when it was still exter- instrumental focus; however, some behaviors are
nal. For example, parents may convey to their chil- motivated by both intrinsic and extrinsic elements.
dren that they will give them more rewards if the These extrinsic motivations may begin as external or
children do well in school and will reward them less, introjected regulations and be transformed into the
or punish them if they do not do well (Grolnick, more autonomous types of extrinsic motivation—
2003). Introjection of these contingencies would namely, identified and integrated regulation—al-
involve the children esteeming themselves to the though they may be retained in a quite controlling
degree that they do well in school. In short, their form. Intrinsic motivation and integrated extrin-
self-esteem would be contingent on doing their sic motivation share various characteristics, such
schoolwork well (Assor, Roth & Deci, 2004; Deci as flexibility and volitional engagement, but they
& Ryan, 1995). If they did not do well, they would are different because intrinsic motivation refers to
feel unworthy and would have a general sense of doing the behavior because it is interesting and
being disapproved of by others (Roth, Assor, Nie- enjoyable in its own right, whereas integrated regu-
miec, Ryan, & Deci, 2009). Although introjected lation refers to doing the behavior because it is per-
regulations are “internal” or intrapersonal, they are sonally, though instrumentally, important, valued,
nonetheless controlling in nature, as the individu- and meaningful for the person. Furthermore, stud-
als are being controlled by these contingencies of ies have shown that, whereas intrinsic motivation
self-worth, which results in negative well-being tends to be the better predictor of being engrossed
consequences (Kernis & Paradise, 2002). Moreover, in an activity, identified and integrated regulations
because the values enforced by these contingencies tend to be better predictors of doing more effortful
are only partially internalized, people typically do tasks that require discipline (e.g., Burton, Lydon,
not feel fully volitional when enacting them so the D’Alessandro, & Koestner, 2006; Koestner, Losier,
behaviors are motivationally unstable and usually Vallerand, & Carducci, 1996).
are either weakly related or unrelated to long-term Having specified different types of motivation,
commitment or performance (Deci & Ryan, 2000). it is important to highlight that any given behavior
Accordingly, external and introjected regulations can be energized by more than one of these moti-
are considered to be the two subtypes of controlled vations. For example, one might be both identified
extrinsic motivation. with the value of an action and also introjected con-
The regulation of extrinsically motivated behav- cerning one’s performance at it. One might pursue
iors is more autonomous when the individuals an activity that one identifies with and also feel
understand and accept the real importance of the intrinsic motivation while enacting at least parts of
activity for themselves. We refer to this type of regu- it. In other words, types of regulation can co-occur,
lation as identified regulation because the individuals and thus within SDT we often look at the overall
have identified with the value of the behavior for relative autonomy of a person’s actions, using pro-
themselves. The experience of identified regulation cedures that aggregate these multiple motives (see
is thus distinct from the experience of introjected Ryan & Connell, 1989).
regulation, and the two have different correlates. For
example, research has shown when the regulation of Outcomes Associated With Autonomous
religious behaviors is introjected, such behaviors are Motivation
negatively associated with well-being, whereas when Ryan, Connell, and Plant (1990) found that
the regulation is identified, religious behaviors are students’ intrinsic motivation for a learning task
positively associated with well-being (Ryan, Rigby, positively predicted their learning and recall both
& King, 1993). Finally, when an identification has immediately following the reading and a week later.
become congruent with other identifications, needs, Wang (2008) found similar results among Chinese
and experiences, the resulting regulation is referred college students in that those who were more intrin-
to as integrated regulation, which represents the most sically motivated performed better on the final
highly autonomous form of extrinsic motivation. exam in the course, thus confirming the relation of

d ec i , rya n 
intrinsic motivation to learning in an Eastern 2008). Stated differently, autonomy facilitates inte-
culture. Boiché, Sarrazin, Grouzet, and Pelletier grated action, need fulfillment, and wellness.
(2008) assessed autonomous motivation (both To summarize, we have reviewed just a few of the
intrinsic and identified) of high school physical hundreds of studies, done in multiple domains and
education students and found that students who cultures and with participants of varied ages, that
were more autonomously motivated performed have shown that more autonomous motivations
better in the course activities than those who were are more effective than controlled motivations with
lower on autonomous motivation. Lévesque, Zue- respect to learning, performing effectively, behaving
hlke, Stanek, and Ryan (2004) found that autono- in healthier ways, and other important outcomes.
mous motivation (as well as perceived competence)
in both German and American college students Causality Orientations
positively predicted their well-being. Pelletiere, For- The autonomous and controlled motivations
tier, Vallerand, and Brière (2001) found that elite being addressed thus far have been either (1)
swimmers who were more autonomously motivated state-level motivations that people experience at
persisted at their sport longer than those who were a particular time and that might be prompted by
more controlled in their motivation. a particular situation, or (2) domain-specific moti-
Studies have also shown that when people are vations—motivations in school, at home, or at
more autonomously motivated for changing their work, for example—that are somewhat more stable
health-compromising behaviors—for example, than state motivations but apply just to specific
stopping smoking, eating a healthier diet, or exer- areas of life. Yet autonomous functioning can also
cising more regularly—they are more successful be studied at a more global, or individual difference,
in changing such behaviors and maintaining the level, as specified within Vallerand’s (1997) hierar-
changes over time. Such findings have been verified chical model of motivation. Individual differences
in multiple ways, including through physiological (i.e., between-person differences) in personality can
indicators such as decreases in glycosylated hemo- have influences across domains and over time. We
globin and LDL cholesterol (e.g., Williams, Freed- refer to this level of analysis of motivational types as
man, & Deci, 1998; Williams, McGregor, Sharp, causality orientations, and SDT specifies three such
Kouides, et al., 2006). In a similar vein, studies have orientations—autonomous, controlled, and imper-
shown that when clients are more autonomous in sonal orientations—and maintains that all people
their motivation for participating in psychotherapy, have each orientation to some degree.
they experience more successful outcomes, such as The autonomy orientation is defined as the degree
decreased depression (Pelletier, Tuson, & Haddad, to which people tend to be generally autonomous
1997; Zuroff et al., 2007). Other research has shown and also to interpret the environment as both being
that people who are more autonomously motivated supportive of their autonomy and providing infor-
behave in healthier ways, such as consuming alcohol mation relevant to choices they are making. When
responsibly (Pavey & Sparks, 2009). autonomy oriented, people regulate behavior on the
Research has further shown that autonomous basis of interests and abiding values. The controlled
motivation promotes not only behaviors that are orientation indexes the level to which people are
personally healthy, but it also leads to behaviors that controlled across domains of their lives and inter-
promote well-being of the collective. For example, pret environments as being pressuring and coer-
Séguin, Pelletier, and Hunsley (1999) found that cive. When control oriented, people are focused on
people who were more autonomously motivated to rewards or punishments, both tangible and social, in
engage in pro-environmental behaviors sought out the regulation of behavior. The impersonal orienta-
more information about environmental health risks tion refers to a general sense of not being intentional
and acted more pro-environmentally. or motivated and of seeing the environment as pro-
Furthermore, across these and other domains, viding obstacles to getting desired outcomes. When
research suggests that people who are more autono- impersonally oriented, people feel little agency, and
mously motivated display greater psychological often fail to regulate their behavior effectively.
wellness (e.g., Ryan et al., 1993; Sheldon, Ryan, Considerable research has shown that the auton-
Deci, & Kasser, 2004). This is important because omy orientation is positively associated with self-
SDT assumes that when afforded autonomy people actualization, self-esteem, ego development, and
are more apt to behave in ways that further their the tendency to support autonomy in others; it is
own capabilities and thriving (Vansteenkiste et al., also negatively associated with many indicators of

 an overview of self-determination theory


ill-being. The controlled orientation is associated at particular times. This happens in part because
with such characteristics as public self-consciousness factors in the environment prime (i.e., noncon-
and the Type-A coronary-prone behavior pattern. sciously prompt) specific orientations regardless of
The impersonal orientation is correlated with vari- the ongoing levels in the strengths of the three ori-
ables such as self-derogation, social anxiety, external entations (e.g., Friedman, Deci, Elliot, Moller, &
locus of control, and depressive symptoms. Aarts, 2010; Hodgins, Yacko, & Gottlieb, 2006).
Williams, Grow, Freedman, Ryan, and Deci In the Weinstein et al. (2011) research mentioned
(1996) found that morbidly obese patients who earlier, some of the studies assessed causality ori-
were in a medically supervised dietary program were entations with the self-report measure, but others
more likely to lose weight and keep it off for 2 years primed the autonomous and controlled orienta-
if they were high rather than low on the autonomy tions in people randomly assigned to conditions,
orientation. Baard, Deci, and Ryan (2004) found thus making the primed orientations more salient.
that employees of a banking firm who were high The researchers found that those primed to be
rather than low on the autonomy orientation autonomous integrated both positive and negative
experienced greater need satisfaction at work and past identities, whereas those primed to be con-
received better performance evaluations from their trolled integrated positive past identities but not
managers. Knee, Patrick, Vietor, Nanayakkara, and negative ones, thus paralleling exactly the findings
Neighbors (2002) found in a laboratory study of by these authors in which individual differences
romantic partners that individuals who were high in causality orientations (assessed with a question-
on the autonomy orientation displayed fewer nega- naire) were used as predictors of integrating past
tive emotions, more positive behaviors, and more identities. Similarly, Hodgins et al. (2006) primed
relationship-maintaining coping strategies; whereas causality orientations in college students and found
those high on the controlled orientation were more that autonomy-primed individuals displayed lower
negative and wanted their partners to be more like use of the defenses such as self-serving bias and self-
themselves. handicapping, whereas control-primed individuals
Weinstein, Deci, and Ryan (2011) found that were higher in self-serving responding and self-
people high in the autonomy orientation inte- handicapping, results that paralleled those found
grated both positive and negative past identities in studies by Knee and Zuckerman (1996, 1998)
into their current sense of self, whereas those high who had used causality orientations assessed as indi-
on the controlled orientation accepted their past vidual differences.
positive, but rejected their past negative, identi- To summarize, research on causality orientations
ties. These findings were mediated by defensiveness, has indicated that being more autonomy oriented
with people high in the controlled orientation also has far more positive outcomes for effective perfor-
being higher in defense. The results followed up on mance and psychological health than being high on
a study by Hodgins, Koestner, and Duncan (1996), the other two orientations. Furthermore, studies
which found that college students who were high have shown that subliminal prompts can prime cau-
in autonomy were less defensive (i.e., more honest sality orientations and produce effects that parallel
and disclosing) with their parents, reported more those that are predicted by self-reported individual
pleasant affect, and felt better about themselves in differences in causality orientations.
those interactions than were students high in the
controlled orientation. Other studies by Hodgins Intrinsic and Extrinsic Life Goals
and colleagues (Hodgins & Liebeskind, 2003; Hod- Although SDT focuses primarily on the “why”
gins, Liebeskind, & Schwartz, 1996) revealed that of people’s goals and behaviors (i.e., on autono-
people higher in autonomy, relative to the other ori- mous versus controlled motivations), we have also
entations, used fewer lies in explaining wrongdoings studied the contents of people’s goals, or the “what”
and provided more apologies when they had caused of behavior. A central notion is that, because the
harm to others. effects of any behavior on wellness is mediated by
As noted, the SDT perspective maintains that all basic psychological need satisfactions, “not all goals
people have some level of all three causality orienta- are created equal” (Ryan, Sheldon, Kasser, & Deci,
tions. One orientation may be much stronger than 1996), because some goals are more directly satisfy-
the others and thus, on average, be the dominant ing of basic needs and some are less satisfying or
personality-level influence on a person’s function- even thwarting of basic need satisfaction, and thus
ing; however, other orientations may be dominant have different effects on psychological wellness.

d ec i , rya n 
Pursuing Intrinsic and Extrinsic Aspirations The SDT interpretation of the results for aspi-
Especially studied in this regard are aspirations or rations is that the intrinsic goals are quite directly
life goals that people value, pursue, and sometimes related to satisfaction of the basic psychologi-
attain. Kasser and Ryan (1996) found that, when cal needs. Personal growth, for example, is closely
participants reported how much importance they related to becoming more integrated and autono-
placed on a variety of life goals, the goals separated mous, as well as somewhat more competent and,
into two factors that the investigators referred to as most likely, more related to others because personal
extrinsic and intrinsic aspirations or life goals. The growth tends to make satisfying relationships easier.
extrinsic aspirations that have been studied most Furthermore, both meaningful relationships and
are accumulating wealth, becoming famous, or hav- community involvement are strongly tied to satis-
ing an attractive image. The intrinsic ones that have faction of the relatedness need and they are likely
gotten considerable empirical attention are personal to relate to people feeling more autonomous and
growth, developing meaningful relationships, con- competent to the extent that the goals are pursued
tributing to the community, and being physically volitionally (e.g., see Weinstein & Ryan, 2010). In
fit and healthy. contrast, the extrinsic aspirations are typically at
Much of the research has examined the associa- best only indirectly related to basic need satisfac-
tion between the relative strength of the extrinsic tion, and they may in many cases be antagonistic
versus intrinsic life goal pursuits and their relations to satisfaction of the basic needs. For example, the
to various indicators of psychological health and pursuit of wealth is likely to leave people feeling less
well-being. Consistently, the studies have shown autonomous, as acting in the service of monetary
that when people’s aspirations for pursuing extrinsic rewards has been shown to undermine autonomy,
outcomes are relatively stronger than their aspira- and, furthermore, the time devoted to the pursuit
tions for pursuing intrinsic outcomes, individuals of wealth is likely to interfere with relatedness sat-
tend to have lower self-esteem and self-actualization, isfaction. Similar kinds of arguments can be made
as well as higher depression, anxiety, narcissism, and for fame and image when they are highly valued as
Machiavellianism, among other outcomes (e.g., life goals.
Kasser & Ryan, 1993; McHoskey, 1999). They also
engage more in high risk behaviors (Williams, Cox, Attaining Intrinsic and Extrinsic
Hedberg, & Deci, 2000). Aspirations
Some commentators (e.g., Carver & Baird, 1998) Recently, Niemiec, Ryan, and Deci (2009)
have suggested that the reason pursuing extrinsic goals examined how the attainment (rather than the pur-
is related to poorer psychological health than is pursu- suit) of intrinsic versus extrinsic aspirations contrib-
ing intrinsic goals is that extrinsic goals are likely to be utes to wellness versus distress in early adulthood.
pursued for controlled motives, whereas intrinsic goals The study followed young adults beginning 1 year
are likely to be pursued for autonomous motives. In after they had graduated from college (Time 1) and
other words, they argued that the problem is not in ending 1 year after that (Time 2). At the beginning
“what” was being pursued (the extrinsic goal) but in and end of that year four important concepts were
“why” it was being pursued (the controlled motive). assessed: (1) the personal importance of intrinsic
Sheldon et al. (2004) tested this reasoning in three and extrinsic goals, (2) the level of attainment of
studies. They found that there was indeed a correla- intrinsic and extrinsic goals, (3) the degree of sat-
tion between the “what” and the “why” of behavior— isfaction of the basic psychological needs, and (4)
people more oriented toward extrinsic goals did tend indicators of both well-being and ill-being. Results
to be more controlled and those oriented toward showed first that the importance people place on
the intrinsic goals did tend to be more autonomous. goals at Time 1 strongly predicted attainment of
However, when both the goals and the motives were those goals at Time 2, and this was true for both
entered simultaneously into regression analyses to intrinsic and extrinsic goals. People tended to
predict well-being, results showed that both the what attain that which they considered important. Sec-
and the why accounted for independent variance. ond, increases in the attainment of intrinsic goals
That is, what people pursue and why they pursue it over the year related to increases in well-being and
both make a significant difference in their psycho- to decreases in ill-being over that period. In con-
logical well-being. In short, being controlled in one’s trast, increases in attainment of extrinsic goals did
motivation and pursuing extrinsic aspirations are not predict increases in well-being but did predict
both negative predictors of well-being. symptoms of ill-being. Finally, the research showed

 an overview of self-determination theory


that the relations between changes in intrinsic goal associated with less well-being and greater ill-being.
attainment and changes in well-being were medi- These relations tend to be mediated by satisfaction
ated by corresponding changes in satisfaction of the versus thwarting of the basic psychological needs,
basic psychological needs (Niemiec et al., 2009). such that intrinsic goals tend to have a direct rela-
This study seems to be a warning: “Be careful what tion to satisfaction of the basic psychological needs,
you wish for” because people tend to attain the goals whereas extrinsic goals tend to be either indirectly
they value, but the consequences of doing so may be related to or antagonistic to satisfaction of the basic
negative for some of the goals. needs. Finally, these intrinsic and extrinsic aspira-
tions can also be manipulated by or primed within
Manipulating Goal Orientations social contexts, such that, when extrinsic goals are
Thus far we have treated aspirations as indi- made salient, performance and well-being tend to be
vidual differences that are learned as a function of worse, whereas when intrinsic goals are made salient
satisfaction versus thwarting of the basic psycho- performance and well-being tend to be better. We
logical needs during development, and the pri- turn now to a consideration of how social contexts
mary outcomes in the research have generally been facilitate autonomous versus controlled motivations
well-being indicators. In another important strand and intrinsic versus extrinsic aspirations.
of this research the salience of people’s goals has
been experimentally manipulated. Vansteenkiste, Effects of Social Contexts on Motivation,
Simons, Lens, Sheldon, and Deci (2004) did two Life Goals, Behavior, and Well-Being
studies of college students and one of younger, Autonomous motivation, intrinsic aspirations,
physical education students in which all the stu- effective functioning, and well-being are theorized
dents were engaged in learning activities and the to be facilitated both developmentally and situ-
introduction they received to the task oriented ationally by social contexts. As we noted earlier,
them toward viewing the task as leading to either facilitators of (and obstacles to) optimal function-
an intrinsic goal or an extrinsic goal. For example, ing, of which autonomous motivation and intrinsic
business students were given material to learn about aspirations are a central components, are conceptu-
communication processes; half were told the learn- alized within SDT in terms of supports (or thwarts)
ing would help them understand themselves better for satisfaction of the basic psychological needs for
(i.e., an intrinsic goal of personal growth) and the competence, autonomy, and relatedness.
other half were told that the learning would help
them earn more money in their business careers Specific Contextual Factors and
(i.e., an extrinsic goal of wealth). Results indi- Autonomous Motivation
cated that those who had been oriented toward Some of the studies of social-contextual effects
the intrinsic goal learned the material better than on motivation-related outcomes have been experi-
those who had been oriented toward the extrinsic ments examining specific factors such as the offer
goal. Furthermore, 5 days later, when making a of rewards, provision of choice, imposition of dead-
presentation about the material, the students who lines, or introduction of competition as they affect
had learned with the intrinsic goal set were rated autonomous motivation, and many of these have
as having given better presentations. Additionally, used intrinsic motivation as the dependent variable.
those who had been given the intrinsic goal orien- For example, nearly 100 experiments have investi-
tation spent more subsequent time exploring the gated whether tangible-reward contingencies tend
topic by engaging in voluntary activities related to to promote versus diminish intrinsic motivation,
the learning. and the results confirmed that the use of the most
common reward contingencies tend to decrease the
Summary of Life Goals Research experience of autonomy and promote controlled
Research shows that many life goals can be motivation (Deci et al., 1999). These authors
grouped into two categories: intrinsic aspirations interpreted this as indicating that, on average, the
such as growth, relationships, and community, and rewards thwart satisfaction of the need for auton-
extrinsic aspirations such as wealth, fame, and image. omy while prompting a shift in the perceived locus
Numerous studies further indicate that pursuit and of causality from internal to external (de Charms,
attainment of the intrinsic aspirations is associated 1968). Rewards have frequently been used to con-
with greater well-being and less ill-being, whereas trol people’s behavior, so the rewards have tended
the pursuit and attainment of extrinsic aspirations is to take on a controlling functional significance and

d ec i , rya n 
to end up thwarting autonomy and undermining the individuals are encouraged to experiment and
intrinsic motivation. are provided some choice; and the use of control-
Other research revealed that threats of punish- ling language and contingencies is minimized. In
ment, deadlines, evaluations, surveillance, and contrast, controlling contexts are ones that pres-
pressured competition also decreased intrinsic sure people to think, feel, or behave in specific ways
motivation because they too are often experienced (Deci & Ryan, 2000) through the use of coercive
as thwarting the autonomy need (see Ryan & Deci, or seductive pressures and demands. The concept of
2000a for a review). Like rewards, these other an autonomy-supportive versus controlling climate
specific aspects of social environments tend to be has been assessed with several methods, including
experienced as pressuring rather than supportive. In self-reports from the authority figures in the situ-
contrast, offering choice and acknowledging people’s ations (e.g., teachers, managers, parents, coaches,
feelings enhanced intrinsic motivation and facili- physicians, or therapists); reported perceptions of
tated fuller internalization because they prompted the authorities’ autonomy supportiveness from
an internal perceived locus of causality and satisfied people for whom the authority is responsible; inter-
people’s need for autonomy (e.g., Deci, Eghrari, views with the authorities that are rated or coded for
Patrick, & Leone, 1994; Koestner, Ryan, Bernieri, autonomy support; and direct observations that are,
& Holt, 1984; Oliver, Markland, Hardy, & Pether- similarly, rated or coded.
ick, 2008; Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008). As mentioned earlier, autonomy-supportive con-
Experiments have also examined the effects of texts of course support satisfaction of the autonomy
feedback on autonomous motivation. In general, need. It turns out, furthermore, that these contexts
these studies indicate that positive feedback tends to also tend, to some degree, to support the other basic
enhance intrinsic motivation and facilitate internal- psychological needs—the needs for relatedness
ization, because such feedback provides satisfaction and competence. First, when an authority takes
of the competence need and may also support auton- another’s perspective, the other typically feels like
omy. That is, when people get positive feedback that he or she has been related to in a genuine way, thus
is authentic, they are likely to infer that they are providing support for relatedness (e.g., La Guardia,
responsible for their good performance, thus expe- Ryan, Couchman, & Deci, 2000). In addition,
riencing autonomy as well as competence satisfac- because autonomy support includes perspective tak-
tions. Situations that provide positive feedback and ing, autonomy-supportive authorities will be more
are accompanied by some support for autonomy are mindful of obstacles to satisfaction of the people’s
referred to as informational, and they have consis- other needs—that is, of frustrations to competence
tently been shown to enhance autonomous motiva- and relatedness needs. Finally, when people are in
tion (see Deci & Ryan, 2000). Yet when positive situations where their autonomy is supported, they
feedback is given in a controlling way—for exam- are likely to feel freer to do what is necessary to get
ple, in a form such as, “Good, you did just as you their other needs satisfied.
should”—it tends to be detrimental to autonomous
motivation and to shift the perceived locus of cau- relational supports and structure
sality toward external (Ryan, 1982). When feedback It is important to note that there are factors other
is negative, the message tends to convey “incompe- than autonomy support that specifically facilitate
tence” and decreases autonomous motivation. If the satisfaction of the basic needs for relatedness and
negative feedback is persistent, and especially if it is competence. For example, direct expressions of car-
demeaning, it will tend to result in amotivation. ing, time spent together mutually sharing feelings,
and involvement of one person in the life of another
Autonomy-Supportive and Controlling are examples of factors likely to promote satisfac-
Climates tion of the relatedness need (e.g., Grolnick, Benjet,
Other studies have examined autonomy- Kurowski, & Apostoleris, 1997). Furthermore, pro-
supportive versus controlling social environments as viding noncontrolling structure and informational
concepts that capture the quality of an interpersonal feedback are factors likely to promote satisfaction
climate or the ambience of a situation—be it, say, a of the competence need (e.g., Jang, Reeve, & Deci,
home, classroom, workgroup, or clinic. Autonomy- 2010; Ryan, 1982). In other words, research in SDT
supportive environments are ones in which the suggests specific nutriments that can enhance each
perspectives of individuals in that environment are of the basic need satisfactions, beyond the general
acknowledged (typically by an authority figure); facilitating impact of autonomy support.

 an overview of self-determination theory


studies of social contexts in evaluations than employees who perceived their
various domains managers as more controlling.
Many studies conducted over the past 30 In health care settings numerous studies have
years have examined the relations of autonomy- shown that patients who perceive their practitio-
supportive contexts to motivation and other out- ners as more autonomy supportive tended to make
comes, beginning with Deci, Schwartz, Sheinman, greater improvements in health behaviors and out-
and Ryan (1981). These investigators had elemen- comes. For example, when practitioners (e.g., physi-
tary school teachers report on the degree of their cians, nurses, health counselors, dental professionals)
own autonomy-supportive versus controlling class- were perceived as more autonomy supportive, their
room styles, and these teacher self-reports were patients were more likely to quit smoking (e.g., Wil-
then related to the students’ experiences of intrin- liams, McGregor, Sharp, Levesque, et al., 2006); to
sic motivation, perceived competence for school, more effectively regulate their glucose levels (e.g.,
and self-esteem. It was found that when teachers Williams, McGregor, Zeldman, Freedman, & Deci,
were more autonomy supportive, their students 2004); to achieve and maintain more weight loss
became more intrinsically motivated and perceived (Williams et al., 1996); and to attend dental clin-
themselves more positively by the end of the first ics more regularly (Münster Halvari, Halvari,
2 months of the school year. Ryan and Connell Bjørnebekk, & Deci, 2010). Philippe and Vallerand
(1989) found that when elementary school teach- (2008) found that when nursing home staff were
ers were perceived as more autonomy supportive, more autonomy supportive, residents both reported
their students showed greater internalization of more autonomy and displayed greater well-being.
achievement-related values. Black and Deci (2000)
found that college students taking organic chemis- Mutual Autonomy Support in Peer
try from instructors who were more autonomy sup- Relationships
portive became more autonomous and got higher In friendships and romantic relationships auton-
grades in the course, after controlling for SAT scores omy support also matters. Here, however, the situ-
and grade point averages. ation is a bit different. In each of the relationships
Grolnick and Ryan (1989) in an interview study discussed earlier there was an authority differen-
of the parents of elementary school students found tial between the two people in the relationship—
that parents who were rated by interviewers as teachers-students, managers-employees, and
more autonomy supportive had children who were coaches-athletes, for example. With friends, rela-
more autonomously motivated to do schoolwork, tionships are typically more mutual. A study of
were rated by their teachers as more competent and friends by Deci, La Guardia, Moller, Scheiner, and
better behaved, and got better grades. Landry et al. Ryan (2006) found that the mutuality of autonomy
(2008) found that when mothers trusted in the nat- support was indeed advantageous. Each relational
ural developmental process, they were more auton- partner benefited in terms of well-being not only
omy supportive, and both the mothers and children when receiving autonomy support from his or her
evidenced more positive adaptation. Gagné, Ryan, partner but also when giving autonomy support
and Bargmann (2003) found that when coaches to the partner. Other studies of intimate relation-
were more autonomy supportive, gymnasts evi- ships (Knee et al., 2002; La Guardia & Patrick,
denced greater vitality, autonomous motivation, 2008) similarly attest to the impact of need-related
and well-being. These and many other similar stud- supports in close relationships for enhancing ver-
ies have shown the pervasive effects of support for sus debilitating people’s healthy functioning and
autonomy across youth development. wellness.
Similar evidence is found in the workplace (see Other research has shown positive effects on
Gagné & Deci, 2005). For example, managers of a well-being of both giving and receiving help even
Fortune 500 company who were more autonomy to a stranger, assuming that the helping was autono-
supportive had employees who were more satisfied mously done rather than being controlled. Weinstein
with their jobs and more trusting of the company’s and Ryan (2010) found in a series of diary studies
top management (Deci, Connell, & Ryan, 1989). and experiments that the more autonomously moti-
Baard et al. (2004) found that banking industry vated an individual was to provide help to another,
employees who perceived their managers as more the more the helping predicted well-being outcomes
autonomy supportive displayed better psychologi- not only in the helper but also in the recipients of
cal well-being and received higher performance that help. In short, it seems that giving to and caring

d ec i , rya n 
for others can be very positive for both the giver and perpetuating a cyclical negative dynamic of need
the receiver so long as the giver is autonomous in his thwarting, causing a stronger extrinsic orientation
or her actions. and that in turn causing greater need thwarting.

Contextual Effect on Intrinsic and More Distal and Pervasive Influences


Extrinsic Aspirations Certainly proximal social contexts—our imme-
Studies have examined the effects of social con- diate interpersonal worlds—typically provide the
texts not only on autonomous and controlled moti- most phenomenally salient experiences of auton-
vation but also on the development of intrinsic and omy support or control both situationally and
extrinsic life goals or aspirations. We saw earlier developmentally. But from an SDT perspective the
that having a strong extrinsic life-goal orientation cultural, economic, and political contexts within
tends to thwart basic psychological need satisfac- which people live have overarching and pervasive,
tion, resulting in more negative outcomes, such as yet often hidden, roles in supporting or thwarting
increased ill-being. The SDT perspective suggests the fulfillment of their basic needs. These distal con-
that it is also the case that the development of a strong texts both set horizons on people’s possibilities and
extrinsic life-goal orientation tends to result from also introduce norms, constraints, and policies that
thwarting of basic psychological need satisfaction either facilitate or diminish need fulfillments.
over time. This thwarting creates a sense of anxiety
and inadequacy that leads to the pursuit of exter- Cultural Values
nal indicators of worth in order to make up for the autonomous motivation and wellness
lack of inner feelings of worth. For example, Kasser, Chirkov et al. (2003) examined the relations of
Ryan, Zax, and Sameroff (1995) studied the impor- cultural values to the well-being of individuals in
tance late teenagers’ placed on the extrinsic aspira- those cultures. The researchers focused on whether
tion for wealth, relative to intrinsic aspirations for the cultures of South Korea, Russia, Turkey, and
growth, relatedness, and community. They found the United States tended to emphasize individual-
that teenagers who placed the strongest importance ism or collectivism and whether the societies were
on wealth had mothers, according to reports from more horizontal or vertical in their structures. The
both the adolescents and their mothers, who were researchers found first that although the cultures
more authoritarian, controlling, and cold. This differed in terms of the values placed on individual-
early thwarting of children’s basic need satisfaction ism versus collectivism, it was not the values them-
is assumed to have created an inner insecurity in the selves that predicted well-being; it was instead the
children for which they developed strong extrinsic degree to which people had internalized the values
aspirations in an attempt to compensate. that predicted their well-being. In other words,
Similarly, Williams, Cox, Hedberg, and Deci autonomy (resulting from full internalization) was
(1999) found that adolescents who rated their par- important for individuals’ well-being in all these
ents as low in autonomy support placed stronger cultures, regardless of whether the cultures were ori-
importance on the extrinsic relative to intrinsic ented more toward the collective or more toward
aspirations, and this in turn was associated with the individual.
the adolescents engaging in more risky behaviors The study further found that vertical structures
such as early engagement in tobacco use, alcohol were, on average, more difficult to internalize than
consumption, and sexual intercourse. Sheldon and were horizontal structures. We understand this as
Kasser (2008) found that when college students indicating that a vertical or hierarchical system is
experienced psychological threats (including exis- likely (though not invariantly) to be experienced as
tential, financial, and interpersonal threats), they more controlling than a horizontal one and would
tended to become more strongly focused on extrin- thus tend to thwart people’s need for autonomy,
sic life goals. It seems from these various studies that making it more difficult to accept the hierarchical
when young people experience thwarting of their structure as their own. This of course is merely an
basic psychological needs they tend to become more “on average” finding, and it remains for the issue
oriented toward the extrinsic goals in order to com- of hierarchy to be disentangled from the experience
pensate for the inner feelings of anxiety. Unfortu- of autonomy versus control at the cultural level. In
nately, as we saw earlier, becoming more strongly principle, according to SDT, one could congruently
oriented toward extrinsic aspirations in turn causes assent to some hierarchical arrangements with-
greater need thwarting and poorer outcomes, thus out losing a sense of autonomy in following that

 an overview of self-determination theory


arrangement. This kind of integrated identification at least this U.S. cultural subgroup toward more
with a hierarchical structure would be facilitated by distress and lower wellness.
authorities who behave in a need-supporting way
toward the individual or group, making acceptance cultural values and need satisfaction
and assimilation more possible. Together, these findings about cultures and val-
ues are important because they suggest that cultures
culture, aspirations, and well-being play a role in whether the people can experience
The issue of broader contextual effects and well- satisfaction of their basic needs. The results further
being can also be addressed with respect to aspira- suggest that having the need for autonomy satis-
tions or life goal. The goals of wealth, fame, and fied in any culture, regardless of the culture’s val-
image are very central to what we think of as the ues, would contribute to psychological wellness.
“American Dream”—that is, the set of values our Conversely, studies show that people feel more
culture and its economic system tend to promote. estranged from and less accepting of their cultures
Indeed, having people within the culture be ori- to the degree that they are not experiencing basic
ented toward the pursuit of wealth, fame, and need satisfaction (e.g., Chirkov, Ryan, & Willness,
attractiveness is necessary for the culture to grow 2005). Thus, one basis for alienation and instability
(Kasser et al., 2007), so the advertising industry within any culture may be the extent to which the
within America has become enormous, and its pri- culture’s ambient values and behavioral regulations
mary purpose is to get people to buy more goods fail to support or facilitate opportunities for basic
and services that represent external indicators of need satisfaction among its constituents.
worth—namely, appearing attractive, wealthy, and
popular. Of course, doing that requires actually pur- evaluating cultural values and
suing more wealth to pay for the goods and services, practices
thus contributing to economic growth. It is of course treacherous for social scientists to
Our cultural context, emphasizing the American evaluate cultural value systems or practices. Many
Dream, supplies the backdrop in which schools and scholars today in fact ascribe to a cultural relativism
parents are embedded as they influence children’s view, which maintains that it is not appropriate to
motivation. Simply stated, the culture with its capi- evaluate any cultural milieu from the “outside.” In
talist economic system strongly promotes extrinsic this regard, SDT is in a particularly interesting posi-
aspirations, through direct means such as advertis- tion. In our view, need support means supporting
ing and through indirect means such as prompting the self-regulation of individuals so they can have
stress within families and thus creating conditions the freedom to experience and pursue their own pre-
of insecurity that conduce toward extrinsic valuing. ferred values and life projects. In taking this internal
While these psychological conditions may facilitate frame of reference, SDT is therefore not imposing
consumption and spending, they are unlikely to be particular contents, but rather is specifying a crite-
conducive to wellness and high quality of life. rion for evaluating any cultural value or practice. To
Twenge, Gentile, DeWall, Ma, Lacefield, and the extent that the value or practice supports (rather
Schurtz (2010) recently examined cultural trends in than thwarts) satisfaction of the basic human needs
the mental health of U.S. college students over the underlying growth, effective functioning, and well-
past seven decades. They first found a rather discon- ness, the evaluation would be positive.
certing pattern: Over this time U.S. college students
evidenced increases in symptoms of depression, Economic Systems
anxiety, and antisocial tendencies as detected with Just as cultural systems set affordances, con-
equated assessments. Examining numerous factors straints, and boundaries that affect people’s pursuit
that might account for this negative trend toward and attainment of basic psychological need satisfac-
greater distress and psychopathology, they ruled out tions, so too do economic systems. From the way
numerous issues from prosperity to family structure. in which work behavior is managed and regulated,
What they concluded was that the increasing shift to the macro-arrangements responsible for distrib-
toward poorer mental health and psychopathol- uting wealth and caring for citizens, SDT takes
ogy may be “due to an increased focus on money, interest in how various economic structures sup-
appearance, and status rather than on community port or thwart people’s basic psychological needs.
and close relationships” (p. 153). That is, the slide We have already discussed how proximal manage-
toward more extrinsic life goals appears to have led ment styles impact work motivation. Variations

d ec i , rya n 
in macro-structures—from the direct deprivations capitalist system. Perhaps no other economic system
of autonomy and competence often witnessed in in history has prompted so much productivity, both
central planning economies, to the economic and constructive and wasteful.
social oppression that can be experienced within SDT research makes a compelling case for the
market capitalisms also differentially support or significant role played by autonomy in fostering
diminish human wellness. SDT uses the construct both creative development and amplifying “human
of basic psychological needs as mediating factors to capital” within an economic system. Within the
link these distal structures to the experiences and workplace we know, for example, that autonomy
outcomes of particular individuals and communi- maximizes creativity and flexible problem solving
ties. Although we have interest in varied economic (Gagné & Deci, 2005). Similarly, at a systems level
systems (e.g., Deci et al., 2001), in what follows, provision of choice and opportunities to develop
we focus primarily on features found in modern competence (e.g., education, training) allow indi-
corporate capitalism to illustrate how the standard viduals to cultivate and apply more preferred talents
market economy can affect individuals’ motivation and passions, which overall makes the system more
and well-being (e.g., Frey, 1997). effective and generative (e.g., Sen, 1999). Autonomy
Corporate capitalism is most notable in part too is evident in the freedom to pursue innovation
because of its increasing global reach and its capac- and expression, and it is enriched by shared ideas
ity to transform the other cultures it reaches (Kasser and flow of information. Autonomy, that is, is an
et al., 2007). Still, the capitalist system has variations, engine of growth in its own right.
in part because cultures have varied in the degree to Accompanying these advantages, however, SDT
which they have also embraced social-welfare poli- highlights a number of less positive motivational
cies. It is clear, for example, that these additions and implications associated with capitalism. Perhaps
variations to an unbridled or laissez-faire economic most salient is capitalism’s capacity to externally
approach modify and constrain some of capitalism’s regulate people’s behavior. The outcomes of pay and
influences on need satisfactions. other tangible rewards are viewed as the primary
Briefly, capitalism is characterized by private motivators of behavior and are offered with contin-
ownership of capital and sale of goods and services gencies that are either directly (e.g., commissions,
at the highest price attainable. The basic idea is that piece-rate payments, stock-option bonuses) or indi-
people are acting in terms of their “self-interests” in rectly (salaries) related to people’s performance on
a competitive system, so they are “free to choose” the job. As outlined by Deci et al. (1999), the con-
(Friedman & Friedman, 1990) how to act in an tingencies widely used within the capitalist system
assumed attempt to maximize their earnings and are nearly always either engagement-contingent
wealth, whether they own capital or merely work (pay depends on doing the job) or performance-
for others who do. One’s own labor, ideas, time, and contingent (pay depends on the quality of the work
products of effort are thus all commodities that can produced) with most of the people whose pay is
be exchanged or sold to others. From the perspec- performance-contingent not receiving the maxi-
tive of SDT, capitalist systems are complex and have mum amount possible. These contingencies are
elements that both support and thwart autonomy. considered the key motivators of the principal-agent
First, modern corporate capitalism, relative to theory of the modern market economy (Petersen,
other styles of economic organization, provides 1993), and yet they are the ones that have been
a multitude of choices, and, when coupled with found to be most detrimental to human autonomy.
democracies, it also manifests as a relative absence Specifically, research has shown that these reward
of direct external control over lifestyles and voca- contingencies have the negative consequences of
tions. Of course, choices may be highly constrained undermining autonomy and intrinsic motivation
for some individuals or groups within capitalist (Deci et al., 1999) and can lead to poorer quality
societies, especially those without access to educa- performance as reflected in more superficial learn-
tion or resources and who thus have few options to ing, less flexible problem solving and heuristic
develop competencies or the autonomy to pursue processing, and less creativity (e.g., Ariely, Gneezy,
valued ends. Yet the array of opportunities for many Loewenstein, & Mazar, 2009; McGraw & McCull-
and the relative freedom to pursue preferred voca- ers, 1979). Thus, the controlling use of rewards,
tions and projects are in large part responsible for which is common within capitalism, can interfere
the high levels of entrepreneurial activity, produc- with effort and quality of engagement, and, as we
tivity, and creativity evidenced within sectors of the shall see later, can even lead people to distort or

 an overview of self-determination theory


ignore organizational goals. Thus, as explained by is evidenced, and where autonomy is undermined,
Frey (1997), the external intervention of financial alienation, passivity, and gaming the system typi-
incentives can have the positive price effect empha- cally result.
sized by economics but at the same time have the
negative undermining effect emphasized in SDT. High-Stakes Rewards and Sanctions
Furthermore, competition, which is a central An example of traditional “rewards gone wrong”
aspect of capitalism, has also been found on aver- within capitalism is the current use of high-stakes
age within the American culture to be controlling bonuses, rewards, and sanctions to pressure people
and undermining of autonomous motivation, espe- or organizations to attain specific outcomes. From
cially when there is interpersonal pressure to beat the gargantuan financial bonuses and stock options
the opponents (Reeve & Deci, 1996). Additionally, doled out to executives for hitting stock-price tar-
performance evaluations, which are a feature of most gets, to sanctions on schools for not raising test
corporate organizations, have similarly been found scores, “accountability” enforced by high-stakes
to be detrimental to autonomy (e.g., Harackiewicz, contingencies is in fashion. SDT has a particular
Manderlink, & Sansone, 1984). In short, research take on such rewards that, sadly, is showing its valid-
has linked the controlling use of rewards, competi- ity across these multiple settings.
tion, and evaluations to decreases in basic psycholog- Notably, for example, around the globe various
ical need satisfaction and autonomous motivation. nations have applied high-stakes tests to drive higher
Because these are all commonly used motivational achievement in students, with the ultimate aim of
elements in a capitalist system, the economic sys- fostering greater economic productivity. The strat-
tem can be expected to yield negative effects on the egy of placing high-stakes contingencies behind test
autonomous motivation and heuristic information scores is based on an undifferentiated view of moti-
processing of individuals. Furthermore, because the vation that considers external incentives to be the
capitalist system, by its very nature, holds people effective way to foster motivation for all behaviors,
directly accountable for results, it is likely that the from widget making to intellectual creativity. Poli-
system will make the managers who work within it cies such as the Bush-era No Child Left Behind and
more controlling (e.g., Deci et al., 1982), which is the Obama-era Race to the Top have threatened to
likely to represent yet another blow to the autonomy close schools that did not perform sufficiently well
of employees. on specific tests and to reward schools that excelled.
Another feature of capitalism is its explicit sup- Analogous strategies of rewarding states for hold-
port for lifestyles focused on achievement, competi- ing their schools accountable, of paying teachers for
tiveness, consumerism, and material accumulation. their students’ performance on achievement test,
Each of these orientations is fostered through and even of paying students for attending school
embedded contexts, from parental and school ide- or doing well on tests have been increasingly advo-
ologies to national values, that vary both in how cated and, in some places, implemented despite
controllingly they instill values and in the contents well-known evidence of the negative effects of such
of those values. We maintain that capitalism as a reward structures on motivation, learning, and
system both directly and indirectly promotes extrin- persistence.
sic aspirations or life goals that focus on accumu- Accordingly, as SDT predicts, accompanying the
lation, personal gains, and recognition. As such, it increased emphasis on high stakes has been research
is inherently in opposition to goals for community showing an increase in a variety of negative conse-
and thus global welfare (e.g., Kasser & Ryan, 1996). quences that follow from it (see Nichols & Berliner,
Kasser et al. (2007) discuss at length the antipodal 2007, Ryan & Brown, 2005; Ryan & Weinstein,
nature of intrinsic and extrinsic values and their 2009, for reviews). These include increases in stu-
relation to capitalist social climates. dent dropout, failing to categorize all students
In sum, capitalism, like every complex economic who have left school as dropouts, excluding some
system, has structures and features that can either students from taking the high-stakes tests so they
support or thwart the autonomy, competence, and wouldn’t lower the school’s scores, more teaching
relatedness needs of the people within the system. to the tests, more teaching of test-taking strategies,
The vitality and quality of the system’s functioning and less teaching of content. Additionally, there is
reflects its supports for people’s basic needs, includ- evidence that “improvements” on high-stakes test
ing the three psychological needs. For example, scores often do not generalize to independent indi-
where features enhance autonomy, more thriving ces of achievement (e.g., the National Assessment

d ec i , rya n 
of Educational Progress exams) that do not have intrinsic values, that they support interventions that
teacher- or school-level stakes associated with them. are likely to fail or backfire. Beyond obvious calcu-
This simply highlights that a “teach to the test” cul- lations of incentive or loss, external contingencies
ture has been realized and appears not to be improv- impact outcomes in part by thwarting versus sat-
ing achievement. isfying people’s basic psychological needs. To make
Erroneously, backers of high stakes in education effective predictions outside of formal exchange
and elsewhere often suggest that their strategies are situations requires a focus on human psychologi-
supported by behaviorist (i.e., operant) principles cal needs and on the conditions that support versus
(Skinner, 1953). But in fact operant theory advo- undermine them. That focus provides SDT with a
cates rewarding behaviors and not outcomes (see critical lens through which to view both micro- and
Ryan & Brown, 2005; Ryan & Weinstein, 2009). In macro-economic factors.
contrast, SDT predicts that making rewards contin-
gent on outcomes typically has the functional effect Political Systems
of reinforcing any route to the rewarded end (e.g., In addition to an economic system, each coun-
Shapira, 1976). In addition, because the approach try has a political system that tends toward either
is controlling in nature, such rewards inspire a totalitarianism or democracy. Totalitarian systems
shortest-route mentality. This contamination effect are centered on individual dictators who hold abso-
is manifest in counterproductive activities intended lute power over the lives of their citizens. Through
to increase reward attainment, including “gaming” the use of propaganda via state-controlled media
outcome data, sacrificing long-term organizational and the support from the military, most aspects of
goals to reach short-term targets, and even out- citizens’ lives are subjugated to the dictates of gov-
right cheating. SDT attributes such contamination ernment. Typically, totalitarianism is accompanied
effects to the controlling nature of outcome-focused by central planning economies, although modified
rewards, and thus has anticipated many of the unin- totalitarian systems may have some degree of capital-
tended negative results of high-stakes approaches, ism. Totalitarianism is straightforwardly antagonis-
including how they undermine intrinsic motivation tic to satisfaction of the basic psychological needs.
and internalization of values in students and teach- There is little opportunity for autonomy, except per-
ers alike, and drive out best practices. haps if one is working under the political umbrella
As we noted, high-stakes reward structures are of the dictator, but even then there is always the
not limited to schools. High-stakes bonus structures implicit or explicit threat of serious consequences
have been widely implemented to “drive results” on if one offends someone higher in the hierarchy. As
Wall Street, Tokyo, and other stock markets or to noted earlier, more vertical or hierarchical systems
reap short-term profits. Where implemented, they have been found to be more difficult to internalize,
have also driven myriad “bad behaviors” by execu- and this may be because the more hierarchical sys-
tives, from excessive risk taking to outright “cooking tems tend to be associated with at least some degree
the books.” Indeed, the aforementioned contami- of totalitarianism.
nation effect (Ryan & Brown, 2005) in which all Democracies, in contrast, are inherently oriented
routes to stock inflations or profits are reinforced has toward giving individuals some say in the processes
recently been the source of much economic stress that govern their lives. Through direct voting and by
and human misery across the globe, as well as selfish having representatives in all levels of government,
profit taking by the players at the top of many cor- individuals can, ideally, have the opportunity to
porations, including failing ones. Here we see the contribute to the political process and to experience
direct relation between a distal structure involving freedom from constraints other than those necessary
controlling rewards that ultimately thwarts human to keep the system functioning effectively. There can
need satisfaction and wellness on a broad scale. As be little doubt that, in general, the democratic sys-
high-stakes contingencies are imported into other tem has great advantages relative to the totalitarian
spheres of life from health care to the coaching of system in terms of human autonomy and satisfac-
sports, SDT suggests that there are strong, evidence- tion of the needs for competence and relatedness,
based reasons for concern. which also tend to be diminished within a totalitar-
It is precisely because economists, policy mak- ian system.
ers, politicians, and pundits often ignore the nega- Nonetheless, democracies are vulnerable to dis-
tive consequences associated with the controlling tortion by forces within the countries. For exam-
use of rewards, and the interplay of extrinsic and ple, in democratic systems individuals or groups

 an overview of self-determination theory


can attain power by amassing huge wealth. Thus, for autonomy, competence, and relatedness, which
through financial support of activities such as adver- in turn led to greater subjective vitality, with the
tising and lobbying, the influence of wealth-based autonomy and competence needs each being partial
power can, through subtle or overt coercion or mediators of the relations. Deci et al. (2001) found
seduction, overrun the voices of the average citizens. support for a cross-cultural structural model in Bul-
Power through force can similarly yield undue influ- garian and American work organizations in which
ence within a democratic system and thus diminish autonomy support led to greater need satisfaction,
the autonomy of individuals within the system. In which in turn led to higher work engagement and
such cases, it is the role both of the legislative sys- greater well-being.
tem to create laws and regulatory agencies to limit Studies of within-person need satisfaction over
the undue influence of the few and of the judicial time (e.g., Reis, Sheldon, Gable, Roscoe, & Ryan,
system to prevent individuals and groups from act- 2000; Sheldon, Ryan, & Reis, 1996), using multi-
ing outside the laws. Different cultures have been level modeling have further found that in addition
differentially effective in these regards. to between-person relations of need satisfaction
to well-being, daily fluctuations in satisfaction of
Summary of Distal Social-Contextual each of the three needs predict unique variance
Influences in daily well-being. On days when people experi-
Theory and research on social contexts, across lev- ence satisfaction of their basic psychological needs,
els of analysis from dyads to cultures, indicates that, they also feel happier and even physically healthier.
to the degree that environmental factors are expe- For example, Ryan, Bernstein, and Brown (2010)
rienced as need supportive, they will foster greater recently demonstrated that the “weekend effect,” in
autonomous motivation, more intrinsic aspirations, which U.S. workers experienced greater vitality and
more effective performance, and higher well-being. positive mood on weekends, is primarily a function
Our analysis points to the embedded nature of con- of the low autonomy and relatedness satisfactions
texts and suggests that the varied levels of analysis most workers experience in their Monday through
that support versus thwart human needs must be Friday jobs. This brings us back to our earlier point
considered. In this section we have seen how the that although capitalism offers its constituents
broadest level, cultural, economic, and political choices and options, many of the vocations that are
systems have substantial influences on individuals available and that people “have to” adopt involve
both directly and as mediated by various embedded jobs that thwart psychological needs.
contexts contained within the culture. Need satisfaction also turns up in odd places. For
example, having identified that exposure to natu-
Basic Psychological Needs ral elements engenders greater vitality (Ryan et al.,
As a motivational theory, SDT must account for 2010), Weinstein, Przybylski, and Ryan (2009)
the energization of behavior. The basic psychological showed that these positive effects were at least par-
needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness tially due to the increased autonomy and connect-
represent one very important energizer of behav- edness people feel when nature is salient to them.
ior. In support of the assertion that there are three Przybylski, Ryan, and Rigby (2009) found that
universal psychological needs, we have reviewed psychological need satisfaction could derive from
evidence indicating that when social contexts in certain elements in video games, but not from their
multiple cultures supported people’s basic psycho- violent content. Weinstein and Ryan (2010) studied
logical needs, the people tended to thrive relative to the benefits of helping others, finding that autono-
people for whom the social contexts were thwarting mous helping fulfilled all three basic psychological
of need satisfaction. In line with this, various studies needs, but controlled helping did not. These exam-
have examined need satisfaction as a mediator both ples show how SDT research is always in search of
between autonomy-supportive contexts and posi- sources, moderators, and obstacles to basic need sat-
tive outcomes such as well-being and between the isfactions across the varied activities of life.
pursuit and attainment of intrinsic versus extrinsic Earlier in the chapter we reviewed research by
aspirations and positive outcomes. Niemiec et al. (2009) showing that people who
For example, Adie, Duda, and Ntoumanis (2008) attained intrinsic aspirations such as personal growth
found that when coaches were more autonomy sup- and community tended to display greater well-being
portive, their adult athletes playing various team and less ill-being but that those who attained extrin-
sports experienced greater satisfaction of the needs sic aspirations such as wealth and fame did not

d ec i , rya n 
display enhanced well-being although they did given moment (Brown & Ryan, 2003). Research
display greater ill-being. The important point for has shown that mindfulness is associated with
the present discussion is that these effects on well- enhanced autonomous functioning—that is, people
being and ill-being were mediated by satisfaction of are more likely to act in accord with abiding values
the basic psychological needs for autonomy, compe- and interests when they are mindful (e.g., Niemiec
tence, and relatedness. et al., 2010). Moreover, mindfulness is associated
Although need satisfaction supplies the essential with less focus on extrinsic values, more effective
nutriments for growth, integrity, and wellness, active coping, and greater wellness, again in part because
thwarting of needs produces a range of negative out- of its enhancement of autonomous functioning and
comes, from defensiveness and aggression to psy- fulfillment of the basic psychological needs (Wein-
chopathology (Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, Ryan, & stein, Brown, & Ryan, 2009). As such, awareness or
Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2011; Ryan, Deci, Grolnick, mindfulness represents a very important means to
& LaGuardia, 2006). Indeed, from an SDT view, take greater responsibility for oneself and thus to be
behaviors such as aggression and violence are not less vulnerable to the controlling and amotivating
themselves inherently motivated (Przybylski et al., forces that are all too prevalent in our social envi-
2009) but are instead consequences of need thwart- ronments.
ing. That is, people are prone to aggression whenever In this chapter, we have devoted considerable
basic functioning concerned with autonomy, com- attention to social-environmental influences for
petence, or relatedness is frustrated or threatened, two primary reasons. First, knowledge about the
rather than because it is an inherent drive or interest. effects of social environments on the motivation,
More generally, the “dark sides” of human behavior performance, and well-being of individuals pro-
can typically be traced to persistent or severe need vides a basis for creating systems—ranging, for
thwarting and the substitute needs or compensatory example, from families, to corporations, to politi-
activities related to it (Ryan & Deci, 2000b). cal policies—that conduce toward satisfaction of
the basic psychological needs of individuals within
Awareness As an Important Part of those systems. Second, when people understand
Autonomy and Well-Being how social environments affect individuals, those
Reviewing the powerful effects of proximal and people are more able to avoid or resist having the
distal contexts on human motivation, effective potentially negative effects impact them. Although
performance, and wellness, as mediated by basic we have emphasized social-contextual effects, we
psychological need satisfaction, might suggest to are equally as interested in people expanding their
many a very deterministic and even passive view of own awareness, for example, though mindfulness
human nature. But that is not the SDT viewpoint. training, psychotherapy, and other such methods,
It bears repeating that SDT assumes that people in order to be more autonomous in managing their
have an active, growth-oriented, challenge-seeking own lives and to be more autonomy supportive
nature unless they experience pervasive conditions with others.
of threat and need thwarting, in which case defen-
sive behaviors, need substitutes, and controlled and Summary and Conclusions
impersonal orientations can be catalyzed. When Herein, we have provided an overview of self-
needs are satisfied, the inherent, active, and growth- determination theory, arguing that the distinc-
oriented processes flourish. Part of the active nature tion between autonomous and controlled forms of
that is supported by need satisfaction involves the motivation are crucial for making predictions about
development of integrative awareness (Hodgins & the quality of performance, well-being, and other
Knee, 2002). important outcomes. We discussed the intrinsic,
According to SDT, autonomy is facilitated by integrated, and identified forms of autonomous
awareness, which entails the authentic attempt to motivation, as well as the external and introjected
experience and become conscious of what is occur- forms of controlled motivation. Autonomous and
ring within and around oneself. It is a relaxed and controlled motivations were discussed in terms of
interested attention to what is happening within the state level, the domain level, and the personal-
and without. One concept closely aligned with ity level, the last being referred to as causality ori-
awareness is mindfulness, which refers to an open, entations. Considerable research has verified that
receptive stance regarding what is occurring in any more autonomous motivation, both situationally

 an overview of self-determination theory


and dispositionally considered, generally leads to functioning and wellness of both persons and the
more positive consequences than controlled moti- communities within which they are embedded.
vation.
We then discussed research on people’s life goals
References
or aspirations, pointing out that these goals tend to Adie, J., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2008). Autonomy
fall into two categories, referred to as extrinsic aspi- support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal func-
rations (e.g., wealth, fame, and image) and intrinsic tioning of adult male and female sport participants:
aspirations (e.g., growth, relationships, and com- A test of basic needs theory. Motivation and Emotion, 32,
munity). The pursuit and attainment of extrinsic, 189–199.
Ariely, D., Gneezy, U., Loewenstein, G., & Mazar, N. (2009).
relative to intrinsic, aspirations has been shown to Large stakes and big mistakes. Review of Economic Studies,
be associated with poorer psychological health and 75, 1–19.
inferior performance, because the intrinsic aspira- Assor, A., Roth, G., & Deci, E. L. (2004). The emotional costs
tions more directly lead to satisfaction of the basic of parents’ conditional regard: A self-determination theory
psychological needs, whereas extrinsic aspirations analysis. Journal of Personality, 72, 47–89.
Baard, P. P., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2004). Intrinsic need
are less closely instrumental to basic need satisfac- satisfaction: A motivational basis of performance and well-
tion and may be hostile to it. being in two work settings. Journal of Applied Social Psychol-
People’s autonomous and controlled motiva- ogy, 34, 2045–2068.
tions, as well as their intrinsic and extrinsic life Bandura, A. (1996). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control.
goals are influenced to a significant degree, in both New York: Freeman.
Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., Ryan, R. M., &
the immediate situation and developmentally over Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C. (2011). Psychological need
time, by the degree to which their social contexts thwarting in the sport context: Assessing the darker side of
support versus thwart their basic psychological athletic experience. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
needs for competence, relatedness, and auton- 33, 75–102.
omy. We reviewed research and theory indicating Benware, C., & Deci, E. L. (1984). Quality of learning with an
active versus passive motivational set. American Educational
that social contexts may be relatively proximal or Research Journal, 21, 755–765.
increasingly more distal, with the more proximal Black, A. E., & Deci, E. L. (2000). The effects of student self-
being embedded within various levels of more dis- regulation and instructor autonomy support on learning in
tal contexts. An example of a proximal context is a a college-level natural science course: A self-determination
child’s home environment, and examples of more theory perspective. Science Education, 84, 740–756.
Boiché, J. C., Sarrazin, P. G., Grouzet, F. M., & Pelletier, L. G.
distal contexts are the neighborhood and, even (2008). Students’ motivational profiles in physical education
more broadly, the culture, with its values, econom- and achievement outcomes: A self-determination theory per-
ics, and politics. spective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 688–701.
Today’s increasingly global economic trends por- Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development.
tend increased concentrations of wealth and power Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being
in the hands of a few, and at this global level raise present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being.
concerns about increased top-down control and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 822–848.
deprivations of autonomy, which is a threat to both Burton, K. D., Lydon, J. E., D’Alessandro, D. U., & Koestner,
national (Downie, Koestner, & Chua, 2007) and R. (2006). The differential effects of intrinsic and identified
personal wellness (Twenge et al., 2010). Yet there motivation on well-being and performance: Prospective,
experimental and implicit approaches to self-determination
are potentially offsetting trends as well. We live in theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91,
a “wiki” world of fast Internet connections and 750–762.
instant communications that allow for much per- Carver, C. S., & Baird, E. (1998). The American dream revisited:
sonal expression as well as bottom-up organization Is it what you want or why you want it that matters? Psycho-
(e.g., Tapscott & Williams, 2006). In the context of logical Science, 9, 289–292.
Chirkov, V. Ryan, R. M., Kim, Y., & Kaplan, U. (2003). Differ-
these complex forces, understanding the basic needs entiating autonomy from individualism and independence:
of persons that are essential to wellness is crucial A self-determination theory perspective on internalization
for interventions from levels of global policy (see, of cultural orientations and well-being. Journal of Personality
for example, work by the New Economics Founda- and Social Psychology. 84, 97–110.
tion; http://www.neweconomics.org/) to individual Chirkov, V. I., Ryan, R. M., & Willness, C. (2005). Cultural
context and psychological needs in Canada and Brazil: Test-
psychotherapy (Ryan & Deci, 2008). The hope is ing a self-determination approach to the internalization of
that SDT research informs these policies and inter- cultural practices, identity, and well-being. Journal of Cross-
ventions and in doing so promotes more optimal Cultural Psychology, 36, 423–443.

d ec i , rya n 
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, well-being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
autonomy and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self- 15, 372–390.
system processes. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Grolnick, W. S. (2003). The psychology of parental control: How
The Minnesota symposium on child psychology: Self-processes in well-meant parenting backfires. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
development (Vol. 22, pp. 43–77). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Grolnick, W. S., Benjet, C., Kurowski, C. O., & Apostoleris,
de Charms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective N. H. (1997). Predictors of parent involvement in children’s
determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press. schooling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 538–548.
Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self- Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). Autonomy in children’s
determination in a work organization. Journal of Applied learning: An experimental and individual difference inves-
Psychology, 74, 580–590. tigation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52,
Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). 890–898.
Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated
perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119–142. with children’s self-regulation and competence in school.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 143–154.
review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic Harackiewicz, J. M., Manderlink, G., & Sansone, C. (1984).
rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, Rewarding pinball wizardry: The effects of evaluation on
627–668. intrinsic interest. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Deci, E. L., La Guardia, J. G., Moller, A. C., Scheiner, M. J., & 47, 287–300.
Ryan, R. M. (2006). On the benefits of giving as well as Hodgins, H. S., & Knee, C. R. (2002). The integrating self and
receiving autonomy support: Mutuality in close friendships. conscious experience. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.),
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 313–327. Handbook of self-determination research (pp.87–100). Roch-
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). The General Causality Ori- ester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
entations Scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Hodgins, H. S., Koestner, R., & Duncan, N. (1996). On the
Research in Personality, 19, 109–134. compatibility of autonomy and relatedness. Personality and
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 227–237.
for true self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Agency, efficacy, and Hodgins, H. S., & Liebeskind, E. (2003). Apology versus
self-esteem (pp. 31–49). New York: Plenum. defense: Antecedents and consequences. Journal of Experi-
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of mental Social Psychology, 39, 297–236.
goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of Hodgins, H. S., Liebeskind, E., & Schwartz, W. (1996). Getting
behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. out of hot water: Facework in social predicaments. Journal of
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating optimal moti- Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 300–314.
vation and psychological well-being across life’s domains, Hodgins, H. S., Yacko, H. A., & Gottlieb, E. (2006). Auton-
Canadian Psychology, 49, 14–23. omy and nondefensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 30,
Deci, E. L., Ryan, R. M., Gagné, M., Leone, D. R., Usunov, J., & 283–293.
Kornazheva, B. P. (2001). Need satisfaction, motivation, and Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to
well-being in the work organizations of a former Eastern behavior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Bloc country. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, Jang, H., Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (2010). Engaging students in
930–942. learning activities: It’s not autonomy support or structure,
Deci, E. L., Schwartz, A. J., Sheinman, L., & Ryan, R. M. but autonomy support and structure. Journal of Educational
(1981). An instrument to assess adults’ orientations toward Psychology, 102, 588–600.
control versus autonomy with children: Reflections on Kasser, T., Cohn, S., Kanner, A. D., & Ryan, R. M. (2007).
intrinsic motivation and perceived competence. Journal of Some costs of American corporate capitalism: A psycho-
Educational Psychology, 73, 642–650. logical exploration of value and goal conflicts. Psychological
Deci, E. L., Spiegel, N. H., Ryan, R. M., Koestner, R., & Inquiry, 18, 1–22.
Kauffman, M. (1982). Effects of performance standards on Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the Ameri-
teaching styles: Behavior of controlling teachers. Journal of can dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life
Educational Psychology, 74, 852–859. aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,
Downie, M., Koestner, R., & Chua, S. N. (2007). Political sup- 410–422.
port for self-determination, wealth, and national subjective Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the
well-being. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 188–194. American dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and
Frey, B. S. (1997). Not just for the money: An economic theory of extrinsic goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
personal motivation. Northampton, MA: Edward Elgar. 22, 80–87.
Friedman, M., & Friedman, R. (1990). Free to choose. New York: Kasser, T., Ryan, R. M., Zax, M., & Sameroff, A. J. (1995). The
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. relations of maternal and social environments to late ado-
Friedman, R., Deci, E. L., Elliot, A. J., Moller, A. C., & Aarts, H. lescents’ materialistic and prosocial values. Developmental
(2010). Motivational synchronicity: Priming motivational Psychology, 31, 907–914.
orientations with observations of others’ behaviors. Motiva- Kasser, V. G., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). The relation of psycho-
tion and Emotion, 34, 34–38. logical needs for autonomy and relatedness to vitality, well-
Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and being, and mortality in a nursing home. Journal of Applied
work motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, Social Psychology, 29, 935–954.
331–362. Kernis, M. H., & Paradise, A. W. (2002). Distinguish-
Gagné, M., Ryan, R. M., & Bargmann, K. (2003). Auton- ing between secure and fragile forms of high self-esteem.
omy support and need satisfaction in the motivation and In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of

 an overview of self-determination theory


self-determination research (pp. 339–360). Rochester, NY: Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects
University Of Rochester Press. of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A
Knee, C. R., Patrick, H., Vietor, N. A., Nanayakkara, A., & meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 134,
Neighbors, C. (2002). Self-determination as growth moti- 270–300.
vation in romantic relationships. Personality and Social Pavey, L., & Sparks, P. (2009). Reactance, autonomy, and paths
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 609–619. to persuasion: Examining perceptions of threats to free-
Knee, C. R., & Zuckerman, M. (1996). Causality orientations dom and informational value. Motivation and Emotion, 33,
and the disappearance of the self-serving bias. Journal of 277–290.
Research in Personality, 30, 76–87. Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Brière, N. M.
Knee, C. R., & Zuckerman, M. (1998). A nondefensive person- (2001). Associations among perceived autonomy support,
ality: Autonomy and control as moderators of defensive cop- forms of self-regulation, and persistence: A prospective study.
ing and self-handicapping. Journal of Research in Personality, Motivation and Emotion, 25, 279–306.
32, 115–130. Pelletier, L. G., Séguin-Lévesque, C., & Legault L. (2002). Pres-
Koestner, R., Losier, G. F., Vallerand, R. J., & Carducci, D. sure from above and pressure from below as determinants of
(1996). Identified and introjected forms of political inter- teachers’ motivation and teaching behavior. Journal of Educa-
nalization: Extending self-determination theory. Journal of tional Psychology, 94, 186–196.
Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1025–1036. Pelletier, L. G., Tuson, K. M., & Haddad, N. K. (1997). Client
Koestner, R., Ryan, R. M., Bernieri, F., & Holt, K. (1984). motivation for therapy scale: A measure of intrinsic motiva-
Setting limits on children’s behavior: The differential effects tion, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation for therapy. Jour-
of controlling versus informational styles on intrinsic nal of Personality Assessment, 68, 414–435.
motivation and creativity. Journal of Personality, 52, Perls, F. S. (1973). The Gestalt approach and eyewitness to therapy.
233–248. Ben Lomond, CA: Science and Behavior Books.
La Guardia, J. G., & Patrick, H. (2008) Self-determination the- Petersen, T. (1993). The economics of organization: The
ory as a fundamental theory of close relationships. Canadian principal-agent relationship. Acta Sociologica, 36, 277–293.
Psychology, 49, 201–209. Philippe, F. L., & Vallerand, R. J. (2008). Actual environments
La Guardia, J. G., Ryan, R. M., Couchman, C. E., & Deci, E. L. do affect motivation and psychological adjustment: A test
(2000). Within-person variation in security of attachment: of self-determination theory in a natural setting. Motivation
A self-determination theory perspective on attachment, need and Emotion, 32, 81–89.
fulfillment, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Przybylski, A. K., Ryan, R. M., & Rigby, C. S. (2009). The moti-
Psychology, 79, 367–384. vating role of violence in video games. Personality and Social
Landry, R., Whipple, N., Mageau, G., Joussemet, M., Psychology Bulletin, 35, 243–259.
Koestner, R., DiDio, L., . . . Haga, S. M. (2008). Trust in Reeve, J., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Elements within the competi-
organismic development, autonomy support, and adaptation tive situation that affect intrinsic motivation. Personality and
among mothers and their children. Motivation and Emotion, Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 24–33.
32, 173–188. Reis, H. T., Sheldon, K. M., Gable, S. L., Roscoe, J., & Ryan,
McGraw, K. O., & McCullers, J. C. (1979). Evidence of a detri- R. M. (2000). Daily well-being: The role of autonomy, com-
mental effect of extrinsic incentives on breaking a mental set. petence, and relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 15, 285–294. Bulletin, 26, 419–435.
McHoskey, J. W. (1999). Machiavellianism, intrinsic versus Roth, G., Assor, A, Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci,
extrinsic goals, and social interest: A self-determination the- E. L. (2009). The emotional and academic consequences of
ory analysis. Motivation and Emotion, 23, 267–283. parental conditional regard: Comparing conditional posi-
Münster Halvari, A. E. M., Halvari, H., Bjørnebekk, G., & tive regard, conditional negative regard, and autonomy sup-
Deci, E. L. (2010). Motivation and anxiety for dental treat- port as parenting practices. Developmental Psychology, 45,
ment: Testing a self-determination theory model of oral self- 1119–1142.
care behavior and dental clinic attendance. Motivation and Ryan, R. M. (1982). Control and information in the intraper-
Emotion, 34, 15–33. sonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory.
Niemiec, C. P., Brown, K. W., Kashdan, T. B., Cozzolino, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 450–461.
P. J., Breen, W. E., Levesque-Bristol, C., & Ryan, R. M. Ryan, R. M., Bernstein, J. H., & Brown, K. W. (2010). Week-
(2010). Being present in the face of existential threat: The ends, work, and well-being: Psychological need satisfactions
role of trait mindfulness in reducing defensive responses to and day of the week effects on mood, vitality, and physi-
mortality salience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, cal symptoms. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29,
99, 344–365. 95–122.
Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). The path Ryan, R. M., & Brown, K. W. (2005). Legislating compe-
taken: Consequences of attaining intrinsic and extrinsic aspi- tence: The motivational impact of high stakes testing as an
rations in post-college life. Journal of Research in Personality, educational reform. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.)
43, 291–306. Handbook of competence (pp. 354–374). New York: Guilford
Nichols, S. L., & Berliner, D. C. (2007). Collateral damage: How Press.
high-stakes testing corrupts America’s schools. Cambridge, MA: Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of cau-
Harvard Education Press. sality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in
Oliver, E. J., Markland, D., Hardy, J., & Petherick, C. M. two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
(2008). The effects of autonomy-supportive versus control- 749–761.
ling environments on self-talk. Motivation and Emotion, 32, Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Deci, E. L. (1985). A motiva-
200–212. tional analysis of self-determination and self-regulation in

d ec i , rya n 
education. In C. Ames & R. E. Ames (Eds.), Research on Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., & Reis, H. T. (1996). What makes
motivation in education: The classroom milieu (pp. 13–51). for a good day? Competence and autonomy in the day and
New York: Academic Press. in the person. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22,
Ryan, R. M., Connell, J. P., & Plant, R. W. (1990). Emotions 1270–1279.
in non-directed text learning. Learning and Individual Dif- Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York:
ferences, 2, 1–17. Macmillan.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000a). Intrinsic and extrinsic moti- Tapscott, D., & Williams, A. D. (2006). Wikinomics: How mass
vations: Classic definitions and new directions. Contemporary collaboration changes everything. New York: Penguin.
Educational Psychology, 25, 54–67. Twenge, J., Gentile, B., DeWall, C. N., Ma, D., Lacefield, K.,
Ryan, R. M. & Deci, E. L. (2000b). The darker and brighter & Schurtz, D. R. (2010). Birth cohort increases in psycho-
sides of human existence: Basic psychological needs as a uni- pathology among young Americans, 1938–2007: A cross-
fying concept. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 319–338. temporal meta-analysis of the MMPI. Clinical Psychology
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2003). On assimilating identities Review, 30, 145–154.
to the self: A self-determination theory perspective on inter- Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic
nalization and integrity within cultures. In M. R. Leary & and extrinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in
J. P. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (pp. experimental social psychology (Vol. 29, pp. 271–360). San
255–273). New York: Guilford. Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). A self-determination Vansteenkiste, M., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). Self-
approach to psychotherapy: The motivational basis for effec- determination theory and the explanatory role of psychologi-
tive change. Canadian Psychology, 49, 186–193. cal needs in human well-being. In L. Bruni, F. Comim, &
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., Grolnick, W. S., & LaGuardia, J. G. M. Pugno (Eds.), Capabilities and happiness (pp. 181–223).
(2006) The significance of autonomy and autonomy sup- Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
port in psychological development and psychopathology. In Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Sheldon, K. M., &
D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopa- Deci, E. L. (2004). Motivating learning, performance, and
thology. Vol. 1: Theory and methods (2nd ed., pp. 295–849). persistence: The synergistic effects of intrinsic goal contents
New York: John Wiley & Sons. and autonomy-supportive contexts. Journal of Personality and
Ryan, R. M., Rigby, S., & King, K. (1993). Two types of reli- Social Psychology, 87, 246–260.
gious internalization and their relations to religious orien- Wang, F. (2008). Motivation and English achievement: An
tations and mental health. Journal of Personality and Social exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis of a new mea-
Psychology, 65, 586–596. sure for Chinese students of English learning. North Ameri-
Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). can Journal of Psychology, 10, 633–646.
All goals are not created equal: An organismic perspective Weinstein, N., Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). A multi-
on the nature of goals and their regulation. In P. M. Goll- method examination of the effects of mindfulness on stress
witzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking attribution, coping, and emotional well-being. Journal of
cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 7–26). New York: Research in Personality, 43, 374–385.
Guilford. Weinstein, N., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2011). Motiva-
Ryan, R. M., & Weinstein, N. (2009). Undermining quality tional determinants of integrating positive and negative past
teaching and learning: A self-determination theory perspec- identities. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100,
tive on high-stakes testing. Theory and Research in Education, 527–544.
7, 224–233. Weinstein, N., Przybylski, A. K., & Ryan, R. M. (2009). Can
Ryan, R. M., Weinstein, N., Bernstein, J., Brown, K. W., Mistretta, nature make us more caring? Effects of immersion in nature
L. & Gagne, M. (2010). Vitalizing effects of being out- on intrinsic aspirations and generosity. Personality and Social
doors and in nature. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 30, Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1315–1329.
159–168. Weinstein, N., & Ryan, R. M. (2010). When helping helps:
Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of child Autonomous motivation for prosocial behavior and its influ-
rearing. Evanston, IL: Row, Peterson. ence on well-being for the helper and recipient. Journal of
Séguin, C., Pelletier, L. G., & Hunsley, J. (1999). Predicting Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 222–244.
environmental behaviors: The influence of self-determination Williams, G. C., Cox, E. M., Hedberg, V., & Deci, E. L. (2000).
and information about environmental health risks. Journal of Extrinsic life goals and health risk behaviors in adolescents.
Applied Social Psychology, 29, 1582–160. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 1756–1771.
Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. New York: Alfred Williams, G. C., Freedman, Z. R., & Deci, E. L. (1998). Sup-
A. Knopf. porting autonomy to motivate glucose control in patients
Shapira, Z. (1976). Expectancy determinants of intrinsically with diabetes. Diabetes Care, 21, 1644–1651.
motivated behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Williams, G. C., Grow, V. M., Freedman, Z., Ryan, R. M., &
ogy, 34, 1235–1244. Deci, E. L. (1996). Motivational predictors of weight loss
Sheldon, K. M., & Kasser, T. (2008). Psychological threat and weight-loss maintenance. Journal of Personality and Social
and extrinsic goal striving. Motivation and Emotion, 32, Psychology, 70, 115–126.
37–45. Williams, G. C., McGregor, H. A., Sharp, D., Kouides, R. W.,
Sheldon, K. M., Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., & Kasser, T. (2004). Levesque, C., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). A self-
The independent effects of goal contents and motives on determination multiple risk intervention trial to improve
well-being: It’s both what you pursue and why you pursue it. smokers’ health. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 21,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 475–486. 1288–1294.

 an overview of self-determination theory


Williams, G. C., McGregor, H. A., Sharp, D., Levesque, theory process model for promoting glycemic control
C., Kouides, R. W., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2006). Test- through diabetes self-management. Health Psychology, 23,
ing a self-determination theory intervention for motivating 58–66.
tobacco cessation: Supporting autonomy and competence in Zuroff, D. C., Koestner, R., Moskowitz, D. S., McBride, C.,
a clinical trial. Health Psychology, 25, 91–101. Marshall, M., & Bagby, M. (2007). Autonomous motivation
Williams, G. C., McGregor, H. A., Zeldman, A., Freedman, for therapy: A new common factor in brief treatments for
Z. R., & Deci, E. L. (2004). Testing a self-determination depression. Psychotherapy Research, 17, 137–147.

d ec i , rya n 
This page intentionally left blank
PA RT
3
Motivational Processes
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER

Ego Depletion: Theory and Evidence


7
Mark Muraven

Abstract
Self-control all too often fails. Despite people’s best intentions and considerable negative outcomes,
people often find themselves at the losing end of resisting temptation, combating urges, and changing
their behavior. One reason for these failures may be that exerting self-control depletes a limited
resource (ego depletion) that is necessary for the success of self-control. Hence, after exerting
self-control individuals are less able to resist temptations, fight urges, or stop a behavior, which results
in a loss of self-control. This chapter reviews the evidence for this theory in a wide variety of domains
and examines what behaviors appear to deplete ego strength and how depletion affects behavior.
A comprehensive theory that examines how depletion operates is put forth and this theory is used
to examine some factors that might moderate the depletion effect.
Key Words: self-control, ego depletion, willpower, motivation

Introduction desire in order to reach a more desired state in the


As most people can attest, dieting, quitting smok- future. That is, the organism is seeking to gain a
ing, controlling one’s temper, and working instead of larger but delayed reward over a smaller but more
playing are not easy. In fact, it often feels quite dif- immediate reward. To do so, the organism must
ficult to avoid immediate, pressing, or easy behaviors resist the temptation to take the immediate reward.
in order to follow rules, get along with others, or Self-control is the process that allows this to happen
reach long-range goals. Moreover, such self-control (Kanfer & Karoly, 1972; Mischel, Shoda, & Rodri-
efforts fail all too often. The point of the strength guez, 1989).
model of self-control (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000) There are significant and important differences
is to better understand how people resist such temp- between self-control and self-regulation worthy of
tations, understand why it fails (and why it succeeds) mention. Although these terms are sometimes used
and what can be done to improve self-control. interchangeably, self-control is an important subset
Self-control is the process that enables organisms of self-regulation. Self-regulation is the process by
to override, inhibit, or stop urges, emotions and which individuals pursue all goals, both short and
moods, thoughts, or behaviors in order to reach a long term. The process of self-regulation incorpo-
long-term goal. These long-term goals can be per- rates both conscious and unconscious process, such
sonally set, such as losing weight or succeeding in as breathing, eating, or driving to work every day.
school, or can be moral, interpersonal, or societal On the other hand, self-control is a deliberative, con-
rules like not having premarital sex or not gossiping. scious, effortful, and resource-intensive process of
Regardless of the type of goal, it typically requires restraining an impulse in order to reach a long-term
the individual to forgo an immediate pleasure or goal or follow a rule. To the extent that a situation


requires inhibition, it demands self-control. This dis- argued that it was not the noise per se that affected
tinction is important, because tasks that may seem performance, but rather the process of adapting and
effortful, like memorizing a list of words or solving coping with the noise that depleted the individual
simple arithmetic problems only require self-control so that he or she was less able to deal with future
to the extent that the individual has to override an demands.
impulse. Such a depletion model can be contrasted with
The ability to exert self-control is one of the criti- a constant resource or skill model. These models
cal features that differentiate humans from other would predict that self-control should not be affected
organisms (Baumeister, 1998, 2005). Although other by previous demands, or it may even get better as
animals can exert self-control (for instance, squirrels the individual warms up and gains knowledge of the
burying nuts for the winter), it is clear that the self- tasks. The depletion model specifically predicts an
control demands on humans is much greater than the aftereffect of exerting self-control. That is, even after
self-control demands on these other animals. Indeed, the initial self-control demand has been removed
it has been argued (Sedikides & Skowronski, 1997) and a new situation introduced, there should be
that the growth in the ability to exert self-control a carryover effect that leads to poorer self-control.
drove the development of human cognition, society, Moreover, in order to be a unique prediction,
and the development of the self. Hence, understand- this decline in performance should not arise from
ing how self-control operates can give us insight into changes in mood, arousal, frustration, self-efficacy,
many critical features of the human experience. or other well-established psychological processes.
Moreover, of course, understanding self-control Extensive research has strongly suggested that
has immense practical benefits as well. Self-control the depletion model is the best fit for the observed
is critical to both preventing the initiation as well data on self-control. In experimental studies, indi-
as the cessation of addictive behaviors (e.g., Brown, viduals who exert self-control perform more poorly
1998; Wills, Sandy, & Yaeger, 2002). Other research on subsequent tests of self-control as compared to
has illustrated the importance of self-control in individuals who initially worked on a task that did
dieting (Heatherton, Striepe, & Wittenberg, 1998), not require self-control. For example, Muraven,
overspending (Faber, 1992), relationship prob- Collins, and Nienhaus (2002) had social drinkers
lems (Finkel & Campbell, 2001), violence (Stucke either suppress the thought of a white bear (a diffi-
& Baumeister, 2006), and crime (Gottfredson & cult thought inhibition exercise; Wegner, Schneider,
Hirschi, 1990). Given that many health problems Carter, & White, 1987) or solve addition problems.
can be linked to a lack of exercise, smoking, and These tasks did not differ in perceived unpleasant-
poor eating habits, it is apparent that a lack of self- ness, effort, or difficulty; the only reported dif-
control is a major contributor to morbidity and ference was the amount of self-control required.
mortality. Likewise, because many economic prob- Subsequently, participants were given the chance to
lems at both the personal and societal level follow drink alcohol, with the caveat that afterward they
from overspending, lack of consideration of future would take a driving simulator test and those who
demands, and educational underachievement, a bet- did well would win a prize. As compared to those
ter understanding of how self-control operates is who solved addition problems, individuals who had
critical to our prosperity as well. to suppress their thoughts drank more and become
more intoxicated. This suggests that the exercise of
Ego Strength suppressing thoughts leads to poorer control over
An examination of past research on self-control alcohol intake subsequently. Indeed, participants’
(Muraven & Baumeister, 2000) suggested that self- reports of the amount of self-control they exerted
control worsens over time. That is, after exerting self- on the first task were related to the amount of alco-
control, subsequent attempts at self-control suffer. hol they consumed. On the other hand, reports of
For instance, research on the effects of environmen- mood, arousal, frustration, and displeasure were not
tal stress (Glass, Singer, & Friedman, 1969) found related to the amount consumed. This strongly sug-
that individuals who were exposed to uncontrol- gests that the loss of control over alcohol intake is
lable or unpredictable noise subsequently performed being driven by the amount of self-control exerted
more poorly on a test of persistence and frustration in the first part of the experiment.
tolerance, after being moved to a quiet location, as The initial exertion of self-control only affects
compared to individuals who had been exposed to a tasks that require self-control, further giving evi-
controllable or predictable noise. These researchers dence to the specificity of the depletion model. For

 ego depl etion


instance, people who strongly desire to drink alco- What Causes Depletion
hol must exert greater self-control not to drink than Overall, and consistent with the definition of
people who desire alcohol less. Hence, in the alco- self-control given earlier, researchers have found that
hol study (Muraven et al., 2002), individuals who anytime an individual overrides, inhibits, stops, or
were not very tempted to drink were less affected by changes a mood, urge, thought, or behavior, it can
the initial exertion of self-control and drank less as lead to depletion and hence poorer self-control. For
compared to individuals who were higher in temp- example, at the most basic level, Baumeister et al.
tation. That is, the initial act of self-control reduced (1998) showed that after resisting the temptation
subsequent self-control performance, but it did not of eating chocolate chip cookies, participants quit
lead to a general increase in alcohol intake (see also working on a frustrating puzzle sooner than partici-
Muraven, Collins, Shiffman, & Paty, 2005). Addi- pants who had to resist eating radishes, which were
tional research has further illustrated that difficult not seen as tempting as the cookies. This suggests
tasks that do not require self-control are unaffected that overriding basic urges is depleting. Indeed,
by initial acts of self-control (Muraven, Shmueli, & Muraven and Shmueli (2006) found a similar effect
Burkley, 2006). for alcohol and social drinkers, with the magnitude
Given these results, it has been suggested (e.g., of the depletion effect being proportional to partici-
Muraven & Baumeister, 2000) that exerting self- pants’ self-reported desire to drink. Likewise, resist-
control may deplete a conceptual resource called ing the urge to eat cookies was more depleting to
ego strength (alternatively called self-control strength). dieters than nondieters, further suggesting that the
In particular, this resource is critical to any and all strength of the impulse being inhibited may par-
attempts at self-control. It is not needed for any tially determine how depleting an activity is (Vohs
activities except self-control. This ego strength is & Heatherton, 2000).
limited and the amount of strength is critical to
the success of self-control, so that more is better. Interpersonal
The exertion of self-control depletes some of this However, research has shown that many other
resource, so that after engaging in self-control, the behaviors are also depleting. One particular area
individual has less ego strength. People in this state of interest is the depleting nature of interpersonal
are said to be ego depleted (or just depleted). interactions. For instance, Vohs, Baumeister, and
If indeed the level of strength is critical to the Ciarocco (2005) found that people who had to
success of self-control and that the exertion of self- present themselves as competent and likeable to an
control depletes some of this resource, it follows audience motivated to believe otherwise were less
that after exerting self-control, subsequent attempts able to regulate their emotions subsequently as com-
at self-control may be more likely to fail. A good pared to individuals who were asked to act naturally.
amount of research, from around the world, using a Similarly, engaging in an interaction with a difficult,
variety of methods, has found this pattern of results. high-maintenance confederate led to greater deple-
As noted earlier, the observed effects do not tion than interacting with a more receptive person
appear to be a product of mood or arousal. In most (Finkel et al., 2006).
studies, mood and arousal have not been found to The difficulty of high-maintenance interactions
differ between participants who exerted self-control seems to carry over to interracial interactions as well.
and those who did not (e.g., Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Research has found that interracial interactions lead
Muraven, & Tice, 1998; Muraven, Tice, & Baumeis- to poorer performance subsequently on the Stroop
ter, 1998). Likewise, mood and arousal were not interference task as compared to same-race interactions
related to final self-control performance. The same is (Richeson & Shelton, 2003; Richeson & Trawalter,
true for more specific mood items, such as frustration, 2005). This effect seems to exist for Black as well as
irritation, annoyance, boredom, or interest as well. White individuals (Richeson, Trawalter, & Shelton,
Likewise, Wallace and Baumeister (2002) directly 2005). Being the target of prejudice and stigma also
manipulated feedback about success and failure of appears to be depleting, as individuals try to cope with
the self-control efforts and also found no effect. In the negative feelings and behaviors of being the target
short, the decline in self-control performance after of stigma (Inzlicht, McKay, & Aronson, 2007). Even
exerting self-control appears to be directly related more powerfully, people who were asked to ostracize
to the amount of self-control exerted and cannot be someone by not talking to the person quit working
easily explained by other, well-established psycho- on an unsolvable anagram task sooner than people
logical processes. who did not have to ostracize someone (Ciarocco,

mur aven 


Sommer, & Baumeister, 2001). These studies suggest (see later for a further discussion of the nature of
that self-presentation and maintaining (or ending) self-control depletion), the researchers argued that
relationships are tasks that often require self-control. imagining oneself exerting self-control may both
activate expectancies about depletion as well as
Changing the Self actually require self-control. This further illustrates
Other activities that require the individual to that although tasks that require self-control appear
change his or her behavior have also been found to be to deplete self-control resources, we must look
depleting. For instance, as would be expected, indi- beyond simple inhibition if we wish to understand
viduals who had to suppress their emotional reaction how individuals exert executive control and guide
to a film designed to evoke sadness exhibited greater themselves to long-term goals.
depletion than individuals who did not have to con-
trol their emotions (Muraven et al., 1998). However, Consequences of Depletion
individuals who had to control their emotions and Understanding how a state of ego depletion affects
increase their sadness in response to a video clip subsequent performance may also help to illuminate
of animals dying in an environmental disaster also the processes involved in self-control. First, consistent
exhibited greater depletion, as evidenced by less per- with definitions outlined earlier, the most clear-cut
sistence on a frustrating task subsequently. This indi- consequence of depletion is a loss of self-control. This
cates that the direction of control is far less important has been demonstrated in many different domains,
than the exercise of control. Subsequent research rep- some of which have already been described. For
licated this effect with disgust (Schmeichel, Demaree, instance, after controlling their thoughts and not
Robinson, & Pu, 2006), while indicating that these thinking about a white bear, participants had a harder
effects were not due to arousal. Hence, behavior time not smiling, laughing, or showing amusement
change in all its forms appears to be depleting. at a humorous film as compared to individuals who
The act of making choices also seems to be deplet- did not control their thoughts (Muraven et al., 1998).
ing. In one study, participants were told that they This effect has carried over to many different domains.
were going to have to give a speech on an issue that For instance, depleted individuals (especially dieters)
ran counter to their existing opinions. As compared tend to eat more (Kahan, Polivy, & Herman, 2003;
to those who had no choice, those who were given a Vohs & Heatherton, 2000). Perhaps most artfully,
choice whether to make the speech persisted for less Hofmann, Rauch, and Gawronski (2007; see also
time on a difficult task afterward compared to those Friese, Hofmann, & Wanke, 2008) found that the
who were not given a choice (Baumeister et al., amount of candy individuals ate when not depleted
1998). In later research, participants who made a was related to their self-report views toward food.
series of consumer decisions subsequently drank However, when depleted, their implicit attitudes were
less of a bad tasting drink than those who rated the a much better predictor of their consumption, which
products but did not make a choice (Vohs et al., suggest that depletion reduced their ability to inten-
2008; see also Bruyneel, Dewitte, Vohs, & Warlop, tionally regulate their food intake (a similar study by
2006). This effect was even observed when making Ostafin, Marlatt, and Greenwald, 2008, found the
choices about pleasant outcomes and appears to be same pattern with implicit and explicit measures of
separate from implementing the choice. attitudes toward drinking and actual alcohol con-
Perhaps most intriguing is recent research that sumption). Consistent with that perspective, depletion
suggests that mentally simulating the self-control has been found to affect the controlled components
actions of others may also be depleting (Ackerman, of stereotype-based responses, but not the automatic
Goldstein, Shapiro, & Bargh, 2009). Participants component (Govorun & Payne, 2006).
read a story about a waiter or waitress who was Research on depletion has found similar pat-
hungry but unable to eat the food that he or she terns that depletion leads to poorer control over
served. Half the participants were told to simply other behaviors of consequence as well. For instance,
read the story, whereas the other half were asked to Muraven, Collins, and Nienhaus (2002) found
imagine themselves as this waiter or waitress. The that after controlling their thoughts, social drink-
people who imagined themselves as the hungry ers consumed more alcohol despite incentives not to
but self-denying server reported being more will- as compared to social drinkers who solved difficult
ing to overpay for consumer products as compared and frustrating math problems that nonetheless did
to those who merely read the story. Although the not require self-control. In a follow-up, a field study
exact mechanism for this effect remains unclear of underage social drinkers who carried palm top

 ego depl etion


computers for 3 weeks to report their self-control Interpersonal Effects
demands and drinking behavior found that on days There are clear interpersonal consequences to
that their self-control demands were higher than their depletion as well. As expected from the decline in
average, these drinkers were more likely to drink to self-control performance, depleted people appear
excess (Muraven et al., 2005). The increased drinking to be less likely to follow basic social norms, both
was not apparently related to increased urges, greater prescriptive and descriptive (DeBono, Shmueli, &
negative affect, or a lack of desire to control drink- Muraven, 2011). For instance, depleted individuals
ing; instead it appeared to be related to an inability are more likely to cheat (Muraven, Pogarsky, &
to regulate alcohol intake. Depletion of ego strength Shmueli, 2006), lie, and steal (DeBono et al., 2011).
has also been found to affect smoking behavior People whose self-control was depleted also engaged
(Leeman, O’Malley, White, & McKee, 2010), regu- in more inappropriate social interactions, by talking
lation of sexual urges (Gailliot & Baumeister, 2007), too much, making too intimate interpersonal disclo-
and impulse spending (Vohs & Faber, 2007). sures, or being arrogant (Vohs et al., 2005). Clearly,
Although the predominant model is that deple- being socially appropriate and following norms
tion reduces individuals’ ability to inhibit urges requires self-control and is affected by depletion.
(Govorun & Payne, 2006; Muraven & Shmueli, However, the effects of depletion extend beyond
2006; Ostafin et al., 2008), there is also some evidence simple impulsive control. For example, depleted
that depletion can also lead to stronger emotions and individuals are also more easily persuaded. Burkley
urges. For instance, Schmeichel, Harmon-Jones, (2008) found that resisting a persuasive attempt
and Harmon-Jones, (2010) found that depletion leads to a pattern of self-control outcomes consis-
increased individuals’ approach motivation, so that tent with depletion. In later studies, he found that
they focused more on a reward-relevant stimulus than depleted individuals were more easily persuaded,
a reward-irrelevant stimulus. Ego depletion could especially by strong arguments. Wheeler, Brinol, and
therefore lead to poorer self-control by strengthening Hermann (2007) found a similar pattern of results
impulses, rather than undermining inhibition. and persuasively argued that depleted individuals
were more likely to agree with counterattitudinal
Broader Perspective statements. Interestingly, they found that depleted
Less well investigated but important to under- and nondepleted individuals thought equally hard
standing how depletion affects performance are stud- about the message, but only depleted individuals
ies on perception of time and passivity in depleted were less likely to come up with counterarguments
individuals. In particular, depletion apparently affects to the message. These results suggest that depletion
people’s sense of the passage of time. Depleted indi- leads to passivity and agreement among people.
viduals estimated that more time had passed while This passivity and general lack of ability to engage
exerting self-control than nondepleted individuals in counterarguments may affect interpersonal per-
(Vohs & Schmeichel, 2003). This might contribute ception as well. For example, individuals who were
to the poorer self-control among depleted individu- depleted rated African American targets more nega-
als, especially on persistence-based tasks, as they may tively than European American targets (Muraven,
misjudge how long they have been acting on con- 2008b; Park, Glaser, & Knowles, 2008). Depleted
trolling themselves. More research is needed to better people may be less willing to override their stereo-
integrate these results into a more general theory of types and less likely to think of reasons to do so
how self-control operates. (Devine, 1989). Research has also found that deple-
Similarly, Baumeister et al. (1998) found that tion makes people less helpful (DeWall, Baumeister,
depleted individuals were more passive than non- Gailliot, & Maner, 2008; Fennis, Janssen, & Vohs,
depleted individuals. When quitting a boring task 2009). Consistent with that reduced helpfulness,
(watching an unchanging video of a blank wall) depleted people are less forgiving in their relation-
required participants to initiate a button push, ships: Individuals who were depleted were less likely
depleted participants watched longer than nonde- to engage in accommodation (Yovetich & Rusbult,
pleted participants. On the other hand, when quit- 1994) and therefore responded less constructively
ting was the passive option and continuing required a to the negative behavior of their partner (Finkel &
response, depleted participants quit sooner. The extent Campbell, 2001). Depleted individuals lie more as
to which many of the effects associated with depletion compared to nondepleted people as well (Mead,
may spring from passivity and a general unwillingness Baumeister, Gino, Schweitzer, & Ariely, 2009),
to initiate an action is an unanswered question. which can also damage relationships.

mur aven 


The effects of depletion further extend into aggres- affect and risk taking was also found to be partially
sive behavior. It is likely that individuals have to mediated by depletion—people’s attempt to regulate
learn to restrain aggressive urges in order to maintain their negative moods is depleting and this depletion
harmonious relationships and therefore depletion of leads to greater risk taking (Bruyneel, Dewitte, Franses,
self-control resources may lead to a breakdown in & Dekimpe, 2009). Depleted individuals also rely to
this restraint. Indeed, depleted individuals have been a greater extent than nondepleted individuals on heu-
found to react to provocations with greater aggression ristics and fail to consider all options carefully in a
than nondepleted individuals (DeWall, Baumeister, consumer decision-making task, which leads to a sub-
Stillman, & Gailliot, 2007; Stucke & Baumeister, optimal decision (Masicampo & Baumeister, 2008;
2006). As compared to participants who were asked Pocheptsova, Amir, Dhar, & Baumeister, 2009). In
to not eat radishes, participants who were asked to addition, it appears that depleted individuals are
not eat a donut placed in front of them slipped more more likely to seek confirmatory information that
hot sauce into the foods that were to be given to a is consistent with their existing viewpoints (Fischer,
participant who gave them negative feedback on an Greitemeyer, & Frey, 2008). Exerting self-control
essay. When participants received neutral feedback appears to reduce the motivation to search for and
on their essay, there were no differences between process new information. This research is particularly
depleted and nondepleted conditions. This suggests notable for showing that the effects of depletion on
that depletion was simply potentiating the aggressive decision making and information processing appear
responses and was not a direct cause of it. to be different from the effects observed from cogni-
tive load, ego threat, and mood.
Cognition This change in decision making and risk taking
Depletion also appears to affects cognition. For goes hand in hand with changes in self-perception.
instance, Schmeichel (2007) found that individu- Depleted individuals are less optimistic about their
als who were depleted by regulating their emotions, abilities, have a lower sense of control, and are less
controlling their attention, writing in a nonnatural optimistic about the future (Fischer, Greitemeyer, &
way, or taking a working memory test performed Frey, 2007). Indeed, depleted individuals set lower
more poorly on subsequent tests of working mem- standards for themselves and had less confidence in
ory span, reverse digit span, and response inhibi- their ability to reach a goal than nondepleted indi-
tion. Basic cognitive processes appear to be affected viduals (DeBono & Muraven, in press). Optimistic
by depletion; the affected tests are considered to perspectives and positive illusions are apparently
require substantial executive control and response not automatic but instead require the individual to
inhibition. This decrease in mental efficiency appar- override doubts and negativity. These findings need
ently carries over to higher order functioning as to be better integrated into the idea of “automatic
well, as depleted individuals do worse on tests of egotism” (Paulhus, Graf, & Van Selst, 1989), as well
logic and reasoning, reading comprehension, and as the findings that depletion leads to greater heu-
a general test of fluid cognitive functioning than ristic processing. They suggest that processes that
nondepleted individuals (Schmeichel, Vohs, & consider egotism and self-enhancement automatic
Baumeister, 2003; see also Shamosh & Gray, 2007). either need to be revised or that depletion contrib-
Depletion did not affect performance on a test of utes to a decline in positive illusions in a novel way.
general knowledge or memorization and recall— In short, the underlying mechanism of maintain-
tests that are presumed to require less higher order ing illusions and why it is vulnerable to depletion
cognitive functioning. requires attention.
As would be expected from the observed changes in
cognition, depletion appears to affect decision making Physiological Markers of Depletion
as well. In general, it appears that depleted individuals Finally, research on biological markers of effort
take greater risks, make poorer decisions, and fail to and motivation similarly point to decreased cog-
consider all alternatives as well as nondepleted indi- nitive control among depleted individuals. For
viduals. For instance, Freeman and Muraven (2010) example, Bray et al. (2008) measured electromyo-
found that people who had to control their attention graphic (EMG) activation in depleted and nonde-
by ignoring information presented at the bottom of pleted individuals as they isometrically squeezed a
a video screen subsequently made more pumps on handgrip. Consistent with previous research (e.g.,
the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (Lejuez et al., 2003), Muraven et al., 1998), depleted individuals were not
a measure of risk taking. The link between negative able to hold the handgrip as long as nondepleted

 ego depl etion


individuals. Moreover, depleted individuals had self-control fails because individuals hold certain
greater neuromuscular activation than nondepleted beliefs about how self-control should operate. The
individuals, despite no differences in maximum biological account, on the other hand, suggests that
strength. This indicates that the depletion is not ego depletion is more than a metaphor and actu-
the same as reduced motivation (which presumably ally represents the loss of crucial biological resources
would lead to reduced maximal output) and also needed for the success of self-control. Ultimately,
represents increased effort as if the person needs to as with many dichotomies, the truth may lie in the
overcome a motivational deficit. integration of these accounts.
A similar conclusion can be drawn from Seger-
strom and Nes (2007), who found that resisting eat- Expectancies
ing cookies led to greater heart rate variability than Arguments for the expectancy account for the
resisting eating carrots and this heart rate variability depletion effect suggest that individuals hold beliefs
correlated with persistence on a subsequent anagram that self-control is limited and therefore after exert-
task. This suggests that exerting self-control requires ing self-control, they expect to fail in subsequent
the mobilization of effort. The inability to maintain attempts at self-control. For instance, individuals
that effort over time may help explain how depletion who felt that self-control was limited and depletes
arises. Indeed, that is the argument raised by Wright a limited resource were more affected by the ini-
and colleagues (Wright et al., 2007; Wright, Martin, tial self-control demand than individuals who did
& Bland, 2003; Wright, Stewart, & Barnett, 2008): not subscribe to such a belief (e.g., Job, Dweck, &
High levels of fatigue require increased mobilization Walton, 2010; Martijn, Tenbuelt, Merckelbach,
of effort (as indexed by cardiovascular output), but Dreezens, & de Vries, 2002). The researchers argued
when the effort required is perceived to be too great, that this suggests that the depletion effect springs
then all efforts cease (Stewart, Wright, Azor Hui, & from expectancies about the nature of self-control.
Simmons, 2009). Indeed, in subsequent research, they found that
Intriguingly, research using electroencephalo- individuals who were not paying attention to their
graphic (EEG) methodology suggests that depletion self-control efforts exhibited less of a decline in self-
can also be measured by changes in the event- control than individuals who were not distracted
related potential of error-related negativity (Inzlicht (Alberts, Martijn, Nievelstein, Jansen, & de Vries,
& Gutsell, 2007). Error-related negatively has been 2008). This leads credence to the idea that some of
linked to preconscious error monitoring and correc- the effects of exerting self-control on subsequent self-
tion and thus may be related to cognitive control control performance are psychologically mediated
over behavior. Individuals who had to control their and based on expectations of self-control demands.
emotions exhibited weaker error-related negativity Similarly, Clarkson et al. (2010) found that
signal while working on a Stroop task as compared people’s perceived levels of depletion predicted their
to individuals in a control condition. Moreover, per- performance on tasks that required self-control.
formance on the Stroop was related to magnitude of Depleted (or not depleted) individuals were given
the error-related negativity signal, so that this neural (false) feedback about this depleting task that led
signal mediated the link between initial self-control them to attribute their resources to external or inter-
exertion and subsequent self-control performance. nal sources. For instance, participants crossed off the
In short, depletion may affect neurological func- letter “e” that is next to or one away from another
tioning and may be tied to specific changes in the vowel (those in the control condition simply crossed
neural system used for conflict monitoring. off all e’s). Crossed with this, participants were told
that the color of the paper could either “exhaust
How Depletion Operates and deplete their ability to attend to information”
These biological markers of depletion provide or “energize and replenish one’s ability to attend to
some insight into how and why prior acts of self- information” (p. 33, italics in original). In the low-
control lead to subsequent self-control failure. As depletion condition, the replenishment feedback
noted earlier, most research has been founded on led to greater persistence on a subsequent task than
the idea that depletion reduces individuals’ ability to the depletion feedback. This pattern was reversed
inhibit behaviors. The exact process underlying that in the high-depletion condition. In short, people’s
effect is still an area of active investigation, with two perception of their level of self-control resource was
main theoretical lines. The first focuses on a moti- a predictor of their subsequent self-control perfor-
vation or expectancy account, which suggests that mance regardless of their actual level of resource.

mur aven 


In summary, the expectancy account of deple- saw the same clip but did not have to ignore the
tion suggests that people fail at self-control because words. This change in glucose correlated with subse-
they believe it should fail. Most people apparently quent self-control performance. Later experiments
subscribe to the belief that self-control is a limited showed that this decline in self-control performance
resource and after exerting self-control this belief after exerting self-control could be negated, however,
is typically activated, which leads to poorer self- by the ingestion of glucose (in the form of orange
control subsequently. This may explain many of the juice). Given that these patterns closely mirror the
outcomes described earlier, although it may have predictions of the ego strength model, it seems likely
greater difficulty in explaining situations in which that some of the observed effects are being driven
the exertion or need for self-control is not apparent by the depletion of glucose in the brain. Sugar only
(e.g., cognitive performance) nor does it explain why improves the performance of depleted individu-
seemingly difficult tasks (Muraven et al., 1998), like als and has no effect on nondepleted individuals,
solving math problems, does not lead to a decline in which suggests that depletion may be related to
self-control performance. This model suggests that reduced levels of sugar (Masicampo & Baumeister,
people have a finely attuned sense of what requires 2008; also found in dogs by Miller et al., 2010).
self-control and what does not. This study also showed that the ingestion of a non-
nutritive sugar substitute (Splenda) had no effect on
Biology depletion, which further indicates that the effects
There are also some persuasive arguments that are not simply due to expectations or merely drink-
the effects of depletion may be biologically medi- ing a pleasant drink. Denson et al. (2010) replicated
ated. In particular, there is evidence that levels of these findings in a study that looked at the effects of
glucose, particularly in the brain, may also explain ego depletion on aggression.
the decline in self-control performance after exert- In short, there are persuasive arguments that
ing self-control. Glucose is the primary source of exerting self-control may require and deplete glu-
energy for all brain activity and therefore a decline cose and this drop in glucose may drive the decline
in glucose may negatively affect executive function- in self-control performance. This helps to explain
ing (Siesjö, 1978). For instance, low levels of glu- the specificity of the depletion on self-control and
cose are related to poorer cognitive functioning in executive control and may fit well with the physi-
both rats (McNay, McCarty, & Gold, 2001) and ological effects associated with depletion. However,
humans (Benton, 1990; Martin & Benton, 1999). changes in glucose cannot be easily integrated with
More recent research has directly linked glucose to the fact that changes in expectancy for self-control
self-control, as individuals with lower levels of blood apparently also lead to changes in self-control per-
glucose have been found to engage in greater dis- formance. The biological account also leaves little
counting of the future. Consistent with this argu- room for motivation in depletion.
ment, the ingestion of sugar negates this drop in
self-control (Wang & Dvorak, 2010). Integration: Effects of Motivation and
Like the hypothesized ego strength, glucose can Conservation
be consumed faster than it can be replenished under Even if the depletion of glucose in the brain
heavy cognitive demands. Hence, exerting self- is a contributor to poorer self-control outcomes, the
control may deplete glucose, a vital fuel for cognitive final result must be psychologically mediated. It is
efforts. Indeed, recent research found that dogs which very unlikely that exerting self-control depletes all
were required to follow rules and resist a temptation available glucose so that self-control becomes impos-
had lower levels of glucose than dogs which were sible. Indeed, except in very rare and unusual cir-
not required to be obedient (Miller, Pattison, DeW- cumstances, most individuals who fail at self-control
all, Rayburn-Reeves, & Zentall, 2010). A similar do not lose control over all actions and become
effect has been found in humans after engaging completely animalistic. Individuals who exert self-
in tasks that likely require self-control (Fairclough control on a laboratory task and hence exhibit
& Houston, 2004; Gailliot, Baumeister, et al., poorer self-control subsequently do not urinate on
2007). For example, Gailliot, Baumeister, DeWall themselves but instead ask to be excused to go to
et al. (2007) reported that individuals who were the bathroom. Thus, a complete model of depletion
instructed to deliberately ignore words at the bot- needs to explain both the specificity of depletion
tom of a video clip suffered a greater drop in blood to self-control and how it can be moderated by
glucose levels from baseline than individuals who motivation.

 ego depl etion


Motivation plays a critical role in contributing to self-control
For instance, Muraven, Pogarsky, and Shmueli failures, the question then arises why past self-
(2006) found that depleted individuals were more control efforts matter at all. Further research, based
likely to lie and cheat on a laboratory task than non- on the idea of the conservation of limited resources,
depleted individuals. However, this was only true suggests they do.
when the perceived probability of getting caught
was low. When the odds of the experimenter discov- Conservation
ering the deviance were high, depleted individuals In particular, if self-control requires glucose or
were no more likely to cheat than nondepleted indi- other limited resources, it makes sense to use this
viduals. Likewise, Wan and Sternthal (2008) found resource as wisely as possible. People should be
that depleted individuals who were encouraged to judicious in how and when they exert self-control,
engage in self-monitoring by being given a clock so they can have resources for future demands or
while working on a persistence task worked as long emergencies. This self-control resource can be com-
as nondepleted individuals. As self-monitoring has pared to other limited resources, such as money. The
been found to increase motivation and goal adher- sensible person keeps a cushion of money in his or
ence (Carver & Scheier, 1998), it is likely that this her checking account, to pay for unexpected events.
self-monitoring feedback led to increased motiva- Moreover, consistent with prospect theory (Tversky
tion that negated the effects of depletion. & Kahneman, 1981), the less money one has, the
Even more directly, depleted individuals who were more the remaining money should be valued.
given an incentive to exert self-control, in the form Analogously, individuals should be concerned
of either money, social acceptance, or moral expec- with conserving ego strength to the extent that it
tations, performed just as well on a subsequent self- is perceived to be a limited resource. Depletion of
control task as participants who were not depleted this resource, through the exercise of self-control,
(Muraven & Slessareva, 2003). For instance, when should heighten this desire to conserve the remain-
paid a cent per cup, individual who had to suppress ing resource. It then follows that individuals who
their emotional reaction to a humorous video clip are motivated to conserve ego strength may be less
drank less of a vinegar-flavored drink as compared likely to exert self-control, which therefore leads to
to individuals who simply watched the video with poorer self-control performance. This may explain
no instructions to control their emotional reaction. why depleted individuals typically perform more
On the other hand, when the incentive for drinking poorly on tasks that matter less to them, but per-
was high (25 cents per cup), individuals who had form just as well as nondepleted people on impor-
to suppress their emotional reaction drank just as tant, self-relevant, or externally motivated tasks.
much of the sour drink as individuals who did not Muraven, Shmueli, and Burkley (2006; see rep-
suppress their reaction. lication by Tyler & Burns, 2009) tested this idea
The motivation can apparently be unconsciously by manipulating participants’ expectations for the
activated as well. For instance, Alberts et al. (2007) future. If people expect to exert self-control in
found that depleted individuals who were given the future, their motivation to conserve should be
primes related to persistence (either by unscram- increased; this should be especially likely if their ego
bling sentences with persistence words in them strength was already depleted. In one experiment,
or seeing a screensaver with motivational images) participants first had to control a well-learned pat-
performed better than depleted individuals not tern by typing a paragraph without hitting the “e”
given these primes. Likewise, thinking of good self- key (participants in the control condition just typed
control exemplars led to better self-control among the paragraph as they saw it). They were then told
depleted individuals than thinking of a neutral that they would take two more tests. The first was
example (Martijn et al., 2007). Even the mere sym- a Stroop test, where they would have to state the
bolic presence of family members appears to lead to font color of word. After that, they would have to
better self-control in depleted individuals (Stillman, solve anagrams that were either described as requir-
Tice, Fincham, & Lambert, 2009). ing them to “think hard” (low self-control) or
The results indicate that people can overcome “override impulses” (high self-control). Participants
depletion if sufficiently motivated. Thus, a reduc- who had to exert self-control in the first part of the
tion in glucose levels may increase the likelihood of experiment and who expected to exert self-control
self-control failure, but only when the individual in the future exhibited poorer self-control on the
is insufficiently motivated. Given that motivation Stroop task as compared to those who did not exert

mur aven 


self-control in the past or those who did not expect (Gollwitzer & Brandstätter, 1997) should help in
to exert self-control in the future. the elimination of ego depletion. Implementation
Further evidence for conservation came from intentions help automatize behaviors by creating a
participants’ actual performance on the final task. clear link between when, where, and how an indi-
In particular, how long they persisted on difficult vidual will strive for a goal. This helps reduce the
and frustrating anagrams before quitting. There was self-control demand of a situation, as it forges an
a negative correlation between Stroop performance association between a cue and a response, thereby
and time spent on the anagrams, suggesting a trade- reducing the need for conscious control.
off in resource use. That is, worse performance on In an examination of the effects of implemen-
the Stroop (which would suggest conserving) was tation intentions on depletion, participants who
associated with greater self-control on the anagram. created implementation intentions for the Stroop
Janssen, Fennis, and Pruyn (2010) found a similar task (e.g., “As soon as I see the word I will ignore
effect: Depleted individuals who were warned about its meaning”) subsequently persisted longer on a
an upcoming persuasive attempt conserved strength frustrating task than individuals who did the same
and hence generated better counterarguments and Stroop task without the benefit of creating an imple-
resisted the compliance more than depleted indi- mentation intention (Webb & Sheeran, 2003). In a
vidual who were not forewarned. second study, the opposite of this effect was found:
In short, people appear to manage their self- Depleted individuals who created implementation
control resources based on their past efforts and intentions for a Stroop task read the list of words
future demands. The desire to conserve strength faster than depleted individuals who did not cre-
can help both explain the specificity of the deple- ate a plan. Making a plan apparently reduced the
tion effect to self-control and how motivation and resources required for self-control, so that tasks were
expectancies can moderate this effect. The desire to less depleting and less affected by depletion.
conserve should not be necessarily interpreted as a
conscious process, however. There is very limited Rest and Replenishment
evidence that people are aware of their self-control There clearly must be some way to recover lost
states; instead, there appears to be a complex process resources. However, to date, this topic has not
of unconscious weighing of alternatives. Further received extensive attention. It is likely that rest
work is necessary to understand how these motiva- from exerting self-control is one way in which
tional processes work together to lead to self-control resources are recovered. For instance, Shiffman et al.
failures. (1996) reported that although the urge to smoke
is strongest in the morning, most lapses occur in
Moderators of Depletion the evening. This is consistent with the idea that in
This conservation model of self-control failure the morning people are rested and therefore have
points the way toward understanding when self- the strength to deal with their urges. Later in the
control is more likely to fail and when it is less day, however, more strength has been depleted and
likely. Hence, there may be processes that moderate therefore their ability to resist the temptation to
the link between depletion and self-control failure. smoke has been diminished. Research that focused
That is, there are some situations in which the link more directly on depletion on a smaller scale found
between depletion of ego strength and final self- a similar pattern (Tyler & Burns, 2008). Partici-
control performance is weakened (or strengthened). pants who had a 10-minute break between the first
These moderators may give some further insight self-control task and the subsequence measure per-
into how depletion works and some limitation to formed better than participants who did not have
our self-control. the break and equal to nondepleted individuals.
A similar effect was found for participants who were
Automatization induced into relaxing between tasks.
Intuitively, anything that reduces the self- It may be possible to accelerate this recovery
control demand on a behavior should reduce how process through positive affect (Tice, Baumeister,
depleting it is. Indeed, as noted earlier, there is a Shmueli, & Muraven, 2007). Participants who were
relationship between how much self-control a task depleted by resisting the temptation of cookies and
required and the subsequent decline in self-control candy failed to persist as long on a frustrating task
performance (e.g., Muraven et al., 2002). Hence, as compared to those who had to resist the tempta-
it was suggested that implementation intentions tion of eating radishes. However, if they watched

 ego depl etion


a comedy between resisting the food and persist- better self-control subsequently as compared to
ing, those who could not eat the cookies persisted people who were told that although they were free
just as long as those who could not eat the radishes. to choose, the experimenter really needed them to
The general conclusion is that positive experiences select a particular activity. This effect was found to be
negate the effects of ego depletion and the effects are mediated by feelings of self-determination, but not
not driven by arousal and distraction but are spe- mood.
cific to tasks that require self-control. The research- People’s own reasons for exerting self-control
ers argued that positive affect may help to replenish can have a similar effect. A plate of cookies was
lost ego strength. It may do this directly, by serving placed in front of participants, with the instructions
as a resource, or it may just increase motivation or to please not eat them unless absolutely necessary
willingness to exert self-control. That is, it might be (Muraven, 2008a). They were then asked why they
like giving coffee to a tired person—it gets him or did not eat the cookies, to measure their feelings of
her going for a while, but a crash is inevitable, or self-determination. People who did not eat the cook-
it might be like getting a good night’s sleep. Fur- ies for more self-determined reasons (e.g., because
ther empirical research is required to differentiate it matters to me) exhibited better self-control by
between these accounts. squeezing a handgrip longer, as compared to those
Finally, affirming the self (Steele, 1988) appears who did not eat the cookies for more extrinsic rea-
to help negate the effects of depletion (Schmeichel sons (e.g., the experimenter would get mad at me).
& Vohs, 2009). Individuals who were depleted by These results suggest that autonomously driven
having to write a story without using any words self-control is less depleting than self-control that is
containing the letter “a” or “n” removed their hand compelled by others or the situation. Further research
from ice water sooner than participants who wrote suggested that this outcome may be driven by the
a story without such restrictions. However, if the replenishment effect described earlier. Participants
depleted individuals were given the chance to rank instructed to avoid thinking about a white bear by
11 values and personal characteristics in order of per- a warm, open, and friendly experimenter who tried
sonal importance, this effect disappeared: Depleted to engage participants as a vital contributor to the
individual held their hand in the water as long as research project subsequently exhibited better self-
nondepleted individuals. The effects were not related control on a dependent measure of self-control than
to changes in mood. Instead, the researchers argued participants who were instructed by a more distant
that the self-affirmation led individuals to consider and cold experimenter who treated participants like a
abstract, long-range outcomes, which improved “cog in the machine” (Muraven, Gagné, & Rosman,
their self-control performance. 2008; see also Muraven, Rosman, & Gagné, 2007).
The participants in the autonomy supportive condi-
Autonomy tion had greater feelings of subjective vitality (Nix,
Perhaps related to these replenishment findings Ryan, Manly, & Deci, 1999; Ryan & Frederick,
is research on the effects of autonomy support on 1997), a positive state of aliveness and energy that
depletion. The idea of autonomy support is that arises from acting in self-actualizing ways. These
some situations encourage and are more support- feelings of vitality mediated the link between experi-
ive of behaviors that are intrinsic and self-driven, mental condition and self-control outcomes, so that
whereas others take that feeling away (Ryan & the reason why people in the autonomy supportive
Deci, 2000). Time pressures, external rewards, and condition exhibited less depletion was because they
authority figures cause individuals to feel com- felt more vital. This is consistent with the replenish-
pelled to act, which in turn diminishes the extent ment idea, and it further suggests that self-control
to which the behavior feels intrinsic, genuine, and behaviors that are associated with positive states
self-motivated. should lead to less depletion of strength.
Several studies have clearly shown self-control
that feels compelled by the situation is much more Building Strength
depleting than self-control that feels more autono- The research described earlier focuses primarily
mously driven. For instance, Moller, Deci, and Ryan on the short-term effects of exerting self-control.
(2006) gave some participants a choice between sev- A quick summary would suggest that people act as
eral different activities and then measured their self- if self-control is a muscle, which gets fatigued with
control performance. They found that people given use. This fatigue effect is moderated by several dif-
such a choice without any constraints exhibited ferent processes, and it may be related to glucose

mur aven 


levels in the brain, as well as beliefs that self-control amount of dental floss and toothpaste used), and
is limited. reported better health-related behaviors.
Muscles do get fatigue through work, much like Recent research by Muraven (2010a, 2010b) has
the effects observed with self-control. However, further extended these findings to make it clear that
muscles also get stronger, providing they are worked the effects of practicing self-control are above and
hard and frequently and rest is taken. Is the same beyond any effects expected from expectation or
true with self-control? That is, can people strengthen self-efficacy. Smokers who were interested in quit-
their self-control muscle by exercising their self- ting were assigned one of four tasks to practice for
control? The evidence suggests that they can. 2 weeks before beginning a cessation attempt. Two
In the first study on this process, Muraven, of these conditions required self-control (avoid eat-
Baumeister, and Tice, (1999) asked participants to ing sweets and squeeze a handgrip exercise for as
practice self-control by either maintaining a diary long as possible twice a day) and two did not (main-
of food intake, keeping good posture, or regulating tain a diary of any time they exerted self-control and
their moods to avoid negative emotions as much as work on difficult math problems). Consistent with
possible. The control group had no special instruc- previous research, smokers who practiced tasks that
tions and went about their daily life. Participants’ required self-control remained abstinent longer than
handgrip squeezing time (relative to their baseline) smokers who practiced tasks that did not require
after engaging self-control was assessed at the start self-control. Moreover, the control tasks evoked
of the study and again after they had practiced their awareness of self-control, increased self-monitoring,
assigned task for 2 weeks. Those who practiced self- increased self-efficacy, and participants expected
control were less affected by the depleting task as these tasks to be helpful in their cessation attempt.
compared to those who did not practice self-control This means that the effects of practicing self-control
and the effects were stronger for participants who on subsequent improvements in self-control are
practiced more. This is evidence that practicing self- above and separate from the smokers’ expectation
control can increase self-control endurance, so that that it should help them quit smoking, improve-
people are less affected by depletion. Put another ments in self-efficacy, or greater self-monitoring.
way, practicing self-control increased their stamina, Put another way, practicing self-control has a direct
so that they were able to exert self-control even effect on subsequent self-control performance.
when already fatigued.
Comparably, research by Oaten and colleagues Conclusion
(Oaten & Cheng, 2006a, 2006b, 2007), as well as The depletion model of self-control suggests that
Gailliot and colleagues (Gailliot, Plant, Butz, & self-control is bounded by a limited resource that
Baumeister, 2007) found that practicing self-control gets depleted with use. A growing body of research,
can also increase self-control power. That is, even in in a variety of domains, suggests that after exert-
nondepleted states, participants who practiced self- ing self-control, individuals have greater difficulty
control exhibited better self-control outcomes. This resisting subsequent self-control demands as this
is the equivalent of strength, so that after practicing resource is taxed. Although this resource may be
self-control individuals could overcome more pow- biologically mediated (possibly glucose), the process
erful impulses. For instance, in Hui et al. (2009), of self-control failure is also psychologically driven,
participants either engaged in a strong training pro- as individuals use social cues in motivating them-
gram (work on the Stroop task for 5 minutes twice selves to exert self-control. It appears that the pro-
daily for 2 weeks and rinse with a mouthwash that cess of depletion can be moderated by individuals’
produces a powerful burning sensation) or a weak mood, feelings toward the self-control activity, and
training program (no conflict between ink color ability to recover lost resources.
and word; diluted mouthwash). At the end of this The model suggests that managing this resource
training, participants returned to the laboratory and is vital to the success of self-control. Given the wide
engaged in several tasks that required self-control. range of behaviors that have been shown to be
As compared to those who had no training or those affected by depletion and depleting in themselves,
who had the weak training, those who underwent including high-order cognition, controlling aggres-
the strong training held their hand in ice water sig- sion, getting along with others, regulating moods,
nificantly longer. They also performed better on a and resisting the temptations of sex, food, and drugs,
visual search task that requires regulating attention clearly a better idea of how self-control operates is
and concentration, had better dental care (based on critical at the personal and societal level.

 ego depl etion


Future Directions Alberts, H. J. E. M., Martijn, C., Nievelstein, F., Jansen, A., &
de Vries, N. K. (2008). Distracting the self: Shifting atten-
Indeed, the findings on building strength and
tion prevents ego depletion. Self and Identity, 7, 322–334.
how self-control is moderated by motivation may Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske,
point the way for further research. A better under- & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4 ed.,
standing of how processes underlying the improve- pp. 680–740). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.
ments observed after practicing self-control is critical Baumeister, R. F. (2005). The cultural animal: Human nature,
meaning, and social life. New York: Oxford University Press.
to both advancing theory and crafting the best inter-
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M.
ventions. In particular, the process of building (1998). Ego-depletion: Is the active self a limited resource?
strength should probably be tied in the conserva- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.
tion model that links the biological and motivation Benton, D. (1990). The impact of increasing blood glucose on
accounts of strength. Such a complete theory should psychological functioning. Biological Psychology, 30, 13–19.
Bray, S. R., Ginis, K. A., Hicks, A. L., & Woodgate, J. (2008).
help in predicting which tasks will lead to the most
Effects of self-regulatory strength depletion on muscular per-
improvement in self-control, how long they should formance and emg activation. Psychophysiology, 45, 337–343.
be practiced, and how often. Brown, J. M. (1998). Self-regulation and the addictive behav-
The conservation model also would benefit from iors. In W. R. Miller & N. Heather (Eds.), Treating addictive
additional research to refine its predictions. For behaviors (2nd ed., pp. 61–73). New York: Plenum.
Bruyneel, S. D., Dewitte, S., Franses, P. H., & Dekimpe, M. G.
instance, it is not clear how people judge their future
(2009). I felt low and my purse feels light: Depleting mood
and past self-control demands, as well as introspect regulation attempts affect risk decision making. Journal of
their levels of resource. This is an area ripe for inves- Behavioral Decision Making, 22, 153–170.
tigation, as it may illuminate how people process Bruyneel, S. D., Dewitte, S., Vohs, K. D., & Warlop, L. (2006).
information critical to self-control, which would Repeated choosing increases susceptibility to affective prod-
uct features. International Journal of Research in Marketing,
lead to more powerful theories of self-control. Such
23, 215–225.
a model of conservation might also better explain Burkley, E. (2008). The role of self-control in resistance to persua-
the processes underlying the replenishment effect of sion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 419–431.
positive affect, as well as the findings that autono- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of
mously driven self-control is less depleting than self- behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Ciarocco, N. J., Sommer, K. L., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001).
control that feels compelled.
Ostracism and ego depletion: The strains of silence. Personal-
The findings that depletion leads to greater pas- ity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1156–1163.
sivity and changes in time estimation likely need to Clarkson, J. J., Hirt, E. R., Jia, L., & Alexander, M. B. (2010).
be better integrated into the literature as well. These When perception is more than reality: The effects of perceived
results may help to explain a wide variety of out- versus actual resource depletion on self-regulatory behavior.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, 29–46.
comes and may present an opportunity for creating
DeBono, A., & Muraven, M. (in press). Keeping it real: Self-
a more unified theory of depletion. Ultimately, it control depletion increases accuracy but decreases confidence
may be possible to link these findings to changes for performance. Journal of Applied Social Psychology.
in brain operation, for example, whether glucose is DeBono, A., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. (2011). Rude and inap-
connected to passivity and a lack of motivation. propriate: The role of self-control in following social norms.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 136–146.
In the end, a comprehensive theory of self-
Denson, T. F., von Hippel, W., Kemp, R. I., & Teo, L. S. (2010).
control may be of profound practical and theoreti- Glucose consumption decreases impulsive aggression in
cal importance. Many of the problems facing people response to provocation in aggressive individuals. Journal of
and society arise from or could be addressed from Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 1023–1028.
improvements in self-control. Moreover, because Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic
and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social
self-control is so critical to what makes us human
Psychology, 56, 5–18.
and enables us to reach our full potential, a theory DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Gailliot, M. T., & Maner,
of self-control may help in explaining other impor- J. K. (2008). Depletion makes the heart grow less helpful:
tant aspects of the human condition. Helping as a function of self-regulatory energy and genetic
relatedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34,
References 1653–1662.
Ackerman, J. M., Goldstein, N. J., Shapiro, J. R., & Bargh, J. A. DeWall, C. N., Baumeister, R. F., Stillman, T. F., & Gailliot, M.
(2009). You wear me out: The vicarious depletion of self- T. (2007). Violence restrained: Effects of self-regulation and
control. Psychological Science, 20, 326–332. its depletion on aggression. Journal of Experimental Social
Alberts, H. J. E. M., Martijn, C., Greb, J., Merckelbach, H., & Psychology, 43, 62–76.
de Vries, N. K. (2007). Carrying on or giving in: The role Faber, R. J. (1992). Money changes everything: Compulsive buy-
of automatic processes in overcoming ego depletion. British ing from a biopsychosocial perspective. American Behavioral
Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 383–399. Scientist, 35, 809–819.

mur aven 


Fairclough, S. H., & Houston, K. (2004). A metabolic measure Hui, S-K., Wright, R., Stewart, C., Simmons, A., Eaton, B., &
of mental effort. Biological Psychology, 66, 177–190. Nolte, R. (2009). Performance, cardiovascular, and health
Fennis, M., Janssen, L., & Vohs, K. D. (2009). Acts of benevo- behavior effects of an inhibitory strength training interven-
lence: A limited resource account of compliance with chari- tion. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 419–434.
table requests. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 906–924. Inzlicht, M., & Gutsell, J. N. (2007). Running on empty:
Finkel, E. J., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Self-control and accom- Neural signals for self-control failure. Psychological Science,
modation in close relationships: An interdependence analy- 18, 933–937.
sis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 263–277. Inzlicht, M., McKay, L., & Aronson, J. (2007). Stigma as ego
Finkel, E. J., Campbell, W. K., Brunell, A. B., Dalton, A. depletion: How being the target of prejudice affects self-
N., Scarbeck, S. J., & Chartrand, T. L. (2006). High- control. Psychological Science, 18, 262–269.
maintenance interaction: Inefficient social coordination Janssen, L., Fennis, B. M., & Pruyn, A. T. H. (2010). Fore-
impairs self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- warned is forearmed: Conserving self-control strength to
chology, 91, 263–277. resist social influence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., & Frey, D. (2007). Ego depletion ogy, 46, 911–921.
and positive illusions: Does the construction of positivity Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion:
require regulatory resources? Personality and Social Psychology Is it all in your head? Psychological Science, 21, 1686–1693.
Bulletin, 33, 1306–1321. Kahan, D., Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2003). Conformity
Fischer, P., Greitemeyer, T., & Frey, D. (2008). Self-regulation and dietary disinhibition: A test of the ego-strength model
and selective exposure: The impact of depleted self-regulation of self-regulation. International Journal of Eating Disorders,
resources on confirmatory information processing. Journal of 33, 165–171.
Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 382–395. Kanfer, F. H., & Karoly, P. (1972). Self-control: A behavioristic
Freeman, N., & Muraven, M. (2010). Self-control depletion excursion into the lion’s den. Behavior Therapy, 3, 398–416.
leads to increased risk taking. Social Psychological and Person- Leeman, R. F., O’Malley, S. S., White, M. A., & McKee, S. A.
ality Science, 1, 175–181. (2010). Nicotine and food deprivation decrease the ability to
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Wanke, M. (2008). When impulses resist smoking. Psychopharmacology, 212, 25–32.
take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and Lejuez, C. W., Aklin, W. M., Jones, H. A., Richards, J. B.,
implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and Strong, D. R., Kahler, C. W., & Read, J. P. (2003). The bal-
consumption behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, loon analogue risk task (BART) differentiates smokers and
47, 397–419. nonsmokers. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology,
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). Self-regulation and 11, 26–33.
sexual restraint: Dispositionally and temporarily poor self- Martijn, C., Alberts, H. J. E. M., Merckelbach, H., Havermans,
regulatory abilities contribute to failures at restraining sex- R., Huijts, A., & de Vries, N. K. (2007). Overcoming ego
ual behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, depletion: The influence of exemplar priming on self-control
173–186. performance. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37,
Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., 231–238.
Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., . . . Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Martijn, C., Tenbuelt, P., Merckelbach, H., Dreezens, E., &
Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: de Vries, N. K. (2002). Getting a grip on ourselves:
Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality Challenging expectancies about loss of energy after self-
and Social Psychology, 92, 325–336. control. Social Cognition, 20, 441–460.
Gailliot, M. T., Plant, E. A., Butz, D. A., & Baumeister, R. F. Martin, P. Y., & Benton, D. (1999). The influence of a glucose
(2007). Increasing self-regulatory strength can reduce the drink on a demanding working memory task. Physiology and
depleting effect of suppressing stereotypes. Personality and Behavior, 67, 69–74.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 281–294. Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Toward a physi-
Glass, D. C., Singer, J. E., & Friedman, L. N. (1969). Psychic ology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: Lemonade,
cost of adaptation to an environmental stressor. Journal of willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis. Psychological
Personality and Social Psychology, 12, 200–210. Science, 19, 255–260.
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation McNay, E. C., McCarty, R. C., & Gold, P. E. (2001). Fluctua-
intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality tions in brain glucose concentration during behavioral test-
and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199. ing: Dissociations between brain areas and between brain and
Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of blood. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 75, 325–337.
crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Mead, N. L., Baumeister, R. F., Gino, F., Schweitzer, M. E., &
Govorun, O., & Payne, B. K. (2006). Ego-depletion and preju- Ariely, D. (2009). Too tired to tell the truth: Self-control
dice: Separating automatic and controlled components. resource depletion and dishonesty. Journal of Experimental
Social Cognition, 24, 111–136. Social Psychology, 45, 594–597.
Heatherton, T. F., Striepe, M., & Wittenberg, L. (1998). Emo- Miller, H. C., Pattison, K. F., DeWall, C. N., Rayburn-Reeves,
tional distress and disinhibited eating: The role of the self. R., & Zentall, T. R. (2010). Self-control without a “self ”?
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 301–313. Psychological Science, 21, 534–538.
Hofmann, W., Rauch, W., & Gawronski, B. (2007). And Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M. L. (1989). Delay of
deplete us not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.
restraint, and self-regulatory resources as determinants of eat- Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and
ing behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Personality
497–504. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1024–1036.

 ego depl etion


Muraven, M. (2008a). Autonomous self-control is less depleting. Park, S. H., Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008). Implicit moti-
Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 763–770. vation to control prejudice moderates the effect of cognitive
Muraven, M. (2008b). Prejudice as self-control failure. Journal of depletion on unintended discrimination. Social Cognition,
Applied Social Psychology, 38, 314–333. 26, 401–419.
Muraven, M. (2010a). Building self-control strength: Practic- Paulhus, D. L., Graf, P., & Van Selst, M. (1989). Attentional
ing self-control leads to improved self-control performance. load increases the positivity of self-presentation. Social Cog-
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 465–468. nition, 7, 389–400.
Muraven, M. (2010b). Practicing self-control lowers the risk Pocheptsova, A., Amir, O., Dhar, R., & Baumeister, R. (2009).
of smoking lapse. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 24, Deciding without resources: Resource depletion and choice
446–452. in context. Journal of Marketing Research, 46, 344–355.
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and Richeson, J. A., & Shelton, J. (2003). When prejudice does
depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a not pay: Effects of interracial contact on executive function.
muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. Psychological Science, 14, 287–290.
Muraven, M., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1999). Richeson, J. A., & Trawalter, S. (2005). Why do interracial
Longitudinal improvement of self-regulation through prac- interactions impair executive function? A resource deple-
tice: Building self-control strength through repeated exercise. tion account. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88,
Journal of Social Psychology, 139, 446–457. 934–947.
Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., & Nienhaus, K. (2002). Self- Richeson, J. A., Trawalter, S., & Shelton, J. N. (2005). African
control and alcohol restraint: An initial application of the americans’ implicit racial attitudes and the depletion of exec-
self-control strength model. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, utive function after interracial interactions. Social Cognition,
16, 113–120. 23, 336–352.
Muraven, M., Collins, R. L., Shiffman, S., & Paty, J. A. (2005). Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory
Daily fluctuations in self-control demands and alcohol and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop-
intake. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 19, 140–147. ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
Muraven, M., Gagné, M., & Rosman, H. (2008). Helpful self- Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality,
control: Autonomy support, vitality, and depletion. Journal and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-
of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 573–585. being. Journal of Personality, 65, 529–565.
Muraven, M., Pogarsky, G., & Shmueli, D. (2006). Self-control Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating,
depletion and the general theory of crime. Journal of Quanti- and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for
tative Criminology, 22, 263–277. executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Gen-
Muraven, M., Rosman, H., & Gagné, M. (2007). Lack of auton- eral, 136, 241–255.
omy and self-control: Performance contingent rewards lead Schmeichel, B. J., Demaree, H. A., Robinson, J. L., & Pu, J.
to greater depletion. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 322–330. (2006). Ego depletion by response exaggeration. Journal of
Muraven, M., & Shmueli, D. (2006). The self-control costs Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 95–102.
of fighting the temptation to drink. Psychology of Addictive Schmeichel, B. J., Harmon-Jones, C., & Harmon-Jones, E.
Behaviors, 20, 154–160. (2010). Exercising self-control increases approach moti-
Muraven, M., Shmueli, D., & Burkley, E. (2006). Conserving vation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99,
self-control strength. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- 162–173.
ogy, 91, 524–537. Schmeichel, B. J., & Vohs, K. (2009). Self-affirmation and self-
Muraven, M., & Slessareva, E. (2003). Mechanisms of self- control: Affirming core values counteracts ego depletion.
control failure: Motivation and limited resources. Personality Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 770–782.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 894–906. Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003).
Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self- Intellectual performance and ego depletion: Role of the
control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. self in logical reasoning and other information processing.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 33–46.
Nix, G., Ryan, R. M., Manly, J. B., & Deci, E. L. (1999). Revi- Sedikides, C., & Skowronski, J. J. (1997). The symbolic self
talization through self-regulation: The effects of autonomous in evolutionary context. Personality and Social Psychology
and controlled motivation on happiness and vitality. Journal Review, 1, 80–102.
of Experimental Social Psychology, 35, 266–284. Segerstrom, S. C., & Nes, L. S. (2007). Heart rate variability
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006a). Improved self-control: The reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psycho-
benefits of a regular program of academic study. Basic and logical Science, 18, 275–281.
Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1–16. Shamosh, N. A., & Gray, J. R. (2007). The relation between
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2006b). Longitudinal gains in self- fluid intelligence and self-regulatory depletion. Cognition
regulation from regular physical exercise. British Journal of and Emotion, 21, 1833–1843.
Health Psychology, 11, 717–733. Shiffman, S., Paty, J. A., Gnys, M., Kassel, J. A., & Hickcox, M.
Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2007). Improvements in self-control (1996). First lapses to smoking: Within-subjects analysis of
from financial monitoring. Journal of Economic Psychology, real-time reports. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychol-
28, 487–501. ogy, 64, 366–379.
Ostafin, B. D., Marlatt, G. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Siesjö, B. K. (1978). Brain energy metabolism. Chichester,
Drinking without thinking: An implicit measure of alcohol England and New York: Wiley.
motivation predicts failure to control alcohol use. Behaviour Steele, C. M. (1988). The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustain-
Research and Therapy, 46, 1210–1219. ing the integrity of the self. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances

mur aven 


in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 261–302). experience of time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
New York, NY: Academic Press. ogy, 85, 217–230.
.Stewart, C. C., Wright, R. A., Azor Hui, S. K., & Simmons, Wallace, H. W., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). The effects of suc-
A. (2009). Outcome expectancy as a moderator of mental cess versus failure feedback on further self-control. Self and
fatigue influence on cardiovascular response. Psychophysiol- Identity, 1, 35–42.
ogy, 46, 1141–1149. Wan, E., & Sternthal, B. (2008). Regulating the effects of deple-
Stillman, T., Tice, D., Fincham, F., & Lambert, N. (2009). tion through monitoring. Personality and Social Psychology
The psychological presence of family improves self-control. Bulletin, 34, 32.
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 28, 498–529. Wang, X. T., & Dvorak, R. D. (2010). Sweet future. Psychological
Stucke, T. S., & Baumeister, R. F. (2006). Ego depletion Science, 21, 183–188.
and aggressive behavior: Is the inhibition of aggression a Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation inten-
limited resource? European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, tions help to overcome ego-depletion? Journal of Experimen-
1–13. tal Social Psychology, 39, 279–286.
Tice, D. M., Baumeister, R. F., Shmueli, D., & Muraven, M. Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L.
(2007). Restoring the self: Positive affect helps improve self- (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of
regulation following ego depletion. Journal of Experimental Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.
Social Psychology, 43, 379–384. Wheeler, S. C., Brinol, P., & Hermann, A. D. (2007). Resistance
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions to persuasion as self-regulation: Ego-depletion and its effects
and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458. on attitude change processes. Journal of Experimental Social
Tyler, J. M., & Burns, K. C. (2008). After depletion: The replen- Psychology, 43, 150–156.
ishment of the self ’s regulatory resources. Self and Identity, Wills, T. A., Sandy, J. M., & Yaeger, A. M. (2002). Moderators of
7, 305–321. the relation between substance use level and problems: Test
Tyler, J. M., & Burns, K. C. (2009). Triggering conservation of of a self-regulation model in middle adolescence. Journal of
the self ’s regulatory resources. Basic and Applied Social Psy- Abnormal Psychology, 111, 3–21.
chology, 31, 255–266. Wright, R. A., Junious, T. R., Neal, C., Avello, A., Graham, C.,
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., & Ciarocco, N. J. (2005). Herrmann, L., Junious, S., & Walton, N. (2007). Mental
Self-regulation and self-presentation: Regulatory resource fatigue influence on effort-related cardiovascular response:
depletion impairs impression management and effortful self- Difficulty effects and extension across cognitive perfor-
presentation depletes regulatory resources. Journal of Person- mance domains. Motivation and Emotion, 31,219–231.
ality and Social Psychology, 88, 632–657. Wright, R. A., Martin, R. E., & Bland, J. L. (2003). Energy
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. resource depletion, task difficulty, and cardiovascular
M., Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. M. (2008). Making choices response to a mental arithmetic challenge. Psychophysiology,
impairs subsequent self-control: A limited-resource account 40, 98–105.
of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. Wright, R. A., Stewart, C. C., & Barnett, B. R. (2008). Mental
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 883–898. fatigue influence on effort-related cardiovascular response:
Vohs, K. D., & Faber, R. (2007). Spent resources: Self-regulatory Extension across the regulatory (inhibitory)/non-regulatory
resource availability affects impulse buying. Journal of performance dimension. International journal of psychophysi-
Consumer Research, 33, 537–547. ology: official journal of the International Organization of
Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory Psychophysiology, 69, 127–133.
failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, Yovetich, N. A., & Rusbult, C. E. (1994). Accommodative
11, 243–254. behavior in close relationships: Exploring transformation
Vohs, K. D., & Schmeichel, B. J. (2003). Self-regulation and of motivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30,
extended now: Controlling the self alters the subjective 138–164.

 ego depl etion


CHAPTER

Flow
8
Susan A. Jackson

Abstract
The concept of flow is one that is central to understanding human motivation and the capacity of
humans to function optimally in the psychological realm. In this chapter, the concept of flow is defined
and described from its dimensional perspective. An overview of some of the research that has been
conducted on flow is provided, measurement options are explored, and the critical question of how
to facilitate this optimal psychological state is addressed.
Key Words: flow, optimal experience, flow measurement, facilitating flow

Introduction This chapter will overview flow, providing defi-


Ten years after Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi nition and description of this positive psychology
(2000) presented a rationale for the advancement of construct. Examples of the research that has been
positive psychology in the millennial issue of Ameri- conducted on flow will be overviewed, centering on
can Psychologist, the rebirth of a positive focus in psy- the themes of understanding flow, assessing flow,
chology has grown from strength to strength. In 2007, and factors found to be associated with flow. Future
The International Association for Positive Psychology research directions for this exciting motivational
(IPPA) was formed, and in 3 years, it has grown to concept will be explored.
a membership of 3,000, representing 70 countries.
Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi (2000) outlined three Defining Flow
areas central to the science of positive psychology: pos- Flow represents those moments when everything
itive subjective experience, positive individual traits, comes together to create a special state of absorption
and positive institutions. Seligman characterized the and enjoyment in what one is doing (Csikszentmi-
aim of positive psychology as “to catalyze a change in halyi, 1975, 1990). Flow occurs when one is totally
psychology from a preoccupation only with repairing involved in the task at hand. Characteristics that
the worst things in life to also building the best quali- identify a flow experience include the better-than-
ties in life” (Seligman, 2002, p. 3). The IPPA website average nature of the experience, with heightened
defines positive psychology as “the scientific study of focus and high levels of enjoyment being key fac-
what enables individuals and communities to thrive.” tors. Csikszentmihalyi (1990) considered flow to be
Flow was identified by Seligman and Csikszent- an optimal experience, and he has used the terms
mihalyi (2000) as a key construct in the area of “flow” and “optimal experience” interchangeably.
positive subjective experience, one of the three core Through the 35 years of insightful writings on
areas of study identified for the field of positive psy- flow by Csikszentmihalyi, this construct has come
chology. Thus, flow has found a home in positive to be universally regarded as a special psycho-
psychology. logical state, one that is intimately associated with


motivation and enjoyment. Complete involvement work together effortlessly, so that there is an intrin-
in a task at hand—for the sake of the absorbing sic experience of harmonious enjoyment (Jackson
experience itself—defines flow. This “immersion for & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). This leads to a feeling
the sake of doing it” links flow with intrinsic moti- of being so involved in the activity that nothing
vation, and the initial name given by Csiksentmi- else seems to matter and we continue in it “. . . even
halyi for flow (i.e., autotelic experience) translates at great cost, for the sheer sake of doing it”
as doing something for its own sake, and hence the (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990, p. 4).
links with intrinsic motivation are strong.
Flow can occur at different levels of complex- Dimensions of Flow
ity but, by definition, flow is intrinsically reward- Csikszentmihalyi (e.g., 1990) has conceptual-
ing, regardless of whether it involves a simple game ized the flow construct in terms of a number of
of throw and catch or a complicated and danger- dimensions. Nine dimensions have been articulated,
ous gymnastics routine. Csikszentmihalyi (1975) with more recent descriptions (e.g., Nakamura &
referred to the different levels of flow experience as Csikszentmihalyi, 2002) separating three of these
micro and macro flow. Micro flow experiences are dimensions into what have been described as pre-
those that occur in everyday life, whereas macro flow conditions for flow. The nine dimensions are, with
are those experiences associated with higher levels of the three preconditions listed first: challenge–skill
complexity and demand on the participant. balance, clear goals, unambiguous feedback, action-
Macro flow experiences are similar to the con- awareness merging, concentration on task, sense
cept of peak experience, first described by Abraham of control, loss of self-consciousness, time transfor-
Maslow in the 1960s. Maslow (1968, 1970, 1973) mation, and autotelic experience. Together, they
asked groups of people such questions as what was “the represent the optimal psychological state of flow;
single most joyous, happiest, most blissful moment of singly they signify conceptual elements of the flow
your life” (1973, p. 182). Maslow detailed the peak experience.
experience into 14 characteristics that reflected a vari-
ety of emotional and cognitive changes. These changes Challenge–Skill Balance
included a feeling of being detached from concerns, During the experience of flow, a dynamic bal-
strong concentration, an egoless and unselfish percep- ance exists between challenges in the situation and
tion, disorientation in time and space, and a feeling of a person’s skills. Challenges can be thought of as
life being meaningful, beautiful, and desirable. opportunities for actions, or goals, while skills are
Research has suggested that flow and peak expe- the capacities the individual has to produce desired
rience are overlapping constructs and can co-occur outcomes (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). Both the chal-
(Jackson, 1996, 2000; McInman & Grove, 1991; lenge of the situation and our skills to meet the chal-
Privette & Bundrick, 1997). Notwithstanding, the lenge need to be at personally high levels in order for
notion of peak experience is conceptually distinct us to be in a position to experience flow: “ . . . it is not
from the optimal experience model of flow. For enough for challenges to equal skills; both factors
example, the flow model consists of not only an need to be extending the person, stretching them
affective component but also several important cog- to new levels” (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999,
nitive components, as described in the following sec- p. 16). Critical to finding this balance is “what you
tion. Furthermore, the flow construct is grounded believe you can do” (p. 17) and how you perceive
in a multidimensional theory of optimal experience the situation, rather than the actual demands or an
with a substantive empirical base drawn from a vari- objective level of abilities per se. It is the perception
ety of life domains, such as work, school, leisure, and of the defined challenge that is critical to the occur-
sports (Kowal & Fortier, 1999). rence of flow.
Flow is an internal, conscious process that lifts
experience from the ordinary to the optimal. It is Action-Awareness Merging
the simultaneous experiencing of several positive This dimension involves a feeling of being “at
aspects that makes the flow experience so special one” with the activity being performed. It is through
(Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). Some of these total absorption in what one is doing (see later for
aspects, or dimensions of flow as they have come total concentration dimension of flow) that percep-
to be known, include total focus, involvement, and tion of oneness with the activity results, bringing
absorption in what one is doing, to the exclusion of harmony and peace to an active engagement with
all other thoughts and emotions. Mind and body a task. When people are asked to describe what it

 flow
feels like to be in flow, commentary on the merg- Sense of Control
ing of action and awareness is often central to their Another frequently mentioned flow characteristic
descriptions, exemplified in statements such as a is a feeling of being in control. Sometimes described
sense of ceasing to be aware of themselves as sepa- as a sense of infallibility, this empowering feeling frees
rate from their actions and experiences. This is often one from the all-too-frequent fear of failure that can
accompanied by a sense of effortless ease and flu- so easily creep into performance. Failure thoughts
ency in movement. are nowhere to be found during flow, enabling the
individual to positively approach the challenges at
Clear Goals hand.
Individuals describe the flow state as involving a Control, like the challenge–skills relationship, is
clear sense of knowing what it is they are supposed a delicately balanced component of flow. Although
to do. When in flow, this clarity of purpose occurs the perception of control is inherent to the experi-
on a moment-by-moment basis, keeping the per- ence, it is actually a finely balanced equation where
former fully connected to the task and responsive perceived skills are at a high level, but one com-
to appropriate cues. In flow, it is clear, moment by mensurate with the challenge. One must experience
moment, how one is doing. Goals provide a blue- challenge to experience flow; it is the possibility
print for what we need to do; then, while engaged of being in control that can keep flow alive. If the
in the activity, there is an ongoing awareness about feeling of being in control keeps going indefinitely,
what to do next as actions and goals become seam- then the scales have tipped in favor of skills over
lessly intertwined (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, challenge, and flow is lost.
1999).
Loss of Self-Consciousness
Unambiguous Feedback We can think of flow as non-self-conscious
Hand in hand with clear goals comes the pro- action. In flow, one is so totally absorbed in the
cessing of information on how performance is activity that there is no room for worry about self
progressing relative to these goals. When in flow, or about the evaluations of others. It is liberating to
feedback information is unambiguous and effort- be free of the voice within our head that questions
lessly processed in producing actions consistent whether we are living up to the standards that we
with the actor’s desires. perceive are important to be met. The losing of self-
Unambiguous feedback while undertaking an consciousness is thus one of the “hidden” benefits of
activity can be internal, such as kinaesthetic aware- flow, emanating from being fully present.
ness, or it can come from external sources. Both
types of feedback tell us how we are going and allow Time Transformation
us to appraise the successfulness of our actions in an Deep moments of flow seem to transform our
ongoing manner during the performance process. perception of time. For some, the experience is that
The unambiguous feedback provides a clear idea of time stops. For others, time seems to slow. Or it
the next action and enables us to know we are on may be that time seems to pass more quickly than
track and headed toward achieving our goals. expected. Because nothing else is entering our
awareness during flow, we can be surprised to find
Concentration on Task at Hand that significant time has passed while in this state.
Being totally connected to the task at hand epit- The intensity of focus may also contribute to per-
omizes the flow state, and it is one of its most fre- ceptions of time slowing, with a feeling of having
quently mentioned characteristics. When in flow, one all the time in the world to execute a move that is,
is totally focused in the present moment. There are no in reality, very much time limited. It is through the
extraneous thoughts, and the distractibility that often total absorption that occurs in flow that the pas-
accompanies involvement on any task is wonderfully sage of time can become perceptually transformed
absent. “In flow, there is no room for any thoughts in some way.
other than what you are doing and feeling right
at the moment, the ‘now’ ” (Jackson & Csikszentmi- Autotelic Experience
halyi, 1999, p. 25). Full and sustained concentration, Csikszentmihalyi (1990) coined the term “auto-
including the ability to exclude irrelevant or unhelp- telic experience” to describe the intrinsically rewarding
ful thoughts from consciousness, and tune into the encounter that flow is for the individual. As described
task at hand, characterizes this dimension. by Csikszentmihalyi, the word is derived from two

jac ks on 
Greek words that describe doing something for its According to this model of flow, first devel-
own sake: “auto” = self, and “telos” = goal. Flow is oped by Csikszentmihalyi (1975) when he initially
such an enjoyable experience that one is motivated to began investigating flow, it is the relative balance of
return to this state. Once experienced, flow becomes challenges and skills that defines whether an indi-
much sought after. Csikszentmihalyi described this vidual experiences flow, anxiety, apathy, relaxation,
dimension as the end result of the other eight flow or another psychological state. When the individual
dimensions. For many, flow is the defining motiva- perceives that he or she has the skills to match a
tion to keep pushing toward higher limits. It is gener- challenge, flow can occur. It is the person’s percep-
ally upon reflection that the autotelic aspect of flow is tion of the level of challenge and degree of skill—
realized and provides high motivation toward further and the balance between them—that is essential to
involvement. flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). When our perceived
Considered together, these nine dimensions of challenge of the activity is in balance with our
flow provide an optimal experience. Considerable perceived skill, we are setting ourselves up for an
consistency of flow experience has been found across experience of flow.
many different domains (see Csikszentmihalyi,
1990, 1997; Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, Growing the Knowledge Base on Flow:
1988a). The dimensions of flow provide a conceptu- Research Examples from 1975 to 2010
ally coherent framework for understanding optimal From enquiries into subjective experience, at
experience. times when everything “came together” during per-
formance of one’s chosen activity, Csikszentmihalyi
The Critical Balance of Challenges (1975) developed the concept of flow. He coined the
and Skills term flow to describe these experiences because of
A central consideration to facilitating an envi- the use of this word in the descriptions of absorb-
ronment conducive to flow is the existence of a ing encounters by research participants. From art-
challenging situation. Flow is defined by the appro- ists to rock climbers, surgeons to musicians, a sense
priate mix of challenges and skills in a situation, as of actions flowing from one moment to the next
described earlier in the dimension of challenge–skill prompted Csikszentmihalyi to adopt the term flow
balance. When the challenges of an activity are bal- for what he initially described as autotelic experi-
anced with the skills of the participant, flow can ences. Despite considerable diversity in settings where
occur. It is a delicate balance, as other relative levels people were interviewed about being in flow, there
of challenge and skill can bring about quite differ- was considerable consistency of responses regarding
ent experiential states. These relationships between what was felt during these moments that stood out
challenge and skill, and experience, are depicted in as being special, above-average experiences.
Figure 8.1. After the release of Beyond Boredom and Anxi-
ety in 1975, Csikszentmihalyi and colleagues began
exploring the flow concept at theoretical and empir-
CHALLENGE AXIS High
ical levels. There was interest in flow from a variety
Anxiety Quadrant of domains, and the research of these formative early
FLOW QUADRANT years is presented in Csikszentmihalyi and Csikszent-
mihalyi (1988a). One of the critical developments
during this period of research was the development
Low Average Hi h
High
SKILLS AXIS
of the Experience Sampling Method, a tool to sam-
ple everyday experience, which is described in the
section on “Measurement of Flow” in this chapter.
Apathy Quadrant Relaxation-
Boredom Quadrant Drawn to the flow concept after reading
Csikszentmihalyi’s (1975) portrayal of his early inves-
Low
tigations into this experience, and relating these expe-
riences to her best moments as an athlete, Jackson’s
Fig. 8.1. Model of the flow state. (Adapted with permission early research into flow was also primarily interview
from S.A. Jackson & M. Csikszentmihalyi, 1999, Flow in based, but focused on athletes, to assess the relevance
sports, Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, p. 37. Adapted from
M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csikszentmihalyi, 1988, Optimal
of flow to sport experiences. Elite athletes were the
experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness, with per- initial focus, because of their expected familiar-
mission of Cambridge University Press.) ity with optimal performance and flow experiences

 flow
(Jackson, 1996). More generally, sport has been consistency in flow experience descriptions with
recognised as an excellent setting in which to Csikszentmihalyi’s model and with the figure skat-
examine flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990; Jackson & ing sample from the 1992 study. Antecedent and
Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). preventative flow factors were consistent with the
Sport offers the opportunity to do something skaters’ experiences. These factors influencing flow
better than it has been done before (Csikszentmi- are described in a later section of this chapter.
halyi, 1990), and so, once having made a choice to Grove and Lewis (1996) studied a noncompeti-
engage in a sport activity, a focused mindset gen- tive sport sample of circuit training participants and
erally results. Furthermore, the experience of sport found an association between hypnotic susceptibil-
is generally one of enjoyment—people engage in ity and “flow-like” states. Highly hypnotic suscep-
sport for the quality of experience it provides, as tible participants showed greater changes in their
explained by Jackson and Csikszentmihalyi (1999, flow-like states than those with low susceptibility.
p. 4), “Contrary to what happens in most of life, Catley and Duda (1997) tested psychological
sport can offer a state of being that is so rewarding states both before and during a golf round and found
one does it for no other reason than to be a part of consistency with Jackson’s (1995) qualitative find-
it.” Thus, sport can be considered an autotelic activ- ings of factors influencing flow. Catley and Duda
ity, and this also makes it an environment condu- found confident readiness, positive focus, and pes-
cive to flow. A third advantage to studying flow in simism had the strongest relationships with flow.
sport is that challenges and skills are in-built to the In a confirmation of the findings of Jackson
domain, are easily observable, and can be modified. and Roberts (1992) with regard to flow and task
Thus, in this chapter, the research that has investi- orientation, Kowal and Fortier (1999) found ath-
gated flow in sport, and other performance-based letes motivated by intrinsic, self-determined reasons
settings, will be a focus, to demonstrate what has experienced flow more readily than those not intrin-
been learned about flow in these settings. sically motivated. Their sample of 203 masters-level
Jackson and Roberts (1992) examined the asso- swimmers were described as either being motivated
ciation between peak performance and flow with in a self-determined way, by engaging in swimming
200 elite athletes from a wide-ranging sample of for their own pleasure, satisfaction, or benefit; ver-
sports that included gymnastics, swimming, golf, sus those motivated for more external reasons. They
track and field, cross-country running, tennis, and also found that the situational determinants of per-
diving. Flow was related to athletes’ peak perfor- ceived competence, autonomy, and relatedness were
mances. Furthermore, athletes high in orientation positively related to flow experiences.
toward mastery of the task experienced flow more Jackson and colleagues (Jackson, Kimiecik, Ford,
frequently than athletes low in mastery orientation. & Marsh, 1998) examined the flow experience in
An opportunity to interview a subsample of this nonelite, older athletic participants in World Mas-
group of competitive athletes demonstrated clearly ters Games participants in swimming, triathlon,
that the flow state was not only relevant to athletes; cycling, and track and field. Using an early version
it was a treasured experience. This led Jackson to of the Flow Scale (Jackson & Marsh, 1996; Marsh
investigate flow experiences in athletes in greater & Jackson, 1999), their findings gave support to
depth. Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) concept of an autotelic
Jackson (1992) interviewed U.S. national cham- personality—where participants choose to under-
pion figure skaters to learn how performers in take an activity for its own sake and where the
this graceful sport experience flow. A close agree- activity provides its own reward. Factors found to
ment between the skaters’ perceptions of flow and be predictive of flow were perceived ability, intrinsic
the theoretical descriptions of the flow construct motivation, and anxiety.
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990) was found. Jackson identi- Interest in flow as a research concept has con-
fied factors perceived as important for attaining flow tinued to grow and flourish in the 21st century.
and those perceived to prevent flow from occur- Concurrent with this growing interest has been an
ring. In an extension of this qualitative research interest in development and application of research
of flow, Jackson (1995, 1996) interviewed elite tools to investigate what is by nature a somewhat
athletes from seven sports, to assess whether their elusive concept. One approach to the assessment of
descriptive accounts of their optimal sport experi- flow for research purposes has been the development
ences would also match Csikszentmihalyi’s (1990) of self-report instruments. These developments are
dimensional model of flow. Results showed a strong described in a subsequent section of this chapter.

jac ks on 
One approach to assessing flow that has been of 271 junior athletes. The results showed that trait
developed has been the Flow Scales (state and dis- confidence, imagery use, and action control were
positional) by Jackson and colleagues (e.g., Jackson significantly related to dispositional and state flow,
& Eklund, 2002; Jackson & Marsh, 1996). Using whereas no significant links emerged between absorp-
the original versions of the flow scales, Jackson et tion and flow. In studying U.S. Division I college ath-
al. (2001) found that four of the flow dimensions letes, Wiggins and Freeman (2000) observed higher
assessed by the Flow Scales—challenge–skill balance, flow scores (i.e., global, unambiguous feedback, con-
concentration on the task at hand, sense of control, centration, loss of self-consciousness) among athletes
and clear goals—were found to be most strongly perceiving their anxiety as facilitative compared with
associated with psychological skill proficiency and those who perceived it as debilitative.
self-concept in a study of 236 competitive orien- As has been discussed, sport, and more generally,
teers, surf life savers, and road cyclists Moreover, performance-based domains, provide ideal contexts
athletes with greater psychological skill proficiency in which to research flow. Csikszentmihalyi (1975)
and more positive self-perceptions were more likely initially began investigating flow through interview-
to experience flow. ing performers in varied domains—and was struck
Karageorghis et al. (2000) investigated relation- by the consistency of the flow experience across
ships between subjective feelings of enjoyment domains. Research continues to unfold in different
and flow in exercise. Using Jackson’s flow scale, contexts, including various domains of the perform-
they found in their sample of 1,231 aerobic dance ing arts (e.g., Jackson & Eklund, 2004; Martin &
exercise participants a positive and significant asso- Cutler, 2002; Wrigley, 2005). For example, Wrigley
ciation between levels of flow and the postexercise (2005) using the FSS-2 and his music performance
feelings of revitalization, tranquility, and positive rating scale derived from live evaluations of over
engagement. This suggested that the experience of 30 teaching staff, measured the effect of the flow
flow might play also a role in encouraging adher- state on the performances of more than 200 tertiary
ence to physical activity regimes through the experi- music students from five instrument families—
ence of positive postexercise feelings. strings, piano, brass, woodwind, and voice—during
A group of Italian researchers investigated the their live performance examinations. Most of the
flow experience across a variety of sport settings, and students experienced flow infrequently during their
an edited book (Muzio, 2004) summarizes find- performance and with a very similar pattern of sub-
ings from an array of research into aspects of flow. scale scores across instrument families. Those that
Included are reports on differences on an Italian ver- did experience flow achieved significantly higher
sion of the Flow State Scale (FSS; Jackson & Marsh, global and specific performance ratings from their
1996) between fencers, skiers, swimmers, cyclists, examiners.
and track and field athletes. The fencers were the Perry (1999) studied creative writers, another
most different from the other groups, which the activity conducive to flow. Perry’s descriptions of
researchers explained in terms of the high impor- the writers’ experience of flow provide an in-depth
tance assigned to immediate feedback cues in fenc- analysis of the experience of writing in flow, as well
ing bouts. There was close similarity between the as suggestions about how to make flow happen
swimming, cycling, and track and field groups. while writing.
Vea and Pensgaard (2004) examined the relation- Computer-mediated environments have also
ship between perfectionism and flow in young elite been a setting in which flow has been examined,
Norwegian athletes. Perfectionism has been shown especially Web instruction and design (e.g., Chen,
to have some negative repercussions, and the authors Wigand, & Nilan, 1999; Novak, Hoffman, & Yung,
were interested in understanding the performance 2000). Given the growing importance of computer
and well-being implications of perfectionism. Flow technology, this is a timely direction in which the
was selected as an indicator of potential to perform study of flow can move. Novak and colleagues
at optimal levels, as well as an indicator of subjective (Novak, Hoffman, & Yung, 2000; Novak, Hoffman,
well-being. As expected, most of the perfectionism & Duhachek, 2003) found support for their propo-
dimensions correlated negatively with flow dimen- sition that compelling online experiences are depen-
sions, although there were a couple of unexpected dent on facilitating flow state. The experience of flow
positive associations. for Web users (Chen, Wigand, & Nilan, 1999) has
Koehn (2007) investigated the frequency and been found to be similar to flow experiences in other
intensity of flow in tennis competition in a sample settings.

 flow
Measurement of Flow the experience sampling method
Flow is a subjective, experiential phenomenon The Experience Sampling Method (ESM) involves
and approaches to its measurement face the chal- the systematic measurement of individuals’ experi-
lenge that go with assessing a subjective state of ences as they are interacting in their daily environ-
consciousness. Csikszentmihalyi has presented a ment (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987; Hormuth,
convincing argument that any measure of flow is 1986). Participants carry some sort of paging system,
only a “partial reflection” of the human experience which provides them with a randomly occurring sig-
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1992, p. 183). A multimodal nal several times during their day. Each time they
approach that incorporates both qualitative and receive the signal, they complete a questionnaire
quantitative methods of measurement is likely to about their momentary experiences in the situation
yield the greatest gains. A diversity of methodolo- at the time. These measures are generally taken over
gies will offer the greatest potential to explain the a period of 1 week.
“what” and “how” questions posed by the unique Analysis of the ESM data provides a descrip-
phenomenon of the flow experience. The measure- tion of the patterns of respondents’ daily experi-
ment approaches that are described next are tools to ences. Compilation of many individuals’ responses
tap into the flow experience, and they are presented in particular situations can lead to development
with the understanding that no one empirical tool of patterns of commonality in experience in sam-
can fully capture flow. pled settings (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987).
Csikszentmihalyi and Larson (1987) stated, “ESM
Qualitative Methods data allow examination of the magnitude, duration,
Csikszentmihalyi’s (1975) early research, which and sequences of states, as well as an investigation
brought to light the flow concept, involved quali- of correlations between the occurrences of different
tative interviews with people from a variety of life experiences” (p. 533).
domains. Simarly, initial research of flow in sport The ESM is a reliable and valid tool for assess-
(Jackson, 1992, 1995, 1996) used qualitative meth- ing flow (Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987), which
ods to assess the flow experience. Jackson developed has been used in many varied life settings (see
in-depth interviews with elite athletes and inductive Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi, 1988b). The
content analysis of their descriptions led to iden- ESM offers several advantages that are particularly
tification of factors inherent in their experiences. useful in the study of flow. Measures of momentary
A qualitative approach was adopted in this early thoughts and feelings and their effects can be taken
research because it was felt that this would facilitate in the person’s naturalistic environment, without too
the understanding of athletes’ flow experiences, par- much disruption. It is ecologically valid and offers
ticularly as little prior research had been conducted a fine-grained assessment of temporal relationships
in the area. By interviewing athletes about their flow between affects and changeable antecedents (Cerin,
experiences, it was possible to explore the under- Szabo, & Williams, 2001).
standing and meaning of flow from the perspective There are obvious difficulties with implement-
of the elite athlete. ing the ESM approach in sport settings and in
other settings where performance is of a continu-
The Development of Quantitative Methods ous nature and evaluated. Creative ways of apply-
Csikszentmihalyi (1975) developed his model ing the ESM approach will enable the benefits of
of flow through the use of experience sampling this type of assessment to be realized in sport and
(Csikszentmihalyi & Larson, 1987). A concomitant other performance settings. A study by Cerin et al.
quantitative approach has been developed in sport (2001) demonstrated that the ESM can be used in
by Jackson (e.g., Jackson & Marsh, 1996; Jackson performance-based settings. These authors randomly
& Eklund, 2002), through the development of assigned 62 male competitive tae-kwon-do practi-
both the dispositional and flow state scales. These tioners into three different measurement groups to
self-report scales were developed to facilitate the ascertain their emotional states. While this study
examination of the flow experience among sport did not measure the flow experience, it provided
and exercise participants and to assist with teasing strong evidence that the ESM can be used to assess
out those factors that may be associated with its dynamic psychological states during competitive
occurrence. Both the Experience Sampling Method sport. Continuing to develop creative and effective
(ESM) and Jackson’s Flow Scales are described in ways to adapt and apply the ESM approach to per-
the following sections. formance-based settings has the potential to open up

jac ks on 
greater understanding of flow through this dynamic All versions of the scales have been validated
measurement approach. through confirmatory factor analyses, and the scales
have demonstrated good psychometric properties.
Each of the scales is briefly described next.
the flow scales
Jackson and colleagues (Jackson & Eklund, 2002;
The LONG Flow Scales
Jackson & Marsh, 1996; Jackson, Martin, & Eklund,
Flow State Scale-2 (FSS-2). The FSS-2 is a 36-item
2008; Martin & Jackson, 2008) have, over a num-
self-report questionnaire designed to measure the
ber of years, developed a suite of scales—the LONG,
state of flow when participating in a specific activ-
SHORT, and CORE Flow Scales—providing a range
ity. The FSS-2 was a revision of the original Long
of self-report instruments to suit a diversity of research
Flow scale developed by Jackson and Marsh (1996).
and applied purposes. One general characteristic of
It is designed to be given immediately or soon after
this approach to assessing flow has been to do so at
a participant has completed an activity. The ques-
two levels: (a) the dispositional level, or frequency of
tionnaire has nine subscales each with four items, to
flow experience across time in particular domains (e.g.,
assess the nine flow dimensions. Respondents in-
sport, work, school), and (b) the state level, or extent of
dicate the extent to which they agree with each
flow experienced in a particular event or activity (e.g.,
statement on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1
a race, a work project, or a test). The dispositional and
(Strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree).
state flow scales are parallel forms, with wording differ-
ences reflecting whether the disposition to experience
Dispositional Flow Scale-2 (DFS-2). A disposition-
flow, or a specific flow experience, is being assessed.
al version of the flow scale was developed to measure
By designing two versions of the scales, it is possible
the frequency with which one typically experiences
to assess both a general tendency to experience flow, as
flow in a specific activity or setting (Jackson et al.,
well as particular incidence (or nonincidence) of flow
1998). The dispositional flow scale was developed to
characteristics during a particular event.
help understand the autotelic personality (see, e.g.,
There are three main flow instruments (each of
Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), or individual differences in
which has a dispositional and a state version):
propensity to experience flow (Jackson et al., 1998).
1. LONG Flow Scales: These are 36-item Jackson and Eklund (2002) developed the DFS-2,
instruments, designed to assess the nine dimensions a revision of the original Long Flow scale developed
of flow (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). The Long Flow by Jackson and Marsh (1996). The DFS-2 is essen-
Scales (i.e., Dispositional & State) are particularly tially a parallel version of the FSS-2. It has 36 items,
useful when a detailed picture of flow experience which are designed to assess to what degree an in-
is important for research or applied purposes. The dividual generally experiences the flow state while
Long Scales provide a multidimensional approach participating in a specified activity. It is therefore
to assessing flow. These scales are the instruments designed to be answered away from an immediate
of choice for targeted interventions, and/or when involvement in one’s activity. The questionnaire has
a detailed understanding of the flow dimensions is nine subscales with four items each, corresponding
important. to the nine flow dimensions. Respondents indicate
2. SHORT Flow Scales: These are 9-item the frequency of each statement on a 5-point Likert
(Dispositional and State) scales, which are scale, ranging from 1 (Never) to 5 (Always).
abbreviated versions of Long Flow. One item is
used to represent each of the nine flow dimensions. Psychometric Characteristics of the Long Flow Scales.
The Short Flow Scales provide a brief assessment A comprehensive construct validation approach
useful when research or practical constraints has been undertaken for the purpose of evaluating
prevent use of a longer scale. the psychometric characteristics of the FSS-2 and
3. CORE Flow Scales: These are 10-item DFS-2. Using confirmatory factor analyses (CFA),
(Dispositional and State) scales, designed to Jackson and Marsh (1996) demonstrated accept-
assess the global phenomenology of flow. The able fit values for the FSS, as did Marsh and Jack-
Core Flow Scales provide an assessment of the son (1999) for the FSS and DFS. In both studies,
central subjective experience and complement a nine-factor, first-order model and a hierarchical
the dimensional assessments afforded by the model with one global flow factor were evaluated.
Long and Short Flow Scales. The first-order model received stronger support

 flow
due to the slightly weaker fit of the hierarchical construct is not the central focus and can be reason-
model. ably estimated with a short measure. It was for practi-
Across two large psychometric studies (i.e., cal reasons such as these that Jackson and colleagues
Jackson & Eklund, 2002; Jackson, Martin, & (Jackson et al., 2008; Martin & Jackson, 2008)
Eklund, 2008), the FSS-2 and DFS-2 have dem- developed two short scales to assess flow: the Short
onstrated good reliability, with FSS-2 alphas rang- Flow Scales and the Core Flow Scales.
ing between .76 to .92, and the DFS-2 range being The Short flow scales are abbreviated versions of
.78 to .90. their predecessors, the FSS-2 and DFS-2. Both the
The DFS-2 and/or FSS-2 have been translated Short Flow State Scale and the Short Dispositional
into several languages, including Greek (Stavrou Flow Scale contain nine items, one for each of the
& Zervas, 2004), French (Fournier et al., 2007), nine flow dimensions. The rating scales of the abbre-
Japanese (Kawabata, Mallett, & Jackson, 2007), viated instruments are the same as those used in their
Finnish (Pekka Hämäläinen & Veli-Pekka Räty, per- parent scales. They provide succinct measures of the
sonal communication, 2008), Spanish (Martínez- higher order dimensional flow model described in
Zaragoza, Benavides, Solanes, Pastor, & Martin del CFA research with the 36-item scales. Initial psycho-
Rio, personal communication, 2008), Hungarian metric support for the Short Flow Scales is promising.
(Bimbo, personal communication, 2009), and Hindi Being new scales, there is a need for research to assess
(Singh, personal communication, 2009) versions, their validity and utility across domains. Both dispo-
with more translations presently underway. sitional and state forms have demonstrated good reli-
Research has been conducted with the Long flow ability in initial validation studies (Jackson, Martin,
scales in a range of activities in settings including sport & Ekluund, 2008; Martin & Jackson, 2008).
(e.g., Jackson et al., 1998), exercise (e.g., Karageorghis
et al., 2000), yoga (Penman, Cohen, Stephens, & The CORE Flow Scales
Jackson, 2006), music performance (Wrigley, 2005), The rationale behind the Core flow scales was to
and Web-based instructional activity (Chan & Rep- devise a somewhat different approach to assessing
man, 1999). The author has communicated with flow to the Long and Short Flow Scales. The Core
researchers from diverse areas (such as gifted educa- flow scales contain 10 items that are descriptions
tion, work addiction, yoga, and business) regarding of what it feels like to be in flow during a target
application of the flow scales to their research setting. activity. The items were derived from qualitative
Moreover, there is considerable interest in examining research, specifically, elite athlete descriptions of the
flow in relationship to other psychological constructs experience of being in flow (Jackson, 1992, 1995,
across diverse settings. Relationships with concepts 1996). Expressions used by athletes to describe what
such as hope, cohesion, personality type, intrinsic it is like to be in flow were adapted into short state-
motivation, burnout, self-efficacy, self-esteem, and ments that are rated on similar rating scales to the
anxiety have all captured the interest of flow research- other flow scales. Model fit and reliability for these
ers. There is considerable interest in examining flow scales have been strong in initial validation studies
across a range of settings and in relation to a diverse (Martin & Jackson, 2008).
set of psychological constructs. With the recent intro-
duction of two new brief measures of flow, research- the potential uses of the flow scales
ers have several options for including flow as a focal, The triad of flow scales developed by Jackson and
ancillary, or outcome measure. colleagues (e.g., Jackson & Eklund, 2002; Jackson
et al., 2008; Martin & Jackson, 2008) provides
The SHORT Flow Scales researchers and practitioners with a choice of mea-
Despite the psychometric advantages to longer, surement instruments to assess flow. The 36-item,
multidimensional instruments, practical consider- or Long flow, scales have solid research evidence
ations may dictate the need for shorter versions. For indicating they are robust instruments that can
example, during a sports event, athletes and coaches provide a detailed assessment of the dimensional
may be willing to complete a 10-item scale, but flow model. When a fine-grained description of flow
not one four times that length. In large-scale proj- characteristics according to the dimensional flow
ects involving many measures, short forms may be model of Csikszentmihalyi (1990) is desired, then
preferable to keep a questionnaire package to a rea- the Long flow scales are the best option. The Long
sonable size for participants, or because a particular scales are also ideally suited to intervention-based

jac ks on 
research, providing assessment of modifiable flow attitude, experienced positive precompetitive and
characteristics in the nine-dimensional approach. competitive affect, maintained appropriate focus,
The Short flow scales provide a practical tool for a felt physically ready, and experienced a unity with
brief assessment of flow from the nine-dimensional their dance partner. Skaters’ experience of flow was
conceptualization. Grounded in a solid psychomet- more likely to be prevented or disrupted if they expe-
ric base, the nine-item Short Flow Scales provide an rienced physical problems and made mistakes, had
aggregate measure of the nine flow dimensions. The an inability to maintain focus, held a negative atti-
equally short (10-item) Core Flow Scales provide a tude, or experienced a lack of audience response.
valid and reliable assessment of the central, or core, In an extension of her 1992 study, Jackson (1995)
subjective experience of being in flow. These two considered the factors that facilitated, disrupted,
brief flow instruments offer different but comple- and prevented the experience of flow—together
mentary ways of assessing flow and open up possi- with the perceived controllability of flow—with a
bilities for including flow as a focal construct across larger and more diverse range of elite athletes across
a range of settings. The items in Short and Core seven sports. Results showed considerable consis-
Flow Scales are applicable across domains, and the tency with results from the earlier study with figure
initial CFA research conducted with these scales has skaters (Jackson, 1992). Ten factors (see Box 8.1)
demonstrated good fit indices in sport, work, aca- were described by the athletes to influence their
demic school work, and extracurricular activities. development of flow and included physical, psycho-
In summary, Jackson and colleagues have devel- logical, nutritional, and situational variables. Sup-
oped a suite of self-report scales that provide mul- port for the multidimensional nature of antecedent
tiple ways of tapping into flow. All of the current and preventive flow factors was provided by these
versions of the scales, and a manual detailing their findings. In relation to the question on perceived
use, are described in Jackson, Eklund, and Martin controllability of flow, athletes reported a range of
(2010). responses, from perceiving little control to perceiv-
ing considerable control. A large percentage of the
Facilitating Flow factors seen to facilitate or prevent flow were seen as
Understanding factors that facilitate flow has controllable, whereas the factors seen as disrupting
been a focus of Jackson’s research (e.g., 1992, 1995, flow were largely perceived as being uncontrollable.
1998, 2001), with the goal of developing the knowl- Karageorghis et al. (2000) made some sugges-
edge base of what can increase the likelihood of tions to facilitate the flow experience among school
achieving this rewarding optimal experience. students, which are equally applicable to other indi-
In a qualitative study with U.S. national cham- viduals interested in attaining flow. They suggested
pion figure skaters, Jackson (1992) found that there that students “set personal goals that are attain-
were certain factors that helped or hindered the able, challenging and well-defined”; “give pupils
achievement of this state. Skaters were more likely a choice from time to time in the activities they
to achieve flow when they held a positive mental engage in” to increase their autotelic experience; and

Box 8.1 Factors Influencing Flow


1. Being motivated to perform well.
2. Achieving an optimal arousal level before performing.
3. Having precompetitive and competitive plans so that the performer felt totally prepared and
knew clearly what to do.
4. Knowing they had done the training and felt physically ready.
5. Optimal environmental and situational conditions and influences.
6. Feeling good during a performance.
7. Holding strong focus and concentration.
8. Feeling confident and having a positive mental attitude.
9. Having positive team play and interaction.
10. Feeling experienced as a competitor and in having experienced flow in the past.
Source: Jackson, 1995.

 flow
“use skill-learning techniques” to encourage persis- The small sample size of four increased their service
tence in mastering the tasks to increase their sense of and groundstroke performance, and three partici-
control (p. 243). pants attained higher flow levels following the inter-
Jackson and colleagues (Jackson, 1995; Jackson vention phase (Koehn, 2007).
et al., 1998, 2001; Jackson & Roberts, 1992) have Understanding the factors that facilitate flow has
found that a high perception of their sporting ability obvious important applied implications. While it
was a crucial factor facilitating flow. This led Jackson is not possible to engineer a flow experience, it is
(Jackson et al., 2001) to suggest that the perceived possible to increase its occurrence, as Jackson and
skills component of the challenge–skill balance that Csikszentmihalyi (1999, p.138) argued: “It is not
defines flow is a critical aspect in the acquisition of possible to make flow happen at will . . . and attempt-
the flow state in sport or other performance-based ing to do so will only make the state more elusive.
domains. However, removing obstacles and providing facili-
Perceptions of skill and challenge in a situation, tating conditions will increase its occurrence . . .”
as described in the flow model (see Fig. 8.1) also (Jackson & Csikszentmihalyi, 1999, p. 138).
help to explain when the experience of anxiety,
rather than flow, is likely to occur. When the chal- Future Directions
lenges are greater than perceived skills, anxiety is the The pioneering efforts of Csikszentmihalyi (e.g.,
predicted outcome, according to the flow model. 1975, 1990, 1997, 2003) has opened a new level
There has been considerable research support for of understanding of what is involved when people
anxiety as a factor preventing flow (Jackson, 1995; become totally absorbed in what they are doing.
Jackson & Roberts, 1992; Jackson et al., 1998; Csikszentmihalyi has examined flow across settings
Stein, Kimiecik, Daniels, & Jackson, 1995; Taylor, ranging from daily living (e.g., Csikszentmihalyi,
2001) and, furthermore, that the cognitive, rather 1997) to research endeavor leading to major scien-
than the physiological, components of anxiety are tific discoveries (e.g., Csikszentmihalyi, 1996). As
seemingly more detrimental to the flow experience. was observed in Csikszentmihalyi’s (1975) initial
As would be expected, self-determined, intrinsic investigations, remarkable consistency has contin-
motivation is the best type of motivation to facili- ued to be encountered across the broadening array of
tate flow. A study by Jackson et al. (1998), using a activities and settings examined.
multidimensional measure of intrinsic motivation, The way forward for research into the optimal
showed that only an intrinsic motivation factor experience of flow is promising and exciting. The
demonstrated substantive relationships with flow; measurement tools thus far developed to help assess
the extrinsic factors were unrelated to flow. Further flow provide much scope for researchers interested
research in this area will increase understanding of in furthering understanding of flow. The ESM, as
how different forms of motivation are related to had been discussed, has been the central method
flow. used by Csikszentmihalyi and colleagues for over
There has been some research suggesting that flow 20 years (e.g., Csikszentmihalyi & Csikszentmihalyi,
may be enhanced by hypnotic capacity and training 1988a; Csikszentmihalyi & Nakamura, 1989) to
in psychological skills. The results of a study by Grove assess flow, and it continues to provide an innova-
and Lewis (1996) showed that the flow state can be tive way to tap into experiential states as they are
enhanced by the capacity for hypnotic susceptibil- occurring.
ity. They found that high-susceptibility exercisers The Flow Scales developed by Jackson and col-
had greater increases in flow than low-susceptibility leagues (e.g., Jackson & Marsh, 1996; Jackson &
participants. Case studies by Pates and Maynard Eklund, 2002; Jackson, Martin, & Eklund, 2008)
(2000) and Pates, Cummings, and Maynard (2002) provide valid and reliable tools for assessing specific
found that hypnotic interventions using imagery, experiences, as well as assessing general tendency
relaxation, hypnotic induction, regression, and trig- to experience flow, respectively. The dispositional
gers enhanced their experience of, and personal con- versions of the flow scales facilitate investigation
trol over, flow. Using an imagery-based intervention of correlates of flow, while the state versions pro-
in combination with relaxation techniques, Koehn, vide assessment of experience of flow characteristics
Morris, and Watt (2006) and Koehn (2007) used within an event. The brief flow measures discussed
an imagery script that aimed to increase athletes’ in this chapter provide opportunity to assess flow
confidence and action control in order to facilitate during, or immediately after, performance, without
flow state and performance in tennis competition. imposing a time demand on participants. Both Short

jac ks on 
and Core flow can be completed in less than 2 min- contexts, do organizational cultures or administra-
utes and could be used to gain multiple assessments tion style impact on flow?
of experience across time. Other advantages of the One of the most important research pursuits for
brief flow measures have already been highlighted, the future will be the unravelling of the complex
such as the inclusion of flow in multimeasure studies interplay between person and situational variables
investigating a large number of constructs, gaining such as these. How do certain dispositional char-
self-other ratings, and their generic item format that acteristics of an individual interact with situational
makes them easily applicable to many domains. variables to affect the experience of flow? Further-
Knowing what is occurring at a neuro- or psycho- more, do different individuals, or different settings,
physiological level during flow has long been regarded influence the relative endorsement of the nine flow
as a critical area for furthering understanding of dimensions that have been described in this chapter?
this concept. Csikszentmihalyi (Csikszentmihalyi & Future research is required to tease out the interplay
Csikszentmihalyi, 1988a) referred to early neurologi- between the nine dimensions to flow and the pattern
cal studies conducted by Hamilton (e.g., Hamilton, of relationships between these dimensions within dif-
1976, 1981), where attentional patterns associated ferent contexts and individuals (Jackson et al., 1998,
with flow were first described. Mental effort, as mea- 2001). For example, the loss of self-consciousness and
sured by cortical activation, was shown to decrease in time transformation dimensions have been found to
individuals with good ability to concentrate deeply receive lower endorsement and lower factor loadings
on a task. The challenges of assessing internal, physi- in sport research (Jackson, 1996; Jackson & Marsh,
ological states during flow have meant this area of 1996; Kowal & Fortier, 1999; Vlachopoulos et al.,
research has moved ahead slowly. A promising study 2000). One future research direction would be to
by de Manzano, Theorell, Harmat, and Ullen (2010) investigate how these two dimensions are experienced
examined several psychophysiological parameters in different settings, and across different levels of per-
and flow state. With piano players as the participants, formers. An exploratory study of the dimensions
they found significant associations between flow and of flow using Rasch analyses (Tenenbaum, Fogarty,
heart rate, blood pressure, zygomaticus muscle activ- & Jackson, 1999) suggested that the loss of self-
ity, and respiratory depth. With continuing techno- consciousness and time transformation dimensions
logical advances, new levels of understanding of what might only be experienced in deeper levels of flow.
happens when people are in flow will be possible. Another interesting direction for future research
Standard psychological measures, such as self- is to continue to examine the challenge–skill bal-
report, also hold promise for advancing under- ance model of flow, how person and/or situational
standing of factors influencing flow. Although there variables might influence the balance of challenges
has been considerable research already conducted, and skills, and whether there is individual variation
future research could continue to explore disposi- in relative levels of challenge and skill for flow to
tional characteristics that may make it more or less occur. Although the operational definition of flow
likely that flow will be experienced. Some individ- describes flow as occuring when challenges and skills
ual difference factors that researchers could explore are balanced, and extending the individual, there
include such areas as general level of capacity to may be specific situations where the relative levels
experience enjoyment and fun; emotional and per- of challenges and skills vary from this standard defi-
sonality characteristics, such as the autotelic person- nition. This potential variability has been discussed
ality trait; motivational orientations; cognitive styles by Csikszentmihalyi (e.g., Moneta & Csikszent-
and processes, including the capacity to concentrate mihalyi, 1996), and a recent empirical study by
and immerse oneself in an activity; and experience Engeser and Rheinberg (2008) demonstrated that
with, and use of, psychological skills. the challenge–skill relationship was moderated by
In addition to individual difference factors, perceived importance of the activity and by achieve-
there are a host of situational factors that can help ment motivation. As measurement tools are refined,
to extend understanding of flow. For example, do and creative ways of tapping into flow developed,
competitive or noncompetitive environments facili- understanding of the concept of flow will continue
tate flow better? How does flow operate in group or to evolve.
team settings? Social factors, such as the impact of
teaching and coaching styles, are likely to influence Conclusion
flow. Can we design programs that help to facilitate Flow is a critical area to continue to research in
flow in learning environments? In organizational order to help with finding out more about fostering

 flow
the positive side of people’s lived experience. In the Engeser, S., & Rheinberg, F. (2008). Flow, performance, and
end, our experience of life is what matters most, as moderators of challenge-skill balance. Motivation and Emo-
tion, 32, 158–172.
Csikszentmihalyi so eloquently expressed when he Fournier, J., Gaudreau, P., Demontrond-Behr, P., Visioli, J.,
wrote, “Subjective experience is the bottom line Forrest, J., & Jackson, S. A. (2007). French translation of
of existence” (Csikszentmihalyi, 1982). Or, as an the Flow State Scale-2: Factor structure, cross-cultural invari-
elite athlete interviewed about his flow experience ance, and associations with goal attainment. Psychology of
described, “I strive to get to that state of perfec- Sport and Exercise, 8, 897–916.
Grove, J. R., & Lewis, M. A. (1996). Hypnotic susceptibility and
tion” (Jackson, 1996). The concept of flow provides the attainment of flow like states during exercise. Journal of
researchers and practitioners with the lofty goal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 380–391.
understanding those moments in time that make life Hamilton, J. A. (1976). Attention and intrinsic rewards in the
worth living. control of psychophysiological states. Psychotherapy and
Psychosomatics, 27, 54–61.
Hamilton, J. A. (1981). Attention, personality, and self-
References regulation of mood: Absorbing interest and boredom.
Catley, D., & Duda, J. L. (1997). Psychological antecedents of
In B.A. Maehr (Ed.), Progress in experimental personality
flow in golfers. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28,
research, 10, 282–315.
309–322.
Hormuth, S. E. (1986). The sampling of experiences in situ.
Cerin, E., Szabo, A., & Williams, C. (2001). Is the Experience
Journal of Personality, 54, 262–293.
Sampling Method (ESM) appropriate for studying pre-
Jackson, S. A. (1992). Athletes in flow: A qualitative investiga-
competitive emotions? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 2,
tion of flow states in elite figure skaters. Journal of Applied
27–45.
Sport Psychology, 4, 161–180.
Chan, T. S., & Repman, J. (1999). Flow in web based instruc-
Jackson, S. A. (1995). Factors influencing the occurrence of flow
tional activity: An exploratory research project. International
states in elite athletes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 7,
Journal of Educational Telecommunications, 5, 225–237.
135–163.
Chen, H., Wigand, R. T., & Nilan, M. S. (1999). Optimal expe-
Jackson, S. A. (1996). Toward a conceptual understanding of the
rience of web activities. Computers in Human Behavior, 15,
flow experience in elite athletes. Research Quarterly for Exer-
585–608.
cise and Sport, 67, 76–90.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San
Jackson, S. A. (2000). Joy, fun, and flow state in sport. In Y. Hanin
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
(Ed.), Emotions in sport (pp. 135–156). Champaign, IL:
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1982). Towards a psychology of optimal
Human Kinetics.
experience. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and
Jackson, S. A., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sports:
social psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 13–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
The keys to optimal experiences and performances. Champaign,
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal
IL: Human Kinetics.
experience. New York: Harper & Row.
Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2002). Assessing flow in
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1992). A response to the Kimiecik &
physical activity: The FSS-2 and DFS-2. Journal of Sport and
Stein and Jackson papers. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology,
Exercise Psychology, 24, 133–150.
4, 181–183.
Jackson, S. A., & Eklund, R. C. (2004, September). Relation-
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology
ships between quality of experience and participation in diverse
of discovery and invention. New York: Harper Collins.
performance settings. Paper presented at the 39th Annual
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1997). Finding flow: The psychology of
Conference of the Australian Psychological Society, Sydney,
engagement with everyday life. New York: Harper Collins.
Australia. [Australian Journal of Psychology, 56, (Supplement),
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2003). Good business: Leadership, flow, and
p. 193].
the making of meaning. London: Hodder & Stoughton.
Jackson, S. A., Eklund, R. C., & Martin A. J. (2010). The FLOW
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. (Eds.). (1988a).
Manual. Mind Garden Inc. Retrieved from, www.mindgar-
Optimal experience: Psychological studies of flow in conscious-
den.com/products/flow.htm
ness. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Jackson, S. A., Kimiecik, J., Ford, S., & Marsh, H.W. (1998).
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Csikszentmihalyi, I. (1988b). Mea-
Psychological correlates of flow in sport. Journal of Sport and
surement of flow in everyday life: Introduction to part IV.
Exercise Psychology, 20, 358–378.
In M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. Csikszentmihalyi. (Eds.), Opti-
Jackson, S. A., & Marsh, H. W. (1996). Development and
mal experience: Psychological studies of flow in consciousness
validation of a scale to measure optimal experience: The
(pp. 251–265). New York: Cambridge University Press.
flow state scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18,
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reli-
17–35.
ability of the experience-sampling method. The Journal of
Jackson, S. A., Martin, A. J., & Eklund, R. C. (2008). Long and
Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 526–536.
short measures of flow: Examining construct validity of the
Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Nakamura, J. (1989). The dynamics of
FSS-2, DFS-2, and new brief counterparts. Journal of Sport
intrinsic motivation: A study of adolescents. In C. Ames &
and Exercise Psychology, 30, 561–587.
R. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education. Vol. 3:
Jackson, S. A., & Roberts, G. C. (1992). Positive performance
Goals and cognitions (pp. 45–71). New York: Academic
states of athletes: Toward a conceptual understanding of peak
Press.
performance. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 156–171.
De Manzano, O., Theorell, T., Harmat, L., & Ullen, F. (2010).
Jackson, S. A., Thomas, P. R., Marsh, H. W., & Smethurst, C. J.
The psychophysiology of flow during piano playing.
(2001). Relationships between flow, self-concept, psychological
Emotion, 10, 301–311.

jac ks on 
skills, and performance. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, A structural modeling approach. Marketing Science, 19,
154–178. 22–44.
Karageorghis, C. I., Vlachopoulos, S. P., & Terry, P. C. (2000). Pates, J., & Maynard, I. (2000). Effects of hypnosis on flow
Latent variable modelling of the relationship between flow states and golf performance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 91,
and exercise-induced feelings: An intuitive appraisal perspec- 1057–1075.
tive. European Physical Education, 6(3), 230–248. Pates, J., Cummings, A., & Maynard, I. (2002). Effects
Kawabata, M., Mallett, C., & Jackson, S. A. (2007). The Flow of hypnosis on flow states and three-point shooting per-
State Scale-2 and Dispositional Flow Scale-2: Examina- formance in basketball players. The Sport Psychologist, 16,
tion of factorial validity and reliability for Japanese adults. 34–47.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 465–485. Penman, S., Cohen, M., Stephens, P., & Jackson, S. A. (2006,
Koehn, S., (2007). Propensity and attainment of flow state. April). Results of the ‘Yoga in Australia’ survey. International
Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Victoria University, Mel- Aruyveda and Yoga Conference, Sydney, Australia. Retrieved
bourne, Australia. from http://www.yogainaustralia.com/downloads/YogaIn
Koehn, S., Morris, T., & Watt, A. P. (2006). Efficacy of an Australia.pdf, November 27, 2011.
imagery intervention on flow and performance in tennis Perry, S. K. (1999). Writing in flow. Cincinnati, OH: Writer’s
competitions Society for Tennis Medicine and Science, 11, Digest.
12–14. Privette, G., & Bundrick, C. M. (1997). Psychological processes
Kowal, J., & Fortier, M. (1999). Motivational determinants of of peak, average, and failing performance in sport. Interna-
flow: Contributions from self-determination theory. Journal tional Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 323–334.
of Social Psychology, 139(3), 355–368. Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Positive psychology, positive preven-
Marsh, H. W., & Jackson, S. A. (1999). Flow experience in tion, and positive therapy. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez
sport: Construct validation of multidimensional, hierarchi- (Eds.), Handbook on positive psychology (pp. 3–9). Oxford,
cal state and trait responses. Structural Equation Modelling, England: Oxford University Press.
6, 343–371. Seligman, M. E., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2000). Positive
Martin, J. J., & Cutler, K. (2002). An exploratory study of psychology: An introduction. American Psychologist, 55(1),
flow and motivation in theater actors. Journal of Applied 5–14.
Sport Psychology, 14, 344–352. Stavrou, N. A., & Zervas, Y. (2004). Confirmatory factor analy-
Martin, A. J., & Jackson, S. A. (2008). Brief approaches to assess- sis of Flow State Scale in sports. International Journal of Sport
ing task absorption and enhanced subjective experience: and Exercise Psychology, 2, 161–181.
Examining ‘short’ and ‘core’ flow in diverse performance Stein, G., Kimiecik, J., Daniels, J., & Jackson, S. A. (1995).
domains. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 141–157. Psychological antecedents of flow in recreational sport.
Maslow, A. H. (1968). Toward a psychology of being (2nd ed.). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 125–135.
New York: D. Van Nostrand. Taylor, M. K. (2001). The relationships of anxiety intensity and
Maslow, A. H. (1970). Religions, values and peak-experiences. direction of flow in collegiate athletes. Unpublished Master’s
New York: The Viking Press. thesis, University of North Carolina at Greensboro, North
Maslow, A. H. (1973). The farther reaches of human knowledge. Carolina.
Pelican Books. Tenenbaum, G., Fogarty, G. J., & Jackson, S. A. (1999). The
McInman, A. D., & Grove, J. R. (1991). Peak moments in sport: flow experience: A Rasch analysis of Jackson’s Flow State
A literature review. Quest, 43, 333–351. Scale. Journal of Outcome Measurement, 3, 278–294.
Moneta, G. B., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). The effect of Vea, S., & Pensgaard, A.M. (2004, September). The relation-
perceived challenges and skills on the quality of subjective ship between perfectionism and flow among young elite athletes.
experience. Journal of Personality, 64, 274–310. Paper presented at the Advancement of Applied Sport Psy-
Muzio, M. (Ed.). (2004). Sport: Flow e prestazione eccellente. chology Annual Conference, Minneapolis, MN.
Milano, Italy: FrancoAngeli. Vlachopoulos, S. P., Karageorghis, C. I., & Terry, P. C. (2000).
Nakamura, J., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept Hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis of the Flow State
of flow. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of Scale in exercise. Journal of Sport Sciences, 18, 815–823.
positive psychology (pp. 89–105). Oxford University Press. Wiggins, M. S., & Freeman, P. (2000). Anxiety and flow:
Novak, T. P., Hoffman, D., & Duhachek, A. (2003). The influ- An examination of anxiety direction and the flow experience.
ence of goal-directed and experiential activities on online International Sports Journal, 4, 78–87.
flow experiences. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 13, 3–16. Wrigley, W. J. (2005). An examination of ecological factors in
Novak, T. P., Hoffman, D. L., & Yung, Y. F. (2000). Measur- music performance assessment. Unpublished Ph.D. disserta-
ing the customer experience in online environments: tion, Griffith University, Brisbane, Australia.

 flow
CHAPTER

Implicit–Explicit Motive Congruence


9
Todd M. Thrash, Laura A. Maruskin, and Chris C. Martin

Abstract
This chapter reviews the literature on congruence (consistency) between implicit (unconscious) and
explicit (conscious) motives. The prevailing wisdom that implicit and explicit motives are uncorrelated
is shown to be incorrect. When methodological shortcomings of past research (e.g., unreliability of
measurement) are overcome, implicit and explicit motives are positively correlated. Nevertheless, the
relation is weak enough that the discrepancy between implicit and explicit motives carries important
information about personality congruence. The relation between implicit and explicit motives has been
found to vary systematically and meaningfully as a function of substantive moderator variables, such as
self-determination and self-monitoring. Motive congruence is predicted distally by satisfaction of basic
needs during childhood and proximally by stress among individuals who have difficulty regulating affect.
Motive congruence predicts important outcomes, including volitional strength, flow, and well-being.
The chapter closes with a discussion of future research directions, such as the distinction between
congruence and integration constructs.
Key Words: motive congruence, incongruence, implicit motives, explicit motives, moderators,
self-determination, self-monitoring, well-being, personality, motivation

Introduction or her motivation using a set of numerical response


A motive is a predisposition to approach a partic- options. In the TAT, the participant is shown a series
ular class of incentives, such as achievement, affilia- of pictures and is asked to tell a story about each.
tion, or power, or to avoid a particular class of threats, The researcher interprets the stories in terms of their
such as failure, rejection, or domination by others. motivational content. David McClelland and other
The study of motives began with the work of Henry researchers later developed refined versions of the
Murray (1938), who, along with his colleagues at the TAT, now called Picture-Story Exercises (PSEs),
Harvard Psychological Clinic, pioneered a sophisti- in which subjective interpretation of participants’
cated, multimethod approach to the assessment of stories is replaced by application of experimentally
human motives. Of the methods that Murray devel- derived coding systems (e.g., McClelland, Atkinson,
oped, two were particularly influential—self-report Clark, & Lowell, 1953). After 60 years of research
motive questionnaires and the Thematic Appercep- in the McClelland tradition, the prevailing wisdom
tion Test (TAT). Motive questionnaires consist of is that scores on questionnaire and PSE measures of
a series of statements regarding motivation in a par- motives tend to be uncorrelated, even when they
ticular content domain, such as achievement, affilia- concern the same content domain. For instance,
tion, or power. The participant reports the level of his within the achievement domain, the extent to which


individuals report valuing achievement on a ques- ment is much higher or lower than the level of
tionnaire is often unrelated to the level of concern concern with achievement that they reveal through
with achievement that they reveal indirectly through their stories. These individuals display a low level
the stories that they tell. of motive congruence (or, equivalently, a high level
Most contemporary researchers interpret the of incongruence). Motive congruence in important
lack of correlation between questionnaire and PSE life domains, such as achievement, affiliation, and
measures as a substantive fact about underlying power, is regarded as a fundamental indicator of
constructs. PSEs and questionnaires are thought to personality coherence and has been posited to have
assess different kinds of motive constructs that are important implications for well-being (McClelland,
conceptually and statistically independent. Ques- Koestner, & Weinberger, 1989; Weinberger &
tionnaires assess explicit motives—verbally encoded McClelland, 1990).
values that are consciously accessible. PSEs assess This interpretation of the lack of correlation
implicit motives—spontaneously expressed con- between PSE and questionnaire measures pro-
cerns or preferences that are not verbally encoded or vides a rather unflattering portrait of the human
directly accessible to consciousness. condition—it suggests that the conscious sense of
An important implication of the substantive self is fundamentally divorced from underlying
interpretation of statistical independence is that unconscious motivations. However, an alternative
individuals differ markedly in motive congruence— possibility is that PSEs and questionnaires are
the extent to which an individual’s levels of implicit uncorrelated due to methodological shortcomings
and explicit motives are aligned. For the sake of of one or both assessment methods. Thus, it may
illustration, we have depicted a correlation of r = .00 be the assessment methodology, rather than the
between implicit and explicit need for achievement human psyche, that is in disarray. Of course, these
(nAch) in Figure 9.1, based on hypothetical data. two possibilities are not mutually exclusive. Our
Each data point represents the implicit nAch and objective in this chapter is to review the literature
explicit nAch scores for a particular individual. Some on the substantive and methodological factors that
individuals—those whose scores fall near the line influence the relation between PSE and question-
y = x (which runs from the lower left corner to the naire motive measures, as well as the literatures on
upper right corner)—have similar levels of implicit the antecedents of motive congruence and the con-
and explicit achievement motives; that is, the extent sequences of congruence for well-being and other
to which they value achievement is comparable to outcomes. In the following, we begin by providing a
the level of concern with achievement that they more detailed historical overview of traditional and
reveal spontaneously through the stories that they contemporary perspectives on the relation between
tell. These individuals display a high level of motive PSE and questionnaire motive measures.
congruence. Other individuals—those whose scores
fall far from the line y = x (those near the upper
left corner or lower right corner)—have dissimilar The Relation Between PSE and
levels of implicit and explicit achievement motives; Questionnaire Measures of Motives
that is, the extent to which they value achieve- In their classic book The Achievement Motive,
McClelland et al. (1953) described a study in which
their PSE measure of nAch was found to be uncor-
related with a three-item questionnaire concern-
ing effort to achieve. Although significant positive
Explicit nAch

correlations emerged in some subsequent studies


(e.g., deCharms, Morrison, Reitman, & McClelland,
1955), most studies failed to document a significant
relation between PSE and questionnaire measures
of nAch (Spangler, 1992). Findings were similar for
Implicit nAch PSE and questionnaire measures of need for affilia-
tion (nAff) and need for power (nPow). Accordingly,
Incongruent Congruent McClelland (1987) and others concluded that these
Fig. 9.1. Hypothetical data in which the correlation between
two kinds of measures are statistically independent.
implicit and explicit nAch is r = .00. Individuals who are more McClelland et al. (1989) described the lack of rela-
congruent have data points that fall closer to the line y = x. tion between PSE and questionnaire measures as

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


being among the most well-established findings in McClelland’s case for interpreting the lack of cor-
psychology. relation between PSEs and questionnaires as evidence
of independent constructs rests on data showing that
Statistical Independence as a Problem of they have distinct nomological networks. DeCharms
Measurement et al. (1955) provided early evidence of distinct
McClelland et al. (1989) observed that most behavioral correlates, and McClelland et al.’s (1989)
researchers up to that point had interpreted the literature review made a particularly compelling case
lack of correlation between PSEs and question- for distinct nomological networks more generally
naires as evidence that one method or the other is (see also Biernat, 1989; McClelland & Pilon, 1983;
invalid (see also Weinberger & McClelland, 1990). Schultheiss, 2001a). Implicit motives, according to
The assumption underlying these interpretations is McClelland and other theorists, appear to develop in
that PSEs and questionnaires represent alternative early childhood through preverbal, affect-based asso-
potential methods of assessing the same construct ciative learning, respond to task-based or experiential
and therefore ought to converge if both are valid. incentives, predict spontaneous behavior trends, and
This assumption has been most explicit among are introspectively inaccessible; explicit motives, in
researchers employing the multitrait–multimethod contrast, are thought to develop later in childhood
(MTMM) matrix framework (Burwen & Campbell, through verbally mediated learning, are responsive to
1957; Campbell & Fiske, 1959). In this approach, social-extrinsic or verbal-symbolic incentives, predict
convergent validity depends upon robust relations deliberate choices, and are accessible in the form of
between measures that concern the same content consciously articulated values. These findings provide
(here, motive domains) but that involve different evidence that PSEs and questionnaires are indicators
assessment methods. Campbell and colleagues have of different constructs; therefore, the lack of correla-
reported that PSEs tend to show poor convergence tion between PSEs and questionnaires was attributed
with other methods, including self-reports. Camp- to the independence of these underlying constructs.
bell and Fiske (1959) concluded—at least as inter- Our reading of McClelland’s various writings
preted by McClelland et al. (1989)—that PSEs suggests that his position was stronger than one of
are therefore invalid. In contrast, Raven (1988) attempting to explain post hoc why PSEs and ques-
cited questionnaires as the cause of poor conver- tionnaires are unrelated; he argued that, in theory,
gence, which he attributed to improper design. they ought to be unrelated (e.g., McClelland, 1987).
McClelland, similarly, has questioned the validity McClelland’s theoretical grounds for predicting sta-
of questionnaires for the assessment of motives, tistical independence appear to consist of two com-
particularly in his earlier writings (e.g., deCharms plementary arguments (see also Thrash, Elliot, &
et al., 1955; McClelland, 1980). Schultheiss, 2007): (a) implicit and explicit motives
have different developmental antecedents (e.g., they
Statistical Independence as a Substantive are acquired through nonverbal and verbal forms of
Fact of Personality Structure socialization, respectively), and (b) implicit motives,
Although McClelland has questioned the which develop first, do not influence the develop-
validity of questionnaires for the assessment of ment of explicit motives, because implicit motives
motives, his more central and long-standing expla- are not accessible to consciousness. In short, if
nation of the lack of correlation was that PSEs and implicit and explicit motives develop independently,
questionnaires assess distinct and independent con- then there should be no correlation between them
structs. That is, he challenged the alternative-methods across individuals—developmental independence
assumption. McClelland’s early position was that implies statistical independence.
PSEs assess motives, whereas so-called motive ques-
tionnaires assess schemata (McClelland, 1951) or Critique of McClelland’s Substantive
values (McClelland, 1980). Later, McClelland con- Interpretation of Independence
cluded that the schemata or values assessed by ques- It is difficult to overstate the importance
tionnaires satisfy his criteria for the definition of a of McClelland’s contribution. Virtually all motive
motive—they energize, direct, and select behavior— researchers today acknowledge the discriminant
but he argued that PSEs and questionnaires assess validity of implicit and explicit motives and regard
different kinds of motives, which he called implicit the implicit–explicit distinction as fundamentally
motives and self-attributed (i.e., explicit) motives, important. Nevertheless, being a pioneer meant
respectively (McClelland et al., 1989). getting a few things wrong. We turn our attention

th r a s h , ma rus ki n , ma rti n 


now to two problems with McClelland’s perspective are positively related rather than uncorrelated. Span-
and then propose a model that addresses these and gler (1992) conducted a meta-analysis of 105 stud-
other problems. ies and found that, on average, implicit and explicit
One problem is that McClelland’s justifications nAch were significantly positively correlated, albeit
for expecting statistical independence do not with- very weakly (r = .09). Although McClelland him-
stand scrutiny. We emphasize that we are not ques- self documented a significant positive correlation
tioning the discriminant validity of implicit and in some studies (e.g., deCharms et al., 1955),2 he
explicit motives; we accept that implicit and explicit tended to downplay the meaningfulness of such
motives are distinct in the sense that the correla- findings. For instance, in his textbook on human
tion between them is substantially less than r = 1.00 motivation, McClelland (1987) stated: “The most
and that they have distinct nomological networks. reasonable interpretation of such findings is that
Rather, we are questioning the justification for the these two types of measures are essentially indepen-
stronger claim that these distinct constructs are sta- dent, as they ought to be on theoretical grounds,
tistically independent (i.e., correlated at r = .00).1 and that when occasional correlations appear
Consider the argument that distinct develop- between them, they are the product of a peculiar
mental antecedents (e.g., nonverbal and verbal social- set of circumstances related to the particular group
ization practices) lead to statistical independence. being tested” (p. 521). McClelland’s strong stance
This argument rests on the unstated assumption on independence is understandable in its historical
that these antecedents are themselves uncorrelated. context; an emphasis on independence may have
After all, if the parents who promote achievement been necessary to convince skeptics that PSEs and
through nonverbal behavior are more likely than questionnaires assess different constructs. However,
other parents to also promote achievement through now that the discriminant validity of implicit and
verbally mediated training, then implicit and explicit explicit motives is no longer in question, there is no
nAch would be positively correlated across chil- reason to downplay evidence of a positive relation
dren; distinct but related developmental processes between them.
would produce distinct but related motives. Thus, The other problem with McClelland’s perspec-
the expectation of independence derives from an tive is that, while challenging the assumption that
assumption of independence and amounts to tautol- a single construct underlies PSE and questionnaire
ogy. Moreover, the identification of particular devel- measures, McClelland introduced a problematic
opmental antecedents that distinctively promote assumption of his own—that PSEs and question-
implicit motives and of others that distinctively naires are perfectly valid as measures of implicit and
promote explicit motives does not imply that there explicit motives, respectively. To be precise, McClel-
exist no antecedents that promote the development land certainly recognized that neither kind of mea-
of both, thereby producing a correlation between sure is perfectly valid; nevertheless, he neglected
the implicit and explicit motives (Thrash, Cassidy, this fact when he attributed the lack of correlation
Maruskin, & Elliot, 2010). between PSEs and questionnaires to the indepen-
Consider next the argument that implicit motives dence of implicit and explicit motive constructs
do not influence explicit motives because the former instead of (rather than in addition to) the imperfect
are not consciously accessible. We are aware of no relation between constructs and measures. The fol-
affirmative evidence (not based on null effects) that lowing quotation (in which n refers to implicit need
individuals have no awareness of their levels of implicit and v refers to explicit value) illustrates his treat-
motives, and there is some evidence to the contrary ment of methodological and substantive explana-
(e.g., Sherwood, 1966). Even if implicit motives are tions as mutually exclusive alternatives:
not directly accessible to consciousness, individuals
may learn about their implicit motives indirectly, This lack of correlation bothers a lot of people and
such as through feedback from others (Murray, they have used it as an argument that therefore, since
1938). Moreover, implicit and explicit motives could the v Achievement measures are more reliable, this
come into alignment through processes that do not proves that the n Achievement is not valid. To me,
require conscious awareness of implicit motives, such it demonstrated that these measures get at different
as those based on the reinforcing emotional conse- aspects of personality—n Achievement at operant
quences of motive congruence (Thrash et al., 2010). trends I called motives and v Achievement at
Theoretical issues aside, the available empirical values I called schemas.
evidence suggests that implicit and explicit motives (McClelland, 1980, p. 13)

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


Although Spangler’s analysis suggests that the the implications stemming from this general model,
implicit–explicit correlation is very weak (r = .09), we briefly discuss the model in the upper left, which
one could discover that the true correlation is con- is even more general (Fig. 9.2a).
siderably larger if the assumption of perfect valid- The general model in Figure 9.2b includes
ity is relaxed and threats to validity and reliability two underlying constructs (in ovals): an implicit
are actively identified and overcome. Indeed, the motive (e.g., implicit nAch) and the correspond-
history of other literatures (e.g., attitude–behavior ing explicit motive within the same content domain
consistency, trait–behavior consistency, and implicit– (e.g., explicit nAch). The curved arrow represents the
explicit attitude consistency) indicates that as mea- correlation between them. The underlying implicit
surement and methods improve, effect sizes tend to and explicit motive constructs are posited to influ-
increase. ence scores on PSE and questionnaire measures,
respectively (rectangles). Because the measures are
An Integrative, General Model posited not to be perfectly reliable or valid, each is
We present the models in Figure 9.2 as a frame- also posited to be influenced by an error term (cir-
work for understanding researchers’ explanations of cles). These error terms represent all influences on
the lack of correlation between questionnaires and the measure other than the underlying construct,
PSEs, as well as the assumptions that have led to these including extraneous constructs, method variance,
interpretations. Models representing researchers’ and random error.
explanations of the lack of correlation are illustrated If one begins with the general model in Fig-
in the right panel of Figure 9.2; models depicting ure 9.2b and imposes the assumption that implicit
the tacit assumptions that have led to these interpre- and explicit motive constructs are correlated at
tations are shown in the middle panel; and a general r = 1.00, then one arrives at the alternative-methods
model, in which these assumptions are relaxed, is model shown in Figure 9.2c, in which there is no
shown in the lower left (Fig. 9.2b). After discussing distinction between implicit and explicit motive

General models Tacit models Explanatory models


e.
Motive

a. c. PSE Que
Implicit Explicit Motive

PSE Que PSE Que f.


Motive

PSE Que

b.
Implicit Explicit
d. g.
Imp/PSE Exp/Que Imp/PSE Exp/Que
PSE Que

h.
Implicit Explicit

PSE Que

Fig. 9.2. Models that have been used to explain the lack of correlation between implicit and explicit motives (right); models depicting
the tacit assumptions that underlie the explanatory models (center); and general models that are relatively free of assumptions (left).

th r a s h , ma rus ki n , ma rti n 


constructs. It is understandable that researchers who Before moving on to those literatures, we pause
relied on this tacit model attributed the lack of cor- to specify some of the assumptions that underlie
relation between measures to inadequate validity of our own general model. In particular, the general
questionnaires (Fig. 9.2e) or PSEs (Fig. 9.2f ). The model in Figure 9.2b rests on the assumptions that
reasoning is sound but the premise (assumption) is (a) implicit motives do not influence scores on
not, thus leading these researchers to the incorrect questionnaires; (b) explicit motives do not influence
conclusion that one measure or the other is invalid. scores on PSEs; and (c) PSEs and questionnaires do
If one begins with the general model and instead not share method variance. A model in which these
imposes the assumption that each measure is corre- assumptions are relaxed is shown in Figure 9.2a. We
lated at r = 1.00 with its underlying construct (i.e., note that the assumptions underlying the model in
each measure is perfectly valid), then one arrives at Figure 9.2b are consistent with most existing the-
the model in Figure 9.2d, in which there is no dis- ory; therefore, this model has greater parsimony and
tinction between constructs and measures. Relying heuristic value than the model in Figure 9.2a for
on this tacit model, McClelland attributed the lack representing the diversity of assumptions and inter-
of correlation between measures to the independence pretations to date. However, we acknowledge that
of the underlying constructs (Fig. 9.2g). Again, the there may be grounds for challenging each of the
reasoning is sound but the premise (assumption) is assumptions that underlie the model in Figure 9.2b.
not, leading McClelland to conclude, incorrectly Whereas we have used this model as a framework for
it seems, that the implicit and explicit motive con- depicting problematic assumptions in past research,
structs are statistically independent. a future reviewer may profit from using the model
We propose that the best route forward is to in Figure 9.2a for the same purpose.
make neither assumption and to allow the gen-
eral model itself (Fig. 9.2b) to guide theory and Methodological Factors That Influence
research. This model implies that the correlation Estimates of the Implicit–Explicit
between PSEs and questionnaires is a multipli- Motive Correlation
cative function (for details, see Bollen, 1989) of Omnibus Effect of Multiple
(a) the true correlation between underlying implicit Methodological Factors
and explicit motive constructs, (b) the degree of Sherwood (1966) noted that a variety of fac-
relation between the implicit motive construct tors, such as lack of clarity about the task of intro-
and the PSE measure, and (c) the degree of rela- spection, defensiveness, and social desirability, may
tion between the explicit motive construct and the compromise the validity of questionnaire mea-
questionnaire measure. Because assumptions have sures, at least as they are typically administered.
been relaxed, this model is underdetermined in the Sherwood administered questionnaire measures of
sense that a weak correlation between measures is of nAch and nAff under special conditions designed
uncertain origin; it may result from a weak relation to minimize these problems. Specifically, he taught
between underlying constructs, from poor validity participants in detail about the implicit nAch and
of one or both measures, or from some combina- nAff constructs about which they were asked to
tion of these. We presume that each of these factors report explicitly; he sought to maximize motivation
contributes to the weak relation between PSE and to be accurate by framing the study as an opportu-
questionnaire measures (Fig. 9.2h), but this is an nity to develop self-insight; and he conducted the
empirical question. Our general model therefore study in the context of a trusting and nonevaluative
calls for research on two kinds of influences on the relationship with the experimenter. Implicit nAch
relation between PSE and questionnaire measures: and nAff were assessed under standard conditions
methodological factors concerning the relation at the beginning of the study. Also noteworthy is
between constructs and measures, and substantive a methodological refinement to which Sherwood
factors concerning the relation between underly- himself drew little attention: His questionnaires
ing constructs. This dual emphasis parallels similar were designed to correspond closely in content to
historical developments in the attitude-behavior the implicit motive coding systems. His findings
consistency and trait-behavior consistency litera- were striking. Across two studies, the relationships
tures (Kraus, 1995). Next, we review the emerging between implicit and explicit measures were posi-
literatures on methodological and substantive fac- tive and significant for both nAch (rs =.35, .42) and
tors that influence the relation between PSE and nAff (rs =.40, .34). These correlations are among the
questionnaire measures. strongest reported to date, suggesting that implicit

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


and explicit motives are robustly related when care when the measures are designed to cover the same
is taken to avoid problems that may compromise content universe.
the validity of explicit measures. Unfortunately, Schultheiss, Yankova, Dirlikov, and Schad (2009)
it is impossible to know to what extent each of sought to address the same issue but came to a dif-
Sherwood’s various methodological refinements ferent conclusion. These researchers assessed implicit
was responsible for his findings. Next we turn to nAch, nAff, and nPow using Winter’s (1994) coding
studies that have isolated particular methodological system and assessed explicit nAch, nAff, and nPow
factors. with a new measure that corresponded closely in
content to the implicit measure. The implicit and
Correspondence of Content explicit measures were also made to correspond in
Ajzen and Fishbein (1977) showed that attitude- a second respect. The explicit motive questionnaire
behavior consistency is attenuated when attitudes items were assessed with respect to the picture cues
and behaviors do not correspond closely in content used in the PSE; participants were asked to look at
or specificity. Thrash et al. (2007) suggested that the picture and respond to the questions as if they
a similar issue may apply to congruence between were a character in the picture. Schultheiss et al.
implicit and explicit motives. These authors argued reported that there was no relation between scores
that the relationship between implicit and explicit on these two sets of measures. They concluded,
motives may have been underestimated in past “Statistical independence between both construct
research, because questionnaires and PSEs gener- types can also be observed when the explicit mea-
ally have not been designed to correspond directly sure of motivation is made as similar as possible to
in content. For example, many measures of explicit the method of implicit motive assessment” (p. 78).
nAch are based on Murray’s (1938) early concep- The contradiction between the conclusions of
tualization of nAch (e.g., Edwards, 1959), whereas Thrash et al. (2007) and Schultheiss et al. (2009) may
McClelland’s coding system for implicit nAch was be reconciled if one distinguishes issues of statistical
derived empirically (McClelland et al., 1953) and independence (i.e., whether the implicit–explicit
deviates from Murray’s conceptualization (Koestner correlation equals zero) and effect size (i.e., whether
& McClelland, 1990). a nonzero correlation is small or large). Consider
To examine the impact of correspondence of first the issue of statistical independence. Schultheiss
content, Thrash et al. (2007) administered a PSE et al. reported the following correlations between
measure of implicit nAch and four questionnaire measures of implicit motives and matched-content
measures of explicit nAch. Implicit nAch was measures of explicit motives: nPow, r = .18, p < .05;
assessed using Schultheiss’s (2001b) translation of nAch, r = .11, ns; nAff, r = .12, ns. Interpretation of
Heckhausen’s (1963) coding system for hope for these findings depends on whether one accepts the
success. Three of the measures of explicit nAch were logic of null hypothesis testing. If so, then one rejects
traditional measures that had not been designed the null hypothesis of independence in the case of
to correspond in content to Heckhausen’s coding nPow and draws no conclusion in the cases of nAch
system. The fourth measure was a new question- and nAff. If not, then the correlations may be inter-
naire (Schultheiss & Murray, 2002) that consisted preted as the midpoints of the following 95% confi-
of five pairs of items that corresponded directly to dence intervals: nPow, r = .18 ± .14; nAch, r = .11 ±
categories of Heckhausen’s coding system (need for .14; nAff, r = .12 ± .14. In this case, there is no more
success, instrumental activity, expectation of suc- reason to interpret any of these effects as equaling
cess, praise, and positive affect). Results indicated r = .00 (which is near the bottom of the confidence
that the traditional measures of explicit nAch were intervals and therefore improbable as an estimate of
uncorrelated with implicit nAch (rs = .00 to .02), the population effects) than there is to interpret them
whereas the matched-content measure of explicit as equaling, say, r = .27 (which is near the top of the
nAch was significantly related to implicit nAch confidence intervals and therefore also improbable).
(r = .17). The latter effect with matched-content The best point estimates of the population effects
measures was significantly stronger than two of are the correlations themselves, which are uniformly
the three null effects that were obtained using weak, positive, and similar to the correlation of
nonmatched measures. This study shows that cor- r = .17 reported by Thrash et al. (2007). Regard-
respondence of content influences estimates of the less, both interpretations provide at least qualified
implicit–explicit correlation; implicit and explicit support for a proposal that, when the problem of
motives are positively related rather than unrelated correspondence of content is addressed, implicit and

th r a s h , ma rus ki n , ma rti n 


explicit motives are positively related. On the other matched-content measures, because the true (dis-
hand, these effects are certainly small—this is the attenuated) correlation would be expected to be
take-home message of the Schultheiss et al. findings robust only in the latter case. As expected, disat-
in our view. We conclude that when the problem of tenuation was found to modestly increase the effect
poor correspondence is addressed, across studies the size for nonmatched measures. As noted earlier, the
correlation between implicit and explicit motives is implicit–explicit correlations for nonmatched mea-
consistently positive but weak.3 sures ranged from r = .00 to .02 (ns). Use of latent
These effects are not so weak that they should variables increased the implicit–explicit correlation
be disregarded, however. One reason is that, as sug- for nonmatched measures to r = .07 (ns). More
gested by Sherwood’s (1966) findings, particular important, disattentuation had a pronounced effect
methodological factors are likely to have individu- for matched measures. As noted earlier, the implicit–
ally small but cumulatively robust effects. To further explicit correlation for matched measures reported
explore this possibility, we now consider a second by Thrash et al. (2007) was r = .17. Use of latent vari-
specific methodological factor. ables increased the implicit–explicit correlation to
r = .38 (p < .01). Although the impact of disattenua-
Reliability of Measurement tion was itself substantial, particularly striking are the
All measures demonstrate some degree of ran- combined effects of addressing the correspondence
dom measurement error, which attenuates effect and measurement error problems simultaneously.
sizes. If the correlation between implicit and explicit With nonmatching measures and without correct-
measures is not corrected for the unreliability of the ing for measurement error, the implicit-explicit
measures—and generally it is not—then the cor- correlations ranged from r = .00 to. 02; with match-
relation between underlying constructs is likely to ing measures and with correction for measurement
be underestimated. The proper means of correction error, the implicit–explicit correlation was r = .38.
depends on the theorized measurement model—that The latter correlation is in the range reported
is, the relation between the measured variables and by Sherwood (1966) who, as noted, also addressed
the construct of interest. Unfortunately, measure- multiple methodological problems simultaneously.
ment models are rarely explicitly specified or tested A second approach to disattenuation is to correct
in PSE research, and the measurement models that an observed correlation based on the reliabilities of
have been proposed are often unconventional (e.g., the two variables. A standard approach based on
Atkinson & Birch, 1970; for other possible models, traditional psychometric theory is to divide a cor-
see McClelland, 1987; Thrash et al., 2010). Because relation by the square root of the product of the
the issue of measurement models remains unre- internal consistencies of the two measures (Guilford
solved, we present the two most widely employed & Fruchter, 1978). We used results reported by
approaches to disattenuation. We illustrate the Schultheiss et al. (2009) to implement this tech-
first approach using data from Thrash et al. (2007) nique. Schultheiss et al. (2009) did not report inter-
and the second using data from Schultheiss et al. nal consistency values for the PSE variables, as is
(2009). customary in PSE research. Internal consistency is
One method of disattenuation is to use confir- underestimated for PSE measures, researchers have
matory factor analysis to remove the unique error argued, because traditional psychometric models
variance from particular indicators of a construct, are not appropriate for the PSE. We therefore used
resulting in latent variables that correspond more the internal consistencies of Schultheiss et al.’s
closely to the construct of interest. Thrash et al. measures of explicit nPow (Cronbach’s α = .64),
(2010) used this approach to reanalyze data from nAff (Cronbach’s α = .74), and nAch (Cronbach’s
Thrash et al. (2007). An implicit nAch latent vari- α = .84) as estimates of the internal consistencies
able was modeled using separate nAch scores for of the corresponding PSE measures. This approach
each of five stories as indicators. A nonmatched- is reasonable in that the measures were designed to
content explicit nAch latent variable was modeled be as similar as possible. Correcting the implicit–
using the three nonmatched measures as indica- explicit correlations for unreliability using the equa-
tors. Finally, a matched-content explicit nAch latent tion described earlier increases the implicit–explicit
variable was modeled using the 10 items from the correlations as follows: for nPow, the correlation
matched-content questionnaire as indicators. increases from r = .18 (p < .05) to .28 (p < .0001);
In theory, disattenuation should have less for nAff, the correlation increases from r = .12
impact for nonmatched-content measures than for (ns) to .16 (p < .05); and for nAch, the correlation

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


increases from r = .11 (ns) to .13 (p = .07). Thus, geometry, implicit and explicit measures are more
after disattenuation, two of the effects are signifi- strongly related when they concern the same domain
cant and one is marginally significant. These analy- than different domains. This finding is at odds with
ses provide further evidence that unreliability may the independent-constructs perspective.
lead the implicit–explicit correlation to be under- In sum, empirical evidence indicates that implicit
estimated. Moreover, although the effects remained and explicit motives are positively related, rather than
modest in size in this data set, these findings never- unrelated, when methods are refined by improving
theless provide additional unambiguous support for correspondence of content, correcting for unre-
the conclusion that the independence hypothesis liability, or using contemporary modeling tech-
does not hold when problems of correspondence of niques. The effects of methodological factors tend
content and measurement error are addressed. to be individually modest but cumulatively robust.
A large variety of other methodological factors
MTMM Analysis remains to be investigated. We encourage research-
As noted earlier, Campbell and colleagues con- ers to look to other literatures, such as the attitude–
cluded, based on inspection of MTMM matrices, behavior consistency, trait–behavior consistency,
that PSEs and questionnaires failed tests of conver- and implicit–explicit attitude consistency litera-
gence. However, now that the discriminant validity tures, for precedents. Epstein’s (1979) research, for
of implicit and explicit motives is well established, instance, suggests that implicit–explicit correlations
the relevant question is not whether they converge will increase if test–retest reliability is enhanced by
strongly enough to be considered alternative indi- aggregating motive scores from multiple occasions.
cators of the same construct (i.e., whether the cor-
relation approaches r = 1.00), but rather whether Substantive Variables That Moderate
they converge at all (i.e., whether the correlation is Implicit–Explicit Motive Congruence
greater than r = .00). We also note that Campbell The argument that implicit and explicit motive
and Fiske’s (1959) approach is outdated in an constructs are positively correlated rather than
important respect. Among their criteria for validity uncorrelated is more than an academic quibble;
was the requirement that measures have more trait these two scenarios have very different implications
variance than method variance. With contemporary for theory and application. A correlation of r = .00
modeling techniques (e.g., CFA, multidimensional between underlying constructs suggests (but does
scaling [MDS]), this issue is relatively unimportant, not guarantee) that no integrative mechanisms are
because trait variance and method variance may be operative. In contrast, a positive correlation suggests
unconfounded through analysis of underlying latent (but does not guarantee) that an integrative process
variables or dimensions. is operative and calls for research focused on identi-
Recently, Bilsky and Schwartz (2008) used MDS fying such processes.
to conduct a MTMM analysis of three previously Moreover, a weak correlation is not necessarily
published data sets in which both PSEs and ques- evidence that integrative mechanisms tend to be
tionnaires were used to measure motivations in the ineffectual and therefore neglible. Another possibil-
achievement, affiliation, and power domains. The ity is that integrative mechanisms are effectual but
aim of the MDS analysis was to derive a spatial operative among some rather than all members of
representation (i.e., in two or three dimensions) of the population. Imagine that the true correlation
motive domain and method facets, such that more between implicit and explicit nAch is r = .35. This
highly correlated measures are located closer together correlation could be the net result of combining two
in physical space. The MDS approach is well suited subgroups of individuals: one in which integrative
for the question at hand, because it is based on the processes are operative (resulting in a correlation of,
relative strengths of the correlations among measures say, r = .74 within this subgroup), and another in
and does not require that any of the correlations be which integrative processes are not operative (result-
strong in an absolute sense. In all three data sets, ing in a correlation of, say, r = .00 within this sub-
Bilsky and Schwartz found that motive domains group). The effect of combining these two subgroups
formed pie-piece-like wedges in a two-dimensional into one group is illustrated with hypothetical data
space, whereas methods were represented by con- in Figure 9.3. Within the past 10 years, researchers
centric circles, which varied from more implicit to have begun to investigate substantive moderating
more explicit or vice versa as the radius increased. variables that specify the groups of individuals (e.g.,
This structure indicates that, as may be shown with individuals with high or low levels of particular

th r a s h , ma rus ki n , ma rti n 


Explicit nAch

Explicit nAch

Explicit nAch
Implicit nAch Implicit nAch Implicit nAch
r = .74 + r = .00 r = .35

Fig. 9.3. Hypothetical data showing how a relatively weak correlation may be the net result of combining two subgroups, one in
which the correlation is strong and one in which the correlation equals zero.

traits) among whom the implicit–explicit relation is chance if these individuals internalize values regard-
weaker or stronger. We focus our review on studies less of their fit with implicit motives.
of implicit and explicit motives per se. For recent More recently, Hofer, Busch, Bond, Kärtner,
reviews of studies of congruence between motives Kiessling, and Law (2010) tested the generalizabil-
and goals, some of which preceded the motive con- ity of the self-determination finding across cultures,
gruence studies reviewed here (e.g., Schultheiss & using data from Cameroon, Germany, and Hong
Brunstein, 1999), see Brunstein (2010) and Thrash Kong. Consistent with the finding reported by
et al. (2010). Thrash and Elliot (2002), Hofer et al. reported that
self-determination moderated the relation between
Self-Determination implicit nAch and explicit achievement goals, such
The topic of personality congruence has been of that implicit nAch and explicit achievement goals
interest not only in the motive literature but also in were positively related among individuals high,
humanistic theories, including traditional theories, but not low, in self-determination. This moderation
such as that of Rogers (1959), and contemporary effect was found to be invariant across cultures.
theories, such as Deci and Ryan’s self-determination
theory (Deci & Ryan, 1991). Thrash and Elliot Private Body Consciousness,
(2002) sought to integrate motive and humanistic Self-Monitoring, and Preference
approaches by showing that individuals who are more for Consistency
self-determined display greater motive congruence. Thrash et al. (2007) argued that at least three
Self-determination refers to self-regulation in distinct processes contribute to motive congruence:
accord with one’s authentic or true self. Individuals access to one’s implicit motives, integration of one’s
differ in self-determination, such that some individ- explicit motives with one’s implicit motives, and
uals live according to their core interests and values, resistance to competing sources of values. Regarding
whereas others live according to others’ expecta- access to implicit motives, Thrash et al. argued that
tions or controlling influences in the environment. motive congruence may be greater among individuals
Thrash and Elliot (2002) argued that the experience higher in private body consciousness; these individu-
of self-determination may reflect (at least in part) als are sensitive to bodily states and therefore may per-
the integration of explicit values with one’s preexist- ceive the effects of implicit motive arousal. Regarding
ing and deep-seated implicit motivational tenden- integration, Thrash et al. argued that congruence may
cies, as opposed to the internalization of explicit be greater among individuals higher in preference for
values arbitrarily from the environment regardless consistency; these individuals would be particularly
of their fit to one’s implicit motives. As expected, motivated to reconcile their explicit motives with any
self-determination was found to moderate the rela- rudimentary knowledge of their implicit motives.
tion between implicit and explicit nAch. Among Regarding resistance to competing sources of values,
individuals high in self-determination, implicit Thrash et al. argued that congruence may be greater
nAch robustly predicted explicit nAch, r = .40, among individuals lower in self-monitoring; these
p < .01; in other words, self-determined individuals individuals are less likely to monitor others’ expecta-
tended to be congruent. Among individuals low in tions and to internalize others’ values arbitrarily. As
self-determination, implicit and explicit nAch were predicted, implicit nAch was found to predict explicit
largely unrelated, r = –.07, ns; in other words, indi- nAch among individuals high but not low in private
viduals low in self-determination tended to be either body consciousness, high but not low in preference for
congruent or incongruent, as would be expected by consistency, and low but not high in self-monitoring.

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


Among individuals with the most advantageous pro- self-determination findings reported earlier, these
file of traits (i.e., high in private body consciousness, findings suggest that explicit motives become inte-
high in preference for consistency, and low in self- grated with implicit motives to the extent that the
monitoring), the correlation between implicit and socialization environment supports satisfaction of the
explicit nAch was r = .46 (p < .05); among individu- basic needs theorized to underlie self-determination.
als with the opposite profile of traits, the correlation
was r = –.30 (ns). All three moderators were found to Stress and Affect Regulation
account for unique variance in motive congruence. Whereas Schattke et al. (2011) examined dis-
Building on this multiple-process perspective, Thrash tal, developmental antecedents of congruence,
et al. (2010) recently developed a general meta- Baumann, Kaschel, and Kuhl (2005) examined
theoretical framework that may be useful in iden- a proximal antecedent: stress. Working from the
tifying additional processes through which motive perspective of personality interactions theory, these
congruence may emerge. authors argued that stress leads to motive incongru-
ence among state-oriented (as opposed to action-
Antecedents of Motive Congruence oriented) individuals. State-oriented individuals
Next we review studies that address essentially have difficulty generating positive affect in response
the same issue as those in the last section, except to demand-related stressors and/or difficulty over-
that congruence is modeled differently. The studies coming negative affect in response to threat-related
in the prior section concerned independent variables stressors. In two correlational studies and an experi-
that moderate the relation between implicit and ment in which stress was manipulated, Baumann
explicit motives. For instance, self-determination et al. reported that state orientation interacted with
was found to moderate the relation between implicit stress, such that greater stress predicted a greater dis-
and explicit nAch. The studies in this section con- crepancy between implicit and explicit nAch among
cern variables that predict the discrepancy between state-oriented individuals but not among action-
implicit and explicit motives; that is, the implicit oriented individuals. The relation between stress
and explicit motive variables are reduced to a sin- and motive incongruence among state-oriented
gle incongruence variable, which is treated as the individuals was theorized to reflect a disruption of
dependent variable. For instance, if one were to test communication between the memory systems asso-
the self-determination hypothesis in this way, one ciated with implicit and explicit motives (extension
would expect to find that individuals higher in self- and intention memory, respectively).
determination have less of a discrepancy between The studies in the prior section (regarding mod-
implicit and explicit nAch. Consistent with the dif- erators) and this section (regarding antecedents) pro-
ference in modeling strategies, we refer to the pre- vide additional evidence that implicit and explicit
dictor variables in this section as antecedents rather motives are not statistically independent. The posi-
than moderators. tive correlation between implicit and explicit motives
may be viewed as the overall or average relation. The
Need Satisfaction studies in the moderator section showed that the cor-
Schattke, Koestner, and Kehr (2011) examined relation varies systematically as a function of third
the childhood antecedents of incongruence in adults, variables. The studies in this section, similarly, indi-
with hypotheses grounded in self-determination cate that one’s standing as congruent or incongruent
theory. These authors predicted that childhood expe- varies systematically as a function of predictor vari-
riences that interfere with the development of ables. We caution, however, that stress was the only
self-determination—specifically, those that thwart variable that was manipulated in these studies. Stress
satisfaction of the basic needs for relatedness and appears to play a role in causing incongruence, but
autonomy—would predict incongruence later in the other moderators and antecedents of congruence
life. Based on new analyses of an archival data set may or may not be causes of congruence.
(McClelland & Pilon, 1983; Sears, Maccoby, &
Levin, 1957), Schattke et al. reported that experi- Consequences of Motive Congruence
ences involving deprivation of the need for auton- Although, as we have shown, implicit and explicit
omy (e.g., maternal inhibition of sexuality) or of the motives are positively related when methodological
need for relatedness (e.g., separation from the mother shortcomings of past research are overcome, the cor-
during the second year of life) predicted levels of relation remains weak enough that the discrepancy
incongruence 26 years later. Consistent with the between them carries important information about

th r a s h , ma rus ki n , ma rti n 


congruence of personality. This conclusion is sug- Consistent with past findings (Muraven & Baumeis-
gested by the aforementioned moderator and ante- ter, 2000), such regulation was posited to deplete
cedent findings and would be further supported by limited volitional resources. In Kehr’s study, implicit
evidence that individuals’ levels of congruence have motives were assessed using the Multi-Motive Grid
theoretically meaningful consequences. Researchers (MMG; Sokolowski, Schmalt, Langens, & Puca,
are in agreement that congruence is beneficial rela- 2000), an instrument that we classify as implicit for
tive to incongruence. In this section, we review the present purposes but that also has some properties of
literature on variables that have been posited to be explicit measures. As predicted, Kehr found that dis-
consequences of motive congruence. crepancies between managers’ implicit and explicit
motives, averaged across content domains (achieve-
Identity Status ment, affiliation, power), predicted lower levels of
Marcia’s (1966) theory of identity recognizes volitional strength 5 months later.
four identity statuses: moratorium, achievement,
diffusion, and foreclosure. Individuals who have Flow
actively searched for an identity have the status of Flow refers to an optimal state in which one is
moratorium if they have not yet committed them- completely involved in an activity, to the point of
selves to an identity, or the status of identity achieve- becoming unaware of anything else (Csikszentmi-
ment if they have. Individuals who have not actively halyi, 1990). Rheinberg (2008) argued that the
searched for an identity have the status of diffusion volitional regulation necessitated by motive incon-
if they have not yet committed to an identity, or gruence hinders flow experiences. Consistent with
the status of foreclosure if they have internalized an this argument, Clavadetscher (2003) found that the
identity from the social environment despite lack of discrepancy between implicit and explicit motives
exploration. predicted less flow among workers. Schüler (2010)
Regarding individuals who have committed argued that such effects are likely to be manifest
themselves to an identity, Hofer, Busch, Chasiotis, only when the situation involves achievement cues;
and Kiessling (2006) argued that identity achieve- in the presence of achievement cues, motive con-
ment stems from discovering one’s implicit motives flicts that are otherwise dormant are aroused and
and adopting explicit motives that are consistent interfere with task engagement. Schüler confirmed
with them, whereas foreclosure stems from adopt- this hypothesis in a series of three studies, including
ing others’ explicit values regardless of their com- a longitudinal study in which the dependent vari-
patibility with one’s implicit motives. As predicted, able represented change in flow and an experiment
Hofer et al. found that implicit and explicit nAff in which the presence of achievement incentives was
interacted in the prediction of identity achievement, experimentally manipulated.
such that explicit nAff was a more positive predictor
of identity achievement among individuals higher Well-Being
in implicit nAff. Also as predicted, implicit and Well-being is the outcome variable that has
explicit nAff interacted in the prediction of fore- received the most attention to date. Kehr (2004)
closure, such that explicit nAff was a more positive found that implicit–explicit motive discrepancies
predictor of foreclosure among individuals lower in predicted lower levels of affective well-being longi-
implicit nAff. No effects emerged for the statuses tudinally. Lower levels of volitional strength medi-
that do not involve commitment to an identity (i.e., ated (explained) this effect. Baumann et al. (2005)
moratorium, diffusion). These findings suggest that found that incongruence between implicit and
attaining a sense of identity requires discovery of explicit nAch predicted lower levels of subjective
one’s implicit motives and embracing them as the well-being and more psychosomatic complaints.
foundation of one’s values. Baumann et al. also showed that incongruence
mediated the effect of the stress × state orientation
Volitional Strength interaction on these outcomes. Hofer, Chasiotis,
Kehr (2004) examined the relationship between and Campos (2006) found that congruence in the
implicit–explicit motive congruence and volitional power domain, but not in the affiliation domain,
regulation within a sample of managers. Kehr pos- predicted greater life satisfaction in three cultures.
ited that discrepancies between implicit and explicit Kazén and Kuhl (2011) found that a directional
motives lead to psychological conflict and that reso- nPow discrepancy, in which explicit nPow was
lution of this conflict requires volitional regulation. strong and implicit nPow was weak, predicted lower

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


levels of well-being and higher levels of stress among Future Directions
managers. How Should Motive Congruence
Null effects of motive congruence on well-being Be Modeled?
have also been reported (McAuley, Bond, & Ng, Analytic strategies for modeling congruence have
2004). For this empirical reason, and based on the- varied across studies. These strategies include the
ory about the conditions under which incongruence following: (a) testing moderation of the implicit-
is more or less likely to be problematic, research- explicit relation (e.g., Thrash & Elliot, 2002);
ers have begun to document factors that moder- (b) computing difference scores based on standard-
ate the effect of motive congruence on well-being. ized implicit and explicit motive variables (e.g., Kehr,
Motive incongruence has been found to be less 2004); (c) testing whether one motive moderates the
problematic when the incongruent motives are not effect of the other (i.e., implicit motive × explicit
aroused through motive-relevant activity (Schüler, motive interactions; e.g., Hofer, Chasiotis et al.,
Job, Fröhlich, & Brandstätter, 2008), when motive 2006); and, most recently, (d) using polynomial
expression is inhibited by a dispositional trait called regression with response surface analysis (Kazén &
activity inhibition (Langens, 2007), when the indi- Kuhl, 2011), a sophisticated variant of the implicit
vidual uses emotional disclosure as a coping strategy × explicit motive interaction approach (Edwards,
(Langan-Fox, Sankey, & Canty, 2009; Schüler, Job, 2002). In most cases, implicit and explicit motives
Fröhlich, & Brandstätter, 2009), and among indi- are modeled as separate variables, but in some cases
viduals high in self-directedness or internal locus of they are combined into a single discrepancy index.
control (Langan-Fox et al., 2009). Most studies have examined nondirectional incon-
In sum, motive congruence appears to have a gruence, which results from either motive being
variety of important consequences. Individuals who higher than the other, but some studies have exam-
are more congruent have greater volitional strength, ined incongruence in a particular direction. These
are more prone to flow experiences, and experience various operationalizations of congruence, which
higher levels of well-being. These variables are gen- imply subtly different conceptualizations of con-
erally posited to be effects of motive congruence. gruence and vary in statistical rigor, have not yet
However, whether motive congruence is the cause received detailed discussion. We encourage research-
of these outcomes has not been shown definitively; ers to articulate the rationale for their operational-
indeed, demonstrating effects of motive congruence ization and its suitability to the substantive issue
is difficult, because motives (as aspects of personal- that is being addressed. The robustness of findings
ity) and the discrepancy between them are not read- across operationalizations of congruence is also in
ily amenable to experimental control. Thrash et al. need of greater scrutiny.
(2010) identified several strategies for documenting
causality that may be useful in future research on What Can Motive Researchers
motive congruence. Learn From Freud?
It is striking how similar the issues surround-
Conclusion ing the topic of motive congruence are to the issues
In this chapter, we have shown to be incorrect the that interested Sigmund Freud and subsequent
prevailing wisdom that implicit and explicit motives psychoanalysts. Nevertheless, motive researchers,
are uncorrelated. When care is taken to address like empirical researchers more generally, tend not
methodological problems of past research, implicit to cite Freud’s work (see Weinberger & McClel-
and explicit motives are positively correlated across land, 1990, for a notable exception). It is not clear
individuals. Nevertheless, the correlation is weak whether this inattention to Freud is based on the
enough that the discrepancy between implicit and belief that Freud’s writings have nothing to offer, or
explicit motives carries important information. We a fear that citing Freud will undermine the appear-
have reviewed evidence showing that the relation ance of scientific credibility, or some other reason.
between implicit and explicit motives varies system- Whatever the reason, we have found that Freud’s
atically as a function of moderator variables. Motive writings contain many important insights (as well
congruence also has meaningful antecedents and as claims that warrant skepticism) and believe that
important consequences. We are excited about the inattention to these insights undermines rather than
rapid development of the motive congruence litera- serves scientific credibility and progress. We there-
ture and, in the following, identify several impor- fore encourage researchers to explore more fully the
tant questions to be addressed in future research. theoretical and historical underpinnings of their

th r a s h , ma rus ki n , ma rti n 


subject matter. This recommendation brings us to congruence between implicit and explicit motives
our next question for future research. (or between the levels of implicit and explicit motiva-
tional states) is likely to vary not only across individ-
May Incongruence Be Integrated? uals but also across time (for a particular individual),
Freud (1909/1989) argued that an individual across content domains (for a particular individual),
has three healthy options after bringing uncon- across countries (for the average individual), and
scious material into awareness: accept it, reject it, so on. We believe that generalizing and extending
or sublimate it. In contrast, the prevailing assump- conceptualizations of congruence to new levels of
tion in the motive literature seems to be that there analysis will be among the most fruitful avenues for
is one healthy option: accept it—that is, embrace future research.
one’s implicit motives as the basis of explicit motive
adoption. We encourage researchers to entertain the Notes
possibility that rejecting or rechanneling (“sublimat- . McClelland and others who have argued that implicit
and explicit motives are statistically independent have some-
ing”) one’s implicit motives may sometimes be the
times qualified this claim by stating that implicit and explicit
healthier option, particularly in the case of implicit motives are “generally,” “essentially,” or “largely” independent.
motives (e.g., implicit nPow or implicit avoidance We acknowledge this fact but emphasize McClelland’s focus
motives) that do not promote, or that thwart, satis- on independence for the following reasons: (a) McClelland
faction of fundamental human needs. Thrash et al. appeared to use these qualifications in order for his language to
be consistent with the empirical facts, but these empirical facts
(2010) proposed that a self-determined, mindful
appear not to have influenced his theorizing; (b) quite often,
decision to reject an implicit motive represents McClelland did not qualify the claim of statistical independence;
a form of integrated incongruence that may be (c) McClelland (1987) explicitly denied the meaningfulness of
healthier than incongruence arising through other implicit–explicit correlations when they emerged; and (d) for
processes (e.g., chance). Integration, which refers theory to progress, it is necessary to sharpen distinctions (e.g.,
between “independent” and “essentially independent”) that have
to unity of structure and coordination of function,
been obscured in the past.
may ultimately be more important than the simpler . McClelland et al. (1989), and many others since, mistak-
mathematical notion of congruence or discrepancy. enly stated that implicit and explicit nAch were uncorrelated in
this study.
May Congruence Be Unintegrated? . Part of Schultheiss et al.’s basis for concluding that implicit
and explicit motives were statistically independent was that an
Regarding the literature on moderators of con- omnibus test of a full correlation matrix was nonsignificant. This
gruence, we have argued that integrative processes matrix included all nine pairwise correlations between implicit
may be operative for individuals at one pole of a and explicit measures of nAch, nPow, and nAff. The three on-
moderating variable and not operative for individu- diagonal correlations in this matrix (i.e., the correlations between
als at the other pole. A potentially important impli- implicit and explicit nPow, between implicit and explicit nAch,
and between implicit and explicit nAff) are the congruence coef-
cation is that congruent individuals are found at ficients that we have discussed. The six off-diagonal correlations
both poles of the moderator, but they are congruent involve mismatches of content domain (e.g., the correlation
for different reasons. For instance, one would expect between implicit nPow and explicit nAff). We have disregarded
most individuals high in self-determination to be this omnibus test, because it confounds our hypothesis (i.e.,
congruent as a result of an integrative process, and that implicit and explicit measures within the same content
domain are correlated if their content is carefully matched) with
one would expect roughly half of the individuals low a hypothesis that is at odds with our hypothesis (i.e., that implicit
in self-determination to be congruent as a result of and explicit measures from entirely different content domains are
chance. In the literatures on antecedents and conse- correlated). Campbell and Fiske (1959) observed that the mean-
quences of congruence, there has been no distinction ingfulness of the on-diagonal correlations is enhanced when the
between these two sets of congruent individu- off-diagonal correlations are weak rather than strong.
als. We speculate that unintegrated congruence—
congruence that arises by chance (for a broader defi- References
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations:
nition of unintegrated congruence, see Thrash et al., A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research.
2010)—may be less stable and beneficial than con- Psychological Bulletin, 84, 888–918.
gruence that arises through an integrative process. Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action.
New York: Wiley.
Baumann, N., Kaschel, R., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Striving for
Does Congruence Vary at Other Levels of
unwanted goals: Stress-dependent discrepancies between
Analysis? explicit and implicit achievement motives reduce subjective
Researchers to date have focused on congruence well-being and increase psychosomatic symptoms. Journal of
at the between-person level of analysis. However, Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 781–799.

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


Biernat, M. (1989). Motives and values to achieve: Different satisfaction across three cultures. European Journal of Person-
constructs with different effects. Journal of Personality, 57, ality, 20, 305–324.
69–95. Kazén, M., & Kuhl, J. (2011). Discrepancies between explicit
Bilsky, W., & Schwartz, S. H. (2008). Measuring motivations: and implicit power motive decreases managers’ well-being.
Integrating content and method. Personality and Individual Motivation and Emotion. doi: 10.1007/s11031–011-9219–8
Differences, 44, 1738–1751. Kehr, H. M. (2004). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and
Bollen, K. A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social
New York: Wiley. Psychology Bulletin, 30, 315–327.
Brunstein, J. C. (2010). Implicit motives and explicit goals: Koestner, R., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Perspectives on
The role of motivational congruence in emotional well- competence motivation. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of
being. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.), personality: Theory and research (pp. 527–548). New York:
Implicit motives (pp. 347–374). New York: Oxford Univer- Guilford.
sity Press. Kraus, S. J. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior:
Burwen, L. S., & Campbell, D. T. (1957). The generality of atti- A metaanalysis of the empirical literature. Personality and
tudes toward authority and nonauthority figures. Journal of Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 58–75.
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 54, 24–31. Langan-Fox, J., Sankey, M. J., & Canty, J. M. (2009). Incon-
Campbell, D. T., & Fiske, D. (1959). Convergent and discrimi- gruence between implicit and self-attributed achievement
nant validation by the multitrait–multimethod matrix. Psy- motives and psychological well-being: The moderating role
chological Bulletin, 56, 81–105. of self-directedness, self-disclosure and locus of control.
Clavadetscher, C. (2003). Motivation ehrenamtlicher Arbeit im Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 99–104.
Verein Mahogany Hall, Bern [Motivation for voluntary work Langens, T. A. (2007). Congruence between implicit and explicit
in the association Mahogany Hall, Bern]. Bern, Switzerland: motives and emotional well-being: The moderating role of
Abschlussarbeit NDS BWL/UF, Hochschule für Technik activity inhibition. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 49–59.
und Architektur. Marcia, J. E. (1966). Development and validation of ego identity
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 551–558.
experience. New York: Harper & Row. McAuley, P. C., Bond, M. H., & Ng, I. W. (2004). Antecedents
deCharms, R., Morrison, H. W., Reitman, W., & McClelland, D. C. of subjective well-being in working Hong Kong adults. Jour-
(1955). Behavioral correlates of directly and indirectly mea- nal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 5, 25–49.
sured achievement motivation. In D. C. McClelland (Ed.), McClelland, D. C. (1951). Personality. New York: Sloane.
Studies in motivation (pp. 414–423). New York: Appleton- McClelland, D. C. (1980). Motive dispositions. The merits
Century-Crofts. of operant and respondent measures. In L. Wheeler (Ed.),
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1991). A motivational approach Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 10–41).
to self: Integration in personality. In R. Dienstbier (Ed.), Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Nebraska symposium on motivation: Perspectives on motiva- McClelland, D. C. (1987). Human motivation. New York:
tion (Vol. 38, pp. 237–288). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Cambridge University Press.
Press. McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell,
Edwards, A. L. (1959). Edwards Personality Preference Schedule E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. New York: Appleton-
manual—revised. Cleveland, OH: Psychological Corporation. Century-Crofts.
Edwards, J. R. (2002). Alternatives to difference scores: Polyno- McClelland, D. C., Koestner, R., & Weinberger, J. (1989). How
mial regression analysis and response surface methodology. do self-attributed and implicit motives differ? Psychological
In F. Drasgow & N. W. Schmitt (Eds.), Advances in measure- Review, 96, 690–702.
ment and data analysis (pp. 350–400). San Francisco, CA: McClelland, D. C., & Pilon, D. A. (1983). Sources of adult
Jossey-Bass. motives in patterns of parent behavior in early childhood.
Epstein, S. (1979). The stability of behavior: I. On predicting Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 564–574.
most of the people much of the time. Journal of Personality Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and
and Social Psychology, 37, 1097–1126. depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a
Freud, S. (1909/1989). Five lectures on psycho-analysis: The stan- muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259.
dard edition. New York: Norton & Co. Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York:
Guilford, J. P., & Fruchter, B. (1978). Fundamental statistics in Oxford.
psychology and education (6th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. Raven, J. (1988). Toward measures of high-level competencies:
Heckhausen, H. (1963). Hoffnung und Furcht in der Leistungs- A re-examination of McClelland’s distinction between needs
motivation [Hope and fear components of achievement motiva- and values. Human Relations, 41, 281–294.
tion]. Meisenheim am Glan, Germany: Anton Hain. Rheinberg, F. (2008). Intrinsic motivation and flow experience.
Hofer, J., Busch, H., Bond, M. H., Kärtner, J., Kiessling, F., & In H. Heckhausen & J. Heckhausen (Eds.), Motivation and
Law, R. (2010). Is self-determined functioning a universal action (pp. 323–348). Cambridge, England: Cambridge
prerequisite for motive-goal congruence? Examining the University Press.
domain of achievement in three cultures. Journal of Personal- Rogers, C. R. (1959). A theory of therapy, personality, and inter-
ity, 78, 747–779. personal relationships, as developed in the client-centered
Hofer, J., Busch, H., Chasiotis, A., & Kiessling, F. (2006). framework. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a sci-
Motive congruence and interpersonal identity status. Journal ence: Vol. 3. Formulations of the person and the social context
of Personality, 74, 511–541. (pp. 184–215). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Hofer, J., Chasiotis, A., & Campos, D. (2006). Congruence Schattke, K., Koestner, R., & Kehr, H. M. (2011). Childhood
between social values and implicit motives: Effects on life correlates of adult levels of incongruence between implicit

th r a s h , ma rus ki n , ma rti n 


and explicit motives. Motivation and Emotion. doi, 35, Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., & Levin, H. (1957). Patterns of
306–316. child rearing. Evanston, IL: Row Peterson.
Schüler, J. (2010). Achievement incentives determine the effects Sherwood, J. J. (1966). Self-report and projective measures of
of achievement-motive incongruence on flow experience. achievement and affiliation. Journal of Consulting Psychology,
Motivation and Emotion, 34, 2–14. 30, 329–337.
Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S. M., & Brandstätter, V. (2008). Sokolowski, K., Schmalt, H-D., Langens, T. A., & Puca, R. M.
A high implicit affiliation motive does not always make you (2000). Assessing achievement, affiliation, and power motives
happy: A corresponding explicit motive and corresponding all at once: The Multi-Motive Grid (MMG). Journal of Per-
behavior are further needed. Motivation and Emotion, 32, sonality Assessment, 74, 126–145.
231–242. Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire and TAT
Schüler, J., Job, V., Fröhlich, S. M., & Brandstätter, V. (2009). measures of need for achievement: Two meta-analyses.
Dealing with a ‘hidden stressor’: Emotional disclosure as a Psychological Bulletin, 112, 140–154.
coping strategy to overcome the negative effects of motive Thrash, T. M., Cassidy, S. E., Maruskin, L. A., & Elliot, A. J.
incongruence on health. Stress and Health, 25, 221–233. (2010). Factors that influence the relation between implicit
Schultheiss, O. C. (2001a). An information processing account and explicit motives: A general implicit-explicit congruence
of implicit motive arousal. In M. L. Maehr & P. Pintrich framework. In O. C. Schultheiss & J. C. Brunstein (Eds.),
(Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement (Vol. 12, Implicit motives (pp. 308–346). New York: Oxford Univer-
pp. 1–41). Greenwich, CT: JAI. sity Press.
Schultheiss, O. C. (2001b). Manual for the assessment of hope of suc- Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Implicit and self-attributed
cess and fear of failure (English translation of Heckhausen’s need achievement motives: Concordance and predictive validity.
for achievement measure). Department of Psychology, Univer- Journal of Personality, 70, 729–755.
sity of Michigan, Ann Arbor: Unpublished manuscript. Thrash, T. M., Elliot, A. J., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). Method-
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: ological and dispositional predictors of congruence between
Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. implicit and explicit need for achievement. Personality and
Journal of Personality, 67, 1–38. Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 961–974.
Schultheiss, O. C., & Murray, T. (2002). Hope of success/fear of Weinberger, J., & McClelland, D. C. (1990). Cognitive versus
failure questionnaire. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan, traditional motivational models: Irreconcilable or complemen-
Department of Psychology. tary? In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook
Schultheiss, O. C., Yankova, D., Dirlikov, B., & Schad, D. J. of motivation and cognition (Vol. 2, pp. 562–597). New York:
(2009). Are implicit and explicit motive measures statistically Guilford.
independent? A fair and balanced test using the Picture Story Winter, D. G. (1994). Manual for scoring motive imagery in run-
Exercise and a cue- and response-matched questionnaire ning text (4th ed.). Unpublished manuscript, Department of
measure. Journal of Personality Assessment, 91, 72–81. Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor.

 im pl icit–explicit motive congruen c e


CHAPTER

Curiosity and Motivation


10
Paul J. Silvia

Abstract
Curiosity is an old, intriguing, and vexing construct in the psychology of motivation. This chapter reviews
the major strands of thought on curiosity and motivation: (1) curiosity as a motive to reduce negative
states, such as uncertainty, novelty, arousal, drive, or information gaps; (2) curiosity as a source of
intrinsic motivation that fosters learning and exploring for their own sakes; and (3) curiosity as a stable
motivational difference between people that leads to differences in knowledge, goals, achievement, and
experience. The scope of psychological thought on curiosity defies a simple integration, but it offers
inspiration for researchers curious about why people learn and explore in the absence of obvious
external rewards.
Key Words: curiosity, interest, intrinsic motivation, personality

The joys of history are many. Some are guilty modesty is called for. Instead, we’ll aim to cultivate
joys, like snickering at the weird use of semicolons a sense of perspective—some of psychology’s prob-
and em-dashes in old scholarly books, but some are lems should be appreciated rather than solved—and
more noble, like recognizing that most of the major a sense of interest in the diverse, wide-ranging, and
problems in psychology were identified prior to the occasionally weird body of thought on curiosity.
invention of air conditioning, paperback books, or This chapter extracts and explores the three
hierarchical linear modeling. Curiosity is an old major strands of thought on curiosity’s motivational
concept in the study of human motivation, and nature. The first strand proposes that curiosity is a
like many of psychology’s venerable problems, the kind of deficit motivation: It motivates people to fill
problem of curiosity seems tractable enough to be gaps in knowledge, reduce unpleasant uncertainty,
intriguing but too complicated to ever solve. The and minimize aversive states of drive. The second
history of psychology thus offers modern research- strand proposes that curiosity is a kind of intrinsic
ers a lot of interesting takes on curiosity: Most of motivation: It motivates people to explore and learn
the major schools of thought in motivation science for their own sakes. The third strand proposes indi-
have had something to say about what curiosity is, vidual differences in curiosity: Variation in curiosity
how it works, and what it does, if anything. translates into big differences in behavior and life
In this chapter, we’ll consider some of the major outcomes, although the models disagree about the
themes that have emerged in the history of thought motivational nature of between-person variation.
on curiosity and motivation. Our goal isn’t to
develop a unified model of curiosity by reinterpret- Curiosity Is Motivation to Reduce Novelty
ing past work in terms of a modern theory. History and Uncertainty
provides many such models, and the fact that there Our first major strand of thought on curiosity’s
are so many suggests that some history-induced motivational nature proposes that curiosity and


exploratory behavior represent attempts to reduce research that showed that drive-increasing stimuli—
novelty and uncertainty. In such models, gaining such as novelty and intense stimulation—could
knowledge and exploring the world are instrumen- reinforce learning. For example, the opportunity to
tal acts, not ultimate goals in themselves. Instead, view a novel maze compartment or complex display
learning and exploring allow people to reduce or to manipulate puzzles can reinforce other behav-
something unpleasant, such as feelings of uncer- iors (e.g., Butler, 1953; Harlow & McClearn, 1954;
tainty, ignorance, information deprivation, arousal, Myers & Miller, 1954).
or drive. Curiosity is thus akin to scratching a men- Eventually, it became clear that the classical
tal itch or filling a mental hole. drive-reduction model simply couldn’t accom-
Motivation psychology’s grandest drive- modate the many demonstrations of curiosity and
reduction model comes from Clark Hull’s body exploration. Motivation psychologists thus searched
of work, which had a tremendous influence in its for extensions and modifications of Hull’s approach.
time. Hull certainly wasn’t the first psychologist to The earliest models simply proposed a new drive,
propose a hydraulic approach to motivation, but such as a curiosity drive, an exploration drive, or a
his model of drive and reinforcement was probably boredom drive (see Fowler, 1965). These new drives
the first major scientific theory of motivation to didn’t stick, but they were an ironic development—
inspire extensive empirical research. We can’t cover the behavior theorists had often mocked instinct
the complexity and development of Hull’s model theories of motivation for proposing new instincts
here—for the details, curious and motivated read- to deal with challenging findings.
ers can consult Hull’s (1943, 1952) own books or Berlyne (1960) proposed a particularly clever
reviews by his contemporaries (Atkinson, 1964; revision of drive theory in his first major model of
Bolles, 1967). curiosity. He suggested that organisms do indeed
In broad strokes, Hull formalized several ideas prefer a low level of arousal, but arousal is non–lin-
that had been proposed during the early writings early related to the novelty, complexity, uncertainty,
on psychological drives. First, organisms experience and conflict of stimuli in the environment. He
a state of drive due to internal deficits or strong suggested a U-shaped relationship between actual
stimulation from the environment. Second, reduc- arousal and novelty: Arousal is high when stimuli
ing the state of drive is rewarding, so behaviors are very low and very high in novelty. Understim-
that result in drive reduction are reinforced and ulation and overstimulation thus both increase
hence more likely to happen during later periods arousal, which then motivates arousal reduction.
of drive. This model thus accommodated both the Understimulation promoted diversive exploration,
intensity and direction aspects of motivation, and in which bored organisms search for anything that
it integrated problems of motivation with prob- would boost arousal potential; overstimulation pro-
lems of learning. But from the outset curiosity and moted specific exploration, in which activated organ-
exploratory behavior seemed hard to explain. Seek- isms examine the arousal-producing stimulus to
ing stimulation and approaching unfamiliar stimuli reduce its novelty and arousingness. The nonlinear
increase drive; within Hull’s model, it’s not obvious function relating arousal and arousal potential was
why an organism at rest would cease resting and clever, but it didn’t catch on (Berlyne, 1967).
start exploring. Another modification of Hull’s approach was the
Unfortunately, the accumulation of facts inter- optimal arousal approach, a family of models that
fered with Hull’s elegant theory, a common occupa- proposed that the ideal level of arousal was moderate
tional hazard in science. One peculiar phenomenon, instead of low (Fiske & Maddi, 1961; Hebb, 1955;
and one predating Hull’s work, was spontaneous Hunt, 1965; Leuba, 1955; Zuckerman, 1969). As
alternation. Rats in T-mazes will often alternate a result, many classic Hullian findings could be
between arms of the maze, presumably for no real retained—organisms will act to reduce drive when
reason apart from the variety afforded by the arm arousal exceeds the optimal level—while accom-
less traveled (Montgomery, 1951, 1952). Harlow modating curiosity. This is a neat idea, but optimal
showed similar findings with primates. When given arousal models never inspired much research despite
puzzles to work with, the primates often worked the broad interest in them in their time. As Hull’s
on them vigorously in the absence of food rewards; model ebbed and alternative models of motivation
if anything, food rewards and prior food depriva- emerged, researchers moved away from the notion
tion seemed to interfere with learning these com- that reinforcement depends on arousal (Atkinson,
plex tasks (Harlow, 1953). Most fatal, however, was 1964). Once this notion was dropped, optimal

 curiosit y and motivation


arousal models became a compromise for a conflict reducing unpleasant states will struggle to explain
that no longer existed. a large literature on exploratory behavior, which
Modern work continues to develop the notion suggests that novelty, variety, and uncertainty can
that curiosity is motivated by drive reduction. themselves be valued and rewarding. As a result,
After reviewing past work on curiosity, Loewen- most research on curiosity since the 1970s has shifted
stein (1994) proposed an information gap model of from drive models to intrinsic motivation models,
curiosity, which has its roots in information theory. the research tradition that we consider next.
His model proposes that someone becomes curious
“when attention becomes focused on a gap in one’s Curiosity Is “For Its Own Sake” Motivation
knowledge. Such information gaps produce the Our second strand of thought views curiosity
feeling of deprivation labeled curiosity. The curious as a source of “for its own sake” motivation, as a
individual is motivated to obtain the missing infor- source of inquiring, learning, and exploring in the
mation to reduce or eliminate the feeling of depri- absence of external sources of reward and punish-
vation” (p. 87). Information gaps are represented ment. In this tradition, curiosity is motivating in its
using information theory’s uncertainty formula. own right, not because it allows people to reduce an
This model thus has all the hallmarks of a Hullian- unpleasant state. This tradition is the mainstream
inspired curiosity model: Increases in information modern view of curiosity, so naturally it goes way
gaps create an aversive feeling of deprivation, which back. J. Clark Murray (1904), author of an early
then motivates exploratory behavior. textbook of psychology, considered curiosity one of
The cardinal problem with the information the intellectual feelings: “The exertion of intellect,
gaps model is that it is a subset of Berlyne’s (1960) when not overstrained, is itself an agreeable activ-
model. To someone who has read Berlyne’s work, ity. . . . It is not, therefore, difficult to explain the love
Loewenstein’s article seems odd. Berlyne first of knowledge,—the feeling commonly treated by
made his name by applying information theory to psychologists under the name of curiosity” (p. 470).
curiosity—this idea was developed in a Psychologi- Typical of his time (cf. McDougall, 1908/1960;
cal Review article (Berlyne, 1957) and in Berlyne’s Smith & Hall, 1907), there was a tacit snobbery to
(1960, 1965, 1971) books. Berlyne’s concept of Murray’s view of curiosity:
specific curiosity covers the dynamics of information
During the earlier years of life, until the familiar facts
gaps—uncertainty motivates exploration aimed at
of the world are mastered, curiosity forms a strong
reducing it. And as a subset of Berlyne’s work, Loe-
and useful impulse. In later life it is only among men
wenstein’s work is encumbered by the same prob-
of some education that it forms a useful and refining
lems that drive-reduction models of curiosity face,
power. In vulgar minds it allies itself with the more
such as the classic studies of exploratory behavior
petty instincts, and even with the malicious passions
from the 1950s that we reviewed earlier. Berlyne’s
of human nature, degenerating into a prurient crav-
concept of diversive curiosity at least attempted to
ing after the knowledge of facts too trivial or too
explain such findings, but the information gaps
pernicious to be worth knowing.
model has little to say about them. It is in this sense
(pp. 470, 471)
that the information gaps model is a subset of Ber-
lyne’s model: They both concern specific curiosity, Although trivial and pernicious are in the eye of
but only Berlyne’s model grappled with the prob- the beholder, one wonders what Murray would have
lems of diversive curiosity and novelty seeking. We thought of social networking Web pages and reality
can thus agree that research supports Loewenstein’s TV shows.
predictions, but it is perhaps embarrassing that the The intrinsic love of knowledge received a deeper
predictions come from the 1960s. treatment by Felix Arnold (1910) and John Dewey
With the perspective of history and the benefit of (1913), both of whom considered how interest
modern research, what can we conclude about the aided education. Arnold viewed interest as having
novelty-reduction tradition? People do seek infor- motor aspects and ideational aspects. The motor
mation for many reasons, and some of those reasons aspect of interest was essentially motivational—
resemble scratching mental itches. In the parlance “interest is dynamic, it points ahead, it is a form
of the lay epistemics model (Kruglanski & Fishman, of striving, of motor impulsion, and it is felt as a
2009), people sometimes seek information because conation or motor attitude” (p. 186)—a view that
of a need for cognitive closure. At the same time, it prefigures later models of interest as a source of moti-
seems obvious that a model of curiosity founded on vated action. The ideational aspect was essentially

s i lvi a 
cognitive—interest evoked mental images related to depended on arousal reduction. As an alternative,
the object of interest and the actions and goals it he proposed that several brain systems interacted
entailed. to promote exploratory behavior or avoidance.
John Dewey (1913), the better known of the pair, Figure 10.1 depicts how two of these systems relate
set forth a model of interest in Interest and Effort in to positive and negative affect. The first system, the
Education, a small book that was overshadowed by primary reward system, generates positive affect as
his many later landmark works. Dewey contended stimuli become more novel, complex, and uncertain.
that educators typically used one of two methods to The positive affect motivates and rewards engage-
motivate students, neither of which worked well. In ment with new things, so this model qualifies as a
the effort method, instructors use self-discipline and for-its-own-sake model of curiosity and motivation.
willpower as the main mechanisms of learning—if A second system, the primary aversion system, gen-
the material is tedious, students must simply slog erates negative affect as stimuli become more novel,
through it. In the interest method, instructors use complex, and uncertain. The aversion system, how-
diverting, novel, and flashy elements to attract stu- ever, has a temporal offset—it takes higher levels of
dents’ attention to basically boring material—“when stimulation for it to kick in—so the combined effect
things have to be made interesting, it is because of the two systems is to create positive approach and
interest itself is wanting,” he quipped (p. 11). engagement and then eventual aversive withdrawal
As an alternative, Dewey proposed connecting as stimuli increase in intensity.
material to students’ own interests. Children already Berlyne’s (1971) reward–aversion model is
had important interests, and new activities could important for historical reasons. Berlyne developed
become interesting in their own right if they fur- the model as part of a shift into the psychology
thered or connected to an existing interest. Dewey of aesthetics (e.g., Berlyne, 1972, 1974), and his
argued that interest was a developmental process research team profoundly affected how the small but
rather than a momentary state—it motivated people valiant community of aesthetics researchers thought
to immerse themselves in the activity, and it made about aesthetic preference and experience (Silvia, in
the process valuable and self-relevant. Dewey’s con- press). The model itself, however, never stuck, per-
ception of interested action probably represents psy- haps because behavior theory was obviously waning
chology’s first fleshed-out treatment of interest and in the 1970s and emerging approaches to curios-
intrinsic motivation: Interested actions are internal- ity and to the neuroscience of reward seemed more
ized and autonomous, and over time they cultivate fresh and exciting.
competence and successful development. Within emotion psychology, research on curios-
In the modern era, the notion of curiosity as a ity and interest got its start with the work of Silvan
source of intrinsic, for-its-own-sake motivation Tomkins (1962), who gave much of early emotion
emerged in many areas of psychology. Within social science its start. Tomkins argued that emotions
and personality psychology, the rise of models of were the organism’s primary motivational system.
intrinsic motivation in the 1970s (Deci, 1975)— First, at the level of action dynamics, emotions
particularly the line of work that developed into made some events significant and thus gave them
self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985, priority. Tomkins proposed a central assembly that
2000)—is probably the best known example of a
model of curiosity as a source of intrinsic motiva-
tion. Deci and Ryan (2000, p. 233) have argued
that “intrinsically motivated behaviors are those that
are freely engaged out of interest without the neces-
Positive

sity of separable consequences,” and research in this Primary Reward


area commonly measures engagement, exploration, System
and interest (Deci, 1992, 1998; Krapp, 2002). This
area of work is reviewed in Chapter 6 of this vol-
Primary Aversion
Negative

ume, so here we’ll only express awe at the enormous System


amount of research that self-determination theory
has inspired.
Within behavior theory, Berlyne (1971, 1973)
proposed a new model of curiosity that shifted from
the position that reinforcement and exploration Fig. 10.1. Berlyne’s model of reward and aversion systems.

 curiosit y and motivation


organized the components of behavior into effective and how interest develops and operates in infancy
action. People can’t do many behaviors at once, so and early childhood (Izard, 1978; Langsdorf, Izard,
they need a mechanism that elevates some tasks and Rayias, & Hembree, 1983).
situations over others. Tomkins’s theory thus falls Most relevant to motivation, however, is the
into the category of prioritization models of moti- body of work on interest’s influence on behavior.
vation, which tackle the problems of why people are A lot of research supports Tomkins’s view of inter-
doing a particular thing instead of something else est as a source of intrinsic motivation. One line of
and how action is stopped and redirected (cf. Atkin- work considers the action tendencies associated with
son & Birch, 1970). being interested. Interest predicts many behavioral
Second, at the level of life span development, markers of exploration and knowledge seeking, such
Tomkins proposed that emotions have broad adap- as how long people look at or listen to interesting
tational functions. Darwin’s (1872/1998) work things and how long they spend exploring objects
on facial expressions heavily influenced Tomkins, or working on tasks (Berlyne, 1971; Sansone &
whose theory of emotion was one of the first modern Smith, 2000; Sansone & Thoman, 2005). Oddly
emotion theories colored by adaptational concepts. enough, interest predicts behavioral exploration
He gave particular attention to the communicative much more strongly than enjoyment does (Silvia,
functions of emotion, such as expressions in the 2006). Another line of work considers learning out-
face, voice, and body; how the emotions developed comes, particularly text comprehension and educa-
in early childhood; and how emotions over the life tional achievement. Educational research has found
span shaped a person’s personality and worldview that people get better grades in interesting classes,
(Tomkins, 1965, 1979). use deeper level reading and studying strategies for
Curiosity appeared in Tomkins’s (1962) work as interesting texts and domains, and retain interest-
the basic emotion of interest–excitement, which he ing material better (Hidi, 1990, 2001; Krapp, 1999,
felt was the “affect which has been most seriously 2002; Sadoski & Paivio, 2001; Silvia, 2006).
neglected” (p. 337). Interest makes new things For the most part, the intrinsic motivation
appealing and thus provides an innate incentive for approach to curiosity is more vibrant than the
engaging with new, unfamiliar things. Events that drive-reduction approach—it’s the backdrop for
are novel enough to evoke interest—but not novel the majority of contemporary work. The three
enough to evoke fear—thus gained priority in the domains we have covered—social and personality
central assembly over mundane events that evoked psychology, behavior theory, and emotion psychol-
no affect. For example, people do forego food and ogy—capture most of modern research. Social and
sleep to pursue interesting things, which fits a prior- personality psychology contains much of the exper-
ity view of motivation—interest make those actions imental research on situational and dispositional
more significant—but poses yet another problem influences on curiosity and interest; behavior theory
for drive models of motivation. In the long run, the has evolved into mature psychobiological models
broad adaptational function served by interest is to of novelty seeking and reward, such as Panksepp’s
motivate learning and development. In early child- (1998) SEEKING system; and emotion psychology
hood, interest is an engine of perceptual and cog- continues to explore what makes things interest-
nitive development; in adulthood, it is the source ing, how interest operates across domains, and how
of intrinsically motivated learning and intellectual interest develops.
creativity.
Since Tomkins’s work, interest has been a con- Some People Are More Curious
troversial emotion: Some emotion theories retained Than Others
it (e.g., Ellsworth & Smith, 1988; Izard, 1977) Our third strand of thought concerns individ-
and others omitted it (e.g., Ekman, 1992; Lazarus, ual differences in curiosity. William McDougall
1991). Nevertheless, a lot of work has accumulated (1908/1960), the notorious and maligned instinct
on interest’s emotional qualities (Silvia, 2006, 2008). theorist, was one of the first psychologists to spec-
For example, researchers have examined the produc- ulate about stable between-person variability in
tion and recognition of facial and vocal expressions curiosity. In modern terms, his instincts are mod-
of interest (Banse & Scherer, 1996; Reeve, 1993; ular and automated motivational systems that are
Sauter & Scott, 2007), the cognitive appraisals evoked by stimuli and opportunities in the environ-
that predict interest and distinguish it from other ment. Instinctive action has a cognitive aspect (an
states (Silvia, 2005, 2010; Turner & Silvia, 2006), apprehension of an object), a behavioral tendency

s i lvi a 
(a motivational urge to act), and an affective aspect thus tolerate more uncertainty and novelty before
(an emotional feeling). withdrawing.
McDougall proposed an instinct of curiosity, For the most part, recent research has focused on
which was the instinct associated with exploratory global curiosity as a source of intrinsic motivation.
behavior—the impulse “to approach and examine Kashdan (2004, 2009), for example, approaches
more closely the object that excites it” (p. 49)—and individual differences in curiosity from the per-
with the emotional state of wonder. He argued that spective of positive psychology. In Kashdan’s work,
“the native excitant of the instinct would seem to be curiosity is a tendency to notice, seek, value, and
any object similar to, yet perceptibly different from, embrace novelty, uncertainty, and challenge. The
familiar objects habitually noticed” (p. 47), while Curiosity and Exploration Inventory, a brief self-
recognizing that higher levels of novelty and uncer- report scale, was developed to capture this model
tainty evoked fear. It is tempting to make fun of poor of curiosity (Kashdan et al., 2009; Kashdan, Rose,
McDougall and his many instincts, but for many & Fincham, 2004). The model is explicitly moti-
decades the notion of a curiosity instinct was the pre- vational, in that curious people respond to novelty
vailing model of the motivational basis of curiosity, with an appetitive, approach-oriented response.
and it had a big influence (e.g., Cameron, 1922). As a consequence of exploring new and challeng-
McDougall (1908/1960) thought that the curi- ing things across the life span, curious people are
osity instinct was less important to humans, so he expected to have a wide range of positive outcomes
believed that it was more variable as a result: (Kashdan & Silvia, 2009; Silvia & Kashdan, 2009).
For example, curious people typically have better
This instinct, being one whose exercise is not of
academic achievement (Kashdan & Yuen, 2007),
prime importance to the individual, exhibits great in-
higher subjective well-being (Gallagher & Lopez,
dividual differences as regards its innate strength; and
2007), and a stronger sense that their daily lives and
these differences are apt to be increased during the
goals are meaningful (Kashdan & Steger, 2007).
course of life, the impulse growing weaker for lack
Another model of trait curiosity, Litman’s
of use in those in whom it is innately weak, stronger
(2005) I-D model, combines the drive reduction
through exercise in those in whom it is innately
and intrinsic motivation traditions of curiosity
strong. In men of the latter type it may become the
research. Litman proposes that curiosity consists of
main source of intellectual energy and effort; to its
two major facets: curiosity as a feeling of interest
impulse we certainly owe most of the disinterested
(I-curiosity) and curiosity as a feeling of deprivation
labors of the highest types of intellect.
(D-curiosity). Interest is curiosity motivated by a
(p. 50)
desire to seek information for its own sake, whereas
This passage is doubly intriguing: It proposes deprivation is curiosity motivated by a desire to
individual differences in curiosity, a topic that has reduce the uncertainty and frustration caused by
since been widely studied, and it suggests that these knowledge gaps. The I-D model is thus a compro-
differences become exaggerated across the life span, mise between the two competing traditions. Most
a topic that hasn’t. of the work testing Litman’s model has used mea-
Modern individual differences research got sures of individual differences (Litman & Jimerson,
started a long time after McDougall’s work, and 2004). Curiosity scales seem to sort into these two
models of trait-like curiosity represent both themes I and D factors (Litman & Silvia, 2006), which are
we have considered so far. It’s hard to organize the highly correlated, and the I and D traits predict
hurly-burly world of self-report curiosity scales— different kinds of knowledge seeking in response
there are a lot of scales and a lot of constructs. to knowledge gaps (Litman, Hutchins, & Russon,
Some models have their roots in Berlyne’s mod- 2005).
els of curiosity. Researchers have developed scales Most research on individual differences in curios-
to measure individual differences in epistemic, ity has been conducted as part of research on openness
sensory, specific, and diversive forms of curiosity to experience, a higher order trait that encompasses
(e.g., Collins, Litman, & Spielberger, 2004; Lit- curiosity and related constructs. McCrae and Sutin
man & Spielberger, 2003), and Spielberger and (2009) describe open people as “imaginative, sen-
Starr (1994) proposed that trait curiosity corre- sitive to art and beauty, emotionally differentiated,
sponds to Berlyne’s (1971) primary reward system behaviorally flexible, intellectually curious, and lib-
and that trait anxiety corresponds to the primary eral in values” (p. 258). Closed people, in contrast,
aversion system. People high in trait curiosity can are “down-to-earth, uninterested in art, shallow

 curiosit y and motivation


in affect, set in their ways, lacking curiosity, and in openness behave more creatively, ranging from
traditional in values” (p. 258). Curiosity is thus one everyday creativity (e.g., having creative hobbies) to
of several facets of openness. significant creative accomplishment (Carson, Peter-
Of the five factors, openness to experience has son, & Higgins, 2005; Silvia et al., 2009). Third,
been the most controversial. It is the least well cap- life span research shows that openness to experience
tured by trait terms, so the lexical tradition in per- predicts the accumulation of creative accomplish-
sonality research has yielded inconsistent pictures ment and the likelihood of becoming creatively emi-
of how openness to experience is represented in nent. Feist and Barron (2003), for example, found
natural languages (De Raad, 1994; Goldberg, 1994; that indicators of openness to experience measured
McCrae, 1994). As a result, models of openness to at age 27 predicted creative achievements at age 72.
experience define the construct somewhat differ- McDougall seems to have been on to something
ently. Some models cast openness to experience as with his notion of individual differences in curios-
an intellectual trait (Goldberg, 1990) or propose ity: Curious people and incurious people are pretty
that openness and intellect are primary facets of different. Modern researchers would disagree with
the higher order trait of openness to experience McDougall that curiosity is both more variable
(DeYoung, Quilty, & Peterson, 2007). and less important than other motivational traits,
Differences between models aside, openness to but they would agree that curiosity is important to
experience is an important source of variability in achievements across the life span.
curiosity. The literature on openness is massive, so
we’ll focus on two domains: aesthetic experience Conclusion
and creativity. Research on aesthetics finds wide Curiosity touches on some of motivational psy-
differences in how interesting and pleasing people chology’s most vexing problems: Why do people
find works of art. People high in openness to experi- do something instead of nothing? Why do people
ence generally find art more interesting than people persist on seemingly trivial activities in the absence
low in openness (e.g., Feist & Brady, 2004), pos- of obvious external rewards and in the presence of
sess more expertise and knowledge related to the more important goals? Why do apparently trivial
arts (Silvia, 2007; Silvia & Berg, 2011), listen to things capture attention and action? The well-
more complicated kinds of music in everyday life springs of action proposed by motivation theories
(Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2007; Rentfrow are usually abstract and vaunted, but the kinds of
& Gosling, 2003), experience aesthetic chills—a actions motivated by curiosity are usually mundane
feeling of goose bumps and chills down the spine and capricious. Nevertheless, curiosity does seem to
(Grewe, Kopiez, & Altenmüller, 2009)—more be an important mechanism in the development of
often in response to art (McCrae, 2007), and find knowledge and competence, so it’s reassuring that
abstract, disturbing, and bizarre art intriguing and something is coming out of the hours people spend
appealing (Rawlings, 2003; Rawlings, Twomey, scrambling around playgrounds, reading low-brow
Burns, & Morris, 2002). books, and street tuning old Honda Civics.
In addition to their interest in the arts, people
high in openness to experience are more creative, in Future Directions
several senses of the word. Openness to experience 1. How can the different strands of thought on
consistently appears as a major predictor of creativ- curiosity be connected? The urge to weave beats
ity across many domains (Feist, 1998, 2006), and strong in the hearts of motivation psychologists.
it seems to foster creativity in part for motivational Within each strand, there could be interesting
reasons. To do creative work, people must value bridges. In the intrinsic motivation strand,
novelty: They should see merit in doing something there could be fertile connections between self-
differently and in challenging established ideas. Peo- determination theory and emotion psychology,
ple who like things the way they are and who prefer two areas that haven’t had much to do with
the traditional over the novel lack the basic mindset each other. In the drive-reduction strand,
needed for innovative behavior (Joy, 2004). researchers could explore the value of a single
A large literature shows that people high in open- broad model—something akin to Kruglanski’s
ness to experience are more creative. First, openness (2004) lay epistemics model or Litman’s (2005)
predicts divergent thinking, a creative cognitive I-D model—for unifying curious and incurious
trait (McCrae, 1987; Silvia et al., 2008, 2009). Sec- motives for seeking knowledge. Between each
ond, cross-sectional studies show that people high strand, researchers could consider bridges between

s i lvi a 
intrinsic motivation and drive reduction. Such Berlyne, D. E. (1967). Arousal and reinforcement. Nebraska
bridges would shift what the field means by Symposium on Motivation, 15, 1–110.
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York:
curiosity, but they could illuminate the broader Appleton-Century-Crofts.
workings of cognitive motives. Berlyne, D. E. (1972). Ends and means of experimental aesthet-
2. How do curious traits influence curious ics. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 26, 303–325.
states? Many models of individual differences Berlyne, D. E. (1973). The vicissitudes of aplopathematic and
invoke motivational mechanisms, largely along the thelematoscopic pneumatology (or the hydrography of
hedonism). In D. E. Berlyne & K. B. Madsen (Eds.), Plea-
lines of drive reduction, intrinsic motivation, or sure, reward, preference (pp. 1–33). New York: Academic
both. Despite this conceptual overlap, there isn’t Press.
much integration between the state and the trait Berlyne, D. E. (Ed.). (1974). Studies in the new experimental
approaches to curiosity—with many interesting aesthetics: Steps toward an objective psychology of aesthetic
exceptions, naturally—perhaps because self-report appreciation. Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
Bolles, R. C. (1967). Theory of motivation. New York: Harper &
assessment and cross-sectional designs dominate Row.
individual differences research. Future work on Butler, R. A. (1953). Discrimination learning by rhesus monkeys
curious people ought to tackle the paradigms to visual-exploration motivation. Journal of Comparative and
used in state research, particularly the measures Physiological Psychology, 46, 95–98.
of experience, cognition, and behavior used to Cameron, E. H. (1922). Psychology and the school. New York:
Century.
illuminate knowledge seeking and exploration. Carson, S. H., Peterson, J. B., & Higgins, D. M. (2005).
3. What does the midrange level of curious Reliability, validity, and factor structure of the Creative
motivation look like? Most work on curiosity Achievement Questionnaire. Creativity Research Journal, 17,
has explored situational aspects or stable 37–50.
individual differences. The midrange—the level of Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2007). Personality and
music: Can traits explain how people use music in everyday
idiosyncratic personal goals—remains obscure. In life? British Journal of Psychology, 98, 175–185.
everyday curiosity, people are probably exploring Collins, R. P., Litman, J. A., & Spielberger, C. D. (2004). The
activities and domains that they have engaged measurement of perceptual curiosity. Personality and Indi-
with before and know a lot about (Prenzel, vidual Differences, 36, 1127–1141.
1992)—their hobbies and interests are important Darwin, C. (1872/1998). The expression of the emotions in man
and animals (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
to understanding how curiosity influences their Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
behavior. So far, most of the work on hobbies and Deci, E. L. (1992). The relation of interest to the motivation
interests has been done in educational research, of behavior: A self-determination theory perspective. In
which emphasizes a distinction between situational K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of
interest and individual interest (Chapter 11, this interest in learning and development (pp. 43–70). Hillsdale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
volume; Schiefele, 2009). Future work should Deci, E. L. (1998). The relation of interest to motivation and
examine how interests relate to situational states human needs—The self-determination theory viewpoint.
and enduring traits (e.g., Durik & Harackiewicz, In L. Hoffman, A. Krapp, K. A. Renninger, & J. Baumert
2007) and how idiosyncratic interests develop (Eds.), Interest and learning (pp. 146–162). Kiel, Germany:
(Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Silvia, 2006). IPN.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
References Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of
Arnold, F. (1910). Attention and interest: A study in psychology and goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of
education. New York: Macmillan. behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268.
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. New York: De Raad, B. (1994). An expedition in search of a fifth universal
Van Nostrand. factor: Key issues in the lexical approach. European Journal of
Atkinson, J. W., & Birch, D. (1970). The dynamics of action. Personality, 8, 229–250.
New York: Wiley. Dewey, J. (1913). Interest and effort in education. Boston, MA:
Banse, R., & Scherer, K. R. (1996). Acoustic profiles in vocal Riverside.
emotion expressions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- DeYoung, C. G., Quilty, L. C., & Peterson, J. B. (2007). Between
ogy, 70, 614–636. facets and domains: 10 aspects of the Big Five. Journal of
Berlyne, D. E. (1957). Uncertainty and conflict: A point of con- Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 880–896.
tact between information-theory and behavior-theory con- Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2007). Different strokes
cepts. Psychological Review, 64, 329–339. for different folks: Individual interest as a moderator of
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York: the effects of situational factors on task interest. Journal of
McGraw–Hill. Educational Psychology, 99, 597–610.
Berlyne, D. E. (1965). Structure and direction in thinking. Ekman, P. (1992). An argument for basic emotions. Cognition
New York: Wiley. and Emotion, 6, 169–200.

 curiosit y and motivation


Ellsworth, P. C., & Smith, C. A. (1988). Shades of joy: Patterns Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Curious? Discover the missing ingredient to
of appraisal differentiating positive emotions. Cognition and a fulfilling life. New York: William Morrow.
Emotion, 2, 301–331. Kashdan, T. B., Gallagher, M. W., Silvia, P. J., Winterstein,
Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of personality in scien- B. P., Breen, W. E., Terhar, D., & Steger, M. F. (2009). The
tific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychology curiosity and exploration inventory–II: Development, factor
Review, 2, 290–309. structure, and psychometrics. Journal of Research in Personal-
Feist, G. J. (2006). The psychology of science and the origins of the ity, 43, 987–998.
scientific mind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Kashdan, T. B., Rose, P., & Fincham, F. D. (2004). Curiosity and
Feist, G. J., & Barron, F. X. (2003). Predicting creativity from exploration: Facilitating positive subjective experiences and
early to late adulthood: Intellect, potential, and personality. personal growth opportunities. Journal of Personality Assess-
Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 62–88. ment, 82, 291–305.
Feist, G. J., & Brady, T. R. (2004). Openness to experience, non- Kashdan, T. B., & Silvia, P. J. (2009). Curiosity and interest: The
conformity, and the preference for abstract art. Empirical benefits of thriving on novelty and challenge. In C. R. Sny-
Studies of the Arts, 22, 77–89. der & S. J. Lopez (Ed.), Handbook of positive psychology (2nd
Fiske, D. W., & Maddi, S. R. (1961). A conceptual framework. ed., pp. 367–374). New York: Oxford University Press.
In D. W. Fiske & S. R. Maddi (Eds.), Functions of varied Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (2007). Curiosity and pathways
experience (pp. 11–56). Homewood, IL: Dorsey. to well-being and meaning in life: Traits, states, and everyday
Fowler, H. (1965). Curiosity and exploratory behavior. New York: behaviors. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 159–173.
Macmillan. Kashdan, T. B., & Yuen, M. (2007). Whether highly curious
Gallagher, M. W., & Lopez, S. J. (2007). Curiosity and well- students thrive academically depends on the learning envi-
being. Journal of Positive Psychology, 2, 236–248. ronment of their school: A study of Hong Kong adolescents.
Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personal- Motivation and Emotion, 31, 260–270.
ity”: The Big Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Krapp, A. (1999). Interest, motivation and learning: An
Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229. educational-psychological perspective. European Journal of
Goldberg, L. R. (1994). Resolving a scientific embarrassment: Psychology of Education, 14, 23–40.
A comment on the articles in this special issue. European Krapp, A. (2002). An educational-psychological theory of
Journal of Personality, 8, 351–356. interest and its relation to self-determination theory. In
Grewe, O., Kopiez, R., & Altenmüller, E. (2009). The chill E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-
parameter: Goose bumps and shivers as promising measures determination research (pp. 405–427). Rochester, NY: Uni-
in emotion research. Music Perception, 27, 61–74. versity of Rochester Press.
Harlow, H. F. (1953). Mice, monkeys, men, and motives. Psycho- Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). The psychology of closed-mindedness.
logical Review, 60, 23–32. New York: Psychology Press.
Harlow, H. F., & McClearn, G. E. (1954). Object discrimi- Kruglanski, A. W., & Fishman, S. (2009). The need for cognitive
nation learned by monkeys on the basis of manipulation closure. In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook
motives. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, of individual differences in social behavior (pp. 343–353).
47, 73–76. New York: Guilford.
Hebb, D. O. (1955). Drives and the C.N.S. (conceptual nervous Langsdorf, P., Izard, C. E., Rayias, M., & Hembree, E. A. (1983).
system). Psychological Review, 62, 243–254. Interest expression, visual fixation, and heart rate changes in
Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental resource 2- to 8-month old infants. Developmental Psychology, 19,
for learning. Review of Educational Research, 60, 549–571. 375–386.
Hidi, S. (2001). Interest, reading, and learning: Theoretical and Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York:
practical considerations. Educational Psychology Review, 13, Oxford University Press.
191–209. Leuba, C. (1955). Toward some integration of learning theories:
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of The concept of optimal stimulation. Psychological Reports, 1,
interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127. 27–33.
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Litman, J. A. (2005). Curiosity and the pleasures of learning:
Appleton–Century–Crofts. Wanting and liking new information. Cognition and Emo-
Hull, C. L. (1952). A behavior system. New Haven, CT: Yale Uni- tion, 19, 793–814.
versity Press. Litman, J. A., Hutchins, T. L., & Russon, R. K. (2005). Epistemic
Hunt, J. M. (1965). Intrinsic motivation and its role in psycho- curiosity, feeling-of-knowing, and exploratory behavior. Cog-
logical development. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 13, nition and Emotion, 19, 559–582.
189–282. Litman, J. A., & Jimerson, T. L. (2004). The measurement of
Izard, C. E. (1977). Human emotions. New York: Plenum. curiosity as a feeling of deprivation. Journal of Personality
Izard, C. E. (1978). On the development of emotions and Assessment, 82, 147–157.
emotion–cognition relationships in infancy. In M. Lewis & Litman, J. A., & Silvia, P. J. (2006). The latent structure of
L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The development of affect (pp. trait curiosity: Evidence for interest and deprivation curi-
389–413). New York: Plenum. osity dimensions. Journal of Personality Assessment, 86,
Joy, S. (2004). Innovation motivation: The need to be different. 318–328.
Creativity Research Journal, 16, 313–330. Litman, J. A., & Spielberger, C. D. (2003). Measuring epistemic
Kashdan, T. B. (2004). Curiosity. In C. Peterson & curiosity and its diversive and specific components. Journal
M. E. P. Seligman (Eds.), Character strengths and virtues: of Personality Assessment, 80, 75–86.
A handbook and classification (pp. 125–141). New York: Loewenstein, G. (1994). The psychology of curiosity: A review
Oxford University Press. and reinterpretation. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 75–98.

s i lvi a 
McCrae, R. R. (1987). Creativity, divergent thinking, and open- Sauter, D. A., & Scott, S. K. (2007). More than one kind of
ness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- happiness: Can we recognize vocal expressions of different
ogy, 52, 1258–1265. positive states? Motivation and Emotion, 31, 192–199.
McCrae, R. R. (1994). Openness to experience: Expanding the Schiefele, U. (2009). Situational and individual interest. In
boundaries of Factor V. European Journal of Personality, 8, K. R. Wenzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
251–272. at school (pp. 197–222). New York: Routledge.
McCrae, R. R. (2007). Aesthetic chills as a universal marker of Silvia, P. J. (2005). What is interesting? Exploring the appraisal
openness to experience. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 5–11. structure of interest. Emotion, 5, 89–102.
McCrae, R. R., & Sutin, A. R. (2009). Openness to experience. Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York:
In M. R. Leary & R. H. Hoyle (Eds.), Handbook of indi- Oxford University Press.
vidual differences in social behavior (pp. 257–273). New York: Silvia, P. J. (2007). Knowledge-based assessment of expertise in
Guilford. the arts: Exploring aesthetic fluency. Psychology of Aesthetics,
McDougall, W. (1908/1960). An introduction to social psychology. Creativity, and the Arts, 1, 247–249.
London: Methuen. Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest—The curious emotion. Current
Montgomery, K. C. (1951). The relationship between explor- Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 57–60.
atory behavior and spontaneous alternation in the white Silvia, P. J. (2010). Confusion and interest: The role of knowl-
rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 44, edge emotions in aesthetic experience. Psychology of Aesthet-
582–589. ics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4, 75–80.
Montgomery, K. C. (1952). A test of two explanations of spon- Silvia, P. J. (in press). Human emotions and aesthetic experience:
taneous alternation. Journal of Comparative and Physiological An overview of empirical aesthetics. In A. Shimamura (Ed.),
Psychology, 45, 287–293. Aesthetic science: Connecting minds, brains, and experience.
Murray, J. C. (1904). An introduction to psychology based on the New York: Oxford University Press.
author’s handbook of psychology. Boston, MA: Little, Brown, Silvia, P. J., & Berg, C. (2011). Finding movies interesting: How
& Company. expertise and appraisals influence the aesthetic experience of
Myers, A. K., & Miller, N. E. (1954). Failure to find a learned film. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 29, 73–88.
drive based on hunger; Evidence for learning motivated by Silvia, P. J., & Kashdan, T. B. (2009). Interesting things and curi-
“exploration.” Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psy- ous people: Exploration and engagement as transient states
chology, 47, 428–436. and enduring strengths. Social and Personality Psychology
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of Compass, 3, 785–797.
human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Silvia, P. J., Nusbaum, E. C., Berg, C., Martin, C., & O’Connor,
Press. A. (2009). Openness to experience, plasticity, and creativity:
Prenzel, M. (1992). The selective persistence of interest. In Exploring lower-order, higher-order, and interactive effects.
K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 1087–1090.
interest in learning and development (pp. 71–98). Hillsdale, Silvia, P. J., Winterstein, B. P., Willse, J. T., Barona, C. M., Cram,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. J. T., Hess, K. I., . . . Richard, C. A. (2008). Assessing creativ-
Rawlings, D. (2003). Personality correlates of liking for “unpleas- ity with divergent thinking tasks: Exploring the reliability
ant” paintings and photographs. Personality and Individual and validity of new subjective scoring methods. Psychology of
Differences, 23, 395–410. Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 2, 68–85.
Rawlings, D., Twomey, F., Burns, E., & Morris, S. (2002). Per- Smith, T. L., & Hall, G. S. (1907). Curiosity and interest. In
sonality, creativity, and aesthetic preference: Comparing T. L. Smith (Ed.), Aspects of child life and education (pp.
psychoticism, sensation seeking, schizotypy, and openness to 84–141). Boston, MA: Ginn & Company.
experience. Empirical Studies of the Arts, 16, 153–178. Spielberger, C. D., & Starr, L. M. (1994). Curiosity and explor-
Reeve, J. (1993). The face of interest. Motivation and Emotion, atory behavior. In H. F. O’Neil, Jr. & M. Drillings (Eds.),
17, 353–375. Motivation: Theory and research (pp. 221–243). Hillsdale, NJ:
Rentfrow, P. J., & Gosling, S. D. (2003). The do re mi’s of every- Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
day life: The structure and personality correlates of music Tomkins, S. S. (1962). Affect, imagery, consciousness. Vol. 1: The
preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, positive affects. New York: Springer.
1236–1256. Tomkins, S. S. (1965). Affect and the psychology of knowledge.
Sadoski, M., & Paivio, A. (2001). Imagery and text: A dual cod- In S. S. Tomkins & C. E. Izard (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and
ing theory of reading and writing. Mahwah, NJ.: Lawrence personality (pp. 72–97). New York: Springer.
Erlbaum Associates. Tomkins, S. S. (1979). Script theory: Differential magnification
Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2000). Interest and self-regulation: of affects. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, 26, 201–236.
The relation between having to and wanting to. In Turner, S. A., Jr., & Silvia, P. J. (2006). Must interesting things
C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrin- be pleasant? A test of competing appraisal structures. Emo-
sic motivation (pp. 341–372). San Diego, CA: Academic. tion, 6, 670–674.
Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2005). Interest as the miss- Zuckerman, M. (1969). Theoretical formulations. In
ing motivator in self-regulation. European Psychologist, 10, J. P. Zubeck (Ed.), Sensory deprivation (pp. 407–432). New
175–186. York: Appleton–Century.

 curiosit y and motivation


CHAPTER

Interest and Its Development


11
K. Ann Renninger and Stephanie Su

Abstract
This chapter focuses on interest as a cognitive and affective motivational variable that develops and can
be supported to develop. Interest and interest development as described by Hidi and Renninger’s (2006)
Four-Phase Model of Interest Development are (a) defined and then (b) contextualized in light of other
conceptualizations that focus on specific aspects of interest (such as emotion, experience, task features,
value, and vocational interest) and issues pertaining to the operationalization and measurement of
interest. Following this, research addressing the development of interest is overviewed, with particular
attention to (a) the triggering of interest in both earlier and later phases of interest, (b) maintaining
interest once it has been triggered, (c) fluctuations in interest, and (d) shifts between phases in the
development of interest. Finally, a Punnett square is employed to suggest next steps and open questions
in the study of interest development.
Key Words: achievement, affect, interest, interest development, knowledge, metacognition, value

Introduction autobiography, is used to illustrate critical aspects


This chapter overviews research that contributes in the development of interest, the conditions that
to understanding interest as a cognitive and affec- support interest to develop and deepen, with which
tive motivational variable that both develops and researchers and practitioners continue to wrestle.
can be supported to develop. It includes studies that These are elaborated on later in this chapter, start-
have been conducted in varying domains using dif- ing with the initial triggering of interest through to
ferent methods. The chapter centers on aspects of the point where she asks questions, reflects on these,
development that are not yet well understood. It and independently follows through to seek answers
begins with the case of Helen Keller and an analysis and feedback. Helen’s case of interest development
of a part of her autobiography, The Story of My Life is paraphrased briefly below:
(Keller, 1903).
Helen Keller was the first blind person to receive Rendered both deaf and blind at a young age, Helen
a bachelor’s degree. She became a world-famous stumbled around like a feral animal for many years.
activist, wrote books about her experience and The adults around her were unable to reach or tame
beliefs, and is now widely considered one of the most her, pitying her and letting her do anything she
inspirational people of the 20th century. Through wanted. When Anne Sullivan, a young and financial-
the support of her tutor Anne Sullivan, Helen “dis- ly strapped tutor, was hired to help Helen, she found
covered” language, communication, and society. a bright but horribly spoiled 7-year-old girl who was
Helen’s case, particularly as it is presented in her unable to see the implications of her own behavior


and its effect on other people. Anne did not approve until the incident with the water. Even at that
of the way that Helen grabbed food from various point, it is not clear that she would have described
people’s dinner plates and broke things during finger spelling, or communication more generally,
temper tantrums. Anne disciplined, and Helen as something in which she was personally invested
fought back both physically and with pranks. and that would hold her interest.
Anne recognized that Helen was bright and Third, Helen’s interest developed in a context
decided to teach her how to finger spell, thinking where her strengths and needs were accounted for
that this might help her to communicate with others. and she was not being graded or assessed: Anne
Anne would put an object in one of Helen’s hands, worked with her so that she would understand and
and in the other quickly spell the name for the ob- be able to think and explore. She was extremely suc-
ject. Even though Helen could imitate well, she did cessful by any number of measures, once her inter-
not understand what Anne was trying to teach her. est began to develop.
Her patience ran out quickly, and the lessons would Fourth, Helen’s interest continued to develop
end in tears and yelling. because, once she made the connection between
Everything changed one day when Anne pumped finger spelling and communicating, she then had
water into Helen’s hands and spelled “water.” The curiosity questions for which she wanted answers—
event appeared to trigger Helen to make a connec- curiosity questions are questions that are novel to the
tion between the fluttering movement in her hand learner but may not be novel to others (Renninger,
and the cold liquid spilling over her skin. All of a 2000). Finding answers to these questions led her to
sudden, Helen realized what Anne had been trying continue to stretch her own understanding.
to show her as she had doggedly spelled word after Fifth, once she began asking curiosity questions,
word into her hand all those weeks. From then on, Helen also began to self-regulate and to explore and
Anne could hardly keep up with Helen, who dragged seize opportunities to learn—opportunities that
her around demanding a word for everything she were ostensibly present before but that she was not
encountered, everything that had been there before. in a position to see.
It is not until Helen makes the connection
Anne’s efforts to help Helen make connections between finger spelling and communication that
between signs and what they represent could be she begins to pose her own curiosity questions, seek
described as potential triggers for interest, and the answers, and reflect on them—a point when her
incident with the water was a trigger that worked interest is clearly developing. However, as Helen’s
because with it she discovered the connection case reveals, the development of interest has phases
between the sign and water. We do not know why that precede what to the outside observer would be
or how the trigger of the water served as a catalyst. identified as “interest.” Her interest also continues
In fact, Helen thought at first that this was some to develop beyond the phase that is detailed here.
kind of game. It seems likely that many factors con- The present chapter focuses on the development of
tributed to her revelation. interest, from the point of potential triggering that
We know, however, that a few elements of Helen’s “takes” to the point when the learner begins to ask
story are particularly important to the description his or her own curiosity questions, and then follows
and understanding of interest development. First, through to reflect on these and seek answers.
the development of her interest involved extended,
seemingly ineffective, external support before she Misunderstood Aspects of Research on
made a connection between the finger spelling and Interest Development
the water and then engaged the challenge of revis- We next call attention to two often misunder-
iting the prior lessons that had been so very frus- stood aspects of research on the development of
trating to her. She did not make a decision to be interest: awareness of interest (the learner’s ability to
interested in communication. Rather, it seems that cognitively evaluate engagement), and the essential
she needed to encounter the connection in order to role of knowledge, in addition to feelings and value,
communicate, and it was the connection that trig- as an indicator of interest, especially in later phases
gered her eventual interest in communication more of interest development.
generally.
Second, Helen was not aware that she was devel- Awareness of Interest
oping an interest as her tutor worked with her. The As Helen’s case illustrates, the development of
potential triggers of finger spelling did not “take” interest does not necessarily involve metacognitive,

 interest a nd its development


or reflective, awareness. This point has three impli- allowed communication. It was only when she made
cations for researchers, educators, and the learners this connection and began to build her knowledge
themselves. First, learners are not necessarily depen- that she then also had questions that she wanted to
dent on their will to develop interest or be interested answer. This led to her continued reengagement to
(Lipstein & Renninger, 2007). They may be depen- understand.
dent largely on supports to find ways to connect
with the content that they are to learn, and while Defining Interest and Interest Development
they need to make their own connections, they are In the present chapter, which focuses on interest
also likely to need support to perceive them (Ren- and its potential to develop, interest is conceptual-
ninger, 2010). Second, while learners may make a ized as:
cognitive evaluation about some content, like Helen
(a) referring to both a learner’s state as well as
they also may not be aware that their interest has
his or her predisposition to return to engagement
been triggered until much later in the process of its
with a particular class of ideas (disciplinary
development. In later phases of interest develop-
content), events, or objects, and
ment too, they can be so engrossed in engagement
(b) developing through four phases: triggered
that they are not reflecting on it.
situational, maintained situational, emerging
A third implication is that having and developing
individual, and well-developed individual interest
an interest is not the same as being metacognitively
(see Table 11.1; Hidi & Renninger, 2006).
aware of the role of interest in one’s learning. The
presence of metacognition impacts a learner’s ability In this section of this chapter, the Four-Phase
to take stock of his or her own goals and to act on Model of Interest Development is described. This is
them (see Flavell, 1976). In this sense, the learner’s followed by an overview of other approaches to the
goals refer to what the learner wants to understand study of interest in order to provide a context for
or do, not whether his or her goals would be consid- understanding a developmental approach. In later
ered mastery or performance goals, since a learner sections, research specific to interest development is
may possess both types of goals. Thus, while a person reviewed and issues central to next steps in under-
may or may not be aware of the process of engaging standing its development are considered.
with an identified interest, the extent to which he
or she is metacognitively aware of his or her interest The Four-Phase Model of Interest
and its role in learning is likely to impact how, not Development
whether, he or she organizes as a learner and follows Hidi and Renninger’s (2006) model identifies
through to engage. four phases in the development of interest based on
existing empirical literature and extended previous
Knowledge and Interest discussions suggesting that there were two types of
In its earliest phases, interest may be consid- interest: situational and individual interest (e.g.,
ered an emotion, or measured based on affect, or Hidi, 1990; Krapp, Hidi, & Renninger, 1992; Ren-
emotional response, and have minimal knowledge ninger, 1990). Briefly, situational interest refers to
requirements (Ainley, 2007; Hidi, 2006; Reeve, the likelihood that particular content, activities, or
Jang, Hardre, & Omura, 2002). As interest devel- events will trigger a response in the moment that
ops, knowledge and value, in addition to affect, may hold over time (Hidi & Baird, 1986; Mitch-
need to be present (Renninger, 1990, 2000). More ell, 1993). Individual interest, in contrast, refers
specifically, Hidi and Renninger (2006) argue to an ongoing and possible deepening of a person’s
that as interest develops and deepens, the desire relation to particular content. It includes a more
for knowledge and value develop concurrently, enriched kind of value than situational interest,
while affect continues to be an important aspect of as well as an increasingly consolidated base of dis-
interest. course knowledge (Renninger, 1990, 2000).
In distinguishing among the phases of interest In the Four-Phase Model, Hidi and Renninger
development, content knowledge is also an impor- (2006) suggested that findings from studies of situ-
tant indicator. Without knowledge, a learner is not ational and individual interest were complementary
in a position to develop the types of curiosity ques- and could be used to map the development of inter-
tions that lead to reengagement, as well as the value est, beginning with forms of initial triggering that
that comes from asking these questions. Helen, for might be sustained to the relatively enduring pre-
example, had no knowledge that finger spelling disposition to return to particular classes of content

ren n i n g er, s u 


Table 11.1. The four phases of interest development (Hidi & Renninger, 2006): Definitions and learner
characteristics.
Phases of Interest Development
Phase I: Phase 2: Phase 3: Phase 4:
Triggered Maintained Emerging Well-Developed
Situational Situational Individual Individual

Definition
• Psychological state • Psychological state that • Psychological state and • Psychological state and
resulting from short-term involves focused atten- the beginning of relatively a relatively enduring
changes in cognitive and tion and persistence over enduring predisposition predisposition to
affective processing extended period, and/or to seek reengagement reengage particular
reoccurs and persists with particular classes of classes of content
content

Learner Characteristics
• Attends to content, if • Reengages content • Is likely to indepen- • Independently reengages
only fleetingly that previously triggered dently re-engage content content
• Needs support to attention • Has curiosity questions • Has curiosity questions
engage from others and • Is supported by others that leads and seeks • Self-regulates easily to reframe
through instructional to find connections answers questions and seek answers
design among their skills, • Has positive feelings • Has positive feelings
• May experience either knowledge, and prior • Has stored knowledge • Can persevere through
positive or negative experience and stored value frustration and challenge in
feelings • Has positive feelings • Is very focused on his order to meet goals
• May or may not be • Is developing knowledge or her own questions • Recognizes others’
reflectively aware of the of the content contributions to the
experience • Is developing a sense of discipline
the content’s value • Actively seeks feedback

over time. It was suggested that situational interest of the water and the finger signing: It represented
could develop into individual interest, but it was the presence of a new concept, communication. Her
also suggested that situational interest could occur interest for communicating using finger spelling
simultaneously with individual interest. was maintained following the triggering provided
As described in Table 11.1, the four phases of by the water, and although she first engaged com-
interest are considered to be sequential and discrete, munication as a game, it began to take on mean-
but as Hidi and Renninger (2006) also noted, they ing for her. It also led her to ask questions because
are phases rather than stages because the length and she wanted to understand, marking a shift in her
character of a given phase may vary among indi- phase of interest. Based on what Helen tells us in
viduals based on, among other factors, experience her autobiography, she appears to have transitioned
and temperament. The first phase in the develop- through the phase of maintained situational interest
ment of interest is conceptualized as being initiated almost immediately, possibly because she had Anne
by a triggered situational interest. If sustained, this to respond and work with her to find answers to the
first phase evolves into the second phase, main- curiosity questions she posed, as they emerged. As
tained situational interest. The third phase of inter- her autobiography also indicates, Helen continued
est, emerging individual interest, may develop out to want to ask questions that allowed her to develop
of the second phase and may then lead to the fourth her knowledge. Her emerging individual interest
phase, a well-developed individual interest. rapidly developed into a well-developed individual
Helen’s experience with finger signing provides interest.
an illustration of triggered and eventually main- The example of Helen demonstrates that once
tained situational interest that evolved almost interest is triggered, it can be maintained and then
immediately into an emerging interest. Helen’s progress as an individual interest. Her interactions
interest was triggered it seems by the juxtaposition with others were critical, a characteristic of inter-

 interest a nd its development


est development that is now well established (e.g., feelings about the worth (the value, task interest,
Barron, 2006; Nolen, 2007; Pasupathi & Rich, utility, cost) of continued engagement (e.g., Wig-
2005). At first these interactions could be charac- field, Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean,
terized as supporting the generation of her interest 2006). Whether a person is in a position to make
(e.g., Mitchell, 1993; Palmer, 2004, 2009). Later an independent decision to reengage has also been
they involved the provision of information that led found to impact the relation between the affective
her to continue to stretch, engage, and explore the and cognitive components of interest, a relation
content of her interest (see Renninger, 2010) or to that affects the experience of interest (Ainley, 2007;
self-generate interest (Sansone, Weir, Harpster, & Sansone & Thoman, 2005a, b) as well as the likeli-
Morgan, 1992). hood that interest will develop and deepen (Ren-
The match between the strengths and needs ninger, 2000).
of the learner and available support, described Although learners at all ages with varying experi-
by Eccles and Midgley (1989) as the stage-fit of ences can develop new interests at any time, age also
the environment, was critical to Helen, and more affects how and whether interest is likely to develop.
generally to the development of interest. Available Undergraduates, for example, may be able to self-
supports can include interactions with others, such generate ways in which to sustain interest in view of
as teachers, peers, parents, or museum personnel, a task that they find boring by finding some reason
and the tools that they have created (e.g., books, that the task could be beneficial to them (e.g., San-
tasks, software, exhibits). However, the presence of sone, et al., 1992). This capacity is related to their
supports and intended triggers does not necessarily metacognitive awareness of the situation (a boring
guarantee triggering. Instead, interest appears to be task that needs to be completed) and their abil-
both triggered and supported to develop when a task ity to generate strategies to address it. Conversely,
such as an assignment to set a goal for a class at the younger children are more likely to generate means
beginning of the term leads learners to find mean- to continue to engage only when tasks are already
ing for themselves (Hulleman, Durik, Schweigert, of interest, although they also may be more open
& Harackiewicz, 2008; Hulleman & Harackiewicz, than older learners to trying to learn new topics or
2009), or when learners are allowed or take charge participate in new activities (Renninger, Sansone, &
of shaping class activity (Cobb & Hodge, 2004; Smith, 2004). At about 8 to 10 years of age, they
Meyer & Turner, 2002). begin comparing their own capacities to those of
When support from the learning environment is others and then need a different form of support
lacking (or perceived to be lacking), however, inter- to persevere on tasks that they have not yet tried,
est can fall off, go dormant, or disappear altogether or that they are aware others already do at a much
(Bergin, 1999). Renninger (2000), for example, more advanced level then they (Renninger, 2009).
described the case of a talented chess player who
ceased to continue to play chess because there was Conceptualizations of Interest Not
no one else to challenge him. Renninger and Lip- Specifically Focused on Development
stein (2006) also reported declines in interest when Understanding how interest can be supported to
students did not perceive opportunities to connect develop is of particular concern to those who sup-
to the work they are doing and/or for their ideas to port others to learn, whether in or outside the school
be respected and heard. Their findings appear to be context. However, the conceptualization of interest
consistent with those of Kunter, Baumert, and Köller as a cognitive and affective motivational variable
(2007) who found that within the same classroom that develops is only one of the ways in which inter-
there were students whose interest would develop est is defined and studied. Krapp (2002, 2007), for
and students whose interest would decrease. They example, describes interest development as a process
observe that the development of interest is likely to of developing one’s identity. Other conceptualiza-
be more related to students’ personal experience of tions of interest reflect a range of research questions
the classroom—for example, whether they feel they and as a result address different aspects of the way
understand what is expected of them and have a in which a person engages (or does not engage) con-
teacher who is responsive and provides support for tent to be learned. These perspectives contribute to
autonomy (see related discussions in Frenzel, Goetz, understanding interest and its relation to learning
Pekrun, & Watt, 2010; Tsai, Kunter, Lüdtke, Traut- but may not address the development of interest per
wein, & Ryan, 2008). The stage-fit of the person to se. However, each is a conceptualization on which
the environment has been described as supporting the understanding of interest development builds.

ren n i n g er, s u 


Detailed considerations of interest can be Conceptualizations that have focused on interest
described as focusing on emotion (e.g., Ainley, in terms of vocational or conceptual interest address
2007; Silvia, 2006), task features and environment the relation between a person’s present abilities and
(e.g., Mayer, 2005; Sansone & Thoman, 2005 a, b), possible occupations (e.g., Holland 1985/1997; see
value (e.g., Schiefele, 2009; Wigfield et al., 2006), also Armstrong, Allison, & Rounds, 2008) or cat-
and vocational interest (e.g., Alexander, Johnson, egories of children’s interest engagement such as sci-
Leibham, & Kelley, 2008; Holland, 1985/1997; ence or art (e.g., Alexander et al., 2008) and school
Lent, Brown, & Hackett, 1994; see Renninger & readiness. One line of work within this framework
Hidi, 2011). Briefly, conceptualizations of interest draws on counseling psychology to suggest that
that focus on emotion are often concerned with the environmental support can be provided to encour-
state of interest, rather than with interest as both age those who presently lack interest to develop
a state and a predisposition to reengage particular it (e.g., women who lack interest for engineering;
content over time. They have determined, for exam- Brown & Lent, 1996). Lent, Brown, and Hack-
ple, that mood, disposition, and situation combine ett’s (1994, 2000) Social Cognitive Career Theory
to influence students’ affective reactions to tasks describes interest development as determined by the
(Ainley & Patrick, 2006), and that interest may individual’s perceptions of his or her own compe-
be either pleasant or unpleasant (Turner & Silvia, tence, or ability to succeed.
2006), but little is known about whether and how Each of the conceptualizations overviewed indi-
the intensity and valence of affect changes with the cates that interest can beneficially influence learning
development of interest. (although it can also be distracting) and that it is
Conceptualizations that have focused on interest always linked to a particular disciplinary content,
in terms of task features or the environment have object, event, or idea. The conceptualizations also
also pointed to the importance of the experience of all acknowledge the role of affect, or feelings, as a
interest to engagement. They find that interest is component of interest, but they tend to vary in the
essential to the feelings of competence that accom- extent to which affect, knowledge, and value are the
pany this experience and self-regulation (Sansone & focus of inquiry and measurement.
Thoman, 2005 a, b), and they have indicated that Some of the conceptualizations describe knowl-
interest can be distracting (e.g., Mayer, Griffith, edge and value as components of interest (Ainley,
Jurkowitz, & Rothman, 2008). However, because 2007; Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Mayer, 2005; San-
these approaches to interest address the state of sone & Thoman, 2005 a, b; Silvia, 2006), whereas
interest in earlier phases of interest development, others focused on affect and value as established
it is not clear whether and how the experience of through cognitive evaluation (Krapp, 2005, 2007;
interest then varies with development. Schiefele, 2009; Wigfield et al., 2006). Differences
Conceptualizations that have focused on interest among the conceptualizations with respect to the
as value have further indicated that interest that is role of knowledge reflect differences among research
operationalized in terms of how much the respon- aims. The research questions being addressed do not
dent says he or she likes particular content will dif- necessarily assess change over time but instead focus
ferentiate first in the expectancy value framework on one or another aspect of interest that may be
(Wigfield et al., 2006) and is linked to intrinsic present and/or a factor in each phase of interest.
motivation (Schiefele, 2009). In cross-sectional
work with middle and high school students, Operationalization and Measurement
Denissen, Zarrett, and Eccles (2007) reported that Considerations
self-concept of ability and interest are coupled, There presently is no single correct measure or
but they also point out that when achievement is indicator of interest or interest development, and
introduced, there is a higher degree of coupling as Renninger and Hidi (2011) have noted, such a
between self-concept of ability and achievement specification may not be possible because of differ-
than between interest and achievement. Because, ences in the structure of disciplinary domains, with
however, the focus of studies of interest conceptu- some being more hierarchical than others (Law-
alized in terms of value has been on an assessment less & Kulikowich, 2006), and/or differences in
of value at one point in time, little is understood researchers’ questions.
about possible change in the development of inter- To date, interest development has been mea-
est in terms of expectancy value (see Wigfield & sured using both surveys (e.g., Chen, Darst, &
Cambria, 2010). Pangrazi, 1999; Häussler & Hoffmann, 2002;

 interest a nd its development


Linnenbrink-Garcia et al., 2010; Schiefele, Krapp, ences. Both were mixed-method studies of middle
Wild, & Winteler, 1993; Schraw, Bruning, & school-age students; taken together, they suggest
Svoboda, 1995) and behavioral measures, such as the need to further consider the role of the learn-
online experience sampling (Ainley, Hidi, & Bern- ing environment as another potential indicator of
dorff, 2002), functional magnetic resonance imag- interest.
ing (fMRI; Kim, Lee, & Bong, 2009), or participant In each study, assessment of interest was based
observation (Pressick-Kilborn & Walker, 2002; on an assessment of the feelings, value, and knowl-
Nolen, 2007; Renninger & Wozniak, 1985). In its edge of participants relative to the other content,
most well-developed form, interest has also been or subject matter, with which they were engaged.
assessed based on participation (Azevedo, 2006; In the first, Lipstein and Renninger (2007) used
Barron, 2006; Fink, 1998) or membership (e.g., survey items (Likert ratings and open-ended ques-
recreational figure skaters, see Green-Demers, Pel- tions) and in-depth interviews to assess students’
letier, Stewart, & Gushue, 1998; mathematicians, phase of interest for writing, and then developed
Gisbert 1998). However, Renninger, Cai, Lewis, portraits of students in each phase of interest. In the
Adams, and Ernst (2011) report that interest that second, Renninger and Riley (in press) used partici-
is not well developed is not accurately predicted by pant observation notes and interviews collected at
participation alone. three time points during each of the 5 years to assess
While surveys capture respondent perceptions, phase of interest. Their assessment procedures were
behavioral measures capture respondent behaviors. informed by Renninger and Wozniak’s (1985, see
Triangulating assessments is likely to be necessary in also Renninger, 1990) use of ethnographic methods
order to accurately capture differences among phases to identify developed interest as including all of the
of interest. For example, while triggered interest following in relation to a particular class of objects,
may be assessed through behavioral measures, it is events, or ideas:
not likely to be easily assessed in the earlier phases
a. more engagement relative to other
of its development using self-reports alone given
engagements,
that respondents in this phase are often not aware
b. voluntary return to engagement over time,
that interest has been triggered. On the other hand,
c. the ability to engage independently, and
a respondent is in a position to report whether he
d. engagement that is not simply exploratory.
or she works on more math problems than those
that are assigned, suggesting that survey items that Lipstein and Renninger (2007) undertook their
specify more developed forms of interest may be study of student writers in order to explore poten-
expected to provide a way to partition a sample. tial indicators of each of the four phases of interest
Hidi and Renninger (2006) have suggested development. They developed portraits of middle
that while the earliest phases and the state of inter- school students’ interest for writing by coupling
est may be characterized and assessed by affective information from surveys of 172 students and fol-
response, the identification of developed interest low-up in-depth interviews. Each portrait provided
needs to account for the relation between feelings, an exemplar or generalized characterization of a
value, and knowledge, and that change in this rela- writer in a given phase of interest and described the
tion might be expected with development. Pres- student’s wants and needs as a learner.
ently, efforts to distinguish phases of interest have As depicted in the description of the closed envi-
focused on dichotomies such as situational and ronment of Table 11.2, Lipstein and Renninger
individual interest, earlier and later phases of inter- (2007) found that students with only a triggered
est, less developed and more developed interest, or situational interest were those with little knowl-
low interest and high interest. Methods for identi- edge of and value for writing but whose interest for
fying interest specific to each of the four phases of writing could be triggered by the assignment of the
interest are presently being explored. “right” topic and/or feedback that appreciated their
Two quasi-experimental studies of interest are ideas and provided concrete suggestions for revi-
described that suggest potential indicators of inter- sion. Students with a maintained situational inter-
est in each phase of development. These consider est thought of writing in terms of rules, and they
the relation among the phase of interest and other could be assisted to begin thinking like writers if
variables that describe learning and motivation, they were provided with topics that were of inter-
such as understanding of the discipline, goals, est to them and given supportive feedback. Students
strategies, effort, self-efficacy, and feedback prefer- with an emerging individual interest for writing had

ren n i n g er, s u 


Table 11.2. Learner characteristics and needs in interest development generally, and by learning environment.
Reprinted from Renninger, K. A. & Riley, K. R. (in press). Interest, cognition, and the case of L__ and science.
In Kreitler, S. (Ed). Cognition and motivation: Forging an interdisciplinary perspective. New York: Cambridge
University Press.
Phases of Interest Development
Phase I: Phase 2: Phase 3: Phase 4:
Triggered Maintained Emerging Well-Developed
Situational Situational Individual Individual

Learner Characteristics
• Attends to content, if • Reengages content that • Is likely to • Independently reengages
only fleetingly previously triggered independently content
• Needs support to attention re-engage content • Has curiosity questions
engage • Is supported by others • Has curiosity • Self-regulates easily to
o From others to find connections questions that reframe questions and seek
o Through between skills, lead to seeking answers
instructional knowledge, and prior answers • Has positive feelings
design experience • Has positive feelings • Can persevere through
• May experience either • Has positive feelings • Has stored frustration and challenge in
positive or negative • Is developing knowledge knowledge and order to meet goals
feelings of the content stored value • Recognizes others’ contribu-
• May or may not be • Is developing a sense of • Is very focused tions to the discipline
reflectively aware of the content’s value on his/her own • Actively seeks feedback
triggered interest questions

Needs/More Closed Learning Environmen


• To have his/her ideas • To have his/her ideas • To have his/her ideas • To have his/her ideas
respected respected respected respected
• To feel genuinely • To feel genuinely appre- • To feel genuinely • Information and feedback
appreciated for his/her ciated for efforts appreciated for his/her • To balance his/her personal
efforts • Support to explore his/ efforts standards with more widely
• To have others under- her own ideas • To feel that his/her accepted standards in the
stand how hard work ideas and goals are discipline
with this content is understood • To feel that his/her ideas
• A limited number of • Feedback that enables have been heard and under-
concrete suggestions him/her to see how stood
goals can be more • Constructive feedback
effectively met • Challenge

Needs/More Open Learning Environment


• To have his/her ideas • To have his/her ideas • To have his/her ideas • To have his/her ideas
respected respected respected respected
• To feel genuinely • To feel genuinely appre- • To express his/her • Information and feedback
appreciated for efforts ciated for the efforts he/ ideas • To balance his/her personal
made she has made • Not to be told to standards with more widely
• To know that he/ • To know what he/she revise present efforts accepted standards in the
she understands the has learned and what • To feel that this/her discipline
content he/she still wants to ideas and goals are • To feel that his/her ideas
learn understood have been heard and
• To feel genuinely understood
appreciated for his/her • Constructive feedback
efforts • Challenge
• Feedback that enables
him/her to see how
his/her goals were met

 interest a nd its development


begun to think of themselves as writers and were two types of studies are described separately because
not interested in receiving feedback about either the they offer different insights. Studies that track the
organization or development of their writing. Stu- behaviors of individuals over time provide rich descrip-
dents with a well-developed individual interest for tive information that provides a basis for developing
writing also thought of themselves as writers, but, inductive models. Studies that have examined earlier
unlike those with an emerging individual interest, and/or later phases of interest focus on studying one
sought feedback and recognized that through feed- or more aspects of findings identified in more descrip-
back they could strengthen their abilities to com- tive data with samples and methods that generalize.
municate their ideas to others. A parsimonious selection of these studies is over-
The characteristics of the learners in each of the viewed with particular attention to four questions
four phases of interest suggest a preliminary set of central to supporting interest development: (a) the
indicators for each phase that includes information triggering of interest in both earlier and later phases
about what and how content is engaged and the of interest, (b) how and why interest is maintained
forms of support that might be needed in order to once it has been triggered, (c) fluctuations in interest,
enable it to develop (additional information is pro- and (d) shifts between phases in the development of
vided in both Lipstein & Renninger, 2007 and Ren- interest. Following this, the generative potential of
ninger & Lipstein, 2006). However, Renninger and thinking across studies is suggested, using articles by
Riley’s (in press) 5-year in-depth case study of inner- Frenzel et al. (2010) and Pugh et al. (2010).
city participants in an out-of-school summer sci-
ence workshop reveals a slightly different trajectory Studies That Track Interest Over Time
that they attribute to the workshops’ out-of-school, Interest development as described in studies
nongraded context (see the description of the open that have tracked individuals over time is collected
environment in Table 11.2). The participants in the through interviews with the participant and/or sig-
science workshops were in an environment that pro- nificant others in the participant’s life, surveys and
vided a lot of possible triggers for interest, and once interviews, the development of portraits based on
their interest was maintained, it quickly shifted to interviews or surveys and interviews, experience
being an emerging individual interest, where they sampling, course enrollments, and/or observation.
sought input, readily asking and answering ques- Analysis of these studies together describes the
tions. This differed from the resistance to feedback development of interest as primarily a sequential
that characterized the middle school writers iden- process that evolves through interactions with the
tified as having an emerging individual interest, environment.
suggesting the possibility of the effects of environ-
mental differences in constraint and opportunities
triggers for interest development
for learning on the learners’ interest trajectories.
Findings from studies that track the development
Like findings reported by Frenzel et al. (2010),
of interest over time generally describe a changing
who studied the decline in students’ interest for
relation between affect and knowledge as interest
mathematics in three academic achievement tracks,
develops. They also describe triggers for interest
it appears that trajectory of interest development
(in this case, triggers that actually result in inter-
may be impacted by how open the environment is
est development) as supporting the making of con-
to inquiry, or the press of the learning environment
nections to content in earlier phases of interest and
on achievement. Such findings suggest the need to
opportunities to continue to develop understanding
consider not only the learners’ feelings, value, and
of content in later phases. Some examples include
knowledge as a predictor of interest development
the following: girls in earlier phases of interest who
but also the role of the environment.
wanted to pursue hard science were triggered by
their desire to get their father’s approval and also
Research on Interest Development by opportunities to pursue mathematics (Gisbert,
Studies that track the behaviors of individuals over 1998), children’s desires to express themselves as
time and studies of learners in earlier and/or later members of a “literate community” in their class-
phases of interest (also described as situational and room acted as a trigger for their interest in reading
individual, less developed and more developed, or low and writing (Nolen, 2007), and instructional meth-
interest and high interest) provide our present under- ods in Latin that students personalized themselves
standing of interest development. Findings from these were successful triggers (Renninger et al., 2004).

ren n i n g er, s u 


Changes in the relation between affect and encouraging learners to fully engage, explore, and
knowledge are also referenced in later phases of work with the content of the interest (Renninger,
development, when the interest being studied 2010). The studies allow identification of recurrent
already exists. Some examples include the follow- patterns within the ecology of the larger learning
ing: descriptions of self-initiated work with technol- environment (Barron, 2006) and point to indica-
ogy in which adolescents seek additional resources, tors that warrant further study and consideration.
create new activities, pursue structured learning, Such patterns are descriptive and specific to a par-
and develop mentoring/knowledge-sharing rela- ticular context. Next steps to examine the issues that
tionships (Barron, 2006); the dyslexic adolescent are uncovered include the kind of replication and
who uses his or her more well-developed interest as validation undertaken in studies that have targeted
a context within which to work on reading skills earlier and/or later phases of interest.
needed to develop further understanding (Fink,
1998); and business students whose interests were Studies of Earlier and/or Later
refined with the introduction of new opportunities Phases of Interest
(Krapp & Lewalter, 2001). Once interest is triggered Studies contributing to the understanding of ear-
and a connection to content occurs, it appears to lier and/or later phases of interest have typically not
continue to be triggered as interest develops, either been undertaken to address interest development,
by other people or the environment, challenging but rather to understand and/or demonstrate the
reading materials, or the development of nuanced impact of interest as a motivational variable. In these
understanding. studies, researchers usually partition the sample of
participants whom they are studying into earlier or
sustaining interest, fluctuations, and later phases of interest based on responses to survey
shifts between phases items, rather than studying one or more individuals
Studies that have examined the development of over time. Some of these studies have focused on
interest over time suggest that, once triggered, inter- participants in a particular phase of interest, while
est is sustained based on the availability of oppor- others have compared the responses of participants
tunities to continue to learn and of support to be in two phases. The relation between affect and cog-
autonomous—meaning that there is ready scaf- nition in these studies is not central unless connec-
folding available for the learner who needs it. Such tions between the findings and a model of interest
opportunities (or constraints on opportunities) can development is specified, in which case the shifting,
take the form of finances, timing, or access (Bar- or change, from one to another phase of interest is
ron, 2006, Barron, Kennedy-Martin, Takeuchi, & addressed (e.g., Harackiewicz, Durik, K. Barron,
Fithian, 2009), although the types of support or Linnenbrink, & Tauer, 2008). Most often, this type
feedback required may depend on the phase of the of study focuses on earlier phases of interest and
interest. Mismatches between the learner’s phase of has measured interest in terms of affect and value,
interest and available supports have been found to rather than knowledge. Taken together, the stud-
result in marginalization and lack of identification ies confirm the importance of the relation among
(Nolen, 2007), a decrease in feelings of competence achievement, feelings of competence, and the devel-
(Azevedo, 2006), and the falling off of interest (Ren- opment of interest. They also suggest a potentially
ninger & Lipstein, 2006). Shifts between phases of critical role for metacognitive awareness.
interest and the development of interest, on the
other hand, have been characterized as including triggers for interest development
developing feelings of competence, the acquisition Findings from studies addressing earlier and/or
of skills and knowledge (Nolen, 2007; Lipstein & later phases of interest development have focused
Renninger, 2007), and/or identification with the on (a) the impact of triggers for situational or indi-
domain of interest (see Krapp, 2003, 2005). vidual interest on learning and (b) the experience of
Assessed in terms of the individual learner and the learning environment as a contributor to inter-
his or her development over time, studies that have est. Both situational interest and individual interest
mapped trajectories of interest development point have been found to trigger interest. Situational inter-
to the critical role of environmental supports in est has been found to promote reading comprehen-
triggering and sustaining interest. As interest devel- sion and motivation among third graders (Guthrie
ops, the supports need to shift from helping learn- et al., 2006), help high school students develop pos-
ers to make connections to particular content to itive attitudes toward science (Palmer, 2009), and

 interest a nd its development


promote undergraduates’ reading engagement and or trigger interest for himself or herself. Thus, while
essay production (Flowerday, Schraw, & Stevens, present perception and values may inform pres-
2004). Similarly, individual interest has been found ent interest, the experience of interest can change
to trigger learners to persevere in working with con- through the process of triggering that is provided
tent that is complex and challenging. For example, either by other people or situations (e.g., Hulleman
middle school students were found to be more likely et al., 2008; Mitchell, 1993; Palmer, 2009) or by
to persevere in working on math problems into individuals who are in a position to self-generate
which an individual interest had been inserted as a interest (e.g., by finding a reason to persevere; San-
context (e.g., basketball) than problems into which sone et al., 1992).
content of less developed interest (e.g., football)
were inserted (Renninger, Ewen, & Lasher, 2002; sustaining, fluctuations, and shifts in
see also Hoffmann, 2002). interest development
Having interest has also been described as a Studies of both earlier and later phases of interest
buffering factor that helps students to cope with development suggest that situational factors, chal-
unfavorable learning conditions (Katz, Assor, lenge, and personal investment are potential triggers
Kanat-Maymon, & Bereby-Meyer, 2006). For for sustaining interest, and they provide a basis for
example, Tsai et al. (2008) reported that the climate shifts that occur in interest development. For exam-
of the classroom (e.g., the levels of autonomy sup- ple, experiences in which students are led to explore
port, controlling behaviors) influenced those with and work with the everyday meaning of science con-
less developed interest more than those with well- cepts in new ways are designed to promote mean-
developed interest. Similarly, Durik and Harackie- ingfulness and sustain engagement (e.g., Mitchell,
wicz (2007) found that level of interest for math 1993; Palmer, 2004, 2009; Pugh et al., 2010). They
influenced the impact of catch (collative factors) and also support learners to set goals for themselves that
hold (situational factors that sustain interest) in an involve them in asking curiosity questions, reflect-
experimental manipulation of triggers for interest in ing on these, and seeking resources to answer them
solving math problems. Those with less interest for (Renninger, 2000; Renninger, Bachrach, & Posey,
mathematics showed more interest in the collative- 2008).
rich environment that provided triggers for novelty, Like the process of triggering interest, whether
and less interest in triggers for challenge, while those interest is sustained and continues to develop
with more developed interest for mathematics were appears to be linked to learners’ perceptions of their
negatively affected by triggers for novelty and posi- experiences, as well as their abilities to set goals
tively influenced by triggers for challenge. for themselves and self-regulate (see Sansone &
Findings such as these suggest both that poten- Thoman, 2005b). Harackiewicz et al.’s (2008) find-
tial triggers for interest differ for learners with more ings indicate, for example, that the process of trigger-
and less developed interest, and the potential trig- ing interest and goal adoption differs for those who
gers of the learning environment may be particu- come to class with an already developed interest and
larly critical for those in earlier phases of interest those who do not. They found that undergraduates
development. They also suggest that the associa- with low initial interest who reported having their
tion between interest and experience that is inde- interest triggered were also those who experienced
pendent of achievement, as is reported by Schiefele shifts in the development of interest, suggesting
and Csikszentmihalyi (1994), is further indication that the triggering of interest can promote mastery
that changed experience can impact interest (see goals and that mastery goals can promote interest
also Pugh et al., 2010). However, Schiefele and development.
Csikszentmihalyi (1995) also reported a correla- Harackiewicz et al. (2008) also found, however,
tion between grades and interest that, like Jacobs, that the simple presence of a trigger did not predict
Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, and Wigfield’s (2002) find- continued interest. Rather, the triggering of interest
ings, points to links between grades and valuing and in addition to students’ final grades in the course
contributes to the experience of interest. predicted their continued interest. These find-
Sansone and her colleagues’ work suggest that ings suggest the importance of both mastery and
interest experience reliably predicts task choice and performance goals to the development of interest
persistence and is essential to self-regulation (e.g., (see Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, & Elliot, 2002;
Sansone & Thoman, 2005a, b). With interest the Harackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich, Elliot, & Thrash,
learner has a clear goal and is able to self-generate 2002). Moreover, analyses to examine the relation

ren n i n g er, s u 


between interest (measured in terms of feelings and Two Studies of Interest Development
value) and background knowledge in the Harackie- Reviewing articles and chapters for this chapter
wicz et al. (2008) study revealed that initial inter- called attention to the range of studies that con-
est was a particularly strong predictor of continued tribute to our present understanding of interest
interest when paired with a high level of background development. It also pointed to the importance of
knowledge, indicating the importance of content their complementarities as sources of validation and
knowledge for interest development. emergent insight. In this section of this chapter, the
Interest that has been triggered has also been questions, methods, and findings from two solid and
found to fluctuate, however. Consistent with the seemingly different studies by Frenzel et al. (2010)
Harackiewicz et al. (Harackiewicz, Barron, Tauer, and Pugh et al. (2010) are reviewed, and their joint
& Elliot, 2002; Harackiewicz, et al., 2008) findings, contributions to interest development are consid-
Denissen et al. (2007) found that students between ered. Two other studies could as easily have been
6 and 17 years of age felt competent and interested selected for consideration; our choice was informed
in the subjects in which they achieved and in which by the differences of their methods and the similar-
they perceived themselves to have ability. They also ity of the age group that each addressed.
found an increase with age in the coordination of Frenzel et al.’s study is a quantitative longitudi-
achievement, self-concept of ability, and/or interest, nal study of early adolescents’ mathematics interest;
suggesting an increasingly influential role of student Pugh et al.’s is a short-term qualitative study of high
perceptions when the content with which they are school students’ transformative experiences with
working in school also reflects increases in difficulty biology. Both studies assess the trajectory of interest
(see Hidi & Ainley, 2008). development. Whereas Frenzel et al.’s study implies
Repeated but not specifically examined in studies that the students’ environment (e.g., teachers, par-
of earlier and/or later phases of interest development ents, school) may influence and account for differ-
is the role of the learner’s metacognitive awareness. ences in their achievements and interest trajectories,
Discussion has centered instead on perceptions of Pugh et al.’s study suggests that individual learner
the environment and whether the learner responds characteristics contribute significantly to interest
to potential triggers or opportunities in the envi- development. Together, these studies can be under-
ronment. The evidence suggests that when learner stood to suggest that interest development involves
interest is triggered, it can be sustained, but if inter- both internal and external factors and point to
est fluctuates this is linked to the learner’s percep- potential indicators and questions that the research
tions or experience of the environment. on interest development still needs to address.
The Harackiewicz et al. (2008) findings suggest
that when undergraduates reported having a trig- frenzel, goetz, pekrun, and watt (2010)
gered situational interest, their interest developed. Frenzel et al. (2010) reported on a longitudinal
Harackiewicz et al. (2008) did not go on to suggest study of the mathematical interests of 3,193 students
that if participants report having their interest trig- (51% female) in grades 5 to 9 in the German school
gered on a survey, then it also is the case that they system based on surveys administered to the stu-
have at least some level of metacognitive awareness dents and their parents. Using Likert scales assessing
and are positioned to set goals for themselves and self- feelings, value, and knowledge to measure interest,
regulate. This is an emergent finding of this review. four issues were addressed: (a) the characteristics of
One of the critiques of using surveys to assess earlier trajectories of interest development in mathematics,
phases of interest has been that learners are not likely (b) the role of gender in the development of inter-
to know that their interest has been triggered. What est in mathematics, (c) the role of ability grouping
the Harackiewicz et al. findings do suggest, however, in the development of interest in mathematics, and
is that when learners are able to report having a trig- (d) the role of the values of significant others’ in the
gered interest, their interest then develops. This is development of interest in mathematics.
not to say that interest cannot develop without this Frenzel et al. predicted that students would expe-
reflective awareness, but rather that interest can be rience a generalized loss of interest across time. In
expected to develop if this reflective awareness is particular, they hypothesized that students’ intrinsic
present. Without metacognitive awareness, it may be motivations for learning were likely to be in increas-
that the learner can be supported to engage with con- ing conflict with school-ordained restrictions such
tent but may lack self-direction and need additional as required courses, increased task complexity, and
support to reflect on and continue to explore it. demands for academic effort and achievement.

 interest a nd its development


Frenzel et al. focused on mathematics, noting that slightly lower initial levels of interest but by grade
mathematics has long been considered a field pre- 9 had managed to sustain interest, whereas students
ferred by males, and predicted that gender would in both Realschule and Gymnasium evinced steeper
influence the level of mathematics interest, in that declines in interest levels, leveling out at a lower
females would have less interest than males, but level than Hauptschule students by grade 9. Finally,
that the level of female and male interest would while family, peer, and teacher influences affected
not impact the expected decline in the trajectory of the formation of students’ interest, it appears that,
interest development given findings suggesting gen- based on an assessment of interest trajectories, they
der differences in the level of interest but not in the did not influence the development of interest.
trajectory of its development (Fredricks & Eccles,
2002; Jacobs et al., 2002; Watt, 2004). pugh, linnenbrink-garcia, koskey,
Frenzel et al. also predicted that ability grouping stewart, and manzy (2010)
would impact interest development based on Marsh’s Pugh et al. (2010) reported on a short-term
(1987) findings from a study of the “Big Fish Little study of transformative experience among 166
Pond Effect,” which suggests negative effects for (66% female) 9th- and 10th-grade biology students,
students placed into high-achievement groups and where transformative experience is characterized by
positive effects of placement into low-achievement “motivated use, expansion of perception, and expe-
groups. Taking advantage of the organization of the riential value” (p. 7), and interest and task value are
German school system, which tests and places stu- described as supporting conceptual change (Dole &
dents into one of three academic tracks based on Sinatra, 1998). Prestudy, poststudy, and follow-up
academic achievement by the fourth grade, Frenzel assessments of students’ science knowledge, iden-
et al. posited that students in Hauptschule (the low- tity, and goals were undertaken using survey data.
est track) would report higher interest levels than Interest was not assessed independently but as part
students in either Realschule (the middle track) or of the construct of experiential value; Likert-items
Gymnasium (the highest track), due to the pressure were used to assess student opinion about the value
in Realschule and Gymnasium to focus on achieve- and utility of information about natural selection.
ment instead of personal development. Pugh et al.’s research questions focused on three
Finally, based on the findings of social cognitive issues: (a) the prevalence of transformative experi-
theorists (e.g., Eccles, Wigfield, Harold, & Blumen- ences among high school biology students learning
feld, 1993; Pekrun, 2000), Frenzel et al. predicted about natural selection; (b) the relation among trans-
that significant others such as family, classmates/ formative experience, science identity, and mastery
peers, and teachers would influence the forma- goal orientation; and (c) the relation between trans-
tion of students’ values and interest for mathemat- formative experience and both initial and enduring
ics. Family members, especially parents, have been conceptual change and transfer. Based on findings
found to be role models for their children’s eventual from his earlier work, Pugh (2004) had described
interests and educational values (Jacobs, Davis- transformative experiences as occurring when stu-
Kean, Bleeker, Eccles, & Malanchuk, 2005) and dents are motivated to apply what they have learned
students can be expected to develop interests and in the classroom outside of the classroom, experi-
values similar to those of their parents (Jacobs & ences that led to expanded perception and value.
Eccles, 2000). These findings suggested that transformation is
Findings from Frenzel et al’s study revealed an best measured by observing changes in students’
overall decline in mathematical interest over time, conceptual understandings of science and whether
regardless of variables such as gender. In terms of they transfer their learning to other aspects of their
gender, Frenzel et al. reported that girls had a lower lives, see aspects of the world in new ways, and find
initial level of interest, but as expected: There were value in doing so (Pugh, 2004). Like findings from
no differences between the shapes of the trajectories Girod, Twyman, and Wojcikiewicz’s (2010) work
of girls boys, suggesting that the areas of decline and with fifth graders, Pugh (2002) showed that biology
stabilization on the growth trajectories may be the students who had transformative experiences had
result of an intensification at earlier ages. In addi- more gains in conceptual understanding than those
tion, differences were identified in the level of inter- who did not. In the study examined here, he and his
est of students in each of the different ability groups. colleagues sought to explore transformative experi-
General/universal longitudinal interest declines ences in an expanded sample and sought to explore
aside, students in Hauptschule in grade 5 had science identity and achievement goal orientation as

ren n i n g er, s u 


predictors of transformative experience. They chose of goal orientation, which in turn predicted the
to focus on natural selection in the biology class- occurrence of transformative experiences. It appears
room because this is a topic about which students that mastery orientation increased the likelihood of
often have misconceptions. transformative experiences. On the strength of these
Based on both Girod and Wong (2002) and Pugh findings, Pugh et al. pointed to the role of individual
(2004), it was expected that students who identified characteristics in the development of interest.
as having had transformative experiences were also
those who saw the relevance of the science being complementary aspects of the frenzel
taught. They conceptualized interest in this context et al. and pugh et al. studies
in terms of value, and science identity as comprised The findings of the Frenzel et al. and Pugh et al.
of one’s prior knowledge and one’s identification studies mirror and extend present discussions of
with science as a strength or weakness. When stu- interest development. Together, their findings sug-
dents believed that they had a strong science iden- gest that, at least for adolescents, interest develops
tity, it was expected that they would thus see the in relation to both academic and personal satisfac-
relevance of the science unit being taught and were tion, and the keys to these lie in the balance and
more likely to undergo transformative experiences personalizing of external and internal, environmen-
(Girod & Wong, 2002; Pugh, 2004). Similarly, it tal and individual, factors.
was expected that a student’s achievement goal ori- Frenzel et al.’s findings confirm the existence of
entation would affect the likelihood of transforma- a general decline of interest over time spent within
tive experiences. the academic system, and the influence of ability
Thus, Pugh et al. predicted that students with a groups on students’ interest development. Students
mastery goal orientation would be more likely to in Hauptschule (the lower track) showed less steep
report transformative experiences, given the focus declines in interest over time, compared to students
of mastery goal orientation on learning. Pugh et al. in Realschule and Gymnasium. Frenzel et al. sug-
also expected that students who either strongly iden- gested that this might be a result of the less competi-
tify with science or have a mastery approach toward tive atmosphere with fewer achievement-oriented
learning would be more likely to experience trans- demands in Hauptschule as compared to Realschule
formative experiences than those with a less defined and Gymnasium. These findings also underscore the
science identity and a performance approach (i.e., role of the learning environment as a support for (or
displaying competence but not necessarily compre- constraint on) academic development, interest, and
hension). performance.
Pugh and his colleagues found that both science Similarly, Pugh et al.’s findings point to the
identity and mastery goal orientations were posi- importance of the learning environment as a sup-
tively associated with transformative experience. port for comprehension and transfer, suggesting the
Students who both identified with science and had further need to attend to the role of learner char-
a mastery approach to learning were more likely to acteristics in the development of interest. When
experience transformative experiences; they retained prior knowledge was controlled, students with a
information and were able to independently apply mastery approach to learning were found to have
it outside of the classroom. However, students more comprehension and a greater ability to retain
with initially higher levels of knowledge about the and transfer what they had learned to other aspects
information taught in the unit also reported having of their lives. These findings further suggest that a
more transformative experiences, suggesting that mastery goal orientation may compensate for less
the acquisition and development of knowledge and than ideal situational factors such as unsupportive
interest (defined as value and utility) may be mutu- environments, limited opportunities, and grade-
ally reinforcing. oriented pressure. It also appears that whether inter-
Their results further suggested that students with est develops depends on the learner: It may be the
a mastery goal orientation were more likely to report individual’s approach to learning that most influ-
experiencing transformative experiences, and that ences both comprehension and transfer.
mastery goal orientation mediated the relationship Frenzel et al.’s and Pugh et al.’s studies also
between science identity and transformative experi- indicated that both situational and individual fac-
ence when prior science knowledge related to the tors can result in a falling off of interest. According
unit taught was controlled. In other words, a strong to the Frenzel et al. study, placement into a high-
science identity predicted a stronger endorsement achievement ability group negatively influenced

 interest a nd its development


interest development, whereas placement in a low- earlier phase, consisting of triggered situational and
achievement ability group had a positive effect. The maintained situational interest, and a later phase
interest levels of Hauptschule students declined at a of emerging individual interest (see Table 11.1).
slower rate than Realschule or Gymnasium students Whereas, because they have included knowledge in
and eventually stabilized toward the later grades of their assessment of interest, Frenzel et al. appear to
high school. As this difference in decline in interest be distinguishing between earlier and later phases of
occurred regardless of the student’s initial interest interest for mathematics.
level, it suggests that the right combination of envi- Consistent with descriptions of students in earlier
ronmental factors and amount of external pressure phases of interest as mapped by Lipstein and Ren-
can cause someone’s interest to change, whether ninger (2007; see also Renninger & Riley, in press),
positively or negatively. Similarly, Pugh et al. found the Pugh et al. and the Frenzel et al. studies sug-
that intrinsic motivation, such as a mastery goal ori- gest that it is the student who ultimately makes use
entation, increased the likelihood of transformative of available supports, and whether students make
experiences, more advanced conceptual understand- this choice depends on whether they are enabled
ing, and the transfer of learning. to make personalized, individualized connections
to content that is a function of both their learning
knowledge, a component of characteristics and the learning environment. This
developing interest is an important point. While personalized content
Pugh et al.’s decision to assess interest using has for some time been recognized as important in
items addressing feelings and value (e.g., “During generating interest (e.g., Mitchell, 1993), the find-
science class, I think the stuff we are learning about ings from the Pugh et al. and Frenzel et al. studies
adaptation and/or natural selection is interesting.” point to the fact that it is the learner, not the teacher
[p. 22]) and to control for prior knowledge influ- or the researcher, who decides what is meaningful—
ences what they can say about interest development. and also that this is the case whether intrest is in ear-
If interest develops through phases, and if transi- lier or later phases of development. In other words,
tions between phases of interest are dependent on whether the student is positioned to take advantage
developing understanding, then knowledge needs of available resources may have to do with how and
to be included in measures intended to distinguish whether he or she understands the situation or the
between earlier and later phases of interest devel- goal and his or her ability to recognize the utility
opment. Pugh et al.’s findings provide information of the particular resources or practices that would
about the roles of knowledge and value in the pro- allow the goal to be realized. These findings fur-
cess of making connections to content to be learned. ther suggest that the degree to which the learner is
However, their findings do not for sure address dif- metacognitively aware of his or her interest may be
ferences between those in earlier and later phases of a critical indicator of interest development. Having
interest, and their abilities to pose questions, seek an interest is not the same as being metacognitively
answers, and so forth. While some participants may aware of the role of interest in one’s learning. Meta-
well have been in later phases of interest, distin- cognition should allow for the possibility of change
guishing among students in terms of the possibil- by enabling goal setting and self-regulation.
ity that some were in later phases of interest was
not undertaken. Frenzel et al., on the other hand, Conclusions
used items to assess interest that tapped feelings and The studies by Frenzel et al. and Pugh et al. point
value, as well as the participants’ relation to knowl- to some potentially critical aspects and indicators of
edge: “I would like to find out more about some of interest development, in particular the roles of situ-
the things we deal with in our mathematics class.” ational influences such as the achievement demands
And, “I like to read books and solve brainteasers of the learning environment and experiential valu-
related to mathematics.” (p. 532) ing. Like the other studies of earlier and/or later
While both Pugh et al. and Frenzel et al. described phases of interest development, they note the role of
their studies of interest in terms of low and high inter- the learner’s metacognitive awareness as an indicator
est, the relation between their outcomes and inter- of what types of supports might be needed in order
est theory suggest that what they are each describing for interest to develop.
differs. Pugh et al. appear to be describing either These aspects of interest development together
earlier and later phases of situational interest (trig- form the basis of an inductive model for understand-
gered situational and maintained situational), or an ing the relation among the learner’s phase of interest,

ren n i n g er, s u 


achievement demands of the learning environment, est and/or its development, using different measures
and metacognitive awareness. Depicted in Fig- and methods, and resulting in seemingly contradic-
ure 11.1 as a Punnett square, phase of interest forms tory findings and conclusions between studies. It
one dimension and the achievement demands of appears that for research on interest development it
the learning environment the other. The level of the is important to look for complementarities among
learner’s metacognition, or reflective ability to think findings—a consideration that also requires atten-
about interest and learning, is also included in each tion to the way in which interest and its develop-
quadrant. Framed in this way, it appears that meta- ment is conceptualized and measured, how it is
cognitive awareness, both in terms of the learner’s studied, with which populations (age and experi-
reflection on content and abilities to self-regulate, ence), and in what type of context (domain of study,
is beneficial to the learner and supports interest achievement expectations, etc.).
to develop. Development of content knowledge The proposed Punnett square anchors the
can also support the learner’s capacity to develop repeated evidence that interest develops through the
meaningful connections to the content, regardless interaction of the learner’s individual learning char-
of his or her initial phase of interest. However, the acteristics and his or her environment. It includes
achievement demands of the learning environment information about a particular aspect of the learn-
may positively or negatively affect the learner’s abil- ing environment: its achievement demands. It also
ity to make connections to the content and ask curi- calls attention to the emergent finding from the lit-
osity questions about it. If the learner is negatively erature review in this chapter, which suggests that
affected, his or her content knowledge may develop metacognitive awareness contributes to whether
but interest may not, thereby compromising his or a learner responds to potential triggers. The Pun-
her possibilities for learning. nett square can also be used to describe the focus of
To date, research on interest development has support needed to enable shifts in interest develop-
tended to focus on one or another aspect of inter- ment. Vertical movement along the Punnett square

High Achievement Demands Low Achievement Demands

More Metacognition More Metacognition

Competitive and competent Reflective and easily absorbed in all facets of


Needs: the content
Less Developed Interest

• To reflect on his or her interest Needs:


• To explore curiosity questions • Opportunities that include targeted
Passionate and Passionate but
challenges
successful, may lack lacking in self-awareness
• To self-regulate engagement
self-awareness and direction and possibly self-direction
with content
Needs: Needs:
• To reflect on content • To reflect on and self-regulate
• To self-regulate engagement his/her engagement with content
with content • To link to present interests and
content knowledge

Less Metacognition Less Metacognition


More Metacognition

Attentive to achievement (e.g. grades) Reflective about what needs to be accomplished


Needs: Needs:
• To continue to develop his/her Little personal • To stretch present understanding with
More Developed Interest

understanding of content knowledge investment in either content-related learning challenges


learning and/or Little personal
• To link understanding to present • To explore curiosity questions
understanding of how to investment in either
interests and content knowledge
engage the content learning and/or
Needs: understanding of how to engage
• To develop his/her the content
understanding of content Needs:
knowledge • To develop his/her
• To make connections understanding of content
between content knowledge knowledge
and present interests • To make connections to
present interests
Less Metacognition

Fig. 11.1. Punnett square of the possible relations among learner phase of interest, metacognitive abilities, and achievement
demands of the learning environment.

 interest a nd its development


indicate shifts between earlier and later phases of and the focus and quality of supports that enabled
interest, whereas horizontal movement refers to shifts in her interest development).
altering the learning environment, or achievement
context. Future Directions
Used for the purpose of revisiting findings from Research on interest has demonstrated that it
both studies of interest development over time and is a variable that develops over time and can be
the studies of earlier and/or later phases in inter- supported to develop at any age. Its presence has
est development, the Punnett square facilitates the been repeatedly found to positively impact learn-
discerning of patterns among individuals sharing ers’ attention, goal setting, and learning. Research
trajectories of interest development. Patterns such on interest development, however, is in its infancy.
as these are useful for researchers studying interest This chapter has examined research on interest and
development, and for educators or anyone working its development, paying particular attention to little
with and hoping to support the interest develop- understood aspects of the development of interest:
ment of others. the triggering of interest in both earlier and later
Mapping what we know of Helen’s experience to phases of interest, how interest is maintained once
the Punnett square, for example, suggests that her it is triggered, fluctuations in the development of
achievement demands were low. She was in an ear- interest, and shifts from one to another phase of
lier phase of interest development at the beginning interest development.
of her anecdote: She was less metacognitively aware It is provocative, for example, that interest should
and was unresponsive to potential triggers for inter- be able to be sustained once a respondent can indi-
est. She then shifted from being less metacognitively cate that his or her interest is triggered. This finding
aware and less developed in her interest to being also raises other questions, however. For example:
more metacognitively aware and more developed in Why and when is a potential trigger likely to come
her interest. to the attention of a learner and work? Are poten-
Helen’s interest developed outside of the school tial triggers for interest the same in all disciplinary
environment; it could be said to have been a con- contexts, in naturally occurring and experimental
text with low achievement demands, and that Anne, contexts? Do potential triggers (e.g., novelty) hold
her tutor, provided appropriate types of support in the same meaning for learners in one versus another
order to allow her interest to develop. Based on phase of interest and at different ages?
Helen’s account, she appears to have almost skipped Similarly, findings suggesting that fluctuations in
through the phase of maintained situational inter- interest are likely to be due to the learners’ percep-
est once she made the connection between finger tions or experience of the environment are critical
signing and the water, suggesting that maybe the and raise questions for further study. For example:
maintaining of interest is an artifact of school- Are there particular learner characteristics, or con-
based learning, an interpretation that is suggested figurations of learner characteristics, that contribute
by the Renninger and Riley (in press) study as well. to how the environment is perceived, experienced,
Reflecting on Helen’s case, and the overviews of the and whether interest can be expected to develop?
literature provided, it is also noted that Helen is sig- What types of environmental supports are needed
nificantly younger than the adolescent learners of for learners in different phases of interest? What is
the Pugh et al. and Frenzel et al. studies, which sug- the role of metacognition in the development of
gests that for her, the development of this interest interest and how might it be fostered?
was possibly easier than it might have been for an In the present chapter we worked with aspects
older, more self-conscious student (see Renninger, and dimensions of interest development that
2009). emerged in reviewing the research literature. Any
Further questions to be considered on the basis of number of Punnett squares could have been devel-
the quadrants of the Punnett square in Figure 11.1 oped, drawing on already existing studies. Little
include the following: whether Helen’s age changes research has yet been done on how findings from
the trajectory of interest development in some way; different studies interact with one another and/
what difference a high achievement demand context or contribute to interest development. The Pun-
would contribute to what is understood presently; nett square proposed in this chapter is an example
and the particulars of her engagement with both less of a framework that could support the continued
and more metacognitive awareness (her response to examination of complementarities among inter-
potential triggers, how and why she reacted to them, est research. In selecting studies to examine, we

ren n i n g er, s u 


strove to find complementarities, recognizing that between domain-specific achievement, self-concept, and
differences of measures, methods, and disciplines interest. Child Development, 78, 430–447.
Dole, J. A., & Sinatra, G. M. (1998). Reconceptualizing change
provide insight and also present particular chal- in the cognitive construction of knowledge. Educational Psy-
lenges. We suggest that forward progress in the chologist, 33, 109–128.
understanding of interest and its development Durik, A., & Harackiewicz, J. M., (2007). Different strokes for
involves revisiting the differences and challenges of different folks: How individual interest moderates the effects
what has already been found. of situational factors on task interest. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 99(3), 597–610.
Eccles, J. P., & Midgley, C. (1989). State/environment fit: Devel-
Acknowledgments opmentally appropriate classrooms for early adolescents. In
We are appreciative of the thoughtful comments we received R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in educa-
from Jessica E. Bachrach at different phases in this project, tion (Vol. 3, pp. 139–181). New York: Guilford Press.
Nadine Kolowrat’s editorial support, and Allison L. Gannett’s Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993).
help with figures. We also gratefully acknowledge funding for Age and gender differences in children’s self- and task percep-
work on this chapter from a Joel Dean Summer Research Grant tions during elementary school. Child Development, 64(3),
and the Swarthmore College Faculty Research Fund. 830–847.
Fink, R. (1998). Interest, gender, and literacy development in
References successful dyslexics. In L. Hoffmann, A. Krapp, K. A. Ren-
Ainley, M. (2007). Being and feeling interested: Transient state, ninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and learning: Proceedings
mood, and disposition. In P. Schutz (Ed.), Emotion in educa- of the Seeon Conference on Interest and Gender (pp. 402–408).
tion (pp. 141- 157). New York: Academic Press. Kiel, Germany: IPN.
Ainley, M., & Patrick, L. (2006). Measuring self-regulated learn- Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solv-
ing processes through tracking patterns of student interac- ing. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), The nature of intelligence (pp.
tion with achievement activities. Educational Psychology 231–236). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Review, 18, 267–286. Flowerday, T., Schraw, G., & Stevens, J. (2004). The role of
Ainley, M., Hidi, S., & Berndorff, D. (2002). Interest, learning, choice and interest in reader engagement. The Journal of
and the psychological processes that mediate their relation- Experimental Education, 72(2), 93–114.
ship. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(3), 545–561. Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. (2002). Children’s competence and
Alexander, J. M., Johnson, K. E., Leibham, M. E., & Kelley, value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth
K. (2008). The development of conceptual interests in young trajectories in two male-sex-typed domains. Developmental
children. Cognitive Development, 23, 324–334. Psychology, 38, 519–533.
Armstrong, P. I., Allison, P., & Rounds, J. (2008). Development Frenzel, A. C., Goetz, T., Pekrun, R., & Watt, H. M. G. (2010).
and initial validation of brief public domain RIASEC marker Development of mathematics interest in adolescence: Influ-
scales. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 73, 287–299. ences of gender, family, and school context. Journal of
Azevedo, F. S. (2006). Personal excursions: investigating the Research on Adolescence, 20(2), 507–537.
dynamics of student engagement. International Journal of Girod, M., Twyman, T., & Wojcikiewicz, S. (2010). Teaching
Computers for Mathematical Learning, 11, 57–98. and learning science for transformative, aesthetic experience.
Barron, B. (2006). Interest and self-sustained learning as cata- Journal of Science Teacher Education, 21, 801–824.
lysts of development: A learning ecology perspective. Human Girod, M., & Wong, D. (2002). An aesthetic (Deweyan) per-
Development, 49, 193–224. spective on science learning: Case studies of three fourth
Barron, B., Kennedy-Martin, C., Takeuchi, L., & Fithian, R. graders. The Elementary School Journal, 87, 574–587.
(2009). Parents as learning partners in the development of Gisbert, K. (1998). Individual interest in mathematics and female
technological fluency. International Journal of Learning and gender identity: Biographical case studies. In L. Hoffmann,
Media, 1(2), 55–77 A. Krapp, K. A. Renninger, & J. Baumert (Eds.), Interest and
Bergin, D. (1999). Influences on classroom interest. Educational learning: Proceedings of the Seeon Conference on Interest and
Psychologist, 34(2), 87–98. Gender (pp. 387–401). Kiel, Germany: IPN.
Brown, S., & Lent, R. (1996). A social cognitive framework for Green-Demers, I., Pelletier, L. G., Stewart, D. G., & Gushue, N.
career choice counseling. The Career Development Quarterly, R. (1998). Coping with the less interesting aspects of train-
44, 355–367. ing: Toward a model of interest and motivation enhance-
Chen, A., Darst, P. W., & Pangrazi, R. P. (1999). What consti- ment in individual sports. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,
tutes situational interest? Validating a construct in physical 20(4), 251–261.
education. Measurement in Physical Education and Exercise Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Humenick, N. H., Perencevich,
Science, 3, 157–180. K. C., Taboada, A., & Barbosa, P. (2006). Influences of
Cobb, P., & Hodge, L. (2004, June). An initial contribution stimulating tasks on reading motivation and comprehension.
to the development of a design theory of mathematical Journal of Educational Research, 99, 232–247.
interests: The case of statistical data analysis. Paper pre- Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot,
sented as part of the symposium, Design Theories of Inter- A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal
est, Motivation, and Engagement for the Learning Sciences. theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational
Sixth International Conference of the Learning Sciences, Psychology, 94, 638–645.
Santa Monica, CA. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Tauer, J. M., & Elliot, A. J.
Denissen, J. H., Zarrett, N. R., & Eccles, J. S. (2007). “I like to (2002). Predicting success in college: A longitudinal study
do it, I’m able, and I know I am”: Longitudinal couplings of achievement goals and ability measures as predictors of

 interest a nd its development


interest and performance from freshman year through gradu- feedback processing. Paper presented at the Meetings of the
ation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 562–575. American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.
Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Linnenbrink, Krapp, A. (2002). An educational-psychological theory of inter-
L. & Tauer, J. M. (2008). The role of achievement goals in est and its relation to SDT. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan
the development of interest: Reciprocal relations between (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 405–427).
achievement goals, interest, and performance. Journal of New York: University of Rochester Press.
Educational Psychology, 100(1), 105–122. Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest
Häussler, P., & Hoffmann, L. (2002). An intervention study and intrinsic motivational orientations. Learning and Instruc-
to enhance girls’ interest, self-concept, and achievement in tion, 12, 383–409.
physics classes. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 39(9), Krapp, A. (2007). An educational—psychological conceptual-
870–888. ization of interest. International Journal of Educational and
Hidi, S. (1990). Interest and its contribution as a mental Vocational Guidance, 7, 5–21.
resource for learning. Review of Educational Research, 60(4), Krapp, A. (2003). Interest and human development: An
549–571. educational-psychological perspective. In L. Smtih,
Hidi, S. (2006). Interest: A motivational variable with a differ- C. Rogers, & P. Tomlinson (Eds.), Development and moti-
ence. Educational Research Review, 7, 323–350. vation: Joint perspectives (pp. 57–84). British Journal of
Hidi, S., & Ainley, M. (2008). Interest and self-regulation: Rela- Educational Psychology Monograph Series, Development and
tionships between two variables that influence learning. In Motivation, 2 (series II).
D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Motivation and Krapp, A., Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (1992). Interest, learn-
self-regulated learning: Theory, research, and application (pp. ing, and development. In K. A. Renninger, S. Hidi, &
77–109). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and develop-
Hidi, S., & Baird, W. (1986). Interestingness—A neglected vari- ment (pp. 3–25). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
able in discourse processing. Cognitive Science, 10, 179–194. Krapp, A., & Lewalter, D. (2001). Development of interests
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The Four-Phase Model and interest-based motivational orientations: A longitudinal
of Interest Development. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), study in vocational school and work settings. In S. Volet &
111–127. S. Järvela (Eds.), Motivation in learning contexts: Theoreti-
Hoffmann, L. (2002). Promoting girls’ learning and achievement cal advances and methodological implications (pp. 209–232).
in physics classes for beginners. Learning and Instruction, 12, New York: Elsevier.
447–465. Kunter, M., Baumert, J., & Köller, O. (2007). Effective class-
Holland, J. L. (1985/1997). Making vocational choices: A theory room management and the development of subject-related
of vocational personalities and work environments. Englewood interest. Learning and Instruction, 17(5), 494–509.
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Lawless, K. A., & Kulikowich, J. M. (2006). Domain knowl-
Hulleman, C. S., Durik, A. M., Schweigert, S. A., & Harackie- edge and individual interest: The effects of academic level
wicz, J. M. (2008). Task values, achievement goals, and inter- and specialization in statistics and psychology. Contemporary
est: An integrative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, Educational Psychology, 31, 30–43.
100(2), 398–416. Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (1994). Toward
Hulleman, C., & Harackiewicz, J. (2009). Promoting inter- a unifying social cognitive theory of career and academic
est and performance in high school science classes. Science, interest, choice, and performance. Journal of Vocational
326(5698), 1410–1412. Behavior, 45, 79–122.
Jacobs, J. E., Davis-Kean, P., Bleeker, M. M., Eccles, J. S., & Lent, R. W., Brown, S. D., & Hackett, G. (2000). Contextual
Malanchuk, O. (2005). ‘I can, but I don’t want to’: The supports and barriers to career choice: A social cognitive
impact of parents, interests, and activities on gender differ- analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 47, 36–49.
ences in math. In A. M. Gallagher & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Durik, A. M., Conley, A. M., Barron,
Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological K. E., Tauer, J. M., Karabenick, S. A., & Harackiewicz,
approach (pp. 246–263). New York: Cambridge University J. M. (2010). Measuring situational interest in academic
Press. domains. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 70(4),
Jacobs, J. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Parents, task values, and 647–671.
real-life achievement-related choices. In C. Sansone & J.M. Lipstein, R., & Renninger, K. A. (2007). “Putting things into
Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The words”: 12–15-year-old students’ interest for writing. In
search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 405–439). P. Boscolo & S. Hidi (Eds.), Motivation and writing: Research
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. and school practice (pp. 113–140). New York: Kluwer Aca-
Jacobs, J., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, demic/Plenum Press.
A. (2002). Ontogeny of children’s self-beliefs: Gender and Marsh, H. W. (1987). The big-fish-little-pond effect on academic
domain differences across grades one through 12. Child self-concept. American Psychologist, 79, 280–295.
Development, 73, 509–527. Mayer, R. E. (2005). Cognitive theory of multimedia learning.
Katz, I., Assor, A., Kanat-Maymon, Y., & Bereby-Meyer, Y. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multime-
(2006). Interest as a motivational resource: Feedback and dia learning (pp. 31–48). New York: Cambridge University
gender matter, but interest makes the difference. Social Press.
Psychology of Education, 9, 27–42. Mayer, R. E., Griffith, E., Jurkowitz, I. T. N., & Rothman, D.
Keller, H. (with A. Sullivan & J. A. Macy). (1903). The story of (2008). Increased interestingness of extraneous details in a
my life. New York: Doubleday, Page & Co. multimedia science presentation leads to decreased learn-
Kim, S., Lee, M. J., & Bong, M. (2009, April). fMRI study on the ing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(4),
effects of task interest and perceived competence during negative 329–339.

ren n i n g er, s u 


Meyer, D. K., & Turner, J. C. (2002). Discovering emotion in mathematics workshop for teachers. R. Small (Ed.), Educa-
classroom motivation research. Educational Psychologist, 37, tional Technology, Research and Development [Special Issue:
107–114. Motivation and New Media], 59 (2), 229–247.
Mitchell, M. (1993). Situational interest: Its multifaceted struc- Renninger, K. A., Ewen, E., & Lasher, A. K. (2002). Individual
ture in the secondary school mathematics classroom. Journal interest as context in expository text and mathematical word
of Educational Psychology, 85, 424–436. problems. Learning and Instruction, 12, 467–491.
Nolen, S. B. (2007). Young children’s motivation to read and Renninger, K. A., & Hidi, S. (2011). Revisiting the conceptu-
write: Development in social contexts. Cognition and Instruc- alization, measurement, and generation of interest. Educa-
tion, 25, 219–270. tional Psychologist, 46(3), 168–184.
Palmer, D. H. (2004). Situational interest and the attitudes Renninger, K. A., & Lipstein, R. (2006). Come si sviluppa
towards science of primary teacher education students. Inter- l’interesse per la scrittura; Cosa volgliono gli studenti e di
national Journal of Science Education, 26(7), 895–908. cosa hannobisogno? [Developing interest for writing: What
Palmer, D. H. (2009). Student interest generated during an do students want and what do students need?] Età Evolutiva,
inquiry skills lesson. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 84, pp. 65–83.
46(2), 147–165. Renninger, K. A., Ewen, E., & Lasher, A. K. (2002). Individual
Pekrun, R. (2000). A social-cognitive, control-value theory of interest as context in expository text and mathematical word
achievement emotions. In J. Heckhausen (Ed.), Motivational problems. Learning and Instruction, 12, 467–491.
psychology of human development (pp 143–163). Oxford, Renninger, K. A., & Riley, K., (in press). Interest, cognition, and
England: Elsevier. the case of L− and science. In S. Kreitler (Ed.). Cognition and
Pugh, K. J. (2002). Teaching for idea-based, transformative motivation: Forging an interdisciplinary perspective. New York:
experiences in science: An investigation of the effectiveness Cambridge University Press.
of two instructional elements. Teachers College Record, 104, Renninger, K. A., Sansone, C., & Smith, J. L. (2004). Love of
1101–1137. learning. In C. Peterson & M.E.P. Seligman (Eds.), Char-
Pugh, K. J. (2004). Newton’s laws beyond the classroom walls. acter strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification (pp.
Science Education, 88, 182–196. 161–179). New York: Oxford University Press.
Pugh, K. J., Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Koskey, K. L., Stewart, Renninger, K. A., & Wozniak, R. (1985). Effect of interest on
V. C., & Manzey, C. (2010). Motivation, learning, and attentional shift, recognition, and recall in young children.
transformative experience: A study of deep engagement in Developmental Psychology, 21, 624–632.
science. Science Education, 94, 1–28. Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2005a). Does how we feel affect
Pressick-Kilborn, K., & Walker, R. (2002). The social construc- what we learn? Some answers and new questions. Learning
tion of interest in a learning community. In D. M. McIner- and Instruction, 15, 507–515.
ney & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Research on sociocultural influences Sansone, C., & Thoman, D.B. (2005b). Interest as the missing
on motivation and learning: An historical perspective (Vol. 2, motivator in self-regulation. European Psychologist, 10(3),
pp.153–182). Greenwich, CT: Information Age. 175–186.
Reeve, J., Jang, H., Hardre, P., & Omura, M. (2002). Providing Sansone, C., Weir, C., Harpster, L., & Morgan, C. (1992). Once
a rationale in an autonomy-supportive way as a strategy to a boring task always a boring task? Interest as a self-regulatory
motivate others during an uninteresting activity. Motivation mechanism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63,
and Emotion, 26, 183–207. 379–390.
Renninger, K. A. (1990). Children’s play interests, representation, Schiefele, U. (2009). Situational and individual interest. In
and activity. In R. Fivush & J. Hudson (Eds.), Emory cogni- K. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of Motivation at
tion series. Vol. III: Knowing and remembering in young children School (pp. 197–222). New York: Routledge.
(pp. 127–165). New York: Cambridge University Press. Schiefele, U., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1994). Interest and the
Renninger, K. A. (2000). Individual interest and its implications quality of experience in classrooms. European Journal of Psy-
for understanding intrinsic motivation. In C. Sansone & chology of Education, 9(3), 251–270.
J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation: Controversies Schiefele, U., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1995). Motivation
and new directions (pp. 373–404). San Diego, CA: Academic and ability factors in mathematics experience and achieve-
Press. ment. Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 26(2),
Renninger, K. A. (2009). Interest and identity development in 163–181.
instruction: An inductive model. Educational Psychologist, Schiefele, U., Krapp, A., Wild, K-P., & Winteler, A. (1993). Eine
44(2), 1–14. neue Version des Fragebogens zum Studieninteresse (FSI).
Renninger, K. A. (2010). Working with and cultivating interest, Untersuchungen zur Reliabilität und Validität [A new ver-
self-efficacy, and self-regulation. In D. Preiss & R. Sternberg sion of the Study Interest Questionnaire (SIQ)]. Diagnostica,
(Eds.), Innovations in educational psychology: Perspectives on 39, 335–351.
learning, teaching and human development (pp. 107–138). Schraw, G., Bruning, R., & Svoboda, C. (1995). Sources of situ-
New York: Springer. ational interest. Journal of Reading Behavior, 27, 1–17.
Renninger, K. A., Bachrach, J. E., & Posey, S. K. E. (2008). Silvia, P. J. (2006). Exploring the psychology of interest. New York:
Learner interest and achievement motivation. In M. L. Maehr, Oxford University Press.
S. Karabenick, & T. Urdan (Eds.), Social psychological per- Tsai, Y. M., Kunter, M., Lüdtke, O., Trautwein, U., &
spectives. Vol. 15: Advances in motivation and achievement. Ryan, R. M. (2008). What makes lessons interesting?
(pp.461–491) Bingley, England: Emerald Group. The role of situational and individual factors in three
Renninger, K. A., Cai, M., Lewis, M., Adams, M., & Ernst, K. school subjects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(2),
(2011). Motivation and learning in an online, unmoderated, 460–472.

 interest a nd its development


Turner, S. A., Jr., & Silvia, P. J. (2006). Must things be pleasant? A ment, and relations to achievement outcomes. Developmental
test of competing appraisal structures. Emotion, 6(4), 670–674. Review, 30, 1–35.
Watt, H. M. G. (2004). Development of adolescents’ self- Wigfield, A., Eccles, J., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R., & Davis-Kean,
perceptions, values, and task perceptions according to gender P. (2006). Development of achievement motivation. In In
and domain in 7th-through 11th-grade Australian students. R. Lerner & W. Damon (Series Eds.), N. Eisenberg (Vol.
Child Development, 75(5), 1556–1574. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional,
Wigfield, A., & Cambria, J. (2010). Students’ achievement and personality development (6th ed., pp. 933–1002). New
values, goal orientations, and interest: Definitions, develop- York: Wiley.

ren n i n g er, s u 


This page intentionally left blank
PA RT
4
Goals and Motivation
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER

Achievement Goals
12
Kou Murayama, Andrew J. Elliot, and Ron Friedman

Abstract
In this chapter, we describe the achievement goal construct’s origin and highlight noteworthy
developments in the literature. We then use this historical overview to provide the context for several
key theoretical and empirical issues surrounding the current achievement goal approach, including the
precise definition of achievement goals, the possible inclusion of additional goals into the achievement
goal approach, the measurement of achievement goals, the debates surrounding performance-approach
and performance-avoidance goals, contextual effects on achievement goals, and the consideration of
methodological expansion.
Key Words: achievement motivation, achievement goals, approach-avoidance

Introduction initial conceptualization to its present-day form. We


Central to the study of human motivation is the then use this historical overview to provide the con-
concept of goals, which can be defined as a form text for several key theoretical and empirical issues sur-
of self-regulation that guides people toward future- rounding the current achievement goal literature.
directed aims (Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Elliot
& Fryer, 2008). Goals focus people’s attention and Historical Overview
facilitate responses that are compatible with their The establishment of the achievement goal con-
objectives, thereby promoting the achievement of struct has occurred through the combined efforts of
desired outcomes or the avoidance of undesired out- several theorists working both independently and col-
comes. The content of people’s goals varies widely, laboratively over a number of decades. Unlike most
and researchers studying the antecedent and conse- theoretical approaches in psychology, the achievement
quences of goals have categorized goals by specific goal approach did not arise through the refinement of
features or common themes, in order to compare a single theoretical framework but emerged through
goals across different domains. One goal category the fusion of several distinct lines of thinking. In this
that has received considerable attention within the sense, the research tradition on achievement goals
field of psychology is that of achievement goals does not constitute “achievement goal theory;” rather,
(Duda, 2005; Elliot, 2005; Kaplan & Maehr, 2007; it is best construed in terms of “theories of achieve-
Meece, Anderman, & Anderman, 2006; Senko, ment goals” or “the achievement goal approach.”
Durik, & Harackiewicz, 2008).
The achievement goal construct was originally Dichotomous Model
developed in the late 1970s and has evolved consider- The achievement goal construct originally emerged
ably from its original form over 30 years ago. In this from the work of psychologists Carole Ames, Carol
chapter, we start by presenting a history of the achieve- Dweck, Marty Maehr, and John Nicholls, each of
ment goal construct, charting its development from whom led an independent research program at the


University of Illinois. In the fall of 1977, they began suggests that children experience helplessness when
meeting regularly in a series of seminars on motiva- they attribute failure to insufficient ability, but it
tion at the Institute for Child Behavior and Devel- does not explain what factors cause this maladap-
opment in the Children’s Research Center (Elliot, tive attribution pattern. The achievement motive
2005; Roberts, 2001). Shortly thereafter, a series of tradition, on the other hand, overemphasized dis-
papers emerged (Maehr & Nicholls, 1980; Nicholls positions without sufficiently addressing the role
& Dweck, 1979) articulating the foundational ideas of cognitions in predicting achievement behavior
of the achievement goal approach. (Dweck & Wortman, 1982). In her work, Dweck
It should be noted that research on achievement sought to address these limitations and proposed
motivation in the 1970s was heavily influenced that children’s responses to failure were related to
by the causal attribution tradition (Weiner, 1985; the goals they held for completing the task (Dweck,
Weiner, Heckhausen, & Meyer, 1972; Weiner & 1986, 1999; Dweck & Leggett, 1988).
Kukla, 1970) and the achievement motive tradition According to Dweck’s theorizing, children who
(Atkinson & Raynor, 1978; McClelland, Atkinson, adopt learning goals view achievement pursuits as
Clark, & Lowell, 1976). The combined efforts of opportunities to learn and increase their compe-
Ames, Dweck, Maehr, and Nicholls can be viewed, tence. Children with learning goals also view fail-
in part, as an attempt to overcome the weaknesses ure as important feedback on their progress, rather
and limitations of the causal attribution and achieve- than an indictment of their ability. Consequently,
ment motive traditions. The achievement goal con- the experience of failure leads children with learning
struct was, therefore, not created ex nihilo. goals to redouble their efforts, which is consistent
A common feature of early work on the achieve- with a mastery response pattern. In contrast, chil-
ment goal construct is the usage of a dichotomous dren who hold performance goals view achievement
model, which distinguishes between two types of pursuits not as opportunities to learn but as oppor-
achievement goals. These distinctions typically cen- tunities to demonstrate their competence (Elliott &
ter on the different foci an individual might bring Dweck, 1988; Smiley & Dweck, 1994). To these
to a given achievement activity (Dweck & Elliott, children, failure is a signal that they do not pos-
1983; Nicholls, 1984). Two dichotomous models sess the ability to succeed. After experiencing fail-
that were highly influential in the development of ure, children with performance goals reduce their
the achievement goal construct are Dweck’s frame- efforts, which is consistent with a helpless response
work, grounded in the learning-performance distinc- pattern.
tion, and Nicholls’s model, grounded in the task-ego Dweck further posited that the adoption of
distinction. The following section reviews these different achievement goals is influenced by one’s
models and outlines the influence of each approach implicit theories of ability. Implicit theories rep-
on later work. resent a person’s beliefs about the relative stability
or malleability of objective forms of competence.
dweck’s conceptualization A belief that ability is a stable entity and not ame-
Dweck’s achievement goal conceptualization nable to change (called entity theory) was posited to
emerged from her research with late grade-school- lead to performance goal adoption, while a belief
age children (Diener & Dweck, 1978, 1980; that ability is malleable and highly amenable to
Dweck, 1975; Dweck & Reppucci, 1973). Dweck change (called incremental theory) was posited to
found that after experiencing failure on a task, some lead to learning goal adoption (Bempechat, Lon-
children exhibited “helpless” responses (character- don, & Dweck, 1991). Taken together, Dweck’s
ized by decrements in persistence and performance, work characterizes achievement goals as proximal
the onset of negative affect and expectancies, the predictors of failure responses that are influenced by
attribution of failure to insufficient ability, and the a person’s implicit theories of competence.
avoidance of subsequent challenge), while others
exhibited “mastery” responses (characterized by sus- nicholl’s conceptualization
tained or enhanced persistence and performance, Nicholls’s work on achievement goals emerged
continued positive affect and expectancies, the attri- from his research on the way children conceptualize
bution of failure to insufficient effort, and the pur- ability. Nicholls argued that both the achievement
suit of subsequent challenge). Dweck was interested motive and causal attribution traditions had failed
in identifying the underlying root of these distinct to recognize that ability may be construed in differ-
response patterns. The causal attribution tradition ent ways (Nicholls, 1983). According to Nicholls

 achievem ent goals


(Jagacinski & Nicholls, 1984, 1987; Nicholls, 1976, This convergence was not limited to the work
1978, 1980), children hold an undifferentiated view of Dweck and Nicholls. Similar dichotomies were
of ability through most of their early development; proposed by other achievement goal theorists, such
that is, they do not distinguish between ability and as Maehr (Maehr, 1983), Ames (Ames, 1984), and
effort. Success is essentially equated with effort, and Covington (Covington & Omelich, 1984). By the
those who expend more effort are generally regarded late 1980s, a wealth of empirical work had emerged
as having greater ability. By roughly the age of 12, supporting the idea that different achievement goals
however, children acquire a more differentiated view predict distinct achievement-related outcomes, yet
of ability; that is, they gain the capacity to distinguish researchers were using different (albeit overlapping)
between ability and effort. Within this differentiated terminologies to describe these effects.
view, high ability is only inferred when one outper- Noting the conceptual similarities, Ames and
forms others while expending equal effort, or when Archer (1987, 1988) argued that the achievement
one performs the same as others while expending goal literature could be unified into a single frame-
less effort. work that distinguishes between two types of goals:
Nicholls argued that adolescents and adults can mastery and performance. This was a milestone
construe achievement situations in either an undif- event—the introduction of an integrative frame-
ferentiated or a differentiated fashion, and these work that brought cohesion to the field. Conse-
distinct views of ability form the basis for the two quently, researchers have largely adopted Ames and
major achievement goals (Nicholls, 1984, 1989). Archer’s (1987, 1988) terminological recommenda-
People who pursue competence in an undifferenti- tion of mastery and performance goals, and research
ated sense—meaning that they simultaneously focus on achievement goals proliferated widely thereafter.
on effort and learning—are said to be in a state of Early empirical work revealed a relatively clear
task involvement. People who pursue competence in and consistent picture of the consequences of mas-
a differentiated sense—meaning that they focus on tery goal adoption. Mastery goals were shown to
outperforming others with limited effort—are said to lead to positive processes and outcomes, such as task
be in a state of ego involvement. Importantly, task and value and self-efficacy (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990;
ego involvement were posited by Nicholls to interact Pintrich & Garcia, 1991), deep-processing learning
with perceived ability in predicting processes and out- strategies (Kaplan & Midgley, 1997; Nolen, 1988;
comes. For example, ego involvement was said to lead Nolen & Haladyna, 1990a), self-regulated learning
to the selection of moderately challenging tasks when strategies (Bouffard, Boisvert, Vezeau, & Larouche,
accompanied by high perceived ability (an adaptive 1995; Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Miller, Greene,
response), but it was said to lead to the selection of Montalvo, Ravindran, & Nicholls, 1996; Wolters,
very easy or very difficult tasks when accompanied by 1998), persistence (Elliott & Dweck, 1988), and
low perceived ability (a maladaptive response). adaptive help seeking (Butler & Neuman, 1995;
Newman, 1998; Ryan & Pintrich, 1997). In sum,
ames’s integration mastery goals were shown to lead to a host of adap-
While there are clear differences in the achieve- tive outcomes.
ment goal conceptualizations proffered by Dweck The consequences of performance goal adoption
and Nicholls, the similarities are far more striking. were less clear. A number of theorists argued that
First, within each theory, one goal (learning/task) performance goal adoption is associated with nega-
is characterized in terms of developing ability and tive, maladaptive outcomes, due to its inherent focus
seeking task mastery, while the other (performance/ on outperforming others (e.g., Ames, 1992; Urdan,
ego) is characterized in terms of demonstrating abil- 1997). Empirical findings, however, did not provide
ity (often through normative competence). Second, clear support for this assumption. For example, per-
both distinctions identify goals that ultimately yield formance goals were shown to have a null or posi-
fairly comparable outcomes. And third, in both con- tive influence on adaptive outcomes in certain types
ceptualizations, individuals’ perceptions of their own of achievement contexts (Koestner, Zuckerman,
ability are predicted to moderate the effect of achieve- & Koestner, 1987; Miller & Hom, 1990; Sansone,
ment goal adoption. That is, performance/ego goals Sachau, & Weir, 1989). In addition, among those
were posited to exert the most negative impact when with certain types of personality dispositions, per-
accompanied by low perceptions of competence, formance goals were associated with positive out-
whereas learning/ego goals were posited to exert the comes (Harackiewicz & Elliot, 1993; Harackiewicz
same positive impact across competence perceptions. & Sansone, 1991).

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


Approach-Avoidance Distinction: pursuing performance-approach goals, individuals
Trichotomous and 2 x 2 Models aim to attain positive outcomes relative to others (e.g.,
To help resolve ambiguities surrounding the “My goal is to perform better than others”), whereas
consequences of performance goals, and further individuals pursuing performance-avoidance goals
refine the mastery-performance distinction, Elliot aim to avoid negative outcomes compared to others
and colleagues (Elliot & Church, 1997; Elliot & (e.g., “My goal is to avoid doing poorly comparing to
Harackiewicz, 1996) introduced the approach- others”). The mastery goal construct was virtually the
avoidance distinction to the achievement goal lit- same in the dichotomous and trichotomous models.
erature. The approach-avoidance distinction centers Importantly, distinguishing between performance-
on whether an individual is focused on approaching approach goals and performance-avoidance goals
a positive possibility (e.g., success) or on avoiding helped elucidate when performance-based goals were
a negative possibility (e.g., failure). Although the most likely to have adaptive or maladaptive conse-
approach-avoidance distinction had been largely quences. Reanalyses of extant data (Elliot & Moller,
ignored within the achievement goal literature, it had 2003; Harackiewicz, Barron, Pintrich, Elliot, &
been readily acknowledged by a long line of research- Thrash, 2002; Rawsthorne & Elliot, 1999), as well as
ers and theorists early in the study of achievement newly emerging data, revealed similar findings (Elliot
behavior. For example, Lewin and colleagues’ (Lewin, & Church, 1997; Elliot, McGregor, & Gable, 1999;
Dembo, Festinger, & Sears, 1944) theory of resul- Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Skaalvik, 1997; Vande-
tant valence (the first formal model of achievement walle, 1997). Performance-avoidance goals were clearly
motivation) incorporated the desire for success and the linked to maladaptive learning behaviors and outcomes
desire to avoid failure as the two primary independent (e.g., disorganized study strategies, lower grades, lower
motivational orientations. Similarly, McClelland pos- intrinsic motivation), while performance-approach
ited that the approach-avoidance distinction consti- goals were linked to several positive behaviors and
tutes two different types of achievement motivation outcomes (e.g., effort, persistence, higher grades). This
(McClelland, 1951). Atkinson introduced his classic pattern of findings was observed in both experimental
need achievement theory, a mathematical framework and correlational studies.
that designated the desire to approach success and the
desire to avoid failure as the primary determinants of 2 x 2 model of achievement goals
achievement behavior (Atkinson, 1957). Elliot and colleagues (Elliot, 1999; Elliot &
The approach-avoidance distinction has also been McGregor, 2001; see also Pintrich, 2000a) then
incorporated well beyond the achievement motiva- extended the approach-avoidance distinction to mas-
tion literature, ranging from traditional behavioral tery goals, resulting in a 2 x 2 achievement goal model
theories (e.g., Hull, 1943; Skinner, 1953), to devel- comprised of mastery-approach, mastery-avoidance,
opmental theories (e.g., Bowlby, 1969), to personality performance-approach, and performance-avoidance
theories (e.g., Eysenck, 1967; Gray, 1987), to cogni- goals (Fig. 12.1). A key feature of the 2 x 2 model
tive theories (e.g., Kahneman & Tversky, 1979), to was the addition of mastery-avoidance goals, which
neuroscience theories (e.g., Davidson, 2000; Rolls, focused on not doing worse than before or not failing
2005). Given the prevalence of the approach- to master a task (e.g., “My goal is not to do worse
avoidance distinction throughout psychology, it is than before” or “My goal is to avoid not mastering
not surprising that its application to the achievement a task”). Extant empirical work on mastery goals at
goal literature has proven highly generative. that time focused exclusively on positive possibili-
ties (i.e., approaching success), which were termed
trichotomous model of mastery-approach goals (e.g., “My goal is to do better
achievement goals than before” or “My goal is to master a task” within
Initially, the approach-avoidance distinction was the 2 x 2 framework).
applied to performance goals to help explain incon- Although mastery-avoidance goals were pre-
sistent findings related to the consequences of perfor- sumed to be less prevalent than other achievement
mance goal adoption. Elliot and Harackiewicz (1996) goals (mastery approach, performance approach,
separated the conventional performance goal con- and performance avoidance), they were also thought
struct into conceptually independent approach and to be common in certain instances. For example,
avoidance goals, introducing three distinct achieve- mastery-avoidance goals were thought to become
ment goals comprised of mastery, performance- more salient as individuals enter later adulthood.
approach, and performance-avoidance goals. When As a person’s physical and mental skills begin to

 achievem ent goals


decline, the person may shift his or her focus to “not operative word in this definition, “purpose,” can take
losing important abilities.” Similarly, athletes may on several different meanings. According to the Ran-
also be prime candidates for mastery-avoidance goal dom House Dictionary of the English Language (1993),
adoption. When athletes reach their peak perfor- the word purpose can be defined in two primary
mance, they may begin to focus on not perform- ways: as “the reason for which something exists or is
ing worse than they have performed to date. Factor done, made, used, etc.” and “an intended or desired
analytic studies confirmed the validity of the 2 x 2 result; end; aim; goal.” Researchers who adopt this
structure of achievement goals (Elliot & McGregor, approach have implicitly employed a combination
2001; Elliot & Murayama, 2008; Finney, Pieper, of both of these meanings simultaneously. That is,
& Barron, 2004), and a cross-cultural replication the term achievement goal has been used as both the
was also largely supportive (Murayama, Zhou, & reason for behavior in an achievement situation (e.g.,
Nesbit, 2009). Mastery-avoidance goals have been the development or demonstration of ability) and as
linked to fewer adaptive processes and outcomes the aim or outcome that is sought in an achievement
than mastery-approach goals but also fewer mal- situation (e.g., normative ability or self-referential
adaptive processes and outcomes than performance- ability). Accordingly, this approach can be problem-
avoidance goals (Bong, 2009; Conroy, Elliot, & atic because it defines the achievement goal construct
Hofer, 2003; Cury, Elliot, Fonseca, & Moller, 2006; in two different ways. For example, performance-
Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Elliot & Murayama, 2008; approach goals can be conceptualized in terms of
Elliot & Reis, 2003; Sideridis, 2008). Research on both the reason of impressing others and the aim of
mastery-avoidance goals, however, remains at a very doing better than other people.
early stage, and a full understanding of their ante- The second approach, which emerged through
cedents and consequences is still developing. the conceptual integration of Ames and Archer
(1987, 1988), characterizes achievement goals as
Current Theoretical and Empirical Issues a network or integrated pattern of beliefs and feel-
Over the past few decades, the achievement goal ings about success, effort, ability, errors, feedback, and
construct has emerged as a central variable in the standards of evaluation that together provide a wide-
study of motivation. However, there remain a num- ranging framework or schema toward achievement
ber of outstanding theoretical and empirical issues tasks (Ames, 1992; Kaplan & Maehr, 2007; Pintrich,
yet to be explored. In this section, using the his- 2000a). This comprehensive account—often labeled
torical context that we have discussed, we overview achievement goal orientation—is initially appealing
these issues, providing a broader perspective for the in that it unifies many achievement-relevant vari-
evolution of the achievement goal construct. ables into a single organizational system. However,
upon careful consideration, this strength can also
Definition of Achievement Goal Constructs be viewed as a limitation. Because this definition
Motivational theorists often assume that a con- includes a collection of variables, it is difficult to
sensual definition of “achievement goal” has been identify exactly which aspect of the achievement
established. However, a careful reading of the litera- goal construct is responsible for any hypothesized
ture reveals a somewhat inconsistent picture (Elliot & or observed effects. The inability to differentiate
Thrash, 2001; Kaplan & Maehr, 2007). Those offer- antecedents and consequences from achievement
ing definitions of the achievement goal construct have goals per se may present significant impediments to
typically adopted one of three basic approaches. researchers utilizing this approach.

achievement goals as an omnibus achievement goals as specific aims


construct A third approach, which was developed to
The first approach, introduced by early research- address the limitations of previous definitions, is
ers, describes achievement goals in terms of the pur- simply to describe achievement goals as an aim
pose for which a person engages in achievement behavior with competence at its conceptual core (Elliot &
(Dweck, 1986; Maehr, 1989; Nicholls, 1989). This Murayama, 2008; Elliot & Thrash, 2001). Within
approach is appealing because it intuitively captures this account, competence is integral with regard to
what achievement motivation theorists, lay and both the definition and valence of a goal. That is,
trained alike, want to know: “For what purpose is definition and valence are construed as necessary
the person engaging in behavior in this achievement features of achievement goals because it is not pos-
situation?” However, it should be noted that the sible to fully specify an achievement goal construct

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


without identifying how competence is defined Definition
and valenced. The performance-mastery distinction Absolute/
Intrapersonal Normative
maps onto the definition of competence, whereas (performance)
(mastery)
approach-avoidance distinction maps on the valence
of competence. Positive Mastery Performance-
Competence is defined by the standard or referent (approaching approach approach
success) goal goal
that is used in evaluating it. Three different standards
may be used: an absolute standard (the requirements Valence
of the task itself ), an intrapersonal standard (one’s Negative Mastery- Performance-
(avoiding
own past attainment or maximum potential attain- failure)
avoidance avoidance
goal goal
ment), and a normative standard (the performance
of others). That is, competence may be evaluated
and therefore defined, in absolute terms according to Fig. 12.1. The 2 x 2 achievement goal model. Definition and
one’s mastery of a task, in intrapersonal terms accord- valence represent the two dimensions of competence. Absolute/
ing to one’s personal trajectory, and in interpersonal intrapersonal and normative represent the two ways that
competence may be defined; positive and negative represent
terms according to one’s attainment relative to oth- the two ways that competence may be valenced.
ers. Absolute and intrapersonal competence share
many conceptual and empirical similarities and, at
present, may be considered jointly rather than sepa- There is, however, room for debate as to how achieve-
rately. As such, competence may be defined in abso- ment goals should be conceptualized, and continued
lute/intrapersonal terms or in interpersonal terms, discussion on this issue should result in theoretical
and two types of achievement goals may be delin- and empirical progress in the field.
eated according to the type of competence that an
individual commits to in an achievement situation. Other Achievement Goals
Mastery goals are posited to map onto an absolute/ A number of theorists have introduced addi-
intrapersonal standard, while performance goals are tional goal constructs to the established mastery-
posited to map onto an intrapersonal standard. performance and approach-avoidance dichotomies
Competence is also valenced in that it can be con- (for early examples, see Maehr & Braskamp, 1986;
strued in positive terms (i.e., competence or success) Maehr & Nicholls, 1980). Several candidates for
or in negative terms (i.e., incompetence or failure). inclusion began to receive consideration and scru-
Two types of achievement goals may be delineated tiny in the early to mid 1990s, most notably, work-
according whether the competence-relevant focus avoidance goals, extrinsic goals, and social goals (see
is on approaching the positive possibility of com- Urdan, 1997, for a review).
petence, or on avoiding the negative possibility of Work-avoidance goals (also labeled “academic
incompetence. Approach goals represent positive alienation”) were defined in terms of trying to get
concerns for competence, while avoidance goals away with putting as little work or effort as pos-
represent negative concerns for incompetence. sible into achievement tasks (Meece, Blumenfeld, &
These two aspects of competence are combined to Hoyle, 1988; Nicholls, Patashnick, & Nolen, 1985;
form the four different types of goals represented Nolen, 1988). Work-avoidance goals were thought
in the aforementioned 2 x 2 framework, which to differ from traditional avoidance goals in that it
consists of mastery-approach, mastery-avoidance, is the avoidance of work—and not failure—that is
performance-approach, and performance-avoidance the main focus. Research findings have consistently
goals (Fig. 12.1). shown a negative pattern of cognition, affect, and
In sum, we have described three approaches to behavior for individuals pursuing work-avoidance
defining and conceptualizing achievement goal con- goals (Archer, 1994; Meece et al., 1988; Nicholls
structs. Our preferred perspective is that achievement et al., 1985; Nolen, 1988; Skaalvik, 1997).
goals are best defined by the third approach, which Extrinsic goals were defined in terms of striving
provides conceptual clarity—a critical requirement to succeed in order to earn a reward or avoid a pun-
for scientific investigation. This approach also distin- ishment (Maehr, 1983; Midgley & Urdan, 1995;
guishes achievement goals from the many different Pintrich & Garcia, 1991). Extrinsic goals were ini-
dispositions, tendencies, processes, and outcomes tially considered a form of performance goal (e.g.,
to which they are associated, allowing researchers Pintrich, Smith, Garcia, & Mckeachie, 1993); how-
to study the relationship between these constructs. ever, recent studies have shown that while extrinsic

 achievem ent goals


goals may overlap with performance goals, the two guideline for consideration of additional achieve-
constructs are not identical (Malka & Covington, ment goals. As noted earlier, within the definition of
2005; Midgley et al., 1998). Several studies also mastery goal in the competence-based framework,
found that in general, this orientation is associated competence is defined in terms of success relative
with a maladaptive attitude toward achievement that to either an absolute (task-based) or intrapersonal
includes placing a lower value on the task, reporting standard. Accordingly, a straightforward theoreti-
higher achievement anxiety, admitting to relatively cal extension would entail separating these abso-
more cheating, and using self-handicapping strategies lute and intrapersonal standards, resulting in a 3 x
(Anderman, Griesinger, & Westerfield, 1998; Ryan 2 framework comprising six types of achievement
& Pintrich, 1997; Wolters, Yu, & Pintrich, 1996). goal: task-approach goals (“do a task well”), task-
Social goals were defined as strivings that focus on avoidance goals (“avoid doing poorly on a task”),
interpersonal relationships (Maehr & Nicholls, 1980; self-approach goals (“do better than before”), self-
Wentzel, 1989), and a number of different variants avoidance goals (“avoid doing worse than before”),
were introduced, including social approval goals, other-approach goals (“do better than others”), and
social responsibility goals, social status goals, proso- other-avoidance goals (“avoid doing worse than oth-
cial goals, and affiliation goals (Urdan & Maehr, ers”). Empirical investigation of a 3 x 2 framework
1995). Among these iterations, social responsibility has just started (see Elliot, Murayama, & Pekrun,
goals were posited to have the most positive impact 2011) and is an intriguing topic that merits future
on student learning (Wentzel, 1989, 1991, 1998). inquiry.
While each of these classifications offers a unique
and potentially valuable way of classifying people’s Measurement of Achievement Goals
strivings, the achievement goal literature has yet to Another current issue in the achievement goal
develop an explicit criterion for determining inclu- literature that warrants consideration is the usage
sion. Our perspective is that establishing compe- of different measurement tools. Over the past two
tence as the conceptual core of the achievement decades, a host of achievement goal measures have
goal construct (the third definitional approach that appeared in the educational psychology, industrial/
we discussed earlier) provides clear guidelines for organizational psychology, social-personality psycho-
evaluating additional achievement goal candidates. logy, and sport and exercise psychology literatures.
According to this approach, achievement goals are Some have focused on the mastery-performance dis-
considered cognitive aims that one adopts when tinction alone, while others have focused on both the
striving for competence, and distinct achievement mastery-performance and approach-avoidance dis-
goals are characterized in terms of the definition tinctions (for reviews, see Elliot & Murayama, 2008;
and valence of competence (Elliot & Thrash, 2001). Fulmer & Frijters, 2009; Kaplan & Maehr, 2007).
Using these guidelines, we can better assess the value The use of different measurement tools is important
of incorporating these additional classifications. because not all achievement goal measures are cre-
Work-avoidance goals differ from striving for ated equal; different measures tend to reflect distinct
competence, because work-avoidant individuals are theoretical backgrounds. We therefore encourage
trying to get away from commitment to a task and researchers to consider measurement tools carefully
are therefore not focused on achieving competence when comparing results across studies.
or avoiding incompetence. Similarly, extrinsic goals As noted earlier, some research construes the
do not focus on a competence-based striving per se, achievement goal construct as an omnibus combina-
but on external factors such as monetary rewards. tion of variables reflecting an integrated pattern of
Some extrinsic goals may qualify as achievement beliefs and feelings. Accordingly, those who subscribe
goals; however, not all extrinsic goals are achievement to this view utilize assessments that combine sev-
goals. Social goals involve a focus on interpersonal eral distinct components of achievement goals into
considerations, rather than a focus on competence a single measure. For example, performance-approach
considerations (Wentzel, 1991). Therefore, social goal measures commonly include items that combine
goals, as traditionally defined, are not achievement a focus on demonstrating competence with a focus on
goals (although see Ryan and Shim, 2006, 2008; outperforming a normative standard (e.g., “I’d like to
for goals that focus on social competence and are, show my teacher that I’m smarter than the other stu-
therefore, a form of achievement goal). dents”; Button, Mathieu, & Zajac, 1996; Elliot &
The competence-based conceptualization of Church, 1997; Greene & Miller, 1996; Harackiewicz,
achievement goals also suggests a systematic Barron, Elliot, Carter, & Lehto, 1997; Meece et al.,

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


1988; Midgley et al., 2000; Roberts & Treasure, care to craft manipulations that correspond to ele-
1995; Skaalvik, 1997; Stipek & Gralinski, 1996). ments of achievement goals of focal theoretical inter-
Alternatively, some performance-based goal scales est. As is the case with achievement goal measures,
focus only on demonstrating competence, excluding achievement goal manipulations are not uniform
a normative standard (Middleton & Midgley, 1997; and researchers have used many different procedures
Vandewalle, 1997; Zweig & Webster, 2004). More- in the past (Rawsthorne & Elliot, 1999; Utman,
over, some measures include items that do not really 1997). Here again, we recommend first settling on
focus on goals at all but attend to other aspects of a theoretical position regarding the achievement
achievement, including intrapersonal definitions of goal construct, and then choosing (or designing) a
success (e.g. “I feel most successful when . . .”, Button manipulation that corresponds to that position.
et al., 1996; Duda, Chi, Newton, Walling, & Catley,
1995; Duda & Nicholls, 1992; Roberts & Treasure, Debates on Performance-Approach and
1995; Skaalvik, 1997), the value of certain out- Performance-Avoidance Goals
comes or experiences (e.g., “It is important for me As noted earlier, motivation researchers have
to . . . ”, Bouffard et al., 1995; Conroy et al., 2003; long debated whether performance-based goal adop-
Elliot & Church, 1997; Elliot & McGregor, 2001; tion ultimately facilitates or impedes achievement
Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Stipek & Gralinski, (Brophy, 2005; Midgley, Kaplan, & Middleton,
1996; Vandewalle, 1997; Zweig & Webster, 2004), 2001; Urdan & Mestas, 2006). In 2001, Midgley,
affective components (e.g., “I feel really pleased Kaplan, and Middleton attempted to resolve this
when . . .”, Bouffard et al., 1995; Button et al., dispute by suggesting that performance-approach
1996; Elliot & Church, 1997; Harackiewicz et al., goals are adaptive, but only when mastery-approach
1997; Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Skaalvik, 1997; goal adoption is also present (see also Kaplan &
Stipek & Gralinski, 1996; Vandewalle, 1997), and the Middleton, 2002). Later research has not supported
consequences of failure (e.g., “My fear of performing this hypothesis, in that performance-approach goals
poorly in this class is often what motivates me,” Elliot alone are related to several types of positive outcomes
& Church, 1997; Elliot & McGregor, 2001; Meece (Elliot & Moller, 2003; Harackiewicz et al., 2002);
et al., 1988; Skaalvik, 1997; Vandewalle, 1997). however, Midgley et al.’s suggestion raises the interest-
Elliot and Murayama (2008) recently devel- ing possibility that certain interactions of achievement
oped a new 2 x 2 measure labeled the Achievement goals (e.g., high mastery-approach goals and high
Goal Questionnaire Revised (AGQ-R), which elim- performance-approach goals) may produce a pattern
inates goal-irrelevant features, such as motives and of results that differs from a simple combination of
affect, common to previous measures. This measure main effects. This perspective is known as the multiple
specifically focuses on tapping cognitive aims and goals model (Pintrich, 2000b; for other definitions of
therefore makes it possible to assess achievement multiple goals model, see Barron & Harackiewicz,
goals based on the competence-based framework 2001; Senko, Hulleman, & Harackiewicz, 2011) and
discussed earlier. has been investigated using interaction effect analyses
Due in part to the wide variety of achievement (e.g., Ames & Archer, 1988; Harackiewicz et al., 1997;
goal measures, researchers in the field have yet to Pintrich, 2000b; Wolters, 2004) and profile analyses
converge on a single scale that best captures the (e.g., Daniels et al., 2008; Levy-Tossman, Kaplan, &
achievement goal construct. From our perspective, Assor, 2007; Meece & Holt, 1993). Unfortunately,
the choice of achievement goal measure depends on empirical investigations of multiple goal models have
one’s theoretical background. Ultimately, research- failed to yield consistent results to date. These incon-
ers must be attentive to the correspondence between sistencies, however, may be due to statistical difficul-
how goals are conceptualized and how they are ties in detecting interaction effects in field research
operationalized within a given assessment device. (McClelland & Judd, 1993). We therefore believe that
For example, when interested in assessing the influ- further research is needed to investigate the interactive
ence of achievement goals on emotional states, we influence of multiple achievement goals. Of particu-
would recommend choosing an achievement goal lar interest is the combination of approach-based and
measure that does not contain affective components avoidance-based goals. This issue has failed to receive
in the items, so that the obtained relationship can- attention from researchers, despite considerable inter-
not be attributed to the overlap between items. est from a number of pioneering achievement motiva-
Similarly, when manipulating achievement goals tion theorists (see Atkinson, 1957; Lewin et al., 1944;
in an experimental setting, researchers must take Miller & Dollard, 1941).

 achievem ent goals


Another intriguing possibility is that the reasons there is value in attending to this construct. Second,
or motives underlying goal pursuits may strongly there is considerable evidence showing that social
influence the consequences of specific goal adop- comparison (normative competence) is automati-
tion. According to this view, individuals may adopt cally, nonconsciously processed (Mussweiler, 2003;
the same goal for fundamentally different reasons, Mussweiler & Epstude, 2009; Stapel & Koomen,
and their experiences and outcomes may differ as a 2001). The fact that performance-avoidance goals
function of their underlying motivation. For exam- are not frequently mentioned in open-ended mea-
ple, individuals may strive to outperform others sures does not, therefore, necessarily indicate that
because this goal represents an enjoyable challenge, these goals are not guiding people’s behaviors in
provides meaningful competence information, or achievement situations.
provides an opportunity to exercise their skills and A final criticism of the performance-approach/
capabilities. On the other hand, individuals may performance avoidance-distinction highlights the
pursue performance-approach goals because they high correlation between the two goals, suggesting
feel compelled to demonstrate their abilities, per- that these goals are functionally indistinguishable
haps because they see doing so as a means of obtain- (Duda, 2005; Roeser, 2004; Roeser et al., 2006;
ing positive regard from others. When pursued for Urdan & Mestas, 2006). In fact, some have indicated
this reason, performance-approach goals are likely to that students cannot readily distinguish between
be experienced as stressful and anxiety inducing, and performance-approach and performance-avoidance
they may lead to less adaptive outcomes. In short, goals (Urdan & Mestas, 2006). However, a num-
some reasons for pursuing goals may lead to highly ber of studies using factor analytic techniques have
positive experiences and adaptive outcomes, whereas supported differentiating between performance-
others may lead to less positive experiences and less approach and performance-avoidance goals in both
adaptive outcomes. This combinatorial construct, the trichotomous (Elliot & Church, 1997; Midgley
which represents both the goal and the underlying et al., 1998; Vandewalle, 1997) and 2 x 2 models
reason that it is pursued, is known as a goal complex (Baranik, Barron, & Finney, 2007; Campbell, Barry,
(Elliot, 2006; Elliot & Thrash, 2001). Goal complex Joe, & Finney, 2008; Elliot & McGregor, 2001;
research is now in the very early stages (see Domp- Murayama et al., 2009), indicating that repondents
nier, Darnon, & Butera, 2009), and it would greatly do indeed distinguish between these two constructs
benefit from additional work that directly and sys- (see Murayama, Elliot, & Yamagata, 2011, for a
tematically tests the consequences of distinct goal- broader analysis on this issue). The high correlation
based combinations. between them may reflect the fact that the two goals
Another issue that has been debated to some degree share a normative standard for evaluating compe-
within the achievement goal literature concerns tence. Furthermore, there may be additional fac-
whether performance-avoidance goals warrant their tors at work, and we believe that future research is
own category. A number of psychologists have chal- needed to explore moderators of this relationship.
lenged the performance-approach and performance-
avoidance distinction based on the finding that Contextual Effects on Achievement Goals
respondents rarely mention performance-avoidance Within the achievement goal literature, the term
goals in open-ended goal measures (Brophy, 2005; achievement goal orientation is often used to refer to
Lemos, 1996; Roeser, 2004; Roeser, Peck, Nasir, a broad network of beliefs and feelings, as well as a
Alexander, & Winne, 2006; Urdan & Mestas, 2006; dispositional tendency to adopt a certain goal. Indeed
but see Senko, Hulleman, & Harackiewicz, 2011). many researchers in this area utilize the achievement
In our view, this criticism is unconvincing for goal construct in a dispositional manner in empiri-
several reasons. First, when it comes to assessing the cal work. This strong dispositional focus is surprising,
value of psychological constructs, what matters most because the achievement goal approach originated, in
is not the mean level of occurrence but the construct’s part, as a critique of dispositional constructs (espe-
relations with other psychological outcomes. Many cially the need for achievement), reflecting a desire
studies have clearly demonstrated the predictive to move toward a more specific, context-based level
utility of performance-avoidance goals (e.g., Bong, of analysis (see Dweck & Wortman, 1982; Maehr
2009; Elliot & Church, 1997; Elliot & Murayama, & Nicholls, 1980). When construed as a disposi-
2008; Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Pekrun, Elliot, tion, it is difficult to see how the achievement goal
& Maier, 2009), and to the extent that performance- construct differs from the self-attributed achievement
avoidance goal adoption predicts unique outcomes, motive construct that has been articulated within

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


the classic achievement motive tradition (see, for classroom goal structure), teacher self-report, or
example, Spence and Helmreich’s [1983] distinction classroom observation.
between work-mastery and competitiveness in the Research on classroom goal structures has revealed
self-attributed need for achievement). Furthermore, a relationship between achievement goals, classroom
if achievement goal orientations are portrayed as goal structures, and achievement-relevant outcomes
general tendencies to adopt particular achievement (for reviews, see Linnenbrink, 2004; Meece et al.,
goals in specific situations, and achievement goals in 2006; Murayama & Elliot, 2009; Urdan & Turner,
specific situations are viewed as direct regulators of 2005). For example, a number of studies have doc-
achievement behavior, then it seems that achievement umented that classroom goal structures indirectly
goal orientations merely serve a descriptive rather than influence achievement-relevant outcomes through
an explanatory function. Thus, although the achieve- their impact on personal achievement goal adop-
ment goal construct has been utilized at both the dis- tion (Bong, 2005; Church, Elliot, & Gable, 2001;
positional and situation-specific levels, we believe it is Greene, Miller, Crowson, Duke, & Akey, 2004;
best suited to the situation-specific level. Kaplan & Maehr, 1999; Midgley, Anderman, &
A good deal of research has been done to inves- Hicks, 1995; Nolen & Haladyna, 1990b; Roeser,
tigate the joint influence of contextual factors and Midgley, & Urdan, 1996; Urdan, 2004). Within
achievement goals (Elliot, 2006; Elliot & Church, these studies, classroom goal structures prompt
1997). Classroom achievement goal structure, for the adoption of corresponding achievement goals
example, which refers to competence-relevant envi- (e.g., mastery-approach goal structure facilitates
ronmental emphases made through teachers’ commu- the adoption of mastery-approach goals). Another
nications and general classroom practices, has received set of studies showed a direct effect of classroom
a good deal of theoretical and empirical attention goal structure on achievement goals; that is, they
(Ames, 1992; Covington & Omelich, 1984; Epstein, demonstrated that classroom goal structures have
1988; Maehr & Midgley, 1996). The concept of an effect on outcomes over and above the effect
classroom goal structure originated from early work of (personal) achievement goals (Kaplan, Gheen,
by Ames, who was particularly interested in the con- & Midgley, 2002; Karabenick, 2004; Lau & Nie,
textual determinants of achievement goals (Ames, 2008; Midgley & Urdan, 1995, 2001; Wolters,
1981; Ames & Ames, 1981, 1984). Research on 2004; for a statistical formulization of contextual
classroom goal structures initially centered on two effects, see Enders & Tofighi, 2007; Hoffman &
distinct types of structures: A mastery goal structure, Stawski, 2009). These studies show that mastery
in which an emphasis is placed on mastery, personal goal structures positively influence achievement
improvement, and understanding in the classroom, outcomes, while performance-approach goal struc-
and a performance goal structure, in which an empha- tures have null or negative effects. The effects of
sis is placed on relative ability and competition in performance-avoidance goal structures are not well
the classroom. Midgley and her colleagues (Midgley established; however, Karabenick (2004) has shown
et al., 2000) subsequently applied the trichotomous that they can be associated with the avoidance of
model of personal achievement goals to the class- help-seeking behavior.
room context, differentiating the performance-based Taken together, these results highlight the con-
goal structure in terms of approach and avoidance. textual factors that play an important role in the
This resulted in three separate classroom goal struc- functioning of achievement goals. These studies
tures: a mastery goal structure, in which the classroom also underscore the need for future research in this
environment focuses on engaging in academic work domain. To date, only a few studies have investigated
in order to develop competence, especially task-based interaction effects, in which classroom goal struc-
and intrapersonally based competence; a performance- tures moderate the influence of achievement goals
approach goal structure, in which the classroom envi- on achievement-relevant outcomes (e.g., Lau &
ronment focuses on engaging in academic work in Nie, 2008; Linnenbrink, 2005; Murayama & Elliot,
order to demonstrate competence, often normative 2009). Given the extensive literature documenting
competence; and a performance-avoidance goal struc- the importance of person by situation interactions
ture, in which the classroom environment focuses on (Bretz & Judge, 1994; Cronbach & Snow, 1977;
engaging in academic work in order to avoid dem- Higgins, 2000; Hunt, 1975; Mischel & Shoda,
onstrating incompetence, often normative incom- 1995; Oishi, Diener, Suh, & Lucas, 1999), there
petence. These goal structures were assessed either is considerable need for additional insight in this
by student self-report (thus constituting “perceived” domain. Among the few studies that reveal an inter-

 achievem ent goals


action, for example, Murayama and Elliot (2009) Methodological Expansion
showed that the influence of performance-approach As this chapter demonstrates, the achievement
goals on intrinsic motivation varies between class- goal literature has yielded a wealth of interesting
rooms, and that the effect is positive in classrooms findings over the past few decades. That said, we
with high performance-approach goal structures. believe there remains substantial room for growth in
In addition, empirical work on classroom goal the methodology approaches used by achievement
structures has yet to incorporate the mastery- goal researchers.
avoidance distinction. There is good reason to believe To date, nearly all achievement goal research has
that investigating the influence of mastery-avoidance focused on between-person covariation, using per-
goal structures would be valuable, because instruc- sons as the unit of analysis (Borsboom, Mellenbergh,
tional practices that convey mastery-avoidance goals & van Heerden, 2003). This focus on between-
(i.e., a mastery-avoidance goal structure) are rela- person comparisons is limiting, particularly when
tively common in classroom setting (e.g., “Be care- the rest of personality/social psychology is moving
ful not to make mistakes”). This mastery-avoidance in the direction of focusing on both between- and
goal structure could therefore have a substantial within-person covariation (Cervone & Shoda, 1999;
impact on the learning process. Hamaker, Dolan, & Molenaar, 2005; Molenaar &
While research on contextual factors has primar- Campbell, 2009). Within-person analyses, which
ily focused on classroom goal structures, the notion involve collecting repeated measurements of items
of context can be thought of more broadly. For across time-points or situations and computing the
example, a number of researchers have raised the covariance of the scores using the time-points or
possibility that achievement goals are pursued dif- situations as the unit of analysis, allow researchers
ferently in different cultures (Maehr & Nicholls, to directly investigate how psychological elements
1980; Urdan, 2004; Zusho & Njoku, 2007). To vary within individuals and interact with each other.
date, few empirical studies have tested cultural dif- Goal pursuit across time and situations, and regula-
ference in achievement goals (e.g., Murayama et al., tory shifts therein, can be monitored using within-
2009; see Elliot, Chirkov, Kim, & Sheldon, 2001 for person analyses, potentially shedding much needed
an analogous point regarding approach and avoid- light on an area about which little is presently
ance goals more generally). From our perspective, known (see Murayama, Elliot, & Yamagata, 2011;
competence strivings (i.e., achievement goals) are Schantz & Conroy, 2009). Furthermore, the use
common to all individuals, across cultural bound- of within-person methodologies, such as diary stud-
aries (Li, 2003; Sheldon, Elliot, Kim, & Kasser, ies, can be used to test whether the psychological
2001; Van de Vliert & Janssen, 2002). Indeed, it is mechanisms identified through between-person
impossible to imagine a culture in which individuals research can be extended to the within-person
do not have any achievement goals. However, the level.
form that these strivings take may differ for people Another limitation of the methodologies com-
with distinct cultural backgrounds (Heine, Lehman, monly used in achievement goal research is the
Markus, & Kitayama, 1999). Research has shown informal assumption that achievement goals are
that relative to Western cultures (e.g., Canada, the consciously accessible. While few present-day
United States, Western Europe), Eastern cultures achievement goal researchers would argue that all
(e.g., China, Japan, South Korea) appear to be goals are consciously accessible, the reality is that
more group and socially oriented (Chang, Wong, within the achievement goal literature, goals are
& Teo, 2000), more grounded in obligation and operationalized as if they must be conscious. That is,
responsibility (Fuligni, Tseng, & Lam, 1999), more in the vast majority of studies, achievement goals
avoidance oriented (Eaton & Dembo, 1997; Elliot are assessed by self-report questionnaire or experi-
et al., 2001), and more focused on improvement mentally manipulated through verbal instruction
(Heine et al., 2001). Furthermore, empirical stud- (for an exception, see Niiya, Crocker, & Bartmess,
ies show that competence-relevant words such as 2004). However, recent research on social cognition
“success,” “failure,” and “learn” have different con- has repeatedly shown that goals can be activated and
notations in different countries (Li, 2003; Maehr & operate in a thoroughly automatic, nonconscious
Nicholls, 1980). In sum, cross-cultural differences fashion (Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar,
in achievement goal strivings may emerge as a func- & Trotschel, 2001; Custers & Aarts, 2005). There is
tion of distinct worldviews promoted within each therefore a strong need for the introduction of exper-
culture. imental priming techniques and implicit assessments

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


into the achievement goal literature. This work could • Investigate the interrelationship between
advance existing research by disentangling conscious achievement goals, including the interaction
and nonconscious elements of achievement goal between different achievement goals, contextual
striving, thereby bringing a richer understanding to moderation effects, and cultural difference in the
the field as a whole. predictors and outcomes of achievement goals.
On a related note, the vast majority of current • Examine the many different achievement
studies on achievement goals have relied on self- goal complexes that energize and direct individuals’
reported questionnaire studies. Although a number behavior in real-world achievement settings.
of studies have utilized experimental manipulations,
interventions, and observational methods, these References
studies are relatively rare. In our view, these meth- Ames, C. (1981). Competitive versus cooperative reward struc-
odologies offer important findings that cannot be tures: The influence of individual and group performance
factors on achievement attributions and affect. American
obtained by depending solely on questionnaire Educational Research Journal, 18, 273–287.
studies, including causal relationships. They also Ames, C. (1984). Achievement attributions and self-instructions
minimize bias inherent in subjective self-reports. under competitive and individualistic goal structures. Journal
We therefore encourage future studies to incorpo- of Educational Psychology, 76, 478–487.
rate these methodologies. Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student
motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271.
Conclusion Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1981). Competitive versus individualistic
goal structures: The salience of past performance informa-
In sum, the achievement goal construct has pro-
tion for causal attributions and affect. Journal of Educational
vided considerable conceptual and empirical utility Psychology, 73, 411–418.
over the past three decades. What began with the Ames, C., & Ames, R. (1984). System of student and teacher
work of four independent researchers has quickly motivation: Toward a qualitative definition. Journal of Edu-
developed into a robust literature examining the cational Psychology, 76, 535–566.
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the class-
thoughts, attitudes, and behaviors that charac-
room: Students’ learning strategies and motivation processes.
terize people’s competence-based pursuits. Today, Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260–267.
the achievement goal construct is utilized within a Ames, C., & Archer, R. (1987). Mother’s beliefs about the role
diverse range of psychological literatures, including of ability and effort in school learning. Journal of Educational
developmental psychology, educational psychol- Psychology, 79, 409–414.
Anderman, E. M., Griesinger, T., & Westerfield, G. (1998).
ogy, industrial-organizational psychology, social-
Motivation and cheating during early adolescence. Journal of
personality psychology, cross-cultural psychology, Educational Psychology, 90, 84–93.
and sport/exercise psychology. For the achievement Archer, J. (1994). Achievement goals as a measure of motivation
goal literature to continue to grow, however, it must in university students. Contemporary Educational Psychology,
address the next generation research questions that 19, 430–446.
Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking
we have highlighted herein, delivering more insight
behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.
into the processes underlying goal pursuit. In this Atkinson, J. W., & Raynor, J. O. (1978). Personality, motivation,
chapter, we have identified a number of exciting and achievement: Oxford, England: Hemisphere.
opportunities for building on existing research, Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in
leading us to believe that the future of the achieve- psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological
Bulletin, 120, 338–375.
ment goal literature is bright, with a number of
Baranik, L. E., Barron, K. E., & Finney, S. J. (2007). Measuring
unanswered empirical questions awaiting inquiry. goal orientation in a work domain: Construct validity evi-
dence for the 2 x 2 framework. Educational and Psychological
Future Directions Measurement, 67, 697–718.
• Consider the possible extension of Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., &
Trotschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious
the achievement goal framework to a
activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Person-
3x2 model. ality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027.
• Conduct additional research on the Barron, K. E., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2001). Achievement goals
consequences of mastery-avoidance goal and optimal motivation: Testing multiple goal models. Jour-
adoption. nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 706–722.
• Integrate broader methodologies Bempechat, J., London, P., & Dweck, C. S. (1991). Children’s
conceptions of ability in major domains: An interview and
such as priming methodologies, diary experimental study. Child Study Journal, 21, 11–36.
methodologies, and continue work on Bong, M. (2005). Within-grade changes in Korean girls’
interventions. motivation and perceptions of the learning environment

 achievem ent goals


across domains and achievement levels. Journal of Educa- Davidson, R. J. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of affective
tional Psychology, 97, 656–672. style. In R. Lane, L. Nadel, J. Allen, A. Kaszniak, S. Rapcsak
Bong, M. (2009). Age-related differences in achievement & G. Schwartz (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion
goal differentiation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, (pp. 371–388). New York: Oxford University Press.
879–896. Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1978). An analysis of learned
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, G. J., & van Heerden, J. (2003). helplessness: Continuous changes in performance, strategy,
The theoretical status of latent variables. Psychological Review, and achievement cognitions following failure. Journal of Per-
110, 203–219. sonality and Social Psychology, 36, 451–462.
Bouffard, T., Boisvert, J., Vezeau, C., & Larouche, C. (1995). Diener, C. I., & Dweck, C. S. (1980). An analysis of learned
The impact of goal orientation on self-regulation and perfor- helplessness. The processing of success. Journal of Personality
mance among college students. British Journal of Educational and Social Psychology, 39, 940–952.
Psychology, 65, 317–329. Dompnier, B., Darnon, C., & Butera, F. (2009). Faking the
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment. New York: Basic Books. desire to learn: A clarification of the link between mastery
Bretz, R. D., & Judge, T. A. (1994). The role of human-resource goals and academic achievement. Psychological Science, 20,
systems in job applicant decision-processes. Journal of Man- 939–943.
agement, 20, 531–551. Duda, J. L. (2005). Motivation in sport: The relevance of compe-
Brophy, J. (2005). Goal theorists should move on from perfor- tence and achievement goals. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck
mance goals. Educational Psychologist, 40, 167–176. (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 318–335).
Butler, R., & Neuman, O. (1995). Effects of task and ego New York: Guilford Press.
achievement goals on help-seeking behaviors and attitudes. Duda, J. L., Chi, L., Newton, M. L., Walling, M. D., & Catley, D.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 261–271. (1995). Task and ego orientation and intrinsic motivation in
Button, S. B., Mathieu, J. E., & Zajac, D. M. (1996). Goal sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 40–63.
orientation in organizational research: A conceptual and Duda, J. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1992). Dimensions of achieve-
empirical foundation. Organizational Behavior and Human ment motivation in schoolwork and sport. Journal of Educa-
Decision Processes, 67, 26–48. tional Psychology, 84, 290–299.
Campbell, H. L., Barry, C. L., Joe, J. N., & Finney, S. J. (2008). Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in
Configural, metric, and scalar invariance of the modified the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality
achievement goal questionnaire across African American and and Social Psychology, 31, 674–685.
white university students. Educational and Psychological Mea- Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational process affects learning.
surement, 68, 988–1007. American Psychologist, 41, 1010–1018.
Cervone, D., & Shoda, Y. (1999). Beyond traits in the study Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, per-
of personality coherence. Current Directions in Psychological sonality, and development: New York: Psychology Press.
Science, 8(1), 27–32. Dweck, C. S., & Elliott, E. (1983). Achievement motivation.
Chang, W., Wong, W., & Teo, G. (2000). The socially oriented In E. M. Hetherington (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology
and individually oriented achievement motivation of Singa- (4th ed., Vol. 4, pp. 643–691). New York: Wiley.
porean and Chinese students. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive
Societies, 1, 39–63. approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review,
Church, M. A., Elliot, A. J., & Gable, S. L. (2001). Perceptions of 95, 256–273.
classroom environment, achievement goals, and achievement Dweck, C. S., & Reppucci, N. D. (1973). Learned helpless-
outcomes. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 43–54. ness and reinforcement responsibility in children. Journal of
Conroy, D. E., Elliot, A. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2003). A 2 x 2 Personality and Social Psychology, 25, 109–116.
achievement goals questionnaire for sport: Evidence for fac- Dweck, C. S., & Wortman, C. (1982). Learned helplessness,
torial invariance, temporal stability, and external validity. anxiety, and achievement motivation: Neglected parallels in
Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 25, 456–476. cognitive, affective, and coping responses. In H. Krohne &
Covington, M. V., & Omelich, C. L. (1984). Task-oriented L. Laux (Eds.), Achievement, stress, and anxiety (pp. 92–126).
versus competitive learning structures: Motivational and per- Washington, DC: Hemisphere.
formance consequences. Journal of Educational Psychology, Eaton, M. J., & Dembo, M. H. (1997). Differences in the moti-
76, 1038–1050. vational beliefs of Asian American and non-Asian students.
Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instruc- Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 433–440.
tional methods: A handbook for research on interactions: Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and
Oxford, England: Irvington. achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189.
Cury, F., Elliot, A. J., Fonseca, D. D., & Moller, A. C. (2006). Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal
The social-cognitive model of achievement motivation and construct. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of
the 2 X 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality competence and motivation (pp. 52–72). New York: Guilford
and Social Psychology, 90, 666–679. Press.
Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit moti- Elliot, A. J. (2006). The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance
vator: On the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals. motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 30(2), 111–116.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 129–142. Elliot, A. J., Chirkov, V. I., Kim, Y., & Sheldon, K. M. (2001).
Daniels, L. M., Haynes, T. L., Stupnisky, R. H., Perry, R. P., A cross-cultural analysis of avoidance (relative to approach)
Newall, N. E., & Pekrun, R. (2008). Individual differences personal goals. Psychological Science, 12, 505–510.
in achievement goals: A longitudinal study of cognitive, Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of
emotional, and achievement outcomes. Contemporary Edu- approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of
cational Psychology, 33, 584–608. Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232.

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


Elliot, A. J., & Fryer, J. W. (2008). The goal construct in application of multivariate stationary time series analysis.
psychology. In J. Shah & W. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of Multivariate Behavioral Research, 40, 207–233.
motivation science (pp. 235–250). New York: Guilford Press. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Elliot, A. J., Carter, S. M., &
Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and Lehto, A. T. (1997). Predictors and consequences of achieve-
avoidance achievement goals and intrinsic motivation: ment goals in the college classroom: Maintaining interest and
A mediational analysis. Journal of Personality and Social making the grade. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Psychology, 70, 461–475. 73, 1284–1295.
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2×2 achievement Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J.,
goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal
80, 501–519. theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational
Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Gable, S. (1999). Achievement Psychology, 94, 638–645.
goals, study strategies, and exam performace: A mediational Harackiewicz, J. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1993). Achievement goals
analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 549–563. and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social
Elliot, A. J., & Moller, A. C. (2003). Performance-approach Psychology, 65, 904–915.
goals: Good or bad forms of regualtion? International Journal Harackiewicz, J. M., & Sansone, C. (1991). Goals and intrinsic
of Educational Research, 39, 339–356. motivation: You can get there from here. In M. L. Maehr
Elliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of & P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achieve-
achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application. ment (Vol. 7, pp. 21–49). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 613–628. Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E.,
Elliot, A. J., Murayama, K., & Pekrun, R. (2011). A 3 x 2 Leung, C., & Matsumoto, H. (2001). Divergent conse-
achievement goal model. Journal of Educational Psychology, quences of success and failure in Japan and North America:
103, 632–648. An investigation of self-improving motivations and mal-
Elliot, A. J., & Reis, H. T. (2003). Attachment and exploration leable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81,
in adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 599–615.
317–331. Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S.
Elliot, A. J., & Thrash, T. M. (2001). Achievement goals and the (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard?
hierarchical model of achievement motivation. Educational Psychological Review, 106, 766–794.
Psychology Review, 13, 139–156. Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit.
Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230.
motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social Hoffman, L., & Stawski, R. S. (2009). Persons as contexts: Eval-
Psychology, 54, 5–12. uating between-person and within-person effects in longitu-
Enders, C. K., & Tofighi, D. (2007). Centering predictor vari- dinal analysis. Research in Human Development, 6, 97–120.
ables in cross-sectional multilevel models: A new look at an Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: an introduction to
old issue. Psychological Methods, 12, 121–138. behavior theory: Oxford, England: Appleton-Century.
Epstein, J. L. (1988). Effective schools or effective students: Hunt, D. E. (1975). Person-environment interaction: A chal-
Dealing with diversity. In R. Haskins & D. Macrae (Eds.), lenge found wanting before it was tried. Review of Educa-
Policies for America’s public schools: Teachers, equity, and indi- tional Research, 45, 209–230.
cators (pp. 89–126). Norwood, NJ: Ablex. Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Conceptions of abil-
Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The biological basis of personality: Spring- ity and related affects in task involvement and ego involve-
field, IL: Thomas. ment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 909–919.
Finney, S. J., Pieper, S. L., & Barron, K. E. (2004). Examining Jagacinski, C. M., & Nicholls, J. G. (1987). Competence and
the psychometric properties of the achievement goal ques- affect in task involvement and ego involvement: The impact
tionnaire in a general academic context. Educational and of social comparison information. Journal of Educational Psy-
Psychological Measurement, 64, 365–382. chology, 79, 107–114.
Fuligni, A. J., Tseng, V., & Lam, M. (1999). Attitudes toward Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An anal-
family obligations among American adolescents with Asian, ysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.
Latin American, and European backgrounds. Child Develop- Kaplan, A., Gheen, M., & Midgley, C. (2002). Classroom goal
ment, 70(4), 1030–1044. structure and student disruptive behaviour. British Journal of
Fulmer, S. M., & Frijters, J. C. (2009). A review of self-report Educational Psychology, 72, 191–212.
and alternative approaches in the measurement of student Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (1999). Achievement goals and student
motivation. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 219–246. well-being. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 24, 330–358.
Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd ed.). Kaplan, A., & Maehr, M. L. (2007). The contributions and
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. prospects of goal orientation theory. Educational Psychology
Greene, B. A., & Miller, R. B. (1996). Influences on achieve- Review, 19, 141–184.
ment: Goals, perceived ability, and cognitive engagement. Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. J. (2002). Should childhood be
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 181–192. a journey or a race? Response to Harackiewicz et al.(2002).
Greene, B. A., Miller, R. B., Crowson, H. M., Duke, B. L., & Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 646–648.
Akey, K. L. (2004). Predicting high school students’ cogni- Kaplan, A., & Midgley, C. (1997). The effects of achievement
tive engagement and achievement: Contributions of class- goals: Does level of perceived academic competence make a dif-
room perceptions and motivation. Contemporary Educational ference? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 22, 415–435.
Psychology, 29, 462–482. Karabenick, S. A. (2004). Perceived achievement goal structure
Hamaker, E. L., Dolan, C. V., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2005). and college student help seeking. Journal of Educational Psy-
Statistical modeling of the individual: Rationale and chology, 96, 569–581.

 achievem ent goals


Koestner, R., Zuckerman, M., & Koestner, J. (1987). Praise, Meece, J. L., & Holt, K. (1993). A pattern analysis of students’
involvement, and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85,
and Social Psychology, 53, 383–390. 582–590.
Lau, S., & Nie, Y. (2008). Interplay between personal goals and Middleton, M. J., & Midgley, C. (1997). Avoiding the
classroom goal structures in predicting student outcomes: demonstration of lack of ability: An underexplored aspect
A multilevel analysis of person-context interactions. Journal of goal theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89,
of Educational Psychology, 100, 15–29. 710–718.
Lemos, M. S. (1996). Students’ and teachers’ goals in the class- Midgley, C., Anderman, E., & Hicks, L. (1995). Differences
room. Learning and Instruction, 6, 151–172. between elementary and middle school teachers and stu-
Levy-Tossman, I., Kaplan, A., & Assor, A. (2007). Academic goal dents: A goal theory approach. Journal of Early Adolescence,
orientations, multiple goal profiles, and friendship intimacy 15, 90–113.
among early adolescents. Contemporary Educational Psychol- Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. (2001). Performance
ogy, 32, 231–252. approach goals: Good for what, for whom, under what cir-
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). cumstances, and at what cost? Journal of Educational Psychol-
Level of aspiration. In Personality and the behavior disorders ogy, 93, 77–86.
(pp. 333–378). Oxford, England: Ronald Press. Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., Middleton, M., Maehr, M. L., Urdan, T.,
Li, J. (2003). U.S. and Chinese cultural beliefs about learning. Anderman, L. H., . . . Roeser, R. (1998). The developmen-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 258–267. tal and validation of scales assessing students’ achievement
Linnenbrink, E. A. (2004). Person and context: Theoreti- goal orientations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23,
cal and practical concerns in achievement goal theory. 113–131.
In P. R. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr (Eds.), Advances in motiva- Midgley, C., Maehr, M. L., Hruda, L. Z., Anderman, E.,
tion and achievement: Motivating students, improving schools: Anderman, L., Freeman, K. E., . . . Urdan, T. (2000). Man-
The legacy of Carol Midgley (Vol. 13, pp. 159–184). Greewich, ual for the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Scales (PALS). Ann
CT: JAI. Arbor: University of Michigan Press.
Linnenbrink, E. A. (2005). The dilemma of performance- Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (1995). Predictors of middle school
approach goals: The use of multiple goal contexts to promote students’ use of self-handicapping strategies. The Journal of
students’ motivation and learning. Journal of Educational Early Adolescence, 15, 389–411.
Psychology, 97, 197–213. Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. (2001). Academic self-handicapping
Maehr, M. L. (1983). On doing well in science: Why Johnny no and achievement goals: A further examination. Contemporary
longer excels, why Sarah never did. In S. Paris, G. Olson, & Educational Psychology, 26, 61–75.
H. Stevenson (Eds.), Learning and motivation in the classroom Miller, A., & Hom, H. L. (1990). Influence of extrinsic and
(pp. 179–210). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ego incentive value on persistence after failure and con-
Maehr, M. L. (1989). Thoughts about motivation. In C. Ames tinuing motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82,
& R. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in education: 539–545.
Goals and cognitions (Vol. 3, pp. 299–315). San Diego, CA: Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation.
Academic Press. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Maehr, M. L., & Braskamp, L. A. (1986). The motivation factor: Miller, R. B., Greene, A., Montalvo, G. P., Ravindran, B., &
A theory of personal investment. Lexington, MA: Lexington Nicholls, J. D. (1996). Engaging in academic work: The role
Books. of learning goals, future consequences, pleasing others, and
Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school perceived ability. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21,
cultures. Boulder, CO: Westview Press. 388–422.
Maehr, M. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1980). Culture and achieve- Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system
ment motivation: A second look. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispo-
in cross cultural psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 221–267). New York: sitions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure.
Academic Press. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268.
Malka, A., & Covington, M. V. (2005). Perceiving school Molenaar, P. C. M., & Campbell, C., G. (2009). The new
performance as instrumental to future goal attainment: person-specific paradigm in psychology. Current Directions
Effects on graded performance. Contemporary Educational in Psychological Science, 18, 112–116.
Psychology, 30, 60–80. Murayama, K., & Elliot, A. J. (2009). The joint influence of
McClelland, D. C. (1951). Personality. New York: William personal achievement goals and classroom goal structures
Sloane Assoc. on achievement-relevant outcomes. Journal of Educational
McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & Lowell, E. L. Psychology, 101, 432–447.
(1976). The achievement motive: Oxford, England: Irvington. Murayama, K., Elliot, A. J., & Yamagata, S. (2011). Separa-
McClelland, G. H., & Judd, C. M. (1993). Statistical difficulties tion of performance-approach and performance-avoidance
of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological achievement goals: A broader analysis. Journal of Educational
Bulletin, 114, 376–390. Psychology, 103, 238–256.
Meece, J. L., Anderman, E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2006). Murayama, K., Zhou, M., & Nesbit, J. C. (2009). A cross-
Classroom goal structure, student motivation, and academic cultural examination of the psychometric properties of
achievement. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 487–503. responses to the Achievement Goal Questionnaire. Educa-
Meece, J. L., Blumenfeld, P. C., & Hoyle, R. H. (1988). tional and Psychological Measurement, 69, 432–447.
Student’s goal orientations and cognitive engagement in Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judg-
classroom activities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, ment: Mechanisms and consequences. Psychological Review,
514–523. 110, 472–489.

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


Mussweiler, T., & Epstude, K. (2009). Relatively fast! Efficiency Pintrich, P. R., & Garcia, T. (1991). Student goal orientation and
advantages of comparative thinking. Journal of Experimental self-regulation in the college classroom. In M. L. Maehr &
Psychology: General, 138, 1–21. P. R. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement
Newman, R. S. (1998). Students’ help seeking during (Vol. 7, pp. 371–402). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
problem solving: Influences of personal and contextual Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A. F., Garcia, T., & Mckeachie, W.
achievement goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, (1993). Reliability and predictive validity of the motivated
644–658. strategies for learning questionaire(MSLQ). Educational and
Nicholls, J. G. (1976). Effort is virtuous, but it’s better to have Psychological Measurement, 53, 801–813.
ability: Evaluative responses to perceptions of effort and abil- Rawsthorne, L. J., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Achievement goals and
ity. Journal of Research in Personality, 10, 306–315. intrinsic motivation: A meta-analytic review. Personality and
Nicholls, J. G. (1978). The development of the concepts of effort Social Psychology Review, 3, 326–344.
and ability, perception of academic attainment, and the Roberts, G. C. (2001). Understanding the dynamics of motiva-
understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child tion in physical activity: The influence of achievement goals
Development, 49, 800–814. on motivational processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances
Nicholls, J. G. (1980). The development of the concept of dif- in motivation in sport and exercise (pp. 1–50). Champaign,
ficulty. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental IL: Human Kinetics.
Psychology, 26, 271–281. Roberts, G. C., & Treasure, D. C. (1995). Achievement goals,
Nicholls, J. G. (1983). Conceptions of ability and achievement motivational climate and achievement strategies and behav-
motivation. In R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on iors in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26,
motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 185–218). New York: 64–80.
Academic Press. Roeser, R. W. (2004). Competing schools of thought in achieve-
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of ment goal theory? In P. R. Pintrich & M. L. Maehr (Eds.),
ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Advances in motivation and achievement. Vol. 13: Motivat-
Psychological Review, 91, 328–346. ing students, improving schools: The legacy of Carol Midgley
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic edu- (pp. 265–299). Oxford, England: Elsevier.
cation: Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Roeser, R. W., Midgley, C., & Urdan, T. C. (1996). Perceptions
Nicholls, J. G., & Dweck, C. S. (1979). A definition of achieve- of the school psychological environment and early adoles-
ment motivation. Unpublished manuscript. University of cents’ psychological and behavioral functioning in school:
Illinois at Champaign-Urbana. The mediating role of goals and belonging. Journal of Educa-
Nicholls, J. G., Patashnick, M., & Nolen, S. B. (1985). tional Psychology, 88, 408–422.
Adolescents’ theories of education. Journal of Educational Roeser, R. W., Peck, S. C., Nasir, N. S., Alexander, P. A., &
Psychology, 77, 683–692. Winne, P. H. (2006). Self and identity processes in school
Niiya, Y., Crocker, J., & Bartmess, E. N. (2004). From motivation, learning, and achievement. In P. Alexander
vulnerability to resilience: Learning orientations buffer con- & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology
tingent self-esteem from failure. Psychological Science, 15, (pp. 391–424). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
801–805. Rolls, E. T. (2005). Emotion explained. Oxford, England: Oxford
Nolen, S. B. (1988). Reasons for studying: Motivational University Press.
orientations and study strategies. Cognition and Instruction, Ryan, A. M., & Pintrich, P. R. (1997). Should I ask for help? The
5, 269–287. role of motivation and attitudes in adolescents’ help seeking in
Nolen, S. B., & Haladyna, T. M. (1990a). Personal and envi- math class. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 329–341.
ronment influences on students’ beliefs about effective Ryan, A. M., & Shim, S. O. (2006). Social achievement goals:
study strategies. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 15, The nature and consequences of different orientations toward
116–130. social competence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Nolen, S. B., & Haladyna, T. M. (1990b). Personal and envi- 32, 1246–1263.
ronmental influences on students’ beliefs about effective Ryan, A. M., & Shim, S. S. (2008). An exploration of young
study strategies. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 15, adolescents’ social achievement goals and social adjustment
116–130. in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100,
Oishi, S., Diener, E., Suh, E., & Lucas, R. E. (1999). Value as 672–687.
a moderator in subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, Sansone, C., Sachau, D. A., & Weir, C. (1989). Effects of instruc-
67, 157–184. tion on intrinsic interest: The importance of context. Journal
Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2009). Achievement of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 819–829.
goals and achievement emotions: Testing a model of their Schantz, L. H., & Conroy, D. E. (2009). Achievement motiva-
joint relations with academic performance. Journal of Educa- tion and intraindividual affective variability during compe-
tional Psychology, 101, 115–135. tence pursuits: A round of golf as a multilevel data structure.
Pintrich, P. R. (2000a). An achievement goal theory perspective Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 472–481.
on issues in motivation terminology, theory, and research. Senko, C., Durik, A. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2008).
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 92–104. Historical perspectives and new directions in achievement
Pintrich, P. R. (2000b). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The goal theory: Understanding the effects of mastery and
role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal performance-approach goals. In J. Shah & W. Gardner
of Educational Psychology, 92, 544–555. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science (pp. 100–113). New
Pintrich, P. R., & DeGroot, E. V. (1990). Motivational and York: Guilford Press.
self-regulated learning components of classroom academic Senko, C., Hulleman, C. S., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2011).
performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 33–40. Achievement goal theory at the crossroads: Old controver-

 achievem ent goals


sies, current challenges, and new directions. Educational Utman, C. H. (1997). Performance effects of motivational state:
Psychologist, 46, 26–47. A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 1,
Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). What 170–182.
is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate Van de Vliert, E., & Janssen, O. (2002). Competitive societies
psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- are happy if the women are less competitive than the men.
ogy, 80, 325–339. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative Social
Sideridis, G. D. (2008). The regulation of affect, anxiety, and Science, 36(4), 321–337.
stressful arousal from adopting mastery-avoidance goal Vandewalle, D. (1997). Development and validation of a work
orientations. Stress and Health: Journal of the International domain goal orientation instrument. Educational and Psycho-
Society for the Investigation of Stress, 24, 55–69. logical Measurement, 57, 995–1015.
Skaalvik, E. M. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeating ego Weiner, B. (1985). Spontaneous causal thinking. Psychological
orientation: Relations with task and avoidance orientation, Bulletin, 97, 74–84.
achievement, self-perceptions, and anxiety. Journal of Educa- Weiner, B., Heckhausen, H., & Meyer, W-U. (1972). Causal
tional Psychology, 89, 74–81. ascriptions and achievement behavior: A conceptual analysis
Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior: Oxford, of effort and reanalysis of locus of control. Journal of Person-
England: Macmillan. ality and Social Psychology, 21, 239–248.
Smiley, P. A., & Dweck, C. S. (1994). Individual differences in Weiner, B., & Kukla, A. (1970). An attributional analysis of
achievement goals among young children. Child Develop- achievement motivation. Journal of Personality and Social
ment, 65, 1723–1743. Psychology, 15, 1–20.
Spence, J. T., & Helmreich, R. L. (1983). Achievement-related Wentzel, K. R. (1989). Adolescent classroom goals, standards for
motives and behavior. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and performance, and academic achievement: An interactionist
achievement motives: Psychological and sociological approaches perspective. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(2), 131–142.
(pp. 10–74). San Francisco, CA: Freeman. Wentzel, K. R. (1991). Social competence at school: Relation
Stapel, D. A., & Koomen, W. (2001). The impact of interpre- between social responsibility and academic achievement.
tation versus comparison mindsets on knowledge accessi- Review of Educational Research, 61(1), 1–24.
bility effects. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motivation
134–149. in middle school: The role of parents, teachers, and peers.
Stipek, D., & Gralinski, J. H. (1996). Children’s beliefs about Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 202–209.
intelligence and school performance. Journal of Educational Wolters, C. A. (1998). Self-regulated learning and college
Psychology, 88, 397–407. students’ regulation of motivation. Journal of Educational
Urdan, T. C. (1997). Achievement goal theory: Past results, future Psychology, 90, 224–235.
directions. In M. L. Maehr & A. L. Pincus (Eds.), Advances Wolters, C. A. (2004). Advancing achievement goal theory:
in motivation and achievement (Vol. 10, pp. 99–141). Using goal structures and goal orientations to predict
Greenwich, CT: JAI Press. students’ motivation, cognition, and achievement. Journal of
Urdan, T. C. (2004). Predictors of academic self-handicapping Educational Psychology, 96, 236–250.
and achievement: Examining achievement goals, classroom Wolters, C. A., Yu, S. L., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The relation
goal structures, and culture. Journal of Educational Psychol- between goal orientation and students’ motivational beliefs
ogy, 96, 251–264. and self-regulated learning. Learning and Individual Differ-
Urdan, T. C., & Maehr, M. L. (1995). Beyond a two-goal the- ences, 8, 211–238.
ory of motivation and achievement: A case for social goals. Zusho, A., & Njoku, H. (2007). Culture and motivation to
Review of Educational Research, 65, 213–243. learn: Exploring the generalizability of achievement goal the-
Urdan, T. C., & Mestas, M. (2006). The goals behind ory. In F. Salili & R. Hoosain (Eds.), Culture, motivation, and
performance goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, learning: A multicultural perspective (pp. 91–113). Charlotte,
354–365. NC: Information Age Publishing.
Urdan, T. C., & Turner, J. C. (2005). Competence motivation Zweig, D., & Webster, J. (2004). Validation of a Multidimen-
in the classroom. In A. J. Elliot & C. S Dweck (Eds.), Hand- sional Measure of Goal Orientation. Canadian Journal of
book of competence and motivation (pp. 297–317). New York: Behavioural Science/Revue canadienne des sciences du comporte-
Guilford Press. ment, 36, 232–248.

mur aya ma , elli ot, f r i ed ma n 


CHAPTER

Goal Pursuit
13
Peter M. Gollwitzer and Gabriele Oettingen

Abstract
We start out with describing how the goal concept emerged in the history of the psychology of
motivation to better understand the important role it plays in current research on motivation. We then
suggest a differentiation between studies targeting the setting of goals versus the implementation of goals
to get a grip on the host of empirical work the goal concept has triggered. With respect to goal setting,
we first discuss studies that explore determinants affecting the content and structure of set goals (e.g.,
entity vs. incremental theories of intelligence influence the setting of performance vs. learning goals).
We then turn to studies on the self-regulation of goal setting and discuss in detail how a self-regulation
strategy called mental contrasting of future and reality facilitates strong commitment to feasible goals but
dissolves commitment to unfeasible ones. With respect to goal implementation we first refer to studies
on the determinants of effective goal striving (e.g., the framing of the set goal in terms of approach vs.
avoidance) and then turn to analyzing the effective self-regulation of goal implementation. Here we focus
on the strategy of forming implementation intentions (i.e., if-then plans) and explicate in detail how
such planning helps in overcoming classic hurdles to goal attainment (e.g., distractions). We will end the
chapter by reporting the results of recent intervention studies that successfully enhanced goal attainment
in the health, academic, and interpersonal domains by combining the self-regulation strategy of mental
contrasting with that of forming implementation intentions.
Key Words: goal setting, goal implementation, goal commitment, obstacles, mental contrasting,
implementation intentions, self-regulation, self-control, willpower, behavior change interventions

It is Friday afternoon. On Monday, there is an Goals Versus Motivation


important presentation you have to give. Even though The term motivation is commonly used to explain
you are highly motivated to give a great talk (i.e., desir- why a person in a given situation selects one response
ability and feasibility are high), you did not find the over another or makes a given response with great ener-
time to prepare the talk during the week. So you set gization or frequency. Imagine a person looking for
yourself the goal to use the weekend to prepare a nice someone else in a crowd. She gets excited when she
presentation. But how do you arrive at a strong com- finds that person, and then she runs toward him.
mitment to attain this goal? And how do you ensure Each of these responses involves motivation, which
that you will indeed implement your goal? In the pres- can manifest itself cognitively (e.g., looking), affec-
ent chapter we will discuss research on self-regulation tively (e.g., excitement), and behaviorally (e.g., run-
strategies that benefit (a) committing to goals and ning). To the question of what drives motivation,
(b) implementing goals that one wants to attain (i.e., the history of the psychology of motivation has
one feels committed to). offered ever more sophisticated answers.


Based on learning theory advanced by early ani- Spence (1956) as the concept of need and the con-
mal psychologists (Hull, 1943; Spence, 1956), the cept of incentive. With respect to need, the cogni-
strength of the tendency to make a response was at tively inspired psychology of motivation ventured
first considered to be a function of an organism’s skills into the concept of motives (for a summary, see
(or habit strength), its needs, and the incentive value McClelland, 1985a), defined as the class of incen-
of the desired outcome. For example, how fast an tives that a person finds attractive (e.g., achieve-
animal runs toward a box containing food depends ment, power, affiliation, intimacy). More important,
on its habit strength, its hunger, and the quality and McClelland (1985b) discovered that depending on
quantity of food. However, with the advance of the whether this preference for certain classes of incen-
cognitive revolution in psychology, these determi- tives was measured implicitly (as assessed by the
nants of motivation as well as the concept of motiva- Thematic Apperception Test; TAT) or explicitly
tion itself became more elaborated. Tolman (1932) (as assessed by attitude questionnaires), it predicts
postulated various mental processes “which interme- the execution of different types of motive-related
diate in the causal equation between environmental responses: actions peoplespontaneously engage in
stimuli and . . . overt behavior” (Tolman, 1932, p. 2). versus actions people decide to engage in after
These intermediate processes entailed concepts of thoughtful deliberation.
purpose (ends and means) as well as expectations It was also found that whether an incentive is
(e.g., means-expectations, end-expectations, and hoped for versus feared matters. For instance, a
means-end-expectations). A few years later, Festinger person with a strong achievement motive, longing
(1942) and Atkinson (1957) drew on that work in for the pride associated with success, will choose
their research on what motivates humans to select a task of medium difficulty to pursue; this level
and perform tasks of varying difficulty. They sug- of difficulty provides the most information about
gested that people weight the incentive value of the achievement level. However, a person who abhors
desired outcome with the expectancy that it would the shame associated with failure (Atkinson, 1958)
actually occur. will choose either a very easy or a very difficult task,
Social cognitive learning theorists (e.g., Bandura, which is an effective strategy to avoid shame (as
1977) went a step further, factoring in whether very easy tasks are likely to be solved, and failure on
one could successfully perform the necessary too-difficult tasks can easily be explained). Finally,
behavior required to arrive at a desired outcome researchers have differentiated among types of
(so-called efficacy or control beliefs). These theo- incentives as well (Heckhausen, 1977). For instance,
rists also alluded to further relevant expectancies, in the realm of achievement, anticipation of positive
such as whether the situation by itself would pro- self-evaluations (e.g., “I did really well!”), positive
duce the desired outcome (Heckhausen, 1977; evaluations by others (e.g., praise by the teacher),
Mischel, 1973), whether performing a given behav- higher order positive consequences (e.g., success-
ior would lead to the desired outcome (Bandura, ful professional career), and consequences that go
1977), whether achieving the desired outcome beyond achievement (e.g., having a good time with
would be instrumental to accruing further posi- coworkers) can all motivate people to do well on
tive consequences (Vroom, 1964), whether the given tasks.
desired outcome could be brought about somehow Given this increasing differentiation in think-
by one’s actions (Oettingen, 1996), and whether ing about the determinants of motivation (i.e.,
the future in general would be bright (Abramson, needs, incentives, and expectancies), one may won-
Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Scheier & Carver, der whether the concept of goals is at all needed.
1987). In our opinion, the concept of goals helps the cog-
Adding these expectancy-related variables helped nitive explication of the readiness to make a certain
to explicate in more detail the can-aspect (or feasi- response. Importantly in this regard, Ajzen and
bility aspect) of the motivation to make a certain Fishbein (1969) suggested that this readiness should
response: Can the desired outcome be brought be assessed in terms of a person’s intention to make
about? But the cognitive revolution also helped to the response. Mischel (1973) went a step further
explain the want-aspect (or desirability aspect) of and argued that such intentions can be conceived
the motivation to make a certain response: Do I as self-imposed or assigned goals that imply stan-
really want the desired outcome? This desirability dards that the person intends to meet (with respect
issue was originally captured by Hull (1943) and to quality and quantity criteria). Doing so allows

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


asking new questions such as how people arrive at feasibility of studying harder for the class, poten-
their goals and how they strive to achieve them. tially leading to the goal to study harder for this
Noticing the unique nature of both of these prob- class in the future.
lems, Kurt Lewin (Lewin, Dembo, Festinger, & It is important to recognize that goals of the same
Sears, 1944) suggested adopting a distinct theo- content can be framed in different ways. Accord-
retical perspective for goal setting versus goal striv- ingly, goal research has analyzed what makes people
ing. Present-day researchers have rediscovered Kurt favor a certain framing over another. For instance, a
Lewin’s approach (see, e.g., the action phases model; person who wants to be a good student may frame
Gollwitzer, 1990; Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987). the goal of doing well in class as either approach-
Today, research on goals explicitly targets either the ing good grades (earning A’s and B’s) or avoiding
determinants and processes of goal setting or the bad grades (no C’s and D’s). Whether approach or
determinants and processes of goal striving and suc- avoidance framing is chosen depends on various
cessful goal attainment. In the subsequent discus- attributes of the person (e.g., the trait disposition of
sion of current research on goals, we will therefore extraversion vs. neuroticism, Larsen & Augustine,
group the presented research into goal setting versus 2008; reward sensitivity vs. punishment sensitivity,
goal striving (goal implementation). Gray, 1994; their motive dispositions of hope for
success versus fear of failure, Elliot, 1997, Gable,
Goal Setting 2006).
Determinants of Goal Content A further framing variation pertains to pro-
and Structure motion strategy goals versus prevention strategy
Most theories addressing the issue of goal setting goals (Higgins, 1997; Scholer & Higgins, 2008)
focus on the question of what goals people are set- as one may want to approach a desired end state
ting themselves: What types of contents and what either by promotion strategies (i.e., with eager-
type of framing is preferred? With respect to con- ness) or prevention strategies (i.e., with vigilance).
tent, the perceived desirability and feasibility of the Equally, when one moves away from an unde-
goal matters. Perceived desirability is high when the sired end state, one can also use either promo-
goal is in line with the person’s needs (e.g., needs tion strategies (eagerness) or prevention strategies
for autonomy, competence, and social integration; (vigilance). The framing of strategy goals in terms
Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Harris, 2006; Ryan, Shel- of promotion versus prevention has been found
don, Kasser, & Deci, 1996), wishes (e.g., possible to be a consequence of whether people construe
selves; Oyserman, Bybee, & Terry, 2006), higher their self either as an ideal self that they desire to
order goals (e.g., identity goals; Gollwitzer & Kich- be or as an ought self that they feel compelled to
hof, 1998), and attitudes (i.e., the expected value of be: Ideal-self individuals prefer a promotion fram-
achieving the goal at hand; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980). ing, whereas ought-self individuals favor a preven-
But perceived feasibility also matters. As suggested by tion framing.
Bandura (1997), feasibility concerns play an impor- Dweck (1996) has suggested a framing distinc-
tant role in setting goals of certain contents as well. tion between performance goals and learning goals.
It matters whether people feel that they can make Goals in the achievement domain, for example,
the responses that produce the desired goal. Self- may either focus on finding out how capable one is
efficacy beliefs need to be high (or control beliefs as (performance goals) or on learning from the task
referred to by Ajzen, 1991, in his theory of planned (learning goals). Molden and Dweck (2006) argue
behavior) for strong intentions (goal commitments) that implicit theories on the nature of ability deter-
to emerge. mine the preference for performance versus learning
Recent research has turned its focus on the ques- goals. If people believe that ability is fixed and can-
tion of what makes people reflect on the desirability not be easily changed (i.e., hold an entity theory of
or feasibility (or both) of a given goal choice. For ability), they prefer setting performance goals. How-
instance, Epstude and Roese (2008; McCrea, 2008) ever, if people believe that ability can be improved
observed that failing to reach a set goal (e.g., not by learning (i.e., hold an incremental theory of abil-
doing well in a midterm exam where one set out ity), they prefer setting learning goals.
to receive an A) triggers thoughts such as: “If only Another structural feature of goals is their level
I had studied harder, I would have done better on of abstractness. People generally prefer to set them-
the midterm exam!” Such counterfactual thought selves abstract goals. They adopt concrete goals pre-
in turn triggers reflections on the desirability and dominantly when they run into problems attaining

 goal pursuit


an abstract goal (see action identification theory; the present negative reality). Neither of these mental
Vallacher & Wegner, 1987). Finally, goals of any strategies produces any discrepancy between future
content (e.g., solving a math problem, writing a and reality, and thus the individual fails to recog-
book, getting to know a stranger) can be specified nize that actions (making responses) are necessary to
at different levels of difficulty. Which level is pre- achieve the desired future. Therefore, expectations
ferred depends on whether a person’s achievement of success do not become activated, and goal setting
motive is dominated by hope for success or fear of does not reflect the perceived likelihood of reach-
failure (Atkinson, 1957), whether the goal is made ing the desired future. Individuals who indulge and
public (Hollenbeck, Williams, & Klein, 1989), and dwell show a medium level of goal commitment,
whether one has successfully achieved an earlier goal even though the resource-efficient strategy to follow
(Bandura, 1997). would be for no engagement in the case of low expec-
Knowing the determinants of the content and tations of success, and full engagement in the case
structure of the goals people set themselves still does of high expectations of success. For example, when
not answer the question of what people can do to it comes to the goal of giving a good presentation
promote strong goal commitments. Perceiving a at a conference, both an indulging and a dwelling
goal as desirable and feasible does not guarantee person will show moderate preparation, regardless
that one actually commits strongly and then sets of whether a successful performance is perceived as
out to strive for this goal. For instance, one may within one’s reach or as hardly possible.
wish to learn to play the violin because one loves to Various experiments support these claims (e.g.,
make music and feels capable of doing so (after all Oettingen, 2000; summary by Oettingen & Ste-
one knows how to sing well), yet actually commit- phens, 2009). In one study (Oettingen, Pak, & Sch-
ting oneself to realize this wish takes a further step, netter, 2001, Study 4), first-year students enrolled in
and there are certain self-regulatory strategies that a vocational school for computer programming indi-
facilitate making this step. cated their expectations of excelling in mathematics.
Next, they named positive aspects that they associated
Self-Regulation of Goal Setting with excelling in mathematics (e.g., feelings of pride,
Various mental strategies advance the transition increasing job prospects) and negative aspects of real-
from one’s wishes and fantasies to goal commit- ity, that is, potential obstacles (e.g., being distracted
ments. The theory of fantasy realization specifies three by peers or feeling lazy). In the mental contrasting
respective self-regulation strategies (Oettingen, 2000): condition, participants had to elaborate in writing
mental contrasting, indulging, and dwelling. In men- two aspects of the desired future and two aspects of
tal contrasting, people first imagine the fulfillment of present reality, in alternating order beginning with the
a wish or fantasy (e.g., giving a good presentation at a aspect of the desired future. Participants in the indulg-
conference) and then reflect on the present reality that ing condition were asked to elaborate four aspects of
stands in the way of attaining the desired future (e.g., the desired future only; in the dwelling condition they
evaluation anxiety). Mental contrasting is a problem- instead elaborated four aspects of the present reality
solving strategy that makes people recognize that they only. As a dependent variable, participants indicated
have not yet fulfilled their wish and that they need to how energized they felt with respect to excelling in
take action in order to achieve the desired future. As math (e.g., how active, eventful, energetic).
a consequence, expectations of attaining the desired Two weeks after the experiment, the participants’
future become activated and determine a person’s teachers reported how much effort each student had
goal commitment and subsequent striving to attain invested over the interim and provided each student
the desired future. When perceived expectations of with a grade for that time period. As predicted,
success are high, people will actively commit to real- only in the mental contrasting condition did the
izing the desired future; when expectations of success students feel energized, exerted effort, and earned
are low, people will refrain from doing so, and thus grades based upon their expectations of success.
they will venture on alternative wishes and desired Those with high expectations of success felt the most
futures. In this way, mental contrasting helps people energized, invested the most effort, and received
discriminate between feasible and unfeasible goals. the highest course grades; those with low expecta-
The theory of fantasy realization specifies two fur- tions of success felt the least energized, invested the
ther routes to goal setting. People may engage either least effort, and received the lowest course grades.
in indulging (envisioning only the attainment of the To the contrary, participants in both the indulging
wished-for future) or in dwelling (reflecting only on and dwelling conditions felt moderately energized,

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


exerted medium effort, and received medium grades orient themselves toward alternative, more promising
independent of their expectations of success. endeavors and to actively search for new venues.
A variety of studies pertaining to different life Recent research suggests that mental contrast-
domains replicated this pattern of results, for exam- ing not only regulates goal setting and goal disen-
ple, experiments on studying abroad, acquiring a sec- gagement but also promotes the choice of suitable
ond language, getting to know an attractive stranger, means for effective goal striving. Oettingen, Ste-
finding a balance between work and family life, self- phens, Mayer, and Brinkmann (2010) examined
improvement, and fulfilling idiosyncratic interper- the mental-contrasting effects on seeking and giving
sonal wishes of great importance (Oettingen, 2000; help as means to an end. For college students, men-
Oettingen, Hönig, & Gollwitzer, 2000; Oettingen tal contrasting about attaining academic help (more
et al., 2001; Oettingen, Mayer, Thorpe, Janetzke, & than indulging and dwelling) led to expectancy-
Lorenz, 2005). Furthermore, strength of goal commit- dependent commitment to seek help (Study 1), while
ment was assessed by cognitive (e.g., making plans), for critical care nurses mental contrasting about help-
affective (e.g., feelings of frustration), motivational ing patients’ relatives led to expectancy-dependent
(e.g., feelings of energization), and behavioral (e.g., commitment to give help (Study 2). Thus, next to
amount of invested effort) indicators. These indica- regulating commitment to goals, mental contrasting
tors were measured via self-report or observations, also regulates the selection of appropriate means to
either directly after the experiment or weeks later. All achieve goals.
of these studies evidenced the same patterns of results: Goal commitment instilled by mental con-
Given high expectations of success, participants in the trasting equips people to successfully master nega-
mental-contrasting group showed the strongest goal tive feedback. A series of three studies (A. Kappes,
commitment; given low expectations, mental-con- Oettingen, & Pak, 2011; Oettingen & A. Kappes,
trasting participants showed least goal commitment. 2009) shows that mental contrasting regulates the
Participants who indulged in positive images about the mastery of negative feedback in three different ways.
future or dwelled on negative images of reality showed When expectations of success were high, mental
medium commitment no matter whether expecta- contrasting promoted the processing of relevant
tions of success were high or low. It is important to negative feedback, protected participants’ self-view
note that the outcomes of mental contrasting do not of competence against negative feedback, and led
occur as a result of changes in the level of expectations to optimistic as well as effort-related (rather than
(feasibility) or incentive valence (desirability) but ability-related) attributions in response to negative
rather as a result of the mode of self-regulatory thought feedback. Thus, mental contrasting can be used as an
(i.e., mental contrasting, indulging, dwelling), with effective strategy to strengthen goal commitment in
mental contrasting aligning strength of goal com- the sense that it prepares people to master upcoming
mitment to expectations. Furthermore, the effects of negative feedback.
mental contrasting depend on the person perceiving So far, we reported findings about mental con-
the present reality as an obstacle, that is, as standing trasting of a positive desired future with a negative
in the way of realizing the desired future (Oettingen present reality. However, mental contrasting does
et al., 2001, Study 3). Thus, when mentally contrast- not have to pertain to the attainment of a positive
ing, people need to first elaborate the desired future future; people can also fantasize about a negative
and only then reflect the present reality; the reverse future and contrast fantasies about a negative feared
order (reverse contrasting) fails to connect future and future with reflection on the positive present reality.
reality in the sense of the reality standing in the way of Oettingen, Mayer, Thorpe, Janetzke, and Lorenz
realizing the desired future outcome (Oettingen et al., (2005) observed in a group of xenophobic high
2001; A. Kappes & Oettingen, 2011). school students that when negative fantasies (i.e.,
The pattern of results, seen as a whole, shows that fears that social conflicts would arise from foreign
mental contrasting is a mode of thought that people youth moving into their neighborhood) are con-
can use to wisely regulate their goal pursuit. First, it trasted with reflections on a positive reality standing
helps people to build strong commitments to feasible in the way of the feared future (i.e., youth having
desired future outcomes (i.e., high expectations of wonderful and exciting soccer matches with for-
success); however, equally important, mental con- eigners), mental contrasting produced expectancy-
trasting also fosters disengagement from unfeasible dependent goal commitments as well (i.e., more
desired future outcomes (i.e., low expectations of tolerance and the goal of approaching the foreign-
success). Thereby mental contrasting allows people to ers by investing time and effort in welcoming them

 goal pursuit


into one’s neighborhood). Thus, mental contrast- Mediating processes of mental contrasting on
ing can be used to create approach goals that make goal commitment pertain also to motivational pro-
people successfully conquer a feared future. In addi- cesses. Oettingen et al. (2009) investigated energiza-
tion, Oettingen, Mayer, and Thorpe (2010) found tion as a primary indicator of motivational processes.
that mental contrasting can also be used to create Specifically, they found that mentally contrasting a
avoidance goals that make people successfully evade desired future with present reality leads to energi-
a feared future. In a study with chronic cigarette zation, which in turn creates goal commitments
smokers, they found that setting oneself the goal of strong enough to lead to effective goal striving and
avoiding the feared consequences of smoking can be successful goal attainment. Mediating effects of
facilitated by mentally contrasting the feared future energization on goal commitment are shown on
of negative health consequences with the current physiological indicators of energization (i.e., systolic
positive reality of still having a healthy body. blood pressure) as well as on experiential indicators
The mediating processes of mental contrasting (self-report of feeling energized). Mental contrast-
pertain to both cognitive and motivational processes. ing also spurs various forms of planning, a known
As for cognitive processes, mental contrasting modu- cognitive mediator between expectations of success
lates the strength of the association between future and goal attainment (Oettingen et al., 2001, 2005;
and reality and between reality and instrumental Oettingen & Stephens, 2009).
means. In a series of four studies employing a primed Finally, mental contrasting, as it is a problem-
lexical decision task to measure strength of associa- solving strategy, necessitates heightened cognitive
tion between future and reality, A. Kappes and Oet- activity. A recent experiment attesting to this idea
tingen (2011) observed that when expectations of used continuous magnetoencephalography (MEG),
successfully reaching a desired future were high, men- a brain imaging technique measuring magnetic
tal contrasting strengthened the association between fields produced by electrical activity in the brain
the desired future and the reality; when expectations (Achtziger, Fehr, Oettingen, Gollwitzer, & Rock-
were low, mental contrasting weakened future-reality stroh, 2009). Mental contrasting as compared to
associations. These results were obtained no matter indulging or simply resting produced heightened
whether expectations were measured or manipu- brain activity in areas associated with working
lated. Importantly, the future-reality associations in memory, episodic memory, intention maintenance,
turn mediated mental-contrasting effects on self- action preparation, and vivid visualization. That
reported (e.g., feelings of responsibility) and other- is, mental contrasting implies vividly imagining a
rated goal commitment (e.g., raters scored quality of desired future, anticipating hindrances to realizing
performance on giving a talk and solving a creativity this future, and making plans on how to overcome
test). Finally, mental-contrasting effects on future- these barriers. The brain activity associated with
reality associations vanished when participants were indulging, on the other hand, did not differ from
informed that the goal was achieved, implying that resting.
future-reality associations wax and wane with the Given this latter finding, one might think that
upholding versus accomplishment of the goal that indulging in the future could potentially lead to
was generated by mental contrasting. strong goal commitments as well—if only individu-
Mental contrasting not only links future and als managed to intensely engage in highly positive
reality but also connects present reality to relevant fantasies about the future. But research on engaging
instrumental means (i.e., means instrumental to in positive versus negative fantasies about the future
overcome or circumvent the present reality to attain speaks against this argument. Early on, Oettingen
the desired future). In two studies, A. Kappes, Sing- and Wadden (1991) observed that obese women who
man, and Oettingen (2011) showed that mental spontaneously indulge in positive fantasies about
contrasting paired with high expectations estab- their weight loss were less successful in achieving a
lished strong associations between present reality lower body mass index (after 4 months and 2 years)
and instrumental behavior, whereas paired with low than obese women whose spontaneously produced
expectations of success, it weakened reality-behavior fantasies were more negative. Moreover, Oettingen
associations. Importantly, the strength of the reality- and Mayer (2002) observed that people who indulge
behavior associations mediated goal commitment as in positive fantasies (valence and frequency) show
indicated by actual performance (e.g., performance comparatively weaker goal commitments (as assessed
of taking the stairs instead of the elevator to achieve by their efforts to strive for the goal) in the areas of
the goal of getting physically fit). academic achievement (i.e., achieving a good grade

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


in a psychology class), professional achievement (i.e., need then to move on and to engage in goal striv-
finding a job after graduation), interpersonal rela- ing. Whether a goal is ultimately attained depends
tions (i.e., finding a romantic partner), and health on how well this goal striving is executed. Success-
(i.e., recovering from hip surgery). Importantly, it ful goal striving depends first of all on what kind
did not matter whether the spontaneously produced of goals people have set for themselves; again, the
positive fantasies pertained to the desired outcome relevant variables are goal content and how this
or to the ways of getting there. Additionally, goal content is structured or framed. However, success-
commitment in these studies was assessed 2 weeks or ful goal striving also depends on coping effectively
even 2 years after the assessment of the spontaneously with a few typical problems: getting started with
produced positive future fantasies. More recently, the initiation of goal-directed actions, persisting in
H. B. Kappes and Oettingen (2011) investigated the the face of difficulties, shielding the goal from dis-
effects of experimentally induced positive fantasies tractions, disengaging from ineffective means, and
on energization, hypothesizing that low energy is a not overextending oneself. This self-regulatory issue
mechanism by which positive fantasies translate into of what people can do to make their goal striving
poor achievement. Indeed, induced positive fantasies more effective in the face of these problems (i.e., the
resulted in less energy (as measured by physiological effective self-regulation of goal striving) has recently
and behavioral indicators) than fantasies that ques- received much attention.
tioned the desired future, negative fantasies, or neu-
tral fantasies. Additionally, energy measured right Determinants of Goal Striving
after the induction of the positive fantasies medi- Goal content strongly affects the chances of
ated accomplishment in everyday life a week later. implementing a goal successfully. For instance, Ryan
Finally, positive fantasies yielded a larger decrease in et al. (1996) have argued that goals of autonomy,
energy when they pertained to a more rather than less competence, and social integration favor creativity,
pressing need (e.g., need achievement), further sug- cognitive flexibility, deep processing of information,
gesting that it is the positivity of fantasies that quells and effective coping with failure. These effects are
energization. Altogether the results indicate that one assumed to be mediated by an intrinsic self-regulation
reason positive fantasies predict poor achievement (see the self-concordance model by Sheldon & Elliot,
is because they sap energy required to pursue the 1999), as the needs of autonomy, competence, and
desired future. social integration are associated with intrinsic goal
At first sight, the reported findings seem to be striving in line with a person’s interests or core values,
in contrast to research observing facilitating effects rather than with extrinsic goal striving in line with
of positive affect on performance in executive func- environmental pressures or internal sanctions. Intrin-
tion tasks (Dreisbach & Goschke, 2004; Gable & sic goal striving is preferred by individuals with posi-
Harmon-Jones, 2008; Kazen & Kuhl, 2005). How- tive self-regard (Judge, Bono, Erez, & Locke, 2005),
ever, these facilitating effects evince for individuals and it can be facilitated from outside by teachers who
who perform tasks while being in a positive affec- provide autonomy support (e.g., when law school
tive state. Note that in the studies reported earlier, faculty provide autonomy support, grade point aver-
it is not positive affect per se that is measured or age improves; Sheldon & Krieger, 2007).
manipulated, but the positivity of fantasies that In addition to goal content, structural features
depict the person already having attained the speci- of set goals also affect whether goal striving is suc-
fied desired future. The mental experience of hav- cessful. For example, goal striving is said to depend
ing already reached the desired outcome and of on the strength of the goal (“I really want to reach
savoring the wished-for consequences reduces the goal x!”; Ajzen, 1991; Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980).
energy required to reach the outcome in actuality. But most tests of this goal-behavior relation involve
Only when such positive fantasies pertain to feasible only correlational studies that preclude causal infer-
futures and are mentally contrasted with the imped- ences. A recent meta-analysis by Webb and Sheeran
ing reality will people muster the energy to excel in (2006) took a closer look at this assumption by
actuality (Oettingen et al., 2009). selecting studies where the strength of the goal was
manipulated relative to a control group, and dif-
Goal Striving ferences in subsequent goal-directed behavior were
Once people have set themselves goals, it can- observed. They found 47 experimental tests of the
not be assumed that attaining the goal is inevitable; intention (goal)–behavior relation that actually used
rather, only the first step has been taken. People an experimental manipulation of the strength of

 goal pursuit


the goal (intention). The meta-analysis showed that approach goals benefit more from goal striving that
the medium-to-large change in strength of inten- makes use of eagerness-related approach strategies
tion (d = 0.66) led to a small-to-medium change in (such as pulling things toward oneself ) than from
respective behavior (d = 0.36). vigilance-related avoidance strategies (such as push-
But success in goal striving does not only depend ing things away from oneself ), whereas the reverse
on the strength of the goal; it also depends on what is true for avoidance goals. The assumed reason for
kind of aspiration or standard is specified in the this is value from fit. Higgins (2006) argues that
goal (i.e., whether the person wants to achieve a lot people engage more in goal striving when the strat-
or only a little). Locke and Latham (2002, 2006) egies used match the goal orientation (i.e., eager-
report that participants are more likely to attain ness strategies/positive outcome focus; vigilance
challenging goals that are spelled out in specific strategies/negative outcome focus) than when there
terms than moderately specific goals or challenging is a mismatch (i.e., vigilance strategies/positive out-
but vague goals (i.e., “do your best”). This effect has come focus; eagerness strategies/negative outcome
a number of prerequisites: frequent performance focus). This heightened engagement in turn leads to
feedback, strong goal commitment, low goal com- higher perceived value and strength of attraction to
plexity, and that the necessary skills and means are this outcome.
available to the individual. What does not seem to Framing goals in terms of learning versus perfor-
matter is whether goal setting is determined from mance has been found to have different effects on
outside (assigned goals), freely chosen by individu- achievement (Dweck, 1996). Learning goals lead to
als (self-set goals), or chosen in interaction with oth- better achievement than performance goals because
ers (participative goals). As potential mediators of the former allow for a more effective coping with
the goal-specificity effect, Locke and Latham point negative feedback than the latter. For people with
to heightened persistence, attention to the execu- performance goals, negative feedback signals fail-
tion of goal-directed behaviors, a greater readiness ure and lack of ability and thus causes them to give
to plan the goal pursuit, and to feedback and self- up prematurely. People with learning goals, on the
monitoring advantages. other hand, view negative feedback as setbacks and
Goal implementation is also affected by the struc- as valuable cues on how to focus on new strategies,
tural features of time frame and goal orientation ultimately furthering goal attainment. Elliot and
(i.e., approach vs. avoidance orientation, promotion Church (1997) observed that performance goals are
vs. prevention, learning versus performance orienta- less detrimental when they are framed as approach
tion, low versus high psychological distance, and low goals (e.g., I want to get good grades) rather than
versus high identity-relation). Note that the earlier avoidance goals (e.g., I do not want to get bad
discussion of goal setting pertained to what deter- grades). Recent studies by Darnon, Harackiewicz,
mines that a person sets goals with various structural Butera, Mugny, and Quiamzade (2007), however,
features. Here we asked the question of what kind of show that this is only true when the achievement
consequences choosing one or the other structural context does not allow for the emergence of fear of
framing has for successful goal attainment. failure (i.e., the task is easy; the feedback on one’s
Framing of the orientation of social goals in achievement is unambiguously positive). Recent
terms of approach versus avoidance clearly affects research on the framing of achievement goals in
their attainment. For instance, striving for the goal terms of learning versus performance has also inves-
of making new friends versus striving for the goal of tigated its influence on interactions in social achieve-
not being lonely produces quite different outcomes. ment situations (Poortvliet, Janssen, Van Yperen, &
With respect to the outcome variable of satisfaction Van de Vliert, 2007). People with learning goals are
with one’s social bonds versus loneliness, the latter oriented reciprocally. They give information openly,
leads to less favorable results than the former (Elliot, and they process received information with a focus
Gable, & Mapes, 2006; Strachman & Gable, 2006). on those pieces of information that fit well and add
Recent research suggests that these differences are value to their own chosen task strategy. On the other
mediated by differential attention and memory pro- hand, people with performance goals are oriented
cesses, differential interpretation and weighting of exploitatively. They provide information to others
available information, and differential evaluation of reluctantly, and they process received information
the progress made toward goal attainment. with a suspicious attitude that leads them to focus
Higgins (2000; Förster, Higgins, & Idson, 1998; on detecting and disregarding low-quality informa-
Shah, Higgins, & Friedman, 1998) reports that tion that might hurt their own task performance.

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


Another relevant structural feature is psychological affirming one’s general self-integrity or bolstering
distance. Liberman, Trope, McCrea, and Sherman one’s self-esteem are not sufficient to offset incom-
(2007) had research participants indicate either why pleteness regarding an identity goal; rather, one
or how another person would perform an activity must acquire specific identity symbols (Ledgerwood,
(e.g., open a bank account) and then ask them to Liviatan, & Carnevale, 2007).
guess when this person would enact the activity. Research on self-completion theory has discov-
As it turned out, why-construals of the activity ered that a higher level of completeness is reached
revealed longer time estimates than how-construals. when a social audience notices the individual’s self-
McCrea, Liberman, Trope, and Sherman (2008) symbolizing activities (Gollwitzer, 1986). In addi-
recently assessed actual enactment times of intended tion, incomplete individuals are more concerned
activity (i.e., returning a filled-out questionnaire to with finding an audience for their identity striv-
the experimenter on time) that was framed in terms ings than are completed individuals (Brunstein &
of high versus low psychological distance. Even Gollwitzer, 1996). This self-symbolizing, however,
though psychological distance was manipulated by has its costs. Self-symbolizing individuals see others
a variety of different methods, low psychological only in terms of the potential to notice their com-
distance led to earlier enactment of the intended pensatory efforts; thus, they lack social sensitivity
activity than high psychological distance. It appears, (Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985). Most interest-
then, that framing a goal in terms of high versus low ingly, when people make public their intention to
psychological distance engenders the risk of procras- acquire a certain self-definitional indicator (e.g.,
tinating about the goal pursuit. when a person who wants to become a great student
Finally, it matters whether a person frames a publicly utters the behavioral intention to enroll
given task goal in terms of its identity-relatedness. in an inspiring course), it turns out that actual
For instance, the task of solving a certain arithmetic efforts toward completion are reduced (Gollwitzer,
problem can be approached with the goal of solving Sheeran, Michalski, & Seifert, 2009). Apparently,
it effectively or the goal of identifying oneself as a when others take notice of a stated identity-relevant
mathematician. The latter goal has been referred to behavioral intention, the superordinate goal of
as a self-defining goal or identity goal, as it specifies claiming the identity is already reached, and thus
an identity as a desired end state. Self-completion performing the intended behavior becomes less
theory (Wicklund & Gollwitzer, 1982) proposes necessary. This finding is in line with results of ear-
that people who are committed to identity goals can lier self-completion studies; public, positive self-
undertake a variety of activities to claim identity- descriptions claiming the possession of an identity
goal attainment, because many different behaviors symbol produced the same sense of self-definitional
indicate the possession of such identities. For a sci- completeness as actual identity-relevant achieve-
entist, for example, such self-symbolizing activities ments (Brunstein & Gollwitzer, 1996; Gollwitzer,
might include engaging in professional duties (e.g., Wicklund, & Hilton, 1982).
giving lectures), making positive self-descriptions Striving for a given goal does not only depend on
(e.g., “I discovered a new principle!”), exerting the content of the goal and its structural features.
identity-relevant social influence (e.g., advising It also depends on the context in which the person
students), or acquiring respective skills, tools, and is situated. Recent research on context variables in
material symbols (e.g., programming skills, fast goal striving differentiates various context variables
computers, large office). that relate to the person’s affective state, the array of
Failing to perform an identity-relevant activ- competing action tendencies, and the power posi-
ity or lacking an identity symbol produces a state tion of the goal striver.
of incompleteness; to restore completeness, people For instance, Tice, Bratislavsky, and Baumeister
engage in self-symbolizing efforts (summary by (2001) focused on negative affect and observed
Gollwitzer & Kirchhoff, 1998). People then empha- that feeling emotionally distraught (i.e., having
size the possession of alternative symbols or set out been asked to imagine that one has caused a traf-
to acquire new identity symbols (e.g., engaging in fic accident that killed a child) makes it difficult to
identity-relevant activities, Brunstein & Gollwitzer, follow through with goals of not eating unhealthy
1996; describing oneself as having the required per- food or delaying gratification to attain better long-
sonality attributes, Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985; term rewards. Moreover, this emotionally negative
showing off relevant status symbols, Harmon-Jones, state also intensifies procrastination; for example,
Schmeichel, & Harmon-Jones, 2009). Importantly, people did not use the time provided to study for

 goal pursuit


an upcoming test. In all of these studies it appeared as people open up to competing goals; this effect
that the reason people did not act on their goals was occurs even when people are told merely to intend
simple; they felt that inaction would alleviate their to make progress in the future (Fishbach & Dhar,
negative emotional states. 2005). However, this negative effect of goal prog-
Positive affect, on the other hand, has been ress on goal shielding should only be expected if the
observed to facilitate goal striving. Kazen and Kuhl goal-directed actions taken (or intended) are inter-
(2005; Kuhl & Kazen, 1999) argue that even though preted by the individual as completing the goal;
decreases in positive affect make it easier to maintain if the action is instead interpreted as indicating a
an intention in working memory, it takes an increase strong commitment to the focal goal, then improved
in positive affect to facilitate the successful behav- goal shielding would be expected (Fishbach, Dhar,
ioral implementation of difficult intentions (e.g., to & Zhang, 2006; Koo & Fishbach, 2008). In line
do well on the Stroop task). Tamir and Robinson with this reasoning, Louro, Pieters, and Zeelenberg
(2007) report data suggesting that positive moods (2007) report on the basis of diary and experimen-
(measured or induced) are associated with selective tal studies that the effects of perceived proximity to
attention to reward stimuli. Gable and Harmon- the goal are moderated by the experience of posi-
Jones (2008) observed that positive affect induced tive or negative goal-related emotions. That is, when
by imagining rewards (such as tasty desserts) reduced the attainment of the focal goal is remote, positive
the breadth of attentional focus, which facilitates emotions promote goal shielding, whereas negative
focusing on specific action tendencies and thus tena- emotions hinder it; in this case, positive emotions
cious goal striving. Apparently, positive affect makes apparently indicate a feeling of high goal commit-
people focus on rewarding stimuli that in turn pro- ment. When closer to the goal, positive emotions
duce a narrowing of attentional focus that makes it decrease shielding of the focal goal, whereas nega-
easy to strive for the goal at hand. tive emotions prompt increased goal shielding; here,
Given that positive affect seems to foster goal positive emotions apparently indicate a feeling of
striving on well-structured tasks (e.g., Stroop and high goal attainment.
task-switching paradigms), this does not imply that Recently, researchers have focused on the con-
positive affect is beneficial to striving for all kinds of textual variable of being in a position of power
tasks. Complex and ill-defined tasks require that peo- versus being powerless. Power has been manipu-
ple anticipate potential obstacles and hindrances. This lated experimentally in several ways: Participants
is easier when people experience negative affect. Not remember an incident in which they had power
surprisingly then, for complex and ill-defined tasks over someone or someone had power over them,
positive affect was found to be a hindrance rather than they imagine or actually act in a powerful (manager,
a facilitator of goal attainment (Markman, Lindberg, evaluator) or a powerless role (subordinate, worker),
Kray, & Galinsky, 2007; Oettingen & Mayer, 2002; or power is primed outside of awareness (e.g., by
Taylor, Pham, Rivkin, & Armor, 1998). And people having participants perform a scrambled sentences
are found to prefer to be in negative emotional states if task using words related to having power—e.g.,
those states better facilitate goal striving; for instance, authority, dominate—or to lacking power—e.g.,
soldiers entering battle or football players during a subordinate, obey). These studies have shown that
game prefer an angry, aggressive (negative) mood not only do powerful (as compared to powerless)
rather than a relaxed, positive mood (Tamir, 2009). participants relate differently to people by treating
The success of goal striving in situational contexts them as a means to the attainment of their goals
that are filled with powerful distractions depends on (i.e., objectivation; Gruenfeld, Inesi, Magee, &
whether the individual is capable of shielding goal Galinsky, 2008); they also differ in the ways in
striving from these distractions. Accordingly, the which they strive for personal goals.
analysis of the determinants of effective goal shield- Guinote (2007) observed that people in power
ing has received much research attention recently. procrastinate less in pursuing their goals, they per-
For instance, Shah, Friedman, and Kruglanski sist longer in the face of difficulties, they show more
(2002) observed that high commitment to the willingness to try out different strategies to attain
focal goal facilitates goal shielding (measured in the goal, and they more readily seize good oppor-
terms of reduced accessibility of a competing goal), tunities to make goal-directed responses. In addi-
whereas feeling anxious and sad hinders it. More- tion, they more readily recognize whether a given
over, when people consider the progress they have situation can be used to serve their goals and then
made toward the goal there is less goal shielding, allow suitable situations to guide their behavior

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


(Guinote, 2008). All of this appears to be facilitated creating a strong link between a situational cue and
by a change in executive functioning. Smith, a goal-directed response.
Jostmann, Galinsky, and van Dijk (2008) report Implementation intentions have been found to
that powerful individuals are better than powerless help people close the gap between setting goals and
ones at updating goal-relevant information (i.e., actually realizing these goals. Evidence that forming
new information is monitored for goal relevance, if-then plans enhances rates of goal attainment and
and relevant information replaces old, irrelevant behavioral performance has now been obtained in
information in working memory). They are also many studies on a whole array of different goals.
better at inhibiting responses that may interfere A recent meta-analysis (Gollwitzer & Sheeran, 2006)
with the present goal, and at planning by continu- involving over 8,000 participants in 94 indepen-
ously switching between the main goal and respec- dent studies revealed a medium-to-large effect size
tive subgoals. Finally, these “powerful” participants (d = .65; Cohen, 1992) of implementation inten-
show less goal neglect (i.e., forgetting to strive for tions on goal achievement in a variety of domains
the goal; Kane & Engle, 2003) by actively maintain- (e.g., interpersonal, environmental, health) on top
ing the goal in working memory. Future research of the effects of mere goal intentions. This size of
could profitably explore the extent to which these the implementation intention effect is noteworthy,
effects are mediated by heightened efficacy beliefs or given that goal intentions by themselves already
control beliefs that may be stimulated by the power have a facilitating effect on behavior enactment
manipulations. (Webb & Sheeran, 2006).
Research on the underlying mechanisms of
Self-Regulation of Goal Striving implementation intention effects has discovered that
The earlier discussion considered goal content implementation intentions facilitate goal attain-
and structure, as well as contextual variables (e.g., ment on the basis of psychological mechanisms that
relative power) as determinants of successful goal relate to the anticipated situation (specified in the
striving and goal attainment. The self-regulation if-part of the plan), the intended behavior (speci-
approach to goal striving, on the other hand, focuses fied in the then-part of the plan), and the mental
on what the individual can do to master the prob- link forged between the if-part and the then-part
lems inherent in goal striving. One very powerful of the plan. Because forming an implementation
strategy is planning out goal striving in advance. intention implies the selection of a critical future
Gollwitzer (1993, 1999) has proposed a distinction situation, the mental representation of this situation
between goal intentions and implementation inten- becomes highly activated and hence more accessible
tions. Goal intentions (goals) have the structure of (Gollwitzer, 1999). This heightened accessibility of
“I intend to reach Z!” whereby Z may relate to a the if-part of the plan has been observed in several
certain outcome or behavior to which the individual studies testing this hypothesis by using different
feels committed. Implementation intentions (plans) experimental paradigms: for example, lexical deci-
have the structure of “If situation X is encountered, sion tasks, Webb and Sheeran (2004), Parks-Stamm,
then I will perform the goal-directed response Y!” Gollwitzer, and Oettingen (2007); dichotic-listening
Both goal and implementation intentions are set paradigm, Achtziger, Bayer, and Gollwitzer (in press;
in an act of will: The former specifies the intention Study 1); and cued recall (Achtziger et al., in press,
to meet a goal or standard; the latter refers to the Study 2). There are even some studies showing that
intention to perform a plan. For instance, a possible the heightened accessibility of the mental repre-
implementation intention for the goal intention to sentation of critical cues as specified in an imple-
eat healthy food could link a suitable situational mentation intention mediates the attainment of the
context (e.g., one’s order is taken at a restaurant) respective goal intention (e.g., Aarts, Dijksterhuis,
to an appropriate behavior (e.g., asking for a low- & Midden, 1999). More recent studies indicate that
fat meal). Whereas goal intentions merely specify forming implementation intentions not only height-
desired end states (“I want to achieve goal X!”), the ens the activation (and thus the accessibility) of the
if-component of an implementation intention spec- mental presentation of the situational cues specified
ifies when and where one wants to act on this goal, in the if-component, it also forges a strong associa-
and the then-component of the plan specifies how tive link between the mental representation of the
this will be done. Implementation intentions thus specified opportunity and the mental representation
delegate control over the initiation of the intended of the specified response (Webb & Sheeran, 2007,
goal-directed behavior to a specified opportunity by 2008). These associative links seem to be quite

 goal pursuit


stable over time (Papies, Aarts, & de Vries, 2009), and bottom-up (stimulus) control of action, whereas
they allow for priming the mental representation of brain activity in the former area is known to be
the specified response (the plan’s then-component) related to top-down (goal) control of action (Burgess,
by subliminal presentation of the specified critical Simons, Dumontheil, & Gilbert, 2005).
situational cue (if-component) (Webb & Sheeran, Support for the delegation hypothesis also comes
2007). Moreover, mediation analyses suggest that from studies using critical samples—that is, indi-
cue accessibility and the strength of the cue-response viduals with poor self-regulatory abilities such as
link together mediate the impact of implementation people with schizophrenia and people with sub-
intention formation on goal attainment (Webb & stance abuse disorders (Brandstätter et al., 2001,
Sheeran, 2007, 2008). Studies 1 & 2), people with frontal lobe damage
Gollwitzer (1999) suggests that the upshot of the (Lengfelder & Gollwitzer, 2001), and children with
strong associative (critical situation—goal-directed attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD;
response) links created by forming implementation Gawrilow & Gollwitzer, 2008, Paul et al., 2007).
intentions is that—once the critical cue is encoun- For instance, Brandstätter et al. (2001, Study 1)
tered—the initiation of the goal-directed response assigned hospitalized opiate addicts under with-
specified in the then-component of the implemen- drawal the goal to write a short CV before the end
tation intention exhibits features of automaticity, of the day; half of the participants formed relevant
including immediacy, efficiency, uncontrollabil- implementation intentions (they specified when
ity, and redundancy of conscious intent. Evidence and where they would start to write what), and the
that if-then planners act quickly (Gollwitzer & other half (control group) formed irrelevant imple-
Brandstätter, 1997, Experiment 3), deal effectively mentation intentions (when and where they would
with cognitive demands (i.e., speed up effects still eat what for lunch). Eighty percent of the relevant
evidence under high cognitive load; Brandstätter, implementation intention participants had written
Lengfelder, & Gollwitzer, 2001), show uncontrolled a short CV at the end of the day, whereas none of
attention to the specified cues (Wieber & Sassenberg, the participants with the irrelevant implementation
2006), and do not need to consciously intend to act intention succeeded in doing so.
in the critical moment is consistent with this idea Implementation intentions have also been found
(i.e., implementation intention effects are observed to benefit children with ADHD who are known to
even when the critical cue is presented subliminally; have difficulties with tasks that require response inhi-
Bayer, Achtziger, Gollwitzer, & Moskowitz, 2009). bition (e.g., Go/NoGo tasks). For example, it was
The postulated and observed component pro- observed that the response inhibition performance
cesses underlying implementation intention effects in the presence of stop signals can be improved in
(enhanced cue accessibility, strong cue-response children with ADHD by forming implementation
links, automation of responding) mean that if-then intentions (Gawrilow & Gollwitzer, 2008, Studies 1
planning allows people to see and seize good oppor- & 2). This improved response inhibition is reflected
tunities to move toward their goals. Fashioning an in electrocortical data as well (Paul et al., 2007).
if-then plan thus strategically automates goal striv- Typically, the P300 component evoked by NoGo
ing; people intentionally make if-then plans that stimuli has greater amplitude than the P300 evoked
delegate control of goal-directed behavior to pre- by Go stimuli. This difference is less pronounced
selected situational cues with the explicit purpose in children with ADHD. Paul et al. (2007) found
of reaching their goals. This delegation hypothesis that if-then plans improved response inhibition and
has recently been tested by in a functional magnetic increased the P300 difference (NoGo—Go) in chil-
resonance imaging (fMRI) study reported by Gil- dren with ADHD. Recently, Gawrilow, Gollwitzer,
bert, Gollwitzer, Cohen, Oettingen, and Burgess and Oettingen (2011a) observed that children with
(2009). In this study, participants had to perform ADHD can use implementation intentions to sup-
a prospective memory task on the basis of either port executive functions in addition to inhibition
goal or implementation intention instructions. Act- (i.e., set shifting and working memory).
ing on the basis of goal intentions was associated Additional process mechanisms to the stimulus
with brain activity in the lateral rostral prefrontal perception and response initiation processes docu-
cortex, whereas acting on the basis of implementa- mented in the findings described earlier have been
tion intentions was associated with brain activity in explored. For instance, furnishing goals with imple-
the medial rostral prefrontal cortex. Brain activity mentation intentions might produce an increase
in the latter area is known to be associated with in goal commitment or self-efficacy, which in turn

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


cause heightened goal attainment. However, this and how one wanted to get started on this project
hypothesis has not received any empirical support. were about three times as likely to actually write the
A recent meta-analysis on 66 implementation inten- report than mere goal intention participants. Other
tion studies that assessed goal commitment or self- studies found that implementation intentions foster
efficacy after the formation of if-then plans revealed striving toward goals involving behaviors that are
negligible effects on both of these variables (Webb somewhat unpleasant to perform (e.g., to recycle,
& Sheeran, 2008); accordingly, neither an increase Holland, Aarts, & Langendam, 2006; and to engage
in goal commitment nor self-efficacy qualifies as a in physical exercise, Milne, Orbell, & Sheeran,
potential mediator of implementation intention 2002), even though there is an initial reluctance to
effects. Additionally, having to furnish their goals execute these behaviors. Moreover, implementation
with implementation intentions may suggest to intentions were associated with goal attainment in
research participants that the experimenter wants domains where it is easy to forget to act (e.g., regu-
them to do well on the goal at hand. However, lar intake of vitamin pills, Sheeran & Orbell, 1999;
when experimenter demand was checked in studies attendance for cervical cancer screening, Sheeran
assigning goals versus implementation intentions, & Orbell, 2000; the signing of work sheets by the
participants who performed the task goals at hand elderly, Chasteen, Park, & Schwarz, 2001).
under these different instructions did not in felt But many goals cannot be accomplished by a
experimenter demand (e.g., Schweiger Gallo, Keil, simple discrete one-shot action as they require that
McCulloch, Rockstroh, & Gollwitzer, 2009; Kirk, people keep striving over an extended period of time.
Gollwitzer, & Carnevale, 2011). Finally, one might Such staying on track may become very difficult
argue that implementation intentions have positive when certain internal stimuli (e.g., being anxious,
effects on goal attainment because they provide extra tired, overburdened) or external stimuli (e.g., temp-
strategy knowledge. In fact, several studies have criti- tations, distractions) interfere with ongoing goal
cally tested this idea by adding to the design a further pursuit. Implementation intentions can prevent the
goal condition in which the critical strategy informa- negative influence of interferences from outside the
tion was provided as well. However, this condition person (e.g., disruptions by attractive video shows;
never showed the beneficial effects on goal attain- Gollwitzer & Schaal, 1998). For this purpose,
ment observed in the respective implementation implementation intentions may take very differ-
intention condition (e.g., Oettingen et al., 2000; ent forms. For instance, if a person wants to avoid
Palayiwa, Sheeran, & Thompson, 2010; Webb, being unfriendly to a friend who is known to make
Ononaiye, Sheeran, Reidy, & Lavda, 2010). outrageous requests, she can form implementation
Research on the facilitating effects of forming intentions such as: “And if my friend approaches me
implementation intentions on goal attainment has with an outrageous request, then I will not respond
targeted all of the four major problems that are in an unfriendly manner!” The then-component of
known to doom effective goal striving: getting suppression-oriented implementation intentions
started, staying on track, failing to call a halt to futile does not have to be worded in terms of not showing
goal striving, and overextending oneself. Given that the critical behavior; it may also specify an alterna-
forming implementation intentions automates goal tive antagonistic behavior (“. . . , then I will respond
striving, people who form implementation inten- in a friendly manner!”) or focus on ignoring the crit-
tions should actually have it easier when they are ical cue (“. . . , then I’ll ignore it!”). Recent research
confronted with these four central problems of goal suggests that the negation implementation inten-
implementation. Indeed, numerous studies suggest tion (“. . . , then I will not respond in an unfriendly
that problems of getting started on one’s goals can manner”) is the least effective as it is associated with
be solved effectively by forming implementation an ironic activation of the mental representation of
intentions. For instance, Gollwitzer and Brandstätter the unwanted behavior (Adriaanse, Van Oosten,
(1997, Study 2) analyzed a goal intention (i.e., De Ridder, De Wit, & Evers, 2011). Interestingly,
writing a report about how the participants spent implementation intentions can be used to curb
Christmas Eve) that had to be performed at a time the negative effects not only of interfering external
when people are commonly busy with other things events but also of interfering inner states. Achtziger,
(i.e., during the subsequent 2 days which are family Gollwitzer, and Sheeran (2008), for instance, report
holidays in Europe). Still, research participants who two field experiments concerned with dieting (i.e.,
had furnished their goal intention with an imple- reduce snacking; Study 1) and athletic goals (i.e.,
mentation intention that specified when, where, win a competitive tennis match; Study 2) in which

 goal pursuit


goals were shielded by implementation intentions goals that turn out to be faulty because of a strong
geared toward controlling potentially interfering self-justification motive (i.e., we tend to adhere to the
inner states (i.e., cravings for junk food in Study 1, irrational belief that decisions we have made deliber-
and disruptive thoughts, feelings, and physiological ately must be good; Brockner, 1992). Such escalation
states in Study 2). effects of sticking with a chosen means or goal even
An alternative way of using implementation if negative feedback on goal progress amounts and
intentions to protect ongoing goal striving from alternative means and goals are available are reduced
derailment is to form implementation intentions effectively, however, by the use of implementation
geared toward stabilizing the ongoing goal pursuit at intentions. These implementation intentions only
hand (Bayer, Gollwitzer, & Achtziger, 2010). Using have to specify receiving negative feedback as the criti-
again the example of a person who is approached by cal cue in the if-component and switching to available
her friend with an outrageous request, let us assume alternative means or goals as the appropriate response
that the person who is the recipient of the request in the then-component (Henderson, Gollwitzer, &
is tired or irritated and thus particularly likely to Oettingen, 2007).
respond in an unfriendly manner. If this person has Finally, the assumption that implementation
stipulated in advance in an implementation inten- intentions subject behavior to the direct control of
tion what she will converse about with her friend, situational cues (i.e., strategic automation of goal
the interaction may come off as planned, and being striving; Gollwitzer, 1999) implies that the person
tired or irritated should fail to affect the person’s does not have to exert deliberate effort when behav-
behavior toward her friend. Bayer et al. (2009) ior is controlled via implementation intentions. As
tested this hypothesis in a series of experiments in a consequence, the self should not become depleted
which participants were asked to make plans (i.e., (Muraven & Baumeister, 2000) when task perfor-
form implementation intentions) or not, regard- mance is regulated by implementation intentions,
ing their performance on an assigned task. Prior and thus for individuals using implementation
to beginning the task, participants’ self-states were intentions, not overextending themselves should
manipulated, so that the task at hand became more become easier. Indeed, using different ego depletion
difficult (e.g., a state of self-definitional incomplete- paradigms, research participants who used imple-
ness prior to a task that required perspective taking; mentation intentions to self-regulate in one task do
Gollwitzer & Wicklund, 1985; a good mood prior not show reduced self-regulatory capacity in a sub-
to a task that required evaluation of others nonste- sequent task (e.g., Webb & Sheeran, 2003).
reotypically; Bless & Fiedler, 1995; and a state of
ego depletion prior to solving difficult anagrams; lack of willpower
Baumeister, 2000; Muraven, Tice, & Baumeister, A new line of research on implementation inten-
1998). The results suggested that the induced criti- tions has been stimulated by Aristotle’s concept of
cal self-states negatively affected task performance akrasia (lack of willpower). It is argued that any
only for those participants who had not planned out willful strategy of goal striving (such as if-then plan-
work on the task at hand via implementation inten- ning) has to prove itself under conditions where
tions (i.e., had only set themselves the goal to come people commonly fail to demonstrate willpower.
up with a great performance). Apparently, task per- Three such conditions have been analyzed so far:
formance (i.e., taking the perspective of another (a) situations in which a person’s knowledge and
person, judging people in a nonstereotypical man- skills constrain performance such as taking aca-
ner, solving difficult anagrams) does not suffer any demic tests, (b) situations in which an opponent’s
impairment because of the respective detrimental behavior limits one’s performance such as negotia-
self-states (e.g., self-definitional incompleteness, tion settings, and (c) situations in which the wanted
mood, and ego depletion) if performing these tasks behavior (e.g., no littering) runs into conflict with
has been planned out in advance via implementa- habits favoring an antagonistic response.
tion intentions. The litmus test for any strategy to improve will-
The self-regulatory problem of calling a halt to a power is enhanced performance in a delay of grati-
futile goal striving (i.e., disengaging from a chosen but fication task. Children with impulse control deficits
noninstrumental means or from a chosen goal that has (i.e., children with ADHD) are known to have
become unfeasible or undesirable) can also be amelio- particularly pronounced problems with delaying
rated by forming implementation intentions. People gratifications. Accordingly, Gawrilow, Gollwitzer,
often fail to readily disengage from chosen means and and Oettingen (2011b) analyzed whether delay of

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


gratification can be facilitated by forming imple- research, Trötschel and Gollwitzer (2007) tar-
mentation intentions even in children with ADHD. geted the sharing of a common good and explored
A computer task was developed in line with the whether the self-regulation strategy of forming
delay of gratification paradigms developed by Walter implementation intentions enables negotiators to
Mischel (1974) and Sonuga-Barke (2002)—waiting find agreements even if they have to operate under
in the presence of a suboptimal cue to make money the adverse conditions of a loss frame (i.e., partici-
for a delayed optimal cue to make money led to a pants see how many points they lose rather than win
higher total amount of money earned. In two stud- and thus they are reluctant to make concessions;
ies it was observed that the goal intention to do e.g., Bottom & Studt, 1993). When looking at the
well on the task did not improve performance as agreements achieved (i.e., level of joint outcomes),
compared to a control group that received mere task it was observed that pairs of loss-frame negotiators
instructions specifying the reward contingencies. with a prosocial goal intention managed to some-
However, when the goal intention was furnished what reduce the resistance to concession making
with an implementation intention that linked a arising from the loss-frame negotiation context,
waiting response to the suboptimal cue, a signifi- but that only negotiators who furnished their proso-
cantly higher amount of money was earned, indicat- cial goal intentions with respective implementation
ing a heightened ability to delay gratification. intentions were successful in completely abolishing
Willpower is also called for when working the negative impact of the loss-frame negotiation
on academic performance tests (math tests, general context (i.e., showed a negotiation performance
intelligence tests) as a good performance is com- that was not different from that of gain-frame nego-
monly not only determined by a person’s knowl- tiators). In addition, action control via implementa-
edge, analytic capability, and cognitive skills but also tion intentions was found to be very efficient (i.e.,
by a person’s motivation to do well as a consequence implementation intentions abolished the negative
of perceived desirability and feasibility of success- effects of loss framing by leaving the negotiators’ cog-
ful test performance. To increase test scores on the nitive capacity intact); negotiators who had formed
spot by exerting willpower, a person may thus focus implementation intentions were more likely to use
on holding up her motivation (e.g., by increas- the cognitively demanding integrative negotiation
ing her self-efficacy feelings). Accordingly, Bayer strategy of logrolling (i.e., making greater conces-
and Gollwitzer (2007, Study 2) tested whether it sions on low rather than high priority issues). More
is possible to increase self-efficacy beliefs by form- recent negotiation research by Kirk, Gollwitzer,
ing implementation intentions. They asked college and Carnevale (2011) used a different negotiation
students to take the Raven Intelligence Test: One task: the ultimatum game. The participants acted as
group of participants formed a mere goal intention receivers of a series of fair but also unfair offers. It is
to do well (“I will correctly solve as many test items commonly found that impulsive anger in response
as possible!”), whereas the implementation inten- to unfair offers leads to rejections—and in turn to
tion group added the following if-then plan: “And a financial cost to the receiver. It was found that
whenever I start a new test item, then I’ll tell myself: entering the ultimatum game with goals to make
I can solve it!” Participants in the implementation a personal profit only then curbed impulsive rejec-
intention condition performed better than those tions by increasing the frequency of accepting unfair
in the mere goal intention to perform well condi- offers when these goals were furnished with respec-
tion; implementation intention participants also tive implementation intentions.
performed better than participants in a further con- The self-regulation of one’s goal striving becomes
dition where a self-efficacy strengthening goal inten- particularly difficult when habitual responses conflict
tion had to be performed (“I will tell myself: I can do with initiating and executing the needed goal-directed
these test items!”). responses instrumental to goal attainment (e.g.,
Often our goals are constrained by others Wood & Neal, 2007). In such cases, showing will-
who are competing with us for positive outcomes power means asserting one’s will to attain the chosen
or have competing goals for the use of the situa- goal against unwanted habitual responses. But can the
tion at hand. In such competitive situations exerting self-regulation strategy of forming if-then plans help
willpower involves effectively protecting one’s goal people to let their goals win out over their habitual
striving from the unwanted influences generated responses? By assuming that action control by imple-
by the goals of others (e.g., Martin, Sheeran, Slade, mentation intentions is immediate and efficient, and
Wright, & Dibble, 2009). In their negotiation adopting a simple horse race model of action control

 goal pursuit


(Gurney, Prescott, & Redgrave, 2001a, b), people cue (in the present case, a spider picture) was encoun-
might be in a position to break habitualized responses tered, so that—using the horse race metaphor—the
by forming implementation intentions (e.g., by planned response (i.e., ignore response) could out-
forming if-then plans that spell out a response that run the habitual response (i.e., fear response).
is contrary to the habitualized response to the critical Various studies have analyzed the control of
situation; Holland, Aarts, & Langendam, 2006). habitual behavioral responses. For instance, Cohen,
Research on the control of automatic responses Bayer, Jaudas, and Gollwitzer (2008, Study 2; see
by implementation intentions has targeted cogni- also Miles & Proctor, 2008) explored the suppres-
tive, affective, and behavioral responses. With respect sion of habitual responses by implementation inten-
to cognitive responses it has been shown that auto- tions using the Simon task. In this task paradigm,
matic cognitive biases such as stereotyping can by participants are asked to respond to a nonspatial
successfully controlled by forming implementation aspect of a stimulus (i.e., whether a presented tone
intentions. Extending earlier work by Gollwitzer is high or low) by pressing a left or right key, and to
and Schaal (1998), Stewart and Payne (2008) exam- ignore the location of the stimulus (i.e., whether it
ined whether implementation intentions designed is presented on one’s left or right side). The difficulty
to counter automatic stereotypes (e.g., “When I see of this task is in ignoring the spatial location (left or
a black face, I will then think ‘safe’ ”) could reduce right) of the tone in one’s classification response (i.e.,
stereotyping toward a category of individuals (versus pressing a left or right response key; Simon, 1990).
a single exemplar). The authors used the Process Dis- The cost in reaction times is seen when the location
sociation Procedure (PDP; Jacoby, 1991) to estimate of the tone (e.g., right) and required key press (e.g.,
whether the reduction in automatic stereotyping came left) are incongruent, as people habitually respond
about by reducing automatic stereotyping, increasing to stimuli presented at the right or left side with the
control, or a combination of these two processes. It corresponding hand. Cohen et al. (2008, Study 2)
was found that implementation intentions reduced found that implementation intentions eliminated
stereotyping in a weapon identification task (Stud- the Simon effect for the stimulus that was specified
ies 1 and 2) and an IAT task (Study 3) by reducing in the if-component of the implementation inten-
automatic effects of the stereotype (without increas- tion. Reaction times for this stimulus did not differ
ing conscious control). This reduction in automatic between the congruent and incongruent trials (i.e.,
race bias held for even new members of the category they were fast throughout).
(Study 2). These studies suggest that implementation Further studies on the control of habitual
intentions are an efficient way to overcome automatic behavioral responses by implementation intentions
stereotyping. analyzed reducing the behavioral expression of stereo-
Schweiger Gallo, Keil, McCulloch, Rockstroh, typical bias (using the shooter paradigm; Mendoza,
and Gollwitzer (2009, Study 3) analyzed whether Gollwitzer, & Amodio, 2010), abolishing concept
it is possible to curb habitual affective responses by and goal priming effects on behavior (using differ-
forming implementation intentions. They found ent concept and goal priming methods; Gollwitzer,
that implementation intentions specifying an ignore Sheeran, Trötschel, & Webb, 2011), stopping over-
response in the then-component helped control fear learned responses to critical stimuli (using the stop
in response to pictures of spiders in participants with signal task in children with ADHD, Gawrilow &
spider phobia—to the low level that was experi- Gollwitzer, 2008), and breaking bad eating habits
enced by participants who did not report any spider (using a lexical decision task presenting the unwanted
phobia. The obtained electrocortical correlates (the food item as the critical word; Adriaanse, Gollwitzer,
authors had used dense-array EEG) revealed that De Ridder, De Wit, & Kroese, 2011).
those participants who bolstered their goal intention Still, one wonders whether forming implementa-
to stay calm with an ignore-implementation inten- tion intentions will always block habitual responses.
tion showed significantly reduced early activity in Using a horse race metaphor, the answer has to be
the visual cortex in response to spider pictures, as no. Whether the habitual response or the if-then
reflected in a smaller P1 (assessed at 120 milliseconds guided response will win the race depends on the
[msec] after a spider picture was presented). This relative strength of the two behavioral orientations.
suggests that the ignore-implementation intention If the habitual response is based on strong habits
assigned to spider phobics lead to a strategic automa- (Webb, Sheeran, & Luszczynska, 2009) and the
tion of the specified goal-directed response (in the if-then guided response is based on weak imple-
present case, an ignore response) when the critical mentation intentions, then the habitual response

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


should win over the if-then planned response; and of respective if-then plans. This is in line with find-
the reverse should be true when weak habits are sent ings that the induction of the distinct emotion of
into a race with strong implementation intentions. sadness leads to weaker implementation intention
This implies that controlling behavior that is based effects as compared to the induction of the distinct
on strong habits requires the formation of strong emotion of anger (Maglio, Gollwitzer, & Oettingen,
implementation intentions. One effective strategy in press).
pertains to creating particularly strong links between Not surprisingly, it was found that the commit-
situational cues (if-component) and goal-directed ment to the formed implementation intention also
responses (then-component). Knäuper, Roseman, needs to be strong to produce beneficial effects of if-
Johnson, and Krantz (2009; see also Papies, Aarts, then planning (e.g., Achtziger, Bayer, & Gollwitzer,
& de Vries, 2009) asked participants to use men- 2010, Study 2). When participants doubted the
tal imagery when linking situational cues to goal- appropriateness of forming implementation inten-
directed responses in their if-then plans, and found tions, no implementation intention effects emerged.
that the rate of initiation of the planned response Additionally, people should find it easier to commit
increased by almost 50%. Finally, using the if-then to if-then plans that specify feasible (i.e., high self-
format for spelling out one’s implementation inten- efficacy feelings) and desirable (i.e., high instrumen-
tions benefits their effectiveness. Chapman, Armit- tality beliefs; the intrinsic value or activity incentive
age, and Norman (2009) observed that for the is perceived as high, Koestner et al., 2006) responses
goal to increase one’s fruit and vegetable intake an in their then-part. In any case, the requirement of
if-then implementation intention had greater impact commitment to the if-then plan for implementation
than an implementation intention that settled with intentions to have an effect ensures that incidental
simply listing the when, where, and how of acting if-then plans do not impair flexibility in striving
toward the goal. for goal attainment (e.g., Gollwitzer, Parks-Stamm,
What else strengthens (or weakens) the effects of Jaudas, & Sheeran, 2008).
implementation intentions? For strong implemen- Finally, personality attributes have been exam-
tation intention effects to occur people need to be ined as moderators of implementation intention
highly committed to the superordinate goal intention effects in two lines of research (Powers, Koestner,
(e.g., Gollwitzer 1999; De Nooijer, De Vet, Brug, & Topciu, 2005; Webb, Christian, & Armitage,
& De Vries, 2006; Orbell, Hodgkins, & Sheeran, 2007). In the first set of studies (Powers et al., 2005),
1997; Sheeran, Webb, & Gollwitzer, 2005, Study perfectionism was examined whereby self-oriented
1; Verplanken & Faes, 1999), which is facilitated perfectionism was distinguished from socially pre-
when this goal is self-concordant (Koestner, Lekes, scribed perfectionism. Whereas the standards for
Powers, & Chicoine, 2002) and the self-efficacy to self-oriented perfectionists are set by the people
reach the goal is high (Koestner et al., 2006, Study themselves, socially prescribed perfectionists try to
2; Wieber, Odenthal, & Gollwitzer, 2010); further- conform to standards and expectations that are pre-
more, the goal needs to be in a state of activation scribed by others. Powers et al. assessed goal progress
(Sheeran et al., 2005, Study 2). These prerequisites with respect to New Year’s resolutions (i.e., three
help flexible goal striving because they prevent rigid personal goals) in participants who formed imple-
plan enactment even when people have reached the mentation intentions as compared to participants
goal, disengaged from the goal, or are in a situation who received control instructions only. Whereas for
where striving for the goal is inappropriate; in other participants being high on self-oriented perfection-
words, the automaticity achieved by implementa- ism, forming implementation intentions actually
tion intentions is a goal-dependent automaticity did improve goal progress, social perfectionists failed
(Bargh, 1989). There may, however, be a cost to this to benefit from implementation intentions. Perhaps
flexibility associated with goal dependency: Recent social perfectionists find it difficult to commit to
research by Wieber et al. (2011) finds that people implementation intentions, as they may feel that
who have formed implementation intentions for the the expectations and standards prescribed by others
goal of eating more healthy food, but subsequently often change unexpectedly, and flexibly responding
are induced to reflect on the reasons for striving to to such changes may be seen as impossible when one
reach this goal (such as, e.g., a better health, a more incurs a strong commitment to a given if-then plan.
beautiful body) no longer benefit from their if-then In the second line of research on relevant personal
plans. Apparently, any doubts about the reasons for attributes for implementation intention effects,
striving for a goal can undermine the effectiveness conscientiousness was examined (Webb, Christian,

 goal pursuit


& Armitage, 2007). In an experimental study using commitments (Oettingen et al., 2001). Implemen-
undergraduate students, the goal of regularly attend- tation intentions are also found to show enhanced
ing class was studied as a function of conscientious- benefits when the specification of the if-component
ness and implementation intentions. Whereas class is personalized (Adriaanse, De Ridder, & De Wit,
attendance of highly conscientious students was not 2009), and mental contrasting guarantees the iden-
changed by forming implementation intentions (as tification of personal critical obstacles that can then
it was high to begin with and stayed high), low and used as the critical situation in the if-component of
moderately conscientious people significantly ben- an implementation intention.
efited from forming implementation intentions. Indeed, in a recent intervention study with
If one assumes that being on time is easy for people middle-aged women (Stadler, Oettingen, & Goll-
with high conscientiousness but difficult for people witzer, 2009), participants were taught the cogni-
who are low on this personal attribute, this finding is tive principles and individual steps of the MCII
in line with the general observation that it is in par- self-regulation strategy. This intervention allowed
ticular the difficult goals that benefit from forming participants to apply MCII to their idiosyncratic
implementation intentions (Gollwitzer & Sheeran, everyday wishes and concerns. Specifically, par-
2006). Apparently, when goal striving is easy the ticipants were taught to apply MCII by themselves
help of the self-regulation strategy of forming imple- to the wish of exercising more whenever possible.
mentation intentions is not needed; easy goals can Hence, MCII is referred to as a metacognitive self-
be striven for effectively without having to prepare regulation strategy. Participants were free to choose
goal striving by forming implementation inten- whatever form of exercising they wished, and they
tions (e.g., Wieber, Odenthal, & Gollwitzer, 2010; were encouraged to anticipate exactly those obsta-
Wieber, von Suchodoletz, Heikamp, Trommsdorff, cles that were personally most relevant and to link
& Gollwitzer, 2011). Alternatively, it seems possible them to exactly those goal-directed responses that
that highly conscientious people routinely form personally appeared to be most instrumental. As
implementation intentions by themselves to live up dependent measures, participants maintained daily
to their high standards of self-control (Gollwitzer & behavioral diaries to keep track of the amount of
Brandstätter, 1997, Study 1). time they exercised every day. Overall, teaching the
MCII technique enhanced exercise more than the
Future Goal Research: Interventions information-only control intervention; this effect
How can the research on goal setting and goal showed up immediately after the interventions and
striving reported earlier be used to help people it stayed stable throughout the entire period of the
wisely select their goals and then meet them? First, study (16 weeks after the intervention). More spe-
knowledge about the determinants about effective cifically, participants in the MCII group exercised
goal setting and goal striving allows one to establish nearly twice as much: an average of 1 hour more
these determinants, and this can be done by the indi- per week than participants in the information-only
viduals themselves or people (e.g., parents, teach- control group.
ers, instructors) who want to help others in their Conducting the same MCII intervention to pro-
goal setting and goal striving. Second, knowledge mote healthy eating in middle-aged women (i.e.,
about effective strategies of goal setting and goal eating more fruits and vegetables) also produced the
striving allows one to construct interventions that desired behavior change effects, and these persisted
teach people how to effectively set and implement even over the extensive time period of 2 years (Stadler,
goals by themselves. One such intervention (devel- Oettingen, & Gollwitzer, 2010). In another study,
oped by Oettingen and her colleagues: Oettingen Adriaanse, Oettingen, Gollwitzer, et al. (2010) tar-
& Gollwitzer, 2010; Oettingen & Stephens, 2009; geted the negative eating habit of unhealthy snacking
Adriaanse et al., 2010; Stadler, Oettingen, & Goll- in college students. MCII worked for both students
witzer, 2009, 2010) combines mental contrasting with weak and strong such habits, and notably, it
with forming implementation intentions into one was more effective than mental contrasting or for-
metacognitive strategy called MCII (i.e., Mental mulating implementation intentions alone. More-
Contrasting with Implementation Intentions). over, MCII was observed to benefit chronic back
To unfold their beneficial effects, implementation pain patients in increasing their health behaviors
intentions require that strong goal commitments (Christiansen, Oettingen, Dahme, & Klinger, 2010).
are in place (Sheeran, Webb, & Gollwitzer, 2005, Over a period of both 3 weeks and 3 months patients
Study 1), and mental contrasting creates such strong increased their exercise as compared to a standard

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


treatment control group. Exercise was measured by to change one’s diet work best when tailored to personally
objective (i.e., bicycle ergometer test and number of relevant reasons for unhealthy eating. Personality and Social
Psychology Bulletin, 35, 60–71.
lifts achieved in 2 minutes) and subjective indica- Adriaanse, M. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., De Ridder, D. T. D.,
tors (reported physical functioning). Finally, MCII De Wit, J. B. F., & Kroese, F. M. (2011). Breaking habits with
has shown beneficial effects outside the health area. implementation intentions: A test of underlying processes.
For example, it benefited study efforts in adolescents Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 502–513.
preparing for standardized tests (Duckworth, Grant, Adriaanse, M. A., Oettingen, G., Gollwitzer, P. M., Hennes, E. P.,
De Ridder, D. T. D., & De Wit, J. B. F. (2010). When plan-
Loew, Oettingen, & Gollwitzer, 2011). Together, ning is not enough: Fighting unhealthy snacking habits by
these findings suggest that MCII is a cost- and time- mental contrasting with implementation intentions (MCII).
effective self-regulation technique when it comes to European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 1277–1293.
the effective self-regulation of goal pursuit. Adriaanse, M. A., Van Oosten, J. M. F., De Ridder, D. T. D.,
De Wit, J. B. F., & Evers, C. (2011). Planning what not
to eat: Ironic effects of implementation intentions negating
Conclusion and Outlook unhealthy habits. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
The research on goals presented in this chapter 37, 69–81.
paints a picture of an agentic individual who wisely Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational
sets goals and effectively acts upon them. She only Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211.
needs to apply the self-regulatory strategies of goal Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1969). The prediction of behavioral
intentions in a choice situation. Journal of Experimental
setting and goal striving, that is, mental contrast- Social Psychology, 5, 400–416.
ing and implementation intentions. These strategies Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and
allow people to pursue realizing their idiosyncratic predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-
wishes and timber their own development according Hall.
to principles of what is desirable and feasible. When Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking
behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.
applied in metacognitive form, mental contrasting Atkinson, J. W. (1958). Towards experimental analysis of human
and implementation intentions and especially their motivation in terms of motives, expectancies and incentives.
combined usage (MCII) will liberate people from In J. W. Atkinson (Ed.), Motives in fantasy, action, and society
being bound to erroneous engagement and bad (pp. 288–305). Princeton, NJ: Van Nostrand.
habits. How these strategies can be applied to create Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
goals of content and structure that best serve the Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control.
realizing of people’s individual wishes (e.g., learning New York: Freeman.
goals if the future outcome is to accumulate knowl- Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of
edge and performance goals when outperforming automatic influence in social perception and cognition.
others is at stake) will need to be explored in future In J. S. Uleman & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought
(pp. 3–51). New York: Guilford Press.
research. Baumeister, R. E. (2000). Ego-depletion and the self ’s executive
function. In A. Tesser, R. B. Felson, & J. M. Suls (Eds.),
References Psychological perspectives on self and identity (pp. 9–33).
Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance Bayer, U. C., Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G.
of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, (2009). Responding to subliminal cues: Do if-then plans
29, 971–979. facilitate action preparation and initiation without conscious
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. D. (1978). intent? Social Cognition, 27, 183–201.
Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation. Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2007). Boosting scholastic test
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74. scores by willpower: The role of implementation intentions.
Achtziger, A., Bayer, U. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (in press). Self and Identity, 6, 1–19.
Committing to implementation intentions: Attention and Bayer, U. C., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Achtziger, A. (2010). Staying
memory effects for selected situational cues. Motivation and on track: Planned goal striving is protected from disruptive
Emotion. internal states. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46,
Achtziger, A., Fehr, T., Oettingen, G., Gollwitzer, P. M., & 505–514.
Rockstroh, B. (2009). Strategies of intention formation are Bless, H., & Fiedler, K. (1995). Affective states and the influence
reflected in continuous MEG activity. Social Neuroscience, 4, of activated general knowledge. Personality and Social Psychol-
11–27. ogy Bulletin, 21, 766–778.
Achtziger, A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Implemen- Bottom, W. P., & Studt, A. (1993). Framing effects and the
tation intentions and shielding goal striving from unwanted distributive aspect of integrative bargaining. Organizational
thoughts and feelings. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 56, 459–474.
letin, 34, 381–393. Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001).
Adriaanse, M. A., de Ridder, D. T. D., & de Wit, J. B. F. Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation.
(2009). Finding the critical cue: Implementation intentions Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 946–960.

 goal pursuit


Brunstein, J. C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). Effects of failure Epstude, K., & Roese, N. J. (2008). The functional theory of
on subsequent performance: The importance of self-defining counterfactual thinking. Personality and Social Psychology
goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, Review, 12, 168–192.
395–407. Festinger, L. (1942). A theoretical interpretation of shifts in level
Brockner, J. (1992). The escalation of commitment to a failing of aspiration. Psychological Review, 49, 235–250.
course of action: Toward theoretical progress. Academy of Fishbach, A., & Dhar, R. (2005). Goals as excuses or guides: The
Management Review, 17, 39–61. liberating effect of perceived goal progress on choice. Journal
Burgess, P. W., Simons, J. S., Dumontheil, I., & Gilbert, S. J. of Consumer Research, 32, 370–377.
(2005). The gateway hypothesis of rostral PFC function. Fishbach, A., Dhar, R., & Zhang, Y. (2006). Subgoals as substi-
In J. Duncan, L. Phillips, & P. McLeod (Eds.), Measuring tutes or compliments: The role of goal accessibility. Journal of
the mind : Speed control and age (pp. 215–246). New York: Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 232–242.
Oxford University Press. Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and
Chapman, J., Armitage, C. J., & Norman, P. (2009). Comparing avoidance strength during goal attainment: Regulatory focus
implementation intention interventions in relation to young and the “goal looms larger”effect. Journal of Personality and
adults’ intake of fruit and vegetables. Psychology and Health, Social Psychology, 75, 1115–1131.
24, 317–332. Gable, P. A. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and
Chasteen, A. L., Park, D. C., & Schwarz, N. (2001). Implemen- goals. Journal of Personality, 74, 175–222.
tation intentions and facilitation of prospective memory. Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008). Approach-motivated
Psychological Science, 12, 457–461. positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological
Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, Science, 19, 476–482.
155–159. Gawrilow, C., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). Implementation
Cohen, A-L., Bayer, U. C., Jaudas, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. intentions facilitate response inhibition in ADHD children.
(2008). Self-regulatory strategy and executive control: Imple- Cognitive Therapy and Research, 32, 261–280.
mentation intentions modulate task switching and Simon Gawrilow, C., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2011a).
task performance. Psychological Research, 72, 12–26. If-then plans benefit executive functions in children with
Christiansen, S., Oettingen, G., Dahme, B., & Klinger, R. ADHD. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology 30,
(2010). A short goal-pursuit intervention to improve physi- 616–646.
cal capacity: A randomized clinical trial in chronic back pain Gawrilow, C., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2011b).
patients. Pain, 149, 444–452. If-then plans benefit delay of gratification performance in
Darnon, C., Harackiewicz, J. M., Butera, F., Mugny, G., children with and without ADHD. Cognitive Therapy and
& Quiamzade, A. (2007). Performance-approach and Research, 35, 442–455.
performance-avoidance goals: When uncertainty makes a Gilbert, S. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., Cohen, A-L., Oettingen, G., &
difference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, Burgess, P. W. (2009). Separable brain systems supporting
813–827. cues versus self-initiated realization of delayed intentions.
De Nooijer, J., De Vet, E., Brug, J., & De Vries, N. K. (2006). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and
Do implementation intentions help to turn good intentions Cognition, 35, 905–915.
into higher fruit intakes? Journal of Nutrition Education and Gollwitzer, P. M. (1986). Striving for specific identities: The social
Behavior, 38, 25–29. reality of self-symbolizing. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Public
Dreisbach, G., & Goschke, T. (2004). How positive affect self and private self (pp. 143–159). New York: Springer.
modulates cognitive control: Reduced perseveration at the Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mind-sets.
cost of increased distractibility. Journal of Experimental Psy- In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), Handbook of
chology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30, 343–353. motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior
Duckworth, A. L., Grant, H., Loew, B., Oettingen, G., & (Vol. 2, pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). Self-regulation strategies improve Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of inten-
self-discipline in adolescents: Benefits of mental contrasting tions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.
and implementation intentions. Educational Psychology 31, Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong
17–26. effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.
Dweck, C. S. (1996). Implicit theories as organizers of goals and Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation
behavior. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psy- intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality
chology of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.
(pp. 69–90). New York: Guilford Press. Gollwitzer, P. M., & Kirchhof, O. (1998). The willful pursuit
Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the “classic” and of identity. In J. Heckhausen & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Moti-
“contemporary”approaches to achievement motivation: vation and self-regulation across the life span (pp. 389–423).
A hierarchical model of approach and avoidance achieve- Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
ment motivation. In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances Gollwitzer, P. M., Parks-Stamm, E. J., Jaudas, A., & Sheeran, P.
in motivation and achievement (pp. 243–279). Greewich, (2008). Flexible tenacity in goal pursuit. In J. Shah
CT: JAI Press. & W. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation science
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. A. (1997). A hierarchical model of (pp. 325–341). New York: Guilford Press.
approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Gollwitzer, P. M., & Schaal, B. (1998). Metacognition in action:
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232. The importance of implementation intentions. Personality
Elliot, A. J., Gable, S. L., & Mapes, R. R. (2006). Approach and and Social Psychology Review, 2, 124–136.
avoidance motivation in the social domain. Personality and Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation inten-
Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 378–391. tions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38, Hollenbeck, J. R., Williams, C. R., & Klein, H. J. (1989).
69–119. An empirical examination of the antecedents of commitment
Gollwitzer, P. M., Sheeran, P., Michalski, V., & Seifert, A. E. to difficult goals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 74, 18–23.
(2009). When intentions go public: Does social reality Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: Appleton-
widen the intention-behavior gap? Psychological Science, 20, Century-Crofts.
612–618. Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process-dissociation framework: Separat-
Gollwitzer, P. M., Sheeran, P., Trötschel, R., & Webb, T. L. ing automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of
(2011). Self-regulation of primimg effects on behvaior. Memory and Language 30, 513–541.
Psychological Science, 22, 901–911. Judge, T. A., Bono, J. E., Erez, A., & Locke, E. A. (2005). Core
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Wicklund, R. A. (1985). Self-symbolizing self-evaluations and job and life satisfaction: The role of
and the neglect of others’ perspectives. Journal of Personality self-concordance and goal attainment. Journal of Applied
and Social Psychology, 48, 702–715. Psychology, 90, 257–268.
Gollwitzer, P. M., Wicklund, R. A., & Hilton, J. L. (1982). Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2003). Working-memory capac-
Admission of failure and symbolic self-completion: Extend- ity and the control of attention: The contributions of goal
ing Lewinian theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- neglect, response competition, and task set to Stroop inter-
ogy, 43, 358–371. ference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132,
Gray, J. A. (1994). Three fundamental emotion systems. 47–70.
In P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion Kappes, A., & Oettingen, G. (2011). The emergence of goals:
(pp. 243–247). New York: Oxford University Press. Mental contrasting connects future and reality. Manuscript
Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. under revision.
(2008). Power and the objectification of social targets. Kappes, A., Singman, H., & Oettingen, G. (2011). Mental con-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127. trasting links reality to instrumental means. Manuscript under
Guinote, A. (2007). Power and goal pursuit. Personality and review.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1076–1087. Kappes, A., Oettingen, G. & Pak, H. (2011). Mental contrast-
Guinote, A. (2008). Power and affordances: When the situation ing and the self-regulation of responding to negative feedback.
has more power over powerful than powerless individuals. Manuscript under revision.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 237–252. Kappes, H. B., & Oettingen, G. (2011). Positive fantasies about
Gurney, K., Prescott, T. J., & Redgrave, P. (2001a). A computa- idealized futures sap energy. Journal of Experimental Social
tional model of action selection in the basal ganglia I: A new Psychology 47, 719–729.
functional anatomy. Biological Cybernetics, 84, 401–410. Kazen, M., & Kuhl, J. (2005). Intention memory and achieve-
Gurney, K., Prescott, T. J., & Redgrave, P. (2001b). A compu- ment motivation: Volitional facilitation and inhibition as a
tational model of action selection in the basal ganglia II: function of affective contents of need related stimuli. Journal
Analysis and simulation of behavior. Biological Cybernetics, of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 426–448.
84, 411–423. Kirk, D., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Carnevale, P. J. (2011). Self-regulation
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Harris, J. (2006). in ultimatum bargaining: Goals and plans help accept unfair
From psychological need satisfaction to intentional behav- but profitable offers. Social Cognition 29, 528–546.
ior: Testing a motivational sequence in two behavioral Knäuper, B., Roseman, M., Johnson, P., & Krantz, L. (2009).
contexts. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, Using mental imagery to enhance the effectiveness of imple-
131–148. mentation intentions. Current Psychology, 28, 181–186.
Harmon-Jones, C., Schmeichel, B. J., & Harmon-Jones, E. Koestner, R., Horberg, E. J., Gaudreau, P., Powers, T. A., Di Dio,
(2009). Symbolic self completion in academia: Evidence P., Bryan, C., & Salter, N. (2006). Bolstering implementa-
from department web pages and email signatures files. tion plans for the long haul: The benefits of simultaneously
European Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, boosting self-concordance or self-efficacy. Personality and
311–316. Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1547–1558.
Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its constructs: Koestner, R., Lekes, N., Powers, T. A., & Chicoine, E. (2002).
A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1, 283–329. Attaining personal goals: Self-concordance plus implementa-
Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents tion intention equals success: Journal of Personality and Social
and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional Psychology, 83, 231–244.
states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120. Koo, M., & Fishbach, A. (2008). Dynamics of self-regulation:
Henderson, M., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). How (un)accomplished goal actions affect motivation. Jour-
Implementation intentions and disengagement from a fail- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 183–195.
ing course of action. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, Kuhl, J., & Kazen, M. (1999). Volitional facilitation of difficult
20, 81–102. intentions: Joint activation of intention memory and positive
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psy- affect removes Stroop interference. Journal of Experimental
chologist, 52, 1280–1300. Psychology: General, 128, 382–399.
Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit. Larsen, R. J., & Augustine, A. A. (2008). Basic personality
American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230. dispositions related to approach and avoidance: Extraver-
Higgins, E. T. (2006). Value from hedonic experience and sion/neuroticism, BAS/BIS, and positive/negative affectivity.
engagement. Psychological Review, 113, 439–460. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance moti-
Holland, R. W., Aarts, H. & Langendam, D. (2006). Breaking vation (pp. 151–164). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
and creating habits on the working floor: A field-experiment Ledgerwood, A., Liviatan, I., & Carnevale, P. J. (2007). Group
on the power of implementation intentions. Journal of identity completion and the symbolic value of property.
Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 776–783. Psychological Science, 18, 873–878.

 goal pursuit


Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). Reflective and reflex- perception, and social development. American Psychologist,
ive action control in frontal lobe patients. Neuropsychology, 61, 192–203.
15, 80–100. Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and
Lewin, K., Dembo, T., Festinger, L., & Sears, P. S. (1944). Level depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a
of aspiration. In J. McHunt (Ed.), Personality and the behav- muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259.
iour disorders (Vol. 1, pp. 333–378). New York: Ronald. Muraven, M., Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1998). Self-
Liberman, N., Trope, Y., McCrea, M. S., & Sherman, S. (2007). control as a limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns.
The effect of level of construal on the temporal distance of Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789.
activity enactment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, Oettingen, G. (1996). Positive fantasy and motivation.
43, 143–149. In P. M. Gollwitzer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior
useful theory of goal setting and task performance: A 35 year (pp. 236–259). New York: Guilford.
odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. Oettingen, G. (2000). Expectancy effects on behavior depend on
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2006). New directions in goal- self-regulatory thought. Social Cognition 18, 101–129.
setting theory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Strategies of setting
265–268. and implementing goals: Mental contrasting and imple-
Louro, M. J., Pieters, R., & Zeelenberg, M. (2007). Dynam- mentation intentions. In J. E. Maddux & J. P. Tangney
ics of multiple-goal pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social (Eds.), Social psychological foundations of clinical psychology
Psychology, 93, 174–193. (pp. 114–135). New York: Guilford Press.
Maglio, S. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (in press). Oettingen, G., Hönig, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2000). Effec-
Action control by implementation intentions: The role of tive self-regulation of goal attainment. International Journal
discrete emotions. In A. Clark, J. Kiverstein, & T. Vierkant of Educational Research, 33, 705–732.
(Eds.), Decomposing the will. New York: Oxford University Oettingen, G., & Kappes, A. (2009). Mental contrasting
Press. of the future and reality to master negative feedback.
Markman, K. D., Lindberg, M. J., Kray, L. J., & Galinsky, A. D. In K. D. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr (Eds.), Hand-
(2007). Implications of counterfactual structure for creativ- book of imagination and mental simulation. (pp. 395–412).
ity and analytical problem solving. Personality and Social Psy- New York: Psychology Press.
chology Bulletin, 33, 312–324. Oettingen, G., & Mayer, D. (2002). The motivating function
Martin, J., Sheeran, P., Slade, P., Wright, A., & Dibble, T. (2009). of thinking about the future: Expectations versus fantasies.
Implementation intention formation reduces consultations Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1198–1212.
for emergency contraception and pregnancy testing among Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Sevincer, A. T., Stephens, E. J.,
teenage women. Health Psychology, 28, 762–769. Pak, H., & Hagenah, M. (2009). Mental contrasting and
McClelland, D. C. (1985a). Human motivation. Glenview, IL: goal commitment: The mediating role of energization. Per-
Scott, Foresman. sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 608–622.
McClelland, D. C. (1985b). How motives, skills, and values deter- Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., & Thorpe, J. S (2010). Promotion
mine what people do. American Psychologist, 41, 812–825. and prevention fantasies and the self-regulation of goal com-
McCrea, S. M. (2008). Self-handicapping, excuse-making, and mitment to reduce cigarette consumption. Psychology and
counterfactual thinking: Consequences for self-esteem and Health, 25, 961–977.
future motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Oettingen, G., Mayer, D., Thorpe, J. S., Janetzke, H., & Lorenz,
ogy, 95, 274–292. S. (2005). Turning fantasies about positive and negative
McCrea, S. M., Liberman, N., Trope, Y., & Sherman, S. J. futures into self-improvement goals. Motivation and Emo-
(2008). Construal level and procrastination. Psychological tion, 29, 237–267.
Science 19, 1308–1314. Oettingen, G., Pak, H., & Schnetter, K. (2001). Self-regulation
Mendoza, S. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Amodio, D. M. (2010). of goal-setting: Turning free fantasies about the future into
Reducing the expression of implicit stereotypes: Reflexive binding goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
control through implementation intentions. Personality and 736–753.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 512–523. Oettingen, G., & Stephens, E. J. (2009). Fantasies and moti-
Miles, J. D., & Proctor, R. W. (2008). Improving perfor- vationally intelligent goal setting. In G. B. Moskowitz &
mance through implementation intentions: Are preexisting H. Grant (Eds.), The psychology of goals (pp. 153–178). New
response biases replaced? Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, York: Guilford Press.
15, 1105–1110. Oettingen, G., Stephens, E. J., Mayer, D., & Brinkmann, B.
Milne, S. E., Orbell, S. & Sheeran, P. (2002). Combining (2010). Mental contrasting and the self-regulation of help-
motivational and volitional interventions to promote ing relations. Social Cognition, 28, 490–508.
exercise participation: Protection motivation theory and Oettingen, G., & Wadden, T. A. (1991). Expectation, fantasy,
implementation intentions. British Journal of Health Psychol- and weight loss: Is the impact of positive thinking always
ogy, 7, 163–184. positive? Cognitive Therapy and Research, 15, 167–175.
Mischel, W. (1973). Toward a cognitive social learning reconcep- Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997) Implementation
tualization of personality. Psychological Review, 80, 252–283. intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Personality
Mischel, W. (1974). Processes in delay of gratification. and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 953–962.
In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychol- Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., & Terry, K. (2006). Possible selves
ogy (Vol. 7, pp. 249–292). New York: Academic Press. and academic outcomes: How and when possible selves
Molden, D. C., & Dweck, C. S. (2006). Finding “meaning” in impel action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91,
psychology. A lay theories approach to self-regulation, social 188–204.

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


Palayiwa, A., Sheeran, P., & Thompson, A. (2010). “Words Sheldon, K. M., & Krieger, L. S. (2007). Understanding the
will never hurt me!” Implementation intentions regulate negative effects of legal education on law students: A longitu-
attention to stigmatizing comments about appearance. Jour- dinal test of self-determination theory. Personality and Social
nal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 575–598. Psychology Bulletin, 33, 883–897.
Papies, E., Aarts, H., & de Vries, N. K. (2009). Grounding your Simon, J. R. (1990). The effects of an irrelevant directional
plans: Implementation intentions go beyond the mere cre- cue on human information processing. In R. W. Proctor &
ation of goal-directed associations. Journal of Experimental T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compatibility: An inte-
Social Psychology, 45, 1148–1151. grative perspective (pp. 31–86). Amsterdam: North-Holland.
Parks-Stamm, E. J., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Oettingen, G. (2007). Smith, P. K., Jostmann, N. B., Galinsky, A. D., & van Dijk, W. W.
Action control by implementation intentions: Effective cue (2008). Lacking power impairs executive functions. Psycho-
detection and efficient response initiation. Social Cognition, logical Science, 19, 441–447.
25, 248–266. Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2002). Psychological heterogenity in
Paul, I., Gawrilow, C., Zech, F., Gollwitzer, P. M., Rockstroh, B., ADHD—A dual pathway model of behavior and cognition.
Odenthal, G., Kratzer, W., & Wienbruch, C. (2007). If-then Behavioral Brain Research, 130, 29–36.
planning modulates the P300 in children with attention defi- Spence, K. W. (1956). Behavior theory and conditioning.
cit hyperactivity disorder. NeuroReport, 18, 653–657. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Poortvliet, P. M., Janssen, O., Van Yperen, N. W., & Van de Stadler, G., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Physical
Vliert, E. (2007). Achievement goals and interpersonal activity in women. Effects of a self-regulation intervention.
behavior: How mastery and performance goals shape infor- American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 36, 29–34.
mation exchange. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Stadler, G., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Inter-
33, 1435–1447. vention effects of information and self-regulation on eating
Powers, T. A., Koestner, R., & Topciu, R. A. (2005). Implemen- fruits and vegetables over two years. Health Psychology, 29,
tation intentions, perfectionism, and goal progress: Perhaps 274–283.
the road to hell is paved with good intentions. Personality and Stewart, B. D., & Payne, B. K. (2008). Bringing automatic
Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 902–912. stereotyping under control: Implementation intentions as
Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). efficient means of thought control. Personality and Social
All goals are not created equal: An organismic perspective on Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1332–1345.
the nature of goals and their regulation. In P. M. Gollwitzer Strachman, A., & Gable, S. L. (2006). What you want (and do
& J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking not want) affects what you see (and do not see): Avoidance
cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 7–26). New York: social goals and social events. Personality and Social Psychology
Guilford Press. Bulletin, 32, 1446–1458.
Scheier, M. F., & Carver, C. S. (1987). Dispositional optimism Tamir, M. (2009). What do people want to feel and why? Plea-
and physical well-being: The influence of generalized out- sure and utility in emotion regulation. Current Directions in
come expectancies. Journal of Personality, 55, 169–210. Psychological Science, 18, 101–105.
Scholer, A. A., & Higgins, E. T. (2008). Distinguishing levels of Tamir, M., & Robinson, M. D. (2007). The happy spotlight: Pos-
approach and avoidance: A analysis using regulatory focus itive mood and selective attention to rewarding information.
theory. In A. J. Elliot (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoid- Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 1124–1136.
ance motivation (pp. 489–503). New York: Psychology Press. Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998).
Schweiger Gallo, I., Keil, A., Mc Culloch, K. C., Rockstroh, B., Harnessing the imagination: Mental simulation, self-
& Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Strategic automation of emo- regulation, and coping. American Psychologist, 53, 429–439.
tion control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001).
11–31. Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse
Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forget- control: If you feel bad, do it! Journal of Personality and
ting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal Social, 80, 53–67
shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men.
1261–1280. New York: Appleton-Century.
Shah, J., Higgins, E. T., & Friedman, R. S. (1998). Performance Trötschel, R., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2007). Implementation inten-
incentives and means: How regulatory focus influences goal tions and the willful pursuit of prosocial goals in negotiations.
attainment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 579–589.
285–293. Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Implementation intentions think they’re doing? Action identification and human behav-
and repeated behavior: augmenting the predictive vailidity of ior. Psychological Review, 94, 215–228.
the theory of planned behaviour. European Journal of Social Verplanken, B., & Faes, S. (1999). Good intentions, bad habits,
Psychology 29, 349–369. and effects of forming implementation intentions on healthy
Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000). Using implementation inten- eating. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 591–604.
tions to increase attendance for cervical cancer screening. Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
Health Psychology, 19, 283–289. Webb, T. L., Christian, J., & Armitage C. J. (2007). Helping
Sheeran, P., Webb, T. L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2005). The inter- students turn up for class: Does personality moderate the
play between goal intentions and implementation intentions. effectiveness of an implementation intention intervention?
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 87–98. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 316–327.
Sheldon, K. M., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Goal striving, need satisfac- Webb, T. L., Ononaiye, M. S. P., Sheeran, P., Reidy, J. G., &
tion, and longitudinal well-being: The self-concordance model. Lavda, A. (2010). Using implementation intentions to over-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 482–497. come the effects of social anxiety on attention and appraisals

 goal pursuit


of performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, Webb, T. L., Sheeran, P., & Luszczynska, A. (2009). Planning
612–627. to break unwanted habits: Habit strength moderates imple-
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Can implementation inten- mentation intention effects on behavior change. British
tions help to overcome ego-depletion. Journal of Experimen- Journal of Social Psychology, 48, 507–523.
tal Social Psychology, 39, 279–286. Wicklund, R. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1982). Symbolic self-
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2004). Identifying good opportuni- completion. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
ties to act: Implementation intentions and cue discrimina- Wieber, F., Odenthal, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2010). Self-
tion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 407–419. efficacy feelings moderate implementation intention effects.
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral Self and Identity, 9, 177–194.
intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the Wieber, F., & Sassenberg, K. (2006). I can’t take my eyes off of
experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249–268. it—attention attraction of implementation intentions. Social
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2007). How do implementation Cognition, 24, 723–752.
intentions promote goal attainment? A test of component Wieber, F., von Suchodoletz, A., Heikamp, T., Trommsdorff, G.,
processes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2011). If-then planning helps school-
295–302. aged children to ignore attractive distractions. Social Psychol-
Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Mechanisms of implemen- ogy 42, 39–47.
tation intention effects: The role of goal intentions, self- Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits
efficacy, and accessibility of plan components. British Journal and the habit–goal interface. Psychological Review, 114,
of Social Psychology, 47, 373–395. 842–862.

g ollw i tzer, oet ti n g en 


CHAPTER

Unconscious Goal Pursuit:


14 Nonconscious Goal Regulation
and Motivation
Henk Aarts and Ruud Custers

Abstract
Experimental research in psychology has discovered that human goal pursuit originates and unfolds in
the unconscious. Our behavior is directed and motivated by goals outside of conscious awareness in the
current situation or environment. In this chapter we review past and current research that examines
these goal-priming effects. Our review is organized around two themes. The first theme deals with
research that analyzes how people control their goal pursuit in the absence of conscious awareness, and
it examines goal pursuit as automated behaviors resulting from habits as well as flexible behaviors that
occur when habitual responding is not adequate to attain goals. The second theme concerns the quest
to understand the unconscious source of human goal pursuit, and it explores recent work that considers
the fundamental role of positive affect in reward processing and implicit motivation.
Key Words: priming, goal pursuit, unconscious processes, motivation, regulation

Observing other people’s and one’s own behavior Obviously, the environment plays a crucial role
sometimes may lead to the conclusion that the in directing behavior. However, acting on fixed
human behavioral system throws in responses to stimulus-response rules—such as smashing a beep-
situations by trial and error. Our behavior can be ing alarm clock in the morning—is not the whole
fairly unpredictable and even chaotic. However, story. A substantial part of human behavior seems to
whereas human behavior occasionally appears to be directed at desired outcomes that reliably control
consist of a random selection of responses, often it and motivate the behavioral system in a dynamic
is not. Our behavior is very sensitive to learning and world. This flexibility to produce the same desired
strongly influenced by past experiences, and it tends outcomes under varying circumstances comes from
to be highly organized and structured in the service our capacity to mentally represent what we want
of future action. Indeed, research in the tradition and do: to build and store representations of goals.
of behaviorism has shown that human behavior fol- These goal representations function as beacons for
lows from rigid responses to environmental stimuli behavior, motivating action and guiding its course.
that are learned and reinforced by rewards (Skinner, The idea that our behavior is directed by goals is
1953; Watson, 1925). When chained together—it explicitly articulated in different models and theo-
is reasoned—such responses can even make up ries of human behavior (e.g., Bandura, 1986; Deci
complicated patterns of action, setting each other & Ryan, 1985; Locke & Latham, 1990). Impor-
off like toppling domino stones. According to this tantly, in most of these models and theories peo-
view, the environment organizes and determines ple’s goal pursuit is assumed to be governed by a
human behavior. kind of inner agent such as “consciousness” or “the


will.” There is common agreement that goal setting of positive affect in reward processing and implicit
is accompanied by conscious awareness, and that motivation to offer insight into the unconscious
goal pursuit is associated with conscious intent. sources of our goal pursuit. Before we discuss this
For Descartes, the role of consciousness in goal work in more detail, we will briefly address some
pursuit even provided proof of our very existence. general issues pertaining to the conceptualization of
It is we who consciously decide what we want and unconscious goal pursuit.
do; and it is up to us to consciously act upon it.
It does not come as a surprise, then, that the sci- The Concept of Unconscious Goal Pursuit
entific investigation of human behavior—heavily At first glance, the notion that goal pursuit occurs
influenced by Descartes—has assigned a key role to nonconsciously and is controlled by the environ-
consciousness. ment brings us back to behaviorism. However, the
However, whereas the causal status of conscious- modern conceptualization of unconscious goal pur-
ness in human behavior is often taken for granted, suit is different in one important way (e.g., Aarts &
over the last 15 years or so several lines of experimen- Dijksterhuis, 2000; Bargh, 1990). It follows the
tation have discovered that our goal-directed behav- modern view on human functioning that analyzes
iors can be under “unconscious control” (Custers & the involvement of mental processes and the role
Aarts, 2010). This large body of evidence for the of knowledge acquisition and utilization in non-
occurrence of nonconscious social behavior reso- conscious learning and environmental control over
nates well with prevailing views on the limited role behavior. For instance, several studies on the role of
of consciousness in human functioning. According reinforcement in classical and instrumental condi-
to these views, our thinking and doing is produced tioning have stressed the importance of incentives
by mental processes that are not open to intro- or goals in motivating and directing behavior of ani-
spection, and hence, in essence all our behaviors mals, humans included, in the absence of conscious
start and unfold in the unconscious (Libet, 1985; awareness (Berridge, 2001). Furthermore, basic and
Nørretranders, 1991). In other words, human applied work on preference learning suggests that
behavior is not (always) governed by a social agent humans implicitly form mental representations of
that motivates and directs behavior consciously and rewarding goal-objects when these objects co-occur
intentionally (Bargh, 1997; Wegner, 2002; Wilson, with affective (e.g., rewarding) stimuli (De Houwer,
2002). Instead, our behavior is directed and moti- Thomas, & Baeyens, 2001; Hofmann, De Houwer,
vated by goals outside of conscious awareness in the Perugini, Baeyens, & Crombez, 2010). In addition,
current situation or environment, even though we people have been shown to learn rules that predict
share the experience and belief that we consciously the occurrence of complex sequences of stimuli and
set and pursue goals. This notion not only pertains responses without their ability to consciously verbal-
to simple motor movements and skills such as flex- ize these rules (Reber, 1993). Accordingly, research
ing an index finger, pushing keys on a computer on unconscious goal pursuit goes beyond behavior-
keyboard, or driving a car but also to social behav- ism (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000) by scrutinizing the
ior resulting from higher cognitive processes such as mental processes that underlie the influence of the
our goal pursuit. environment on the activation and operation of
In the present chapter, we discuss social cogni- goal-directed behavior.
tion research that examines the possibility that Analogous to research on conscious goal pursuit,
human goal pursuit emerges in the absence of con- the study on unconscious goal pursuit assumes that
scious intent and without awareness of the cause of goals are mentally represented as desired behaviors or
the goal pursuit. This chapter is organized around outcomes (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1998; Gollwitzer
two themes. The first theme deals with research & Moskowitz, 1996). These goal representations
that analyzes how people are able to control and can differ in their level of abstractness. For instance,
regulate their goal pursuit in the absence of con- while socializing or earning money are representa-
scious awareness. Therefore, we examine goal pur- tions of complex goals that usually require a series
suit as automated behaviors resulting from habits as of actions to be achieved, using a phone or produc-
well as flexible behaviors that occur when habitual ing matching symbols on a slot machine are results
responding is not adequate to attain goals. The sec- which can be attained by a few button presses or a
ond theme concerns the question of what motivates simple hand movement. Thus, human goal-directed
goal pursuit unconsciously. Specifically, we discuss behavior is commonly understood to evolve from
recent work that considers the fundamental role more simple movement goals to more complex

a a rts , c us ter s 


social goals. We first have to learn to orchestrate and exposed students to words such as “strive” and
coordinate our motor movements before we can “succeed” to prime the goal of achievement, and
operate a phone and to make a date to go out, so then gave them the opportunity to perform well
to speak. (finding as many words as possible in an anagram
In contrast to research on conscious goal pursuit, puzzle task). Results indicated that students primed
however, the study on unconscious goal pursuit with the achievement goal outperformed those who
explicitly assumes that goals and their pursuit can be were not primed with the goal. Further experimen-
controlled in the absence of conscious awareness. An tation demonstrated that such goal priming leads
important foundation for this assumption pertains to qualities associated with motivational states or
to the empirical observation that humans represent volition, such as persistence in solving puzzles and
their actions in terms of their observable effects or increased flexibility on the Wisconsin Card Sort-
outcomes, and they establish associations between ing Task (Hassin, 2008), a standard measure of
the outcomes and the motor programs that produce flexibility in cognitive processing (Miyake, Fried-
the outcome (Hommel, Müsseler, Aschersleben, man, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter, & Wager, 2000).
& Prinz, 2001; Jeannerod, 1997; Prinz, 1997; Extensive debriefing revealed that the students did
Vallacher & Wegner, 1987). As a consequence, action not experience an influence of the first task (in
can follow from an ideomotor principle (James, which they were exposed to achievement-related
1890): Merely thinking about or priming a certain words) on their responses to the second. These find-
outcome moves and programs the human body ings indicate that the mere activation of a goal rep-
in the service of achieving that outcome without resentation suffices to motivate and direct people to
a conscious decision to act. In addition, represent- work on the primed goal without conscious thought
ing actions in terms of their potentially desirable and intent.
outcomes allows people to direct their behavior at The work alluded to earlier shows that goal pur-
the level of the specific outcome, in that they serve suit can be automatically put in place if the repre-
as reference points that guide and adjust ongoing sentation of the goal is directly primed (for more
actions toward producing the desired goal. evidence of direct achievement goal priming effects,
Furthermore, our personal goal pursuit is e.g., Bongers, Dijksterhuis, & Spears, 2010; Custers,
assumed to be part of knowledge structures includ- Aarts, Oikawa, & Elliot, 2009; Eitam, Hassin, &
ing the context, the goal itself, and actions as well Schul, 2008; Engeser, Wendland, & Rheinberg,
as opportunities that may aid goal pursuit, that are 2006; Hart & Albarracín, 2009; Oikawa, 2004;
shaped by direct experience and other types of learn- Shantz & Latham, 2009). Recently, researchers have
ing (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2000; Aarts & Dijkster- started to identify the specific aspects in the social
huis 2003; Bargh & Gollwitzer, 1994; Kruglanski environment that may cause people to automati-
et al., 2002). For example, the goal of consuming cally set and pursue goals. Through their associa-
fruit may be related to eating a banana while hav- tions with particular goals, these aspects indirectly
ing lunch in the university cafeteria. Or a visit to prime or activate goal representations. For instances,
an exclusive restaurant or bar may be connected there is research to suggest that goal pursuit is auto-
to interacting with good friends and the desire to matically triggered when goals are inferred from the
socialize and go out. Thus, when activating or prim- behavior of others, an effect termed goal contagion
ing a goal (e.g., eating fruit, socializing), we do not (Aarts, Gollwitzer, Hassin, 2004; Dik & Aarts,
access a single concept, but rather a rich structure 2007; Friedman, Deci, Elliot, Moller, & Aarts,
containing, among others, cognitive, affective, 2010; Loersch, Aarts, Payne, & Jefferis, 2008). Aarts
behavioral information (Bargh, 2006). According to et al. (2004) demonstrated that participants who
the concept of unconscious goal pursuit, then, the observed another person’s behavior that implied
direction and motivation of people’s thinking and the goal of making money were more motivated to
doing can start and proceed outside of conscious make money themselves by engaging in a task that
awareness, because one can directly rely on acces- gave access to a lottery.
sible goal-relevant representations that are primed Furthermore, goals and their pursuit seem to be
by contextual as well as behavioral information. activated in the presence of important others (Fitz-
One of the first empirical demonstrations of simons & Bargh, 2003; Kraus & Chen, 2009; Shah,
this notion comes from Bargh and others’ (2001) 2003). In a study among undergraduate students
research program on goal-priming effects on achieve- (Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2003), subliminal priming
ment. In one of their studies, they unobtrusively of the name of one’s parents was demonstrated to

 unconscious goal pursuit


trigger the motivation to achieve, and exposure to behavior as a result of practice and the automation
names of good friends primed the goal and resultant of skills. In general, there are two different views to
behavior of helping. In a recent line of experimen- this issue that can be characterized as representing
tation, these goal-priming effects have been repli- either low-level stimulus-response learning and per-
cated and extended in the realm of social stereotypes formance or a higher cognitive level of goal-directed
(Aarts et al., 2005; Custers, Maas, Wildenbeest, learning and performance.
& Aarts, 2008; Moskowitz, Gollwitzer, Wasel, &
Schaal, 1999). For instance, priming members of unconscious goal pursuit and
social groups that contain the representation of a stimulus-response rules
goal that is believed to be held by that group has At the lowest level of analysis, habits can be
been shown to cause people to automatically pursue regarded as stimulus-response links that are estab-
the goals, such as the goals of helping or making lished and reinforced by rewards which follow cer-
money that are stereotypical for nurses or stockbro- tain responses to a stimulus. If, for example, one feels
kers, respectively. pleasantly satisfied after eating a crunchy chocolate
While divergent, these findings have a com- bar, the sight of chocolate may later evoke the action
mon theme. They suggest that an appreciation of of grabbing it in order to eat. Eventually, when a
the goals motivating other people we interact with behavior has repeatedly been executed in response
allows one to entertain similar goals and to try to to a certain stimulus and the stimulus-response asso-
attain them oneself. It promotes successful pursuit ciation has become well established, the perception
of one’s own needs, desires, and goals. Furthermore, of the stimulus may automatically trigger the execu-
by pursuing the goals of others, people may become tion of the associated behavior.
more similar in what they desire and strive for, and This view that, after sufficient practice, behavior
hence in their plans for the future (Aarts, Dijkster- becomes completely stimulus controlled and inde-
huis, & Dik, 2008). Given this compelling and per- pendently of the rewards (e.g., reducing hunger) that
vasive social influence on human pursuit, we will initially reinforced the behavior suggests that the
now discuss how goals are primed and control overt behavior is no longer motivational and goal directed
behavior without an act of conscious will. in nature (Dickinson, Balleine, Watt, Gonzales, &
Boakes, 1995). However, there are two scientific
The Control of Unconscious Goal Pursuit discoveries that do not agree with this automatic
In this section we address the question of how stimulus-response habit perspective. First, theories
people control their goal pursuit unconsciously. That on incentive learning propose that stimuli them-
is, we will examine how the mere activation of a goal selves may act as incentives for which the organ-
representation produces actions leading to the goal. ism is willing to work. These theories grew out of
Two issues are important here. First, unconscious several remarkable findings in different animal labs
goal pursuit is the result of well-established habits. that shed new light on the role of reinforcement in
Second, because habits may fail to produce actions learning processes following the stimulus-response
directed at goals, human goal pursuit needs to be habit paradigm (Skinner, 1953; Watson, 1925). For
flexible and adaptive. Accordingly, researchers have instance, operant stereotypes or misbehaviors were
started to examine whether people are capable of discovered during operant conditioning experiments
regulating their personal goals without being aware (Breland & Breland, 1961). One such behavior is
of the activation and operation of the goal. We will autoshaping (Brown & Jenkins, 1968; Williams &
first address the habitual nature of unconscious goal Williams, 1969). It has, for example, been shown
pursuit, and then discuss research that aims to dem- that pigeons, for which free presentation of food is
onstrate that goal pursuit is flexible without an act repeatedly paired with a light signal, start to vigor-
of conscious will. ously pick at the light bulb, although this behavior
is not explicitly reinforced. This phenomenon, in
The Role of Habits in Unconscious which an animal shapes itself, occurs because the
Goal Pursuit positive affect or pleasure aroused by the food has
The nonconscious execution of goal-directed now become linked to the light bulb, which there-
behavior has been initially understood and appreci- fore serves as an incentive for which the animal is
ated in terms of habits. That is, in line with contem- motivated to work.
porary social cognition research on the establishment Biological grounding of this “transfer of posi-
of automatic and unconscious processes, goals prime tive affect to stimuli” effect comes from research

a a rts , c us ter s 


suggesting that so-called pleasure centers in the regarded as goal dependent that are selected in the
brain (mainly targeting the nucleus accumbens) are course of attaining a specific goal.
involved in the mechanism that creates incentives
(Shizgal, 1999). For example, rats that have learned unconscious goal pursuit and skills
to perform an arbitrary behavior such as pressing a directed by goals
lever in a cage that is followed by electrical stimu- Considering habits as single responses to stimuli
lation of the mesolimbic brain area become highly may work well for basic actions such as walking to
motivated to perform that behavior (as the behav- the door when the bell rings. However, most actions
ior activates the brain’s pleasure center, and hence, in daily life—such as making coffee, driving to work,
triggers positive affect; Olds & Milner, 1954). It or even uttering words that make up sentences—are
appears as if pushing the lever becomes a goal in far more complicated. Nonetheless, these actions
itself. Illustrative of the motivational strength of can be executed in a habitual manner without much
this type of incentive learning, it has been estab- conscious thought. How do these skills develop and
lished that animals run uphill and leap over hurdles what do their underlying structures look like?
(Edmonds & Gallistel, 1974) and cross electrified One way to consider these skills is to regard
grids (Olds, 1958) in order to engage in the behav- them as a chain of responses instigated by a par-
ior. Importantly, such enhanced effort effects occur ticular stimulus (e.g., Adams, 1984; Wickelgren,
even in the absence of physiological deprivation 1969). When one prepares coffee in the morning,
states such as thirst or hunger (Shizgal, 1997). This for example, pouring the water may trigger getting
research demonstrates that practice does not only a filter from the cupboard, putting the filter in the
lead to automatic stimulus-response rules, but it can machine triggers getting the coffee powder, and so
also install a form of unconscious goal pursuit that forth. Such action chains can be conceptualized as
is motivational and goal directed, in that the stimu- open-loop mechanisms that enable the efficient exe-
lus acquires goal properties that work as a reward cution of behavior when the exact same sequence of
signal and motivate behavior. actions is required every time the behavior is per-
A second line of research that diverges from formed. In these types of habits, once the behav-
the automatic stimulus-response habit perspec- ior is initiated, it runs to completion in a ballistic
tive comes from context learning. Specifically, sev- fashion and does not allow for adjustments of the
eral studies suggest that, even though habits may ongoing process. Relying on such an open-loop
rely on nonmotivational well-practiced stimulus- mechanism, which does not take into account the
response structures, these structures are not as rigid result of the performed responses, may be the only
and automatic as one may think (Hommel, 2000). way to execute complex behavioral patterns when
Most notably, some stimulus-response links appear there is no time to process such feedback informa-
to be conditional on a particular goal or context, tion (e.g., when playing a fast sequence of notes on a
and as such renders the activation and application of piano). However, this mechanism only works when
stimulus-response links more flexible. Upon hearing the exact same sequence of responses is required.
the sound of the alarm clock, someone may stumble Any small change in the environment or execution
to the shower on a workday when she has to get of previous actions will lead the mechanism astray
to the office, but she may without much thought and cause the chain to break.
stumble downstairs to pick up the Saturday paper on As the execution of behavior often happens under
the weekend. Depending on the person’s goal (work such dynamic conditions, researchers have suggested
or leisure), the same stimulus may set off a different that another type of habitual behavior operates via a
response that promotes the completion of the goal feedback-control system, in which one’s actions are
at hand. This flexibility in switching between differ- directed by goals and can be adjusted in an ongoing
ent stimulus-response relations is reflected in work manner. More specifically, in such closed-loop pro-
demonstrating that people are able to quite easily cesses, the result of one action forms the input for
switch between different well-learned stimulus- the next one, thereby allowing for constant adjust-
response rules according to task instructions, such ments and efficient regulation of skillful actions in
as has been shown for context effects on spatial changing circumstances (e.g., Cooper & Shallice,
mapping of stimulus-response relations (Hommel, 2006; Frith, Blakemore, & Wolpert, 2000; Pow-
1993) or approach/avoidance movements toward ers, 1973). When driving one’s car, for example, the
valenced stimuli (Eder & Rothermund, 2008). required behavior is largely the same every time one
In this way, some stimulus-response habits can be takes the usual route to work. Still, slightly different

 unconscious goal pursuit


actions are needed on different occasions, such as et al., 2005), where it was shown that activating the
when the traffic light is red instead of green, there goal of socializing increased the accessibility of the
is a slow car in front, or a steady side wind requires concept of drinking, but only among those student
adjusting one’s steering wheel. Such adjustments of participants who were regular drinkers of alcohol
one’s habitual behavior can be made in a noncon- in social situations. In addition, after a socializing
scious manner by monitoring the results of one’s prime, these students were more likely to drink alco-
actions and using perceptual feedback to fine-tune hol as a reward for their participation in the experi-
the execution of the necessary skills and responses ment. These results indicate that the activation of a
(Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Custers & Aarts, 2010; goal automatically activates its associated habitual
Fourneret & Jeannerod, 1998). This way, the opera- means, making the repeated selection of this means
tion of a perceptual feedback-control system ensures for goal pursuit more likely.
that the same goals can be attained under different The idea of habits as a form of automatic goal-
circumstances. directed behavior has been pushed even a bit further.
When pursuing a goal, however, how is the selec- Specifically, although most models on goal-directed
tion of a course of action made in the first place? behavior assume that the goals that we set eventu-
Out of a variety of behaviors that could potentially ally originate from a conscious reflection process
lead to the attainment of a goal, how does the men- and thus are the result of conscious intent, research
tal system supporting unconscious goal pursuit on nonconscious goal pursuit suggest that our goals
decide which path to follow? All else being equal, can be activated outside of conscious awareness
one is likely to do things as one did them before, themselves to then have their effect on behavior.
and this is certainly true for nonconscious goal- Recurrent and consistent pursuit of a goal upon per-
directed behavior. Repeatedly pursuing a goal via a ception of a specific (social) situation is thought to
certain course of behavior forges a strong cognitive strengthen the link between the representations of
link between the goal representation and the rep- the situation and the goal. Consequently, the mere
resentation of this behavior, so that activation of a perception of the situation or environment causes
goal can automatically lead to the activation of the the goal-directed behavior to be triggered directly.
habitual means for goal pursuit. This way, for exam- This notion has been corroborated by a host of stud-
ple, we do not have to think deliberately how to ies that show that goal pursuit is launched when
get to work in the morning, as the goal of going to people are exposed to goal-related stimuli (Aarts
work automatically activates the idea of using one’s et al., 2008).
bike or car; we do not have to consider all avail- To recap, practice and habits play an important
able supermarkets when having to do the grocer- role in the automation of goal pursuit. Actions
ies, since the goal of grocery shopping automatically instrumental in attaining goals that are repeatedly
activates the representation of the store we usually and consistently selected and performed in the same
go to. Thus, habitual behavior involves not only the context become habitual and associated with the
skilled execution but also the initial selection of a goal in the given context. Accordingly, goal-directed
means for goal pursuit, which can be automatized behavior no longer needs to be guided by conscious
based on earlier behavior and later executed in an intentions to attain the goal, but, instead, it is acti-
efficient, nonconscious fashion. vated and maintained by the representation of the
The idea that habitual behavior comprises the goal without conscious intervention.
automatic selection of a course of action upon It is important to emphasize here that, in line
the priming of a goal has received empirical sup- with the behaviorists’ perspective, habits have often
port in a number studies, and it was first tested in been (and are still) conceptualized as rigid responses
the domain of travel behavior (e.g., Aarts & Dijk- that are directly triggered by environmental cues,
sterhuis, 2000). Here, participants who had been while goal-directed behaviors are exclusively seen
primed with certain travel goals (e.g., going to fol- as the result of conscious intentional processes (see,
low classes) showed increased activation of certain e.g., Wood & Neal, 2007). In other words, goals are
means for traveling (e.g., biking). However, this treated as inherently equivalent to conscious inten-
effect occurred only among those students who tions and, hence, the obvious cause of the insti-
habitually used the bicycle to reach their travel gation of behavior to attain specific goals always
goals. These findings were replicated and extended relies on consciousness. The research on habitually
in the domain of the habitual drinking of alcohol driven goal-directed behavior discussed here goes
among students in the United Kingdom (Sheeran beyond this conventional approach. Specifically, the

a a rts , c us ter s 


observation that our goal pursuits can emerge from then unconscious goal pursuit is bound to fail and
nonconscious cognitive processes as a result of prac- conscious processes are called to the fore. It is this
tice suggests that intentions and goals are distinct episode of awareness that is said to typify a shift
concepts that can operate independently from each from habitual to intentional control (e.g., James,
other, served by different processes (see also Bargh, 1890; Louis & Sutton, 1991; Norman & Shallice,
1990). In line with this idea, neuroscientific research 1986).
on motor skill control suggests that conscious inten- Although tempting, this suggestion is ques-
tions are recruited and handled by the medial pre- tionable. Given the acknowledged limitations of
frontal cortex, whereas the control of goals occurs conscious attention on the one hand (Kahneman,
in more posterior, parietal areas interconnected 1973), and the dynamic nature of the world on the
with motor and sensory areas in the cortex to enable other (Powers, 1973), it seems that people should
action preparation and execution (e.g. Frith et al., be able to go beyond routines to efficiently adapt
2000). Therefore, priming these goal representations to the environment, even during unconscious goal
causes the organism to recruit the associated means pursuit (see also Wilson, 2002). Indeed, previous
or skills directly, and thus goal-directed behavior is work on goal priming indicates that people who are
triggered and guided in the situation at hand with- unconsciously primed with goals display behavior
out conscious intent and awareness. in novel settings, overcome obstacles, and invest
effort to achieve the primed goal. In other words,
When Habits Fail: Unconscious Goal unconscious goal pursuit is flexible and adaptive.
Pursuit Is Flexible and Adaptive This observation has led researchers to posit that
Thus far, unconscious goal pursuit is mainly nonconscious goals operate via cognitive processes
explored as a form of habit: Once the goal is acti- that follow principles of self-regulation or executive
vated by the situation, habitual behavior follows control and working memory (Aarts, 2007; Hassin,
a well-practiced route to completion with some Aarts, Custers, Eitam, & Kleinman, 2009). Whereas
degrees of freedom by the operation of a perceptual commonly conceived of as belonging to the realm
feedback-control system. Sometimes, however, the of consciousness, executive processes supporting
situation does not allow for a direct execution of goal pursuit are proposed to operate in the absence
habitual means or skills, or it contains distractions of awareness of the activation and operation of the
and temptations that push our current goal out of goal. While this may be a controversial proposition,
the attentional system. In that case, we may need there is accumulating evidence for it.
to postpone our goals, shield them from interfer-
ing or prepotent responses, and act on opportu- active maintenance of goal-relevant
nities to attain these goals. For example, a person representations
who frequently pursues the goal of being popular The activation of semantic items decays in
may need to wait for the right moment or adjust short-term memory over very short periods of
his behavior to the context at hand to successfully time, usually within a couple of seconds, unless
attain the goal (e.g., telling sexually explicit jokes some intervention or goal holds the items active
when sitting in a pub with friends, but gossiping (Baddeley & Logie, 1999; McKone, 1995). Exploit-
about the minister of education during lunch with ing this notion, research has demonstrated that
colleagues). Furthermore, when having the goal to goals that are activated unconsciously can keep rel-
use healthy food at lunch, one may need to resist the evant information active as well (Aarts, Custers, &
temptation to eat the delicious snacks abundantly Holland, 2007; Aarts, Custers, & Marien, 2009;
available in the cafeteria; or one needs to switch to Aarts, Custers, & Veltkamp, 2008). For instance,
alternatives when one discovers that the cafeteria is Aarts, Custers, and Holland (2007) examined how
currently out of one’s habitually selected bananas, in the mental accessibility of a goal after a short inter-
order to eat healthy food. val changes as a function of subliminally priming
An important issue, then, is how unconscious the goal. In one of their studies, participants were
goal pursuit proceeds when habits are inadequate either primed with the goal to socialize or not, and
and fail to produce goals. The traditional answer 2.5 minutes later tested for accessibility of the goal
would be that we do not: Unconscious goal pur- in a lexical decision task by measuring the speed
suit, like every other automatic process, is limited of recognizing words related to the goal as exist-
to circumstances in which habits can be applied ing words. Results showed that the representation
successfully. If they cannot be applied successfully, of the goal remained accessible when participants

 unconscious goal pursuit


were primed to attain the goal. Similar persistent in novel settings without awareness of operation of
activation effects—even after 5 minutes of goal the goal (Aarts et al., 2004; Custers & Aarts, 2005a;
priming—have been obtained for behavioral 2007a). For instance, Aarts et al. (2004) showed that
measures (Aarts et al., 2004; Bargh et al., 2001), priming the goal of earning money encouraged partici-
suggesting that some kind of updating or active pants to engage in a lottery that gave access to money,
maintenance process keeps goal-relevant informa- but only when they were in need of money, that is,
tion alive nonconsciously. From a functional point when the primed desired goal state was discrepant with
of view, this observation makes sense: When a goal the actual state. Participants claimed that they were
operates as a desired state it should be kept actively not aware of the priming effects, thus showing that the
in mind, thereby increasing the probability of acting detection and reduction of discrepancies may occur in
on it when encountering goal-relevant opportuni- the absence of conscious awareness. In another, more
ties (e.g., asking a colleague that enters one’s office compelling study, Custers and Aarts (2007a) investi-
to meet up later on in the bar). gated the goal of looking well groomed, a goal which
typically needs to be maintained over time and was
inhibition of goal-interfering highly desirable to participants. In their study, they
representations subliminally primed the goal or not, just before par-
Furthermore, recent work has started to explore ticipants were confronted with a situation that was
whether humans can keep their eyes on their ongo- discrepant with the goal (e.g., the shoes they put on
ing goal pursuit in a nonconscious manner when were dirty). Then, they implicitly measured the acces-
competing information conflicts with these pursuits sibility of actions that are instrumental in reducing the
(Aarts et al., 2007; Papies, Stroebe, & Aarts, 2008; discrepancy (e.g., polishing) in action-identification
Shah, Friedman, & Kruglanski, 2002). People usually reaction time task. Their findings showed that sub-
engage in this type of self-regulatory process when liminal priming facilitated the identification of instru-
they have to deal with interference that stems from mental actions. Additional studies showed that these
other goals or temptations that compete for attention priming effects do not emerge when the situation is
and behavior; a process that is commonly conceived not discrepant with the primed goal (Custers & Aarts,
of as requiring conscious and intentional control (see, 2005a). Together, these data suggest that unconscious
e.g., work on delay of gratification; Mischel, Shoda, goal pursuit is supported by monitoring and feedback
& Rodriguez, 1989). However, there are studies that processing.
tell a somewhat different story. For instance, Shah and In sum, several lines of research suggest that non-
colleagues (2002) demonstrated that when partici- conscious goals not only run off in a habitual, auto-
pants are nonconsciously instigated to pursue a given matic way, but they may serve flexible functions that
goal (by subliminal exposure to words representing operate via cognitive processes following principles
the goal, e.g., of studying), they inhibit competing of executive control and working memory that rely
accessible goals (e.g., going out), and moreover, this on the mobilization of effort and the recruitment
inhibition facilitated the achievement of the noncon- of resources (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974; Miyake &
sciously activated goal. These findings provide sup- Shah, 1999). Contrary to most current views (e.g.,
port for the existence of a nonconscious attention/ Baars & Franklin, 2003; Baddeley, 1993), however,
inhibition mechanism that shields goals from dis- these processes (and the information on which they
tracting thoughts. Shah et al. speculated that these operate) seem to run below the threshold of con-
goal-shielding effects require extensive practice, thus sciousness (see also Dijksterhuis & Aarts, 2010).
arguing for a habitual and well-learned automated The findings discussed earlier raise the interest-
mechanism. Recent studies, however, indicate that ing issue of how people mobilize effort and allocate
the inhibitory effects in goal-directed behavior may resources to pursue a primed goal without an act
kick in rather rapidly—that is, after a few practice tri- of conscious will. That is, what is the nonconscious
als (Danner, Aarts, & De Vries, 2007; McCulloch, source that motivates people to initiate and regu-
Aarts, Fujita, & Bargh, 2008; Veling & Aarts, 2009). late their goals in the first place? This is the issue to
which we turn now.
goal monitoring and feedback
processing Unconscious Motivation: Positive Affect
Finally, there are a few studies that tested whether and Reward Processing
situations that are discrepant with nonconsciously acti- When scrutinizing the scientific literature on the
vated goals encourage people to exploit opportunities pressing question of what motivates people to set

a a rts , c us ter s 


and pursue their goals, most answers boil down to TOYOTA COROLLA evokes a better gut feeling
the idea that humans are equipped with an internal than the NISSAN SUNNY) often determines what
agent that prioritizes and decides what we want and people decide to do without them being aware of
do. That is, the step from priming a cognitive rep- the influence of the affective signal. Thus, if goal
resentation of a goal to the process of motivating representations would contain an affective compo-
goal pursuit involves an act of conscious will (e.g., nent that reflects the rewarding value of the goal,
Gollwitzer, 1990; Locke & Latham, 1990; Monsell this information could be used to nonconsciously
& Driver, 2000). Research on nonconscious goal determine whether a primed goal is desired, which
pursuit suggests that this assumption may not be renders conscious deliberations redundant.
entirely right: The step from goal priming to moti- In line with this notion, neuroimaging research
vated behavior can be taken nonconsciously. An has discovered that reward cues are processed by
important issue, though, concerns the question of limbic structures such as the nucleus accumbens and
how this works: How do people resolve whether the ventral striatum. These subcortical areas play a
to pursue and to invest effort or recruit resources central role in determining the rewarding value of
to attain a given goal without involvement of con- outcomes and are connected to frontal areas in the
scious will? cortex that facilitate goal pursuit (e.g., Aston-Jones
Current social cognition research tends to answer & Cohen, 2005). These reward centers in the brain
this question by proposing that the process of form- respond to evolutionarily relevant rewards such as
ing an intention or decision to pursue a goal can food and sexual stimuli but also to learned rewards
take place outside awareness. Whereas this proposi- (e.g., money, status) or words (e.g., good, nice)
tion led researchers to come up with original terms that are associated with praise or rewards (Schultz,
to conceptualize the source of human goal pursuit, 2006). This demonstrates that regardless of their
such as automated will (Bargh et al., 2001), implicit shape or form, such positive stimuli induce a reward
intention (Wood, Quinn, & Kashy, 2002), or signal that is readily picked up by the brain (Shizgal,
implicit volition (Moskowitz, Li, & Kirk, 2004), 1997).
these terms merely stretch the applicability of inher- Other recent research has demonstrated that sub-
ently conscious concepts featured in existing mod- liminal primes that are specifically related to rewards
els to the unconscious level. Obviously, this strategy can motivate people to increase the effort they
has certainly helped to put the exciting notion of invest in behaviors. In one study (Pessiglione et al.,
nonconscious goal pursuit on the scientific research 2007), participants could earn money by squeezing
agenda, but it does not tell us much about how the a handgrip. Before each squeeze, the money that
unconscious, rather than consciousness, determines could be earned was indicated by a one-pound or
whether to pursue a goal or not. one-penny coin on the screen. Whereas on some tri-
Another approach is to make an inventory of als the coin was clearly visible, on others it was pre-
what people can do unconsciously and construct a sented subliminally. Thus, effects of conscious and
hypothesis in line with that knowledge. According unconscious reward cues could be compared within
to almost all models of goal pursuit, whether a goal one experiment. It was found that people squeezed
is pursued depends on the rewarding value of the harder on high- than on low-reward trials, regard-
goal state. The best candidate for a mechanism that less of whether the reward was consciously visible.
could determine the value of a primed goal outside Moreover, this effect was accompanied by activation
conscious awareness would therefore be one that in the brain areas that play a role in reward process-
relies on affective processes. It is known that affect ing and in the recruitment of effort for action. Simi-
plays a fundamental role in motivating human action lar effects of unconscious (and conscious) monetary
and can be evoked quite quickly without reaching rewards have been established in cognitive tasks that
conscious awareness (e.g., Damasio, 1994; Dijkster- require flexibility and cognitive resources (Bijleveld,
huis & Aarts, 2003; Fazio, 2001; LeDoux, 1996; Custers, & Aarts, 2009, 2010; Zedelius, Veling, &
Zajonc, 1980). For instance, in several experiments Aarts, 2011). These findings indicate that conscious
it has been shown that affective words (e.g., sum- and unconscious reward cues have similar effects
mer, shark) are classified in terms of their valence on effort and flexible cognitive processing, which
before participants know the meaning of the word. suggests that conscious awareness of rewards is not
Furthermore, the affective tone associated with needed for goal pursuit to occur.
options in a decision problem (e.g., while inspect- The observation that a variety of reward cues
ing the several occasions at the local car garage the are encoded by the same brain system to motivate

 unconscious goal pursuit


cognition and action and can be processed uncon- desired goal is activated, which, because of its asso-
sciously has led to the proposal that a positive ciation with positive affect, sets off a positive reward
reward signal associated with outcomes plays a signal. In theory, this process could be simulated by
crucial role in unconscious goal pursuit (Custers & externally triggering the affective signal just after
Aarts, 2010). Specifically, when a desired outcome activation of a neutral goal concept (i.e., a goal
or goal is primed, activation of the mental repre- concept that provides a reference point for action
sentation of this outcome is immediately followed but does not designate a current desired state that
by the activation of an associated positive affective people are motivated to pursue). This ability to
tag, which acts as a reward signal for pursuing the respond to the mere coactivation of goal representa-
primed goal. The positive reward signal attached to tions and positive affective cues is thought to play
a goal thus unconsciously facilitates the actual selec- a fundamental role in social learning (Miller & Dol-
tion of the goal and the subsequent mobilization lard, 1941) and considered as basic in motivational
of effort and resources to maintain the goal, unless analyses of human behavior (Shizgal, 1997). Thus,
other (e.g., more rewarding) goals gain priority. This when a child observes ones mother’s smile upon
affective-motivational process relies on associations munching homemade cookies, a student witnesses
between the representations of outcomes and posi- a hilarious joke upon entering the classroom, or
tive reward signals that are shaped by one’s history a person strolling around in the mall hears people
(e.g., when a person was happy when making money laugh while reading on a billboard “start your holi-
or performing well). In this case, the goal is said to day here,” this can cause the goal representations
preexist as a desired state in the mind. Priming this that are primed by those situations (eating candy,
goal representation not only prepares the appropri- achieving at school, booking a vacation) to acquire
ate instrumental actions but also motivates behav- an intrinsic reward value, which prepares and regu-
ior, rendering it persistent and flexible, directed at lates goal-directed behavior.
attaining the desired outcome. This hypothesis that the mere coactivation of
A recent set of studies tested investigated the role a neutral goal concept and positive affect produces
of this positive reward signal attached to a goal in unconscious goal pursuit has been tested as well
the effects of subliminal goal priming in teenagers (Aarts, Custers, & Veltkamp, 2008; Van den Bos
and young adults (Custers & Aarts, 2007b; Fergu- & Stapel, 2009; Custers & Aarts, 2005b; Holland
son, 2007). For instance, Custers and Aarts (2007b) et al., 2009; Veltkamp, Aarts, & Custers, 2008,
subliminally primed participants or not with the 2009). In these studies, goal concepts were paired
goal of going out socially. Next, they performed with positively valenced information outside of
a mouse-click task that, if sufficient time was left, conscious awareness by exploiting the evaluative
was followed by a lottery in which they could win conditioning paradigm (De Houwer et al., 2001).
tickets for a popular student party. Thus, working For instance, it has been shown that repeated pair-
hard (or fast) on the task can be seen as a means to ing of the representation of a neutral goal concept
get to the goal of socializing. It was established that (e.g., words such as drinking, cleaning up, doing
participants put more effort into the instrumental puzzles) and positive affect (e.g., words such as sum-
task to attain the goal state when the goal concept mer or nice) motivates participants to work harder
of “socializing” was primed, and that this effect was on an intervening task to secure engagement in
more pronounced when the goal concept was more the behavior (Custers & Aarts, 2005b). In another
positive (which was assessed in a separate implicit study, effects of linking the behavioral concept of
affective association task). These findings show drinking to positive affect were compared with the
that goal-priming effects on motivated behavior deprivation of water on the amount of water that
and action control are conditional on the positive was consumed in a tasting task. The results of this
valence attached to the primed goal. Similar effects study showed that deprivation increased the amount
of positive reward value attached to a goal have been of drinking, and that shaping drinking more posi-
documented for other, perhaps more consequential tively caused participants to drink more water only
behaviors. Priming an egalitarian goal, for instance, when they were not deprived. These findings indi-
changes people’s voting behavior to the extent that cate that linking neutral goal concepts to positive
this goal is represented as positive or rewarding affect simulates effects of actual needs (Veltkamp
(Ferguson, 2007). et al., 2009).
The findings presented earlier indicate that uncon- Furthermore, extending past research into
scious goal pursuit may result when a preexisting the relation between motivation and functional

a a rts , c us ter s 


perception (Bruner & Goodman, 1947; Bruner & as well as flexible behaviors that occur when habitual
Postman, 1948), other research demonstrated that responding is not adequate to attain goals. Fur-
the nonconscious goal-shaping treatment (e.g., of thermore, this unconscious control of goal pursuit
drinking) affected size perception of goal-related is initiated and maintained by the mere processing
objects (e.g., a glass of water): participants saw of reward signals that accompany the priming of
them as being bigger in size (Veltkamp et al., 2008). the representation of a goal. This way, people can
Moreover, these motivated functional percep- pursue and attain desired outcomes without being
tion effects were manifested even after a period of aware of the activation and operation of the goal
3 minutes, suggesting that some kind of rehearsal or leading to the outcome. Whereas the amount of sci-
active maintenance process kept the nonconsciously entific evidence for the occurrence of unconscious
shaped goal alive in the mind (Aarts, Custers, & goal pursuit is still growing, there are several issues
Veltkamp, 2008). Importantly, these effects could that require further scrutiny to offer a more com-
not be attributed to initial differences in activation prehensive understanding and examination of the
of the goal concept as all participants were primed role of nonconscious processes in human goal pur-
with the mental representation of the goal before suit. We will briefly address three of them here.
the dependent variables were assessed. What mat- The first issue pertains to the question of whether
tered was the fact that the goal concept was activated people are truly unaware of the goal that produces
in temporal proximity to the activation of positive their behavior. It is clear that the notion of uncon-
affect, and as a consequence, evoked in people a state scious goal pursuit is not readily appreciated and
of readiness for goal pursuit that not only prepared endorsed by all people. After all, we all share the
but also regulated their behavior to attain the goal. experience that our behavior is accompanied by
A recent study examined the effects of coactivat- conscious awareness, and it feels odd to assume that
ing goal representations and positive reward signals these conscious experiences have no causal status in
on the preparation and motivation of behavior in the process of goal pursuit. Accordingly, the idea
more detail. In this study, healthy young adults had that consciousness does not mediate goal-priming
to squeeze a handgrip in response to a start sign while effects is still a matter of debate.
the timing and persistence of their behavior were mea- To offer compelling evidence that goal pursuit
sured (Aarts, Custers, & Marien, 2008). Prior to this can be truly unconscious, researchers have tried to
task, words pertaining to the goal of physical exertion do their best to conceal the purpose of their study to
were subliminally presented (or not) together with participants (in the so-called unrelated studies setup)
positive words that signal rewards (e.g., good, nice) or took pains to prime goals unconsciously by sub-
or not. In line with the ideomotor principle, research liminal presentation techniques. In addition, some
participants who were subliminally primed with the studies asked participants in retrospect to indicate
goal of exertion started to squeeze earlier. However, whether they were influenced by the primes or moti-
only participants for whom the goal was coactivated vated to pursue the primed goal. The general finding
with a positive reward signal recruited more resources of these checks is that although reported motiva-
to execute this goal, as was evidenced by more forceful tion sometimes correlates with behavior (people
and persistent squeezing. It was found that consciously who worked harder report to be more motivated),
reported motivation did not show any relation with these reports are not influenced by the primes.
the subliminal goal-priming manipulation. Hence, This suggests that subliminal priming of the goal
activating a goal representation gives behavior a head does not affect goal pursuit because people become
start, whereas the accompanying reward signal moti- conscious of their motivation to pursue the goal
vates behavior outside awareness. Other studies have after it is primed. Participants may become conscious
shown that this coactivation procedure yields effects of their motivation after the behavior is performed
that are similar to those of conscious goals (induced and when they are explicitly probed to reflect on it.
by conscious goal instructions or by making people We believe that such checks should be a default part
aware of their current needs) on tasks that require of the experimental setup in the study on uncon-
flexibility and effort in novel situations (Custers & scious goal pursuit, and future research should even
Aarts, 2005b; Veltkamp et al., 2009). try to go further by designing new and more fine-
grained ways to examine whether consciousness
Conclusion and Future Directions plays a causal role in the priming of goal pursuit
The present review indicates that unconscious (e.g., Seth, Dienes, Cleeremans, Overgaard, &
goal pursuit relies on automated habitual behaviors Pessoa, 2008).

 unconscious goal pursuit


The second issue concerns the observation that consciousness as a relatively new knack of human
the control of unconscious goals is flexible and evolution is required to overrule the labor of the
effortful, suited to meet the dynamics of the envi- older reward system involved in unconscious goal
ronment. Such unconscious flexibility fits well with pursuit.
research that has discovered that human function- Recently, researchers have started to explore the
ing (information encoding, memory use, evaluation, role of negative affect in this process, and it turns
inferences, social perception and judgment) is largely out that negative stimuli that are coactivated with
rooted in cognitive processes and does not require the subliminal priming of goals can put unconscious
conscious control. However, understanding exactly goal pursuit on a hold (e.g., Aarts, Custers, & Hol-
how unconscious goals flexibly control behavior land, 2007; Veling, Aarts, & Stroebe, 2011). Impor-
remains a challenge for future research. It has been tantly, this modulating effect of negative affect on
argued that goals direct attention and behavior, even the cessation of unconscious goal pursuit may not be
in the absence of conscious awareness of the goal so general, as other studies suggest that people can
(Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Dijksterhuis & Aarts, also be motivated by negative affect, such as when
2010). That is, the operation of cognitive processes goals are associated with anger (Aarts, Ruys, Veling,
supporting goal pursuit (also conceptualized as Renes, de Groot, van Nunen, & Geertjes, 2010;
working memory or executive control) does not care Carver & Harmon-Jones, 2009). It remains to be
much about the conscious state of the individual. seen, then, whether and how negative affect accom-
This view concurs with recent insights that atten- panying the activation of goals serves as an uncon-
tion and consciousness are distinct (Dehaene et al., scious veto to not engage in goal pursuit itself.
2006; Koch & Tsuchiya, 2006; Lamme, 2003).
The research discussed here suggests that con- Acknowledgment
scious goals (often induced by explicit task instruc- The preparation of this chapter was supported by VICI-grant
tions) and unconscious goals (induced by priming) 453-06-002 from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific
have similar effects on tasks that rely on executive Research.
control. However, it is too early to conclude that
consciousness is redundant in the pursuit of goals, References
as we do not yet know whether there are special Aarts, H. (2007). Health behavior and the implicit motivation
cases in which consciousness (apart from attention) and regulation of goals. Health Psychology Review, 1, 53–82.
facilitates performance. In fact, we only know that Aarts, H., Chartrand, T. L., Custers, R., Danner, U., Dik,
G., Jefferis, V., & Cheng, C. M. (2005). Social Stereotypes
we can become consciously aware of the decisions and automatic goal pursuit. Social Cognition, 23, 464–489.
that we make and the goals we pursue without hav- Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Holland, R. W. (2007). The non-
ing a proper empirical test telling us how conscious- conscious cessation of goal pursuit: When goals and nega-
ness itself exactly influences our behavior. Future tive affect are coactivated. Journal of Personality and Social
research thus will have to explore when consciously Psychology, 92, 165–178.
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Marien, H. (2008). Preparing and
and unconsciously activated goals direct the opera- motivating behavior outside of awareness. Science, 319,
tion of cognitive functions and brain systems sup- 1639.
porting goal pursuit in similar or distinct manners. Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Marien, H. (2009). Priming and
A third issue that may be worth examining is the authorship ascription: When nonconscious goals turn into
idea that a potential role for consciousness may lie conscious experiences of self-agency. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 96, 967–979.
not in the starting and steering of behavior, but in Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Veltkamp, M. (2008). Goal priming
stopping it. Whereas the brain is designed to realize and the affective-motivational route to nonconscious goal
desired outcomes, in today’s society the well-being pursuit. Social Cognition, 26, 497–519.
of the individual may for a large part be depen- Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2000). Habits as knowledge
dent on the ability to prevent oneself from engag- structures: Automaticity in goal-directed behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 53–63.
ing in rewarding behaviors that have undesired Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). The silence of the library:
personal and social long-term consequences (e.g., Environment, situational norm and Social behavior. Journal
eating junk food, derogating others). Whereas it is of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 18–28.
known that ongoing and impulsive behaviors can be Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., Dik, G. (2008). Goal contagion.
inhibited directly by environmental stimuli or well- In J. Y. Shah & W. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
science (pp. 265–280). New York: Guilford.
learned stop rules (Verbruggen & Logan, 2009), Aarts, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Hassin, R. R. (2004). Goal
we do not yet know whether and how people can contagion: Perceiving is for pursuing. Journal of Personality
express an unconscious volitional veto, or whether and Social Psychology, 87, 23–37.

a a rts , c us ter s 


Aarts, H., Ruys, K. I., Veling, H., Renes, R. A., de Groot, Bongers, K. C. A., Dijksterhuis, A., & Spears, R. (2010). On
J. H. B., van Nunen, A. M., & Geertjes S. (2010). The the role of consciousness in goal pursuit. Social Cognition,
art of anger: Reward context turns avoidance responses to 28, 262–272.
anger-related objects into approach. Psychological Science, 21, Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1961). The misbehavior of organ-
1406–1410. isms. American Psychologist, 16, 681–684.
Adams, J. A. (1984). Learning of motor movement. Psychological Brown, P. L., & Jenkins, H. M. (1968). Auto-shaping of the
Bulletin, 96, 3–28. pigeon’s key-peck. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Aston-Jones, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2005). An integrative theory Behavior, 11, 1–8.
of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: Adaptive gain Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as
and optimal performance. Annual Review of Neuroscience, organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal and
28, 403–450. Social Psychology, 42, 33–44.
Baars, B. J., & Franklin, S. (2003). How conscious experience Bruner, J. S., & Postman, L. (1948). Symbolic value as an orga-
and working memory interact. Trends in cognitive science, nizing factor in perception. Journal of Social Psychology, 27,
7(4), 166–172. 203–208.
Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and conscious Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is approach-
awareness. In A. Collins & S. Gathercole (Eds.), Theories related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bul-
of memory (pp. 11–28). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum letin, 135, 183–204.
Associates. Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Cooper, R. P., & Shallice, T. (2006). Hierarchical schemas and
Advances in research and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). New goals in the control of sequential behavior. Psychological
York: Academic Press. Review, 113, 887–916.
Baddeley, A. D., & Logie, R. H. (1999). Working memory: The Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005a). Beyond accessibility: The role
multiple-component model. In A. Miyake & P. Shah (Eds.), of affect and goal-discrepancies in implicit processes of moti-
Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance vation and goal-pursuit. European Review of Social Psychology,
and executive control (pp. 28–61). New York: Cambridge 16, 257–300.
University Press. Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005b). Positive affect as implicit moti-
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: vator: On the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals.
A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 129–142.
Bargh, J. A. (1990). Goal not-equal-to intent: Goal-directed Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2007a). Goal-discrepant situations
thought and behavior are often unintentional. Psychological prime goal-directed actions if goals are temporarily or chron-
Inquiry, 1, 248–251. ically accessible. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In 623–633.
R. S. Wyer (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life: Advances in Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2007b). In search of the nonconscious
social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. sources of goal pursuit: Accessibility and positive affec-
Bargh, J. A. (2006). What have we been priming all these years? tive valence of the goal state. Journal of Experimental Social
On the development, mechanisms, and ecology of noncon- Psychology, 43, 312–318.
scious social behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, Custers, R, & Aarts, H. (2010). The unconscious will: How the
36, 147–168. pursuit of goals operates outside of conscious awareness.
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: Science, 329, 47–50.
On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological Custers, R., Aarts, H., Oikawa, M., & Elliot, A. (2009). The
Bulletin, 126, 925–945. nonconscious road to perceptions of performance: Achieve-
Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1994). Environmental control ment priming, success expectations and self-agency. Journal
of goal-directed action: Automatic and strategic contingen- of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 1200–1208.
cies between situations and behavior. In W. D. Spaulding Custers, R., Maas, M., Wildenbeest, M., & Aarts, H. (2008).
(Ed.), Integrative views of motivation, cognition, and emotion. Nonconscious goal pursuit and the surmounting of physical
Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 41, pp. 71–124). and social obstacles. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38,
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. 1013–1022.
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion reason, and the
Trötschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious human brain. New York: Putnam.
activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Person- Danner, U. N., Aarts, H., & De Vries, N. K. (2007). Habit
ality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027. formation and multiple options to goal attainment:
Berridge, K. C. (2001). Reward learning: Reinforcement, incen- Repeated selection of targets means causes inhibited access
tives, and expectations. In D. L. Medin (Ed.), The psychology to alternatives. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33,
of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory 1367–1379.
(Vol. 40, pp. 223–278). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2009). The unconscious determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
eye opener: Pupil dilation reveals strategic recruitment of Dehaene, S., Changeux, J. P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., &
mental resources upon subliminal reward cues. Psychological Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal
Science, 20, 1313–1315. processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2010). Unconscious 10, 204–211.
reward cues increase invested effort, but do not change De Houwer, J., Thomas, S., & Baeyens, F. (2001). Association
speed-accuracy tradeoffs. Cognition, 115, 330–335. learning of likes and dislikes: A review of 25 years of research

 unconscious goal pursuit


on human evaluative conditioning. Psychological Bulletin, human action (pp. 549–566). New York: Oxford University
127, 853–869. Press.
Dickinson, A., Balleine, B., Watt, A., Gonzales, F., & Boakes, R. A. Hassin, R. R. (2008). Being open minded without knowing why:
(1995). Motivational control after extended instrumental Evidence from nonconscious goal pursuit. Social Cognition,
training. Animal Learning and Behavior, 23, 197–206. 26, 578–592.
Dik, G., & Aarts, H. (2007). Behavioral cues to others’ moti- Hofmann, W., De Houwer, J., Perugini, M., Baeyens, F., &
vation and goal-pursuits: The perception of effort facilitates Crombez, G. (2010). Evaluative conditioning in humans:
goal inference and contagion. Journal of Experimental Social A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 390–421.
psychology, 43, 727–737. Holland, R. W., Wennekers, A. M., Bijlstra, G.,
Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2003). On wildebeests and Jongenelen, M. M., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2009).
humans: The preferential detection of negative stimuli. Self-symbols as implicit motivators. Social Cognition, 27,
Psychological Science, 14, 14–18. 579–600.
Dijksterhuis, A., & Aarts, H. (2010). Goals, attention, and (un) Hommel, B. (1993). Inverting the Simon effect by intention:
consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 467–490. Determinants of direction and extent of effects of irrelevant
Eder, A., & Rothermund, K. (2008). When do motor behaviors spatial information. Psychological Research, 55, 270–279
(mis)match affective stimuli? An evaluative coding view of Hommel, B. (2000). The prepared reflex: Automaticity and con-
approach and avoidance reactions. Journal of Experimental trol in s-r translation. In S. Monsell & J. Driver (Eds.), Con-
Psychology: General, 137, 262–281. trol of cognitive processes: Attention and performance XVIII.
Edmonds, D. E., & Gallistel, C. R. (1974). Parametric analysis (pp. 247–273). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
of brain stimulation reward in the rat: III. Effect of per- Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001).
formance variables on the reward summation function. The theory of event coding (tec): A framework for percep-
Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 87, tion and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24,
876–883. 849–937.
Eitam, B., Hassin, R. R., & Schul, Y. (2008). Nonconscious goal James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. London: Mac-
pursuit in novel environments: The case of implicit learning. millan.
Psychological Science, 19, 261–267. Jeannerod, M. (1997). The cognitive neuroscience of action.
Engeser, S., Wendland, M., & Rheinberg, F. (2006). Noncon- Oxford, England: Blackwell Publishers.
scious activation of behavioral goals, a methodologically Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewoods Cliff,
refined replication. Psychological Reports, 99, 963. NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Fazio, R. H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associ- Koch, C., & Tsuchiya, N. (2006). Attention and consciousness:
ated evaluations: An overview. Cognition and Emotion, 15, Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11,
115–141. 16–22.
Ferguson, M. J. (2007). On the automatic evaluation of end-states. Kraus, M. W., & Chen, S. (2009). Striving to be known by sig-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 596–611. nificant others: Automatic activation of self-verification goals
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: in relationship contexts. Journal of Personality and Social
Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals associated Psychology, 97, 58.
with relationship partners. Journal of Personality and Social Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, R.,
Psychology, 84, 148–163. Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of goal-
Fourneret, P., & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious moni- systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social
toring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsy- psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). New York: Academic
chologia, 36, 1133–1140. Press.
Friedman, R., Deci, E.L., Elliot, A., Moller, A., & Aarts, H. Lamme, V. A. F. (2003). Why visual attention and awareness are
(2010). Motivation synchronicity: Priming motivational ori- different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 12–18.
entations with observations of others’ behavior. Motivation LeDoux, J. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon &
and Emotion, 34, 34–38. Schuster.
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S-J., & Wolpert, D. M. (2000). Abnor- Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role
malities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioural and Brain
Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 355, 1771–1788. Science, 8, 529–566.
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1990). Action phases and mindsets. In Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and
R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of moti- task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
vation and cognition (pp. 53–92). New York: Guilford Press. Loersch, C., Aarts, H., Payne, B. K., & Jefferis, V. E. (2008)
Gollwitzer, P. M., & Moskowitz, G. B. (1996). Goal effects on The influence of social groups on goal contagion. Journal of
action and cognition. In E. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1555–1558.
(Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. Louis, M. R., & Sutton, R. I. (1991). Switching cognitive gears:
361–399). New York: Guilford Press. From habits of mind to active thinking. Human Relations,
Hart, W., & Albarracín, D. (2009). The effects of chronic 44, 55–76.
achievement motivation and achievement primes on the McCulloch, K. C., Aarts, H., Fujita, J., & Bargh, J. A. (2008).
activation of achievement and fun goals. Journal of Personal- Inhibition in goal systems: A retrieval-induced forget-
ity and Social Psychology, 97, 1129–1141. ting account. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
Hassin, R. R., Aarts, H., Custers, R., Eitam, B., & Kleiman, 857–865.
T. (2009). Non-conscious goal pursuit, working mem- Mckone, E. (1995). Short-term implicit memory for words and
ory, and the effortful control of behavior. In E. Morsella, nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning,
J. A. Bargh, & P. M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of Memory and Cognition, 21, 1108–1126.

a a rts , c us ter s 


Miller, N. E., & Dollard, J. (1941). Social learning and imitation. Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forget-
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal
Mischel, W., Shoda,Y., & Rodriguez, M.I. (1989). Delay of shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83,
gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938. 1261–1280.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Shantz, A., & Latham, G. P. (2009). An exploratory field experi-
Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diver- ment of the effect of subconscious and conscious goals on
sity of executive functions and their contributions to com- employee performance. Organizational Behavior and Human
plex “Frontal lobe” Tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Decision Processes, 109, 9–17.
Psychology, 41, 49–100. Sheeran, P., Aarts, H., Custers, R., Rivis, A., Webb, T. L., &
Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mech- Cooke, R. (2005). The goal-dependent automaticity of
anisms of active maintenance and executive control. New York: drinking habits. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44,
Cambridge University Press. 47–63.
Monsell, S., & Driver, J. (2000). Control of cognitive processes: Shizgal, P. (1997). Neural basis of utility estimation. Current
Attention and performance XVIII. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 198–208.
Press. Shizgal, P. (1999). On the neural computation of utility:
Moskowitz, G. B., Gollwitzer, P. M., Wasel, W., & Schaal, B. Implications from studies of brain stimulation reward. In
(1999). Preconscious control of stereotype activation through D. Kahneman, E. Diener, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Well-being:
chronic egalitarian goals. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- The foundations of hedonic psychology (pp. 500–524). New
chology, 77, 167–184. York: Russell Sage Foundation.
Moskowitz, G. B., Li, P., & Kirk, E. R. (2004). The implicit Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. Oxford,
volition model: On the preconscious regulation of temporar- England: Macmillan.
ily adopted goals. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experi- Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1987). What do people
mental social psychology (Vol. 36, pp. 317–404). New York: think they’re doing? Action identification and human behav-
Academic Press. ior. Psychological Review, 94, 3–15.
Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention and action: Van den Bos, A., & Stapel, D. A. (2009). Why people stereo-
Willed and automatic control of behavior. In R. J. Davidson, type affects how they stereotype: The differential influence
G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self- of comprehension goals and self-enhancement goals on
regulation: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 4, pp. 1–18). stereotyping. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35,
New York: Plenum. 101–113.
Nørretranders, T. (1991). The user illusion: Cutting consciousness Veling, H., & Aarts, H. (2009). Putting behavior on hold
down to size. New York: Penguin. decreases reward value of need-instrumental objects outside
Oikawa, M. (2004). Moderation of automatic achievement of awareness. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45,
goals by conscious monitoring. Psychological Reports, 95, 1020–1023.
975–980. Veling, H., Aarts, H., & Stroebe, W. (2011). Fear signals inhibit
Olds, J. (1958). Self-stimulation of the brain. Science, 127, impulsive behavior toward rewarding food objects. Appetite,
315–324. 56, 643–648.
Olds, J., & Milner, P. (1954). Positive reinforcement produced Veltkamp, M., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2008). Perception in the
by electrical stimulation of septal area and other regions of service of goal pursuit: Motivation to attain goals enhances
rat brain. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, the perceived size of goal-instrumental object. Social Cogni-
47, 419–427. tion, 26, 720–736.
Papies, E. K., Stroebe, W., & Aarts, H. (2008). Healthy cogni- Veltkamp, M., Aarts, H., & Custers, R. (2009). Unravelling
tion: Processes of self-regulatory success in restrained eating. the motivational yarn: A framework for understanding the
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1290–1300. instigation of implicitly motivated behaviour resulting from
Pessiglione, M., Schmidt, L., Draganski, B., Kalisch, R., Lau, H., deprivation and positive affect. European Review of Social Psy-
Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). How the brain translates chology, 20, 345–381.
money into force: A neuroimaging study of subliminal moti- Verbruggen, F., & Logan, G. D. (2009). Automaticity of cogni-
vation. Science, 316, 904–906. tive control: Goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms.
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and
Chicago, IL: Aldine. Cognition, 35, 1381.
Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Watson, J. B. (1925). Behaviorism. New York: Norton and Com-
Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154. pany.
Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An Wegner, D. M. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge,
essay on the cognitive unconscious. New York: Oxford Univer- MA: MIT Press.
sity Press. Wickelgren, W. A. (1969). Auditory and articulatory codin-
Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioral theories and the neurophysiology gin verbal short-term memory. Psychological Review, 76,
of reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 87. 233–235.
Seth, A. K., Dienes, Z., Cleeremans, A., Overgaard, M., & Williams, D. R., & Williams, H. (1969). Auto-maintenance
Pessoa, L. (2008). Measuring consciousness: Relating behav- in the pigeon: Sustained pecking despite contingent non-
ioural and neurophysiological approaches. Trends in Cogni- reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behav-
tive Sciences, 12, 314–321. ior, 12, 511–520.
Shah, J. Y. (2003). Automatic for the people: How representa- Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adap-
tions of significant others implicitly affect goal pursuit. Jour- tive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 661–681. University Press.

 unconscious goal pursuit


Wood, W., & Neal, D. T. (2007). A new look at habits and the Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no
interface between habits and goals. Psychological Review, 114, inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175.
843–863. Zedelius, C. M., Veling, H., & Aarts, H. (2011). Boosting or
Wood, W., Quinn, J. M., & Kashy, D. A. (2002). Habits in choking—How conscious and unconscious reward process-
everyday life: Thought, emotion, and action. Journal of ing modulate the active maintenance of goal-relevant infor-
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1281–1297. mation. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 355–362.

a a rts , c us ter s 


CHAPTER

The Motivational Complexity of


15 Choosing: A Review of Theory and
Research
Erika A. Patall

Abstract
Years of research have implicated a complex set of motivational causes and consequences of choice.
Psychological theory has often prescribed the benefits of choosing, though limitations to this view
of choice as being ubiquitously positive are apparent. In this chapter, the relation between choice
and motivation is examined. Conceptualizations of choice as both an outcome of motivation and a
motivational experience are described. The benefits and determinants of receiving and perceiving choice
for motivation are then discussed according to various psychological theories. Next, the complex
and often contradictory findings regarding the relation between choice and motivation are discussed
in light of various factors (e.g., characteristics of choices, persons, and situations) that may influence
those effects. Issues that have yet to be adequately addressed in the research on choice effects and the
directions that future research might take are briefly discussed.
Key Words: provision of choice, perceived choice, decision making, autonomy, motivation

Introduction motivation. In this chapter, the relation between


People make a multitude of choices every day. choice and motivation is examined. First, an array
Considering any one of a number of personal of important factors that influence choice making
choices you may have made today will reveal that are briefly highlighted to illustrate how choice is
choices vary in type and are influenced by numer- often conceptualized as the outcome of a motiva-
ous factors. Not only is the motivation that under- tional process. I then turn to a more central focus
lies choice making complex, but the consequences of the chapter, to discuss how various psychologi-
of choosing may be powerful. Most people believe cal theories have conceptualized choice as an influ-
that having the freedom to choose is an essential ential predictor of motivation. In this section,
determinant of happiness and health, allowing them research emphasizing both the benefits of choos-
to control their fate and express individuality. Few ing and the detriments of choosing is presented.
other beliefs are more fundamental to Western cul- Next, in an attempt to understand the complex
ture than the belief that individuals have the right to and often contradictory relations that have been
freedom, liberty, and choice. revealed between choice and various motivation-
Given this colloquial understanding of the related outcomes, factors that may influence choice
complexity and importance of choosing, it comes effects are discussed. Finally, issues that have yet to
as no surprise that choice is implicated in numer- be adequately addressed in the research on choice
ous psychological theories, either as an outcome effects and the directions that future research might
of some motivational process or as a predictor of take are briefly discussed.


The Motivation to Choose not always conform to conventional assumptions of
Over time, choice has been conceptualized and rational choice. Rather, people are often engaging in
operationally defined in a variety of different ways satisficing, accepting a choice that is good enough,
in theories of human behavior. Consequently, the due to their cognitive limitations, limited informa-
study of choice and its relation with motivation tion, and the complexity of environment that limits
reflect this variability. In much theory and research, their ability to make a perfect choice (Simon, 1982,
choice is an outcome that can be explained by some 1987). Likewise, irrational choices may be a func-
motivational process. That is, choice is the end result tion of a number of judgment heuristics and biases
of a decision-making process in which an individual that influence the way people assess probabilities
has some freedom regarding whether to engage in under uncertain conditions and, thus, influence the
one behavior or not or one behavior over another. decisions they make (Kahneman & Tversky, 1979;
When looking at choice as a behavioral outcome, Tversky & Kahneman, 1974, 1981).
virtually any theory of human behavior might be A great deal of research and theory has high-
conceived to have something to say regarding the lighted the important role that expectancies about
relation between motivation and choice. Likewise, one’s abilities or the outcomes of behaving, as well
the research on what motivates people to choose as as one’s values for a particular object or behavior,
they do provides a long list of influential factors. play in influencing the choices an individual makes
For example, from the standpoint of early drive (Ajzen, 1985; Atkinson, 1964; Bandura, 1977; Eccles
theories (Maslow, 1954; Murray, 1938), people & Wigfield, 2002; Edwards, 1954; Feather, 1988;
choose one object or activity over another to the Lewin, 1936; Rotter, 1954; Tolman, 1932; Vroom,
extent that it satisfies one or more of many basic 1964). Self-efficacy or expectations regarding one’s
physiological and psychological needs (i.e., hunger, ability to adequately execute a specific behavior or
thirst, love, or achievement). According to Maslow sequence of behaviors relative to a particular goal or
(1954), needs exist in a hierarchical organization, criterion influence which behaviors one chooses to
such that lower level “deficit” needs (physiological engage in (Bandura, 1977, 1986, 1997). For exam-
and safety needs) are required to be satisfied before ple, students with higher mathematical self-efficacy
higher level “growth” needs (belongingness, esteem, were found to be more likely to choose to engage
and self-actualization needs). As such, when faced in the math task compared to other types of tasks
with a situation in which an individual has the (Bandura & Schunk, 1981), and measures of self-
choice of objects and activities that satisfy existing efficacy have been found to correlate significantly
needs at varying levels in the hierarchy, the object with career choices and students’ choice of majors
or activity that satisfies lower level needs will be in college (Betz & Hackett, 1981, 1983; Hackett
chosen. & Betz, 1981, 1989). Going further, maladaptive
A number of theories suggest that people simply choices may be made as a result of expectancies for
choose the best or most rewarding option among failure and self-presentational concerns. For exam-
the alternatives and avoid options that lead to unde- ple, Berglas and Jones (1978) demonstrated in their
sirable outcomes. From a traditional behaviorist research on self-handicapping that compared to
standpoint, a particular behavior will be chosen undergraduate students that were led to believe they
and initiated over another behavior to the extent had performed well on a series of analogy problems
that it has been frequently paired with a stimulus due to their high aptitude, students who were led
or followed by a reinforcing consequence in the to believe they had performed well on the task due
past (Skinner, 1953; Thorndike, 1913). Similarly, to lucky guessing (and thus would be unlikely to
according to rational choice theory, an influential do well on future tasks) were more likely to choose
theory in social sciences such as sociology, econom- to take a performance-inhibiting drug rather than
ics, and political science, individuals are motivated a performance-enhancing drug for a second round
to choose the best option given their goals and of problems.
information they have about the conditions under Expectancy-value theories of motivation suggest
which they are acting (Scott, 2000). Rational indi- that the choices an individual makes are a function
viduals must anticipate the outcome of each alterna- of the interaction between an individual’s beliefs
tive course of action and will choose the alternative about his or her ability to produce a particular out-
that will lead to the greatest satisfaction (Carling, come and his or her value for the task or attain-
1992; Coleman, 1973; Heath, 1976). Despite this ing a particular outcome (Ajzen, 1985; Atkinson,
hedonic assumption, the decisions people make do 1964; Edwards, 1954; Feather, 1988; Lewin, 1936;

pata ll 
Rotter, 1954; Vroom, 1964). For example, Vroom’s immediately or in the future and the extent to which
expectancy theory (1964), Atkinson’s theory of an activity is valued. Although early expectancy-value
achievement motivation (1964), and Rotter’s social theory and research (e.g., Atkinson, 1964; Vroom,
learning theory (1954) all predict that an indi- 1964) suggested that expectancies were related to task
vidual’s choices are a function of the product of choices, more recent research has suggested that value
an individual’s beliefs about his or her ability to beliefs are a better predictor of students’ academic
produce a particular outcome or consequence and choices than expectancies (Eccles, 1987; Eccles et al.,
the desirability or his or her value for attaining 1983; Eccles et al., 1984; Feather, 1988; Meece, Wig-
that particular outcome. The greater the product of field, & Eccles, 1990; Wigfield & Eccles, 1992). For
these elements, the more likely an individual will example, Eccles (1984) showed that fifth- through
be to choose a particular option or course of action. twelfth-grade students’ valuing of math predicted
Research has been consistent with these notions. For their intentions to keep taking more math courses
example, the combination of expectancies and val- more strongly than their expectancies for success.
ues has been found to predict occupational choice Likewise, Eccles, Adler, and Meece (1984) showed
(Mitchell, 1974; Van Eerne & Thierry, 1996) and that eighth- through tenth-grade students’ valuing
students’ academic choices (Feather, 1988) rather of math strongly predicted their decisions to enroll
well. Feather (1988) found that college students in advanced high school math courses. Whereas for
who had a higher value and perceptions of their younger students, interest may be the strongest pre-
ability in math were more likely to enroll in science dictor of academic choices, for older students, both
courses, whereas students who had higher value and interest and perceived usefulness may both be strong
perception of their ability in English were less likely predictors (Wigfield & Eccles, 1989).
to enroll in science courses and more likely to enroll A number of theories emphasize how one’s goals
in humanities. motivate choices in the context of self-regulation
Similar predictions are made according to the (Carver & Scheier, 2002; Markus & Nurius, 1986).
theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985). Spe- For example, according to Carver and Scheier’s
cifically, outcome expectancies and values lead to a (1982, 2002; also see Chapter 3, this volume) con-
favorable or unfavorable attitude toward a behavior trol theory, people monitor the discrepancy between
and influence one’s intentions to perform a particu- their current state and a goal and will try to move
lar behavior, along with perceptions of the social toward a desirable goal (or away from a goal with an
pressure to engage or not engage in a behavior undesirable outcome) as a way to reduce the discrep-
and perceptions of one’s ability to perform a given ancy (or enlarge it in the case of goals to avoid unde-
behavior. Consequently, one behavior will be cho- sirable outcomes). Thus, a person’s choice to engage
sen over alternatives to the extent that the intention in one behavior over another is the result of the abil-
to perform that behavior is stronger than the inten- ity of a behavior to reduce the discrepancy between
tion to perform alternative behaviors and when an one’s current state and ideal state or goal. Similarly,
individual has a sufficient degree of actual control individuals’ positive and negative visions of them-
over the behavior (Ajzen, 1988, 2002; Sheppard, selves in the future or their possible selves (Markus
Hartwick, & Warshaw, 1988). For example, people’s & Nurius, 1986; Oyserman & James, 2008) may
voting intentions, assessed a short time prior to a influence the choices an individual makes. Possible
presidential election, tend to correlate with actual selves can act as a guidepost and orient an individual
voting choice in the range of .75 to .80 (see Fishbein to make choices that bring them closer to desired
& Ajzen, 1981). Mothers’ choice of feeding method possible selves and farther from undesirable pos-
(breast versus bottle) for newborn babies has been sible selves (Hoyle & Sherrill, 2006; Oyserman &
found to have a strong correlation with intentions Fryberg, 2006; Oyserman & James, 2008).
expressed several weeks prior to delivery (Manstead, Various personal orientations that people have
Proffitt, & Smart, 1983). will influence their choices. For example, in Atkinson’s
Contemporary expectancy-value theories within (1957, 1964) early expectancy-value theory, an indi-
the academic domain (Eccles, 1987; Eccles et al., vidual’s choice of tasks was hypothesized to be a
1983; Eccles & Wigfield, 2002; Wigfield & Eccles, function of relatively enduring motives to approach
1992, 2001) also assert that the choices that individ- success and avoid failure, in addition to expectan-
uals make when given some autonomy in their deci- cies and value for success. People high in the motive
sion making are influenced by expectancy-related to approach success were expected to choose tasks
beliefs about how well they will do on a task either of intermediate difficulty because motivation was

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


expected to be highest for intermediate tasks. In con- regulatory orientation and behavior (e.g., Förster,
trast, people who were high in the motive to avoid Higgins, & Idson, 1998; Förster, Higgins, & Strack,
failure were expected to choose tasks that were very 2000; Idson, Liberman, & Higgins, 2000).
easy or very difficult. In one case, negative affect (the It is also important to note that the choice an
fear of failure) would be minimized because very individual makes is not only the end result of some
easy tasks would ensure success. Likewise, negative motivational process, but it is an index of motiva-
affect could be minimized by choosing a very diffi- tion. That is, when an individual has the freedom to
cult task because success would not be expected on choose among objects or activities, what he or she
difficult tasks. Empirical findings have supported chooses indicates where the individual’s motivation
predictions of Atkinson’s expectancy-value theory, lies. Fairly common is the experimental design in
showing that most people choose tasks of intermedi- which a factor is manipulated to observe its impact
ate difficulty and that individuals high in the motive on the participant’s motivation to engage in a task
to approach success were more likely to choose tasks by observing his or her selection of a task under free-
of intermediate difficulty compared to individuals choice conditions (e.g., Lepper, Greene, & Nisbett,
high in the motive to avoid failure (Weiner, 1992). 1973; Zuckerman, Porac, Lathin, Smith, & Deci,
For example, Atkinson and Litwin (1960) showed 1978).
that individuals high in the motive to approach suc- Finally, for some choices, motivation may not
cess stood at a moderately challenging distance from even be part of the equation. Theorists have noted
pegs for a ring-toss game, whereas individuals high that some choices may not require intentional analyt-
in the motive to avoid failure stood either very close ical reasoning; rather, some choices are made quickly
or very far from the peg. In the context of an aca- and spontaneously based on nonconcious processes,
demic environment, the extent to which a student including affective feelings (Bargh, 1997; Bargh &
is focused on mastery versus performance goals Chartrand, 1999; Kahneman, 2003). According to
(Ames, 1992; Dweck & Leggett, 1988; Maehr & automaticity scholars, conscious choice based on
Midgley, 1991) or holds an entity versus incremental one’s preferences, goals, and expectations may ini-
theory of intelligence (Dweck, 1986, 1999; Dweck tially be needed to perform a desired behavior. How-
& Bempechat, 1983) may lead a student to choose ever, to the extent the same option is chosen across
options and activities that are reflective of his or her similar situations, choice becomes an automatic
personal orientation. For instance, students with process (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999).
performance goals or entity theories of intelligence As the earlier discussion illustrates, when choice
are likely to choose easier tasks, while students with is conceptualized inclusively as the selection between
mastery tasks or incremental theories of intelligence objects, the initiation of one behavior over another,
are likely to choose more personally challenging tasks or the initiation of behavior over doing nothing at
(Ames, 1992; Anderman & Maehr, 1994; Bandura all, the list of possible psychological and social theo-
& Dweck, 1985 (unpublished data); Dweck, 1986; ries that can illuminate our understanding of the
1999; Elliott & Dweck, 1988, ; Nicholls, 1984). impact of motivation on one’s choices is virtually
As another example, according to regulatory focus endless. However, more limited in scope is theory
theory (Higgins, 1997, 2000; also see Chapter 5, and research that has studied choice as an experi-
this volume), people pursue goals in line with their ence that has consequences for an individual’s moti-
regulatory focus orientation or their particular man- vation to engage in subsequent behaviors. I turn my
ner of approaching pleasure and avoiding pain. Indi- attention to this conceptualization of choice next.
viduals with a promotion focus are concerned with
gains, advancements, and accomplishments, whereas Choice as an Experience
individuals with a prevention focus are primarily Choice as a Motivator
concerned with safety, responsibilities, and avoiding The presumption that feelings of having choice
losses. Regulatory focus will influence the choice of can be a powerful motivator is pervasive in motiva-
behavior an individual engages in to achieve a goal, tion theory and research. Kurt Lewin (1952) pro-
as well as the value of various options. That is, people vided one of the earliest proposals of the notion that
choose to engage in behaviors that allow them to having choice was a powerful motivator, demonstrat-
achieve a goal in a way that is consistent with their ing that people would be more likely to engage in an
regulatory focus orientation (e.g., Crowe & Higgins, activity if they believed they had freely chosen it.
1997) and have greater value for and commitment The motivating role of having choice is apparent
to decisions in which there is a fit between their in many foundational theories in social psychology.

pata ll 
Dissonance theory suggests that people dislike a counterattitudinal essay, participants who felt they
inconsistency between the beliefs they have about had a great deal of choice compared to those who per-
themselves and the behavior they engage in, and ceived having little choice regarding whether to write
therefore, people will strive for their thoughts to be the essay demonstrated greater physiological arousal
consistent with other thoughts and with their behav- construed as a dissonant motivational state. Likewise,
ior (Aronson, 1969, 1992, 1999; Brehm, 1962; individuals who perceived having chosen to write an
Festinger, 1957). When a person’s thoughts are essay that was contrary to their personal beliefs sub-
inconsistent with one another or inconsistent with sequently changed their attitudes, while individuals
one’s behavior, an aversive motivational state of dis- who felt they were forced to write the counterattitu-
sonance is aroused (Elliot & Devine, 1994) in which dinal essay did not change their attitudes (Cooper &
an individual will be motivated to resolve dissonance Fazio, 1984). Similarly, participants who were given a
(Gerard, 1992; Harmon-Jones & Mills, 1999) by one choice compared to no choice as to whether to write
of a number of strategies: (1) removing the dissonant that they liked an unpleasant-tasting drink or that
belief, (2) reducing the importance of the dissonant they thought a boring passage was interesting shifted
belief, (3) adding new beliefs that are consonant with their attitudes in the direction of what they wrote.
behavior, or (4) increasing the importance of the new Furthermore, the more choice participants felt they
consonant belief (Festinger, 1957; Harmon-Jones & had over what they wrote, the more they shifted their
Mills, 1999; Simon, Greenberg, & Brehm, 1995). attitudes (Harmon-Jones et al., 1996).
Choice making plays an important role in cogni- Choice is also at the core of attribution theory
tive dissonance in several ways. First, the experience (Kelley, 1967, 1973) and self-perception theory
of making a difficult choice is one important source (Bem, 1967). According to attribution theory, peo-
of dissonance and motivates strategies to reduce dis- ple will assign dispositional meaning to behavior
sonance (Festinger, 1964). For example, in Brehm’s after a careful assessment of the possible explanatory
(1956) classic study examining the role of choice power of controlling influences in the environment.
making, after receiving a chosen household appli- Thus, if a person feels that a behavior was freely cho-
ance among several options, participants who chose sen, he or she will be more likely to infer from that
between appliances similar in desirability accord- behavior information about the actor’s traits and
ing to their own initial ratings were found to sub- attitudes (Jones & Harris, 1967; Jones, Worchel,
sequently rate the chosen object as more desirable Goethals, & Grumet, 1971).
and the unchosen object as less desirable. However, Self-perception theory makes a similar claim.
participants who were assigned to receive a particu- Challenging the notion that people experience an
lar appliance or who were asked to choose between aversive motivational state known as dissonance, self-
appliances that they initially viewed as highly dis- perception theory suggested that rather “individuals
crepant in desirability, showed little change in their come to know their own attitudes, emotions, and
attitudes toward the appliances after receiving the other internal states partially by inferring them from
one they were assigned or had chosen. Subsequent observations of their own overt behavior and/or the
studies provided additional support for the notion circumstances in which this behavior occurs” (Bem,
that choice making itself can motivate an individual 1972, p. 2). Like dissonance theory, self-perception
to shift his or her attitudes in an effort to reduce dis- theory maintained the notion that that in order for
sonance (Brehm & Cohen, 1959; Gerard & White, individuals to be motivated to form attitudes that
1983; Lyubomirsky & Ross, 1999; Shultz, Léveillé, reflect their overt behavior, they must believe that they
& Lepper, 1999). freely chose to engage in the behavior self-observed
Second, refinements of dissonance theory sug- (Bem, 1972; Bem & McConnell, 1971). That is,
gested that dissonance and corresponding dissonance- individuals who experienced a choice as to whether
reducing strategies will only occur when people to engage in a particular behavior will observe that
perceive that they have freely chosen to engage in a overt behavior and conclude that they must have an
behavior and can therefore accept responsibility for an attitude in line with that behavior. In contrast, indi-
action (Cooper & Fazio, 1984; Goethals & Cooper, viduals who did not experience a choice will observe
1972; Harmon-Jones, Brehm, Greenberg, Simon, & their behavior and evaluate the conditions in which
Nelson, 1996; Helmreich & Collins, 1968; Linder, they acted and conclude that their behavior was
Cooper, & Jones, 1967; Scher & Cooper, 1989; Sher- the result of not having a choice, rather than their
man, 1970). For example, Croyle and Cooper (1983) internal beliefs. Subsequent research attempting to
found that when participants were asked to write resolve the discrepancies between dissonance theory

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


and self-perception theory demonstrated that both self-determination theory holds that having choice
were correct under various circumstances. In partic- should result in enhanced intrinsic motivation,
ular, when initial attitudes are weak or unimportant as well as other positive motivational and perfor-
individuals may form (or change) their beliefs upon mance outcomes supported by intrinsic motivation
observing their own behavior (Chaiken & Baldwin, (Deci, 1980; Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci,
1981; Fazio, 1981; Fazio, Zanna, & Cooper, 1977). 2000). Much research has supported this postu-
In contrast, when initial attitudes are strong or late of self-determination theory, demonstrating
important, attitude change may be motivated by that choice leads to enhanced intrinsic motivation
dissonance (Chaiken & Baldwin, 1981; Fazio, 1981; (e.g., Cordova & Lepper, 1996; Iyengar & Lepper,
Fazio, Zanna, & Cooper, 1977). However, both sit- 1999; Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008; Swann &
uations require that a person believe that he or she Pittman, 1977; Zuckerman, Porac, Lathin, Smith,
freely chose to engage in the behavior in order to & Deci, 1978), as well as enhanced effort, task
motivate the formation or shifting of an attitude in performance, subsequent learning, perceived com-
line with behavior. petence, preference for challenge, and creativity
Furthermore, the motivating effects of choice go (e.g., Amabile, 1979, 1983; Amabile, Hennessey, &
beyond the formation or change of an attitude to Grossman, 1986; Becker, 1997; Cordova & Lepper,
affect other subsequent behaviors. For example, the 1996; Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Kernan, Heimann,
basic finding of research examining the “foot-in-the- & Hanges, 1991; Patall, Cooper, & Robinson,
door” technique is that once a person freely chooses 2008). For example, in one seminal study on the
to comply with a small request, he or she will be effect of choice on intrinsic motivation, Zuckerman,
more likely to comply with more substantial subse- Porac, Lathin, Smith, and Deci (1978) found that
quent requests (Freedman & Fraser, 1966; Uranow- participants who were asked to choose three puzzles
itz, 1975). Similarly, people may remain committed to work on among six options spent more time
to a course of action if they freely chose it in the engaged in the puzzle-solving task in a subsequent
first place. For example, even when presented with free-play period compared to participants who were
information suggesting that a chosen investment assigned to work on three of the six puzzles. Iyengar
was not profitable, participants given a choice of and Lepper (1999) found that Caucasian American
investments subsequently allocated more money to elementary school students were the most motivated
their chosen investment compared to people who and demonstrated the best performance when they
were not given a choice (Staw, 1976). made personal choices about which tasks to engage
The role of choice is well defined in studies in rather than having the task chosen for them. Sim-
evaluating its effects on intrinsic motivation and ilarly, children provided with various choices during
outcomes related to intrinsic motivation from a a computerized math activity, such as the oppor-
self-determination perspective. According to self- tunity to choose their game name or various icons
determination theory, people are naturally inclined to in the math game, demonstrated greater intrinsic
interact with the environment in ways that promote motivation and learning as measured by the num-
learning and mastery (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Ryan & ber of problems answered correctly on a math test
Deci, 2000). The theory posits that autonomy, com- compared to children who were not provided with
petence, and relatedness are three fundamental needs choices (Cordova & Lepper, 1996). Amabile and
that underlie people’s intrinsic motivation or the pro- Gitomer (1984) found that children who were given
pensity to engage in a behavior for its own sake (or choices of which task materials to use when creat-
out of enjoyment; Deci, 1971). Social contexts that ing a collage produced collages that were assessed to
satisfy these needs will enhance intrinsic motivation be more creative than those produced by children
(Ryan & Deci, 2000). Therefore, intrinsic motiva- given no choice. Likewise, Greenberg (1992) found
tion is enhanced when an individual feels autono- that participants who were given choice in select-
mous and in control of his or her outcomes and ing which problems to work on produced more
when information is provided about the individual’s creative outputs. Most recently, a meta-analysis of
competence at navigating the social environment. 41 studies examining the effect of choice on intrinsic
When the environment is experienced as control- motivation and related outcomes in a variety of set-
ling, self-determination and intrinsic motivation are tings indicated that overall, providing choice indeed
diminished (Deci, Connell, & Ryan, 1989). enhanced intrinsic motivation, effort, task perfor-
Given the importance placed on one’s need mance, and perceived competence, among other
for autonomy in supporting intrinsic motivation, outcomes (Patall, Cooper, & Robinson, 2008).

pata ll 
In addition to being an important antecedent of even in the wake of initial failure (Mikulincer,
feelings of autonomy, subsequent motivational and 1988). Consistent with these notions, Henry
performance benefits of choice may lie in its ability (1994) found that allowing male participants to
to support the perception or experience of having choose between two sets of almanac questions pro-
control (Langer, 1975; Rotter, 1966; Taylor, 1989) duced enhanced perceptions of competence prior to
and the need for competence (Cordova & Lepper, engaging in the task, though the only information
1996; Henry, 1994; Henry & Sniezek, 1993; Katz provided was uninformative labels for the questions
& Assor, 2007; Monty & Perlmuter, 1987; Patall (i.e., Set A or Set B). Similarly, Langer (1975) found
et al., 2008; Perlmuter & Monty, 1977; Perlmuter, that expectancies for success were greater when par-
Scharff, Karsh, & Monty, 1980; Tafarodi, Milne ticipants were given a choice of ticket in the lottery,
& Smith, 1999; Tafarodi, Mehranvar, Panton, & even though outcomes in a lottery are determined
Milne, 2002). Based on a series of studies showing by chance.
that choice of either stimulus or response words in In line with self-determination theorists and
a paired-associates memory task led to enhanced others, the importance of providing choices has also
performance outcomes (Monty & Perlmuter, 1975; been emphasized in theories of academic achieve-
Monty, Rosenberger, & Perlmuter, 1973; Perlmuter ment motivation. For example, achievement goal
& Monty, 1973; Perlmuter, Monty, & Kimble, orientation theorists have suggested that to the
1971), Perlmuter and Monty (1977) argued that extent that it is a way to support students’ feelings of
the performance benefits of choice are a result of autonomy and control and emphasizes the process
increased motivation in the form of enhanced per- of learning, the provision of choice may be one of a
ceived control and subsequent increased arousal number of key instructional practices that differen-
and cognitive engagement with the task (Monty & tiate mastery compared to performance classroom
Perlmuter, 1987; Perlmuter et al., 1980). Similarly, goal structure (Ames, 1992; Ames & Archer, 1988;
Henry (1994; Henry & Sniezek, 1993) argued that Epstein, 1989; Maehr & Midgley, 1991; Meece,
the benefits of choice lie in its ability to increase Anderman, & Anderman, 2006). In particular, pro-
perceived control and subsequent self-efficacy. Fur- viding students with options and opportunities to
thermore, the power of choice to enhance percep- make choices regarding tasks, materials, learning
tions of control and perceived competence has been methods, or pace of learning in the classroom has
found to be independent of its effect on either actual been theorized to be an important part of imple-
performance or intrinsic motivation. For example, menting a mastery goal structure (e.g., an emphasis
in two studies examining the effects of choice, col- on developing skills and learning), whereas restrict-
lege students were asked to read and understand a ing options and choices may be consistent with a
short story. Those who selected names to be used in performance goal structure (e.g., an emphasis on
the story reported enhanced perceived control and demonstrating competence and performing better
felt more confident about their performance than than others). Though few empirical examples exist
did those who were assigned names, although the to support the supposition, the provision of choice
groups in fact performed equally and did not dif- in the classroom is expected to encourage students
fer in reports of interest for the task (Tafarodi et al., to adopt a personal mastery goal orientation when
1999). engaging in achievement behaviors (Ames, 1992;
In fact, so strong is the relationship between Epstein, 1989; Maehr & Midgley, 1991; Meece,
choice, perceived control, and feelings of compe- Anderman, & Anderman, 2006) and, in turn, sup-
tence, that choice may have a motivational quality port a constellation of adaptive motivational and
even when the choices one makes have no rela- performance outcomes that mastery goal orienta-
tion with outcomes (Langer, 1975). Langer (1975) tions have generally been found to be positively
argued that as a result of people’s cumulative experi- associated with (e.g., increases in effort, persis-
ence in which having choice allows one to perform tence, self-efficacy, intrinsic motivation, and self-
better on a task by picking options that are tailored regulation; see Ames, 1992; Harackiewicz, Barron,
to one’s preferences and abilities, even providing Pintrich, Elliot, & Thrash, 2002; Linnenbrink-
choices that have no relation with the outcomes Garcia, Tyson, & Patall, 2008; Meece et al., 2006;
obtained can lead to heightened expectations of Midgley, Kaplan, & Middleton, 2001 for relevant
success. Furthermore, the cue that choice provides reviews).
of an increased probability of success (Estes, 1976) Also building on the notion that the provision
may support motivation and increased resilience and/or perception of having choices will support

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


student feelings of autonomy and intrinsic moti- to feel competent and to value schoolwork if they
vation a la a self-determination perspective, inter- felt they had some autonomy about choosing the
est theorists have suggested that choice may be an activity (Midgley & Feldlaufer, 1987). Similarly,
important antecedent to students’ situational inter- college students’ perceptions that they were afforded
est and the subsequent development of individual greater opportunities for decision making in a col-
(personal) interest for a domain (Krapp, 2005; lege course predicted greater course self-efficacy and
Schraw, Flowerday, & Lehman, 2001; Tsai et al., value for the course (Garcia & Pintrich, 1996).
2008; Zahorik, 1996). In sum, a number of psychological theories,
Individual interest is relatively stable and resides including dissonance theory, self-perception theory,
within the individual (c.f., Hidi & Renninger, 2006; attribution theory, self-determination theory, and
Krapp, 2005; Renninger, 2009; Renninger, Hidi, & related academic motivation theories have sug-
Krapp, 1992; Schiefele, 1991, 2001). It includes a gested that choice may be an influential motivator.
deep, personal connection, enjoyment, and valuing According to these perspectives, choice is expected
of a domain as well as a willingness to reengage in to motivate behavior and lead to a variety of ben-
the domain over time. Renninger and her colleagues efits. Formation or change of attitudes, enhanced
(Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Lipstein & Renninger, commitment, as well as greater feelings of auton-
2007; Renninger, 1992, 2009) further suggest that omy, control, intrinsic motivation, and a number of
knowledge is a critical component of well-developed adaptive learning-related outcomes (e.g., perceived
individual interest in that as interest develops, a per- competence, effort, engagement, task performance,
son gains a more in-depth knowledge, value, and learning, creativity, situational and individual inter-
advanced epistemic understanding of the domain est, mastery goals, and task value) are among the
based on his or her experiences. In contrast, situ- many outcomes on which choice has been found to
ational interest refers to interest that emerges from have a motivating effect.
and is supported by the context (Hidi & Baird,
1986; Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Krapp, 2002), and The Effectiveness of Choice and Choice
it may support intrinsic motivation for learning as a Demotivator
and the development of individual interest (Hidi & Despite a great deal of theory and research sug-
Renninger, 2006; Krapp, 2002; Renninger, 2009; gesting that choice is a powerful motivator of behav-
Schiefele, 2009). ior, not all studies within the various motivational
In line with the notion that choice may be an perspectives described previously have found choice
important practice used to support situational inter- to be a ubiquitous motivator. A number of studies
est, Schraw, Flowerday, and Reisetter (1998) reported find that choice may have no effect or even a nega-
that giving college students choices about what they tive effect on adaptive motivation and performance
read increased situational interest in the material. outcomes (Overskeid & Svartdal, 1996; Parker &
Linnenbrink-Garcia, Patall, and Messersmith (unpub- Lepper, 1992; Reeve, Nix, & Hamm, 2003). Like-
lished data) found that controlling for students’ ini- wise, not all studies from dissonance and attribution
tial individual interest or perceived competence for theory perspectives have found that greater choice
science, the perception of having choices supported conditions produce greater dissonance and attitude
talented adolescents’ situational interest for a science change (Collins, Ashmore, Hornbeck, & Whitney,
course during a 3-week summer program, as well as 1970; Melson, Calder, & Insko, 1969), greater
subsequent individual interest and perceived compe- internal attributions (Fitch, 1970), or dispositional
tence in science at the end of the program. attributions to actors by observers (Calder, Ross, &
Likewise, academic expectancy-value theorists Insko, 1973).
have made use of the notion that supporting the For example, in a series of studies, Flowerday and
fundamental need for autonomy can have impor- colleagues (Flowerday & Schraw, 2003; Flowerday,
tant motivating consequences. That is, supporting Schraw, & Stevens, 2004) found choice had few
autonomy by providing students with the oppor- positive effects. For example, giving students a
tunity to make choices may be critical to creating choice between working on a crossword puzzle or
contexts in which values for a task may develop and essay task showed no effect on engagement and task
lead to enhanced self-perceptions and expectancies performance (Flowerday & Schraw, 2003). Students
for success, as well as long-term engagement in that in the choice condition demonstrated reduced effort
task (Jacobs & Eccles, 2000). During the transition compared to students not given a choice of tasks. In a
to junior high school, students were more likely second study, students allowed to choose the pacing

pata ll 
of the task spent less time studying and performed According to the self-regulatory perspective
more poorly on cognitive measures compared to stu- proposed by Baumeister, Muraven, and colleagues
dents whose pace was dictated by the experimenter (Baumeister, Bratslavsky, Muraven, & Tice, 1998;
(Flowerday & Schraw, 2003). In two additional Muraven & Baumeister, 2000; Muraven, Baumeis-
studies, no-choice participants were found to write ter, & Tice, 1999), all acts of choice or self-control
higher quality essays compared to students who are effortful and draw on a limited resource that
were given choice. Choice had no effect on a sub- can be depleted, analogous to a source of energy or
sequent test to assess learning (Flowerday, Schraw, strength. Since all acts of volition or self-regulation
& Stevens, 2004). In models exploring the nature draw on the same resource, any act of volition or self-
of perceived autonomy (or self-determination) and regulation will have detrimental effects on subsequent
its relation to intrinsic motivation, Reeve, Nix, and acts that continue to require self-regulation. Conse-
Hamm (2003) found that internal locus of causality quently, engaging in a choice can result in a state of
(e.g., an individual’s perception that his or her actions fatigue called ego depletion, in which the individual
are initiated and controlled by the individual rather experiences a decrement in the capacity to initiate
than by external forces; deCharms, 1968) and voli- activity, make choices, or further self-regulate.
tion (e.g., sense that individuals feel free rather than Baumeister and colleagues (1998) have proposed
forced to engage in a behavior), but not perceived that making choices is one form of self-control that
choice or the external event of provision of choice, can result in ego depletion. In particular, the pro-
constituted valid indicators of self-determination. cess of deliberation among the options and making
Through structural equation models, the authors a specific choice produces depletion (Vohs et al.,
compared a series of models containing one, two, 2008; Vohs, Finkenauer, & Baumeister, 2011). Sev-
or all three qualities of perceived self-determination eral studies have demonstrated the depleting effect
to find the best-fitting model. The inclusion of per- of choice. Baumeister and colleagues (1998) found
ceived choice or provision of choice was consistently that participants who were given a choice of which
found to reduce the fit of the model and reduced side to take in a debate persisted for less time and
the strength of the relationship between perceived made fewer attempts at solving subsequent puzzles
self-determination and intrinsic motivation. Similar compared to participants who were not asked to
findings were reported by Assor, Kaplan, and Roth make a debating choice. In other studies, partici-
(2002) when they distinguished between three forms pants who made choices among household prod-
of teacher practices meant to support the need for ucts, among college courses, or about the content of
autonomy among students: fostering relevance by courses demonstrated a reduced ability to exert self-
articulating the importance of a task for students’ control on a subsequent task (Vohs et al., 2008).
personal goals, allowing students to express dissat- Specifically, participants who made choices drank
isfaction with learning tasks, and providing oppor- less of a bad-tasting beverage, showed reduced per-
tunities to make choices. They found that while sistence on a cold water pressure task, procrasti-
allowing students to express their concerns if they nated more on a math task, showed less persistence
do not like a task and fostering the relevance of a in the face of failure on an unsolvable tracing task or
task promoted engagement, perceptions of provi- solvable math task, and performed worse on a math
sion of choice had little impact. test. In yet another study, people who reported mak-
In line with null and negative findings, some ing more choices during a shopping trip performed
psychologists have suggested that choice may have more poorly on a subsequent math task (Vohs et al.,
disadvantages. Schwartz (2000) argues that Ameri- 2008).
cans now live in a world in which the ability to Burger (1989) proposed a variety of circum-
choose everything from breakfast cereal to the way stances under which the provision of choice may
one wants to live is greater than ever before, though lead to maladaptive outcomes due to the relation-
depression and unhappiness are on the rise. That is, ship between having choice and perceptions of con-
although some choice may have benefits, as the num- trol. In particular, having a choice can also increase
ber of options and opportunities for making choices self-presentational concerns or feelings of responsi-
becomes excessive, motivation and well-being suffer bility about making the correct choice or perform-
due to the cost of giving up alternatives and regret ing well. This concern over self-presentation may
with the choices made. Accordingly, Schwartz (2000, lead to various negative outcomes. For example, in
2004) and colleagues (2002) argue that constraints a series of studies, participants were given a choice
on choosing may often be beneficial. of three tasks to work on during a 20-min testing

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


session, one of which they had been told they were Milne, & Smith, 1999). That is, as long as the indi-
likely to do well on based on earlier trials. All par- vidual still perceives increased control over outcomes
ticipants chose the task they expected to do well on. as a function of his or her choices, choice may yield
Other participants were given identical feedback, motivational benefits. For example, Dember and
but they were assigned the task for the testing ses- colleagues (1992) found that participants who were
sion. Participants given the choice of tasks scored given a choice between completing an “easy” or a
lower on measures of self-esteem and higher on mea- “hard” version of a vigilance task performed better
sures of anxiety and hostility than did subjects given compared to participants not given a choice, though
no choice of tasks. However, when participants were in reality, everyone was given the same task regard-
led to believe that whether they had made a choice less of the choice made.
would not be known to the experimenter who was However, more surprising might be the notion
to administer the test, there was no increase in neg- that an individual need not have had the opportu-
ative affect. Similarly, participants given a choice nity to make an actual choice (regardless of its rela-
of words in a paired-associate memory study and tionship with outcomes) in order for the effects of
told that both the experimenters and the professor choice to be experienced. It would be expected that
supervising the project would return to discuss their the provision of choice would be an effective way
performance performed worse on the memory task to enhance perceived choice, and in fact, research
than those who anticipated the same discussion but has suggested that having the opportunity to make
did not choose the words (Burger, 1988, unpub- choices does increase the perception of choice (e.g.,
lished data). Patall, Cooper, & Wynn, 2010; Reeve et al., 2003).
In sum, not all research examining the effects of However, akin to the claim made by many control
choice has found choosing to enhance motivation. theorists that perceived control is a more powerful
In some instances choice has been found to have few predictor than actual control (Averill, 1973; Burger,
beneficial effects on intrinsic motivation, perceived 1989; Langer, 1975, 1979), choice is unlikely to have
competence, engagement, learning and performance, an effect if its provision and significance is unrecog-
among other adaptive outcomes. As we have seen, nized by the individual. In this way, perceived choice
some theorists have suggested that in line with null may be a more powerful and essential predictor than
or even negative choice effects that have been found, the actual choice. One study explicitly tested the role
there may be limitations or drawbacks of choosing. of perceived compared to actual choice in motiva-
In particular, choice is an effortful and sometimes tion. Detweiler, Mendoza, and Lepper (1996) asked
ego-involved process that may deplete resources and preschool children to draw a picture using a set of
motivation to engage in subsequent behavior. eight colored markers. Actual choice was manipu-
lated by either allowing the children to choose the
Factors That Influence the Effects of Choice subject of their drawing from a list of eight possibili-
There is little doubt that the relationship between ties or assigning the children subjects for the drawing.
choice and motivation is complex. It would seem Perceptions of choice were manipulated by showing
that choice has the potential to promote motivation, children in the high perceived-choice condition only
to protect motivation, and to diminish motivation eight markers and emphasizing to them that they
depending on a variety circumstances. Characteris- could use any of the eight, while participants in the
tics of the choice, the person, and the situation may low perceived-choice condition were showed a set of
all influence the effects of being provided or perceiv- 32 markers, though they too could use any makers
ing one has a choice. of a set of eight. Participants in the perceived-choice
condition demonstrated greater intrinsic motivation
Characteristics of the Choice for the drawing activity in a later free-play period
perceived versus actual choice compared to students in the low perceived-choice
One factor that may be important to consider in condition. Moreover, the effect of low compared to
determining when choice has versus does not have high perceived choice was greater than that of low
an effect (whether beneficial or detrimental) may be compared to high actual choice.
the extent to which it is real or illusory. The find-
ing that choice can be totally illusory and need not choices that support perceptions of
have any real consequences in order for it to have control, competence, and autonomy
an effect on motivation or performance has already There are a number of constructs, many previ-
been highlighted (e.g., Langer, 1975; Taforodi, ously mentioned, that are so intimately related to

pata ll 
the provision or perception of choice (and each Abramson, Seligman, & Teasdale, 1978; Maier &
other) that they are sometimes viewed as synony- Seligman, 1976; Peterson, Maier, & Seligman, 1993;
mous with choice. Although increased perceptions Peterson & Seligman, 1984; Seligman, 1975). When
or experiences of control, competence, autonomy, choice does not support perceptions of control
volition, or locus of causality may often result as a because the chooser feels insufficiently informed or
function of having or perceiving opportunities to overly rushed to make decisions, choice may be expe-
choose, the provision or perception of having an rienced as stressful and can diminish self-confidence
opportunity to select an option among alternatives (Paterson & Neufeld, 1995; Rodin, Rennert, &
is separable from each of these related constructs. Solomon, 1980). Similarly, Tafarodi and colleagues
That said, a survey of the literature would suggest (1999) showed that perceptions of competence in
that when choice is divorced from some or all of understanding a story and performing well on a sub-
these related constructs, it may lose its power to sequent test were heightened only when participants
motivate. chose names used in the story relative to those who
Control may be conceptualized as the oppor- made no choices or those who chose names not used
tunity or perception that (1) an individual has the in the story. Similarly, choice may be less benefi-
ability to perform a particular behavior, (2) the cial when divorced from the need for competence.
particular behavior will be likely to lead to a desir- For example, Burger (1987, 1989) showed that
able end, and so ultimately, (3) the individual will having choice was more beneficial when it provided
obtain a particular outcome (Bandura, 1977, 1997; an opportunity to demonstrate competence. Under-
Burger, 1989; Skinner, 1996). As previously dis- graduate students allowed to select the response
cussed, some research and theory suggests that the word for a paired-associate memory task performed
effects of choice may be owed at least partly to the better when told that the experimenter would know
power of choice to enhance perceptions of control about their choice and performance compared to
(Burger, 1989; Henry, 1994; Langer, 1975; Monty participants who did not choose or chose but were
& Perlmuter, 1977). Work on reactance theory (see not told the experimenter would know about their
Brehm, 1966; Wortman & Brehm, 1975) has sug- choice and performance (Burger, 1987).
gested that when people expect to be able to con- According to self-determination theory, percep-
trol important outcomes, explicitly eliminating an tions of choice are conceptualized as being an impor-
option in the context of giving choice will cause a tant condition to the experience of autonomy, and
state of psychological reactance in which they will in turn, this experience of autonomy supports other
be highly motivated to regain and defend their per- adaptive outcomes (Deci & Ryan, 1987). How-
sonal freedom. This threat of restriction or elimina- ever, perceived choice is just one of several aspects
tion of the individual’s ability to choose will cause of autonomy. Specifically, autonomy is experienced
the individual to evaluate more positively the alter- when actions are perceived as (a) stemming from
natives he or she was not allowed to choose, while an internal locus of causality or individuals’ percep-
the remaining alternatives are evaluated more nega- tions that their actions are initiated and controlled
tively. People will continue to experience a moti- by them rather than by external forces (deCharms,
vational state of reactance and engage in coping 1968) and (b) volitional, or the sense that indi-
behaviors designed to regain their personal freedom viduals feel free rather than forced to engage in a
as long as behaviors associated with this reactance behavior (Rogers, 1969). Choice is viewed as the
are perceived to affect outcomes. condition needed to induce the experiential shift
In fact, it would seem that when choice does not “from pawn to origin” and “from forced to free”
support the experience of control, it may have no or (Deci & Ryan, 1985).
negative effects. For example, work on learned help- However, not all choices (or perceptions of
lessness has demonstrated that motivation to engage choices) facilitate an internal locus of causality or
in a behavior and learning are impaired when people volition. Reeve and colleagues (2003) suggested as
experience outcomes as independent of their choice much based on a review of existing literature and
of actions and consequently feel they have no con- their own series of studies in which it was shown
trol over a situation (Seligman, 1975). Furthermore, that when choice was designed in such a way that
mental health suffers under conditions of learned it enhanced perceptions of an internal locus of
helplessness; in particular, depression and anxiety causality and volition (e.g., participants were given
result when an individual experiences life choices as ongoing action choices in which the initiation and
irrelevant (e.g., Abramson, Metalsky, & Alloy, 1989; regulation of their behavior could be freely chosen),

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


it effectively enhanced intrinsic motivation. How- study designs in which participants were subtly
ever, when choice was designed to only enhance pressured to choose a particular option or provided
the perception of choice and not locus of causality options grossly dissimilar in attractiveness to increase
or volition (e.g., participants were given choices of the likelihood that all participants would choose a
experimenter-determined task options), then it did particular target option had the undesirable effect of
not successfully enhance intrinsic motivation. reducing the sense of having a true choice, result-
Moller and colleagues (Moller, Deci, & Ryan, ing in a smaller effect size for this group of studies
2006) also highlighted the importance of consider- compared to those with other study designs. Second,
ing how the choices being given support feelings of the meta-analysis found that the effect of choice was
autonomy in an attempt to integrate contradictory essentially zero when a reward external to the choice
predictions from self-determination theory perspec- manipulation was provided compared to when the
tive compared to a self-regulatory strength depletion participants chose a reward they would receive or
framework. Specifically, Moller et al. (2006) sug- when no reward was involved, presumably because
gested that contradictory findings result from the providing rewards in this fashion seems to commu-
lack of differentiation between choices that either nicate to the individual that he or she is being con-
promoted participants’ sense of autonomy or pro- trolled by forces external to the self.
vided them with a controlled form of choice. Moller The importance of autonomy in choice provision
and colleagues (2006) suggested that most often in goes beyond avoiding the overt pressure or control
studies of ego depletion, a controlled form of choice that may characterize some forms of choice. Katz
is implemented in which participants are led to pick and Assor (2007) adopted a useful differentiation
a particular option. That is, while participants are in terminology to express when choice will be most
told they have a choice among options, they are sub- likely to facilitate adaptive motivational outcomes
tly pressured to pick a particular option. In contrast, by differentiating between “picking” and “choosing”
studies coming out of the self-determination perspec- (Ullmann-Margalit & Morgenbesser, 1997). That is,
tive generally provide an unrestricted choice with no choosing allows for one’s preferences to be expressed,
indication provided as to which option should be while picking does not necessarily. Choices that
chosen. In support of their hypothesis that differen- allow for the expression of one’s preferences are
tiating between autonomous and controlled forms likely to better facilitate a sense of autonomy and
of choice would reconcile discrepancies, Moller and subsequent motivation. In fact, there is evidence to
colleagues (2006) found that when an unrestricted suggest that the greater the extent to which choosing
autonomous form of choice was provided, it had a supports feelings of autonomy, the greater the bene-
beneficial effect in terms of persistence and perfor- fits. For example, research has suggested that choices
mance outcomes, whereas ego depletion resulted that allow for personalization may be particularly
when a controlled choice was provided. In fact, the effective (e.g., Cordova & Lepper, 1996). Tafarodi
relatively little controversy over the role of choice Mehranvar, Panton, and Milne (2002) found in two
in dissonance or self-perception motivated attitude studies that only choices reflecting personal prefer-
change may be a function of the fact that the choice ences increased confidence in the task outcome and
manipulation consistently used in these studies is boosted performance-related self-esteem. Choice
intentionally designed to make the participant feel had few effects when participants were asked to select
responsible for his or her action because he or she options based on what they thought the majority
freely chose to engage in an attitude-discrepant of people similar to them would most prefer. Simi-
behavior (Brehm & Cohen, 1962; Cooper & Fazio, larly, variety (or the mere appearance of variety) may
1984). That is, choice manipulations often used in effectively enhance the benefits of choice by increas-
dissonance research may inherently support feeling ing feelings of autonomy from choosing. A series
of autonomy because they usually involve giving of studies found that when choosers are unfamiliar
the participant the choice to engage in an attitude- with the domain or options they are asked to choose
discrepant behavior or not. among, being presented with a greater rather than
A recent meta-analysis my colleagues and I con- fewer number of categories (holding constant the
ducted of the effect of choice on intrinsic motivation number of individual options) positively influenced
provided additional support for the notion that the their satisfaction (Mogilner, Rudnick, & Iyengar,
effects of choice might in part depend on the extent 2008). Presumably, the presence of categories sig-
to which feelings of autonomy are enhanced (Patall nals greater variety among the available options.
et al., 2008). First, this meta-analysis suggested that This in turn may enhance the sense that one can

pata ll 
truly express his or her personal preferences from given two equally preferred activities to choose from
choosing and thus increases feelings of autonomy. compared to when just one preferred activity was
All in all, these examples serve to illustrate the point offered. Likewise, when required to choose among
that more powerful choices are those that effectively unwanted options (as opposed to choosing to do
enhance feelings of autonomy. something undesirable or nothing at all, as is com-
mon in dissonance research), the effort needed to
level of effort make a decision may be greater (Higgins, 1998;
In addition to the extent to which choices Janis & Mann, 1977; Lewin, 1951), diminishing the
provided enhance perceptions of control, compe- benefits of choosing. In one study (Botti & Iyengar,
tence, or autonomy, the extent to which a choice is 2004), participants were asked to either choose or
effortful may also play a role in determining when not choose among either appealing or unappealing
choice may lead to motivational benefits or decre- yogurt flavors. Although all of the subjects preferred
ments. That is, in line with a self-regulatory strength to choose for themselves, choosers reported greater
model, making a choice has a cost for expenditure satisfaction than nonchoosers only when the yogurt
of effort. The greater the extent to which making a options were appealing. When the yogurt options
choice is an effortful process, the less likely it will be were unappealing, choosers were less satisfied than
to afford motivational benefits. Given that choos- nonchoosers. Furthermore, when the yogurt flavors
ing may simultaneously support feelings of control, were unappealing, choosers ate less yogurt than
autonomy, and competence, while being an effort- nonchoosers.
ful process, it seems reasonable that positive and Wang and colleagues (Wang, Novemsky, Dhar,
negative consequences can both occur under vari- & Baumeister, in press) found that the structure
ous conditions and can even occur together. of the options influenced whether making choices
There is some research to support this assertion was depleting. In one condition, participants made
that there are limited benefits to choosing when a choice in which there was a fairly linear trade-off
greater effort is needed to make choices. For exam- between cost and quality across options, so that
ple, in one study designed to investigate the role of each increase in price yielded a roughly proportional
inner effort, Vohs and colleagues (2008) found that increase in quality. In another condition, however,
making choices about a computer and associated the trade-offs were not linear, and one of the choices
services, support options, and accessories required yielded the best value in the sense that it offered much
more effort and resulted in less persistence on a higher quality for only a slight increase in price. Par-
subsequent anagram task than the process of just ticipants who made the latter choice were more likely
thinking about and forming a preference regard- to make more virtuous future decisions compared to
ing the options or implementing choices previously participants who made the former choices, presum-
made by others. Similarly, Vohs, Finkenauer, and ably because the nonlinear trade-off choices pro-
Baumeister (2011) had participants either execute duced greater depletion by requiring greater effort to
preordained choices without thinking about them, make a good decision.
deliberate between options without choosing, or On the flip side, it is often noted that studies in
both deliberate and choose. The last condition was which seemingly trivial choices presumably requir-
the most depleting on subsequent tasks, whereas ing little effort in order to make decisions provide
executing choices without deliberating produced significant motivational, performance, and well-
little depletion. Deliberating without choosing still being benefits. A rather dramatic example is pro-
depleted some resources, but not as much as delib- vided by Langer and Rodin (1976) who conducted
erating and choosing. a field study in a nursing home in which a group
The extent to which options are similar or attrac- of patients were given choices to make relatively
tive may influence the effort needed to make a deci- inconsequential decisions, such as choosing when to
sion and the benefits of choosing. For example, watch a movie or how to arrange their bedroom fur-
choosers make suboptimal choices and delay mak- niture, or had these same decisions made for them
ing choices when confronted with equally attractive by the nursing home staff. The results showed an
or highly risky options (Luce, 1998; Mischel & increase in choosers’ happiness and activity levels
Ebbesen, 1970; Shafir, Simonson, & Tversky, 1993; relative to nonchoosers, as well as better health con-
Shafir & Tversky, 1992; Yates & Mischel, 1979). ditions and even lower death rates in the long run.
Higgins, Trope, and Kwon (1999) found that chil- Cordova and Lepper (1996) examined the effects
dren demonstrated less intrinsic motivation when of choices over “instructionally irrelevant aspects

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


of the task” among elementary school children moderate the effects of providing choice. While a
involved in an educational computer activity. Par- self-determination model might predict that too
ticipants who were given the opportunity to choose few options or choices may not be powerful enough
features such as the icon representing them on the to bolster the individual’s sense of autonomy, a
game board, the name of their spaceship, and the self-regulatory model would suggest that many
name of their opponent’s spaceship demonstrated options or choices requires exertion of more effort
enhanced motivation and learning of the mathemat- and energy. As the cognitive “workload” of deciding
ical concepts involved in the game compared to par- between options and making choices increases with
ticipants who were not given a choice. Tafarodi and the number of options and discrete choices available,
colleagues (1999) intentionally attempted to trivial- choice may come to be experienced as overwhelm-
ize the choice manipulation by giving participants a ing rather than motivating and decision making
choice among unusual names to be used in a story. may become impaired as a consequence (Botti
The name options were made highly similar by trans- & Iyengar, 2006; Greenleaf & Lehmann, 1995;
posing one pair of consonants within a root name to Huffman & Kahn, 1998; Malhotra, 1982; Payne,
create each of the options (e.g., Ojebeta, Obejeta, Bettman, & Johnson, 1993; Shugan, 1980).
Otebeja, Ojeteba, Otejeba, Obeteja). However, even In fact, research has supported the proposition
this somewhat trivial choice led to enhanced percep- that more choice does not necessarily lead to greater
tions of competence. In a meta-analysis of the effects motivation. In a series of three studies, Iyengar and
of choice on intrinsic motivation, my colleagues and Lepper (2000) asked that participants choose between
I (Patall et al., 2008) found evidence to support the 6 or 24 options. They found that people were more
notion that these trivial or instructionally irrelevant likely to purchase gourmet jams or chocolates or
choices are the most effective in supporting intrin- undertake an optional class essay assignment when
sic motivation compared to instructionally relevant offered the small array of options to choose among.
choices, choices among task, versions of a task, or Other studies across a variety of contexts have found
rewards. We hypothesized that this effect was the that offering too many options may have detrimen-
result of variation in self-regulatory effort that vari- tal effects. For example, the presence of more rather
ous kinds of choices requires. That is, instruction- than fewer options has been found to make decision
ally relevant choices (e.g., choosing the method or makers more likely to decide against choosing, even
strategy used to engage in the task, pacing, or who to when the choice of opting out has negative conse-
work with) were hypothesized to be more effortful quences for their future well-being (Iyengar, Jiang,
to the extent that they both provide an opportunity & Kamenica, 2006, (unpublished data); see Botti
to personalize the task and have potentially influ- & Iyengar, 2006). In one study (Chua & Iyengar,
ential consequences for learning and performance. 2005), when participants were given an extensive
Because people are likely aware of the potential choice of initial themes from which to generate ideas
for these types of choice to have important conse- for a print advertisement and told their goal was to
quences, they are more difficult to make, particularly be creative, they demonstrated less creativity and
when the choices are equally desirable, because the more frustration compared to those who were given
consequences of that decision are likely to be impor- fewer themes. Furthermore, participants in the lim-
tant to the individual. In contrast, when a choice is ited choice condition reported more interest in par-
of little instructional significance, such as choosing ticipating in similar future studies than those in the
what color paper to write on or what pen to write extensive choice condition. Similarly, job seekers who
with, it may be relatively “easier” to make a choice pursued more rather than fewer job opportunities
because the implications are minimal, no matter were less satisfied with their accepted job offer and
what option is chosen. Nevertheless, these forms reported less commitment to their position (Iyengar,
of choice still provide the opportunity to personal- Elwork, & Schwartz, 2006). Likewise, as employers
ize the task and gain a sense of autonomy without increase the number of investment options provided
the cost of having expended a great deal of effort. to employees in 401(k) plans, employees become less
Consequently, it may be these “easy” choices require likely to participate in any, even though this often
the least effort and allow for more positive effects of means forgoing an employer match of several thou-
being given a choice. sand dollars per year (Iyengar, Jiang, & Huberman,
For similar reasons, the number of options 2004). However, when the cognitive effort needed
provided or the total number of choices an indi- to make choices is alleviated, the benefits of choos-
vidual makes within a limited time frame may also ing may be apparent even when the set of options

pata ll 
is extensive. For example, providing categories may conditions. Specifically, the effect of choice was
allow choosers to clearly perceive the variety avail- strongest in comparison to no-choice conditions in
able in extensive choice sets and reduce the effort which participants’ lack of choice was made salient
needed to differentiate among options and make a by explicitly denying choice. Similarly, the effect of
choice. Consequently, grouping options into cat- choice on intrinsic motivation was greatest in com-
egories appears to alleviate the detrimental effects of parison to control conditions in which participants
having an extensive set of options to choose among were aware that there were alternatives not open to
(Mogilner et al., 2008). them versus control conditions in which partici-
Furthermore, in addition to the number of pants were not aware of alternatives. Under these
options one has, the number of discrete choices one conditions, participants likely experienced a decre-
makes in a limited time frame may also have limited ment in intrinsic motivation. Consequently, the dif-
benefits. The meta-analysis by myself and colleagues ference in intrinsic motivation between the choice
(Patall et al., 2008) suggested that, indeed, there conditions and these most controlling no-choice
were an optimal number of choices to be made. Spe- conditions was greater than the difference between
cifically, studies in which participants made between the choice condition and less controlling no-choice
two and four choices produced the greatest effect on conditions, such as when participants were ran-
intrinsic motivation compared to when participants domly assigned an option, were assigned an option
made more or fewer choices. Similarly, Vohs and by an individual who was not significant to the par-
colleagues (2008) found that as the amount of time ticipant in any way (e.g., the experimenter), or were
engaging in a choice-making task (choosing items unaware that options other than their assignment
for a wedding registry) and therefore the number of existed.
choices made increased, the more depletion partici-
pants demonstrated. Specifically, participants asked summary
to engage in the choice-making task for a longer Clearly, choices vary in form and type. Not all
compared to shorter duration waited more time to choices are equally motivating and in fact some
tell the researcher that there was a technical prob- choices may be depleting. A review of existing
lem occurring on a subsequent task that prevented research suggests several underlying themes may
them from continuing. All in all, it would seem that explain when choice has positive, negative, or no
making more choices requires more effort expendi- effects. In particular, while choice need not be
ture and there is likely a point at which the balance explicit, individuals need to perceive that they have
among motivational effects of choosing are tipped the freedom to choose in order for effects (whether
from beneficial to null or even deleterious. beneficial or detrimental) to be experienced. Choices
that effectively enhance feelings of autonomy, con-
frame of reference trol, and competence are likely to successfully moti-
Clearly, there are a variety of factors that influ- vate behavior and facilitate adaptive outcomes.
ence the motivational effects of choice. However, Likewise, choices that lack the ability to support
it also matters how one is treated when choices are feelings of autonomy, control, or competence, or
intentionally restricted. The assumptions of theories are experienced as controlling, are unlikely to lead
previously mentioned have attested to this. Accord- to beneficial effects and may even diminish motiva-
ing to self-determination theory, conditions that are tion or lead to detrimental outcomes. The previous
experienced as controlling will diminish intrinsic review also highlights that choices may often come
motivation (Deci et al., 1989). Similarly, reactance with a cost. That is, choice can be an effortful pro-
theory (Brehm, 1966) suggests that when an option cess. A look at the literature on various types and
or alternative course of action is explicitly elimi- forms of choice suggests that when choice is made
nated, people will evaluate whatever options are left to be a more effortful process (e.g., choices require
more negatively, and those they were not allowed to greater deliberation, more complex cognitive evalu-
choose more positively. ation in order to determine the best option, reflect
In line with both theories, the meta-analysis of greater similarity between options, or the number
empirical studies on the impact of choice on intrin- of options and choices are extensive), the benefits of
sic motivation I conducted with colleagues (Patall choosing become more unlikely and the detriments
et al., 2008) suggested that the effect of choice was of choosing become more likely. Given that choice
stronger when participants given choice were com- may vary both in the level of effort required as well
pared to the most controlling forms of no-choice as the extent to which feelings of autonomy, control,

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


or competence is supported, choice may have simul- an activity, the ability to choose aspects of it may
taneously positive and negative effects that must be create interest and motivation where little previ-
negotiated. On one side, choices that are highly ously existed. This is particularly relevant to the
effortful and lack support for important psychologi- educational setting. In a phenomenological study
cal needs are likely to lead to detrimental outcomes. of teachers’ beliefs about instructional choice
Conversely, choices that minimize the cognitive (Flowerday & Schraw, 2000), teachers reported that
effort needed to make a decision, while still enhance choice was perceived to be especially beneficial for
feelings of control, autonomy, or competence, may students who had low interest and little motivation
provide the greatest benefits. However, the complex- for the task at hand. In particular, many teachers
ity of choosing becomes clear upon considering that reported that they believed choice increased the
most choices will require both some level of effort motivation of students with low initial interest
and provide some level of support for important because it gave them a sense of control. A similar
psychological needs. How the effects of these choices pattern was found in one set of experiments that
become negotiated in a cost–benefit-like analysis is isolated the effects of choice and personal interest
less clear, but it would seem that trade-offs must on situational interest and text comprehension.
occur in most choice situations. Finally, the previ- Personal interest ratings were made prior to read-
ous review illustrated that not only does the type ing a text. Some students then were given a choice
or form of choice given impact the effects observed, of what to read, whereas others were assigned a
but it also matters how individuals are treated when text. Results suggested that only for students with
not given a choice. In line with the notion that it low personal interest, having a choice of what to
is the sense of control, autonomy, and competence read significantly increased situational interest (see
that underlies the powerful effects of choosing, when Schraw, Flowerday, & Lehman, 2001 for a descrip-
these needs are diminished in no-choice situations, tion of this unpublished finding). In fact, this
the difference between choosing and not choosing notion may be the very base on which a dissonance
may become even more apparent. effect operates. That is, in the traditional dissonance
study, it is inherent to the design that participants
Characteristics of the Individual have little motivation to perform a behavior counter
Clearly, the type, number, and various other to their existing attitudes. However, freely choosing
characteristics of the choice and options provided to engage in that counterattitudinal behavior moti-
can influence the effects of receiving or perceiv- vates a shift in attitudes.
ing choices. However, people bring with them an Likewise, an individual’s perception of competence
array of individual characteristics that are likely to for the tasks involved may also influence the effects of
influence how choice is interpreted. An individual’s choice. On one hand, when individuals feel that they
initial levels of interest, value, and perceived compe- may be somewhat inadequate in performing a task,
tence for the options or tasks, cultural or socioeco- having a choice may alleviate some of those concerns
nomic background, or developmental level may all by allowing them to choose aspects of the task most
influence the effect of choice. preferred and tailor the task to their particular inter-
ests, values, and skills. That is, choosing may lead to
initial levels of interest, perceived motivational benefits for individuals with low percep-
competence, and value for tions of competence, in particular because choosing
tasks and options allows them to feel that the task is more manageable
Individuals are expected to vary in their ini- than would have been the case if they had not been
tial levels of interest and motivation for the tasks given the opportunity to choose. Whereas for indi-
involved, as well as their preferences and values viduals with high perceptions of competence for a
for the particular options available in any choice- task, choosing may confer little additional advantage
making situation. As such, this individual variation if they feel that the task is manageable regardless of
may influence how choice is experienced by one their ability to tailor it to their particular preferences,
person to the next. Some research has suggested that interests, and skills. Alternatively, it seems reasonable
providing choices may be particularly beneficial for to propose the opposite pattern. That is, individuals
those individuals who lack existing motivation and who perceive themselves to be highly competent on
personal interest for the task at hand (Flowerday a task may benefit most from having choice, given
& Schraw, 2000; Schraw, Flowerday, & Lehman, their greater expertise and potentially enhanced abil-
2001). That is, for those who lack initial interest in ity to make effective decisions. Whereas individuals

pata ll 
who perceive themselves to lack competence on a task (Baumeister & Vohs, 2007; Deci & Ryan, 1985;
may feel overwhelmed by having to make choices. Katz & Assor, 2007; Reeve, Nix, & Hamm, 2003;
Furthermore, when the number of options or choices Ryan, 1993; Williams, 1998). Conversely, when an
to be made becomes excessive, individuals with low individual has little value for the options provided,
perceived competence for the task at hand may be choice making may be experienced ambivalently or
particularly likely to feel overwhelmed, given their even negatively. This assertion seems probable in light
perception of having more limited expertise. There is of the evidence previously reviewed suggesting that
some evidence to suggest possibilities. For example, choices that are intentionally constructed to facili-
exploratory analyses in one series of experimental tate preference matching and that allow individu-
studies repeatedly revealed a trend in which the effect als to experience a greater sense of autonomy (i.e.,
of choice appeared to be more beneficial for individu- through personalization, variety, or unconstrained
als with low perceived competence and the effect of options) often yield impressive benefits. However,
restricting choice appeared to be more detrimental to even in situations in which choices are intention-
individuals with high perceived competence (Patall, ally created to allow individuals to freely choose and
2009). Another study suggested that among college express their preferences among options, individuals
students who were given feedback to lead them to may still vary in their value for the options avail-
believe they would perform very well on an upcoming able or the choice-making process. There is some
task, choice had a positive effect on motivation for the evidence to suggest that indeed, experiencing the
task. However, for college students who were led to choice-making process as enjoyable may protect
believe that they would perform poorly on an upcom- individuals from experiencing costs associated with
ing task, choice had a negative effect on motivation choice making, at least within limits. For example,
(Patall, Dacy, & Han, unpublished data). Further, in one study (Vohs et al., 2008), participants were
Chua and Iyengar (2005) found that an individual’s asked to make a series of choices in the context of
level of self-efficacy may moderate when an extensive creating a bridal registry (i.e., selecting wedding
set of options becomes overwhelming and when it is presents to receive). Some participants enjoyed the
motivating. That is, in one experiment, participants choice-making task, whereas others found it aver-
were assigned to either a high-choice condition, in sive. Results suggested that individuals who liked
which they were given six types of ribbon and four the bridal registry task did not experience deple-
types of wrapping paper for a gift-wrapping task, or a tion on subsequent activities as long as the task was
low-choice condition in which they were given only brief, that is, only a moderate number of choices
two types of ribbon and two types of wrapping paper. were made. When the bridal registry task was long,
For each of the choice conditions, half of the subjects that is, many decisions had to be made, participants
were told that the goal was to come up with as cre- experienced depletion on subsequent tasks, regard-
ative a gift wrap as possible (creativity goal) while the less of whether they liked the task or detested it.
other half of the subjects were simply told to do their While little direct evidence exists on the topic as
‘‘best’’ in the gift-wrapping task (performance goal). of yet, an individual’s personal choice-making strat-
Results showed that there was no difference in diver- egy may be one factor likely to influence the motiva-
gent thinking (i.e., use of more unusual materials tional benefits and detriments of choosing. According
in gift wrapping) among subjects with high creative to Simon (1955, 1956, 1957), choice-making strat-
self-efficacy as a function of choice set provided. egies can be distinguished as either ‘‘maximizing’’
However, participants with low creative self-efficacy versus ‘‘satisficing.’’ For the individual who engages
were more likely to think divergently when given in a maximizing choice-making strategy, the strat-
fewer as opposed to greater options. More specifically, egy is to seek out the best alternative by engaging
for subjects with high creative self-efficacy, a greater in an exhaustive search of all possibilities (Schwartz
number of options led to more divergent thinking et al., 2002). For the individual who engages in a
when the goal was to be as creative as possible. satisficing choice-making strategy, the strategy is to
Once in the choice-making situation, if an indi- determine an acceptable option or option that is
vidual personally values the options and enjoys the “good enough.” Thus, a satisficing strategy requires
process of making a choice, he or she may be more only that the individual search until an option is
likely to experience the benefits and less likely to encountered that crosses the threshold of accept-
experience the costs of choice making due to an ability. Research has suggested that individuals who
increased sense of autonomy from making choices engage in maximizing choice-making strategies may
reflective of the self and his or her personal autonomy make better decisions. For example, in one study,

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


maximizers performed better in the job search pro- However, many times people do not know their
cess than satisficers, earning higher salaries in their own preferences before making a decision and con-
resulting job (Iyengar, Wells, & Schwartz, 2006). struct them on the spot during the decision-making
However, despite being more likely to make success- process (Feldman & Lynch, 1988; Payne, Bettman,
ful decisions and obtain desirable outcomes in the & Johnson, 1993). In this case in which personal
long run, maximizers may also be less satisfied with preferences are not well known, choice may have
the choice process and the outcomes of choosing. more limited benefits as the cognitive effort needed
In this same study, maximizers reported feeling to make a decision can be great even in the con-
more ‘‘pessimistic,’’ ‘‘stressed,’’ ‘‘tired,’’ ‘‘anxious,’’ text of a limited number of options or decisions to
‘‘worried,’’ ‘‘overwhelmed,’’ and ‘‘depressed’’ through- be made (Chernev, 2003; Dhar, 1997; Huffman &
out the entire choice process and with their resulting Kahn, 1998; Shafir, Simonson, & Tversky, 1993).
jobs (Iyengar, Wells, & Schwartz, 2006). Given this Furthermore, the limited benefits and potential
relation, it seems likely that one’s preferred choice- costs of making choices when one’s preferences are
making strategy will impact the motivational ben- unclear may only be exacerbated as the number of
efits or detriments of choosing to the extent that the options increases (Chua & Iyengar, 2006).
experience of choice will be less enjoyable, satisfy- In sum, individual differences in levels of initial
ing, or rewarding for maximizers compared to satis- interest and perceived competence for tasks, as well
ficers, particularly when an extensive set of options as variation in one’s value for the options and the
are provided. While choosing may lead to motiva- choice-making process, play a complex role in deter-
tional benefits for satisficers, maximizers may be mining the effects of choice. Though the evidence is
more likely to experience the costs associated with currently limited, findings suggest that choice may
choosing. be especially beneficial for those who lack initial
An individual’s regulatory focus might also be interest for the task at hand. It remains unclear as
particularly important in determining how different of yet whether choice is more or less beneficial for
individuals will perceive choice or will value various those who feel least competent for the task, though
options (Higgins, 1998). According to the regulatory the benefits of providing an extensive amount of
focus theory (Higgins, 1998) all goal-directed behav- choice may be limited to those who are the most
iors are regulated by two distinct motivational systems: confident. Furthermore, once in a choice-making
promotion and prevention focus. Promotion-focused situation, it may be important that individuals value
individuals are primarily concerned with accom- the options provided and enjoy the choice-making
plishment and advancement. They in turn, tend to process in order for choice making to afford motiva-
use an ‘‘approach’’ strategy to gain accomplishments. tional advantages and avoid costs. One’s regulatory
In contrast, prevention-focused individuals are more orientation and preferred choice-making strategy
concerned with safety and fulfillment of responsibili- may be important in determining which options
ties. They, in turn, tend to use an ‘‘avoidance’’ strategy are likely to be valued and when the choice-making
to avoid losses. Consequently, an individual’s per- process will be enjoyable for particular individuals.
sonal orientation to either approach may influence Furthermore, the extent to which preferences are
the value attributed to options provided in a choice- clearly known to the individual may also determine
making context. Research has in fact suggested that when choosing is more or less effortful, enjoyable,
when an object or course of action is consistent and in turn, motivating or detrimental. Additional
with one’s regulatory focus, that option will be allo- research is needed to investigate these possibilities.
cated more value (Forster et al., 1998, 2000; Idson,
Liberman, & Higgins, 2000). Based on this relation cultural and socioeconomic
between regulatory focus and value for a course of differences
action, it seems reasonable to suggest that benefits of An individual’s cultural or socioeconomic back-
choosing will be more apparent if available options ground has also been found to influence the motivat-
are consistent with an individual’s regulatory focus. ing effects of choice. Culture has been hypothesized
However, again, there is little evidence that addresses to moderate the effects of choice or lack of personal
these predictions. choice in light of seminal work suggesting that in
Going further, to the extent that one knows what individualistic cultures (including the United States),
he or she wants and can therefore make a choice based personal agency and independence may be central
on his or her own values and preferences, having the to one’s self-concept (Markus & Kitayama, 1991).
ability to choose is likely to empower and motivate. However, in more collectivistic cultures (such as those

pata ll 
in Asian countries) agency may have less importance. employees in nine different countries ranging in
Instead, non-Westerners may have more interdepen- levels of individualism (Iyengar, Lepper, Hernan-
dent self-concepts in which the goal of belonging- dez, DeVoe, & Alpert, 2001 (unpublished data); see
ness is achieved by acting in accordance with one’s Chua & Iyengar, 2006), results suggested that per-
social obligations to others (Markus & Kitayama, ceptions of choice predicted job satisfaction, intrin-
1991). Consequently, making personal choices with- sic motivation, perceptions of fair treatment at work,
out reference to significant others may not hold as and job performance (as reported by the employees
much importance for individuals from interdepen- managers) significantly better for employees in the
dent cultures as it does for those from independent United States as compared to employees in Asian
cultures. In fact, evidence suggests that individuals countries. Furthermore, comparisons among vari-
from more interdependent cultures construe fewer ous ethnic groups in the United States indicated that
actions of themselves and others as choices, are more the perception of choice was a stronger predictor of
likely to construe actions as choices when it involved these outcomes for European, African, and Hispanic
a response to another person, and are less likely to Americans as compared to Asian Americans.
choose based on their own personal preferences Experimental evidence has further teased apart
(Savani, Markus, & Conner, 2008; Savani, Markus, cultural variation in the effects of choice on motiva-
Naidu, Kumar, & Berlia, 2010). Furthermore, under tion. Iyengar and Lepper (1999) found that intrinsic
circumstances in which significant others are con- motivation was enhanced for students of all cultural
sidered, not choosing may support important psy- backgrounds when a personal choice was made com-
chological needs, including autonomy, and enhance pared to when an unfamiliar person (i.e., the experi-
subsequent motivation for individuals from interde- menter) made a choice for the individual. However,
pendent opposed to independent cultures. among Asian American students, intrinsic motiva-
In line with this reasoning, previous research tion, performance, learning, and other related out-
has suggested that one’s cultural background may comes were enhanced most when trusted authority
influence the extent to which a motivational state figures or peers made choices for them compared to
of dissonance is experienced as a function of choos- when a personal choice was made. For Caucasian
ing. For example, Heine and Lehman (1997) found American students, motivation and performance
that after choosing among similarly desirable CDs, were most enhanced when making a personal choice.
Canadians demonstrated the classic dissonance find- Similarly, in another study (Katz & Assor, 2003),
ing, expressing increased liking for chosen CDs and the provision of choice was found to undermine the
decreased liking for unchosen CDs. However, Japa- intrinsic motivation of collectivistically oriented Bed-
nese participants demonstrated no such spreading ouin children. Somewhat in contrast, Bao and Lam
of alternatives. Similarly, Kitiyama, Snibbe, Markus, (2008) found that Chinese students reported greater
and Suzuki (2008) showed that the standard disso- motivation for a word task or a school course and
nance paradigm did not produce dissonance reduc- greater task performance for a word task when they
tion among Japanese participants. However, when had made a personal choice compared to when their
asked to estimate the preference of the average col- mothers or teachers had made the choice for them.
lege student before making choices, Japanese par- However, this effect was moderated by the level of
ticipants demonstrated a spreading of alternatives, closeness Chinese students felt toward their mother
whereas American participants demonstrated a sig- or teacher. For Chinese students who were close to
nificant spread of alternatives regardless of whether their mothers or teachers, having a choice made for
they were primed to think of others or not. That is, them by their mother or teacher enhanced motiva-
for European American participants, choosing pro- tion for the target task just as well as making a per-
duces dissonance to the extent that one is worried sonal choice. But for students who were not close to
about his or her own competence. However, for Jap- their mothers or teachers, making a personal choice
anese participants, choosing produces dissonance to enhanced motivation more than when mothers or
the extent that one is worried about possible rejec- teachers chose for the student. Finally, Bao and Lam
tion by others. (2008) showed in an additional study that unlike
Furthermore, the effect of choice on intrinsic their findings regarding choosing, students’ level of
motivation, learning, and performance-related out- autonomous motivation positively related to self-
comes may vary depending on the culture of the reported behavioral engagement regardless of how
individual. For example, in a field study in which close students felt to their teacher. Together these
an extensive questionnaire was given to Citigroup findings suggest a point made previously, namely,

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


choosing is not synonymous with autonomy. To the evaluations of chosen objects remained unchanged.
extent that having another person make a choice for In a second study, participants approached in a
an individual is perceived to be an autonomous expe- shopping center were assigned to one of two condi-
rience (as may be the case for individuals from more tions. Participants in the free-choice condition were
collectivistic cultures in which the needs or desires of asked to choose a pen that they liked from among
personally significant others are internalized as part five types of pens, test out the pen, and complete a
of one’s own self-concept), a lack of choice may be pen evaluation survey. Participants in the no-choice
equally or more motivating than making a personal condition were denied the pen of their choice due
choice. to scarcity and offered a different pen that they sub-
Somewhat parallel to theoretical perspectives of sequently tested and evaluated. The results indicated
cultural differences in the choice experience, individ- that college graduate subjects in the no-choice con-
uals’ socioeconomic background may influence their dition evaluated their pens less favorably than those
choice-making experience. Scholars have suggested in the free-choice condition. Conversely, high school
that choice may be particularly relevant to higher graduate subjects evaluated their pens equally favor-
socioeconomic status individuals (i.e., those with a ably irrespective of the experimental condition.
college degree or higher) because agency for these
individuals emphasizes the expression of unique- developmental level
ness and control over the environment (Snibbe & Developmental level may also be an important
Markus, 2005). To the extent that choice is a way to characteristic of the individual that may influence
express one’s unique preferences and exert control how choice is experienced and the effect of choice on
over the environment, choice may be particularly subsequent cognitive and motivational outcomes.
important to individuals of high socioeconomic sta- In particular, developmental level may be impor-
tus. In contrast, among lower socioeconomic status tant because individuals of different ages may vary
individuals (i.e., those without a college degree), in their cognitive capacity to process choices and
notions of agency emphasize integrity, honesty, and may be afforded fewer opportunities for choosing
the expression of self-control. For these individuals, (Bereby-Meyer, Assor, & Katz, 2004; Patall et al.,
personal choice may be less important or may reflect 2008). Limited evidence exists to suggest that there
one’s connection to others (Snibbe & Markus, 2005; are differences in the effects of choice depending on
Stephens, Markus, & Townsend, 2007). developmental level. In fact, there is no single study
In line with this reasoning that individuals from in which age has been varied in order to explore pos-
different classes differ in their models of agency, sible differences in the effects of choice among indi-
Stephens, Markus, and Townsend (2007) found viduals of different developmental levels. However,
that the choices of individuals from higher socio- in a recent meta-analysis (Patall et al., 2008) my col-
economic status backgrounds reflected attempts to leagues and I found that choice had a greater effect
differentiate themselves from others, whereas the on intrinsic motivation for children than for adults.
choices of individuals from lower socioeconomic We hypothesized that this effect might be a func-
status backgrounds reflected attempts to be similar tion of the possibility that children experience fewer
and connected to others. For example, participants opportunities to make choices and to enhance their
from working class compared to middle class back- sense of autonomy than do adults. Consequently,
grounds more often chose pens that appeared similar when a child encounters an opportunity to make
to, rather than different from, other pens in the set choices and to experience a sense of autonomy,
of options, and they more often chose images they the effect is more powerful. Future research should
had previously been told were chosen by another systematically investigate how the experience and
participant. Participants from working class relative effects of choice may vary by developmental level.
to middle class backgrounds also liked their chosen
objects better when someone else had chosen simi- summary
larly (either a confederate or a friend). Going fur- Not only does the form and type of the choice
ther, Snibbe and Markus (2005) found that subjects given influence the observed effects of choosing but
who held college degrees demonstrated the classic individual differences among the people doing the
dissonance effect of evaluating chosen objects more choosing are likely to influence when choice is more
positively and rejected objects less positively. How- or less beneficial. While the research is limited, there
ever, this effect was not found among those subjects is some evidence to suggest that choice will confer
who were high school graduates; for these subjects, greater motivational benefits and fewer costs when an

pata ll 
individual has a clear conception of his or her pref- indeed lead to motivational and performance ben-
erences, values the options available, and enjoys the efits through its impact on perceptions of control.
process of choosing and its outcomes. Likewise, one’s For example, in one experiment (Burger, 1987),
initial level of interest and feelings of competence for undergraduates either were or were not allowed
the tasks involved may influence the extent to which to select the response words for a paired-associate
choice is perceived to afford advantages or not and memory task. In addition, half the subjects were led
thus have a motivating effect. Due to its relation to believe that the experimenter who knew of their
with one’s construal of the self and the importance choice would also know of their performance on the
of personal choice to the self, an individual’s cul- task. The other half thought that the experimenter
tural or socioeconomic background may influence would not know of their choice or their perfor-
the experience and effects of choice. In particular, mance. It was found that choice improved perfor-
agency may be less central and social connected- mance on the task only when subjects also believed
ness more central to the self-concepts of individuals the experimenter would know of their choice and
from more interdependent cultures or lower socio- performance. In a second experiment, participants
economic backgrounds. Consequently, the benefits given a choice of which cognitive aptitude test to
and detriments of making personal choices or lack- take did better on the test than the no-choice partic-
ing personal choice may be different depending on ipants, but only when they believed their choice and
one’s cultural or socioeconomic background and the performance would be known by the experimenter
social conditions under which choices are provided and other participants.
or limited. Finally, given the cognitive complexity of However, while the intensification of self-
choosing and the extent to which choosing is avail- presentational concerns at moderate levels may
able to children compared to adults, the effects of often facilitate the benefits of choice on motiva-
choice may vary depending on one’s developmental tion and performance, motivation may be hindered
age. Limited evidence has suggested thus far that when choice is accompanied by factors that raise
choosing may be particularly beneficial for chil- awareness of public evaluation to distracting or
dren compared to adults, possibly due to children anxiety-provoking levels (Burger, 1989). For exam-
having fewer opportunities for choice. This review ple, Burger (1988) found that participants who
of existing research pointed to a number of poten- were given a choice of words in a paired-associates
tially influential characteristics of the individual that task and a told that both the experimenters and
may explain when choice is more or less motivating. the professor supervising the project would return
However, additional research is needed. to discuss their performance did worse on the
memory task than those who anticipated the same
Characteristics of the Situation discussion but did not choose the words. That is,
Finally, in addition to characteristics of the when self-presentational concerns were high, choice
choices provided and characteristics of the individ- had few motivational and performance benefits.
ual doing the choosing, various characteristics of the However, consistent with choice findings in which
situation may also influence the effects of choice. In only a moderate level of self-presentational concern
particular, the extent to which the situation empha- was created, participants who believed their perfor-
sizes self-presentational concerns and the level of mance would be known only to one undergraduate
realism within which choices are provided may be experimenter showed an increase in performance
situational factors that influence when choice leads when allowed to select the words compared to those
to motivational benefits or detriments. Although who were not given a choice.
little evidence is available in order to examine
their impact, several additional situational factors level of realism
may also be important, including type of the tasks The context in which choice is administered may
involved, the desirability of subsequent outcomes, also affect its impact on motivation. That is, choice
and the correspondence between the choices made may be particularly beneficial in settings in which
and the target for which motivation is measured. it makes intuitive sense to have choices, seems
most realistic, or is most meaningful. For example,
situations that enhance choice may be expected to have a larger effect when
self-presentational concerns it is administered in a classroom with students or
Burger (1989) suggested that at moderate levels in a workplace with workers as opposed to in a
of self-presentational concern, providing choice may contrived laboratory setting in which choice may

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


often be experienced as meaningless or inauthentic. fewer benefits and greater costs of choosing under
In fact, in a recent meta-analysis (Patall et al., 2008), tragic circumstances in which undesirable outcomes
my colleagues and I found that the effect of choice will be experienced regardless of the choice made.
varied as a function of setting. Specifically, choice For example, Beattie and colleagues (1994) found
had the greatest effect on intrinsic motivation when that when people were asked to make choices about
the choice manipulation was implemented in a very consequential decisions with undesirable out-
well-controlled natural setting, for example, when comes regardless of the decision (e.g., a parent was
students were taken to a separate room within the confronted with having to choose which of two
school they attend or employees were taken to a sep- children would receive a bone marrow transplant
arate room within their workplace in order to exam- when both children will die without the proce-
ine the effects of giving them choices, compared to dure), not only did parents experience guilt, regret,
when choices were examined in a traditional labo- and psychological distress, but they also preferred
ratory setting. That is, when the choices given are that the decision be left to fate or another decision
experienced as more authentic, their impact may maker. Similarly, Botti, Orfali, and Iyengar (2009)
be greater. found that when faced with the consequential deci-
sion of having to discontinue their infants’ life sup-
type of tasks port, people experienced greater negative feelings
It has been suggested that choice may be par- and less coping ability than when the same choices
ticularly motivating when it involves a task that is were made by a physician. Although participants
not interesting to begin with (e.g., Tafarodi et al., faced with this kind of tragic decision were resis-
1999), although there is little evidence to examine tant to giving up the option to choose, they had
this supposition. That is, the increase in motivation a weaker desire for autonomy and disliked having
as a function of choosing will be more consequential to make a decision. Given such aversive affective
for a task that is not especially motivating to begin and well-being responses to choosing, as well as
with than for one that is already highly motivating. the reduced desire to choose, it seems reasonable to
For highly engaging and naturally interesting activi- suggest that the motivation to engage in behaviors
ties, it is possible that the benefits of choosing may related to such tragic choices would be limited to
be more limited. In line with this notion, it is worth nonexistent.
noting that many demonstrations of improved moti-
vation and performance due to choice have involved correspondence between choice
neutral or lackluster activities, such as solving ana- options and motivation target
gram puzzles and paired-associate word learning Finally, one potentially influential factor may be
(e.g., Iyengar & Lepper, 1999; Monty, Rosenberger, the correspondence between the choices made and
& Perlmuter, 1973; Perlmuter & Monty, 1973). the target for which motivation is being consid-
A recent demonstration of the classroom effects ered. According to a self-determination perspective,
of choice with a traditionally boring task, home- choice making is expected to enhance subsequent
work, showed that when high school students were persistence and liking for those objects and activi-
allowed to choose among homework options iden- ties which relate to the choices previously made.
tical in the content covered, they reported greater In contrast, a self-regulatory strength depletion
interest and enjoyment for their homework, greater model suggests that choice making will diminish
perceived competence for homework, enhanced persistence on subsequent activities. However, there
homework completion, and improved test scores at is an important methodological difference between
the end of the instructional unit (Patall, Cooper, & the self-regulatory depletion and self-determination
Wynn, 2010). perspectives. Namely, the effect of making choices
within the context of the self-regulatory depletion
desirability of outcomes framework has always been tested by examining
Related to the notion that the benefits of choos- subsequent persistence on a task separate from the
ing are limited to conditions in which the choice choice-making activity. Within the self-determination
involves attractive rather than unappealing options theory perspective, the effects of choice are typically
(Botti & Iyengar, 2004), motivation and related examined for the same tasks for which choices were
benefits of choosing may also be restricted to situ- originally made. Going further, even from a self-
ations in which the outcomes of choosing bring determination perspective, choice making would be
about desirable outcomes. That is, there may be expected to have few benefits when motivation was

pata ll 
assessed for targets unrelated to the choice-making The Role of Preferences
activity. It seems reasonable to suggest that choice One such issue is that of the role of preference
making may provide motivational benefits for those matching in the effect of choice. That is, it remains
activities and objects related to the choices previ- in question as to whether there is any motivational
ously made and simultaneously incur undesirable effect of choosing beyond that of having one’s pref-
motivational and self-regulatory costs for activities erences. This issue has recently emerged in the cog-
and objects unrelated to the choice made. While the nitive dissonance literature (Chen, 2008; Chen &
correspondence between choice content and moti- Risen, 2009; Sagarin, & Skowronski, 2008). Specif-
vational target has yet to be the focus of study in ically, Chen (2008) has argued that a methodology
understanding the effects of choices, the preponder- central to the cognitive dissonance literature (the
ance of evidence supporting both self-determination free-choice paradigm in which participants’ rank
and self-regulatory depletion perspectives (as previ- several options, choose or do not choose between
ously reviewed) suggest that this is likely to be an two approximately equally attractive options, then
important factor. rerank the items) has suffered from an inability to
separately measure how much choices affect peo-
summary ple’s preferences and how much they simply reflect
In this section, the roles of various situational those preferences. The argument is that the tradi-
characteristics in choice effects were described. tional paradigm cannot properly test for dissonance
Though existing evidence is limited, several factors because it fails to account for revealed preferences.
may help to explain variation in the effects of choice. That is, preranking only ensures that the choices are
First, choices may be less beneficial when self- approximately equal in attractiveness, not exactly
presentational concerns are heightened to anxiety- equal and that people simply choose the option
producing levels or when outcomes are undesirable that they prefer, even if the difference is minimal.
despite having been chosen. Second, settings which Thus, even without the motivating effect of cogni-
promote the perception that choosing is a mean- tive dissonance, a subject’s choices among objects
ingful and authentic experience are more likely to will change subsequent rankings. This is because if
produce strong effects of choice. Benefits of choos- the initial ranking is an imperfect measure of pref-
ing may also be particularly likely when the tasks erences, then a subject’s choice is new information
involved are not interesting to begin with. That is, about his or her preferences; choices reflect how
actors may be more likely to benefit from having subjects feel about the goods they are choosing
choices for dull tasks because choosing allows them between. The option chosen is not random, and
to make the task more personally interesting, rel- comparisons which use that option must take this
evant, or meaningful. Finally, it seems important into account. In defense of the traditional para-
to consider the methodological underpinnings of digm, other scholars have argued that people do not
studies looking at the effects of choosing in order always choose the option most preferred (Sagarin
to explain variation in effects. In particular, there & Skowronski, 2008). Even so, it seems reasonable
may be little reason to expect a motivating effect to suggest that when testing for choice effects, it is
of choosing when there is little correspondence necessary to begin with an experimental design that
between the subject of the choice and the motiva- explicitly controls for revealed preferences (Chen,
tional target. 2008).
A similar issue exists in the literature looking
Lingering Issues in Determining the at the effects of choosing on intrinsic motivation,
Motivational Effects of Providing Choices effort, persistence, performance, learning, and related
As the previous review suggests, the experience outcomes. In fact, few if any studies designs have suc-
and effects of choice are not uniform across out- cessfully addressed the confounding between choos-
comes, settings, and people. A look at the cumulative ing and having one’s preference. A common design
evidence suggests that in many cases, opportunities among studies looking at the effects of choosing on
for choosing may motivate behavior in various ways intrinsic motivation and related outcomes is to use
and through a variety of mechanisms. However, yoking or matching in order to control for the par-
there appear to be many limits to the benefits of ticular task or options that the individual receives.
choosing. That said, a number of lingering issues Specifically, in a yoked design, the experimenter
continue to obfuscate our understanding of the matches a control participant with an experimental
effects of choosing. participant so that in both conditions there are an

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


equal number of participants doing the same task the reading material reported after engaging in the
or having the same task options (e.g., Iyengar & task. In contrast, students’ reports of the interest expe-
Lepper, 1999; Zuckerman et al., 1978). In this way, rienced while reading the text significantly predicted
a yoked design will perfectly control for the con- attitudes and engagement in the task.
founding effect of the task or option the participant With such limited information on the effects of
chose or is assigned. Matched designs also attempt to choice when separated from the role of preferences it
control for the confounding effect of task; however, is difficult as of yet to tell whether the power of choice
this is accomplished by excluding participants who goes beyond the effect of having one’s preferences.
do not engage in a target activity or option. While However, several points seem warranted based on the
these design strategies rule out the possibility that accumulated evidence and theory on choice effects.
differences between the choice and no-choice condi- Given that much of the power of choice seems to
tions can be attributed to the fact that participants be dependent on the extent to which choosing sup-
in the choice condition chose different options than ports feelings of control, competence, and autonomy,
those that were assigned to participants in the no- choices that are made without information on which
choice condition, they do not successfully control to base a decision are unlikely to have benefits. That
for the preference matching that is possible in the is, undecipherable choices are unlikely to have ben-
choice condition. That is, even though a no-choice efits because, aside from not being able to preference
participant was assigned the same option as his or match, these sorts of choices do not lead the actor to
her choice participant counterpart, this assigned feel more in control of outcomes, more competent,
option may not reflect the no-choice participant’s or more autonomous. As such, these designs may not
preferences. As such, differences attributed to choice be the best way to separate the independent effect of
making may actually be attributable to the fact that choosing from having one’s preference. In fact, based
participants in one group received options that they on the previous review, being asked to choose without
prefer, while participants in the other condition did information to base one’s decision would be expected
not. It is possible that the act of personally choos- to yield no or even negative effects to the extent this
ing may confer no additional benefit beyond having type of situation may be perceived as highly control-
received one’s preferences. ling. A preferred research design strategy would be
There is little evidence that partitions the effect of to assess preferences in advance of providing choices
choosing from the effect of having one’s preference. so that no-choice participants may be assigned their
One study suggested that choosing, even when pref- preferences. This suggestion would be relevant even
erence matching is near impossible, may still confer in cases in which choice is illusory. For example, in
motivational benefits (Henry, 1994). Henry (1994) a case in which the options participants choose have
attempted to separate the effect of choosing from pref- no bearing on the task they are asked to engage in, as
erence by having participants choose or be assigned we have seen, a powerful effect may still lie in the
almanac questions based only on an uninformative mere perception of having chosen one’s preferences.
label (Set A or Set B) with no information on charac- It remains relevant to assess one’s prior prefer-
teristics that might differentiate the options given. She ences even if in the end the choice or assignment
found that among males, having a choice produced is only between different labels for a single option.
enhanced perceptions of competence prior to engag- Furthermore, given the seeming importance of peo-
ing in the task, although having this type of choice ple’s needs for control and autonomy under many
produced no performance benefits. Other studies circumstances, it seems reasonable to suggest that
have found that choice may have few if any benefits choosing is likely to yield small benefits above and
when information about the options is unavailable, beyond having one’s preference. However, certainly
and therefore preference matching is impossible. In the most pronounced effects of choosing are likely to
two studies, Flowerday and colleagues (Flowerday emerge when comparing those who choose based on
et al., 2004) had participants choose between two personal preferences to those who do not choose or
sealed packets of materials (Packet A and Packet B) receive their preferences.
without other information presented on which to
base a decision. In this case, choice had no benefits for Future Research on Factors That
participants’ attitudes (i.e., motivation, enjoyment, Enhance or Inhibit Choice Effects
satisfaction) toward the reading task, their engage- This review has highlighted a number of poten-
ment in the task, or subsequent learning, controlling tially influential factors in determining when
for prior interest in the reading topic or interest for choosing is more or less motivating and when it may

pata ll 
even be detrimental. However, as has been high- implicated in numerous psychological theories, as
lighted throughout, many of these factors have yet well as in social science theories more broadly. The
to be the systematic focus of study in single experi- motivational factors implicated to influence choice
mental designs. Rather, in this narrative review (as making are numerous—too numerous in fact to even
is often the case, even in quantitative reviews using adequately review here. However, perhaps equally
meta-analysis), many important factors have been complex is the influence of the experience of choice
identified using patterns that emerge across studies on motivation. The cumulative evidence suggests that
rather than within studies. For example, much of the under most circumstances, the presence of choice
evidence drawn on to conclude that choice effects may have powerful motivating effects, helping to
vary with the extent to which choosing is an effortful shape attitudes, enhance perceptions of control and
process makes assumptions about particular kinds of competence, intrinsically motivate behavior, as well
choices being more or less effortful and draw com- as enhance a variety of adaptive outcomes (e.g., learn-
parisons across studies. Research in which the effort ing, performance, effort, satisfaction, and creativity,
exerted in making a choice is systematically varied in a among others). However, choosing is likely not equal
single experimental design is needed to explicitly test across all types of choices, people, and circumstances,
these assertions. Similarly, though an initial look at and the benefits of choosing are not had without cost.
the existing research suggests that factors such as the Whether for bad or good, choosing is a necessity in
frame of reference, initial levels of value, interest, or most people’s lives. Furthermore, the potential ben-
perceived competence for options and tasks, develop- efits and limits of choice suggest that there are prac-
mental age, type of task, or correspondence between tical implications of understanding choice effects for
the choice options and motivational target are likely classroom, workplace, and therapeutic settings. As
to be important factors, there is limited evidence on such, it is important for both theory and practice that
which such claims are made. Furthermore, influen- future research continue to provide a firm founda-
tial factors are likely to interact in complex ways. The tion on which to base conclusions about the circum-
burden falls on future research to systematically inves- stances under which choice is more or less beneficial.
tigate factors suggested here and elsewhere in well- Although simplicity is always desirable, an accurate
controlled experimental designs. Additional research understanding of choice effects requires this more
is needed to reveal which factors are truly influential complex analysis of the phenomena.
moderators as well as examine the complex relation-
ships that may exist among these moderators. Finally, Future Directions
as can be garnered by this review, choice has been 1. Few of the choice-, person-, and situation-
linked to a variety of motivation-related outcomes. related characteristics proposed to moderate the
It seems reasonable to suggest that various outcomes effect of choice on motivation are well established
may be differentially related to choice when vari- based on existing literature. As such, what are
ous moderators are taken into consideration. As one the most important factors in determining when
example, some choices may more effectively enhance choosing will lead to motivational benefits or
feelings of control, while other forms of choice may decrements, and stronger or weaker effects?
more effectively enhance feelings of autonomy. Vari- 2. How do important choice-, person-, and
ous motivational consequences, such as intrinsic situation-related factors moderating the role of
motivation or perceived competence, may be dif- choice interact to affect the relation between choice
ferentially impacted to the extent that choices are and motivation?
intentionally designed to satisfy each of these various 3. How do the effects of choice vary from one
needs. Likewise, the interaction of choice making motivational outcome to the next (e.g., intrinsic
with other important moderators may lead to differ- motivation versus perceived competence versus
ent effects depending on what motivational outcome task value) when considering the role of important
is considered. Future research is needed to tease apart choice-, person-, and situation-related
the differential pathways by which choice may lead moderating factors?
to various motivational outcomes. 4. What is the effect of choice on motivation after
accounting for the role of preference matching?
Conclusions
A review of the relations between choice and moti- References
vation suggests that the relationship is far reaching, Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory
reciprocal, and complex. Choice making has been of planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckman (Eds.),

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


Action-control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11–39). Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. New
Heidelberg, Germany: Springer. York: Freeman.
Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Chicago, IL: Bandura, A., & Schunk, D. H. (1981). Cultivating competence,
Dorsey Press. self-efficacy, and intrinsic interest through proximal self-
Ajzen, I. (2002). Perceived behavioral control, self-efficacy, locus motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41,
of control, and the theory of planned behavior. Journal of 586–598.
Applied Social Psychology, 32, 665–683. Bao, X., & Lam, S. (2008). Who makes the choice? Rethinking
Amabile, T. M. (1979). Effects of external evaluation on artis- the role of autonomy and relatedness in Chinese children’s
tic creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, motivation. Child Development, 79, 269–283.
221–233. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life.
Amabile, T. M. (1983). The social psychology of creativity: In R. S. Wyer, Jr. (Ed.), The automaticity of everyday life:
A componential conceptualization. Journal of Personality and Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp. 1–61). Mahwah,
Social Psychology, 45, 357–376. NJ: Erlbaum.
Amabile, T. M., & Gitomer, J. (1984). Children’s artistic creativ- Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automa-
ity: Effect of choice in task materials. Personality and Social ticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.
Psychology Bulletin, 10, 209–215. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M.
Amabile, T. M., Hennessey, B. A., & Grossman, B. S. (1986). (1998). Ego-depletion: Is the active self a limited resource?
Social influence on creativity: The effects of contract-for Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1252–1265.
rewards. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, Baumeister, R. F., & Vohs, K. D. (2007). Self-regulation, ego
14–23. depletion, and motivation. Social and Personality Psychology
Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, F. I., & Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hope- Compass, 1, 1–14.
lessness depression: A theory based subtype of depression. Beattie, J., Baron, J., Hershey, J. C., & Spranca, M. D. (1994).
Psychological Review, 96, 358–372. Psychological determinants of decision attitude. Journal of
Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Teasdale, J. (1978). Behavioral Decision Making, 7, 129–144.
Learned helplessness in humans. Critique and reformulation. Becker, D. A. (1997). The effects of choice on auditors’ intrinsic
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74. motivation and performance. Behavioral Research in Account-
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student ing, 9, 1–19.
motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271. Bem, D. J. (1967). Self-perception: An alternative interpretation
Ames, C., & Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the class- of cognitive dissonance phenomena. Psychological Review, 74,
room: Students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. 183–200.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 260–267. Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.),
Anderman, E. M., & Maehr, M. L. (1994). Motivation and Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 1–62).
schooling in the middle grades. Review of Educational New York: Academic Press.
Research, 64, 287–309. Bem, D. J., & McConnell, H. K. (1971). Testing the self-
Aronson, E. (1969). A theory of cognitive dissonance: A current perception explanation of dissonance phenomena: On the
perspective. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimen- salience of premanipulation attitudes. Journal of Personality
tal social psychology (Vol. 4, pp. 1–34) New York: Academic and Social Psychology, 14, 23–31.
Press. Bereby-Meyer, Y., Assor, A., & Katz, I. (2004). Children’s choice
Aronson, E. (1992). The return of the repressed: Dissonance the- strategies: The effects of age and task demands. Cognitive
ory makes a comeback. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 303–311. Development, 19, 127–146.
Aronson, E. (1999). Dissonance, hypocrisy, and the self-concept. Berglas, S., & Jones, E. E. (1978). Drug choice as a self-
In E. Harmon-Jones & J. Mills (Eds.), Cognitive dissonance: handicapping strategy in response to noncontingent success.
Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 103–126). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36(4), 405–417.
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1981). The relationship of career-
Assor, A., Kaplan, H., & Roth, G. (2002). Choice is good, but related self-efficacy expectation to perceived career options
relevance is excellent: Autonomy-enhancing and suppress- in college women and men. Journal of Counseling Psychology,
ing teacher behaviors predicting students’ engagement in 28, 399–410.
school work. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, Betz, N. E., & Hackett, G. (1983). The relationship of math-
261–278. ematics self-efficacy expectations to the selection of science-
Atkinson, J. W (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking based college majors. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 23,
behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372 329–345.
Atkinson, J. W. (1964). An introduction to motivation. Princeton, Botti, S., & Iyengar, S. S. (2004). The psychological pleasure and
NJ: Van Nostrand. pain of choosing: When people prefer choosing at the cost of
Atkinson, J. W., & Litwin, G. H. (1960). Achievement motive subsequent outcome satisfaction. Journal of Personality and
and test anxiety as motive to approach success and motive Social Psychology, 87, 312–326
to avoid failure. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Botti, S., & Iyengar, S. S. (2006). The dark side of choice: When
60, 52–63. choice impairs social welfare. Journal of Public Policy and
Averill, J. R. (1973). Personal control over aversive stimuli and its Marketing, 25, 24–38.
relationship to stress. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 286–303. Botti, S., Orfali, K., & Iyengar, S. S. (2009). Tragic choices:
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of Autonomy and emotional response to medical decision.
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. Journal of Consumer Research, 36, 337–352.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: Brehm, J. (1956). Post-decision changes in desirability of alterna-
A social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. tives. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 52, 384–389.

pata ll 
Brehm, J. W. (1962). A dissonance analysis of attitude-discrepant decision-making. Organizational Behavior and Human
behavior. New York: Gaines Dog Research Center. Decision Processes, 69, 117–132.
Brehm, J. W. (1966). A theory of psychological reactance. Croyle, R. T., & Cooper, J. (1983). Dissonance arousal: Physi-
New York: Academic Press. ological evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1959). Re-evaluation of choice 45, 782–791.
alternatives as a function of their number and qualitative DeCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation. New York: Academic
similarity. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 58, 373–378. Press.
Brehm, J. W., & Cohen, A. R. (1962). Explorations in cognitive Deci, E. L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on
dissonance. New York: Wiley. intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychol-
Burger, J. M. (1987). Increased performance with increased per- ogy, 18, 105–115.
sonal control: A self-presentation interpretation. Journal of Deci, E. L. (1980). The psychology of self-determination. Lexing-
Experimental Social Psychology, 23, 350–360. ton, MA: Heath.
Burger, J. M. (1989). Negative reactions to increases in perceived Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self-
personal control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, determination in a work organization. Journal of Applied
56, 246–256. Psychology, 74, 580–590.
Calder, B., Ross, M., & Insko, C. (1973). Attitude change and Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
attitude attribution: Effects of incentive, choice, and con- determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
sequence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 25, Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy
84–99. and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A use- Psychology, 53, 1024–1037.
ful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and Dember, W. N., Galinsky, T. L., & Warm, J. S. (1992). The
health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111–135. role of choice in vigilance performance. Bulletin of the Psy-
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (2002). Control processes and chonomic Society, 30, 201–204.
self-organization as complementary principles underlying Detweiler, J. B., Mendoza, R. J., & Lepper, M. R. (1996,
behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 304–315. June). Perceived versus actual choice: High perceived choice
Carling, A. (1992). Social divisions. London: Verso. enhances children’s task engagement. Paper presented to the
Chaiken, S., & Baldwin, M. W. (1981). Affective–cognitive con- 8th Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Society,
sistency and the effect of salient of behavioral information San Francisco, CA.
on the self-perception of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Dhar, R. (1997). Consumer preference for a no-choice option.
Social Psychology, 41, 1–12. Journal of Consumer Research,24, 215–231.
Chen, K. (2008). Rationalization and cognitive dissonance: Do Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning.
choices affect or reflect preferences? [Discussion Paper No. 1669]. American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048.
Cowles Foundation for Research in Economics, Yale Uni- Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, per-
versity. Retrieved July 2011, from http://cowles.econ.yale. sonality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: The Psychology
edu/P/cd/d16b/d1669.pdf Press.
Chen, M. K., & Risen, J. L. (2009). Is choice a reliable predictor Dweck, C., & Bempechat, J. (1983). Children’s theories of intel-
of choice? A comment on Sagarin and Skowronski. Journal of ligence. In S. Paris, G. Olsen, & H. Stevenson (Eds.), Learn-
Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 425–427. ing and motivation in the classroom (pp. 239–256). Hillsdale,
Chernev, A. (2003). When more is less and less is more: The role NJ: Erlbaum.
of ideal point availability and assortment in choice. Journal Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive
of Consumer Research, 30, 170–183 approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review,
Chua, R. Y. J., & Iyengar, S. (2005). The effects of choice, goal, 95, 256–273.
and creative self-efficacy on divergent thinking and creative out- Eccles, J. S. (1984). Sex differences in achievement patterns. In T.
comes. Academy of Management Conference. Hawaii. Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1983
Chua, R. Y.J., & Iyengar, S. (2006). Empowerment through (pp. 97–132). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
choice? A critical analysis of the effects of choice in organiza- Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women’s achievement related
tions. Research in Organizational Behavior 27, 41–79. decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11, 135–172.
Coleman, J. (1973). The mathematics of collective action. London: Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M.,
Heinemann. Meece, J., & Midgley, C. (1983). Expectancies, values and
Collins, B. E., Ashmore, R. D., Hornbeck, F. W., & Whitney, academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and
R. E. (1970). Studies in forced compliance: XIII and XV in achievement motives (pp. 75–146). San Francisco, CA: W. H.
search of dissonance producing forced compliance paradigm. Freeman.
Representative Research in Social Psychology, 1, 11–23. Eccles, J. S., Adler, T., & Meece, J. L. (1984). Sex differences in
Cooper, J., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance achievement: A test of alternate theories. Journal of Personal-
theory. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social ity and Social Psychology, 46, 26–43.
psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 229–266). New York: Academic Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational belief, values,
Press. and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132.
Cordova, D., & Lepper, M. (1996). Intrinsic motivation and the Edwards, W. (1954). The theory of decision making. Psychologi-
process of learning: Beneficial effects of contextualization, cal Bulletin, 51, 380–417.
personalization, and choice. Journal of Educational Psychol- Elliot, A. J., & Devine, P. G. (1994). On the motivational
ogy, 88, 715–730. nature of cognitive dissonance: Dissonance as psychological
Crowe, E., & Higgins, E. T. (1997). Regulatory focus and discomfort. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,
strategic inclinations: Promotion and prevention in 382–394.

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to Goethals, G. R., & Cooper, J. (1972). Role of intention and
motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social postbehavioral consequence in the arousal of cognitive dis-
Psychology, 54, 5–12. sonance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 23,
Epstein, J. (1989). Family structure and student motivation: 293–301
A developmental perspective. In C. Ames & R. Ames (Eds.), Greenberg, E. (1992). Creativity, autonomy, and evaluation of
Research on motivation in education (Vol. 3, pp. 259–295). creative work: Artistic workers in organizations. Journal of
San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Creative Behavior, 26, 75–80
Estes, W. K. (1976). The cognitive side of probability learning. Greenleaf, E. A., & Lehmann, D. R. (1995). Reasons for
Psychological Review, 83, 37–64. substantial delay in consumer decision making. Journal of
Fazio, R. H. (1981). On the self-perception explanation of the Consumer Research, 22, 186–199.
overjustification effect: The role of salience of initial attitude. Hackett, G., & Betz, N. E. (1981). A self-efficacy approach
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 17, 417–426. to the career development of women. Journal of Vocational
Fazio, R. H., Zanna, M. P., & Cooper, J. (1977). Dissonance and Behavior, 18, 326–336.
self-perception: An integrative view of each theory’s proper Hackett, G., & Betz, N. E. (1989). An exploration of the
domain of application. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- mathematics self-efficacy/mathematics performance corre-
ogy, 13, 464–479. spondence. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,
Feather, N. T. (1988). Values, valences, and course enrollment: 20, 261–273.
testing the role of personal values within an expectancy- Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Pintrich, P. R., Elliot, A. J.,
value framework. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, & Thrash, T. M. (2002). Revision of achievement goal
381–391. theory: Necessary and illuminating. Journal of Educational
Feldman, J. M., & Lynch, J. G., Jr. (1988). Self-generated valid- Psychology, 94, 638–645.
ity and other effects of measurement on belief, attitude, Harmon-Jones, E., Brehm, J. W., Greenberg, J., Simon, L., &
intention and behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 73, Nelson, D. E. (1996). Is the production of aversive conse-
421–435. quences necessary to create cognitive dissonance? Journal of
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford, Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 5–16.
CA: Stanford University Press. Harmon-Jones, E., & Mills, J. (1999). Cognitive dissonance: Prog-
Festinger, L. (1964). Conflict, decision, and dissonance. Stanford, ress on a pivotal theory in social psychology. Washington, DC:
CA: Stanford University Press. American Psychological Association.
Fitch, G. (1970). Effects of self-esteem, perceived performance Helmreich, R., & Collins, B. E. (1968). Studies in forced com-
and choice on causal attributions. Journal of Personality and pliance: Commitment and magnitude of inducement to
Social Psychology, 16, 311–315 comply as determinants of opinion change. Journal of Person-
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1981). Attitudes and voting ality and Social Psychology, 10, 75–81.
behaviour: An application of the theory of reasoned action. Heath, A. (1976). Rational choice and social exchange. Cambridge,
In G. M. Stephenson & J. M. Davis (Eds.), Progress in applied England: Cambridge University Press.
social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 95–125). London: Wiley. Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (1997). Culture, dissonance, and
Flowerday, T., & Schraw, G. (2000). Teacher beliefs about self-affirmation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23,
instructional choice: A phenomenological study. Journal of 389–400.
Educational Psychology, 92, 634–645. Henry, R. A. (1994). The effects of choice and incentives on the
Flowerday, T., & Schraw, G. (2003). Effect of choice on cog- overestimation of future performance. Organizational Behav-
nitive and affective engagement. Journal of Educational ior and Human Decision Processes, 57, 210–225.
Research, 96, 207–215. Henry, R. A., & Sniezek, J. A. (1993). Situational factors
Flowerday, T., Schraw, G., & Stevens, J. (2004). The role of affecting judgments of future performance. Organizational
choice and interest in reader engagement. The Journal of Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 54, 104–132.
Experimental Education, 72, 93–114 Hidi, S., & Baird, W. (1986). Interestingness—A neglected vari-
Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and able in discourse processing. Cognitive Science, 10, 179–194.
avoidance strength during goal attainment: Regulatory focus Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four-phase model of
and the “goal looms larger” effect. Journal of Personality and interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127.
Social Psychology, 75, 1115–1131. Higgins, E. T. (1998). From expectancies to worldviews: Regula-
Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Strack, F. (2000). When stereotype tory focus in socialization and cognition. In J. M. Darley
disconfirmation is a personal threat: How prejudice and & J. Cooper (Eds.), Attribution and social interaction: The
prevention focus moderate incongruency effects. Social legacy of Edward E. Jones (pp. 243–309). Washington, DC:
Cognition, 18, 178–197. American Psychological Association.
Freedman, J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without Higgins, E. T. (2000). Making a good decision: Value from fit.
pressure: The foot-in-the-door technique. Journal of Personal- American Psychologist, 55, 1217–1230.
ity and Social Psychology, 4, 196–202 Higgins, E. T., Trope, Y., & Kwon, J. (1999). Augmentation and
Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). The effects of autonomy undermining from combining activities: The role of choice
on motivation and performance in the college classroom. in activity engagement theory. Journal of Experimental Social
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 477–486. Psychology, 35, 285–307.
Gerard, H. (1992). Dissonance theory: A cognitive psychology Hoyle, R. H., & Sherrill, M. R. (2006). Future orientation in
with a engine. Psychological Inquiry, 3, 323–327 the self-system: Possible selves, self-regulation, and behavior.
Gerard, H. B., & White, G. L. (1983). Post-decisional reevalu- Journal of Personality, 74, 1673–1696.
ation of choice alternatives. Personality and Social Psychology Huffman, C., & Kahn, B. E. (1998). Variety for sale: Mass custom-
Bulletin, 9, 365–369. ization or mass confusion? Journal of Retailing, 74, 491–513.

pata ll 
Idson, L. C., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2000). Distin- Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest
guishing gains from nonlosses and losses from nongains: and intrinsic motivational orientations. Learning and Instruc-
A regulatory focus perspective on hedonic intensity. Journal tion, 15, 381–395.
of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 252–274. Langer, E. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality
Iyengar, S. S., Elwork, R. F., & Schwartz, R. (2006). Doing bet- and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.
ter but feeling worse: Looking for the ‘best’ job undermines Langer, E. (1979). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality
satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17, 143–150. and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.
Iyengar, S. S., Jiang, W., & Huberman, G. (2004). How much Langer, E. J., & Rodin, J. (1976). The effects of choice and
choice is too much: Determinants of individual contri- enhanced personal responsibility for the aged: A field experi-
butions in 401K retirement plans. In O. S. Mitchell & ment in an institutional setting. Journal of Personality and
S. Utkus (Eds.), Pension design and structure: New Lessons Social Psychology, 34, 191–198.
from Behavioral Finance, (pp. 83–97). Oxford, England: Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermin-
Oxford University Press. ing children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic rewards: A test
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Rethinking the value of the “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality
of choice: A cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. and Social Psychology, 28, 129–137.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 349–366. Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of topological psychology. New York:
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demoti- McGraw-Hill.
vating: Can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Lewin, K. (1951). Field theory in social science; selected theoretical
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 995–1006. papers. (D. Cartwright Ed.). New York: Harper & Row.
Iyengar, S. S., Wells, R., & Schwartz, B. (2006). Doing better Lewin, K. (1952). Group decision and social change.
but feeling worse: Looking for the ‘‘best’’ job undermines In G. E. Swanson, T. M. Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley (Eds.),
satisfaction. Psychological Science, 17, 143–150. Readings in social psychology (pp. 459–473). New York:
Janis, I. L., & Mann, L. (1977). Decision making, a psychological Henry Holt.
analysis of conflict, choice and commitment. New York: The Linder, D. E., Cooper, J., & Jones, E. E. (1967). “Decision free-
Free Press. dom as a determinant of the role of incentive magnitude in
Jacobs, J., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Parents, task values, and attitude change.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
real life achievement-related choices. In C. Sansone & 6, 245–254.
J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motiva- Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., Tyson, D., & Patall, E. A. (2008).
tion: The search for optimal motivation and performance When are achievement goal orientations beneficial for
(pp. 405–439). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. academic achievement? A closer look at main effects and
Jones, E. E., & Harris, V. A. (1967). The attribution of attitudes. moderating factors. International Review of Social Psychology,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 1–24. 21, 19–70.
Jones, E. E., Worchel, S., Goethals, G. R., & Grumet, J. F. Lipstein, R., & Renninger, K. A. (2007). “Putting things into
(1971). Prior expectancy and behavioral extremity as deter- words”: The development of 12–15-year-old students’ inter-
minants of attitude attribution. Journal of Experimental Social est for writing. In P. Boscolo & S. Hidi (Eds.), Motivation
Psychology, 7, 59–80. and writing: Research and school practice (pp. 113–140).
Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice. New York: Elsevier.
American Psychologist, 58, 697−720. Luce, M. (1998). Choosing to avoid: Coping with negatively
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An anal- emotion-laden consumer decisions. The Journal of Consumer
ysis of decisions under risk. Econometrika, 47, 263−291. Research, 24, 409–433.
Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2003). Is autonomy important for non- Lyubomirsky, S., & Ross, L. (1999). Changes in attractiveness of
western students? Examining autonomy as a universal human elected, rejected, and precluded alternatives: A comparison
propensity. Paper presented at the 84th Annual Meeting of happy and unhappy individuals. Journal of Personality and
of the American Educational Research Association, Social Psychology, 76, 988–1007.
Chicago, IL. Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1991). Enhancing student moti-
Katz, I., & Assor, A. (2007). When choice motivates and when it vation: A schoolwide approach. Educational Psychologist, 26,
does not. Educational Psychology Review, 19, 429–442. 399–427.
Kelley, H. H. (1967). Attribution theory in social psychology. Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1976). Learned helplessness:
In D. Levine (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation. Theory and evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. General, 105, 3–46.
Kelley, H. H. (1973). The process of causal attribution. American Malhotra, N. K. (1982). Information load and consumer
Psychologist, 28, 107–128. decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 8, 419–430.
Kernan, M. C., Heimann, B., & Hanges, P. J. (1991). Effects Manstead, A. S. R., Proffitt, C., & Smart, J. L. (1983). Predict-
of goal choice, strategy choice, and feedback source on goal ing and understanding mothers’ infant feeding intentions
acceptance, performance, and subsequent goals. Journal of and behavior: Testing the theory of reasoned action. Journal
Applied Social Psychology, 21, 713–733 of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 657–671.
Kitiyama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T. Markus, H., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self:
(2008). Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two Implications for cognition, emotion and motivation. Psycho-
cultures. Psychological Science, 15, 527–533. logical Review, 98, 224–253.
Krapp, A. (2002). An educational-psychological theory of interest Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American
and its relation to SDT. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Psychologist, 41, 954–969.
The handbook of self-determination research (pp. 405–427). Maslow, A. (1954). Motivation and personality. New York:
Rochester, NY: Rochester University. Harper.

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Predictors of Possible selves: Theory, research, and application (pp. 17–39).
math anxiety and its influence on young adolescents’ course Huntington, NY: Nova.
enrollment intentions and performance in mathematics. Oyserman, D., & James, L. (2008). Possible selves: From con-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 60–70. tent to process. In K. Markman, W. M. P. Klein, & J. A. Suhr
Meece, J., Anderman E. M., & Anderman, L. H. (2006). (Eds.), The handbook of imagination and mental stimula-
Structures and goals of educational settings: Classroom goal tion (pp. 373–394). New York: Psychology Press.
structure, student motivation, and academic achievement. In Overskeid, G., & Svartdal, F. (1996). Effects of reward on
S. T. Fiske, A. E. Kazdin, & D. L. Schacter (Eds.), Annual subjective autonomy and interest when initial interest is low.
review of psychology (Vol. 57, pp. 487–504). Stanford, CA: The Psychological Record, 46, 319–331.
Annual Reviews. Patall, E. A. (2009). The motivational benefits and limits of
Melson, W. H., Calder, B. J., & Insko, C. A. (1969). The social choice. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Duke Univer-
psychological status of reward. Psychonomic Science, 17, sity, Durham, NC.
240–242. Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Robinson, J. C. (2008). The effects
Midgley, C., & Feldlaufer, H. (1987). Students’ and teachers’ of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes:
decision-making fit before and after the transition to junior A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin,
high school. Journal of Early Adolescence, 7, 225–241. 134, 270–300.
Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., & Middleton, M. J. (2001). Patall, E. A., Cooper, H., & Wynn, S. R. (2010). The effective-
Performance-approach goals: Good for what, for whom, ness and relative importance of providing choices in the
under what circumstances, and at what cost? Journal of classroom. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 896–915.
Educational Psychology, 93, 77–86. Parker, L. E., & Lepper, M. R. (1992). Effects of fantasy
Mikulincer, M. (1988). Freedom of choice, control, and learned contexts on children’s learning and motivation: Making
helplessness. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 7, learning more fun. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
203–213. ogy, 62, 625–633.
Mischel, W., & Ebbesen, E. B. (1970). Attention in delay of Paterson, R. J., & Neufeld, R. W. J. (1995). What are my
gratification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16, options? Influences of choice availability on stress and the
329–337 perception of control. Journal of Research in Personality, 29,
Mitchell, T. R. (1974). Expectancy models of job satisfaction, 145–167.
occupational preference and effort: A theoretical, method- Payne, J. J., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. R. (1993). The
ological, and empirical appraisal. Psychological Bulletin, 81, adaptive decision maker. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
1053–1077 University Press.
Mogilner, C., Rudnick, T., & Iyengar, S. S. (2008). The mere Perlmuter, L. C., & Monty, R. A. (1973). Effect of choice of
categorization effect: How the presense of categories increases stimulus on paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental
choosers’ perceptions of assortment variety and outcome Psychology, 99, 120–123.
satisfaction. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 202–215. Perlmuter, L. C., & Monty, R. A. (1977). The importance of
Moller, A. C., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2006). Choice and perceived control: Fact or fantasy? American Scientist, 65,
ego-depletion: The moderating role of autonomy. Personality 759–765.
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 1024–1036. Perlmuter, L. C., Monty, R. A., & Kimble, G. A. (1971). Effect
Monty, R. A., & Perlmuter, L. C. (1975). Persistence of the of choice on paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimen-
effects of choice on paired-associate learning. Memory and tal Psychology, 91, 47–53.
Cognition, 3, 183–187. Perlmuter, L. C., Scharff, K., Karsh, R. A., & Monty, R. A.
Monty, R. A., & Perlmuter, L. C. (1987). Choice, control, and (1980). Perceived control: A generalized state of motivation.
motivation in the young and the aged. In M. L. Maehr & Motivation and Emotion, 4, 35–45.
D. L. Kleiber (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achieve- Peterson, C., Maier, S. F., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1993). Learned
ment. Vol. 5: Enhancing motivation (pp. 99–122). Greenwich, helpessness. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
CT: JAI. Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P. (1984). Causal explanations
Monty, R. A., Rosenberger, M. A., & Perlmuter, L. C. (1973). as a risk factor for depression: Theory and evidence. Psycho-
Amount and locus of choice as sources of motivation in logical Review, 91, 347–374.
paired-associate learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Reeve, J., Nix, G., & Hamm, D. (2003). Testing models of the
97, 16–21. experience of self-determination in intrinsic motivation and
Muraven, M., & Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Self-regulation and the conundrum of choice. Journal of Educational Psychology,
depletion of limited resources: Does self-control resemble a 95, 375–392.
muscle? Psychological Bulletin, 126, 247–259. Renninger, K. A. (1992). Individual interest and development:
Muraven, M., Baumeister, R. F., & Tice, D. M. (1999). Self- Implications for theory and practice. In K. A. Renninger,
control as limited resource: Regulatory depletion patterns. S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 774–789. development (pp. 361–376). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Renninger, K. A. (2009). Interest and identity development in
Oxford University Press. instruction: An inductive model. Educational Psychologist,
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of 44, 1–14.
ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Renninger, K. A., Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (1992). The role of
Psychological Review, 91, 328–346. interest in learning and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Oyserman, D., & Fryberg, S. (2006). The possible selves of Rodin, J., Rennert, K.., & Solomon, S. K. (1980). Intrin-
diverse adolescents: Content and function across gender, race sic motivation for control: Fact or fiction. In A. Baum &
and national origin. In C. Dunkel & J. Kerpelman (Eds.), J. E. Singer (Eds.), Advances in environmental psychology.

pata ll 
Vol 2: Applications of personal control (pp. 131–148). Sheppard, B. H., Hartwick, J., & Warshaw, P.R (1988). The the-
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ory of reasoned action: A meta-analysis of past research with
Rogers, C. R. (1969). Freedom to learn. Columbus, OH: recommendations for modifications and future research.
Merrill. Journal of Consumer Research, 15, 325–343.
Rotter, J. B. (1954). Social learning and clinical psychology. Sherman, S. J. (1970). Attitudinal effects of unforeseen con-
New York: Prentice-Hall. sequences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 16,
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus 510–520.
external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, Shugan, S. M. (1980). The cost of thinking. Journal of Consumer
80, 1–28. Research, 7, 99–111.
Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic motiva- Shultz, T. R., Leveille, E., & Lepper, M. R. (1999). Free-choice
tion, autonomy, and the self in psychological development. and cognitive dissonance revisited. Personality and Social
In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Psychology Bulletin, 25, 40–48.
Vol. 40: Developmental perspectives on motivation (pp. 1–56). Simon, H. A. (1955). A behavioral model of rational choice.
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 59, 99–118.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory Simon, H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the
and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop- environment. Psychological Review, 63, 129–138.
ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. Simon, H. A. (1957). Models of man, social and rational: Math-
Sagarin, B. J., & Skowronski, J. J. (2008). The implications of ematical essays on rational human behavior. New York: Wiley.
imperfect measurement for free choice carry-over effects: Simon, H. A. (1982). Models of bounded rationality. Cambridge,
Reply to M. Keith Chen (2008) Rationalization and cog- MA: MIT Press.
nitive dissonance: Do choices affect or reflect preferences? Simon, H. A. (1987). Rationality in psychology and economics.
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 421–423. In R. M. Hogarth & M. W. Reder (Eds.), Rational choice:
Savani, K., Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. L. (2008). Let your The contrast between economics and psychology (pp. 25–40).
preference be your guide? Preferences and choices are more Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
tightly linked for North Americans than for Indians. Journal Simon, L., Greenberg, J., & Brehm, J. (1995). Trivialization: The
of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 861–876. forgotten mode of dissonance reduction. Journal of Personal-
Savani, K., Markus, H. R., Naidu, N. V. R., Kumar, S., & ity and Social Psychology, 68, 247–260.
Berlia, N. (2010). What counts as a choice? U.S. Americans Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York:
are more likely than Indians to construe actions as choices. Macmillan.
Psychological Science, 21, 391–398. Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of
Scher, S., & Cooper, J. (1989). The motivational basis of disso- Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570.
nance: The singular role of behavioral consequences. Journal Snibbe, A., & Markus, H. R. (2005). You can’t always get what
of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 899–906. you want: Educational attainment, agency, and choice.
Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning, and motivation. Educa- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 703–720.
tional Psychologist, 26, 299–323. Staw, B. M. (1976). Knee-deep in the big muddy: A study of
Schiefele, U. (2001). The role of interest in motivation and escalating commitment to a chosen course of action. Organi-
learning. In J. Collis & S. Messick (Eds.), Intelligence and zational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 27–44.
personality: Bridging the gap in theory and measurement Stephens, N., Markus, H. R., & Townsend, S. S. M. (2007).
(pp. 163–194). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Choice as an act of meaning: The case of social class. Journal
Schraw, G., Flowerday, T., & Lehman, S. (2001). Promoting of Personality and Social Psychology, 93. 814–830.
situational interest in the classroom. Educational Psychology Swann, W., & Pittman, T. (1977). Initiating play activity of
Review, 13, 211–224. children: The moderating influence of verbal cues on intrin-
Schraw, G., Flowerday, T., & Reisetter, M. F. (1998). The role of sic motivation. Child Development, 48, 1128–1132.
choice in reader engagement. Journal of Educational Psychol- Tafarodi, R. W., Mehranvar, S., Panton, R. L., & Milne, A. B.
ogy, 90, 705–714. (2002). Putting oneself in the task: Choice, personalization,
Schwartz, B. (2000). Self-determination: The tyranny of and confidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28,
freedom. American Psychologist, 55, 79–88. 648–658.
Schwartz, B. (2004, April). The tyranny of choice. Scientific Tafarodi, R. W., Milne, A. B., & Smith, A. J. (1999). The con-
American, 71–75. fidence of choice: Evidence for an augmentation effect on
Schwartz, B., Ward, A. H., Monterosso, J., Lyubomirsky, S., self-perceived performance. Personality and Social Psychology
White, K., & Lehman, D. (2002). Maximizing versus satis- Bulletin, 25, 1405–1416.
ficing: Happiness is a matter of choice. Journal of Personality Taylor, S. (1989). Positive illusions: Creative self-deception and the
and Social Psychology, 83, 1178–1197. healthy mind. New York: Basic Books.
Scott, J. (2000). Rational choice theory. In G. Browning, Thorndike, E. (1913). Educational psychology: The psychology of
A. Haleli, & F. Webster (Eds.), Understanding contemporary learning. New York: Teachers College Press.
society: Theories of the present (pp. 126–138). London: Sage. Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men.
Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depression, develop- New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
ment, and death. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman. Tsai, Y., Kunter, M., Ludtke, O., Trautwein, U., & Ryan, R. M.
Shafir, E., Simonson, I., & Tversky, A. (1993). Reason-based (2008). What makes lessons interesting? The role of situa-
choice. Cognition, 49, 11–36. tional and individual factors in three school subjects. Journal
Shafir, E., & Tversky, A. 1992. Thinking through uncertainty: of Educational Psychology, 100, 460–472.
Nonconsequential reasoning and choice. Cognitive Psychol- Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncer-
ogy, 24, 449–474. tainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185, 1124–1131.

 the m otivational complexit y of ch oos i n g


Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (1989). Test anxiety in elementary and sec-
decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, ondary school students. Educational Psychologist, 24, 159–183.
453–458. Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (1992). The development of achieve-
Ullmann-Margalit, E., & Morgenbesser, S. (1997). Picking and ment task values: A theoretical analysis. Developmental
choosing. Social Research, 44, 757–785. Review, 12, 265–310.
Uranowitz, S. W. (1975). Helping and self-attributions: A field Wigfield, A. L., & Eccles. J. S. (2001). Development of achieve-
experiment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, ment motivation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
852–854. Williams, S. (1998). An organizational model of choice: A theo-
Van Eerne, W., & Thierry, H. (1996). Vroom’s expectancy retical analysis differentiating choice, personal control, and
models and work-related criteria: A meta-analysis. Journal of self-determination. Genetic, Social and General Psychology
Applied Psychology, 81, 575–586. Monographs, 124, 465–492.
Vohs, K. D., Baumeister, R. F., Schmeichel, B. J., Twenge, J. M., Wortman, C. B., & Brehm, J. W. (1975). Response to uncontrol-
Nelson, N. M., & Tice, D. M. (2008). Making choices lable outcomes: An integration of reactance theory and the
impairs subsequent self-control: A limited resource account learned helplessness model. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances
of decision making, self-regulation, and active initiative. in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 8, pp. 277–336). New
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 883–898. York: Academic Press.
Vohs, K., Finkenauer, C., & Baumeister, R. F. (2011). The sum Yates, B. T., & Mischel, W. (1979). Young children’s preferred
of friends’ and lovers’ self-control scores predicts relation- attentional strategies for delaying gratification. Journal of
ship quality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2, Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 286–300.
138–145. Zahorik, J. A. (1996). Elementary and secondary teachers’
Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley reports of how they make learning interesting. Elementary
Wang, J., Novemsky, N., Dhar, R. and Baumeister, R. (in press). School Journal, 96, 551–564.
Tradeoffs and depletion in choice. Journal of Marketing Zuckerman, M., Porac, J., Lathin, D., Smith, R., & Deci,
Research. E., (1978). On the importance of self-determination for
Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation: Metaphors, theories, and intrinsically-motivated behavior. Personality and Social
research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Psychology Bulletin, 4, 443–446.

pata ll 
CHAPTER

On Gains and Losses, Means and


16 Ends: Goal Orientation and Goal
Focus Across Adulthood
Alexandra M. Freund, Marie Hennecke, and Maida Mustafić

Abstract
Personal goals guide behavior toward a desired outcome, motivate behavior over time and across
situations, provide direction and meaning, and contribute to the acquisition of skills and subjective
well-being. The adaptiveness of goals, however, might vary with dimensions such as their orientation
toward the achievement of gains, maintenance of functioning, or the avoidance of losses. We argue that
goal orientation is most adaptive when it corresponds to the availability of resources and the ubiquity of
losses. In line with this argument, younger adults show a predominant orientation toward the promotion
of gains, whereas goal orientation shifts toward maintenance and avoidance of loss across adulthood.
This shift in goal orientation seems adaptive both regarding subjective well-being as well as engagement
in goal pursuit. A second goal dimension that has been largely overlooked in the literature is the
cognitive representation of goal pursuit primarily in terms of its means (i.e., process focus) or its ends
(i.e., outcome focus). This chapter investigates the antecedents and consequences of goal focus.
In particular, it highlights the importance of factors related to chronological age (i.e., the availability
of resources, future time perspective, goal orientation, motivational phase) for the preference for
and adaptiveness of an outcome or process focus. Finally, we posit that a process focus leads
to more adaptive behavioral and affective reactions when people encounter failure during
goal pursuit.
Key Words: adult development, goal orientation, goal focus, means, ends, resources, time perspective,
failure

Introduction be evident. First, the orientation of goals is likely to


Imagine a young woman in her early 20s and her shift from gains in young adulthood to maintenance
grandmother, an older woman in her early 70s. Now in middle adulthood and the prevention of losses in
think about the personal goals they might pursue. older age (e.g., Freund & Ebner, 2005). For example,
Most likely, the younger woman will pursue goals a young woman might aim at improving her fitness
related to finding a life partner, finishing her educa- level, whereas her grandmother might be more likely
tion, and establishing a professional career. The goals to try to maintain her physical fitness in the face of
of the older woman are more likely to center around age-related losses in health. Second, younger adults
the domains of health, cognitive functioning, inde- might focus more on the outcome of goal pursuit,
pendence, and the well-being of her loved ones (Fre- whereas older adults might focus more on the process
und & Riediger, 2006). Beyond the differences in (Freund, Hennecke, & Riediger, 2010). For exam-
content, however, two other age-related differences ple, the young woman might focus on the desired
in the goals of a younger and an older adult might outcome of exercising regularly such as her body


shape and her overall fitness. In contrast, her grand- Such goal dimensions influence the adaptiveness
mother might think primarily about how she can of goals. Various goal dimensions have been distin-
exercise regularly in a manner that even makes her guished, such as approach—avoidance (e.g., Elliott
feel good while she is exercising. In this chapter we & Friedman, 2007), promotion—prevention (e.g.,
aim at integrating these two dimensions of personal Higgins, 1997), intrinsic—extrinsic (e.g., Deci,
goals and discuss their change across adulthood. Koestner, & Ryan, 1999), and mastery—perfor-
First, however, we want to highlight the importance mance (e.g., Dweck & Leggett, 1988). This chapter
of personal goals throughout the life span. centers around two goal dimensions that we believe
to change systematically across adulthood: (1) Goal
The Importance of Goals for Adult orientation: the orientation of personal goals toward
Development gains, maintenance, or the prevention of losses
Laypeople as well as motivation researchers seem (e.g., Freund & Ebner, 2005), and (2) Goal focus:
to agree that setting and pursuing goals has positive whether a person focuses on the outcome of goal pur-
consequences. Goals give life meaning, direction, suit (short-term and long-term consequences) or on
and contribute to happiness and subjective well- the process of goal pursuit (means of goal attainment)
being (e.g., Emmons, 1996; Klinger, 1977; Little, (e.g., Freund et al., 2010; Sansone & Thoman, 2005;
1989). Goals have been defined as cognitive repre- Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997).
sentations of personally desired (or dreaded) states The importance of personal goals for adult devel-
to be approached (or avoided) through action, such opment has been acknowledged by different action-
as becoming a nurse (or not becoming like one’s theoretical approaches (e.g., Brandtstädter & Renner,
parents). More specifically, they encompass means 1990; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Heckhausen &
of goal pursuit and desired outcomes of it (e.g., Schulz, 1995). In particular, the model of selection,
Kruglanski, 1996). The activation of goals affects the optimization, and compensation (SOC model, Bal-
encoding, storage, and retrieval of information and tes & Baltes, 1990) has stressed the importance of
guides attention as well as behavior (e.g., Wyer & setting, pursuing, and maintaining personal goals
Srull, 1986). As goals are comprised of means and for successful development.
ends, goals might channel and organize information
in terms of means and ends (e.g., Woike, Lavezzary, Successful Development Through Personal
& Barsky, 2001). Each time a goal is activated, the Goals
associated means and ends (as well as their emo- One of the central propositions of lifespan psy-
tional correlates such as enjoyment or fear) are also chology is the multidirectionality of development.
activated. Consequently, the activation of goals That is, development comprises not only trajecto-
enhances the likelihood of engaging in goal-relevant ries of growth but also trajectories of decline (Baltes,
behaviors (i.e., means), which can occur even auto- 1987; Labouvie-Vief, 1981). Successful development
matically (e.g., Bargh & Ferguson, 2000; Bargh has often been defined as the maximization of gains
& Gollwitzer, 1994). Goals, then, direct attention and the simultaneous minimization of losses (see
and information processing and motivate behav- Freund & Riediger, 2003, for a review of definitions
ior. Thereby, goals organize behavior over time and of successful development). According to the SOC
across situations, and provide a sense of direction model (Baltes & Baltes, 1990), an optimal ratio of
and purpose in life (Freund, 2007). Moreover, gains to losses can be achieved by the orchestrated
research suggests that goal pursuit enhances perfor- use of three processes of developmental regulation,
mance (e.g., Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Emmons, namely selection, optimization, and compensa-
1989, 1996; Freund, 2007). Therefore, it is not sur- tion. As elaborated in more detail elsewhere (e.g.,
prising that the goal concept seems particularly well Freund, 2006; Freund & Baltes, 2000; Freund, Li,
suited for understanding how people develop suc- & Baltes, 1999), the action-theoretical specification
cessfully over time. of the SOC model posits that developing and com-
However, as Ryan and colleagues put it: Not all mitting to a hierarchy of personal goals (i.e., elective
goals are created equal (Ryan, Sheldon, Kasser, & selection) and engaging in goal-directed actions and
Deci, 1996). Goals differ in their content, concrete- means (i.e., optimization) are essential for achieving
ness, difficulty, time frame, and their orientation higher levels of functioning (i.e., maximizing gains).
toward gains and losses (e.g., Austin & Vancou- To maintain a given level of functioning in the face
ver, 1996; Freund & Ebner, 2005; Little, 1989; of inevitable losses in resources people encounter
Locke & Latham, 2002; Wiese & Freund, 2005). throughout their lives, people need to compensate

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


for their losses (e.g., by substituting goal-relevant life domains contributs to higher facilitation among
means that are no longer available). When the costs goals, which, in turn, leads to stronger goal engage-
for optimization or compensation outweigh the ment and achievement (Riediger & Freund, 2006).
expected gains, according to the SOC model it is Age-related increases in motivational selectivity,
more adaptive to reconstruct one’s goal hierarchy by then, are one way of managing the increasing limi-
focusing on the most important goals, developing tation of resources in adulthood. Another way of
new goals, or adapting goal standards (i.e., loss-based dealing with conflicts due to goals competing for
selection). Thus, the SOC model conceptualizes the same limited resources is prioritizing. Wiese and
processes promoting gains (elective selection, opti- Freund (2001) showed that young adults who expe-
mization) but also processes to counteract losses rience conflicts between work- and family-related
(compensation, loss-based selection). goals report fewer strains and higher subjective well-
Empirical evidence supports the adaptiveness being when they prioritize one goal (and temporally
of self-reported selection, optimization, and com- postpone the other). Taken together, this research
pensation throughout adolescence (Gestsdottir & supports the importance of selection as a key pro-
Lerner, 2007), adulthood, and into very old age (e.g., cess for successfully managing multiple goals.
Freund & Baltes, 1998; 2002; Wiese, Freund, &
Baltes, 2000; 2001; Ziegelmann & Lippke, 2007). Optimization and Compensation: A Tale
The use of SOC strategies seems to be particularly of the Shifting Goal Orientation Across
helpful for persons with fewer resources (Jopp & Adulthood
Smith, 2006; Lang, Rieckmann, & Baltes, 2002; As mentioned earlier, one of the central tenets
Young, Baltes, & Pratt, 2007). of lifespan developmental psychology holds that
development encompasses both gains and losses
Goal Selection: Managing Multiple Goals throughout the life span. Examples of ubiquitous
A series of studies by Riediger and colleagues losses in later adulthood are health-related and cog-
(Riediger & Freund, 2004, 2006, 2008; Riediger, nitive decline or the loss of social partners and social
Freund, & Baltes, 2005) demonstrated the role of status through retirement (Baltes & Smith, 2003).
the selection of goals for successful goal pursuit. More In contrast, affective well-being (e.g., Röcke, Li, &
specifically, results by Riediger and colleagues stress Smith, 2009), motivational competence (e.g., Rie-
the importance of considering the interrelations of diger & Freund, 2008), and self-regulation (Hen-
personal goals. Conflict between goals might occur necke & Freund, 2010) appear to increase across
because resources are insufficient to support both adulthood and into old age. The ratio of gains
goals at the same time through incompatible strate- to losses, however, changes across the life span,
gies. For instance, wanting to enjoy food and trying encompassing decreasing gains and increasing losses
to lose weight imply incompatible eating behaviors, throughout adulthood and into old age (e.g., Baltes,
leading to goal conflict. Goals can facilitate each 1997; Baltes, Lindenberger, & Staudinger, 1998;
other by sharing the same strategies. For example, Heckhausen, Dixon, & Baltes, 1989). Address-
the two goals to lose weight and to lead a healthy ing this changing ratio of gains to losses, the SOC
lifestyle are both served by the same strategy of model holds that goals directed at the optimiza-
working out regularly. Goal conflict and facilita- tion of gains might be more important at younger
tion are two largely independent goal dimensions ages, whereas goals directed at the maintenance and
and show differential associations with affective avoidance of losses might gain in importance with
experience and goal-relevant behavior. Goal conflict increasing age.
seems to impair affective well-being; facilitation is Arguing from an evolutionary standpoint as well
associated with goal pursuit in everyday life and as from a developmental perspective, it is advanta-
subsequent goal attainment (Riediger et al., 2005). geous to possess as many resources as possible (see
Interestingly, older adults appear to gain in motiva- Freund & Riediger, 2001). Resources are essential
tional competence regarding the selection of goals. for reproductive success and survival. They signal
They report more goal facilitation and less conflict success, relative social standing, and good genetic
among their goals than younger adults (Riediger material to potential mates. They enhance attrac-
et al., 2005). Importantly, this result is not simply tiveness and successful reproduction and provide
due to a reduction in the number of goals but to for the upbringing of offspring (Buss, 1999). Gain-
focusing on personally important, superordinate ing resources appears to be a primary motivation in
goals. Focusing one’s goals on central and similar young adulthood, a phase in life when most people

 on gains and losses, means and end s


have not yet had opportunities to accumulate many and avoidance of loss, older adults’ subjective
resources that are advantageous for their reproduc- well-being was positively related to a maintenance
tive success. Moreover, social expectations and devel- orientation. Using behavioral indicators of goal
opmental tasks for young adults are geared toward pursuit, Freund (2006) showed that younger adults
gains (e.g., gaining education or professional skills, pursue a given goal more persistently when it is ori-
founding a family, building a home, establishing a ented toward achieving gains (optimization goal),
career). Young adults have large potentials for func- whereas older adults are more persistent when pur-
tional gains and still need to realize these potentials. suing the goal to counteract losses (compensation
As Raynor (1982) puts it, younger adults are still in goal). In addition, when confronted with a resource
the process of “becoming.” In other words, before loss, compensatory activities are related to positive
younger adults can start protecting and conserving affect in older adults (Duke, Leventhal, Brownlee,
resources, they need to acquire skills and resources & Leventhal, 2002).
and build upon their status. In contrast, with increas- In sum, then, goal orientation toward gains and
ing age, one is increasingly likely to have reached losses appears to change with the shifting ratio of
one’s personal asymptote of performance in many gains to losses across adulthood. Moreover, this shift
areas of life, making the achievement of new gains in goal orientation seems adaptive both regarding
less and less likely. Moreover, throughout their lives subjective well-being as well as actual goal pursuit.
older adults have accumulated resources, including
skills, material belongings, as well as social relations Goal Focus: Process or Outcome
that need to be protected against losses. Given the The previous sections focused on goal selection
ubiquity of losses in older adulthood and the cor- and the shift in goal orientation toward gains and
responding social expectations (Heckhausen et al., losses across adulthood. In the following, we want
1989), older adults are likely to be chronically aware to address how the cognitive representation of goal
of threatening losses. pursuit primarily in terms of its means (process
In late adulthood, then, preserving resources focus) or its outcome (outcome focus) might affect
and counteracting losses may become the primary goal-relevant behavior as well as affect, and how it
motivation outweighing tendencies to accumulate might change with age.
new resources (Freund & Ebner, 2005; Staudinger, Let us open this section with an example of pro-
Marsiske, & Baltes, 1995). cess and outcome focus. Two people pursuing the
Consistent with this hypothesis, J. Heckhausen goal of completing a 20-km hike in the Alps within
(1999) found that younger adults reported more 5 hours may focus on very different aspects of this
goals in domains associated with striving for gains goal: One of them might focus primarily on the
and fewer goals in domains reflecting the avoidance consequences of successfully reaching the destina-
of losses than middle-aged or old adults. Similarly, tion within the allotted time, while the other might
Ebner, Freund, and Baltes (2006) showed that, focus more on pacing herself by monitoring her
compared to older adults, younger adults rated their pulse rate and breathing. What factors determine
personal goals as having a stronger focus on gains. whether a person focuses more on the outcome
Conversely, older adults reported a higher focus on or the process when pursuing goals? Are there dif-
maintenance and prevention of loss in their personal ferences in adaptiveness of a stronger focus on the
goals than younger adults. Moreover, in two further outcome or the process of goal pursuit? We posit
studies using a forced-choice paradigm for tasks that factors related to chronological age, namely the
pertaining to physical fitness and cognitive func- availability of (physical and cognitive) resources,
tioning, younger adults were more likely to adopt future time perspective, and a goal orientation
goals focusing on achieving new gains compared toward achieving gains or maintenance of function-
to older adults who preferred goals focusing on the ing contributes to a preference for and adaptive-
maintenance of their level of functioning. Attesting ness of either an outcome or a process focus during
to the role of resources for goal orientation, Ebner goal pursuit. In addition, taking a closer look at the
et al. (2006) showed that younger adults shifted to dynamics of goal setting and pursuit, we posit that
a preference for maintenance goals when resources the motivational phase and the closeness to a dead-
were perceived as being limited. line determine whether people focus on the process
The shift in goal orientation across adulthood or the outcome of goal pursuit. Finally, we discuss
seems adaptive. Whereas younger adults seem to the role of goal focus when goal pursuit is hampered
suffer from a goal orientation toward maintenance by setbacks or failure.

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


The concept of outcome and process focus is a focus on the consequences of attaining a certain
related—but not identical—to the concepts of outcome. The opposite is not true, however, as the
extrinsic and intrinsic motivation as well as per- concept of goal focus is mute regarding the underly-
formance and mastery orientation. In accordance ing reasons for engaging in goal pursuit. For instance,
with Sansone and Thoman (2005), we define out- a person might focus on the outcome of goal pur-
come focus as the motivation to engage in an activity suit (e.g., a beautiful painting) for a goal that was
because it is a means to a certain end. We define set autonomously and will bear no further conse-
process focus as the cognitive salience of aspects of quences such as praise or tangible rewards. Extrinsic
the goal that are related to the means, whereas San- motivation implies a concern about the consequences
sone and Thoman define it as the (expected) experi- of attaining an outcome (e.g., receiving a monetary
ence of interest in an activity. It is likely that people reward from parents for achieving a good grade), not
only persist in a certain activity for longer periods of about the outcome itself. Regarding process focus,
time, however, if they experience it as being some- a person might focus on the process of goal pursuit
how rewarding, be it due to their interest in it, their (e.g., painting) because she is positively reinforced
positive affect, or its instrumentality for achieving for doing so (e.g., through teachers’ praise for her
a desired outcome. Focusing on the outcome or the talent and perseverance). Process focus, then, is not
process of goal pursuit is like beaming a flashlight necessarily associated with intrinsic motivation.
on either the means or the end of goal pursuit, thus
highlighting aspects of goal pursuit either related to linking outcome and process focus
the process (e.g., Do I have the means necessary to to performance and mastery
achieve this goal?) or the outcome (e.g., When will goal orientation
I achieve the goal?). Another goal dimension related to goal focus is
performance and mastery goal orientation. Dweck
Differentiating Goal Focus From Related (e.g., Dweck & Leggett, 1988) defines performance
Constructs goal orientation as a focus on how well one is doing
linking outcome and process focus to (particularly as compared to others), whereas mas-
extrinsic and intrinsic motivation tery goal orientation represents a focus on learning
Extrinsic motivation is characterized by a focus and mastering a skill. Dweck traces these two types
on the consequences of goal achievement (e.g., of goal orientation back to beliefs about skills as
external rewards for achieving a certain goal), fixed (i.e., an entity) or malleable (i.e., incremental),
whereas intrinsic motivation is typically defined as respectively. In the first case (entity theory), perfor-
a focus on the task at hand (e.g., enjoyment of or mance is seen as an indicator of the underlying abil-
interest in the goal-relevant activity). Compared to ity and provides feedback about an unchanging trait.
extrinsic motivation, intrinsic motivation is asso- In the latter case (incremental theory), feedback is
ciated with voluntary involvement, more interest, a means of improving one’s skill level. A number
and higher persistence in a task (e.g., Deci, Koest- of studies in educational settings have shown that
ner, & Ryan, 1999; Krapp, 2005; Lepper, 1981). setting mastery goals promotes interest in and
Intrinsic motivation implies that a person focuses enjoyment of goal pursuit, but that performance
on the satisfaction derived from the activity rather goals are typically associated with a higher level of
than on the external consequences of goal achieve- performance (e.g., Harackiewicz, Barron, Trauer,
ment. For instance, when one’s goal is to paint a Carter, & Elliot, 2000; for a review, see Dweck
picture, either the amount of money the picture & Molden, 2005). In the area of organizational
will bring in at the next exhibition (i.e., extrinsic behavior, however, mastery goals (in this context
motivation) or the enjoyment of and interest in often labeled “learning” goals) have been shown to
the activity of painting (i.e., intrinsic motivation) be positively linked to the successful acquisition of
could be in the foreground. Engaging in goal pur- new skills, feedback seeking, and performance (e.g.,
suit for tangible, external rewards has been shown to VandeWalle, 2001; VandeWalle, Brown, Cron, &
undermine intrinsic motivation (Deci, Koestner, & Slocum, 1999).
Ryan, 1999). Seijts and Latham (2005) posit that the adap-
At first glance, the definition of intrinsic and tiveness of goal focus depends on the goal at hand.
extrinsic motivation greatly resembles process and If the means and strategies of goal pursuit are not
outcome focus. Intrinsic motivation entails a focus (yet) known or mastered, learning goals should
on the process, whereas extrinsic motivation entails enhance performance because attention is focused

 on gains and losses, means and end s


on the means of goal pursuit, whereas focusing on (e.g., achieving a certain SAT score). Another feature
performance might actually distract and hinder distinguishing outcome and process focus is the
successful goal pursuit. In a similar vein, and using clarity of standards of comparison between actual
the terminology of process and outcome focus, and desired states. Outcome focus is more likely
Zimmerman and Kitsantas (1997, 1999) point out than process focus to provide a clear standard of
that, when learning to master a new task, people comparison because outcomes typically entail cri-
are more likely to adopt a process focus, defined teria regarding when they are reached (e.g., arriving
by these authors as a focus on the acquisition of at a destination within 5 hours). By comparison,
(strategic) skills (i.e., mastering the various ele- it is much more difficult to define the standards of
ments and steps of a complex skill such as writing comparison for the means of goal pursuit without
or dart throwing) or, in other words, on the means referring to the outcome (e.g., enjoying a hike is
for achieving a given outcome. Outcome focus, in less clearly defined than reaching the destination in
contrast, presupposes mastery of the different ele- a given amount of time). Finally, researchers agree
ments of which a complex skill is comprised and that higher-order, abstract goal representations (i.e.,
denotes a focus on the actual outcome (i.e., perfor- outcome focus) provide direction and meaning in
mance level). In line with Seijts and Latham (2005), life, whereas lower-order, concrete goal represen-
Zimmerman and Kitsantas found that a focus on tations (i.e., process focus) provide guidelines for
the acquisition of skills and means (i.e., adopting a action (e.g., Emmons, 1996; Klinger, 1977; Little,
process focus) is beneficial when learning a new skill, 1989). As Little (1989) pointed out, however, peo-
whereas adopting an outcome focus enhances perfor- ple do not want to know why they are doing some-
mance when the means need to be implemented as thing but also what they should be doing. It seems,
an integrated whole in the service of goal attainment then, that neither of the two is in and of itself more
(Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997, 1999). This result adaptive. Instead, as discussed in the next section,
can be taken as first evidence for the hypothesis that the effects of goal focus are hypothesized to depend
goal focus and its adaptiveness depend on skill level. on factors related to chronological age.
Before we elaborate on the role of age for goal
focus, let us summarize the main differences between Age and Goal Focus
process and outcome focus. As for the development of skills during adult-
hood, one could argue that skill level is associated
main differences between process and with age. In many domains of life, young adults
outcome focus are still in the process of acquiring the means and
Table 16.1 summarizes the main differences skills relevant for goal pursuit, such as skills needed
between process and outcome goal focus, which will in the professional/work domain or in the area
be elaborated below. of establishing a long-term partnership and fam-
First, let us point out that the differences high- ily. This might force young adults to focus more
lighted in Table 16.1 are relative, not absolute. closely on the acquisition of skills or the process of
Typically, however, actions and the means of goal goal pursuit (see Zimmermann & Kitsantas, 1997,
pursuit are more concrete than outcomes (Carver 1999). Middle-aged and older adults are more
& Scheier, 1998). Similarly, actions take place in likely to have acquired most of the skills necessary
specific situational contexts (e.g., studying for the to pursue their goals in both the work as well as
SAT), whereas outcomes are more decontextualized the social domain and, thus, could be seen as being

Table 16.1. Differences Between Process and Outcome Goal Focus


Process Goal Focus Outcome Goal Focus

action/means end state

subordinate goals (concrete) superordinate goals (abstract)

contextualized decontextualized

provides vague or no standard of comparison provides clear standard

provides guidelines for action provides direction, meaning

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


more likely to focus on the outcome of goal pursuit. should lead to a more concrete representation of the
Moreover, as Kanfer and Ackerman (2004) point means (“do” goals, according to Carver & Scheier,
out, skills can also be defined in terms of the bal- 1998). Taken together, preference for a certain goal
ance between investment of resources and payoff. In focus might vary by variables such as time perspec-
the context of work-related motivational develop- tive (Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999) and
ment during adulthood, they argue that the payoff availability of resources (e.g., Freund & Ebner,
for resource investment decreases with age, lead- 2005). Both time perspective and available resources
ing younger adults to be more focused on resource have been shown to be negatively related to chron-
investment and older adults on the outcome. Next, ological age (e.g., Baltes & Smith, 2003; Lang &
we will argue, however, that other factors related Carstensen, 2002). Therefore, one could expect an
to chronological age—the availability of resources, increase in process focus and a decrease in outcome
future time perspective, goal orientation toward focus during adulthood (see Fig. 16.1).
gains or maintenance/avoidance of loss—suggest As pointed out earlier, the developmental tasks of
that, overall, the primary goal focus is expected to young adults entail the achievement of growth goals,
shift from the outcome to the process of goal pur- which have an inherent outcome-oriented aspect due
suit across adulthood. to the tangible nature of task achievement conse-
Some goals might lend themselves more to a pro- quences (viz., a diploma, a job, a mate, a child). Thus,
cess focus than others. For instance, goals related young adults may develop a more outcome-oriented
to an enduring characteristic (e.g., to be a friendly approach to task achievement, and outcomes are
person) or maintaining some state (e.g., to stay likely to become highly salient during young adult-
healthy) require working constantly on the goal and hood. Later on, however, adults’—especially older
might therefore be more suitable for a process focus adults’—goal orientation shifts toward maintaining
than goals specifying an endpoint (e.g., to pass an one’s level of functioning and avoiding losses (Ebner
exam). Therefore, maintenance goals may be more et al., 2006; Freund, 2006). Orientation toward
likely to be associated with a process focus, whereas maintenance/avoidance of losses implies a constant
goals involving the achievement of new outcomes monitoring of one’s actual performance vis-à-vis
(i.e., growth) should be more likely to invoke an a progressively declining level of functioning. Thus,
outcome focus. As has been shown by Ebner et al. orientation toward maintenance and loss avoidance
(2006), availability of resources is one of the fac- has an inherent process-oriented aspect. Accordingly,
tors determining whether growth or maintenance older adults may develop a more process-oriented
goals are adopted. When resources are perceived approach to goal achievement. In addition, achiev-
as being limited, people might feel that achieving ing new outcomes typically takes time. However,
new outcomes (growth) is less likely and desirable when one’s future becomes more and more limited,
than focusing on the task at hand, namely, the growth goals with their inherently more distant out-
process of goal pursuit. Similarly, as suggested by comes might be viewed as less applicable to one’s
construal level theory (Trope & Liberman, 2003), own life than maintenance goals with their inher-
goals that are temporally distant are more likely to ently more immediate nature (as necessitated by
be represented in an abstract way and in terms of constant monitoring). Thus, given that future time
ends, whereas shorter temporal distance of goals perspective decreases with age (Lang & Carstensen,

Orientation of
goal focus

Process focus

Outcome focus

Adulthood
Young Middle-Aged Older

Fig. 16.1. Hypothesized relation of the development of goal focus across adulthood.

 on gains and losses, means and end s


2002), one might expect older adults to be more effort” (p. 451). This proposition is consistent with
process focused. the view that older adults’ goal focus shifts from
being primarily concerned with achieving a specific
resources and goal focus outcome (i.e., performance level) and more with
The importance of achieving gains and accu- the process of goal achievement (i.e., investment of
mulating new resources in young adulthood (see effort).
earlier) is likely to result in a focus on achieving cer-
tain outcomes. Middle-aged adults might hold an time perspective and goal focus
equally strong process and outcome focus because, Attempting to achieve certain outcomes requires
on the one hand, they are starting to experience a adopting a future time perspective. Zimbardo and
shift in resources toward decline and are, in many Boyd (1999) even view outcome focus and the abil-
areas, at their peak in performance, making achieve- ity to postpone immediate gratification in order to
ment of new outcomes less likely. This should lead attain a goal at some later point in time as part of
to a stronger focus on the process of goal pursuit. their concept of future time perspective. In contrast,
At the same time, middle-aged adults typically present orientation is characterized by a more hedo-
still experience their resources such as (life-) time nic approach to life with a focus on more immediate
and vigor as plentiful, and they might therefore gratification and less concern for consequences that
still aspire to reach certain outcomes because gains lie in the farther future. Therefore, one could argue
are still possible (Baltes et al., 1998; Freund & that an extended future time perspective is more
Ebner, 2005; Staudinger et al., 1995). This pattern likely to be associated with outcome focus, whereas
clearly changes in old age, when resources decline shorter future time expansion might be associated
(Baltes & Smith, 2003) and achieving new out- with a focus on the process of goal pursuit that is
comes becomes less likely and goal orientation shifts taking place in the present. Investing into the future
toward maintenance and loss avoidance. As mainte- only makes sense when there is a future in which to
nance goals lend themselves more to process focus reap the fruits of one’s efforts. Consistent with this
than do growth goals, older adults should also be view, in their studies testing socioemotional selec-
more likely than younger or middle-aged adults to tivity theory (SST), Carstensen and her colleagues
adopt a process focus. (e.g., Carstensen, Isaacowitz, & Charles, 1999)
This hypothesis is also consistent with Kan- consistently show that a limited future time per-
fer’s resource model (e.g., Kanfer, 1987; Kanfer & spective is related to focusing on emotionally mean-
Ackerman, 2004), which proposes that motivation ingful social goals. In contrast, a longer future time
(defined here as effort) depends on the perceived perspective is associated with information seeking,
effort-performance function (i.e., the expected level which can be seen as an investment in the future.
of performance upon investing a certain amount of As Fung and Carstensen (2004) put it, “When the
effort into a task at hand), the performance-utility future is perceived as open-ended, future-oriented
function (i.e., the consequences of attaining a cer- goals weigh most heavily and individuals pursue
tain level of performance), and the effort-utility goals that optimize long-range outcomes” (p. 68),
function (i.e., the payoff for investing effort into a and “when time is perceived as limited, emotionally
task at hand). When resources decrease (e.g., as does meaningful goals ( . . . ) are pursued because such
fluid intelligence during adulthood), the expected goals have more immediate payoffs” (p. 68).
payoff for investing effort declines, so older adults In her studies, Carstensen shows that, different
are expected to invest less effort into tasks involving to younger adults, older people are more likely to
resources on the decline. When resources are plenti- restrict their social contacts to close social partners
ful or even increasing (e.g., crystallized intelligence and emotionally meaningful social interactions. It
during adulthood), the expected payoff for investing is not old age per se, SST argues, but the shorter
effort increases, so effort will be invested into tasks future time perspective of older people that is
involving resources that are increasing. Applied to responsible for this shift in social goals. In fact, Lang
the work domain, Kanfer and Ackerman (2004) and Carstensen (2002) show that age is negatively
propose that “among older workers, work motiva- related to future time perspective. Moreover, when
tion will be less determined by level of performance experimentally restricting younger adults’ time
achievement and, rather, more determined by judg- perspective, they orient themselves more toward
ments of how much effort is required for requisite meaningful interactions with close social partners
performance . . . and the utility of allocating that rather than investing into the future by selecting

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


partners that might provide useful information (for next, we posit that a change goal orientation might
a summary, see Carstensen, et al., 1999). Research be associated with a stronger outcome focus and sta-
on SST suggests that an extended future time per- bility goal orientation might be related to a stronger
spective is likely to be associated with a focus on process focus.
the outcomes of goal pursuit, whereas a limited One of the main reasons why change and stabil-
time perspective brings about a focus on the pres- ity goal orientation might contribute to goal focus
ent and, therefore, a more immediate payoff. With is that they imply a different discrepancy between
a limited future time perspective, people should be the actual and the desired state. The very definition
more concerned with the more immediate process of a change goal is that it entails a significant dis-
of goal pursuit rather than the more distant out- crepancy between the actual and the desired state.
come thereof. In contrast, there is no discrepancy between the
actual and the desired state in a stability goal—the
Change Versus Stability Orientation and desired state is to maintain this lack of a discrep-
Goal Focus ancy. Feedback-loop models of goals (Carver &
In this section, we take a different perspective Scheier, 1998; Miller, Galanter, & Pribram, 1960)
on gain and maintenance/avoidance of loss goal suggest that, as long as a discrepancy reduction
orientation by shifting the emphasis of this distinc- between the actual and desired state is intended and
tion away from gains and losses toward stability and the outcome is not reached, a “tension state” toward
change. From a developmental viewpoint, striving the outcome exists; that is, the cognitive accessibil-
for the achievement of new gains implies an orienta- ity of outcome-related information might be higher
tion toward change (e.g., “I want to become better in before than after goal fulfillment (see Förster, Liber-
Spanish”), whereas striving for maintenance/avoid- man, & Friedman, 2007). In a change goal orienta-
ance of loss implies an orientation toward stability tion, a person attempts at reducing the discrepancy
(e.g.., “I want to maintain my Spanish at the current to the outcome (“negative feedback loop,” Miller,
level and not get worse”). Different to the distinc- Galanter, & Pribram, 1960). This should render the
tion of gain versus maintenance/loss-avoidance ori- outcome cognitively more accessible than a stability
entation, change as well as stability goal orientation goal orientation, where the desired outcome state
might be approach- as well as avoidance-motivated. has already been achieved.
In other words, change and stability goals can Another line of argument for the association
be either approach or avoidance oriented (see of change versus stability orientation and process
Table 16.2). When approaching a change goal, peo- versus outcome goal focus stems from the tempo-
ple are oriented toward a future state (e.g., “I want ral value asymmetry assumptions (Caruso, Gilbert,
to become better”), whereas approaching a stability & Wilson, 2008). Accordingly, people value future
goal implies the wish to maintain an actual state events more than equivalent events in the equi-
(e.g., “I want to stay good”). Similarly, avoiding distant past. Future outcomes in change goal ori-
change is directed at an actual state (e.g., “I do not entation should therefore have a higher value than
want to change”), whereas avoiding stability com- outcomes already reached in stability goal orienta-
prises a future state (e.g., “I do not want to become tion. Consequently, change goal orientation should
different”). lead to a stronger focus on the outcome than sta-
Goal orientation toward stability or change is bility goal orientation. Taken together, then, the
theoretically related to goal focus and thereby con- larger discrepancy of the actual and desired state
tributes to the hypothesized age-related differences in a change goal should lead to a stronger outcome
in process and outcome focus. As we will elaborate focus when compared to a stability goal. Conversely,

Table 16.2. Difference Between Change and Stability Goal Orientation with Respect to Approach and Avoidance
Motivation
Goal Orientation

Motivational system change stability

Approach Future state Actual state

Avoidance Actual state Future state

 on gains and losses, means and end s


stability goals should be associated with a process compared to a change goal orientation, means
focus because there is no discrepancy between the should also be more cognitively accessible.
desired and the actual state.
Furthermore, change and stability goal orienta- adaptiveness of goal focus for change
tion might lead to different goal foci due to (1) how and stability goal orientation
resource demanding the pursuit of a goal is, and (2) There might be an adaptive correspondence
the frequency of means usage for change and stabil- between mental representations of either means or
ity goals over time. outcomes and change or stability goal orientation.
As the pursuit of change and stability goals poses
ad (1): resource demands different challenges to goal pursuit, process and
Means might vary in different regards, as making outcome focus might be differentially adaptive. In
one of them more desirable, for example, for being particular, we posit that the challenge of a change
less resource demanding than the other. Investing goal lies in successfully reducing the discrepancy
highly resource-demanding means might be accept- between the actual and desired state within a cer-
able if they help achieving a certain goal quickly and tain time (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1998), which
the investment of the means does not have to be should require more intense and immediate effort
repeated often. This is more likely to be the case in mobilization, whereas the challenge of pursuing a
a change as compared to a stability goal that typi- stability goal lies in maintaining it potentially end-
cally requires investment of resources as long as the lessly, which should demand adaptive adjustment of
goal itself exists (e.g., maintaining a certain diet in means.
order to keep one’s weight stable). Consequently, as Let us first address the challenge of pursuing
means have to be selected more carefully when pur- a change goal, namely to reduce efficiently the dis-
suing a stability goal, the focus should also be on crepancy between the actual and desired state. We
means rather than the outcome of goal pursuit. maintain that an outcome focus might provide
motivational resources helpful when people expe-
ad (2): frequency of means usage rience goal pursuit as effortful and demanding. As
Successful stabilization of achieved outcomes is decision theories propose, outcomes are generally
often achieved by repeating already established goal- evaluated compared to the costs of attaining them,
relevant behavior that helped attaining the now to- that is, the effort invested in the pursuit of a goal
be-maintained state. Maintaining a certain state (e.g., Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). Given the same
typically requires engaging in goal-relevant behav- costs, the higher (i.e., the more abstract) an out-
iors as long as people hold the respective goal. Sta- come is set, the more it is perceived to be worth
bility goals (e.g., “I want to maintain my weight”) investing energy in it. Furthermore, Fujita, Trope,
are typically not achieved at one specific point in Liberman, and Levin-Sagi (2006) demonstrated
time and therefore do not render themselves to one- that focusing on higher-order goals (i.e., outcomes)
shot goal pursuit. Stability goals, then, are more increases people’s motivation and mobilizes efforts
likely to be pursued for longer periods of time than for outcome attainment. A focus on outcomes leads
change goals that typically specify a certain end to a preference for delayed outcomes compared to
point when the goal is achieved (e.g., “I want to lose immediate ones, greater physical endurance, more
5 pounds”). Therefore, as goal pursuit stretches over self-control, and less positive evaluations of tempta-
a longer period of time, people are also more likely tions that undermine self-control. Fujita and Han
to use the means for goal pursuit more often than (2009) showed that changes in the evaluation of
when they pursue change goals that are more likely temptations depend on whether a goal is repre-
to specify certain end points. Frequency here refers sented in more concrete or more abstract terms.
to the absolute number of times means are applied This, in turn, might explain that an outcome focus
(not to the interval between using the means during can foster self-control when facing temptations.
a fixed time period). According to semantic memory Additionally, Manderlink and Harackiewicz (1984)
theories (Collins & Loftus, 1975) or spreading acti- theorize that a focus on outcomes increases intrinsic
vation models (Bower, 1981), the more recently or motivation. Therefore, an outcome focus should be
frequently a concept (such as a goal orientation) has more likely than a process focus to mobilize moti-
been used in the past, the more often it is activated, vational resources for optimal outcome attainment.
and the more cognitively accessible it is. Therefore, Furthermore, the approach toward the desired
if people use means more often in a stability as outcome and the reduction of the actual-desired

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


state discrepancy is evaluated and experienced as outcome of their exercise goal, whereas older adults
more positive the nearer one gets to the outcome focused more on the process thereof. Moreover, out-
(Carver & Scheier, 1982). In contrast, focusing on come and process focus were differentially associ-
a discrepancy where none exists, as in the case of a ated with goal-relevant exercise outcomes. Adults
stability goal, does not provide any further informa- with a stronger process focus tended to experience
tion regarding goal pursuit or potential for experi- a decrease in the distance to their goal over time and
encing positive emotions. rated it as more attainable and important; they also
Turning to stability goals, the main challenge reported higher goal involvement and satisfaction as
is the length of goal pursuit. For instance, keeping compared to adults with an outcome focus. One of
one’s weight is not reached at a certain point in time the shortcomings of this study is that outcome and
but instead requires constant adherence to a certain process focus were assessed indirectly via the moti-
eating or exercising regimen. Because of the long- vation to exercise.
term aspect of stability goal orientation, the means Addressing this shortcoming, Freund and
must have the potential to be used for as long as the Hennecke (in press) demonstrated that a directly
goal is held. This is not necessarily true for change assessed process focus helped overweight women
goals where, once a goal is reached, it is either aban- to successfully pursue the goal to lose weight over
doned (e.g., I want to pass this exam) or translated a period of six weeks. Moreover, in another study
into a stability goal (e.g., “I want to lose 10 pounds,” reported by Freund et al. (2010) presented four goals
once achieved, might turn into “I want to keep my (e.g., to quit smoking) to younger and older adults.
weight down”). Because of the longer time frame of Each goal was described by five process-related
a stability goal, people have to pay more attention statements (e.g., throw away cigarettes) and five
to how resource demanding their means are. Taken outcome-related statements (e.g., improve health).
together, this suggests that, when pursuing a change Participants were asked to select five out of these ten
goal, an outcome focus might be more adaptive, statements per goal. As hypothesized, younger but
whereas the pursuit of a stability goal should profit not older adults showed a significant preference for
more from a process focus. outcome-related descriptors, indicating their stron-
As was elaborated earlier, older adults report ger outcome orientation. A third study investigated
a stronger orientation toward the maintenance of age-related differences in and affective consequences
functioning, whereas younger adults are more ori- of goal focus. Both younger and older adults were
ented toward achieving new gains. Taking a stabil- to choose between two “thinking exercises,” one of
ity versus change perspective, older adults should them focusing on the desired outcomes of personal
be more stability oriented, younger adults more goals (i.e., outcome-related exercise), the other one
change oriented. If, as we posit, stability orientation focusing on means to pursue these personal goals
is related to a stronger process focus and change ori- (i.e., process-related exercise). Participants who
entation to a stronger outcome focus, once again, selected the process-related exercise then had to list
we would predict that younger adults should focus two successive means by which one could pursue
more on the outcome of goal pursuit, whereas older the goal of having a good vacation. Participants who
adults should focus more on the process. selected the outcome-related exercise had to list two
successive desired outcomes of having a good vaca-
does process and outcome goal focus tion (see also Freitas, Gollwitzer, & Trope, 2004).
change with age? Again, younger adults showed a preference for the
A short-term longitudinal study by Freund, outcome-focused exercise, whereas older adults
Hennecke, and Riediger (2010) provides first evi- showed no preference for either type. Affect measures
dence for an age-related shift in primary goal focus. were administered after conducting the exercises. A
In this study, younger and older exercise begin- significant age by goal focus interaction indicated
ners’ process and outcome focus were assessed that older adults showed higher positive affect after
using an exercise motivation scale. Outcome focus the process-related exercise. Interestingly, younger
comprised such items as wanting to lose weight, adults showed more intense negative affect after
becoming more physically attractive, or improv- conducting the outcome-focused exercise, which
ing one’s appearance in general. Process focus was they had chosen more often. Even though younger
operationalized as wanting to have fun, socializ- adults appear to prefer an outcome focus, then, they
ing with friends, or making new acquaintances. experience more negative affect when adopting an
As expected, younger adults focused more on the outcome rather than a process focus.

 on gains and losses, means and end s


Motivational Phase and Goal Focus If a goal is not externally set (e.g., by teachers,
Integrating goal focus into the model of action parents, boss), people have to come to a decision
phases by H. Heckhausen (1989/1991) and the if they want to adopt a certain goal or not. During
related model of cognitive mindsets accompanying this phase, the predecisional phase, we propose that
the different motivational phases (Gollwitzer, 1996; people are likely to adopt an outcome focus. This is
Gollwitzer, Heckhausen, & Steller, 1990), we because during this phase, they deliberate about the
hypothesize that goal focus changes according to advantages and disadvantages of one or more tem-
motivational phase. porally distant outcomes. Weighing consequences
In brief, H. Heckhausen distinguishes four con- of different options is likely to direct attention to
secutive phases in the motivational process1: In the abstract, global features of the goal rather than the
first, predecisional phase, people deliberate about concrete goal process. At this stage, people think
pros and cons of different goals, their short- and about whether they want or like to attain something
long-term consequences, as well as their subjec- in general before engaging in laying out a roadmap
tive attainability. Once a decision is made, people as to how to reach the goal. This is not to say that
no longer engage in comparing different options considerations about whether one believes to have,
(e.g., Gollwitzer, Heckhausen, & Steller, 1990). in principle, good chances of achieving the goal do
In the preactional phase, they focus on formulating not play a role. They clearly do, as research on goal
binding intentions and concrete action plans that setting shows (for an overview of this literature, see
are realized in the actional phase. In the final post- H. Heckhausen, 1989/1991). As the literature in
actional phase goal achievement is evaluated. Note the context of bounded rationality and the use of
that the sequence of motivational phases is idealized. heuristics for making decisions suggests, however,
Throughout the motivational process, people might people do not typically have elaborate lists in mind
step back, reevaluate their goal (i.e., reentering the for integrating the various goal-relevant means,
predecisional phase), the means they employ (i.e., weighted by subjective likelihood of attaining each
reentering the preactional phase), maybe leading to step (Gigerenzer, Todd, & the ABC research group,
changes in goal standards or the chosen means. The 1999; see also H. Heckhausen, 1989/1991). Even
action phase model by H. Heckhausen proposes if all the necessary information were available, such
(and empirical studies provide evidence) that the an approach would overburden cognitive capaci-
proposed sequence is the most likely and prototypi- ties and might not even lead to better decisions
cal one. Figure 16.2 summarizes the hypothesized (Gigerenzer et al., 1999). Therefore, focusing on
goal focus during the goal process in the action the outcome and the value attached to the conse-
phase model by H. Heckhausen (1989/1991), aug- quences of a potential goal before making a decision
mented by the deadline model by J. Heckhausen seems more likely and more adaptive than taking a
(1999). detailed stock of the necessary means attached to

Decision Action
(intention formation) initiation Deadline

Pre-decisional Pre-actional Phase Actional Phase Post-actional


Phase Non-urgent Urgent Phase

Focus on outcome Focus on process Focus on Focus on Focus on


of possible goal: of goal pursuit: process of goal outcome of goal outcome:
positive and Planning of which pursuit: pursuit: positive Evaluation of
negative goal strategies Proximal and negative goal
consequences and should be consequences consequences achievement
likelihood of goal employed, of goal-relevant and likelihood
attainment specification of actions are at of goal
good the fore attainment
opportunities for
goal pursuit

Fig. 16.2. Integrating goal focus into H. Heckhausen’s model of action phases.

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


the different outcomes also into account. In fact, In the actional phase, the primary task is to
H. Heckhausen and Gollwitzer (1987) showed invest goal-relevant means and engage in goal-
that people focus more on the values of the out- relevant actions in the interest of goal achievement.
come than on strategies of goal pursuit during the H. Heckhausen and colleagues claim that a focus on
predecisional phase. the outcome on a rather abstract level of cognitive
If a goal is not self-selected but instead exter- representation might be predominant and adap-
nally imposed (and accepted as a goal by the indi- tive during this phase. In contrast, we posit that
vidual), the predecisional phase is not relevant and focusing on the outcome might distract from good
people move directly to the preactional phase, which opportunities to implement goal-relevant plans and
describes the phase after having committed to a might thereby actually hinder goal achievement.
goal and before actually engaging in goal-relevant Particularly when long-term goals are pursued that
actions. In the preactional phase, people plan the require maintenance of goal-relevant actions over
implementation of intentions as to how, when, and an extended period of time, focusing on the activi-
where to start goal-relevant actions and means. If the ties related to goal pursuit (rather than the nega-
means of goal pursuit are well established and highly tive discrepancy to a desired outcome) should help
routinized, it is likely that people will immediately maintaining motivation even in the face of hin-
proceed to implementing goal-relevant actions, drances or setbacks (see Kuhl & Beckmann, 1994).
sometimes even in an automatic way, as Bargh and This should be the case because, if the very process
Gollwitzer (1994) posit in their automotive theory of goal pursuit is in the foreground, the distance
of goal pursuit. If, however, the means are not yet to the outcome becomes less salient. For instance,
known and routinized, the focus is likely to lie on when the goal is to lose weight and the goal-relevant
finding out the best way to pursue the goal (see also means is exercising regularly, a lack of weight loss
Zimmerman & Kitsantas, 1997, 1999). In line with over a certain period of time is less likely to discour-
this, H. Heckhausen and Gollwitzer (1987) dem- age from exercising if the focus is on jogging every
onstrated that the postdecisional phase is associated morning rather than on the weight one brings to
with the elaboration of plans and strategies of how the scale every morning. If an outcome orientation
to implement goal pursuit. Findings on the imple- prevails, the person might give up exercising if no
mentational mindset are highly compatible with the weight reduction is seen within a certain period of
assumption of a predominant process focus during time. This might also be why many weight loss pro-
this motivational phase. Moreover, in a number of grams advise not to get on the scale too often.
studies, Gollwitzer and his colleagues (for an over- In line with this idea, Houser-Marko and Shel-
view, see Gollwitzer, 1996) showed repeatedly and don (2006) found that formulating an existing goal
consistently that clear and strong implementation in terms of “self as a doer” (e.g., “jogger” instead of
intentions contribute to goal achievement. Imple- “jogging regularly”) leads to higher goal attainment
mentation intentions specify goal-related means and in the domains of academic performance (Study 1)
actions, situations in which to apply those means, and exercising (Study 2). In contrast, research
and also the right timing of acting on a given goal. on positive fantasies, which can be defined as an
Moreover, implementation intentions have impor- extreme version of a positive outcome focus, has
tant cognitive effects (i.e., implemental mindset): been shown to have detrimental effects for actual
They focus attention on goal-relevant information goal pursuit during the actional phase. Oettingen
and ward off distractions (including questioning and colleagues have demonstrated repeatedly and in
the value of the selected goal), they heighten the different goal domains (e.g., academic achievement,
accessibility of situational cues allowing goal-related dieting) that indulging in positive fantasies about
actions (thereby enhancing the likelihood of seiz- the desired outcome seems to undermine actual
ing the right moment and opportunity), and lead goal pursuit (see Oettingen & Hagenah, 2005). The
to being particularly optimistic about achieving the authors speculate that the rewarding experience of
goal. All of these characteristics of planning enhance anticipated goal attainment on an imaginary level
the likelihood of actually initiating and complet- might seduce people to fantasize rather than engage
ing intended goal-related actions or applying goal- in the more laborsome process of the acquisition
related means (Gollwitzer & Brandstätter, 1997). and investment of goal-relevant means.
Taken together, the literature suggests that during The hypothesis of a predominant focus on goal
the preactional phase, people focus on the actual pursuit during the actional phase is also in line with
process of goal pursuit rather than the outcome. research on automatic goal pursuit. According to

 on gains and losses, means and end s


the automotive model by Bargh and Gollwitzer process to outcome focus, thereby motivating them-
(1994), the repeated activation of a goal in a certain selves to strive for the positively valued goal instead
situation leads to an association of the respective of focusing on negative aspects of goal pursuit. Note
goal and situational cues. Such situational features that not only achievement-related goals can have
can then automatically trigger goal-relevant actions such deadlines, but they can be applied to other life
without being consciously aware of the respective domains as well. An example of a (external) devel-
goal (Bargh & Ferguson, 2000). This suggests that, opmental deadline in the family domain is meno-
during the actional phase, conscious awareness of pause for reproduction in women.
the outcome is not necessary in order to pursue a Adopting an outcome goal when a deadline is
goal. It might even happen that—temporarily or looming might, on the one hand, help to mobilize
permanently—the process itself takes over as the increased efforts of goal pursuit and attain a goal
goal and the outcome is either regarded as rela- within a certain time frame. On the other hand,
tively unimportant or abandoned as irrelevant (e.g., however, outcome focus might also hinder flexible
jogging every morning for 45 minutes becomes adjustment of means and emphasize the importance
a goal and techniques are acquired to improve run- of investing maximum effort over efficient use of
ning performance, whereas losing weight might be goal pursuit strategies (Schmitz & Wiese, 1999).
seen as a nice side effect of jogging but no longer as Hence, if a deadline is introduced too early in the
the goal). As these examples show, means and ends motivational process, that is, when the most adap-
can change their status during the motivational tive means or strategies of pursuing the goal are
process (see Kruglanski, 1996). Means sometimes not yet established, goal attainment might come at
become outcomes. Attention then shifts to the sub- a relatively high cost or people might not live up to
ordinate means to achieve the new goal (formerly their optimal performance level (see also Ariely &
known as means). Wertenbroch, 2002). In cases where no deadline is
A different situation arises when a (self-set or set, people are expected not to undergo a shift from
imposed) deadline is approaching (J. Heckhausen, process to outcome focus during goal pursuit. The
1999). In this case the outcome will again become same holds true for goals consisting of a state to be
more salient. A deadline (e.g., losing 3 pounds until reached and maintained (e.g., “I want to be happy.”)
the night of the high school prom a week from rather than an endpoint (e.g., “I want to get mar-
now) revives the importance of the outcome and ried.”). State goals do not have clear endpoints but
decreases the importance of the valence of the pro- instead stretch over an extended period of time. As
cess. In such cases, the most effective (and not nec- continued engagement in goal pursuit is needed for
essarily the most enjoyable) way of attaining one’s such goals, they should be generally more condu-
goal needs to be identified and implemented so as to cive to process focus. This contrasts with goals that
reach it in time. Closely monitoring the distance to specify an outcome that can be reached at a certain
an outcome becomes adaptive and adjusting means point in time. Upon reaching such a goal—or after
of goal pursuit accordingly is required (e.g., Schmitz deciding to give it up (e.g., because a deadline has
& Wiese, 1999). passed)—people enter the postactional phase, in
If the means for achieving a goal are not posi- which they evaluate the means and the degree to
tively valued, even if the outcome is, people are which they reached the outcome. If the goal will have
tempted to procrastinate and not engage in goal- to be reached again (e.g., taking an exam in school),
relevant activities. In this case, a deadline and the it is likely that people are motivated to evaluate the
perceived negative consequences of missing it (i.e., quality of the means in order to be able to optimize
not achieving the outcome) serve as incentives to get goal pursuit in the next round (i.e., maintain a focus
to work. The valence of the more abstract outcome on processes for some time). With increasing tem-
representation (i.e., the positive valence of achiev- poral distance, however, people will focus primarily
ing the outcome, or the negative valence of failure) on the outcome (Trope & Liberman, 2003).
is helpful for overriding the negative valence of the Taken together, goal focus is proposed to change
concrete goal-relevant means. In fact, research sug- relative salience depending on motivational phase.
gests that deadlines increase performance and goal During the predecisional and, again, when urgency
attainment and, moreover, that people even self- in attaining the goal is experienced, outcome focus
impose binding deadlines to counteract procrasti- should be predominant. During the preactional and
nation (e.g., Ariely & Wertenbroch, 2002). Thus, nonurgent actional phase, process focus is expected
people might use deadlines to induce a shift from to be more salient.

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


Consequences of Goal Focus After Failure of explicit feedback, people can internally frame fail-
After having discussed antecedents of goal focus ure either as failure to implement the right means or
related to age, resources, time perspective, goal as failure to achieve the desired outcome. Whether
orientation, and motivational phase, we now turn failure is framed as means- or as outcome-related
to the consequences of goal focus when people have should partly depend on goal focus. Thinking about
to cope with failure. means (process focus) should be associated with
Previous research on the consequences of goal the cognitive accessibility of these means, whereas
focus has shown that mentally simulating the pro- thinking about outcomes (outcome focus) should
cess of goal pursuit (e.g., studying for an exam) is be associated with the cognitive accessibility of
more beneficial than mentally simulating its attain- these outcomes. Conversely, as highly accessible
ment (e.g., receiving a good grade; Pham & Tay- goals or constructs influence information process-
lor, 1999; Taylor, Pham, Rivkin, & Armor, 1998). ing (e.g., Bargh, Gollwitzer, Lee-Chai, Barndollar,
However, not much is known about the underlying & Trötschel, 2001; Bargh & Pratto, 1986; Förster,
mechanisms that render process or outcome focus Liberman, & Higgins, 2005; Higgins, Bargh, &
more adaptive. We propose that one mechanism Lombardi, 1985), a person who primarily focuses
might be the reaction to failures and setbacks that on means will be more likely to frame her setbacks as
might depend on the goal focus. More specifically, a failure to implement the necessary or appropriate
we put forth that a process focus is more beneficial means of goal pursuit (e.g., “I did not use the right
because it fosters adaptive affective and behavioral dieting strategies to lose weight.”), whereas a person
reactions in the event of failure. who primarily focuses on the outcome will be more
There are many typical situations in which goal likely to frame her failure as failure to attain desired
pursuit is hampered by setbacks or failure: Dieters outcomes (e.g., “I did not achieve the weight loss I
are frustrated when their weight goes up instead was hoping for.”). In other words: Beaming a flash-
of down, students fail to pass their exams, and light on the means of goal pursuit will more likely
sportsmen do not win a competition. As setbacks also highlight the blocked path, whereas beaming it
and failures are a major threat to future persistence on the desired outcomes will highlight the blocked
and subjective well-being (Carver & Scheier, 1990; outcome. Failure, then, should be framed as process-
Pomerantz, Saxon, & Oishi, 2000), psychologi- related in an outcome focus and as outcome-related
cal research has long been interested in how peo- in an outcome focus.
ple cope with them: Under what conditions does
a person remain persistent and substitutes his or her behavioral consequences of goal focus
means of goal pursuit? When will someone give up after failure
his or her goal and decide to head for other desirable Framing failure as process-related should have
outcomes instead? One prominent determinant of different effects on subsequent behavior than fram-
affective and behavioral consequences of failure is ing failure as outcome-related. After experiencing
attribution to internal or external, stable or instable, failure, people usually face different behavioral
global or unspecific causes (Abramson, Seligman, & options: First, means that are thwarted or resulted
Taesdale, 1978). We argue that goal focus is another in failure can be substituted by others (equifinality;
important determinant of affective and behav- Kruglanski, 1996; Kruglanski et al., 2002). Differ-
ioral reactions to failure because it might influence ent outcomes can be attained via the same means
whether the inappropriate implementation of means (multifinality; Kruglanski & Jaffe, 1988; Kruglanski
or the failed accomplishment of desired outcomes is et al., 2002). A person trying to lose weight could,
in the foreground of failure identification. for example, try another diet if he realized that the
one he has tried before does not bring about the
framing failure as means- versus desired results. In a process focus, when the means
outcome-related of goal pursuit are identified as problematic and
Feedback is essential to evaluate progress toward inappropriate, means substitution (i.e., compensa-
a desired outcome (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1981, tion; see Freund & Baltes, 2000, 2002) seems like
1982). Sometimes such feedback can refer explic- the self-evident behavioral reaction.
itly to either the processes of goal pursuit (i.e., fail- Second, a person can decide to pursue another
ure to implement the right means) or the outcome goal, if she perceives the desired outcome as blocked.
states (i.e., failure to achieve a desired result; Earley, Switching to another desirable outcome, that is, dis-
Northcraft, Lee, & Lituchy, 1990). In the absence engaging from the goal at hand and selecting a new

 on gains and losses, means and end s


one (outcome substitution or loss-based selection; According to Carver and Scheier (e.g., 1981), feel-
Freund & Baltes, 2000, 2002), should be the more ings arise as a consequence of an automatic feedback
straightforward reaction in an outcome focus. process. The feedback process continually checks
In line with this rationale, some researchers have how well one’s actions reduce the discrepancy
also argued that “what the hell” cognitions result between the actual and a desired state. If goal prog-
from identifying behaviors on higher, more abstract ress is below a criterion that refers to an acceptable
levels (Cochran & Tesser, 1996). “What the hell” rate of discrepancy reduction, negative affect arises.
cognitions typically occur in dieters. After having If goal progress exceeds the criterion, positive affect
failed to resist a temptation (e.g., a piece of cake), arises. If it is identical with the criterion, no affect
they interrupt their dieting for a day or even com- arises (Carver, 2004). Failure of goal pursuit can be
pletely disengage from their weight loss goal. As defined as a progress rate below this criterion or even
a consequence, they show disinhibited eating (e.g., stagnation. Accordingly, failure elicits negative affect
more pieces of cake; Polivy & Herman, 1985). This (see also Hsee & Abelson, 1991). We propose that,
breakdown of self-regulation might be caused by especially when goals are difficult to attain and goal
framing failure as failure to bring about desired out- pursuit is hampered by setbacks, focusing on and
comes (“I am not successful in reducing my weight”). valuing primarily the outcome has negative conse-
Perceiving a goal as blocked might cause people to quences because it makes the discrepancy between
disengage from it and switch to the tangible goal of the actual and the desired state more salient.
eating enjoyment (for a similar argumentation, see A second explanation for the detrimental effects
also Stroebe, Mensink, Aarts, Schut, & Kruglanski, of outcome focus on affective well-being is based
2008). In fact, we have shown that dieters who on the hierarchic organization of goals and goal-
focus on a more abstract and outcome-related level directed behavior (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1982,
of their goal (weight loss, improving their appear- 1990; Emmons, 1996; Vallacher & Wegner, 1985).
ance, health, and well-being) show more disinhib- Means are often referred to as subgoals that serve
ited eating after failure than dieters who focus on the attainment of more abstract, superordinate
a more concrete process-related level (the way they goals, the respective outcomes. As goals that are
diet, persist, resist temptations, and change their placed higher in a personal goal hierarchy are more
eating behavior; Hennecke & Freund, in revision). important and central to the self (Austin & Van-
In addition, a recent study by Burnette (2010) has couver, 1996; Boden, 1973), outcomes, by defini-
shown that dieters who might tend to attribute fail- tion, should be more valuable than their respective
ure to the outcome of dieting, as they believe body subgoals or means; they might even be valuable only
weight to be fixed (entity theorists) rather than mal- to the extent that they serve a desired outcome. Self-
leable by the use of appropriate means (incremental regulation is required when people engage in activi-
theorists), report less persistence following setbacks. ties that are not intrinsically motivated or positively
Moreover, findings of our own self-report study valued in and of themselves (e.g., eating low-caloric
(Hennecke & Freund, in revision) also supported food instead of tasty but high-caloric food) but
the predicted link between goal focus and a prefer- instead represent means in the service of pursuing
ence for means substitution versus loss-based selec- higher-order goals (e.g., becoming more attractive).
tion after failure in other goal domains. Participants If means of goal achievement come to bear intrinsic
were asked to name two personal goals and indicate value (e.g., if someone joins a gym to lose weight
how much they think about the means of goal pur- and experiences exercising as fun), the former means
suit (process focus) and about the desired outcomes might change their status to a desired outcome (e.g.,
(outcome focus). As expected, process focus was wanting to have the fun experience of exercising).
strongly positively related to means substitution as However, the opposite effect can come about
opposed to loss-based selection. Outcome focus was when intrinsically rewarding activities become
slightly negatively related to means substitution; means of achieving extrinsic rewards. The vast
hence, it had a positive impact on the loss-based literature on the detrimental effects of extrinsic
selection of new outcomes after failure. rewards on intrinsic motivation demonstrates that
activities can lose their intrinsic appeal if they are
affective consequences of goal focus tied to extrinsic rewards (Deci et al., 1999; Lep-
after failure per, Greene, & Nisbett, 1973). Moreover, Newman
What are the affective consequences of process and Taylor (1992) have demonstrated the relatively
and outcome focus when people encounter failure? lower value of means as compared to outcomes in

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


a study with children who were given a snack as Conclusion
a reward for consuming another snack. Indepen- Goals have wonderful qualities: They motivate
dent of prior ratings of how much children liked behavior, help us organize behavior into action
the snacks, they ended up liking the snack that sequences over time and situations, and thus provide
was given as a reward for consuming another snack our lives with direction and meaning. Although we
more than the “means-snacks,” even though the wholeheartedly agree with this assessment, we would
position of the respective snack in the goal system like to distinguish at least two goal dimensions that
was assigned arbitrarily. modulate the adaptiveness of goals. Depending on
Martin and Tesser (1989) also assume that the the availability of resources, it might be better to
higher a goal in the hierarchy, the more likely it is orient one’s goals toward gains, maintenance, or the
that a threat to it will elicit rumination, the ten- avoidance of loss. Goal orientation, in turn, might
dency to carry negative thoughts and feelings after affect goal focus on the process or the outcome of
being exposed to unpleasant events. Taken together, goal pursuit. We argued that a gain (change) orien-
as a means is subordinate to its desired outcome, a tation is likely to be related to an outcome focus,
threat to a means should be less severe than a threat whereas maintenance (stability) orientation is likely
to an outcome. Houser-Marko and Sheldon (2008) to be related to a process focus. Moreover, we elabo-
have supported this hypothesis when showing that rated that the motivational phase might influence
failure feedback has stronger negative effects on the goal focus (during the predecisional phase and
mood when it is related to the process (in their terms: close to a deadline, an outcome focus is more likely
primary goal level) as compared to the outcome (in to occur, whereas during the actional phase a process
their terms: subgoal level). Moreover, Emmons focus should prevail). Importantly, regarding the
(1992) demonstrated that people who focus on consequences of goal focus, we argued that process
concrete goals show less severe depressive symptoms focus might lead to higher persistence and higher
than people whose goals are rather abstract. Our affective well-being when people encounter diffi-
own research supports our assumptions as well: We culties during goal pursuit. Research on goal focus
have found that framing failure experience during a is just at the beginning of empirically testing these
low-calorie diet as failure to attain desired outcomes hypotheses. Initial results, however, are largely sup-
was related to significantly lower levels of affective portive of the ideas presented here. Future research
well-being (Hennecke & Freund, in revision). will have to prove the incremental validity of goal
In addition to these direct effects of goal focus on focus over other constructs such as intrinsic and
affect, an indirect effect might result from the behav- extrinsic motivation.
ioral outcomes of each focus. When goals are higher
in the goal hierarchy than their subordinate means, Acknowledgments
disengaging from a goal to switch to another (out- This chapter represents work that is currently pursued in a
come substitution or loss-based selection) should grant to the first author and financially supporting the second
impede affective well-being more strongly than dis- and third author. The grant (“Process and outcome focus—
The role of age,” ID: 100013–116528) is financed by the Swiss
engaging from a means and switching to another
National Foundation.
(means substitution). In fact, we have found that
means substitution (as opposed to loss-based selec-
tion) is positively related to affective well-being Note
Unlike H. Heckhausen (1989), we use the term “motiva-
(Hennecke & Freund, in revision). tional phase” to refer to all phases from setting to attaining (or
In sum, then, a process focus might be gener- abandoning) a goal.
ally more adaptive after failure because it should
lead to failure framing that refers to the means
References
rather than to the desired outcomes of goal pur- Abramson, L. Y., Seligman, M. E. P., & Taesdale, J. P. (1978).
suit. This, in turn, should foster the substituting Learned helplessness in humans: Critique and reformulation.
of means rather than the loss-based selection of a Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 49–74.
new outcome. Finally, focusing on means has posi- Ariely, D., & Wertenbroch, K. (2002). Procrastination, dead-
lines, and performance: Self-control by precommitment.
tive effects on affective reactions to failure, whereas
Psychological Science, 13, 219–224.
focusing on the outcome should make the discrep- Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in psy-
ancy between the actual and the desired state even chology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological Bul-
more salient. letin, 120, 338–375.

 on gains and losses, means and end s


Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span devel- Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of
opmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psycho-
decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626. logical Review, 97, 19–35.
Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1998). On the self-regulation of
ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as behavior. New York: Cambridge University Press.
foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, Cochran, W., & Tesser, A. (1996). The “what the hell” effect:
52, 366–380. Some effects of goal proximity and goal framing on
Baltes, P. B., & Baltes, M. M. (1990). Psychological perspectives performance. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), Striving
on successful aging: The model of selective optimization with and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect and self-regulation
compensation. In P. B. Baltes & M. M. Baltes (Eds.), Success- (pp. 99–120). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ful aging: Perspectives from the behavioral sciences (pp. 1–34). Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading activation
New York: Cambridge University Press. theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 83,
Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (1998). 407–428.
Life-span theory in developmental psychology. In Deci, E. L, Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic
R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 1: review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic
Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 1029– rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125,
1143). New York: Wiley. 627–668.
Baltes, P. B., & Smith, J. (2003). New frontiers in the future of Duke, J., Leventhal, H., Brownlee, S., & Leventhal, E. A. (2002).
aging: From successful aging of the young old to the dilem- Giving up and replacing activities in response to illness. Jour-
mas of the fourth age. Gerontology, 49, 123–135. nal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 57, 367–376.
Bargh, J. A., & Ferguson, M. J. (2000). Beyond behaviorism: Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to
On the automaticity of higher mental processes. Psychological motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.
Bulletin, 126, 925–945. Dweck, C. S., & Molden, D. C. (2005). Self-theories: Their
Bargh, J. A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1994). Environmental control impact on competence motivation and acquisition. In
of goal-directed action: Automatic and strategic contingen- A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence
cies between situations and behavior. Nebraska Symposium on and motivation (pp. 122–140). New York: Guilford Press.
Motivation, 41, 71–124. Earley, P. C., Northcraft, G. B., Lee, C., & Lituchy, T. R. (1990).
Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, A., Barndollar, K., & Impact of process and outcome feedback on the relation of
Trötschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious goal setting to task performance. Academy of Management
activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Person- Journal, 33, 87–105.
ality and Social Psychology, 81, 1014–1027. Ebner, N. C., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2006). Develop-
Bargh, J. A., & Pratto, F. (1986). Individual construct accessibil- mental changes in personal goal orientation from young to
ity and perceptual selection. Journal of Experimental Social late adulthood: From striving for gains to maintenance and
Psychology, 22, 293–311. prevention of losses. Psychology and Aging, 21, 664–678.
Boden, M. A. (1973). The structure of intentions. Journal for the Elliott, A. J., & Friedman, R. (2007). Approach-avoidance:
Theory of Social Behaviour, 3, 23–46. A central characteristic of personal goals. In B. R. Little,
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, K. Salmela-Aro, J. E. Nurmi, & S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Per-
36, 129–148. sonal project pursuit: Goals, action and human flourishing (pp.
Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit 97–118). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related Emmons, R. A. (1989). The personal striving approach to per-
analysis of assimilative and accommodative strategies of cop- sonality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Goal concepts in personality and
ing. Psychology and Aging, 5, 58–67. social psychology (pp. 87–126). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Burnette, J. L. (2010). Implicit theories of body weight: Entity Emmons, R. A. (1992). Abstract versus concrete goals: Personal
beliefs can weigh you down. Personality and Social Psychology striving level, physical illness, and psychological well-being.
Bulletin, 36, 410–422. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 292–300.
Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of Emmons, R. A. (1996). Striving and feeling: Personal goals and
the mind. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. subjective well-being. In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition and motiva-
Taking time seriously: A theory of socio-emotional selectiv- tion to behavior (pp. 313–337). New York: Guilford Press.
ity. American Psychologist, 54, 165–181. Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Friedman, R. S. (2007). Seven
Caruso, E. M., Gilbert, D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2008). A wrin- principles of goal activation: A systematic approach to distin-
kle in time: Asymmetric valuation of past and future events. guishing goal priming from priming of non-goal constructs.
Psychological Science, 19, 796–801. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11, 211–233.
Carver, C. S. (2004). Self-regulation of action and affect. In R. F. Förster, J., Liberman, N., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). Accessibility
Baumeister & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self regulation: from active and fulfilled goals. Journal of Experimental Social
Research, theory, and applications (pp. 13–39). New York: Psychology, 41, 220–239.
Guilford Press. Freitas, A. L., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Trope, Y. (2004). The influ-
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1981). Attention and self- ence of abstract and concrete mindsets on anticipating and
regulation: A control-theory approach to human behavior. guiding others’ self-regulatory efforts. Journal of Experimental
New York: Springer. Social Psychology, 40, 739–752.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A use- Freund, A. M. (2006). Differential motivational consequences of
ful conceptual framework for personality, social, clinical, and goal focus in younger and older adults. Psychology and Aging,
health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111–135. 21, 240–252.

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


Freund, A. M. (2007). Levels of goals: Understanding motivational Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). The volitional benefits of planning. In
processes across adulthood. In B. R. Little, K. Salmela-Aro, & P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action
S. D. Phillips (Eds.), Personal project pursuit: Goals, action and (pp. 287–312). New York: Guilford Press.
human flourishing (pp. 87–116). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (1998). Selection, optimization, and intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality
compensation as strategies of life-management: Correlations and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.
with subjective indicators of successful aging. Psychology and Gollwitzer, P. M., Heckhausen, H., & Steller, B. (1990). Delib-
Aging, 13, 531–543. erative and implemental mind-sets: Cognitive tuning toward
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). The orchestration of selec- congruous thoughts and information. Journal of Personality
tion, optimization and compensation: An action-theoretical and Social Psychology, 59, 119–127.
conceptualization of a theory of developmental regulation. Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., Trauer, J. M., Carter, S. M.,
In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), Control of human behav- & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Short-term and long-term conse-
ior, mental processes, and consciousness: Essays in honor of the quences of achievement goals: Predicting interest and per-
60th birthday of August Flammer (pp. 35–58). Mahwah, NJ: formance over time. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92,
Erlbaum. 316–330.
Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2002). Life-management strat- Heckhausen, H. (1991). Motivation and action (P. K. Leppmann,
egies of selection, optimization, and compensation: Mea- Trans.). New York: Springer. (Original work published 1989).
surement by self-report and construct validity. Journal of Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents
Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 642–662. and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional
Freund, A. M., & Ebner, N. C. (2005). The aging self: Shift- states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120.
ing from promoting gains to balancing losses. In W. Greve, Heckhausen, J. (1999). Developmental regulation in adulthood:
K. Rothermund, & D. Wentura (Eds.), The adaptive self: Age-normative and sociostructural constraints as adaptive chal-
Personal continuity and intentional self-development (pp. 185– lenges. New York: Cambridge University Press.
202). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber. Heckhausen, J., Dixon, R. A., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Gains and
Freund, A. M., & Hennecke, M. (in press). Changing eating losses in development throughout adulthood as perceived
behavior vs. losing weight: The role of goal focus for weight by different adult age groups. Developmental Psychology, 25,
loss in overweight women. Psychology and Health. 109–121.
Freund, A. M., Hennecke, M., & Riediger, M. (2010). Age- Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (1995). A life-span theory of con-
related differences in outcome and process goal focus. Euro- trol. Psychological Review, 102, 284–304.
pean Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 198–222. Hennecke, M., & Freund, A. M. (2010). Staying on and get-
Freund, A. M., Li, K. Z. H., & Baltes, P. B. (1999). Successful ting back on the wagon: Age-related improvement in self-
development and aging: The role of selection, optimization, regulation during a low-calorie diet. Psychology and Aging,
and compensation. In J. Brandtstädter & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), 25, 876–885.
Action and self-development: Theory and research through the Hennecke, M., & Freund, A. M. (in revision). The path or
life span (pp. 401–434). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. the goal? Process versus outcome focus and the mastery of
Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2001). What I have and what I failure.
do: The role of resource loss and gain throughout life. Applied Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psy-
Psychology: An International Review, 50, 370–380. chologist, 52, 1280–1300.
Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2003). Successful aging. In Higgins, E. T., Bargh, J. A., & Lombardi, W. (1985). Nature
R. M. Lerner, A. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), Compre- of priming effects on categorization. Journal of Experimental
hensive handbook of psychology. Volume 6: Developmental psy- Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 58–69.
chology (pp. 601–628). New York: Wiley. Houser-Marko, L., & Sheldon, K. (2006). Motivating behav-
Freund, A. M., & Riediger, M. (2006). Goals as building ioral persistence: The self-as-doer construct eyes on the prize
blocks of personality and development in adulthood. In or nose to the grindstone: The effects of level of goal evalua-
D. K. Mroczek & T. D. Little (Eds.), Handbook of personality tion on mood and motivation. Personality and Social Psychol-
development (pp. 353–372). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. ogy Bulletin, 32, 1037–1049.
Fujita, K., & Han, H. A. (2009). Moving beyond deliberative Houser-Marko, L., & Sheldon, K. (2008). Eyes on the prize or
control of impulses: The effect of construal levels on evalu- nose to the grindstone: The effects of level of goal evaluation
ative associations in self-control conflicts. Psychological Sci- on mood and motivation. Personality and Social Psychology
ence, 20, 799–804. Bulletin, 34, 1556–1569.
Fujita, K., Trope, Y., Liberman, N., & Levin-Sagi, M. (2006). Hsee, C. K., & Abelson, R. P. (1991). Velocity relation: Sat-
Construal levels and self-control. Journal of Personality and isfaction as a function of the first derivative of outcome
Social Psychology, 90, 351–367. over time. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
Fung, H. H., & Carstensen, L. L. (2004). Motivational changes 341–347.
in response to blocked goals and foreshortened time: Testing Jopp, D., & Smith, J. (2006). Resources and life-management
alternatives to socioemotional selectivity theory. Psychology strategies as determinants of successful aging: On the pro-
and Aging, 19, 68–78. tective effect of selection, optimization, and compensation.
Gestsdottir, S., & Lerner, R. M (2007). Intentional self-regulation Psychology and Aging, 21, 253–265.
and positive youth development in early adolescence: Find- Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An anal-
ings from the 4-H Study of Positive Youth Development. ysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47, 263–291.
Developmental Psychology, 43, 508–521. Kanfer, R. (1987). Task-specific motivation: An integrative
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & The ABC Research Group. approach to issues of measurement, mechanisms, processes,
(1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart: Evolution and and determinants. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology,
cognition. London, England: Oxford University Press. 5, 237–264.

 on gains and losses, means and end s


Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). Aging, adult develop- Newman, J., & Taylor, A. (1992). Effect of a means-end contin-
ment, and work motivation. Academy of Management Review, gency on young children’s food preferences. Journal of Experi-
3, 440–458. mental Psychology, 64, 200–216.
Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the Pham, L. B., & Taylor, S. E. (1999). From thought to action:
incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis: University of Min- Effects of process- versus outcome-based mental simulations
nesota Press. on performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
Krapp, A. (2005). Basic needs and the development of interest 25, 250–260.
and intrinsic motivational orientations. Learning and Instruc- Oettingen, G., & Hagenah, M. (2005). Fantasies and the self-
tion, 15, 381–395. regulation of competence. In A. Elliot & C. Dweck (Eds.),
Kruglanski, A. W. (1996). Goals as knowledge structures. In Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 647–665). New
P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: York: Guilford.
Linking cognition and motivation to behavior (pp. 599–618). Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (1985). Dieting as a problem in
New York: Guilford Press. behavioral medicine. In E. Katkin & S. Manuck (Eds.),
Kruglanski, A. W., & Jaffe, Y. (1988). Curing by knowing: The Advances in behavioral medicine (pp. 1–37). New York:
epistemic approach to cognitive therapy. In L. Abramson SAI.
(Ed.), Social cognition and clinical psychology (pp. 254–291). Pomerantz, E. M., Saxon, J. L., & Oishi, S. (2000). The psy-
New York: Guilford Press. chological trade-offs of goal investment. Journal of Personality
Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Fishbach, A., Friedman, and Social Psychology, 79, 617–630.
R., Chun, W. Y., & Sleeth-Keppler, D. (2002). A theory of Raynor, J. O. (1982). A theory of personality functioning and
goal systems. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimen- change. In J. O. Raynor & E. E. Entin (Eds.), Motivation,
tal social psychology (Vol. 34, pp. 331–378). San Diego, CA: career striving and aging (pp.249–302). Washington, DC:
Academic Press. Hemisphere Publishing Corporation.
Kuhl, J., & Beckmann, J. (1994). Volition and personality: Action Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (2004). Interference and facilita-
versus state orientation. Seattle, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. tion among personal goals: Differential association with sub-
Lang, F. R., & Carstensen, L. L. (2002). Time counts: Future jective well-being and persistent goal pursuit. Personality and
time perspective, goals, and social relationships. Psychology Social Psychology Bulletin, 30,1511–1523.
and Aging, 17, 125–139. Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (2006). Focusing and restricting:
Lang, F. R., Rieckmann, N., & Baltes, M. M. (2002). Adapting Two aspects of motivational selectivity in adulthood. Psychol-
to aging losses: Do resources facilitate strategies of selection, ogy and Aging, 21, 173–185.
compensation, and optimization in everyday functioning? Riediger, M., & Freund, A. M. (2008). Me against myself: Moti-
The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and vational conflicts and emotional development in adulthood.
Social Sciences, 57, 501–509. Psychology and Aging, 23, 479–494.
Labouvie-Vief, G. (1981). Proactive and reactive aspects of con- Riediger, M., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2005). Managing
structivism: Growth and aging in life-span perspective. In life through personal goals: Intergoal facilitation and inten-
R. M. Lerner & N. A. Busch-Rossnagel (Eds.), Individuals sity of goal pursuit in younger and older adulthood. Journals
as producers of their development (pp. 197–230). New York: of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 60B, 84–91.
Academic Press. Röcke, C., Li, S-C., & Smith, J. (2009). Intraindividual vari-
Lepper, M. (1981). Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in ability in positive and negative affect over 45 days: Do older
children: Detrimental effects of superfluous social con- adults fluctuate less than young adults? Psychology and Aging,
trols. In W. W. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on 24, 863–878.
Child Psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 155–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erl- Ryan, R. M, Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). All
baum. goals are not created equal: An organismic perspective on the
Lepper, M. R., Greene, D., & Nisbett, R. E. (1973). Undermin- nature of goals and their regulation. In P. M. Gollwitzer &
ing children’s intrinsic interest with extrinsic reward: A test of J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cognition
the “overjustification” hypothesis. Journal of Personality and and motivation to behavior (pp. 7–26). New York: Guilford
Social Psychology, 28, 129–137. Press.
Little, B. R. (1989). Personal projects analysis: Trivial pursuits, Sansone, C., & Thoman, D. B. (2005). Interest as the miss-
magnificent obsessions, and the search for coherence. In ing motivator in self-regulation. European Psychologist, 10,
D. M. Buss & N. Cantor (Eds.), Personality psychology: 175–186.
Recent trends and emerging directions (pp. 15–31). New York: Schmitz, B., & Wiese, B. S. (1999). Eine Prozessstudie selbstreg-
Springer. ulierten Lernenverhaltens im Kontext aktueller affektiver und
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically motivationaler Faktoren [A process study of self-regulated
useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year learning behavior in the context of motivational and emo-
odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. tional states]. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und
Manderlink, G., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1984). Proximal versus Pädagogische Psychologie, 31, 157–170.
distal goal setting and intrinsic motivation. Journal of Person- Seijts, G. H., & Latham, G. P. (2005). Learning versus perfor-
ality and Social Psychology, 47, 918–928. mance goals: When should each be used? Academy of Man-
Martin, L. L., & Tesser, A. (1989). Toward a motivational and agement Executive, 19, 124–131.
structural theory of ruminative thought. In J. S. Uleman & Staudinger, U. M., Marsiske, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1995). Resil-
J. A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought (pp. 306–326). New ience and reserve capacity in later adulthood: Potentials and
York: Guilford Press. limits of development across the life span. In D. Cicchetti &
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology (Vol. 2,
the structure of behavior. New York: Henry Holt. pp. 801–847). New York: Wiley.

f reun d , h en n ec ke, mus ta f i  


Stroebe, W., Mensink, W., Aarts, H., Schut, H., & Kruglanski, to work and partnership. Journal of Vocational Behavior, 57,
A. (2008). Why dieters fail: Testing the goal conflict model of 273–300.
eating. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 26–36. Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Longitudinal
Taylor, S. E., Pham, L. B., Rivkin, I. D., & Armor, D. A. (1998). predictions of selection, optimization, and compensation.
Harnessing the imagination: Mental simulation, self- Journal of Vocational Behavior, 59, 1–15.
regulation, and coping. American Psychologist, 53, 429–439. Woike, B., Lavezzary, E., & Barsky, J. (2001). The influence of
Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psycho- implicit motives on memory processes. Journal of Personality
logical Review, 110, 403–421. and Social Psychology, 81, 935–945.
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1985). A theory of action Wyer, R. S., & Srull, T. K. (1986). Human cognition in its social
identification. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. context. Psychological Review, 93, 322–359.
VandeWalle, D. (2001). Goal orientation: Why wanting to look Young, L. M., Baltes, B. B., & Pratt, A. K. (2007). Using selec-
successful doesn’t always lead to success. Organizational tion, optimization, and compensation to reduce job/family
Dynamics, 30, 162–171. stressors: Effective when it matters. Journal of Business and
VandeWalle, D., Brown, S. P., Cron, W. L., & Slocum, J. W., Jr. Psychology, 21, 511–539.
(1999). The influence of goal orientation and self-regulation Ziegelmann, J. P., & Lippke, S. (2007). Use of selection, optimi-
tactics on sales performance: A longitudinal field test. Journal zation, and compensation strategies in health self-regulation:
of Applied Psychology, 84, 249–259. Interplay with resources and successful development. Journal
Wiese, B. S., & Freund, A. M. (2001). Zum Einfluss of Aging and Health, 19, 500–518.
persönlicher Prioritätensetzungen auf Masse der Stimulus- Zimbardo, P. G., & Boyd, J. N. (1999). Putting time in perspec-
präferenz [The impact of personal priorities on stimulus tive: A valid reliable individual-differences metric. Journal of
preference]. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1271–1288.
57–73. Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1997). Developmental
Wiese, B. S., & Freund, A. M. (2005). Goal progress makes phases in self-regulation: Shifting from process to outcome
one happy, or does it? Longitudinal findings from the work goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 29–36.
domain. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol- Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1999). Acquiring writ-
ogy, 78, 287–304. ing revision skill: Shifting from process to outcome self-
Wiese, B. S., Freund, A. M., & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Selection, regulatory goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91,
optimization, and compensation: An action-related approach 241–250.

 on gains and losses, means and end s


PA RT
5
Motivation in
Relationships
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER

Self-Enhancement and
17 Self-Protection Motives

Constantine Sedikides and Mark D. Alicke

Abstract
People desire to maximize the positivity, and minimize the negativity, of their self-views. The tendency
to exalt one’s virtues and soften one’s weaknesses, relative to objective criteria, manifests itself in many
domains of human striving. We focus illustratively on three strivings: the self-serving bias (crediting
the self for successes but blaming others or situations for failures), the better-than-average effect
(considering the self superior to the average peer), and selective self-memory (disproportionately poor
recall for negative self-relevant information). Nonmotivational factors (e.g., expectations, egocentrism,
focalism, individuated-entity versus aggregate comparisons) are not necessary for the emergence of these
strivings. Instead, the strivings are (at least partially) driven by the self-enhancement and self-protection
motives, as research on self-threat and self-affirmation has established. The two motives serve vital
functions: They confer benefits to psychological health and psychological interests (e.g., goal pursuit).
Key Words: self-enhancement, self-protection, self-serving bias, better-than-average effect, self-memory,
psychological health

Introduction self-relevant information, broadly defined (Sedikides,


Individuals routinely appraise their qualities, 1993; Sedikides & Strube, 1997). Self-enhancement
performance, behavior, and feedback they receive in particular refers to the desire and preference for
from others. They also choose activities in which maximizing the positivity of self-views, whereas self-
to engage, allocate credit or blame for dyadic and protection refers to the desire and preference for mini-
group task outcomes, recollect events from their mizing the negativity of self-views. Self-enhancement
lives, use self-knowledge to understand other peo- and self-protection are reflected in individuals’ ten-
ple, and judge the value of their relationships or the dency to exaggerate their strengths and to underrate
groups to which they belong. We suggest, in the their weaknesses more so than objective standards
current chapter, that these and similar domains of would warrant. The two motives are also reflected in
human functioning can be motivated, and we pro- individuals’ tendency to construe or remember events
ceed to discuss the role of two pivotal motives: self- in a manner that places their self-attributes in the most
enhancement and self-protection. favorable light that is credible to themselves and to
Self-enhancement and self-protection are instances others (Sedikides & Gregg, 2003). Finally, the motives
of self-evaluation motives (Sedikides & Strube, 1995), energize and guide attributions, task involvement,
which themselves are a class of the hedonic or plea- and behavior. In the long run, self-enhancement and
sure/pain drive (Alicke & Sedikides, 2011a). Self- self-protection foster psychological health (Sedikides,
evaluation motives guide processing and appraisal of Gregg, & Hart, 2007) and assist in the advancement


and protection of psychological interests (e.g., goals; unbridled pursuit of pleasure rather than by a failure
Alicke & Sedikides, 2009). to grasp a priori truths. “Men [are] vehemently in
We begin our excursion into self-enhancement and love with their own opinions” (p. 48), he proclaimed.
self-protection with a brief historical overview. We then The position that humans have an excessively positive
provide key examples of motive instantiation, what view of themselves and of the objects (e.g., persons,
we call self-enhancement and self-protection strivings possessions) associated with them was reflected in
(Alicke & Sedikides, 2011b; Sedikides & Gregg, 2008). the utilitarianism of Bentham (1789/1982) and John
These striving are the self-serving bias, the better-than- Stuart Mill (1863/2004), the forewarning of Nietz-
average effect, and selective self-memory. In discuss- sche (1886/1972) for the power of pride to rewrite
ing each of these strivings, we consider the perennial memory (Maxim 68, p. 72), and the contemplations
“cognition-motivation” debate. We acknowledge, of of La Rochefoucauld (1678/1827), Schopenhauer
course, that cognition and motivation are closely inter- (1844/1996), and Freud (1905/1961a) on the curi-
twined (Kruglanski, 1989; Kunda, 1990; Pyszczynski ous human capacity for self-deception.
& Greenberg, 1987). Yet we aim to provide evidence William James (1890) was the first psychologist to
that the strivings are motivated and, in particular, that systematize various philosophical accounts and pro-
they cannot be exclusively accounted for by the vaga- pose a unifying principle. He observed that thinking
ries of information processing (Sedikides, 2012). Next, about one’s self gives rise to the emotions of “self-
we discuss the functional benefits of the two motives: complacency and self-dissatisfaction” (p. 305). He also
promotion of psychological health and psychological remarked on “social self-seeking,” people’s persistent
interest. We conclude with a consideration of issues concern with the achievement of tangible successes
worthy of further empirical attention. and public acclaim. “Each of us,” James stated, “is
animated by a direct feeling of regard for his [self]”
A Historical Overview (p. 308). He proceeded to define the self (empirical
The seeds for modern theorizing on self- “me”) as a repository of ego-relevant matters. James’
enhancement and self-protection motivation were key animating principle, self-enhancement, found
sown in classical times. The Cyrenaics (founder: fertile ground in Gordon Allport’s (1937) theoriz-
Aristippus; Tatarkiewicz, 1976) and Epicureans ing. He advocated that humans have a need for self-
(founder: Epicurus; De Witt, 1973) thought that positivity, and he also regarded self-protection as
hedonism drives human action. They observed “nature’s eldest law.” Heider (1958) similarly argued
that people want to feel good, or avoid feeling bad, that subjective needs, desires, and preferences partially
about themselves, and they further proposed that serve to maintain an individual’s positive outlook.
humans want and pursue pleasurable experiences, Rogers (1961) proposed the construct of positive
while detesting and eschewing unpleasant ones. self-regard, a form of self-appreciation achieved by
Notably, Demosthenes, the orator of antiquity, satisfying one’s own, rather than others’, standards
remarked insightfully on self-deception: “Nothing and expectations. In the meantime, Sigmund Freud
is so easy as to deceive oneself; for what we wish, we (1915/1961b, 1923/1961c, 1926/1961d) and Anna
readily believe.” Freud (1936/1946) were pioneering the analysis of
The role of hedonism as the master motive receded defense mechanisms. The scientific study of self-
while rationalism was in ascendance. This philosoph- enhancement and self-protection was born.
ical school, building on Plato’s ideas (Bloom, 1991),
depicted an objective reality that all individuals with Instantiations of Self-Enhancement and
correct understanding (“orthodoxy”) could readily Self-Protection
discern (Kenny, 1986; Loeb, 1981). Continental How have scientists approached self-enhancement
rationalists (Descartes, Leibniz, Spinoza), for exam- and self-protection? They have done so through
ple, opined that selfish, irresponsible, or malicious experimental and correlational investigations of over
behavior was due to flawed knowledge. Erudition 60 instantiations (or implementations) of the motives.
would cure personal and social ills such as immorality These marks of self-enhancement and self-protection
or the prioritization of personal over societal goals. have recently been summarized through factor-analytic
The pendulum swung back with Renaissance phi- techniques, with both Western (Hepper, Gramzow, &
losophers (Macfarlane, 1978) and the British empiri- Sedikides, 2010) and East-Asian (Hepper, Sedikides,
cists. Mandeville (1705) argued that humans overvalue & Cai, in press) samples, into four factors: positivity
themselves and expect others to do the same. Hobbes embracement, defensiveness, favorable construals, and
(1651/1991) believed that behavior was driven by the self-affirming reflections.

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


Positivity embracement reflects the acquisition (Mezulis, Abramson, Hyde, & Hankin, 2004). And
of positive feedback (e.g., self-serving attributions it is found both in Western and non-Western cultures
for success), whereas defensiveness reflects the pro- (Brown & Kobayashi, 2002; Mezulis et al., 2004).
tection of self from threat (e.g., self-serving attribu- Next, we will consider reasons why the self-serving
tions for failure). A striving that exemplifies both bias is motivated or why it cannot be accounted for
factors is the self-serving bias, the tendency to credit solely by nonmotivational factors. Specifically, we
the self for successes but to blame others (e.g., dyadic will discuss the role of self-threat, self-affirmation,
partners, ingroup, situations) for failures. Favorable expectancies, and impression management. We will
construals reflects flattering portrayals of the self in offer representative examples in each case.
the social world. An exemplary striving here is the
better-than-average effect, the tendency to regard the self-threat
self as superior to others in many domains of func- From a self-protection perspective, when people
tioning. Finally, self-affirming reflections refers to feel threatened, they become defensive (Roese &
securing favorable, or bypassing unfavorable, self- Olson, 2007). Given an outlet, such as the opportu-
views and outcomes. A key mechanism through nity to deflect attributions regarding task outcomes,
which this process is attained is selective self-memory, they will grab it to footprint their defensiveness.
or disadvantageous recall for negative as opposed to Assuming that the self-protection motive under-
positive feedback. lies the SSB, the more threatened people feel, the
Next we review literature on the self-serving bias, stronger the magnitude of the SSB will be. A meta-
the better-than-average effect, and selective self- analysis by Campbell and Sedikides (1999) tested
memory. Although we fully endorse the close inter- whether the SSB waxes and wanes as function of
weaving of cognition and motivation (Kruglanski, self-threat, operationalized as negative feedback.
1989; Kunda, 1990; Pyszczynski & Greenberg, This meta-analysis examined several moderators of
1987), we venture to make the case for motivation. the SSB, such as role, self-focused attention, and
That is, we attempt to document that this class of interpersonal orientation.
purposive goal strivings cannot be accounted for In particular, each moderator was classified as
purely and exclusively by nonmotivational anteced- high or low in self-threat potential. For example,
ents. Instead, each striving is, at least in part, an out- the moderator role was classified in terms of actor
come of the self-enhancement and self-protection or observer. Actors presumably experience more
motives in action. self-threat than observers, given that actors’ self-
views are directly challenged by negative feedback.
The Self-Serving Bias The moderator self-focused attention was classified as
“If more than one person is responsible for a self-focused or other-focused attention. Self-focused
miscalculation, none will be at fault,” Murphy’s law attention presumably involves more threat, given
advocates. Weiner’s (1972) attributional analysis of that participants in this experiential state are more
achievement motivation documented this pattern. likely to become aware of the discrepancy between
Actors attribute their successful outcomes to inter- their actual and ideal/ought self. Hence, their focus
nal factors (e.g., ability, effort, discipline) and their on performance standards would intensify the psy-
unsuccessful outcomes to external factors (e.g., bad chological impact of negative feedback. Finally, the
luck, task difficulty, harsh course instructor). More moderator interpersonal orientation was classified as
generally, assuming the lion’s share of responsibil- competitive or cooperative. Some participants com-
ity for desirable events and denying responsibility peted (actually or ostensibly) with another person,
or displacing it to external causes for undesirable whereas others cooperated (actually or ostensibly)
events has come to be known as the self-serving bias with another person, on a task. Failed competitive
(SSB; Miller & Ross, 1975). participants would presumably experience the high-
The SSB is a robust and pervasive phenomenon. est level of self-threat because they would have the
It is evident among university students (Zuckerman, most at stake on the task outcome.
1979), athletes (De Michele, Gansneder, & Solomon, The meta-analysis proceeded to test the effective-
1998), and drivers (Stewart, 2005). It occurs in the ness of the SSB moderators. The proposition that
arena of interpersonal influence (Arkin, Cooper, & self-threat magnifies the SSB was supported. For
Kolditz, 1980), naturalistic sports (Mullen & Riordan, example, actors, self-focused, and competing partic-
1988), and organizations (Corr & Gray, 1996). It ipants displayed the SSB, but their respective coun-
is manifested by children, adolescents, and adults terparts (observers, other-focused, and cooperative

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
participants) did not do so. In all, this meta-analysis scale corresponding to their most important value.
illustrated that, the more threatened individuals Each item consisted of two statements, one describ-
feel, the more likely they are to resort to the SSB. ing a facet of the relevant value, the other being neu-
This conclusion is bolstered in research by tral (i.e., filler). Participants proceeded to rate their
Kernis, Cornell, Sun, Berry, and Harlow (1993) agreement with each statement. Participants in the
and by Crocker, Voelkl, Testa, and Major (1991). control condition displayed the SSB. However, par-
Undergraduate students are quick to find flaws in a ticipants in the self-affirmation condition refrained
test when they fail it but quick to stress its validity from it. In all, self-affirmation eclipsed the procliv-
when they pass it (Wyer & Frey, 1983). This pat- ity to respond defensively to self-threat, a pattern
tern is especially pronounced among individuals tracked by the vanishing of the SSB.
with unstable self-esteem, suggesting that these indi-
viduals use the SSB when threatened to shore up a nonmotivational explanations
fragile sense of personal worth (Kernis et al., 1993). We will now turn to the nonmotivational explana-
Black American students experience a drop in self- tions of expectancies and impression management.
esteem when the negative feedback is administered
by a White evaluator believed to be unaware of their Expectancies
race; however, their self-esteem is unaffected when It has been argued that differential expectancies
the evaluator is believed to be aware of their race. for success and failure account for the SSB (Miller
In the latter case, participants attribute their failure & Ross, 1975). Based on prior experience (Kelley &
to racial prejudice, thus denying the validity of the Michela, 1980; Tetlock & Levin, 1982), individu-
test (Crocker et al., 1991). Here, the SSB is not only als expect success more frequently than failure. As
a mode to respond to self-threat but also a means such, they make internal attributions for expected
to alleviate the consequences of threat (i.e., drop in outcomes and external attributions for unexpected
self-esteem). outcomes (i.e., SSB).
There is evidence that expectations can influ-
self-affirmation ence the SSB. For example, individuals with chronic
As discussed earlier, self-threat intensifies the SSB. expectations of superior task performance (e.g., high
It follows that the SSB will be attenuated or cancelled self-esteemers, normals) manifest strongly the SBB
when the self-threat is assuaged. One way of reduc- relative to individuals with chronic expectations
ing self-threat is via self-affirmation (Sherman & of inferior task performance (low self-esteemers,
Hartson, 2011). Here, individuals affirm a domain depressed; Blaine & Crocker, 1993; Tennen &
(e.g., values) irrelevant to self-threat. For example, Herzberger, 1987). Similarly, participants who regard
they explain in writing, before or after they receive a task as important (and hence likely have chronic
negative feedback, why some values are important to expectations of superior performance) demonstrate
them. This self-affirmation procedure reduces defen- the SSB to a greater degree than participants who
siveness (and even buffers neuroendoctrine and psy- regard a task as unimportant (Miller, 1976).
chological responses to stress; Creswell et al., 2005) Nevertheless, expectations are not a neces-
by making individuals feel more secure in their self- sary component of the SSB (Weary, 1979; Weary
worth. Self-affirmation, then, would reduce, if not Bradley, 1978; Zuckerman, 1979). Of the various
eliminate, the SSB. moderators in the Campbell and Sedikides (1999)
Sherman and Kim (2005) tested these ideas in field meta-analysis discussed earlier, expectations did
experiments with volleyball and basketball athletes. not play a substantial role. Actors and observers
The experiments were conducted at the conclusion approach the experimental situation with the same
of a game, with positive feedback operationalized as expectations, yet only actors display the SSB. Fur-
a win and negative feedback as a loss. Immediately thermore, it is not clear why a momentary state of
after the game, athletes were escorted into a confer- self-focused versus other-focused attention, or a
ence room and undertook a self-affirmation manipu- state of competitive versus cooperative interpersonal
lation. They rated and ranked five values (aesthetics, orientation, would influence task expectancies. Yet
religion, social, political, theoretical) in terms of per- the SSB was manifested by some of these partici-
sonal importance. Then, participants in the control pants (i.e., actors, state-self-focused persons, com-
condition received a 10-item scale corresponding petitive persons) but not others. Finally, the SSB is
to their least important value, whereas participants observed even when controlling for task importance
in the self-affirmation condition received a 10-item (Sedikides, Campbell, Reeder, & Elliot, 1998).

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


Impression Management condition. In all, impression management concerns
Participants may display the SBB in a strategic cannot fully account for the SSB.
maneuver to present themselves favorably to others
(Miller, 1978; Weary, 1979). Impression manage- summary
ment, of course, aims at the enhancement or pro- Although nonmotivational factors play a role in
tection of one’s public image (Forsyth & Schlenker, the SSB, they cannot account singly for it. Expecta-
1985), although such aims are not always felici- tions or strategic self-management is not necessary
tous (Miller & Schlenker, 1985; Sedikides, Gregg, for the emergence of the SSB. In contrast, research
et al., 2007). Nevertheless, strategic enhancement/ on self-threat and self-affirmation makes a compel-
protection of one’s public image does not necessitate ling case that the SSB is a valid signature of the self-
the concurrent enhancement/protection of one’s enhancement and self-protection motives.
private self. Impression management may be super-
ficial and short lived (i.e., driven by the moment The Better-Than-Average Effect
or situation) rather than authentic. It may merely Garrison Keillor’s Lake Wobegon is a fictional
reflect putting on a persona or playing a role rather location, where “all the women are strong, all the men
than expressing a cherished self-belief. are good looking, and all the children are above aver-
Impression management concerns can influence age.” This characterization describes succinctly the
the SSB (Arkin, Appelmen, & Burger, 1980; House, human tendency for overestimation of one’s merits
1980). Such concerns, however, are not necessary and underestimation of one’s liabilities, in compari-
for its occurrence. Sedikides et al. (1998) tested son to other persons. Research has confirmed this
undergraduate students at a large university. The tendency. Most people judge themselves as better
participants worked together, as members of a dyad, than their average peer (Alicke & Govorun, 2005;
on an interdependent-outcomes task. They were Brown, 1998; Dunning, Heath, & Suls, 2004), and
unacquainted and thus unlikely to anticipate future they truly believe they are so (Williams & Gilovich,
interactions. In addition, care was taken to ensure 2008). The phenomenon of rating oneself above the
that participants expected not to meet each other average peer standing on positive characteristics, or
after the experiment and not to discuss this experi- rating oneself below the average peer standing on
ment even if they happened to encounter each other negative characteristics, has been labeled the better-
on campus. Finally, all procedures were private, than-average effect (BTAE).
anonymous, and confidential, with each participant The BTAE is robust and pervasive. It is found
being unaware of the other’s contribution to the among undergraduate students rating their leader-
interdependent-outcomes task. These procedures ship skills, athletic prowess, ability to get along with
were intended to minimize impression management others (Brown, 1986; College Board Exams, 1976),
concerns. The experimental task ostensibly assessed intentions (Kruger & Gilovich, 2004), resistance
creativity. Following bogus success or failure feed- to socially undesirable media messages (Davison,
back at the dyadic level, participants did manifest 1983), complexity of personality (Sande, Goethals,
the SSB. & Radloff, 1988), possessions (Nesselroade, Beggan,
Greenberg, Pyszczynski, and Solomon (1982) & Allison, 1999), and, indeed, their very humanness
put the impression management explanation of the (Haslam, Bain, Douge, Lee, & Bastian, 2005); driv-
SSB directly to test. Participants took an alleged ers rating their driving skills, while in a hospital due
intelligence test (“Culture Fair Test of g”). Half of to a car accident they had caused (Preston & Harris,
them learned that the experimenter was interested 1965); college instructors rating their teaching ability
in their performance on the test and therefore would (Cross, 1977); social psychologists rating the quality
collect their named answer sheets and record their of their research (Van Lange, Taris, & Vonk, 1997);
scores (public performance condition: presence of students assessing their dating popularity (Preuss &
impression management concerns). The other half Alicke, 2009) or couples assessing the quality of their
of participants learned that the experimenter was marriage (Rusbult, Van Lange, Wildschut, Yovetich,
disinterested in their performance and had no way & Verette, 2000); and adults assessing their happiness
of knowing how well they had done on the test (pri- (Freedman, 1978). In addition, individuals suffering
vate performance condition: absence of impression from rheumatoid arthritis rate their symptoms as less
management concerns). Participants displayed the severe than those of the average patient (DeVellis
SSB in both conditions. Remarkably, the SSB was et al., 1990), and elderly persons judge that they are
stronger in the private than public performance less at risk for age-related problems than their peers

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
(Schulz & Fritz, 1987). The BTAE has also been likely to assert their self-superiority on their impor-
found among preschoolers (Weiner, 1964), elemen- tant (e.g., trustworthy) than their unimportant
tary school children (Albery & Messer, 2005), high (e.g., punctual) attributes (Sedikides & Strube,
school students (Kurman, 2002), and representative 1997). This principle is illustrated in recent work
community samples (Andrews & Whitey, 1976; by Brown (2011, Studies 1–4), where participants
Heady & Wearing, 1988). Ironically, people believe indeed showed a stronger tendency to evaluate
that they are less prone to the BTAE than the average themselves more positively on important than
person (Pronin, Lin, & Ross, 2002). unimportant traits (Study 1). This principle is also
Next we will discuss five reasons why the BTAE illustrated when placing the BTA effect in cultural
is motivated. These pertain to attribute valence context.
and controllability, attribute importance (in cross- Important self-attributes are those that imply
cultural context), attribute verifiability, self-threat, successful role fulfillment or enactment of culturally
and self-affirmation. We will also consider nonmo- sanctioned roles. They imply that one is a valued
tivational accounts of the effect. member of a given culture, given that one excels on
culturally (and personally) important characteris-
attribute valence and controllability tics, no matter if one falls behind on culturally (and
Self-enhancement and self-protection strivings are personally) unimportant characteristics. Members
tactical (Sedikides & Strube, 1997; see also Sedikides of all cultures, then, will appraise themselves posi-
& Gebauer, 2010). People do not self-enhance or self- tively on important (but not necessarily on unim-
protect across the board; instead, they are selective on portant) attributes.
the attributes that they will tout or undervalue. For For Western culture important attributes are those
example, they may be more likely to self-enhance on conveying agency (e.g., personal effectiveness, com-
positive attributes over which they have high control petence), whereas for Eastern culture important attri-
(e.g., resourceful) than positive attributes of which butes are those conveying communion (e.g., personal
they have low control (e.g., mature). Conversely, integration, other-orientation). Hence, Westerners
they may be more likely to self-protect on negative will display the BTAE on agentic attributes, whereas
attributes over which they have high control (e.g., Easterners will display the BTAE on communal attri-
unappreciative) than negative attributes over which butes. Westerners, for example, will rate themselves
they have low control (e.g., humorless). as better than their average peer on originality or
The results of a study by Alicke (1985) demon- independence but not on loyalty or respectfulness,
strated that the BTAE effect indeed varies as a function but Easterners will rate themselves as better than
of attribute valence and controllability. Undergradu- their average peer on loyalty or respectfulness but
ates rated themselves more favorably on positive not on originality or independence. This hypothesis
traits, and less favorably on negative traits, compared has been confirmed both by primary studies (Brown
to their average peer. Thus, the BTAE increased as & Kobayashi, 2002; Gaertner, Sedikides, & Chang,
the valence of the self-attribute increased. In addi- 2008; Sedikides, Gaertner, & Toguchi, 2003) and
tion, participants rated themselves more favorably on meta-analytic investigations (Sedikides, Gaertner, &
positive controllable traits, and more unfavorably on Vevea, 2005, 2007; for more general discussions, see
negative controllable traits, compared to their average Brown, 2003, 2010). The findings attest to the pan-
peer. Finally, they rated themselves more favorably culturality of the BTAE.
on positive controllable than positive uncontrollable
traits, and rated themselves less favorably on nega- attribute verifiability
tive uncontrollable than negative controllable traits, There is another way in which self-enhancement
compared to their average peer. This latter finding and self-protection are tactically expressed. It involves
in essence illustrates that people self-aggrandize the attribute verifiability. Some attributes (e.g., those
most when they feel responsible for their positive belonging to the moral or social domain) are more
traits, and self-aggrandize the least when they believe difficult to verify objectively than others (e.g., those
that fate is responsible for their negative traits. belonging to the intellectual or physical domain;
Reeder & Brewer, 1979; Rothbart & Park, 1986).
attribute importance: on the Therefore, moral attributes leave more latitude for
panculturality of the btae self-enhancement strivings than intellectual ones.
Self-enhancement and self-protection strivings The BTAE, then, will be stronger in the case of moral
are also tactical in another way. People are more than intellectual attributes.

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


This pattern has been empirically supported. described an accomplishment or achievement that
Participants firmly believe that they have enacted made them feel good about themselves. Control par-
more moral behaviors than their average peer. How- ticipants, on the other hand, described the student
ever, they believe rather tentatively that they have union building on campus. Subsequently, all par-
enacted more intellectual behaviors than their peers ticipants rated their standing, relative to that of their
(Allison, Messick, & Goethals, 1980; Van Lange & average academic peer, on a variety of traits (e.g.,
Sedikides, 1998). In addition, participants rate them- cooperative, truthful, athletic, attractive, imagina-
selves as better than average on traits that are either tive, tolerant).
preclassified as ambiguous or are manipulated to be The results were revealing. The BTAE emerged,
ambiguous (Critcher, Helzer, & Dunning, 2011). as expected, among participants in the control con-
These findings illustrate that self-enhancement and dition, but it was attenuated among participants
self-protection strivings, albeit “dying to come out,” in the self-affirmation condition. Self-affirmation
are susceptible to reality constraints (Gramzow, reduced defensiveness or the need to assert one’s
2011; Sedikides & Gregg, 2008). superiority over others. These findings attest to the
motivational underpinnings of the BTAE.
self-threat
A self-protection perspective would predict that, nonmotivational explanations
when individuals feel threatened, they will become The three most prominent nonmotivational
defensive (Roese & Olson, 2007). We have dis- explanations for the BTAE effect are egocentrism,
cussed evidence that self-threat intensifies the SSB. focalism, and individuated-entity versus aggregate
Does self-threat also intensify the BTAE? comparisons. We consider them next along with
Research by Brown (2011, Study 4) showed that a fourth possibility, that the BTAE reflects simple
it does. All participants took the Remotes Associates contrast of oneself from the average peer.
Test (RAT; Mednick, 1962), ostensibly a test of the
cognitive ability of integrative orientation (defined Egocentrism
as creativity). The RAT consists of a series of three According to egocentrism, when participants
words; in each case, participants are asked to gener- compare their attributes to those of the average peer,
ate a fourth word that relates in some way to the they think selectively about their own strengths or
other three. All RAT problems were difficult, and about their peer’s weaknesses (Champers, Windschitl,
participants received either bogus negative feedback & Suls, 2003; Moore, 2007; Moore & Kim, 2003;
or no feedback. Subsequently, participants com- Weinstein, 1980). However, selective recruitment of
pleted a BTAE task: They rated both themselves and one’s assets or of peers’ liabilities may themselves be
most other people on important and unimportant expressions of self-enhancement and self-protection
traits. Participants who received negative feedback (Brunot & Sanitioso, 2004; Sanitioso & Niedenthal,
manifested a stronger BTAE effect (compared to 2006). In addition, egocentrism cannot explain why
those who did not receive feedback). In particu- the BTAE is obtained not only with direct measures
lar, they rated themselves as superior to others on (where participants compare the self to the average
important than unimportant traits, but they rated peer on a single scale) but also with indirect mea-
others as superior on unimportant than important sures (where participants rate the self and average
traits. These results underscore the motivational peer on separate and scales that are counterbalanced)
relevance of the BTAE (see also: Brown, Collins, (Alicke & Govorun, 2005). Moreover, egocentrism
& Schmidt, 1988; Brown & Gallagher, 1992; has trouble accounting for why the BTAE is stron-
Dunning, Leuenberger, & Sherman, 1995). ger on unverifiable than verifiable traits (Allison
et al., 1989; Critcher et al., 2011) and for why self-
Self-Affirmation affirmation reduces the BTAE (Guenther, 2011).
Does self-affirmation reduce the BTAE? An exper- Finally and importantly, the BTAE is observed even
iment by Guenther (2011) addressed this question. when behavioral evidence for attributes is equated
Participants were assigned to either a self-affirmation for self and others. This pattern was demonstrated
or a control condition. The manipulation was a by Alicke, Vredenburg, Hiatt, and Govorun (2001).
hybrid of two established procedures introduced by Participants first estimated the percentage of times
Blanton, Pelham, DeHart, and Carvallo (2001) and they enacted various trait-relevant behaviors (e.g.,
by Wiesenfeld, Brockner, Petzall, Wolf, and Bailey percentage of times they were uncooperative or
(2001). Specifically, self-affirmation participants cooperative, when the opportunity arose). A month

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
and a half later, participants received the very same present when the individuated entity is the self; that
estimates but were led to believe that the estimates is, the effect emerges even when the self is compared
were provided by their average peer. Still, participants to any other individuated entity (Alicke et al., 1995).
rated themselves more favorably than “their average In addition, this nonmotivational alternative cannot
peer” on almost all traits. Participants claimed that explain why the effect ebbs and flows as a function
they were superior to themselves. of the motivational significance of the judgment
(e.g., attribute valence, controllability, verifiability,
Focalism importance). Moreover, the alternative cannot easily
According to focalism, people put greater weight explain why self-affirmation weakens the effect and,
on whatever entity is currently the focus of their importantly, why the effect emerges even under cog-
attention. By asking participants to compare their nitive load (Alicke et al., 1995, Study 7)—a pattern
attributes to those of their average peer, research indicative of automatic self-enhancement (Paulhus,
on the BTAE places the self in the focal position 1993). Finally, the alternative cannot explain why
and the average peer in the referent position. Self- participants manifest a stronger BTAE on impor-
representations consist of a higher number of unique tant than unimportant traits, even when they com-
attributes than other-representations (Karylowski, pare themselves with a single person (Brown, 2011,
1990; Karylowski & Skarzynska, 1992). Hence, Study 2).
focusing on the self highlights those unique attri-
butes and leads to perceiving the self as less simi- Assimilation and Contrast
lar than the average peer (Moore & Kim, 2003; Although some researchers have conjectured
Otten & van der Pligt, 1996; Pahl & Eiser, 2006, that self versus average peer judgments are made
2007; Windschitl, Kruger, & Sims, 2003). How- by anchoring on the self and contrasting the aver-
ever, focalism cannot provide an adequate account age peer from that point (e.g., Kruger, 1999), until
of why the BTAE varies as a function of attribute recently, no studies had been designed specifically
valence, controllability, importance, and verifiabil- to examine this facet of the BTAE. To address this
ity. In addition, focalism cannot explain why the question, Guenther and Alicke (2010) constructed
BTAE is obtained with indirect measures (Alicke an experimental design that was equipped to test
& Govorun, 2005), when behavioral base rates for whether self versus average peer judgments repre-
relevant traits are the same for self and other (Alicke sent assimilation or contrast, and in what direction
et al., 2001), and even when the referent is highly assimilation or contrast might occur. In the first
concretized (Alicke, Klotz, Breitenbecher, Yurak, & study, participants first made either self or aver-
Vredenburg, 1995). Finally, focalism cannot explain age peer ratings in a pretesting session. Later in the
why participants manifest a stronger BTAE on semester, their original ratings were returned and
important than unimportant traits, even when the they were now asked to rate the other target (i.e.,
self constitutes the referent and “most other people” those who rated the self in the first phase now rated
constitute the target (Brown, 2011, Study 3). the average peer in relation to their self-ratings, and
those who rated the average peer in the first phase
Individuated-Entity Versus Aggregate Comparisons now rated the self in relation to their average peer
This nonmotivational account refers to a single ratings). Comparisons with the ratings provided by
entity (e.g., a person, an object) being compared a group that simply made simultaneous ratings of
with an aggregate (e.g., the average peer, the aver- self and the average peer showed that self-ratings
age object). Klar and his colleagues (Giladi & Klar, were unaltered as a result of whether self and aver-
2002; Klar, 2002; Klar & Giladi, 1997) showed age peer were rated simultaneously, self was rated
that any member of a liked group (e.g., a randomly in relation to the average peer, or the average peer
selected student at one’s university, police officer, was rated in relation to the self. This shows clearly
soap fragrance) is rated more positively than the that the self anchors these judgments. The findings
group average (e.g., average student at one’s univer- also demonstrated that ratings of the average peer
sity, average police officer, average fragrance), and were higher when made in relation to self-ratings
that any member of a disliked group is rated more than when self and average peer were rated simulta-
negatively than the group average. These findings neously. Contrary to the common assumption that
raise the possibility that the BTAE is due to the judgments of an average peer are contrasted from
self being an individuated entity and the average the self, average peer ratings were assimilated toward
peer being an aggregate. However, the BTAE is still the self.

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


The fact that people move evaluations of the aver- that this effect is a legitimate signature of self-
age peer closer to the self seems to contradict self- enhancement and self-protection motivation.
enhancement assumptions. However, most modern
self-enhancement perspectives (Alicke & Sedikides, Selective Self-Memory
2009; Sedikides & Gregg, 2003, 2008) acknowl- “It’s not only the most difficult thing to know
edge that such tendencies occur in concert with one’s self, but the most inconvenient,” quipped Josh
many nonmotivational forces, including relatively Billings. The empirical evidence has treated Bill-
automatic anchoring and adjustment processes. ings kindly. People indeed remember poorly their
Guenther and Alicke (2010) next designed a study weaknesses compared to their strengths, a memorial
to assess whether self-enhancement motives could pattern that does not occur for other people’s weak-
be discerned in light of these assimilative compara- nesses and strengths (Sedikides & Green, 2009;
tive judgments. Skowronski, 2011). We refer to this phenomenon as
In this study (Guenther & Alicke, 2010, Study selective self-memory. Next we discuss it by review-
2), participants made self-judgments on various ing research both from the autobiographical and
trait dimensions during pretesting. The returned experimental literatures.
later in the semester and were provided with the Selective self-memory is robust and pervasive.
self-ratings they had completed during pretesting. It has been observed in the domain of feedback
This time, they were asked to evaluate the average (Crary, 1966; Sedikides & Green, 2000), social act
college student with reference to these self-ratings. frequencies (Gosling, John, Craik, & Robins, 1998),
Most important, half of the participants were led to possessions and places (Zauberman, Ratner, & Kim,
think that the ratings they now received were those 2009), relationship-relevant behaviors (Van Lange,
provided by a randomly selected student instead Rusbult, Semin-Goossens, Goerts, & Stalpers, 1999),
of by themselves. The critical comparison was personality traits (Messick, Bloom, Boldizar, &
between ratings of the average peer made with refer- Samuelson, 1985; Mischel, Ebbesen, & Zeiss, 1976),
ence to scale points that participants believed were life events (Ross & Wilson, 2002; Skowronski, Betz,
their own ratings, and those made with reference Thompson, & Shannon, 1991), and emotionally
to identical points that were believed to belong to charged (i.e., pride-inducing and shame-inducing)
another student. Participants assimilated their rat- events (D’Argembeau & Van der Linden, 2008).
ings of average toward the scale points provided to a It has also been observed not only in Western but
lesser degree when those scale points were described also in non-Western or East-Asian cultures (Kwon,
as self-ratings compared to when the identical Scheibe, Samanez-Larkin, Tsai, & Carstensen, 2009;
points were attributed to another individual. Thus, Schrauf & Hoffman, 2007). Selective self-memory
although anchoring comparative judgments on emerges early in life. Children, for example, ascribe
the self induces average-peer assimilation because more serious transgressions to their siblings than to
of the fact that self-ratings constitute high scale themselves in their recollections of sibling conflict
points, participants’ desire to maintain favorable (Wilson, Smith, Ross, & Ross, 2004). Finally, selec-
self-concepts restricts this assimilative process and tive self-memory is found both among younger and
thereby maximizes the distance between the self and older adults (Field, 1981, 1997; Wagenaar & Groe-
the average peer. neweg, 1990; Yarrow, Campbell, & Burton, 1970).
Selective self-memory may be due to an encod-
summary ing bias. People avoid attending to unfavorable
As with the SSB, nonmotivational explanations feedback (Baumeister & Cairns, 1992; Sedikides &
for the BTAE are rather unsatisfactory. Egocentrism, Green, 2000, Experiment 3), thus impeding its reg-
focalism, individuated-entity versus aggregate com- istration. However, selective self-memory may also
parisons, and assimilation/contrast cannot account be due to a retrieval bias. Evidence for this process-
for the fluctuation of the BTAE as a function of ing mechanism is found in memory for behaviors
assessment technique (i.e., indirect measures, equa- that exemplify desirable traits (Sanitioso, Kunda, &
tion of behavioral evidence for self and other, cogni- Fong, 1990), satisfying interpersonal relationships
tive load), motivational relevance (attribute valence, (Murray & Holmes, 1993), and health-boosting
controllability, importance, verifiability), and refer- habits (Ross, McFarland, & Fletcher, 1981). Finally,
ent individuation. On the other hand, research on selective self-memory may be due to retention. The
self-threat, self-affirmation, and the motivational negative affect associated with autobiographical
relevance of the BTAE makes a compelling case memories fades faster across time than the positive

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
affect associated with such memories (Landau & the more defensive participants become (i.e., more
Gunter, 2009; Ritchie, Skowronski, Hartnett, Wells, likely to exhibit mnemic neglect). For example,
& Walker, 2009; Walker, Skowronski, & Thompson, the effect is obtained when the behaviors are high
2003). on diagnosticity (e.g., “you would be unfaithful
We will examine next why selective self-memory when in an intimate relationship”), but it is can-
is motivated. In particular, we will zero in on the celled when the behaviors are low on diagnosticity
role of self-threat and self-affirmation in selective (e.g., “would forget for a week to return a borrowed
self-memory. We will also consider the nonmoti- book to a friend”) (Green & Sedikides, 2004). This
vational accounts of differential expectancies and is because high-diagnosticity behaviors can really
inconsistency between information valence and self- reveal whether one is untrustworthy or unkind,
view valence. and are thus threatening. In addition, the effect is
obtained when participants are led to believe that
Self-Threat their traits are unmodifiable, but it is cancelled
Sedikides and colleagues (Sedikides & Green, when they are led to believe their traits are modi-
2009; Sedikides, Green, & Pinter, 2004) tested fiable (Green, Pinter, & Sedikides, 2005). This is
experimentally the role of self-threat in selective because learning that one was born untrustworthy or
self-memory. In the standard paradigm, participants unkind and will be so for life makes untrustworthi-
first receive behavioral feedback. Some are then asked ness or unkindness feedback threatening. Relatedly,
to imagine, or are led to believe, that they are likely the effect is obtained when participants are deprived
to perform the behaviors contained in the feedback. of the opportunity to improve on feedback-relevant
Other participants are asked to imagine, or are led dimensions (e.g., to become less untrustworthy or
to believe, that another person (Chris) is likely to less unkind) and are thus threatened, but it is can-
perform the very same behaviors. These behaviors celled when participants are offered the opportu-
are either negative or positive, and they exemplify nity to improve (Green, Sedikides, Pinter, & Van
either central (e.g., unkind vs. kind, untrustworthy Tongeren, 2009). In all, this research shows that
vs. trustworthy) or peripheral (e.g., complaining vs. selective self-memory is motivated.
uncomplaining, unpredictable vs. predictable) traits.
Next, participants engage in a surprise recall task. Self-Affirmation
The typical finding is that participants recall poorly Does self-affirmation reduce or negate selective
behaviors that are negative, exemplify central traits, self-memory? Green, Sedikides, and Gregg (2008,
and refer to the self (e.g., unkind or untrustworthy Experiment 2) addressed this question. All partici-
behaviors) compared to all other categories of behav- pants took a test ostensibly assessing their cognitive
ior (e.g., those that are positive, exemplify central ability (i.e., creativity). In the self-threat condition,
traits, and refer to the self; those that are negative participants learned that they had performed poorly
exemplify central traits but refer to Chris). For exam- on the test. In the self-affirmation condition, how-
ple, participants recall poorly the behaviors “you ever, participants learned that they had performed
would borrow other people’s belongings without well on the test. Subsequently, all participants pro-
their knowledge” (untrustworthy) and “you would ceeded to an “impression” task, which was actually
refuse to lend classnotes to a friend who was ill” the standard mnemic neglect paradigm (i.e., behav-
(unkind). However, participants recall relatively well ioral feedback).
the behaviors “Chris would borrow other people’s The results were, once again, telling. Self-
belongings without their knowledge” and “Chris threatened participants evinced mnemic neglect,
would refuse to lend classnotes to a friend who was whereas self-affirmed participants did not. Self-
ill” (unkind). Additionally, they recall relatively well affirmation relaxed defensiveness, as tracked by the
the behaviors “you would keep secrets when asked abolishment of mnemic neglect. These results are
to” (trustworthy) and “you would offer to care for a consistent with the idea that mnemic neglect is a
neighbor’s child when the babysitter couldn’t come” motivated phenomenon.
(kind). This recall discrepancy has been labeled mne-
mic neglect and has been attributed to the self-threat Nonmotivational Explanations
potential of the feedback. We next turn to two nonmotivational explana-
Research has consistently supported the idea tions of selective self-memory: differential expectan-
that self-threat underlies mnemic neglect. In gen- cies and inconsistency between information valence
eral, the more threatening the feedback is perceived, and self-view valence.

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


differential expectancies session after the usual exposure to and recall of behav-
In a review of the literature, Walker et al. (2003) ioral feedback, participants provided expectancies
concluded that the base rate of negative versus posi- for each behavior for either the self or Chris. That
tive life events is unequal. That is, negative events is, they estimated the extent to which they could
are half as frequent as positive events (25% vs. imagine either themselves or Chris performing the
50%). Differential base rates may also be involved behavior. Expectancies and recall were uncorrelated
in mnemic neglect. People may process shallowly for most but a subset of participants. This subset
and recall negative feedback poorly because they do was defensive pessimists, who as hypothesized, did
not expect to receive it; based on prior experience, not show the typical mnemic neglect pattern. In
such feedback is implausible. conclusion, differential expectancies, albeit relevant
Can differential expectancies account for selective to recall of autobiographical information (Walker
self-memory? We (Sedikides et al., 2004; Sedikides et al., 2003), cannot account solely for mnemic
& Green, 2009) addressed this issue in the context of neglect and more generally selective self-memory.
the mnemic neglect paradigm. As described earlier,
this research was concerned with the on-line pro- inconsistency between information
cessing of a concrete and experimentally provided valence and self-view valence
array of feedback as opposed to the reconstruction Another alternative, though, is worth considering,
of pleasant or unpleasant life events, thus exerting specifically, inconsistency between the valence of one’s
tight control over the to-be-remembered material. self-views and the valence of feedback (Abelson et al.,
The ratio of negative to positive information was 1968). Mnemic neglect, in particular, may reflect
equal. In addition, the relevance of self versus other processing of information whose valence is incon-
memories was taken into consideration: The same sistent with the valence of self-conceptions. Most
information was self-referent in one condition and participants have a positive self-concept (Ogilvie,
other-referent in another condition. More impor- 1987; Schwartz, 1986). Hence, they recall negative
tant, the research addressed the issue of whether feedback poorly because it is inconsistent with their
mnemic neglect is due to expectancies (Sedikides & self-views. This alternative explanation leads to an
Green, 2004, Experiment 1). interesting prediction. Inconsistency will also drive
All participants received hypothetical behavioral mnemic neglect among participants with a negative
feedback. However, the referent of the feedback var- self-concept. These participants will recall positive
ied. A quarter of the participants received feedback feedback poorly, because it is inconsistent with their
about themselves, and another quarter about Chris. self-views.
The third quarter of participants received feedback An experiment (Sedikides & Green, 2004, Exper-
about a person described in glowing terms, such iment 2) tested whether feedback inconsistency
as extraordinarily trustworthy and kind (glowing (behaviors that are inconsistent with the self-view)
Chris condition). The fourth quarter of participants or feedback negativity (behaviors that are negative
received feedback about a close friend. Pretest had regardless of whether they are consistent or incon-
established that participants held the most positive sistent with the self-view) drives mnemic neglect.
expectancies for glowing Chris, considering him or A pretest identified two groups of participants: those
her as most likely to enact positive behaviors and with positive self-views (i.e., trustworthy, kind) and
least likely to enact negative behaviors. Expectancies those with negative self-views (i.e., untrustworthy,
for close friend and self were virtually identical, and unkind). These participants were then brought
they were both more positive than expectancies for in the laboratory and exposed to the usual mne-
(mere) Chris. If expectancies constituted a sufficient mic neglect paradigm. The inconsistency alterna-
explanation for mnemic neglect, then the effect tive would predict that participants with positive
would be more strongly evident in the glowing Chris self-views would recall poorly untrustworthy and
than the self condition, and it would be equally unkind behaviors, whereas participants with nega-
strong in the close friend and self conditions. This tive self-views would recall poorly trustworthy and
was not the case. Participants evidenced the most kind behaviors. The results ran contrary to this alter-
neglect in the self condition, followed by the friend native. All participants, regardless of the valence of
condition, and then by the glowing Chris and Chris their self-conception, manifested mnemic neglect.
conditions (which did not differ significantly). That is, even individuals who regarded themselves
These findings were conceptually replicated by as untrustworthy or unkind recalled poorly untrust-
Newman, Nibert, and Winer (2009). In a separate worthy or unkind behaviors. This is additional

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
evidence that feedback negativity (i.e., self-threat) Seligman, & Vaillant, 1998), and poorer athletic, aca-
underlies mnemic neglect. In conclusion, incon- demic, and work performance (Peterson & Barrett,
sistency between the valence of one’s self-views and 1987; Seligman, Nolen-Hoeksema, Thornton, &
the valence of feedback, albeit relevant to autobio- Thornton, 1990). The positive association between
graphical recall (Gramzow & Willard, 2006), can- the SSB and psychological health has been found not
not account singly for mnemic neglect and more only in Western culture but also in East-Asian culture
generally selective self-memory. (China; Anderson, 1999).
The BTAE is also strongly linked to psychological
summary health. For example, the BTAE is positively related to
As with the SSB and the BTAE, nonmotivational indices of thriving (e.g., subjective well-being, pur-
explanations for selective self-memory are not par- pose in life, positive relations, self-acceptance), posi-
ticularly persuasive. Differential expectancies and tively related to resources (optimism, extraversion,
inconsistency between information valence and self- self-esteem, family support), and negatively related
view valence cannot provide a satisfactory account to indices of distress (e.g., loneliness, depression,
for poor recall of negative, central, self-referent anxiety) (Brown, 1991, 1998; Marshall & Brown,
feedback. Instead, the threat potential of such feed- 2007; Taylor, Lerner, Sherman, Sage, & McDowell,
back, including research on self-affirmation, can. 2003a). Similar patterns have been obtained in sev-
The extant evidence points to mnemic neglect as a eral East-Asian cultures such as China (Brown & Cai,
valid signature of the self-protection motive. 2009; Cai, Wu, & Brown, 2009; O’Mara, Gaertner,
But is self-threatening feedback always recalled Sedikides, Zhou, & Liu, 2010), Japan (Kobayashi
poorly? Research on trauma would seem to indicate & Brown, 2003), Korea (Chang, Sanna, & Yang,
that it is not: Traumatic events are well remembered 2003), Taiwan (Gaertner et al., 2008), and Singapore
(Berntsen, 2001; McNally, 2003). Such events, (Kurman & Sriram, 1997). In addition, longitudinal
though, are extreme, and event extremity is associ- studies, in Western and non-Western culture, indi-
ated with superior recall (Thompson, Skowronski, cate that the BTAE promotes subsequent psychologi-
Larsen, & Betz, 1996). And yet event valence cal health under adverse conditions (Bonanno, Field,
predicts recall independently of event extremity Kovacevic, & Kaltman, 2002; Bonanno, Rennicke,
(Thompson et al., 1996, Chapter 4). Finally, in the & Dekel, 2005; Gupta & Bonanno, 2010; Zucker-
mnemic neglect paradigm, behavioral feedback was man & O’Loughlin, 2006). Moreover, the BTAE
moderate rather than extreme (Sedikides & Green, serves a stress-buffering function: As a response to
2000, pilot studies). Selective self-memory, then, stress, the BTAE is related to lower cardiovascular
is applicable to the domain of mild, as opposed to response, more rapid cardiovascular recovery, and
extreme, feedback or events. lower baseline cortisol level (Taylor, Lerner, Sherman,
Sage, & McDowell, 2003b).
What Are Self-Enhancement and Finally, selective self-memory in autobiographi-
Self-Protection Good For? cal recall is also associated with psychological health.
Self-enhancement and self-protection strivings For example, selective self-memory is related to lack
have functional advantages for the individual. Next of dysphoria (Walker, Skowronski, Gibbons, Vogl,
we will consider two critical domains of functional- & Thompson, 2003), reduced depression (Williams
ity: psychological health and psychological interests. et al., 2007), a future orientation (Brunson, Wheeler,
& Walker, 2010), social connectedness or better
Psychological Health interpersonal relations (Wildschut, Sedikides, Arndt,
The SSB is linked to a variety of psychological & Routledge, 2006), felt continuity between one’s
health benefits. For example, the SSB is related to past and one’s present (Sedikides, Wildschut, Gaert-
positive mood (McFarland & Ross, 1982) and high ner, Routledge, & Arndt, 2008), perceptions of life
subjective well-being (Rizley, 1978), improved prob- as meaningful (Routledge et al., 2011), and reduced
lem solving (Isen & Means, 1983), reduced depres- existential anxiety (Juhl, Routledge, Arndt, Sedikides,
sion (Abramson & Alloy, 1981), better immune & Wildschut, 2010). Relatedly, selective self-memory
functioning (Taylor et al., 2000), and lower mortal- is linked to fewer symptoms of psychopathology and
ity and morbidity longitudinally (Peterson & Selig- better psychological health over time (Bonanno,
man, 1987). On the other hand, a weak or absent Keltner, Holen, & Horowitz, 1995; Bonanno,
SSB is related to depression (Sweeney, Anderson, & Znoj, Siddique, & Horowitz, 1999; Newton &
Bailey, 1986), deteriorating physical health (Peterson, Contrada, 1992). In conclusion, self-enhancement

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


and self-protection strivings are associated with, or Test) and initial depression. Students prone to SSB
promote, psychological health. were more likely to have specific academic goals and
to make use of academic advising.
Psychological Interests The BTAE is similarly implicated in the facilita-
Psychological interests include love/security, social tion of psychological interests. Taylor et al. (2003a)
status, and popularity, as well as skills and abilities showed that the BTAE is positively related to active
(e.g., musicality, athleticism, intelligence). Interests coping, positive reframing, planning, achievement,
are hierarchically organized from the general (e.g., mastery, and personal growth. In addition, Wright
being a good student, being a good friend) to the spe- (2000) demonstrated that undergraduate students
cific (e.g., performing well on a task, providing sup- who are more likely to manifest the BTAE (assessed
port to a friend in need) ones. Furthermore, interests in the beginning of the semester) achieved higher
can entail private matters (e.g., meeting one’s personal grades during the semester compared to students
standards) or public matters (e.g., meeting organiza- less likely to manifest the BTAE. Moreover, students
tional standards) and can extend to close relations who exaggerate reporting of their grade point average
or important groups. Finally, interests can be nega- perform better than those who do not (Gramzow,
tive or positive. Negative interests include matters 2011). In general, the BTAE is associated with work-
that individuals wish to circumvent or shun (e.g., ing harder and longer on tasks (Taylor & Brown,
relationship breakup, achievement failure), whereas 1988) and with performing better on tasks (Armor
positive interest include matters that individuals wish & Taylor, 2003).
to possess or attain (e.g., two-story house, managerial Finally, selective self-memory in autobiograph-
position) (Alicke & Sedikides, 2009). ical recall is also involved in the promotion of
A vital function of self-enhancement and self- psychological interests. Such memory has approach
protection is the pursuit of psychological interests rather than avoidance consequences (Stephan et al.,
(Alicke & Sedikides, 2009). This pursuit is carried out 2011; Walker & Skowronski, 2009) and, as such,
through either primary or secondary means. (These it can motivate individuals to engage and persist in
constructs correspond to notions of primary and sec- goal pursuit (Sedikides & Hepper, 2009; Walker
ondary control; Rothbaum, Weisz, & Snyder, 1982.) & Skowronski, 2009). Indeed, forms of selective
Primary means refer to changing an objective state self-memory have been found to be associated
of affairs by assuming instrumental action. In that with resilience (Coifman, Bonanno, Ray, & Gross,
capacity, self-enhancement entails effective action 2007), improved coping following traumatic life
that promotes oneself and one’s prospects. Secondary events (Janoff-Bulman, 1992), and, in general, the
means refers to psychological mechanisms that regu- implementation of active coping strategies in times
late events by altering how one perceives or interprets of stress (Langens & Moerth, 2003) and in attempt-
them. In that capacity, self-protection entails effec- ing to master life challenges (Walker & Skowronski,
tive intervention that obviates failing below one’s 2009).
standards. Self-enhancement and self-protection,
then, contribute effectively to the successful pursuit summary
of psychological interests of the effective avoidance A psychological health and psychological inter-
of harm to those interests. ests analysis addresses squarely the issue of why
The three self-enhancement and self-protection people self-enhance and self-protect. They do not
strivings serve psychological interests. Let us first do so for a whim, or just to feel good, or for short-
consider the SSB. Seligman et al. (1990) examined lived impression management purposes. Rather,
the role of the SSB in predicting athletic perfor- they do so, and they do so persistently, because self-
mance. They found that varsity swimmers prone to enhancement and self-protection strivings confer
the SSB (assessed at the start of the season) performed both momentary and long-term benefits (i.e., ways
better at sporting competitions than swimmers not in which psychological health and psychological
prone to the SSB. Additionally, Peterson and Bar- interests are advanced) and deter both momentary
rett (1987) reported that undergraduate students and long-term harms (i.e., ways in which psycho-
prone to the SSB (assessed at the beginning of their logical health interests are regressed or thwarted).
first year at university) received higher grades during
their freshman year compared to students not prone Conclusions
to the SSB. This pattern held after controlling for In his An Outline of Intellectual Rubbish (1943),
initial ability (measured by the Scholastic Aptitude Bertrand Russell was duly impressed by the influence

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
of motives on human judgment. “Man is a rational Future Directions
animal—so at least I have been told. [ . . . ] I have There are several issues in need of further empiri-
looked diligently for evidence in favor of this state- cal attention. We will briefly touch upon four of
ment, but so far I have not had the good fortune to them. First, what is the interplay between the two
come across it [ . . . ],” he exclaimed in wonder (p. 73). motives? Although self-enhancement and self-
We have focused in this chapter on two self-evaluation protection are occasionally treated as polar ends of
motives that might have confounded Russell, self- a single dimension, the empirical evidence suggests
enhancement and self-protection. that a lot will be gained if they are treated separately
We defined self-enhancement as the desire and (Elliot & Mapes, 2005). Yet the relation between
preference for maximizing the positivity of one’s the two motives is complex. They can operate inde-
self-views, and we defined self-protection as the pendently, one motive may facilitate the other, or
desire and preference for minimizing the negativ- one motive may impede the other. Second, and
ity of one’s self-view. We argued that the tendency relatedly, what is the interplay between implicit
to exalt one’s virtues and make light of one’s weak- and explicit self-enhancement and self-protection?
nesses, relative to impartial criteria, manifests itself In particular, what is the relation between implicit
in a variety of strivings. Due to space limitations, and explicit self-enhancement and self-protection
we restricted our discussion to three key strivings: strategies (Arndt & Goldenberg, 2011) or between
the SSB (crediting the self for successes but blam- implicit and explicit self-esteem (Gregg & Sedikides,
ing others for failures), the BTAE (considering the 2010)? Third, what is the interplay between the
self superior to others), and selective self-memory self-enhancement and self-protection motives on
(disadvantageous recall for negative feedback). the one hand and other self-evaluation motives on
Although we acknowledged that cognition and the other? These other motives are self-assessment
motivation are closely intertwined, we proceeded (i.e., pursuit of accurate self-knowledge; Gregg,
to make a case for the motivational underpinnings Sedikides, & Gebauer, 2011), self-improvement
of these strivings. We aimed to provide evidence (i.e., pursuit of one’s betterment; Sedikides & Hep-
that self-enhancement and self-protection strivings per, 2009), and self-verification (i.e., pursuit of self-
cannot be exclusively accounted for by nonmoti- confirmation; Swann, Rentfrow, & Guinn, 2003).
vational (i.e., information processing) factors. The Finally, what are the boundary conditions—both
nonmotivational explanations of expectations and situational demands and individual differences—
impression management were not deemed neces- that constrain self-enhancement or self-protection
sary for the occurrence of the SSB. Likewise, ego- (Gramzow, 2011)? And what are the intrapersonal
centrism, focalism, and individuated-entity versus and interpersonal consequences of such constraints
aggregate comparisons were not deemed necessary upon motive emergence or manifestation? These and
for the occurrence of the BTAE. And similarly, dif- other issues are worth exploring. As La Rouchefou-
ferential expectancies and inconsistency between cauld (1678/1827) prophetically noted, “Whatever
self-view valence and feedback were not deemed nec- discoveries have been made in the land of self-love,
essary for the occurrence of selective self-memory. In many territories remain to be discovered.”
contrast, evidence from research on self-threat and
self-affirmation testifies to the motivational under- References
pinnings of the strivings. The SSB, BTAE, and Abelson, R, Aronson, E., McGuire, W., Newcomb, T.,
Rosenberg, M., & Tannenbaum, P. (Eds.). (1968). The cog-
selective self-memory are driven, in part, by the self- nitive consistency theories: A source book. Chicago, IL: Rand
enhancement and self-protection motives. McNally.
We drew to a conclusion by asking why individ- Abramson, L. Y., & Alloy, L. B. (1981). Depression, nondepres-
uals self-enhance and self-protect. A partial answer sion, and cognitive illusions: Reply to Schwartz. Journal of
lies in the functionality of self-enhancement and self- Experimental Psychology, 110, 436–447.
Albery, I. P., & Messer, D. (2005). Comparative optimism about
protection strivings: They accrue benefits pertaining health and nonhealth events in 8- and 9-year old children.
to psychological health and psychological interests. Health Psychology, 24, 316–320.
Self-enhancement and self-protection strivings are Alicke, M. (1985). Global self-evaluation as determined by the
associated with, or confer, a host of psychologi- desirability and controllability of trait adjectives. Journal of
cal health advantages, and they advance a host of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 1621–1630.
Alicke, M., & Govorun, O. (2005). The better-than-average
psychological interests. Mild self-enhancement and effect. In M. D. Alicke, D. A. Dunning, & J. I. Krueger
self-protection continue to be markers of psycho- (Eds.), The self in social judgment (pp. 85–106). New York:
logical health. Psychology Press.

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


Alicke, M. D., Klotz, M. L., Breitenbecher, D. L., Yurak, T. J., & Bloom, A. (Trans.). (1991). The republic of Plato (2nd ed.). New
Vredenburg, D. S. (1995). Personal contact, individuation, York: Basic Books.
and the better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Bonanno, G. A., Field, N. P., Kovacevic, A., & Kaltman, S. (2002).
Social Psychology, 68, 804–825. Self-enhancement as a buffer against extreme adversity: Civil
Alicke, M., & Sedikides, C. (2009). Self-enhancement and War in Bosnia and traumatic loss in the United States. Person-
self-protection: What they are and what they do. European ality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 184–196.
Review of Social Psychology, 20, 1–48. Bonanno, G. A., Keltner, D., Holen, A., & Horowitz, M. J.
Alicke, M. D., & Sedikides, C. (2011a). Self-enhancement and (1995). When avoiding unpleasant emotions might not be
self-protection: Historical overview and conceptual frame- such a bad thing: Verbal-autonomic response dissociation
work. In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides (Eds.), The handbook and midlife conjugal bereavement. Journal of Personality and
of self-enhancement and self-protection (pp. 1–19). New York: Social Psychology, 69, 975–989.
Guilford Press. Bonanno, G. A., Rennicke, C., & Dekel, S. (2005). Self-
Alicke, M. D., & Sedikides, C. (2011b). (Eds.). The handbook enhancement among high-exposure survivors of the Septem-
of self-enhancement and self-protection. New York: Guilford ber 11th terrorist attacks: Resilience or social maladjustment?
Press. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 984–998.
Alicke, M. D., Vredenburg, D. S., Hiatt, M., & Govorun, O. Bonanno, G. A., Znoj, H., Siddique, H. I., & Horowitz, M.
(2001). The “better than myself effect.” Motivation and Emo- J. (1999). Verbal autonomic dissociation and adaptation to
tion, 25, 7–22. midlife conjugal loss: A follow-up at 25 months. Cognitive
Allison, S. T., Messick, D. M., & Goethals, G. R. (1989). Therapy and Research, 23, 605–624.
On being better but not smarter than others: The Muhammad Brown, J. D. (1986). Evaluations of self and others: Self-
Ali effect. Social Cognition, 7, 275–296. enhancement biases in social judgments. Social Cognition, 4,
Allport, G. W. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. 353–376.
New York: Holt. Brown, J. D. (1991). Accuracy and bias in self-knowledge.
Anderson, C. A. (1999). Attributional style, depression, and In C. R. Snyder & D. F. Forsyth (Eds.), Handbook of social
loneliness: A cross-cultural comparison of American and and clinical psychology: The health perspective (pp. 158–178).
Chinese students. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, New York: Pergamon Press.
25, 482–499. Brown, J. D. (1998). The self. New York: McGraw-Hill.
Andrews, F. M., & Whitey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well- Brown, J. D. (2003). The self-enhancement motive in collectivis-
being. New York: Plenum Press. tic cultures: The rumors of my death have been greatly exag-
Arkin, R. M., Appelmen, A. J., & Burger, J. M. (1980). Social gerated. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 603–605.
anxiety, self-presentation, and the self-serving bias in causal Brown, J. D. (2010). Across the (not so) great divide: Cultural
attribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38, similarities in self-evaluative processes. Social and Personality
23–25. Psychology Compass, 4, 318–330.
Arkin, R. M., Cooper, H. M., & Kolditz, T. A. (1980). A statisti- Brown, J. D. (2011). Understanding the better than average effect:
cal review of the literature concerning the self-serving attri- Motives matter. Unpublished manuscript, University of
bution bias in interpersonal influence situations. Journal of Washington.
Personality, 48, 435–448. Brown, J. D., & Cai, H. (2009). Self-esteem and trait impor-
Armor, D. A., & Taylor, S. E. (2003). The effects of mindset tance moderate cultural differences in self-evaluations.
on behavior: Self-regulation in deliberative and implemental Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41, 116–122.
frames of mind. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, Brown, J. D., Collins, R. L., & Schmidt, G. W. (1988). Self-
86–95. esteem and direct versus indirect forms of self-enhancement.
Arndt, A., & Goldenberg, J. L. (2011). When self-enhancement Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 445–453.
drives health decisions: Insights from a terror manage- Brown, J. D., & Gallagher, F. M. (1992). Coming to terms with
ment health model. In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides failure: Private self-enhancement and public self-effacement.
(Eds.), The handbook of self-enhancement and self-protection Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 3–22.
(pp. 380–398). New York: Guilford Press. Brown, J. D., & Kobayashi, C. (2002). Self-enhancement in
Baumeister, R. F., & Cairns, K. J. (1992). Repression and self- Japan and America. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 5,
presentation: When audiences interfere with self-deceptive 145–168.
strategies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, Brunot, S., & Sanitioso, R. B. (2004). Motivational influences
851–862 on the quality of memories: Recall of general autobiographi-
Bentham, J. (1982). An introduction to the principles of morals cal memories related to desired attributes. European Journal
and legislation (J. H. Burns & H. L. A. Harts, Eds.). London, of Social Psychology, 34, 627–635.
England: Methuem. (Original work published 1789). Brunson, C. A., Wheeler, D., & Walker, W. R. (2011). Testing
Berntsen, D. (2001). Involuntary memories of emotional two alternative explanations for a fading affect bias in autobio-
events: Do memories of traumas and extremely happy graphical memory. Manuscript under review.
events differ? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 135–158. Cai, H., Wu, Q., & Brown, J. D. (2009). Is self-esteem a uni-
Blaine, B., & Crocker, J. (1993). Self-esteem and self-serving bias versal need? Evidence from the People’s Republic of China.
in reactions to positive and negative events: An integrative Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 12, 104–120.
review. In R. F. Baumeister (Ed.), Self-esteem: The puzzle of Campbell, K. W., & Sedikides, C. (1999). Self-threat magnifies
low self-regard (pp. 55–85). New York: Plenum. the self-serving bias: A meta-analytic integration. Review of
Blanton, H., Pelham, B. W., DeHart, T., & Carvallo, M. (2001). General Psychology, 3, 23–43.
Overconfidence as dissonance reduction. Journal of Experi- Champers, J. R., Windschitl, P. D., & Suls, J. (2003). Egocentrism,
mental Social Psychology, 37, 373–385 event frequency, and comparative optimism: When what

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
happens frequently is “more likely to happen to me.” Field, D. (1997). “Looking back, what period of your life brought
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 1343–1356. you the most satisfaction?” International Journal of Aging and
Chang, E. C., Sanna, L. J., & Yang, K. (2003). Optimism, pes- Human Development, 45, 169–194.
simism, affectivity, and psychological adjustment in US and Forsyth, D. R., & Schlenker, B. R. (1985). Attributing the causes
Korea: A test of mediation model. Personality and Individual of group performance: Effects of performance quality, task
Differences, 34, 1195–1208. importance, and future testing. Journal of Personality, 45,
Coifman, K. G., Bonanno, G. A., Ray, R. D., & Gross, J. J. 220–236.
(2007). Does repressive coping promote resilience? Affective- Freedman, J. (1978). Happy people: What happiness is, who has it,
autonomic response discrepancy during bereavement. Jour- and why. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 745–758. Freud, A. (1946). The ego and the mechanisms of defense.
College Board. (1976–1977). Student descriptive questionnaire. New York: International Universities Press. (Originally work
Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service. published in 1936).
Corr, P., & Gray, J. (1996). Attributional style as a personality Freud, S. (1961a). Three essays on the theory of sexuality.
factor in insurance sales performance in the UK. Journal of In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the
Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 69, 83–87. complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 19,
Crary, W. G. (1966). Reactions to incongruent self-experiences. pp. 3–66). London, England: Hogarth Press. (Original work
Journal of Consulting Psychology, 30, 246–252. published 1905).
Creswell, J. D., Welch, W. T., Taylor, S. E., Sherman, D. K., Freud, S. (1961b). Instincts and their vicissitudes. In J. Strachey
Gruenewald, T. L., & Mann, T. (2005). Affirmation of per- (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete works of
sonal values buffers neuroendocrine and psychological stress Sigmund Freud (Vol. 14, pp. 111–142). London, England:
responses. Psychological Science, 16, 846–851. Hogarth Press. (Original work published in 1915).
Critcher, C. R., Helzer, E. G., & Dunning, D. (2011). Self- Freud, S. (1961c). The ego and the id. In J. Strachey (Ed. &
enhancement via redefinition: Defining social concepts to Trans.), The standard edition of the complete works of Sigmund
ensure positive views of self. In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides Freud (Vol. 19, pp. 12–66). London, England: Hogarth
(Eds.), The handbook of self-enhancement and self-protection Press. (Original work published in 1923).
(pp. 69–91). New York: Guilford Press. Freud, S. (1961d). Inhibitions, symptoms, and anxiety.
Crocker, J., Voelkl, K., Testa, M., & Major, B. (1991). Social In J. Strachey (Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the
stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambigu- complete works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 20, pp. 77–178).
ity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 218–228. London, England: Hogarth Press. (Original work published
Cross, P. (1977). Not can but will college teachers be improved? in 1926).
New Directions for Higher Education, 17, 1–15. Gaertner, L., Sedikides, C., & Chang, K. (2008). On pancultural
Davison, W. P. (1983). The third-person effect in communica- self-enhancement: Well-adjusted Taiwanese self-enhance on
tion. Public Opinion Quarterly, 47, 1–15. personally-valued traits. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
D’Argembeau, A., & Van der Linden, M. (2008). Remem- 39, 463–477.
bering pride and shame: Self-enhancement and the Giladi, E. E., & Klar, Y. (2002). When standards are wide of
phenomenology of autobiographical memory. Memory, 16, the mark: Nonselective superiority and bias in comparative
538–547. judgments of objects and concepts. Journal of Experimental
De Michele, P. E., Gansneder, B., & Solomon, G. B. (1998). Psychology: General, 131, 538–551.
Success and failure attributions of wrestlers: Further evi- Gosling, S. D., John, O. P., Craik, K. H., & Robins, R. W.
dence of the self-serving bias. Journal of Sport Behavior, 21, (1998). Do people know how they behave? Self-reported
242–255. act frequencies compared with on-line codings by observers.
DeVellis, R. F., Holt, K., Renner, B. R., Blalock, S. J., Blanchard, Journal Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1337–1349.
L. W., Cook, H. L., . . . Harring, K. (1990). The relationship Gramzow, R. H. (2011). Academic exaggeration: Pushing self-
of social comparison to rheumatoid arthritis symptoms and enhancement boundaries. In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides
affect. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11, 1–18. (Eds.), The handbook of self-enhancement and self-protection
De Witt, N. W. (1973). Epicurus and his philosophy. Westport, (pp. 455–471). New York: Guilford Press.
CT: Greenwood Publishing Group. Gramzow, R. H., & Willard, G. (2006). Exaggerating current
Dunning, D., Heath, C., & Suls, J. M. (2004). Flawed and past performance: Motivated self-enhancement versus
self-assessment: Implications for health, education, and reconstructive memory. Personality and Social Psychology
the workplace. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, Bulletin, 32, 1114–1125.
69–106. Green, J. D., Pinter, B., & Sedikides, C. (2005). Mnemic
Dunning, D., Leuenberger, A., & Sherman, D. A. (1995). A new neglect and self-threat: Trait modifiability moderates
look at motivated inference: Are self-serving theories of suc- self-protection. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35,
cess a product of motivational forces? Journal of Personality 225–235.
and Social Psychology, 69, 58–68. Green, J. D., & Sedikides, C. (2004). Retrieval selectivity in the
Elliot, A. J., & Mapes, R. R. (2005). Approach-avoidance moti- processing of self-referent information: Testing the boundar-
vation and self-concept evaluation. In A. Tesser, J. V. Wood, ies of self-protection. Self and Identity, 3, 69–80.
& D. A. Stapel (Eds.), On building, defending and regulating Green, J. D., Sedikides, C., & Gregg, A. P. (2008). Forgotten
the self: A psychological perspective (pp. 171–196). New York: but not gone: The recall and recognition of self-threatening
Psychology Press. memories. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44,
Field, D. (1981). Retrospective reports by healthy intelligent 547–561.
elderly people of personal events of their adult lives. Interna- Green, J. D., Sedikides, C., Pinter, B., & Van Tongeren, D.
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 77–97. R. (2009). Two sides to self-protection: Self-improvement

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


strivings and feedback from close relationships eliminate Kenny, A. (1986). Rationalism, empiricism and idealism. Oxford,
mnemic neglect. Self and Identity, 8, 233–250. England: Oxford University Press.
Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., & Solomon, S. (1982). The self- Kernis, H. H., Cornell, D. P., Sun, C-R., Berry, A., &
serving attributional bias: Beyond self-presentation. Journal Harlow, T. (1993). There’s more to self-esteem than whether
of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 56–67. it’s high or low: The importance of stability of self-esteem.
Gregg, A. P., & Sedikides, C. (2010). Narcissistic fragility: Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 1190–1204.
Rethinking its links to explicit and implicit self-esteem. Self Klar, Y. (2002). Way beyond compare: Nonselective superiority
and Identity, 9, 142–161. and inferiority biases in judging randomly assigned group
Gregg, A. P., Sedikides, C., & Gebauer, J. E. (2011). Dynamics members relative to their peers. Journal of Experimental Social
of identity: Between self-enhancement and self-assessment. Psychology, 38, 331–351.
In S. J. Schwartz, K. Luyckx, & V. L. Vignoles (Eds.), Hand- Klar, Y., & Giladi, E. E. (1997). No one in my group can be
book of identity theory and research (Vol. 1, pp. 305–327). below the group’s average: A robust positivity bias in favor of
New York: Springer. anonymous peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Guenther, C. (2011). Self-affirmation and the better-than-average 73, 885–901.
effect. Manuscript submitted for publication, Creighton Kobayashi, C., & Brown, J. D. (2003). Self-esteem and self-
University. enhancement in Japan and America. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Guenther, C. L., & Alicke, M. D. (2010). Deconstructing the Psychology, 34, 567–580.
better-than-average effect. Journal of Personality and Social Kruger, J. (1999). Lake Wobegon be gone! The “below-average
Psychology, 99, 755–770. effect” and the egocentric nature of comparative ability
Gupta, S., & Bonanno, G. A. (2010). Trait self-enhancement as judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77,
a buffer against potentially traumatic events: A prospective 221–232.
study. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Kruger, J., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Actions, intentions, and self-
Policy, 2, 83–92. assessment: The road to self-enhancement is paved with
Haslam, N., Bain, P., Douge, L., Lee, M., & Bastian, B. (2005). good intentions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
More human than you: Attributing humanness to self 30, 328–339.
and others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98, Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge:
937–950. Cognitive and motivational biases. New York: Plenum Press.
Heady, B., & Wearing, A. (1988). The sense of relative superi- Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychologi-
ority—central to well-being. Social Indicators Research, 20, cal Bulletin, 108, 480–498.
497–516. Kurman, J. (2002). Measured cross-cultural differences in self-
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. enhancement and the sensitivity of the self-enhancement
New York: Wiley. measure to the modesty response. Cross-Cultural Research,
Hepper, E. G., Gramzow, R., & Sedikides, C. (2010). Individual 36, 73–95.
differences in self-enhancement and self-protection strategies: Kurman, J., & Sriram, N. (1997). Self-enhancement, general-
An integrative analysis. Journal of Personality, 78, 781–814. ity of self-evaluation, and affectivity in Israel and Singapore.
Hepper, E. G., Sedikides, C., & Cai, H. (in press). Self- Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 28, 421–441.
enhancement and self-protection strategies in China: Cul- Kwon, Y., Scheibe, S., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Tsai, J. L. &
tural expressions of a fundamental human motive. Journal of Carstensen, L. L. (2009). Replicating the positivity effect in
Cross-Cultural Psychology. picture memory in Koreans: Evidence for cross-cultural gen-
Hobbes, T. (1991). Leviathan. Cambridge, England: Cambridge eralizability. Psychology and Aging, 24, 748–754.
University Press. (Original work published 1651). La Rochefoucauld, F. (1827). Reflections: Or sentences and moral
House, W. C. (1980). Effects of knowledge that attributions will maxims (J. W. W. Bund & J. H. Friswell, Trans.). London,
be observed by others. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- England: Simpson, Low, Son, & Marston. (Original work
chology, 14, 528–545. published 1678).
Isen, A. M., & Means, B. (1983). The influence of positive Landau, J. D, & Gunter, B. C. (2009). “Don’t worry; you really
affect on decision-making strategies. Social Cognition, 2, will get over it”: Methodological investigations of the fading
18–31. affect bias. American Journal of Psychology, 122, 209–217.
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology (Vols. 1–2). New Langens, T. A., & Moerth, S. (2003). Repressive coping and the
York: Holt. use of passive and active coping strategies. Personality and
Janoff-Bulman, R. (1992). Shattered assumptions: Towards a new Individual Differences, 35, 461–473.
psychology of trauma. New York: Free Press. Loeb, L. (1981). From Descartes to Hume: Continental metaphysics
Juhl, J., Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Sedikides, C., & Wildschut, and the development of modern philosophy. Ithaca, NY: Cor-
T. (2010). Fighting the future with the past: On the death- nell University Press.
anxiety buffering function of nostalgia. Journal of Research in Macfarlane, A. (1978). The origins of English individualism: The
Personality, 44, 309–314. family, property, and social transition. New York: Cambridge
Karylowski, J. (1990). Social reference points and accessibility of University Press.
trait-related information in self-other comparisons. Journal Mandeville, B. (1705). The fable of the bees: or private vices, public
of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 975–983. benefits (Vol. 1). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.
Karylowski, J., & Skarzynska, K. (1992). Asymmetric self-other Marshall, M. A., & Brown, J. D. (2007). On the psychologi-
similarity judgments depend on priming self-knowledge. cal benefits of self-enhancement. In E. Chang (Ed.), Self-
Social Cognition, 10, 235–254. enhancement and self-criticism: Theory, research, and clinical
Kelley, H. H., & Michela, J. L. (1980). Attribution theory and implications (pp. 19–35). New York: American Psychological
research. Annual Review of Psychology, 31, 457–501. Association.

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
McFarland, C., & Ross, M. (1982). Impact of causal attributions Ogilvie, D. M. (1987). The undesired self: A neglected variable
on affective reactions to success and failure. Journal of Person- in personality research. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
ality and Social Psychology, 45, 937–946. chology, 52, 379–385.
McNally, R. J. (2003). Remembering trauma. Cambridge, MA: O’Mara, E. M., Gaertner, L., Sedikides, C., Zhou, X., & Liu, Y.
Belknap Press/Harvard University Press. (2010). An experimental test of the panculturality of self-
Mednick, S.A. (1962). The associative basis of the creative enhancement: Self-enhancement promotes psychological
process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232. well-being both in the West and the East. Manuscript sub-
Messick, D. M., Bloom, S., Boldizar, J. P., & Samuelson, C. D. mitted for pulbication, University of Dayton.
(1985). Why are we fairer than others? Journal of Experimen- Otten, W., & van der Pligt, J. (1996). Context effects in the
tal Social Psychology, 21, 480–500. measurement of comparative optimism in probability judg-
Mezulis, A. H., Abramson, L. Y., Hyde, J. S., & Hankin, B. ments. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 15, 80–101.
L. (2004). Is there a universal positive bias in attributions? Pahl, S., & Eiser, J. R. (2006). The focus effect and self-positivity
A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and in ratings of self-other similarity and difference. British Jour-
cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. nal of Social Psychology, 45, 107–116.
Psychological Bulletin, 130, 711–747. Pahl, S., & Eiser, J. R. (2007). How malleable is comparative
Mill, J. S. (2004). Utilitarianism. Adelaide, Australia: ebooks@ self-positivity? The effects of manipulating judgemental
Adelaide. Retrieved July 2011, from, http://etext.library. focus and accessibility. European Journal of Social Psychology,
adelaide.edu.au/m/mill/john_stuart/m645u/ (Original work 37, 617–627.
published 1863). Paulhus, D. L. (1993). Bypassing the will: The automatization of
Miller, D. T. (1976). Ego involvement and attributions for suc- affirmations. In D. M. Wegner & J. M. Pennebaker (Eds.),
cess and failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Handbook of mental control (pp. 573–587). Englewood Cliffs,
34, 901–906. NJ: Prentice Hall.
Miller, D. T. (1978). What constitutes a self-serving attributional Peterson, C., & Barrett, L. C. (1987). Explanatory style and aca-
bias? A reply to Bradley. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- demic performance among university freshmen. Journal of
chology, 36, 1221–1223. Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 603–607.
Miller, D. T., & Ross, M. (1975). Self-serving bias in the attribu- Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. (1987). Explanatory style and
tion of causality: Fact or fiction? Psychological Bulletin, 82, illness. Journal of Personality, 55, 237–265.
213–225. Peterson, C., Seligman, M. E., & Vaillant, G. E. (1998). Pes-
Miller, R. S., & Schlenker, B. R. (1985). Egotism in group mem- simistic explanatory style is a risk factor for physical illness:
bers: Public and private attributions of responsibility for A thirty-five-year longitudinal study. Journal of Personality,
group performance. Social Psychology Quarterly, 48, 85–89. 55, 23–27.
Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E. B., & Zeiss, A. R. (1976). Determi- Preston, C. E., & Harris, S. (1965). Psychology of drivers
nants of selective memory about the self. Journal of Consult- in traffic accidents. Journal of Applied Psychology, 49,
ing and Clinical Psychology, 44, 92–103. 284–288.
Moore, D. A. (2007). Not so above average after all: When Preuss, G. S., & Alicke, M. D. (2009). Everybody loves me: Self-
people believe they are worse than average and its implica- evaluations and metaperceptions of dating popularity. Per-
tions for theories of bias in social comparison. Organizational sonality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 937–950.
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102, 42–58. Pronin, E., Lin, D. Y., & Ross, L. (2002). The bias blind spot:
Moore, D. A., & Kim, T. G. (2003). Myopic social prediction Perceptions of bias in self versus others. Personality and Social
and the solo comparison effect. Journal of Personality and Psychology Bulletin, 3, 369–381.
Social Psychology, 85, 1121–1135. Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, J. (1987). Toward an integration
Murray, S. L., & Holmes, J. G. (1993). Seeing virtues in faults: of cognitive and motivational perspectives on social infer-
Negativity and the transformation of interpersonal narratives ence: A biased hypothesis-testing model. Advances in Experi-
in close relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- mental Social Psychology, 20, 297–340.
chology, 65, 707–722. Reeder, G. D., & Brewer, M. B. (1979). A schematic model of
Mullen, B., & Riordan, C. A. (1988). Self-serving attributions dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. Psycho-
for performance in naturalistic settings: A meta-analytic logical Review, 86, 61–79.
review. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 18, 3–22. Ritchie, T., Skowronski, J. J., Hartnett, J. Wells, B., &
Nesselroade, K. P., Beggan, J. K., & Allison, S. T. (1999). Posses- Walker, W. R. (2009). The fading affect bias in the context
sion enhancement in an interpersonal context: An extension of emotion activation level, mood, and personal theories of
of the mere ownership effect. Psychology and Marketing, 16, emotion change. Memory, 17, 428–444.
21–34. Rizley, R. (1978). Depression and distortion in the attribution of
Newman, L. S., Nibert, J. A., & Winer, E. S. (2009). Mnemic causality. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 32–48.
neglect is not an artifact of expectancy: The moderating role Roese, N. J., & Olson, J. M. (2007). Better, stronger, faster:
of defensive pessimism. European Journal of Social Psychology, Self-serving judgment, affect regulation, and the optimal
39, 477–486. vigilance hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2,
Newton, T. L., & Contrada, R. L. (1992). Repressive coping 124–141.
and verbal-autonomic dissociation: The influence of social Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person. Boston, MA:
context. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, Houghton Mifflin.
159–167. Ross, M., & Wilson, A. E. (2002). It feels like yesterday:
Nietzsche, F. (1972). Beyond good and evil (R. J. Hollongdale, Self-esteem, valence of personal past experiences, and judg-
Trans.). London, England: Penguin Books. (Original work ments of subjective distance. Journal of Personality and Social
published 1886). Psychology, 82, 792–803.

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


Ross, M., McFarland, C., & Fletcher, G. J. (1981). The effect of Sedikides, C., & Gebauer, J. E. (2010). Religiosity as self-
attitude on the recall of personal histories. Journal of Person- enhancement: A meta-analysis of the relation between
ality and Social Psychology, 40, 627–634. socially desirable responding and religiosity. Personality and
Rothbart, M., & Park, B. (1986). On the confirmability and Social Psychology Review, 14, 17–36.
disconfirmability of trait concepts. Journal of Personality and Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (2000). On the self-protective
Social Psychology, 50, 131–142. nature of inconsistency/negativity management: Using the
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R., & Snyder, S. S. (1982). Changing person memory paradigm to examine self-referent memory.
the world and changing the self: A two-process model of per- Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 906–922.
ceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (2004). What I don’t recall can’t
42, 5–37. hurt me: Information negativity versus information inconsis-
Routledge, C., Arndt, J., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Hart, C. M., tency as determinants of memorial self-defense. Social Cogni-
Juhl, J., & Vingerhoets, A., & Schlotz, W. (2011). The past tion, 22, 4–29.
makes the present meaningful: Nostalgia as an existential Sedikides, C., & Green, J. D. (2009). Memory as a self-protective
resource. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101, mechanism. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3,
638–652. 1055–1068.
Rusbult, C. E., Van Lange, P. A. M., Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Green, J. D., & Pinter, B. (2004). Self-protective
Yovetich, N. A., & Verette, J. (2000). Perceived superiority memory. In D. R. Beike, J. M. Lampinen, & D. A. Behrend
in close relationships: Why it exists and persists. Journal of (Eds.), The self and memory (pp. 161–179). Philadelphia, PA:
Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 521–545. Psychology Press.
Russell, B. (1943). An outline of intellectual rubbish. Girard, KS: Sedikides, C., & Gregg, A. P. (2003). Portraits of the self.
Haldeman-Julius. In M. A. Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), Sage handbook of
Sande, G., Goethals, G., & Radloff, C. (1988). Perceiving one’s social psychology (pp. 110–138). London, England: Sage
own traits and others’: The multifaceted self. Journal of Per- Publications.
sonality and Social Psychology, 54, 13–20. Sedikides, C., & Gregg, A. P. (2008). Self-enhancement: Food for
Sanitioso, R., Kunda, Z., & Fong, G. T. (1990). Motivated thought. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 102–116.
recruitment of autobiographical memories. Journal of Person- Sedikides, C., Gregg, A. P., & Hart, C. M. (2007). The impor-
ality and Social Psychology, 59, 229–241. tance of being modest. In C. Sedikides & S. Spencer
Sanitioso, B. R., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2006). Motivated self- (Eds.), The self: Frontiers in social psychology (pp. 163–184).
perception and perceived ease in recall of autobiographical New York: Psychology Press.
memories. Self and Identity, 5, 73–84. Sedikides, C., & Hepper, E. G. D. (2009). Self-improvement.
Schopenhauer, A. (1996). The world as will and representation Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 899–917.
(Vol. 2; E. F. J. Payne, Trans.) New York: Dover Publications. Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. (1995). The multiply motivated self.
(Original work published 1844). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1330–1335.
Schrauf, R. W., & Hoffman, L. (2007). The effects of revision- Sedikides, C., & Strube, M. J. (1997). Self-evaluation: To thine
ism on remembered emotion: The valence of older, volun- own self be good, to thine own self be sure, to thine own self
tary immigrants’ pre-migration autobiographical memories. be true, and to thine own self be better. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.),
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 895–913. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29, 209–269.
Schulz, R., & Fritz, S. (1987). Origins of stereotypes of the New York: Academic Press.
elderly: An experimental study of the self-other discrepancy. Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., Gaertner, L., Routledge, C., &
Experimental Aging Research, 13, 189–195. Arndt, J. (2008). Nostalgia as enabler of self-continuity.
Schwartz, R. M. (1986). The internal dialogue: On the asymme- In F. Sani (Ed.), Self-continuity: Individual and collective
try between positive and negative coping thoughts. Cognitive perspectives (pp. 227–239). New York: Psychology Press.
Therapy and Research, 10, 591–605. Seligman, M. E., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Thornton, N., & Thorn-
Sedikides, C. (1993). Assessment, enhancement, and verification ton, K. M. (1990). Explanatory style as a mechanism of
determinants of the self-evaluation process. Journal of Person- disappointing athletic performance. Psychological Science, 1,
ality and Social Psychology, 65, 317–338. 143–146.
Sedikides, C. (2012). Self-protection. In M. R. Leary & J. P.Tangney Sherman, D. K., & Hartson, K. A. (2011). Reconciling self-
(Eds.), Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed.) (pp. 327–353). protection with self-criticism: Self-affirmation theory.
New York: Guilford Press. In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides (Eds.), The handbook of self-
Sedikides, C., Campbell, W. K., Reeder, G., & Elliot, A. J. enhancement and self-protection (pp. 128–151). New York:
(1998). The self-serving bias in relational context. Journal of Guilford Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 378–386. Sherman, D. K., & Kim, H. S. (2005). Is there an “I” in “Team”?
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Toguchi, Y. (2003). Pancultural The role of the self in group-serving judgments. Journal of
self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 108–120.
ogy, 84, 60–70. Skowronski, J. J. (2011). The positivity bias and the fading affect
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Vevea, J. L. (2005). Pancultural self- bias in autobiographical memory: A self-motives perspective.
enhancement reloaded: A meta-analytic reply to Heine (2005). In M. D. Alicke & C. Sedikides (Eds.), The handbook of self-
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 539–551. enhancement and self-protection (pp. 211–231). New York:
Sedikides, C., Gaertner, L., & Vevea, J. L. (2007). Inclusion of Guilford Press.
theory-relevant moderators yield the same conclusions as Skowronski, J. J., Betz, A. L., Thompson, C. P., & Shannon, L.
Sedikides, Gaertner, and Vevea (2005): A meta-analytic reply (1991). Social memory in everyday life: Recall of self-events
to Heine, Kitayama, and Hamamura (2007). Asian Journal of and other-events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
Social Psychology, 10, 59–67. 60, 831–843.

s ed i ki d es , a li c ke 
Stephan, E., Sedikides, C., Wildschut, T., & Routledge, C., Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., Gibbons, J. A., Vogl, R. J., &
Zhou, X., Kuang, L., & Vingerhoets, J. J. M. (2011). Nostal- Thompson, C. P. (2003). On the emotions accompanying
gia regulates avoidance and approach motivation. Unpublished autobiographical memory: Dysphoria disrupts the fading
manuscript, University of Southampton. affect bias. Cognition and Emotion, 17, 703–724.
Stewart, A. E. (2005). Attributions of responsibility for motor Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., & Thompson, C. P. (2003).
vehicle crashes. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 37, Life is pleasant—and memory helps to keep it that way.
681–688. Review of General Psychology, 7, 203–210.
Swann, W. B., Jr., Rentfrow, P. J., & Guinn, J. (2003). S Weary, G. (1979). Self-serving biases in attribution process:
elf-verification: The search for coherence. In M. Leary A re-examination of the fact or fiction question. Journal of
& J. Tangney (Eds.), Handbook of self and identity Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 56–71.
(pp. 367–383). New York: Guilford. Weary Bradley, G. (1978). Self-serving attributional biases:
Sweeney, P. D., Anderson, K., & Bailey, S. (1986). Attributional Perceptual or response distortions? Journal of Personality and
style in depression: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Person- Social Psychology, 37, 1418–1420.
ality and Social Psychology, 50, 974–991. Weiner, B. (1972). Theories of motivation: From mechanism to
Tatarkiewicz, W. (1976). Analysis of happiness. Warsaw, Poland: cognition. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
Polish Scientific Publishers. Weiner, P. S. (1964). Personality correlates of self-appraisal in
Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: four-year-old children. Genetic Psychologic Monographs, 70,
A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psycho- 329–365.
logical Bulletin, 103, 193–210. Weinstein, N. D. (1980). Optimistic biases about personal risks.
Taylor, S. E., Klein, L. C., Lewis, B. P., Gruenewald, T. L., Science, 246, 1232–1233.
Gurung, R. A. R., & Updegraff, J. A. (2000). Biobehavioral Wiesenfeld, B., Brockner, J., Petzall, B., Wolf, R., & Bailey, J.
responses to stress in females: Tend-and-befriend, not fight- (2001). Stress and coping among layoff survivors: A self-
or-flight. Psychological Review, 107, 411–429. affirmation analysis. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 1, 15–34.
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & Wildschut, T., Sedikides, C., Arndt, J., & Routledge, C. D.
McDowell, N. K. (2003a). Portrait of the self-enhancer: (2006). Nostalgia: Content, triggers, functions. Journal of
Well-adjusted and well-liked or maladjusted and friendless? Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 975–993.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 165–176. Williams, E. F., & Gilovich, T. (2008). Do people really believe
Taylor, S. E., Lerner, J. S., Sherman, D. K., Sage, R. M., & they are above average? Journal of Experimental Social Psychol-
McDowell, N. K. (2003b). Are self-enhancing cogni- ogy, 44, 1121–1128.
tions associated with healthy or unhealthy biological Williams, J. M. G., Barnhofer, T., Crane, C., Hermans, D.,
profiles? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, Raes, F., Watkins, E., & Dalgeish, T. (2007). Autobiographi-
605–615. cal memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological
Tennen, H., & Herzberger, S. (1987). Depression, self-esteem, Bulletin, 133, 122–148.
and the absence of self-protective attributional biases. Journal Wilson, A. E., Smith, M. D., Ross, H. S., & Ross, M. (2004).
of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 72–80. Young children’s personal accounts of their sibling dis-
Tetlock, P. E., & Levin, A. (1982). Attribution bias: On the putes. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 50, 39–60.
inconclusiveness of the cognition-motivation debate. Journal Windschitl, P. D., Kruger, J., & Sims, E. N. (2003). The influ-
of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 68–88. ence of egocentrism and focalism on people’s optimism and
Thompson, C. P., Skowronski, J. J., Larsen, S. F., & Betz, A. competition: When what affects us equally affects me more.
(1996). Autobiographical memory: Remembering what and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 389–408.
remembering when. New York: Erlbaum. Wright, S. S. (2000). Looking at the self in a rose-colored mirror:
Van Lange, P. A. M., Rusbult, C. E., Semin-Goossens, A., Unrealistically positive self-views and academic performance.
Goerts, C. A, & Stalpers, M. (1999). Being better than oth- Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 19, 451–462.
ers but otherwise perfectly normal: Perceptions of uniqueness Wyer, R. S., & Frey, D. (1983). The effects of feedback about self
and similarity in close relationships. Personal Relationships, 6, and others on the recall and judgments of feedback-relevant
269–289. information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19,
Van Lange, P. A. M., & Sedikides, C. (1998). Being more 540–559.
honest but not necessarily more intelligent than others: Yarrow, M. R., Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R. V. (1970). Recol-
Generality and explanations for the Muhammad Ali lections of childhood: A study of the retrospective method.
effect. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 675–680. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development,
Van Lange, P. A. M., Taris, T. W., & Vonk, R. (1997). The social 35, 1–83.
psychology of social psychologists: Self-enhancing beliefs Zauberman, G., Ratner, R. K., & Kim, K. (2009). Memories
about own research. European Journal of Social Psychology, as assets: Strategic memory protection in choice over time.
27, 675–685. Journal of Consumer Research, 35, 715–728.
Wagenaar, W. A., & Groeneweg, J. (1990). The memory of Zuckerman, M. (1979). Attribution of success and failure
concentration camp survivors. Applied Cognitive Psychology, revisited, or: The motivational bias is alive and well in attri-
4, 77–87. butional theory. Journal of Personality, 47, 245–287.
Walker, W. R., & Skowronski, J. J. (2009). The Fading Affect Zuckerman, M., & O’Loughlin, R. E. (2006). Self-enhancement
Bias . . . .But what the hell is it for? Applied Cognitive Psychol- by social comparison: A prospective analysis. Personality and
ogy, 23, 1122–1136. Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 751–760.

 sel f- enhancement and self- protect i on moti ves


CHAPTER

The Gendered Body Project:


18 Motivational Components of
Objectification Theory
Tomi-Ann Roberts and Patricia L. Waters

Abstract
In this chapter, we attempt to explore the motivational questions that arise when we view the
psychology of women through the lens of objectification theory, which highlights the centrality
of appearance concerns, or “body projects,” for girls and women today. We examine theoretical
perspectives on what motivates the sexual objectification of women, considering the ways this treatment
may reflect an adaptive evolutionary mating strategy, may serve as a tool for the maintenance of
patriarchal power, or may lend existential “protection” against the creaturely, death reminders that
women’s bodies provide. We then investigate both developmental processes and situational/contextual
features that motivate girls and women to internalize a sexually objectifying view on their physical selves.
And, finally, we review evidence that self-objectification, though motivating in itself, carries significant
consequences for their health and well-being.
Key Words: sexual objectification, self-objectification, body, gender, motivation

New Year’s Resolution: I will try to make myself of their bodies becomes the primary expression of
better in any way I possibly can, with the help their individual identity. Because of the centrality
of my budget and babysitting money. I will of appearance concerns to girls and women today,
lose weight, get new lenses, a new haircut, Brumberg called this their “body projects.”
good makeup, new clothes and accessories. Objectification theory (Fredrickson & Roberts,
(adolescent girl’s diary entry, Brumberg, 1997, p. xxi) 1997), published the same year as Brumburg’s book,
provided a theoretical framework from within psy-
In a fascinating if disheartening historiography, chology for understanding the gendered body proj-
Joan Brumberg (1997) examined the ways adoles- ect. This framework argues that the ubiquitous
cent girls described their self-improvement goals in sexual objectification of the female body provides
their diaries over the past 100 years. The change over the cultural milieu in which girls develop into
time was clear. Whereas girls of yesteryear focused women. The theory proposes that girls and women
on improving their manners or their study habits, are coaxed through both social and cultural experi-
in the more recent years, girls’ focus has become ences of sexual objectification to treat themselves as
almost exclusively the enhancement of their physical objects to be gazed at and evaluated based on physi-
appearance. It was not that 19th-century girls were cal appearance, an effect termed “self-objectification”
not aware of beauty imperatives, but rather that these (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997).
were not linked to self-worth or personhood in the In this chapter, we explore the motivational
ways they appear to be for 21st-century girls, whose questions that arise when we view the psychology of
motivational concern with the shape and appearance women through the lens of objectification theory.


First, we ask: What motivates the sexual objectifi- bear on this question. First, evolutionary psycholo-
cation of women, both culturally and interperson- gists argue that such treatment of women serves an
ally? When, where, and why does this perspective adaptational function. Second, feminists argue that
on the female body get adopted, and what function such treatment serves to uphold hegemonic mas-
does it serve? Next we examine the question of what culine patriarchal structures of power. And finally,
motivates self-objectification, or the internalization existential theorists point to the psychic distancing
of a sexually objectifying perspective on the bodily from the animal body that objectification serves.
self, among girls and women. How do cultural and
interpersonal experiences of objectification translate Objectification as Evolutionary Adaptation
into and take hold of girls’ and women’s own trait- Evolutionary psychologists argue that the sexual
level self-concepts? What developmental processes objectification of women’s bodies is part and parcel of
are involved? What situational or contextual fea- the naturally selected mating strategy of human males.
tures of girls’ and women’s environments motivate This perspective takes as a starting point that physi-
states of self-objectification? Finally we argue that, cal appearance in women provides a wealth of cues
despite its motivating elements, the “body project” to fertility and reproductive value. Secondly, these
that girls and women appear to be engaged in car- theorists argue that standards of physical attractive-
ries a host of cognitive, emotional, behavioral and ness in human females are not arbitrary or infinitely
even health consequences. culturally variable (Buss, 2007). So this framework
argues for an evolution of standards of female beauty:
Theoretical Frameworks on the Sexual Visually observable cues to fertility and reproductive
Objectification of Women value will become essential to what humans find
Philosopher Martha Nussbaum (1995) defined attractive in females. As David Buss (2007) has put
sexual objectification as the treating of persons as it, “beauty is in the psychological adaptations of the
sexual “things” or objects, separating them from their beholder” (p. 506).
human attributes or characteristics. Nussbaum (1999) In a large cross-cultural study, Buss and his col-
identified seven components of objectification: instru- leagues (Buss, 1989; Buss et al., 1990) found that
mentality and ownership involve treating a person as a men place a greater emphasis on physical attractive-
tool or commodity; denial of autonomy and inertness ness (or “good looks”) in potential mates than do
involve seeing a person as lacking self-determination women and men want mates who are more youthful
and agency; fungibility is characterized by viewing than they themselves are. Theorists have suggested
a person as interchangeable with others of his or that the visually observable cues to fertility and
her “type”; violability represents someone as lacking reproductive value that are considered “good look-
boundary integrity; and denial of subjectivity involves ing” in women include a low waist-to-hip ratio, full
believing that a person’s experiences and feelings can lips, lustrous hair, long hair, clear skin, and facial
be neglected. femininity (Buss, 2007).
Sexual objectification has been discussed by So the evolutionary perspective on sexual objec-
feminist philosophers and social scientists for over a tification argues that men’s visual inspections of
century, and it has typically been linked to cultural women’s bodies, the emphasis placed on women’s
representations of women in pornography (e.g., bodies as the most important feature of them, and
LeMoncheck, 1985). Indeed, each of Nussbaum’s even women’s own attention to and enhancement
(1999) components can be seen in such cultural of their attractiveness are all motivated by a drive
representations, as well as interpersonal treatment for heterosexual mating. There are several problems
of women and girls. In psychology, Fredrickson with this explanation for sexual objectification,
and Roberts (1997) argued that sexual objectifica- however. First, in Buss et al.’s (1990) cross-cultural
tion occurs along a continuum, extending beyond study, physical attractiveness was ranked by males,
pornography to the wider cultural context, which on average, not first but third on a list of desir-
normalizes the commodification of women’s bodies able characteristics of a potential female sex part-
just about everywhere, and that this cultural context ner. Both kindness and intelligence ranked ahead
induces girls and women to adopt a third-person per- of physical attractiveness on the list of what men
spective on their own bodies (i.e., to self-objectify). want in a mate. Perhaps some men’s treatment of
But what motivates this cultural and interpersonal women as sexual objects is motivated by desire to
treatment of women’s bodies? Here we will outline reproduce, but some men are not as motivated as
three theoretical views that have been brought to others by heterosexual sex, and clearly some sexual

 the gende red body project


objectification by men is not a reflection of wanting This framework links objectification with power.
to mate. However, the literature reveals a somewhat complex
Of the many features of women’s bodies considered psychological relationship between felt power and
signals of reproductive value by evolutionary psychol- likelihood to objectify. Is it that feelings of power
ogists, only the waist-to-hip ratio has received much motivate objectification, or is objectification a defen-
attention in research. Here, studies show that men do sive reaction against feelings of powerlessness? There
find a lower ratio more attractive than a higher (e.g., is evidence for both.
Singh, 1993). However, in a study of the eye move- In one study, when a female participant was cast
ments male and female participants made while judg- as subordinate, males who possessed power over her
ing the attractiveness of photographs of female bodies, and who endorsed survey items indicating a likeli-
researchers found no evidence for fixations on the pel- hood to sexually harass found her more attractive than
vic or hip area. That is, waist-to-hip ratio assessments those without power or the propensity toward sexual
had no impact on attractiveness judgments overall harassment (Bargh, Raymond, Pryor, & Strack, 1995).
(Cornelissen, Hancock, Kiviniemi, George, & Tovée, A more recent study examined the relationship
2009). We do not know the role the other hypoth- between power and the instrumentality feature of
esized signals of female attractiveness play in actually objectification, and it showed that felt power funda-
attracting men to want to mate with them. mentally alters how targets are perceived. Gruenfeld,
Finally, numerous critics have pointed out that, Inesee, Magee, and Galinsky (2008) demonstrated
although heterosexual intercourse is necessary for sexual objectification of a moderately competent
species survival, it is not necessary for individual female prospective work partner by men assigned to
survival or even well-being (e.g., Hartlaub, 2007). the role of boss. When participants had concurrent
Plenty of people have lived long prosperous lives sex and performance goals, those in the high-power
without having heterosexual intercourse. Therefore, condition were more interested in working with a
hinging the ubiquitous cultural and interpersonal female target who was instrumental for sexual pur-
sexual objectification of women on men’s evolved poses than were those in the low-power condition.
strategy for mating falls seriously short. Clearly other So felt power appears to heighten approach toward
motivations for this phenomenon must exist. instrumental (“useful”) social targets. In other words,
this feature of sexual objectification (treating others
Objectification as Maintenance of as sexual tools) appears to be motivated in part by
Hegemonic Masculinity power. Indeed, news stories of men in positions of
Hegemonic masculinity is defined as “the main- power sexually exploiting others are so common as to
tenance of practices that institutionalize men’s domi- be almost humdrum. From Italy’s Sylvio Berlusconi
nance over women” (Connell, 1987, p. 185). One of to our own Eliot Spitzer, Clarence Thomas, Bill
these practices is the sexual objectification of women. Clinton, or John Edwards, the list of powerful men
Dworkin (1987) argues that women are socialized treating women as sexual tools is long. Such men
into heterosexual womanhood, which is the same as seem willing to risk a great deal—even the loss of
being socialized into subordination. For Dworkin, their hard-earned power—to do so.
heterosexuality is organized around male dominance, On the other hand, evidence also exists for quite
which is heterosexual maleness, and female subordi- the opposite relationship between power and objec-
nation, which is heterosexual femaleness. Women tification. For example, Krings and Facchin (2009)
are socialized to be heterosexual females, and hence found that men’s sexual harassment of women (which
sexualized to the liking of heterosexual men, which is we would argue is an interpersonal form of sexual
to be subordinate, which is to be unable to meaning- objectification) is motivated by the perception of
fully consent to what is actually their own subordina- low interactional justice in their workplace. That is,
tion. To the extent that women are complicit in this when men feel they themselves are not being treated
arrangement, it is because they have formed a kind of fairly, this appears to fuel a desire to exert power, to
slave mentality to escape punishment or curry favor. “get even,” by harassing or objectifying women.
Dworkin’s colleague Catherine MacKinnon wrote, A similar theme is found in the literature on the
“sexual objectification is the primary process of the motives of rapists. Numerous studies have found
subjection of women. It unites act with word, con- that men who rape confess to underlying feelings of
struction with expression, perception with enforce- insecurity about their masculinity, and they report
ment, myth with reality. Man fucks woman; subject feeling a need to exert power over women. Studies
verb object” (1983, p. 635). show similar feelings in a wide range of men who

rob erts , water s 


engage in sexual violence, from those incarcerated for Building on Dinnerstein’s view, Goldenberg and
attacking strangers (e.g., Scully & Marolla, 1985), Roberts (2004, 2011) further articulated an existen-
to college students who coerce unwanted sex from tial motivation for the sexual objectification of wom-
acquaintances (Lisak & Roth, 1988; Malamuth, en’s bodies and women’s own self-objectification.
1986). As Lisak and Roth (1990) showed, such They point out that there are two conflicting views
men feel threatened by women and compensate for historically on women’s bodies. On the one hand,
their insecurity by seeking to dominate and control there is a long tradition of construing women as
them. closer to animals and nature (Ortner, 1974), yet on
Contrary to the evolutionary view, this perspec- the other hand there are also ample cultural exam-
tive on objectification argues that it is not motivated ples of women being elevated above nature, ideal-
by sex or mating goals alone. In the Gruenfeld et al. ized, even worshipped as goddesses. So stereotypes
(2008) study, having sex on their mind was not suf- about women are paradoxical, because they contain
ficient motivation for males to prefer to work with both negative and seemingly positive judgments,
an attractive female on a complex task. Men did not as Glick and Fiske’s (1996) revelation of “benevo-
act on their sex goal unless they were also powerful. lent sexism” attests. With respect to women’s bod-
Whether sexual objectification is fueled by a feeling ies, research shows that women’s reproductive and
of being in power over others, who become attrac- bodily functions (menstruation, pregnancy, breast-
tive sexual instruments, or whether objectification of feeding) are often viewed with derision, but, on the
others is a way for the relatively disenfranchised to other hand, other features of their bodies are revered
reinstate dominance, it is clear that power plays a role. as cultural symbols of beauty and male desire.
Furthermore, one structural outcome of the sexual Goldenberg and Roberts (2004, 2011) argue that
objectification of women is certainly the maintenance the objectification of women is motivated by a desire
of the societal power inequity between the sexes. to strip women of their connection to nature, and it
serves as a form of symbolic drapery that enables a
Objectification as Existential Protection transformation of “natural,” creaturely woman (and
The previous two explanations for what motivates thus a reminder of our animal nature, and hence also
the sexual objectification of women, while offering of death) into “object” of beauty and desire. What
significant insights, fail in at least two important ways. becomes clear when scrutinizing the ubiquitous
First, they do not adequately account for the fact that cultural presentation of women’s idealized bodies
objectification occurs along a continuum from dehu- is that, regardless of the particular features deemed
manizing (e.g., rape, trafficking) to seemingly benign essential by a culture for feminine beauty, it is spe-
or even benevolent (e.g., widespread cultural displays cifically when the more creaturely features and func-
of idealized women’s bodies). In other words, sexual tions of women’s bodies are actually or symbolically
objectification does not just “keep women down” it removed from the presentation that the female body
also sometimes “puts them up.” Second, they do not is publicly acceptable and attractive.
provide satisfying explanations for why women them- Breasts provide an illustrative example. Iris Marion
selves often take this perspective on their own bodies; Young once wrote of women’s breasts, “Cleavage is
they do not, in other words, provide a meaningful good; nipples are a no-no” (1992; p. 220). Breasts
account of the phenomenon of self-objectification. are multidimensional. They are not only a source
More psychodynamically oriented feminist of food for offspring but also the objects of sexual
thinkers, such as Dorothy Dinnerstein (1976) add desire. Interestingly, we do not seem capable of sus-
anxiety to the question of what motivates this treat- taining both of these orientations toward breasts at
ment of women and their bodies. In this view, once. Studies indeed show that the extent to which
women are regarded as dangerous because they are both men and women view breasts as objects to be
the ones who first introduce all of us to the mixed enjoyed by men predicts negative attitudes toward
blessing of being human (Snitow, 1978). For Din- breastfeeding. In one qualitative study, a mother
nerstein, men have subordinated women across cul- negatively disposed toward breastfeeding her infant
ture and time because women, in giving life, also, by was quoted as saying, “Yuck, those are for your
association, give death. The threat men feel toward husband!” (Morse, 1989, p. 239). In a more recent
women’s life-and-death giving “powers” is the impe- quantitative investigation, Ward, Merriwether, and
tus behind their efforts to control women. For if Caruthers (2006) found that the more men engaged
men can control women’s bodies they also, in effect, with popular men’s magazines, the more they con-
control nature and mortality itself. strued women as sexual objects, and this attitude

 the gende red body project


predicted more negative views toward breastfeeding The Hows and Whys of Self-Objectification
and more concern that breastfeeding interferes with We come now to the question of how and why
sexual relations. girls and women adopt an observer’s point of view
Menstruation provides another example. Roberts, on their bodily selves, how and why they undertake
Goldenberg, Power, and Pyszczynski (2002) found their own “body projects.” As Brumberg’s (1997)
that male and female participants exhibited nega- work on diaries illustrates, today’s girls have come to
tive reactions to a woman who made her menstrual focus almost exclusively on the enhancement of their
status known by inadvertently dropping a wrapped physical appearance as their most important self-
tampon out of her backpack. Not only was the improvement goal. Empirical studies indeed show
woman viewed as less competent and less likable that girls as young as 12 years old self-objectify; they
than when she dropped a less “offensive” but equally place significantly greater emphasis on their body’s
feminine item—a hair barrette—from her bag, but appearance than on its competence, health, or well-
the mere presence of the tampon also led partici- being (Slater & Tiggemann, 2002). Other evidence
pants to distance themselves physically from the shows that even younger girls are already dissatis-
woman by sitting farther away from her. Further- fied with their bodies, and especially their weight
more, not only were negative reactions exhibited (Phares, Steinberg, & Thompson, 2004). And what
in response to the individual woman who dropped starts young, sticks around. For example, one sur-
the tampon, but when participants were asked to vey showed that more than 70% of normal-weight
describe their attitudes toward women more gen- women aged 30–75 are dissatisfied with their body’s
erally, those who had seen the tampon rather than appearance (Allaz, Bernstein, Rouget, Archinard, &
the hair barrette were particularly likely to sexually Morabia, 1998). Feminists have argued that, over
objectify women’s bodies. the course of women’s development across the life
Construing women as objects also requires span, their dissatisfaction with their weight and
downplaying their explicit sexuality as well. Put- physical attractiveness is so widespread as to consti-
ting “beautiful” (good) women on an objectified tute a “normative discontent” (Rodin, Silberstein,
pedestal may also serve the function of protect- & Striegel-Moore, 1984).
ing men from the threat associated with their Objectification theory provided a comprehen-
own animalistic urges toward them. Landau et al. sive framework for understanding how the cultural
(2006) found that inducing male participants to milieu of the sexual objectification of female bodies
contemplate their mortality led to reduced sex- delivers a kind of instructional backdrop for wom-
ual interest in a seductive woman, but this effect en’s development across the life span (Fredrickson
was eliminated when the woman appeared more & Roberts, 1997). Girls and women encounter this
wholesome. treatment second hand, through media and market-
This position fits with the findings of Glick and ers’ representations (e.g., Kilbourne & Jhally, 2000),
Fiske (e.g., 2001), who have observed that prejudice as well as in actual interpersonal encounters (e.g.,
against women takes the form not only of overtly Swim, Hyers, Cohen, & Ferguson, 2001). The the-
hostile sexism but also benevolent sexism (i.e., ory argues that girls and women are coaxed through
“characterizing women as pure creatures who ought these social and cultural experiences of sexual objec-
to be protected, supported, and adored and whose tification to treat themselves as objects to be gazed at
love is necessary to make a man complete”; Glick and evaluated based on physical appearance, that is,
& Fiske, 2001, p. 109). The primary theoretical to self-objectify (Fredrickson & Roberts, 1997).
explanation for benevolent sexism is that it enables Exactly how does this coaxing occur? What moti-
interaction between the sexes, while simultaneously vates self-objectification? Fredrickson and Roberts
pacifying women. Seemingly benevolent sexual (1997) argued that this view of self can manifest on
objectification, when viewed as motivated by exis- a trait level, but it also can be induced by situational
tential concerns, appears not only to pacify women and contextual cues. On the one hand, some people
but also to protect both men and women. Men’s phys- are more likely to define themselves stably in ways
ical, animal desires should be rendered less threat- that emphasize a third-person, over a first-person,
ening if the target of these desires is construed as a point of view on their bodies. Women consistently
pure and wholesome object of worship. And, to the score higher than men in trait self-objectification
extent that women themselves self-objectify, they (e.g., Fredrickson, Roberts, Noll, Quinn, & Twenge,
are afforded the protection of psychically distancing 1998). Furthermore, similarities override differences
from their own animal nature. among subgroups of women in self-objectification.

rob erts , water s 


White and non-White women as well as heterosexual organizing gender-relevant information. From this
and sexual minority women report similar levels of perspective, children mark and quarter their realities
self-objectification (Downs, James, & Cowan, 2006; along gender lines, allowing stereotype-consistent
Harrison & Fredrickson, 2003; Kozee & Tylka, behavior in and ruling stereotype-inconsistent behav-
2006). The adoption of this perspective on the self ior out (Martin, Ruble, & Szkrybalo, 2002). How-
is likely a developmental process in which a sexually ever, the extent to which cultural modeling about
objectified standard of femininity is cultivated. beauty is internalized by young girls and whether
On the other hand, certain situations appear there is a critique of cultural ideals is influenced by
to call greater attention to the body as observed, cognitive development.
encouraging a state of self-objectification. Again, While gender is only one part of the identity
women appear to be more susceptible than men equation, it is one of the earliest emerging facets of
to the experimental inductions of a state of self- identity and, since it develops so early, it can appear
objectification (Moradi & Huang, 2008). Here we quite rigid. Parents often remark on their daughters’
will attempt to organize our understanding of what desire to dress in highly feminine outfits even when
motivates the gendered body project both in terms the situation might not warrant it, and this has led
of the development of trait-level self-objectification researchers to conclude that children both observe
in girls and women, as well as the more situation- and construct notions about gender-appropriate
specific, proximal contexts that induce this perspec- behavior based on their cognitive abilities (Maccoby,
tive on self. 1998). Researchers argue that rigidity about gender-
appropriate behavior (and dress) reflect preschool-
Girls’ Gender Development in the Culture ers’ erroneous beliefs that it is not biology, but the
of Sexual Objectification clothes (and other trappings of gender) that make
Theories of gender development may help to the man (or woman). While this confusion is cleared
make sense of how cultural messaging around sexual up by the advent of gender constancy around 4 to
objectification, as well as interpersonal sexualized or 5 years old (Kohlberg & Zigler, 1967; Ruble et al.,
sexually objectifying treatment, get translated and 2007), the fact that girls as young as 3 years old
incorporated into the self system of girls as they are already policing the femininity of their outfits
develop within a sexually objectifying culture. suggests that the roots of self-objectification are
deep indeed.
socialization theories The basic schema for gender continues to become
Ample research suggests that from the moment elaborated and solidified as part of one’s larger iden-
children are born they are treated differently based tity across childhood and adolescence. However,
on their sex (Maccoby, 1998). By as early as 2 years in these earliest years, information about gender is
old, children consistently label themselves and those consumed relatively uncritically. In preschool and
around them as either male or female, and they are early childhood, it is clear that children are pre-
busy about the business of modeling behaviors that occupied with learning the rules of the game, not
are consistent with cultural gender norms (Campbell, with dismantling them (Piaget, 1965). Acquiring
Shirley, & Candy, 2004). Social learning theorists rules for gender-appropriate appearance and behav-
(e.g., Bussey & Bandura, 1999) posit that children iors comes in a variety of sources, including family
learn gender-appropriate behaviors by observing the members, peers, teachers, but one of these ways is
praise and recognition others receive for gender- through media and toy exposure. The average pre-
consistent behavior, and by noting sanctions against schooler watches more than 21 hours of TV a week
gender-inconsistent behavior (Rust, Golombok, and yet they are the least able to critically evaluate
Hines, Johnston, & Golding, 2000). From a social media messaging (Kunkel et al., 2004; Robinson &
learning theory perspective then, little girls emulate Bianchi, 1997). This is especially problematic given
feminine models because they are praised for doing that even among young women and adults, the
so (“My, how pretty you look!”), and because they more exposure to mainstream media content the
see that others are positively reinforced for doing so. more likely females are to uncritically endorse sex-
role stereotypes (Ward, 2002; Ward & Rivadeneyra,
cognitive developmental theories 1999).
Cognitive developmentalists emphasize the child’s Across early and middle childhood advances in
active participation in forming an identity and argue perspective-taking ability pave the way for increas-
that children construct a schema or framework for ingly sophisticated interpretations of the social realm

 the gende red body project


(Higgins, 1991; Selman, 1980). In school-aged chil- on the development of the self concept demonstrates
dren these comparisons center around concrete attri- that while early adolescents are able to generate mul-
butes such as appearance. Given this, how explicitly tiple self-descriptors, they see no apparent contradic-
sexually objectifying material is incorporated into tion in describing themselves as entirely opposite in
the self at different ages is likely to change with the one setting versus another (Harter, 1999). Harter
type of gender role models children encounter. The argued that this is due to emergent formal opera-
subtle shift in toys from Barbie with the hypersexu- tional development in which the individual is able to
alized body to Bratz dolls with a girlish body juxta- generate multiple perspectives on the self but is not
posed with hypersexualized clothes and facial features yet able to hold contradictory descriptors as objects
makes room for ever younger girls to don the mantle for purposes of comparison. It is plausible then that
of sexuality even if they don’t have the body for it the 12-year-old girl could hold completely contra-
(Roberts, in press). dictory views, recognizing at once that the media
At adolescence, normative developmental pro- images are “stupid and unrealistic” and yet unprob-
cesses may heighten girls’ sensitivity to sexually objec- lematically working mightily to attain the ideal body.
tifying cultural messages. At puberty, girls begin to Appreciating the inconsistencies between these two
acquire formal operational thought and the capacity ideas is likely to cause increasing distress only at mid-
to compare themselves against multiple standards. adolescence when contradictory information about
However, the capacity to understand the self in the self becomes highly salient but integration of
increasingly complex and differentiated terms devel- inconsistencies is still beyond the cognitive reach of
ops gradually and inconsistently across adolescence the mid-adolescent (Harter, 1999). In the context
and may be accompanied by cognitive errors. Elkind of objectification theory, it is conceivable that self-
(1967) dubbed one of these the “imaginary audience,” objectification contributes to increased body surveil-
which he argued was a “failure of under differentia- lance and body shame, as the mid-adolescent works
tion” between the self and other that leads adolescents to integrate what she knows (e.g., “These media
to believe that others are as preoccupied with them as images are unrealistic and stupid, nobody dresses like
they are with themselves. In terms of objectification that!”) with what she feels (e.g., “If I look hot for this
theory, the heightened self-consciousness associated party, people will like me”).
with this cognitive error leaves early adolescents ripe This assertion, however, is largely speculative
for the self-surveillance aspects associated with self- given the lack of research examining the rela-
objectification. tionship between cognitive development and self-
During the same period at the neurological level, objectification. Even among older samples, studies
a gradual process of synaptic pruning and consolida- that explicitly examine developmental processes in
tion of neural architecture occurs in the prefrontal the motive to self-objectify have been few and far
cortex—the seat of judgment, reasoning, and plan- between. One notable exception is McKinley’s (2006a;
ning—and this is coupled with more pronounced 2006b) sequential study of mothers and daughters
development in the amygdala (emotion regulation) at two points in time, 10 years apart. McKinley
and changes in the availability of dopamine and (2006a) found that college women reported higher
GABA (Baird et al., 1999; Kalivas, Churchill, & levels of body shame and body surveillance than did
Klitenick, 1993; Spear, 2000). Taken together, these their middle-aged mothers or their male peers and
changes are associated with a heightened sensitiv- the link between body shame and body surveillance
ity to emotionally evocative material (e.g., media/ was stronger in younger women than in middle-aged
advertising) coupled with relatively few cognitive women. Ten years later, both groups of women con-
“breaks” on the system. Researchers have associated tinued to report higher levels of self-objectification
these shifts with adolescents’ penchant for risk tak- than males. However, the younger cohort of women
ing and decision making that favors immediate over reported significantly less self-objectification at the
long-term rewards (Reyna & Farley, 2006; Spear, second assessment, and they no longer differed sig-
2000), but in the context of objectification theory, nificantly from their mothers (McKinley, 2006a).
it is conceivable that they are also associated with McKinley explained that during emerging adult-
difficulty in critically analyzing provocative cultural hood, young women may be more motivated to self-
messaging around appearance. objectify because they are engaged in life span tasks
Self-objectification may also play a role in the such as establishing intimate relationships (Erikson,
social comparisons associated with creating a coherent 1959) and beginning careers (Arnett, 2000), both of
and integrated identity during adolescence. Research which implicate appearance for women. Ten years

rob erts , water s 


later, as careers and intimate relationships are solidi- include exposure to sexually objectifying words or
fying, the motivation to self-objectify may lessen. images (Aubrey, 2006; Monro & Huon, 2005; Rob-
To sum up, objectification theory’s concept of erts & Gettman, 2004), overhearing other women
self-objectification provides an intrapsychic contri- speak disparagingly of their own bodies (Gapinski,
bution to our understanding of gender development Brownell, & LaFrance, 2003), and being filmed
in girls. During their early years of gender social- by a male with a focus on the body, not the face
ization, perhaps through reinforcements as social (Gay & Castano, 2010). The salience of these primes
learning theorists would argue, or even through suggests that contextual factors contribute to self-
normative cognitive and neurological changes as objectification and lend support for the argument
constructivists would argue, girls learn to inter- that self-objectification derives from cultural messag-
nalize a sexually objectified view of their bodies. ing around expected appearance norms.
Once they have done so, they set about the work However, not all girls and women read cultural
of solidifying their identity as a physically attractive messages about beauty with the same urgency, nor
female. The media steps in to supply increasingly do all females interpret cultural beauty ideals as
impossible standards of sexy attractiveness to which mandates for body projects. The extent to which
they can aspire and marketers sell them the products individual differences in self-objectification are trait-
and procedures that promise to help them meet that like or are the product of the social milieu remains
standard (Lamb & Brown, 2006). Once in place at speculative. Several studies have demonstrated con-
the trait level, self-objectification provides the moti- nections between self-reported self-objectification
vational fuel for the body project in which girls and (the trait), body surveillance, body shame, and neg-
women appear to remain engaged for much of their ative outcomes. Jones (2004), for example, found
lives. that the more 7th and 10th grade girls engaged in
appearance conversations with friends and the more
The Situational Motivators of social comparison they made concerning appear-
Self-Objectification ance, the more body dissatisfaction they reported.
In addition to its cultivation within girls and In this study, appearance conversations with friends
women as a trait, a number of studies manipulating played as significant a role as internalizing media
the level of exposure to sexually objectifying ver- ideals in shaping body dissatisfaction. It could
sus control situations lend support for the notion be that conversations with peers concerning body
that self-objectification can be induced as a state issues are analogous to co-rumination, a phenom-
by subtle cues in the environment (e.g., Calogero, enon that has been tied to depressive symptoms
2004; Fredrickson et al., 1998; Quinn, Kallen, (Broderick & Korteland, 2002; Rose, 2002; Rose &
& Cathey, 2006; Roberts & Gettman, 2004). The Rudolph, 2006). Could these conversations amount
classic study of heightening self-objectification to co-objectification? When a girl asks a question like
by manipulating appearance pressure involved “Do these pants make my butt look big?” is she
women performing tasks in a mirrored dressing enlisting peers’ help in her own body surveillance?
room while wearing either a swimsuit or a sweater If co-objectification is one of the factors contribut-
(Fredrickson et al., 1998). Those in the heightened ing to body dissatisfaction in the middle and high
self-objectification condition (wearing swimsuits) school years, interventions in the context of peer
reported higher levels of body shame, reduced task interactions may be especially important.
performance, and restrained food intake compared
to control groups dressed in sweaters. Restrained Summary
eating occurred even after delaying the food intake We have argued that one of the reasons girls
assessment until after participants redressed, sug- and women are motivated to self-objectify is that
gesting that the effects of the prime remained bringing the self into compliance with sociocul-
even after the initial manipulation (Quinn et al., tural norms yields positive benefits. We have also
2006). suggested that how self-objectification motivates
In other experiments, priming women to antici- behavior will vary depending on age and normative
pate an interaction with a male stranger increased developmental processes, including cognitive and
state self-objectification phenomena, including body neurological maturation. It seems likely that ado-
shame and appearance anxiety (Calogero, 2004). lescent girls may be especially sensitized to cultural
Still other inductions that have been demon- information about appearance as they work to cre-
strated to lead to heightened state self-objectification ate a coherent self-concept. Research also suggests

 the gende red body project


that young women who are in the emerging adult- the past 40 years has increased the disparity between
hood phase of life will be particularly motivated to women’s real bodies and media ideals. This widening
engage in self-objectification because it is during gap motivates the self-surveillance that results in feel-
these times that the social benefits of conforming ings of failure (shame) at attaining the increasingly
to appearance standards are particularly salient. It is impossible ideal (McKinley, 1998; Stice, 1994).
also clear that just how self-objectification informs Moradi and Huang (2008) also reviewed
girls’ and women’s choices varies depending on con- correlational studies that support links between self-
textual phenomena such as the extent to which girls objectification and body shame with negative out-
co-objectify by talking to each other about appear- comes for girls’ and women’s mental health, including
ance concerns, and whether and how they encoun- low self-esteem and depressive symptoms. The body
ter objectifying treatment and media. shame associated with self-objectification appears to
motivate eating disordered behaviors and even smok-
The Motivating But Consequential ing behaviors (e.g., Fiissel & Lafreniere, 2006; Har-
Body Project rell, Fredrickson, Pomerleau, & Nolen-Hoeksema,
Self-objectification can be thought of as a kind 2006; Tylka & Hill, 2004). Self-objectification and
of external locus of control, for it frames girls’ and its attendant body shame have also been linked with
women’s lives around their physical attractiveness poor outcomes related to women’s sexual motivations,
or sexual appeal to others. On the other hand, the such as reduced interest in sex, nonassertive and risky
“body project” has its internally motivated elements sexual behaviors, greater appearance concern dur-
as well. Girls and women have every reason to value ing sexual intimacy, and even lower reported sexual
the positive feedback they receive from their own pleasure and arousability (e.g., Hirschman, Impett,
efforts at appearance enhancement. Studies have & Schooler, 2006; Sanchez & Kiefer, 2007).
shown that women’s appearance control beliefs However, while mere exposure to thin media
have a paradoxical relationship to their well-being. ideals increases self-objectification and thus moti-
That is, those who endorse a more internal locus of vates unhealthy behaviors like disordered eating,
control with respect to their appearance (“buying,” smoking, or poor sexual decision making, it is
in other words, what advertisers for the myriad of increasingly clear that self-objectification is associ-
body-enhancing products and procedures marketed ated not only with exposure, but with the extent to
to females in this culture are really selling) tend to which women internalize the beauty ideal (Moradi,
have somewhat higher body esteem, but engage in Dirks & Matteson, 2005; Morry & Staska, 2001).
more restricted eating and other body-altering prac- In Morry and Staska’s study, college women’s self-
tices that can be hazardous (McKinley & Hyde, reported exposure to beauty (as compared to fitness)
1996). Despite its motivating elements, a decade magazines was tied to self-objectification and to
of research since the publication of “Objectification symptoms of disordered eating, but this effect was
theory” has demonstrated that the body project in mediated by self-reported internalization. In other
which girls and women appear to be engaged carries words, it is likely not exposure alone, but the degree
a host of cognitive, emotional, behavioral, and even to which girls and women take on ideals for beauty
health consequences. as self-relevant and motivating that predicts noxious
The most well-documented consequences of self- outcomes.
objectification are in the arena of body esteem. Moradi To sum up, the internalization of sexual objecti-
and Huang (2008) reviewed this literature and con- fication means that many girls and women chroni-
cluded that experimental data strongly suggest that cally monitor their body’s appearance. Their body
heightened self-objectification promotes body shame, projects can be very motivating indeed, and advertis-
appearance anxiety, and negative affect in both White ers promise the products, clothing, procedures, and
and racially and ethnically diverse samples of women diets that provide the tools for their undertaking. But
as well as girls as young as 12 years old. Why do girls the project is costly, not only in terms of time and
and women experience body shame upon viewing money, but, studies have clearly shown, also in terms
the self as an object? The conceptual reasons for this of girls’ and women’s well-being. It is also never fin-
rest on observations that a disparity between the real ished. The comparisons girls and women make to
and the ideal self contributes to low self-esteem, dis- the idealized feminine beauty proliferated by 24–7
satisfaction with one’s body, and dysphoria (Harter, media nearly invariably result in body shame, which,
1999; Horney, 1945; James, 1892; McKinley, 1998). in turn, leads to a host of negative consequences for
The thinning down of the feminine body ideal over the quality of their lives.

rob erts , water s 


Concluding Comments of the amygdala in children and adolescents. Journal of the
American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 38(2),
We have suggested that objectification and self-
195–199.
objectification can be viewed as motivated processes. Bargh, J. A., Raymond, P., Pryor, J. B., & Strack, F. (1995).
We have suggested alternative explanations for why Attractiveness of the underling: An automatic power—sex
objectification takes place in the culture, and we have association and its consequences for sexual harassment and
developed a rationale for viewing self-objectification, aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
768–781.
and its attendant self-surveillance and body shame,
Broderick, P. C., & Korteland, C. (2002). Coping styles and
as a motivational set. The empirical literature on depression in early adolescence: Relationships to gender, gen-
self-objectification suggests that it is contingent der role, and implicit beliefs. Sex Roles, 46, 201–213.
upon a complex interaction between the person and Brumberg, J. J. (1997). The body project: An intimate history of
the situation. In a decade of investigation, much American girls. New York: Vintage.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences:
has been illuminated about the activating role of
Evolutionary hypotheses testing in 37 cultures. Behavioral
self-objectification in motivating girls’ and women’s and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49.
body projects. In this chapter, we have attempted to Buss, D. M. (2007). The evolution of human mating. Acta
illustrate how these projects can be seen as the logi- Psychologica Sinica, 39, 502–512.
cal outcome of normative developmental processes Buss, D. M., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A., Biaggio, A., Blanco-
Villasenor, A., Bruchon-Schweitzer, M. . . . Yang, K-S. (1990).
within a framework on gender where femininity is
International preferences in selecting mates: A study of
narrowly construed as emphasizing sexually attrac- 37 societies. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 21, 5–47.
tive physical appearance. Gender identity formation Bussey, K., & Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory and
in childhood, self-concept in adolescence, and inti- gender development and differentiation. Psychological Review,
macy and career establishment in emerging adult- 106, 676–713.
Calogero, R. M. (2004). A test of objectification theory: The
hood are all inflected through this lens.
effect of the male gaze on appearance concerns in college
Just how these processes unfold and the extent to women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 28, 16–21.
which individual differences impact the motivation Campbell, A., Shirley, L., & Candy, J. (2004). A longitudinal
to engage in body projects and produce maladapted study of gender-related cognition and behavior. Developmen-
outcomes have been the subjects of considerable tal Science, 7, 1–9.
Connell, R. W. (1987). Gender and power: Society, the person and
investigation, some of which has been reviewed in
sexual politics. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.
this chapter. And it is clear from the evidence gath- Cornelissen, P. L., Hancock, P. J. B., Kiviniemi, V., George, H. R.,
ered to this point that the motivation to self-objectify & Tovée, M. J. (2009). Patterns of eye movements when
varies in response to priming. It remains unclear, male and female observers judge female attractiveness, body
however, why some individuals are motivated to fat and waist to hip ratio. Evolution and Human Behavior,
30, 417–428.
engage in self-objectification to the detriment of
Dinnerstein, D. (1976). The mermaid and the minotaur: Sexual
their health and psychological well-being and self- arrangements and human malaise. New York: Harper Collins.
esteem and others remain resilient in the face of Downs, D. M., James, S., & Cowan, G. (2006). Body objec-
pressures to conform to appearance ideals. Unravel- tification, self-esteem, and relationship satisfaction: A com-
ing these questions will be important for research if parison of exotic dancers and college women. Sex Roles, 54,
745–752.
we are to imagine interventions to undermine the
Dworkin, A. (1987). Intercourse. London: Arrow Books.
sexual objectification of girls and women, as well as Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Develop-
to relieve so many of them of the maladaptive body ment, 38, 1025–1034.
projects in which they are engaged. Erikson, E. (1959). Childhood and society. New York: Norton.
Fiissel, D. L., & Lafreniere, K. D. (2006). Weight control
References motives for cigarette smoking: Further consequences of the
Allaz, A. F., Bernstein, M., Rouget, P., Archinard, M., & sexual objectification of women? Feminism and Psychology,
Morabia, A. (1998). Body weight preoccupation in middle 16, 327–344.
age and ageing women: A general population survey. Interna- Fredrickson, B. L., & Roberts, T-A. (1997). Objectification theory:
tional Journal of Eating Disorders, 23, 287–294. Toward understanding women’s lived experiences and mental
Arnett, J. (2000). Emerging adulthood: A theory of development health risks. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 21, 173–206.
from the late teens through the twenties, American Psycholo- Fredrickson, B. L., Roberts, T-A., Noll, S. M., Quinn, D. M.,
gist, 55(5), 469–480. & Twenge, J. M. (1998). That swimsuit becomes you: Sex
Aubrey, J. S. (2006). Effects of sexually objectifying media on differences in self-objectification, restrained eating, and math
self-objectification and body surveillance in undergraduates: performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75,
Results of a 2-year panel study. Journal of Communication, 269–285.
56, 366–386. Gapinski, K. D., Brownell, K. D., & LaFrance, M. (2003). Body
Baird, A. A., Gruber, S. A., Cohen, B. M., Renshaw, P. F., objectification and “Fat Talk”: Effects on emotion, motiva-
Steingard, R. J., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (1999). fMRI tion, and cognitive performance. Sex Roles, 48, 377–388.

 the gende red body project


Gay, R. K., & Castano, E. (2010). My body or my mind: The Kozee, H. B., & Tylka, T. L. (2006). A test of objectification
impact of state and trait objectification on women’s cogni- theory with lesbian women. Psychology of Women Quarterly,
tive resources. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 30, 348–357.
695–703. Krings, F., & Facchin, S. (2009). Organizational justice and
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (1996). The ambivalent sexism inven- men’s likelihood to sexually harass: The moderating role of
tory: Differentiating hostile and benevolent sexism. Journal sexism and personality. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94,
of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 491–512. 501–510.
Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: Hostile Kunkel, D., Wilcox, B. L., Cantor, J., Palmer, E., Linn, S. &
and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for Dowrick, P. (2004, February 20). Report of the APA task force
gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118. on advertising and children. Washington, DC: American
Goldenberg, J. L., & Roberts, T-A. (2004). The beast within the Psychological Association.
beauty: An existential perspective on the objectification and Lamb, S., & Brown, L. M. (2006). Packaging girlhood: Rescuing
condemnation of women. In J. Greenberg, S. L. Koole, & our daughters from marketers’ schemes. New York: St. Martin’s
T. Pyszczynski (Eds.), Handbook of experimental existential Press.
psychology (pp. 71–85). New York: Guilford Press. Landau, M. J., Goldenberg, J. L., Greenberg, J., Gillath, O.,
Goldenberg, J. L., & Roberts, T-A. (2011). The birthmark: Solomon, S., Cox, C., . . . & Pyszczynski, T. (2006). The siren’s
An existential account of the objectification of women. call: Terror management and the threat of sexual attraction.
In R. M. Calogero, S. Tantleff-Dunn, & J. K. Thompson Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 129–146.
(Eds.), Self-objectification in women: Causes, consequences, and LeMoncheck, L. (1985). Dehumanizing women: Treating persons
counteractions (pp. 77–99). Washington, DC: American Psy- as sex objects. Totowa, NJ: Rowman & Allanheld.
chological Association Press. Lisak, D., & Roth, S. (1988). Motivational factors in nonincar-
Gruenfeld, D. H., Inesi, M. E., Magee, J. C., & Galinsky, A. D. cerated sexually aggressive men. Journal of Personality and
(2008). Power and the objectification of social targets. Jour- Social Psychology, 55, 795–802.
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 111–127. Lisak, D., & Roth, S. (1990). Motives and psychodynamics of
Harrell, Z. A. T., Fredrickson, B. L., Pomerleau, C. S., & Nolen- self-reported, unincarcerated rapists. American Journal of
Hoeksema, S. (2006). The role of trait self-objectification in Orthopsychiatry, 60, 268–280.
smoking among college women. Sex Roles, 54, 735–743. Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming
Harrison, K., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2003). Women’s sport together. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.
media, self-objectification, and mental health in black and MacKinnon, C. (1983). Feminism, Marxism, method, and the
white adolescent females. Journal of Communication, 53, state: Toward feminist jurisprudence. Signs: Journal of Women
216–232. in Culture and Society, 8, 635–658.
Harter, S. (1999). The construction of self: A developmental perspec- Malamuth, N. M. (1986). Predictors of naturalistic sexual
tive. New York: Guilford Press. aggression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50,
Hartlaub, M. G. (2007). Celebrating the natural. Journal of Sex 953–962.
Research, 44, 220–222. Martin, C. L., Ruble, D. N., & Szkrybalo, J. (2002). Cognitive
Higgins, E. T. (1991). Development of self-regulatory and theories of early gender development. Psychological Bulletin,
self-evaluative processes: Costs, benefits, and tradeoffs. 128, 903–933.
In M. R. Gunnar & A. Sroufe (Eds.), Self-processes and McKinley, N. M. (1998). Gender differences in undergraduates’
development: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Development body esteem: The mediating effect of objectified body con-
(Vol. 23, pp. 135–166). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. sciousness and actual/ideal weight discrepancy. Sex Roles, 39,
Hirschman, C., Impett, E. A., & Schooler, D. (2006). Dis/ 113–123.
embodied voices: What late-adolescent girls can teach us McKinley, N. M. (2006a). Longitudinal gender differences in
about objectification and sexuality. Sexuality Research and objectified body consciousness and weight-related attitudes
Social Policy: A Journal of the NSRC, 3, 8–20. and behaviors: Cultural and developmental contexts in the
Horney, K. (1945). Our inner conflicts. New York: Basic Books. transition from college. Sex Roles, 54, 159–173.
James, W. (1892). Psychology: The briefer course. New York: McKinley, N. M. (2006b). The development and cultural con-
Henry Holt. texts of objectified body consciousness: A longitudinal anal-
Jones, D. (2004). Body image among adolescent girls and syis of two cohorts of women. Developmental Psychology,
boys: A longitudinal study. Develomental Psychology, 40(5) 42(4), 679–687.
823–835. McKinley, N. M., & Hyde, J. S. (1996). The objectified body
Kalivas, P. W., Churchill, L., & Klitenick, M. A. (1993). consciousness scale: Development and validation. Psychology
The circuitry mediating the translation of motivational of Women Quarterly, 20, 181–215.
stimuli into adaptive motor responses. In P. W. Kalivas & Monro, F., & Huon, G. (2005). Media-portrayed idealized
C. D. Barnes (Eds.), Limbic motor circuits and neuropsychiatry images, body shame, and appearance anxiety. International
(pp. 237–287). Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. Journal of Eating Disorders, 38, 85–90.
Kilbourne, J. (Producer), Jhally, S. (Director). (2000). Killing us Moradi, B., Dirks, D., & Matteson, A. (2005). Roles of sexual
softly 3: Advertising images of women [videorecording]. Avail- objectification experiences and internalization of sociocul-
able from Media Education Foundation. http://www.mediaed. tural standards of beauty in eating disorder symptomatology:
org/cgi-bin/commerce.cgi?preadd=action&key=206 An examination and extension of objectification theory. Jour-
Kohlberg, L., & Zigler, E. (1967). The impact of cognitive nal of Counseling Psychology, 52, 420–428.
maturity upon the development of sex-role attitudes in Moradi, B., & Huang, Y. P. (2008). Objectification theory and
the years four to eight. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 75, psychology of women: A decade of advances and future
91–165. directions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 32, 377–398.

rob erts , water s 


Morry, M. M., & Staska, S. L. (2001). Magazine exposure: Ruble, D. N., Taylor, L. J., Cyphers, L., Greulich, F. K.,
Internalization, self-objectification, eating attitudes, and Lurye, L. E., & Shrout, P. E. (2007). The role of gender
body satisfaction in male and female university students. constancy in early gender development. Child Development,
Canadian Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 4, 269–279. 78(4), 1121–1136.
Morse, J. M. (1989). “Euch, those are for your husband!” Rust, J., Golombok, S., Hines, M., Johnston, K., & Golding, J.
Examination of cultural values and assumptions about breast (2000). The role of brothers and sisters in the gender devel-
feeding. Health Care for Women International, 11, 223–232. opment of preschool children. Journal of Experimental Child
Nussbaum, M. C. (1995). Objectification. Philosophy and Public Psychology, 77, 292–303.
Affairs, 24, 249–291. Sanchez, D. T., & Kiefer, A. K. (2007). Body concerns
Nussbaum, M. C. (1999). Sex and social justice. Oxford, in and out of the bedroom: Implications for sexual
England: Oxford University Press. pleasure and problems. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36,
Ortner, S. B. (1974). Is female to male as nature is to culture? 808–820.
In M. Z. Rosaldo & M. Lamphere (Eds.), Women, culture Selman, R. L. (1980). The growth of interpersonal understand-
and society (pp. 173–253). Stanford, CA: Stanford University ing. New York: Academic Press.
Press. Scully, D., & Marolla, J. (1985). “Riding the bull at Gilley’s:”
Phares, V., Steinberg, A. R., & Thompson, J. K. (2004). Convicted rapists describe the rewards of rape. Social Prob-
Gender differences in peer and parental influences: Body lems, 32, 251–263.
image disturbance, self-worth and psychological functioning Singh, D. (1993). Body shape and women’s attractiveness—
in preadolescent children. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, the critical role of waist-to-hip ratio. Human Nature, 4,
33, 421–429. 297–321.
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. New York: Slater, A., & Tiggemann, M. (2002). A test of objectification
Free Press. theory in adolescent girls. Sex Roles, 46, 343–349.
Quinn, D. M., Kallen, R. W., & Cathey, C. (2006). Body on Snitow, A. (1978). Thinking about the Mermaid and the Mino-
my mind: The lingering effect of state self-objectification. taur. Feminist Studies, 4, 190–198.
Sex Roles, 55, 869–874. Spear, L. (2000). The adolescence brain and age-related behav-
Reyna, V. F., & Farley, F. (2006). Risk and rationality in adoles- ioral manifestations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews,
cent decision-making: Implications for theory, practice and 24(4) 417–463.
public policy. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7(1) Stice, E. (1994). Review of the evidence for a sociocultural
1–44. model of bulimia nervosa and an exploration of the
Roberts, T-A. (in press). “She’s so pretty, she looks just like a mechanisms of action. Clinical Psychology Review, 14(7),
Bratz doll!” Theoretical foundations for understanding 633–661.
girls’ and women’s self-objectification. In E. Zurbriggen & Swim, J. K., Hyers, L. L., Cohen, L. L., & Ferguson, M. J.
T-A. Roberts (Eds.), The sexualization of girls and girlhood (2001). Everyday sexism: Evidence for its incidence, nature,
New York: Oxford University Press. and psychological impact from three daily diary studies. Jour-
Roberts, T-A., & Gettman, J. Y. (2004). Mere exposure: Gender nal of Social Issues, 57, 31–53.
differences in the negative effects of priming a state of self- Tylka, T. L., & Hill, M. S. (2004). Objectification theory as it
objectification. Sex Roles, 51, 17–27. relates to disordered eating among college women. Sex Roles,
Roberts, T-A., Goldenberg, J. L., Power, C., & Pyszczynski, T. 51, 719–730.
(2002). “Feminine protection:” The effects of menstruation Ward, L. M. (2002). Does television exposure affect emerging
on attitudes toward women. Psychology of Women Quarterly, adults’ attitudes and assumptions about sexual relationships?
26, 131–139. Correlational and experimental confirmation. Journal of
Robinson, A. P., & Bianchi, S. (1997). The children’s hours. Youth and Adolescence, 31, 1–15.
American Demographics, 19,, 20–23. Ward, L. M., Merriwether, A., & Caruthers, A. (2006). Breasts
Rodin, J., Silberstein, L., & Striegel-Moore, R. (1984). Women are for men: Media use, masculinity ideology, and men’s
and weight: A normative discontent. Nebraska Symposium on beliefs about women’s bodies. Sex Roles, 55, 703–714.
Motivation, 32, 267–307 Ward, L. M., & Rivadeneyra, R. (1999). Contributions of
Rose, A. J. (2002). Co-rumination in the friendships of girls and entertainment television to adoelscents’ sexual attitudes
boys. Child Development, 73, 1830–1843. and expectations: The role of viewing amount versus viewer
Rose, A. J., & Rudolph, K. D. (2006). A review of sex differ- involvement. Journal of Sex Research, 36, 237–249.
ences in peer relationship processes: Potential tradeoffs for Young, I. M. (1992). Breasted experience: The look and the
the emotional and behavioral development of girls and boys. feeling. In D. Leder (Ed.), The body in medical thought and
Psychological Bulletin, 132, 98–131. practice (pp. 215–230). Boston, MA: Kluwer.

 the gende red body project


CHAPTER

Relatedness Between Children and


19 Parents: Implications for Motivation

Eva M. Pomerantz, Cecilia Sin-Sze Cheung, and Lili Qin

Abstract
A significant goal in many countries around the world is promoting children’s motivation so that
ultimately they achieve at their full potential. There is much evidence supporting the idea that parents
play a significant role in either facilitating or undermining children’s motivation. The focus of this
chapter is on how relatedness between children and their parents shapes the development of children’s
motivation as well as achievement. Three sets of ideas about how relatedness between children and
their parents contributes to children’s motivation are reviewed. An integration of the three is provided
to highlight key themes as well as suggest key directions for future research.
Key Words: achievement, attachment, motivation, parent–child relations, parenting

Feeling related to others is fundamental to development of children’s functioning in the aca-


human functioning (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; demic arena has also been of interest.
Deci & Ryan, 2000). For most humans, their first The central goal of this chapter is to integrate
experience with relatedness takes place in the con- several lines of theory and research in which chil-
text of their relationships with their parents. Such dren’s relationships with their parents serve as a
relationships are unique in that they are often the context for the development of their motivation,
first in children’s lives, with children depending on with implications for their achievement (for recent
their parents to provide them with important physi- reviews of other ways in which parents contribute
cal and psychological resources (Clutton-Brock, to children’s motivation, see Eccles, 2007; Grolnick,
1991; Thompson et al., 2005). It is thus not sur- Friendly, & Bellas, 2009; Pomerantz & Moorman,
prising that even as children enter into relationships 2010; Pomerantz, Moorman, & Litwack, 2007).
with others such as their peers, their relationships After briefly describing the major categories of
with their parents retain substantial significance motivation studied among children, we high-
throughout adolescence, if not into adulthood as light the relevant postulates, as well as supportive
well (Collins & Steinberg, 2006; Offer & Offer, research, of the two foremost theories linking relat-
1975). Indeed, children’s relationships with their edness between children and their parents to chil-
parents have been identified as key contexts for vir- dren’s academic functioning: We review Bowlby’s
tually all aspects of their psychological development (1969, 1973, 1980) attachment theory and its
(for reviews, see Cassidy & Shaver, 2008; Thomp- extensions to the development of such functioning
son, 2006). Although much of the attention in this (e.g., Bretherton, 1985; van IJzendoorn, Dijksta, &
vein has been directed toward the development Bus, 1995); we then discuss Deci and Ryan’s (1985,
of children’s functioning in the social arena, the 2000) self-determination theory, with a focus on


its application to parents’ socialization of children’s and Ainsworth make the case that the quality of
motivation (e.g., Grolnick, Deci, & Ryan, 1997; children’s attachment to their primary caregivers,
Grolnick & Farkas, 2002). We next move beyond who are often their parents, shapes children’s explo-
the focus of these theories on the quality of relation- ration (i.e., examining their environment so that
ships between children and their parents to chil- they are ultimately knowledgeable about it). Among
dren’s sense of responsibility to their parents. In the children who are securely attached, parents serve as
final section before concluding, we integrate the dif- a reliable base from which children can explore their
ferent ideas about the role of children’s relatedness world: Children are able to trust that their parents
to their parents in their academic functioning, sug- will be there for them if they are needed; their par-
gesting key questions to be answered in the future. ents serve as a safe haven, thereby permitting effec-
tive concentration among children, which may be of
Major Categories of Children’s Motivation particular import in the face of challenge. For these
Theory and research concerned with children’s children, their attachment and exploration systems
academic functioning has generally focused on three are balanced, which is unfortunately not the case
major categories of motivation (Eccles & Wigfield, for children who are insecurely attached to their
2002; Eccles, Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998). First, parents. Such children experience anxiety over the
a key category is that of how capable children feel possibility of losing their parents’ attention, which
they are in regard to accomplishing the learning interferes with their exploration.
tasks they encounter. This includes children’s per- Several studies find that when children are
ceptions of competence, expectations for future securely attached to their parents in the early years
performance, feelings of efficacy, and sense of con- of life, they are better able to explore their environ-
trol. Second, children’s investment and engagement ment at this time, often demonstrating enhanced
in learning tasks is of import, including not sim- competence (e.g., Belsky, Garduque, & Hrncir,
ply the value children assign to such tasks and the 1984; Frodi, Bridges, & Grolnick, 1985). For
amount of time they spend on them but also their example, Matas, Arend, and Sroufe (1978) observed
use of effective learning strategies, such as the plan- that children securely (versus insecurely) attached to
ning and monitoring of their learning. A third cate- their mothers at 18 months were more effective in
gory is the reasons behind children’s investment and their problem solving 6 months later in that they
engagement. One of the most studied set of reasons spent more time working on problem-solving tasks,
is children’s internal or autonomous (e.g., enjoy- with heightened enthusiasm and dampened frustra-
ment and personal importance) versus external or tion. Perhaps because of their enhanced exploration,
controlled (e.g., avoidance of shame and attainment securely attached children are more cognitively com-
of rewards) reasons—what is known as intrinsic petent (e.g., their language is more developed) than
(versus extrinsic) motivation. Also receiving sub- are insecurely attached children; notably, this is not
stantive attention is the extent to which children are accounted for by children’s early IQ (e.g., O’Connor
concerned with developing (i.e., mastery motiva- & McCartney, 2007; van IJzendoorn et al., 1995).
tion) rather than demonstrating (i.e., performance Children with an ambivalent insecure attachment
motivation) their competence. All three categories to their parents (i.e., children alternate between
appear to play a role in children’s achievement (for resistance and passivity toward their parents) appear
a review, see Wigfield, Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & to be at greater risk for a lack of exploration than are
Davis-Kean, 2006). Thus, they have all received their counterparts with an avoidant insecure attach-
attention in the theory and research concerned with ment (i.e., children physically and affectively avoid
the role of parents in the development of children’s their parents; e.g., Belsky et al., 1984; Frodi et al.,
academic functioning. 1985). Frodi and colleagues (1985) speculate that
although both types of insecurely attached children
The Attachment Theory Perspective feel anxious over obtaining their parents’ atten-
The idea that children’s relationships with their tion, avoidant children are more likely to displace
parents contribute to their motivation has received their anxiety by engaging in activities that give the
support in Bowlby’s (1969, 1973, 1980) and Ains- appearance of greater exploration compared to their
worth’s (Ainsworth, Blehar, Waters, & Wall, 1978; ambivalent counterparts.
Caldwell & Ricituti, 1973) writings in the context A critical question is whether the early attach-
of attachment theory (see also Bretherton, 1985). ments children have to their parents make contri-
Focusing on the first 2 years of life, both Bowlby butions to children’s academic functioning over the

 rel atedness bet ween children and pa ren ts


longer term as children progress through school. to their parents. Some investigators have put forth
A key mechanism by which such attachments may what Fraley (2002) refers to as revisionist perspec-
do so is through the internal working models chil- tives of attachment in which internal working mod-
dren develop of themselves and others (Jacobsen, els are constantly updated to incorporate ongoing
Wolfgang, & Hofman, 1994; van IJzendoorn et al., attachment experiences so that such models may
1995). Bowlby (1969, 1973) maintains that when or may not map onto children’s early attachment
children have secure attachments to their parents, to their parents. However, Fraley’s (2002) review
they develop internal working models in which the indicates that the data are more in line with what
self is seen as worthy of love and others are seen as Fraley refers to as prototype perspectives in which
trustable. Such models may not only contribute to internal working models are updated but maintain
the effective exploration of securely attached chil- core dimensions of children’s early attachment to
dren described earlier but also generate confidence their parents. In line with the idea that the quality
among them. Seeing oneself as worthy of love may of children’s attachment to their parents early in life
lead to perceptions that one is competent; such per- matters for children’s academic functioning later in
ceptions, in conjunction with seeing others as trust- life, studying children who were raised in their ini-
able and thus as able to provide a safe haven when tial years on an Israeli kibbutz, Aviezer, Sagi, Resnik,
needed, may lead children to feel in control. These and Gini (2002) observed that children’s early (i.e.,
views of the self as capable may set the foundation at 13 to15 months) attachment to their mothers,
for children’s effective investment and engagement. but not fathers, was predictive of children’s motiva-
In contrast, when children are insecurely attached to tion (e.g., persistence and attention) and skills (e.g.,
their parents, they possess internal working models writing and oral abilities), but not grades, in the
in which people, including themselves, are viewed academic arena during early adolescence over and
in a negative light. This may ultimately lead them above children’s early IQ. When Stams and col-
to feel incapable, thereby undermining effective leagues (2002) examined the link between adopted
investment and engagement among children. children’s attachment to their biologically unrelated
Consistent with this perspective, the quality parents at 12 months and a composite of children’s
of children’s attachment to their parents, often as motivation, skills, and grades in the academic arena
manifest in their representations of it, is predictive at 7 years of age, they found that the children with
of their motivation as well as achievement dur- the most insecure attachments to their parents (i.e.,
ing not only the childhood and adolescence years disorganized) experienced problems in the aca-
(e.g., Granot & Mayseless, 2001; Stams, Juffer, demic arena; however, this effect was evident only
& van IJzendoorn, 2002) but also the transition among children who also had a difficult tempera-
to adulthood (e.g., Elliot & Reis, 2003; Larose, ment early in life.
Bernier, & Tarabulsky, 2005). For example, Moss Building on the original tenets of attachment
and St-Laurent (2001) found that children who theory, contemporary investigators have speculated
were securely attached to their mothers at 6 years that several other mechanisms may also contribute
of age were more effectively engaged while working to the role of children’s attachment to their parents
with their mothers on a problem-solving task at this in their academic functioning (for additional mech-
age than were insecurely attached children; more- anisms not discussed here, see Bergin & Bergin,
over, 2 years later, securely attached children were 2009; van IJzendoorn et al., 1995). Such speculation
more mastery motivated in the academic arena. is of import because constructs reflecting children’s
This may be responsible for the effects of children’s exploration (e.g., mastery motivation) and internal
attachment—or at least their representations of working models (e.g., self-confidence) do not fully
it—on their subsequent achievement (Jacobsen & account for the link between children’s attachment
Hofman, 1997). Notably, Jacobsen and colleagues to their parents and their achievement (e.g., Aviezer
(1994) demonstrated that when children hold et al., 2002; Jacobsen & Hofman, 1997). For one,
secure (versus insecure) representations of attach- when children are securely attached to their parents,
ment at 7 years of age, they appear more confident they may be both more able and willing to meet the
at this age, which accounts for the effect of their learning demands of their parents, often adopting
attachment representations on their later cognitive them as their own. In this vein, in what they term
competence, over and above their earlier IQ. the attachment-teaching hypothesis, van IJzendoorn
It is unclear whether these effects reflect the influ- and colleagues (1995) make the case that when
ence of the quality of children’s early attachment children are securely attached to their parents, they

p omer a n tz , c h eun g , qi n 


may be better able than their insecurely attached Consistent with this idea, the effect of children’s
counterparts to attend to their parents’ learning attachment to their parents on their subsequent cog-
practices (e.g., instruction) because they are not dis- nitive competence is due in part to the quality of chil-
tracted by concerns about their relationships with dren’s relationships with their teachers (O’Connor &
their parents; instead they feel safe even when con- McCartney, 2007).
fronted with challenge—a common occurrence in Given that the quality of children’s attachment to
the learning context. In addition, because they trust their parents appears to contribute to their motivation
their parents, securely (versus insecurely) attached as well as achievement in the academic arena, the issue
children may be more willing to take on the val- of how to foster secure attachments between children
ues conveyed by their parents’ learning-related and their parents is of import. Bowlby (1969) postu-
practices (Furrer & Skinner, 2003; Kochanska, lates that the quality of children’s attachment to their
Aksan, Knaack, & Rhines, 2004). Thus, children’s parents is dependent to a large extent on parents’ sen-
secure attachment to their parents may lead parents’ sitivity to children’s needs and desires (see also Ains-
learning-related practices to be particularly effective worth et al., 1978), with much evidence to support
in enhancing children’s motivation and ultimately this idea (for a review, see Wolff & van IJzendoorn,
achievement. Suggestive of this idea, the more 1997). Of particular import to fostering children’s
mothers characterize their relationships with their academic functioning, there is some evidence that
kindergarten children as warm, the more positive when parents are sensitive early in children’s lives,
the effects of their involvement in their children’s such functioning among children is enhanced (e.g.,
learning for children’s achievement at this phase Frodi et al., 1985; Stams et al., 2002). For example,
of development (Simpkins, Weiss, McCartney, Tamis-LeMonda, Bornstein, and Baumwell (2001)
Kreider, & Dearing, 2006). observed that mothers’ heightened sensitivity during
Much of the theory and research generated by the first year of children’s lives is predictive over time
Bowlby’s attachment theory has focused on the of children’s advanced language development. More-
implications of the quality of children’s attachments over, as delineated in the next section, starting in the
to their parents for children’s relationships with preschool years, parents’ emotional support, which
others—particularly, their peers and teachers (for a is considered a core component of their sensitivity
review, see Schneider, Atkinson, & Tardiff, 2001). (Wolff & van IJzendoorn, 1997), is predictive of
The key idea is that the internal working models chil- children’s subsequent motivation as well as achieve-
dren develop in the context of their relationships with ment (e.g., Estrada, Arsenio, Hess, & Holloway,
their parents are applied by children to their relation- 1987; Grolnick, Kurowski, Dunlap, & Hevey, 2000;
ships with others (Bretherton, 1985; Main, Kaplan, Pomerantz, Wang, & Ng, 2005). However, research
& Cassidy, 1985). These relationships, in turn, may is necessary to determine whether children’s attach-
promote children’s motivational development in two ment to their parents—rather than other mechanisms
key ways. First, children’s relationships with their (e.g., children’s feelings of autonomy)—underlies the
peers contribute to their academic functioning (for a effects of parents’ sensitivity or other related dimen-
review, see Ladd, 2003): The more positive children’s sions of parenting, such as emotional support, on
relationships with their peers, the less they are at risk children’s academic functioning.
for motivational as well as achievement problems as
they are not preoccupied with relational difficulties Summary
in the classroom. Van IJzendoorn and colleagues In sum, from an attachment theory perspective,
(1995) also make the case that children’s relation- children’s relationships with their parents serve as
ships with their peers can be cognitively stimulating, an important context for the development of their
but only if there is trust in the relationships which motivation, thereby contributing to their achieve-
allows children to use their peers’ resources optimally. ment. Multiple mechanisms have been posited
Second, the quality of children’s attachment to their to account for the role of the quality of children’s
parents may shape the quality of their relationships attachment to their parents in their academic func-
with their teachers; this may contribute to how chil- tioning. Early on, investigators focused on the
dren are treated by their teachers as well as children’s idea of securely attached children using their par-
responsiveness to their teachers’ instruction, thereby ents as a trusted base from which to explore, with
influencing children’s academic functioning (Bergin positive internal working models also being influ-
& Bergin, 2009; Pianta, Nimetz, & Bennett, 1997). ential. However, as more attention was directed

 rel atedness bet ween children and pa ren ts


to the issue, other mechanisms were posited—for the attachment theory perspective, the relatedness
example, the ensuing response of children to their that ensues from parents’ involvement is postulated
parents’ instruction as well as children’s ensuing to have a validating function in that it indicates
relationships with their peers and teachers. There that the central figures in children’s lives care about
is some evidence that the early attachments chil- them, allowing children to feel worthy. Ultimately,
dren have with their parents play a role in their later children may come to view themselves as capa-
academic functioning, but it remains an area for ble, with such feelings leading to investment and
further investigation. engagement (Grolnick, Deci, et al., 1997; Grolnick
& Farkas, 2002). In addition, children’s relatedness
The Self-Determination Theory Perspective to their parents is viewed as facilitating children’s
Investigators working from a self-determination internalization of their parents’ values (Grolnick &
theory perspective have also deemed children’s Slowiaczek, 1994). In a somewhat different vein,
relatedness to their parents of import in children’s relatedness has been posited to have a direct ener-
academic functioning. In the context of self- gizing function that permits engagement with the
determination theory, Deci and Ryan (1985, 2000) world; when there is a lack of relatedness, disaffec-
posit that critical to motivation is the fulfillment tion occurs (Connell & Wellborn, 1990; Deci &
of basic psychological needs, which they view as Ryan, 2000; Furrer & Skinner, 2003).
specifying “innate psychological nutrients” (p. 227, In line with Grolnick and colleagues’ application
Deci & Ryan, 2000) without which psychological of self-determination theory to parents’ socialization
functioning suffers. Relatedness represents one of of children, parents’ involvement in their children’s
the three central needs, with the other two being lives is associated with enhanced feelings of capabil-
autonomy and competence. The relatedness need ity among children (for recent reviews, see Grolnick
is fulfilled when children have a sense of security et al., 2009; Pomerantz & Moorman, 2010). In both
in the context of their relationships with significant concurrent and longitudinal investigations focusing
others as well as feel that such others consider them on the academic arena, the more parents are involved
as worthy of affection and positive regard (Connell in their children’s learning (e.g., attending school
& Wellborn, 1990). Self-determination theory does events or reading with children), the more positively
not give the relationships children have with their children perceive their competence as well as feel in
primary caretakers in the early years the special sta- control in school (e.g., Dearing, McCartney, Weiss,
tus that such relationships are given in attachment Kreider, & Simpkins, 2004; Grolnick & Slowiaczek,
theory. Thus, relationships with others at various 1994; Hong & Ho, 2005). Such involvement also
points across the life span are viewed as able to ful- foreshadows children’s heightened investment and
fill relatedness needs, relatively independent of early engagement in school (Cheung & Pomerantz, 2011).
relationships with primary caretakers. The motivation developed by parents’ involvement
Deci and Ryan (2000) make the case that appears to pay off as parents’ involvement consis-
although the early relationships between children tently predicts children’s heightened achievement (for
and their parents contribute to feelings of related- reviews, see Fan & Chen, 2001; Hill & Tyson, 2009),
ness later in life, critical to such feelings are the even when children’s earlier achievement (e.g., Che-
psychological resources provided in the proximal ung & Pomerantz, 2011; Izzo, Weissberg, Kasprow,
environment. In this vein, in her application of & Fendrich, 1999) as well as socioeconomic status
self-determination theory to parents’ socialization (e.g., Deslandes, Bouchard, & St-Amant, 1998;
of children, Grolnick and colleagues (e.g., Grol- Jeynes, 2005; Senechal & LeFevre, 2002) is taken
nick, Deci, et al., 1997; Grolnick & Farkas, 2002) into account. Notably, children’s beliefs about their
highlight the import of parents’ involvement in capability in the academic arena account in large part
their children’s lives in facilitating the satisfaction for the association between parents’ involvement and
of children’s need for relatedness. These investiga- children’s achievement (Dearing et al., 2004; Grolnick
tors maintain that parents’ involvement, which they & Slowiaczek, 1994; Hong & Ho, 2005). However,
define as parents’ dedication of resources to children research has not examined whether the effect of par-
as manifest in such practices as spending time with ents’ involvement on children’s academic functioning
children, efforts to learn about children’s lives, and is due to children’s relatedness to their parents.
warmth toward children, fosters the development of As research by Pomerantz, Wang, and Ng
feelings of relatedness among children. Similar to (2005) reveals, parents’ involvement as reflected in

p omer a n tz , c h eun g , qi n 


their warmth is also of import: The more positive Although both parents’ involvement and auton-
mothers’ affect when interacting with their children omy support appear to lead children to experience
on days children have homework, the less children’s themselves as capable, Grolnick and colleagues (e.g.,
negative affect in this often frustrating context is Grolnick, Deci, et al., 1997; Grolnick & Farkas,
detrimental to their subsequent perceptions of 2002) maintain that particularly critical in this vein
competence as well as intrinsic and mastery motiva- is parents’ structure. Structure involves parents’ pro-
tion (see also Hokoda & Fincham, 1995; Nolen- vision of clear and consistent guidelines, expecta-
Hoeksema, Wolfson, Mumme, & Guskin, 1995). tions, and rules for children. Structuring parents
Although parents’ warmth is clearly of import to also communicate predictable consequences for
the development of children’s motivation, it needs children’s actions (e.g., what will happen if rules are
to be unconditional in that it is not expressed only violated). In addition, structure includes providing
when children do as parents desire. When parents children with instruction that takes into account
make their warmth conditional on children think- children’s capacity. Such parenting can promote
ing, feeling, or acting as parents desire, they lead feelings of capability in that it assists children in
children to feel pressured by a fear of losing their not only identifying societally valued standards but
parents’ positive regard (Roth, Assor, Niemiec, also developing the skills to achieve them. Although
Ryan, & Deci, 2009). Indeed, when parents’ posi- parents’ structure is not associated with children’s
tive regard for their children is contingent on their intrinsic motivation (Grolnick & Ryan, 1989), it
children doing well in the academic arena, children is associated with heightened feelings of capability,
are engaged for performance rather than mastery both in terms of perceptions of competence and
reasons (Roth et al., 2009). a sense of control, among children (e.g., Grolnick &
Although relatedness is viewed as important for Ryan, 1989; Skinner, Johnson, & Snyder, 2005).
the development of motivation in self-determination This is likely due in part to the effects of parents’
theory, it is not viewed as the only influence on structure on children’s skills. Research conducted by
motivation—or even the most important. Indeed, Englund and colleagues (2004), for example, reveals
Grolnick and colleagues (e.g., Grolnick, Deci, that the more structured mothers’ instruction when
et al., 1997; Grolnick & Farkas, 2002) argue that children are 3 years old, the higher children’s cogni-
once parents are involved in their children’s lives, tive competence at 5 years old, and the better their
key to promoting children’s intrinsic motivation is achievement in the first and third grades (see also
parents’ autonomy support: When parents support Pianta et al., 1997).
children’s autonomy instead of exerting control over
them by allowing children to take initiative rather Summary
than dictating what children do, parents facilitate Similar to the attachment theory perspective, the
the development of children’s feelings of autonomy, self-determination theory perspective regards chil-
thereby allowing for the development of intrinsic dren’s relatedness to their parents as important in
motivation among children. This process appears to the development of children’s motivation, thereby
begin early in children’s lives as evidenced by Frodi shaping their achievement. Also like the attachment
and colleagues’ (1985) finding that when mothers theory perspective, the self-determination theory
are autonomy supportive during children’s first 2 perspective posits that children’s relatedness to their
years, children display heightened persistence in parents may serve a validating function that allows
problem solving during this phase of development. them to feel worthy, with implications for their feel-
As children move into the school years, the more ings of capability. However, there is a focus not evi-
parents support children’s autonomy rather than dent in attachment theory on the proximal forces
exert control over children, the more children are that lead to children’s relatedness; consequently,
intrinsically motivated in the academic arena (e.g., parents’ involvement in their children’s lives is seen
d’Ailly, 2003; Grolnick & Ryan, 1989; Grolnick, as key to facilitating children’s fulfillment of their
Ryan, & Deci, 1991). Parents’ autonomy support relatedness needs, thereby playing a role in chil-
is also associated with children feeling more capable dren’s academic functioning. Moreover, according
as well as being more invested and engaged, with to self-determination theory, other needs and thus
heightened achievement (e.g., Grolnick & Ryan, dimensions of parenting other than involvement are
1989; Hess & McDevitt, 1984; Ng, Kenney- central in the development of children’s motivation:
Benson, & Pomerantz, 2004; Steinberg, Elmen, & Parents must not only be involved in their children’s
Mounts, 1989; Wang, Pomerantz, & Chen, 2007). lives but also autonomy supportive and structuring

 rel atedness bet ween children and pa ren ts


so that in addition to feeling related, children feel more confident in their capabilities or intrinsically
autonomous and competent. motivated, in the academic arena, which may ben-
efit their achievement.
Beyond Relationship Quality: Children’s Although this process may be driven by con-
Sense of Responsibility to Their Parents trolled motivation – for example, children’s fear
At the heart of both the attachment theory and of losing their parents’ positive regard, it may be
self-determination theory perspectives is children’s effective in ensuring children are engaged in school,
relatedness to their parents as reflected in the qual- particularly during adolescence. Controlled motiva-
ity of their relationships with their parents—that tion, which may be fostered by rewards or punish-
is, the extent to which children have secure (ver- ment, is useful when autonomous motivation does
sus insecure) or positive (versus negative) relation- not already exist (for a review, see Deci, Koestner,
ships with their parents. Although the quality of & Ryan, 1999), as is often the case for children
children’s relationships with their parents is clearly in the academic context during adolescence (for a
of significance to children’s academic functioning, review, see Wigfield & Wagner, 2005). Indeed, con-
it is not the only form that children’s relatedness trolled motivation may promote engagement and
to their parents takes (Hardway & Fuligni, 2006; achievement—at least when deep processing is not
Pomerantz, Qin, Wang, & Chen, 2009). In this sec- necessary (Vansteenkiste, Simons, Lens, Soenens,
tion, we focus on another form that is of import & Matos, 2005). It is also possible that children’s
to children’s motivation with their parents: Chil- sense of responsibility to their parents is in part a
dren’s sense of responsibility to their parents—that is, form of autonomous motivation because it grows
the belief among children that it is important that out of a relationship of reciprocal give and take
they provide psychological or material assistance to between children and their parents (Pomerantz,
their parents (e.g., by meeting their parents’ expec- Qin, Wang, & Chen, 2011), so that children view
tations for their performance or helping with chores their responsibilities to their parents as personally
around the house). Children’s sense of responsibility important. Indeed, children’s sense of responsibility
to their parents has been studied by Fuligni and col- to their parents is associated not only with children’s
leagues (e.g., Fuligni, Tseng, & Lam, 1999; Fuligni controlled, but also autonomous, motivation in the
& Zhang, 2004) as manifest in their feelings of academic context (Pomerantz et al., 2011).
obligation to their family. Pomerantz, Qin, Wang, Growing evidence reveals that children’s sense of
and Chen (2011) make the case that children’s sense responsibility to their parents contributes to their
of responsibility to their parents is also manifest in academic functioning. In both the United States
their motivation to please their parents—that is, and China, the more children feel obligated to their
children’s pursuit of goals to obtain their parents’ family during the high school years, the more they
approval. value school (Fuligni et al., 1999; Fuligni & Zhang,
These two manifestations of children’s sense of 2004; Pomerantz et al., 2011). Research conducted
responsibility to their parents may enhance chil- in the United States also indicates that children of
dren’s academic functioning by leading children to European, Chinese, and Mexican heritage who feel
use their parents’ values as guides as they attempt obligated to their family during adolescence are par-
to fulfill their responsibilities to them (Fuligni & ticularly likely to devote time to their schoolwork
Flook, 2005). Fuligni and colleagues (Fuligni & (Fuligni, Yip, & Tseng, 2002; Hardway & Fuligni,
Flook, 2005; Telzer & Fuligni, 2009) contend 2006). However, children’s feelings of obligation to
that children’s feelings of obligation to their fam- their family are not associated with their enhanced
ily along with the activities motivated by such feel- achievement—perhaps because other obligations
ings are beneficial in that they provide children with to the family (e.g., doing chores and taking care
a sense of purpose, often evident in feelings of role of siblings) interfere with the effectiveness of their
fulfillment. Although some parents do not see chil- academic efforts so that children are not optimally
dren’s learning in the academic arena as a priority, focused. Notably, children’s sense of responsibility
many parents place at least some value on this arena. to their parents as manifest in their motivation in
Thus, children’s sense of responsibility to their par- school to please them appears to foster their moti-
ents may often lead children to place value on the vation as well as achievement. Studying children
academic arena. As a consequence, children harbor- in the United States and China during early ado-
ing a sense of responsibility to their parents may be lescence, Pomerantz and colleagues (2011) found
more invested and engaged, albeit not necessarily that the more motivated children were to please

p omer a n tz , c h eun g , qi n 


their parents, the greater their investment and effects of the quality of their relationships with their
engagement as well as grades in school 2 years later, parents. Future research is needed to identify the
even after taking into account children’s earlier aca- mechanisms by which children’s sense of respon-
demic functioning. sibility to their parents enhances their academic
The greater children’s sense of responsibility functioning.
to their parents (i.e., the more they feel obligated
to their parents and the more motivated they are to Integrating the Different Ideas About
please their parents), the better the quality of their Relatedness
relationships with their parents (e.g., Fuligni et al., The three sets of ideas we have reviewed share an
1999; Fuligni & Zhang, 2004; Pomerantz et al., emphasis on children’s relatedness to their parents
2011). Although the associations are not strong as a significant force in the development of their
enough to suggest that children’s sense of respon- motivation, with implications for their achieve-
sibility to their parents is simply a reflection of the ment. Although there is convergence among the
quality of their relationships with them, they beg three along some lines (e.g., children’s relatedness
the question of whether the effects of children’s to their parents is viewed as leading children to take
sense of responsibility to their parents remain once on their parents’ values), there is also divergence
the quality of their relationships with their parents (e.g., the form of relatedness deemed of import var-
is taken into account. Notably, when Pomerantz ies). In this section, we offer an integration of the
and colleagues (2011) statistically controlled for the three sets of ideas. Such integration is a fruitful step
quality of children’s relationships with their parents, toward fully elucidating how children’s relatedness
children’s sense of responsibility to their parents to their parents contributes to their academic func-
continued to exert an effect over time on children’s tioning across different phases of their development
academic functioning. A key issue that has unfortu- in different cultural contexts. We outline three key
nately not received attention is the extent to which themes that evolve from considering the different
children’s adoption of their parents’ values accounts perspectives together. In doing so, we highlight
for the effects of children’s sense of responsibility to critical questions that may serve to guide future
their parents on children’s motivation. It is possible research and theory.
that other mechanisms may also be at work—for
example, children’s sense of responsibility to their Theme 1: Early Relatedness Matters,
parents may facilitate their spontaneous disclosure But Is Not Deterministic
about their daily lives to their parents which has Consistent with the attachment theory perspec-
been argued to provide an important context for tive, children’s early relatedness to their parents may
parents to gain knowledge about their children’s set an important foundation for children’s later
lives (e.g., Kerr & Stattin, 2000). This may allow academic functioning. Indeed, as reviewed earlier,
parents to provide support to children in the aca- there is much evidence that the security of chil-
demic arena. dren’s attachment to their parents during the first
few years of their life contributes to their motivation
Summary as well as achievement during these years. Moreover,
Although the quality of children’s relationships it appears that such relatedness is of import for chil-
with their parents is of import to children’s aca- dren as they enter the school system where they may
demic functioning as postulated in the attachment be confronted with new challenges. The extent to
theory and self-determination theory perspectives, which children’s early attachment to their parents
other forms of children’s relatedness to their par- continues to exert an influence over their academic
ents appear to be instrumental as well. Here, we functioning in the later years of children’s develop-
have focused on children’s sense of responsibility to ment, however, is unclear. Indeed, research on the
their parents, which appears to facilitate children’s longer term effects is sparse. Although the one study
academic functioning during the adolescent years. in this vein of which we are aware provides evidence
Children’s sense of responsibility to their parents for such effects into adolescence, the effects are
may be particularly functional during this phase quite small, evident among mothers but not fathers,
of development given children’s declining interest and do not reach significance when it comes to chil-
in school. Notably, the effects of children’s sense of dren’s grades (Aviezer et al., 2002).
responsibility to their parents on children’s motiva- These findings along with the ideas put forth by
tion as well as achievement are distinct from the investigators working from the self-determination

 rel atedness bet ween children and pa ren ts


theory perspective suggest that as children develop, however, this important issue has not received
although their early relatedness to their parents empirical attention. Such attention is needed given
plays a role in their motivation, children’s later other viable alternatives: Parents’ involvement—at
environments are influential as well. Ultimately, least in children’s learning—may directly enhance
children’s early attachment to their parents may children’s skills (Senechal & LeFevre, 2002) or indi-
not be deterministic as it may be overridden—at rectly do so by leading teachers to give children
least in part—by the subsequent environment cre- more attention in the classroom (Epstein & Becker,
ated by their parents or other significant figures in 1982), both of which may enhance children’s moti-
their lives such as their peers or teachers. Parents vation as well as achievement.
may undo the costs or benefits of their early attach- The security of children’s early attachment to
ment relationships with their children through their their parents may also work synergistically with par-
involvement in their children’s lives, which is influ- ents’ later involvement to shape children’s academic
enced in part by parents’ physical and psychological functioning. On the one hand, children’s attach-
resources (for a review, see Pomerantz, Moorman, ment may serve an amplifying function such that
& Cheung, 2011). Such resources may change a secure (versus insecure) attachment sets a founda-
over the course of children’s development, thereby tion among children allowing them to reap greater
changing parents’ involvement. For example, when benefits from their parents’ later involvement in
a family’s income increases over time, parents cre- their learning (e.g., van IJzendoorn et al., 1995).
ate better learning environments for their children On the other hand, children’s attachment may serve
along physical as well as psychological lines (e.g., a compensatory function: A secure (versus insecure)
Dearing & Taylor, 2007). Parents may also experi- attachment early in children’s lives may make up for
ence changes in their social support over time that dampened involvement among parents in their chil-
may lead to changes in their involvement in chil- dren’s learning later in children’s lives. That is, when
dren’s lives (e.g., Gavidia-Payne & Stoneman, 1997; parents do not have the desire or resources to be
Grolnick, Benjet, Kurowski, & Apostoleris, 1997; involved in their children’s learning, children may
Sheldon, 2002). still make motivational gains if their secure attach-
The proposal that the effects of children’s early ment to their parents provides them with a trustable
relatedness to their parents are superseded to some base from which to explore as well as other resources
extent by children’s later relatedness to their par- such as positive internal working models.
ents fostered in part by their parents’ involvement Distinguishing between the amplifying and
begs empirical examination of two key questions. compensating functions of children’s early attach-
First, to what extent are the effects of children’s ment to their parents requires research examining
early attachment to their parents and their parents’ whether such relatedness moderates the effect of
later involvement in their lives independent of one parents’ subsequent involvement in their children’s
another? On the one hand, it is possible that the learning on children’s academic functioning. Simp-
two are entirely overlapping. Parents’ early involve- kins and colleagues’ (2006) research showing that
ment may contribute to an early secure attachment; children who have warm relationships with their
over the course of children’s development, parents’ mothers as well as highly involved mothers have
continued involvement may maintain such an higher achievement than other children is a step in
attachment, thereby fostering children’s motivation. this direction. However, the focus of this research,
On the other hand, it is possible that although par- which is suggestive of the amplifying function, was
ents’ involvement may contribute to early attach- not on children’s early attachment to their parents;
ment, parents’ involvement changes over time as thus, it is unclear what kind of foundation such
their physical and psychological resources change, relatedness provides for the success of subsequent
thereby changing children’s motivational trajectory parenting in promoting children’s academic func-
stemming from their early attachment. tioning. Moreover, an endeavor of this sort should
Second, if parents’ later involvement has an inde- look at the mechanisms underlying the synergy of
pendent effect, to what extent does it do so through children’s early attachment to their parents and their
its impact on children’s relatedness to their parents? parents’ later involvement. Also in need of atten-
A key tenet of the self-determination theory perspec- tion is the possibility that children’s early attach-
tive is that parents’ involvement fosters relatedness, ment changes how parents are involved, with secure
which in part is responsible for its role in chil- attachment driving more positive, effective involve-
dren’s academic functioning. As highlighted earlier, ment on parents’ part (van IJzendoorn et al., 1995).

p omer a n tz , c h eun g , qi n 


Indeed, there is some evidence supportive of this significance in children’s lives during adolescence
possibility (O’Connor & McCartney, 2007). (e.g., Furman & Buhrmester, 1992; Larson, Richards,
Moneta, Holmbeck, & Duckett, 1996).
Theme 2: There Are Contextual Variations Direct empirical examination of this develop-
in the Role of Relatedness mental hypothesis is necessary. The contribution
For quite some time the attachment theory and of children’s sense of responsibility to their parents,
self-determination theory perspectives have served along with the quality (e.g., security or positivity)
to guide research on how relatedness between chil- of children’s relationships with their parents, should
dren and their parents contributes to children’s aca- be identified from the earliest to the latest school
demic functioning; this has led to a focus on the years. The effect of children’s sense of responsibility
quality of relationships between children and their to their parents on their motivation may become
parents. However, more recent theory and research larger over time as children move into adolescence,
on the adolescent years has highlighted the signifi- whereas that of the quality of children’s relation-
cance of another form of children’s relatedness to ships with their parents may be maintained. In this
their parents—children’s sense of responsibility to context, attention needs to be given to the possibil-
them—that while related to the quality of children’s ity that children’s sense of responsibility to their par-
relationships with their parents, is distinct from it ents grows out of a positive relationship with them,
with unique effects on children’s academic func- thereby acting as a mechanism through which the
tioning. Children’s sense of responsibility to their quality of children’s relationships with their parents
parents may be of particular import in two key con- enhances their motivation.
texts: (1) during phases of children’s development Also worthy of consideration is that children’s
when their interest in learning declines and (2) in sense of responsibility to their parents may be most
cultures, such as that characteristic of East Asian beneficial when parents remain involved in their
countries, in which children may not have particu- children’s academic lives during adolescence—
larly positive relationships with their parents, but a time when the norm is often for parents to become
learning may be particularly important. less involved in this arena of children’s lives (e.g.,
Although it is likely that children’s sense of Cheung & Pomerantz, 2011; Cooper, Lindsay, &
responsibility to their parents develops prior to ado- Nye, 2000). By being involved in their children’s
lescence, playing a role in their academic function- learning, parents may convey to children that they
ing during these earlier years, its role may be of most value school, leading children to view doing well in
significance during adolescence. It is well docu- school as their responsibility. Indeed, during early
mented that as children move into adolescence their adolescence, the more parents are involved in chil-
investment and engagement in school declines, with dren’s learning, the more children are motivated
a parallel decline in their achievement (for a review, in school to please their parents, which is predic-
see Wigfield et al., 2006). Eccles and colleagues tive of children’s subsequent engagement and ulti-
(1993) have argued that such decrements are due mately achievement (Cheung & Pomerantz, 2009).
in part to a poor fit between children’s concerns as Through their involvement in children’s academic
they move into adolescence (e.g., with establish- lives, parents may also support children as they
ing independence and avoiding negative evalua- attempt to fulfill their academic responsibilities.
tion) and the environment provided by the middle For example, by being involved parents may pro-
schools that they transition into from elementary vide children with encouragement when children
school (e.g., teachers’ heightened control and evalu- experience difficulty as may often be the case dur-
ative feedback). During adolescence, the quality of ing adolescence. In addition, through their involve-
children’s relationships with their parents is predic- ment, parents may provide instruction that allows
tive of their academic functioning (e.g., Furrer & children to develop the abilities necessary to meet
Skinner, 2003; Ryan, Stiller, & Lynch, 1994), but their responsibilities in the academic arena.
alone it may not be enough to sustain children dur- Children’s sense of responsibility to their parents
ing this phase of development. Optimal motivation may be of particular import not only during ado-
may require additional forces that are instrumen- lescence but also in cultures where much emphasis
tal in moving children toward adopting the values is placed on learning as well as filial piety, as is the
of their parents, which are less likely to deviate case in East Asian countries, such as China. In fact,
from those endorsed by society than are the val- considering children’s sense of responsibility to their
ues of children’s peers who often attain heightened parents may be key to understanding the apparent

 rel atedness bet ween children and pa ren ts


paradox that despite reporting poorer quality rela- and Latin heritage accounts for the heightened value
tionships with their parents during adolescence (e.g., they place on school compared to their American
Pomerantz et al., 2009), East Asian children are more counterparts of other ethnic heritage. Further exami-
motivated, with heightened achievement, compared nation of the issue between countries is needed to
to American children during this phase of develop- ensure that the effects are not due to immigrant or
ment (e.g., Stevenson, Chen, & Lee, 1993; Wang minority status. In this context, other dimensions of
& Pomerantz, 2009). This paradox is particularly children’s motivation besides value should be exam-
intriguing given that the quality of children’s rela- ined, as should children’s achievement.
tionships with their parents appears to make equal
contributions to children’s academic functioning in Theme 3: Relatedness Is Not All That Matters
China and the United States (Cheung, Pomerantz, Most reviews focusing on the role of parents in
& Dong, 2010). Thus, a key question is what com- children’s academic functioning highlight the prac-
pensates for the poorer quality relationships among tices that parents employ that facilitate or under-
children and their parents in East Asia (versus the mine such functioning (e.g., Eccles, 2007; Grolnick
United States) in motivating children. Although et al., 2009; Pomerantz & Moorman, 2010; Pomer-
there are a variety of possibilities (e.g., teaching and antz et al., 2007). Indeed, there is a sizeable body
parenting practices), children’s sense of responsibil- of research indicating that multiple dimensions of
ity to their parents is likely to be influential. parenting contribute to children’s motivation as well
The focus on children’s sense of responsibility as achievement. Our goal in this chapter was to go
to their parents emerged largely in the context of beyond these prior reviews to emphasize the import
efforts to understand children from cultural back- of relatedness between children and their parents—
grounds in which the family looms larger than it whether it be established in the earliest years of
does in European American culture (e.g., Fuligni children’s lives or the later years, and whether it be
et al., 1999). For example, in East Asian countries the quality of the relationships between children
where Confucian philosophy is central, children’s and their parents or children’s sense of responsibil-
sense of responsibility to their parents may grow out ity to their parents. However, as is emphasized in
of the notion of filial piety, which involves, among self-determination theory, although relatedness is of
other things, children repaying their family for their import to children’s motivational development, it is
efforts in raising them, bringing honor to their certainly not the only force.
family, making sacrifices for their family, and psy- If parents create an environment, often through
chologically and materially supporting their family their involvement, in which children feel related to
(Chao & Tseng, 2002; Ho, 1996). Doing well in them, this on its own may not necessarily translate
school may be a central way for children to fulfill into motivation among children that optimizes their
their responsibilities to their parents in East Asian achievement. Although parents’ involvement in chil-
countries given the import of learning in Confucian dren’s lives is a critical first step toward promoting
philosophy (Ho, 1994; Yu, 1996) as well as profes- children’s motivation, such involvement likely needs
sional and financial success (Tang, Luk, & Chiu, to be accompanied by autonomy support and struc-
2000). ture (Grolnick et al., 2009; Pomerantz, Grolnick,
In line with such reasoning, soon after entering & Price, 2005; Pomerantz et al., 2007). Autonomy-
adolescence, children of Chinese heritage—both supportive and structuring practices may afford
residing in China and the United States—feel more key resources to children that no degree of related-
obligated to their parents and more motivated in ness may afford. For example, even if children have
school to please them than do children of European established a positive relationship with their parents,
heritage residing in the United States (e.g., Fuligni if their parents do not employ structuring practices,
et al., 1999; Hardway & Fuligni, 2006; Pomerantz the feelings of capability fostered by children’s relat-
et al., 2011). Chinese (versus American) children’s edness with their parents may be undermined as
greater sense of responsibility to their parents may children do not develop necessary skills.
compensate for the poorer quality relationships they In the two earlier themes we delineated, we
have with their parents during adolescence. Indeed, highlighted how children’s relatedness to their par-
focusing on ethnic differences in the United States in ents may work synergistically with their parents’
the value children place on school during adolescence, involvement in shaping their academic functioning.
Fuligni (2001) reports that the heightened feelings Children’s relatedness to their parents, as well as the
of obligation to their families of children of Chinese involvement assumed to foster it, may also work

p omer a n tz , c h eun g , qi n 


synergistically with parents’ autonomy support and the academic context. However, there are still key
structure. On the one hand, children’s relatedness issues to be resolved in understanding the role of
may serve an amplifying function. For example, the children’s relatedness to their parents. We high-
reduced anxiety produced by children’s secure attach- lighted three themes that emerge from an integra-
ment to their parents may allow them to capitalize tion of the three different sets of ideas we reviewed
on their parents’ autonomy support in that they are on the role of relatedness between children and their
able to take full advantage of autonomy opportu- parents in children’s academic functioning: (1) Both
nities, such as making choices or solving problems the early attachment between children and their
on their own. Children’s sense of responsibility to parents and the later environment created by par-
their parents may also lead children to use auton- ents are of import to children’s academic function-
omy opportunities to pursue societally valued goals ing; (2) there is contextual variation in the effects of
held by their parents. On the other hand, children’s children’s relatedness to their parents, particularly in
relatedness to their parents may play a compensa- regard to children’s sense of responsibility to their
tory function by providing them with resources to parents; and (3) although children’s relatedness to
buffer the undermining effects of parenting that is their parents is instrumental in their academic func-
not autonomy supportive or structuring. For exam- tioning, it alone may not be sufficient in optimiz-
ple, children residing in a home without structure ing it. A comprehensive understanding of the role
may have a trustable base from which to find struc- of relatedness between parents and children in chil-
ture outside of their home, such as that supplied by dren’s motivation as well as achievement requires
teachers, coaches, and friends’ parents. empirical tests of these themes.
Empirical tests of the ideas outlined here are
needed. Perhaps most notably, investigation of
the synergies is an important direction for future
References
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & Wall, S.
research. Suggestive of such interactions are findings (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psychological study of the
yielded by research conducted by Kochanska and strange situation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
colleagues (2004): Consistent with the idea of an Aviezer, O., Sagi, A., Resnick, G., & Gini, M. (2002). School
amplifying function, parents’ responsiveness (which competence in young adolesence: Links to early attachment
relationships beyond concurrent self-perceived competence
involves some autonomy support) and gentle dis- and representations of relationships. Internaltional Journal of
cipline (which is an aspect of structure) during the Behavioral Development, 26, 397–409.
early years of children’s lives mattered most for chil- Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong:
dren in terms of their subsequent conscience when Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental
children were securely (versus insecurely) attached to human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Belsky, J., Garduque, L., & Hrncir, E. (1984). Assessing perfor-
their parents during the early years. However, there mance, competence, and executive capacity in infant play:
is no evidence to date as to whether children’s relat- Relations to home environment and security of attachment.
edness moderates the effects of parents’ learning- Developmental Psychology, 20, 406–417.
related practices on their academic functioning in Bergin, C., & Bergin, D. (2009). Attachment in the classroom.
such a manner. Examination of the moderating role Educational Psychology Review, 21, 141–170.
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Vol 1: Attachment.
of children’s relatedness to their parents as manifest New York: Basic Books.
both in the quality of their relationships with their Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss. Vol 2: Separation: Anxiety
parents as well as their sense of responsibility to and anger. New York: Basic Books.
them is of much import. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss. Vol. 3: Loss. New York:
Basic Books.
Bretherton, I. (1985). Attachment theory: Retrospect and prospect.
Conclusions In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points of attach-
ment theory and research. Monographs of the Society for Research
Children’s relatedness to their parents appears to
in Child Development, 50, Serial No. 209 (1–2), pp. 3–35.
play a fundamental role in the development of their Caldwell, B., & Ricituti, H. (Eds.). (1973). The development of
motivation, ultimately having implications for their infant-mother attachment. New York: Russell Sage.
achievement. Such relatedness, whether reflected Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.). (2008). Handbook of attach-
in the quality of children’s relationships with their ment: Theory, research, and clinical application (2nd ed.).
New York: Guilford Press.
parents or their sense of responsibility to their par-
Chao, R. K., & Tseng, V. (2002). Parenting of Asians. In M. H.
ents, is predictive of children’s subsequent academic Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting. Vol. 4: Social condi-
functioning across multiple phases of the life span, tions and applied parenting (2nd ed., pp. 59–93). Mahwah,
taking into account children’s earlier functioning in NJ: Erlbaum.

 rel atedness bet ween children and pa ren ts


Cheung, C. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2011). Parents’ involvement Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values,
in children’s academic lives in the US and China: Implica- and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 109–132.
tions for children’s academic and emotional adjustment. Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to
Child Development, 82, 932–950. succeed. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology.
Cheung, C. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (April, 2009). Social moti- Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed.,
vation mediates the effects of parents’ involvement on children’s pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley.
school engagement in the US and China. Paper presented at the Elliot, A. J., & Reis, H. T. (2003). Attachment and exploration
Society for Research on Child Development, Denver, CO. in adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85,
Cheung, C. S., Pomerantz, E. M., & Dong, W. (March, 2010). 317–331.
Youth’s disclosure of everyday activities to parents in the United Englund, M. M., Luckner, A. E., Whaley, G. J. L., & Egeland, B.
States and China: Implications for youth’s academic functioning. (2004). Children’s achievement in early elementary school:
Paper presented at the Society for Research on Adolescence, Longitudinal effects of parental involvement, expectations,
Philadelphia, PA. and quality of assistance. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1991). The evolution of parental care. 96, 723–730.
Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Epstein, J. L., & Becker, H. J. (1982). Teachers’ reported prac-
Collins, W. A., & Steinberg, L. (2006). Adolescent development tices of parent involvement: Problems and possibilities. Ele-
in interpersonal context. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook mentary School Journal, 83, 103–113.
of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personal- Estrada, P., Arsenio, W. F., Hess, R. D., & Holloway, S. D. (1987).
ity development (6th ed., pp. 1003–1067). Hoboken, NJ: Affective quality of the mother-child relationship: Longitu-
Wiley. dinal consequences for children’s school-relevant cognitive
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1990). Competence, auton- functioning. Developmental Psychology, 23, 210–215.
omy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self-system Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental involvement and students’
processes. In M. Gunnar & A. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota Sym- academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Educational Psychol-
posium on Child Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 43–77). Hillsdale, ogy Review, 13, 1–22.
NJ: Erlbaum. Fraley, C. (2002). Attachment stability from infancy to adult-
Cooper, H., Lindsay, J. J., & Nye, B. (2000). Homework in the hood: Meta-analysis and dynamic modeling of developmen-
home: How student, family, and parenting-style differences tal mechanisms. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 2,
relate to the homework process. Contemporary Educational 123–151.
Psychology, 25, 464–487. Frodi, A., Bridges, L., & Grolnick, W. S. (1985). Correlates of
d’Ailly, H. (2003). Children’s autonomy and perceived control in mastery-related behavior: A short-term longitudinal study
learning: A model of motivation and achievement in Taiwan. of infants in their second year. Child Development, 56,
Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 84–96. 1291–1298.
Dearing, E., McCartney, K., Weiss, H. B., Kreider, H., & Simpkins, Fuligni, A. J. (2001). Family obligation and the academic moti-
S. (2004). The promotive effects of family educational vation of adolescents from Asian, Latin, and European
involvement for low-income children’s literacy. Journal of American backgrounds. In A. J. Fuligni (Ed.), Family obli-
School Psychology, 42, 445–460. gation and assistance during adolescence: Contextual variations
Dearing, E., & Taylor, B. A. (2007). Home improvements: and developmental implications (pp. 61–75). San Francisco,
Within-family associations between income and the quality CA: Jossey-Bass.
of children’s home environments. Journal of Applied Develop- Fuligni, A. J., & Flook, L. (2005). A social identity approach to
mental Psychology, 28, 427–444. ethnic differences in family relationships. In R. Kail (Ed.),
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 33, pp.
review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic 61–75). New York: Academic Press.
rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, Fuligni, A. J., Tseng, V., & Lam, M. (1999). Attitudes toward
627–668. family obligation among American adolescents with Asian,
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self- Latin American, and European American backgrounds.
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum. Child Development, 70, 1030–1044.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of Fuligni, A. J., Yip, T., & Tseng, V. (2002). The impact of fam-
goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of ily obligation on the daily activities and psychological well-
behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. being of Chinese American adolescents. Child Development,
Deslandes, R., Bouchard, P., & St-Amant, J. (1998). Family 73, 302–314.
variables as predictors of school achievement: Sex differences Fuligni, A. J., & Zhang, W. (2004). Attitudes toward family obli-
in Quebec adolescents. Canadian Journal of Education, 23, gation among adolescents in contemporary urban and rural
390–404. China. Child Development, 75, 180–192.
Eccles, J. S. (2007). Families, schools, and developing Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1992). Age and sex differences
achievement-related motivation and engagement. In J. E. in perceptions of networks of personal relationships. Child
Grusec & P. D. Hastings (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Development, 63, 103–115.
Theory and research (pp. 655–691). New York: Guilford Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor
Press. in children’s academic engagement and performance. Journal
Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, of Educational Psychology, 95, 148–162.
D., Flanagan, C., & Iver, D. M. (1993). Development dur- Gavidia-Payne, S., & Stoneman, Z. (1997). Family predic-
ing adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on tors of maternal and paternal involvement in programs for
young adolescents’ experiences in schools and in families. young children with disabilities. Child Development, 68,
American Psychologist, 48, 90–101. 701–717.

p omer a n tz , c h eun g , qi n 


Granot, D., & Mayseless, O. (2001). Attachment security and academic competency in middle childhood and adolescence.
adjustment to school in middle childhood. Internaltional Developmental Psychology, 33, 703–710.
Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 530–541. Jacobsen, T., Wolfgang, E., & Hofman, V. (1994). A longitudi-
Grolnick, W. S., Benjet, C., Kurowski, C. O., & Apostoleris, nal study of the relation between representations of attach-
N. H. (1997). Predictors of parent involvement in children’s ment in childhood and cognitive functioning in childhood
schooling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 538–548. and adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 30, 112–124.
Grolnick, W. S., Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1997). Internaliza- Jeynes, W. H. (2005). A meta-analysis of the relation of parental
tion within the family: The self-determination theory per- involvement to urban elementary school student academic
spective. In J. Grusec & L. Kuczynski (Eds.), Parenting and achievement. Urban Education, 40, 237–269.
children’s internalization of values: A handbook of contemporary Kerr, M., & Stattin, H. (2000). What parents know, how they
theory (pp. 135–161). New York: Wiley. know it, and several forms of adolescent adjustment: Further
Grolnick, W. S., & Farkas, M. (2002). Parenting and the devel- support for a reinterpretation of monitoring. Developmental
opment of children’s self-regulation. In M. Bornstein (Ed.), Psychology, 36, 366–380.
Handbook of parenting. Vol. 5: Practical issues in parenting Kochanska, G., Aksan, N., Knaack, A., & Rhines, H. M. (2004).
(2nd ed., pp. 89–110). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Maternal parenting and children’s conscience: Early security
Grolnick, W. S., Friendly, R. W., & Bellas, V. M. (2009). Par- as a moderator. Child Development, 75, 1229–1242.
enting and children’s motivation at school. In K. R. Wenzel Ladd, G. W. (2003). Probing the adaptive significance of chil-
& A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. dren’s behavior and relationships in the school context:
279–300). New York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis. A child by environment perspective. In R. Kail (Ed.),
Grolnick, W. S., Kurowski, C. O., Dunlap, K. G., & Hevey, C. Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31, pp.
(2000). Parental resources and the transition to junior high. 43–104). Sand Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 10, 465–488. Larose, S., Bernier, A., & Tarabulsky, G. M. (2005). Attachment
Grolnick, W. S., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Parent styles associated state of mind, learning dispositions, and acadmic perfor-
with children’s self-regulation and competence in school. mance during the college transition. Developmental Psychol-
Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 143–154. ogy, 41, 281–289.
Grolnick, W. S., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1991). Inner Larson, R. W., Richards, M. H., Moneta, G., Holmbeck, G., &
resources for school achievement: Motivational mediators of Duckett, E. (1996). Changes in adolescents’ daily interac-
children’s perceptions of their parents. Journal of Educational tions with their families from ages 10 to 18: Disengagement
Psychology, 83, 508–517. and transformation. Developmental Psychology, 32, 744–754.
Grolnick, W. S., & Slowiaczek, M. L. (1994). Parents’ involve- Main, M., Kaplan, N., & Cassidy, J. (1985). Security in infancy,
ment in children’s schooling: A multidimensional conceptu- childhood, and adulthood: A move to the level of representa-
alization and motivational model. Child Development, 64, tion. In I. Bretherton & E. Waters (Eds.), Growing points
237–252. of attachment theory and research. Monographs of the Society
Hardway, C., & Fuligni, A. J. (2006). Dimensions of family for Research in Child Development, 50, Serial No. 209 (1–2),
connectedness among adolescents with Mexican, Chinese, pp. 66–104.
and European backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 42, Matas, L., Arend, R., & Sroufe, A. (1978). Continuity and adap-
1246–1258. tation in the second year: The relationship between quality
Hess, R. D., & McDevitt, T. M. (1984). Some cognitive con- of attachment and later competence. Child Development, 49,
sequences of maternal intervention: A longitudinal study. 547–556.
Child Development, 55, 2017–2030. Moss, E., & St-Laurent, D. (2001). Attachment at school age
Hill, N. E., & Tyson, D. (2009). Parental involvement in middle and academic performance. Developmental Psychology, 37,
school: A meta-analytic assessment of the strategies that pro- 863–874.
mote achievement. Developmental Psychology, 45, 740–763. Ng, F. F., Kenney-Benson, G. A., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2004).
Ho, D. Y. F. (1994). Cognitive socialization in Confucian heri- Children’s achievement moderates the effects of mothers’ use
tage culture. In P. M. Greenfield & R. R. Cocking (Eds.), of control and autonomy support. Child Development, 75,
Cross-cultural roots of minority child development (pp. 764–780.
285–313). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wolfson, A., Mumme, D., & Guskin, K.
Ho, D. Y. F. (1996). Filial piety and its psychological conse- (1995). Helplessness in children of depressed and nonde-
quences. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), Handbook of Chinese psychol- pressed mothers. Developmental Psychology, 31, 377–387.
ogy (pp. 155–165). New York: Oxford University Press. O’Connor, E., & McCartney, K. (2007). Attachment and cogni-
Hokoda, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children’s tive skills: An investigation of mediating mechanisms. Jour-
helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. nal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 458–476.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 375–385. Offer, D., & Offer, J. B. (1975). Teenage to young manhood:
Hong, S., & Ho, H. (2005). Direct and indirect longitudinal A psychological study. New York: Basic Books.
effects of parental involvement on student achievement: Pianta, R. C., Nimetz, S. L., & Bennett, E. (1997). Mother-
Second-order latent growth modeling across ethnic groups. child relationships, teacher-child relationships, and school
Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 32–42. outcomes in preschool and kindergarten. Early Childhood
Izzo, C. V., Weissberg, R. P., Kasprow, W. J., & Fendrich, M. (1999). Research Quarterly, 12, 263–280.
A longitudinal assessment of teacher perceptions of parent Pomerantz, E. M., Grolnick, W. S., & Price, C. E. (2005). The
involvement in children’s education and school performance. role of parents in how children approach school: A dynamic
American Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 817–839. process perspective. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.),
Jacobsen, T., & Hofman, V. (1997). Children’s attachment rep- The handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 259–278).
resentations: Longitudinal relations to school behavior and New York: Guilford Press.

 rel atedness bet ween children and pa ren ts


Pomerantz, E. M., & Moorman, E. A. (2010). Parents’ involve- Stevenson, H. W., Chen, C., & Lee, S-Y. (1993). Mathematics
ment in children’s schooling: A context for children’s develop- achievement of Chinese, Japanese, and American children:
ment. In J. L. Meece & J. Eccles (Eds.), Handbook of research Ten years later. Science, 259, 53–58.
on schools, schooling, and human development (pp. 398–416). Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Bornstein, M. H., & Baumwell, L.
New York: Routledge. (2001). Maternal responsiveness and children’s acheivement
Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Cheung, C. S. (2011). of language milestones. Child Development, 72, 748–767.
Parents’ involvement in children’s learning. In K. R. Harris, Tang, T. L-P., Luk, V. W-M., & Chiu, R. K. (2000). Pay differ-
S. Graham, & T. C. Urdan (Eds.), APA educational psychology entials in the people’s Republic of China: An examination of
handbook (pp. 417–440). Washington DC: APA. internal equity and external competitiveness. Compensation
Pomerantz, E. M., Moorman, E. A., & Litwack, S. D. (2007). Benefits Review, 32, 43–49.
The how, whom, and why of parents’ involvement in chil- Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009). Daily family assistance
dren’s schooling: More is not necessarily better. Review of and the psychological well-being of adolescents from Latin
Educational Research, 77, 373–410. American, Asian, and European backgrounds. Developmental
Pomerantz, E. M., Qin, L., Wang, Q., & Chen, H. (2009). Psychology, 45, 1177–1189.
American and Chinese early adolescents’ inclusion of their Thompson, R. A. (2006). The development of the person:
relationships with their parents in their self-construals. Child Social understanding, relationships, conscience, self. In
Development, 80, 792–807. N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3:
Pomerantz, E. M., Qin, L., Wang, Q., & Chen, H. (2011). Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp.
Changes in early adolescents’ sense of responsibility to their 24–98). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
parents in the United States and China: Implications for their Thompson, R. A., Braun, K., Grossmann, K. E., Gunnar, M. R.,
academic functioning. Child Development, 82, 1136–1151. Heinrichs, M., Keller, H., . . . Wang, S. (2005). Early social
Pomerantz, E. M., Wang, Q., & Ng, F. F. (2005). Mothers’ affect attachment and its consequences: The dynamics of a devel-
in the homework context: The importance of staying posi- oping relationship. In C. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Gross-
tive. Developmental Psychology, 42, 414–427. mann, M. Lamb, S. Porges, & N. Sachser (Eds.), Attachment
Roth, G., Assor, A., Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. and bonding: A new synthesis (Dahlem Workshop report 92)
(2009). The emotional and academic consequences of (pp. 349–383). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
parental conditional regard: Comparing conditional posi- van IJzendoorn, M. H., Dijksta, J., & Bus, A. G. (1995). Attach-
tive regard, conditional negative regard, and autonomy sup- ment, intelligence, and language: A meta-analysis. Social
port as parenting practices. Developmental Psychology, 45, Development, 4, 115–128.
1119–1142. Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, B., & Matos,
Ryan, R. M., Stiller, J. D., & Lynch, J. H. (1994). Represen- L. (2005). Examining the motivational impact of intrin-
tations of relationships to teachers, parents, and friends as sic versus extrinsic goal framing and autonomy-supportive
predictors of academic motivation and self-esteem. Journal of versus internally controlling communication style on early
Early Adolescence, 14, 226–249. adolescents’ academic achievement. Child Development, 76,
Schneider, B. H., Atkinson, L., & Tardiff, C. (2001). Child- 483–501.
parent attachment and children’s peer relations: A quantita- Wang, Q., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2009). The motivational land-
tive review. Developmental Psychology, 37, 86–100. scape of early adolescence in the US and China: A longitudi-
Senechal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvment in the nal investigation. Child Development, 80, 1280–1296.
development of children’s reading skill: A five year longitudi- Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E. M., & Chen, H. (2007). The role of
nal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460. parents’ control in early adolescents’ psychological function-
Sheldon, S. B. (2002). Parents’ social networks and beliefs as pre- ing: A longitudinal investigation in the United States and
dictors of parent involvement The Elementary School Journal, China. Child Development, 78, 1592–1610.
102, 301–316. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., & Davis-
Simpkins, S. D., Weiss, H. B., McCartney, K., Kreider, H. M., & Kean, P. (2006). Development of achievement motivation.
Dearing, E. (2006). Mother-child relationship as a moderator In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3:
of the relation between family educational involvement and Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp.
child achievement. Parenting: Science and Practice, 6, 49–57. 933–1002). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Skinner, E., Johnson, S., & Snyder, T. (2005). Six dimensions Wigfield, A., & Wagner, A. L. (2005). Competence, motivation
of parenting: A motivational model. Parenting: Science and and identity development during adolescence. In A. J. Elliot
Practice, 5, 175–235. & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motiva-
Stams, G. J. M., Juffer, F., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2002). tion (pp. 222–239). New York: Guilford Press.
Maternal sensitivity, infant attachment, and temperament Wolff, D., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and
in early childhood predict adjustment in middle childhood: attachment: A meta-analysis on parental antecedents of
The case of adopted children and their biologically unrelated infant attachment. Child Development, 68, 571–591.
parents. Developmental Psychology, 38, 806–821. Yu, A-B. (1996). Ultimate life concerns, self, and Chinese
Steinberg, L., Elmen, J. D., & Mounts, N. S. (1989). Authorita- achievement motivation. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The hand-
tive parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success book of Chinese psychology (pp. 227–246). Hong Kong:
among adolescents. Child Development, 60, 1424–1436. Oxford University Press.

p omer a n tz , c h eun g , qi n 


CHAPTER

Avoiding the Pitfalls and Approaching


20 the Promises of Close Relationships

Shelly L. Gable and Thery Prok

Abstract
People are powerfully motivated to form and maintain social relationships. Indeed, health and well-being
are strongly tied to the course and quality of social bonds. However, while close relationships provide
people with many advantages such as intimacy and social support, relationships can also be a detriment,
such as when they are wrought with hostility and potential rejection. We present a framework of social
and relationship motivation that simultaneously accounts for people’s tendencies to both approach the
incentives and avoid the threats in close relationships. Research examining the correlates of approach
and avoidance motives and goals in relationships is reviewed and mediating mechanisms are explored.
The implications and advantages of an approach and avoidance model of social motivation are highlighted
and future research directions are discussed.
Key Words: close relationships, approach motivation, avoidance motivation, social isolation, loneliness

Introduction Hawkley, Mesi, Berry, & Cacioppo, 2006; McDade,


Motivation for Social Bonds Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2006), psychological processes
Humans need social contact. Some of the most and disorders (e.g., executive functioning, depres-
compelling evidence for this simple truth can be found sion, anxiety; Cacioppo & Hawkley, 2009; Cacioppo
in reports of what happens to us when we are not able et al., 2006; Joiner, 1997), which likely contribute to
to form or maintain interpersonal bonds. The empiri- increased mortality and decreased well-being.
cal literature on the effects of social isolation clearly The negative impact that social isolation has
shows that social relationships are critical to health on health and well-being likely reflects the rich
and well-being. For decades we have known that social context of human evolutionary history. Social
social isolation is associated with a substantial increase bonds increased fitness, which, over time, likely con-
in all-cause mortality risk and psychological dysfunc- ferred cognitive, affective, and behavioral responses
tion (e.g., Berkman & Syme, 1979; Durkheim, 1897; designed to regulate interpersonal relations (Reis,
House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988). More recent Collins & Berscheid, 2000). In short, the legacy of
work has begun to unravel the complex mechanisms our evolutionary past is that modern human beings
responsible for these links (e.g., Carter, 1996; Cohen, possess a powerful motivation to form and maintain
2005; Kiecolt-Glaser, 1999; Uchino, Cacioppo, & strong and stable interpersonal relationships (e.g.,
Kiecolt-Glaser, 1996; Whisman, 2001). This research Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Cantor & Malley, 1991;
shows that extended loneliness and social isolation is McAdams, 1982). These nontransient relationships
associated with downstream alterations in multiple have a robust influence on daily life and vary in terms
biological systems (e.g., cardiovascular, inflammatory; of their function and structure, taking the form of


attachment bonds, hierarchical relationships, mat- as social support and connection, relationships can
ing relationships, coalitions, and communal bonds also be harmful, such as when they are wrought
(Bugental, 2000; Fiske, 1992). The motivation to with hostility and potential rejection. In the follow-
connect with others is powerful and ubiquitous. For ing section, we briefly outline the evidence that the
example, compared to people whose social needs are road to close relationships contains both promises
being met, those who feel lonely are more likely to and pitfalls.
ascribe human characteristics to inanimate objects,
presumably in an attempt to feel more socially con- Promises and Pitfalls of Close Relationships
nected (Epley, Waytz, Akalis, & Cacioppo, 2008). Close relationships are the source of both pleasure
Even though the motivation to form and main- and pain. Although people cite close relationships
tain strong interpersonal bonds is rooted deep in most often when asked what gives their life mean-
our evolutionary psyche, people are keenly aware of ing (e.g., Klinger, 1977), they are also very likely to
their need for close relationships, as beautifully illus- cite relationship problems when asked what is not
trated in poetry and song lyrics. When asked about going well in their lives (Veroff, Douvan, & Kulka,
their most important life goals, successful close rela- 1981). The potential rewards of close relationships are
tionships are often high on the list (e.g., Emmons, numerous and highly valued; these incentives include
1986. Moreover, people who do not make their companionship, passion, and intimacy. The poten-
social goals a high priority have poorer mental and tial pitfalls are equally plentiful and impactful; these
physical health than those who do rank social goals threats include betrayal, jealously, and criticism.
high among their life goals (Kasser & Ryan, 1996). One potential benefit of close relationships is
The popularly held belief that “people need people” social support, or more accurately the perception
is expounded in the psychological literature. Promi- that others will be there if needed during times of
nent models of well-being propose that satisfying stress. In fact, social support has been widely linked
ongoing relationships are a necessary component to various mental and physical health processes (e.g.,
of psychological and physical health (e.g., Deci & Cohen & Willis, 1985; Cutrona, Russell, & Rose,
Ryan, 1985; Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2008; Keyes, 1986; Dunkel-Schetter & Bennett, 1990; Lakey &
1998; Ryff, 1995). Cassady, 1990). Another potential benefit of social
Recent research has also demonstrated that the bonds is that close others often facilitate one’s ideal
impact of our basic motivation to form and main- personal development and goal pursuit (e.g., Rus-
tain close relationships reaches far beyond our inter- bult, Kumashiro, Kubacka, & Finkel, 2009). On the
action with others. That is, interpersonal motives other hand, threats to relationship stability and prob-
and goals influence cognition, emotion, and behav- lems within relationships also contribute to psycho-
ior in domains that once were thought to be inde- pathological symptoms such as depression, anxiety,
pendent of close relationships. Overall, this body of and substance abuse (e.g., Davila, Bradbuy, Cohan,
research clearly demonstrates that motives for close & Tochluk, 1997; Whisman, 2001; Whisman,
relationships influence how people think, feel, and Uebelacker, & Settles, 2010). Specifically, conflict,
act in novel social interactions and seemingly non- rejection, and abandonment are among the threats
social contexts (e.g., Andersen, Reznik, & Manzella, with well-documented detrimental links to psycho-
1996; Baldwin, Carrell, & Lopez, 1990; Mikulincer, logical health (e.g., Baron et al., 2007; Downey,
1998). For example, the motives and goals people Feldman, & Ayduck, 2000; Mikulincer, 1998).
have for their close relationships affect psychologi- In addition to the psychological impact of social
cal processes in the seemingly unrelated domain of bonds, there are numerous physical benefits and
performance on achievement tasks, and this occurs risks of relationships (e.g., Kiecolt-Glaser, Gouin,
without their awareness (Shah, 2003). & Hantsoo, 2010). For example, emotionally sup-
In short, there is compelling evidence that people portive relationships facilitate recovery from illness,
are motivated to form and maintain stable interper- whereas marital conflict increases the likelihood
sonal bonds. When people are unable to fulfill these of cardiac death (Eaker et al., 2007; Wilcox, Kasl,
needs, their health and well-being suffer. Moreover, & Berkman, 1994). The specific physiological
social motivation influences cognition, affect, and processes that are responsible for seemingly direct
behavior in social and nonsocial contexts. However, effects that relationship incentives and threats have
close relationships in themselves are not necessar- on physical health are also beginning to be unrav-
ily positive and beneficial. That is, while close rela- eled (e.g., Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008; Carter, 1996).
tionships provide people with many benefits such For example, fear of negative social evaluation is

g a b le, p rok 


linked to a biological stress response (Dickerson & motives and goals in the achievement domain.
Kemeny, 2004). He separated approach motives, those consisting of
Taken together this research shows that some the need for achievement, from avoidance motives,
aspects of social bonds and close relationships are those focused on a fear of failure.
positively associated with psychological and physi- What is clear from all of this work is that beyond
cal health, whereas other aspects of social bonds and the content of the motives or goals (e.g., achievement,
close relationships pose significant risks to health power, social), the focus of those motives and goals is
and well-being. Perhaps in no other domain of life critical for understanding a variety of psychological
is the simultaneous potential for rewards and threats processes. That is, the approach and avoidance moti-
so clear as it is for interpersonal relationships. And, vational distinction has been shown to have impor-
given the human need for social connection, one can tant implications for perception, cognition, emotion,
not avoid the potential pitfalls of social bonds (and behavior, health, and well-being (e.g., Derryberry &
forego any possible promises) by simply not having Reed, 1994; Elliot & Sheldon, 1998; Higgins, Shah,
them. Therefore, it is our thesis that people are moti- & Friedman, 1997). For example, in terms of basic
vated to approach the rewards and avoid the pun- attention, individuals with strong approach moti-
ishments inherent in social relationships. Models of vation are biased toward cues indicating gain of an
social and relationship motivation need to account incentive and those with strong avoidance motiva-
simultaneously for the regulation of incentives and tion are biased toward cues indicating threat or loss
threats. Fortunately, decades of research on approach (Derryberry & Reed, 1994). In the area of emotion,
and avoidance motivation laid the groundwork for Carver (2001) argues that the function of emotions
the model described in the remainder of this chapter. lies in their utility to motivate the organism and reg-
ulate behavior regarding incentives (approach) and
Approach and Avoidance Motivation threats (avoidance). Thus, there are positive incentive-
The history of distinguishing approach and related emotions (e.g., joy) and negative incentive-
avoidance dimensions is almost as long as the his- related emotion (e.g., disappointment). Likewise,
tory of psychology itself (Gray, 1970; Pavlov, 1927; there are positive threat-related emotions (e.g., relief )
Schneirla, 1959). For example, Miller (1959, 1961) and negative threat-related emotions (e.g., anxiety).
conducted his classic research on separate approach In the area of well-being, Elliot and Sheldon (1998)
and withdrawal learning processes and made graph- examined the links between approach and avoid-
ical representations of their interplay in the form ance personal goals and found that people with more
of approach and avoidance conflict gradients that avoidance goals for their lives had lower well-being.
continue to inspire current research (e.g., Cacioppo, Goal direction (approach or avoid) was associated
Gardner, & Berntson, 1997). Over time, theoretical with current well-being as well as changes in well-
and empirical work across diverse areas of psychol- being over time.
ogy has either explicitly or implicitly adopted a view Several researchers have argued that the reason
of separate incentive approach and threat avoidance that the approach-avoidance distinction emerges
systems (see Carver, 1996; Gable, Reis, & Elliot, frequently in motivation research is because they are
2003, for reviews). based on distinct neurobiological systems or struc-
Specifically in the areas of motivation and tures (e.g., Gray, 1987). For example, researchers who
behavior regulation, several theories have featured have used electroencephalographic (EEG) technol-
the approach and avoidance distinction (see Elliot, ogy to examine relative prefrontal brain asymmetry
2008). For example, Carver and Scheier described a and activity in these regions have found that strong
model of self-regulation (1990) that entails feedback dispositional approach motives or signals of possible
processes such that the current environment is com- reward correspond to greater left prefrontal activa-
pared to some internal goal, the person acts, the situ- tion and strong dispositional avoidance motives or
ation is reevaluated and the comparison made again, signals of possible punishment correspond to greater
and the process continues. More important, these right prefrontal activation (e.g., Harmon-Jones &
feedback processes can either lead to behaviors that Allen, 1997; Sobotka, Davidson, & Senulis, 1992).
move the person closer to the object of his or her goal The separate biological systems research is consistent
(approach), or they can lead to behaviors that move with work examining the underlying factor structure
the person away from the object of his or her goal of different measures of individual differences in
(avoidance). Similarly, Elliot (1997) made the dis- motivation, personality, and affect (e.g., Gable, Reis,
tinction between approach- and avoidance-focused & Elliot, 2003; Laurenceau, Troy, & Carver, 2005).

 avoiding the pitfalls & approaching the promises of close rel ationships
These studies have found that approach and avoid- However, up to this point most of these studies
ance dimensions, while correlated with one another, were conducted in the context of laboratory-formed
are independent. Also consistent with theoretical groups doing achievement-relevant tasks, not with
models and empirical data is the idea that each existing or naturally formed close relationships.
operates through different processes. For example, Later, Boyatzis (1973) further delineated approach
Gable, Reis, and Elliot (2000) showed that the rela- and avoidance social motivation by pointing that
tionship between approach motivation and positive affiliation motivation can be expressed in two ways;
emotion was accounted for differential exposure pro- one is approach oriented and the other is avoidance
cesses (higher approach motives predicted a greater oriented. The approach affiliative motive is focused
number of pleasant daily events). The relationship on a positive outcome, obtaining closeness and inti-
between avoidance motivation and negative emo- macy, whereas the avoidance affiliative motive seems
tion was mediated by differential sensitivity pro- focused on a negative outcome, avoiding rejection
cesses (higher avoidance motives predicted stronger or loneliness. Boyatzis’ conclusions mirrored those of
reactions to negative events when they occurred, but Mehrabian and colleagues, even though their work
not a higher frequency of them). grew out a different perspective, that of reward and
expectancy theories (Mehrabian, 1976; Mehrabian
Social and Relationship Motivation & Ksionzky, 1974). Specifically, Mehrabian (1976)
early social motivation research: posited that expectancies of positive and negative
affiliation motives reinforcers in interpersonal relationships shaped
Early work on social motivation hinted at the approach and avoidance social motivation, respec-
need for understanding both the regulation of incen- tively, which he called affiliation tendency and sen-
tives and the regulation of threats. For example, sitivity to rejection. Mehrabian found that approach
among Murray’s (1938) several socially based needs motives targeted different outcomes than avoidance
(e.g., deference, nurturance) the one that subse- motives. People high on affiliative tendency were less
quently received the most empirical and theoretical anxious, elicited more positive affect from others,
attention was the need for affiliation (nAff). Shipley were more self-confident, and saw themselves as sim-
and Veroff (1952) viewed the need for affiliation as ilar to others. People high in sensitivity to rejection
stemming from interpersonal insecurity. They mea- were less confident, more anxious, and were judged
sured the strength of the need for affiliation in open- less positively by others than people low on sensitiv-
ended stories written to explain ambiguous pictures ity to rejection.
(Thematic Apperception Test; Morgan & Murray,
1935). Their research showed that themes of social beyond affiliation motivation
separation were more common in the stories of Although most scholars of close relationships
recently rejected men, compared to controls. would agree that the need for close bonds is a funda-
Later work showed that those who had a strong mental human motive, the examination of relation-
need for affiliation were less popular in their social ship motivation seemed to take a hiatus for close to
groups, had more confidence, but sought the approval 25 years. There were, of course, important excep-
of others more often than those with weaker need tions. For example, McAdams (1982) conducted
for affiliation (Atkinson, Heyns, & Veroff, 1954). research on intimacy motivation. The primary assess-
Atkinson and his colleagues explained the seemingly ment of these motives was again the TAT; however,
mixed results of the work on nAff by suggesting that motivation for intimacy and motivation for affilia-
social situations aroused two needs: hope of affiliation tion were assessed separately. He found that those
and fear of rejection. The idea that social needs took with stronger intimacy motivation had more spon-
the form of independent approach and avoidance taneous thoughts about relationships and relation-
motives was further explicated by DeCharms (1957), ship partners, experienced more positive emotions
who attempted to separately measure approach in interpersonal situations, disclosed and listened
affiliative motives and avoidance affiliative motives. more to friends, spent more time communicating
Sticking with the prevailing paradigm of the time, with others, and had greater concern for others’
he further refined and expanded a TAT coding well-being than those with weaker intimacy motives
scheme; coding responses concerned with positive (McAdams & Constantian, 1983; McAdams, Healy,
relationships and attaining affiliation as approach & Krause, 1984).
affiliation (+Aff) and responses concerned with sepa- McAdams and colleagues’ work on intimacy moti-
ration and rejection as avoidance affiliation (–Aff). vation is important to the current thesis because it

g a b le, p rok 


highlights the relevance of social goals to established, approach goals may be concerned with making a
ongoing close relationships. This was a departure good impression and having an interesting con-
from much of the previous work that examined the versation, whereas someone with weak approach
role of social goals in more general social processes goals would be less concerned with these outcomes.
(e.g., popularity, self-confidence) or in the context of Strong approach goals in this scenario might stem
newly or laboratory-formed groups and dyads. More- from high dispositional levels of hope for affiliation
over, subsequent research on intimacy motivation and/or prior cues of warmth and interest from his
found that intimacy motives were associated with a date. Someone with strong avoidance goals may be
variety of important relationship processes, includ- concerned with not making a fool of himself and
ing relationship satisfaction in dating relationships, avoiding controversial conversation topics, whereas
friendships, and marriages (Sanderson & Cantor, someone with weak avoidance goals would be less
1997, 2001; Sanderson, Rahm, & Beigbeder, 2005). concerned with these things. Strong avoidance goals
Although this work expanded the social motivation in this scenario might stem from high dispositional
research beyond the original focus on the need to fear of rejection and/or prior cues of aloofness or
affiliate with others, little attention has been paid, disinterest from his date. Although the content of
until recently, to the need to simultaneously regulate the goals in both of these examples is very similar,
incentives and threats. In the following sections we the manner in which individuals frame their goals
describe such a model of approach and avoidance makes all the difference.
social motivation and the empirical evidence sup-
porting it. motives and current environment
shape goals
Approaching Incentives and Avoiding Empirical evidence supports the premise that indi-
Threats in Relationships vidual differences in distal social motives and attach-
Modeled on other hierarchical motivation and ment dimensions predict more proximal social and
goal theories (e.g., Elliot, 2006) and rooted in the relationship goals (e.g., Gable, 2006; Impett, Gor-
previous work that separated fear of rejection from don, & Strachman, 2008). For example, several stud-
hope for affiliation motives (e.g., Mehrabian, 1976), ies found that people with strong approach motives
Gable (2006) proposed a model of approach and were more likely to adopt short-term approach
avoidance social goals. The model describes pre- social goals such as wanting to make new friends,
dicted associations between approach and avoid- and those with strong avoidance motives were more
ance motives and different interpersonal outcomes likely to adopt short-term avoidance social goals
as well as the processes that mediate those associa- such as not wanting to be lonely (e.g., Gable, 2006).
tions. Specifically, the model proposes that people Similar associations between dispositional motives
can have two distinct types of goals. Approach social and short-term goals for specific interactions in a
goals are aimed at obtaining desired outcomes such romantic relationship have also been demonstrated
as intimacy and pleasure in their close relationships, (e.g., Impett, Gable, & Peplau, 2005).
whereas avoidance social goals direct individuals Although there is less work on the associations
away from potentially undesirable outcomes such as among current cues of incentives and threats in the
conflict and rejection. The model posits that indi- social environment and the adoption of approach
vidual differences in dispositional threat and incen- and avoidance goals in relationships, existing
tive sensitivities (i.e., relatively stable traits) as well data are consistent with the prediction that the
as aspects of a person’s current social environment perception of current incentives is positively asso-
(i.e., recent events in a person’s particular relation- ciated with approach goal strength and the per-
ship) influence the type of short-term goals that ception of current threats is positively associated
people adopt. with avoidance goal strength (e.g., Elliot et al.,
We view goals as the short-term cognitive rep- 2006). However, interpretation of these findings
resentations of wants and fears that are influenced is complicated because current goals are likely to
by both dispositional motivational tendencies bias the interpretation of social cues in the envi-
(e.g., need for affiliation and fear of rejection) and ronment, such as whether neutral or ambiguous
situational cues (e.g., current social incentives and social information is interpreted as an incentive
threats in the environment; Elliot, Gable, & Mapes, or threat (Strachman & Gable, 2006). These pro-
2006; Gable, 2006). For example, when out on a cesses are discussed in more detail in the following
date with a new partner, a person who has strong section.

 avoiding the pitfalls & approaching the promises of close rel ationships
Approach and Avoidance Goals and There are data showing that the goals people
Interpersonal Outcomes have for specific interactions are also important.
Because approach goals are focused on poten- For example, in one study, the type of goals people
tial incentives, and avoidance goals are focused on had for everyday sacrifices (i.e., enacting behaviors
potential threats, they should be linked with differ- that are not preferred for the sake of their partners,
ent outcomes in relationships. Specifically, approach such as accompanying a partner to a dull work
goals should be associated with outcomes defined by function or not spending time on a hobby) influ-
the presence or absence of positive outcomes, such enced their relationship outcomes (Impett, Gable,
as passion and intimacy. Avoidance goals should be & Peplau, 2005). Specifically when people engaged
associated with outcomes defined by the presence in these behaviors for approach motives (e.g., to
or absence of negative outcomes, such as security promote intimacy), they reported greater positive
and conflict. Results from several studies are consis- affect and relationship satisfaction. However, when
tent with this idea. In one study, participants’ self- they did the same behaviors for avoidance motives
generated social goals and romantic goals were later (e.g., to prevent my partner from becoming upset),
coded as either approach or avoidance. Whereas they reported greater negative affect, lower rela-
approach goals were associated with more satisfac- tionship satisfaction, and more conflict. The more
tion and with social life and less loneliness, avoid- often people sacrificed for avoidance motives over
ance goals were associated with increased anxiety the course of the study, the less satisfied they were
and loneliness 8 weeks later (Gable, 2006). Lone- with their relationships at the follow-up assess-
liness has historically been defined as being both ment 6 weeks later and the more likely they were
about lacking positive relationships or having prob- to have broken up. Finally, in a recent observational
lematic or insecure relationships (e.g., Perlman & study of newlywed couples, Laurenceau, Kleinman,
Peplau, 1981); thus, it is theoretically consistent Kaczynski, and Carver (2010) examined how incen-
with the model that both types of goals would be tive and threat sensitivities were associated with out-
associated with changes in loneliness. comes regarding specific interactions. They found
Similarly Elliot, Gable, and Mapes (2006) that relationship incentive sensitivity was positively
developed and validated an eight-item measure of associated with positive affect during a discussion
approach and avoidance social goals in a short-term of the loving aspects of the relationship, whereas
longitudinal study. Consistent with the model, they relationship threat sensitivity predicted greater anxi-
found that approach social goals were associated ety when discussing a significant marital problem.
with greater subjective well-being, whereas avoidance To summarize, the previous section reviewed
social goals were associated with more self-reports several studies that demonstrated the links between
of physical health symptoms 3.5 months later. The approach and avoidance relationship motivation
aforementioned studies examined peoples’ level of and outcomes. These studies employed diverse
relationship goals at a global level, aggregating across methods (cross-sectional, daily experience, longi-
friendships, family relationships, and intimate rela- tudinal data, observational), focused on different
tionships. However, additional studies have focused close relationships such as friendships, romantic
on people’s approach and avoidance goals in a specific relationships, and family bonds, and examined
relationship and have found parallel results that are associations at the level of global social relation-
consistent with the hierarchical model. For example, ships, specific relationships, and particular inter-
in a series of studies, Impett and colleagues found actions. The results all showed that approach and
that people in relatively new romantic relationships avoidance goals were associated with important
with strong approach goals maintained high levels relationship outcomes. In addition, these data sug-
of sexual desire over time, whereas those with weak gest that approach goals are more strongly asso-
approach goals showed the typical decline in sexual ciated with outcomes defined by the presence of
desire that has been observed in other studies (Impett, incentives (e.g., sexual desire), whereas avoidance
Strachman, Finkel, & Gable, 2008). In addition, goals are more strongly associated with outcomes
in a study of married couples, Gable (2000) found defined by the absence of threats (e.g., security).
that those who had strong avoidance goals reported Global outcomes that involve both incentives and
greater insecurity than those with weaker avoidance threats, such as overall relationships satisfaction
goals. On the other hand, the strength of approach and loneliness, were correlated with both approach
goals positively predicted daily positive affect during and avoidance goals. The next question centers
marital interactions. on how goals influence outcomes—the cognitive,

g a b le, p rok 


behavioral, and affective processes that mediate the relationship motives and goals rated them as more
links summarized in this section. important and showed more changes in their well-
being than those with weak avoidance goals. Expo-
Processes Linking Goals to Outcomes sure to negative events was not consistently related
Our theoretical model suggests that the processes to avoidance goals. In the subsequent section, stud-
that mediate the links between approach goals and ies examining other possible mediators of the links
outcomes are not necessarily the same as those that between social goals and outcomes are reviewed.
mediate the links between avoidance goals and out-
comes. An illustration of this point comes from a memory and interpretation of
series of studies examining how threat and reward ambiguous information
sensitivities were associated with daily affect (Gable, Previous research has clearly shown that
Reis, & Elliot, 2000). Specifically, threat sensitivity motives and goals influence basic cognitive processes
was assessed with Carver and White’s (1994) mea- (e.g., Kunda, 1990). Much of this work has exam-
sure of individual differences in the Behavioral Inhi- ined how the content of goals (e.g., self-protection
bition System (BIS), and incentive sensitivity was motives, accuracy goals) directs cognitive processes.
assessed with the corresponding scale assessing indi- For example, Maner and colleagues (2005) reported
vidual differences in the Behavioral Activity System that when participants were motivated by a self-
(BAS); both scales are based on Gray’s reinforcement protection goal, they perceived greater anger in the
sensitivity theory (e.g., Gray, 1987, 1990). The faces of outgroup members and when they had goals
results showed that, as expected, high BIS sensitivity related to finding a mate, male participants per-
was associated with more daily negative affect (NA) ceived more sexual arousal in attractive female tar-
and high BAS sensitivity was predictive of increased gets. In addition to the content of goals influencing
daily positive affect (PA). basic cognitive processes, research has also suggested
Across three studies the occurrence and impact of that the focus of motives and goals influences cogni-
daily positive and negative events were also assessed, tive processes. For example, Higgins and Tykocinski
and the results suggested that the relationship (1992) found that participants who had promotion-
between BAS and PA was explained by a differential focused motives (concerned with aspirations and
exposure process and the relationship between BIS growth) had better recall of information from a
and NA was explained by a differential sensitivity previously presented story when it was related to
hypothesis. People with more sensitive BAS experi- positive outcomes. Those with prevention-focused
enced more daily PA because they experienced more motives (concerned with obligations and security)
frequent positive events (differential exposure), and remembered more information related to negative
people with more sensitive BIS did not report expe- outcomes in the story.
riencing more frequent negative events; however, Strachman and Gable (2006) investigated how
they reacted more strongly to the occurrence of goals focused on social incentives and those focused
negative events (differential reactivity). In another on social threats influenced two basic processes: recall
series of studies focused on social motives and goals of social information and interpretation of ambigu-
and social events, results were consistent with these ous social information. In one study, individual dif-
findings (Gable, 2006). ferences in the strength of approach and avoidance
Specifically, approach social motives and goals goals for current friendships were assessed. After
were associated with increased exposure to social reading a story containing positive, negative, and
positive events, such that strong approach relation- neutral information regarding the interactions of
ship motives and goals predicted increased frequency two relationship partners, participants with strong
of the occurrence of positive social events (Gable, avoidance social goals recalled more of the negative
2006). The frequency of positive social events medi- information in the story than those with weak avoid-
ated the link between approach relationship motives ance goals (Strachman & Gable, 2006, Study 1).
and outcomes; and as expected approach relationship In another study, social goals for an upcoming inter-
motives and goals did not predict the frequency of action with a stranger were experimentally manipu-
negative social events. The link between avoidance lated; half of the participants were provided with an
goals and outcomes was associated with a different approach goal for a conversation with a stranger and
process, reactivity to negative social events (Elliot half were given avoidance goals (e.g., try to make a
et al., 2006; Gable, 2006). Specifically, when negative good impression, try not to make a bad impression).
social events happened, those with strong avoidance They then were given a self-description ostensibly

 avoiding the pitfalls & approaching the promises of close rel ationships
written by their future interaction partner. Those goals likely place more weight on the presence (or
in the avoidance goal condition remembered more absence) of threats in their relationships when making
negative information about and expressed more dis- global evaluations than those with weaker avoidance
like for the other person than those in the approach goals. Consistent with this idea, Updegraff, Gable,
goal condition. and Taylor (2004) found that global life-satisfaction
A significant proportion of social information ratings were more strongly tied to positive affect for
that people receive on a daily basis can be ambigu- people with strong approach motivation than for
ous in nature. A smile from a stranger can be a sign those with weaker approach motivation.
of good manners or genuine warmth and interest. Examining this idea in close relationships, Gable
The quiet demeanor of a spouse can be indicative and Poore (2008) conducted a signal-contingent
of a bad day at work or lingering animosity about daily experience study in which participants were
a marital disagreement the night before. Strachman beeped at several random intervals throughout the
and Gable (2006) also analyzed their participants’ day and reported their feelings of passion for and
interpretation of the information they did recall security regarding their romantic partners at that
from the story they read. That is, they examined very moment. Prior to the beginning of the study,
how closely their recalled information matched they reported the strength of their approach and
the information presented in the story. The results avoidance relationship goals. At the end of the day,
showed that those with strong avoidance goals were they also reported their general feeling of satisfac-
more likely to interpret seemingly neutral and posi- tion with their relationships. The authors found that
tive information from the story with a more negative participants with strong avoidance social goals put
spin than people with avoidance goals. For example, more weight on security in their end-of-day reports
when describing an originally neutral aspect of the of satisfaction than those with weaker avoidance
scenario such as “he picked her up at 10:00,” those social goals; moreover, those with strong approach
with high avoidance goals were likely to view that as goals put more weight on their feelings of passion
being picked up late. Memory of social information in their end-of-day reports of relationship satisfac-
and biases in interpretation are particularly impor- tion than those low in approach goals. Thus, when
tant processes in close relationships because people people had strong avoidance goals they reported less
have repeated interactions with the same people relationship satisfaction on days they felt more inse-
across multiple contexts. Thus, there are ample cure than they typically felt, but if they had weak
opportunities for pertinent memories to form and avoidance goals their satisfaction did not go down
for ambiguous information to be interpreted in a on days they felt more insecure than they typically
manner consistent with goals. Moreover, as demon- felt. Similarly, when people had strong approach
strated by Neuberg (e.g., 1996), these cognitive pro- goals they reported greater satisfaction on days they
cesses lead to expectations, which in turn influence felt more passion than they typically reported feel-
the information sought from social partners, how ing, but if they had weak approach goals they did
people behavior toward a social partner, and how not report a boost in their relationship satisfaction
the social partner behaves in return. on days they felt more passion than they typically
felt. It seems that the very definition of satisfac-
weight of social information tion—the presence of incentives or the absence of
Although the strength of approach goals is pri- threats—was tied to goals.
marily associated with incentive-based outcomes
such as passion, and threat-based outcomes such as emotion in social interaction
security, in the end, people make more global evalu- and close relationships
ations in relationships and decisions to act or not act Motivation and emotion are closely tied (Keltner
in certain ways (e.g., file for divorce or stay in a mar- & Lerner, 2009) and approach and avoidance motives
riage). Thus, another process by which approach and have been tied to positive and negative affect, respec-
avoidance goals influence outcomes is by influenc- tively (e.g., Larsen & Ketelaar, 1991). For example,
ing the weight of different types of information in studies have found that approach motives and goals
global evaluations and decisions. Specifically, those were positively correlated with positive affect on a
with strong approach goals likely weigh the presence daily basis and avoidance motives and goals were cor-
(or absence) of incentives in their relationships more related with negative affect on a daily basis (e.g. Gable
heavily in global evaluations than those with weaker et al., 2000). More important, there is little evidence
approach goals. And those with strong avoidance that approach motivation is associated with negative

g a b le, p rok 


affect and that avoidance motivation is associated with to better these relationships needs to address both
positive affect (Gable et al., 2000). dimensions of the relationship.
Recently, Impett and colleagues (2010) exam-
ined the experience of affect in close relationships. Future Directions
In terms of ongoing relationships, Impett and col- Although there has been empirical progress on
leagues conducted two studies investigating the role approach and avoidance social motivation, there are
of positive emotions in the link between relationship several directions in need of further study. First, the
goals and satisfaction in dating couples. They found bulk of studies have focused on existing individual
that people high in approach goals experienced more differences in social motives, but the situational influ-
positive emotions on a daily basis, which medi- ences on interpersonal goal pursuit have been largely
ated their greater feelings of satisfaction with their unexamined. For example, the cues in the social
relationships. Moreover, participants with high environment that signal incentives or threats, and
approach goals also had partners who experienced their impact on goal adoption in relationships and
more positive emotions, which also contributed to interpersonal interactions, are not well understood.
participants’ higher feelings of satisfaction with the Another future direction is examining whether people
relationship. More research is needed to understand can change their motivation through effort. Given the
how avoidance goals and negative emotions might benefits of approach goal pursuit in close relationships
mediate the link between avoidance goals and rela- (e.g., Gable & Poore, 2008; Impett et al., 2008), exam-
tionship outcomes; however, Impett and colleagues’ ining whether it is possible for people with chronically
findings are consistent with Fredrickson’s (1998) low levels of approach goals or high levels of avoidance
broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. That goals to learn to focus on incentives in relationships is
is, one interpretation of these data is that positive important. In addition, part of the definition of close
emotions broaden people’s attention and thinking relationships is that they are nontransient, consisting
and these broadened outlooks help people to discover of interactions that take place over some period of
and build consequential personal resources such as time. Thus, a future direction of research is to more
social support and enhanced feelings of satisfaction carefully examine how motives and goals unfold over
(Fredrickson, Cohn, Coffey, Pek, & Finkel, 2008). time in long-term relationships.
For example, the manner in which approach and
Conclusions avoidance goals operate in a new friendship may be
People are strongly motivated to form and main- different than the way they operate for two lifelong
tain strong and stable bonds and their health and best friends. Similarly, it may be that the associations
well-being are closely tied to those relationships. between relationship motivation and relationship qual-
However, research has also clearly demonstrated ity may be different in married couples than in dating
that interpersonal relationships present both incen- couples. Another benefit of studying motivation in
tives and threats. Thus, our view is that no model relationships over time is an increased understanding
of social motivation is complete unless it simulta- of how progress on goals is evaluated over time, across
neously addresses the regulation of both rewards repeated interactions with the same person. It is likely
and punishments. However, although the approach that definition of progress and rate of that progress are
and avoidance motivation distinction has a long assessed differently for approach compared to avoid-
and prolific history in several domains of human ance goals (e.g., Carver & Scheier, 1990). For exam-
endeavors, it has not been applied to close rela- ple, the man who has the goal of not saying anything
tionships research consistently until recently. This embarrassing on a first date is only one faux pas away
is likely a lost opportunity because such a frame from failure the whole evening, regardless of how
explicitly describes the regulation of the inherent many funny, smart, and kind comments he makes.
incentives and threats in social relationships and Finally, research on approach and avoidance goals in
may offer insights into understanding different pat- relationships has focused on explicit or consciously
terns of functioning in relationships. For example, accessible goals, but work from several other research-
some unstable and unsatisfying close relationships ers on the power that seemingly nonconscious goal
are lacking incentives and other unstable and unsat- pursuit has on behavior in close relationship is com-
isfying close relationships are ripe with threats. pelling (e.g., Anderson et al., 1996; Baldwin et al.,
Approach and avoidance motives and goals are 1990; Fitzsimons & Bargh, 2003; Scinta & Gable,
likely operating differently in these two types of 2007). Future studies need to examine nonconscious
relationships, and more importantly, any attempt incentive- and threat-based motivation.

 avoiding the pitfalls & approaching the promises of close rel ationships
Note Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibitions,
1. For reviews of the approach and avoidance distinction behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending
in motivation, see Elliot (1999), Higgins (1998), and Schleirna reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Per-
(1959). sonality and Social Psychology, 67(2), 319–333.
Cohen, S. (2005). The Pittsburgh Common Cold Studies: Psy-
chosocial predictors of susceptibility to respiratory infectious
References illness. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 12,
Anderson, S. M., Reznik, I., & Manzella, L. M. (1996). Eliciting 123–131.
facial affect, motivation, and expectancies in transference: Cohen, S., & Willis, T. A. (1985). Stress, social support, and the
Significant-other representations in social relations. Journal buffering hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 310–357.
of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1108–1129. Cutrona, C., Russell, D., & Rose, J. (1986). Social support and
Atkinson, J. W., Heyns, R. W., & Veroff, J. (1954). The effect adaptation to stress by the elderly. Psychology and Aging, 1,
of experimental arousal of the affiliation motive on thematic 47–54.
apperception. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, Davila, J., Bradbury, T. N., Cohan, C. L., & Tochluk, S. (1997).
49(3), 405–410. Marital functioning and depressive symptoms: Evidence for
Baldwin, M. W., Carrell, S. E., & Lopez, D. F. (1990). Priming a stress generation model. Journal of Personality and Social
relationship schemas: My advisor and the pope are watching Psychology, 73(4), 849–861.
me from the back of my mind. Journal of Experimental Social DeCharms, R. (1957). Affiliation motivation and productivity
Psychology, 26(5), 435–454. in small groups. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychol-
Baron, K. G., Smith, T. W., Butner, J., Nealey-Moore, J., ogy, 55(2), 222–226.
Hawkins, M. W., & Uchino, B. N. (2007). Hostility, anger, Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and
and marital adjustment: Concurrent and prospective asso- self-determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum.
ciations with psychosocial vulnerability. Journal of Behavioral Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and atten-
Medicine, 30(1), 1–10. tion: Orienting toward and away from positive and nega-
Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: tive signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 66(6),
Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental 1128–1139.
human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Dickerson, S. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2004). Acute stressors
Berkman, L. F., & Syme, S. L. (1979). Social networks, host- and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and syn-
resistance, and mortality—9-year follow-up-study of alameda thesis of laboratory research. Psychological Bulletin, 130(30),
county residents. American Journal of Epidemiology, 109(2), 355–391.
186–204. Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2008). Happiness: Unlocking
Boyatzis, R. E. (1973). Affiliation motivation: A review and a the mysteries of psychological wealth. Malden, MA: Blackwell
new perspective. In D. C. McClelland & R. S. Steele (Eds.), Publishing.
Human motivation: A book of readings. Morristown, NJ: Gen- Downey, G., Feldman, S., & Ayduk, O. (2000). Rejection sen-
eral Learning Press. sitivity and male violence in romantic relationships. Personal
Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social life: Relationships, 7, 45–61.
A domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 26, 187–209. Dunkel-Schetter, C., & Bennett, T. L. (1990). Differentiat-
Cacioppo, J. T., & Hawkley, L. C. (2009). Perceived social ing the cognitive and behavioral aspects of social support.
isolation and cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(10), In I. G. Sarason, B. R. Sarason, & G. R. Pierce (Eds.), Social
447–454. support: An interactional view (pp. 267–296). New York: Wiley.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness: Human Durkheim, E. (1897/1951). Suicide: A study in sociology.
nature and the need for social connection. New York: (J. A. Spaulding & G. Simpson, Trans.). New York: The Free
W. W. Norton & Co. Press.
Cacioppo, J. T., Hughes, M. E., Waite, L. J., Hawkley, L. C., Eaker, E. D., Sullivan, L. M., Kelly-Hayes, M., D’Agostino, R. B.,
& Thisted, R. A. (2006). Loneliness as a specific risk factor & Benjamin, E. J. (2007). Marital status, marital strain,
for depressive symptoms: Cross-sectional and longitudinal and risk of coronary heart disease or total mortality: The
analyses. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), 140–151. Framingham offspring study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69(6),
Cacioppo, J. T., Gardner, W. L., & Berntson, G. G. (1997). 509–551.
Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures: The case of Elliot, A. J. (1997). Integrating the “classic” and “contemporary”
attitudes and evaluative space. Personality and Social Psychol- approaches to achievement motivation: A hierarchical
ogy Review, 1(1), 3–25. model of approach and avoidance achievement motivation.
Cantor, N., & Malley, J. (1991). Life tasks, personal needs, and In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances in motivation
close relationships. In G. J. O. Fletcher & F. D. Fincham and achievement (Vol. 10; pp. 243–279). Greenwich, CT:
(Eds.), Cognition in close relationships (pp. 101–125). JAI Press.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and
Carver, C. S. (1996). Neuroendocrine perspectives on social achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 149–169.
attachment and love. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23, 779–818. Elliot, A. J. (2006). A hierarchical model of approach and avoid-
Carver, C. S. (2001). Affect and the functional bases of behavior: ance motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 111–116.
On the dimensional structure of affective experience. Person- Elliot, A. J. (2008). Handbook of approach and avoidance motiva-
ality and Social Psychology Review, 5(4), 345–356. tion. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1990). Origins and functions of Elliot, A. J., Gable, S. L., & Mapes, R. R. (2006). Approach and
positive and negative affect: A control-process view. Psycho- avoidance motivation in the social domain. Personality and
logical Review, 97(1), 19–35. Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 378–391

g a b le, p rok 


Elliot, A. J., & Sheldon, K. M. (1998). Avoidance personal goals Higgins, E. T., & Tykocinski, O. (1992). Self-discrepancies and
and the personality-illness relationship. Journal of Personality biographical memory: Personality and cognition at the level
and Social Psychology, 75(5), 1282–1299. of psychological situation. Personality and Social Psychology
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to per- Bulletin, 18(5), 527–535.
sonality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and House, J. S., Landis, K. R. & Umberson, D. (1988). Social rela-
Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068. tionships and health. Science, 241, 540–545.
Epley, N., Akalis, S., Waytz, A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Impett, E., Gable, S. L. Peplau, L. A. (2005). Giving up and
Creating social connection through inferential reproduction: giving in: The costs and benefits of daily sacrifice in intimate
Loneliness and perceived agency in gadgets, gods, and grey- relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89,
hounds. Psychological Science, 19(2), 114–120. 327–344.
Epley, N., Waytz, A., Akalis, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). When Impett, E. A., Gordon, A. M., Kogan, A., Oveis, C., Gable, S. L.,
we need a human: Motivational determinants of anthropo- & Keltner, D. (2010) Approaching happiness: Daily and
morphism. Social Cognition, 26(2), 143–155. long-term consequences of approach and avoidance goals in
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: romantic relationships. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychologi- chology, 99, 948–963.
cal Review, 99(4), 689–723. Impett, E. A., Gordon, A. M., & Strachman, A. (2008). Attach-
Fitzsimons, G. M., & Bargh, J. A. (2003). Thinking of you: ment and daily sexual goals: A study of dating couples.
Nonconscious pursuit of interpersonal goals associated with Personal Relationships, 15(3), 375–390.
relationship partners. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- Impett, E., Strachman, A., Finkel, E. & Gable, S. L. (2008).
chology, 84(1), 148–164. Maintaining sexual desire in intimate relationships: The
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? importance of approach goals. Journal of Personality and
Review of General Psychology, 2, 300–319. Social Psychology, 94, 808–823.
Fredrickson, B. L., Cohn, M. A., Coffey, K. A., Pek, J., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (1997). Shyness and low social support as
Finkel, S. M. (2008). Open hearts build lives: Positive emo- interactive diatheses, with loneliness as mediator: Testing
tions, induced through loving-kindness mediation, build an interpersonal-personality view of vulnerability to depres-
consequential personal resources. Journal of Personality and sive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106(3),
Social Psychology, 95, 1045–1062. 386–394.
Gable, S. L. (2000). Appetitive and aversive social motivation. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the Ameri-
Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of Rochester, can dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic
Rochester, NY. goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin 22, 80–87.
Gable, S. L. (2006). Approach and avoidance social motives and Keltner, D., & Lerner, J. S. (2009) Emotion. In D. Gilbert, S.
goals. Journal of Personality, 71, 175–222. Fiske, & G. Lindsey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology
Gable, S. L., & Poore, J. (2008). Which thoughts count? (5th ed., pp 312–347). New York: McGraw Hill.
Algorithms for evaluating satisfaction in relationships. Keyes, C. L. M. (1998). Social well-being. Social Psychology
Psychological Science, 19, 1030–1036 Quarterly, 61, 121–140.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2000). Behavioral acti- Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1999). Stress, personal relationships, and
vation and inhibition in everyday life. Journal of Personality immune function: Health implications. Brain, Behavior and
and Social Psychology, 78, 1135–1149. Immunity, 13(1), 61–72.
Gable, S. L., Reis, H. T., & Elliot, A. J. (2003). Evidence for bivari- Kiecolt-Glaser J. K., Gouin J. P., & Hantsoo L. (2010). Close
ate systems: An empirical test of appetition and aversion across relationships, inflammation, and health. Neuroscience Biobe-
domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 349–372. havioral Review, 35(1), 33–38.
Gray, J. A. (1970). The psychophysiological basis of introversion- Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and the
extraversion. Behaviour Research & Therapy, 8(3), 249–266. incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis: University of Min-
Gray, J. A. (1987). The psychology of fear and stress (2nd ed.). nesota Press.
New York: Cambridge University Press. Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychologi-
Gray, J. A. (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cal Bulletin, 108, 480–498.
cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269–288. Lakey, B., & Cassady, R B. (1990). Cognitive processes in
Harmon-Jones, E., & Allen, J. J. B. (1997). Behavioral activa- perceived social support. Journal of Personality and Social
tion sensitivity and resting frontal EEG asymmetry: Covaria- Psychology, 59, 331–343.
tion of putative indicators related to risk for mood disorders. Larsen, R. J., & Ketelaar, T. (1991). Personality and suscepti-
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 159–163. bility to positive and negative emotional states. Journal of
Hawkley, L. C., Masi, C. M., Berry, J. D., & Cacioppo, J. T. Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 132–140.
(2006). Loneliness is a unique predictor of age-related differ- Laurenceau, J., Kleinman, B. M., Kaczynski, K. J., &
ences in systolic blood pressure. Psychology and Aging, 21(1), Carver, C. S. (2010). Assessment of relationship-specific
152–164. incentive and threat sensitivities: Predicting satisfaction and
Higgins, E. T. (1998). Promotion and prevention: Regulatory affect in adult intimate relationships. Psychological Assessment,
focus as a motivational principle. Advances in Experimental 22(2), 407–419.
Social Psychology, 30, 1–46. Laurenceau, J., Troy, A. B., & Carver, C. S. (2005). Two distinct
Higgins, E. T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997). Emotional emotional experiences in romantic relationships: Effects of
responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus as perceptions regarding approach of intimacy and avoidance
moderator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(3), of conflict. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(8),
515–525. 1123–1133.

 avoiding the pitfalls & approaching the promises of close rel ationships
Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Robertson, T., ity and the Michelangelo phenomenon. Journal of Personality
Hofer, B., Neuberg, S. L., . . . Schaller, M. (2005). Functional and Social Psychology, 96(1), 61–82.
projection: How fundamental social motives can bias inter- Sanderson, C. A., & Cantor, N. (1997). Creating satisfaction in
personal perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- steady dating relationships: The role of personal goals and
ogy, 88, 63–78. situational affordances. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-
McAdams, D. P. (1982). Intimacy motivation. In A. J. Stewart chology, 73(6), 1424–1433.
(Ed.), Motivation and society (pp. 133–171). San Francisco, Sanderson, C. A., & Cantor, N. (2001). The association of inti-
CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers. macy goals and marital satisfaction: A test of four media-
McAdams, D. P., & Constantian, C. A. (1983). Intimacy and affili- tional hypotheses. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
ation motives in daily living: An experience sampling analysis. 27(12), 1567–1577.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 851–861. Sanderson, C. A., Rahm, K. B., & Beigbeder, S. A. (2005). The
McAdams, D. P., Healy, S., & Krause, S. (1984). Social motives link between the pursuit of intimacy goals and satisfaction in
and patterns of friendship. Journal of Personality and Social close same-sex friendships: An examination of the underlying
Psychology, 47, 828–838. processes. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22(1),
McDade, T. W., Hawkley, L. C., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2006). Psy- 75–98.
chosocial and behavioral predictors of inflammation in middle- Schneirla, T. C. (1959). An evolutionary and developmen-
aged and older adults: The Chicago health, aging, and social tal theory of biphasic processes underlying approach and
relations study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 68(3), 376–381. withdrawal. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol. 7,
Mehrabian, A. (1976). Questionnaire measures of affiliative pp. 1–43). Lincoln, Nebraska: University of Nebraska Press.
tendency and sensitivity to rejection. Psychological Reports, Scinta, A., & Gable, S. L. (2007). Automatic and self-reported
38, 199–209. attitudes in romantic relationships. Personality and Social
Mehrabian, A., & Ksionzky, S. (1974). A theory of affiliation. Psychology Bulletin, 33(7), 1008–1022.
Lexington, MA: D.C. Heath. Shah, J. Y. (2003). The motivational looking glass: How signifi-
Mikulincer, M. (1998). Attachment working models and the cant others implicitly affect goal appraisals. Journal of Person-
sense of trust: An exploration of interaction goals and affect ality and Social Psychology, 85, 424–439.
regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, Shipley, T. E., & Veroff, J. (1952). A projective measure of
1209–1224. need for affiliation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43,
Miller, N. E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: 349–356.
Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation, and social learn- Sobotka, S. S., Davidson, R. J., & Senulis, J. A. (1992). Ante-
ing. In S. Koch (Ed.). Psychology: A study of science, Study 1 rior brain electrical asymmetries in response to reward and
(pp. 198–292). New York: McGraw-Hill. punishment. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysi-
Miller, N. E. (1961). Learning and performance motivated ology, 83, 236–247.
by direct stimulation of the brain. In D. E. Sheer (Ed.), Elec- Strachman, A., & Gable, S. L. (2006). What you want (and
trical stimulation of the brain (pp. 387–396). Austin, TX: don’t want) affects what you see (and don’t see): Avoidance
University of Texas Press. social goals and social events. Personality and Social Psychology
Morgan, Ch. D., & Murray, H. A. (1935). A method for investi- Bulletin, 32, 1446–1458.
gating fantasies: The Thematic Apperception Test. Archives Uchino, B. N., Cacioppo, J. T., & Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K. (1996).
of Neurology and Psychiatry, 34, 289–306. The relationship between social support and physiological
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: processes: A review with emphasis on underlying mecha-
Oxford University Press. nisms and implications for health. Psychological Bulletin, 119,
Neuberg, S. L. (1996). Expectancy influences in social interac- 488–531
tion: The moderating role of social goals. In P. M. Gollwitzer Updegraff, J. A., Gable, S. L., & Taylor, S. E. (2004). What
& J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cogni- makes experiences satisfying? The interaction of approach-
tion and motivation to behavior (pp. 529–552). New York: avoidance motivations and emotions in well-being. Journal
Guilford Press. of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 496–504.
Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes: An investigation into the Veroff, J., Douvan, E., & Kulka, R. A. (1981). Mental health
physiological activity of the cortex (G. Anrep, Trans.). New in America: Patterns of help-seeking from 1957 to 1976. New
York: Dover. York: Basic Books.
Perlman, D., & Peplau, L. A. (1981). Toward a social psychol- Whisman, M. A. (2001). Marital adjustment and outcome fol-
ogy of loneliness. In S. Duck & R. Gilmour (Eds.), Personal lowing treatments for depression. Journal of Consulting and
relationships 3: Personal relationships in disorder (pp. 31–55). Clinical Psychology, 69(1), 125–129.
London: Academic Press. Whisman, M. A., Uebelacker, L. A., & Settles, T. D. (2010).
Reis, H. T., Collins, W. A., & Berscheid, E. (2000). The relation- Martial distress and the metabolic syndrome: Linking social
ship context of human behavior and development. Psycho- functioning with physical health. Journal of Family Psychol-
logical Bulletin, 126(6), 844–872. ogy, 24(3), 367–370.
Ryff, C. D. (1995). Psychological well-being in adult life. Cur- Wilcox, V. L., Kasl, S. V., & Berkman, L. F. (1994). Social
rent Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 99–103. support and physical disability in older people after hospi-
Rusbult, C. E., Kumashiro, M., Kubacka, K. E., & Finkel, E. J. talization: A prospective study. Health Psychology, 13(2),
(2009). “The part of me that you bring out”: Ideal similar- 170–179.

g a b le, p rok 


This page intentionally left blank
PA RT
6
Evolutionary and
Biological Perspectives
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER

Neuroscience and Human Motivation


21
Johnmarshall Reeve and Woogul Lee

Abstract
Recognizing the potential for interdisciplinary research in motivational neuroscience, the goal of the
present chapter is to show the relevance of neuroscience research to human motivation researchers
and to suggest ways to expand their programs of research, methodological options, and theoretical
conceptualizations of the motivational constructs with which they work. To illustrate the neural bases
of human motivation, we highlight 15 key motivation-relevant brain structures, identify the neural core
of reward-based motivated action, and discuss a range of brain-generated motivational states that
extend from those that are relatively automatic and stimulus dependent (e.g., pleasure from taste) to
those that are relatively intentional and context sensitive (e.g., goals). We then examine the following
10 well-researched concepts from the human motivation literature to suggest how each might be
enriched through neuroscientific investigation: agency, volition, value, intrinsic motivation, extrinsic
motivation, flow, expectancy, self-efficacy, self-regulation, and goals. We conclude with suggestions for
future research.
Key Words: motivation, neuroscience, striatum, reward, dopamine, prefrontal cortex

Introduction skepticism are in the air when human motivation


The “and” in the chapter title is important, as it researchers sit down at the table with neuroscientists
reflects the contemporary view that human motiva- to discuss collaborations and points of integration.
tion study and neuroscience are two different fields. A decade ago, Richard Mayer (1998) character-
That is, the people who study human motivation, ized the relationship between neuroscience and his
the journals they publish in, and the empirical field—educational psychology—through the imag-
methods they rely on are not generally populated by ery of dead-end, one-way, and two-way streets. He
a neuroscience focus, though these same researchers characterized (and lamented) the then-present rela-
(and journals) recognize the potential contribution tion between neuroscience and his field as an intel-
of neuroscience to human motivation study. Neu- lectual landscape characterized by dead-end streets
roscientists often study the same content—the same in which the two fields of study had little in com-
motivational constructs, though they routinely mon and contributed little to the enrichment of the
conceptualize these motivational constructs more other. He also observed (and again lamented) an
narrowly. Neuroscientists also tend to study basic, intellectual landscape of one-way streets in which
stimulus-driven motivations, such as hunger, thirst, neuroscience research was unidirectionally applied
pleasure and reward, though more complex motiva- to educational psychology. For instance, neurosci-
tions (e.g., volition, self-regulation) are also inves- entists identified the limits of hippocampal-based
tigated. Overall, equal measures of optimism and short-term memory (e.g., cognitive overload), and


educational psychologists revised their theories of neuroscience. If interdisciplinary motivational neu-
learning and their recommendations for the design roscientists are to become a critical mass of scholars,
of instruction accordingly (e.g., Paas, Tuovinen, researchers in both fields will need to consider the
Tabbers, & Van Gerven, 2003). merits of reengineering these otherwise one-way
The metaphor Mayer offered to enrich interdis- and dead-end streets into two-way streets of infor-
ciplinary activity was that of a two-way street. In mation, methodology, and theory development. To
this scenario, neuroscience study influences moti- facilitate such progress in the present chapter, we first
vation research, while motivation study influences overview the neuroscience research that is broadly
neuroscience research. Such a two-way relationship relevant to probably all contemporary human
is only possible with the emergence and contribu- motivation study as we illuminate the biological
tions of interdisciplinary researchers. Interdisci- substrates of human motivation. We then address
plinary researchers are those who feel free and able conceptual points of convergence and divergence
to traverse not only the landscape of their home between neuroscience and human motivation study
field of study but also the landscape of the allied across the following 10 frequently studied motiva-
field. Several examples of such successful interdis- tional constructs: agency, volition, value, intrinsic
ciplinary research have emerged, including cogni- motivation, extrinsic motivation, flow, expectancy,
tive neuroscience (Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun, self-efficacy, self-regulation, and goals.
2008), affective neuroscience (Davidson & Sutton, Any new field of study (e.g., motivational neurosci-
1995), social neuroscience (Decety & Cacioppo, ence) necessarily begins with description and taxonomy.
2010), and neuroeconomics (Loewenstein, Rick, & In that spirit, Figure 21.1 lists 15 key brain structures
Cohen, 2008). identified by neuroscience research as motivation rel-
The goal of the present chapter is to embrace this evant and illustrates the anatomic location for each.
two-way street imagery and, in doing so, embrace Five structures reside within the neocortex: prefrontal
the potential value in interdisciplinary motivational cortex, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral

(A) (B) Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex


Ventral
Anterior striatum: Globus Dorsal striatum:
cingulate Nucleus pallidus Caudate nucleus
cortex accumbens
& Putamen

Prefrontal Hypothalamus
cortex Substantia
nigra

Insular cortex

(C) Caudate nucleus

Orbitofrontal Putamen
cortex Insular cortex

Ventral Globus pallidus


Ventromedial
prefrontal cortex tegmental
Amygdala Hippocampus
area

Ventral Hippocampus
palladium

Fig. 21.1. Anatomic location of 15 key motivation-relevant brain structures. (A) A medial sagittal section of the brain. The dotted line
represents the point that a coronal section of the brain (C) is acquired. (B) A lateral sagittal section of the brain.

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


prefrontal cortex, orbitofrontal cortex, and anterior cin- fashion. In this way, past reward-related information
gulate cortex. Six structures reside with the basal gan- helps establish an anticipatory motivational value
glia: dorsal striatum—caudate nucleus and putamen, of objects and events. Reward receipt and reward
ventral striatum—nucleus accumbens, globus pallidus, expectation both involve neural activations that typ-
ventral tegmental area, substantia nigra, and ventral ically give rise to pleasant feelings and a good mood
palladium. And four structures reside within the lim- and, hence, to the subjective experiences of pleasure
bic system: amygdala, hypothalamus, hippocampus, and positive affect (at least in humans). This same
and insular cortex. It is with these 15 brain struc- reward-related information also serves as the basis
tures that we will illustrate the neural bases of human of future goals, which are mental representations of
motivation. sought-after (reward-related) environmental events.
When defined in the context of behavioral sci- In addition, when the reward values of multiple
ence, motivation concerns the study of all those environmental events are compared, people show
processes that give behavior its energy and direc- preferences (in terms of choice and the amount of
tion (Reeve, 2009). In neuroscience, motivation effort expended) for different objects and events.
is generally conceptualized as energy for behaviors Hence, biologically experienced reward serves as
related to obtaining rewarding stimuli or situa- the basis not only for reward but for the additional
tions (Mogenson, Jones, & Yim, 1980; Robbins & motivational constructs of value, expectancy, plea-
Everitt, 1996). That which energizes behavior is sure/affect, goal, and preference.
subscribed to a rather narrow set of neural pro- The neural substrates of this dopaminergic family
cesses, such as those in the mesolimbic dopamine of reward-based motivational states appear in Figure
system. While these basic neural processes energize 21.2. The neural core of goal-directed motivated action
behavior, the sources that activate these basic neu- is the pathway from the motivation-generating dop-
ral processes are many (e.g., natural rewards, social amine system to the movement-preparation and
rewards; Berridge, 2004; Berridge & Robinson, behavior-generating supplementary motor area and
2003; Wise, 2004). In the next section, we summa- premotor cortex (see right side of Fig. 21.2). Within
rize the basic subcortical neural core that energizes the phrase “motivated action,” the Dopamine system
reward-related action. Once done, we overview the box represents the fundamental core of “motivated”
more specific types of motivation that activate these while the Substantia nigra, globus pallidus box rep-
basic subcortical neural processes. resents the fundamental core of “action.” Feeding
into this basic reward processing core are a number
Neural Core of Reward-Based of brain areas that process reward information by
Motivated Action releasing dopamine, such as responsiveness to natural
From a biological perspective, the role of reward rewards (hypothalamus), the particular characteris-
in motivation is fundamental. It is fundamental to tics of any one particular reward in the limbic regions
survival, to learning, to well-being, and to the gen- (e.g., amygdala), and the interoceptive information
eration of goal-directed effort (Schultz, 2000). The of rewards in the limbic-related regions (e.g., insular
energization or generation of goal-directed effort cortex) as well as responsiveness to the values (and
(motivated action) follows from and is dependent relative values) of various rewards (orbitofrontal cor-
on first extracting reward-related information from tex), the mental representation of reward as a goal
environmental objects, events, and circumstances, object (dorsolateral prefrontal cortex), and executive
and this reward-related information consists largely control over goal-directed action (anterior cingu-
of the release of the neurotransmitter dopamine late cortex). In addition, as depicted in the boldface
(Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008).1 The reward- double-sided arrow on the left-hand side of the
related information that people extract from their figure, reciprocal relations connect the limbic regions
surroundings includes the presence and availability with the prefrontal cortex as limbic regions gener-
of reward, the value of the available reward, the pre- ally feed-forward projections into the prefrontal
dictability of the reward, and the costs associated cortex while prefrontal regions generally feed-back
with trying to obtain that reward. projections to the various limbic regions. Lastly, as
In addition, repeated experiences with objects depicted in the six double-sided arrows in the center
and events allow people to form mental repre- of the figure, reciprocal relations connect the dop-
sentations in which these environmental stimuli amine system with the limbic regions and prefrontal
come to signal reward information in a predictive cortex.

reeve, lee 


Limbic Regions
Hypothalamus
Responsive to natural rewards.

AMYGDALA
Responsive to particular reward characteristics. Dopamine system
Substantia Nigra,
Insular cortex Ventral tegmental area
Globus Pallidus
Representation of emotional states of rewards. Relay information of rewards to striatum.
Behavior preparation and execution.
These basal ganglia structures project
into the pre-supplemental motor area,
STRIATUM the supplemental motor area, and the
Prefrontal Cortex Responsive to information of rewards. primary motor cortex (i.e., the motor
Orbitofrontal Cortex * Nucleus Accumbens areas of the cortex).
Responsive to the value of rewards; responsive * Caudate Nucleus
to preferences between rewards. * Putamen

Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex


Mental representation of reward as a goal object.

Anterior Cingulate Cortex


Executive control of goal-directed action.

Fig. 21.2. Neural core of reward-based motivated action.

Sources of Reward-Based Motivation Relatively Automatic Motivational States


It is important to understand the nature of Neuroscientific approaches to motivation do
various biological sources of motivation (depicted a particularly good job of explaining relatively auto-
on the left-hand side of Fig. 21.2) because differ- matic homeostatic motivational processes that are
ent sources of motivation lead to different types driven by ingestibles (or consumables), such as food
of motivation. For instance, some sources of and water. Ingested substances are natural rewards
motivation are implicit and objective (e.g., thirst, (e.g., food, water) that play a key role in energizing
hunger), while other sources are more conscious consumatory behaviors that then lead to changes in
and cognitive (e.g., ultimate goals). As we will homeostatic and hedonic motivational states. These
see, the types of motivation induced by relatively motivational states are closely monitored and regu-
implicit and objective sources tend to generate lated by subcortical limbic structures (Saper, Chou,
rather automatic motivational states, whereas the & Elmquist, 2002), as the hypothalamus plays an
types of motivation induced by more conscious important role in relatively automatic consumatory
and cognitive sources tend to be rather rational behavior while the dopamine-based mesolimbic sys-
motivational states. Accordingly, to classify and tem plays an important role in learned instrumental
to understand the different types of biologically behaviors. Homeostatic motivational states such as
generated motivational states, we need to think hunger (appetite) and satiety arise rather automati-
carefully about (1) what the sources of the moti- cally (and reliably) from cooperative networks dis-
vational state are, (2) how much the source of the tributed throughout the body, including those in
motivational state is implicit and objective (versus the brain (hypothalamus, mesolimbic system) but
explicit and cognitive), and (3) how much the var- also those in the endocrine/hormonal and auto-
ious sources of motivation conflict when sources nomic systems (Powley, 2009).
of motivation are divergent. Based on these con- Thirst is a brain-generated motivational state that
siderations, we present four sections to illustrate arises when people physiologically need to ingest
a range of brain-generated motivational states water to maintain adequate fluid balance through-
that extend from those that are largely subcorti- out the body. Reduced water generates thirst—the
cal, relatively automatic, and stimulus dependent urge to ingest water, and the body’s remarkable
(e.g., pleasure from taste) to those that are largely constancy of intracellular and extracellular water
cortical, intentional, and context sensitive (e.g., is regulated by neural, hormonal/endocrine, and
personal strivings). behavioral mechanisms (McKinley, 2009). Though

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


hypothalamic-based thirst contributes to water mainly stored in the hippocampus and insular
intake (drinking) and to the involuntary regulation cortex, though it is also stored in cortical regions as
of water conservation (e.g., hormone release, kid- well, including the orbitofrontal cortex. The more
ney function), most human beverage consumption automatic or simple the incentive-based information
is determined by the reward aspects of the ingested is, the more likely it is that it will be stored subcorti-
fluid, including those related to taste, odor, tem- cally in the limbic system or in the limbic-related
perature, alcohol, caffeine, and social consequences regions (e.g., insular cortex); the more cognitive and
(Booth, 1991). Thus, brain structures such as the less automated the incentive-based information is,
orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala respond to the the more likely it is that it will be stored cortically
rewarding properties of fluid intake (Rolls, 2000), in the orbitofrontal cortex. When instrumental
and these brain structures then feed this reward- behaviors are needed, these various brain regions
related information into the striatum that underlies deliver their stored incentive value information to
the dopamine reward system that energizes fluid the mesolimbic dopamine system, which then ener-
intake (Wise, 2002), as depicted in Figure 21.2. gizes consumatory motivated action (when intense
Recognizing the important motivational role of enough to exceed a threshold of response). In addi-
the rewarding properties of ingestibles (e.g., sweet tion, the nucleus accumbens (within the ventral stri-
taste) expands the neural bases of motivation from atum) is active in the experience of rewarding and
hypothalamic-centric homeostatic motivational pleasurable feelings, as the presentation of pleasant
states to include stimulus-driven, dopamine-centric images, pleasant tastes, and many addictive drugs
motivational states (i.e., incentive motivation). (e.g., opiates, cocaine, amphetamine) are dopamine
releasers in the nucleus accumbens (Sabatinelli
Motivational States Based on Associative et al., 2007; Wise, 2002).
Learning (Close to Automatic) To explain how associative learning processes
Environmental incentives are those we tend to occur, some researchers parse reward information
approach and return to after experiencing their into three psychological components—learning,
rewarding properties. Incentives have rewarding affect (emotion), and motivation (Berridge, 2004;
properties and promote approach-oriented behav- Berridge & Robinson, 2003). Learning has two
ior because they send information through the five forms—associative and cognitive. Associative learn-
senses that reach the mesolimbic dopamine-based ing refers to the relatively automatic forms of incen-
reward circuitry to (1) activate those reward path- tive learning, while cognitive refers to the relatively
ways (e.g., Fig. 21.2), (2) activate those reward more complex and less automatic learning related
pathways powerfully (above threshold), (3) activate to activities in the cortex (e.g., orbitofrontal cortex).
those reward pathways with little delay in reinforce- Affect also has two forms: liking and conscious plea-
ment (so to yield a high degree of reward effective- sure. Liking is one’s implicit (nonconscious), hedo-
ness), and (4) produce rewarding effects that decay nic reaction to an objective environmental stimulus
rapidly (half-second after onset) (Wise, 2002). Some (e.g., sweet taste) that arises from nondopamine
incentive values are universal or objective, such as mesolimbic activity (e.g., opioid neurotransmis-
a sweet taste or a toxic smell. Other incentive val- sion). Conscious pleasure is a more general form of
ues (e.g., color preference) are learned subjectively liking that involves awareness and arises from corti-
or circumstantially. The more an incentive value is cal activity. Motivation too has two forms—want-
universal or objective, the more it will be associated ing which is implicit (nonconscious) and objective,
with motivational states that are automatic. and wanting that is cognitive, conscious, and goal
The learning (remembering, conditioning) of directed.
the incentive value of environmental events takes The affective distinction between implicit liking
place in several brain areas. The amygdala evaluates and explicit pleasure and the motivational distinc-
a stimulus as associated with either reward or pun- tion between implicit desire and explicit goal striv-
ishment, signals that it is potentially important (or ing is important for several reasons. First, affect and
not), and evaluates the stimulus as unpredicted or motivation can diverge. Liking and wanting typi-
not (Whalen, 1999, 2007). In this way, amygdala cally converge in natural situations (i.e., we want
activity builds associative knowledge about a stimu- and like the same thing), but they can diverge, as
lus’s motivational and emotional significance (Baxter when a medicine smells or tastes disgusting (no lik-
et al., 2000; Baxter & Murray, 2002; Schoenbaum, ing) yet is wanted for health reasons (cognitive want-
Chiba, & Gallagher, 1999). This information is ing) or when one craves an addictive drug (implicit

reeve, lee 


wanting) that brings little or no pleasure (conscious humor, task success) activates dopamine neurons in
liking). Second, these two forms of liking and these the ventral tegmental area, which sends dopamine
two forms of wanting mean that incentive values projections into many cortical areas, including
will sometimes be conflicting in naturally occurring (a) the prefrontal cortex, which enriches working
behavior (e.g., should I watch the television show memory, openness to information, willingness to
I like or should I go to a social event to meet poten- explore, creative problem solving, and the integra-
tial new friends?). In these situations, people need tion of ideas; and (b) the anterior cingulate cortex,
to resolve these motivational conflicts using higher which increases attention, flexible thinking, switch-
order cognitive, emotional, and motivational pro- ing easily among alternative objects or action plans,
cesses (Litman, 2005). and the sort of enhanced perspective taking that
leads to prosocial behaviors such as cooperativeness,
Implicit Motivational States Involved in generosity, social responsibility, and improved nego-
Decision Making and Action tiating skills (Ashby et al., 1999).
Subcortical (limbic system) processing of envi- Crucially, the dopamine hypothesis of positive
ronmental events plays an important role in deci- affect proposes that it is only mild, everyday positive
sion making and action. In daily life, few situations feelings—the type of positive affect that remains
involve only a single stimulus, as decision making in outside of conscious attention—that produces these
the face of diverging and conflicting incentive values facilitating effects on decision making, problem
is the norm (two restaurants, two social events, 30 solving, creativity, and prosocial behavior (Isen,
different chapters in this Handbook). When people 2003). If the dopamine increase is relatively large
make decisions, they rely on a great deal on both or if the person is made aware of the positive affect
cognitive processes and emotional processes, even state (e.g., “My, aren’t we in a good mood today?”),
to the point that it is difficult to separate out cogni- then research shows that the facilitating effect is lost
tive activity from emotional activity, as the two are (Isen, 1987). The dopamine hypothesis, however,
so neurally intertwined that it makes little sense to seems to contradict the wanting versus liking dis-
treat them as separate entities during decision mak- tinction introduced in the previous section, as lik-
ing. In this section, we review how nonconscious ing is not dopamine based. The difference between
processing creates feelings (e.g., affect, intuition) the two hypotheses might suggest that the positive
that bias what memory content emerges into con- affect (liking) is epiphenomenal and that it is only
scious awareness that is then acted on in terms of the dopamine increase (not the positive affect expe-
decision making and action. Such affectively based rience per se) that facilitates cognitive processes and
decision making can be demonstrated through the prosocial behavior.
dopamine hypothesis of positive affect, priming,
and the somatic marker hypothesis. priming
Priming is the procedure that evokes an implicit
dopamine hypothesis of positive affect response from an individual upon exposure to a
Positive affect influences decision making and stimulus that is outside his or her conscious aware-
problem solving such that people who feel good, ness. While priming occurs outside of the person’s
compared to people in a neutral mood, are more conscious awareness, the prime itself can be deliv-
likely to recall positive material from memory, and ered unconsciously or consciously. An example of
this accessibility has been shown to promote flexibil- an unconsciously delivered prime might be a word
ity in thinking, creative problem solving, efficiency that is flashed so briefly on a computer screen (e.g.,
and thoroughness in decision making, improved 30 msec) that it is not recognized, though it still
thinking on complex tasks, variety seeking, enhanced produces an implicit effect. An example of a con-
intrinsic motivation, and a greater willingness to sciously delivered prime might occur as the person
help (Isen, 1987, 2003). The dopamine hypothesis is asked to judge if a dot appears above or below
of positive affect (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999) a word, a word whose content induces an implicit
proposes that the presence of mild positive feelings effect (e.g., the words “good” or “pleasant” might
systemically affects cognitive processes and that it produce implicit positive feelings).
is increased dopamine in certain brain regions that Primes that activate a mental representation of
produces the mild positive feelings and facilitating a behavior (outside the person’s awareness) prepare
effects on cognition. For instance, the receipt of a people to enact behaviors consistent with that mental
small unexpected positive event (unexpected gift, representation. For instance, the smell of a cleaning

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


solution, the site of a briefcase, and viewing a library processes and learns about risk and uncertainty. This
painting lead people to engage in cleaning behav- is important because the role of the insula seems
ior, competitive behavior, and hushed conversation, to be to integrate current feeling, a risk prediction
compared to the absence of these primes, though forecast (that has a degree of uncertainty) that arises
participants report being unaware of the aroma, from the anticipation or consideration of the future
briefcase, or painting (Aarts & Dijksterhuis, 2003; outcomes of one’s actions, and contextual informa-
Holland, Hendriks, & Aarts, 2005; Kay, Wheeler, tion to produce a global feeling state that guides
Bargh, & Ross, 2004). These findings show that decision making (Singer et al., 2009).
nonconscious primes prepare (i.e., motivate) action. The somatic marker hypothesis was originally
Primes also influence a wide range of motiva- based on observations that patients with ventro-
tions. Primes have been shown to activate implicit medial prefrontal cortex lesions commonly showed
motives such as power and affiliation (Schultheiss, emotional impairments and made destructive social
2008), outcome expectancies (Custers, Aarts, decisions, even though their cognitive capacities
Oikawa, & Elliot, 2009), autonomous motiva- were unaffected. Based on these clinical observa-
tions (Hodgins, Yacko, & Gottlieb, 2006), and so tions, researchers proposed that emotional processes
forth. For instance, students who were asked to (bodily states and feelings in this case) played an
solve language puzzles populated by achievement- important and constructive role in the decision-
related words (“win”) outperformed and outper- making process (Damasio, 1994, 1996). The body’s
sisted students who were asked to solve the same primary inducer of bodily states is the amygdala,
language puzzles populated by neutral words when and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex works as a
both groups worked on a second task unrelated to secondary inducer of bodily states (e.g., pain, heart-
the language-puzzle task (Bargh et al., 2001). This beat awareness, rhythm, affiliation) (Baxter & Mur-
means that the nonconscious activation of the moti- ray, 2002; Baxter et al., 2000; Schoenbaum et al.,
vational state promotes behavioral activation if the 1999). As incentive-related events (those associated
motivational state itself is associated with positive with motivational and emotional significance for
valence (Aarts, Custers, & Marien, 2008; Custers & the person) change the body, the insula integrates
Aarts, 2005). That is, primes facilitate motivated these changes into a conscious, subjective emotional
action by activating mental representations of experience (much in the spirit of the James-Lange
action (i.e., the subliminal presentation of the words theory of emotion; James, 1894).
“exert” and “vigorous”), implicit motivational states,
and positive affect; furthermore, these effects occur Neural Bases of Rational Motivational
even though participants are unaware of the presen- States in Decision Making and Action
tation of the primes. Several regions in the prefrontal cortex exert
executive or cognitive control over decision mak-
somatic marker hypothesis ing and action. For instance, the medial prefrontal
Another hypothesis about the role of feelings in cortex (both dorsal and ventral), inferior frontal
decision making is the somatic marker hypothesis cortex, dorsal section of the anterior cingulate cor-
(Bechara & Damasio, 2005; Bechara, Damasio, & tex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex all work
Damasio, 2000). In this hypothesis, the key brain for cognitive control of decision making and action
structure is the insular cortex (Singer, Critchley, & (Davidson & Irwin, 1999; Ochsner & Gross, 2005;
Preuschoff, 2009). The insula (insular cortex) pro- Ridderinkhof, van den Wildenberg, Segalowitz, &
cesses interoceptive (visceral, homeostatic) informa- Carter, 2004). As a case in point, the dorsolateral
tion about the state of one’s body and allows the prefrontal cortex activations occur when one pur-
person to construct a consciously aware representa- sues a long-term reward in favor of a shorter term,
tion of how he or she feels (Craig, 2009; Wicker striatum-based reward (McClure, Laibson, Loewen-
et al., 2003). Furthermore, insula activity seems to be stein, & Cohen, 2004).
involved in practically all subjective feelings (Craig, In understanding the cognitive control over
2009). In the anterior insula, people consolidate decision making and action, one needs to recognize
this feeling-state information with social-contextual the massive cortical feedback that occurs through-
information about the task they are involved in out the brain. For instance, the amygdala not only
and the people around them to form a basis of the processes the emotional significance of sensory
conscious experience (subjective awareness) of emo- information and sends that information to the
tion or affect (Craig, 2002, 2008). The insular also prefrontal cortex (feed-forward), but the amygdala

reeve, lee 


also receives information from the prefrontal 2006). It is this set of complex executive predictions
cortex (Freese & Amaral, 2005). Similar (and mas- and forecasts that dominate current thinking about
sive) feedback flows of information occur through- human motivation (e.g., goals, plans, expectations,
out cortical and subcortical brain regions (as future time perspective, possible selves), the topic to
depicted by the large double-sided arrow between which we now turn.
them in Fig. 21.2). This prefrontal cortex flow of
feedback information adds information about the Key Motivational Constructs in Human
environmental context and conscious intentions Motivation Study
into the neural core depicted on the right-hand side To this point in the chapter, the conversation
of Figure 21.2. Furthermore, this prefrontal lobe has been rather one sided, as we have presented
information comes in cycles of continuous infor- and summarized the neuroscientific perspective on
mation and, according to some estimates, these top- motivation. In the present section, we focus on sev-
down feedback projections likely exceed the number eral central motivational constructs that are richly
of bottom-up feed-forward projections, at least with studied in the human motivation research litera-
adults (Salin & Bullier, 1995). The result is an inte- ture that occurs outside of a neuroscience focus. In
grated feed-forward and feedback system in which doing so, we will compare and contrast the human
basic sensory information feeds-forward rather auto- motivation understanding of these complex motiva-
matically and rapidly, while top-down deliberative tional states with the neuroscientific understanding
information (intentions, goals), which is affected of these same phenomena. In particular, we discuss
and biased by the aforementioned feed-forward agency, volition, value, intrinsic motivation, extrin-
information, contributes regulatory and intentional sic motivation, flow, expectancy, self-efficacy, self-
processing (Cunningham & Zelazo, 2007; Miller & regulation, and goals.
Cohen, 2001).
Motivationally relevant brain structures are clearly Agency
reactive and responsive to environmental events. In Agency is the sense that “I did that,” and it lies
this sense, motivation “happens” to the person as an at the center of intentional, voluntary, and purpose-
adaptive reaction to these environmental events. It driven action. Human motivation researchers tend
is also true, however, that brain activity is proactive to study agency broadly, defining it, for instance,
in that people regularly anticipate the future (Bar, as self-generated motivation to act on the environ-
2007). According to Bar, people are not so much ment—the proactive desire to create, manipulate,
passively waiting to be activated by environmental influence, and transform the environment that one
events as they are continuously busy generating pre- is in so to improve it in some way (Bandura, 2006).
dictions about the future. These predictions have Neuroscientists study agency more narrowly, as they
motivational and emotional implications and there- contrast an experience of self-as-cause versus other-
fore focus attention on the neural bases of proactive as-cause of an action (Engbert, Wohlschlager, &
and purposive motivational states. Haggard, 2008; Farrer & Frith, 2002; Spengler, von
At one extreme, the brain is involved in proxi- Cramon, & Brass, 2009). In these investigations,
mal predictions, such as expecting to receive a shot the person performs a simple action (e.g., move
upon walking in the doctor’s office. But, at another a joystick) that causes an event to happen (e.g.,
extreme, the brain is involved in distal predictions, as make an image appear on the screen), and the causal
the person anticipates experiences, plans far ahead, source of that action is manipulated experimentally
and uses memory-guided simulations to mentally such that what happens is directly linked to the per-
travel into the future (Szpunar, Watson, & McDer- son’s own intentions and behaviors or is unrelated
mott, 2007; Addis, Wong, & Schacter, 2007). The to them, because a computer program or the experi-
important point is that people plan, imagine, and menter causes the action such that anything done by
project themselves into the future in a way that the participant is superfluous. Results show that an
allows them to better prepare for that future, and experience of agency is closely linked to and depen-
these activities are subserved by brain processes dent on the activities of motor-related brain regions,
specific for complex executive forecasts and predic- such as the supplemental motor area and the presup-
tions. Necessarily, these future-oriented forecasts, plemental motor area, which plan and enact an effer-
intentions, decisions, and plans take place under ent motor command—that is, agency arises from a
conditions of uncertainty (Cohen & Aston-Jones, tight relation between action and effect as the person
2005; Daw, O’Doherty, Dayan, Seymour, & Dolan, must self-generate the motor instruction to perform

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


an action to feel a true sense of personal agency. If researchers need to examine the neural circuits of
the person enacts the same behavior without self- various aspects of cognitive, emotional, and moti-
instruction to do so (e.g., an outside agent actually vational control over action, and some neuroscien-
causes the person’s behavior), little agency is experi- tist have begun to do this (Haggard, 2008; Nachev,
enced. Furthermore, the greater the length of time 2006; Nachev et al., 2005).
that elapses between one’s action and the effect it pro-
duces, the less the resulting sense of agency will be, as Value
the sense of “I did that” is put into doubt by the rival Value is a central concept in contemporary moti-
belief that “maybe something or someone else did vation study, as it serves as the core construct under-
it” (Spengler et al., 2009). Such agency is associated lying the expectancy X value family of motivation
with activation in the insula, while such nonagency theories (Wigfield & Eccles, 2002). In expectancy
is associated with activation in the inferior parietal X value theories, value is a multidimensional con-
cortex (Farrer et al., 2003; Farrer & Frith, 2002). struct composed of four divergent sources: intrinsic
Pressing a button while lying in an fMRI machine interest, utility value, attainment value, and cost.
is a long way from improving one’s working condi- High values on each of these components of value
tions or changing one’s career path, but the premise (cost needs to be reversed scored) generally cor-
is the same—“unless people believe they can produce relate with choice behavior and persistence (Wig-
desired effects by their actions, they have little incen- field & Eccles, 2002). This conceptualization of
tive to act” (Bandura, 2006, p. 170). value is noticeably different from the neuroscience
conceptualization of value, which is the incentive-
Volition based, reward-related information of an object or
Some neuroscientists study mental control over event, and that reward value is sometimes natural
action as volition, rather than as agency (Haggard, (e.g., water, orange juice) but often learned or con-
2008). In this research, neuroscientists use experi- ditioned (Dickinson & Balleine, 2002). When the
mental tasks that give participants freedom whether learned reward-based information is subjective or
to perform actions, when to perform actions, or circumstantial (rather than universal or objective),
how many times to perform actions, and they then orbitofrontal cortex information is active and, once
search for related neural activities (Haggard, 2008; the incentive value of various environmental objects
Libet, Gleason, Wright, & Pearl, 1983; Nachev, and events is learned, activity in the orbitofrontal
2006; Nachev, Rees, Praton, Kennard, & Husain, cortex helps people make choices between options,
2005). The results consistently indicate that (1) vol- consider their options, remember the incentive
untary control activates motor-related brain regions, value associated with each of those options, and
such as the supplementary motor area and the pre- make their selection among the differently valued
supplementary motor area, and (2) conflict moni- objects to pursue (Arana et al., 2003; Rushworth,
toring during this voluntary control activates the Behrens, Rudebeck, & Walton, 2007).
dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, as the individual While expectancy X value theorists emphasize
attempts to cope with the cognitive conflicts that divergent sources of valuing, neuroscientists gener-
arise. In the human motivation research literature, ally do the opposite and emphasize the converging
Heinz Heckhausen distinguished what was termed sources of valuing. The orbitofrontal-striatal circuit
agency in the preceding paragraph from volition by is viewed as a valuation system in which this circuit
defining agency (motivation) as that which initiates continually computes valuation (how rewarding,
action (e.g., need, goal), whereas volition involved how punishing) across a broad range of stimuli and
the persistent striving of that motivated action over environmental events (Montague & Berns, 2002).
time and in the face of obstacles (Heckhausen, It does so by valuing all these potential stimuli and
1977). In other words, human motivation research- events on a common dopamine-based scale, which
ers view volition as the cognitive, emotional, and is sort of like the neural equivalent of monetary
motivational control that occurs over time to carry currency in a nation’s economic system. Rewards
out (not to initiate) goal-directed behavior (Goll- vary on their type, magnitude, salience, and imme-
witzer, 1996). As such, volition encompasses diverse diacy, and the orbitofrontal-striatal circuit (and
cognitive, emotional, and motivational processes the striatum in particular) convert and integrate
(e.g., conflict monitoring). To expand the study these diverse sources of reward-based informa-
of volition beyond that of agency, it would seem tion into a common currency and, by doing so,
that interdisciplinary motivational neuroscience value all rewards on a common scale. Once diverse

reeve, lee 


environmental incentives can be compared and “my feelings about that thing” (Craig, 2009, p. 65).
contrasted via a common currency, people can As one example, people experience greater insular
compare disparate stimuli (which would you rather activity as they enjoy (experience spontaneous satis-
do—drink a glass of orange juice, go for a walk in factions from the experience) music (Koelsch, Fritz,
the park, or play a videogame?) so to assign their Cramon, Muller, & Friederici, 2006).
attention and plan their action. Perhaps some simi- In the neuroscience literature, extrinsic moti-
lar process allows people to integrate the various vation is synonymous with incentive motivation,
sources of value within expectancy X value theory which we reviewed under the heading of “Moti-
(intrinsic value, utility value, and attainment value) vational States Based on Associative Learning.” In
on a common scale to compare the value of an inter- the human motivation literature, extrinsic motiva-
esting but not useful event (play) with an uninter- tion arises from environmental incentives and con-
esting but useful event (work). sequences (e.g., food, money, tokens, extra credit
A second perspective on value in the human points) in which approach motivation is based not on
motivation literature conceptualizes it as an inter- the characteristics of the task itself but on the condi-
nalization process in which socially recommended tioned incentive value of the separate environmental
prescriptions (“do this, believe that”) and pro- event/consequence. As people experience extrinsic
scriptions (“don’t do this, don’t believe that”) are motivation toward a task, they show greater orb-
accepted as one’s own (Ryan & Connell, 1989). The itofrontal cortex activity as they weigh the value of
internalization process of valuing is not so much an the incentive being offered and greater anterior cin-
emotionally associative process (as valuing is studied gulate cortex as they go through a decision-making
in neuroscience) as it is a process in which a particu- process as to whether engagement in the activity will
lar way of thinking, feeling, or behaving is accepted bring enough benefit to justify the effort expendi-
as personally beneficial for self-functioning (similar ture (Plassmann, O’Doherty, & Rangel, 2007). In
to the “utility value” in expectancy X value theories). the human motivation literature, however, extrinsic
It is an active and intentional process that is based motivation is a complex construct in which types of
not on reward but in self-development and adjust- extrinsic motivation exist, including external regula-
ment (Ryan, 1993). In both the expectancy X value tion (the prototype of extrinsic motivation, which
literature and in the internalization literature, value is incentive motivation), introjected regulation (the
(like volition in the previous section) is conceptu- person—rather than the environment per se—self-
alized more broadly than it is in the neuroscience administers rewards and punishments, as in feeling
literature. contingent pride or contingent shame), and iden-
tified regulation (discussed in the previous section
Intrinsic Motivation and Extrinsic as the internalized process of valuing). This differ-
Motivation entiated view of extrinsic motivation has not been
Intrinsic motivation is the inherent propensity to explored in the neuroscientific research literature.
engage one’s interests and to exercise one’s capacities Furthermore, almost no research exists to date on
and, in doing so, to seek out and master optimal the neuroscientific study of intrinsic motivation.
challenges (Deci & Ryan, 1985). When people are Intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motivation
intrinsically motivated, they act out of interest and interact with one another, and the tendency of
because they find the task at hand to be inherently highly salient extrinsic rewards to decrease intrinsic
enjoyable—producing spontaneous satisfactions motivation represents the “undermining effect” in
such as “It’s fun” and “Its interesting” during activ- the human motivation literature (Deci, Koestner, &
ity engagement. This behavior occurs spontaneously Ryan, 1999). To investigate this social psychologi-
and is not enacted for any instrumental (extrinsic) cal process within a neuroscience perspective, one
reasons. Intrinsic motivation is a concept that neu- group of researchers asked participants to engage
roscientists have not been able to explain (or under- themselves in an interesting task either with the
stand). What is known, however, is that during promise of a contingent extrinsic reward (money) or
greater insular cortical activity people become aware simply to experience the inherently interesting sense
of how the task they are engaged in is affecting their of challenge within the task itself (Murayama, Mat-
subjective feelings and they consolidate this feeling- sumoto, Izuma, & Matsumoto, 2010). By itself, the
state information with social-contextual information interesting, challenging task generated meaningful
about their task engagement (e.g., is there a deadline striatal and lateral prefrontal cortical activity, acti-
involved?) to form a global conscious experience of vations that confirmed that the challenging task

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


was inherently rewarding and cognitively engaging. a second case of rather high convergence between
When the same task was paired with the promise these two literatures.
of a contingent monetary reward, striatal and lat-
eral prefrontal cortical activity increased signifi- Expectancy
cantly, suggesting that the extrinsic reward added Expectancy is a central concept in the contem-
to the task-inherent intrinsic motivation. In a sec- porary study of human motivation; it serves as the
ond phase of the study, the extrinsic reward was core explanatory construct underlying motivations
removed. The researchers then examined how much such as personal control beliefs, mastery motiva-
striatal and lateral prefrontal cortical activity the tion, self-efficacy, and learned helplessness, among
task itself could generate. For participants in the others (Skinner, 1995, 1996). These “expectancy-of-
no-reward condition, striatal and lateral prefrontal control” constructs involve the interrelations among
cortical activity were essentially the same on the person, behavior, and outcome such that people have
second encounter with the activity—the task was expectancies of being able to generate effective cop-
just as rewarding and engaging as before. For par- ing behavior (e.g., efficacy expectations) and they
ticipants in the reward condition, however, striatal have expectancies of whether their coping behavior,
and lateral prefrontal cortical activity practically once enacted, will produce the outcome they seek
disappeared—the capacity of the once interesting (outcome expectations). In neuroscientific investi-
and challenging task to generate pleasure (striatum) gations of reward learning, however, expectancy is
and cognitive engagement (lateral prefrontal cortex) largely investigated as how expected a reward is.
had been undermined by the previously contingent This research, which takes place under the
extrinsic reward. This program of research nicely umbrella term of “reward prediction error” (Schultz,
shows how a complex human motivational concept 1998), shows that dopamine neurons are responsive
(intrinsic motivation) can be better understood by when a reward is received unexpectedly. When that
a neuroscience emphasis, and it therefore provides same reward is expected, based on prior experience,
an exemplary model for how future researchers the neurons respond not to reward receipt but to
might integrate neuroscientific methods and per- the informative nature of the predictive cue. Thus,
spectives within human motivation study. dopamine neurons are responsive to reward-related
novelty (Schott et al., 2004), the anticipation of
Flow cued reward (Schott et al., 2008), and the difference
Flow is a state of concentration that involves between expected reward and actual reward, which is
a holistic absorption and deep involvement in an the reward prediction error (Schultz, 1998). Overall,
activity (Csikszentmihalyi, 1990). It is a highly dopamine neurons throughout the basal ganglia—
pleasurable feeling that involves a sense of opti- dorsal striatum, ventral striatum, ventral tegmental
mal challenge and perceived competence, and it is area, and substantia nigra—report ongoing reward
characterized by a loss of time perspective in which prediction errors, and they do so by providing antic-
time passes relatively slowly. The anterior insular ipatory, unexpected, and actual signals of motiva-
cortex integrates feelings generated by homeostatic, tional relevance (i.e., reward cues). This information
environmental, hedonic, motivational, social, and is then passed on to target brain regions, including
cognitive inputs to produce a “global emotional the prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex,
moment,” which represents conscious awareness to coordinate reward-based learning and the moti-
of one’s feelings at one (present) moment in time vation to learn about goals. For instance, once this
(Craig, 2008, 2009). Under conditions of strong information is passed on to the anterior cingulate
emotion (joy, or flow from achieving competent cortex, approach versus avoidance decisional con-
functioning during a challenging task), the anterior flicts can be resolved based on expected probabilities
insular cortex produces a dilation of time in which of reward, payoff, and costs, just as this same infor-
many global emotional moments occur rapidly (Tse, mation can be passed on to the prefrontal cortex to
Intriligator, Rivest, & Cavanagh, 2004). Hence, guide goal setting and prioritizing.
subjective time dilates, as the actor subjectively feels The neuroscientific study of reward prediction
that little time has passed even when engagement errors is similar to the “outcome expectancy” concept
has continued for an objectively long(er) period of in the human motivation literature. Reward predic-
time. Like the study of the undermining effect of tion errors mostly serve the function of learning (not
rewards on intrinsic motivation, the human moti- of motivation per se), as dopamine neurons activate
vation and neuroscientific studies of flow represent with unexpected reward experiences to produce new

reeve, lee 


learning. However, these same dopamine-based their attention not to intermediate goal-directed steps
responses can be used to influence future choice but to the larger aim (as automation of skill occurs).
behavior (Schultz, Dayan, & Montague, 1997). Automation of procedural skills allows one to focus
That is, as people navigate their surroundings, they attention to environmental demands and challenges,
evaluate various courses of action that have differ- retrieve relevant episodic memories, and predict and
ential predictions of reward associated with them. plan effective future courses of action, while it further
These predictions of future rewards (outcomes) are lessens cognitive confusion and anxiety (Bandura,
influenced by past expected reward learning. Hence, 1988). The hippocampus is important to automation
dopamine responses provide information to enact of procedural knowledge, and the downregulation
the most basic expectancy-based motivational prin- of competent self-representations has been shown
ciple—namely, approach and engage in action cor- to lessen negative affect, affect intensity, and cortisol
related with increased dopamine activity and avoid reactivity during coping (Sapolsky, 1992).
action correlated with decreased dopamine activity. Perhaps the most productive way that human
Dopamine-based learning plays a key role in motivation research on self-efficacy can contribute to
reward expectation and receipt, which are closely interdisciplinary motivational neuroscience research is
related to outcome expectancies. But it also facili- to stress the point that neural systems that focus atten-
tates episodic memory formation that is used for tion, mentally represent value, detect the causal struc-
future adaptive behavior. That is, dopamine infor- ture of the world, and integrate this information into
mation during learning helps build and enable the effective decision making and action is only one part
forging of memory from one’s past experience that of the adaptive story (Bandura, 2001). The other part
then becomes the basis for future adaptive behav- of the adaptive story is self-efficacy-fueled agency in
ior (Shohamy & Adcock, 2010). It is this “adaptive which people proactively devise ways to adapt flexibly
memory” that then forms the basis of the second to a wide range of physical and social environments
major type of expectancy motivation studied in the to redesign them to their liking and controllability.
human motivation literature—namely, self-efficacy. Such a perspective places lesser influence on environ-
mentally responsive and adaptive brain processes and
Self-Efficacy relatively greater influence on proactive and agentic
Efficacy expectations are rooted in questions such brain processes in the exercise of personal control over
as “Can I cope well with the task at hand?” and “If environments to be encountered in the future.
things start to go wrong during my performance, do
I have the personal resources within me to cope well Self-Regulation and Goals
and turn things around for the better?” Self-efficacy Self-regulation is an ongoing, cyclical process
is the generative capacity in which the individual that involves forethought, action, and reflection
(the “self ” in self-efficacy) organizes and orches- (Zimmerman, 2000). Forethought involves goal set-
trates his or her skills in the pursuit of goal-directed ting and strategic planning, while reflection involves
action to cope with the demands and circumstances assessment and making adjustments to produce more
he or she faces. Formally defined, self-efficacy is informed forethought prior to the next performance
one’s judgment of how well (or poorly) one will opportunity. What is regulated during self-regulation
cope with a situation, given the skills one possesses are the person’s goals (and, to a lesser extent, the means
and the circumstances one faces (Bandura, 1997). to these goals, such as plans, strategies, emotions, and
The precuneus (embedded within the parietal lobe) environments). In the human motivation literature,
is involved in many of these processes, including goals are future-focused cognitive representations
self-related imagery, episodic memory retrieval, pre- that guide behavior to an end state that the individual
paring future action, and the experience of agency is committed to either approach or avoid (Hulleman,
(Cavanna & Trimble, 2006; den Ouden, Frith, Schrager, Bodmann, & Harackiewicz, 2010). It is
Frith, & Blakemore, 2005). the prefrontal cortex that houses a person’s conscious
The primary determinant of self-efficacy expecta- goals (Miller & Cohen, 2001), and this information
tions is one’s history of episodic memory-based mas- is used in goal-directed action in the top-down flow
tery enactments, which might be conceptualized by of information depicted in Figure 21.2.
neuroscientists as perceived skill in that domain. Stud- From a neuroscience point of view, several brain
ies of motor skill acquisition (Poldrack et al., 2005) structures exercise executive control and inhibition
and cognitive skill acquisition (Fincham & Anderson, over action. The prefrontal cortex contributes top-
2006) show that trained individuals come to direct down control that guides behavior by activating

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


internal representations of action such as goals and theoretical depth and complexity for the motiva-
intentions by sending information to other areas of tional constructs it studies. Motivational concepts
the brain to promote goal-relevant actions. While such as volition, agency, value, intrinsic motiva-
the prefrontal cortex generates goals and intentions, tion, self-efficacy, and self-regulation can be under-
executive control over action seems to be carried out stood more richly when neuroscientific analyses
in many additional prefrontal cortex regions, includ- are supplemented and informed by behavioral and
ing the ventral medial prefrontal cortex, the anterior psychological findings, methodologies, and espe-
cingulate cortex, and the dorsolateral prefrontal cor- cially theories. Once understood in their theoretical
tex, as each is involved in a high-level regulation of richness, these motivational constructs can be stud-
action, including self-control and the self-regulation ied in ways that increasingly map onto and reflect
of action such as planning, organizing, and chang- what is known about them from traditional human
ing action (Damasio, 1994, 2003; Oschsner & motivation study. Such integration, if it is to occur,
Gross, 2005; Rueda et al., 2004). The anterior cin- will likely be carried out by a generation of inter-
gulate cortex, for example, plays a high-level role in disciplinary motivation neuroscience researchers—
the regulation of action, as it not only receives infor- scholars whose interests, professional training, and
mation about sensory events, monitors conflict, and intellectual home is as much in neuroscience as it is
integrates emotional information (Botvinick et al., in human motivation study, and vice versa.
2004; Craig, 2008), it is active during any decision
to change one’s course of action (Devinsky, Mor-
Future Directions
rell, & Vogt, 1995) and is involved in adjusting past
learning about environmental contingencies when 1. Will the relationship between neuroscience
their reliability changes over time (Behrens, Wool- and human motivation become more reciprocal
rich, Walton, & Rushworth, 2007). These research and bidirectional in the future, or will it remain
findings suggest a possible convergence between largely a landscape of one-way—and even dead-
human motivation researchers and neuroscientists, end—streets? This trend will depend on human
as neuroscientists have done an especially impressive motivation researchers’ openness to neuroscience
job in explaining the neural bases of forethought, and to their willingness to form collaborations
decision making, and reflective action. and learn the methods and knowledge base of
neuroscience.
Conclusion 2. Is neuroscience relevant to only some classes
The intellectual landscape that connects human or facets of motivation—for example, homeostasis
motivation study and neuroscience is not currently and reward—or is it more generally relevant
populated by ever-present two-way information to more complex motivations such as intrinsic
highways in which the methodologies, findings, motivation and self-efficacy? This is a question of
and theoretical developments in one field flow into whether the motivation-neuroscience collaboration
the other and return back in a more informed and will be a narrow or a broad one.
sophisticated way. It is clear, however, that human 3. What are the benefits of maintaining the
motivation researchers have a lot to gain from such existing distinction between the two different
interconnectivity. To date, the most obvious benefit levels of analyses (neurological versus behavioral
for human motivation research has been that neuro- and self-report) embraced by neuroscience on
scientific investigations have brought to light the neu- the one hand and human motivation study on
ral meditational processes that underlie the how and the other? How well can the dependent measures
the why of the basic motivation mediation model: used in neuroscience (e.g., reaction times, neural
environment → motivation → adaptive action. That activations) align with the dependent measures
is, neuroscientific investigations have enriched the used in human motivation study (e.g., effort,
understanding of both the generation of motiva- phenomenology)? This future direction will
tional states (i.e., environment → neural activations likely be determined by the extent to which
→ motivation) and their adaptive functions (motiva- neural-dependent measures align (correlate) with
tion → neural activations → adaptive functioning). behavioral and self-report measures of motivation.
It is equally clear that neuroscience researchers 4. Can the brain generate motivation of its
have gained from greater motivation-neuroscience own? Or is brain-based motivation always an
interconnectivity. The most obvious benefit for adaptive response to environmental events?
neuroscience research has been to gain a greater Neuroscientific investigations of motivation have

reeve, lee 


revealed much about environmental sources Bar, M. (2007). The proactive brain: Using analogies and asso-
of motivation and reward. It is still an open ciations to generate predictions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
11, 280–289.
question, however, as to how much this Bargh, J. A., Gollwitzer, P. M., Lee-Chai, K., Barndollar, K., &
paradigm might reveal about intrinsic Trotschel, R. (2001). The automated will: Nonconscious
sources of motivation. activation and pursuit of behavioral goals. Journal of Person-
5. Lastly, the past decade of motivational ality and Social Psychology, 82, 1014–1027.
neuroscience has largely sought to identify the Baxter, M. G., & Murray, E. A. (2002). The amygdala and
reward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 563–573.
neural bases of various motivational states. Baxter, M. G., Parker, A., Lindner, C. C. C., Izquierdo, A. D.,
This has been and continues to be a productive & Murray, E. A. (2000). Control of response selection by
enterprise. As the neural bases of various reinforcer value requires interaction of amygdala and orbital
motivational states become well understood, frontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 4311–4319.
motivational neuroscience will need to ask new Bechara, A., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). The somatic marker
hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games and
questions and take on a new sense of purpose. Economic Behavior, 52, 336–372.
It is interesting to speculate what this future Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion,
direction will be, but it will like be one that decision-making, and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cor-
transcends description (e.g., the amygdala is tex, 10, 295–307.
involved in this, the anterior cingulate cortex is Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., Walton, M. E., & Rush-
worth, M. F. (2007). Learning the value of information in an
involved in that) to address explanation. uncertain world. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 1214–1221.
Berridge, K. C. (2004). Motivation concepts in behavioral neu-
Acknowledgments roscience. Physiology and Behavior, 81, 179–209.
This research was supported by the WCU (World Class Uni- Berridge, K. C., & Kringelbach, M. (2008). Affective neurosci-
versity) Program funded by the Korean Ministry of Education, ence and pleasure: Reward in humans and animals. Psychop-
Science and Technology, consigned to the Korea Science and harmacology, 191, 391–431.
Engineering Foundation (grant R32–2008–000-20023–0). Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Parsing reward.
Trends in Neurosciences, 26, 507–513.
Note Booth, D. (1991). Influences on human fluid consumption.
1. While dopamine is the key neurotransmitter involved In D. J. Ramsay & D. A. Booth (Eds.), Thirst: Physiologi-
in the processing of reward, other neurotransmitters also cal and psychological aspects (pp. 53–73). London: Springer
contribute to the processing of reward, including choline, Verlag.
GABA, glutamate, opiod, and serotonin (Knapp & Botvinick, M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict
Kornetsky, 2009). monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends
in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.
References Cavanna, A. E., & Trimble, M. R. (2006). The precuneus:
Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Marien, H. (2008). Preparing A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural corre-
and motivating behavior outside of awareness. Science, 319, lates. Brain, 129, 564–583.
1639. Cohen, J. D., & Aston-Jones, G. (2005). Cognitive neurosci-
Aarts, H., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2003). The silence of the library: ence: Decision amid uncertainty. Nature, 436, 471–472.
Environment, situational norm, and social behavior. Journal Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: The sense
of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 18–28. of the physiological condition of the body. Nature Reviews
Addis, D. R., Wong, A. T., & Schacter, D. L. (2007). Remember- Neuroscience, 3, 655–666.
ing the past and imaging the future: Common and distinct Craig, A. D. (2008). Interoception and emotion: A neuroana-
neural substrates during event construction and elaboration. tomical perspective. In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones,
Neuropsychologia, 45, 1363–1377. & L. F. Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed., pp.
Arana, F. S., Parkinson, J. A., Hinton, E., Holland, A. J., Owen, 272–288). New York: Guilford.
A. M., & Roberts, A. C. (2003). Dissociable contributions Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel—now? The anterior insula and
of the human amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex to incentive human awareness. Nature Review of Neuroscience, 10, 59–70.
motivation and goal selection. Journal of Neuroscience, 23, Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal
9632–9638. experience. New York: Harper & Row.
Ashby, F. G., Isen, A. M., & Turken, A. U. (1999). A neuropsy- Cunningham, W., & Zelazo, P. D. (2007). Attitudes and evalu-
chological theory of positive affect and its influence on cog- ation: A social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends in
nition. Psychological Review, 106, 529–550. Cognitive Sciences, 11, 97–104.
Bandura, A. (1988). Self-efficacy conception of anxiety. Anxiety Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2005). Positive affect as implicit moti-
Research, 1, 77–98. vator: On the nonconscious operation of behavioral goals.
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 129–142.
New York: W. H. Freeman. Custers, R., Aarts, H., Oikawa, M., & Elliot, A. (2009). The
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspec- nonconscious road to perceptions of performance: Achieve-
tive. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 1–26. ment priming augments outcome expectancies and experi-
Bandura, A. (2006). Toward a psychology of human agency. Per- enced self-agency. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
spectives on Psychological Science, 1, 164–180. 45, 1200–1208.

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Grosset/ Heckhausen, H. (1977). Achievement motivation and its con-
Putnam. structs: A cognitive model. Motivation and Emotion, 1,
Damasio, A. R. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and 283–329.
the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex. Philosophi- Hodgins, H. S., Yacko, H. A., & Gottlieb, E. (2006). Auton-
cal Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 35, omy and nondefensiveness. Motivation and Emotion, 30,
1413–1420. 283–293.
Damasio, A. R. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow and the Holland, R. W., Hendriks, M., & Aarts, H. (2005). Noncon-
feeling brain. New York: Harcourt. scious effects of scent on cognition and behavior. Psychologi-
Davidson, R. J., & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neuro- cal Science, 16, 689–693.
anatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Hulleman, C. S., Schrager, S. M., Bodmann, S. M., &
Science, 3, 11–21. Harackiewicz, J. M. (2010). A meta-analytic review of
Davidson, R. J., & Sutton, S. K. (1995). Affective neuroscience: achievement goal measures: Different labels for the same
The emergence of a discipline. Current Opinion in Neurobiol- constructs or different constructs with similar labels? Psycho-
ogy, 5, 217–224. logical Bulletin, 136, 422–449.
Daw, N. D., O’Doherty, J. P., Dayan, P., Seymour, B., & Dolan, Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social
R. J. (2006). Cortical substrates for exploratory decisions in behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental
humans. Nature, 441, 876–879. social psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 203–253). New York: Aca-
Decety, J., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2010). Handbook of social neurosci- demic press.
ence. New York: Oxford University Press. Isen, A. M. (2003). Positive affect as a source of human strength.
Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic In L. G. Aspinwall & U. Staudinger (Eds.), A psychology of
review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic human strengths (pp. 175–195). Washington, DC: American
rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, Psychological Association.
627–668. James, W. (1894). The physical basis of emotion. Psychological
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self- Review, 1, 516–529.
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum. Kay, A. C., Wheeler, S. C., Bargh, J. A., & Ross, L. (2004).
den Ouden, H. E. M., Frith, U., Frith, C., & Blakemore, Material priming: The influence of mundane objects on
S-J. (2005). Thinking about intentions. NeuroImage, 28, situational construal and competitive behavioral choice.
787–796. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 95,
Devinsky, O., Morrell, M. J., & Vogt, B. A. (1995). Contribu- 83–96.
tions of anterior cingulate cortex to behaviour. Brain, 118, Knapp, C. M., & Kornetsky, C. (2009). Neural basis of plea-
279–306. sure and reward. In G. G. Berntson & J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.),
Dickinson, A., & Balleine, B. (2002). The role of learning in Handbook of neuroscience for the behavioral sciences (Vol. 2,
the operation of motivational systems. In C. R. Gallistel pp. 781–806). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
(Ed.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Learning, Koelsch, S., Fritz, T., Cramon, D. Y. V., Muller, K., & Friederici,
motivation, and emotion (Vol. 3, pp. 497–534). New York: A. D. (2006). Investigating emotion with music: An fMRI
Wiley. study. Human Brain Mapping, 27, 239–250.
Engbert, K., Wohlschlager, A., & Haggard, P. (2008). Who is Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983).
causing what? The sense of agency is relational and efferent- Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of
triggered. Cognition, 107, 693–704. cerebral activity (readiness-potential). The unconscious ini-
Farrer, C., Franck, N., Georgieff, N., Frith, C. D., Decety, tiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 106, 623–642.
J., & Geannerod, M. (2003). Modulating the experience of Litman, J. (2005). Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: Want-
agency: A positron emission tomography study. NeuroImage, ing and liking new information. Cognition and Emotion, 19,
18, 324–333. 793–814.
Farrer, C., & Frith, C. D. (2002). Experiencing oneself vs. Loewenstein, G., Rick, S., & Cohen, J. D. (2008). Neuroeco-
another person as being the cause of an action: The neu- nomics. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 647–672.
ral correlates of the experience of agency. NeuroImage, 15, Mayer, R. E. (1998). Does the brain have a place in educational
596–603. psychology? Educational Psychology Review, 10, 389–396.
Fincham, J. M., & Anderson, J. R. (2006). Distinct roles of McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen,
the anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortext in the acquisi- J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems value immediate and
tion and performance of a cognitive skill. Proceedings of the delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306, 503–507.
National Academy of Sciences USA, 103, 12941–12946. McKinley, M. J. (2009). Thirst. In G. G. Berntson & J. T.
Freese, J. L., & Amaral, D. G. (2005). Neuroanatomy of the pri- Cacioppo (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience for the behavioral
mate amygdala. In R. J. Whalen & E. A. Phelps (Eds.), The sciences (Vol. 2, pp. 680–709). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
human amygdala (pp. 3–42). New York: Guilford Press. Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of
Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R., & Mangun, G. R. (2008). Cogni- prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience,
tive neuroscience: The biology of the mind (3rd ed.). New York: 24, 167–202.
W. W. Norton. Mogenson, G. J., Jones, D. L., & Yim, C. Y. (1980). From moti-
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1996). The volitional benefits of planning. vation to action: Functional interface between the limbic
In P. M. Gollwitzer & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology system and the motor system. Progress in Neurobiology, 14,
of action: Linking cognition and emotion to behavior (pp. 69–97.
287–312). New York: Guilford Press. Montague, P. R., & Berns, G. S. (2002). Neural econom-
Haggard, P. (2008). Human volition: Towards a neuroscience of ics and the biological substrates of valuation. Neuron, 36,
will. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 934–946. 265–284.

reeve, lee 


Murayama, K., Matsumoto, M., Izuma, K., & Matsumoto, K. Sapolsky, R. (1992). Stress, the aging brain and the mechanisms of
(2010). Neural basis of the undermining effect of monetary neuron death. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
reward on intrinsic motivation. Proceedings of the National Schoenbaum, G., Chiba, A. A., & Gallagher, M. (1999). Orbito-
Academy of the Sciences USA, 107, 20911–20916. frontal cortex and basolateral amygdala encode expected out-
Nachev, P. (2006). Cognition and medial frontal cortex in health comes during learning. Nature: Neuroscience, 1, 155–159.
and disease. Current Opinion in Neurology, 19, 586–592. Schott, B. H., Sellner, D. B., Lauer, C-J., Habib, R., Frey, J. U.,
Nachev, P., Rees, G., Praton, A., Kennard, C., & Husain, M. Guderian, S., . . . Duzel, E. (2004). Activation of midbrain
(2005). Volition and conflict in human medial frontal cor- structures by associative novelty and the formation of explicit
tex. Current Biology, 15, 122–128 memory in humans. Learning and Memory, 11, 383–387.
Oschsner, K. N., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The cognitive control of Schott, B. H., Minuzzi, L., Krebs, R. M., Elmenhorst, D.,
emotion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 242–249. Lang, M., Winz, O. H., . . . Bauer, A. (2008). Mesolimbic
Paas, F., Tuovinen, J. E., Tabbers, H., Van Gerven, P. W. M. functional magnetic resonance imaging activations dur-
(2003). Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance ing reward anticipation correlate with reward-related ven-
cognitive load theory. Educational Psychologist, 38, 63–71. tral striatal dopamine release. Journal of Neuroscience, 28,
Plassmann, H., O’Doherty, J., & Rangel, A. (2007). Orbitofron- 14311–14319.
tal cortex encodes willingness to pay in everyday economic Schultheiss, O. C. (2008). Implicit motives. In O. P. John,
transactions. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 9984–9988. R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personal-
Poldrack, R. A., Sabb, F. W., Foerde, K., Tom, S. M., Asarnow, R. F., ity: Theory and research (3rd ed., pp. 603–633). New York:
Bookheimer, S. Y., & Knowlton, B. J. (2005). The neural Guilford.
correlates of motor skill automaticity. Journal of Neuroscience, Schultz, W. (1998). Predictive reward signal of dopamine neu-
25, 5356–5364. rons. Journal of Neurophysiology, 80, 1–27.
Powley, T. L. (2009). Hunger. In G. G. Berntson & Schultz, W. (2000). Multiple reward signals in the brain. Nature
J. T. Cacioppo (Eds.), Handbook of neuroscience for the behav- Reviews: Neuroscience, 1, 199–207.
ioral sciences (Vol. 2, pp. 659–679). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural sub-
Reeve, J. (2009). Understanding motivation and emotion (5th strate of prediction and reward. Science, 275, 1593–1599.
ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. Shohamy, D., & Adcock, R. A. (2010). Dopamine and adaptive
Ridderinkhof, K. R., van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., Segalowitz, memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 14, 464–472.
S. J., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms Singer, T., Critchley, H. D., & Preuschoff, K. (2009). A common
of cognitive control: The role of prefrontal cortex in action role of insula in feelings, empathy, and uncertainty. Trends in
selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and Cognitive Sciences, 13, 334–340.
reward-based learning. Brain and Cognition, 56, 129–140. Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, and coping.
Robbins, T. W., & Everitt, B. J. (1996). Neurobehavioral mecha- Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
nisms of reward and motivation. Current Opinion in Neuro- Skinner, E. A. (1996). A guide to constructs of control. Journal of
biology, 6, 228–236. Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 549–570.
Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cerebral Spengler, S., von Cramon, D. Y., & Brass, M. (2009). Was it
Cortex, 10, 284–294. me or was it you? How the sense of agency originates from
Rueda, M. R., Posner, M. I., & Ruthbart, M. K. (2004). Atten- ideomotor learning revealed by fMRI. NeuroImage, 46,
tional control and self-regulation. In R. F. Baumeister & 290–298.
K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, Szpunar, K. K., Watson, J. M., & McDermott, K. B. (2007).
and applications (pp. 283–300). New York: Guilford Press. Neural substrates of envisioning the future. Proceedings of the
Rushworth, M. F., Behrens, T. E., Rudebeck, P. H., & Walton, National Academy of Sciences USA., 104, 642–647.
M. E. (2007). Contrasting roles for cingulate and orbitofron- Tse, P. U., Intriligator, J., Rivest, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2004).
tal cortex in decisions and social behaviour. Trends in Cogni- Attention and the subjective Expansion of time. Perceptual
tive Sciences, 4, 168–176. Psychophysics, 66, 1171–1189.
Ryan, R. M. (1993). Agency and organization: Intrinsic motiva- Whalen, P. J. (1999). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neu-
tion, autonomy, and the self in psychological development. roimagining studies of the human amygdala. Current Direc-
In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: tions in Psychological Science, 7, 177–187.
Developmental perspectives on motivation (Vol. 40, pp. 1–56). Whalen, P. J. (2007). The uncertainty of it all. Trends in Cognitive
Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Science, 11, 499–500.
Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of cau- Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J., Gallese, V., & Riz-
sality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in zolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The
two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron,
749–761. 40, 655–664.
Sabatinelli, D., Bradley, M. M., Lang, P. J., Costa, V. D., & Ver- Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (2002). The development of achievement
sace, F. (2007). Pleasure rather than salience activates human motivation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex. Journal of Wise, R. A. (2002). Brain reward circuitry: Insights from
Neurophysiology, 98, 1374–1379. unsensed incentives. Neuron, 36, 229–240.
Salin, P. A., & Bullier, J. (1995). Corticocortical connections Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature
in the visual system: Structure and function. Physiological Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 1–12.
Review, 75, 107–154. Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social
Saper, C. B., Chou, T. C., & Elmquist, J. K. (2002). The need cognitive perspective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, &
to feed: Homeostatic and hedonic control of eating. Neuron, M. Zeidner’s (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 13–39).
36, 199–211. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

 neuroscience and human motivatio n


CHAPTER

Evolved Individual Differences


22 in Human Motivation

Larry C. Bernard

Abstract
Three social science approaches—evolutionary psychology, behavioral ecology, and behavioral
genetics—share the metatheory of evolution. They also suggest several mechanisms that may account
for heritable individual differences in personality and motivation, including stabilizing selection,
fluctuating selection, trade-offs, balancing selection, life history theory, and behavioral syndromes.
These mechanisms are discussed as possible explanations for individual differences in the five-factor
model of personality and in a new theory of human motivation. The theory postulates that 15 latent
motive dimensions evolved in humans to facilitate behavior in five social domains. Trade-offs that, in
combination with fluctuating and balancing selection, might have maintained individual differences in
motive phenotypes are described. The reliability and validity of a method to assess individual differences
in the strength of these motive dimensions is also discussed.
Key Words: individual differences, motivation, personality, evolutionary psychology, behavioral ecology,
life history theory, behavioral genetics, trade-offs, balancing selection, behavioral syndromes

Introduction same level of outgoing behavior, they would not


Many evolutionary psychologists would con- describe each other as “introverted” or “extraverted”
sider the phrase “evolved individual differences” to and psychologists would not be interested in the
be an oxymoron because it would appear to violate assessment of Extraversion.
two important assumptions of evolutionary biology Personality psychologists accept dimensions like
and evolutionary psychology: (1) that advantageous Extraversion as important descriptors and predictors
traits spread and become species typical while any of behavior, and they are generally satisfied when a
heritable individual differences that arise in them personality dimension can be demonstrated to be
are eliminated, and (2) that individual differences relatively stable, expressed across situations, and a
that do exist may be no more than noise in the oper- valid predictor of behavior (Chamorro-Premuzic,
ation of psychological adaptations (Buss, 2009). In 2007; McAdams & Pals, 2007). But evolutionary
contrast, the psychology of individual differences psychologists would not be satisfied until at least
is a science of variation. Its primary research meth- two additional questions were answered: (1) Is a per-
ods, such as factor analysis and multiple regression, sonality dimension related to important outcomes
are correlational and depend on variance (Revelle, such as fitness? and (2) If so, what mechanisms can
2007). The identification and confirmation of a account for its evolution?
dimension, latent trait, or construct in personality, These questions are rarely asked by individual
require that people differ in it. If all people had the differences psychologists and the dependence of


personality psychology on variation has been a barrier in motivation. However, some of the first applica-
to its acceptance by evolutionary psychologists (Buss, tions of evolutionary theory to individual differences
2009). Selection pressures are thought to work in only involved personality constructs, specifically those
one direction, variance reduction, to the point where in the five-factor model (FFM; Costa & McCrae,
a trait emerges universally, and nearly identically, in 1992; Digman, 1996; Goldberg, 1993). Therefore,
all members of a species (Tooby & Cosmides, 1990, this chapter will begin with the FFM, because it laid
1992). The primary challenge for individual differ- the groundwork for development of an evolutionary
ences psychologists, then, is to conceptualize mecha- approach to individual differences in motivation.
nisms by which selection can maintain variance in a
functional dimension of behavior and put them to The Five-Factor Model of Personality
the test. Recent developments in behavioral ecology, Background
behavioral genetics, evolutionary psychology, and com- The FFM is one of the most widely studied person-
parative psychology have led to more willingness to ality models (e.g., Costa & McCrae, 1992; Digman,
explore the possibility of evolved individual differences 1996; Goldberg, 1993). It is a taxonomic model of
in personality. These developments include life history personality that emerged from factor analyses of
theory (MacArthur & Wilson, 1967; Reznick, Bryant, the English lexicon of adjectives describing human
& Bashey, 2002; Rushton, 2004; Stearns, 1992), bal- behavior. It is based on the assumption that terms
ancing selection (Penke, Denissen, & Miller, 2007), used to describe the most salient and socially relevant
fluctuating selection and trade-offs (Nettle, 2007), individual differences in behavior will become part
and behavioral syndrome theory (Roff, 2001; Sih, of common language usage. Hundreds of such terms
Bell, & Johnson, 2004a, 2004b) all of which suggest were factor analyzed until five meaningful dimen-
mechanisms by which heritable individual differences sions emerged: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness,
may evolve. Buss (1991) was one of the first evolu- Conscientiousness, and Agreeableness. Although the
tionary psychologists to recognize these developments FFM does have its limitations, it will probably con-
and suggest the importance of their implications for tinue to be the most popular model for personality
personality, as well as evolutionary, psychology. These assessment in the foreseeable future (Merenda, 1999,
developments may one day turn “evolved individual 2008). Meta-analyses of the FFM and both work per-
differences” into a congruous term. formance (Barrick & Mount, 1991; Hough, Eaton,
Evolutionary theory has played an important role Dunnette, Kamp, & McCloy, 1990) and academic
in unifying the biological sciences, and it may play performance (Poropat, 2009) support its relationship
a similar role in the social sciences (de Waal, 2002). with important behavioral outcomes. An emerging
Evolutionary theory has great explanatory power, body of literature has proposed an evolutionary
particularly when it addresses the interaction of brain basis to, and the heritability of, the FFM (e.g., Jang,
and social (cultural) development, and may ulti- McCrae, Angleitner, Riemann, & Livesley, 1998;
mately help in understanding the origin of individual Reimann, Angleitner, & Strelau, 1997). Research
differences in personality and motivation. Evolution- has found that between 40% and 50% of variance
ary psychology emphasizes the function of evolved in the FFM dimensions is due to genetic influences
solutions to adaptive problems, the recognition that (Plomin, Happe, & Caspi, 2002; see Nettle, 2006,
selective processes are involved in the development of for a review of the heritability of the FFM). This
the brain, and the expectation that there should be a has led to several proposed mechanisms that may
large number of domain-specific psychological mecha- be relevant to the evolution of individual differences
nisms (Buss & Greiling, 1999; Ermer, Cosmides, & dimensions in the FFM and a description of them
Tooby, 2007). (For a discussion of current issues in follows.
evolutionary psychology, see Confer et al., 2010.)
This last emphasis is consistent with personality psy- Proposed Mechanisms for Evolution of the
chology’s interest in multiple latent factors that may Five-Factor Model Dimensions
be responsible for individual differences in observed stabilizing selection
behavior. And personality psychologists are begin- Wilson (1994) was one of the first evolution-
ning to recognize the heuristic value of evolutionary ary psychologists to propose that phenotypic dif-
psychology’s adaptationist approach in their work ferences between individuals could be the result of
(Penke et al., 2007). underlying genotypic polymorphisms, rather than
This chapter presents a recent application of evo- primarily the result of proximal causes (i.e., pheno-
lutionary theory to the study of individual differences typic plasticity). MacDonald (1995, 1998) shared

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


this view and addressed the key assumption of evo- there is no trade-off, and natural selection would be
lutionary biology—that individual differences in directional toward the higher value of the trait. If
adaptations should be eliminated by selection—by selection were directional, then it would decrease
proposing that a personality dimension represents individual differences over time. The effect of trade-
a continuous distribution of phenotypes. The phe- offs over time should be more variance in levels
notypes represent different behavioral strategies for of a trait. This consideration of trade-offs has also
negotiating a competitive social environment, and been called the “optimality approach” (Kaplan &
“stabilizing selection” is selection against extremes Gangestad, 2007). This approach takes into account
of the distribution (MacDonald, 2005). Stabilizing that individuals live with finite resource “budgets,”
selection is based partly on Hogan’s (1996) pro- which must be allocated among various behavioral
posal that the environment of evolutionary adapt- strategies. Because a finite budget necessitates trade-
edness (EEA; see Hogan, 2005, for a discussion of offs, there should be no optimal solutions to adap-
this term) of Homo sapiens was comprised of hierar- tive problems.
chical living groups wherein competition for social Fluctuating selection and trade-offs are not entirely
status could result in reproductive success or failure. incompatible with the key assumption of evolution-
Hogan contended that the FFM provided an indi- ary psychology—that individual differences are mere
cation of an individual’s “reputation,” of how well noise in the operation of psychological adaptations
that individual “ . . . is doing in the game of life . . . as (Tooby & Cosmides, 1990, 1992). However, they
it concerns reproductive success” (1996, p. 173). suggest a different conclusion about how variation
Phenotypic behavioral strategies that fall in the can persist in the face of fitness-relevant selection pro-
tails of a normally distributed personality dimen- cesses (Nettle, 2006). First, genetic influences should
sion might represent higher risk evolutionary strate- be thought of as constraining, rather than deter-
gies that could result in reduced fitness outcomes. mining, traits (Kagan, 2003). Second, mutation, by
In fact, extremes on FFM dimensions are associated altering the constraining influence of a trait, is the
with psychopathology (Costa & Widiger, 1994). In starting point of variation. If a trait is the result of a
stabilizing selection, at different times in evolution- single gene, selection pressures will keep variations
ary history, phenotypes at different points on a per- close to zero because mutations occur infrequently
sonality dimension would be favored by selection, and there is time for selection to operate. Single gene
leading to niches for risk takers and risk avoiders mutations account for rare serious disorders such as
alike. Over time, in the aggregate, selection against severe mental retardation and early-onset Alzheim-
the extremes could provide a moderating influence, er’s disease (Plomin, DeFries, Craig, & McGuffin,
with the majority—but not all—individuals ending 2003). However, because the probability of muta-
up in the middle of the distribution of phenotypes. tions rises with the number of genes involved in a
trait, a polygenic trait has ample room for variance.
fluctuating selection and trade-offs Additionally, selection does not remove the muta-
There might be fitness disadvantages to pheno- tions expediently, so there remains some variation in
types at the extremes of normal personality dimen- a trait even in the face of strong selection pressures
sions. However, any such disadvantages may not be (Houle, 1998). Polygentic traits—quantitative trait
stable, as proposed by stabilizing selection; rather, loci (QTL)—are more likely to result in quantita-
the effects of selection pressures themselves may fluc- tive distributions (such as those found in dimensions
tuate to sometimes increase a trait and sometimes of personality) than they are to result in qualitative
decrease it. Therefore, stabilizing selection may not dichotomies such as those that underlie severe mal-
have made adequate use of the concept of trade-offs adaptive or psychopathological disorders (Plomin
to explain variance, particularly in the middle range et al., 2003).
of a personality dimension, or to take into account The personality dimensions that may arise from
possible disadvantages and advantages of phenotypes QTLs would not simply increase or decrease fitness.
in the extremes of a distribution (Nettle, 2006). Instead, overall fitness is a trade-off that balances the
When two levels along the continuum of a advantages and disadvantages of occupying a particu-
particular trait produce a relatively equal fitness lar position in the distribution of a dimension. There
outcome, if a different level of the trait increases should be fitness disadvantages associated with being
a component of fitness, then it must also decrease in the tails of a personality dimension, but “The
other components of fitness. The benefit produced retention of a normal distribution is a consequence of
by increasing a trait must come at a cost, otherwise the inconsistency of the direction of selection, not its

b er n a rd 
stabilizing form” (Nettle, 2006, p. 628, italics added). on a continuum, could result from social environ-
Observations of the size to which male pygmy sword- mental variation across time and space.
tail, Xiphophorous nigrensis, grow and its function in Both of these variations are consistent with fluc-
reproductive fitness demonstrate this point (Nettle, tuating selection/trade-offs, but balancing selection
2006). Large males are preferred by females, engage has led to a different emphasis in research with the
in elaborate courtship displays, and take 27 weeks FFM (Penke et al., 2007). In particular, personal-
to reach maturity at great cost in time and meta- ity dimensions have been conceptualized in terms
bolic investment. In contrast, small males take only of reaction range, which might help account for the
14 weeks to mature and do not engage in the same dual role of the environment as a source of pheno-
courtship displays; therefore, more time and energy typic plasticity and fluctuating selection pressures.
are available to “sneak” copulation with females Reaction range incorporates the idea of genes setting
(Zimmer & Kallman, 1989). Each variation in size constraints that restrict outcomes along a personal-
has its fitness advantages and disadvantages and dif- ity dimension, rather than determining outcomes.
ferent sizes can be maintained through selection. Reaction range emphasizes the Person x Situation
Trade-offs are also applicable to the FFM (Nettle, interaction in assessment. To illustrate this point,
2006). For example, people who score higher on Penke et al. (2007) reasoned that “. . . some people
Extraversion have more sexual partners and greater may be socially confident at informal parties but not
social support (Buchanan, Johnson, & Goldberg, at public speaking, whereas for others, the opposite
2005) but also engage in more risky behavior and may apply. To class them both as ‘extraverts’ may
are more likely to be hospitalized (Nettle, 2005) and conflate disparate genotypes that lead to distinct
arrested (Samuels et al., 2004). By implication, there endophenotypes, behavioural strategies, reaction
should also be fitness trade-offs associated with low norms, and fitness payoffs” (p. 574). The applicabil-
extraversion scores, for example, people who score ity of this approach has been demonstrated with the
lower may be exposed to less risk but would also have FFM dimensions (Denissen & Penke, 2008). The
less opportunity to find mates of higher quality. dimensions were reconceptualized as motivational
Both MacDonald’s and Nettle’s views of the evo- systems, and questionnaire items were developed to
lutionary basis for personality dimensions have had specifically tap reaction norms in the form of Person
to rely on post hoc analysis. Nettle (2006) acknowl- x Situation interactions. The results indicated that,
edged potential criticisms of trade-off theory as spec- while reaction norm-based items were different in
ulative and anticipated problems with the post hoc content from those in other FFM measures, this did
nature of his analysis of trade-offs in maintaining not negatively affect the factor structure or predic-
human personality variation. Many explanations of tive validity of the FFM dimensions.
personality variation are open to such criticisms, and
Nettle attempted to address them by making several comparative psychology
specific testable predictions based on the theory. Animal models have largely been missing from
personality psychology, which could partially account
balancing selection for our poor understanding of the evolutionary basis
In balancing selection, selective forces are bal- of human personality (Figueredo et al., 2005). In
anced for either extreme of a trait and both extremes contrast, animal models play a very important role
could be favored by selection to the same extent but in behavioral ecology, which focuses on the behav-
under different conditions (Penke et al., 2007). Sev- ioral roles that enable a species to adapt to its envi-
eral variations of balancing selection are likely can- ronmental niche (Burkhardt, 2005; Krebs & Davies,
didates for maintaining personality variation (Penke 1997; Smith, 2000). Fairly compelling evidence has
et al., 2007). One is environmental heterogeneity, been developed for evolved personality differences in
which relates more to the physical environment and nonhuman species (Gosling, 2001; Gosling & Vazire,
occurs when a trait’s fitness effect varies across space 2002), for example, shyness-boldness in wolves
and time. When selection pressures vary spatially or (MacDonald, 1983) and sunfish (Wilson, 1994). In
temporally, a trait’s fitness effect would be nearly neu- particular, three FFM dimensions—Extraversion,
tral when averaged across them. Another variation is Agreeableness, and Neuroticism—have been found
negative frequency-dependent selection, and it is a in diverse species, such as primates, guppies, and
special case of environmental heterogeneity in which octopuses (Gosling & John, 1999). Evolutionary
the spatial and temporal variations occur in the social biologists and ecologists have also found evidence of
environment. Different phenotypic personalities, some non-FFM dimensions used to describe human

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


personality in birds and fish (Coleman & Wilson, unstable environment with a high risk of mortality
1998; Groothuis & Carere, 2005; Wilson, Clark, and varying availability of resources. Many species
Coleman, & Dearstyne, 1994). can be identified as K or r strategists. Populations
As a result of such findings, Wolf, van Doorn, of K-selected species, such as humans, tend to be
Leimar, and Weissing (2007) suggested that the near the density/resource capacity of their environ-
“phenomenon of animal personalities is one of ments, while populations of r-selected species, such
the most intriguing challenges to the adaptation- as rabbits, may exceed the density/resource capacity
ist programme in behavioural research” (p. 581). of their environments from time to time.
This challenge goes directly to the key assumption With subsequent modifications (e.g., Stearns,
of evolutionary psychology—that individual differ- 1992), life history theory has received empirical sup-
ences may be no more than noise in the operation port and become influential in behavioral ecology
of psychological adaptations—because the finding (Reznick et al., 2002; Rushton, 2004). Life history
of some of the same individual differences dimen- theory is relevant to the present discussion because
sions in humans and other species suggests that the it has been used to predict within-species variation
dimensions may have evolved and are not the result among r- and K-selected species (Figueredo et al.,
of proximal sources of variation alone. 2005; Figueredo, Vásquez, Brumbach, & Schneider,
To demonstrate that individual differences in 2007). Adaptive individual within-group differences
animal personalities could have evolved, Wolf et al. in life history strategies have been found in several
(2007) conducted a simulation study based on the species in addition to humans (Rowe, 2000).
prediction that current and future reproduction Figueredo et al. (2005) hypothesized that
trade-offs could result in polymorphic populations K-selection theory could lend itself to a latent vari-
of individuals, some of whom would emphasize able, individual differences model comprised of a
future fitness returns more than others. Those whose single factor (K) that underlies various life history
expectation is for future fitness should engage in less variables such as sexual, reproductive, parental, and
risk-taking behavior, operationalized in personality social behavior, reflective of a K-selected species.
terms as less “aggressiveness” and “boldness.” Wolf They provided empirical evidence of a single com-
et al.’s mathematical simulation model showed mon factor, identified as “K,” that was consistent
that, over 1,000 generations, stable individual dif- with life history theory and weakly correlated with at
ferences traits of aggressiveness and boldness could least one FFM dimension, Neuroticism. Figueredo
evolve under conditions in which predation risk and et al. (2007) also identified three common factors:
resource quality were varied. Their model was lim- (1) “K,” which was related to life history theory and
ited to these risk-taking dimensions of personality reflected personal, familial, and social functioning
and was not intended to explain other dimensions consistent with a K-selected species; (2) “Covital-
such as “cooperativeness.” However, it does suggest ity,” which reflected well-being, negative and posi-
that selection pressures can give rise to stable indi- tive affect, general health, and medical symptoms;
vidual differences. and (3) “Personality,” which reflected FFM dimen-
sions. These factors were all moderately intercorre-
life history theory: k- and lated, suggesting relationships between K, health/
r-selected species well-being, and personality.
Life history theory originated with the work
of MacArthur and Wilson (1967) to explain how behavioral syndromes
selection pressures may operate to produce variation The concept of behavioral syndromes developed
in the life histories of species. The original notion in behavioral ecology and has not been applied spe-
was that different strategies may ultimately produce cifically to the FFM. “Behavioral syndromes” refers to
the same fitness outcome depending on population “suites” of correlated characteristics or behaviors that
density and resource availability. Two particular life occur across situations (Roff, 2001). Cross-situational
history strategies came to be known as “K” and “r” manifestation of behavior—its consistency—is an
(Pianka, 1970). K-selected species utilize a low- essential assumption for individual differences in
fecundity, high-survivorship strategy that is pre- human personality dimensions (Pervin & John,
sumed to have evolved in a stable environment with 1999) and may be observed in diverse animal species
a low risk of premature death. In contrast, r-selected (Sih et al., 2004a, 2004b). Until recently, behavioral
species utilize a high-fecundity, low-survivorship ecologists had been studying animal behavior indi-
strategy that is presumed to have evolved in an vidually, but behavioral syndrome theory suggests

b er n a rd 
that behaviors should be studied “as a package,” with first-order factors (i.e., with a narrower range or band-
a species exhibiting a “behavioral syndrome” and width of behavioral influence) and the FFM dimen-
individuals manifesting different types (e.g., bold or sions are conceived as second-order factors (i.e., with
shy) (Sih et al., 2004a, 2004b). a broad range or bandwidth of behavioral influence).
Behavioral syndromes are conceived as heritable This can complicate interpretation because statisti-
styles of responding that are maintained by trade-offs. cally significant relationships may be observed with
In some contexts, a behavioral syndrome could result the five broad dimensions that are due to differences
in maladaptive behavior but be maintained because within only one or two of the narrow facets that com-
of fluctuating environmental selection. There may prise them (Ones & Viswesvaran, 1996; Tett, Steele,
be environments in which no particular phenotype & Beauregard, 2003). Bandwidth would be an issue
is optimum, for example, situations in which high when trying to understand which dimension or
and low Extraversion may be equally adaptive. Thus, dimensions are heritable. Is it Extraversion or one or
variation could persist due to multiple optima in a more of its facets—warmth, gregariousness, assertive-
single environment. This is in contrast to behavioral ness, activity, excitement-seeking, and positive emo-
ecology’s tendency to ignore variation and evolution- tions—that is heritable? And it is conceivable that
ary psychology’s concept of it as noise. there are different selection pressures and trade-offs
Behavioral syndrome theory has generated much for each of these facets. Narrow bandwidth dimen-
research, most of it in nonhuman species. Intrigu- sions are probably best to control potentially con-
ingly, again some of the behavioral syndromes that founding factors and also fit better with evolutionary
have been identified in nonhumans are similar to psychology’s emphasis on multiple, independent,
human personality traits. For example, evidence has domain-specific, rather than domain-general, evolved
been found for a boldness-aggression syndrome in mental mechanisms (Cosmides & Tooby, 1992).
zebrafish (Danio rerio) (Moretz, Martins, & Robi-
son, 2007), an exploratory syndrome in the lizard Evolved Individual Differences
(Eulamprus heatwolei) (Stapley & Keogh, 2005), and in Motivation
boldness, aggression, and risk-avoidance syndromes Background
in farmed and wild fish (Huntingford & Adams, Research with the FFM suggests that at least
2005). The identification of a bold syndrome in the some heritable personality dimensions may be forms
fishing spider (D. triton) is particularly noteworthy, of strategic individual differences shaped by envi-
because an individual spider’s boldness was consis- ronmentally fluctuating, but ultimately balancing,
tent across situations and boldness in one context selection forces and trade-offs. It may be possible to
could be predicted from boldness in other contexts extend this reasoning to dimensions of motivation.
(Johnson & Sih, 2007). From the beginnings of modern psychology—from
James (1890), Freud (1955), McDougall (1933), to
summary Murray (1938)—motivation has been an integral
The widespread use of the FFM, and its rela- part of personality psychology (Fiske, 2008). How-
tionships with important social outcomes, prob- ever, there are some important distinctions between
ably accounts for its central role in most of these personality and motivational psychology. Personality
proposed mechanisms to explain evolved individ- psychology focuses more on temperament, traits, and
ual differences. However, the task of providing an types. Motivational psychology focuses more on the
evolutionary rationale for personality dimensions dynamics of action, on instincts, drives, needs, values,
may have been limited by the fact that the FFM incentives, and goals (McAdams & Pals, 2007). Per-
was developed empirically and is atheoretical. In sonality psychologists attempt to answer the question:
addition, at its inception, no claim was made, nor “What is the structure of individuality?” Motivational
rationale provided, for an evolutionary basis to its psychologists attempt to answer the question: “What
dimensions; the heritability of its dimensions was energizes and directs people’s behavior?” Therefore,
investigated only after the fact. personality and motivational constructs are concep-
Another problem with the use of the FFM dimen- tually different. Traits are, by definition, enduring
sions is their relatively broad bandwidth. The five characteristics. Is it possible there can be enduring
dimensions are general personality domains. In some dynamics of action; can there be individual differ-
measures of the FFM, such as the NEO PI-R (Costa ences dimensions—traits—in motivation?
& McCrae, 1992), the dimensions are comprised From a recent categorization of 75 years of mea-
of facets or subscales. The facets are conceived as sures used in motivation research it appears that many

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


researchers would answer “yes;” dozens of instru- with mainstream evolutionary psychology, behav-
ments, many of them trait measures, have been used ioral ecology, and behavioral genetics—the three
in hundreds of studies (Mayer, Faber, & Xu, 2007). social science approaches that share the metatheory
While many of these instruments are unidimen- of evolution (Smith, 2000). Buss’s (2009) rationale
sional (e.g., The Need for Uncertainty; Sorrentino, and the promising work of those who proposed evo-
Holmes, Hanna, & Sharp, 1995), many are multi- lutionary mechanisms for FFM dimensions both
dimensional. One multidimensional instrument is figured prominently in the development of this the-
R. B. Cattell and his colleagues’ Motivation Analy- ory. The development of this theory is ongoing and
sis Test (MAT; Cattell, Horn, Sweney, & Radcliffe, has followed a logical, step-by-step procedure.
1964), which is comprised of scales to assess these The first step was to describe the EEA in which
dimensions: Career, Home-Parental, Fear, Narcism human motivational dimensions may have evolved.
(sic), Superego, Self-sentiment, Mating, Pugnacity, Bugental (2000) and Kenrick, Li, and Butner (2003)
Assertiveness, and Sweetheart-Spouse. The MAT had suggested that five or six overlapping social
incorporated innovations not widely used then or domains existed in the EEA of humans. Each of
now. Unfortunately, one of these, ipsative scoring, these domains is related to a different sized group.
is psychometrically problematic and the MAT has Evidence had suggested that the size of the human
not been revised since it was introduced (Bernard, neocortex and the size of social groups in which
Walsh, & Mills, 2005). humans interact had coevolved (Dunbar, 2007).
Another multidimensional measure of individual Bernard et al. (2005) took this into account in orga-
differences in motivation is the Personality Research nizing the domains hierarchically by size: Individual,
Form (PRF; Jackson, 1999), which is based on Dyad, Small Group, Large Group, and Very Large
Murray’s (1938) concept of needs. The PRF Form Group.
A/B is comprised of scales to assess these dimensions: The second step was to propose recurring fitness
Achievement, Affiliation, Aggression, Autonomy, challenges that may have been present in each social
Dominance, Endurance, Exhibition, Harmavoid- domain in the EEA of humans. Kenrick et al.’s
ance, Impulsivity, Nurturance, Order, Play, Social (2003) suggestions formed a basis for this. We iden-
Recognition, and Understanding. tified seven fitness challenges. The social domains,
Both the MAT and PRF are comprised of rela- their sizes, constituents, and fitness challenges are
tively independent, narrow bandwidth scales assess- listed in Table 22.1. Three social groups, similar to
ing purported dimensions of motivation. However, the first three listed in Table 22.1, have also been
while Cattell did propose a genetic basis to person- identified in the social ecology of other primates:
ality and motivational dimensions (e.g., Cattell & Solitary, Pairs, and Group-Living (Kappeler & van
Dreger, 1977; Cattell, Radcliffe, & Sweney, 1963), Schaik, 2002). The primate Group-Living domain
neither MAT nor PRF scales were based on evolu- was operationalized as at least three adults of both
tionary theory. In 75 years of motivation research sexes and the upper limit size for gorillas in this
only two instruments were categorized as biologi- domain is probably 20 members (Harcourt & Stew-
cally based, and none were identified as evolution- art, 2007), which places the primate Group-Living
ary based (Mayer et al., 2007). domain between the human Small Group and Large
Group domains in terms of size. The larger social
An Individual Differences Theory of domains that humans negotiate probably necessi-
Human Motivation tated the evolution of additional motive dimensions
Buss (1991) proposed a rationale for joining evo- and larger and more complex brains.
lutionary, motivational, and personality psychology. The third step was to identify the behavioral syn-
My colleagues and I were influenced by Buss’s pro- dromes that might have addressed each of the fitness
posal, as well as Cattell’s attempt to assess motiva- challenges. A behavioral syndrome represents cor-
tional traits multidimensionally, when we developed related strategies for overcoming a fitness challenge,
a new approach to the study of individual differences and a brain that is prepared to facilitate certain strat-
in human motivation based on evolution (Bernard, egies may have a fitness advantage (Sih et al., 2004a,
Mills, Swenson, & Walsh, 2005). We could have 2004b). Each syndrome was hypothesized to be a
undertaken a post hoc analysis of the MAT or PRF dimension that varies in strength (i.e., the amount of
dimensions from an evolutionary perspective, like resources in the form of time, effort, energy, money,
what had been done with the FFM, but opted for etc.) an individual expends on it. Under some envi-
a theory-based approach that would be compatible ronmental conditions, maximum strength would

b er n a rd 
Table 22.1. Human Social Domains, Size, Constituents, and Recurring Fitness Challenges
Domain Size Constituents Recurring Fitness Challenge

Individual 1 Single individual (1) How to protect oneself


(2) How to identify environmental and
interpersonal resources and hazards

Dyadic 2 Pairs of intrasexual and inter- (3) How to compete for mates
sexual individuals (4) How to develop social status and
mating desirability

Small Group 2–20 Mate and kin (5) How to establish cooperative relationships
for mating and support of kin

Large Group 20–150 Coalitions of mostly nonkin (6) How to develop individual-to-individual
(neighborhoods, towns, reciprocation among nonkin
social clubs)

Very Large Group 1,000+ Coalitions of overwhelmingly (7) How to develop group-to-individual
nonkin (political parties, reciprocation among nonkin (“institutionalized
religions, ethnic groups, reciprocity”)
nations)

Note: Once a successful adaptation to a fitness challenge in a domain has developed, it may be successful in meeting challenges in other
domains as well.

produce better fitness, in others, minimal strength, The fourth step was to propose a rationale for the
and in most, a moderate level of strength. An indi- development and heritability of these individual dif-
vidual’s strength level in a dimension is phenotype, ferences dimensions. Balancing/fluctuating selection
partly influenced by the genotype of a brain pre- (Penke et al., 2007) and trade-offs (Nettle, 2006)
pared to set a certain level (reaction range) for the provide the basis for the theory. When environmen-
latent trait that produces the behavioral syndrome tal conditions fluctuate such that different levels of a
and partly influenced by local environmental cir- trait are favored at different times, genetic variation
cumstances, including threats, resource availability, in the trait can be maintained. An example of this
cultural expectations, roles, learning, and cues. The is depicted in Figure 22.1 . As Figure 22.1 suggests,
dimensions associated with each phenotypic behav- polygenic variations could result in structural and
ioral syndrome are considered latent variables— functional differences in brain development. Geno-
called “motives” for short—and each was identified typic differences in brain development could result in
by a label for convenience. different reaction ranges that restrict potential phe-
Research has suggested that adapted mental notypic outcomes along a motive dimension. Dif-
mechanisms are not domain general, but rather ferent phenotypes produce different levels of strength
evolved independently to accomplish domain- in a latent predisposition—a motive—to engage in
specific tasks (e.g., Cosmides & Tooby, 1992; Gig- a particular behavioral syndrome at a certain level of
erenzer & Hug, 1992). Each motive was assumed to activity. Different levels of strength in a behavioral
have developed independently and to be mediated syndrome have different trade-offs. Higher strength
by a relatively independent, domain-specific mental generally requires more resources (e.g., time, effort,
mechanism. The brain contains many such mental energy, wealth), while lower strength generally con-
mechanisms, each one of which evolved to facilitate serves resources. Any level of strength of a behav-
behavior that increased fitness in the EEA (Hagen ioral syndrome could increase fitness, depending on
& Symons, 2007; see Bernard et al., 2005, for a environmental conditions and the return on invest-
discussion of the neuropsychological structures that ment of resources (trade-offs) in terms of survival
may underlie various motive mental mechanisms). and reproductive/inclusive fitness.
The motives, operational definitions, and behavioral Various levels of a motive’s strength would
syndromes are listed in Table 22.2. be favored by selection to some extent but under

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


Table 22.2. Motives, Operational Definitions, and Behavioral Syndromes
Social Motive Operational Definition Behavioral Syndrome Examples
Domain

Individual Environmental Use resources to explore the Investigating the physical environment and
Inquisitiveness physical environment; evaluate the unknown; investigating places; finding
resource availability and hazards in out how things work; trying out resources
new and different things, places,
and situations

Illness Use resources to maintain bodily Avoiding unhealthy or toxic substances;


Avoidance integrity and health maintaining physical integrity; maintaining
cleanliness of one’s person and surround-
ings; having health checkups; maintaining
a healthy diet

Threat Use resources to maintain the safety Taking precautionary actions; avoiding
Avoidance of one’s person; avoid challenges to potential conflicts and threats; securing
one’s person and resources one’s person and property; pacifying others

Dyad Aggression Use resources to acquire and control Threatening or assaulting others; striving
additional resources; challenge for leadership; engaging in challenging and
and intimidate others for control dangerous activities that signal fearlessness
of resources; approach challenges and dominance
to one’s person and resources with
combative and intimidating displays
and actions

Interpersonal Use resources to explore the social Investigating the social environment;
Inquisitiveness environment; test limits, traditions, jesting, mocking, playful engagement with
and how others act, react, and inter- others; game playing; using humor and
act; compare oneself to others sarcasm; testing interpersonal limits

Appearance1 Use resources to compete for status Grooming and adornment; attempting to
on the basis of physical appearance increases one’s attractiveness comparatively
and competitively (tanning, cosmetic
surgery, makeup, fragrances, jewelry)

Mental1 Use resources to compete for status Increasing knowledge; exploiting talent;
on the basis of intellectual capacities developing creative or artistic abilities;
and knowledge, as well as skills, participating in competitions that signal
abilities, and talents (gender-appropriate) intellectual ascen-
dancy (spelling bees, chess and card
tournaments, dance and music
competitions)

Physical1 Use resources to compete for status Increasing bodily strength and endurance;
on the basis of physical strength, participating in competitions that signal
endurance, size, shape, and stature (gender-appropriate) physical ascendancy;
altering and enhancing bodily shape to
culturally appropriate norms

Wealth1 Use resources to compete for status Obtaining wealth and material possessions;
on the basis of acquiring material signaling ascendancy on the basis of access
resources to and control of resources

Sex Seek sexual activity Pursuing sexual partners and sexual variety;
engaging in frequent sexual activity

(Continued)


Table 22.2. Continued
Social Motive Operational Definition Behavioral Syndrome Examples
Domain

Small Commitment2 Transfer of resources to mates and Giving mates and offspring
Group offspring; development of tender, resources and tangible signs of
intimate, supportive attachments affection and emotional support;
with others fidelity (e.g., refraining from sharing
resources with others); signaling a
desire for intimacy

Large Altruism2 Transfer of resources to Using resources to protect kin and


Group kin without expectation of help kin succeed
immediate self-benefit (but at
a cost to oneself that is generally
lower than the benefits to others
multiplied by their degree of
genetic relatedness)

Social Enter into reciprocal, Demonstrating reliability and trustworthi-


Exchange2 mutually beneficial exchanges ness; not taking advantage of others; not
of resources with nonkin; share cheating others; living up to contracts and
resources fairly and without bargains; following social expectations,
cheating; do what is legally and rules, regulations, and laws
socially prescribed and avoid
what is proscribed

Very Legacy2 Transfer of resources to institutions Contributing resources to


Large that benefit nonkin as much as, or the commonweal and future
Group more than, kin without the expecta- generations; bequeathing
tion of direct reciprocity to oneself resources to institutions
(schools, hospitals, charities);
self-sacrifice for nonkin

Meaning Use resources to identify Using resources to investigate and


with, construct, and maintain support meaning-generating paradigms
a philosophy, purpose, or involving spirituality, religion, philosophy,
rationalization for existence science or any other system of knowledge;
(and nonexistence); attempt constructing a belief system; studying or
to arrive at an understanding engaging in the practice of philosophy,
and peace with the presumed science, or religion
purpose of life

Note. Resources include time, energy, effort, wealth, and reputation. Since the theory was introduced, the motives have been renamed to better
reflect their underlying dimensions and redefined and recategorized to conform more closely to evolutionary theory; this table reflects the
theory in its current form.
1
“S” or “status motive” the goal of which is to increase one’s social standing through competitive behavior intended to increase personal
resources within its behavioral domain.
2
“C” or “cooperative motive” the goal of which is to increase one’s reputation for reliability as a partner in relationships and coalitions through
direct and indirect reciprocal transfers of resources.

different environmental conditions. There should history (MacArthur & Wilson, 1967; Reznick et al.,
also be trade-offs in the costs and benefits of differ- 2002; Rushton, 2004; Stearns, 1992). Extreme
ent levels of a behavioral syndrome such that differ- environmental conditions have probably been less
ent levels of strength in a motive dimension could common over time, such that very high or very
produce relatively equal fitness outcomes at differ- low strength in a motive dimension is less com-
ent times and in different contexts in evolutionary mon than a moderate level. Therefore, aggregating

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


GENES BRAIN LATENT FACTOR BEHAVIORAL ENVIRONMENT
SYNDROME
“Commitment”

Genetic Produce differences Which result in Which result in Which are


Variations in brain structure measurable individual different levels of expressed in the
and function difference in the correlated behaviors environment
reactivity and strength
of a dimension

Low
Fluctuating
Trade-Offs Environments

>Transfer of resources
to mate and offspring In some environments,
>Expression of LOWER Commitment
emotional support and results in better fitness;
affection in some environments
>Signaling a desire for HIGHER Commitment
intimacy and sharing results in better fitness

High

Balancing Selection
A certain environment, for a certain time, will favor (i.e., select for) the genetic variation
producing the highest level of fitness; over a long period of time, aggregating across the
differences in individuals, this will result in a normal distribution of phenotypic strength
levels between individuals on a motive dimension

Fig. 22.1. The development of motive dimensions maintained by fluctuating/balancing selection: Example with the
commitment motive.

motive strength across individuals should produce bert, 2000). Interestingly, one submission strategy
a relatively normal distribution of strength along a is to signal cooperation, which could overlap with
motive dimension. the cooperative motives described later. However,
To complete the fourth step, it is necessary because submission to others results in aggressors
to identify some of the possible trade-offs that gaining more access to available resources, submis-
may have allowed individual differences in the pro- sive strategies should also involve costs that could
posed motives to evolve. A preliminary rationale for maintain heritable individual differences.
motive trade-offs follows.
Illness Avoidance
Hypothesized Motive Trade-Offs Illness Avoidance may also be related to Pank-
individual domain sepp’s (1998, 2000) fear system. In this sense,
Threat Avoidance engaging in many healthy behaviors could represent
Threat Avoidance may be a better fitness strategy a “flight into health” in order to escape fear of con-
than Aggression in some cases where cost is high, tamination and illness. In a study of 37 cultures,
and even “posturing,” a mixture of aggression and both women and men judged “good health” to be
threat avoidance, may be a better fitness strategy in highly desirable in a mate (Buss, 1989). Individual
other cases (Fournier, Moskowitz, & Zuroff, 2002). differences in Illness Avoidance may be selectively
Panksepp (1998, 2000) identified several genetical- maintained because it could increase fitness and
ly-coded emotional operating systems, one of which the probability of selection as a mate. Avoiding ill-
he called “fear.” This system mediates freezing, ness may require more resources but result in better
flight, or escape. Threat Avoidance combines a fear health and more access to mates. Illness avoidance
of harm to oneself and a desire to escape to safety should also vary with the presence and ease of trans-
(O’Connor, Berry, Weiss, Schuweiter, & Sevier, mission of pathogens in a particular environment,
2000). Social comparison helps humans determine and this has been shown to affect sociosexuality and
when to submit (Buunk & Brenninkmeyer, 2000)— personality (Schaller & Murray, 2008).
when the costs outweigh the benefits of standing Research on disgust offers another perspective
ground—and humans have developed a variety of on the Illness Avoidance motive. Disgust has been
submission strategies (Fournier et al., 2002; Gil- identified as a basic human facial expression in

b er n a rd 
infants and has been part of many evolutionary and have been identified, such as relational, dramatic,
neural theories of emotion (Plutchik, 2003). Dis- constructive, sensorimotor, and rough-and-tumble
gust has been defined as revulsion at the prospect (Scott & Panksepp, 2003; Slade & Wolf, 1994).
of putting an offensive substance in one’s mouth The Interpersonal Inquisitiveness motive was
(Rozin & Fallon, 1987), but this seems overly nar- operationalized as rough-and-tumble play and simple
row. People disgusted by something actually avoid fun rather than other types of play. Human rough-
proximity with it and do not want to touch or taste and-tumble play probably allows individuals to “size
it (Rozin, Haidt, McCauley, Dunlop, & Ashmore, each other up” in mock situations that are not sup-
1999; Rozin, Millman, & Nemeroff, 1986). Fur- posed to lead to outright aggression. For example,
thermore, even foods once preferred can become research suggests that children value sports achieve-
disgusting if a person has become nauseated and ments higher than any other (Eccles, Wigfield, Har-
vomited after eating it once (Logue, 1985; Rozin old, & Blumenfeld, 1993), and men appear to have
& Kalat, 1971). Experimental stimuli that evoke evolved mental mechanisms to allow them to assess
such responses include rotting meat, feces, vomit, each other’s fighting ability and do so more than
cockroaches, phlegm, and similar bodily fluids, and women (Fox, 1997). However, there are only modest
all of these could spread illness. In an evolutionary differences in boys’ and girls’ rough-and-tumble play
sense, individual differences in a motive to avoid (Scott & Panksepp, 2003).
contamination could be maintained because it may In addition, there is evidence that Environmen-
have resulted in trade-offs in survival and fitness tal Inquisitiveness, in the form of play, improves
benefits. Extreme illness avoidance could reduce cognitive functioning (Dansky & Silverman, 1973)
exposure to toxins but could also restrict food intake and helps individuals learn to negotiate the social
and calories, while indiscriminate food intake could environment (Berkoff, 2001). A strong motive to
increase calories and nutrition but might lead to be inquisitive about other people and the social
more frequent exposure to unwholesome or toxic environment could improve knowledge about other
substances. The fitness outcomes of high versus low individuals’ cooperation, fairness, and trustworthi-
Illness Avoidance would also depend on the fluctu- ness (Berkoff, 2001). On the other hand, rough-
ating availability and quality of food. and-tumble play could lead to increased exposure to
pathogens, and the risk of escalation of aggression
Environmental Inquisitiveness could lead to physical injury.
Panksepp (1998, 2000) called this the “seeking/
expectancy” system, which controls general engage- Aggression
ment with the environment, leading to exploration, When attempting to survive, reproduce, and
investigation, and anticipation of rewards. He also acquire resources, animals will threaten, attack, and
noted its role in learning and memory and sug- kill each other (Mason & Mendoza, 1993). Another
gested that it allows an organism “to develop a sense of Panksepp’s (1998, 2000) four genetically coded
of causality from the perception of correlated envi- emotional operating systems is “rage,” which medi-
ronmental events” (2000, p. 160). A strong motive ates aggressive behaviors such as attacking, biting,
to be inquisitive about the environment and explore and fighting in non-humans. In humans, subtler
it should involve trade-offs. Exploratory behav- forms of aggression, such as school yard bullying in
ior requires resources in terms of time and effort. order to gain resources (toys, designer clothes, or
Knowledge gained may increase survival and fit- lunch money), are common in children (Campbell,
ness; however, exploration may involve exposure to 1993; Olweus, 1978). In a coalition-forming spe-
unknown dangers that could reduce fitness. cies such as humans, competition for friends can
also result in “relational aggression” (Crick, Casas, &
dyadic domain Mosher, 1997). Men sometimes kill other men who
Interpersonal Inquisitiveness have cuckolded them, coopting their reproductive
This is related to Panksepp’s (1998, 2000) “play resource, and males commit more than 99% of intra-
and dominance system,” which recognizes the role sex homicides (Daly & Wilson, 1988). Aggression, as
play can have in establishing social dominance. a strategy, can be used to acquire and secure resources
Individual differences in play are observed in rats. of all kinds (mates, territory, food), but it can lead
For example, rats that exhibited the highest levels of to counteraggression, and cycles of escalation can
juvenile play also became more dominant as adults. incur costs for the initial aggressor (Berkowitz, 1993).
Human play is quite complex and numerous types Therefore, aggressive behavior involves trade-offs in

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


terms of costs such as physical injury. Aggression the important role status and resources play in choos-
should be context dependent, and heritable individ- ing a mate (Buss & Schmitt, 1993; Gangestad &
ual differences could develop in it (Buss & Duntley, Simpson, 2000). The four status/competitive motives
2006). Innovative research with chimpanzees (Pan could be maintained as individual differences dimen-
troglodytes) has found that dominance is heritable and sions because, although they can require considerable
correlated with subjective well-being (Weiss, King, & resources, their benefit is increased access to mates
Enns, 2002), which may be a trade-off for the costs and reproductive fitness.
incurred by the risk of counteraggression and escalat-
ing violence. Appearance
Research has suggested that face and body sym-
Sex metry—which can indicate genetic soundness and
Humans engage in pluralistic mating strategies developmental stability—are the markers of “beauty”
(Gangestad & Simpson, 2000) and are prepared for in mate selection (e.g., Gangestad, Thornhill, &
short- and long-term relationships (Buss & Schmitt, Yeo, 1994; Shakelford & Larsen, 1997). Therefore,
1993). Human females have “concealed ovulation” physical appearance is a proxy for good health.
with a vague estrus cycle that is not evident through The nucleus accumbens of heterosexual males is
physical changes as in other primates. Therefore, differentially activated by female faces that range
human sexuality is not linked directly to reproduc- in attractiveness (Aharon et al., 2001). It becomes
tion. As Panksepp (1998) noted, “It was a remark- hyperactivated in response to images of attractive
able feat of nature to weave powerful sexual feelings females but not to images of average females and
and desires in the fabric of the brain, without reveal- attractive and average males. This region is the “plea-
ing the reproductive purposes of those feelings to sure center” of the brain and part of its reward cir-
the eager participants” (p. 228). In the absence of cuitry. Cross-cultural research also supports a very
such knowledge, humans had to be motivated to strong desire for physical attractiveness in a long-
engage in sex so that reproduction could occur. Sex- term mate, although it is consistently stronger in
ual motivation is complex and mediated partly by males than females (Buss & Schmitt, 1993).
neurohormonal activation and partly by the reward The Appearance motive is the strength of interest
of physical contact and orgasm. The inherent level in increasing status through appearance-enhancing
of neurohormonal activation should be correlated behaviors and can be measured by the amount of
with the strength of the Sex motive, which is a test- resources one puts into them. Examples of such
able hypothesis. behaviors include cosmetic surgery (which could
During orgasm, activity in the cerebral cortex correct asymmetries in body and face), dieting and
decreases and activity in the hypothalamus and fat reduction procedures (liposuction) where food is
adjacent midline structures increases (Georgiadis abundant and inexpensive, greater food intake where
et al., 2006; Holstege et al., 2003). These structures food is not abundant and costly, and body decora-
also respond more actively to heroin exposure and tion and adornments that may serve to camouflage
rely on dopamine, which is involved in pleasure or distract from physical shortcomings. These can
and reward circuits (Panksepp, 1998). The benefits/ involve considerable costs in terms of resources, but
rewards of sex are among the most intense pleasures a trade-off may be better quality mates.
experienced. However, that sex can have its costs
is evident in some species, such as the wolf spider Mental
(Hogna helluo) where females eat males after mating Psychometric intelligence or “g” is related to socio-
presumably as nourishment for offspring. economic status (SES) and the control of resources
The remaining four motives in the Dyadic (e.g., Gottfredson, 1997, 2004; Jensen, 1998; Nyborg
Domain all involve different strategies to increase & Jensen, 2001; Scullin, Peters, Williams, & Ceci,
social status and desirability as a mate. It is likely that 2000). Individuals with higher SES also have better
these motives arose through intrasexual and inter- physical health and a longer life span (Adler et al.,
sexual competition as well as sexual selection consis- 1994; Bradley & Corwyn, 2002), which suggests
tent with parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972). the evolutionary benefits of intelligence. Therefore,
One strategy for maintaining friendships and coali- it is not surprising that intelligence is a highly valued
tions may be to increase one’s personal attributes, quality in mate selection (Buss et al., 1990).
reputation, and irreplaceability, in short, one’s status Bernard et al. (2005) posited that individuals
(Tooby & Cosmides, 1996). This is consistent with might compete for status by attempting to increase

b er n a rd 
and signal knowledge acquisition. For example, strength, and flexibility. Such activities can involve
many cultures hold educators and the educated considerable costs in terms of effort, energy, and
in high regard. Education costs time, wealth, and injury, yet they may result in higher status and bet-
effort, but it can increase earning potential, reputa- ter quality mates.
tion, and status. However, Bernard et al. adopted
a broader definition of knowledge than academic Wealth
achievement. They proposed that the Mental motive A mate who has material resources may be valued
behavioral syndrome encompasses the development as more irreplaceable or indispensable than a poorer
of nonintellectual talents and abilities such as in rival (Tooby & Cosmides, 1996). Consistent with
the creative arts (painting, sculpture) or perform- parental investment theory (Trivers, 1972), women
ing arts (acting, dancing, singing, playing a musi- place a premium on economic resources in a mate
cal instrument, composing). All of these might be (e.g., Buss et al., 1990; Buss, Shackelford, Kirkpat-
avenues for increasing one’s status, reputation, and rick, & Larsen, 2001; Kenrick, Sadalla, Groth, &
irreplaceability (Tooby & Cosmides, 1996). This is Trost, 1990; Wiederman, 1993). Men do not dis-
consistent with the description of nonacademic tal- count the importance of economic resources, but
ents and abilities as “multiple intelligences,” which women accord it more weight than men do in mate
include musical intelligence and bodily/kinesthetic selection. Men should also value a mate who has
intelligence (Gardner, 1993). The time and effort her own resources more, because this could leave his
to develop any of these talents and abilities is costly resources intact. This could be the basis for some
and must be diverted from other endeavors, but the arranged marriages and would certainly be repre-
benefit in better quality mates may offset this under sented in the tradition of the dowry. People who
certain environmental conditions. are more acquisitive may accumulate more wealth
and gain an advantage in competition for mates,
Physical but acquiring wealth may require a greater expense
Physical stature and status are important in mate of other resources such as energy and time. The
selection. Women prefer potential partners to be accumulation of material resources would, there-
tall, physically strong, and athletic (Buss & Schmitt, fore, involve trade-offs that could permit individual
1993) and men are intimidated by other men who differences to develop in the strength of resource
are taller, have greater shoulder width, and more acquisition behavior.
upper body musculature (Barber, 1995). Men prefer
women whose waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) is between small group domain
0.67 and 0.80 (Singh, 1993, 2000; Singh & Young, Commitment
1995). WHR of men is in the range of 0.85 to 0.95, Commitment involves the transfer of resources to
similar to what both men’s and women’s WHR is mate and offspring and provides the framework for
before puberty. The reduction in the ratio in women family interrelationships. Comparative research sug-
after puberty is an accurate indicator of women’s gests that a variety of neural circuits—for example,
better reproductive status. However, preferences in the dorsal preoptic area (Jirikowski, Caldwell, Stumpf,
weight and body fat tend to be context dependent. & Pedersen, 1988)—and hormones and neurotrans-
In areas where food is more available, attractive- mitters—vasopressin, oxytocin, prolactin, and pro-
ness is enhanced by a thinner appearance (Symons, gesterone (e.g., Carmichael, Warburton, Dixen, &
1979) and in areas where food is less available, Davidson, 1994; Insel, Winslow, Wang, & Young,
attractiveness is enhanced by a heavier appearance 1998; Mann & Bridges, 2001; Panksepp, 1981; Rosen-
(Rosenblatt, 1974). blatt, 2001)—are found in males and females and are
Bernard et al. (2005) were aware of this and involved in sexual as well as affectionate (caregiving or
attempted to operationalize the Physical motive so “maternal”)behavior. The concept of “love” appears
that the behavioral syndrome would not be restricted to be nearly universal in human cultures (Jankowiak,
to certain contexts and would reflect general attempts 1995; Jankowiak & Fischer, 1992), and it seems to be
to improve one’s physical stature and status. In con- defined by men and women specifically in terms of
trast with the Appearance motive, which concerns commitment behaviors such as giving up other sexual
body adornment, the Physical motive concerns and romantic partners, remaining faithful, marriage,
actual attempts to alter one’s physique, body type, and a desire to have children (Buss, 1988). The extent
and physical prowess. These may involve exercising of commitment predicts satisfaction in relationships
and attempts to improve physical fitness, endurance, (Rusbult, 1983) and groups (Van Vugt & Hart, 2004)

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


and commitment may be necessary in order for coop- the cooperative motives. The mechanisms of balanc-
erative motives such as Altruism and Social Exchange ing/fluctuating selection and trade-offs are usually
(see later) to develop (Nesse, 2001; Rusbult & Van understood to apply at the level of individual selec-
Lange, 2003; Van Vugt & Hart, 2004). tion, but selection can operate at multiple levels.
In addition, women prefer men who are more Groups can become higher-level “organisms” sub-
affectionate to children (La Cerra, 1994, as cited in ject to the pressures of natural selection and this is
Buss, 2004), supporting the evolutionary concept probably necessary for adaptations such as altruism
that men may have been selected by women not and morality to develop. Group selection had been
only for their ability to commit to a partner but also rejected earlier but is now viewed as plausible in some
for their ability to commit to offspring. The forma- circumstances (Wilson, 2007). Human evolution
tion of pair bonds increases inclusive fitness, because may represent a major transition in multilevel selec-
of the beneficial effects a partnership has on raising tion and this may be expressed in the cooperative
offspring (Geary, 2000). However, alternative strate- motives (Wilson, Van Vugt, & O’Gorman, 2008).
gies to increase fitness—such as promiscuity—also
exist. As mammals, one of the most outstanding fea- Altruism
tures of parental care is human males’ involvement Bernard et al. (2005) noted Hamilton’s (1964)
in parenting that is facultatively expressed based on “rule” that altruism could develop if the costs to
contingent conditions and therefore could evolve oneself were outweighed by the benefit to the recipi-
(Geary, 2005). In terms of trade-offs, a cost of Com- ent, multiplied by the degree of genetic relatedness
mitment is reduced resources for the individual, to the recipient. Indeed, research has found that
while a benefit would be increased inclusive fitness. altruistic behavior is more likely between relatives
(Burnstein, Crandall, & Kitayama, 1994), and it
large group domain is mediated by the degree of perceived emotional
Bernard et al. (2005) proposed that the four closeness (Korchmaros & Kenny, 2001; Neyer &
remaining motives operated in Large and Very Large Lang, 2003). Social species of insects engage in
Group domains of mostly nonkin. Three of these— self-sacrificing behaviors but usually only within
Altruism, Social Exchange, and Legacy—are con- families of genetically related individuals when their
sidered “cooperative motives,” due to their role in interests overlap (Wilson, 1975).
promoting coalition formation. Coalitions are typi- Bernard et al. (2005) originally defined the Altru-
cally defined as alliances of two or more individu- ism motive in terms of “strong reciprocity,” the pre-
als who take collective action to accomplish a goal. disposition to cooperate with nonkin. However, the
The cooperative motives help individuals balance the theory now defines Altruism in terms of Hamilton’s
status/competitive motives that are involved in more “rule.” The Altruism motive applies to kin and facil-
individualistic and/or self-protective behavior. The itates sharing resources with a wider circle of kin
benefit of coalitions is increased survivability of one- than the Commitment motive does. It supports the
self and one’s kin through the mutual protection and development of larger family groups than Commit-
shared resources that a large group provides. For sep- ment. “Strong reciprocity” is now operationalized
arately reproducing individuals, humans are excep- in the Social Exchange and Legacy motives, which
tionally cooperative, and we cooperate in groups to involve exchange or transfer of resources to nonkin.
compete with other groups (Alexander, 2005; Wrang- In terms of trade-offs, as with Commitment, a cost
ham, 1999). This involves a fundamental trade-off of Altruism is reduced resources for the individual,
in terms of the individual costs of being in a coali- while a benefit would be increased inclusive fitness
tion versus perhaps the greater costs of not being in a through resource sharing with kin who share at least
coalition (Flinn & Coe, 2007). The increased social some of the individual’s genes.
challenges and computations that group-living pri-
mates would have faced required greater social intel- Social Exchange
ligence, which in turn bought increased brain size as Altruism involves an apparent unequal
well as developments in computational mechanisms exchange—a transfer of resources to kin—whereas
to serve these additional adaptive motivations (Byrne Social Exchange involves an equal—or reciprocal—
& Corp, 2004; Byrne & Whiten, 1988; Dunbar, exchange of resources with nonkin, either with
2007; Kudo & Dunbar, 2001). immediate reciprocity or with the expectation of
Multilevel selection theory (Wilson & Sober, future reciprocity (Trivers, 1971). Altruism can
1998) probably also plays a role in the evolution of increase inclusive fitness, but, as Hamilton’s (1964)

b er n a rd 
“rule” states, it would decline as the degree of genetic while limiting costs such as increased potential for
relatedness is reduced. Altruism would, therefore, conflicts and exposure to pathogens (Kurzban &
not facilitate the formation of nonkin coalitions, Leary, 2001). Formalizing membership may help
but the Social Exchange motive would. Social identify in-group members from out-group mem-
Exchange can operate in large social groups whereby bers and insure that members participate at some
an individual transfers resources to another mem- minimal level, thereby reducing the costs of free-
ber of the group and receives reciprocation from the loading. Collateral institutions also develop that
individual or a third party or institution (Alexander, help members track each other’s reputations for
1987). For example, a soldier risks his or her life for trustworthiness, for example, public trials, gossip,
a comrade in battle and receives veterans’ benefits online social networking, and news media.
such as mortgage assistance or college tuition from
the government in return. very large group domain
The Social Exchange motive promotes individ- Very Large groups can command a strong sense of
ual sacrifice in the form of not taking advantage of affiliation and can result in extreme sacrifices to the
others to gain resources, even when there is oppor- group, including death. Tooby and Cosmides (1988)
tunity to gain an advantage that could increase have noted that war is a highly cooperative endeavor
an individual’s fitness. Gradually, an individual’s and, as such, it should involve the cooperative
reputation for trustworthiness is enhanced through motives. Bernard et al. (2005) originally identified
social exchanges and results in others being more this very large social domain of nonkin as “memetic,”
willing to engage in exchanges to the mutual ben- because motives in this social domain are dependent
efit of both parties. Those who enter into reciprocal on cultural transmission of memes of identity, mean-
exchanges of resources more frequently risk being ing, and obligation (Dawkins, 1989). These memes
taken advantage of, but they may benefit from can operate as cultural norms—as exemplars of cul-
enhancing their own reputation as a trusted partner. turally valued behavior—that increase cooperation
Research suggests that about 60% of people appear and sacrifice in very large groups (de Waal, 1996).
to be interested in maximizing mutual, rather than Motives in this domain are primarily cultural adapta-
individual, gain (Van Lange & Kuhlman, 1994; tions (Boyd, 2007) and learning plays an important
Van Lange, Otten, De Bruin, & Joireman, 1997). role in their shaping and expression. This is because
Humans do engage in strong reciprocity—transfer humans are a species that is not only subject to oper-
of resources to nonkin—in large social groups (Fehr ant conditioning (Thorndike’s law regarding learn-
& Fischbacher, 2003) and this can be maintained ing from the consequences of one’s actions) but also
by frequency-dependent selection (Mealey, 1995). aware of its subjectivity to it. Group selection acting
In larger groups the detection of freeloaders can be on culture may be partly responsible for the devel-
more difficult, but larger groups provide better pro- opment of motives in the very large group domain
tection and access to resources as a trade-off. and cultures may transmit valuable adaptive as well
Social Exchange requires the development of as maladaptive information (Boyd, 2007). Bernard
strong social institutions as well as cheater detec- et al. (2005) proposed two evolved individual dif-
tion to reduce exploitation (Cosmides & Tooby, ferences dimensions in this very large social domain,
1992). Therefore, Social Exchange should work the Legacy and Meaning motives.
best where reputations are known. However, some
human groups are so large that it is impossible to be Legacy
acquainted with more than a fraction of their mem- The third of the cooperative motives, Legacy, is
bers personally (Van Vugt, Snyder, Tyler, & Biel, not based on an equal exchange of resources. Legacy
2000). Examples of such groups are nations, reli- mediates the transfer of resources to nonkin without
gions, linguistic, and ethnic groups. In large groups, an expectation of reciprocity. Bernard et al. (2005)
membership is often formalized through require- used the example of donating money to a university
ments (e.g., place of birth or parentage), declara- as an example of Legacy. Such a transfer of resources
tions of allegiance (e.g., loyalty oaths, catechism), may provide no direct fitness benefit to the donor
initiation rituals (e.g., baptism, circumcision), and or his or her kin. The university is culturally val-
obligations (dues, taxes, military service). Formal- ued and, presumably, perpetuates values consistent
ization of membership is consistent with the idea with the donor’s, but a donation to the university
that humans should exhibit “discriminate sociality”; probably tangibly benefits rivals’ kin more than
they should seek the benefits of socializing in groups one’s own. Another example of the Legacy motive is

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


a soldier who gives his or her life in battle. This is the Such a personal ontology would be essential in a
ultimate self-sacrifice to a perceived higher purpose species that is aware of its mortality.
(honor, duty, patriotism) that exists only as a meme. As social animals, humans must accommodate or
In the nonmemetic, material world, where resources reconcile their nature in terms of balancing the trade-
are finite (nothing more so than mortality), such offs of individual self-interest versus communal group
extreme self-sacrifice cannot benefit the individual’s deference and cooperation. Bernard et al. (2005)
fitness. However, it may increase the self-sacrificing believed that this situation can lead to alienation and
individual’s inclusive fitness, because his or her kin despair. In fact, alienation and anonymity are associ-
may benefit from membership in a stronger, safer ated with increased selfishness and antisocial behavior
nation. Very large groups also develop institution- (Prentice-Dunn & Rogers, 1980; Zimbardo, 1970).
alized systems for third-party reciprocity through Although behavior motivated by Meaning could
which, for example, individuals are “paid back” for increase an individual’s level of cooperation with oth-
dying (keeping their part of the bargain with soci- ers, this would be a byproduct. Therefore, Meaning is
ety) by the government transferring resources (e.g., not categorized as a cooperative motive.
“survivor benefits”) to a dead soldier’s spouse and Instead, Bernard et al. (2005) proposed that
children. there are individual differences in the extent to
This third-party reciprocal arrangement can work which people are motivated to construct meaning
in evolutionary terms only if it increased the group’s out of existence. This is controversial, but in evolu-
(society’s or nation’s) ability to prevail in competi- tionary terms, being motivated to seek, adopt, and/
tion with other groups or otherwise had the effect or construct meaning may result in benefits such
of increasing the commonweal, the resources gener- as better psychological coping abilities—hardiness,
ally available to the group. The reputation of the resilience, fitness, even—when it comes to meeting
donor or dead soldier may also be enhanced and, in existential challenges. Cultural myths, particularly
turn, increases the donor’s attractiveness as a partner of legendary heroes, which are a form of meme
in social exchange or the soldier’s offspring’s status (Dawkins, 1989), exist because they function to
as a hero’s progeny (Alexander, 1987; McAndrew, provide ready models of meaning for members of
2002). In this way, a good reputation is extended to a group (Campbell, 2008; Campbell & Moyers,
kin, thereby increasing the donor’s and the soldier’s 1991). Some people could find meaning by accept-
inclusive fitness. In terms of trade-offs, the costs in ing a particular religious belief, and most religions
such cases are often large, while the benefits in terms come with an explanation of the individual’s place
of a return on investment beyond reputation are in the world. In fact, an epidemiological analysis
difficult to discern, which is why cultural memes of mental representations suggested that motiva-
must play an important role. There are, perhaps, tion may even be driving the evolution of religious
some quantifiable returns to society and the strength representations (Nichols, 2004). Others may find
of its institutions that benefit its members, but meaning in scientific pursuits or the enjoyment of
many of the benefits to the individual who engages nature. Some may even find meaning in amassing
in Legacy-motivated behaviors are perceptions and great fame and fortune and, with them, influence.
abstractions based on memes about generosity, In terms of trade-offs, even the pursuit of mean-
patriotism, sacrifice, religious duty, and heroism. ing has resource costs associated with it in terms
of time, energy, effort, and wealth. There may be
Meaning benefits, however, in terms of psychological fitness,
Access to resources in contemporary human which may increase one’s attractiveness as a mate.
existence involves living in smaller groups of kin,
while simultaneously living in and depending on Summary
very large groups and institutions for resources As noted in the introduction to this chapter,
(Tooby & Cosmides, 1996). And obtaining resources the primary challenge for individual differences
in such large groups requires the negotiation of psychologists is to conceptualize a means by which
numerous complex social exchanges. Bernard et al. selection can maintain variance in a functional
(2005) suggested that meaning—the development dimension of behavior. These proposed trade-offs
of a personal ontology, a rationale or explanation for that may have existed in the evolutionary history of
one’s existence and place in the world—serves a sur- the motive dimensions offer promise in addressing
vival function as an antidote to the despair that can this challenge. However, the trade-offs proposed for
arise in the social environment of very large groups. the motive dimensions are only tentative first steps.

b er n a rd 
Furthermore, the proposal of an evolved indi- personality constructs. The AIM-Q consists of three
vidual differences theory of motivation is a highly versions of somewhat different questionnaire tasks
speculative endeavor. First, in the long history of (see Bernard et al., 2008, for descriptions of the
motivational psychology, except for the MAT and versions). This permitted a multitrait-multimethod
PRF, there are very few attempts to study motivation analysis of its psychometric properties. Overall, the
from a multidimensional, trait-based perspective. AIM-Q scales assessing the 15 motive dimensions
Second, the relevance of individual differences to have good psychometric properties and the scales
evolutionary psychology is a source of much debate have good convergent and discriminant validi-
and is not widely accepted (see Gangestad & Simp- ties in a multitrait-multimethod matrix (Bernard
son, 2007). The advantage of combining individual et al., 2008). This suggests that the dimensions are
differences and evolutionary theory, particularly not method-dependent artifacts and emerge across
trade-offs, should be the ability to explain why cer- different assessment strategies. It also suggests that
tain motives are expressed today in human behavior the dimensions, as represented in the AIM-Q, are
and to put the development of these motives into relatively independent. The motive scales also have
historical context with respect to selection rather good internal consistency reliabilities and test-retest
than to merely describe proximal processes in moti- reliabilities across several different samples (Bernard
vation (Buss, 1991, 1999). Due to their presumed et al., 2008).
heritability, evolved motives should be operating in, Another method of establishing a new scales’
and should be able to be measured as, individual dif- reliability is consensual methodology. This approach
ferences in contemporary behavior (Bernard et al., compares self-reports to observer ratings on the same
2005). At this point, the theory and the motive dimensions (Costa & McCrae, 1992). Mean corre-
constructs required empirical support, which neces- lations between self-friend and self-relative ratings of
sitated development of a means of assessing them. AIM-Q motive scores compared favorably to mean
self-peer correlations for the five NEO PI-R domain
Development of a Motive Assessment scores (Costa & McCrae, 1992, Table 8). They also
Strategy compared favorably to the mean of self-peer corre-
The development of new psychological con- lations reported by Cheek (1982) for a variety of
structs requires these steps: (1) base constructs on aggregated personality measures (Bernard, 2009).
a theory, (2) operationally define the constructs, This convergence of self-friend and self-relative rat-
and (3) determine the relationships between the ings on the AIM-Q scales suggests that the motives
proposed constructs and behavior and other con- represent dimensions on which there is agreement
structs (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). The previous that people differ in meaningful ways (McCrae &
discussion has described how Steps 1 and 2 were Weiss, 2007).
accomplished. Step 3 required a programmatic series
of studies designated the “Assessment of Individual validity
Motives.” The term “individual” was chosen to con- To better understand the meaning of scores on
note the presumed domain-specific (i.e., indepen- the AIM-Q motive scales, it is necessary to study
dent) nature of the motive dimensions. These studies their relationships to other important individual
resulted in several questionnaire versions of the 15 differences dimensions (Messick, 1995). Scores on
motive dimensions referred to together as the Assess- the AIM-Q scales are correlated with scores on other
ment of Individual Motives-Questionnaire (AIM-Q; reliable and valid measures of aggression, cognition,
Bernard, Mills, Swenson, & Walsh, 2008). This sec- playfulness, and sexuality (Bernard, 2007a), provid-
tion summarizes the psychometric properties of ing evidence of the construct validity of some of the
the AIM-Q. Our theory of motivation depends on AIM-Q dimensions. It is also necessary to investigate
whether the motive dimensions are able to be identi- relationships between the AIM-Q motive dimen-
fied and assessed reliably and validly in contempo- sions and personality dimensions, especially those
rary samples. represented in the FFM because of its proposed evo-
lutionary basis. Recent research tested hypotheses
reliability predicting relationships between the motive dimen-
The first challenge for the theory was to pro- sions and dimensions of the FFM (Bernard, 2010).
vide empirical evidence that the motive constructs Two general hypotheses predicting that the motive
could be reliably assessed, are unidimensional, and and personality dimensions would be related, but
are relatively independent of one another and other not strongly overlap, were supported. This suggests

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


that motivational and personality constructs are such as German and Spanish. These new versions
conceptually different and that motivation, while may provide crucial additional information about
related to personality, requires assessment by dif- the validity of the motive dimensions, because
ferent psychological dimensions. In addition, 21 the theory requires that the motive dimensions be
of 24 hypotheses of specific relationships between found independently of language and culture in all
the motives and FFM dimensions based on Nettle’s human societies.
(2006, 2007) analysis of potential trade-offs in FFM
dimensions were also supported. Future Research Directions
AIM-Q items were written and edited to have Together, these results suggest that the AIM-Q
face sex neutrality (Bernard et al., 2008). However, motive dimensions predicted by the theory are reli-
even with sex-neutral wording of the items, there able and, to some degree, valid. However, the dem-
should be mean sex differences in certain motive scale onstration of adequate psychometric properties is
scores consistent with sexual selection and parental not evidence in support of the evolutionary theory
investment theory (Trivers, 1972). In fact, empirical or the proposed trade-offs on which the motives are
evidence suggests that sex differences in scores on based. Such evidence will require additional research
the Affection, Aggression, Appearance, Physical, and as well as support from other avenues of investiga-
Sex motives are consistent with evolutionary theory tion, including comparative psychology and behav-
(Bernard, 2007b). ioral genetics, and it may be some time before
One of the most important methods of estab- “evolved individual differences” is fully accepted as a
lishing the validity of new dimensions is to use congruent term by evolutionary biologists and psy-
them to predict behaviors of some social impor- chologists. An example of other avenues of investiga-
tance. If a dimension can account for a meaningful tion that could facilitate this acceptance is Hogan’s
amount of the variance in “real-world” behaviors, (2005) description of a number of behavior motiva-
then it may be more than just an artifact of mea- tional systems in other species that includes parenting
surement on paper. Therefore, AIM-Q scale scores (Commitment?), aggression, sex, self-maintenance,
have also been used to predict the frequency of a fear (Threat Avoidance? Illness Avoidance?), and
variety of behaviors (Bernard, 2009). Although exploration (Environmental Inquisitiveness? Inter-
these behaviors were self-reported, their self-report personal Inquisitiveness?). Hogan suggested that
was confirmed through observer ratings. Some of these systems can interact to release and inhibit
the more important relationships included the fol- one another hydraulically to affect the direction of
lowing: (1) Sex motive scores accounted for 11% behavior. If there are links between the motives and
of the variance in the frequency of sex; (2) Wealth these animal behavior systems, the links might be
motive scores accounted for 14% of the variance in used to identify an evolved basis for the motives in
money spent on clothing; (3) Illness Avoidance and other species.
Physical motive scores accounted for 15% of the Bernard et al. (2005) anticipated this notion of
variance in cigarette smoking and 42% of the vari- “hydraulic” interaction among motive dimensions,
ance in exercising; (4) Interpersonal Inquisitiveness suggesting that multiple, even conflicting, motives
motive scores accounted for 17% of the variance in may combine to shape behavior. The motives reflect
illegal substance use; and (5) Aggression and Mental complex decision rules that underlie the behav-
motive scores accounted for 29% of the variance in ioral syndromes and even these rules may vary as
leadership among peers. These effect sizes are not a function of local trade-offs and the characteris-
trivial because behaviors such as these have multiple tics of other individuals in the local social network
determinants. They suggest that the motive dimen- (Kenrick & Sundie, 2007). Our theory also incor-
sions are related to socially important behaviors. porates roles for energy, emotion, and cognition,
which help determine a direction for action when
summary motives conflict. These roles are reflected in two
These studies have relied on relatively large additional individual differences dimensions that
samples that now cumulatively total more than complete the theory: Vigor, which is an individual’s
3,500 adults. This research has been done only with level of raw behavioral activation/energy, and Delib-
English-language versions of the AIM-Q; however, eration, which is the extent to which an individual
there is ample evidence of the reliability and valid- delays action/gratification in order to consider the
ity of the motive dimensions in these samples. The consequences of conflicting motivated behaviors
AIM-Q is being translated into other languages (Bernard, 2008). The deliberation process involves

b er n a rd 
cognition and emotions that inform and expedite psychology is dedicated to studying human nature
the process. in all of its individually different manifestations,
Another promising avenue of investigation is this field is uniquely positioned to contribute to,
Kurzban and Aktipis’ (2006) evolutionary psychol- and become informed by, evolutionary psychology”
ogy approach to social cognition. It suggests viewing (p. 460). Almost two decades later, there is scant
social cognition from the perspective of “multiple evidence that Buss’s call has been answered by per-
minds, multiple motives,” a brain architecture con- sonality psychologists. For example, there is but a
sisting of multiple discrete information processing single brief reference to evolutionary theory in the
systems. The motives were hypothesized to emerge 37 chapters of a recent volume that its editors call:
from brain architecture and function (Bernard et al., “. . . a guide for researchers . . . that describes . . . all
2005) and the AIM-Q scales could be used in stud- of the resources in the methodological toolkit of
ies of individual differences in social cognition and the personality psychologist” (Robins, Fraley, &
discrete informational processing systems. Hypo- Krueger, 2007, p. ix; italics added).
thetically, different strengths in motive dimensions Despite this situation, there have been a few
should shape the direction and content of social attempts to approach individual differences research
cognition. from an evolutionary perspective. Stabilizing selec-
One last promising avenue of investigation is tion (MacDonald, 1998), fluctuating selection/
experimental personality research (Revelle, 2007). trade-offs (Nettle, 2006), and K-factor selection the-
In this approach, individual differences dimensions ory (Figueredo et al., 2005) all suggest that dimen-
are assessed and then experimental conditions are sions of personality, such as those that comprise the
manipulated. The effects of both are then jointly ana- FFM, can be understood through the lens of evo-
lyzed on states that mediate ongoing behavior. Such lution and may ultimately be explained as evolved
designs could be developed within one of the most adaptations. In addition, behavioral syndromes (Sih
active research paradigms in behavioral economics, et al., 2004a, 2004b) and balancing selection (Penke
“game theory” (for an overview see Camerer, 2003). et al., 2007) have much in common, even though
Game theory concerns people’s interactive behav- the former is an outgrowth of behavioral ecology
ior. In the “games,” people interact over obtaining, and the latter of behavioral genetics. Both use the
controlling, sharing, and using resources, which are concept of “reaction norms,” which is also used
behaviors of interest to evolutionary psychologists as in Person x Situation assessments in personality
well as economists. In some games, there are theo- research. This suggests that the three social science
retically “optimal” strategies (equilibria), which, if fol- approaches that share the basic metatheory of evolu-
lowed, should result in the maximization of resources tion may yet find common ground within person-
for each player. However, players sometimes adopt ality psychology. Some behavioral geneticists have
strategies that do not maximize their payoffs. Games indeed been enthusiastic about the role the field
can be used to investigate such phenomena as domi- may play in personality psychology (e.g., Ebstein,
nance, bargaining, social reputation, trust, and coop- Benjamin, & Belmaker, 2003).
erativeness and, overall, there are substantial individual As with personality, there have been only a few
differences observed in games (Camerer, 2003). Typi- attempts to apply evolutionary theory to motiva-
cally, in a game theory experiment, individual differ- tion. Control theory (Heckhausen, 2000), behavior
ences are treated as nuisance variance and relegated production theory (Aunger & Curtis, 2008), and
to the error term. In the proposed experimental per- the Zurich Model of Social Motivation (Bischof,
sonality approach, the strength of certain motives and 1975, 1993, 2001) have all suggested specific
motive combinations would be used to predict who evolved motivational modules or behavior pro-
would be more dominant, trustworthy, or coopera- duction systems in the human brain. All of these
tive, for example. Thus, variations from game strat- attempts are intriguing and may have potential for
egy equilibria due to individual differences would be broadening our understanding of human motiva-
“captured” and used to predict behavior. The accuracy tion from an evolutionary perspective. However,
of such predictions could provide further evidence for none of these initially used an individual differences
the validity of the motive dimensions. approach. Recently, a new questionnaire for the
Zurich Model of Social Motivation has been devel-
Conclusion oped to assess individual differences in the model’s
Buss (1991) issued an early call for an evolution- three dimensions: Security, Arousal, and Autonomy
ary personality psychology: “Because personality (Schönbrodt, Unkelbach, & Spinath, 2009).

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


Our theory combines individual differences References
and evolution and suggests that there may be mul- Adler, N. E., Boyce, T., Chesney, M. A., Cohen, S., Folkman, S.,
tiple heritable individual differences dimensions of Kahn, R. L., & Syme, S. L. (1994). Socioeconomic status
and health: The challenge of the gradient. American Psycholo-
motivation that could be used to help understand gist, 49, 15–24.
and predict purposeful behavior. Some issues with Aharon, I., Etcoff, N., Ariely, D., Chabris, C. F., O’Connor, E.,
the motive dimensions remain to be addressed & Breiter, H. C. (2001). Beautiful faces have variable reward
empirically. For example, are they defined in too value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron, 32, 537–551.
narrow a bandwidth, and are they too domain Alexander, R. D. (1987). The biology of moral systems. Hawthorne,
NY: De Gruyter.
specific or not specific enough? Ultimately, the Alexander, R. D. (2005). Evolutionary selection and the nature
motives may provide a bridge between understand- of humanity. In V. Hosle & C. Illies (Eds.), Darwinism and
ing human evolutionary past and understanding philosophy (pp. 301–348). South Bend, IN: University of
contemporary behavior. They should permit moti- Notre Dame Press.
vational psychologists to theorize about and pre- Aunger, R., & Curtis, V. (2008). Kinds of behaviour. Biology and
Philosophy, 23, 317–345.
dict behavior with some understanding of why it Barber, N. (1995). The evolutionary psychology of physical
takes the forms and directions that it does, partic- attractiveness: Sexual selection and human morphology.
ularly when contemporary behavior is abnormal. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 395–424.
The motive dimensions should also have applica- Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The Big Five personal-
bility in basic research fields beyond motivation, ity dimensions and job performance: A meta-analysis. Per-
sonality Psychology, 44, 1–26.
such as behavioral genetics, behavioral economics, Berkoff, M. (2001). Social play behavior: Cooperation, fairness,
and neuroscience. With respect to the last of these, trust, and the evolution of morality. Journal of Consciousness
the original theory suggested some of the neurop- Studies, 8, 81–90.
sychological structures that may be involved with Berkowitz, L. (1993). Aggression. New York: McGraw-Hill.
specific motives (Bernard et al., 2005). The avail- Bernard, L. C. (2007a). Assessing individual differences in moti-
vation: Convergent validity of the Assessment of Individual
ability of reliable and valid motive scales should Motives-Questionnaire [AIM-Q] and measures of aggres-
permit these predictions to be tested with imaging sion, cognition, playfulness, and sexuality. Individual Differ-
techniques. ences Research, 5, 158–174.
Despite early critics (e.g., Mischel, 1968), per- Bernard, L. C. (2007b). Sex and motivation: Differences in evo-
sonality and individual differences psychology has lutionary psychology-based motives. In P. W. O’Neal (Ed.),
Motivation of health behavior (pp. 65–84). Hauppauge, NY:
demonstrated its usefulness in the clinic (e.g., Meyer Nova Science Publishers.
et al., 2001) and as a valid predictor of important Bernard, L. C. (2008). Individual differences in vigor and
life outcomes (Roberts, Kuncel, Shiner, Caspi, & deliberation: Development of two new measures from an
Goldberg, 2007). Therefore, one of the more prac- evolutionary psychology theory of human motivation. Psy-
tical outcomes of the study of evolved individual chological Reports, 103, 243–270.
Bernard, L. C. (2009). Consensual and Behavioral Validity of a
differences in motivation may be its potential util- Measure of Adaptive Individual Differences Dimensions in
ity in applied areas such as educational, industrial/ Human Motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 33, 303–319.
organizational, health, and clinical psychology. Bernard, L. C. (2010). Motivation and personality: Relation-
Years after his initial call for an evolutionary ships between putative motive dimensions and the five factor
personality psychology, and noting the scant progress model of personality. Psychological Reports, 106, 1–19.
Bernard, L. C., Mills, M., Swenson, L., & Walsh, R. P. (2005).
that had been made in the interim, Buss (1999) was An evolutionary theory of human motivation. Genetic, Social,
still convinced that “Goal-directed tactics and strat- and General Psychology Monographs, 131, 129–184.
egies . . . are promising units for personality psychol- Bernard, L. C., Mills, M., Swenson, L., & Walsh, R. P. (2008).
ogy . . . Discovery of the underlying species-typical Measuring motivation multidimensionally: Development of
goal structure and the corresponding evolved stra- the assessment of individual motives-questionnaire [AIM-Q].
Assessment, 15, 16–35.
tegic solutions will constitute a major and lasting Bernard, L. C., Walsh, R. P., & Mills, M. (2005). The Motiva-
scientific contribution of personality psychologists tion Analysis Test: An historical and contemporary evalua-
informed by evolutionary theory” (p. 485). tion. Psychological Reports, 96, 464–492.
Bischof, N. (1975). A systems approach toward the functional
connections of attachment and fear. Child Development, 46,
Acknowledgments 801–817.
The author is grateful to his many colleagues and students Bischof, N. (1993). Untersuchungen zur Systemanalyse der soz-
who have contributed to the development and study of this ialen Motivation I: Die Regulation der sozialen Distanz—
theory, in particular, to Michael Mills, Leland Swenson, and R. Von der Feldtheorie zur Systemtheorie [On the regulation
Patricia Walsh, who were part of the original research team that of social distance—From field theory to systems theory].
conceived the theory. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 201, 5–43.

b er n a rd 
Bischof, N. (2001). Das Rätsel Öedipus. Die biologischen An evolutionary approach to mood disorders and their therapy
Wurzeln des Urkonflikts von Intimität und Autonomie [The (pp. 147–164). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
riddle of Oedipus: The biological roots of the core con- Byrne, R. W., & Corp, N. (2004). Neocortex size predicts
flict between intimacy and autonomy]. Munich, Germany: deception rate in primates. Proceedings of the Royal Society of
Piper. London B, 271, 1693–1699.
Boyd, R. (2007). Cultural adaptation and maladaptation: Of Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (Eds.). (1988). Machiavellian intel-
kayaks and commissars. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson ligence: Social expertise and the evolution of intellect in mon-
(Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and keys, apes and humans. Oxford, England: Oxford University
controversies (pp. 327–331). New York: Guilford Press. Press.
Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in
and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, strategic interaction. New York: Princeton University Press.
371–399. Campbell, A. (1993). Men, women, and aggression. New York:
Buchanan, T., Johnson, J. A., & Goldberg, L. R. (2005). Imple- Basic Books.
menting a five-factor personality inventory for use on the Campbell, J. (2008). The hero with a thousand faces. Novato, CA:
internet. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, New World Library.
115–127. Campbell, J., & Moyers, B. (1991). The power of myth.
Bugental, D. B. (2000). Acquisition of the algorithms of social New York: Anchor Books.
life: A domain-based approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126, Carmichael, M. S., Warburton, V. L., Dixen, J., & Davidson,
187–219. J. M. (1994). Relationships among cardiovascular, muscu-
Burkhardt, R. W., Jr. (2005). Patterns of behavior: Konrad Lorenz, lar, and oxytocin responses during human sexual activity.
Niko Tinbergen, and the founding of ethology. Chicago, IL: Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 59–79.
University of Chicago Press. Cattell, R. B., & Dreger, R. M. (Eds.). (1977). Handbook of
Burnstein, E., Crandall, C., & Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo- modern personality theory. Washington, DC: Hemisphere
Darwinian decision rules for altruism: Weighing cues for Publishing.
inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance Cattell, R. B., Horn, J. L., Sweney, A. B., & Radcliffe, J. A.
of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, (1964). Handbook for the motivation analysis test. Cham-
67, 773–789. paign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing.
Buss, D. M. (1988). Love acts: The evolutionary biology Cattell, R. B., Radcliffe, J. A., & Sweney, A. B. (1963). The
of love. In R. J. Sternberg & M. L. Barnes (Eds.), The psychol- nature and measurement of components of motivation.
ogy of love (pp. 100–118). New Haven, CT: Yale University Genetic Psychology Monographs, 68, 49–211.
Press. Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2007). Personality and individual differ-
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: ences. Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.
Evolutionary hypotheses testing in 37 cultures. Brain and Cheek, J. M. (1982). Aggregation, moderator variables, and the
Behavioral Sciences, 12, 1–49. validity of personality tests: A peer-rating study. Journal of
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychology. Annual Personality and Social Psychology, 43, 1254–1269.
Review of Psychology, 42, 459–491. Coleman, K., & Wilson, D. (1998). Shyness and boldness in
Buss, D. M. (1999). Evolutionary personality psychology. Annual pumpkinseed sunfish: Individual differences are context-
Review of Psychology, 42, 459–491, specific. Animal Behaviour, 56, 927–936.
Buss, D. M. (2004). Evolutionary psychology: The new science of Confer, J. C., Easton, J. A., Fleischman, D. S., Goetz, C. D.,
the mind. Boston, MA: Pearson. Lewis, D. M. G., Perilloux, C., & Buss, D. M. (2010). Evo-
Buss, D. M. (2009). How can evolutionary psychology success- lutionary psychology: Controversies, questions, prospects,
fully explain personality and individual differences? Perspec- and limitations. American Psychologist, 65, 110–126.
tives on Psychological Science, 4, 359–366. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). Cognitive adaptations for
Buss, D. M., Abbott, M., Angleitner, A., Asherian, A., Biaggio, A., social exchange. In J. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby
Blanco-Villasenor, A. . . . Yang, K. (1990). International pref- (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 163–228). New York: Oxford
erences in selecting mates: A study of 37 cultures. Journal of University Press.
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21, 5–47. Costa, P. T., Jr., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Revised NEO Person-
Buss, D. M., & Duntley, J. D. (2006). The evolution of aggres- ality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory
sion. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick, Evolu- (NEO-FFI) professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological
tion and social psychology (263–285). New York: Psychology Assessment Resources, Inc.
Press. Costa, P. T., Jr., & Widiger, T. A. (1994). Summary and unresolved
Buss, D. M., & Greiling, H. (1999). Adaptive individual differ- issues. In P. T. Costa, Jr. & T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality
ences. Journal of Personality, 67, 209–243. and the five-factor model of personality (pp. 1–20). Washington,
Buss, D. M., & Schmitt, D. P. (1993). Sexual strategies theory: DC: American Psychological Association.
An evolutionary perspective on human mating. Psychological Crick, N. R., Casas, J. F., & Mosher, M. (1997). Relational and
Review, 100, 204–232. overt aggression in preschool. Developmental Psychology, 33,
Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Kirkpatrick, L. A., & Larsen, 579–588.
R. J. (2001). A half century of American mate preferences. Cronbach, L. J., & Meehl, P. E. (1955). Construct validity in
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 63, 491–503. psychological tests. Psychological Bulletin, 52, 281–302.
Buunk, B. P., & Brenninkmeyer, V. (2000). Social comparison Daly, M., & Wilson, M. (1988). Homicide. New York: Aldine.
processes among depressed individuals: Evidence for the evo- Dansky, J. L., & Silverman, I. W. (1973). Effects of play on asso-
lutionary perspective on involuntary subordinate strategies? ciative fluency in preschool-aged children. Developmental
In L. Sloman & P. Gilbert (Eds.), Subordination and defeat: Psychology, 9, 28–43.

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


Dawkins, R. (1989). The selfish gene. Oxford, England: Oxford Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of
University Press. human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral
Denissen, J. A., & Penke, L. (2008). Motivational individual and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–587.
reaction norms underlying the Five-Factor model of person- Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (Eds.). (2007). The evolution
ality: First steps towards a theory-based conceptual frame- of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies. New York:
work. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 1285–1302. Guilford Press.
de Waal, F. (1996). Good natured: The origins of right and wrong Gangestad, S. W., Thornhill, R., & Yeo, R. A. (1994). Facial
in humans and other animals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard attractiveness, developmental stability, and fluctuating asym-
University Press. metry. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15, 73–85.
de Waal, F. B. M. (2002). Evolutionary psychology: The wheat Gardner, H. (1993). Frames of mind (2nd ed.). London: Harper-
and the chaff. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, Collins.
187–191. Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of
Digman, J. M. (1996). The curious history of the f human paternal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126,
ive-factor model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model 55–77.
of personality: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 1–20). New York: Geary, D. C. (2005). Evolution of parental investment.
Guilford. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary
Dunbar, R. (2007). Evolution of the social brain. In S. W. Gangestad psychology (pp. 483–505). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley &
& J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamental Sons.
questions and controversies (pp. 280–286). New York: Guilford Georgiadis, J. R., Kortekaas, R., Kuipers, R., Nieuwenburg, A.,
Press. Pruim, J., Reinders, A. A., Holstege, G. (2006). Regional
Ebstein, R. P., Benjamin, J., & Belmaker, R. H. (2003). Behav- cerebral blood flow changes associated with clitorally induced
ioral genetics, genomics, and personality. In R. Plomin, orgasm in healthy women. European Journal of Neuroscience,
J. C. Defries, I. W. Craig, & P.McGuffin (Eds.), Behavioral 24, 3305–3316.
genetics in the postgenomic era (pp. 365–388). Washington, Gigerenzer, G., & Hug, K. (1992). Domain-specific reasoning:
DC: American Psychological Association. Social contracts, cheating, and perspective change. Cogni-
Eccles, J., Wigfield, A., Harold, R. D., & Blumenfeld, P. (1993). tion, 43, 127–171.
Age and gender differences in children’s self- and task per- Gilbert, P. (2000). Varieties of submissive behavior as forms
ceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64, of social defense: Their evolution and role in depression.
830–847. In L. Sloman & P. Gilbert (Eds.), Subordination and defeat:
Ermer, E., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2007). Functional special- An evolutionary approach to mood disorders and their therapy
ization and the adaptationist program. In S. W. Gangestad (pp. 3–46). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
& J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamen- Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality
tal questions and controversies (pp. 153–160). New York: traits. American Psychologist, 48, 26–34.
Guilford Press. Gosling, S. D. (2001). From mice to men: What can we learn
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2003). The nature of human about personality from animal research? Psychological Bulle-
altruism. Nature, 425, 785–791. tin, 127, 45–86.
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., & Schneider, Gosling, S. D., & John, O. P. (1999). Personality dimensions
S. M. R. (2007). The K-factor, covitality, and personality. in nonhuman animals: A cross-species review. Current Direc-
Human Nature, 18, 47–73. tions in Psychological Science, 8, 69–75.
Figueredo, A. J., Vásquez, G., Brumbach, B. H., Sefcek, J., Gosling, S. D., & Vazire, S. (2002). Are we barking up the
Kirsner, B. R., & Jacobs, W. J. (2005). The K-factor: Indi- right tree? Evaluating a comparative approach to personality.
vidual differences in life history strategy. Personality and Indi- Journal of Research in Personality, 36, 607–614.
vidual Differences, 39, 1349–1360. Gottfredson, L. (1997). Why g matters: The complexity of every-
Fiske, S. T. (2008). Core social motivations: Views from the day life. Intelligence, 24, 79–132.
couch, consciousness, classroom, computers, and collectives. Gottfredson, L. (2004). Intelligence: Is it the epidemiologists’ elu-
In J. Y. Shah & W. I. Gardner (Eds.), Handbook of motivation sive “fundamental cause” of social class inequalities in health?
science (pp. 3–22). New York: Guilford Press. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 174–199.
Flinn, M., & Coe, K. (2007). The linked red queens of human Groothuis, T. G. G., & Carere, C. (2005) Avian personalities:
cognition, coalitions, and culture. In S. W. Gangestad & Characterization and epigenesis. Neuroscience and Biobehav-
J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamen- ioral Reviews, 29, 137–150.
tal questions and controversies (pp. 339–347). New York: Hagen, E. E. (2005). Controversial issues in evolutionary psy-
Guilford Press. chology. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary
Fournier, M. A., Moskowitz, D. S., & Zuroff, D. C. (2002). psychology (pp. 145–174). New York: Wiley.
Social rank strategies in hierarchical relationships. Journal of Hagen, E. H., & Symons, D. (2007). Natural psychology: The
Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 425–433. environment of evolutionary adaptedness and the structure
Fox, A. (1997, June). The assessment of fighting ability in humans. of cognition. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.),
Paper presented to the Ninth Annual Meeting of the Human The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies
Behavior and Evolution Society, University of Arizona, (pp. 38–52). New York: Guilford Press.
Tucson, AZ. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social
Freud, S. (1955). Beyond the pleasure principle. In J. Strachey behavior. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.
(Ed. & Trans.), The standard edition of the complete psycho- Harcourt, A. H., & Stewart, K. J. (2007). Gorilla society: What
logical works of Sigmund Freud (4th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 3–64). we know and don’t know. Evolutionary Anthropology, 16,
London: Hogarth Press. 147–158.

b er n a rd 
Heckhausen, J. (2000). Evolutionary perspectives on human moti- Kenrick, D. T., & Sundie, J. M. (2007). Dynamical evolutionary
vation. The American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 1015–1029. psychology and mathematical modeling. In S. W. Gangestad
Hogan, J. (2005). Motivation. In J. J. Bolhuis (Ed.), The behavior & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamen-
of animals: Mechanisms, function, and evolution (pp 41–70). tal questions and controversies (pp. 137–144). New York:
Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. Guilford Press.
Hogan, R. (1996). A socioanalytic perspective on the five-factor Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2001). Emotional closeness
model. In J. S. Wiggins (Ed.), The five-factor model of personal- as a mediator of the effect of genetic relatedness on altruism.
ity: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 163–179). New York: Guilford. Psychological Science, 12, 262–265.
Holstege, G., Georgiadis, J., R., Paans, A. M., Meiners, L. C., Krebs, J. R., & Davies, N. B. (Eds.). (1997). Behavioural ecol-
van der Graaf, F. H., & Reinders, A. A. (2003). Brain activa- ogy: An evolutionary approach (4th ed.). Oxford, England:
tion during human male ejaculation. Journal of Neuroscience, Blackwell.
23, 9185–9193. Kudo, H., & Dunbar, R. I. M. (2001). Neocortex size and social
Hough, L. M., Eaton, N. K., Dunnette, M. D., Kamp, J. D., & network size in primates. Animal Behaviour, 62, 711–722.
McCloy, R. A. (1990). Criterion-related validities of person- Kurzban, R., & Aktipis, C. A. (2006). Modular minds, multiple
ality constructs and the effect of response distortion on those motives. In M. Schaller, J. A. Simpson, & D. T. Kenrick
validities. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 581–595. (Eds.), Evolution and social psychology (pp. 39–53). Madison,
Houle, D. (1998). How should we explain variation in the CT: Psychosocial Press.
genetic variance of traits? Genetica, 102–103, 241–253. Kurzban, R., & Leary, M. R. (2001). Evolutionary origins of
Huntingford, F., & Adams, C. (2005). Behavioural syndromes in stigmatization: The functions of social exclusion. Psychologi-
farmed fish: Implications for production and welfare. Behav- cal Bulletin, 127, 187–208.
iour, 142, 1207–1221. Logue, A. W. (1985). Conditioned food aversion learning in
Insel, T. R, & Winslow, J. T. (1998). Serotonin and neuropeptides humans. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 443,
in affiliative behaviors. Biological Psychiatry, 44, 207–219. 316–329.
Jackson, D. N. (1999). Personality research form manual MacArthur, R. H., & Wilson, E. O. (1967). The theory of island
(3rd ed.). Port Huron, MI: Sigma Assessment Systems, Inc. biogeography. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (2 vols.). New York: MacDonald, K. B. (1983). Stability of individual differences
Holt. in behavior in a litter of wolf cubs (Canis Lupus). Journal of
Jang, K. L., McCrae, R. R., Angleitner, A., Riemann, R., & Comparative Psychology, 2, 99–106.
Livesley, W. J. (1998). Heritability of facet-level traits in a MacDonald, K. (1995). Evolution, the 5-factor model, and lev-
cross-cultural twin study: Support for a hierarchical model els of personality. Journal of Personality, 63,525–567.
of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, MacDonald, K. (1998). Evolution, cu lture, and the five-factor
1556–1565. model. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 119–149.
Jankowiak, W. (Ed.). (1995). Romantic passion: A universal MacDonald, K. (2005). Personality, evolution, and develop-
experience? New York: Columbia University Press. ment. In R. L. Burgess & K. MacDonald (Eds.), Evolutionary
Jankowiak, W., & Fischer, R. (1992). A cross-cultural perspective perspectives on human development (2nd ed., pp. 207–242).
on romantic love. Ethnology, 31, 149–155. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Mann, P. E., & Bridges, R. S. (2001). Lactogenic hormone regu-
Westport, CT: Praeger. lation of maternal behavior. Progress in Brain Research, 133,
Jirikowski, G. F., Caldwell, J. D., Stumpf, W. E., & Pedersen, 251–262.
C. A. (1988). Estradiol influences oxytocin immunoreactive Mason, W. A., & Mendoza, S. P. (1993). Primate social conflict.
brain systems. Neuroscience, 25, 237–248. Albany: State University of New York Press.
Johnson, J. C., & Sih, A. (2007). Fear, food, sex, and parental Mayer, J. D., Faber, M. A., & Xu, X. (2007). Seventy-five years
care: A syndrome of boldness in the fishing spider, Dolom- of motivation measures (1930–2005): A descriptive analysis.
edes triton. Animal Behaviour, 74, 1131–1138. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 83–103.
Kagan, J. (2003). A behavioral science perspective. In R. Plomin, McAdams, D. P., & Pals, J. L. (2007). The role of theory
J. C. DeFries, I. W. Craig, & P. McGuffin (Eds.), Behavioral in personality research. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, &
genetics in the postgenomic era (pp. xvii-xx). Washington, DC: R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in person-
American Psychological Association. ality psychology (pp. 3–20). New York: Guilford Press.
Kaplan, H. S., & Gangestad, S. W. (2007). Optimality McAndrew, F. T. (2002). New evolutionary perspectives on
approaches and evolutionary psychology. Evolution of the altruism: Multi-level selection and costly signaling theories.
social brain. In S. W. Gangestad & J. A. Simpson (Eds.), Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 79–82.
The evolution of mind: Fundamental questions and controversies McCrae, R. R., & Weiss, A. (2007) Observer ratings of personal-
(pp. 121–129). New York: Guilford Press. ity. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.),
Kappeler, P. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2002). Evolution of pri- Handbook of research methods in personality psychology. New
mate social systems. International Journal of Primatology, 23, York: Guilford Press.
707–740. McDougall, W. (1933). The energies of men. New York: Scribner.
Kenrick, D. T., Li, N. P., & Butner, J. (2003). Dynamical evolu- Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: An inte-
tionary psychology: Individual decision rules and emergent grated evolutionary model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18,
social norms. Psychological Review, 110, 3–28. 523–599.
Kenrick, D. T., Sadalla, E. K., Groth, G., & Trost, M. R. (1990). Merenda, P. F. (1999). Theories, models, and factor approaches
Evolution, traits, and the stages of human courtship: Quali- to personality, temperament, and behavioral styles: Postu-
fying the parental investment model. Journal of Personality, lations and measurement in the second millennium A.D.
58, 97–116. Psychological Reports, 85, 905–932.

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


Merenda, P. F. (2008). Update on the debate about the existence Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Craig, I. W., & McGuffin, P. (2003).
and utility of the big five: A ten-year follow-up on Carroll’s Behavioral genetics. In R. Plomin, J. C. DeFries, I. W. Craig,
“The five factor personality model: How complete and satis- & P. McGuffin (Eds.), Behavioral genetics in the postgenomic
factory is it?” Psychological Reports, 103, 931–942. era (pp. 3–15). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Messick, S. (1995). Validity of psychological assessment: Valida- Association.
tion of inferences from persons’ responses and performances Plomin, R., Happe, F., & Caspi, A. (2002). Personality and
as scientific inquiry into score meaning. American Psycholo- cognitive abilities. In P. McGuffin, M. J. Owen, &
gist, 50, 741–749. I. I. Gottesman (Eds.), Psychiatric genetics and genomics
Meyer, G. J., Finn, S. E., Eyde, L. D., Kay, G. G., Moreland, K. (pp. 77–112). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
L., Dies, R. R. . . . Reed, G. M. (2001). Psychological test- Plutchik, R. (2003). Emotions and life: Perspectives from
ing and psychological assessment. American Psychologist, 56, psychology, biology, and evolution. Washington, DC:
128–165. American Psychological Association.
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. New York: Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the Five Factor Model
Wiley. of personality and academic performance. Psychological
Moretz, J. A., Martins, E. P., & Robison, B. D. (2007). Behav- Bulletin, 135, 322–338.
ioral syndromes and the evolution of correlated behavior in Prentice-Dunn, S., & Rogers, R. W. (1980). Effects of deindi-
zebrafish. Behavioral Ecology, 18, 556–562. viduating situational cues and aggressive models on subjec-
Murray, H. (1938). Explorations in personality. New York: Oxford tive deindividuation and aggression. Journal of Personality
University Press. and Social Psychology, 39, 104–113.
Nesse, R. M. (Ed.) (2001). Evolution and the capacity for Reimann, R., Angleitner, A., & Strelau, J. (1997). Genetic and
commitment. New York: Russell Sage Fondation. environmental influences on personality: A study of twins
Nettle, D. (2005). An evolutionary approach to the extraversion reared together using the self- and peer report NEO-FFI
continuum. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 363–373. scales. Journal of Personality, 65, 449–475.
Nettle, D. (2006). The evolution of personality variation Revelle, W. (2007). Experimental approaches to the study of
in humans and other animals. American Psychologist, 61, personality. In R. W. Robins, R. C. Farley, & R. F. Krueger
622–631. (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in personality psychology
Nettle, D. (2007). Traits and trade-offs are an important tier. (pp. 37–61). New York: Guilford Press.
American Psychologist, 62, 1074–1075. Reznick, B. M., Bryant, M. J., & Bashey, F. (2002). r- and
Neyer, F. J., & Lang, F. R. (2003). Blood is thicker than water: K-selection revisited: The role of population regulation in
Kinship orientation across adulthood. Journal of Personality life-history evolution. Ecology, 83, 1509–1520.
and Social Psychology, 84, 310–321. Roberts, B. W., Kuncel, N. R., Shiner, R., Caspi, A., &
Nichols, S. (2004). Is religion what we want? Motivation and the Goldberg, L. R. (2007). The power of personality: The com-
cultural transmission of religious representations. Journal of parative validity of personality traits, socioeconomic status,
Cognition and Culture,4, 347–371. and cognitive ability for predicting important life outcomes.
Nyborg, H., & Jensen, A. R. (2001). Occupation and income Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 313–345.
related to psychometric g. Intelligence, 29, 45–55. Robins, R. W., Fraley, R. C., & Krueger, R. F. (2007). Preface.
O’Connor, L. E., Berry, J. W., Weiss, J., Schweitzer, D., & In R. W. Robins, R. C. Fraley, & R. F. Krueger (Eds.), Hand-
Sevier, M. (2000). Survivor guilt, submissive behaviour book of research methods in personality psychology (pp. ix-x).
and evolutionary theory: The down-side of winning social New York: Guilford Press.
competition. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 73, Roff, D. A. (2001). The evolution of life histories: Theory and anal-
519–530. ysis. New York: Chapman & Hall.
Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in schools. New York: Wiley. Rosenblatt, J. (2001). Hormone-behavior relationships
Ones, D. S., & Viswesvaran, C. (1996). Bandwidth-fidelity in the regulation of parental behavior. In J. B. Becker,
dilemma in personality measurement for personnel selection. S. M. Breedlove, & D. Crews (Eds.), Behavioral endocrinol-
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 17, 609–626. ogy (pp. 219–260). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Panksepp, J. (1981). Brain opioids: A neurochemical substrate Rosenblatt, P. C. (1974). Cross-cultural perspectives on attrac-
for narcotic and social dependence. In S. Cooper (Ed.), Prog- tiveness. In T. L. Huston (Ed.), Foundations of interpersonal
ress in theory in psychopharmacology (pp. 149–175). London: attraction (pp. 79–95). New York: Academic Press.
Academic Press. Rowe, D. C. (2000). Environmental and genetic influences
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of on pubertal development: Evolutionary life history traits?
human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University In J. L. Rodgers, D.C. Rowe, & W. B. Miller (Eds.), Genetic
Press. influences on human fertility and sexuality: Recent empirical
Panksepp, J. (2000). Emotions as natural kinds within the and theoretical findings (pp. 147–168). Boston, MA: Kluwer.
mammalian brain. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland-Jones Rozin, P., & Fallon, A. (1987). A perspective on disgust. Psycho-
(Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 137–156). New York: logical Review, 94, 23–41.
Guilford Press. Rozin, P., Haidt, J., McCauley, C., Dunlop, L., & Ashmore, M.
Penke, L., Denissen, J. J. A., & Miller, G. F. (2007). The evolu- (1999). Individual differences in disgust sensitivity:
tionary genetics of personality. European Journal of Personal- Comparisons and evaluations of paper-and-pencil ver-
ity, 21, 549–587. sus behavioral measures. Journal of Research in Personality,
Pervin, L., & John, O. P. (Eds.). (1999). Handbook of personality: 33, 330–351.
Theory and research (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. Rozin, P., & Kalat, J. W. (1971). Specific hungers and poison
Pianka, E. R. (1970). On r- and K-selection. American Natural- avoidance as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychologi-
ist, 104, 592–596. cal Review, 78, 459–486.

b er n a rd 
Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation relationships: Individual differences in cognitive styles.
of the laws of sympathetic magic in disgust and other Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 314–327.
domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, Stapley, J., & Keogh, J. S. (2005). Behavioral syndromes influ-
703–712. ence mating systems: Floater pairs of a lizard have heavier
Rusbult, C. E. (1983). A longitudinal test of the investment offspring. Behavioral Ecology, 16, 514–520.
model: The development (and deterioration) of satisfaction Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford,
and commitment in heterosexual involvements. Journal of England: Oxford University Press.
Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 101–117. Symons, D. (1979). The evolution of human sexuality. New York:
Rusbult, C. E., & Van Lange, P. A. M. (2003). Interdependence, Oxford.
interaction, and relationships. Annual Review of Psychology, Tett, R. P., Steele, J. R., & Beauregard, R. S. (2003). Broad and
54, 351–375. narrow measures on both sides of the personality-job perfor-
Rushton, J. P. (2004). Placing intelligence into an evolu- mance relationship. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24,
tionary framework, or how g fits into the r-K matrix of 335–356.
life history traits, including longevity. Intelligence, 32, Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1988, April). The evolution of war
321–328. and its cognitive foundations. Institute for Evolutionary Stud-
Samuels, J., Bienvenu, O. J., Cullen, B., Costa, P. T., Eaton, W. W., ies, Technical Report #88–1. Paper presented at the Evolu-
& Nestadt, G. (2004). Personality dimensions and criminal tion and Human Behavior Meetings, Ann Arbor, Michigan,
arrest. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 45, 275–280. retrieved from: http://www.psych.ucsb.edu/research/cep/
Schaller, M., & Murray, D. R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, papers/Evolofwar.pdf
and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide vari- Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1990). On the universality of
ability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to human nature and the uniqueness of the individual: The
experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, role of genetics and adaptation. Journal of Personality, 58,
212–221. 17–68.
Schönbrodt, F. D., Unkelbach, S. R., & Spinath, F. M. (2009). Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological founda-
Broad motives in short scales: A questionnaire for the tion of culture. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby
Zurich model of social motivation. European Journal of (Eds.), The adapted mind (pp. 19–136). New York: Oxford
Psychological Assessment, 25, 141–149. doi: 10.1027/1015– University Press.
5759.25.3.141. Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1996). Friendship and the
Scott, E., & Panksepp, J. (2003). Rough-and-tumble play in banker’s paradox: Other pathways to the evolution of adap-
human children. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 539–551. tations for altruism. Proceedings of the British Academy, 88,
Scullin, M. H., Peters, E., Williams, W. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2000). 119–143.
The role of IQ and education in predicting later labor mar- Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quar-
ket outcomes: Implications for affirmative action. Psychology, terly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.
Policy, and Law, 6, 63–89. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual
Shakelford, T. K., & Larsen, R. J. (1997). Facial asymmetry selection. In B. Campbell (Ed.), Sexual selection and the
as indicator of psychological, emotional, and physiologi- descent of man, 1871–1971 (pp. 136–179). Chicago, IL:
cal distress. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, Aldine.
456–466. Van Lange, P. A. M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (1994). Social value
Sih, A., Bell, A., & Johnson, J. C. (2004a). Behavioral syn- orientations and impressions of partner’s honesty and intel-
dromes: An ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in ligence: A test of the might versus morality effect. Journal of
Ecology and Evolution, 19, 372–378. Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 126–141.
Sih, A., Bell, A., Johnson, J. C., & Ziemba, R. E. (2004b). Van Lange, P. A. M., Otten, W., De Bruin, E. M. N., &
Behavioral syndromes: An integrative overview. The Quar- Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial, individu-
terly Review of Biology, 79, 241–277. alistic, and competitive orientations: Theory and preliminary
Singh, D. (1993). Body shape and women’s attractiveness: evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73,
The critical role of waist-to-hip ratio. Human Nature, 4, 733–746.
297–321. Van Vugt, M., & Hart, C. M. (2004). Social identity as social
Singh, D. (2000). Waist-to-hip ratio: An indicator of female glue: The origins of group loyalty. Personality and Social Psy-
mate value. International Research Center for Japanese Studies, chology Bulletin, 25, 731–745.
International Symposium 16, pp. 79–99. Van Vugt, M., Snyder, M., Tyler, T., & Biel, A. (2000). Coopera-
Singh, D., & Young, R. K. (1995). Body weight, waist-to-hip tion in modern society: Promoting the welfare of communities,
ratio, breasts, and hips: Role in judgments of female attrac- states, and organizations. London: Routledge.
tiveness and desirability for relationships. Ethology and Socio- Weiss, A., King, J. E., & Enns, R. M. (2002). Subjective well-
biology, 16, 483–507. being is heritable and genetically correlated with dominance
Slade, A., & Wolf, D. P. (Eds.). (1994).Children at play. in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Personality and
New York: Oxford University Press. Social Psychology, 83, 1141–1149.
Smith, E. A. (2000). Three styles in the evolutionary analysis Wiederman, M. W. (1993). Evolved gender differences in mate
of human behavior. In L. Cronk, N. Chagnon, & W. Irons preferences: Evidence from personal advertisements. Ethology
(Eds.), Adaptation and human behavior: An anthropological and Sociobiology, 14, 331–352.
perspective (pp. 27–46). New York: Aldine De Grutyer. Wilson, D. S. (1994). Adaptive genetic variation and human
Sorrentino, R. M., Holmes, J. G., Hanna, S. E., & Sharp, evolutionary psychology. Ethology and Sociobiology, 15,
A. (1995). Uncertainty orientation and trust in close 219–235.

 evolved individual differences in h uma n moti vati on


Wilson, D. S. (2007). The role of group selection in Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The new synthesis. Cambridge,
human psychological evolution. In S. W. Gangestad & MA: Harvard University.
J. A. Simpson (Eds.), The evolution of mind: Fundamen- Wolf, M., van Doorn, G. S., Leimar, O., & Weissing, F. J.
tal questions and controversies (pp. 213–220). New York: (2007). Life-history trade-offs favour the evolution of animal
Guilford Press. personalities. Nature, 447, 581–585.
Wilson, D. S. Clark, A. B., Coleman, K., & Dearstyne, T. Wrangham, R. W. (1999). Evolution of coalitionary killing.
(1994). Shyness and boldness in humans and other animals. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 42, 1–30.
Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 9, 442–446. Zimbardo, P. G. (1970). The human choice: Individuation, rea-
Wilson, D. S. & Sober, E. (1998). Multilevel selection and the son, and order versus deindividuation, impulse, and chaos.
return of group-level functionalism. Brain and Behavioral In W. J. Arnold & D. Levine (Eds.), Nebraska symposium on
Sciences, 21, 305–306. motivation (Vol. 17, pp. 237–307). Lincoln: University of
Wilson, D. S., Van Vugt, M., & O’Gorman, R. (2008). Mul- Nebraska.
tilevel selection theory and major evolutionary transitions: Zimmer, E. J., & Kallman, K. D. (1989). Genetic basis for alter-
Implications for psychological science. Current Directions in native reproductive tactics in the pygmy swordtail, Xiphopho-
Psychological Science, 17, 6–9. rus nigrensis. Evolution, 43, 1298–1307.

b er n a rd 
CHAPTER

Moods of Energy and Tension That


23 Motivate

Robert E. Thayer

Abstract
Moods are perfect barometers of physiological and psychological functioning. Two biopsychological
dimensions, Energetic and Tense Arousal, are keys to understanding moods. These dimensions
interact under different activating conditions to form four complex moods that range from calm
energy (pleasurable mood associated with full attentional focus, happiness, optimism, favorable athletic
performance, and self-control) to tense tiredness (bad mood associated with depression, negative
perception of problems, yielding to unwanted urges such as sugar snacking or smoking). Two other
complex moods include tense energy (moderately positive state that combines vigor with moderate
tension) and calm tiredness (ideal for restful sleep). Energetic and tense arousal are associated with many
kinds of motivational processes, including various strategies of self-regulation (best strategy combines
relaxation techniques, exercise, and cognitive control). Important to these moods are natural processes
such as health, sleep, diet, diurnal energy cycles, movement (exercise), and stress. These moods are
mediated by general bodily arousal states.
Key Words: mood, energy, tension, self-regulation, diet, exercise, arousal, sleep, stress, diurnal cycles

If we think of our moods as emphasizing Unlike emotions that usually have an under-
meaning and enhancing or reducing the standable cause, everyday moods are background
pleasure in our lives, we can understand how feelings that seem to come and go with no apparent
central they really are. In this respect they are reason, although as we shall see they are integrally
more important than daily activities, money, related to knowable bodily systems and processes.
status, and even personal relationships because Moods can be regarded as dispositional variables
these things are usually filtered through our that tend to amplify or inhibit existing causal rela-
moods. In many respects our moods are at the tionships. I believe that the best moods involve high
core of our being. energy and lower tension, and the worst moods
(Thayer, 1996, p. 4) involve reduced energy or tiredness and increased
tension.
Contrary to a mistaken assumption that every- Let me introduce an often overlooked motiva-
day moods are meaningless background feelings, tional concept in this chapter with a personal anec-
these low-level emotional states appear to be barom- dote. Being reminded of the approaching deadline
eters of both physiological and psychological func- for a draft of this work, I noticed that my motiva-
tioning (Thayer, 2000). They have a physiological tion to finish it varied with the amount of require-
substrate. They are integrally tied to our thoughts ments in my life and with time of day. The matter
and other cognitive precesses. They subtly influence of other requirements being important to changing
our behavior, and they motivate us. motivation levels is no surprise to any writer with


many commitments, but often overlooked is time rhythm. In the time of day when energy is high,
of day as a motivational variable. At times of day motivation is strong, especially for physical activity.
when I felt more energy, my motivation to com- But as energy declines one is inclined to rest and to
plete this important task was unflagging because of decrease energy expenditure. Although gross physi-
its obvious significance, but when I was tired my cal activity is most influenced by energy level in my
motivation was reduced, often resulting in delay or view, cognitive activity also is influenced, and there
procrastination. is abundant evidence for this in the mood-cognition
literature with the understanding that energy is a
An Endogenous Cycle of Energetic Arousal
core component of mood.
This variation with time of day concerns a
general biopsychological variable that I have Two Arousal Systems Mediate Mood
named Energetic Arousal. Once established, this I regard mood as mediated by two bipolar
is an endogenous energy cycle (a kind of biologi- biopsychological dimensions, Energetic Arousal
cal clock) that repeats itself with more or less the and Tense Arousal, and the conjunction of these
same pattern day after day unless there is signifi- dimensions form four complex moods: calm
cant change in such variables as physical activity, energy, tense energy, calm tiredness, and tense
food intake, unusual emotional arousal, or cross- tiredness (see Fig. 23.2).
ing time zones with accordant behavioral changes.
Energetic arousal directly affects level of motivation Energetic Arousal
for everyday activities that require energy expendi- I think of energetic arousal as a kind of “go sys-
ture, and from this we can see one of the reasons for tem.” When it is high, we are motivated to move,
changes in motivation to finish this chapter. While to act, to do things. And when energetic arousal is
experiencing more energy, my writing motivation low, our motivation predisposes rest and recupera-
increased, but with less energy (tiredness) motiva- tion. There are a number of prominent associations
tion declined. of energetic arousal. Among the more important,
There are individual differences in this energy this activation pattern is closely associated with
cycle such as the morningness/eveningness trait health and illness (Thayer, 1989; cf. Ryan & Deci,
(moderately heritable), but in my experience feel- 2008; Ryan & Frederick, 1997). This is immedi-
ings of energy and the physiological substrate that ately evident by the way in which a core element
underlies it is highest in the first third of the wak- in the experience of sickness is fatigue and tired-
ing day for most people, often reaching its peak ness, while good health brings feelings of vitality
at mid to late morning, dropping off in late after- and energy. Moreover, there are reliable correlations
noon, reaching a subpeak in the early evening and between mood (energy) and immune system func-
then declining to the lowest point just before sleep tioning (Segerstrom, 2007). Subjective energy is an
at night (see Fig. 23.1). Motivation for everyday excellent indication of health and there are a variety
activities varies directly with this diurnal (circadian) of kinds of evidence for this. But this assertion was

13 10.0

12 9.5
11 9.0
Energetic Arousal

Tense Arousal

10
8.5
9
8.0
8
7.5
7

6 7.0

5 6.5
0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16
7:45 12:00 5:00 11:28

Fig. 23.1. Energy (line) and tension (bar) levels from waking to sleep. (Reproduced from Thayer, 2001, Oxford University Press.)

th ayer 
ENERGY that are affected by food, including the immediate
subjective reactions as well as changing reactions an
Calm Energy Tense Energy hour or two later as the food is metabolized. And
then there may be thoughts about what was eaten.
For example, one may experience an immediate
CALMNESS TENSION
energy surge from a sugar snack (pleasant feeling)
followed moments later by guilt about having bro-
Calm Tiredness Tense Tiredness ken one’s diet (negative feeling), and as happened in
my research, tense tiredness (negative feeling) some
TIREDNESS time later. These and other reasons are why I have
used a short-term within-subjects approach such as
Fig. 23.2. Two biopsychological dimensions and four complex
moods. (Reproduced from Thayer, 2001, Oxford University
those in which food-mood associations are repeat-
Press.) edly assessed and results are aggregated for maximal
reliability (e.g., Thayer, 1987a, b; Thayer, Peters,
Takahashi, & Birkhead-Flight, 1993).
nicely evidenced by a convincing study involving Although there is no clear consensus about what
a panel of 310 masters-level nurses who systemati- type of food most affects energy and mood, there is
cally rated energy at present and in past decades much more unambiguous evidence about the way
(Dixon, Dixon, & Hickey, 1993). Their finding: that mood affects diet. For example, in one review
Energy level was the best predictor of both physical of over 50 scientific studies the causes of obesity
and psychological health. and overeating could be traced to emotional eating
Another important association of energetic (Ganley, 1989). Included here were such negative
arousal is diet. Although there is no clear consensus emotions as depression, anxiety, anger, boredom,
about exactly what type of diet brings the greatest and loneliness. A common pattern of these negative
and most sustained energy feelings, we know from emotions was low energy and tension, or what I call
classic studies of starvation (e.g., Keys, Brozek, tense tiredness (Thayer, 2001). And the scientific lit-
Henchel, Mickelsen, & Taylor, 1950) that reduced erature on overeating contains a number of studies
food intake has a primary effect of tiredness (low showing that the mood conditions which are corre-
energy). As one example of my research in this area, lated with overeating are low energetic arousal and
an experiment involving sugar ingestion (Thayer, increased tense arousal. These negative moods exert
1987a,) indicated that this resulted in increased powerful influences on behavior. When resources
energy followed an hour later by energy decreases decline and feelings of low energy and increased
and tension increases. tension prevail, people are often motivated to eat
One of the most meaningful experimental dem- good tasting (energy-intensive) food as a way of
onstrations of the relationship between mood and feeling better. Eating such food can raise energy and
a physiological substrate associated with food was reduce tension and from this we can see the locus of
carried out by Deary and his colleagues in their the motivation: It is to escape these negative feelings
studies of hypoglycemia (e.g., Gold, MacLeod, and enhance positive ones.
Deary & Frier, 1995). Using random assignment Another important association with energetic
and a placebo compared method of reducing blood arousal is sleep. This is evident both from clas-
glucose with an insulin drip, and at the same time sic studies of sleep deprivation on feelings that are
periodically assessing mood over a 3-hour period, produced (Murrey, 1965; Thayer, 1989), as well as
this group showed that as the blood glucose of the from correlational and quasi-experimental research
participants dropped from normal to near hypogly- in which energy level is manipulated and tracked
cemia tension increased and energy declined. The with differing amounts of sleep. One very impor-
participants were left experiencing tense tiredness. tant reason why sleep and mood are related is the
In my view, one important matter to bear in way that sleep or lack of sleep directly affects energy
mind about the food-mood connection is that the and tiredness and these feelings are core constitu-
moods which are experienced from typical food ents of energetic arousal. As was true with diet men-
ingestion are subtle and require very reliable assess- tioned earlier, time of day is likely to be important
ment for adequate understanding. Moreover, good in assessment of sleep on mood, so for example,
understanding of the effects of food on mood is very partial sleep deprivation one day may have its major
complicated, and one must consider many variables mood effects the next day in the late afternoon or in

 m oods of energy and tension th at moti vate


the evening of the next day. But on the morning of smoke (or snack) if they walked than if they had
the day following sleep deprivation there may be no engaged in some sedentary activity. It appeared that
apparent effect. our hypothesis was correct and that the positive
Physical activity also is an important component mood-regulating effect of exercise was verified.
of energetic arousal. With regard to exercise, with Cognition and mood are quite interrelated, and
which there are many scientific studies (e.g., Reed this can be illustrated by the extensive research on
& Ones, 2006), certain conclusions seem apparent mood-thought congruency (e.g., Blaney, 1986).
(also cf. Ryan, Williams, Patrick, & Deci, 2009). In And as I have indicated, energy and mood are
my view (Thayer, 2001) the primary mood effect closely related. But there is another often over-
of moderate exercise (e.g., a short brisk walk) is looked way that cognition and energy are likely to
increased energetic arousal and a secondary mood be closely associated. This has to do with the fact
effect (less reliable) is reduced tense arousal. A ter- that motivation to act (particularly physical activ-
tiary effect is increased feelings of optimism. But ity) is related to self-perception of the energy that
with more intense exercise (e.g., an hour of intense would be necessary to act. This involves a kind of
aerobics) the primary mood effect is both reduced cybernetics analysis (cf., Carver & Scheier, 1982)
tense and energetic arousal, although there is some incorporating feedback loops. Energy and motiva-
evidence that after recovery from intense exercise, tion usually are integrated so that thoughts about a
there may be a resurgence of energetic arousal. perspective action are associated with the perception
My work in recent years has involved self- of how much or little energy will be necessary. Usu-
regulation of mood, and this will be discussed more ally the sensing of energy levels and the subsequent
fully later. However, here let me say that exercise is integration is so rapid and of such low awareness
a remarkably effective regulator of mood. Moreover, that it isn’t noticed, but when one is tired it is easy
the amount of exercise necessary to impact mood is to be unrealistic about the amount of energy that
relatively little. We have focused on short exercise will be necessary for some future act and to believe
interventions that could be introduced into typi- it will not be possible (Thayer, 1987b).
cal daily lifestyle—in particular, short brisk walks Still another way that cognition and mood are
(Thayer, 1987a, b, 1989; Thayer et al., 1993). As interrelated was the subject of a series of our stud-
indicated earlier, the primary mood effect of this ies in which perception of personal problems and
form of exercise is increased energy. also optimistic thoughts was studied. The impetus
One effective use of short brisk walks that indi- for this research was my perception that seemingly
cates well the importance of exercise as a mood unchanging personal problems did in fact vary sub-
regulator occurred with a set of studies in which we stantially depending on the mood that preceded
assumed that unwanted behaviors such as smoking their consideration.
and sugar snacking often occur as a way of self- A group of volunteer participants who were expe-
regulating mood, a kind of self-medication. Our riencing a chronic personal problem were enlisted
idea was that if alternative ways of self-regulating to rate the apparent seriousness of the agreed-upon
mood were available the unhealthy behavior would problem as well as how optimistic they were that the
diminish. In one experiment that focused on ciga- problem would be solved. This was done five times
rette smoking and a second on sugar snacking, par- during the day over several days in a 3-week period:
ticipants were randomly assigned to 5 minutes of a beginning of day, late morning, late afternoon, and
short brisk walk or to an alternative sedentary activ- just before sleep. Additionally, they rated the prob-
ity (Thayer et al., 1993) at times when they desired lem after taking a 10-minute brisk walk as various
to smoke or snack. Before and after that 5 minutes, times during the day.
participants rated their urge to smoke (or snack) The same problem was rated as significantly more
and their mood. After those post ratings they were serious (also less optimistic of being solved) at late
free to smoke a cigarette or eat the sugar snack, but afternoon than late morning. Moreover, regardless
the time before smoking or snacking was recorded. of time of day, the changing degree of apparent seri-
The brisk walk significantly reduced the urge ousness was rated as more serious when self-ratings
to smoke (or snack) and significantly lengthened of energy were low relative to higher tension (tense
the time before the cigarette was smoked or the tired) compared to times when energy was high and
snack was eaten. Moreover, the walk significantly tension low (calm energy). Lastly, the problem was
increased self-rated energy in both experiments. rated as significantly less serious when consideration
Finally, participants waited almost twice as long to occurred after a 10-minute brisk walk. These findings

th ayer 
were replicated in several quasi-experiments (Thayer, bipolar dimensions rather than several independent
1987b; Thayer, Takahashi, & Pauli, 1988). factors as I had previously hypothesized. I then pro-
One last prominent association with energetic posed a two-dimensional model (Thayer, 1978),
arousal that should be mentioned is stress. Stress and based in part on that empirical and theoretical
is evidenced by the subjective tension that it cre- work, four prominent models of affect and several
ates in interaction with energetic arousal. I believe less prominent ones exist today as the likely map-
that stress is related to a balance between resources pings of affect (Yik, Russell, & Barrett, 1999). My
and requirements (Thayer, 1996). When resources, two-dimensional model involving energetic and
which are indicated by one’s energy level, are tense arousal, plus combinations of the two kinds of
exceeded by requirements the result is tension and arousal forming complex moods, currently stands as
stress. One of the interesting implications of this is one of at least four competing models of mood. This
that the same experience may be sought out as plea- model is the one I believe is the most valid represen-
surable when resources are adequate but be stressful tation of mood.
when ones resources are not equal to the require- There is a substantial similarity between my two-
ments of the situation. More about this relationship dimensional model and other proposed models of
will be discussed in the next section in relation to affect. As alternatives to my two bipolar dimensions,
the interaction of energetic and tense arousal. two other prominent mappings of affect comprise
similar but slightly different two-dimensional mod-
Tense Arousal els. These include a model involving two dimen-
In addition to energetic arousal, I named the sec- sions named pleasure/displeasure and arousal,
ond of the two main mood systems Tense Arousal. proposed by Russell (1980), and a second model
This is a system that mediates danger, real or imag- involving two dimensions named positive and nega-
ined (Thayer, 1989, 2009). While energetic arousal tive affect by Watson and Tellegen (1985). A third
is a go system, tense arousal is a kind of stop sys- multidimensional circumplex model, including
tem. Energy predisposes us to move and to act, but pleasantness and activation, also has been proposed
tension predisposes caution, waiting, or stopping. by Larsen and Diener (1992).
A primary marker that differentiates tense and ener- The similarity between my two-dimensional
getic arousal is subjective experience, including ten- model and Watson’s model of positive and negative
sion/calmness denoting one dimension and energy/ activation is quite apparent. In fact, the two models
tiredness denoting the second. Other differences, are substantially the same although the dimensions
which will be briefly described next, include some were given different names. The similarity between
physiological patterns (primarily muscle tension) my model and Russell’s model is also apparent with
and also differences at the level of the brain (e.g., reference to combinations of energetic and tense
limbic system, cerebral cortical asymmetry). arousal, which yield complex mood states. These com-
Cognitive differences also exist between ener- plex mood states in my model match Russell’s basic
getic and tense arousal. A major difference in my dimensions, especially the dimension of pleasure/
view is that cognition often is scattered with tension displeasure (Yik et al., 1999).
but is directed with energy. A tentative evolution- That these multidimensional models are valid
ary explanation for this distinction may be that in indicators of the most basic underlying dimensions
a cautious (tense) mood it is adaptive to continu- of affect is suggested by various kinds of evidence.
ously scan the environment for danger. This primar- However, considering the four models of affect that
ily applies during circumstances in which the source are most prominent, which one is most valid?
of the danger is not fully known (e.g., generalized I believe that energetic and tense arousal best cap-
anxiety), but if the source of the danger were known ture the most elemental biopsychological processes
it is likely that attention would be directed to that that underlie affect. However,, there is substantial
source even under high tension. On the other hand, precedent for a basic psychological dimension of
the go system of energetic arousal facilitates fully pleasure/displeasure (e.g., Russell). Affective states
directed attention (Thayer, 1989). of energy and tiredness are more central from a bio-
logical perspective in my view. This energy-tiredness
Alternate Models of Mood (Affect): dimension underlies all health and general behav-
Similarities and Differences ior. Wakefulness (closely correlated with energy) as
In the 1970s it became increasingly clear from well as sleep (tiredness) is a primary dimension of all
factor analytic studies that affective space forms two life forms; thus, energetic arousal is a fundamental

 m oods of energy and tension th at moti vate


dimension of life. Also essential for survival is the of tense tiredness. In a similar way, as activating
ability to react to danger, and these reactions are conditions increase energetic arousal from low to
mediated by tense arousal. Without the capacity moderate levels, tense arousal also increases. But as
to react to hostile environments, survival would be tense arousal is increased further energetic arousal
limited. Based on this very general biological per- is decreased, thus yielding the complex mood that
spective, energetic and tense arousal appear to be I call “tense tiredness.”
the most fundamental dimensions of affect. In statistical terms, energetic and tense arousal
This being said, I believe that all four models bear a positive correlation from low to moderate
are basically compatible. Exploring this point fur- levels of activating conditions and a negative corre-
ther, cross-sectional studies have shown energetic lation from moderate to high levels. Thus, it follows
and tense arousal to be orthogonal as the other that these two types of correlation together result in
competing models similarly exhibit orthogonality. orthogonal dimensions in cross-sectional studies. It
But I believe that this apparent independence does is notable that these positive and negative correla-
not reflect the way that energy and tension interact tions are the same regardless of whether activating
under different activating conditions. conditions that drive energy result at high levels of
To illustrate this point, as we change from tired- energy and low tension (calm energy) or if activating
ness to high energy, tension states change and com- conditions that drive tension result in high tension
plex moods occur. At high levels of energy together and low energy (tense tiredness). These relationships
with low tension, pleasurable moods are experi- are portrayed in Figure 23.3.
enced. I call this complex mood “calm energy.” This moderate level at which a positive cor-
On the other hand, for the moderately energized relation becomes negative has not been indepen-
individual, as tension increases to the highest level, dently established from psychophysiological criteria
very negative moods occur and I call this complex other than affective response, but rather it must be
negative mood “tense tiredness.” Just as calm energy observed on the basis of the shifting correlations.
is very pleasurable, tense tiredness is experienced as Speculating somewhat loosely, the moderate point
displeasure. It is apparent then that my bipolar com- at which increasing tense arousal results in decreas-
plex mood dimension represented by calm energy ing energetic arousal probably is related to current
on one end and tense tiredness on the other end is physiological resources that can be assessed on the
the same as Russell’s (2003) core mood that extends basis of subjective levels of energy. The moderate
from pleasure to displeasure. Other combinations point at which increasing energy results in decreas-
of the energy and tension dimensions yield the ing tense arousal is likely to be based on tempera-
complex mood states that I call “tense energy” and ment (e.g., neuroticism).
“calm tiredness.” Tense energy and calm tiredness Finally, as energy declines and tension increases
represent different levels of arousal and from this there is a point at which energy is at such low ebb
we can see a similarity to all four models of affect. that exhaustion occurs. In my experience, this state
Thus, the four most prominent models of mood are of exhaustion may be quite pleasant, but in this
compatible. condition an individual operates almost like an
automaton in which behavior is directed cognitively
How Complex Moods Are Formed by without the usual subjective feedback that comes
Energetic and Tense Arousal Interactions from sensing energy resources to sustain ongo-
As indicated earlier, the four most prominent ing behavior. Although this may be experienced as
two-dimensional models of affect or mood are usu- a pleasant waking state, rest or sleep is the primary
ally derived by cross-sectional studies in which all motivational directive. There is also some indication
levels of both dimensions are represented (e.g., Yik that this exhaustion state leaves one particularly vul-
et al., 1999). But in everyday experience changing nerable to serious physical breakdown (e.g., Prescott
activating conditions influence the two dimensions et al., 2003).
differentially. Thus, in my model, as activating These interactions between the two arousal
conditions increase tense arousal, energetic arousal dimensions carry a number of motivational implica-
increases as well, yielding the complex mood of tions. For example, at different times of day differ-
tense energy. But this only occurs up to a moderate ent activities are likely to be more or less attractive.
level of activating conditions. Beyond that as tense In the first part of the day as subjective energy
arousal increases further, energetic arousal begins to increases in its natural circadian cycle physical activ-
decline, eventually resulting in the complex mood ity is likely to be more attractive. But in the last part

th ayer 
HIGH
Subjective ENERGY

SELF-RATINGS
Subjective TENSION

MOD

LOW

LOW MODERATE HIGH


ACTIVATING CONDITIONS
Some influencing factors: parceived danger, stress, pain, noise

HIGH
SELF-RATINGS

MOD

LOW

LOW MODERATE HIGH


ACTIVATING CONDITIONS
Some influencing factors: circadian rhythms, exercise, food and
drug substances, cognition

Fig. 23.3. Interactions of energy and tension under different activating conditions. (Reproduced from Thayer, 1996, Oxford
University Press.)

of the day as energy declines and tiredness increases tension. I regard this as a Zen-like state in which
physical activity is likely to be less attractive. Like- there are little or no tension-related stress reactions,
wise, cognitive demands probably vary in degree of a state in which natural cycles of greater and lesser
attractiveness. energy occur, but tension is absent or at a low level.
Calm energy also bears a similarity, in my view,
Calm Energy to Csikszentmihalyi’s flow (1990), particularly on
It is useful to describe further the combination of the basis of pleasurable attentional focus with full
energetic and tense arousal because this complex state involvement. I also think of calm energy as enhanc-
relates to common emotions and thus leads to greater ing athletic performance. In this regard, Morgan’s
understanding of mood. One primary component is (e.g., Morgan & Pollock, 1978) concept of the ice-
calm energy, the mixed mood state that I regard as berg profile (high vigor coupled with low negative
most pleasurable and in many respects optimal for mood), which characterizes world-class athletes,
cognitive and physical functioning in waking hours is relevant. Probably related to this are anecdotal
when activity is appropriate. This mood is often not descriptions by athletes when their performance is so
fully recognized. For example, in one talk I gave, outstanding that they are in the zone. Calm energy
a rather intense young man challenged the concept also is an optimal predisposition in the martial arts
by saying that he does not understand calm energy such as karate and judo and is a valued state dur-
because whenever he feels energetic he always feels ing the meditation in movement of Tai Chi Chuan.
slightly wired. (The feeling he was describing was Calm energy is such an attractive state that people
a state I would call tense energy, a common mood in may seek it through self-medicating drugs such as
today’s stress-filled society.) caffeine, nicotine, and amphetamines.
Calm energy does exist during waking hours
(primarily higher activity hours of day) when people Tense Tiredness
experiencing it feel energetic and yet are very calm. The bipolar opposite to calm energy in psycho-
In this state they experience little general muscular metric space is tense tiredness, a complex mood that

 m oods of energy and tension th at moti vate


is as negative as calm energy is positive. As resources to be confused with the trait of happiness, which we
decline and feelings of energy change to tiredness, know is often correlated with the trait of extraver-
vulnerability to stressful circumstances develops sion (Pavot, Diener, & Fujita, 1990). Instead, it has
and tension arises. This state often develops late in to do with the state or the actual experience when
the afternoon or later in the evening when stress is one is feeling happiness (Yik et al., 1999). This calm
present. But tense tiredness may occur at any time energetic state is also related to feelings of optimism,
when personal resources are depleted and stressful a time when problems are perceived as most easily
circumstances are present. solvable (Thayer, 1987b).
Although optimism and happiness are calm ener- In my view, anger is likely to represent a combi-
getic states, feelings of depression and pessimism nation of energy and tension (tense energy). I base
about the future are fostered by tense tiredness. As this idea on the observation that anger is dimin-
we shall see later when I discuss self-regulation, tense ished by extreme fatigue (moderate fatigue may
tiredness is a state that people self-regulate to feel increase tension and disinhibit angry thoughts).
better, often with food or drugs. For example, there Additionally, one can observe that when angry and
is good evidence tense tiredness motivates breaking beginning to exercise—a common way of increasing
diets (also succumbing for other proscribed sub- energy—anger may increase at least until the exer-
stances such as cigarettes) (Thayer, 2001). And in cise leads to exhaustion. Finally, boredom is often
general, food urges are motivated by tense tiredness misunderstood as a low arousal state, but it is better
and overeating can be traced to this mood state. interpreted as a state of tense tiredness (cf Berlyne,
1960). Boredom has been found to be an anteced-
Tense Energy ent to overeating, and I have interpreted this as an
Two other complex mood states should be example of self-regulation (Thayer, 2001).
described to complete the picture of the interac-
tions of energetic and tense arousal. One very General Bodily Arousal
common complex mood will be familiar to many General bodily arousal which underlies the mood
people engaged in high-energy productive activity, dimensions that I have proposed represents interre-
but where stress-related tension is present. This is lated multiple systems within the body on an arousal
tense energy. Like calm energy, tense energy is also continuum that is associated with energy expendi-
positively evaluated by many people. It is a state ture. When a resting individual becomes physically
that combines energy and tension together. This is a active, a wide variety of bodily systems are mobilized
common condition of a modern stress-filled society as energy expenditure increases. A similar pattern of
in which people may be quite productive and feel mobilization occurs as a sleeping individual becomes
energetic but are never fully relaxed. maximally alert or as a calm person becomes intensely
emotional. This mobilization occurs in a more or less
Calm Tired integrated fashion, although the pattern of activa-
The fourth complex mood to be described is tion across bodily systems is not perfectly correlated.
calm tiredness. This state is optimal for sleep and Reduced intraindividual correlation is likely due to
is often sought through drugs by tired people who differing latencies and strength of system responses.
need sleep but suffer from the common condi- Plus there are unique responsibilities of each system
tion of insomnia, which usually is due to low-level for bodily homeostasis.
tense tiredness. In my view these sleep aids are not That a broad pattern of integration generally is
nearly as effective for restful sleep as naturally occur- the rule as the individual changes from low to high
ring tiredness together with the absence of tension arousal with increasing amounts of energy expen-
because they involve side effects and they interfere diture is evident by comparing system levels from
with natural bodily processes. states of baseline or low arousal and reduced energy
Other commonly experienced emotions may expenditure to states of high arousal and high-energy
be understood in relation to these interactions of expenditure. As energy expenditure increases, each
energetic and tense arousal. Mentioned earlier is the system evidences its own activation pattern. These
observation that depression is a condition of low patterns are not perfectly correlated, but the general
energy and increased tension (especially agitated interrelationship is apparent with reference to the
depression). Other common emotions that can be low and high end points of energy expenditure.
understood in this context are happiness, optimism, To pursue this matter a bit further, consider an
anger, and boredom. Feelings of happiness are not example of a resting individual who stands and

th ayer 
begins to physically exercise (e.g., walks with increas- response. The fight-or-flight pattern made famous
ing rapidity). Bodily arousal is reflected in a very by Walter Cannon 100 years ago is preceded in dan-
general way throughout the body with increased ger situations by freezing, which optimizes avoid-
cell metabolism, respiration, heart rate, blood pres- ance of detection (Thayer, 1989). Although the
sure, adrenaline, cortisol, and other physiological fight-or-flight response is much better known than
systems associated with energy expenditure. At the the kind of freeze response to which I refer, Can-
brain level, infusions of neurotransmitters such as non himself did note this initial reaction to danger
norepinephrine, dopamine, and serotonin mediate (Cannon, 1929/1963). In my view, this activated
bodily arousal. freeze response plays an important role in the every-
In my view, growing subjective energy and ten- day experience of such states as nervousness, jitteri-
sion are part of this integrated pattern that consti- ness, agitation, anxiety, and fear. This tension state
tutes general bodily arousal as increased feelings of (with the subjective opposite of calmness) experi-
energy and tension occur (Thayer, 1970, 1989). enced on a chronic level can produce headaches, as
To summarize, in the bigger picture the subjec- well as pain in the jaw, back, and shoulders.
tive experience of energy and tension can be traced Besides the degree and type of skeletal-muscular
throughout the body from a basic level of the cel- tension that differentiates energetic and tense
lular mitochondria, up through neurotransmitter arousal, other physiological differences probably
effects on relevant brain mechanisms (Brown, 1999; exist such as the particular physiological patterns
Duffy, 1962; Malmo, 1975; Thayer, 1989). Subjec- that differentiate adrenaline and cortisol (Dienst-
tive energy and tension are the conscious represen- bier, 1989). Moreover, at the level of brain processes
tations of this general bodily arousal pattern. there undoubtedly are differences leading to the two
kinds of subjective experience associated with the
Muscular Tension: The Activated Freeze two dimensions of energy-tiredness and tension-
Response calmness. Relevant here would be brain structures
The similar activation pattern for both energetic such as the reticular activating system, the limbic
and tense arousal follows from a biological perspec- system, and cerebral cortical lateralized activation.
tive since both arousal systems predispose mobili-
zation for action—ongoing actions with energetic Self-Regulation of Mood
arousal and preparation for (emergency) action People seek pleasure and avoid pain. This hedo-
with tense arousal. The most definitive difference nic principle has governed my work in relation to
between the two arousal patterns is in affect. How- the self-regulation of mood (Thayer, 1989). When
ever, there are other differences as well, although the behavior is not required by schedules and previously
full extent of the psychophysiological differences, made plans, when there is choice about what to do
including brain patterns, is not fully known. But next, the preferred chosen behavior follows this
several differences should be noted. hedonic principle. Sometimes this involves aware-
One physiological difference may be that tense ness of a negative mood followed by a conscious
arousal is associated more with anaerobic metabo- decision to take some action to feel better. But often
lism, whereas energetic arousal reflects aerobic there is only a low-level awareness of this process.
metabolism (Thayer, 1989). For example, prepara- In general, this probably is initiated by a thought
tory emergency arousal in the case of tension dif- about a pleasant activity that is sufficiently attractive
fers from energetic arousal at least in respect to to motivate action.
skeletal-muscular activation. Thus, subjective states I maintain that people prefer moods of increased
of tension, stress, and anxiety are characterized energy and reduced tension (calm energy), and they
by preparatory muscle tension with an absence of behave in various ways to achieve this state. They
directed motor activity. As one good indication, also act in various ways to reduce moods of tension
muscles around the thoracic cavity are tight, reflect- and tiredness (tense tiredness). The direction of the
ing a pattern of restraint. Exemplifying this, breath- preferred motivational process is from tense tired-
ing occurs at the top of the lungs in a short panting ness (mood to be avoided) and toward calm energy
pattern. But with energetic arousal that is associated (optimal mood). For some types of people (e.g.,
with ongoing activity, diaphramatic breathing is Type As), however, tense energy may be preferred.
more the rule. Furthermore, when sleep is desired, the motiva-
The skeletal-muscular inhibition associated with tional direction would involve change from tense
tension reflects what I have called the activated freeze tiredness to calm tiredness.

 m oods of energy and tension th at moti vate


To test this theory, my colleagues and I con- choices involved cognitive responses, thus indicating
ducted a series of correlational studies in which a the importance that cognition has in mood regula-
representative sample of adults from high school age tion. This is a point that I have made in many ways,
through mid-eighties were first questioned about including the idea that the differences between emo-
what they do when they are in a bad mood (also tions must involve the interactions of energetic and
need increased energy and reduced tension) and tense arousal and cognitive interpretations (Thayer,
what works (Thayer, Newman, & McClain, 1994). 1989, 1996).
Hundreds of ways to self-regulate their mood were We also looked at the 29 categories of mood
indicated and these were content analyzed yielding regulation chosen by our research participants using
29 categories of behaviors people regularly employ factor analyses. Six factors were readily interpre-
to try to change a bad mood. The categories that table, and I have come to look at these factors as
are used were then placed in an anonymous ques- mood regulation strategies that have varying degrees
tionnaire that was administered to a representative of effectiveness. These degrees of effectiveness were
sample of 308 respondents from 16 to 89 years who judged both by our participants and also by a panel
indicated methods they usually use to change a bad of 26 doctoral-level psychotherapists who graciously
mood as well as the most common method they use. volunteered to make these kinds of assessments.
This most common method was then rated for suc- Of the six interpretable factors, the first one
cess. Finally, people were given the opportunity to which we named Active Mood Management was
indicate things they do to change a bad mood but judged the most effective both by our research par-
were not included in the 29 categories. Since none ticipants and the panel of psychotherapists. This
of these alternatives appeared to be sufficiently dif- strategy nicely supported the mood theory that was
ferent from the list of 29, we assumed that the list one of the bases of our research. The five most highly
was fairly all inclusive. loaded items of this strategy included, first and sec-
Of the 29 categories the most common response ond, two ways for reducing tense arousal: relaxation
involved seeking social interaction (i.e., call, talk to, techniques (e.g., deep breathing, stretching, muscle
or be with someone) and this was endorsed by 54% relaxation), and stress management activities (e.g.,
of the respondents. Since we collected demographic get organized, plan ahead). And the fifth item on
information it was possible to determine that this most effective strategy included the way that
females were significantly more likely to endorse was judged the most effective way of changing
this activity, but many males chose this activity as a bad mood: exercise. The third and fourth items on
well. Males, on the other hand, were significantly this most effective strategy were cognitive items: put
more likely to endorse the second more common feelings in perspective and evaluate or analyze the
response (51%), control thoughts (i.e., think posi- situation. Thus, the best strategy for changing a bad
tively, concentrate on something else, don’t let mood involves simultaneously reducing tension,
things bother, give self pep talk), but many females raising energy, and employing cognitive control.
chose this way as well.
The third most common response and judged Conclusion
second in effectiveness (listen to music, 47%) was The moods that influence our lives in all major
a surprise to me at the time of the research, but since aspects are affected in one biopsychological dimension
that time it has become more evident that our find- by natural processes such as health, sleep, diet, and
ing of a median split indicting that younger people exercise (a go system) and in the second dimension by
were significantly more likely to chose this response stress and perceived danger (a stop system). Complex
than older people was valid. With audiences to moods arising from interactions of these dimensions
whom I have spoken involving many young people, account for many elements of motivation. These are
music listening to regulate mood is endorsed by the not insignificant feelings, but rather they are excellent
vast majority by a show of hands. Why listening to barometers of the overall psychology and physiology
music would be so prevalent in regulating mood is of the individual. One of the most important impli-
not clear, but this mood regulation method cur- cations for motivation is the way that people both
rently is a vigorous research area, some focusing consciously and unconsciously self-regulate these
on the mood variables that I have proposed (e.g., moods in a wide variety of ways, and this generally
Hirokawa, 2004; Lim, 2008). follows the hedonic principle of motivation to opti-
Although seeking social interaction was the most mize positive moods such as calm energy and reduce
commonly endorsed item, three of the first seven negative moods such as tense tiredness.

th ayer 
Future Directions Ganley, R. M. (1989). Emotion and eating in obesity: A review
of the literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 8,
Further empirical and theoretical research could
342–361
productively focus on the way that complex moods Gilbert, D. G. (1979). Paradoxical tranquilizing and emotion-
occur from the interactions of the two major biopsy- reducing effects of nicotine. Psychological Bulletin, 86,
chological dimensions. This suggests potential solu- 643–661.
tions for previous points of confusion such as those Gold, A. E., MacLeod, K. M., Deary, I. J., & Frier, B. M. (1995).
Changes in mood during acute hypoglycemia in healthy
concerning “paradoxical” effects of nicotine—that
participants. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68,
cigarettes both activate and deactivate simultane- 498–504.
ously (Gilbert, 1979). Other seemingly paradoxical Hall, E. C., Ekkekakis, P., & Petruzello, S. J. (2007). Regional
effects of drugs such as caffeine and cocaine may be brain activity and strenuous exercise: Predicting affective
important in this same respect. Likewise, these seem- responses using EEG asymmetry. Biological Psychology, 75,
194–200.
ingly paradoxical effects occur with hyperactivity,
Hirokawa, E. (2004). Effects of music listening and relax-
a condition in which activating drugs such as caffeine ation instructions on arousal changes and the working
and behaviors such as exercise appear to improve memory task in older adults. Journal of Music Therapy, 41,
the condition. And in the exercise science literature, 107–127.
one finds similar dual arousal effects such as the fact Keys, A., Brozek, J., Henchel, A., Mickelsen, O., & Taylor, H. L.
(1950). The biology of human starvation. Minneapolis: Uni-
that vigorous exercise produces calmness and tired-
versity of Minnesota Press.
ness simultaneously (Hall, Ekkekakis, Petruzello, Larsen, R. J., & Diener, E. (1992). Promises and problems with
2007). These seemingly paradoxical effects may the circumplex model of emotion. Review of Personality and
be understood, of course, by the orthogonality of Social Psychology, 13, 25–59.
energetic and tense arousal in cross-sectional stud- Lim, H. A. (2008). The effect of personality type and musical
task on self-perceived arousal. Journal of Music Therapy, 45,
ies but the complex interaction of these dimen-
147–164.
sions at different activation levels. Finally, what is Malmo, R. B. (1975). On emotions, needs, and our archaic brain.
not known well is how best to designate the mod- New York: Holt
erate point at which increasing tension leads first Morgan, W. P., & Pollock, M. L. (1978). Psychological charac-
to increased energy but at some point to reduced terization of the elite distance runner. Annals of the New York
Academy of Science, 301, 382–403.
energy. Similarly, what is not known well is how
Murrey, E. J. (1965). Sleep, dreams and arousal. New York:
to designate the moderate point at which increas- Appleton-Century-Crofts.
ing energy can lead first to increasing tension (in Pavot, W., Diener, E., & Fujita, F. (1990). Extraversion and
a stressful context) but at some moderate point to happiness. Personality and Individual Differences, 11,
reduced tension. 1299–1306.
Prescott, E., Holst, C., Gronbaek, M., Schnohr, P., Jensen, G., &
Barefoot, J. (2003). Vital exhaustion as a risk factor for
Acknowledgment ischaemic heart disease and all-cause mortality in a com-
Thanks to Martin Fiebert and Ralph Hupka for comments munity sample. International Journal of Epidemiology, 32,
on a draft of this chapter. 990–997
Reed, J., & Ones, D. S. (2006). The effect of acute aerobic exer-
References cise on positive activated affect: A meta-analysis. Psychology of
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York: Sport and Exercise, 75, 477–514.
McGraw-Hill Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1161–1178.
Bulletin, 99, 229–246. Russell, J. A. (2003). Core affect and the psychological construc-
Brown, G. (1999). The energy of life. New York: The Free Press tion of emotion. Psychological Review, 110, 145–172.
Cannon, W. B. (1929/1963). Bodily changes in pain, hunger, fear Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). From ego depletion to vital-
and rage. New York: Harper & Row. ity: Theory and findings concerning the facilitation of energy
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1982). Control theory: A use- available to the self. Social and Personality Psychology Compass,
ful conceptual framework for personality-social, clinical, and 2, 702–717.
health psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 111–135. Ryan, R. M., & Frederick, C. (1997). On energy, personality
Csíkszentmihályi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal and health: Subjective vitality as a dynamic reflection of well-
experience. New York: Harper and Row being. Journal of Personality, 65, 529–565.
Dixon, J. K., Dixon, J. P., & Hickey, M. (1993). Energy as Ryan, R. M., Williams, G. C., Patrick, H., & Deci, E. L. (2009).
a central factor in the self-assessment of health. Advances in Self-determination theory and physical activity: The dynam-
Nursing Science, 15, 1–12. ics of motivation in development and wellness. Hellenic Jour-
Dienstbier, R. A. (1989). Arousal and physiological toughness: nal of Psychology, 6, 107–124.
Implications for mental and physical health. Psychological Segerstrom, S. C. (2007). Stress, energy, and immunity: An eco-
Review, 96, 84–100. logical view. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16,
Duffy, E. (1962). Activation and behavior. New York: Wiley. 326–330.

 m oods of energy and tension th at moti vate


Thayer, R. E. (1970). Activation states as assessed by verbal Thayer, R.E. (2000). Mood. In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia
report and four psychophysiological variables. Psychophysiol- of psychology (pp. 394–395). Washington, DC: Oxford Uni-
ogy, 7, 86–94. versity Press and American Psychological Association.
Thayer, R. E. (1978). Toward a psychological theory of multi- Thayer, R. E. (2001). Calm energy: How people regulate mood with
dimensional activation (Arousal). Motivation and Emotion, food and exercise. New York: Oxford University Press
2, 1–33. Thayer, R. E. (2009). Tension. In D. Sander & K. R. Scherer
Thayer, R. E. (1987a). Energy, tiredness, and tension effects of (Eds.), Emotion and the affective sciences (pp. 390–391). New
a sugar snack versus moderate exercise. Journal of Personality York: Oxford University Press.
and Social Psychology, 52, 119–125. Thayer, R. E., Peters, D. P., Takahashi, P. J., & Birkhead-Flight, A.
Thayer, R. E. (1987b). Problem perception, optimism, and M. (1993). Mood and behavior (smoking and sugar snacking)
related states as a function of time of day (diurnal rhythm) following moderate exercise: A partial test of self-regulation
and moderate exercise: Two arousal systems in interaction. theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 14, 97–104.
Motivation and Emotion, 11, 19–36. Thayer, R. E., Takahashi, P. J., & Pauli, J. S. (1988). Multidimen-
Thayer, R. E. (1989). The biopsychology of mood and arousal. sional arousal states, diurnal rhythms, cognitive and social
New York: Oxford University Press. processes, and extraversion. Personality and Individual Dif-
Thayer, R. E., Newman, J., & McClain, T. M. (1994). Self- ferences, 9, 15–24.
regulation of mood: Strategies for changing a bad mood, Watson, D., & Tellegen, A. (1985). Toward a consensual struc-
raising energy and reducing tension. Journal of Personality ture of mood. Psychological Bulletin, 98, 219–235.
and Social Psychology, 67, 910–925. Yik, M. S. M., Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (1999). Structure of
Thayer, R. E. (1996). The origin of everyday moods. New York: self-reported current affect: Integration and beyond. Journal
Oxford University Press. of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 600–619.

th ayer 
CHAPTER

Effort Intensity: Some Insights From


24 the Cardiovascular System

Guido H. E. Gendolla, Rex A. Wright, and Michael Richter

Abstract
This chapter deals with the psychological process that determines effort intensity in instrumental
behavior. According to motivation intensity theory, effort should be proportional to experienced task
difficulty as long as success is possible and justified and low when success is impossible or excessively
difficult, given the available benefit. When task difficulty is unspecified or unknown, effort should be
proportional to the importance of success. We report a program of experimental studies that have
operationalized effort intensity as cardiovascular reactivity during task performance and used multiple
manipulations of variables influencing subjective task difficulty (e.g., performance standards, ability, mood)
and the amount of justified effort (e.g., material incentive, instrumentality, evaluation). The empirical
evidence is in clear support of the principles of motivation intensity theory and challenges a number of
other theoretical accounts. Directions for future research are discussed.
Key Words: effort, motivation intensity, cardiovascular response

Introduction effort is mobilized. Over the years, this basic princi-


Motivation can be briefly defined as the pro- ple has been elaborated and modified, but it still has
cess that determines the direction and energiza- a major impact on current research. Its most influ-
tion of behavior (Elliot, 2006). Traditionally, the ential elaboration has been formulated in Brehm’s
first aspect—what people do—has received more motivation intensity theory (Brehm & Self, 1989;
attention in motivation research than the second Brehm, Wright, Solomon, Silka, & Greenberg,
aspect—how much effort people mobilize to exe- 1983). In the first part of this chapter we will dis-
cute instrumental behavior. However, over the last cuss this theory and studies that have been inspired
25 years a number of variables have been identified by it. In the second part we will discuss the rela-
that influence effort intensity—that is, resource tion of this approach to other theoretical accounts
mobilization for instrumental behavior at a point in as well as future directions for research.
time (Gendolla & Wright, 2009). Drawing on the
idea that effort has the function to deal with obsta- Motivation Intensity Theory
cles in the goal pursuit process, it was postulated at Motivation intensity theory (Brehm et al., 1983;
the beginning of the last century that effort mobi- Brehm & Self, 1989) draws on the basic idea that
lization follows the “difficulty law of motivation” individuals try to avoid wasting resources. Follow-
(e.g., Ach, 1935; Hillgruber, 1912). Accordingly, ing this resource conservation principle, performers
effort is mobilized in proportion to the experienced are expected to expend effort (1) only to the degree
difficulty of instrumental behavior—the higher the that is needed, and (2) only when expenditure yields
obstacles encountered during goal pursuit, the more a return (i.e., a benefit) justifying the effort expended.


The need for effort is determined by the difficulty To illustrate, first consider a person offered at
of instrumental behavior, which is the difficulty of different points in time $10, $20, and $30 to lift
activity that must be carried out to attain desired a weight that he or she could lift and was willing
outcomes and avoid aversive ones. Thus, a central to lift for $10. The present view suggests that the
proposition of motivation intensity theory is that person’s effort should correspond to the difficulty
effort investment should vary nonmonotonically of the lift, and not the value of the incentive, at the
with the perceived difficulty of imminent or ongo- different points. Next, consider what would happen
ing behavior (see Ach, 1935; Hillgruber, 1912). As under conditions where this person was unwilling
long as success is viewed as both possible and worth to lift the weight even for $30. The present view
the investment that it will require, effort should suggests that his or her effort should be consistently
correspond to difficulty. That is, performers should low at the different points. Finally, consider what
invest greater effort the more difficult they perceive would happen if the person were offered the dif-
their task to be. On the other hand, if success is ferent incentives for lifting a weight that he or she
viewed as impossible or excessively difficult, given knew exceeded his or her strength. Once again,
the benefit that it will accrue, then effort should effort should be low at all points.
be low (Fig. 24.1). Effort should also be low in the
impossible case because it would yield no return, Where Difficulty Is Unknown
and it should be low in the excessively difficult case A further point to note about the central propo-
because it would yield a return lower than the value sition discussed earlier is that it assumes that per-
of the effort expended. formers know what will be required to succeed,
which will not always be the case. Consider here,
The Role of Benefit for example, (1) a student confronted with an exam
An important point about the central proposi- from a new teacher, (2) a woman woken up in the
tion mentioned earlier is that it suggests that con- middle of the night by a noise in her bedroom closet,
siderations of benefit in performance circumstances or (3) a soldier confronted with random sniper fire
do not determine effort directly. Rather, they deter- over the course of a morning. People sometimes
mine effort indirectly by setting the upper limit of know that action is called for but are unsure what
what is justified and what performers are willing to exactly needs to be done and how much effort needs
do. Brehm referred to this upper limit as the per- to be mobilized for it. Motivation intensity theory
former’s level of potential motivation. In theory, fac- asserts that effort in such circumstances will be pro-
tors related to benefit (e.g., the value of and need portional to potential motivation.
for an available incentive) should have no impact At first glance, the preceding assertion might
on effort as long as success is possible and benefit is seem contrary to the guiding principle of resource
great enough to justify the required effort. conservation. However, it arguably is not when one

(a) (b)
(Potential Motivation)
Effort

Effort

(Potential Motivation)

Unspecified Low Moderate High Extreme Unspecified Low Moderate High Extreme
Task Difficulty Task Difficulty

Fig. 24.1. Theoretical predictions of the joint impact of task difficulty and potential motivation on effort intensity. A shows predictions
for effort mobilization when low effort is justified (i.e., low potential motivation). B shows predictions for the condition that high effort
is justified (i.e., high potential motivation). (Adapted from Gendolla & Wright, 2009, p. 134. Copyright: Oxford University Press.)

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


considers that using potential motivation to guide benefit directly, but rather with the difficulty of
effort investment allows performers to avoid invest- behavior necessary to attain goals. So long as success
ing more resources than is justified. Given that is possible and worthwhile, effort should correspond
performers do not know what will be required to to difficulty. Where success is deemed impossible or
succeed when task difficulty is unknown, they are excessively difficult, given the available benefit, effort
at risk of investing more than necessary. However, should be low. In theory, the role of benefit should be
by investing effort proportional to potential motiva- to determine the upper limit of what performers will
tion they can assure that they will never exceed the be willing to do, thus determining the drop point of
amount of justified effort. effort along possible levels of a difficulty continuum.
Motivation intensity theory recognizes that people
Where Difficulty Is Not Fixed sometimes believe that action is—or might be-
A third point to note about the central propo- needed, but they are unsure what exactly needs to be
sition discussed earlier is that it recognizes that done and that people sometimes can decide on their
performance contingencies (i.e., benefits) do not own how much effort they want to invest. In such
always have an all-or-none character. The contin- circumstances, people are expected to expend effort
gencies sometimes do, as would be the case if a per- in proportion to their potential motivation—that is,
son learned that he or she could earn an amount of their willingness to act.
money by lifting a given amount of weight. How-
ever, they sometimes do not. Consider, for example, Measuring Effort
a person told that he or she can earn $1 for every lift Motivation intensity theory provides an elegant
he or she makes or, alternatively, a child told he or picture of effort investment. However, its validity
she can earn a bite of dessert for every bite of broc- is by no means self-evident. Like any other theory
coli that he or she takes. In circumstances like these, worth serious consideration, it requires empirical
benefit rises in constant proportion to performance testing and thus a measure of effort intensity. One
quality. Brehm referred to such circumstances as idea to quantify effort intensity, applied for decades,
ones in which difficulty is not fixed (e.g., Brehm has been to obtain self-reports of effort mobiliza-
& Self, 1989). Additionally, persons can be asked tion under different task conditions (e.g., Efklides,
to do their best (or what they want) without being Kourkoulou, Mitsiou, & Ziliaskopoulou, 2006;
confronted with a clear performance standard (see Meyer & Hallermann, 1977; Roets, Van Hiel, Cor-
Locke & Latham, 1990). Also under this condition nelis, & Soetens, 2008). However, self-report mea-
task difficulty is not fixed. sures of effort are problematic for several reasons. For
Once again, motivation intensity theory assumes one thing, effort self-reports have been shown to be
that effort will be proportional to potential motiva- highly vulnerable to self-presentational influences
tion (total benefit that can be accrued) up to the (Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1983; Rhodewalt &
point that people can try no harder. Thus, the per- Fairfield, 1991). In addition, there is concern that
son in the previous example would be expected to people might not always apprehend how hard
exert more effort the more money he or she could they are (or are not) trying, because introspection
make until attaining the effort peak. Similarly, the abilities are (very) limited (Wilson, 2002). A sec-
child would be expected to exert more effort the ond idea has been to measure performance—an
more delectable the available dessert until he or outcome that multiple motivation research-
she attained his or her effort peak and persons who ers have directly linked to the intensity aspect of
are asked to do their best should invest more effort motivation (e.g., Aarts, Custers, & Marien, 2008;
the higher the benefits of success. Arguably, under- Atkinson & Raynor, 1974; Bandura & Cervone,
lying processes are similar to those operating in 1983; Eisenberger, 1992; Kukla, 1972; Locke &
unknown difficulty circumstances. Specifically, Latham, 1990). However, this is problematic as
performers (1) aim to perform at the highest level well because of obvious disconnections between
that is both possible and justified, and (2) expend effort and different performance outcomes, includ-
effort in proportion to the difficulty of that behavior ing the speed and quality of responses (see Har-
(Wright & Kirby, 2001). kins, 2006). Consider, for example, two people,
one asked to memorize two nonsense trigrams
Summary and the other asked to memorize four. Within a
To summarize, motivation intensity theory main- period of 2 minutes, they both would be likely to
tains that effort intensity does not vary with potential succeed and therefore perform perfectly. But they

 effort intensit y


would have to expend different degrees of effort With the preceding background in place, we can
to achieve their success. Moreover, performance is now turn to relevant studies. The studies can be
determined by more factors than effort alone—at organized into two groups: (1) ones that addressed
least ability and strategy use also have (and some- basic implications of the motivation intensity analy-
times even stronger) influences on it (see Locke & sis and focused on psychological variables influenc-
Latham, 1990). ing subjective task difficulty, and (2) studies that
A third idea is, in our view, more promising. It is focused on manipulations of variables that should
to assess effort physiologically, that is, by examining impact potential motivation—the level of maxi-
adjustments in bodily systems that should—in the- mally justified effort—and its interaction with task
ory—be involved in mobilizing people for action. difficulty.
This approach (1) takes as a given that effort either
leads to energization or is simply part and parcel Empirical Evidence
of the energy mobilization process, and (2) draws We will now present a series of studies that
attention to the cardiovascular system. have systematically investigated the principles of
motivation intensity theory and operationalized
Effort-Related Cardiovascular Reactivity effort intensity as cardiovascular response—that is,
Effort investigators have looked to the cardiovas- performance-related changes in cardiovascular activ-
cular system for two reasons. One reason is because ity with reference to baseline values. The experimen-
there is growing agreement that the cardiovascu- tal protocol of a typical study consists of two phases.
lar system functions chiefly to sustain behavior Participants are first habituated to the laboratory for
(Papillo & Shapiro, 1990). The second is because a period of about 10 minutes. During that time,
research in psychophysiology—particularly that by participants are inactive and cardiovascular baseline
Elliott (1969) and Obrist (1976)—has indicated activity is assessed. Then participants work on a task,
that effort not only affects cardiovascular responses typically for about 5 minutes, and cardiovascular
but does so by way of certain sympathetic nervous activity is again assessed during task performance.
system mechanisms, that is, mechanisms associated In some studies, cardiovascular activity was addi-
with the branch of the autonomic nervous system tionally assessed immediately before task perfor-
involved in activation. It is beyond the scope of mance. Participants’ cardiovascular reactivity—the
this chapter to delve into all details of sympathetic dependent variable referring to effort intensity—is
cardiovascular influence or to examine carefully the expressed in the mean performance-related changes
evidence linking effort to it. For present purposes, in cardiovascular activity with reference to the indi-
it is sufficient to make two observations. First, in vidual baseline values.
theory (Wright, 1996), the best indicators of the
sympathetic mechanisms mentioned earlier should The Role of Variables Affecting Difficulty
be heart contraction force (i.e., cardiac contrac- Numerous studies have investigated simple task
tility—typically measured as pre-ejection period difficulty effects on effort-related cardiovascular
[PEP]) and outcomes affected by it, most notably response. Examples are experiments conducted by
systolic blood pressure (SBP—pressure at the peak Wright (1984) and Smith, Baldwin, and Christensen
of a pulse). Heart rate (HR—the pace of heart (1990). Wright (1984) told some participants that
contraction) should tend to be indicative as well. they could avoid an electric shock by performing
However, it is affected not only by sympathetic an easy or difficult motor activity (a toggle switch
activity but also by parasympathetic activity that grip or dynamometer grip, respectively) and other
can sometimes mask or even reverse the relevant participants that they were in a control condition
sympathetic effects. Thus, HR must be interpreted in which no (shock) avoidant behavior would be
cautiously in performance contexts. Other com- made available. HR data collected in the 30 seconds
mon cardiovascular parameters may have also some prior to a point at which participants were to either
potential for reflecting effort, but less for reasons (1) perform their task (avoidance conditions) or (2)
that we will not go into here. Interested readers are pause for further instructions (no avoidance) indi-
thus referred to relevant discussions by Berntson, cated greater responsiveness in the difficult avoid-
Cacioppo, and Quigley (1993), Brownley, Hurwitz, ance condition. A measure of finger pulse volume
and Schneiderman, (2000), Kelsey (2011), Obrist (reflecting digital blood flow) indicated a similar
(1981), Papillo and Shapiro (1990), and Wright response pattern. Smith et al. (1990) provided par-
and Kirby (2001). ticipants the chance to earn a monetary incentive

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


by making a speech that was mildly, moderately, or to the features of the task), with less capable per-
extremely convincing to an audience. As expected, formers viewing success at any given objective dif-
SBP, diastolic blood pressure (DBP—pressure ficulty level as harder than more capable performers
between pulses) and HR responses measured just (Heider, 1958; Hockey, 1997; Kukla, 1972; cf. Ban-
before and during the speech were greater when the dura, 1982, 1986). It follows that (1) effort should
performance standard was moderate than when it be stronger for low- than high-ability performers as
was low or extreme. long as the low-ability performers perceive success
The most recent study to provide documenta- as both possible and worthwhile; (2) low-ability per-
tion for simple difficulty effects is an experiment formers should withhold effort and display reduced
by Richter, Friedrich, and Gendolla (2008) that cardiovascular responses at a lower difficulty level
included a measure of cardiac contractility (PEP) as than should high-ability performers, creating a win-
well as measures of blood pressure and HR. Note dow of difficulty levels within which effort is weaker
that PEP is assessed in milliseconds and increased for low- than high-ability performers; and (3) effort
contractility is expressed by negative PEP reactivity should be low for both ability groups under condi-
values. The study presented participants a recogni- tions where success calls for more than high-ability
tion memory task that required them to indicate performers can or will do (Fig. 24.3).
repeatedly whether a probe character was in a pre- One of the earliest ability studies (Wright, Wadley,
ceding character string (Sternberg, 1966). Difficulty Pharr, & Butler, 1994) examined ability as a mea-
was manipulated by displaying the initial string for sured, rather than a manipulated, factor. Investigators
1,000 milliseconds (difficulty low), 550 milliseconds first administered in a mass testing session an ability
(difficulty moderate), 100 milliseconds (difficulty questionnaire that allowed them to identify students
high), or 15 milliseconds (difficulty extreme). As who viewed themselves as highly incapable or highly
expected, contractility and SBP responses assessed capable with respect to math. They then invited the
during performance rose progressively from the targeted students to participate in a study that gave
low- to the moderate- to the high-difficulty condi- them the chance to avoid noise by meeting a particu-
tion, and then dropped (Fig. 24.2). lar performance standard on a set of math problems.
For some students, the problems were described as
ability and difficulty easy; for others, they were described as difficult; for
An ability elaboration from the motivation still others, they were described as extremely difficult.
intensity analysis, which has been discussed in detail Analysis of cardiovascular responses measured just
elsewhere (e.g., Wright, 1998), builds on two ideas. before the performance period showed a difficulty
The first is that the proximal determinant of effort x ability interaction for SBP. Among high-ability par-
should be performers’ personal appraisals of how ticipants, anticipatory responses rose across the three
difficult it will be to attain a goal. The second is that difficulty levels. Among low-ability participants,
personal difficulty appraisals should be influenced they were higher at the easy- and high-difficulty lev-
by the performer’s ability (i.e., capacity with respect els, but low at the highest difficulty level.

Panel A- PEP Reactivity Panel B- SBP Reactivity


2
10
Reactivity Scores (in mmHg)
Reactivity Scores (in ms)

0
8

–2 6

–4 4

–6 2

–8 0
Low Moderate High Impossible Low Moderate High Impossible
Task Difficulty Task Difficulty

Fig. 24.2. PEP (A) and SBP (B) reactivity in dependence on task difficulty in the Study by Richter, Friedrich, and Gendolla (2008).
(Reprinted with permission of John Wiley and Sons.)

 effort intensit y


Possible Difficulty Levels - Ability Low

Possible Difficulty Levels - Ability High

Drop Points Determined by


Potential Motivation Where
Success is Possible
EFFORT

ow
yL
ilit
Ab
gh
y Hi
ilit
Ab

TASK DIFFICULTY

Fig. 24.3. Relation between effort and difficulty for people with low and high ability. (Adapted from Wright and Franklin, 2004, p.
190, with permission of Erlbaum Press.)

Other studies experimentally manipulated ability SBP and DBP. Among high-ability participants,
appraisals. One by Wright and Dill (1993) first used responses were or tended to be stronger under dif-
a performance feedback procedure to lead half of its ficult conditions. Among low-ability participants,
participants to believe that they had low ability with the reverse was true. Once again, whereas ability
respect to a scanning task and half to believe that tended to be negatively associated with cardiovas-
they had high ability with respect to the task. Later, cular responsiveness when difficulty was low, it was
it presented a related scanning task and told the positively associated with cardiovascular responsive-
participants that they could earn a prize by meet- ness when difficulty was high.
ing a high (85th percentile) or low (15th percentile)
performance standard. As expected, SBP responses fatigue and difficulty
measured immediately before and during the work Following from the ability reasoning and studies
period were in an interactional pattern, reflecting discussed in the previous section has been a series of
relatively greater responsiveness for low-ability par- studies concerned with fatigue influence on effort-
ticipants when the standard was low, but greater related cardiovascular responses. Studies in the
responsiveness for high-ability participants when series have assumed that ability falls as fatigue rises.
the standard was high. Whereas ability tended to be Accordingly, they have predicted that fatigue should
negatively associated with SBP responsiveness when interact with difficulty to determine effort-related
difficulty was low, it was positively associated with cardiovascular responses in the same way that abil-
cardiovascular responsiveness when difficulty was ity should do so, with high fatigue corresponding to
high. Analysis of DBP data revealed an interactional low ability and low fatigue corresponding to high
response pattern corresponding closely with the ability (Fig. 24.3).
SBP means. The earliest of the fatigue studies was an experi-
A later study by Wright and Dismukes (1995) ment that involved a muscular challenge (Wright
involved a similar procedure, but it utilized an aver- & Penacerrada, 2002). Its central purpose was to
sive rather than an attractive incentive. As before, evaluate the implication that effort-related cardio-
investigators first led participants to believe that vascular response should be proportional to fatigue
they had low or high ability with respect to a scan- where a performance challenge can and will be met.
ning task. They then told participants that they A secondary purpose was to evaluate the idea that
could avoid noise by attaining a low (20th percen- muscular fatigue effects should tend to be challenge
tile) or high (95th percentile) performance standard specific, that is, confined to challenges that involve
on a version of the task. Results indicated a cross- the fatigued system. Participants first performed
over response pattern for HR and, to a lesser degree, left- or right-handedly a set of 12 easy (low fatigue)

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


or difficult (high fatigue) hand dynamometer grips. same interactions with means in similar crossover
Following the initial grip period, the participants patterns.
made and held with their right hand a modest Several mental fatigue studies aimed not only
grip while cardiovascular measures were taken. As to reproduce cardiovascular effects like those men-
expected, SBP responses were stronger under high tioned previously but also to evaluate the extent to
fatigue conditions among participants who gripped which mental fatigue effects are challenge specific,
first with their right hand, but not among partici- that is, limited to challenges highly relevant to the
pants who gripped first with their left hand. mental activity that induced the fatigue. An example
Findings have been conceptually replicated and is an experiment that (1) first required participants
extended in multiple subsequent investigations, to perform an easy (fatigue low) or difficult (fatigue
some involving muscular tasks and some involving high) counting task, and then (2) presented the par-
mental tasks (for a review, see Wright & Stewart, ticipants with either an arithmetic challenge (fatigue
2011). One mental fatigue study (Wright, Martin, relevance high) or a letter scanning challenge (fatigue
& Bland, 2003) examined the ideas that fatigue relevance low) with instructions that they would
should (1) augment effort-related cardiovascular avoid a noise if they attained a modest (50th per-
responses when it leaves unaltered a belief that suc- centile) performance standard (Wright et al., 2007,
cess is possible and worthwhile, but (2) retard those Experiment 1). Analysis of the cardiovascular data
responses when it causes success to appear impos- collected during the second work period indicated
sible or excessively difficult. Investigators created stronger DBP and MAP responses for high fatigue
low and high levels of mental fatigue by requiring participants regardless of the character of the task.
participants initially to perform for 5 minutes an SBP responses corresponded with DBP and MAP
easy or difficult counting task. After the counting responses, although the fatigue effect in that case
period, the investigators presented participants fell short of reliability. The other studies of this type
mental arithmetic problems with instructions that also yielded no evidence that mental fatigue effects
they could earn a prize if they attained a low (30th are challenge specific.
percentile) or high (80th percentile) performance
standard. As expected, analysis of cardiovascular mood
data collected during the arithmetic work period Moods are defined as relatively long-lasting
indicated a fatigue x difficulty interaction for SBP. affective states that are experienced without con-
High fatigue participants tended to have stronger current awareness of their origins (Frijda, 1993).
responses than low fatigue participants when the Typical examples are feeling elated or depressed.
standard was low; by contrast, they had weaker A series of experiments tested the idea of the mood-
responses than low fatigue participants when the behavior-model (Gendolla, 2000) that moods have
standard was high (Fig. 24.4). Analysis of the DBP a systematic informational impact on effort intensity
and mean arterial pressure (MAP) data revealed the during task performance because they function as
task-relevant information and have mood congru-
ency effects on task appraisals. If effort mobilization
6
is basically guided by a resource conservation prin-
5 Fatigue Low
Fatigue High
ciple, it follows that the default appraisal in face of
4
a task should be that of the level of task demand. In
SBP Reactivity (mmHg)

3
those studies elated and depressed moods were first
2
manipulated with exposure to funny or depress-
1
ing video excerpts, elating or depressing music, or
0
autobiographical recollection of positive or negative
–1
personal events. Subsequently, participants worked
–2
on a cognitive task (memory, attention, or verbal
–3
creativity). After an initial habituation period,
–4
cardiovascular measures were taken during mood
Low High
inductions and task performance (see Gendolla &
Performance Standard
Brinkmann, 2005; Gendolla, Brinkmann, & Silves-
Fig. 24.4. SBP reactivity as a function of difficulty for low- and
trini, 2011 for more detailed reviews).
high fatigued participants. (Based on data presented by Wright In support of the idea that moods systemati-
et al., 2003.) cally influence effort-related cardiovascular response

 effort intensit y


through their informational impact, it was found Mood had no effect on cardiovascular reactivity in
that (1) demand appraisals before and SBP reactiv- the incidental learning task. The results support the
ity during task performance were higher in a nega- idea that moods influence effort when they can be
tive mood than in a positive mood for tasks with used as task-relevant information for task apprais-
“do your best” instructions (e.g., Gendolla, Abele, als. However, when resources do not have to be
& Krüsken, 2001; Gendolla & Krüsken, 2001a, mobilized, mood loses this informational impact.
2002b); (2) these mood congruency effects on This influence of moods is clearly different from
experienced demand and SBP response during task that of specific, object-related, and short-lived emo-
performance disappeared when moods’ value as tions, which directly mobilize resources for adaptive
task-relevant information was called into question actions (see Kreibig, 2010).
by providing a hint for the mood manipulations In summary, the studies on mood effects on
(Gendolla & Krüsken, 2002a); (3) when participants effort intensity discussed in this section show that
performed tasks with fixed performance standards, moods function as task-relevant information. When
they used both their mood and the performance tasks have an achievement character and judgments
standard to appraise demand, resulting in the antic- of task demand are possible, mood influences effort
ipated crossover pattern of mood and objective task by informing about subjective task difficulty and has
difficulty (Gendolla & Krüsken, 2001b, 2002b) similar effects on effort intensity as ability beliefs, as
that resembles the interactive effect of ability beliefs discussed earlier.
and task difficulty discussed earlier: For easy tasks,
SBP reactivity was stronger in a negative mood than The Role of Variables Affecting the
in a positive mood, because subjective demand for Importance of Success
participants in a negative mood was higher than A number of studies have investigated the impact
for those in a positive mood. However, for diffi- of variables influencing the level of potential motiva-
cult tasks, SBP reactivity was stronger in a positive tion as determinant of the level of maximally justi-
mood than in a negative mood, because here subjec- fied effort for goal attainment. Most of these studies
tive difficulty was high but still feasible in a positive have focused on tasks with manipulated fixed dif-
mood, whereas it was too high in a negative mood, ficulty levels. Others have focused on tasks where
resulting in disengagement. Corresponding effects difficulty was unspecified. As outlined in the first
on effort-related cardiovascular response were found section of this chapter, motivation intensity theory
for individual differences in dysphoria/depression predicts that effort intensity should be proportional
(Brinkmann & Gendolla, 2007, 2008) and extra- to task difficulty as long as success is justified. Con-
version (Kemper et al., 2008). sequently, high potential motivation justifies the
A recent study by de Burgo and Gendolla high effort that is necessary to cope with highly dif-
(2009) tested the mood-behavior-model postulate ficult demands, while low potential motivation does
that that moods themselves are not motivational not, resulting in earlier disengagement on lower dif-
states and that they thus do not have an impact on ficulty levels. Moreover, effort intensity should be
effort-related cardiovascular response until they can proportional to potential motivation when task dif-
be used as task-relevant information for demand ficulty is unspecified or unknown. These ideas were
appraisals. After being induced in positive versus tested in experiments using various manipulations
negative moods with video excerpts, participants of potential motivation ranging from a material
were exposed to a list of letter series. In an inten- incentive to affective and self-esteem-related conse-
tional learning condition, participants were explic- quences of success.
itly instructed to correctly memorize the series
within 5 minutes; in an incidental learning condi- material incentive
tion, the list was merely presented without framing Participants in a study by Eubanks, Wright, and
it as achievement task. As in the mood studies dis- Williams (2002) worked on a computerized recog-
cussed earlier, SBP did not differ between the mood nition memory task. Depending on the difficulty
conditions during the mood inductions, although condition, participants were presented with letter
the verbal mood manipulation checks indicated suc- series that contained between three (very easy) and
cessfully manipulated positive and negative mood thirteen (very difficult) letters, followed by a target
states. Most relevant, during task performance SBP letter that was presented after a short delay. Par-
reactivity in the intentional learning task was stron- ticipants learned before task onset that they could
ger in a negative mood than in a positive mood. gain either $10 (low incentive) versus $100 (high

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


incentive) if they responded correctly in at least The most recent experiment of this type (Richter &
90% of the trials. The results were most pronounced Gendolla, 2009a) examined PEP as well as blood
for changes in HR, which increased over the whole pressure and HR. The experimental task required
range of difficulty conditions in the high-incentive participants to match patterns displayed on a com-
condition. In the low-incentive condition, HR reac- puter monitor to a target pattern, with success
tivity first increased with difficulty but dropped on earning 1, 15, or 30 Swiss Francs. Instructions left
the difficult and very difficult levels. This supports details ambiguous and, furthermore, indicated that
the idea that high monetary incentive leads to high at the end of the work period, the computer would
effort for difficult tasks by justifying the neces- assign randomly the performance standard that
sary high effort. When task difficulty is rather low, would define success. As expected, PEP responsive-
incentive has no increasing effect on effort, because ness increased steadily across the incentive value
low effort is sufficient for succeeding. (potential motivation) conditions. SBP responses
Evidence for the incentive effects on effort- tended to rise with incentive value as well, although
related cardiovascular response when task difficulty the comparison of the 15- and 30-Swiss Francs con-
is unknown has come primarily from studies by ditions did not approach significance.
Richter and Gendolla. The first study from these In summary, studies that manipulated potential
investigators (Richter & Gendolla, 2006, Experi- motivation by means of material incentive have well
ment 1) used a computer to present participants over supported the predictions of motivation intensity
a 5-minute period in sequence a series of four non- theory. It has been shown that high incentive justi-
sense quadrams, that is, character strings made up of fies the high necessary effort for coping with diffi-
four letters that convey no meaning (e.g., AEGD). cult tasks, while incentive itself does not boost effort
Preliminary instructions informed participants that when task difficulty is low and success only neces-
they could earn an attractive poster (incentive value sitates the mobilization of low resources. Moreover,
high) or an unattractive poster (incentive value there is replicated evidence that effort rises with the
low) if they memorized their quadrams success- value of a material incentive when task difficulty is
fully, providing quadram and procedural details for not specified.
some participants (difficulty low), but not for other
participants (difficulty unknown). As expected, outcome expectancy (instrumentality)
SBP reactivity during task performance was jointly A series of studies by Wright and colleagues
determined by difficulty and incentive (potential operationalized potential motivation in terms of
motivation). Whereas reactivity was stronger under outcome expectancy, that is, the perceived likeli-
high- than low-incentive value conditions when dif- hood that success on a task will lead to a desired
ficulty was unknown, it was relatively low and con- outcome (Maddux, 1995). Outcome expectancy
stant across incentive value levels when difficulty also is referred to as the instrumentality of behavior.
was low. In theory, importance should be greater where it is
Later studies conceptually replicated and high than where it is low. Thus, outcome expectancy
extended the preceding cardiovascular effects. For should determine performers’ upper effort limit—
example, Richter and Gendolla (2006, Experiment the level of potential motivation.
2) used highly similar known and unknown dif- An early experiment by Wright and Gregorich
ficulty protocols and manipulated incentive value (1989) provided participants a low (1/15) or high
by offering participants either 10 Swiss Francs or (14/15) chance of winning a modest prize (a paper
nothing for a good performance. As expected, SBP notebook) by succeeding on an easy (two-trigram)
responses were greater in the payment condition or moderately difficult (five-trigram) memorization
than in the no-payment condition if difficulty was task. Analysis of cardiovascular responses assessed
unknown, but not if difficulty was known and low. just prior to work revealed difficulty x instrumental-
Another follow-up (Richter & Gendolla, 2007) ran ity (i.e., potential motivation) interaction patterns
a highly similar unknown difficulty protocol and for SBP and HR. Among the high-chance partici-
manipulated incentive value across four levels, offer- pants, responses were proportional to difficulty;
ing participants 10, 20, or 30 Swiss Francs, or noth- among the low-chance participants, the responses
ing for success. Results showed that SBP responses were low irrespective of difficulty.
measured during the performance period were A later experiment by Wright, Williams, and Dill
directly proportional to the value of the incentive (1992) manipulated the expectancy in an avoid-
offered (i.e., to potential motivation). ance context rather than in an appetitive one and

 effort intensit y


measured cardiovascular responses during, rather sion of a letter-cancellation task (see e.g., Gendolla &
than immediately prior to, the work (see also Krüsken, 2001b). They then were presented a set of
Manuck, Harvey, Lechleiter, & Neal, 1978). Partici- single-digit multiplication problems with instruc-
pants were presented trials of a recognition memory tions that they would earn a strong (51/52—success
task similar to the one used by Richter et al. (2008). importance high) or weak (1/52—success impor-
Specifically, they were presented a series of character tance low) chance of winning a prize if they attained
strings, each followed by a question asking whether a moderate (50th percentile) performance standard.
a probe was in the string. For half, each string had The central prediction was that fatigue would poten-
three characters, rendering success relatively easy. For tiate effort-related cardiovascular responses during
the rest, each string had seven characters, rendering the second period when the chance of winning
success relatively difficult. Instructions indicated (and, thus, importance) was high, but not when the
that a good performance (90% success rate) would chance of winning (and, thus, importance) was low.
ensure a strong (19/20) or weak (1/20) chance of Potentiation was not expected under low chance
avoiding an aversive noise, thus yielding the same (importance) conditions because available benefit
basic experimental design seen in the study men- under those conditions was not expected to be great
tioned earlier. Once again, there was a joint effect enough to justify the added effort requirement asso-
of chance and difficulty on cardiovascular response. ciated with fatigue. SBP responses assessed during
Whereas responses were greater in the moderately the period were supportive. Specifically, they were
difficult condition when outcome expectancy was proportional to fatigue for high-chance partici-
high, they were low in both difficulty conditions pants, but low regardless of fatigue for low-chance
when outcome expectancy (potential motivation) participants. Analyses revealed the same interac-
was low. tional response pattern for DBP and MAP.
A recent study by Richter and Gendolla (2009b) In summary, outcome expectancy studies have
extended the evidence on informational mood yielded effects highly compatible with the effects
impact on effort mobilization discussed earlier. This from the material incentive studies. In accordance
experiment tested the idea that mood can influ- with the principles of motivation intensity theory,
ence effort mobilization through its informational they suggest that outcome expectancy moderates
impact on outcome expectancies when difficulty is the relation between difficulty and effort when task
unknown. Participants were induced into positive, difficulty is fixed and predicts effort directly when
neutral, or negative moods by an autobiographical task difficulty is unknown.
recollection task and then worked on a memory
task of unknown task difficulty—a setting in which social evaluation
participants orient effort mobilization on potential Some studies have tested the idea that social eval-
motivation, as discussed earlier. Before task onset, uation of one’s performance augments the impor-
participants learned that they could earn the chance tance of success and thus increases the amount
of winning a monetary reward if they succeeded of justified effort (potential motivation). Wright,
and were asked to rate the probability of winning. Tunstall, Williams, Goodwin, and Harmon-Jones
Those subjective probability ratings were higher in a (1995, Study 2) confronted participants with a
positive mood than in a negative mood—suggesting recognition memory task that was either easy or
that potential motivation was higher in a positive had an unfixed performance standard. In the easy
mood (high probability to get the reward) than in a condition, participants tried to correctly identify in
negative mood (low probability to get the reward). 90% of the trials if a target letter was part of a string
Corresponding to this, SBP reactivity during task of three letters that was presented for 5 seconds.
performance increased from the negative via the In the unfixed difficulty condition, participants
neutral to the positive mood condition. The SBP were also presented with strings of three letters
effect was statistically mediated by participants’ sub- but tried to attain 90% correct responses as fast as
jective probability ratings of winning the monetary they could. Social evaluation was manipulated by
reward for successful performance. telling participants explicitly that their responses
Another study—by Stewart, Wright, Hui, and could, versus could not, be monitored by the exper-
Simmons (2009)—investigated combined effects imenter. The pattern of performance-related SBP
of fatigue and outcome expectancy on effort-related reactivity occurred as expected: In the unfixed dif-
cardiovascular response. Participants first performed ficulty condition, social evaluation increased reac-
an easy (fatigue low) or difficult (fatigue high) ver- tivity. But in the easy condition reactivity was low

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


regardless of whether participants’ responses could a task was difficult and thus necessitated high
be monitored. A study by Wright, Killebrew, and resources, but not when it was easy, and required
Pimpalapure (2002) revealed corresponding results only relatively low resources.
for social evaluation by high-, but not low-, status
observers. ego involvement
An experiment by Wright, Dill, Geen, and Ego involvement refers to an increased sense of
Anderson (1998) involved five difficulty levels, success importance that occurs when people are in
ranging from very easy to extremely difficult. Specif- a performance setting and believe that a valuable
ically, participants performed a recognition memory ability is being evaluated (Klein & Schoenfeld,
task and tried to correctly identify at least 90% of 1941). Studies by Gendolla and Richter tested
40 trials in which two, four, six, eight, or ten let- the idea that ego involvement thus increases the
ters were presented. Social evaluation was manipu- level of potential motivation. In an experiment by
lated as in the previous experiment by Wright et al. Gendolla and Richter (2006b, Study 1), high-ego-
(1995). In the social-evaluation condition, SBP involvement participants were told that a memory
reactivity increased from the two-letter condition to task would be diagnostic of an important ability—
the six-letter condition and then sharply decreased, learning under time pressure—whereas low-ego-
describing the anticipated saw-tooth shaped curve. involvement participants were told that the task was
In the no-evaluation condition, task difficulty had only a filler without any diagnostic meaning. Task
no significant impact on SBP reactivity, which was difficulty was low, moderate, or high. Additionally,
relatively low in all conditions. A study by Gendolla there was a condition in which participants were
and Richter (2006a) found corresponding effects instructed to “do their best” (unfixed difficulty).
for implicit social evaluation manipulated by the Figure 24.5 shows for the high-ego-involvement
mere presence of an observer. condition that SBP reactivity was, as expected, high
It is of note that the preceding social evaluation in the unfixed difficulty condition where participants
studies challenge other approaches that have pos- were instructed to correctly memorize as many items
ited that social observation should lead to a general as they could. Moreover, SBP reactivity increased
increase in autonomic nervous system activity (e.g., from the easy to the difficult condition. The unfixed
Baron, 1986; Cottrell, 1968; Zajonc, 1965) or a and difficult conditions did not differ, suggesting
general increase in effort (Harkins, 2006). As out- that participants in these conditions mobilized effort
lined earlier and supporting the principles of motiva- up to the level of potential motivation, which was
tional intensity theory, social evaluation resulted in defined by the ego involvement manipulation. No
relatively strong response of one specific autonomic significant differences emerged among the four dif-
arousal measure—cardiovascular activity—when ficulty cells in the low-ego—involvement condition.

15 Low ego-involvement
14 High ego-involvment
13
12
11
SBP reactivity (mmHg)

10
9
8
7
6
5
4
3
2
1
0
Unfixed Easy Moderate Difficult

Fig. 24.5. SBP reactivity in dependence on ego involvement and task difficulty in Study 1 by Gendolla and Richter (2006b).
(Reproduced with permission from Sage Publications.)

 effort intensit y


The effects for unfixed and fixed easy difficulty which participants received “do your best” instruc-
levels replicate the findings of a previous study using tions. For the high-self-focus condition, where rela-
a mental concentration task that found identical tively much effort should have been justified, SBP
effects on SBP, DBP, HR, and electrodermal reac- reactivity was anticipated to be high in both the
tivity (Gendolla & Richter, 2005). unfixed and the difficult conditions. By contrast,
A second experiment by Gendolla and Richter reactivity should have been low in the extremely
(2006b) involved a “do your best” condition and an difficult condition where participants were expected
excessively difficult, actually impossible, condition. to disengage. In the low-self-focus condition, where
Under “do your best” instructions high ego involve- only low effort should have been justified, reactiv-
ment led again to higher SBP reactivity than low ego ity was anticipated to be low in general. The results
involvement. More important, when task difficulty confirmed these predictions. A recent follow-up
was so high that success was obviously impossible, experiment by Silvia, McCord, and Gendolla (2010)
participants disengaged even when ego involvement conceptually replicated the joint effect of self-focus
was high, resulting in low SBP reactivity. and task difficulty on effort-related cardiovascular
The present studies on ego involvement chal- response and clarified that self-focused attention
lenge earlier views of Nicholls (1984) and Dweck also leads to high effort when success expectancies
(1986) who had formulated reservations against the are low due to high task difficulty.
assumption that the difficulty law of motivation
applies to evaluations of important abilities. Those hedonic incentive: moderating effects
authors have suggested that the difficulty-effort rela- of mood and depressive symptoms
tionship is only proportional when people do not try A number of studies have also investigated the
to demonstrate valuable abilities—that is, not under combined effects of mood, task difficulty, and the
ego involvement. The findings of the Gendolla and hedonic incentive of success on effort-related car-
Richter studies suggest, however, that the principle diovascular response. These studies tested the idea
of resource conservation applies well under ego of the mood-behavior-model (Gendolla, 2000) that
involvement and that effort intensity also follows actions that are instrumental for mood regulation
the principles of motivation intensity theory when (maintaining a positive mood, repairing a negative
valuable abilities are evaluated. mood) justify relatively high resources. One conse-
quence of this suggestion is that positive hedonic
self-evaluation incentive should eliminate the previously discussed
Self-awareness theory (Duval & Wicklund, effort withdrawal of people who face a difficult task
1972) posits that focusing individuals’ attention to in a negative mood (e.g., Brinkmann & Gendolla,
their self induces a state of self-evaluation: Persons 2008; Gendolla & Krüsken, 2001b). Building on
compare their actual behavior with the momentarily initial evidence for this hypothesis (Gendolla &
relevant standards. In the context of achievement Krüsken, 2002c), Silvestrini and Gendolla (2009a)
behavior, self-focused attention should, thus, jus- first induced participants into a positive or nega-
tify relatively high resources, because self-evaluation tive mood with film clips and then presented them
makes success relatively important. Drawing on this a memory task that was either easy or difficult.
logic, Gendolla, Richter, and Silvia (2008) tested Before performance, participants were informed
the idea that self-focused attention determines the about the hedonic consequences of success. In
level of potential motivation. In their second study, a positive incentive condition they were promised
participants performed a computer-based attention the presentation of a comedy video after success; in
task. In a high-self-focus condition participants’ face a negative incentive condition, participants expected
was filmed from the left-hand-side perspective dur- the presentation of a distressing video after success.
ing task performance. The picture was transmitted As depicted in Figure 24.6 SBP reactivity during
to a video monitor that was placed next to partici- task performance described the predicted pattern:
pants’ computer screen, exposing them to a picture When success incentive was negative, and thus did
of their own face during task performance. In a not justify high effort, SBP reactivity conformed
contrasting low-self-focus condition, participants to the crossover interaction pattern anticipated and
were not filmed because the camera was ostensibly shown for the joint effect of mood and objective task
out of order. The study involved a difficult and an difficulty on experienced demand and correspond-
extremely difficult (actually impossible) version of ing effort intensity (e.g., Brinkmann & Gendolla,
the attention task. It also included a condition in 2008; Gendolla & Krüsken, 2001b). However,

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


Negative Incentive Positive Incentive
16

14

12

SBP reactivity [mm Hg]


10

–2
Easy Difficult Easy Difficult

Negative mood Positive mood

Fig. 24.6. Cell means and standard errors of systolic blood pressure reactivity during memory task performance in the experiment by
Slivestrini and Gendolla (2009a). (Reproduced with permission from Elsevier.)

when success incentive was positive, SBP reactivity Gendolla (2009, Study 2) therefore directly tested
of participants who worked on the difficult task in the hypothesis of a reduced reward responsiveness
a negative mood increased significantly. The antici- in dysphoric individuals in terms of effort mobili-
pated pleasant consequences of success justified zation. The studies used a task with unclear diffi-
here the very high effort that was perceived as nec- culty, because this type of task permits a direct test
essary when participants faced a difficult task in a of reward effects on effort mobilization, as discussed
negative mood. These results highlight the hedonic earlier. In the reward condition participants learned
aspects of achievement motivation. Accordingly, that they could win money (10 Swiss Francs) for
it is not success per se that justifies the mobiliza- correctly solving an arithmetic problem. Partici-
tion of high effort—success did not justify high pants received instructions to perform arithmetic
resources when it led to unpleasant consequences. operations over a 5-minute period, leading to a final
Rather, success has to be bound up with positive correct or incorrect result in the end. No incentive
hedonic aspects to justify high effort. Another was mentioned in the no-reward condition. Results
study that manipulated the hedonic aspects of showed strong increases in SBP, DBP, HR, and PEP
task performance itself brought compatible results for nondysphoric participants in the reward condi-
(Silvestrini & Gendolla, 2009b): A pleasant version tion, reflecting the typical incentive effect in tasks
of a sentence completion task justified higher effort with unclear difficulty (Richter & Gendolla, 2006,
than an unpleasant version. 2009a). In contrast, dysphorics’ reactivity was signif-
Other research investigated the impact of incen- icantly lower and did not differ from the no-reward
tive in individuals suffering from depressive symp- condition. Taken together, these studies show that
toms. As discussed earlier, the effects of depressive depressives’ reduced responsiveness to reward is also
symptoms on effort-related cardiovascular response evident in effort mobilization.
resemble those of a negative mood when incentive
is not manipulated (Brinkmann & Gendolla, 2007, Conclusions
2008). But it is questionable whether promised The present analysis has highlighted the question
performance-contingent reward has the same influ- of what determines effort intensity in instrumental
ences on effort mobilization in depressed/dysphoric behavior. We have discussed empirical evidence
individuals as in nondepressed/nondysphoric peo- from studies that have operationalized effort inten-
ple. The reason is evidence that depressed individu- sity as cardiovascular response in the context of
als do not behaviorally respond to monetary reward task performance. The analysis was guided by the
and punishment (e.g., Henriques & Davidson, predictions of motivation intensity theory (Brehm
2000). Brinkmann, Ancel Joye, Schüppach, and et al., 1983; Brehm & Self, 1989), which state in

 effort intensit y


brief that effort intensity corresponds to subjective intensity theory may draw a picture of effort mobi-
difficulty as long as success is seen as possible and lization that is “too rational.” The point was that
justified and that success importance (i.e., potential the theory would not really capture the dynamics
motivation) only influences effort directly when task of human action, because it implies that people
difficulty is unspecified. As outlined in this chapter, always consciously calculate how much effort they
many studies have investigated several psychologi- want and need to mobilize and decide to do so or
cal variables that have a systematic impact on sub- to disengage—for example, in a deliberate process
jective difficulty and potential motivation. Those like this: “I’m highly able, thus this task must be
studies have brought highly concordant evidence easy for me, so I’ll only invest little effort.” This
for the predictions. Additionally to supporting interpretation may have been caused in part by the
the principles of motivation intensity theory, this fact that the function of effort has been regarded for
evidence challenges a number of other ideas about long as allowing coping with obstacles and tempta-
the determination of effort intensity. tions during volitional goal pursuit. Some authors
Some of these challenges were already men- have thus seen effort as a proxy of willpower (e.g.,
tioned earlier in the context of the presentation of Ach, 1910; Dewey, 1897) and the typical charac-
our empirical work. In addition to that, our find- teristic of an action phase in models of volition and
ings also limit approaches suggesting that reward action control (Heckhausen & Gollwitzer, 1987;
directly determines effort mobilization (e.g., Eisen- Kuhl, 1983).
berger, 1992; Fowles, 1983). The studies discussed We agree that motivation intensity theory is well
here have revealed that a direct effect of incentive applicable to deliberate effort investment. How-
on the intensity of motivation only occurs when ever, according to our understanding the theory has
difficulty is unspecified or unknown rather than in not been conceptualized as being limited to this—
general. Our findings are also not compatible with although effort is surely necessary for self-control,
the idea that motivation is less intense if people because resisting temptations, changing habits, and
perform tasks without a clear performance stan- coping with obstacles are difficult endeavors. How-
dard (“do your best”) than if they perform tasks ever, according to our view, the principles of effort
with fixed high standards (Locke & Latham, 1990). mobilization can be so well learned, or even inter-
According to the findings presented here, these con- nalized, that they also work implicitly. Some empir-
ditions produce the same effects on effort-related ical support for this idea comes from recent studies
cardiovascular response. Moreover, our findings that manipulated effort intensity with implicitly
contradict the (historically popular) idea that the processed stimuli.
intensity of motivation is maximal on intermediate
task difficulty levels, as, for example, expressed in Implicit Determination of Effort
Atkinson’s (1957) influential risk taking model. The Participants in recent experiments by Gendolla
studies presented here have found that effort inten- and Silvestrini (2011) performed an attention or rec-
sity is maximal on the highest possible and justified ognition memory task under “do your best” instruc-
rather than an intermediate difficulty level. Com- tions. During the task, participants were exposed to
patible with this, and further challenging the risk briefly flashed and backward-masked happy, sad, or
taking model, recent experiments by Capa, Audif- angry low-resolution pictures of facial expressions.
fen, and Ragot (2008a, 2008b) have found that The results revealed stronger reactivity of cardiac
a strong achievement orientation justifies high PEP and SBP in the masked sadness condition than
effort, resulting in stronger cardiovascular reactivity in both the masked happiness and masked anger
when task difficulty is high. cells. Moreover, self-report measures found that,
corresponding to this, task difficulty was experi-
Future Directions enced as higher in the masked sadness condition
One interesting issue for future research might than in both the masked happiness and anger cells.
be the question of whether people are (always) cog- The effects of masked sadness and happiness expres-
nizant of the effort they expend and if the factors sions resemble those of conscious moods discussed
influencing effort have their impact because they earlier—sadness was associated with difficulty and
are deliberatively taken into account. Related to this happiness was associated with ease (see Gendolla &
issue is a point of critique that was occasionally raised Brinkmann, 2005)—although the masked emo-
in the earlier phases of the research presented here. tional expressions did not have any effects on con-
The critique was that the principles of motivation scious feelings here. The effect of masked anger

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


stimuli is explicable by the accumulating evidence condition and weaker reactivity in the inaction
that anger is associated with behavioral facilitation prime condition. These results suggest that it is
and experiences of control (Carver & Harmon-Jones, possible to influence effort intensity directly—that
2009), making a task subjectively easier. However, is, without taking effect on difficulty or potential
the important point is that effort intensity can be motivation—with implicit cues that are processed
systematically manipulated with masked affective online during task performance. This effect mer-
cues that are processed “on line”—that is, during its further attention because it suggests that effort
performance—and have an effect on experienced intensity may not only be determined by experi-
task difficult. enced difficulty and potential motivation.
Beside the effects of implicit affective stimuli
on effort-related cardiovascular response, there is Personality and Individual Differences
also evidence for the impact of masked incentive Another issue that merits more attention in
cues on performance-related sympathetic arousal. future research is the role of personality and indi-
Stimulated by a study by Pessiglione et al. (2007) vidual differences in effort mobilization. Some data
on implicit monetary incentive cues on the exer- are already available and show that personality vari-
tion of physical force, Bijleveld, Custers, and Aarts ables can moderate the effects of task difficulty and
(2009) briefly flashed backward-masked pictures of incentive on effort intensity. The studies on dys-
low (1 cent coin) or high (50 cent coin) monetary phoria/depression by Brinkmann and colleagues,
incentive during a digit-retention task that was discussed earlier, provide an example, the studies
either easy or difficult. Measures of pupil dilata- by Wright and colleagues on ability differences
tion during task performance (which is related to and fatigue provide another (see also Schmidt,
sympathetic arousal and has been suggested to mir- Richter, Gendolla, & van der Linden, 2010).
ror mental effort; Kahneman, 1973) found higher Other studies have investigated the moderating
arousal when the task was difficult and partici- effects of individual differences in achievement
pants were flashed with pictures of valuable coins. motivation (Capa, 2011), agentic extraversion
This suggests that implicitly processed information (Kemper, Leue, Chavanon, Henninghausen, &
about monetary incentive can augment potential Stemmler, 2008), or self-focused attention (Silvia,
motivation and justify the necessary effort for a dif- Jones, Kelly, & Zibaie, 2011) on effort-related car-
ficult task—a finding that fully fits the principles diovascular response. However, personality factors
of motivation intensity theory. Moreover, a recent in effort mobilization are still far from being fully
study by Capa, Cleeremans, Bustin, Bouquet, and understood.
Hansenne (2011) primed university students with
the goal of studying and combined the goal primes Task Context
either with positive (i.e., rewarding) words or not. Finally, another important issue that merits
During the subsequent performance of a learning more attention in future research is the role of task
task of unspecified difficulty, participants in the goal context or task framing. Recent work by Richter
priming-positive group showed larger decreases of (2010) shows that relationships among task dif-
mid-frequency band heart rate variability and pulse ficulty, potential motivation, and cardiovascular
transit time, reflecting higher effort, according to reactivity are very flexible. Richter could show that
the authors. the variables that take effect on effort-related cardio-
Another recent experiment by Gendolla and vascular reactivity largely depend on the task con-
Silvestrini (2010) even went further and tested the text. If participants had rated some “manipulation
possibility to manipulate effort intensity directly check” referring to task difficulty before perform-
with masked stimuli that were processed “online” ing a task, cardiovascular reactivity was a function
during task performance. Inspired by studies by of task difficulty. However, if participants had rated
Abarracín, et al. (2008) on the priming of general questions related to the reward that they could earn,
behavioral activation, participants were exposed to cardiovascular reactivity was determined by reward
masked action versus inaction words during the value. This indicates that more variables than those
trials of a recognition memory task. Additionally, referring to task difficulty and potential motivation
there was a control condition in which participants have to be considered to fully understand the deter-
were exposed to nonwords. Compared with the mination of effort intensity. Apparently, task con-
control group, the results showed stronger changes text can determine which information individuals
in cardiac contractility (PEP) in the action prime use for effort mobilization.

 effort intensit y


Link to Other Physiological Correlates of Suisse (100011–108144, 100014–122604) awarded to Guido
Gendolla, from the National Science Foundation (BCS-
Effort Intensity
0450941) awarded to Rex Wright, and from the Fonds National
Taken together, there is solid evidence for the Suisse (100014–118220) awarded to Michael Richter.
idea that effort-related cardiovascular response is
systematically influenced by experienced task dif-
References
ficulty and potential motivation (the level of maxi- Aarts, H., Custers, R., & Marien, H. (2008). Preparing
mally justified effort), supporting the principles and motivating behavior outside awareness. Science, 319,
of motivation intensity theory. Moreover, there 1639.
are new lines for future research addressing issues Ach, N. (1910). Über den Willensakt und das Temperament [On the
will and temperament]. Leipzig, Germany: Quelle und Meyer.
like the moderating role of individual differences,
Ach, N. (1935). Analyse des Willens [Analysis of the will]. Berlin,
the impact of implicit cues on effort intensity, and Germany: Urban Schwarzenberg.
the important role of context variables influencing Abarracín, D., Handley, I. M., Noguchi, K., McCulloch,
effort mobilization. K. C., Li, H., Leeper, J., . . . Hart, W. P. (2008). Increasing
Still another research field is the link between other and decreasing motor and cognitive output: A model of
general action and inaction goals. Journal of Personality and
physiological correlates and indicators of effort inten-
Social Psychology, 95, 510–523.
sity. Earlier we have already mentioned pupil dilata- Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk-taking
tion as another measure of sympathetic nervous system behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.
activity that has been related to effort (see also Gra- Atkinson, J. W., & Raynor, J. O. (1974). Motivation and achieve-
nholm, Verney, Perivoliotis, & Miura, 2007). Other ment. Washington, DC: Winston & Sons.
Bandura, A. (1982). Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency.
researchers have linked the investment of mental effort
American Psychologist, 37, 122–147.
to glucose consumption (e.g., Fairclough & Houston, Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action.
2004) and have provided evidence that glucose admin- Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.
istration can restore or even foster effortful cognitive Bandura, A., & Cervone, D. (1983). Self-reactive mechanisms in
performance (Kennedy & Scholey, 2000; see also Gail- goal motivators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
45, 1017–1028.
liot & Baumeister, 2007). Referring to the interplay
Bijleveld, E., Custers, R., & Aarts, H. (2009). The unconscious
between difficulty and reward on motivational central eye opener: Pupil dilation reveals strategic recruitment of
nervous system responses, Harmon-Jones et al. (2007) resources upon presentation of subliminal reward cues. Psy-
found greater left frontal cortical activation in manic chological Science, 20, 1313–1315.
individuals who were expecting to start working on a Baron, R. S. (1986). Distraction-conflict theory: Progress and
problems. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 19,
difficult anagram task where they could win money for
1–40.
correct answers. Moreover, research on brain activity Berntson, G. G., Cacioppo, J. T., & Quigley, K. S. (1993). Car-
and cardiovascular arousal during effortful cognitive diac psychophysiology and autonomic space in humans:
processing has demonstrated that activity of the ante- Empirical perspectives and conceptual implications. Psycho-
rior cingulate cortex—which responds to the difficulty logical Bulletin, 114, 296–322.
Brehm, J. W., & Self, E. A. (1989). The intensity of motivation.
of mental challenges (Paus, Koski, Zografos, Carama-
Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 109–131.
nos, & Westbury, 1998)—is related to cardiovascular Brehm, J. W., Wright, R. A., Solomon, S., Silka, L., & Green-
reactivity (Critchley, Corfield, Chandler, Mathias, & berg, J. (1983). Perceived difficulty, energization, and the
Dolan, 2000; Gianaros et al., 2005; Gray & Critchley, magnitude of goal valence. Journal of Experimental Social Psy-
2011). Further investigations in the interplay between chology, 19, 21–48.
Brinkmann, K., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2007). Dysphoria and
central and autonomic nervous system responses
mobilization of mental effort: Effects on cardiovascular reac-
during the performance of cognitive tasks could still tivity. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 71–82.
enlarge the knowledge about physiological correlates Brinkmann, K., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Does depression
and objective measures of the long disregarded but interfere with effort mobilization? Effects of dysphoria and
important intensity aspect of motivation. Although task difficulty on cardiovascular response. Journal of Personal-
ity and Social Psychology, 94, 146–157.
past research has revealed solid evidence for factors that
Brinkmann, K., Ancel Joye, I., Schüppach, L., & Gendolla,
systematically influence the mobilization of resources G. H. E. (2009). Anhedonia and effort mobilization in
for instrumental behavior, the process of effort mobili- dysphoria: Reduced cardiovascular response to reward and
zation is certainly not yet fully understood. punishment. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 74,
250–258.
Brownley, K. A., Hurwitz, B. E., & Schneiderman, N. (2000).
Acknowledgments Cardiovascular sychophysiology. In J. T. Cacioppo,
The research reported in this chapter was facilitated by L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psy-
research grants from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft chophysiology (pp. 224–264). New York: Cambridge Univer-
(Ge 987/1-1, Ge 987/3–1, Ge 987/7–1) and the Fonds National sity Press.

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


Capa, R. L. (2011). Clarifying achievement motives and effort: Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of
Studies of cardiovascular response. In R. A. Wright & willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-control. Personality
G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation affects cardiovas- and Social Psychology Review, 11, 303–327.
cular response: Mechanisms and applications (pp. 383–398). Gendolla, G. H. E. (2000). On the impact of mood on behavior:
Washington, DC: APA. An integrative theory and a review. Review of General Psychol-
Capa, R. L., Audiffen, M., & Ragot, S. (2008a). The interactive ogy, 4, 378–408.
effect of achievement motivation and task difficulty on mental Gendolla, G. H. E., Abele, A. E., & Krüsken, J. (2001). The
effort. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 70, 144–150. informational impact of mood on effort mobilization:
Capa, R. L., Audiffen, M., & Ragot, S. (2008b). The effects of A study of cardiovascular and electrodermal responses. Emo-
achievement motivation, task difficulty, and goal difficulty on tion, 1, 12–24.
physiological, behavioral, and subjective effort. Psychophysiol- Gendolla, G. H. E., & Brinkmann, K. (2005). The role of
ogy, 45, 859–868. mood states in self-regulation: Effects on action prefer-
Capa, R. L., Cleeremans, A., Bustin, G. M., Bouquet, C. A., & ences and resource mobilization. European Psychologist, 10,
Hansenne, M. (2011). Effects of subliminal priming on 187–198.
nonconscious goal pursuit and effort-related cardiovascular Gendolla, G. H. E., Brinkmann, K., & Silvestrini, N. (2011).
response. Social Cognition, 29, 430–444. Gloomy and lazy? On the impact of mood and depressive
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is approach- symptoms on effort-related cardiovascular response. In
related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bul- R. A. Wright & G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation
letin, 135, 183–204. affects cardiovascular response: Mechanisms and applications
Cottrell, N. B. (1968). Performance in the presence of other (pp. 139–155). Washington, DC: APA.
human beings: Mere presence, audience, and affiliation Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2001a). Mood state and
effects. In E. C. Simmel, R. A. Hoppe, & G. A. Milton cardiovascular response in active coping with an affect-
(Eds), Social facilitation and imitative behavior (pp. 91–110). regulative challenge. International Journal of Psychophysiology,
Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon. 41, 169–180.
Critchley, H. D., Corfield, D. R., Chandler, M. P., Mathias, C. J., & Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2001b). The joint impact
Dolan, R. J. (2000). Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardio- of mood state and task difficulty on cardiovascular and elec-
vascular arousal: a functional neuroimaging investigation in trodermal reactivity in active coping. Psychophysiology, 38,
humans. Journal of Physiology, 523, 259–270. 539–548.
De Burgo, J., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009). Are moods moti- Gendolla, G. H. E., & Krüsken, J. (2002a). Informational mood
vational states? A study on effort-related cardiovascular impact on effort-related ardiovascular response: The diagnos-
response. Emotion, 9, 892–697. tic value of moods counts. Emotion, 2, 251–262.
Dewey, J. (1897). The psychology of effort. Psychological Review, Gendolla, G. H. E, & Krüsken, J. (2002b). Mood, task demand,
6, 43–56. and effort-related cardiovascular response. Cognition and
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self- Emotion, 16, 577–603.
awareness. New York: Academic Press. Gendolla, G. H. E, & Krüsken, J. (2002c). The joint effect of
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. informational mood impact and performance-contingent
American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. incentive on effort-related cardiovascular response. Journal of
Efklides, A., Kourkoulou, A., Mitsiou, F., & Ziliaskopoulou, D. Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 271–285.
(2006). Metacognitive knowledge of effort, personality fac- Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2005). Ego involvement
tors, and mood state: Their relationships with effort-related and effort: Cardiovascular, electrodermal, and performance
metacognitive experiences. Metacognition and Learning, 1, effects. Psychophysiology, 42, 595–603.
33–49. Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2006a). Cardiovascular
Eisenberger, R. (1992). Learned industriousness. Psychological reactivity during performance under social observation: The
Review, 99, 248–267. moderating role of task difficulty. International Journal of Psy-
Elliot, A. J. (2006). The hierarchical model of approach-avoidance chophysiology, 62, 185–192.
motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 30, 111–116. Gendolla, G. H. E., & Richter, M. (2006b). Ego involvement
Elliott, R. (1969). Tonic heart rate: Experiments on the effects of and the difficulty law of motivation: Effects on performance-
collative variables lead to a hypothesis about its motivational related cardiovascular response. Personality and Social Psychol-
significance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12, ogy Bulletin, 32, 1188–1203.
211–228. Gendolla, G. H. E., Richter, M., & Silvia, P. (2008). Self-focus
Eubanks, L., Wright, R. A., & Williams, B. J. (2002). Reward and task difficulty effects on effort-related cardiovascular
influence on the heart: Cardiovascular response as a function reactivity. Psychophysiology, 45, 653–662.
of incentive value at five levels of task demand. Motivation Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2010). The implicit “go”:
and Emotion, 26, 139–152. Masked action cues directly mobilize mental effort. Psycho-
Fairclough, S. H., & Houston, K. (2004). A metabolic measure logical Science, 21, 1389–1393.
of mental effort. Biological Psychology, 66, 177–190. Gendolla, G. H. E., & Silvestrini, N. (2011). Smiles make it
Fowles, D. C. (1983). Motivational effects on heart rate and elec- easier and so do frowns: Masked affective stimuli influence
trodermal activity: Implications for research on personality mental effort. Emotion, 11, 320–328.
and psychopathology. Journal of Research in Personality, 17, Gendolla, G. H. E., & Wright, R. A. (2009). Effort. In D. Sander &
48–71. K. R. Scherer (Eds.), Oxford companion to the affective sciences
Frijda, N. H. (1993). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. (pp. 134–135). New York: Oxford University Press.
In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions Gianaros, P. J., Derbyshire, S. W. G., May, C., Siegle, G.,
(pp. 381–404). New York: Guilford. Gamalo, M. A., & Jennings, R. (2005). Anterior cingulate

 effort intensit y


activity correlates with blood pressure during stress. Psy- adjustment: Theory, research, and application (pp. 3–27).
chophysiology, 42, 627–635. New York: Plenum.
Gray, M. A., & Critchley, H. D. (2011). Integration of cardiac Manuck, S. B., Harvey, S. H., Lechleiter, S. L., & Neal, K. S.
function with cognitive, motivational, and emotional pro- (1978). Effects of coping on blood pressure responses to
cessing: Evidence from neuroimaging. In R. A. Wright & threat and avoidance stimulation. Psychophysiology, 15,
G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.), How motivation affects cardio- 544–549.
vascular response: Mechanisms and applications (pp. 21–42). Meyer, W. U., & Hallermann, B. (1977). Intended effort and
Washington, DC: APA Press. informational value of task outcome. Archiv für Psychologie,
Granholm, E., Verney, S. P., Perivoliotis, D., & Miura, T. (2007). 129, 131–140.
Effortful cognitive resource allocation and negative symptom Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of
severity in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance.
831–842. Psychological Review, 91, 328–346.
Harkins, S. G. (2006). Mere effort as the mediator of the Obrist, P. A. (1976). The cardiovascular-behavioral interaction as
evaluation-performance relationship. Journal of Personality it appears today. Psychophysiology, 13, 95–107.
and Social Psychology, 91, 436–455. Obrist, P. A. (1981). Cardiovascular psychophysiology: A perspec-
Harmon-Jones, E., Abramson, L. Y., Nusslock, R., Sigelman, J. D., tive. New York: Plenum Press.
Urosevic, S., Turonie, L. D., . . . Fearn, M. (2007). Effect of bipo- Papillo, J. F., & Shapiro, D. (1990). The cardiovascular system.
lar disorder on left frontal cortical responses to goals differing in In J. T. Cacioppo, & L. G. Tassinary (Eds), Principles of psy-
valence and task difficulty. Biological Psychiatry, 63, 693–698. chophysiology (pp. 465–512). New York: Cambridge Univer-
Heckhausen, H. & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents sity Press.
and cognitive functioning in motivational versus volitional Paus, T., Koski, L., Caramanos, Z., & Westbury, C. (1998).
states. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120. Regional differences in the effect of task difficulty and motor
Heider, F. (1958). The psychology of interpersonal relations. output on blood flow response in the human cingulated cor-
New York: Wiley. tex: A review of 107 PET activation studies. NeuroReport, 9,
Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2000). Decreased rspon- R37-R47.
siveness to reward in depression. Cognition and Emotion, 14, Pessiglione, M., Schmidt, L., Draganski, B., Kalisch, R., Lau, H.,
711–724. Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2007). How the brain translates
Hillgruber, A. (1912). Fortlaufende Arbeit und Willensbetätigung. money into force: A neuroimaging study of subliminal moti-
[Continuous work and the will]. Leipzig, Germany: Quelle vation. Science, 316, 904–906.
und Meyer. Pyszczynski, T., & Greenberg, T. (1983). Determinants of
Hockey, G. R. (1997). Compensatory control in the regulation reduction in intended effort as a strategy for coping with
of human performance under stress and high workload: anticipated failure. Journal of Research in Personality, 17,
A cognitive-energetical framework. Biological Psychology, 45, 412–422.
73–93. Rhodewalt, F., & Fairfield, M. (1991). Claimed self-handicaps
Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs, and the self-handicapper: The relation of reduction in
NJ: Prentice-Hall. intended effort to performance. Journal of Research in Person-
Kelsey, R. M. (2011). Beta-adrenergic cardiovascular reactiv- ality, 25, 402–417.
ity and adaptation to stress: The cardiac pre-ejection period Richter, M. (2010). Pay attention to your manipulation checks!
as an index of effort. In R. A. Wright, & G. H. E. Gendolla Reward impact on cardiac reactivity is moderated by task
(Eds.), How motivation affects cardiovascular response: Mecha- context. Biological Psychology, 84, 279–289.
nisms and applications (pp. 43–60). Washington, DC: APA Richter, M., Friedrich, A., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2008). Task
Press. difficulty effects on cardiac activity. Psychophysiology, 45,
Kemper, C. J., Leue, A., Wacker, J., Chavanon, M-L., Henning- 869–875.
hausen, E., & Stemmler, G. (2008). Agentic extraversion as Richter, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2006). Incentive effects
a predictor of effort-related cardiovascular response. Biologi- on cardiovascular reactivity in active coping with unclear
cal Psychology, 76, 191–199. task difficulty. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61,
Kennedy, D. O., & Scholey, A. B. (2000). Glucose administra- 216–225.
tion, heart rate and cognitve performance: Effects of increas- Richter, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2007). Incentive value,
ing mental effort. Psychopharmacology, 149, 63–71. unclear task difficulty, and cardiovascular reactivity in
Klein, G. S., & Schoenfeld, N. (1941). The influence of ego active coping. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 63,
involvement on confidence. Journal of Abnormal and Social 294–301.
Psychology, 36, 249–258. Richter, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009a). The heart contracts
Kreibig, S. D. (2010). Autonomic nervous system activity in to reward: Monetary incentives and preejection period. Psy-
emotion: A review. Biological Psychology, 84, 394–421. chophysiology, 46, 451–457.
Kuhl, J. (1983). Motivation, Konflikt und Handlungskontrolle Richter, M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009b). Mood impact on
[Motivation, conflict, and action control]. Berlin, Germany: cardiovascular reactivity when task difficulty is unclear. Moti-
Springer. vation and Emotion, 33, 239–248.
Kukla, A. (1972). Foundations of an attributional theory of per- Roets, A., Van Hiel, A., Cornelis, I., & Soetens, B. (2008). Deter-
formance. Psychological Review, 79, 454–470. minants of task performance and invested effort: A need for
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting task closure by relative cognitive capacity interaction analysis.
performance. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, Inc. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 779–795.
Maddux, J. E. (1995). Self-efficacy theory: An introduction. Schmidt, R. E., Richter, M., Gendolla, G. H. E., & van der Lin-
In J. E. Maddux (Ed.), Self-efficacy, adaptation, and den, M. (2010). Young poor sleepers mobilize extra effort

g en d oll a , w r i g h t, r i c h ter 


in an easy memory task: Evidence from cardiovascular mea- and high levels of avoidant task demand. Psychophysiology,
sures. Journal of Sleep Research, 19, 487–495. 32, 172–176.
Silvestrini, N., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009a). Mood-regulative Wright, R. A., & Franklin, J. (2004). Ability perception deter-
hedonic incentive interacts with mood and task difficulty to minants of effort-related cardiovascular response: Mood,
determine effort-related cardiovascular response and facial optimism, and performance resources. In R. A. Wright,
EMG. Biological Psychology, 82, 54–63. J. Greenberg, & S. S. Brehm (Eds), Motivational analyses of
Silvestrini, N., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2009b). The joint effect of social behavior: Building on Jack Brehm’s contributions to psy-
mood, task valence, and task difficulty on effort-related car- chology (pp. 187–204). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
diovascular response and facial EMG. International Journal of Wright, R. A., & Gregorich, S. (1989). Difficulty and instru-
Psychophysiology, 73, 226–234. mentality of imminent behavior as determinants of cardio-
Silvia, P. J., Jones, H. C., Kelly, C. S., & Zibaie, A. (2011). Trait vascular response and self-reported energy. Psychophysiology,
self-focused attention, task difficulty, and effort-related car- 26, 586–592.
diovascular reactivitgy. International Journal of Psychophysiol- Wright, R. A., Junious, T. R., Neal, C., Avello, A., Graham, C.,
ogy, 79, 335–340. Herrmann, L., Junious, S., & Walton, N. (2007). Mental
Silvia, P. J., McCord, D. M., & Gendolla, G. H. E. (2010). fatigue influence on effort-related cardiovascular response:
Self-focused attention, performance expectancies, and the Difficulty effects and extension across cognitive performance
intensity of effort: Do people try harder for harder goals? domains. Motivation and Emotion, 31, 219–231.
Motivation and Emotion, 34, 363–370. Wright, R. A., Killebrew, K., & Pimpalapure, D. (2002). Cardio-
Smith, T. W., Baldwin, M., & Christensen, A. J. (1990). Inter- vascular incentive effects where a challenge is unfixed: Dem-
personal influence as active coping: Effects of task difficulty onstrations involving social evaluation, evaluator status, and
on cardiovascular reactivity. Psychophysiology, 27, 429–437. monetary reward. Psychophysiology, 39, 188–197.
Sternberg, S. (1966). High-speed scanning in human memory. Wright, R. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Effort determination of
Science, 153, 652–654. cardiovascular response: An integrative analysis with applica-
Stewart, C. C., Wright, R. A., Hui, S. A., & Simmons, A. tions in social psychology. Advances in Experimental Social
(2009). Outcome expectancy as a moderator of mental Psychology, 33, 255–307.
fatigue influence on cardiovascular response. Psychophysiol- Wright, R. A., Martin, R. E., & Bland, J. L. (2003). Energy
ogy, 46, 1141–1149 resource depletion, task difficulty, and cardiovascular
Wilson, T. D. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adap- response to a mental arithmetic challenge. Psychophysiology,
tive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 40, 98–105.
Wright, R. A. (1984). Motivation, anxiety, and the difficulty Wright, R. A., & Penacerrada, D. (2002). Energy resource
of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, depletion, ability perception, and cardiovascular response to
1376–1388. behavioral challenge. Psychophysiology, 39, 182–187.
Wright, R. A. (1996). Brehm’s theory of motivation as a model Wright, R. A., & Stewart, C. C. (2011). Multifaceted effects of
of effort and cardiovascular response. In P. M. Gollwitzer & fatigue on effort and associated cardiovascular responses. In
J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cogni- R. A. Wright & G. H. E. Gendolla (Eds.) How motivation
tion and motivation to behavior (pp. 424–453). New York: affects cardiovascular response: Mechanisms and applications
Guilford. (pp. 199–218). Washington, DC: APA Press.
Wright, R. A. (1998). Ability perception and cardiovascular Wright, R. A., Tunstall, A. M., Williams, B. J., Goodwin, J. S., &
response to behavioral challenge. In M. Kofta, G. Weary, & Harmon-Jones, E. (1995). Social evaluation and cardiovas-
G. Sedek (Eds.), Personal control in action: Cognitive and moti- cular response: An active coping approach. Journal of Person-
vational mechanisms (pp. 197–232). New York: Plenum. ality and Social Psychology, 69, 530–543.
Wright, R. A., & Dill, J. C. (1993). Blood pressure responses Wright, R. A., Wadley, V. G., Pharr, R. P., & Butler, M. (1994).
and incentive appraisals as a function of perceived ability and Interactive influence of self-reported ability and avoidant task
objective task demand. Psychophysiology, 30, 152–160. demand on anticipatory cardiovascular responsivity. Journal
Wright, R. A., Dill, J. C., Geen, R. G., & Anderson, C. A. (1998). of Research in Personality, 28, 68–86.
Social evaluation influence on cardiovascular response to a Wright, R. A., Williams, B. J., & Dill, J. C. (1992). Interac-
fixed behavioral challenge: Effects across a range of difficulty tive effects of difficulty and instrumentality of avoidant
levels. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 20, 277–285. behavior on cardiovascular reactivity. Psychophysiology, 29,
Wright, R. A., & Dismukes, A. (1995). Cardiovascular effects 677–686.
of experimentally induced efficacy (ability) appraisals at low Zajonc, R. B. (1965). Social facilitation. Science, 149, 269–274.

 effort intensit y


PA RT
7
Motivation in
Application
This page intentionally left blank
CHAPTER

Motivation in Psychotherapy
25
Martin Grosse Holtforth and Johannes Michalak

Abstract
Motivational issues are central to human life. Correspondingly, they are also central to the challenging
endeavor of psychotherapy. Assisting patients to change involves motivational issues at various levels
and at various stages of therapy. Patients might be more or less motivated to begin and to participate
in the different stages of psychotherapy (therapy motivation). Besides these differences in therapy
motivation, an understanding of the broader concepts of motivation in psychotherapy should mandate
that motivational issues be considered in the treatment of all patients and not only in those with
obvious deficits in therapy motivation: Motivational issues influence the therapeutic relationship, they
should be considered in tailoring specific interventions, and they might be important factors for the
onset and maintenance of psychological disorders. This chapter presents theoretical and empirical
background information and illustrates therapeutic approaches for dealing with patients’ motivation.
Moreover, it summarizes the implications of basic and clinical research for a motivationally informed
psychotherapy.
Key Words: motivation, goals, psychotherapy, treatment, psychopathology

Motivation in Psychotherapy change behavior and experiences in order to enable


Motivation is central in life and governs most them to live independently after therapy.
psychological processes. According to Heckhausen Because motivational issues are central to human
and Heckhausen (2008): life, they are also central to the challenging endeavor of
psychotherapy. Assisting patients to change involves
the psychology of motivation is specifically concerned
motivational issues at various levels and at various
with activities that reflect the pursuit of a particular
stages of therapy. First of all, patients might be more
goal and, in this function, form a meaningful unit of
or less motivated to begin psychotherapy and to par-
behavior. Motivational research seeks to explain these
ticipate in the different stages of the psychotherapy
units of behavior in terms of their whys and hows.
process. Often, patients are willing to work very hard
(p. 1)
during the therapeutic process and to invest a lot to
Consequently, motivational processes also have a change their lives and their way of behavior. However,
central importance for the change of experience and there are also patients who are ambivalent during dif-
behavior, which is the main purpose of psychother- ferent stages of the therapeutic process. They may be
apy. Psychotherapy patients seek help for the parts of ambivalent about whether they should start therapy,
their lives that they failed to cope with themselves. whether they should frame a certain kind of behav-
Therapists strive to optimally assist their patients to ior as a problematic behavior, or whether they should


take steps to change a problematic behavior. All psy- aversive outcomes such as diminished well-being or
chotherapists are confronted with these variations of psychopathology might be consequences (Flanagan,
therapy motivation in their patients, and they know 2010). Various lists of needs have been proposed, for
that it is useful to skillfully deal with the patients who example, self-enhancement, attachment, pleasure, and
show reduced therapy motivation. orientation/control by Epstein (1990), or relatedness,
However, a broader perspective on motivation in competence, and autonomy by Deci and Ryan (1985,
psychotherapy would indicate that it might be use- 1995). Overarching metaneeds have also been sug-
ful to consider motivational issues in the treatment gested, such as a need for meaning (Heine, Proulx, &
of all patients and not only in those with obvious Vohs, 2006) or consistency as the most basic compo-
deficits in therapy motivation: Motivational forces nent of psychological functioning (Grawe, 2004).
influence the therapeutic relationship, and it may be Within the concept of motives, a general dis-
wise to consider them when trying to build a helpful tinction can be made between implicit and explicit
therapeutic alliance. Moreover, specific interventions motives. Implicit motives are seen as enduring indi-
might be tailored to the motivational background vidual motive dispositions, whereas explicit motives
of the patients. For example, the specific situation refer to goals that are conscious or consciously
patients are confronted with in exposure therapy accessible (Heckhausen & Heckhausen, 2008). The
might not only be chosen because of the nature of implicit motivational system consists of a relatively
their avoidance behavior but also because of the small number of motives (i.e., achievement, power,
important personal goals the patient strives for. and affiliation motives) that are unconscious, holis-
On the most fundamental level, some authors have tically represented, and are more directly linked to
identified insufficient satisfaction of basic psycho- emotional processes. It becomes apparent that the
logical needs as an important factor in the etiology concept of implicit motives as used by Heckhausen
of various psychological problems and psychological and Heckhausen (2008) is very similar to the con-
disorders (Grawe, 2004; Ryan, 2005; Ryan, Deci, cept of needs described earlier, and the concept of
Grolnick, & La Guardia, 2006). Accordingly, from explicit motives is very similar to the concept of high-
this perspective, the overall goal of psychotherapy level goals. Another general motivational construct
should be to increase the degree of satisfaction of is the construct of values. According to Rokeach
motivational needs in order to reduce psychopatho- (1973), “Values generally are defined as preferences
logical conditions. for certain outcomes or modes of conduct” (as cited
Therapists dealing with motivational issues in in Locke, 2000, p. 250). Such preferences can be
psychotherapy could profit from a deeper under- shared by a whole community (cultural values) or be
standing of basic motivational principles in human individual (personal values). The concept of personal
life. Therefore, the first part of our chapter presents values is also very similar to high-level goals.
theoretical and empirical background information A central and well-documented assumption of
as well as selected methods of assessing motivational goal-oriented approaches is that, to a considerable
constructs that might be relevant for psychother- extent, people’s daily behaviors, thoughts, and emo-
apy research and practice. The second part of the tions are linked to the pursuit of personal goals and
chapter reviews important theoretical and empirical are regulated by feedback regarding goal attainment
literature from clinical research pointing to the rel- (Austin & Vancouver, 1996; Carver & Scheier, 1996;
evance of motivational variables in psychotherapy Klinger, 1977). Personal goals can be defined as elabo-
and illustrating therapeutic approaches for dealing rate cognitive representations of what a person wants
with patients’ motivation. In the final part of this to achieve or avoid in his or her current life circum-
chapter, we will summarize the implications of basic stances and are conscious, symbolically represented,
and clinical research for a motivationally informed and stored in a language-related manner (Brunstein,
psychotherapy. Schultheiss, & Graessmann, 1998). It is assumed that
approach goals are developed to satisfy psychological
Clinically Relevant Motivational Constructs needs, whereas avoidance goals are developed to pre-
We will briefly introduce the following motiva- vent these needs from being hurt (Grawe, 2004; see
tional constructs: psychological needs, motives, per- also Elliot, 2008, for a review). People may pursue
sonal goals, values, therapy motivation, and treatment personal goals as diverse in content as, for example,
goals. The concept of psychological needs implies that making new friends, improving my professional situ-
everybody has the same needs, everybody must satisfy ation, learning how to be more spontaneous, trying
them, and, if the individual fails to satisfy these needs, to be a better parent, or overcoming fear of rejection

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


(Chulef, Read, & Walsh, 2001). Grosse Holtforth experience participation in treatment as a freely
and Grawe (2000) empirically identified the con- made choice emanating from themselves” (Zuroff
tents of personal goals that therapists considered to et al., 2007, p. 137). If the patient has a favorable
be especially relevant for their patients. Examples are therapy motivation, he or she will cooperate with the
to perform well, to be in a committed relationship, therapist and the protocol, will disclose private expe-
to avoid being humiliated, or to avoid showing weak- riences, will test out new patterns of behavior, will
nesses. The totality of all goals a person strives for (the show low resistance to the therapist’s intervention,
person’s goal structure) can be viewed as his or her and will unlikely drop out of treatment (basic behav-
individual future-oriented side giving purpose, struc- ior). The patient’s therapy expectations may also influ-
ture, and meaning to life (Cantor, 1990; Emmons, ence the therapy process and outcome. Examples for
1986; Klinger, 1977; Michalak & Grosse Holtforth, therapy expectations are hope for improvement of
2006). well-being, hope for improvement of relationships,
A patient usually seeks psychotherapy when he fear of adverse side effects, or fear of being ridiculed
or she experiences an unbearable level of suffering (Schulte & Eifert, 2002). The patient’s motivation as
being caused by unpleasant changes, experiences, well as his or her expectations may also be influenced
and/or behaviors. Examples for such changes are psy- by the patient’s concepts of psychological illness and
chopathological symptoms, unpleasant social inter- change (Calnan, 1987).
actions, inadequate performance at work, reduced Treatment goals can be defined as “intended
enjoyment of leisure activities, or a general dissatis- changes in behavior and experience to be attained
faction with life. The person evaluates these experi- by therapy, which patient and therapist agree upon
ences as problems because they are discrepant with at the beginning of treatment and on which success-
his or her personal goals. When the person evaluates ful psychotherapy should be instrumental” (Grosse
these changes as abnormal, believes that he or she is Holtforth & Grawe, 2002, p. 79). Treatment goals
incapable of dealing with them alone, and has at least have various functions in psychotherapy (Driessen
minimal hope that he or she will receive some relief et al., 2001). On an individual level, treatment goals
from suffering through psychotherapy, the person focus patients’ and therapists’ attention, guide treat-
may seek professional help (Schulte & Eifert, 2002). ment planning, and provide criteria for outcome
Apart from unbearable levels of suffering, other fac- assessment (e.g., goal-attainment scaling, see ear-
tors may also contribute to the person’s decision to lier). In addition, treatment goals fulfill an ethical
seek help, such as expectations or pressures by signifi- function by providing transparency for the patient,
cant others or the wish to receive an absolution from by balancing the power between the therapist and
guilt feelings by receiving a medical diagnosis, which the patient, and by supporting the patient in giving
may have been caused by a sense of burdensomeness his or her informed consent. Treatment goals also
to one’s loved-ones (Schneider, 1990). help to define similarities and differences between
Therapy motivation is influenced by various expe- different therapeutic approaches. Finally, informa-
riences, including (a) suffering, (b) positive experi- tion on patients’ treatment goals may support the
ences (hope of relief ), (c) fear of change, and (d) the optimization of treatment programs by providing
therapeutic bond (wish to maintain the therapeutic feedback on the needs of specific patient groups
relationship). Patient suffering may also be influ- (Uebelacker et al., 2008). Table 25.1 shows an
enced by a multitude of factors, that is, impairment empirically constructed list of patients’ goals in psy-
(psychopathological symptoms and negative affect), chotherapy (Grosse Holtforth & Grawe, 2002).
the experience of being abnormal, or the feeling of
helplessness (Schulte & Eifert, 2002). Other factors Assessing Motivational Constructs
may also be gains from illness, be they material (e.g., in Psychotherapy
wish for a pension) or psychological (e.g., attention, As implicit motives are unconscious by definition,
support, protection; Schulte & Eifert, 2002). they cannot be assessed directly by self-report. Implicit
Probably the most favorable approach that patients motives have traditionally been assessed using pro-
can bring to therapy would be one that is character- jective techniques like the Thematic Apperception
ized by interest, curiosity, and commitment. The term Test (TAT; Smith, 1992). During the TAT, respon-
autonomous motivation (Ryan & Deci, 2000) closely dents are asked to write fantasy stories in response to
resembles this ideal kind of therapy motivation. For several pictures depicting motive-arousing scenarios.
the purposes of psychotherapy, autonomous motiva- Several systems for deriving motive scores (achieve-
tion can be defined as “the extent to which patients ment, power, and affiliation) from TAT stories have

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


Table 25.1. Bern Inventory of Treatment Goals (BIT-T)
Problem-/ Interpersonal Well-Being/ Existential Personal Residual cat-
Symptom- Oriented Functioning Growth egory (R)

Depression Intimate Exercise, activity Self- Attitude Regeneration


Suicidality relationships Relaxation/ reflection toward self Psychosocial
Fears/anxiety Current family composure and future Desires and rehabilitation
Obsessions/comp. Family of origin Well-being Finding wishes Somatic
Traumatic experiences Other specific meaning Self-control rehabilitation
Substance use relationships Emotion
Eating behavior Loneliness, grief regulation
Sleep Assertiveness
Sexuality Contact/closeness
Somatic problems
Stress
Medication

been developed (Smith, 1992). Although suitable rate these individual goals on various dimensions to
for clinical research, these systems are not yet suited allow for inter-individual comparisons. Goals can be
for routine clinical use due to missing norms, ques- rated by the participants themselves or by indepen-
tionable or unknown test-retest reliability, and so on dent raters (e.g., categorization of goals by content or
(Lilienfeld, Wood & Garb, 2000). Recently, several according to the approach and avoidance quality of
alternatives for assessing implicit motives (especially the goals). Examples of this idiographic-nomothetic
the achievement motive) have been introduced. approach are the Personal Projects Matrix (Little,
For example, Brunstein and Schmitt (2004) devel- 1983), the Personal Concerns Inventory (Cox &
oped an Implicit Association Test (IAT; Greenwald, Klinger, 2002), and its immediate antecedents, such
McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998) to assess achievement as the Concern Dimensions Questionnaires (Klinger,
motivation by evaluating the strength of association Barta, & Maxeiner, 1980) and the Interview Ques-
between achievement-related adjectives and the self- tionnaire (Klinger, 1987). These instruments pre-
concept. However, such newer techniques are too ceded and/or gave rise to other methods such as
time consuming for routine clinical use. the Striving Assessment Scale (Emmons, 1986) and
Explicit motivational constructs can be assessed the Goal Assessment Battery (Karoly & Ruehlman,
efficiently using questionnaire methods. A sample 1995). All of these approaches make it possible to
questionnaire to assess various dimensions of therapy assess theory-derived indices that seek to achieve
motivation is the Patient Motivation for Therapy a multilevel understanding of goals (e.g., goal impor-
Scale (CMTS; Pelletier, Tuson, & Haddad, 1997). tance, goal achievement, goal conflict, etc.).
Based on the self-determination theory proposed Furthermore, various interviews to assess per-
by Deci and Ryan (1985), the CMTS measures sonal goals have been developed (e.g., AIMS; Wad-
patients’ intrinsic motivation, four forms of regula- sworth & Ford, 1983), allowing for an extensive
tion for extrinsic motivation (integrated, identified, description of the various goals and their mutual
introjected, and external regulation), and motivation relationships. When standardized goal question-
for therapy. An example for the assessment of values naires (e.g., Ford & Nichols, 1991; Grosse Holtforth
in the interpersonal realm is the Circumplex Scales of & Grawe, 2000; Grosse Holtforth & Grawe, 2003;
Interpersonal Values (CSIV; Locke, 2000). The prin- Grosse Holtforth, Grawe, & Tamcan, 2004; Reiss
ciple of a circumplex structure implies that variables & Havercamp, 1998; Ryff, 1989) are utilized, the
that measure interpersonal relations are arranged participants are presented with goals that have to be
around a circle in a two-dimensional space, with the evaluated with respect to dimensions such as impor-
dimensions being agency and communion. tance, strain, progress, or realization. Compared to
Personal goals are most often assessed in basic other methods, standardized questionnaires offer
research using a combined idiographic-nomothetic the advantage that the goal contents are comparable.
approach. The first step (the idiographic part) is to Furthermore, goal assessment is less dependent on
ask the participants to generate a list of personal goals. recall processes. However, the personal salience and
In the second step (the nomothetic part), patients ecological sensitivity of the idiographic-nomothetic

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


approach associated with the individual formula- Kunkel & Newsom, 1996; Miller & Thompson,
tion of goals is diminished by the standardized goal 1973). The attainment of individually formulated
presentation. Conflict matrixes are used to examine therapy goals can be measured using, for example,
the interrelationship and possible conflicts among the Goal Attainment Scaling (GAS) procedure (Kire-
goals (e.g., Cox, Klinger, & Blount, 1999; Emmons suk, Smith, & Cardillo, 1994). In the first step, GAS
& King, 1988). To measure the amount of conflict procedures define possible areas of change. Then, the
that exists between pairs of goals, participants com- patient and therapist join forces to explore and for-
pare each goal with every other goal and ask them- mulate as concretely as possible for each area what
selves, “Does being successful in this goal have a would constitute an improvement to, stagnation in,
helpful, a harmful, or no effect on the other goal?” or a deterioration of the current state. The degree of
A strategy to assess the person’s clinically rele- goal attainment can be assessed in the course of treat-
vant personal goals is Plan Analysis (Caspar, 2007). ment or after termination by patients, therapists, or
According to Caspar, Grossmann, Unmüssig, and independent raters. The goal-attainment scaling may
Schramm (2005), a “person’s Plan structure is the then be viewed as individualized measures of treat-
total of conscious and unconscious strategies this ment success (for a discussion of methodological
person has developed to satisfy his or her needs” limitations of GAS, see Hill & Lambert, 2004).
(p. 92). The patient’s Plan structure can be derived
from various sources of information (e.g., biograph- Personal Goals, Well-Being, and
ical information, behavioral observations, and the Psychological Problems
patient’s impact on others). The main question guid- After considering methodological issues in assess-
ing the assessment process is: What is the explicit ing motivational constructs, we will now present
or implicit purpose of this patient’s behavior? In research findings that underscore the importance of
a simplified form, Plan Analysis can be done and personal goals for well-being and psychological prob-
used in collaboration with the patient to enhance lems. We will focus on personal goals because most of
his or her understanding of certain parts of his or the empirical research has used personal goals as a unit
her functioning (Caspar, 2007). The result of a Plan of analysis. In everyday life, humans pursue multiple
Analysis is a graphic display of the structure of the goals in various areas, such as family, work, career, rec-
patient’s most important approach and avoidance reation, or spirituality using various individual strate-
goals as well as his or her individual means (Plans gies (Karoly, 2006). As there is a multitude of ways
and behaviors) toward pursuing these goals (see to pursue one’s goals successfully, there are also many
Fig. 25.1). ways to fail in reaching one’s goals. How effectively a
Treatment goals can also be formulated and person strives for his or her goals can be seen as key
assessed in a more or less standardized form. Vari- criterion for effective adjustment (Karoly, 2006). Suc-
ous questionnaires and checklists for a standardized ceeding or failing to reach one’s goals not only has
assessment of treatment goals have been developed important consequences for the individual’s happiness
(e.g., Driessen et al., 2001; Grosse Holtforth, 2001; and well-being (for reviews see Brunstein, Schultheiss,

Fit of goals (motive-


congruence, self-
concordance,
content of goals)

Commitment to
Progress in goal Emotional
pursue personal
striving well-being
goals

Realizability of
personal goals

Fig. 25.1. Teleonomic model of subjective well-being (Brunstein & Maier, 2002, p. 163, modified).

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


& Maier, 1999; Emmons & Kaiser, 1996; Schmuck than healthy subjects (Watkins, 2011). In addition,
& Sheldon, 2001) but can also contribute to the more goal progress can be also expected if the social
development and maintenance of serious psychologi- network supports a person’s goals (Brunstein, 1993;
cal problems and disorders. Brunstein, Dangelmayer, & Schultheiss, 1996; Rue-
In the following, we will highlight selected findings hlman & Wolchik, 1988). However, which goals the
as pertaining to the associations of goal functioning social network supports and what the consequences
to well-being and psychopathology. According to Per- for the individual’s well-being are may also depend on
vin (1990), Karoly (1999), and Grawe (2004), psy- the cultural context. For example, whereas indepen-
chopathology can develop when a person is unable dent goal pursuit (fun and enjoyment) increased well-
to attain his or her goals over an extended period of being among European Americans but less among
time, when goal attainment is threatened by personal Asian Americans, interdependent goal pursuit (pleas-
or external circumstances, or when dysfunctional pro- ing parents and friends) increased the well-being of
cesses occur in goal-oriented self-regulation. Concur- Asian Americans but not of European Americans
rently, psychopathology can be seen as “disturbances to (Oishi & Diener, 2001).
the normal processes and structures by which humans A central assumption in the teleonomic model is
consciously and nonconsciously guide their actions, that successful goal striving does not inevitably lead
emotions, and thoughts in the service of achieving to happiness and well-being, but rather if the goals
meaningful life goals” (Karoly, 2006, p. 367). fit the person. Goals may be pursued for extrinsic
Brunstein and Maier (2002) summarized find- or intrinsic reasons, and the various content of goals
ings from basic research on personal goals in a teleo- also seems to make a difference. Many studies have
nomic model of subjective well-being (see Fig. 25.1). shown that the pursuit of goals is especially associated
In the teleonomic model, goal commitment, as well with a sense of well-being if the goals are well inte-
as goal realizability is assumed to causally influence grated into the person’s self-system (self-concordance;
well-being. If someone pursues his or her goals with Sheldon, 2001), that is, if goals are pursued because
commitment (i.e., he or she identifies with the goals one has consciously accepted the values underlying
and feels motivated to realize them), and if his or such behavior as personally important and mean-
her life situation facilitates the attainment of these ingful (identified regulation) or because the pursuit
goals, progress in goal striving is more likely. Goal of these goals is in itself satisfying and rewarding
progress, in turn, is assumed to contribute to the (intrinsic regulation). In contrast, goals are not inte-
person’s emotional well-being. grated into the self-system if they are mainly pursued
The basic assumptions of the teleonomic model because of external reward (“extrinsic regulation”) or
of emotional well-being have been supported by internal pressure (e.g., feelings of guilt or embarrass-
a multitude of research findings (Brunstein et al., ment, “introjected regulation”; for reviews see Deci
1999). For example, through longitudinal studies & Ryan, 2002; Ryan & Deci, 2000; Ryan, Sheldon,
on various groups of participants (Brunstein, 1999; Kasser, & Deci, 1996; Sheldon, 2001). Whenever
Maier & Brunstein, 2001, Wiese & Freund, 2005), goals correspond with a person’s personal values and
it has been demonstrated that people who (a) are interests, or the pursuit of the goals is itself satisfying,
strongly committed to strive for their goals, and he or she will—even in times when the pursuit of
(b) who view their life circumstances as favorable for goals is fraught with difficulties or exertion—be more
goal striving, achieved a greater degree of progress in able to activate emotional resources and thus persist
goal attainment and greater increase in emotional in the pursuit. Concurrently, in a meta-analytical
well-being than people who were less committed review by Koestner, Lekes, Powers, and Chicoine
to goals for which conditions were more unfavor- (2002), the positive effects of self-concordant goals
able. In addition, people whose goals are in conflict on goal attainment were consistently shown even
with each other or are poorly integrated (Michalak, after controlling for other relevant variables such as
Heidenreich, & Hoyer, 2011) or who have goals that neuroticism, goal efficiency, and commitment.
are abstract and not clearly formulated (Emmons, A closely related, but not quite the same aspect as
1996) show lower subjective well-being and satisfac- self-concordance (Kasser & Ryan, 2001) refers to the
tion with life. Furthermore, dysregulated goal/action content dimension of goals. Kasser and Ryan (1993,
identification seems to relate to various psychological 1996) have coined the term external goals for goals the
symptoms and disorders such as depression or social focus of which is to increase one’s status in the eyes of
anxiety in the sense that patients with these disor- others, as compared to internal goals, which are geared
ders identify negative events at a more abstract level toward the fulfillment of inherently personal needs,

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


such as competence, autonomy, and relatedness, and Wilson, 1999). Such negative experiences can relate
therefore fit people’s deeper, psychologically funda- to various types of events, such as abandonment,
mental needs. A series of studies (e.g., Kasser & Ryan, criticism, or failure (Grosse Holtforth, Grawe, Egger,
1993, 1996; Schmuck, Kasser, & Ryan, 2000; Shel- & Berking, 2005).
don & Kasser, 1998) showed that people who mainly Empirical research showed that psychotherapy
strove for external goals displayed a lower level of well- patients pursue more avoidance goals than normal
being than people who devoted their lives to the attain- controls, and that the intensity of avoidance goals
ment of internal goals. Püschel, Schulte, and Michalak correlates with the decreased levels of goal satisfac-
(2011) investigated associations between goal fit tion, poor well-being, severity of psychopathology,
and psychopathology in a sample of 61 psychotherapy and other psychological problems in psychotherapy
outpatients with heterogeneous diagnoses. In accor- patients as well as in normal controls (Grosse Holtforth
dance with the teleonomic model, results showed that & Grawe, 2000; Grosse Holtforth & Grawe, 2003).
only motive-congruent goal progress was related to However, avoidance goals do not have to be uniformly
depressivity. Patients who made more progress at goals maladaptive but might protect individuals from being
that matched their implicit motives experienced fewer deeply frustrated and hurt in harmful environments
depressive symptoms, whereas patients who failed to (Grosse Holtforth, Bents, Mauler, & Grawe, 2006).
make progress at motive matching goals experienced Michalak, Püschel, Joormann, and Schulte (2006)
more depressive symptoms. Motive-incongruent prog- examined avoidance tendencies in the explicit and the
ress did not have any effect on depressive symptoms. implicit modes. In a sample of students and psycho-
Several studies with student samples demon- therapy patients, avoidance tendencies within the
strated associations between goal functioning and explicit system of personal goals as well as in implicit
psychopathology. For example, in a study by Lecci, motives were associated with symptoms, even when
Karoly, Briggs, and Kuhn (1994) “negative” goal controlling for the other mode.
characteristics such as high stress and difficulty, low Several studies examined goal functioning in
goal structure, low expectations regarding control, specific mental disorders (Michalak, Klappheck, &
perceived insufficiency of own capabilities, and low Kosfelder, 2004; Pöhlmann, 1999; Stangier, Ukrow,
expectations of success were associated with increased Schermelleh-Engel, Grabe, & Lauterbach, 2007).
depression and anxiety. Correspondingly, Cohen and Pöhlmann (1999) compared the personal goals of
Cohen (1996, 2001) found in an extensive prospec- psychosomatic patients with the goals of a psycho-
tive longitudinal study that children and adolescents logically healthy sample. Psychosomatic patients
who set high priority on materialistic and hedonistic generally pursued more goals than psychologically
goals (i.e., external goals) showed a higher incidence healthy subjects. However, the goals were formulated
of almost all Axis-I and Axis-II DSM-III diagno- as avoidance goals rather than approach goals, and
ses later in life. Furthermore, a number of studies the scope of the goals was narrower. Compared to
found associations between goal conflicts, impaired psychologically healthy subjects, goals expressing the
well-being, and psychopathology (for a review, see desire to change oneself were mentioned more often,
Michalak, Heidenreich, & Hoyer, 2011). as well as health-related goals. Michalak et al. (2004)
Pursuing a high proportion of avoidance goals investigated the aspect of goal attainment as well as
relative to approach goals is associated with less goal fit in a study with psychotherapy outpatients
perceived goal progress and seems to be particularly with anxiety and mood disorders. Both probability
detrimental to one’s well-being and functioning (see of goal attainment as well as the self-concordance
Elliot & Friedman, 2007; Tamir & Diener, 2008, of goals (intrinsically vs. extrinsically motivated goal
for reviews). Avoidance goals may exert this negative striving) correlated highly with symptom distress.
influence because the monitoring and management In addition, Stangier and colleagues (2007) found
of goal process is harder for avoidance goals than for that depressed inpatients showed higher scores for
approach goals, because avoidance goals elicit more inconsistencies among different goals/values as well
negative cognitions and emotions (Tamir & Diener, as between goals/values and their perceived realiza-
2008), and avoidance goals hinder the satisfaction tion as compared with controls. Strauman (2002)
of important personal goals as well as associated proposed in his self-regulation model of depression
psychological needs (Grawe, 2004). Particularly, an that individuals who are unable to pursue promo-
avoidance of aversive experiences may contribute to tion goals effectively are at risk for mood disorder
the development and perpetuation of mental prob- because of their chronic inability to satisfy these
lems (Grosse Holtforth, 2008; Hayes, Strosahl, & goals. In this model, depression results from and

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


maintains disruption of the mechanisms of incentive motivational factors in psychotherapy as they occur
motivation (Dickson & MacLeod, 2004; Strauman naturally in the process of treatment, then we will
et al., 2006). Empirical studies indeed indicate that outline therapeutic interventions that explicitly aim
an inability to attain promotion goals is predictive at changing motivational factors.
of dysphoric mood and depressive symptoms (e.g.,
Scott & O’Hara, 1993). goals, therapy motivation,
In the motivational model of alcoholism by Cox and motive change
and Klinger (1988, 1990), dysfunctional goal char- Zuroff and colleagues (2007) examined the role
acteristics possess a central role as pathogenic factors of autonomous motivation in a randomized con-
in the development and maintenance of substance trolled trial comparing interpersonal therapy, cog-
abuse and addictions. The model assumes that the bal- nitive behavior therapy, or pharmacotherapy with
ance between the expectation of positive and negative clinical management for depressed outpatients. They
affective consequences of substance use determines found that autonomous motivation assessed at ses-
the course and outcome of an episode of alcohol con- sion 3 was a stronger predictor of outcome than
sumption. The satisfaction a person is able to draw therapeutic alliance across all three treatments. In
from other areas of his or her life strongly contrib- addition, patients who perceived their therapists as
utes to the decision for or against the consumption of more autonomy supportive reported higher auton-
alcohol. When a person is unable to get satisfaction omous motivation. Thus, it seems that fostering
from other sources of reinforcement, the risk increases autonomous motivation in psychotherapy is a gen-
that he or she will use alcohol or other substances to eral facilitator of favorable treatment outcomes.
find pleasure and emotional relief. In various studies Certain goal characteristics might influence the
with students with more pronounced alcohol-related therapeutic process by enhancing or decreasing the
problems, Cox et al. (2002) showed that unfavorable motivation to actively engage in treatment and to
goal structures were associated with the amount of attain treatment goals. Ryan, Plant, and O’Malley
alcohol consumed. Compared with students, alco- (1995) found that alcoholic patients who were pres-
holic patients reported fewer goals and reported less sured to participate in a treatment program showed
average commitment to them but also reported higher dropout rates compared to patients who
less average commitment relative to the return they experienced their participation as more intrinsi-
expected from their goal striving (Man, Stuchlik- cally motivated. In addition, Michalak, Klappheck,
ova, & Klinger, 1998). It might be that alcoholics and Kosfelder (2004) investigated the correlation of
need more expected rewards to become committed optimism about goal attainment and the intrinsic
to goals. A recent study by Sevincer and Oettingen orientation of patients’ general goals with session
(2009) demonstrated that the relationship between outcome. Optimism as well as goal fit (i.e., self-
goal striving and psychopathology is not unidirec- concordance of goals) correlated strongly with ses-
tional but a “double-edged causal sword” (Karoly, sion success. This correlation was not mediated by
2006, p. 369). Sevincer and Oettingen (2009) found the patients’ psychopathological state, so it can be
in an experimental study that alcohol consumption assumed that motivational aspects are responsible
creates strong commitments even in light of low for the correlation. Klappheck and Michalak (2009),
expectations. However, in a longitudinal study, once analyzing the same sample, found that only opti-
sober again, formerly intoxicated participants with mism to reach the goal of symptom relief was related
low expectations did not follow up on their strong to treatment outcome. However, self-concordance
commitments over a 3-week period. The authors of goals failed to predict treatment outcome.
interpreted the findings as showing that alcohol seems Various studies have investigated the question of
to produce “empty goal commitments,” as commit- whether conflicts between a patient’s general goals
ments are not based on individuals’ expectations. and poor integration of the goal structure (i.e., the
extent of mutual support or hindrance of goals) is
Motivational Factors in Psychotherapy associated with the motivation to become actively
Research and Practice involved in therapy (for a review, see Michalak
In the following, we will examine various ways et al., 2011). For example, in a study conducted
in which motivational factors are relevant in the by Michalak and Schulte (2002), goal conflicts in
practice of psychotherapy—either as facilitators of patients with anxiety disorders were negatively cor-
treatment process and outcome or as targets of psy- related with patients’ basic behavior during therapy
chological change. First, we will examine the role of as well as treatment success. Conflict was measured

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


using the Striving Instrumentality Matrix (SIM; behavior is assumed to foster each of the essential
Emmons & King, 1988). Moreover, basic behav- parts of the therapeutic alliance: therapeutic bond,
ior also correlated with some measures of treatment agreement on therapeutic tasks, and agreement
success (Michalak, Kosfelder, Meyer, & Schulte, on therapeutic goals (Bordin, 1979). The Motiva-
2003). However, Michalak et al. (2004) were unable tional Attunement approach is similar to Alliance
to replicate correlations between goal conflicts and Fostering Therapy proposed by Crits-Christoph
basic behavior in another sample of psychotherapy et al. (2006), which is conceptualized as a supple-
outpatients. In two studies with alcoholic inpatients ment to existing empirically supported therapies.
and inpatients receiving treatment for drug addic- Among others, maximally informing patients about
tion, Heidenreich (2000) showed negative correla- the indicated therapeutic procedures and discussing
tions between the degree of conflict concerning the them with the patients until reaching an agreement
goal “personal change” and attitudes toward change- will foster task agreement (Crits-Christoph et al.,
relevant topics. These attitudes were operationalized 2006). The therapist may also bolster motivation-
according to the transtheoretical model developed by ally unattractive techniques by putting the task into
Prochaska, DiClemente, and Norcross (1992): Will- the service of other approach goals and by activating
ingness to contemplate changing problematic abuse other resources. For example, the therapist might
(“Contemplation”) and to actively cope with the say, “This exposure exercise will finally enable you to
abuse (“Action”; see also see Cox & Klinger, 2002). spend relaxed afternoons downtown shopping with
your daughter and enjoying life a little more.” Moti-
motivational interventions as vational attunement may also help to prevent alli-
facilitators of change ance ruptures (Safran & Muran, 2000). Empirical
Therapists can use motivational factors to facili- results show that potential precipitants of alliance
tate change in at least two ways. Therapists can fos- ruptures occur as either “therapist does something
ter a good therapeutic relationship by tailoring the that the patient does not want or need” or as “the
therapy to the patient’s motives and they can try to therapist fails to do something that the patient wants
formulate maximally helpful treatment goals. or needs” (Ackerman & Hilsenroth, 2001, p. 183).
Therefore, a therapist will be well advised to be aware
Fostering the Therapeutic Relationship of the patient’s salient approach and avoidance goals
One of the most important tasks of a therapist in order (a) not to miss important expectations or
at the beginning of psychotherapy is to establish needs or (b) commit interactional blunders that fit
a good therapeutic relationship and productive the patient’s individual vulnerabilities.
working alliance (Horvath, 1995). From a motiva-
tional perspective, how well a behavior is received by Formulating Treatment Goals
another person depends on how effectively a behav- Treatment goals may first come to mind when
ior helps to satisfy the other person’s needs and goals. thinking about goals in psychotherapy. As patients
According to Ryan and Deci (2000), a therapeutic and therapists may have different goals for therapy,
relationship promoting the fundamental needs for it is important to distinguish treatment goals from
competence, autonomy, and relatedness constitutes (a) naïve treatment concerns presented by the
a prerequisite for a successful integration of goals patients and (b) from treatment goals defined exclu-
into the self-system. However, a therapist can go sively by the therapist. An agreement on therapy
beyond supporting generic psychological needs that goals is considered a central ingredient of the work-
are assumed to be shared by all human beings. For ing alliance. Naïve treatment concerns are what a
this, the therapist may individualize his or her behav- patient hopes to accomplish in the course of therapy
ior to accommodate the patient’s individual goals. and are usually closely connected to the problem
In their motivational attunement approach, Grosse the person suffers from. These naïve treatment con-
Holtforth and Castonguay (2005) described ways cerns can be seen as a subset of the patient’s general
to tailor therapeutic interventions to the patient’s personal goals (Pöhlmann, 1999). Naïve treatment
goals and motives in order to foster the therapeu- concerns may or may not parallel the goals for ther-
tic relationship and therapeutic outcome. By show- apy that the therapist holds. On the other hand,
ing motivationally attuned behavior, the therapist what the therapist sees as the goals of treatment may
attempts to satisfy important approach goals of the be strongly influenced by various factors, includ-
patient while activating avoidance goals no more ing therapeutic orientation (Arnow & Castonguay,
than necessary. A motivationally attuned therapist 1996; Dirmaier, Harfst, Koch, & Schulz, 2006;

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


Philips, 2009). Empirically, agreement between the anxiety disorder show a higher proportion of explic-
patients’ and the therapists’ treatment goals seems itly symptom-oriented goals (e.g., “having fewer epi-
to have positive effects on the process and outcome sodes of binge-eating,” “being able to go shopping
of psychotherapy (Tryon & Winograd, 2001; see by myself again”; Faller & Goßler, 1998) than do
also Orlinsky, Ronnestad, & Willutzki, 2004). patients with mood disorders. The latter show—in
However, the rather low correspondence between addition to disorder-typical goals (e.g., “being able
patients’ goals and therapists’ goals found in earlier to find pleasure in everyday activities”)—many goals
studies indicates that it is necessary for the therapist that focus on interpersonal or existential issues (e.g.,
to explicitly strive for this agreement (Dimsdale, “resolving my marital conflicts”). Similar diagnostic
1975; Dimsdale, Klerman, & Shershow, 1979; differences could be found in other samples of inpa-
Polak, 1970; Thompson & Zimmerman, 1969). tients and outpatients (Berking, Grosse Holtforth,
In the process of goal definition, the therapist Jacobi, & Kröner-Herwig, 2005; Dirmaier et al.,
helps the patient to translate the often vaguely 2006; Grosse Holtforth & Grawe, 2002; Grosse
worded treatment objectives and wishes into well- Holtforth, Reubi, Ruckstuhl, Berking, & Grawe,
formed therapeutic goals, so that they optimally 2004; Grosse Holtforth, Wyss, Schulte, Trachsel, &
fulfill the aforementioned functions. Several char- Michalak, 2009; Uebelacker et al., 2008).
acteristics of a well-formed therapeutic goal can be So far, only a few studies have examined the
formulated (Michalak & Grosse Holtforth, 2006; association between the content of therapeutic goals
Willutzki & Koban, 2004). Optimally, treatment and therapy outcome. In a study analyzing the treat-
goals are negotiated and agreed upon with the ment goals of 2,770 inpatients in psychosomatic
patient. In the goal selection process, those goals rehabilitation, Berking et al. (2005) found that the
should be preferred that correspond with a patient’s level of goal attainment differed between goal cat-
intrinsic (approach) goals so that they hold a maxi- egories. For example, goals such as “reducing my
mum positive valence, urgency, and importance for panic attacks” or “learning to accept myself better”
the patient. The goals should describe a change, an had much better prospects of success than the goal
increase in skills, or the preservation of facilitative “coping with my sleep problems” or “experiencing
conditions. Therapists should ensure that the goals less pain.” Such findings help to adjust therapist
a patient chooses or formulates are attainable. Com- expectations and may serve as references for evalu-
plex and long-term goals should be divided into suf- ating treatment progress in various disorders.
ficiently concrete and feasible low-level goals, and For patients experiencing difficulties formulating
they should be divided into steps small enough for clear and self-concordant therapy goals, Willutzki
the patient to be able to translate them into action. and Koban (2004) introduced the EPOS interven-
Goal attainment should be initiated and maintained tion (Development of Positive Perspectives in Psy-
by the patient. Goals should not be in conflict with chotherapy). In the imagination phase, the therapist
one another, and goal formulation should entail guides the patient in activating positive perspectives
simple, concrete, specific, observable, and detailed beyond presenting problems (e.g., “When your life
descriptions of the current problematic state as well progresses fine within the next years, what will a
as the aspired goal state. In addition, therapeutic day in 5 years look like?”). In the analysis phase,
goals should be formulated positively as approach the therapist supports the patient in specifying the
goals (“to be able to go shopping by myself ”) as personally relevant goals and relates these more con-
opposed to avoidance goals (“no longer scared when crete goals to the patient’s imaginary activation of
alone outside”). The goal-striving process should positive perspectives. These interventions usually
be supported by implementation intentions (i.e., take two to three sessions.
specifications as to when and where the goal will be
pursued and how obstacles will be dealt with; see changing motivational factors
Gollwitzer & Brandstätter, 1997) and the patient’s by psychotherapy
social environment should be motivated to support First, we will describe general models of psycho-
the patient’s goals. Finally, goals should be clearly logical change that ascribe a central role to moti-
measurable so the patient can see the progress and vational factors for change in psychotherapy. Then
feel motivated to further engage in therapy. we will demonstrate examples of interventions that
Empirical studies show that patients’ treatment have an explicit motivational focus. A fundamental
goals differ depending on patients’ diagnoses. For assumption of attempts to change motivational fac-
example, psychotherapy patients with eating or tors in psychotherapy is that motivational change

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


will contribute to symptom change and improve- Goldfried, 1980). Motivational clarification, which
ment of well-being. is of particular relevance for this chapter, involves
becoming aware of the motivational background
General Models of Change of unpleasant emotions and reevaluating negative
We will describe three general models of behav- primary appraisals of situations and events (Grosse
ior change that are particularly relevant to psycho- Holtforth, Grawe, & Castonguay, 2006). The psy-
therapy. These models are the Rubicon Model of chotherapist for each patient individually combines
Action Phases (Heckhausen, Gollwitzer & Weinert, empirically supported interventions that corre-
1987), the change model of General Psychotherapy spond to these mechanisms of change based on a
(GPT; Grawe, 1997), as well as the Transtheoretical case formulation and treatment plan (Caspar &
Model (TTM; Prochaska et al., 1992). Grosse Holtforth, 2010). In an experimental study
The Rubicon Model of Action Phases (Heckhausen with heterogeneous outpatients, Grosse Holtforth,
et al., 1987) is a well-established psychological model Grawe, Fries, and Znoj (2008) demonstrated dif-
for goal-oriented action that can be profitably applied ferential effects for general psychotherapy depend-
to psychotherapy. Figure 25.2 shows the different ing on motivational factors. General psychotherapy,
phases of the model. In the motivation phase, a person which combines motivationally clarifying interven-
contemplates on his or her goals, which is completed tions with mastery-oriented interventions, yielded
by the formulation of an intention, constituting the stronger reductions of interpersonal problems for
shift from choosing to wanting. From here on, all pro- patients with high levels of avoidance motivation, as
cesses are directed toward the implementation of the compared to a cognitive-behavioral condition that
decision, that is, toward the attainment of a particular focuses on mastery-oriented interventions only.
goal. Subsequently, the person plans for how to reach Grawe (2004) adapted the Rubicon Model
the goal, screens out competing intentions, and ulti- described earlier for the systematization of change
mately executes adequate action. After realizing the processes in psychotherapy. In the Rubicon Model,
action, the person evaluates the action consequences the therapist’s goal is to help the patient to move
with reference to the pursued goal. through the phases of action to enable the patient
In General Psychotherapy (GPT; Grawe, 1997) to realize an action that fulfils his or her psychologi-
the term generally denotes that rather than defin- cal needs. As the Rubicon constitutes the difference
ing one’s interventions by therapy schools, therapists between choosing and wanting, the therapist may
conceptualize their interventions in terms of general help the patient on both sides of the Rubicon. The
change factors. The assumed general change factors therapist may help the patient to form clear inten-
are resource activation, problem actuation, motiva- tions (motivational clarification) or to realize his
tional clarifications, and problem mastery. Whereas or her intentions (problem mastery), or both. By
resource activation and problem actuation are con- motivationally clarifying interventions, the thera-
sidered catalysts for change, motivational clarifica- pist guides the patient’s attention to the process of
tion and problem mastery refer to specific types of choice, raises awareness for the involved motivational
corrective experiences (Alexander & French, 1946; forces (wishes, fears, expectations, standards, etc.),

Intention Intention Intention


formation initiation deactivation
Intention
realization
Motivation Volition Volition Motivation
“Rubicon”

Choosing Pre-action phase Action Evaluation

Fig. 25.2. The action phase model (Rubicon Model) by Heckhausen. (Adapted from Grawe, 2004, p. 50;
reproduced by permission from Psychological Therapy by Klaus Grawe, ISBN 0-88937-217-9
©2004 by Hogrefe& Huber Publishers www.hogrefe.com.)

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


and attempts to strengthen the patient’s volition by were found to change in psychotherapy were strong
changing the patient’s intentions in clarity, direction, avoidance motivation (Grosse Holtforth et al., 2005),
or strength. For example, psychodynamic therapies goal conflicts (Heidenreich, 2000; Hoyer, Fecht,
predominantly use interventions aiming at motiva- Lauterbach, & Schneider, 2001; Michalak, 2000),
tional clarification. To the right of the Rubicon, that or goal satisfaction (Berking, Grosse Holtforth, &
is, when a patient has clear and strong intentions, Jacobi, 2003; Grosse Holtforth & Grawe, 2002).
the therapist’s activities are geared toward supporting Even though motivation can change even in
the patient in implementing his or her intentions. therapies that do not explicitly target motivational
Behaviorally oriented therapies work predominantly variables, several approaches have been developed
on the right side of the Rubicon. directly targeting motivational change. Some inter-
Perhaps the most testable of the general mod- ventions have been tested in studies with nonclini-
els is the Transtheoretical Model (TTM; Prochaska cal populations that might be used as a heuristic for
et al., 1992). The TTM describes the process of psy- clinical interventions. For example, Schultheiss and
chological change as going through six subsequent Brunstein (1999) used guided goal imagery to foster
stages: precontemplation, contemplation, preparation, motive-congruent goal commitment (goal fit). They
action, maintenance, and termination. Individuals found that those subjects who had used guided goal
may also return from action or maintenance to an imagery were more committed to goals that corre-
earlier stage (relapse). Heckhausen et al.’s (1987) sponded with their implicit motive dispositions. In
decisional Rubicon could be located between the an extension of this research, Job and Brandstätter
contemplation and preparation phase. The authors (2009) showed in a series of experiments that goal
also advise therapists to adjust their relational stance fantasies focusing on affective incentives that are spe-
toward the patient to each individual’s stage of cific to a given motive promoted motive-congruent
change. Prochaska and Norcross (2001) proposed goal setting. Sheldon, Kasser, Smith, and Share
that in the precontemplation stage, the therapist (2002) designed an intervention to improve self-
should behave like an understanding parent, later concordance and integration of goals. In the inter-
morphing into a Socratic teacher in the contem- vention, participants are asked to reflect upon their
plation phase. In the preparation phase, the thera- goals and are taught four specific strategies for the
pist should assume the position of an experienced regulation of goal-related experience, such as Own
trainer, stepping behind again as a counselor in the the goal, Make it fun, Remember the big picture,
action and maintenance phases. Research indicates and Keep a balance (Sheldon et al., 2002). Empiri-
that tailoring the therapy relationship and treatment cal results indicate that participants already high on
intervention to the stage of change can enhance personality integration benefited the most from the
outcome, specifically in the percentage of patients program in terms of goal attainment, whereas par-
completing therapy and in the ultimate success of ticipants with low levels of self-concordance did not
treatment (Prochaska & Norcross, 2001). profit from the program. Also the formation of imple-
Although the TTM has been criticized, for mentation intentions specifying the where, when, and
example, for inconclusive empirical foundation as how of goal pursuit may further goal attainment
well as unresolved assessment problems, the TTM, (Gollwitzer, 1993; Koestner et al., 2002).
like the Rubicon Model and the change model of In the following we will highlight sample thera-
General Psychotherapy, possesses great heuristic peutic approaches that attempt to influence goal
value for practicing clinicians to conceptualize and processes at various levels. Whereas motivational
structure their interventions. change is at the core of psychodynamic approaches
and these therapeutic approaches are rather well
Motivational Change as a Central Mechanism known (e.g., Luborsky & Crits-Christoph, 1998),
Motivational factors may change in psychotherapy, we will focus here on more recent approaches for
even if motivational change is not explicitly part of the the treatment of emotional problems that explicitly
therapy rationale, or motivational factors may be the target motivational change. First, we will describe
central mechanism of change that therapists explic- Motivational Interviewing (MI; Miller & Rollnick,
itly try to implement. As an example of naturalistic 2002), an approach that combines various motiva-
motivational change, several authors found that unfa- tional strategies to further treatment motivation and
vorable goal characteristics changed, even without an therapy outcome, as well as other approaches that
underlying explicit rationale for working on these goal aim at the resolution of patient ambivalence. Two
characteristics. For example, motivational factors that approaches for the treatment of emotional disorders

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


that explicitly work on motivational factors are the patient in making a deliberate decision for or
Self-System Therapy (SST; Strauman et al., 2006) against beginning psychotherapy. For this purpose,
and Well-Being Therapy (WBT; Fava & Tomba, the intervention helps the patient to clarify the
2009). Another approach that focuses on motiva- respective advantages and disadvantages of begin-
tional change as a main change mechanism and has ning psychotherapy (or not) using a 2 x 2 matrix.
been explicated for several psychological disorders is The therapist tries to activate the goals involved
the Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT; but takes an absolutely neutral stance toward the
Hayes et al., 1999). Finally, in the field of personal- options. It is assumed that an autonomous deci-
ity disorders, Clarification Oriented Therapy targets sion to begin psychotherapy will lead to a greater
motivational change for changing patients’ dysfunc- commitment and endurance. The aim of the two-
tional interactional behaviors (Sachse, 2004). chair-exercise is to create awareness of both sides of
Motivational Interviewing (MI; Miller & an ambivalent experience and to prepare the two
Rollnick, 2002) is both a treatment philosophy and “split” sides for a later integration. As mentioned,
a set of methods employed to help people increase this ambivalence may concern other ambivalences
intrinsic motivation by exploring and resolving than starting a treatment. For this purpose, each of
ambivalence about behavioral change. MI is highly the two chairs used reflects one side of the ambiva-
compatible with the therapeutic goal of fostering lence. The therapist actively guides the patient to
autonomous motivation for therapy (Ryan & Deci, activate and express the thoughts, feelings, and
2008). In MI, the therapist neither persuades nor action tendencies of the different sides of the self
coerces patients to change, but instead attempts to by requesting the patient to switch chairs, when-
explore and resolve the patients’ ambivalence, allow- ever the patient assumes one respective side of the
ing them to decide for themselves about whether ambivalence. After some dialogue between the two
wanting to change or not (Ryan & Deci, 2008). sides, usually conflict/ambivalence weakens and the
When working with ambivalence and resulting patient comes closer to a decision that integrates the
resistance against change, the therapist supports the two competing sides of himself or herself.
patient by confronting, exploring, and challenging The Decision-Fostering Intervention (DFI) is a
introjected past experiences of conditional regard structured short group intervention aimed at moti-
(Assor, Roth, & Deci, 2004). Such introjects may, vating patients to actively participate in personal
for example, hinder a patient from disclosing his or change. Central to the intervention is to frame the
her feelings to the therapist out of fear of disapproval. motivational problem as a decisional problem. DFI
By providing an autonomy-supportive atmosphere, consists of the following successive parts: Evaluation
the MI therapist helps his or her patient find an and Prioritization, Justification, and Planning. In the
internal source of motivation that guides and fuels Evaluation and Prioritization phases, patients imag-
his or her future change efforts (Markland, Ryan, ine the change process, the range of possible out-
Tobin, & Rollnick, 2005). The therapist tries to comes, along with their own emotional reactions,
improve intrinsic motivation by helping the patient the reactions of their significant others, and then
become the primary agent of change (Arkowitz & subsequently prioritize their behavioral options. In
Westra, 2009). Initially, MI was established in the the Justification phase, patients are asked to choose
area of alcohol and other substance abuse (Miller & a decision and justify their decision in front of the
Rollnick, 2002), and more recently it has been used group, while the therapist assumes the position of
in the context of a wider range of clinical problems a (friendly) devil’s advocate that the patient needs
(see Buckner, 2009, for a review). to convince. This dispute will lead to a Decisional
Patients may experience ambivalence not only Statement that the patient and therapist fixate in
regarding therapeutic change but also in other parts writing with the general structure: “I want to do X,
of their lives (e.g., the continuation of a marriage, because the consequence Y is more important to me
finding a new job, etc.). Several methods are avail- than the consequence Z.” To foster the Presence of
able to psychotherapists to help their patients deal the decision, the patients find a symbol for the deci-
with or resolve their ambivalences. A very pragmatic sion (picture, posture, movement, etc.) and place
approach to this purpose is the work with the Deci- it in a highly visible or noticeable position in their
sion Cube. More general interventions to change daily lives. In the Planning phase, patients plan the
patient ambivalence are the Two-Chair-Exercise implementation of their decision by defining the
and the Decision-Fostering Intervention. The basis where, when, and how of the associated actions,
for the application of the Decision Cube is to assist anticipate likely obstacles, and generate adequate

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


responses. In addition, patients plan the specifics of dimensions are very similar to other motivational con-
coming out with their decision to significant others, structs, for example, the dimensions of goal satisfaction
and write these plans down. The therapist in DFI (Grosse Holtforth & Grawe, 2003). A WBT therapist
closely monitors and guides the patient through the strives to help the patient achieve better well-being on
process of decision formation, up to the implemen- all six dimensions to increase resilience (Fava, 1999;
tation of the formulated decision. Fava & Ruini, 2003). To reach this goal, WBT inte-
Self-System Therapy (SST) has been developed grates psychological techniques from various sources
by Strauman and colleagues (Strauman et al., 2006; within a short-term format (8–12 sessions), that is,
Vieth et al., 2003) based on a model of depression as cognitive restructuring, scheduling of activities, and
a disorder of motivation and goal pursuit resulting assertiveness training, and problem solving in addition
from chronic failure to attain certain kinds of per- to self-observation of positive experiences.
sonal goals. In an earlier study, Strauman et al. (2001) In early therapy the therapist helps the patient
had found that various empirically supported treat- to develop skills to continuously attend to positive
ments (cognitive therapy, CT; Beck, Rush, Shaw, & aspects of daily experience or positive emotions
Emery, 1979; Beck, 1995), interpersonal psychother- using structured diaries. Subsequently the therapist
apy (Klerman, Weissman, Rounsaville, & Chevron, helps the patient to identify thoughts and beliefs
1984), and pharmacotherapy with selective serotonin leading to premature interruption of well-being by
reuptake inhibitors were less effective for depressed self-observation, and the therapist challenges these
patients with chronic self-perceived failure in promo- thoughts (Beck et al., 1979). In parallel, the therapist
tion goal pursuit than for other patients. According reinforces and encourages activities that are likely to
to Regulatory Focus Theory (RFT; Higgins, 1989), elicit well-being by graded task assignments. In the
promotion goals are aimed at making good things final sessions, the therapist instructs the patient to
happen, whereas prevention goals are aimed at pre- self-monitor the course of episodes of well-being
venting bad things from happening. If this subgroup and optimize behaviors aiming at the attainment
of depressed patients was vulnerable to depression and preservation of well-being. WBT can be applied
because of inadequate socialization toward pursuing as a stand-alone therapy or as an addition to other
promotion goals, then SST interventions to enhance forms of psychotherapy or pharmacotherapy. How-
promotion goal pursuit might help them recover ever, WBT is considered most appropriate for treat-
from depression more completely. Strauman et al. ing nonacute depression to address aspects that have
(2006) summarized SST in four questions directed to been omitted by other approaches (Fava & Tomba,
the patient: “What are your promotion and preven- 2009). In an empirical test of WBT, 20 patients
tion goals? What are you doing to try to attain them? with depressive and/or anxiety disorders (major
What is keeping you from making progress? What depression, panic disorder with agoraphobia, social
can you do differently?” (Strauman et al., 2006, phobia, generalized anxiety disorder, obsessive com-
p. 368). To improve the patient’s pursuit of promo- pulsive disorder) who had been successfully treated
tion goals, SST flexibly combines techniques from by behavioral (anxiety disorders) or pharmacological
other empirically supported psychotherapies, includ- (mood disorders) methods were randomly assigned
ing cognitive therapy, interpersonal psychotherapy, to either WBT or cognitive-behavioral therapy
and behavioral activation therapy in the service of (CBT). Whereas both therapies showed a significant
improved goal pursuit. In a randomized trial compar- reduction of residual symptoms as well as increase
ing SST with cognitive therapy (CT) in a sample of in psychological well-being, WBT showed a signifi-
45 patients with depression, SST and CBT on aver- cantly greater reduction of residual symptoms imme-
age showed equal efficacy between treatments, but diately after treatment (Fava & Tomba, 2009).
patients whose socialization history lacked an empha- Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT;
sis on promotion goals showed significantly greater Bach & Hayes, 2002; Hayes et al., 1999) focuses on
improvement with SST (Strauman et al., 2006). reducing experiential avoidance, which is considered
Well-Being Therapy (WBT; Fava & Tomba, 2009) a pathological factor. Avoiding private experiences
is based on Ryff’s cognitive model of psychological (i.e., certain thoughts, feelings, or body sensations)
well-being (Ryff, 1989), which proposes six dimen- is assumed to result in failure to behave in a way
sions of psychological well-being: environmental that is in accord with one’s values. As the acronym
mastery, personal growth, purpose in life, autonomy, ACT indicates, patients learn to Accept their reac-
self-acceptance, and positive relations with others. tions and be present, Choose a valued direction, and
Although labeled as dimensions of well-being, these Take action in order to develop more psychological

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


flexibility (Hayes et al., 1999). Patients are guided to emotions, show maladaptive behavioral strategies
face and overcome experiential avoidance by learn- (e.g., oversensitive counterattacks), or retreat to mal-
ing to perceive their inner experiences (thoughts, adaptive coping strategies (e.g., abusing drugs or alco-
images, emotions, memories etc.) without any eval- hol). Consequently, the goal of motivation-focused
uation, by allowing and accepting them to come and interventions for personality disorders according to
go without resisting them, by experiencing the here Horowitz’s model is to change problematic interper-
and now with openness and interest, and by observ- sonal behavior by changing the assumedly underly-
ing the processes within the self. The therapist focuses ing motivational structure. This should lead to more
on values by helping the patient to discover what he adaptive interpersonal strategies and behaviors that,
or she considers most important in life, to set goals in turn, should lead to a better satisfaction of the full
according to these values, and to carry them out. range of the person’s goals.
A characteristic of ACT that distinguishes it from, Sachse (2010) proposed a conceptualization and
for example, CBT is that ACT focuses less on symp- treatment of personality disorders (Clarification-
tom reduction than on empowering patients to pur- Oriented Psychotherapy, COP-PD) that is compat-
sue their goals in accordance with their important ible with these assumptions. Like Horowitz (2004),
values. ACT has shown significant effects with a Sachse (2010) assumed that over their lives, people
variety of clinical disorders and problems; however, with personality disorders have developed a prepon-
the body of well-controlled studies does not suffice derance of certain motives as well as dysfunctional
yet to conclude that ACT is generally more effec- interactional goals, strategies, behaviors, and cogni-
tive than other active treatments (Hayes, Luoma, tions for the satisfaction of these motives, which he
Bond, Masuda, & Lillis, 2006). For example, For- calls game structures. These game structures (i.e., often
man, Herbert, Moitra, Yeomans, and Geller (2007) manipulating and intransparent styles of interaction
found in a randomized controlled effectiveness trial to force a partner to satisfy motives) constitute the
of ACT and CT for anxiety and depression that characteristics of people with personality disorders.
patients in CT and ACT showed large and equiva- Thus, the actions of a patient with a personality dis-
lent improvements in depression, anxiety, and other order are governed by predominant motives as well
outcomes. However, the mechanisms of action dif- as the developed game structures. In COP-PD, the
fered between the therapies; in CT observing and therapist tries to help the patient explicate his or her
describing one’s experiences mediated outcomes for interpersonal motives, change the associated dys-
patients in the CT, whereas experiential avoidance, functional interpersonal schemas (goals, strategies,
acting with awareness and acceptance, mediated and behaviors), and establish new need-satisfying
outcomes for those in the ACT group. motivational-behavioral patterns in real-life interac-
Personality disorders can be characterized by tions. For example, with narcissistic patients, thera-
the interpersonal problems the patients experience. pists attune their behavior to the assumed narcissistic
According to the interpersonal theory of person- motives by normalizing the patient’s problems, max-
ality disorders, salient frustrated motives are the imally validating the patient’s resources (but not the
potential reasons underlying interpersonal problems dysfunctional strategies), and cautiously directing
(Horowitz, 2004). Accordingly in Horowitz’s (2004; attention to the discrepancy between the patient’s
Horowitz et al., 2006) model of interpersonal motives, a self-doubts and his or her resources. This discrepancy
person with a certain personality disorder is assumed in conjunction with awareness of the costs of the
to feel frustrated with respect to some salient motive. maladaptive behavior is assumed to fuel the patient’s
For example, for patients with a narcissistic person- motivation for change. Subsequently, the therapist
ality disorder, the assumed organizing motive is the moves to reconstructing the schemas and to practic-
unrestricted admiration by other people. Conse- ing new, more adaptive strategies and behaviors for
quently, narcissistic people may show behaviors like motive satisfaction in real-life interactions with less
bragging about their exceptional achievements. As adverse side effects. Clarification-Oriented Psycho-
a result, they often appear rather arrogant and repel therapy has been explicated also as a general strategy
other people, so that the person reports characteristic for other clinical disorders (Sachse, 2003).
recurring cognitions, fears, and interpersonal prob-
lems, which are described as the diagnostic criteria Conclusion: A Motivationally Informed
in the DSM-IV-TR. In response to failures to sat- Psychotherapy
isfy the predominant interpersonal motive, a person In the concluding section, we will try to summa-
with a personality disorder may experience negative rize the implications of basic and clinical research

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


for a motivationally informed psychotherapy. In a seriously hamper the patient’s wishes to be in con-
motivationally informed psychotherapy, psycho- trol of one’s life). Goal satisfaction may also be
therapists are faced with at least two tasks: to help hindered if the person has not developed adequate
the patient change the factors maintaining his or strategies for goal satisfaction (e.g., bragging behav-
her psychological disorder(s), and to foster condi- ior of narcissists). A motive-oriented treatment plan
tions that support the patient’s treatment motiva- will then outline motive-related ways to improve
tion (see Schulte & Eifert, 2002). the conditions for change as well as ways to change
A primary goal of psychotherapy is to decrease motivational factors that contribute to lacking goal
psychopathology and associated suffering. From satisfaction.
a motivational perspective, the overall goal of psy- The therapist can create favorable conditions
chotherapy can be defined more broadly as helping for change by fostering a good therapy relation-
the patient to better satisfy his or her psychological ship via motivational attunement and by formu-
needs, which should go along with better well-being lating adequate therapy goals. As shown earlier,
and life satisfaction. Under this perspective, psycho- motivational attunement aims at strengthening the
pathology is considered the principal source of psy- working alliance by attuning to the patient’s most
chological suffering that psychotherapy is supposed important approach goals, by avoiding to inad-
to change. However, part of the therapeutic enter- vertently activate avoidance goals, as well as using
prise may also be to assist the patient in accepting and the motivational information to understand and
coping with problems that are unchangeable, such as resolve occurring alliance ruptures. The treatment
losses of significant others or one’s own limitations goals that the patient and therapist agree on at the
as, for example, caused by physical disabilities. outset of treatment should concretize the directions
To optimally be aware of, use, and change motiva- the therapy should take for satisfying the patient’s
tional factors in psychotherapy, a therapist has vari- personal goals. If a patient experiences difficulties in
ous options for assessing motivational constructs. For formulating clear treatment goals, strategies of goal
reasons of practicality, the therapist will use observa- imagery and goal concretization (e.g., EPOS) may
tional, interview, or questionnaire methods of assess- help the patient to get a clearer picture of what he
ment most likely in conjunction with anamnestic or she wants to achieve with the help of the thera-
information, rather than more stringent yet time- pist. The therapist will focus goal formulation on
consuming methods of assessing implicit motives. positive outcomes associated with the planned
Motivational constructs that the therapist may assess changes and define goals that correspond well with
at intake are therapy motivation, values, personal the patient’s values and motives and are compatible
goals, motivational conflicts, and treatment goals. with each other.
If the therapist wants to use motive-related measures Treatment goals should be formulated as approach
for quality assurance purposes, the improvement goals (“I will be able to confront my boss assertively
of goal satisfaction may be the central construct to when I disagree”) instead of avoidance goals (“I no
assess (Grosse Holtforth & Grawe, 2004). longer avoid confronting my boss when I disagree”),
For a motivation-focused case formulation, it will should be controllable and attainable, should be
be central to get a clear picture of which approach formulated as concretely as possible, and the crite-
goals the patient deems important, which avoidance ria for goal attainment should be explicitly stated.
goals he or she dreads most strongly, how strong the Well-chosen and well-formulated therapy goals will
suffering from goal dissatisfaction is, which goals the strengthen the patient’s commitment and endurance
patient cannot satisfy enough, and what the sources in goal striving and participation in therapy. Goal
of the dissatisfaction are. The sources of lacking goal attainment promises to be most likely if the patient
satisfaction may be heterogeneous, such as marital develops clear intentions for goal attainment, as well
problems, academic failure, and discrimination at as implementation intentions. Strategies like “Own
work. Motivational sources of goal dissatisfaction the goal,” “Make it fun,” “Remember the big pic-
may be approach goals that are too strong (e.g., very ture,” or “Keep a balance” may help the patient in
high standards for personal achievement) or too the process of goal striving (Sheldon et al., 2002,
strong avoidance goals (e.g., being very easily hurt pp. 14–15).
by critical remarks of others). Additionally, goal To facilitate change, therapists may also ques-
conflicts or ambivalences may be powerful sources tion potential defensive justifications for preserving
of goal dissatisfaction (e.g., being ambivalent about old goals and behaviors (Karoly, 2006). Another
ending the relationship to an alcoholic spouse may powerful force working against new goal striving

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


may be the influence of automaticity. Patients may Future Research/Open Questions
make the experience that “old” maladaptive behav- As we have seen in this chapter, previous research
iors or feelings are automatically triggered by situ- on motivational factors in psychotherapy has already
ational cues. Such reactions may especially arise in yielded findings that have great potential for advanc-
situations that the patient previously dreaded and ing psychotherapy. However, as we have also seen, a
avoided. The therapist will need to make these multitude of questions remains to be investigated by
automatized associations consciously accessible to future research. A few examples of such questions
the patient and work with the patient on accepting are as follows:
(not avoiding) his or her own aversive experiences.
The sequence of awareness, acceptance, and new 1. By which psychological mechanisms do
reactions/new behaviors will have to be repeatedly motivational factors (treatment goals, motive
run through with the patient to result in more sus- satisfaction, conflicts, etc.) contribute to the
tainable change. Such a training of deautomatizing development and maintenance of psychological
previously automatic dysfunctional reactions and disorders and problems? For example, how do
behaviors will help the patient to recognize and the goals of patients with personality disorders
override the effects of automaticity in service of pur- influence their interpersonal behavior, and how
suing more need-satisfying goals (Karoly, 2006). An does this behavior relate to the satisfaction of
important factor to consider is the context of goal specific needs and goals?
striving. The more the patient’s social network sup- 2. Which motivation-focused interventions
ports the patient’s goals, the more likely the patient can foster motivational change, and how does
will reach them. Depending on how changeable this relate to therapy effectiveness? For example,
patient and therapist perceive the level of support is a treatment that fosters the formulation of
for his or her goals, either couple, family, or sys- well-formed therapy goals more effective than a
temic interventions will be indicated for trying to treatment that does not?
change the level of support. 3. Can therapists foster the quality of
We have highlighted several therapeutic the therapeutic relationship by motivational
approaches that attempt to influence goal pro- attunement and does this improve therapy
cesses at various levels. Motivational Interviewing outcome? For example, do cognitive-behavioral
as well as other techniques (Decision Cube, Two- therapists who try to tailor their interventions to
Chair exercise, Decision-Fostering Interventions) the patient’s motives attain better outcomes than
are suited to change patient ambivalence toward therapists who do not?
treatment as well as other forms of ambivalence. 4. What are the motivational mechanisms of
Both Self-System Therapy and Well-Being Therapy change in psychotherapeutic treatments of various
are designed to improve goal and need satisfac- orientations? For example, how does the intensity
tion in acutely or chronically depressed patients, of avoidance motivation change in cognitive-
respectively, with varying emphases. Acceptance behavioral as compared to psychodynamic
and Commitment Therapy emphasizes overcom- therapies?
ing experiential avoidance by accepting unpleasant 5. Which motivational factors predict the
experiences, developing clear values guiding one’s process and outcome of psychotherapy, and how
actions, and trying to efficiently pursue one’s goals. is this prediction mediated? For example, do
Clarification-Oriented Therapy for personality dis- high levels of ambivalence over the expression of
orders intends to improve the patient’s need and goal emotion predict collaboration in treatment, and
satisfaction by raising awareness for dysfunctional worse outcomes?
interactional strategies and behaviors and attempt- 6. Do various psychotherapeutic approaches
ing to change these strategies and behaviors. change explicit and implicit motivation
We hope to have demonstrated the centrality differentially? What are the consequences for
of motivational factors in psychotherapy, either outcome? For example, do motivation-focused
as facilitators of change or as targets of change. treatments change implicit motivational conflicts
Motivational considerations and interventions may more effectively, and is this associated with longer-
be used in all kinds of psychotherapy and have a lasting improvements?
great potential for helping patients in overcom- 7. Do ethnically and culturally diverse
ing their problems and living a happier and more groups of patients differ in motivational factors
satisfying life. (values, treatment goals, response to motivational

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


interventions), how does this affect treatment, Brunstein, J. C. (1993). Personal goals and subjective well-being:
and how should practicing psychotherapists tailor A longitudinal study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 65, 1061–1070.
their interventions to the ethnical backgrounds Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Persönliche Ziele und subjektives Wohl-
of their patients? For example, can clarification- befinden bei älteren Menschen [Personal goals and subjective
focused interventions be applied similarly with well-being in elderly persons]. Zeitschrift für Differentielle
Swiss patients with Asian background as with und Diagnostische Psychologie, 20, 58–71.
Moroccan patients with sub-Saharan African Brunstein, J., Dangelmayer, G., & Schultheiss, O. (1996).
Personal goals and social support in close relationships:
background, and which adaptations are necessary? Effects on relationship mood and marital satisfaction. Journal
8. Which brain areas are associated with of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 1006–1019.
motivational characteristics of psychotherapy Brunstein, J., & Maier, G. W. (2002). Das Streben nach persön-
patients and how do they change over treatment? lichen Zielen: Emotionales Wohlbefinden und proaktive
For example, can intense avoidance motivation be Entwicklung über die Lebensspanne [Striving for personal
goals: Emotional well-being and proactive development dur-
localized in the brain, and is change in avoidance ing the lifespan]. In H. Thomae (Ed.), Persönlichkeit und
motivation over therapy associated with changes Entwicklung [Personality and development] (pp. 157–189).
in these brain areas? Weinheim, Germany: Beltz Verlag.
Brunstein, J. C., & Schmitt, C. H. (2004). Assessing individual
Generally, future research on motivational fac- differences in achievement motivation with the Implicit Asso-
tors would profit from a more frequent use of exper- ciation Test. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 536–555.
imental methods, as well as causal hypotheses using Brunstein, J. C., Schultheiss, O. C., & Graessmann, R. (1998).
longitudinal designs. Personal goals and emotional well-being: The moderating
role of motive dispositions. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 75, 494–508.
References Brunstein, J., Schultheiss, O., & Maier, G. W. (1999). The
Ackerman, S. J., & Hilsenroth, M. J. (2001). A review of thera- pursuit of personal goals: A motivational approach to well-
pist characteristics and techniques negatively impacting the being and life adjustment. In J. Brandstädter (Ed.), Action
therapeutic alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, self-development: Theory and research through the life span
Training, 38, 171–185. (pp. 169–196). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Alexander, F., & French, T. M. (1946). Psychoanalytic therapy. Buckner, J. D. (2009). Motivation enhancement therapy can
Principles and applications. New York: Wiley. increase utilization of cognitive-behavioral therapy: The case
Arkowitz, H., & Westra, H. A. (2009). Introduction to the spe- of social anxiety disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65,
cial series on motivational interviewing and psychotherapy. 1195–1206.
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 65, 1149–1155. Calnan, M. (1987). Health and illness. The lay perspective.
Arnow, B. A., & Castonguay, L. G. (1996). Treatment goals and London: Tavistock.
strategies of cognitive-behavioral and psychodynamic thera- Cantor, N. (1990). From thought to behavior: “Having” and
pists: A naturalistic investigation. Journal of Psychotherapy “doing” in the study of personality and cognition. American
Integration, 6, 333–347. Psychologist, 45, 735–750.
Assor, A., Roth, G., & Deci, E. L. (2004). The emotional costs Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1996). Perspectives on personality
of parents’ conditional regard: A self-determination theory (3rd. ed.). Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.
analysis. Journal of Personality, 72, 47–88. Caspar, F. (2007). Beziehungen und Probleme verstehen. Eine
Austin, J. T., & Vancouver, J. B. (1996). Goal constructs in Einführung in die psychotherapeutische Plananalyse [Under-
psychology: Structure, process, and content. Psychological standing relationships and problems: Introduction into Plan
Bulletin, 120, 338–375. Analysis] (3rd. ed.). Bern, Switzerland: Huber.
Bach, P., & Hayes, S. C. (2002). The use of acceptance and com- Caspar, F., & Grosse Holtforth, M. (2010). Klaus Grawe: On
mitment therapy to prevent rehospitalization of psychotic a constant quest for a truly integrative and research-based
patients. A randomized controlled trail. Journal of Consulting psychotherapy. In L. G. Castonguay, J. C. Muran, L.
and Clinical Psychology, 70, 1129–1139. Angus, J. A. Hayes, N. Ladany, & T. Anderson (Eds.), Bring-
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F., & Emery, G. (1979). ing psychotherapy research to life (pp. 113–123). Washington,
Cognitive therapy of depression. New York: Guilford Press. DC: American Psychological Association.
Beck, J. S. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Basics and beyond. Caspar, F., Grossmann, C., Unmüssig, C., & Schramm, E.
New York: Guilford Press. (2005). Complementary therapeutic relationship: Therapist
Berking, M., Grosse Holtforth, M., & Jacobi, C. (2003). Reduc- behavior, interpersonal patterns, and therapeutic effects. Psy-
tion of incongruence in inpatient psychotherapy. Clinical chotherapy Research, 15, 91–102.
Psychology and Psychotherapy, 10, 86–92. Chulef, A. S., Read, S. J., & Walsh, D. A. (2001). A hierarchical tax-
Berking, M., Grosse Holtforth, M., Jacobi, C., & Kröner- onomy of human goals. Motivation and Emotion, 25, 191–232.
Herwig, B. (2005). Empirically based guidelines for goal- Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (1996). Life values and adolescent mental
finding procedures in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, health. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
15, 316–324. Cohen, P., & Cohen, J. (2001). Life values and mental health
Bordin, E. S. (1979). The generalizability of the psycho-analytic in adolescence. In P. Schmuck & K. M. Sheldon (Eds.), Life
concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy: Theory, goals and well-being: Towards a positive psychology of human
Research, and Practice, 16, 252–260. striving (pp. 167–181). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (1988). A motivational model of Emmons, R. A., & Kaiser, H. A. (1996). Goal orientation and
alcohol use. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 168–180. emotional well-being: Linking goals and affect through the
Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (1990). Incentive motivation, self. In A. Tesser (Ed.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among
affective change, and alcohol use: A model. In W. M. Cox goals, affects, and self-regulation (pp. 79–98). Hillsdale, NJ:
(Ed.), Why people drink: Parameters of alcohol as a reinforcer Erlbaum.
(pp. 291–314). New York: Amereon Press. Emmons, R. A., & King, L. A. (1988). Conflict among personal
Cox, W. M., & Klinger, E. (2002). Motivational structure strivings: Immediate and long-term implications for psycho-
relationships with substance use and processes of change. logical and physical well-being. Journal of Personality and
Addictive Behaviors, 27, 925–940. Social Psychology, 54, 1040–1048.
Cox, W. M., Klinger, E., & Blount, J. P. (1999). Systematic moti- Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive-experiential self-theory.
vational counseling: A treatment manual. Manual available In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and
from W. M. Cox, Professor of The Psychology of Addictive research (pp. 165–192). New York: Guilford.
Behaviours, School of Psychology, Bangor University, UK. Faller, H., & Goßler, S. (1998). Probleme und Ziele von Psy-
Cox, W. M., Schippers, G. M., Klinger, E., Skutle, A., chotherapiepatienten [Problems and goals of psychotherapy
Stuchlikova, I., Man, F., King, A. L., & Inderhaug, R. (2002). patients]. Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psychol-
Motivational structure and alcohol use in university students ogie, 48, 176–186.
with consistency across nations. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, Fava, G. A. (1999). Well-being therapy. Psychotherapy and
63, 280–285. Psychosomatics, 68, 171–178.
Crits-Christoph, P., Connolly Gibbons, M. B., Crits-Christoph, Fava, G. A., & Ruini, C. (2003). Development and character-
K., Narducci, J., Schamberger, M., & Gallop, R. (2006). istics of a well-being enhancing psychotherapeutic strategy:
Can therapists be trained to improve their alliances? A pilot Well-being therapy. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experi-
study of Alliance-Fostering Therapy. Psychotherapy Research, mental Psychiatry, 34, 45–63.
13, 268–281. Fava, G. A., & Tomba, E. (2009). Increasing psychological well-
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self- being and resilience by psychotherapeutic methods. Journal
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press. of Personality, 77, 1903–1934.
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1995). Human autonomy: The basis Flanagan, C. M. (2010). The case for needs in psychotherapy.
for true self-esteem. In M. Kernis (Ed.), Efficacy, agency, and Journal of Psychotherapy Integration, 20, 1–36.
self-esteem (pp. 31–49). New York: Plenum Press. Ford, M. E., & Nichols, C. W. (1991). Using goal assessments to
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). Handbook of self-determination identify motivational patterns and facilitate behavioral regu-
research. Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press. lation and achievement. In M. L. Maehr & P. R. Pintrich
Dickson, J. M., & MacLeod, A. K. (2004). Anxiety, depression, (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement. (Vol. 7,
and approach and avoidance goals. Cognition and Emotion, pp. 51–84). Greenwich, CT: JAI.
18, 423–430. Forman, E. M., Herbert, J. D., Moitra, E., Yeomans, P. D., &
Dimsdale, J. E. (1975). Goals of therapy on psychiatric inpatient Geller, P. A. (2007). A randomized controlled therapy and
units. Social Psychiatry, 10, 1–7. cognitive therapy for anxiety and depression. Behaviour Mod-
Dimsdale, J. E., Klerman, G., & Shershow, J. C. (1979). ification, 31, 772–799.
Conflict in treatment goals between patients and staff. Social Goldfried, M. R. (1980). Toward the delineation of therapeutic
Psychiatry, 14, 1–14. change principles. American Psychologist, 35, 991–999.
Dirmaier, J., Harfst, T., Koch, U., & Schulz, H. (2006). Therapy Gollwitzer, P. M. (1993). Goal achievement: The role of inten-
goals in inpatient psychotherapy: Differences between diag- tions. European Review of Social Psychology, 4, 141–185.
nostic groups and psychotherapeutic orientations. Clinical Gollwitzer, P. M., & Brandstätter, V. (1997). Implementation
Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 34–46. intentions and effective goal pursuit. Journal of Personality
Driessen, M., Sommer, B., Röstel, C., Malchow, C. P., and Social Psychology, 73, 186–199.
Rumpf, H-J., & Adam, B. (2001). Therapieziele in der Psy- Grawe, K. (1997). Research-informed psychotherapy. Psycho-
chologischen Medizin: Stand der Forschung und Entwick- therapy Research, 7, 1–20.
lung eines standardisierten Instruments. [Treatment goals in Grawe, K. (2004). Psychological therapy. Cambridge, MA:
psychological medicine: State of research and development Hogrefe.
of standardized instruments]. Zeitschrift für Psychotherapie, Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998).
Psychosomatik und Medizinische Psychologie, 51, 239–245. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The
Elliot, A. J. (2008). Handbook of approach and avoidance motiva- Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social
tion. New York: Psychology Press. Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.
Elliot, A. J. & Friedman R. (2007). Approach-avoidance: Grosse Holtforth, M. (2001). Was möchten Patienten in ihrer
A central characteristic of personal goals. In B. R. Little, Therapie erreichen?—Die Erfassung von Therapiezielen
K. Salmela-Aro, & S. D. Philips (Eds.), Personal project mit dem Berner Inventar für Therapieziele (BIT) [What do
pursuit: Goals, action, and human flourishing (pp. 97–118). patients want to accomplish in psychotherapy?—Assessment
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. and classification of treatment-goal themes with the Bern
Emmons, R. A. (1986). Personal strivings: An approach to per- Inventory of Treatment Goals (BIT)]. Verhaltenstherapie und
sonality and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality and psychosoziale Praxis, 33, 241–258.
Social Psychology, 51, 1058–1068. Grosse Holtforth, M. (2008). Avoidance motivation in psycho-
Emmons, R. A. (1996). Striving and feeling, personal goals logical problems and psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research,
and subjective well-being. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), The psychol- 18, 147–159.
ogy of action: Linking cognition and motivation to behavior Grosse Holtforth, M., Bents, H., Mauler, B., & Grawe, K. (2006).
(pp. 313–337). New York: Guilford Press. Interpersonal distress as a mediator between avoidance goals

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


and goal satisfaction in psychotherapy inpatients. Clinical Psy- [Intrapsychic conflict and therapy motivation in the treat-
chology and Psychotherapy, 13, 172–182. ment of substance abuse]. Regensburg, Germany: Roderer.
Grosse Holtforth, M., & Castonguay, L. G. (2005). Relationship Heine, S. J., Proulx, T., & Vohs, K. D. (2006). The meaning
and techniques in CBT—A motivational approach. Psycho- maintenance model: On the coherence of social motivations.
therapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 42, 443–455. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 88–110.
Grosse Holtforth, M., & Grawe, K. (2000). Fragebogen zur Higgins, E. T. (1989). Continuities and discontinuities in self-
Analyse Motivationaler Schemata (FAMOS) [Question- regulatory and self-evaluative processes: A developmental
naire for the analysis of motivational schemata]. Zeitschrift theory relating self and affect. Journal of Personality, 57,
für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie: Forschung und 407–444.
Praxis, 29, 170–179. Hill, C. E., & Lambert, M. J. (2004). Methodological
Grosse Holtforth, M., & Grawe, K. (2002). Bern inventory or issues in studying psychotherapy process and outcomes.
treatment goals: Part 1: Development and first application of In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Handbook of psychotherapy and
a taxonomy of treatment goal themes. Psychotherapy Research, behavior change (pp. 84–135). New York: Wiley.
12, 79–99. Horowitz, L. M. (2004). Interpersonal foundations of psychopa-
Grosse Holtforth, M., & Grawe, K. (2003). Der Inkongru- thology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Associa-
enzfragebogen (INK)—Ein Messinstrument zur Analyse tion.
motivationaler Inkongruenz [Questionnaire for assessing Horowitz, L. M., Wilson, K. R., Turan, B., Zolotsev, P.,
incongruence—an instrument for analysis of motivational Constantino, M. J., & Henderson, L. (2006). How interper-
incongruence]. Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und sonal motives clarify the meaning of interpersonal behavior:
Psychotherapie, 32, 315–323. A revised circumplex model. Personality and Social Psychology
Grosse Holtforth, M., & Grawe, K. (2004). Inkongruenz und Review, 10, 67–86.
Fallkonzeption in der Psychologischen Therapie [Incongru- Horvath, A. O. (1995). The therapeutic relationship: From
ence and case formulations in psychological therapy]. Verh- transference to alliance. In Session: Psychotherapy in Practice,
altenstherapie und psychosoziale Praxis, 36, 9–21. 1, 7–18.
Grosse Holtforth, M., Grawe, K., & Castonguay, L. G. (2006). Hoyer, J., Fecht, J., Lauterbach, W., & Schneider, R. (2001).
Predicting a reduction of avoidance motivation in psycho- Changes in conflict, symptoms, and well-being during psy-
therapy: Toward the delineation of differential processes of chodynamic and cognitive-behavioral alcohol inpatient treat-
change operating at different phases of treatment. Psycho- ment. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 70, 209–215.
therapy Research, 16, 639–644. Job, V., & Brandstätter, V. (2009). Get a taste of your goals:
Grosse Holtforth, M., Grawe, K., Egger, O., & Berking, M. Promoting motive-goal congruence through affect-focus goal
(2005). Reducing the dreaded: Change of avoidance motiva- fantasy. Journal of Personality, 77, 1527–1560.
tion in psychotherapy. Psychotherapy Research, 15, 262–271. Karoly, P. (1999). A goal systems-self-regulatory perspective on
Grosse Holtforth, M., Grawe, K., Fries, A., & Znoj, H. (2008). personality, psychopathology, and change. Review of General
Inconsistency as a criterion for differential indication in psy- Psychology, 3, 264–291.
chotherapy: A randomized controlled trial. Zeitschrift für Karoly, P. (2006). Tracking the leading edge of self-regulatory
Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 37, 103–111. failure: Commentary on “Where do we go from here?
Grosse Holtforth, M., Grawe, K., & Tamcan, Ö. (2004). Der The goal perspective in psychotherapy.” Clinical Psychology
Inkongruenzfragebogen (INK)—Handanweisung [Ques- Science and Practice, 13, 366–370.
tionnaire for assessing incongruence—manual]. Göttingen, Karoly, P., & Ruehlman, L. S. (1995). Goal cognition and its
Germany: Hogrefe. clinical implications: Development and preliminary valida-
Grosse Holtforth, M., Reubi, I., Ruckstuhl, L., Berking, M., tion of four motivational assessment instruments. Assessment,
& Grawe, K. (2004). The value of treatment goals in the 2, 113–129.
outcome evaluation of psychiatric inpatients. International Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1993). A dark side of the Ameri-
Journal of Social Psychiatry, 50, 80–91. can dream: Correlates of financial success as a central life
Grosse Holtforth, M., Wyss, T., Schulte, D., Trachsel, M., & aspiration. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65,
Michalak, J. (2009). Some like it specific: The difference 410–422.
between treatment goals of anxious and depressed patients. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (1996). Further examining the Ameri-
Psychology and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, can dream: Differential correlates of intrinsic and extrinsic
82, 279–290. goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 280–287.
Hayes, S. C., Luoma, J., Bond, F., Masuda, A., & Lillis, J. Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (2001). Be careful of what you wish
(2006). Acceptance and commitment therapy. Research and for: Optimal functioning and the relative attainment of
Therapy, 44, 1–25. intrinsic and extrinsic goals. In P. Schmuck & K. M. Sheldon
Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Accep- (Eds.), Life goals and well-being: towards a positive psychology
tance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to of human striving (pp. 116–131). Seattle, WA: Hogrefe.
behavior change. New York: Guilford Press. Kiresuk, T. J., Smith, A., & Cardillo, J. E. (1994). Goal Attain-
Heckhausen, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Weinert, F. E. (1987). ment Scaling: Applications, theory, and measurement. Hills-
Jenseits des Rubikon: Der Wille in den Humanwissenschaften dale, NJ: Erlbaum.
[Beyond the Rubicon: The will in human sciences]. Berlin, Klappheck, M. A., & Michalak, J. (2009). Patientenziele und Ther-
Germany: Springer. apieerfolg [Patients’ goals and treatment outcome]. Zeitschrift
Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2008). Motivation and für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 38, 24–33.
action. Cambridge University Press. Klerman, G. L., Weissman, M. M., Rounsaville, B. J., &
Heidenreich, T. (2000). Intrapsychische Konflikte und Therapi- Chevron, E. S. (1984). Interpersonal psychotherapy of depres-
emotivation in der Behandlung von Substanzabhängigkeit sion. New York: Basic Books.

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


Klinger, E. (1977). Meaning and void: Inner experience and Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 32,
the incentives in people’s lives. Minneapolis: University of 94–103.
Minnesota Press. Michalak, J., Püschel, O., Joormann, J., & Schulte, D. (2006).
Klinger, E. (1987). The Interview Questionnaire technique: Implicit motives and explicit goals: Two distinctive modes
Reliability and validity of a mixed idiographic-nomothetic of motivational functioning and their relations to psychopa-
measure of motivation. In J. N. Butcher & C. D. Spielberger thology. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 13, 81–96.
(Eds.), Advances in personality assessment (Vol. 6., pp. 31–48). Michalak, J., & Schulte, D. (2002). Zielkonflikte und Thera-
Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. piemotivation [Goal conflicts and treatment motivation].
Klinger, E., Barta, S. G., & Maxeiner, M. E. (1980). Motivational Zeitschrift für Klinische Psychologie und Psychotherapie, 31,
correlates of thought content frequency and commitment. 213–219.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 1222–1237. Miller, A., & Thompson, A. (1973). Factor structure of a goal
Koestner, R., Lekes, N., Powers, T. A., & Chicoine, E. (2002). checklist for patients. Psychological Reports, 32, 497–498.
Attaining personal goals: Self-concordance plus implementa- Miller, W. R., & Rollnick, S. (2002). Motivational interviewing:
tion equals success. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- Preparing people for change (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford
ogy, 83, 231–244. Press.
Kunkel, M. A., & Newsom, S. (1996). Presenting problems for Oishi, S., & Diener, E. (2001). Goals, culture, and subjective
mental health services: A concept map. Journal of Mental well-being. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27,
Health Counseling, 18, 53–63. 1674–1682.
Lecci, L., Karoly, P., Briggs, C., & Kuhn, K. (1994). Specific- Orlinsky, D. E., Ronnestad, M. H., & Willutzki, U. (2004).
ity and generality of motivational components in depression: Fifty years of process-outcome research: Continuity and
A personal projects analysis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, change. In M. J. Lambert (Ed.), Handbook of psychotherapy
103, 404–408. and behavior change (pp. 307–389). New York: Wiley.
Lilienfeld, S. O., Wood, J. M., & Garb, H. N. (2000). The sci- Pelletier, L. G., Tuson, K. M., & Haddad, N. K. (1997). Client
entific status of projective techniques. Psychological Science in motivation for therapy scale: A measure of intrinsic moti-
the Public Interest, 1, 27–66. vation, extrinsic motivation, and amotivation for therapy.
Little, B. R. (1983). Personal projects: A rational and method for Journal of Personality Assessment, 68, 414–435.
investigation. Environment and Behavior, 15, 273–309. Pervin, L. A. (1990). Handbook of personality: Theory and research.
Locke, K. D. (2000). Circumplex Scales of Interpersonal Values: New York: Guilford Press.
Reliability, validity, and applicability to interpersonal prob- Philips, B. (2009). Comparing apples and oranges: How do
lems and personality disorders. Journal of Personality Assess- patient characteristics and treatment goals vary between dif-
ment, 75, 249–267. ferent forms of psychotherapy? Psychology and Psychotherapy:
Luborsky, L., & Crits-Christoph, P. (1998). Understanding trans- Theory, Research and Practice, 82, 323–336.
ference: The core conflictual relationship theme method (2nd ed.). Pöhlmann, K. (1999). Persönliche Ziele: Ein neuer Ansatz
Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. zur Erfassung von Therapiezielen [Personal goals: A new
Maier, G. W., & Brunstein, J. C. (2001). The role of personal approach for assessing treatment goals]. Praxis Klinische
work goals in newcomers’ job satisfaction and organizational Verhaltensmedizin und Rehabilitation, 45, 14–20.
commitment: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Psy- Polak, P. (1970). Patterns of discord: Goals of patients, therapists
chology, 86, 1034–1042. and community members. Archives of General Psychiatry, 23,
Man, F., Stuchlikova, I., & Klinger, E. (1998). Motivational 277–283.
structure of alcoholic and nonalcoholic Czech men. Psycho- Prochaska, J. O., DiClemente, C. C., & Norcross, J. C. (1992).
logical Reports, 82, 1091–1106. In search of how people change. Applications to addictive
Markland, D., Ryan, R. M., Tobin, V., & Rollnick, S. (2005). behavior. American Psychologist, 47, 1002–1014.
Motivational interviewing and self-determination theory. Prochaska, J. O., & Norcross, J. C. (2001). Stages of change. Psy-
Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 811–831. chotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 38, 443–448.
Michalak, J. (2000). Zielkonflikte im therapeutischen Prozess [Goal Püschel, O., Schulte, D., & Michalak, J. (2011). Be careful what
conflicts in the therapeutic process]. Wiesbaden, Germany: you strive for. The significance of motive-goal-congruence
Deutscher Universitätsverlag. for depressivity. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 18,
Michalak, J., & Grosse Holtforth, M. (2006). Where do we go 23–33.
from here? The goal perspective in psychotherapy. Clinical Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (1998). Toward a comprehensive
Psychology Science and Practice, 13, 346–365. assessment of fundamental motivation: Factor structure of
Michalak, J., Heidenreich, T., & Hoyer, J. (2011). Goal con- the Reiss Profiles. Psychological Assessment, 10, 97–106.
flicts and goal integration: Theory, assessment, and clinical Rokeach, M. (1973). The nature of human values. New York: Free
implications. In W. M. Cox & E. Klinger (Eds.), Handbook Press.
of motivational counseling: Motivating people for change (2nd. Ruehlman, L. S., & Wolchik, S. A. (1988). Personal goals and
ed., pp. 89–107). London: Wiley interpersonal support and hindrance as factors in psychologi-
Michalak, J., Klappheck, M., & Kosfelder, J. (2004). Personal cal distress and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social
goals of psychotherapy patients: The intensity and the “why” Psychology, 55, 293–301.
of goal-motivated behavior and their implications for the Ryan, R. M. (2005). The development line of autonomy in the
therapeutic process. Psychotherapy Research, 14, 193–209. etiology, dynamics, and treatment of borderline personality
Michalak, J., Kosfelder, J., Meyer, F., & Schulte, D. (2003). disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 987–1006.
Messung des Therapieerfolgs—Veränderungsmessung oder Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory
retrospektive Erfolgsbeurteilung [Assessing treatment suc- and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop-
cess- change scores or retrospective measures of success]. ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.

g ros s e h oltf orth , mi c h a l a k 


Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2008). A self-determination theory Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., Smith, K., & Share, T. (2002).
approach to psychotherapy: The motivational basis for effec- Personal goals and psychological growth: Testing an inter-
tive change. Canadian Psychology, 49, 186–193. vention to enhance goal attainment and personality integra-
Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., Grolnick, W. S., & La Guardia, J. G. tion. Journal of Personality, 70, 5–31.
(2006). The significance of autonomy and autonomy sup- Smith, C. P. (1992). Motivation and personality: Handbook of
port in psychological development and psychopathology. thematic content analysis. Cambridge, England: Cambridge
In D. Cicchetti & D. Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopa- University Press.
thology. Vol. 1: Theory and methods (2nd ed., pp. 295–849). Stangier, U., Ukrow, U., Schermelleh-Engel, K., Grabe, M., &
New York: Wiley. Lauterbach, W.(2007). Intrapersonal conflict in goals and
Ryan, R. M., Plant, R. W., & O’Malley, S. (1995). Initial motiva- values of patients with unipolar depression. Psychotherapy
tions for alcohol treatment: Relations with treatment involve- and Psychosomatics, 76, 162–170.
ment and dropout. Addictive Behaviors, 20, 279–297. Strauman, T. J. (2002). Self-regulation and depression. Self and
Ryan, R. M., Sheldon, K. M., Kasser, T., & Deci, E. L. (1996). Identity, 1, 151–157.
All goals are not created equal: An organismic perspective on Strauman, T. J., Koldon, G. G., Stromquist, V., Davis, N.,
the nature of goals and their regulation. In P. M. Gollwitzer Kwapil, L., Heerey, E., & Schneider, K. (2001). The effects
& J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The psychology of action: Linking cog- of treatments for depression on perceived failure in self-
nition and motivation to behavior (pp. 7–26). New York: regulation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 693–712.
Guilford Press. Strauman, T. J., Vieth, A. Z., Merrill, K. A., Kolden, G. G.,
Ryff, C. D. (1989). Happiness is everything, or is it? Explora- Woods, T. E., Klein, M. H., . . . Kwapil, L. (2006). Self-
tions on the meaning of psychological well-being. Journal of system therapy as an intervention for self-regulatory dysfunc-
Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 1069–1081. tion in depression: A randomized comparison with cognitive
Sachse, R. (2003). Klärungsorientierte Psychotherapie [Clarification- therapy. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74,
Oriented Psychotherapy]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. 367–376.
Sachse, R. (2004). Persönlichkeitsstörungen [Personality disor- Tamir, M., & Diener, E. (2008). Approach-avoidance goals and
ders]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. well-being: One size does not fit all. In A. Elliot (Ed.), Hand-
Sachse, R. (2010). Persönlichkeitsstörungen verstehen: Zum book of approach and avoidance motivation (pp. 415–430).
Umgang mit schwierigen Klienten [Understanding personal- New York: Psychology Press.
ity disorders: Treating difficult patients]. Bonn, Germany: Thompson, H., & Zimmerman, R. (1969). Goals of counsel-
Psychiatrie-Verlag. ing: Whose? When? Journal of Counceling Psychology, 16,
Safran, J. D., & Muran, J. C. (2000). Negotiating the therapeu- 121–125.
tic alliance: A relational treatment guide. New York: Guilford Tryon, G. S., & Winograd, G. (2001). Goal consensus and col-
Press. laboration. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training,
Schmuck, P., Kasser, T., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). Intrinsic and 38, 385–389.
extrinsic goals: Their structure and relationship to well- Uebelacker, L. A., Battle, C. L., Friedman, M. A., Cardemil, E.
being in German and U.S. college students. Social Indicators V., Beevers, C. G., & Miller, I. W. (2008). The importance
Research, 50, 225–241. of interpersonal treatment goals for depressed inpatients.
Schmuck, P., & Sheldon, K. M. (Eds.). (2001). Life goals and Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196, 217–222.
well-being: towards a positive psychology of human striving. Vieth, A., Strauman, T. J., Kolden, G., Woods, T., Michels, J., &
Seattle, WA: Hogrefe. Klein, M. H. (2003). Self-System Therapy: A theory-based
Schneider, W. (1990). Indikationen zur Psychotherapie. Anwend- psychotherapy for depression. Clinical Psychology: Science and
ungsbereiche und Forschungsprobleme [Indication for psycho- Practice, 10, 245–268.
therapy]. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz. Wadsworth, M., & Ford, D. H. (1983). Assessment of per-
Schulte, D., & Eifert, G. H. (2002). What to do when manuals sonal goal hierachies. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 39,
fail? The dual model of psychotherapy. Clinical Psychology: 514–526.
Science and Practice, 9, 312–328. Watkins, E. (2011). Dysregulation in level of goal and action
Schultheiss, O. C., & Brunstein, J. C. (1999). Goal imagery: identification across psychological disorders. Clinical Psychol-
Bridging the gap between implicit motives and explicit goals. ogy Review, 31, 260–278.
Journal of Personality, 67, 1–38. Wiese, B. S., & Freund, A. M. (2005). Goal progress makes
Scott, L., & O’Hara, M. W. (1993). Self-discrepancies in clini- one happy, or does it? Longitudinal findings from the work
cally anxious and depressed university students. Journal of domain. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol-
Abnormal Psychology, 102, 282–287. ogy, 78, 287–304.
Sevincer, A. T., & Oettingen, G. (2009). Alcohol breeds empty Willutzki, U., & Koban, C. (2004). Enhancing motivation
goal commitments. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, for psychotherapy: The eloboration of positive perspectives
623–633. (EPOS) to develop patients’ goal structure. In W. M. Cox
Sheldon, K. (2001). The self-concordance model of healthy goal & E. Klinger (Eds.), Motivating people to change: A hand-
striving: When personal goals correctly represent the person. book of motivational counseling (pp. 337–256). London:
In P. Schmuck & K. Sheldon (Eds.), Life goals and well-being: Wiley.
Towards a positive psychology of human striving (pp. 18–36). Zuroff, D. C., Koestner, R., Moskowitz, D. S., McBride, C.,
Seattle, WA: Hogrefe. Marshall, M., & Bagby, R. M. (2007). Autonomous
Sheldon, K., & Kasser, T. (1998). Pursuing personal goals: Skills motivation for therapy: A new common factor in brief
enable progress but not all progess is benefical. Personality treatments for depression. Psychotherapy Research, 17,
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 1319–1331. 137–147.

 m otivation in psychoth erapy


CHAPTER

Motivation in Education
26
Allan Wigfield, Jenna Cambria, and Jacquelynne S. Eccles

Abstract
In this chapter we discuss the nature of children’s achievement motivation and how it develops over
the school years. We focus on the competence-related belief, value, goal, interest, and intrinsic aspects
of motivation that have been emphasized in much of the research on motivation. We then discuss how
different aspects of classroom and school practices influence motivation, and how teacher–student
relationships and peer relationships impact students’ motivation. We next consider how school
transitions influence students’ motivation, describing important differences in the structure and
organization of schools at different levels of schooling. In the final section we describe some recent
intervention work to boost children’s motivation in different ways. Suggestions for future research
include how students’ motivation varies in different classroom contexts, the need to study motivation
in diverse groups of children, methodological issues with respect to studying motivation, what other
kinds of further motivation intervention studies are needed, and how work on motivation can inform
educational policy.
Key Words: achievement motivation, development of motivation, motivation and instruction,
teacher–student relationships and motivation, peers and motivation

Overview Eccles, 2002; Wigfield, Tonks, & Klauda, 2009).


Motivation theorists are interested in the “whys” Because of this emphasis on self variables, much
of human behavior: what moves people to act research on motivation has focused on motivation
(Weiner, 1992). In terms of motivation and edu- as a characteristic of the individual.
cation, researchers studying school motivation look Motivation researchers also recognize the impor-
at things like the engagement and interest students tance of social influences on learning and moti-
have in different academic activities, the choices stu- vation (Ladd, Herald-Brown, & Kochel, 2009;
dents make about which academic activities to do, Wentzel, 2009). Indeed, many researchers and
their persistence at continuing the activities, and the theorists now posit that learning is an inherently
degree of effort they expend. But what determines social activity (Hickey & Granade, 2004; Vygotsky,
individuals’ choices, effort, and persistence at differ- 1978). Learning in classrooms is not done in isola-
ent academic activities? Many motivation research- tion, but instead occurs in the context of relation-
ers have focused on students’ self-beliefs, values, ships with teachers and peers (O’Donnell, 2006;
and goals and how these relate to their achievement Webb & Palincsar, 1996; Wentzel, 2009). These
behaviors, such as choice, persistence, and perfor- relationships, and the different roles that emerge
mance (Maehr & Zusho, 2009; Ryan & Deci, 2009; for students and teachers in various classrooms,
Schiefele, 2009; Schunk & Pajares, 2009; Wigfield & strongly influence how students learn. Furthermore,


opportunities for social interactions around learn- capacity where different subskills are organized
ing have been shown to improve children’s achieve- into courses of action. Bandura (1997) reviewed
ment in reading and other areas (e.g., Guthrie, research showing that individuals’ efficacy for dif-
McRae, & Klauda, 2007; Johnson & Johnson, ferent achievement tasks is a major determinant of
2009). Along with social relationships, it is increas- activity choice, willingness to expend effort, and
ingly clear that the social contexts and organization persistence in and out of school (see also Schunk &
of classrooms and schools also have major influ- Pajares, 2009).
ences on students’ motivation and achievement Researchers interested in individuals’ control
(Nolen & Ward, 2008; Perry, Turner, & Meyer, beliefs initially made a major distinction between
2006; Wigfield, Eccles, & Rodriguez, 1998). internal and external locus of control (e.g., Crandall,
In this chapter we discuss children’s motivation Katkovsky, & Crandall, 1965; Rotter, 1966. Internal
in school. We begin with a brief discussion of the control means the individual believes that he or she
belief, value, and goal constructs prevalent in cur- controls the outcome; external control means the
rent motivation research and how they develop. outcome is determined by other things. Research-
We then discuss the influence of different aspects ers have confirmed the positive association between
of classrooms and schools on the development of internal locus of control and academic achievement
students’ motivation. We also discuss how school (see Skinner, 1995). Connell and Wellborn (1991)
structures change as children move from elemen- integrated control beliefs into a broader theoreti-
tary into secondary school, and how such changes cal framework based on psychological needs for
affect students’ motivation. We close with discus- competence, autonomy, and relatedness from self-
sion of some recent intervention efforts to improve determination theory (Ryan & Deci, 2009). They
children’s motivation. linked control beliefs to competence needs: Chil-
dren who believe they control their achievement
The Nature of Student Motivation outcomes should feel more competent. When the
Researchers have assessed many different con- family, peer, and school contexts support children’s
structs that are crucial to students’ motivation. autonomy, develop their competence, and provide
To organize our discussion of these constructs, we positive relations with others, then children’s moti-
separate them into two broad groups. One group vation (which Connell and Wellborn conceptual-
includes individuals’ sense of their competence ized as engagement) will be positive, and they will
and agency to achieve different outcomes. Another become fully engaged in different activities, such as
group concerns intrinsic and extrinsic motivation, their school work. When one or more of the needs
interest, values, and goals. Many of these constructs is not fulfilled, children will become disaffected
are discussed in greater detail in other chapters in (see Connell, Spencer, & Aber, 1994; Skinner,
this book. Kindermann, Connell, & Wellborn, 2009).

Individuals’ Sense of Competence and Individuals’ Intrinsic Motivation, Interests,


Control Values, and Goals
Many researchers interested in motivation Although theories dealing with competence,
focus on students’ beliefs about their ability and expectancy, and control beliefs provide powerful
efficacy to perform achievement tasks as crucial explanations of individuals’ performance on differ-
motivational mediators of achievement behavior ent kinds of achievement activities, these theories
(e.g., Bandura, 1997; Eccles et al., 1983; Wigfield, do not systematically address another important
Tonks, & Klauda, 2009). Ability beliefs are chil- motivational question: Does the individual want to
dren’s evaluations of their competence in different do the task? Even if people are certain they can do
areas. Researchers have documented that children’s a task and think they can control the outcome, they
and adolescents’ ability beliefs relate to and predict may not want to engage in it. Once the decision is
their achievement performance in different achieve- made to engage in a task or activity, there are differ-
ment domains like math and reading, even when ent reasons for doing so. The constructs discussed
previous performance is controlled (see Wigfield next focus on these aspects of motivation.
et al., 2009, for review). A basic distinction in the motivation literature is
Bandura’s (1997) construct of self-efficacy also between intrinsic motivation and extrinsic motiva-
deals with individuals’ sense of competence; how- tion (Deci & Ryan, 1985; Harter, 1981). When indi-
ever, Bandura defined self-efficacy as a generative viduals are intrinsically motivated, they do activities

 m otivation in education


for their own sake and out of interest in the activ- interest (Wigfield & Cambria, 2010). Utility value
ity. Deci, Ryan, and their colleagues (e. g., Deci & is determined by how well a task relates to current
Ryan, 1985; Ryan & Deci, 2009) went beyond the and future goals, such as career goals. A task can
extrinsic-intrinsic motivation dichotomy in their have positive value to a person because it facilitates
discussion of internalization, the process of trans- important future goals, even if he or she is not inter-
ferring the regulation of behavior from outside to ested in a task for its own sake. For instance, stu-
inside the individual. They defined several levels in dents often take classes that they do not particularly
the process of going from external to more internal- enjoy but that they need in order to pursue other
ized regulation: external—regulation coming from interests, to please their parents, or to be with their
outside the individual; introjected—internal regula- friends. In one sense then this component captures
tion based on feelings that he or she should or has to the more “extrinsic” reasons for engaging in a task.
do the behavior; identified—internal regulation of But it also relates directly to individuals’ internal-
behavior that is based on the utility of that behavior ized short- and long-term goals. Finally, cost refers
(e.g., studying hard to get grades to get into col- to what one has to give up to do something else;
lege); and finally, integrated—regulation based on spending time on homework means less time for
what the individual thinks is valuable and impor- socializing with friends.
tant to the self. Even though the integrated level Eccles and her colleagues have found that individ-
is self-determined, it still does not reflect intrinsi- uals’ task values predict course plans and enrollment
cally motivated behavior. Intrinsic motivation only decisions in mathematics, physics, and English and
occurs when the individual autonomously controls involvement in sport activities even after controlling
the behavior, which may not be the case even at the for prior performance levels (Durik, Vida, & Eccles,
integrated level of regulation. 2006; Eccles et al., 1983; Meece, Wigfield, &
A construct closely related to intrinsic motiva- Eccles, 1990; Simpkins, Davis-Kean, & Eccles,
tion is interest (Hidi & Renninger, 2006; Schiefele, 2006). They have also shown that both competence
2009), and researchers studying interest distinguish beliefs and values predict career choices (see Eccles,
between individual and situational interest. As the 2005.
name implies, individual or personal interest is The construct perhaps most directly related to the
a characteristic of the individual, and it is concep- purposes for doing an activity is achievement goals.
tualized either as a relatively stable disposition or an Researchers (e.g., Ames, 1992; Dweck & Leggett,
active state. Hidi and Renninger (2006) suggested 1988; Nicholls, 1984) initially distinguished three
that individual interest includes both knowledge broad goal orientations that students can have
and value about a topic or object and represents an toward their learning (see Maehr & Zusho, 2009;
enduring involvement with an activity. By contrast, Elliot, 2005, for review). One orientation, called
situational interest stems from conditions in the learning, task involved, or mastery goal orienta-
environment. Hidi and Renninger (2006) described tion, means that the child is focused on improving
how situational interest generates curiosity, which his or her skills, mastering material, and learning
can lead individuals to explore an activity further new things. The second goal orientation, called per-
and develop individual interest in it. This point is formance or ego orientation, means that the child
a crucial one for this chapter; features of activities focuses on maximizing favorable evaluations of his
that individuals do in school can increase their per- or her competence and minimizing negative evalu-
sonal interest in the activities. Furthermore, there are ations of competence. The different terms used to
significant but moderate relations between interest label the first two goal orientations occurred because
and learning of different kinds (Schiefele, 2009). different researchers were working on them simul-
Eccles and her colleagues have defined different taneously, with each having a somewhat distinctive
ways in which individuals can value activities such as view of each orientation (see Pintrich, 2000a, and
schoolwork (see Eccles et al., 1983; Wigfield et al., Thorkildsen & Nicholls, 1998). Nicholls and his
2009). Eccles et al. (1983) outlined four motiva- colleagues (e.g., Nicholls, Cobb, Wood, Yackel, &
tional components of task value: attainment value, Patashnick, 1990; Nicholls, Cobb, Yackel, Wood, &
intrinsic value, utility value, and cost. Attainment Wheatley, 1990) and Meece (1991, 1994) also
value refers to the importance of the activity to the described a work-avoidant goal orientation, which
individual. Intrinsic value is the enjoyment the indi- means that the child does not wish to engage in aca-
vidual gets from performing the activity, and so it demic activities. This orientation has received less
is conceptually linked to intrinsic motivation and research attention compared to the others.

w i g f i eld , c a mb r i a , ecc les 


In the 1990s, researchers differentiated perfor- Development of Children’s Motivation
mance and mastery goal orientations into approach A substantial body of research shows that chil-
and avoidance components. Elliot and Harackie- dren’s academic motivation declines across the ele-
wicz (1996) and Skaalvik (1997), among others, mentary and secondary school years (see Wigfield,
defined performance-approach goals as students’ Eccles, Schiefele, Roeser, & Davis-Kean, 2006, for
desire to demonstrate competence and outperform review). Many young children are quite optimistic
others. Performance-avoidance goals involve the about their competencies in different areas, and this
desire to avoid looking incompetent. Elliot (1999; optimism changes to greater realism and (some-
Elliot & McGregor, 2001) and Pintrich (2000b) times) pessimism for many children as they go
proposed that the mastery goal orientation also may through school (Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; Jacobs,
be divided into approach and avoid components, Lanza, Osgood, Eccles, & Wigfield, 2002; Watt,
rather than being solely conceived as reflecting an 2004). Children’s intrinsic motivation for different
approach tendency. academic subjects also declines (Gottfried, Fleming,
One issue with the approach-avoidance dis- & Gottfried., 2001), as does their valuing of achieve-
tinction that continues among goal orientation ment (Jacobs et al., 2002). Children also appear to
theorists is debate about their relative merits of focus more on performance goals as they get older
the different kinds of goal orientation. Most goal (Maehr & Zusho, 2009). Although the pattern of
orientation theorists believe in the benefits of mas- these findings is clear, most of the research just men-
tery goals for both students and teachers (because tioned is normative, describing mean-level change
they focus students on meaningful learning and across all children. Researchers have shown that
improvement) and many of these theorists state these patterns do vary for children achieving at dif-
that such goal orientations should be focused on ferent levels (Harter, Whitesell, & Kowalski, 1992;
more strongly in school. Theorists also agree that Wigfield, Eccles, Mac Iver, Reuman, & Midgley,
performance-avoid goals are debilitating. There 1991); more work of this kind is needed.
is debate, however, about the relative merits of These changes have been explained with respect
performance-approach goals. Because these goals to children’s understanding of their performance and
relate positively to some important achievement changes in the school environments children experi-
outcomes such as grades, some theorists believe ence. First, children both receive more information
that performance-approach goals can be beneficial about their performance and learn to interpret it
to students; other theorists continue to think that more clearly. Because they are with same-aged peers
mastery goals are the most favorable goals students in school they also learn to compare themselves
can have. A complete discussion of this debate is more systematically with others, which can lead to
beyond the scope of this chapter; interested read- decreases in motivation for some children (see Wig-
ers should see Linnenbrink (2005) and Maehr and field et al., 2009). Second, schools focus more on
Zusho (2009). evaluation and performance outcomes as children
To conclude this section, researchers have identi- go through school, which can negatively impact
fied a number of important beliefs, value, and goal some children’s motivation. We turn next to a more
constructs that impact students’ motivation. These detailed consideration of schooling’s influences on
variables relate to achievement and choice in many students’ motivation.
different academic areas. We discussed these con-
structs individually and many researchers indeed School’s Influences on Students’ Motivation
have studied each separately. There is increasing How do different kinds of tasks, activities, and
interest currently in how they interrelate and relate structures in school impact children’s motivation?
to various achievement outcomes (e.g., Harackie- How do the relationships children have with their
wicz, Durik, Barron, Linnenbrink-Garcia, & Tauer, teachers and peers influence their motivation? We
2008; Wigfield & Cambria, 2010). For instance, focus on these issues in this section.
having positive competence beliefs, intrinsic moti-
vation, and mastery goals for activities may be the Tasks and Classroom Practices and Student
most adaptive pattern for positive motivation. Motivation
Researchers also have studied how students’ Stipek (1996) reviewed the effects of tasks and
beliefs, values, and goals change across the school classroom practices on student outcomes. She argued
years; that is, how they change across the school that although students do have certain motivational
years; that is the topic of the next section. characteristics that they bring with them to the class-

 m otivation in education


room, teachers and the kinds of environment they to the use of rewards, intrinsic motivation is most
provide are primary influences on students’ motiva- enhanced when evaluation provides information
tion and achievement. Stipek (1996) discussed how about progress. Teachers also can foster intrinsic
teachers can foster motivation by specific classroom motivation by varying the format of tasks, offering
practices that enhance achievement-related beliefs, appropriate levels of challenges for students, and
intrinsic motivation, and learning goals. allowing choice. Each of these classroom practices
From her review of the research, Stipek (1996) is associated with students’ perceptions of control
posited that the classroom practices that are associ- over their academic outcomes, mastery goals, and
ated with changes in achievement-related beliefs are competence beliefs. Increasing these positive beliefs
task-level practices, criteria for success, evaluation, and feelings are associated with task engagement,
rewards, and teacher behaviors toward students. enjoyment, understanding, and ultimately increased
Task-level practices that are motivating are those intrinsic motivation.
with appropriate level of challenge. In addition, Stipek (1996) also described the importance of
tasks should be differentiated over time so that tasks classroom practices on fostering learning or mastery
do not become redundant or uninteresting. Next, goals. This is important because learning goals (also
teachers can foster more positive achievement- called mastery or task goals) are associated with
related beliefs by creating clear achievable criterion developing skills and a desire to master the material.
for success and offer rewards for achieving this crite- This builds on her discussion of fostering intrinsic
rion. Stipek suggested that improvement on previous motivation in students because it includes each of
work be the primary mark of success and students those practices and is supplemented by several other
should receive clear and positive feedback about practices such as adapting instruction to students
how to attain this goal. Rewards should be given on existing background knowledge, providing oppor-
the basis of improvement, effort, and performance. tunities for exploration of the topic, and treating
The final classroom practice that Stipek (1996) dis- mistakes as an expected part of the learning pro-
cussed as a primary influence on achievement-related cess. Engaging in these practices is associated with
beliefs is how teachers treat their students. Teachers’ increase in students’ goals. Students are more likely
behaviors reflect their beliefs about student compe- to understand, gain skills, and learn and master
tence; therefore, they should avoid treating students tasks. These goals are associated with attentiveness,
of varying abilities differently and express that all conceptual learning, and feeling satisfaction from
students can achieve if they put effort into the task. gaining knowledge.
These classroom practices are associated with stu- Stipek’s (1996) discussion of the merit of these
dent competence beliefs, having an internal locus instructional practices on achievement-related
of control, and holding the perception that achieve- beliefs, intrinsic motivation, and learning goals
ment is due to effort and not attributable solely to was based on observational, correlational, and
ability. These beliefs are associated with more posi- experimental research; however, these task-level and
tive achievement-related beliefs, which are related instructional practices likely interact, and it remains
to positive student outcomes such as help seeking, unclear how practices may interact, what the best
persistence, effort, and pride in success. combination of practices is, and at what magnitude
Stipek (1996) also discussed how classroom these practices are most helpful for students’ moti-
practices influence intrinsic motivation in students. vation and learning.
In this case there is significant overlap with her dis-
cussion of fostering achievement-related beliefs. The TARGET
classroom practices that are associated with intrinsic Ames (1992) used the acronym TARGET to
motivation are use of rewards, evaluation, and task- describe a set of instructional practices designed
level practices. When rewards are used as indica- to increase the levels of student motivation, espe-
tors of performance and to provide information as cially their mastery goals. She focused on mastery
opposed to trying to gain control over students, they goals because these goals are associated with more
can enhance intrinsic motivation (see also Ryan & time spent on tasks (Butler, 1987) and the amount
Deci, 2009). Next, overly emphasizing evaluation of effort put into learning (Elliot & Dweck, 1988)
has negative effects on intrinsic motivation. Evalu- than performance goals. TARGET is an evidence-
ation is best used to inform students about their based set of classroom practices that includes task
best or less effective practices as opposed to threat- design, authority, recognition, grouping arrange-
ening or controlling evaluation techniques. Similar ments, evaluation practices, and time allocation.

w i g f i eld , c a mb r i a , ecc les 


Strategic design of tasks is the first element of children’s relations with peers and parents are taken
TARGET. Each task should be designed with useful into account; research measuring support from
learning goals in mind and use a variety of tasks. all three kinds of socialization agents shows that
The focus of these tasks should be gaining skills and teacher support is particularly important for aca-
learning. Such thoughtful planning of classroom demic motivation and adjustment.
tasks is associated with increased interest and skills. Teachers’ relations with students are crucial
Distributing authority in the classroom is essential to to students’ early adjustment in school (Birch &
increasing student autonomy and allows them own- Ladd, 1996), and the emotional quality of student–
ership over their learning by allowing them to make teacher relations during the early school years pre-
decisions and schedules for their tasks. By shifting dicts growth in their reading and math achievement
autonomy in the classroom from teacher to student, (Pianta, Belsky, Vandegrift, Houts, & Morrison,
students gain ownership over their learning activi- 2008). The importance of such relations continues
ties by increasing their participation in classroom into middle school and beyond. Goodenow (1993)
choices. This increases mastery goals and interest. found that students’ perceptions of support from
Recognizing students’ success and improvement is teachers and their sense of belongingness in their
similar to Stipek’s (1996) discussion of rewards and classrooms related strongly to their perceived valu-
evaluation. Students’ effort and successes should be ing of the schoolwork they were doing. Similarly,
recognized and rewarded. Wentzel (2002) found that students’ academic goals
The next element of TARGET is grouping and performance were strongly related to their sense
arrangements, which should be organized to pro- that their middle school teachers were “caring.”
mote discussion that leads to a deeper understand- Wentzel (2009) noted that much of the work
ing of points and support in the face of challenging showing how teacher–student relations impact
tasks. These classroom groups should be heteroge- student achievement is correlational and discussed
neous and should not reflect ability differences (see a variety of design and measurement issues that
Wigfield et al., 1998, for a review of how ability need to be considered in the next generation of this
grouping impacts students’ motivation). Students research. These include the complexity of these rela-
generally are aware of ability-based groups, and this tions and the need to examine students’ impact on
is undermining for mastery goals. Evaluation tech- teachers along with teachers’ impact on students.
niques that encourage learning and not normative Additionally, researchers need to take the “nested”
comparison are essential to TARGET. They should nature of these relationships into account; teacher–
be based on progress and achieving attainable goals. student relations occur in complex classroom set-
The final piece of a TARGET classroom is time allo- tings and the kinds of relations teachers have with
cation. Allowing students to make decisions about individual students likely is influenced by the rela-
how to pace and schedule their assignments and tions they have with others in their classes. Wentzel
understanding that students work successfully at dif- argued that we need a clearer understanding of the
ference paces is essential for fostering mastery goals. mechanisms underlying the observed relations of
With respect to enhancing the probability that stu- teacher emotional support and student motivation
dents adopt a mastery goal orientation, a TARGET and achievement.
classroom structure uses motivational principles
that could contribute or contribute multiplicatively Peers and Motivation
toward an orientation to develop new skills. Peers are another important social influence on
motivation. When children are socially supported
Teacher–Student Relationships and Student and accepted by their peers, they have stronger moti-
Motivation vation, better achievement outcomes, and are more
There is a growing body of literature that shows engaged in school (see Ladd, Herald-Brown, &
how the affective relationships teachers have with Kochel., 2009 for review). Furthermore, social com-
students impact students’ motivation and achieve- petence and social support can help ease school tran-
ment in school (see Juvonen, 2006, and Wentzel, sitions, including the transition from home to school
2009, for review). When teachers support stu- (Ladd et al., 2009. In contrast, socially rejected and
dents emotionally, they have higher school-related highly aggressive children, and also those who are
perceptions of competence, clearer positive social victimized by others, are at risk for poorer achieve-
and academic goals, and willingness to engage in ment and motivation (Birch & Ladd, 1996; Ladd
school activities. These relations emerge even when et al., 2009). Moreover, it appears that both the

 m otivation in education


quantity of children’s friendships with peers and the (Cohen, 1994; O’Donnell, 2006). By working
quality of the friendships are related to positive out- together students can create communities of learn-
comes; in fact, the quality of children’s friendships ers and learn to co-regulate each others’ motivation
may be especially key, particularly as children move and achievement (McCaslin & Good, 1996).
into adolescence (Berndt & Keefe, 1995).
Peer groups in school can have either a posi- School Transitions and Changes in Student
tive or negative effect on motivation across various Motivation.
activity settings. Children who come together in Entrance into kindergarten and then the tran-
peer groups often share similar motivational orien- sition from kindergarten to first grade introduces
tations and activity preferences, and such groupings several systematic changes in children’s social worlds
reinforce and strengthen their existing motivational (Pianta, Rimm-Kaufman, & Cox, 1999). First,
orientation and activity preferences over time (e.g., classes are age stratified, making within-age ability
Berndt & Keefe, 1995; Kindermann, 1993, 2007). social comparison much easier. Second, formal eval-
Whether such effects are positive or negative depends uations of competence by “experts” begin. Third,
on the nature of the peer groups’ motivational ori- formal ability grouping begins usually with reading
entation. High-achieving children who seek out group assignment. Fourth, peers have the oppor-
other high achievers as friends develop even more tunity to play a much more constant and salient
positive academic motivation over time. The role role in children’ lives. Each of these changes can
of peer group influences is likely to vary across age. impact children’s motivational development (Pianta
Peers may play an especially important role vis-à-vis et al., 1999). Unfortunately, very little longitudinal
motivation and achievement during adolescence, research has focused on this transition and how it
for two reasons: Adolescents are more aware of, and influences children’s motivation and achievement
concerned about, peer group acceptance and they (one important exception is Pianta and colleagues’
spend much more unsupervised time with peers work; see Pianta et al., 2008).
groups than younger children (Rubin, Bukowski, & Instead, most of the research on the early ele-
Parker, 2006). Consequently, adolescents should be mentary school years has focused on individual dif-
especially susceptible to peer group influences on ferences in the link between children’s early school
motivation and achievement. experiences and their subsequent development.
This research suggests significant long-term conse-
Learning in Groups and Motivation quences of children’s experiences in the early school
There is an extensive body of research on how years, particularly experiences associated with ability
students learn in groups and the impact of students grouping and within-class differential teacher treat-
on each others’ learning and motivation; much of ment. For example, teachers use a variety of infor-
this work has focused on cooperative learning (see mation to assign first graders to reading groups,
O’Donnell, 2006; Webb & Palincsar, 1996, for sys- including temperamental characteristics like interest
tematic reviews of the research on group processes and persistence, race, gender, and social class (e.g.,
in the classroom, and Johnson & Johnson, 2009, Alexander, Dauber & Entwisle, 1993; Brophy &
for a review of the effects of cooperative learning). Good, 1974). Alexander et al. (1993) demonstrated
One issue motivation researchers have focused on is that differences in first grade reading group place-
how cooperative and competitive reward structures ment and teacher–student interactions predict
in classrooms influence students’ motivation. Ames subsequent motivation and achievement even after
(1984) discussed how competitive reward structures controlling for initial differences in reading com-
heighten social comparison and a focus on one’s abil- petence. Furthermore, these effects are mediated
ity relative to others. Cooperative reward structures by both differential instruction and the amplifying
help children focus on shared effort and interdepen- impact of ability group placement on parents’ and
dence. More broadly, the research on cooperative teachers’ views of the children’s abilities, talents, and
learning shows that children’s achievement often motivation (Pallas et al., 1994).
improves, social relations are more positive, and stu- As noted earlier, there are substantial changes
dents’ motivation is enhanced (Johnson & Johnson, in academic motivation and achievement across
2009). Peers can also help each other understand the upper elementary and secondary school years,
and learn the material through group discussion, including changes in grades, interest in school,
sharing of resources, modeling academic skills, and perceptions of competence in different areas, and
interpreting and clarifying the tasks for each other increases in performance goals at the expense of

w i g f i eld , c a mb r i a , ecc les 


mastery goals. These changes are particularly large Pintrich, 1996). These characteristics, in turn, are
for students who are doing poorly (either emotion- likely to lead to an increase in performance rather
ally or academically) in school (Lord, Eccles, & than mastery goal focus in the classroom and the
McCarthy, 1994). The transition from elementary to school building. As noted earlier, these changes
middle school can accelerate these negative changes. are likely to undermine low-performing students’
In explaining them, Eccles et al. (1998) discussed sense of competence.
how the multiple changes that occur during this Research on the transition to high school suggests
time period (puberty, school transitions, changing that similar changes occur at this transition (Lee &
relations with parents, increasing cognitive maturity, Smith, 2001; Mac Iver et al., 1995; NRC, 2004).
increasing concern with identity, increasing sexual- For example, high schools are typically even larger
ity and heterosociality, and increasing focus on peer and more bureaucratic than middle and junior high
relationships) likely have an impact on students’ schools. Lee and Smith (2001) provide numerous
motivation and achievement. They also discussed examples of how the sense of community among
how differences in school environments between teachers and students is undermined by the size and
elementary and secondary schools could contribute bureaucratic structure of most high schools. There
to these changes (see also NRC, 2004). Traditional is less opportunity for students and teachers to get
secondary schools differ structurally in important to know each other and, likely as a consequence,
ways from elementary schools. Most secondary there is distrust between them and little agreement
schools are substantially larger than elementary on a common set of goals and values. There is also
schools. As a result, students’ friendship networks less opportunity for the students to form mentor-
often are disrupted as they attend classes with stu- like relationships with the teachers, and there is
dents from several different schools. In addition, little effort to make instruction meaningful to the
students are likely to feel more anonymous and students.
alienated because of the large size of many second- Such environments are likely to undermine the
ary schools. Finally, the opportunity to participate motivation and involvement of many students,
in and play leadership roles in school activities often especially those not doing particularly well academi-
declines over these school transitions due to the lim- cally, and those who are alienated from the values of
ited number of slots in such niches and the increas- the adults in the high school. Furthermore, research
ing size of the student body. These kinds of changes based upon both teacher and student reports shows
should affect the students’ sense of belonging as well that schools become more socially comparative and
as their sense of social competence. competitive in orientation as students progression
The nature of instruction also changes: Secondary from elementary to middle to high school (Wigfield
school instruction is organized and taught depart- et al., 1996). The coincidence of declining social
mentally—making it likely that secondary school support and increased social comparison and com-
teachers teach several different groups of students petition at both the middle and high school levels
each day and are unlikely to teach any particular likely contributes to some adolescents’ decisions,
students for more than one year. This departmental especially those who are already on the margins of
structure can create a number of difficulties for stu- the school community, to withdraw from school
dents. First, the curriculum often is not integrated prior to graduation (Finn, 1989).
across different subjects. Second, students typically
have several teachers each day with little opportu- Middle School Reform Efforts and
nity to interact with any one teacher on any deeper Student Motivation
dimension beyond the academic content of what is Based in part on the research just reviewed, dur-
being taught and disciplinary issues. As a result, the ing the 1990s different middle school reform efforts
likelihood of students and teachers forming close, were undertaken; many of the recommendations
supportive bonds is much less in secondary than in were included in a report by the Carnegie Founda-
elementary schools. tion (1989). There are a number of important ways
Finally, grading systems are more likely to be in which these recommendations have been imple-
based on social comparative performance, abil- mented in different middle schools. One is replac-
ity level tracking via curricular tracking is com- ing departmentalized curriculum structures with
mon, and teachers are more likely to hold entity, teams of teachers working with the same group of
rather than incremental, views of ability differ- students. This practice allows groups of teachers to
ences (Eccles & Midgley, 1989; Wigfield Eccles, & spend more time with the same group of adolescents,

 m otivation in education


thus getting to know them better. It also allows for One of the challenges for motivation researchers
greater integration across the curriculum. Teachers is that many of the reform efforts they espouse do
serving as advisors and counselors has become more not fit well with the current focus on performance,
prevalent, so that adolescents can develop closer testing, and accountability that are hallmarks of the
relationships with their teachers. To create smaller No Child Left Behind era. The severe pressure that
learning communities in often-large middle schools, many teachers and principals face to produce higher
“schools within schools” have been created, in part test scores and other indicators of student perfor-
through the teaming approach just discussed. This is mance can lead to a strong focus on performance
particularly likely to occur for the youngest group in on the tests used to gauge student growth, at the
a middle school, be they fifth graders, sixth graders, expense of focusing on how efforts to enhance stu-
or seventh graders. Cooperative learning practices dents’ interest and mastery can enhance their per-
are used more frequently, in part to reduce the use formance (Deci & Ryan, 2002). We understand
of ability grouping or tracking. Juvonen (2007) dis- the importance of students showing continual
cussed middle school reform efforts designed spe- progress in their learning and believe teachers and
cifically to promote student engagement in school principals need to be accountable for this progress.
by facilitating social relationships among students However, we also believe that a focus on enhancing
and between students and teachers. students’ sense of competence, interest and enjoy-
Relatively few of the middle school reform efforts ment of learning, and mastery also can lead to gains
focused specifically on students’ motivation. An in students’ performance, and it is more likely to
important exception is the work of Maehr and foster students’ healthy development in other areas
Midgley (1996), who worked with teachers and as well. We turn next to a discussion of some suc-
administrators to change the culture organization and cessful interventions that have enhanced students’
climate of a middle school and an elementary school motivation and achievement
in a city in Michigan from performance goal based
to mastery goal based (similar to Ames, 1992). The Motivation Interventions
school-university team worked extensively to restruc- Many researchers studying students’ motiva-
ture the school toward a focus on mastery goal; they tion have used correlational methods measuring
spent 3 years in each school. At the middle school aspects of students’ motivation and relating them
they focused on creating teams of teachers, “schools to different achievement outcomes, experimen-
within the school,” lessening the use of ability group- tal studies done in controlled laboratory settings,
ing practices, and changing the student recognition or classroom-based observation studies. From this
patterns so that not just the “honor roll” students work we now have a substantial body of information
were recognized. They also worked to loosen the about the nature of students’ motivation and how
rigid bell schedule so that longer class periods were different teaching practices and classroom environ-
sometimes possible. Changing the school culture in ments impact motivation and achievement. There
the middle school was very difficulty due to some is a growing body of work examining the effective-
teachers’ (especially the math teachers) resistance to ness of different kinds of interventions designed to
change. Despite these difficulties, the changes had improve students’ motivation; we discuss examples
positive effects on students’ motivation (E. Ander- of this kind of work in this section (see also Went-
man, Maehr, & Midgley, 1999). zel & Wigfield, 2007). This work can be done at
Even less work has been done on high school different levels, including working with individual
reform effort with respect to motivation and the students to improve aspects of their motivation such
results of this work are less consistent (NRC, 2004). as their self-efficacy or changing their failure attri-
Reform efforts have followed similar principles butions to focus on lack of effort rather than lack of
aimed at creating schools that better meet the com- ability (e.g., Dweck, 1975; Schunk, 1983) or work-
petence, belonging, autonomy, and mattering needs ing at the classroom or school levels; we focus here
of the adolescent students. As is true for the middle on the latter kind of work.
school reform efforts, when these principles are well Blackwell, Trzesniewski, and Dweck (2007) pro-
implemented, improvements in students’ motiva- vided seventh grade students with an eight-session
tion, school engagement, and academic perfor- intervention designed to help them develop an
mance are obtained (NRC, 2004). But successfully incremental rather than entity view of their ability.
implementing these kinds of changes has proven to Dweck and her colleagues have shown that students
be very difficult at the high school level. who believe that their intelligence is incremental

w i g f i eld , c a mb r i a , ecc les 


or modifiable are more positively motivated than includes instruction in math, English, and science.
those who believe it is fixed (see Dweck & Master, The program focuses on developing strong learning
2009, for review). The intervention involved teach- communities and providing an engaging curricu-
ing children that they can grow their intelligence lum in each subject area. Analyses of the effects of
through their efforts and students were assigned the program on student outcomes indicate that the
randomly by classroom to the intervention or con- program has been effective in improving student
trol group. The intervention produced significant achievement and engagement because the specific
changes in the intervention group’s theories of kinds of instruction are important for motivation
intelligence; these children became more incremen- and achievement.
tal in their views about their intelligence. Overall, Guthrie, McRae, and Klauda (2007) review the
students’ math grades declined over the course of research on how different kinds of reading instruc-
the year; this decline was reversed for the students tion programs influence motivation and achieve-
in the intervention group. Interestingly, there was ment, focusing specifically on Concept-Oriented
an (marginally significant) interaction of student’s Reading Instruction (CORI), a reading comprehen-
initial theory of intelligence and condition showing sion instruction program that integrates science and
that students who initially endorsed the entity view reading. CORI focuses on instructional practices
of intelligence were most impacted by the interven- to enhance students’ motivation for reading and
tion with respect to their grades. The decline in ability to use cognitive reading strategies. The class-
grades for such students in the intervention group room practices that are designed to enhance reading
reversed, whereas for control group students hold- motivation include increasing personal relevance of
ing this view the decline continued. Blackwell et al. the material being learned, providing choices with
concluded that altering students’ views of their respect to learning activities, ensuring enough suc-
intelligence impacted both their motivation and cess that students’ self-efficacy is fostered, giving
mathematics achievement. many opportunities for student collaboration, and
Felner, Seitsinger, Brand, Burns, and Bolton teaching in thematic units so that students have
(2007) reviewed their work on reforming middle clear content goals for learning. Guthrie et al.’s
schools to create effective small learning commu- meta-analysis of the work to date on CORI’s effec-
nities in middle school. They call their approach tiveness in boosting students’ reading motivation,
the High Performance Learning Communities reading comprehension, and strategy use showed
Project (Project HiPlace). Project HiPlace is being that CORI is indeed effective in increasing these
conducted in middle schools across the country, outcomes for students in grades 3–5.
including many middle schools serving minority Hudley, Graham, and Taylor (2007) focused on
students living in poverty. Felner and his colleagues a somewhat different issue that has implications
have conducted a number of large-scale studies on for students’ school engagement and achievement,
the effectiveness of Project HiPlace, and they have helping students interact more positively with
documented that creating smaller learning commu- their peers. Their interventions focus on reducing
nities in middle school has positive effects on stu- elementary school aged children’s aggression toward
dents’ motivation and achievement. peers. Many of their interventions have been con-
Balfanz, Herzog, and Mac Iver (2007) and other ducted with African American boys. The theoreti-
colleagues developed a system for identifying middle cal grounding of their work is attribution theory,
school students who are most at risk for academic a theory that characterizes individuals’ understand-
disengagement and later dropping out of school. ing of their own and others’ actions. Hudley et al.
They focus on student grades, attendance, and discuss how aggressive children often interpret the
behavior in school (as rated by the teacher) as pos- acts of others as hostile, even when the intention
sible indicators of disengagement, and they examine behind the act is benign. Their interventions focus
how each of these factors individually and together on changing these children’s interpretations of oth-
affect student engagement and school attendance ers’ actions, and reducing their aggressive responses
over the middle and high school years. These to other children. Results of studies evaluating the
researchers also developed and evaluated a middle effectiveness of the interventions show that they
school reform effort called the Talent Development have had positive effects both on students’ social
Middle School Project, a broad-scale instructional behavior and attitudes, and academic motivation.
program implemented in urban middle schools in Hudley et al. discuss how this work shows that
Philadelphia and other cities. Talent Development children’s social behaviors indeed can be altered in

 m otivation in education


positive ways during the elementary school years, A number of motivation researchers taking
leading to increased achievement or improved moti- sociocultural approaches to motivation have made
vation. They also discuss the importance of tailoring similar points about the dynamic, situation-based
the interventions to the cultural and ethnic back- nature of motivation (Hickey, 1997, 2008; Hickey
grounds of the participants, in order for the inter- & Granade, 2004; Nolen, 2007; Nolen & Ward,
ventions to be optimally effective. 2008; Urdan, 1999). These researchers argue that
In sum, research on the nature of motivation motivation is not a stable individual characteristic
has informed important interventions that aim to that operates similarly in different settings. Addi-
increase motivation in various domains, increase tionally, classrooms themselves are fluid structures
achievement, and improve students’ social behav- that change depending upon who is in them and the
iors and perceptions. Work is needed to take these mutual influences teachers, students, and activities
successful interventions to scale and to examine all have on one another. Motivation theorists tak-
them for different groups of children and children ing a sociocultural perspective challenge some of the
of different ages. premises of the social cognitive models of motivation
that focus on the individual, and they also challenge
Conclusion and Future Directions the notion that practices thought to optimize moti-
We have learned much about the nature of stu- vation will operate similar in different classrooms (see
dents’ motivation and how it changes across the in particular Hickey, 2008, and Hickey & Granade,
school years. We also have learned much about how 2004). An important research implication of these
different kinds of tasks, activities, and other char- points is that we need to look carefully at how prac-
acteristics of school and classroom environments tices shown to facilitate motivation operate in differ-
impact students’ motivation, as well as how teach- ent classroom settings, to understand the breadth of
er–student and peer relations influence motivation. their impact as well as their limits.
Although the frequently observed declines in stu-
dent motivation continue to be cause for concern, How Does Motivation Vary in Different
various motivation-based intervention studies have Groups of Children?
shown that the declines can be reversed. We have Researchers studying children’s motivation have
a growing body of knowledge about how students’ long been interested in group differences, with
motivation can be enhanced in classrooms, which a particular focus on gender and ethnic differences
is a reason for optimism. We close our chapter with in motivation (see Graham & Hudley, 2005; Meece,
some issues that need research attention over the Glienke, & Askew, 2009; Murdock, 2009; and
next few years. Wigfield et al., 2006 for review). This work shows
that boys’ and girls’ competence-related beliefs and
How Do Different Classroom Contexts values tend to follow gender stereotypic patterns,
Influence Students’ Motivation? with boys having more positive beliefs and values
Perry et al. (2006) reviewed research on how in domains such as math and sports, and girls in
the interactions among students, teachers, and the reading/English and music (Eccles, 1984; Eccles
contextual features of different classrooms impact et al., 1993). More recent studies that these patterns
students’ motivation. They take a sociocultural may be changing; for instance, Jacobs et al. (2002)
approach to student motivation and learning, argu- did not find significant gender differences in value
ing that students’ participation in different class- of math, though gender differences in competence
room environments and interactions with others beliefs in math (favoring boys) and English (favor-
co-create motivation. They discuss research on some ing girls) were found, along with gender differences
of the same topics that Stipek (1996) reviewed such in English value (favoring girls) and sport value and
as how different kinds of academic tasks influence competence beliefs favoring boys.
students, how different kinds of instructional prac- Furthermore, researchers working in other coun-
tices influence students, and how teacher–student tries find somewhat different patterns in gender dif-
relations and student–student relations influence ferences in children’s competence and values (Watt,
motivation. They agree with many of Stipek’s points 2004). The changing patterns in gender differences
about instructional practices and tasks, but they as well as the different findings from studies done
note that the dynamic and complex nature of class- in different cultural context demonstrate the impor-
rooms means that the practices may have different tance of continuing to assess gender differences in
meanings in different classrooms. achievement motivation.

w i g f i eld , c a mb r i a , ecc les 


With respect to ethnic differences in motivation, interrelations, and relations to outcomes. Classroom
some research shows that African American chil- observations and interviews can provide a richer
dren have more positive competence beliefs than depiction of situated motivation. Measures of actual
do European American children, but that these choices, persistence, and effort can provide informa-
beliefs do not relate as strongly to achievement for tion about outcomes tied to motivation. Teacher
the African American children (see Graham, 1994). and parent ratings of children’s motivation have
Graham, Taylor, and Hudley have found interesting been used successfully, and they have been shown
interactions of ethnicity and gender, using a peer to relate to various achievement outcomes (e.g., see
nomination measure asking who students admire Wigfield et al., 2008 for a teacher rating measure of
in their school that they describe as a way to mea- student engagement). Having multiple informants
sure task value (Graham, Taylor, & Hudley, 1998; and multiple kinds of measures adds complexity to
Taylor & Graham, 2007). They found that during a study, but it has many benefits as well.
elementary school African American, European
American, and Hispanic American children chose What Are the Next Steps in Motivation
students who were fashionable, athletic, and high Interventions?
achievers as ones they admired; females in all groups We are encouraged by the results from the dif-
and European American boys continued to do so ferent motivation intervention studies discussed
in middle school. However, in middle school Afri- earlier. There are several important next steps for
can American and Hispanic American males nomi- research of this kind. We continue to need both
nated classmates who were fashionable and athletic quasi-experimental and randomized trial design
but who were not high achievers. This work shows intervention studies done in classrooms, to build a
why it is important to consider gender and ethnic- strong experimental support for effective interven-
ity together, as there are different patterns for boys tions. Second, we need to do these kinds of studies
and girls in different ethnic groups with respect to at different grade levels, to see how effective pro-
motivation-related beliefs. grams are with different aged students, and how they
Researchers interested in ethnic differences in need to be modified for use with students of differ-
motivation point to broader cultural and societal ent ages. We predict that it may be easier to change
issues such as the perceived opportunity structure students’ motivation when they are younger, before
for different groups (e.g., if I work hard in school, long-term patterns of failure and avoidance set in
will it lead to more economic opportunities for me for children performing poorly in school. However,
and members of my group), discrimination, and because some motivation problems emerge later we
stereotypes about the capabilities of individuals need effective interventions for middle and high
from different groups as impacting motivation and school students as well.
achievement (Aronson, 2002; Graham & Hudley,
2005; Murdock, 2009). The complex influences of What Are the Implications of Motivation
these factors on students’ motivation need further Research for School Reform?
research attention. As noted earlier, many of the principles derived
from research on motivation about how to enhance
How Should Motivation Be Studied? students’ motivation in school do not mesh well
Much of the research on children’s beliefs, values, with the current press for more assessments and
interests, and goals has relied on self-report ques- evaluation of student performance, and teacher and
tionnaires. There are numerous reasons why student principal accountability for student performance on
self-report is a good way to measure motivation; if these tests. These pressures can lead to a strong focus
one is interested in measuring individuals’ beliefs, on teaching to the tests being used and the (some-
then self-report needs to be used. However, such times) surface learning needed to do well on such
measures are subject to social desirability effects, tests, rather than a deep engagement in meaningful
and it is challenging to use them in studies of young and interesting learning activities. We do not believe
children, despite the efforts of researchers to develop that the motivational principles we have discussed
good measures for use with younger children. are antithetical to student achievement in school;
We urge researchers to include other kinds of indeed, we think that when students believe they
measures along with participant self-report mea- are competent, see that what they are learning is
sures, to get a more complete picture of students’ relevant and interesting, and have the goals of mas-
motivation-related beliefs, values, and goals, their tering material and increasing their skills they will

 m otivation in education


perform very well in school. Motivation researchers, processes. In R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), Minnesota
particularly those doing intervention work, should symposia on child psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 43–77). Hillsale,
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
work with policy makers to be sure that motivation Crandall, V. C., Katkovsky, W., & Crandall, V. J. (1965). Chil-
is included in the debates about effective education dren’s beliefs in their own control of reinforcements in
policy and how best to assess children’s learning and intellectual-academic achievement situations. Child Develop-
promote both achievement and motivation. One ment, 36, 91–109.
good example of this type of work is the National Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press.
Research Council’s (2004) book on engaging Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002). The paradox of achievement:
schools. More of this kind of work is needed. The harder you push, the worse it gets. In J. Aronson (Ed.),
Improving academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors
References on education (pp. 61–87). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Alexander, K. L., Dauber, S. L., Entwisle, D. R. (1993). First- Durik, A. M., Vida, M., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Task values and
grade classroom behavior: Its short- and long-term con- ability beliefs as predictors of high school literacy choices:
sequences for school performance. Child Development, 64, A developmental analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology,
801–803 98, 382–393.
Ames, C. (1984). Competitive, cooperative, and individual- Dweck, C. S. (1975). The role of expectations and attributions in
istic goal structures: A cognitive-motivational analysis. In the alleviation of learned helplessness. Journal of Personality
R. E. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in edu- and Social Psychology, 31, 674–685.
cation (Vol. 1). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. (1988). A social-cognitive approach
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95,
motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271. 256–273.
Anderman, E. M., Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1999). Declin- Dweck, C. S., & Master, A. (2009). Self-theories and motiva-
ing motivation after the transition to middle school: Schools tion: Students’ beliefs about intelligence. In K. R. Wentzel &
can make a difference. Journal of Research and Development in A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp.
Education, 32, 131–147. 123–140). New York: Routledge.
Aronson, J. (2002). Stereotype threat: Contending and coping Eccles, J. S. (1984). Sex differences in achievement patterns. In T.
with unnerving expectations. In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving Sonderegger (Ed.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (Vol.
academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors on educa- 32, pp. 97–132). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.
tion (pp. 279–300). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Eccles, J. S. (2005). Subjective task values and the Eccles et al.
Balfanz, R., Herzog, L., & Mac Iver, D. J. (2007). Preventing model of achievement related choices. In A. J. Elliott &
student disengagement and keeping students on the gradu- C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation
ation path in urban middle-grades schools: Early identifica- (pp. 105–121). New York: Guilford.
tion and effective intervention. Educational Psychologist, 42, Eccles, J. S., & Midgley, C. (1989). Stage/environment fit: Devel-
223–236. opmentally appropriate classrooms for early adolescents. In
Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. R. Ames & C. Ames (Eds.), Research on motivation in educa-
New York: W. H. Freeman. tion (Vol. 3, pp. 139–181). New York: Academic Press.
Berndt, T. J., & Keefe, K. (1995). Friends’ influence on ado- Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the achiever:
lescents’ adjustment to school. Child Development, 66, The structure of adolescents’ academic achievement related-
1312–1219. beliefs and self-perceptions. Personality and Social Psychology
Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1996). Interpersonal relation- Bulletin, 21, 215–225.
ships in the school environment and children’s early school Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R., & Blumenfeld, P. B.
adjustment: The role of teachers and peers. In J. Juvonen & (1993). Age and gender differences in children’s self- and
K. R. Wentzel (Eds.), Social motivation: Understanding chil- task perceptions during elementary school. Child Develop-
dren’s school adjustment (pp. 199–225). New York: Cam- ment, 64, 830–847.
bridge University Press. Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M.,
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Meece, J. L., & Midgley, C. (1983). Expectancies, values,
Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement
an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an inter- and achievement motivation (pp. 75–146). San Francisco,
vention. Child Development, 78, 246–263. CA: W. H. Freeman.
Butler, R. (1987). Task-involving and ego-involving properties Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and
of evaluation: Effects of different feedback conditions on achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189.
motivational perceptions, interest, and performance. Journal Elliot, A. J. (2005). A conceptual history of the achievement goal
of Educational Psychology, 79, 474–482. construct. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of
Carnegie Council on Adolescent Development. (1989). Turn- competence and motivation (pp. 52–72). New York: Guilford
ing points: Preparing American youth for the 21st century. Publications.
Washington, DC: Author. Elliot, A. J., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (1996). Approach and avoid-
Connell, J. P., Spencer, M. B., & Aber, J. L. (1994). Educational risk ance goals and intrinsic motivation: A mediational analysis.
and resilience in African American Youth: Context, self, and Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 461–475.
action outcomes in school. Child Development, 65, 493–506. Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal
Connell, J. P., & Wellborn, J. G. (1991). Competence, auton- framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80,
omy, and relatedness: A motivational analysis of self-system 501–509.

w i g f i eld , c a mb r i a , ecc les 


Elliott, E. S., & Dweck, C. S. (1988). Goals: An approach to Hudley, C., Graham, S., & Taylor, A. (2007). Reducing aggres-
motivation and achievement. Journal of Personality and Social sive behavior and increasing motivation in school: The evo-
Psychology,54, 5–12. lution of an intervention to strengthen school adjustment.
Felner, R. D., Seitsinger, A. M., Brand, S., Burns, A., & Bolton, N. Educational Psychologist, 42, 251–260.
(2007). Creating small learning communities: Lessons from Jacobs, J., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield,
the project on high-performing learning communities about A. (2002). Ontogeny of children’s self-beliefs: Gender and
“what works” in creating productive, developmentally enhanc- domain differences across grades one through 12. Child
ing, learning contexts. Educational Psychologist, 42, 209–221. Development, 73, 509–527.
Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Educa- Johnson, D. W., & Johnson, R. T. (2009). An educational
tional Research, 59, 117–142. psychology success story: Social interdependence theory
Fredricks, J., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Children’s competence and and cooperative learning. Educational Researcher, 38,
value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth 365–379.
trajectories in two male sex-typed domains. Developmental Juvonen, J. (2006). Sense of belonging, social bonds, and school
Psychology, 38, 519–533. functioning. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Hand-
Goodenow, C. (1993). Classroom belonging among early adoles- book of educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 655–674). Mah-
cent students: Relationships to motivation and achievement. wah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Journal of Early Adolescence, 13(1), 21–43. Juvonen, J. (2007). Reforming middle schools: Focus on con-
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). tinuity, social connectedness, and engagement. Educational
Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation from childhood Psychologist, 42, 197–208.
through late adolescence: A longitudinal study. Journal of Kindermann, T. A. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts for
Educational Psychology, 93, 3–13. individual development: The case of children’s motivation in
Graham, S. (1994). Motivation in African Americans. Review of school. Developmental Psychology, 29, 970–977.
Educational Research, 64, 55–117. Kindermann, T. A. (2007). Effects of naturally existing peer
Graham, S., & Hudley, C. (2005). Race and ethnicity in the groups on changes in academic engagement in a cohort of
study of motivation and competence. In A. Elliot & sixth graders. Child Development, 78, 1186–1203.
C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation Ladd, G.W., Herald-Brown, S. L., & Kochel, K. (2009). Peers
(pp. 392–413). New York: Guilford Press. and motivation. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.),
Graham, S., Taylor, A. Z., & Hudley, C. (1998).Exploring Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 323–348). New York:
achievement values among ethnic minority early adolescents. Routledge.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 606–620. Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. (2001). Restructuring high schools for
Guthrie, J. T., McRae, A., & Klauda, S. L. (2007). Contribu- equity and excellence: What works. New York: Teachers Col-
tions of concept-oriented reading instruction to knowledge lege Press.
about interventions for motivation in reading. Educational Linnenbrink, E. A. (2005). The dilemma of performance-
Psychologist, 42, 237–250. approach goals: The use of multiple goal contexts to promote
Harackiewicz, J. M., Durik, A. M., Barron, K. E., Linnenbrink- students’ motivation and learning. Journal of Educational Psy-
Garcia, L., & Tauer, J. M. (2008). The role of achievement chology, 97, 197–213.
goals in the development of interest: Reciprocal relations Lord, S., Eccles, J. S., & McCarthy, K. (1994). Risk and protec-
between achievement goals, interest, and performance. Jour- tive factors in the transition to junior high school. Journal of
nal of Educational Psychology, 100, 105–122. Early Adolescence, 14, 162–199.
Harter, S. (1981). A new self-report scale of intrinsic versus Maehr, M. L., & Midgley, C. (1996). Transforming school cul-
extrinsic orientation in the classroom: Motivational and tures. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
informational components. Developmental Psychology, 17, Maehr, M. L., & Zusho, A. (2009). Achievement goal theory:
300–312. Past, present, and future. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield
Harter, S., Whitesell, N. R., & Kowalski, P. (1992). Individual (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 77–104).
differences in the effects of educational transitions on young New York: Routledge.
adolescents’ perceptions of competence and motivational McCaslin, M., & Good, T. L. (1996). The informal curriculum.
orientation. American Educational Research Journal, 29, In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of educa-
809–835. tional psychology (pp 622–670). New York: Macmillan.
Hickey, D. T., (1997). Motivation and contemporary socio- Meece, J. L. (1991). The classroom context and students’ moti-
constructivist instructional perspectives. Educational Psy- vational goals. In M. Maehr & P. Pintrich (Eds.), Advances
chologist, 32, 175–193. in motivation and achievement (Vol. 7, pp. 261–286). Green-
Hickey, D. T. (2008). Sociocultural theories of motivation. In wich, CT: JAI Press,
E. M. Anderman & L. Anderman (Eds.), Psychology of class- Meece, J. L. (1994). The role of motivation in self-regulated
room learning. Farmington Hills, MI: Thomson Gale Pub- learning. In D. H. Schunk & B. J. Zimmerman (Eds.), Self-
lishers. regulation of learning and performance (pp. 25–44). Hillsdale,
Hickey, D. T., & Granade, J. B. (2004). The influence of socio- NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
cultural theory on our theories of engagement and motiva- Meece, J. Glienke, B. B., & Askew, K. (2009). Gender and moti-
tion. In D. M. McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.), Big theories vation. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of
revisited. Vol. 4: Research on sociocultural influences on motiva- motivation at school (pp. 411–431). New York: Routledge.
tion and learning (pp. 223–247). Greenwich, CT: Informa- Meece, J. L., Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (1990). Predictors of
tion Age Publishing. math anxiety and its consequences for young adolescents’
Hidi, S., & Renninger, K. A. (2006). The four phase model of course enrollment intentions and performances in math-
interest development. Educational Psychologist, 41, 111–127. ematics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 60–70.

 m otivation in education


Murdock, T. (2009). Achievement motivation in racial and K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
ethnic context. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), in school (pp. 171–196). New York: Routledge.
Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 433–461). New York: Schiefele, U. (2009). Situational and individual interest. In
Routledge. K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
National Research Council. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering in school (pp. 197–222). New York: Routledge.
high school students’ motivation to learn. Washington, DC: Schunk, D. H. (1983). Developing children’s self-efficacy and
National Academies Press. skills: The roles of social comparative information and goal
Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of setting. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 76–86.
ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance. Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2009). Self-efficacy theory. In
Psychological Review, 91, 328–346. K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook of motivation
Nicholls, J. G., Cobb, P., Wood, T., Yackel, E., & Patashnick, M. in school (pp. 35–54). New York: Routledge.
(1990). Dimensions of success in mathematics: Individual Simpkins, S. D., Davis-Kean, P. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2006). Math
and classroom differences. Journal for Research in Mathemat- and science motivation: A longitudinal examination of the
ics Education, 21, 109–122. links between choice and beliefs. Developmental Psychology,
Nicholls, J. G., Cobb, P., Yackel, E., Wood, T., & Wheatley, G. 42, 70–83.
(1990). Students’ theories of mathematics and their math- Skaalvik, E. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeating ego orien-
ematical knowledge: Multiple dimensions of assessment. In tation: Relations with task and task avoidance orientation,
G. Kulm (Ed.), Assessing higher order thinking in mathematics achievement, self-perceptions, and anxiety. Journal of Educa-
(pp. 137–154). Washington, DC: American Association for tional Psychology, 89, 71–81.
the Advancement of Science. Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, and coping.
Nolen, S. B. (2007). Young children’s motivation to read and Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
write: Development in social contexts. Cognition and Instruc- Skinner, E. A., Kindermann, T. A., Connell, J. P., & Wellborn,
tion, 25, 219–270. J. G. (2009). Engagement and disaffection as organizational
Nolen, S. B., & Ward, C. J. (2008). Sociocultural and situ- constructs in the dynamics of motivational development. In
ative approaches to studying motivation. In M. L. Maehr, K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbool of motivation
S. Karabenick & T. Urdan (Eds.), Advances in motivation at school (pp. 223–245). New York: Routledge.
and achievement. Vol. 15: Social psychological perspectives (pp. Stipek, D. J. (1996). Motivation and instruction. In R. C. Calfee
425–461). Bingley, England: Emerald Publishing Group. & D. C. Berliner (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology
O’Donnell, A. M. (2006). The role of peers and group learn- (pp. 85–113). New York: Macmillan.
ing. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of Taylor, A. Z., & Graham, S. (2007). An examination of the rela-
educational psychology (2nd ed., pp. 781–802). Mahwah, NJ: tionship between achievement values and perceptions of bar-
Erlbaum. riers among low-SES African American and Latino students.
Pallas, A. M., Entwisle, D. R., Alexander, K. L. & Stluka. M. F. Journal of Educational Psychology, 99, 52–64.
(1994). Ability-group effects: Instructional, social, or institu- Thorkildsen, T., & Nicholls, J. G. (1998). Fifth graders’ achieve-
tional? Sociology of Education, 67, 27–46. ment orientations and beliefs: Individual and classroom dif-
Perry, N. E., Turner, J. C., & Meyer, D. K. (2006). Classrooms ferences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 179–201.
as contexts for motivating learning. In P. A. Alexander & Urdan, T. C. (Ed.). (1999). The role of context: Advances in moti-
P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of educational psychology (2nd vation and achievement (Vol. 11). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
ed., pp. 327–348). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind and society: The development of higher
Pianta, R. C., Belsky, J., Vandegrift, N., Houts, R., & Morrison, mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
F. J. (2008). Classroom effects on children’s achievement tra- Watt, H. (2004). Development of adolescents’ self-perceptions,
jectories in elementary school. American Educational Reserach values, and task perceptions. Child Development, 75,
Journal, 45, 365–397. 1556–1574.
Pianta, R. C., Rimm-Kaufman, S. E., & Cox, M. J. (1999). Webb, N. M., & Palincsar, A. M. (1996). Group processes in the
Introduction: An ecological approach to kindergarten tran- classroom. In D. C. Berliner & R. C. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook
sition. In R. C. Pianta & M. J. Cox (Eds.), The transition of educational psychology (pp 841–873). New York: Macmillan.
to kindergarten (pp. 3–12). Baltimore, MD: P. H. Brookes Weiner, B. (1992). Human motivation: Metaphors, theories, and
Publishing. research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Pintrich, P. R. (2000a). An achievement goal theory perspective Wentzel, K. (2002). Are effective teachers like good parents?
on issues in motivation terminology, theory, and research. Teaching styles and student adjustment in early adolescence.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 92–104. Child Development, 73, 287–301.
Pintrich, P. R. (200b). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The Wentzel, K. R. (2009). Students’ relationships with teachers as
role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal motivational constructs. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield
of Educational Psychology, 92, 544–555. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation at school (pp. 301–322).
Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus New York: Routledge.
external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, Wentzel, K. R., & Wigfield, A. (2007). Motivational interventions
80, 1–28. that work. Special Issue,, Educational Psychologist, 42, No. 4.
Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Parker, J. G. (2006). Peer inter- Wigfield, A., & Cambria, J. M. (2010). Children’s achievement
actions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & values, goal orientations, and interest: Definitions, develop-
N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (6th ment, and relations to achievement outcomes. Developmental
ed., Vol. 3, pp. 571–645). New York: Wiley. Review, 35, 1–30.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2009). Promoting self-determined Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of compe-
school engagement: Motivation, learning, and well-being. In tence beliefs, expectancies for success, and achievement values

w i g f i eld , c a mb r i a , ecc les 


from childhood through adolescence. In A. Wigfield & A. Iran-Nejad & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Review of research in
J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation education (Vol. 23, pp. 73–118). Washington, DC: Ameri-
(pp. 91–120). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. can Educational Research Association.
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Mac Iver, D., Reuman, D., & Midgley, C. Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., &
(1991). Transitions at early adolescence: Changes in children’s Davis-Kean, P. (2006). Development of achievement moti-
domain-specific self-perceptions and general self-esteem vation. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology
across the transition to junior high school. Developmental (Vol. 3, pp. 933–1002). New York: John Wiley.
Psychology, 27, 552–565. Wigfield, A., Guthrie, J. T., Perencevich, K., Taboada, A., Klauda,
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., & Pintrich, P. R. (1996). Development S. L., McRae, S., & Barbosa, P. (2008). The role of reading
between the ages of eleven and twenty-five. In D.C. Berliner engagement in mediating the effects of instruction on read-
and R.C. Calfee (Eds.), The handbook of educational psychol- ing outcomes. Psychology in the Schools, 45, 432–445.
ogy, New York: Macmillan Publishing. Wigfield, A., Tonks, S., & Klauda, S. L. (2009). Expectancy—
Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., & Rodriguez, D. (1998). The devel- value theory. In K. R. Wentzel & A. Wigfield (Eds.), Handbook
opment of children’s motivation in school contexts. In. of motivation in school (pp. 55–76). New York: Routledge.

 m otivation in education


CHAPTER

Advances in Motivation in Exercise


27 and Physical Activity

Martin S. Hagger

Abstract
Given the considerable epidemiological evidence linking regular physical activity with good health
and reduced risk of chronic disease, exercise psychologists have adopted theories and models of
motivation to understand the antecedents and processes that give rise to health-related physical activity.
These theories are important because they provide the basis for the development and evaluation of
interventions aimed at promoting increased physical activity in a largely sedentary population. This
chapter reviews three of the leading theories that have been applied in physical activity contexts: the
theory of planned behavior, self-determination theory, and achievement goal theory. Advances in
research that have aimed to promote better understanding of the factors that underpin motivation
in physical activity and the relevant processes are also reviewed, including implementation intentions,
the increasing importance of psychological needs, and theoretical integration. In addition, the role of
methodological improvements such as the measurement of implicit motivational processes and the need
for “gold standard” designs when evaluating physical activity interventions based on these theories are
highlighted. It is concluded that future research needs to develop hybrid interventions adopting both
motivational and implemental strategies to change physical activity behavior, research should extend
knowledge of the relative contribution of implicit and explicit motivational processes on physical activity
behavior, and investigations to evaluate physical activity interventions should pay careful attention to
design and evaluation.
Key Words: exercise, planned behavior, intention, autonomous motivation, achievement goals, implicit
processes, intervention design

Introduction in industrialized nations do not engage in sufficient


There is strong epidemiological evidence linking physical activity to minimize risks from these chronic
low levels of physical activity with chronic health conditions (Bauman et al., 2009; Martinez-Gonzalez
conditions such as cardiovascular disease (Williams, et al., 2001). Such reports have catalyzed consider-
2001), obesity (Ross, Freeman, & Janssen, 2000), able investigation into the motivational variables that
diabetes (Jeon, Lokken, Hu, & van Dam, 2007), are associated with individual leisure-time physical
and cancer (Byers et al., 2002). International reports activity in order to develop population-based inter-
have highlighted the importance of regular physi- ventions to change behavior (Marteau, Dieppe, Foy,
cal activity as an important preventive behavior in Kinmonth, & Schneiderman, 2006).
managing these health risks (U.S. Department of Many behavioral approaches adopted to under-
Health and Human Services, 1996; World Health stand people’s motivation to engage in physical
Organization, 2004). However, it is clear that people activity have been based on social psychological


theories and models. The purpose of these theories that many of these theories have similar compo-
is three-fold: (1) to identify the motivational cor- nents and hypotheses, such that there is consider-
relates and antecedents of physical activity behav- able overlap in the definitions of constructs and the
ior; (2) to identify the mechanisms and processes by proposed mechanisms by which these constructs
which these correlates affect physical activity (e.g., affect physical activity behavior (Hagger, 2009).
mediation and moderation effects); and (3) to use For example, self-efficacy, a very important con-
knowledge of the antecedents and mechanisms to struct in the field of social psychology and derived
inform and design interventions aimed at chang- from Bandura’s (1977, 1995) influential social
ing behavior to promote desirable health outcomes cognitive theory, is a key component in numer-
(Baum & Posluszny, 1999; Hagger, 2009; Taylor, ous theories of motivated social behavior such as
2008). In this chapter I will review three dominant protection motivation theory (Rogers, 1975) and
social psychological approaches to understanding the theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985) and
motivation in physical activity and review recent both have been applied to physical activity (Hag-
advances in the field that have aimed to enhance ger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002b; Rhodes,
understanding and advance knowledge of how to Plotnikoff, & Courneya, 2008). Similarly, the con-
increase motivation and behavior in physical activ- struct of intention, which is a motivational con-
ity. I will first review the research on the motiva- struct reflecting the degree of effort and planning
tional theories and the contribution such research an individual is prepared to invest in pursuing
has made in identifying the key constructs that a behavior, is also a key component of numerous
influence physical activity behavior and which theories such as the theory of planned behavior,
have been most effective in explaining variance in protection motivation theory, the theories of self-
physical activity behavior. The theories are Ajzen’s regulation and trying (Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995),
(1985, 1991) theory of planned behavior, Deci and the theory of goal-directed behavior (Perugini
and Ryan’s (1985b, 2000) self-determination the- & Conner, 2000). Again these theories have been
ory, and Nicholls’ (1989) achievement goal theory. adopted to explain behavior in a physical activity
I will very briefly review the research adopting these context (Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995; Perugini &
approaches and evaluate their importance and level Conner, 2000). Similarly, these theories have dif-
of contribution to the literature on motivation and ferent assumptions and perspectives. For example,
physical activity. Most important, I will identify attitudinal theories like the theory of planned
the advances, theoretically, that researchers in the behavior are belief based and focus on behavioral
physical activity domain have made to each of these predictions based on estimates of the future out-
theories such as the use of implementation inten- comes of a given behavior and individuals’ evalu-
tion strategies, the adoption of new perspectives on ation of those outcomes. In contrast, theories
psychological needs (e.g., “need thwarting”), and such as self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan,
the introduction of a 2 x 2 achievement goal frame- 1985b, 2000) adopt an organismic approach,
work. I will also outline how theoretical integration steeped in the humanist tradition, focusing on the
may benefit theoretical research in physical activity contextual influences on motivated behavior and
contexts. Finally, I will review recent methodologi- motivational orientations derived from the satis-
cal advances in the psychology of physical activity faction of innate psychological needs. In this chap-
such as the use of implicit measures of motivation ter I will focus on three dominant motivational
and the importance of randomized controlled tri- theories applied in physical activity contexts: the
als, intervention mapping, and intervention fidelity theory of planned behavior, self-determination
to ensure that the effective components of motiva- theory, and achievement goal theory. I will outline
tional interventions to change physical activity can how the adoption of these theories has contributed
be identified and replicated precisely. to the understanding of physical activity behavior.
I will also review how these theories might help
Three Key Motivational Theories in move the field forward in terms of developing
Physical Activity a more comprehensive theory of the antecedents
The psychology of physical activity is a theory- and mechanisms of physical activity behavior and
rich discipline with many motivational theories informing interventions and practical solutions
and models proposed to provide comprehensive to increase motivation to participate in physi-
and definitive explanations of health behavior cal activity and promote engagement in physical
(Hagger, 2010a). It is, however, important to note activity behavior.

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


The Theory of Planned Behavior 1991). An indirect measure of perceived behavioral
The theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1985, control is formed from the composite of the control
1991; Fishbein & Ajzen, 2009) is a widely adopted beliefs multiplied by its perceived power (Ajzen &
social cognitive theory aimed at explaining inten- Driver, 1991).
tional behavior. It has been applied to many health- In terms of process and the operationalization
related behaviors, including physical activity (Hagger of the model, intentions are hypothesized to lead
et al., 2002b; Symons Downs & Hausenblas, 2005). directly to behavior and mediate the effects of atti-
In the theory, intention is considered a motivational tudes, subjective norms, and perceived behavioral
construct and represents the degree of planning and control on behavior. This means that intentions
effort people are willing to invest in performing any explain the effects of attitudes, subjective norms,
future planned action or behavior. Intention is con- and perceived behavioral control on behavior. Inten-
ceptualized within the theory as the most proximal tions are therefore necessary to convert these con-
influence on behavior and is a function of a set of structs into behavior. Ajzen (1985) also predicted
personal, normative, and control-related belief- direct and indirect effects for the perceived behav-
based social-cognitive constructs regarding the per- ioral control construct on behavior. The effects of
formance of the future behavior, termed attitudes, perceived behavioral control that are mediated by
subjective norms, and perceived behavioral control, intention reflect the level of perceived volitional
respectively. control an individual has over the performance of
Attitudes refer to an individual’s overall evalua- the behavior in the future, similar to self-efficacy.
tion of the behavior and are usually tapped using However, if perceived behavioral control closely
direct measures and psychometric scales (Ajzen, reflected the degree to which participation in the
2003). However, the sets of personal beliefs that the behavior was impaired by real environmental bar-
target behavior will result in outcomes (behavioral riers or impedances, the construct would serve as a
beliefs) and whether such outcomes are salient (out- “proxy” measure of actual control and directly affect
come expectations) are hypothesized to underpin behavior unmediated by intention.
the direct attitude measure (Ajzen, 2003). These The most frequently cited or “modal” beliefs
can also be measured individually for each belief that underpin the attitude, subjective norms, and
and outcome and are considered indirect measures perceived behavioral control constructs in physical
of attitude. Similarly, subjective norms are typically activity contexts have been identified. The beliefs
measured directly as a person’s overall evaluation are typically elicited from pilot research using open-
that significant others would want them to engage ended measures that are content analyzed to provide
in the target behavior. As with attitudes, subjective sufficient information to develop the salient out-
norms are sourced indirectly from sets of beliefs that comes for the behavioral belief and outcome evalua-
reflect expectations that significant others will exert tion measures, the salient referents for the normative
pressure or cajole the individual to engage in the belief and motivation to comply measures, and the
behavior (normative beliefs) and the individual’s salient barriers and control-related issues for the
propensity to comply with those significant others control beliefs and perceived power measures (Ajzen
(motivation to comply). The construct of perceived & Fishbein, 1980). Research in physical activity has
behavioral control encompasses control-related per- typically identified the following most frequently
ceptions with respect to the target behavior, includ- cited (modal) outcomes: “good companionship,”
ing actual barriers and personal evaluations of “weight control,” “benefit my overall health,” “take
limitation or capacity with respect to the behavior. too much time,” “fun,” “get fit,” “stay in shape,”
This led Ajzen to indicate that perceived behavioral “improve skills,” “get an injury,” and “makes you
control contained elements of Bandura’s (1977) hot and sweaty” (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle,
self-efficacy construct in that it captures judg- 2001). Similarly, important referents identified
ments of how well one can execute required actions include friends, colleagues, and family members
to produce important outcomes. The construct is like parents, grandparents, and siblings (Hagger
also underpinned by a set of beliefs (Ajzen, 1985). et al., 2001). The modal control beliefs identified
Control beliefs refer to the perceived presence of fac- include barriers and facilitators that underpin the
tors that may facilitate or impede performance of direct measure of perceived behavioral control: “bad
behavior, and perceived power refers to the perceived weather,” “age,” “heart pain,” “costs,” “fatigue,” and
impact that facilitative or inhibiting factors may “no time” (Godin, Valois, Jobin, & Ross, 1991).
have on performance of behavior (Ajzen & Driver, As with behavioral and normative beliefs, research

h ag g er 
shows that control beliefs demonstrate considerable most recent decision-making variables remained
variance across different populations and behav- salient as explanations of variance in physical activ-
iors. For example, studies in the physical activity ity intentions and behavior. It was concluded that
domain have identified “age” and “fear of having a “ . . . while past behavior had a significant and direct
heart attack” among the control beliefs for older and influence on intention, attitude, perceived behav-
clinical populations (Godin et al., 1991), but these ioral control, and self-efficacy, these cognitions are
beliefs do not feature among the control beliefs of also necessary for translating past decisions about
younger populations who focus more on inclem- behavioral involvement into action. This is consis-
ent weather and lack of time (Hagger et al., 2001). tent with the notion that involvement in volitional
Interestingly, the comparatively limited research behaviors such as regular physical activity involves
examining relations between the indirect belief- both conscious and automatic influences” (p. 23).
based measures and the direct measures suggests that This evidence indicates the general recognition
multiplicative composites of the belief and value of theory of planned behavior as an important theo-
systems do not account for a high degree of vari- retical approach to the understanding of the motiva-
ance in the direct measures of attitudes, subjective tional influences on physical activity behavior. The
norms, and perceived behavioral control (Hagger considerable attention paid to the theory in the lit-
et al., 2001). Few definitive solutions have been erature is attributable to its effectiveness in account-
put forward for this problem, and the role of beliefs ing for variance in physical activity intention and
and expectancy-value models within the theory of behavior as well as its relative parsimony and role
planned behavior is an area of surprisingly sparse as a flexible framework for the study of psychoso-
attention in the literature (Ajzen & Fishbein, 2008; cial influences and processes that underpin physical
Bagozzi, 1984; French & Hankins, 2003). activity behavior. For example, its role as a “flexible
Formative research adopting the theory of framework” has been supported by research that has
planned behavior in physical activity contexts has shown the attitude, subjective norm, and perceived
demonstrated that attitudes and perceived behav- behavioral control constructs mediate the effect of
ioral control consistently and significantly predict other distal constructs on intentions and behavior
intentions and explain approximately equal propor- such as personality (Bozionelos & Bennett, 1999;
tions of the variance in physical activity behavior Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2008; Conner & Abra-
with a substantially lesser role for subjective norms ham, 2001; Conner, Rodgers, & Murray, 2007;
(Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2005; Hagger et al., Hoyt, Rhodes, Hausenblas, & Giacobbi, 2009;
2002b). In addition to individual empirical studies, Rhodes & Courneya, 2003; Rhodes, Courneya, &
a meta-analysis of 72 studies applying the theory of Jones, 2002, 2003) and other individual difference
planned behavior in physical activity contexts sup- variables (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2007; Fitch &
ported the trends in the physical activity data across Ravlin, 2005; Hagger, Anderson, Kyriakaki, & Dark-
the literature (Hagger et al., 2002b). Using a meta- ings, 2007). However, researchers have also indicated
analytic path analysis, intention was found to be that the theory does not account for all of the vari-
the sole proximal predictor of physical activity and ance in intention and behavior, nor does it mediate
that the effects of attitudes and perceived behavioral the effects of certain “external variables” on inten-
control on intentions were medium in magnitude tions and behavior (e.g., Bagozzi & Kimmel, 1995;
and stronger than the effects of subjective norms. Conner & Abraham, 2001; Conner & Armitage,
In addition, studies that separated measures of self- 1998; Rhodes & Courneya, 2003; Rhodes et al.,
efficacy (reflecting personal capacity and confidence 2002). Paradoxically, this “weakness” has become
estimates) and perceived controllability (reflect- the theory’s greatest strength. Ajzen (1991) states
ing perceived barriers) indicated that self-efficacy that the theory should be viewed as a flexible frame-
explained additional variance in the prediction of work into which other variables can be incorporated
both intentions and behavior. Past behavior also provided they make a meaningful and unique con-
predicted all of the theory constructs and attenuated tribution to the prediction of intentions and there
their effects on intention and behavior. Nevertheless, is a theoretical precedence for the inclusion of such
the influences of the social cognitive constructs on variables.
intentions and behavior remained significant even As a consequence, the theory has been adopted
after controlling for previous experience. This indi- by researchers in physical activity as a general frame-
cated that previous decision-making processes were work to investigate the effect of a number of addi-
accounted for by the variables in the model, but the tional social cognitive constructs on intention and

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


behavior (Conner & Armitage, 1998). To the extent Bagozzi, 2001, 2004). In the same vein, researchers
that such constructs have a unique effect on intention have also investigated the extent to which individu-
or behavior and are not mediated by the core theory als are orientated toward or base their intentions
variables of attitude, subjective norm, and perceived on each of the core theory constructs (Sheeran,
behavioral control, the researcher has evidence to Trafimow, Finlay, & Norman, 2002; Trafimow &
support the inclusion of that construct within the Finlay, 1996). These modifications suggest that the
theory. A number of constructs have been found to antecedents of volitional behaviors, like physical
have a unique effect on intentions and/or behavior, activity, may be more complex than originally con-
including anticipated affect and attitude ambiva- ceived by the theory (Conner & Armitage, 1998).
lence (Armitage & Conner, 2000), anticipated However, it is important to note that many of these
regret (Sheeran & Orbell, 1999a), cultural norms modifications make relatively modest increases in
and ethnicity (Blanchard et al., 2008; Blanchard the predictions within the model and the separation
et al., 2009; Blanchard et al., 2003; Van Hooft & of the theory components into more specific, differ-
De Jong, 2009; Walker, Courneya, & Deng, 2006), entiated constructs does not appear to affect the pre-
descriptive norms (Sheeran & Orbell, 1999a), group diction of intentions and behavior at the global level
norms and membership (Terry, Hogg, & White, (Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2005). Notwithstanding
2000; White, Hogg, & Terry, 2002), health locus these modifications, the theory still performs rela-
of control (Armitage, 2003; Hagger & Armitage, tively well in terms of explaining physical activity
2004), moral norms (Godin, Conner, & Sheeran, behavior and in its most parsimonious form can
2005; Lam, 1999), past behavior (Aarts, Verplanken, inform successful interventions to promote physi-
& van Knippenberg, 1998; Albarracín & Wyer, cal activity (e.g., Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2005;
2000; Conner, Warren, Close, & Sparks, 1999; Hagger Darker, French, Eves, & Sniehotta, 2010).
et al., 2001), prototypes (Norman, Armitage, & Although the theory of planned behavior has
Quigley, 2007), self-identity (Hagger & Chatzisa- demonstrated considerable success in terms of pre-
rantis, 2006), and self-schemas (Sheeran & Orbell, dicting physical activity in numerous contexts and
2000a). groups, the theory and the research that has adopted
In addition to the effects of other constructs, it does have considerable documented limitations.
the influence of variations in the characteristics First, the relationship between intentions and
and nature of the core theory of planned behavior behavior is far from perfect. In fact, it frequently
constructs on intentions, and of intention itself, on falls considerably short of a large effect size and
behavior have been investigated (Sheeran, 2002). meta-analytic studies have typically indicated that
Examples include the stability of intentions (Sheeran, the relationship between intentions and behavior
Orbell, & Trafimow, 1999), the accessibility of atti- is relatively modest (Hagger et al., 2002b), per-
tudes (Doll & Ajzen, 1992; Verplanken, Hofstee, & haps medium in size, according to Cohen’s (1987)
Janssen, 1998), and hypothetical bias (Ajzen, taxonomy of effect sizes. Numerous reasons have
Brown, & Carvahal, 2004). In addition, researchers been cited for this problem such as a lack of cor-
have sought to differentiate between the indepen- respondence between the measures of intention and
dent and fundamental concepts within each of the behavior, the relative instability of intentions, and
psychosocial components that predict intentions. the moderating effect of numerous individual dif-
For example, attitudes have been differentiated into ference factors such as self-schema. These have been
cognitive or instrumental attitudes and affective frequently investigated and research has shown that
attitudes (Lowe, Eves, & Carroll, 2002; Trafimow the intention-behavior “gap” is strengthened under
& Sheeran, 1998), subjective norms have been dif- conditions of high intention stability and among
ferentiated into injunctive norms and descriptive self-schematics (Sheeran & Orbell, 2000a). How-
norms (Rivis & Sheeran, 2003), and, as mentioned ever, the relationship remains relatively modest in
previously, perceived behavioral control has been effect size, which means that people frequently do
differentiated into self-efficacy and perceived con- not convert their “good” intentions to engage in
trollability (Armitage & Conner, 1999a, 1999b; physical activity into actual behavior. Research-
Hagger et al., 2001; Povey, Conner, Sparks, James, ers have therefore sought to develop strategies that
& Shepherd, 2000; Sniehotta, Scholz, & Schwarzer, might assist in moderating the intention–behavior
2005; Terry & O’Leary, 1995). Even intentions relationship, particularly strategies that enable indi-
have been distinguished from desires, the latter viduals to convert their “good” intentions to engage
being “emotional” forms of intention (Perugini & in physical activity behavior into actual action.

h ag g er 
These strategies and advances will be reviewed in the Research adopting the perceived locus of causality
“Theoretical Advances” section of this chapter. from OIT has shown that autonomous forms of reg-
ulation are positively related to adaptive behavioral
Self-Determination Theory and psychological outcomes in the domain of physi-
Self-determination theory (Deci & Ryan, 1985b, cal activity. Autonomous motivation is associated
2000) is a prominent motivational theory adopted with physical activity participation and adherence
to identify the contextual and interpersonal influ- over time (Barbeau, Sweet, & Fortier, 2009; Chatz-
ences on human behavior and has received much isarantis, Biddle, & Meek, 1997; Chatzisarantis,
attention in the physical activity literature (Hagger Hagger, Biddle, & Karageorghis, 2002; Chatzisaran-
& Chatzisarantis, 2007a, 2008, 2007d; Ryan & tis, Hagger, Biddle, Smith, & Wang, 2003; Fortier &
Deci, 2007). Self-determination theory is actually Kowal, 2007; Pelletier, Dion, Slovinec-D’Angelo, &
a meta-theory comprising a number of subtheories Reid, 2004; Vansteenkiste, Simons, Soenens, & Lens,
that seek to explain human motivation and behavior 2004), perceived competence (Goudas, Biddle, &
on the basis of individual differences in motivational Fox, 1994), physical activity intentions (Hagger &
orientations, contextual influences on motivation, Chatzisarantis, 2007b; Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Cul-
and interpersonal perceptions. Central to self- verhouse, & Biddle, 2003; Phillips, Abraham, &
determination theory is the distinction between Bond, 2003; Standage, Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2005;
self-determined or autonomous forms of motivation Wilson & Rodgers, 2004), Csikzentmihalyi’s (1990)
relative to non-self-determined or controlling forms flow state (Fortier & Kowal, 2007), and psychologi-
of motivation. The extent to which people experi- cal well-being (Wilson & Rodgers, 2007). Further-
ence motivation to engage in activities and behav- more, environmental antecedents such as autonomy
iors as autonomous or controlling will determine support (Edmunds, Ntoumanis, & Duda, 2007) and
their persistence with the behavior in the future and people’s perceptions that the motivational context is
whether they gain certain adaptive outcomes such supportive of their autonomous motivation (Hagger,
as satisfaction, enjoyment, and psychological well- Chatzisarantis, Barkoukis, Wang, & Baranowski,
being. Organismic integration theory (OIT), a sub- 2005; Hagger et al., 2003; Hein & Koka, 2007;
theory of self-determination theory, seeks to provide Koka & Hein, 2003; Standage et al., 2005) have also
an explanation for the processes by which people been linked with autonomous motivational regula-
assimilate behaviors that are externally regulated and tions from OIT. Findings from previous research
incorporate them into their repertoire of behaviors have been supported by a recent meta-analysis of the
that are self-determined and integrated into their per- effects of perceived locus of causality on behavior and
sonal system. Central to OIT is the perceived locus outcomes in physical activity settings (Chatzisarantis
of causality, which represents a graduated continuum et al., 2003). The analysis supported the proposed
of motivational styles or regulations. The continuum, effects of the motivational regulations on physical
known as the perceived locus of causality, is charac- activity behavior and outcomes such as perceived
terized by two relatively autonomous forms of moti- competence and physical activity intentions across
vation: intrinsic motivation and identified regulation, a set of 21 studies (Chatzisarantis et al., 2003). Inter-
and two relatively controlling forms of motivation: estingly, autonomous forms of motivation medi-
external regulation and introjected regulation (Ryan ated the effect of perceived competence on physical
& Connell, 1989). Important for researchers and activity intentions, suggesting that competence per-
practitioners in the field of physical activity, indi- ceptions affect behavior because competence percep-
viduals who act for autonomous reasons are more tions tend to be self-determined in nature.
likely to persist in the absence of discernable external Another fundamental subtheory of self-
rewards or contingencies. Therefore, if interventions determination theory is Basic needs theory. Deci
can promote autonomous motives for engaging in and Ryan (2000) suggest that the origins of self-
physical activity among individuals, it is likely to determined motivation stem from individuals’ innate
lead to persistence over time and cede the health propensity to satisfy three basic psychological needs:
benefits of physical activity to those individuals. The autonomy, competence, and relatedness. These needs
major theoretical tenets of self-determination theory are perceived to be fundamental to all humans, and
have been outlined in detail elsewhere in this volume people approach behaviors in an intrinsically moti-
(see Chapter 6), so the present review will focus on vated fashion because they perceive it as being effica-
the specific application of self-determination in the cious in satisfying psychological needs. The existence
domain of health-related physical activity. of these needs has been justified empirically and

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


research has illustrated that these needs are pervasive need satisfaction as well as motivational regulations
across different cultures (Sheldon, Elliot, Kim, & (Edmunds et al., 2007). While research examining
Kasser, 2001). Basic needs theory is linked with OIT the role of psychological need satisfaction as the ori-
because it charts the origins of autonomous or self- gin of autonomous motivation in physical activity,
determined motivational regulations. The perceived this research is relatively new and there is consider-
locus of causality is proposed to reflect the degree to able scope for further investigation to answer ques-
which behaviors have become internalized or “taken tions relating to the role of needs in determining
in.” Behaviors that have the propensity to fulfill per- physical activity behavior. For example, what hap-
sonally relevant goals that are valued by individuals pens to physical activity when needs are not fulfilled
(e.g., participating in physical activity to gain more or thwarted? Such questions will be addressed in the
energy for other activities in life or to increase fitness) “Theoretical Advances” section of this chapter.
are perceived as efficacious in satisfying psychologi-
cal needs. Increased participation in such behaviors Achievement Goal Theory and the 2 x 2
will likely lead to the behavior being internalized Framework
and finally integrated into the person’s repertoire of Achievement goal theory was developed to exam-
behaviors that satisfy these needs. As a result, people ine the effects of perceptions of success and failure
may not perform physical activity for the activity on motivation in education contexts (Nicholls,
itself as in the “classic” definition of intrinsic motiva- 1989). Central to the theory is the manner in which
tion. Rather, they perform it to achieve an intrinsic people tend to view or interpret success or failure
“outcome” that is highly valued and perceived as part when engaged in competence-relevant behaviors.
of the person’s “true self.” The original conceptualization of the theory iden-
It is also important to note that the three basic tified two pervading dispositional and enduring
needs are complementary—that is, optimal func- motivational orientations: mastery oriented and
tioning and truly integrated behavior can only performance oriented. Individuals with a mastery-
result if all three psychological needs are supported. oriented or self-referenced goal orientation tend
For example, competence alone, that is, mastering to view success and failure in terms of personal
a technique or skilled action alone is not sufficient improvement, effort, self-referenced goals, and
for a behavior to be perceived to be need satisfy- learning. Analogously, people with a performance-
ing. Competence along with a perception that the oriented or other-referenced goal perspective tend to
behavior is performed out of a true sense of self, view their success and failure in terms of their per-
without external contingency, perceived or real, and formance compared to others, fulfilling normative
out of choice and volition (i.e., autonomously moti- standards, other-referenced goals, competition, and
vated) and that behavioral engagement is supported normative comparison. This classic dichotomous
by others in an autonomous fashion (i.e., related- conceptualization of achievement motivation has
ness) is necessary for an action to be fully integrated formed the basis of numerous theoretical traditions
and to support psychological needs. Research in the that have viewed achievement goals as generalized
physical activity domain has suggested that the basic orientations that affect individuals’ interpretation of
needs tend to be strongly correlated and can be sub- competence across a wide variety of contexts (Ames,
sumed by a single global factor (Hagger, Chatzisa- 1992; Dweck, 1986; Nicholls, 1984), including
rantis, & Harris, 2006; Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage physical activity (e.g., Cury et al., 1996; Treasure &
et al., 2005) and interventions that provide synergis- Roberts, 2001; Vlachopoulos & Biddle, 1997).
tic support for the needs of autonomy, competence, A relatively recent framework proposed by Elliot
and relatedness tend to result in greater behavioral and others (Elliot, 1999; Pintrich, 2000) views
engagement than support for each individual need achievement goals as more dynamic, flexible, and
alone (Deci, Eghrari, Patrick, & Leone, 1994). changeable interpersonal constructs that not only
Overall, the satisfaction of basic psychological needs vary in terms of the definition of competence in
has been shown to be related to autonomous forms achievement settings but also in their valence as
of motivation in physical activity contexts from either approach or avoidant. The integration of
the perceived locus of causality consistent with an approach-avoidance valence concurrent with
self-determination theory (Edmunds et al., 2007; the mastery-performance dichotomy has led to
Hagger et al., 2006; Standage, Gillison, & Treasure, the development of a 2 x 2 conceptualization of
2007) and interventions supporting autonomous achievement goals (Elliot & Church, 1997; Elliot
motivation were found to increase psychological & Conroy, 2005; Elliot & McGregor, 2001). The

h ag g er 
theory proposes that not only can people define Ntoumanis, 2008; Barkoukis et al., 2007; Conroy,
their competence with respect to future actions as Elliot, & Hofer, 2003; Conroy, Kaye, & Coatsworth,
self-referenced, either according to a personal or 2006). Elliot and Conroy (2005) point out that
absolute standard, or other referenced, but also in relations between the 2 x 2 achievement goal con-
terms of whether it will lead to adaptive, desirable structs and health-related physical activity have not
outcomes or maladaptive, undesirable outcomes. been fully investigated: “Although the value of the
Such evaluations are automatically paired with an expanded 2 x 2 conceptual framework [of achieve-
approach or avoidance response such that courses ment goals] in sport and physical activity domains
of action that are expected to lead to desirable out- is a relatively open empirical question, we are opti-
comes are approach valenced and actions leading to mistic of its potential for enhancing our understand-
undesired outcomes are avoidance valenced (Bargh, ing of achievement motivation in these contexts and
1997; Elliot & McGregor, 2001). As a consequence, eagerly await further investigation” (p. 21).
people will tend to perceive their competence with Recent research has provided evidence to sup-
respect to future actions in terms of both the defini- port to Elliot and Conroy’s suggestion that the 2 x 2
tion and valence dimensions. model may offer a useful framework for the under-
The 2 x 2 framework integrates the defini- standing of motivation in health-related physical
tion and competence dimensions to produce four activity contexts. Variables such as intrinsic motiva-
distinct achievement goal constructs: mastery- tion (Barkoukis et al., 2007), perceived competence
approach goals in which competence is defined (Wang et al., 2007), and self-efficacy (Cumming &
in terms of mastering skills, improving technique, Hall, 2004) have been shown to be related to
and enhancing self-referenced outcomes and is approach goals, whereas fear of failure and extrinsic
positively valenced; performance-approach goals in motivation have been shown to be related to avoid-
which competence is defined in normative terms ance goals (Barkoukis et al., 2007; Conroy & Elliot,
and relative to the performance of others and is pos- 2004). This provides an indication of the utility and
itively valenced; mastery-avoidance goals in which content of achievement goals in this context. For
competence is defined as personally referenced and example, people may perceive engaging in physical
is negatively valenced; and performance-avoidance activity as an opportunity to achieve personally rele-
goals in which competence is defined normatively vant or self-determined outcomes such as mastering
and is negatively valenced. These goal orientations an exercise technique or losing the most weight in
should be viewed as “situation-specific regulators an aerobics class. They are therefore more likely to
of achievement behavior that are energized and develop approach-valenced mastery or performance
impelled by underlying motive dispositions” (Elliot goals toward their behavioral regulation. However,
& Church, 1997, p. 228). Therefore, global goal they may also be motivated to avoid physical activ-
orientations and motivational dispositions may ity contexts if they perceive that they are unlikely
influence or give rise to these goals and the goals are to demonstrate competence and have a high likeli-
also affected by environmental and situational fac- hood of failure. For example, people may perceive
tors that define the behavioral response. that doing physical activities may reveal their lack
Research with the 2 x 2 model has illustrated that of skills or that they are not as competent as others
mastery-approach goals are most strongly related to when it comes to lifting weights or running at speed
adaptive outcomes such as need for achievement on a treadmill. Such undesirable outcomes are likely
(Elliot & Murayama, 2008), self-concept (Hein & to result in the development of avoidance-valenced
Hagger, 2007), perceived competence (Cury, Elliot, mastery or performance goals. Just as high per-
Da Fonseca, & Moller, 2006), self-determined ceived competence and fear of failure may lead to
forms of motivation (Barkoukis, Ntoumanis, & the development of approach and avoidance goals,
Nikitaras, 2007; Hein & Hagger, 2007; Wang, respectively, other variables related to competence
Biddle, & Elliot, 2007), enjoyment (Pekrun, Elliot, & may also be linked to achievement goals (Hein &
Maier, 2006; Wang et al., 2007), and behavioral Hagger, 2007).
persistence (Elliot, Cury, Fryer, & Huguet, 2006; While research in the field of achievement goals
Elliot & Murayama, 2008). Research has also has been somewhat rejuvenated with the introduc-
investigated relations between achievement goals tion of the 2 x 2 conceptual framework, questions
using the 2 x 2 framework in physical activity con- still remain, particularly for the field of health-
texts, but investigations have largely focused on related physical activity. At the forefront of this
competitive sport behavior (e.g., Adie, Duda, & future research should be the development of specific

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


inventories for the physical activity context. Con- Implementation Intention Approaches
roy et al.’s (2003) achievement goal questionnaire One of the problems with motivational interven-
for sport (AGQ-S) has been shown to be a useful tions based on theories like the theory of planned
and valid instrument in measuring constructs from behavior is that their effects on actual behavior have
the 2 x 2 framework in sport contexts, but it is not been relatively modest (Hardeman et al., 2002).
likely to be applicable to noncompetitive, health- The limited success of such interventions has been
related physical activity contexts. In addition, future attributed to the comparatively weak relationship
research in the physical activity domain should be between intentions and behavior observed in for-
directed toward establishing the links between the mative research on the theory. For example, meta-
achievement goals from the 2 x 2 framework and analyses have demonstrated that the average effect
the degree of internalization of physical activity size of the intention–behavior relationship for
behavior using the perceived locus of causality. It many health behaviors, including physical activ-
may be that the graded conceptualization of moti- ity, although significant, is comparatively weak and
vational regulations in the exercise domain may is further compromised by the inclusion of past
discriminate the different goal perspectives. There is behavior (Armitage & Conner, 2001; Hagger et al.,
also the need to examine achievement goals in rela- 2002b). Furthermore, meta-analyses of interven-
tion to constructs from other theories. For example, tions and experimental manipulations based on the
there are recognized congruences between achieve- theory of planned behavior aimed at changing inten-
ment goal and self-determination theories, and tions have corroborated these findings, demonstrat-
these have been well documented, generally (Ryan ing substantially larger effects of interventions on
& Deci, 1989) and in the domain of physical activ- intentions than behavior (Webb & Sheeran, 2006).
ity (Standage, Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2003a). How- These data present a problem for interventions
ever, there is increased need to look at the overlap based on this theory as it seems that even though
and distinctions in the context of the 2 x 2 frame- people may report that they have “good intentions”
work. This will be investigated in more detail in the to engage in physical activity, people do not always
“Integration of Theories” section of this chapter. behave in accordance with their intentions.
Solutions to this problem have been presented in
Theoretical Advances the form of implemental approaches to behavioral
While the three motivational theories have engagement. Heckhausen and Gollwitzer (1987)
informed exercise psychologists’ understanding of presented an action-phase model that identifies two
the factors that influence physical activity behavior complementary processes that lead to action: an
and also provided a useful basis for interventions intentional (motivational) phase and an implemen-
aimed at changing physical activity behavior and tal (volitional) phase. The intentional phase encom-
health-related outcomes, questions remain with passes the processes that lead to the formation of
respect to some of the limitations of the theories and intentions to engage in a behavior captured aptly in
the lack of information or research in particular areas the theory of planned behavior by the antecedents
in the physical activity context. I outlined some of of intention. However, while intentions to engage
these limitations and needs for research in the pre- in health-related behaviors may be a prerequisite
vious sections. For example, the theory of planned for behavioral engagement, they are not always suf-
behavior is limited in that the link between inten- ficient. The implemental phase outlines the pro-
tions and behavior was relatively modest; there is cess of how the identification of critical cues in the
relatively limited information on self-determination environment leads to the enactment of intentions
theory in the role that psychological need satisfac- and promotes strong links between the cue and the
tion plays on physical activity behavior; and there is planned action. Proponents of the action-phase
little research on the conceptual and empirical links model have proposed that engaging in strategies
between theories like achievement goal theory, the that highlight a critical situation or contingency in
theory of planned behavior, and self-determination which the behavior will be initiated will be effective
theory in the physical activity domain. In the next in promoting behavioral engagement. Such strate-
two sections, I will outline recent developments gies, known as implementation intentions, require
in the field of motivation in physical activity that people to propose and write down when and where
attempt to address these outstanding questions with they will enact their planned behavior (e.g., “if situ-
a view to advancing knowledge and understanding ation Y occurs, then I will perform response Z !”).
of physical activity behavior. Such exercises promote behavioral engagement by

h ag g er 
promoting increased accessibility of the critical and regular moderate-to-vigorous physical activity
cue in the environment (Aarts, Dijksterhuis, & (Luszczynska & Haynes, 2009; Prestwich et al.,
Midden, 1999) and developing a link in memory 2008; Prestwich et al., 2009; Stadler, Oettingen,
between the critical situation (Y) and the planned & Gollwitzer, 2009) and in different populations
action (Z) (Brandstätter, Lengfelder, & Gollwitzer, such as those with chronic diseases like obesity
2001). When intentions are furnished with imple- (De Vet et al., 2009) and cardiovascular disease
mentation intentions, behavioral initiation is there- (Luszczynska, 2006; Sniehotta, Scholz, Schwarzer
fore more efficient, guided by automatic processes, et al., 2005). A recent meta-analysis of interventions
and less vulnerable to lapses in memory or reliant and experimental trials adopting implementation
on conscious processing. intentions in the context of physical activity dem-
Augmenting intentions with implementation onstrated a small-to-medium effect size of imple-
intentions has shown to be effective in promoting mentation intentions on physical activity behavior
behavioral engagement in numerous health-related (Bélanger-Gravel, Godin, & Amireault, 2011).
contexts, including cancer screening (Orbell, Suffice to say that the trends in this research gener-
Hodgkins, & Sheeran, 1997; Prestwich et al., ally support the significant and positive effects for
2005; Sheeran & Orbell, 2000b), dietary behaviors implementation intention interventions on physi-
(Chapman, Armitage, & Norman, 2009; Prestwich, cal activity behavior found in studies adopting this
Ayres, & Lawton, 2008; Prestwich, Perugini, & intervention approach in other behavioral contexts.
Hurling, 2009; Scholz, Schuz, Ziegelmann, Lippke, A relatively recent advance in this field is the
& Schwarzer, 2008; van Osch et al., 2009; Verplan- adoption of hybrid intervention approaches that
ken & Faes, 1999), alcohol consumption (Murgraff, target both intention promotion using traditional
Abraham, & McDermott, 2007), and physical intervention approaches targeting the antecedents of
activity (Arbour & Martin Ginis, 2009; Chatzisa- intention from the theory of planned behavior and
rantis, Hagger, & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, 2008; De implementation using implementation intention
Vet, Oenema, Sheeran, & Brug, 2009; Luszczyn- strategies. This research has demonstrated that moti-
ska, 2006; Milne, Orbell, & Sheeran, 2002; Prest- vational interventions combined with implementa-
wich, Lawton, & Conner, 2003; Sniehotta, Scholz, tion intentions have a synergistic effect on physical
Schwarzer et al., 2005). A meta-analysis has also activity behavior (Milne et al., 2002; Prestwich et al.,
demonstrated that implementation exercises have a 2003). Such interventions have demonstrated con-
strong effect on behavioral enactment (Gollwitzer siderable promise and indicate the importance of
& Sheeran, 2006). In addition, investigations have both motivation and implementation when it comes
demonstrated that changes in behavior as a result to intervening to enhance physical activity behavior.
of forming implementation intentions are not due Hybrid interventions should therefore be advocated
to changes in intentions or other constructs from in the development of physical activity interventions
the theory of planned behavior (Orbell et al., 1997; based on motivational theories in the future.
Sheeran & Orbell, 1999b). Instead, there is evi-
dence that the effect of implementation intention Psychological Needs and Need Thwarting
manipulations is mediated by the extent to which Self-determination theory is a relatively unique
participants engage in the implementation intention approach in the melee of psychological theories
exercises and form plans to enact their intentions applied to physical activity because it is an organ-
(Scholz et al., 2008). Such mediators are impor- ismic approach that is based on three innate psy-
tant because they demonstrate the mechanisms for chological needs: autonomy, competence, and
the effects and also highlight the dependence of relatedness (Deci & Ryan, 2000). According to the
the effect on compliance with the implementation theory, it is the environmental support for these
intention manipulations (Michie, 2008). needs as well as the extent to which an individual
In the context of physical activity behavior, perceives these needs to be satisfied that gives rise
implementation intentions have been shown to be to autonomously motivated behavior. The latter
effective in producing increased physical activity “state” of motivation is clearly desirable for exercise
participation and, therefore, reducing the intention- psychologists, practitioners, and interventionists
behavior “gap.” The body of research adopting when it comes to behaviors like physical activity
these kinds of intervention is increasing has been because it means that individuals are more likely to
applied to numerous types of physical activity engage and persist with the behavior (i.e., be more
such as walking (Arbour & Martin Ginis, 2009) effective at self-regulating their physical activity) in

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


the absence of any external contingency (e.g., the likely to occur when the context fails to support
presence of a social agent to “prod” and “coerce”) needs. Recent evidence for this comes from some
or tangible reward (e.g., money). In the theory, it is research conducted on adolescent girls’ unhealthy
assumed that all individuals require these needs to weight-control behaviors (such as skipping meals,
be satisfied to function effectively in their environ- taking laxatives, and vomiting). The research dem-
ment, and research has suggested that people rec- onstrated that low psychological need satisfaction
ognize the value of these needs and that they are was associated with high levels of these behaviors
universal (Sheldon et al., 2001). As a consequence, and this was mediated by body image concerns
there has been considerable recent interest in the (Thøgersen-Ntoumani, Ntoumanis, & Nikitaras,
role of psychological need satisfaction, motivational 2010). However, perceptions that significant social
orientations toward physical activity, and actual agents such a parents supported autonomy was posi-
physical activity engagement and behavior. tively related to psychological need satisfaction. This
For example, several studies have shown global indicates that interventions that target autonomy-
psychological need satisfaction to be associated with supportive behaviors of social agents may be a useful
actual physical activity behavior (Edmunds et al., means to promote psychological need satisfaction
2007; Hagger et al., 2003; Hagger et al., 2006; Wilson, and, as a consequence, autonomous motivation to
Rodgers, Blanchard, & Gessell, 2003). However, engage in physical activity. Indeed, our meta-analysis
this association was, unsurprisingly, mediated by has demonstrated a significant relationship between
contextual-level motivational orientations, indicat- both perceived and actual autonomy support and
ing a process model. Psychological need satisfaction psychological need satisfaction (McLachlan, 2011).
therefore acts as a distal factor influencing physical It may be that thwarted psychological needs in
activity behavior by promoting autonomous forms certain contexts are not irreparable, and the provi-
of motivation toward that specific behavior. Support sion of autonomy support may be most effective
for this process model has been relatively consistent in bringing about changes in motivation for peo-
in the literature for both physical activity and other ple with low need satisfaction. However, there is
health-related behaviors. In fact, a recent meta- relatively little research adopting this approach in
analysis has demonstrated a significant indirect effect a physical activity context, and there is a clear need
of satisfaction of the three psychological needs on for further inquiry in this direction to confirm these
health-related behavior, many of which were con- hypotheses.
ducted in a physical activity context (McLachlan,
2011). This demonstrates the relatively consistent Integration of Theories
effects of psychological needs and the process model The Theory of Planned Behavior and
proposed by Deci and Ryan (1985b, 2000) in their Self-Determination Theory
exposition of the theory. Recently, researchers have sought to integrate
So what does the future hold for research into psychosocial models such as the theory of planned
psychological needs? I propose two new steps. First, behavior with other motivational theories like
there has been comparatively little attention paid to self-determination theory. This is because these
occasions where psychological needs remain unsat- approaches are deemed to provide complementary
isfied or are thwarted (Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, explanations of the processes that underlie moti-
& Thogersen-Ntoumani, 2009). It could be argued vated behavior (Hagger, 2009). This is important
that is equally important to examine the effects of with regard to the theory of planned behavior
occasions when individuals in physical activity con- because it provides information as to the origins
texts fail to have their psychological needs satisfied of the attitudes, subjective norm, and perceived
and whether that impacts on their physical activ- behavioral control constructs. Several researchers
ity participation. If a person perceives the physical have integrated these approaches in mediational
activity domain not to be a context in which his or models to illustrate the processes that lead to deci-
her needs are likely to be satisfied, this will prob- sions to engage in social behavior. For example,
ably have two effects on the person’s behavior in self-determined or autonomous motives from self-
that domain. First, it would likely lead to an avoid- determination theory have been shown to directly
ance response and desistance from physical activity predict behavioral intentions (Chatzisarantis et al.,
participation and, second, the individual may likely 2002; Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002a;
seek the satisfaction of those needs elsewhere, in Standage et al., 2003a; Wilson & Rodgers, 2004).
other behavioral domains. These effects are most However, some researchers have tested a more com-

h ag g er 
plete model in which different regulatory styles of will be likely to find information that points to the
autonomous and controlled motivation from self- importance of activity and thus form a positive atti-
determination theory predict intentions via the tude toward future participation in that physical
mediation of attitudes and perceived behavioral activity. In contrast, people who report high levels
control. This motivational sequence has been sup- of controlled forms of motivation will tend to focus
ported in a number of studies (Chatzisarantis et al., on external contingencies of the future engagement
2002; Hagger et al., 2005; Hagger et al., 2002a; in physical activity, which are likely to have little
Hagger et al., 2003; Hagger et al., 2006). to do with the valued consequences of participating
The proposition that self-determination theory in physical activity. Individuals with high levels of
(Deci & Ryan, 1985b, 2000) can augment social autonomous motivation are likely to feel more con-
cognitive theories such as the theory of planned fident in reaching their goals and engaging in sub-
behavior has been suggested previously, but it has sequent behavior to satisfy these goals because they
only recently received empirical support. Numerous quench their need for competence. Links between
authors have proposed that motivational, organis- autonomous motivation and perceived competence
mic theories such as self-determination theory could have been found in previous research (e.g., Williams,
potentially offer explanations for the origins of con- Gagne, Ryan, & Deci, 2002; Williams, McGregor,
structs in social cognitive theories. As Andersen, Zeldman, & Freedman, 2004).
Chen, and Carter (2000) state, “most informa- The second premise relates to the relative degree
tion processing [social cognitive] models are silent of generality reflected by the constructs from the
on matters central to self-determination theory” two theories. The autonomous motives from self-
(p. 272). Deci and Ryan (1985b) have suggested determination theory reflect dispositional motiva-
that social cognitive theories identify the immedi- tional orientations in a particular context and are
ate antecedents of behavior but neglect the origins therefore expected to predict behavioral engage-
of the antecedents: “Cognitive theories begin their ment across a variety of behaviors in that context.
analysis with what Kagan (1972) called a motive, In the case of physical activity this can mean formal
which is a cognitive representation of some future kinds of exercise (e.g., going to the gym, participat-
desired state. What is missing, of course, is the con- ing in an aerobics class), sport (e.g., training for a
sideration of the conditions of the organism that particular sport and competition), and informal or
makes these future states desired” (p. 228). Con- incidental physical activity (e.g., walking to work,
structs such as attitudes, perceived behavioral con- using stairs instead of the elevator). Vallerand (2000)
trol, and intentions from social cognitive theories labels this form of motivation contextual-level moti-
like the theory of planned behavior are measured vation because it reflects motivational orientations
as explicitly stated expectancies regarding future that affect all forms of behavior in a given context.
behavioral engagement. Therefore, the integration However, the constructs from the theory of planned
of these theories may offer more information as to behavior are expectations for engaging in the behav-
the mechanisms that underlie intentional behavior ior in the future, and measures of these constructs
such as physical activity. therefore specify explicitly the behavior and time
The integration of the theory of planned behav- frame of that bout of behavior. Vallerand suggested
ior and self-determination theory is based on two that contextual-level motivation affects motiva-
key premises. The first premise is based on the tional orientations at the situational level in a top-
hypothesis that the relationship between autono- down fashion (see also Guay, Mageau, & Vallerand,
mous motives from self-determination theory and 2003). Intentions in the theory of planned behavior
the constructs from the theory of planned behavior are hypothesized to be located at this level because
is a formative one. People who have high levels of they reflect expectations for engaging in a specific
autonomous motivation in a given domain are likely target behavior at a specific future point in time.
to experience their behavior in that domain as per- They are therefore conceptualized as orientations to
sonally relevant and valued in that it is concordant engage in a behavior at the situational level. In addi-
with their psychological need for self-determination tion, Vallerand also hypothesized that contextual-
(Sheldon, 2002). As a consequence, autonomously level motivation would also influence cognitions
motivated people will have a greater tendency to at the situational level. It is therefore expected that
critically examine the importance and value of the motivation at the contextual level would influence
outcomes of engaging in any future target behavior. the beliefs that underlie engagement in specific
In the case of physical activity, autonomous people bouts of a behavior in the future, which, according

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


to the theory of planned behavior, are constructs an impetus to the formation of attitudes oriented
like attitudes and perceived behavioral control. In toward servicing personally valued goals and medi-
accordance with this theory, it would be expected ate the effects of autonomous motivation on physi-
that contextual-level motives would predict the per- cal activity intentions.
formance of behavior at the situational level and its Hagger, Chatzisarantis, and Biddle (2002a)
antecedents. found that self-determined forms of motivation
There is a growing body of research that has affected intentions to engage in physical activity
supported the integration of the theory of planned behavior, but only via the mediation of attitudes
behavior and self-determination theory. The devel- and perceived behavioral control. This provided
opment of research in this area began with Chatz- support for the hypothesis that autonomous forms
isarantis, Biddle, and Meek (1997) who found of motivation bias individuals’ decision making in
that intentions based on self-determination theory favor of forming attitudes congruent with their
(autonomous intentions) were a better predictor of personal goals (attitudes) and perceptions that the
behavior than “traditional” forms of intentions. Sim- behavior will lead to competence-related outcomes
ilarly, Sheeran, Norman, and Orbell (1999) found (perceived behavioral control). This was corrobo-
that intentions based on attitudes were more likely rated in a subsequent study that furthered these
to predict behavior than intentions based on subjec- findings to actual behavior. Autonomous motives
tive norms, and they suggested that intentions based affected behavior via a motivational sequence
on attitudes reflected pursuing behaviors for person- beginning with autonomous forms of motivation
ally valued outcomes (akin to an identified regula- and ending with behavioral engagement mediated
tion) and, therefore, for more autonomous reasons by attitudes, perceived behavioral control, inten-
than intentions based on subjective norms, which tions, and effort (Chatzisarantis et al., 2002). Since
reflected more controlling aspects of motivation this initial research, the indirect effect of autono-
such as external or introjected regulations. Together mous motives from self-determination theory on
these results paved the way for more comprehensive intentions and behavior as stipulated by the pro-
studies in which the effects of self-determined forms posed motivational sequence has been corrobo-
of motivation influenced behavior. rated in several studies in the domain of physical
Following these pioneering studies, researchers activity (e.g., Hagger et al., 2005; Hagger et al.,
have been committed to comprehensive tests inte- 2003; Hagger, Chatzisarantis et al., 2009; Shen,
grating the theories adopting hypotheses from both McCaughtry, & Martin, 2007, 2008). A recent
component theories to address hypotheses relat- meta-analysis of all studies adopting these theories
ing to behavior in numerous contexts. Prominent and testing some of the components of the inte-
among these studies are those that outline a clear grated motivational sequence has provided sup-
motivational sequence in which the generalized port for the sequence (Hagger & Chatzisarantis,
motivational orientations from self-determination 2009b). The meta-analysis demonstrated across 36
theory influence constructs from the theory of studies, the majority of which were in a physical
planned behavior in a physical activity context activity context, that the effect of self-determined
(e.g., Chatzisarantis et al., 2002; Hagger et al., motivation on behavior was mediated by the the-
2002a). In such studies, the theory of planned ory of planned behavior variables. This provides
behavior acts as a conduit for the effects of auton- useful information for the process by which social
omous forms of motivation on physical activity contexts influence behavior and provides recom-
behavior. The decision-making constructs from mendations for intervention. For example, we have
the theory of planned behavior reflect the forma- shown that interventions can be designed in such
tion of plans to engage in physical activity in the a way to change perceptions at any stage of the
future and represent situational motivational orien- motivational sequence, targeting either autono-
tations toward physical activity behavior. The self- mous motives as a distal influence on intentions or
determination theory motives serve to indicate a attitudes and perceived control as a proximal influ-
source of information that influences the decision- ence. This may lead to hybrid interventions that
making process. For example, autonomous forms adopt techniques from both self-determination
of motivation from self-determination theory are theory (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2009) and the
hypothesized to influence attitudes from the theory theory of planned behavior (Chatzisarantis &
of planned behavior; an autonomous motivational Hagger, 2005) to promote increased physical activ-
disposition in a particular domain is likely to be ity participation.

h ag g er 
2 x 2 achievement goal perspectives and domain has also supported these theoretical links,
self-determination theory and it seems a mastery motivational climate pro-
Achievement goal theory was developed by motes exercise adherence and is attributable to the
researchers interested in examining the effects of context enhancing intrinsic motivation and compe-
young people’s perceptions of success and failure tence (Cury et al., 1996; Cury, Da Fonséca, Rufo,
on motivation in education contexts (Ames, 1992; Peres, & Sarrazin, 2003; Cury, Elliot, Sarrazin, Da
Nicholls, 1989). An important tenet of the theory Fonseca, & Rufo, 2002; Escarti & Gutierrez, 2001;
is that cues from the social context, known as the Goudas & Biddle, 1994; Hein & Hagger, 2007;
motivational climate, have pervasive effects on moti- Kavussanu & Roberts, 1996; Papaioannou, 2004;
vation and behavior. Two dimensions have emerged Treasure & Roberts, 2001).
from research examining the effects of motivational Recent research has sought to examine the role
climate on motivation in educational settings: of motivational climate in physical activity contexts
a task or mastery-oriented climate and an ego- or in promoting or thwarting autonomous forms of
performance-oriented climate. A mastery-oriented motivation (Ntoumanis, 2001; Standage, Duda,
motivational climate tends to promote hard work, & Ntoumanis, 2003b). Such studies adopt a lon-
effort, cooperation, and personal development gitudinal approach, similar to those examining the
among individuals acting in that climate, whereas effect of perceived autonomy support on moti-
a performance-oriented climate tends to engen- vation and intention (Hagger & Chatzisarantis,
der comparisons with others, competition, success 2007b, 2007c), and there is considerable congru-
based on ability, and reward and punishment sched- ence in the motivational sequences put forward in
ules for success and failure. Research in education these models. However, these models have tended
has suggested that a mastery-oriented climate tends to focus on participation within physical educa-
to engender adaptive motivational patterns and is tion rather than physical activity outside of school.
linked to increased psychological well-being and Importantly, these authors make explicit the links
persistence in behavior (Ames, 1995; Ntoumanis & between a mastery-oriented motivational climate
Biddle, 1999). and contexts that support psychological needs and
The concepts of motivational climate and intrin- recognize the congruences between the features of
sic motivation from self-determination theory have the social context that support autonomous forms
been viewed as providing complementary expla- of motivation from both theoretical perspectives
nations of motivation. A mastery-oriented moti- (Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage et al., 2005; Standage
vational climate, in supporting effort, personal et al., 2007).
improvement, and self-references improvement is The achievement goal perspective has also
directly compatible with autonomous motivation been adopted alongside constructs from self-
because such contexts have been shown to enhance determination theory in terms of dispositional ori-
intrinsic motivation (Butler, 1987). In contrast, entations that reflect perceptions about success and
performance-oriented climates have not been asso- failure (Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage et al., 2003b).
ciated with autonomous forms of motivation, and Until recently, research in achievement goal perspec-
they may even undermine autonomous motivation tives had identified two pervading achievement goal
given its focus on external contingencies for success. orientations: task oriented and ego oriented. A task-
Recently, Deci and Ryan (2000) have explicitly oriented motivational orientation means an individ-
linked a mastery-oriented motivational climate with ual will tend to view success and failure in physical
the development of intrinsic motivation, stating activity contexts relative to personal improvement,
that “both [theories] suggest that the use of salient effort, self-referenced goals, learning, and improve-
performance-based rewards, social comparisons, ment. Analogously, ego-oriented persons will tend
and normatively based goal standards as motiva- to view their success and failure relative to their per-
tional strategies yield manifold hidden costs [and] formance compared to others, fulfilling normative
that environments that are less evaluative and more standards, other-referenced goals, and competition
supportive of the intrinsic desire to learn provide and normative comparison. Research in physi-
the basis for enhanced achievement and well-being” cal activity contexts has suggested that individuals
(p. 260). These theoretical links have been sup- who attach high value to task-oriented goals tend
ported empirically across many achievement-related to have more adaptive motivational patterns and,
behaviors (Rawsthorne & Elliot, 1999). A bur- in particular, report high levels of intrinsic moti-
geoning body of literature in the physical activity vation in tasks (Boyd, Weinmann, & Yin, 2002;

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Brunel, 1996; Hein & Hagger, 2007; Newton that account for the nonconscious, impulsive, and
& Duda, 1999; Standage et al., 2003b; Wang & implicit influences on human behavior (Bargh &
Biddle, 2003). This is irrespective to whether they Chartrand, 1999; Greenwald et al., 2002; Hof-
also endorse an ego-oriented goal perspective, and mann, Friese, & Strack, 2009; Kehr, 2004; Nosek,
it is only when task orientation is comparatively Greenwald, & Banaji, 2007; Strack & Deutsch,
low that maladaptive motivational patterns such as 2004). Such approaches have given rise to so-called
avoiding evaluative situations and low intrinsic moti- dual route models of motivation that recognize
vation arise (Goudas et al., 1994; Goudas, Biddle, that behavior is a function of reflective, delibera-
& Underwood, 1995). It must, however, be stressed tive, volitional, and planned inferences as well as
that there is a relative dearth of research examining those that are impulsive, automatic, nonconscious,
the effects of motivational climate on autonomous and unplanned (Hofmann et al., 2009; Strack &
forms of motivation in physical activity contexts, Deutsch, 2004). Interest in these automatic and
and, most important, even fewer studies that have implicit processes has been mirrored by concomitant
examined the role of interventions to manipulate or advances in methods to measure implicit processes.
change motivational climate and its effects on self- Research adopting implicit processes alongside
determination theory variables and physical activity more traditional self-report measures of cognition
behavior (Hagger, Hein, & Chatzisarantis, 2011). has illustrated that behavior is influenced by both
These should be prioritized in future research. explicit and implicit social cognitive variables and
these effects are relatively independent (Perugini,
Measurement and Methodological Advances 2005; Spence & Townsend, 2007).
In this final section I outline two important Given the increasing attention being paid to
methodological advances that offer much promise implicit processes, recent research has endeavored
in contributing to the understanding of the motiva- to examine the role of implicit processes in self-
tional influences on physical activity behavior. The determined motivation and behavior. This is based
first focuses on the development of new measures on theoretical premises that suggest that people
of implicit, nonconscious constructs in the field have an implicit bias or propensity to approach
of social psychology and applying them alongside behaviors in an autonomous or controlling man-
the explicit measures of motivation traditionally ner. For example, Deci and Ryan (1985a) proposed
operationalized in theories and models of motiva- causality orientations theory, which introduced
tion in physical activity contexts. Such an approach the notion that people have a generalized capac-
acknowledges that motivated behaviors like physical ity to be oriented toward and interpret situations
activity are not simply a function of explicit, con- as supportive of their self-determination. There-
scious decision-making processes but are also sub- fore, people exhibit interindividual differences in
ject to more spontaneous, impulsive psychological their generalized causality orientations, which are
variables (Hagger, 2010c; Hagger, Wood, Stiff, & global and relatively enduring, developed through
Chatzisarantis, 2009, 2010; Hofmann, Friese, & experience, and affect motivation and behavior in a
Wiers, 2008). The second focuses on the impor- variety of contexts. Such orientations may moder-
tance of using cutting-edge methodological features ate the effects of situational factors that support or
when designing interventions based on motivational thwart intrinsic motivation on behavior (Hagger &
theories in physical activity research. I will argue Chatzisarantis, 2011). In addition, it has been sup-
for the consideration of intervention mapping and posed that these causality orientations may affect
reporting of intervention protocols such that there behavior independent of conscious decision making
is clear congruence between the target theoretical (Elliot, McGregor, & Thrash, 2002), in much the
constructs and the intervention components as well same way as individual difference and personality
as the need for state-of-the-art techniques to estab- constructs tend to influence behaviors independent
lish the effectiveness of the intervention, including of intentional processes (Conner & Abraham, 2001;
treatment fidelity checks. Rhodes et al., 2002). Indeed, recent evidence exam-
ining mediational models of motivation adopting
Implicit Motivation OIT and basic needs theory have indicated that
Research in social psychology over the past 10 generalized constructs such as basic need satisfac-
years has begun to shift away from models that focus tion predict exercise behavior directly independent
solely on deliberative, intentional, and explicit influ- of contextual motivational orientations and inten-
ences on behavior and sought to develop theories tions (Hagger et al., 2006). These processes therefore

h ag g er 
transcend the deliberative route by which these psy- In our studies, we used the newly developed
chological constructs lead to behavior and suggests implicit measure of motivation to predict self-
that people’s global causality orientations may affect reported physical activity behavior alongside more
behavior directly, and the process is likely to be one explicit measures of motivational orientations
with which the person is unaware and therefore from the perceived locus of causality. Scores on
implicit in nature. the IAT were such that higher scores represented
Recent research has included implicit moti- a strong link between the positive attribute and
vational constructs in the prediction of behavior self-determined motivation. Although there were
adopting a self-determination theory approach. relations between the implicit and explicit mea-
Levesque and Pelletier (2003) adopted priming sures of motivation, there was no direct effect of
techniques used in previous studies examining the implicit motivational orientations on physi-
implicit processes to activate either autonomous cal activity behavior. Rather, the explicit measures
or nonautonomous (termed heteronomous) motiva- of autonomous and controlling motivation both
tional orientations. Using this method, they found significantly predicted physical activity intentions
that priming autonomous and heteronomous and behavior. In one sample, there was a mediated
motivation influenced participants’ perceptions of effect from the implicit motivational orientation to
intrinsic motivation, choice, and competence as intentions via the mediation of explicit controlling
well as persistence with subsequent problem-solving forms motivation. This preliminary evidence sug-
tasks consistent with explicit, consciously regulated gests that the implicit measure of self-determined
motivational orientations. Similarly, Burton, Lydon, motivation may have a role in influencing physical
D’Alessandro, and Koestner (2006) used a lexi- activity intentions, but the route is subsumed by
cal decision task to measure implicit autonomous explicit forms of motivation. Nevertheless, this is
motivation and found that this measure predicted an important finding because it suggests that physi-
psychological well-being and academic performance cal activity is largely an intentional behavior under
independent of explicit measures of autonomous the volitional control of the individual and requires
motivation. Together these studies suggest that the conscious and deliberative motivational factors to
motivational influences from self-determination be enacted. However, research using implicit mea-
theory can influence behavior and other outcomes sures is in its infancy and requires further validation
implicitly and these effects are independent of work and research examining the independent pre-
explicit motivational orientations. diction of implicit autonomous motives on motiva-
Recently we have conducted a series of studies tion and physical activity. Furthermore, the present
to extend this research to a physical activity context studies focused on self-reported physical activity
and adopt recently developed measures of implicit and, therefore, likely a more considered, intentional
motivational orientations from self-determination form. It may be that implicit motivational orien-
theory (Harris, 2008; Keatley, Clarke, & Hagger, tations are more important in predicting forms of
2011). The studies required the development of an activity which are less to do with explicit, deliberate
implicit measure of motivational orientations based motivational processes. This is clearly an important
on self-determination theory using the Implicit avenue for future research, and it will provide new
Association Test (IAT) and then evaluate the extent and important information on the relative contri-
to which the implicit motives tapped by the new bution of the implicit and explicit motivational sys-
measure predicted variance in physical activity tems on physical activity behavior.
behavior. The IAT is essentially a sorting task that
requires individuals to sort items from two pairs of Randomized Controlled Trials and
contrasted categories into logical sets and in doing Intervention Mapping
so measures the strength of association between The randomized controlled trial (RCT) has often
mental constructs that are bipolar in nature. The been cited as the gold standard for the evaluation of
IAT was developed with the distinction between clinical trials of any intervention, and a consider-
“intrinsic” and “extrinsic” as categories of motiva- able body of evidence has been recently established
tion and “pleasant” or “unpleasant” as the associated examining the efficacy of theory-based psycho-
attributes. The words that represented the category logical intervention on physical activity behavior
were derived from a pilot study in which partici- (Michie & Abraham, 2008). Indeed, meta-analyses
pants were required to write down words associated have extolled the effectiveness of RCT evaluations
with intrinsic and extrinsic categories. of theory-based interventions on physical activity

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


behavior and health-related outcomes such as fit- taxonomy is to provide a more systematic descrip-
ness and weight loss in numerous contexts (e.g., tion of the components of interventions that target
Conn, Hafdahl, Cooper, Brown, & Lusk, 2009; specific constructs from motivational theories of
Harris, Kuramoto, Schulzer, & Retallack, 2009; behavior change. This is an important step forward
Jenkins, Christensen, Walker, & Dear, 2009; Wu, in terms of assisting researchers and intervention
Gao, Chen, & van Dam, 2009). There have also designers in being more explicit in identifying the
been meta-analyses focusing on intervention based specific components of interventions that are pro-
on specific theories like the theory of planned behav- posed to be making the change in behavior (Michie,
ior (Hardeman et al., 2002) and self-determination 2008). This is clearly important when it comes to
theory (McLachlan, 2011) or theory-based inter- translational research aiming to capitalize on the
vention protocols like motivational interviewing research identifying antecedents and mechanisms
(Lundahl, Kunz, Brownell, Tollefson, & Burke, from motivational theories applied to physical activ-
2010). However, many of these systematic reviews ity contexts (Hagger, 2010a; Moss-Morris & Yardley,
and meta-analyses have been hampered and limited 2008). Furthermore, there is now a specific protocol
by the low quality of many of the constituent stud- for the coding of intervention components which
ies. A key quality component that has often be cited provides a blueprint for mapping the intervention
a lacking is the sufficient detail in the reporting of components that are the likely “active ingredients”
the intervention and a lack of provision of clear of interventions (Michie & Prestwich, 2010). This
protocols to permit the replication of the interven- is not only a tool for those conducting systematic
tion and the identification of the components of the reviews and meta-analyses but also for those design-
intervention that are effective in changing behavior ing interventions to consider when it comes to pin-
(Michie & Abraham, 2008; Michie et al., 2005; pointing the components from formative research
Michie, Johnston, Francis, Hardeman, & Eccles, examining psychological correlates likely to be the
2008). These limitations have made it difficult to most viable target for intervention. Interventions
draw definitive conclusions as to the effective- aimed at changing physical activity behavior should
ness of particular interventions based on particu- therefore pay careful attention to providing clear
lar theories. For example, without sufficient detail details of the constructs that are the targets of inter-
it is difficult to establish whether the intervention ventions (based on formative research), the interven-
satisfactorily targeted the theoretical variable pro- tion components that will be adopted to give rise to
posed by the researchers running the intervention the intervention, and a clear protocol, similar to an
and resulted in changes in the dependent variable instruction manual, giving the precise details of the
(Michie & Abraham, 2008). Recent solutions to intervention so that it can be replicated.
this have arisen in the need to clearly map the inter- Two other important methodological issues must
vention components onto the theoretical constructs be considered when it comes to the design, imple-
the components are purported to change (Michie, mentation, and evaluation of theory-based physical
2008; Michie et al., 2008). A further problem is activity interventions. First, it is important that inter-
whether there is sufficient detail and checks regard- vention designers include means to evaluate the treat-
ing whether the intervention has been carried out ment fidelity of the intervention (Bellg et al., 2004).
by those administering the intervention as it is This must come in two forms. First, it is important to
outlined in the intervention protocol. This would evaluate whether the intervention has actually caused
require checks to ensure that those administering change in the specific theoretical variable or variables
the intervention were keeping to task and whether targeted by the intervention, similar to manipulation
the participants reported carrying out the interven- checks in experimental research. It is therefore essen-
tion correctly and accurately. This is known as treat- tial that the intervention not only includes the primary
ment fidelity (Bellg et al., 2004) and has only very outcome variables whether that be physical activity
recently been applied to behavioral interventions in behavior, or any target outcome variables related to
physical activity contexts (Hardeman et al., 2007). physical activity, but also measures of the psychologi-
The aforementioned intervention components cal variables related to the intervention, both before
have been termed the “active ingredients” of inter- and after the implementation of the intervention.
ventions, and this has received much recent atten- Second, it is important that interventionists include
tion in the literature. Abraham and Michie (2008) means to identify whether the intervention has been
have published a taxonomy of health-related behav- carried out according to the proscribed protocol. If
ior change intervention components. The aim of the the intervention is delivered by a clinician or a social

h ag g er 
agent, an example of a fidelity check might include psychologists and those interested in promoting
some sort of observation of a subgroup of the agents physical activity behavior in a largely sedentary
delivering the intervention and coded independently population with important knowledge of the fac-
for the specific behaviors expected of those carrying tors that influence physical activity and the pro-
out the intervention. Of course, it is important that cesses by which these factors affect physical activity:
this is compared to similar observations for the social the theory of planned behavior, self-determination
agents executing the control condition components theory, and achievement goal theory. Although
of the intervention. This will ensure that the inter- these theories have had success in explaining vari-
vention is carried out precisely and effectively in the ance in physical activity behavior and serving as the
manner outlined in the protocol. basis for interventions to change physical activity,
Finally, I mentioned previously the importance there are limitations and shortcoming in the theo-
of including measures relating to the target theory- ries and in current knowledge of the application of
related variables that the intervention components these theories to physical activity. These limitations
are purported to target as a means to establish the include the link between intentions and behavior
effectiveness of the intervention (Hagger, 2010b; and the relations between constructs in the theories.
Hagger & Chatzisarantis, 2009a). However, these I have therefore reviewed recent advances that have
components are also likely to be the salient media- aimed to address these limitations and gaps in the
tors of intervention components and will provide research such as the adoption of implementation
an important test of the mechanisms by which the intentions and theoretical integration. In addition,
intervention affects behavioral outcomes. As an I have also highlighted the importance of recent
illustration, two of our recent interventions adopt- methodological advances in implicit motivational
ing theory-based interventions have demonstrated research and the design of interventions in develop-
the importance of examining the psychological ing future research in physical activity behavior and
mediators of intervention components on behav- advancing knowledge and understanding of physi-
ior and motivational outcomes in physical activ- cal activity behavior. I think the overall message of
ity (Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2005, 2009). For this chapter, distilling the research on motivation in
example, in a school-based intervention aimed at physical activity, is that there is some high-quality
increasing physical activity behavior among school and innovative research that is not only mov-
pupils, we trained teachers to present their lessons ing motivational theory forward but has genuine
in an autonomy-supportive manner versus an application and practical relevance to intervention-
information-only intervention (Chatzisarantis & ists and policy makers to adopt in order to pro-
Hagger, 2009). As predicted, physical activity mote physical activity in populations and produce
behavior increased among the children randomly healthier lifestyles.
allocated to the intervention group, but this was
mediated perceived autonomy support, which also Future Directions
served as the manipulation check, and autono-
mous motivation and behavioral intentions. (1) Can an intervention designed to increase
Similarly, we found that the effects of a school- motivational climate as outlined by achievement
based intervention adopting the theory of planned goal theory result in changes in self-determined
behavior on physical activity intentions was medi- motivation, achievement goal orientations, and
ated by attitudes and perceived behavioral control actual physical activity behavior?
(Chatzisarantis & Hagger, 2005). These data were (2) How do hybrid interventions that use
analyses using path analyses, and the mediation motivational and implemental intervention
analyses were conducted according to the crite- components to promote physical activity affect
ria proposed by Baron and Kenny (1986). These the behavior people who are resistant to change
analyses should be considered essential for the and have low motivation versus those with high
identification the process by which the interven- motivation?
tion exerts its effects on physical activity behavior (3) What are the differential effects of implicit
and is recommended practice. and explicit motivational constructs on different
types of physical activity such as formal exercise
Conclusion (e.g., going to the gym, attending an aerobics class)
In this chapter I have reviewed three important and more “habitual” forms of physical activity
motivational theories that have provided exercise (e.g., walking to work)?

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


References a low fat diet using the theory of planned behavior. Journal of
Aarts, H., Dijksterhuis, A., & Midden, C. (1999). To plan or not Applied Social Psychology, 29, 72–90.
to plan? Goal achievement or interrupting the performance Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (1999b). The theory of planned
of mundane behaviors. European Journal of Social Psychology, behavior: Assessment of predictive validity and “perceived
29, 971–979. control.” British Journal of Social Psychology, 38, 35–54.
Aarts, H., Verplanken, B., & van Knippenberg, A. (1998). Pre- Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2000). Attitudinal ambivalence:
dicting behavior from actions in the past: Repeated decision- A test of three key hypotheses. Personality and Social Psychol-
making or a matter of habit? Journal of Applied Social ogy Bulletin, 26, 1421–1432.
Psychology, 28, 1355–1374. Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of
Abraham, C., & Michie, S. (2008). A taxonomy of behavior planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of
change techniques used in interventions. Health Psychology, Social Psychology, 40, 471–499.
27, 379–387. Bagozzi, R. P. (1984). Expectancy-value attitude models: An
Adie, J. W., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2008). Achievement analysis of critical measurement issues. International Journal
goals, competition appraisals, and the psychological and of Research in Marketing, 1, 295–310.
emotional welfare of sport participants. Journal of Sport and Bagozzi, R. P., & Kimmel, S. K. (1995). A comparison of leading
Exercise Psychology, 30, 302–322. theories for the prediction of goal directed behaviours. Brit-
Ajzen, I. (1985). From intentions to actions: A theory of ish Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 437–461.
planned behavior. In J. Kuhl & J. Beckmann (Eds.), Action- Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of
control: From cognition to behavior (pp. 11–39). Heidelberg, behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.
Germany: Springer. Bandura, A. (1995). Health promotion from the perspective of
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational social cognitive theory. Psychology and Health, 13, 623–649.
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211. Barbeau, A., Sweet, S. N., & Fortier, M. (2009). A path-analytic
Ajzen, I. (2003, April 14). Constructing a TPB questionnaire: model of self-determination theory in a physical activity con-
Conceptual and methodological considerations. Retrieved text. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral Research, 14, 103–118.
September 1, 2009, from http://www-unix.oit.umass. Bargh, J. A. (1997). The automaticity of everyday life. In
edu/~aizen R. Wyer (Ed.), Advances in social cognition (Vol. 10, pp.
Ajzen, I., Brown, T. C., & Carvahal, F. (2004). Explaining the 1–61). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
discrepancy between intentions and actions: The case of Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automa-
hypothetical bias in contingent valuation. Personality and ticity of being. American Psychologist, 54, 462–479.
Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 1108–1121. Barkoukis, V., Ntoumanis, N., & Nikitaras, N. (2007). Compar-
Ajzen, I., & Driver, B. E. (1991). Prediction of leisure partici- ing dichotomous and trichotomous approaches to achieve-
pation from behavioral, normative, and control beliefs: An ment goal theory: An example using motivational regulations
application of the theory of planned behavior. Leisure Sci- as outcome variables. British Journal of Educational Psychol-
ences, 13, 185–204. ogy, 77, 683–702.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1980). Understanding attitudes and Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator
predicting social behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice variable distinction in social psychological research: Concep-
Hall. tual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Person-
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, F. (2008). Scaling and testing multiplica- ality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
tive combinations in the expectancy-value model of attitudes. Bartholomew, K., Ntoumanis, N., & Thogersen-Ntoumani, C.
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38, 2222–2247. (2009). A review of controlling motivational strategies from
Albarracín, D., & Wyer, R. S. (2000). The cognitive impact of a self-determination theory perspective: Implications for
past behavior: Influences on beliefs, intentions, and future sports coaches. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psy-
behavioral decisions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- chology, 2, 215–233.
ogy, 79, 5–22. Baum, A., & Posluszny, D. M. (1999). Health psychology:
Ames, C. (1992). Classrooms: Goals, structures, and student Mapping biobehavioral contributions to health and illness.
motivation. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261–271. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 137–163.
Ames, C. (1995). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and Bauman, A., Bull, F., Chey, T., Craig, C. L., Ainsworth, B. E.,
motivational processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation Sallis, J. F., . . . The IPS Group. (2009). The International
in sport and exercise (pp. 161–176). Champaign, IL: Human Prevalence Study on Physical Activity: Results from 20 coun-
Kinetics. tries. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physi-
Andersen, S. M., Chen, S., & Carter, C. (2000). Fundamental cal Activity, 6, 21.
human needs: Making social cognition relevant. Psychological Bélanger-Gravel, A., Godin, G., & Amireault, S. (2011). A meta-
Inquiry, 4, 269–275. analytic review of the effect of implementation intentions on
Arbour, K. P., & Martin Ginis, K. A. (2009). A randomised physical activity. Health Psychology Review, Advance online
controlled trial of the effects of implementation intentions publication. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2011.560095
on women’s walking behaviour. Psychology and Health, 24, Bellg, A. J., Borrelli, B., Resnick, B., Hecht, J., Minicucci, D. S.,
49–65. Ory, M., . . . Treatment Fidelity Workgroup of the NIH Behavior
Armitage, C. J. (2003). The relationship between multidimen- Change Consortium. (2004). Special NIH committee report—
sional health locus of control and perceived behavioural con- Enhancing treatment fidelity in health behavior change studies:
trol: How are distal perceptions of control related to proximal Best practices and recommendations from the NIH behavior
perceptions of control? Psychology and Health, 18, 723–738. change consortium. Health Psychology, 23, 443–451.
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (1999a). Distinguishing percep- Blanchard, C. M., Kupperman, J., Sparling, P., Nehld, E.,
tions of control from self-efficacy: Predicting consumption of Rhodes, R. E., Courneya, K. S., . . . Rupph, J. C. (2008).

h ag g er 
Ethnicity and the theory of planned behavior in an exercise self-reported leisure-time physical activity participation.
context: A mediation and moderation perspective. Psychology Psychology and Health, 24, 29–48.
of Sport and Exercise, 9, 527–545. Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J. H., &
Blanchard, C. M., Kupperman, J., Sparling, P. B., Nehl, E., Karageorghis, C. (2002). The cognitive processes by which
Rhodes, R. E., Courneya, K. S., & Baker, F. (2009). Do eth- perceived locus of causality predicts participation in physical
nicity and gender matter when using the theory of planned activity. Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 685–699.
behavior to understand fruit and vegetable consumption? Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., Biddle, S. J. H., Smith, B., &
Appetite, 52, 15–20. Wang, C. K. J. (2003). A meta-analysis of perceived locus of
Blanchard, C. M., Rhodes, R. E., Nehl, E., Fisher, J., Sparling, P., & causality in exercise, sport, and physical education contexts.
Courneya, K. S. (2003). Ethnicity and the theory of planned Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 284–306.
behavior in the exercise domain. American Journal of Health Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hagger, M. S., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C.
Behavior, 27, 579–591. (2008). Effects of implementation intentions and self con-
Boyd, M. P., Weinmann, C., & Yin, Z. (2002). The relationship cordance on health behavior. Journal of Applied Biobehavioral
of physical self-perceptions and goal orientations to intrinsic Research, 13, 198–214.
motivation for exercise. Journal of Sport Behavior, 25, 1–18. Cohen, J. (1987). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sci-
Bozionelos, G., & Bennett, P. (1999). The theory of planned ences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
behaviour as predictor of exercise: The moderating influence Conn, V. S., Hafdahl, A. R., Cooper, P. S., Brown, L. M., &
of beliefs and personality variables. Journal of Health Psychol- Lusk, S. L. (2009). Meta-analysis of workplace physical
ogy, 4, 517–529. activity interventions. American Journal of Preventive Medi-
Brandstätter, V., Lengfelder, A., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2001). cine, 37, 330–339.
Implementation intentions and efficient action initiation. Conner, M., & Abraham, C. (2001). Conscientiousness and the
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 81, 946–960. theory of planned behavior: Toward a more complete model
Brunel, P. C. (1996). The relationship of task and ego orientation of the antecedents of intentions and behavior. Personality and
and intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Journal of Sport and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27, 1547–1561.
Exercise Psychology, 17 (Suppl.), S18. Conner, M., & Armitage, C. J. (1998). Extending the theory of
Burton, K. D., Lydon, J. E., D’Alessandro, D., & Koestner, R. planned behavior: A review and avenues for further research.
(2006). The differential effects of intrinsic and identified moti- Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 28, 1429–1464.
vation on well-being and performance: Prospective, experi- Conner, M., Rodgers, W., & Murray, T. (2007). Conscientiousness
mental, and implicit approaches to self-determination theory. and the intention-behavior relationship: Predicting exercise
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 750–762. behavior. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 29, 518–533.
Butler, R. (1987). Task-involving and ego-involving properties Conner, M., Warren, R., Close, S., & Sparks, P. (1999). Alcohol
of evaluation: Effects of different feedback conditions on consumption and the theory of planned behavior: An exami-
motivational perceptions, interest, and performance. Journal nation of the cognitive mediation of past behavior. Journal of
of Educational Psychology, 79, 474–482. Applied Social Psychology, 29, 1676–1704.
Byers, T., Nestle, M., McTiernan, A., Doyle, C., Currie-Williams, Conroy, D. E., & Elliot, A. J. (2004). Fear of failure and achieve-
A., Gansler, T., & Thun, M. (2002). American Cancer Soci- ment goals in sport: Addressing the issue of the chicken and
ety guidelines on nutrition and physical activity for cancer the egg. Anxiety, Stress and Coping, 17, 271–285.
prevention: Reducing the risk of cancer with healthy food Conroy, D. E., Elliot, A. J., & Hofer, S. M. (2003). A 2 x 2
choices and physical activity. CA—Cancer Journal of Clini- achievement goals questionnaire for sport: Evidence for fac-
cians, 52, 92–119. torial invariance, temporal stability, and external validity.
Chapman, J., Armitage, C. J., & Norman, P. (2009). Comparing Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 456–476.
implementation intention interventions in relation to young Conroy, D. E., Kaye, M. P., & Coatsworth, J. D. (2006). Coach-
adults’ intake of fruit and vegetables. Psychology and Health, ing climates and the destructive effects of mastery-avoidance
24, 317–332. achievement goals on situational motivation. Journal of Sport
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Biddle, S. J. H., & Meek, G. A. (1997). and Exercise Psychology, 28, 69–92.
A self-determination theory approach to the study of inten- Csikzentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal expe-
tions and the intention-behaviour relationship in children’s rience. New York.: Harper & Rowe.
physical activity. British Journal of Health Psychology, 2, Cumming, J., & Hall, C. (2004). The relationship between goal
343–360. orientation and self-efficacy for exercise. Journal of Applied
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2005). Effects of Social Psychology, 34, 747–763.
a brief intervention based on the theory of planned behav- Cury, F., Biddle, S. J. H., Famose, J. P., Goudas, M., Sarrazin, P., &
ior on leisure time physical activity participation. Journal of Durand, M. (1996). Personal and situational factors influ-
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 470–487. encing intrinsic interest of adolescent girls in school physical
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). Mindfulness education: A structural equation modeling analysis. Educa-
and the intention-behavior relationship within the theory of tional Psychologist, 16, 305–315.
planned behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Cury, F., Da Fonséca, D., Rufo, M., Peres, C., & Sarrazin, P.
33, 663–676. (2003). The trichotomous model and investment in learn-
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2008). Influences ing to prepare for a sport test: A mediational analysis. British
of personality traits and continuation intentions on physical Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 529–543.
activity participation within the theory of planned behav- Cury, F., Elliot, A. J., Da Fonseca, D., & Moller, A. C. (2006).
iour. Psychology and Health, 23, 347–367. The social-cognitive model of achievement motivation and
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2009). Effects of the 2x2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality
an intervention based on self-determination theory on and Social Psychology, 90, 666–679.

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Cury, F., Elliot, A. J., Sarrazin, P., Da Fonseca, D., & Rufo, M. Fitch, J. L., & Ravlin, E. C. (2005). Willpower and perceived
(2002). The trichotomous achievement goal model and behavioral control: Influences on the intention-behavior
intrinsic motivation: A sequential mediational analysis. Jour- relationship and postbehavior attributions. Social Behavior
nal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 473–481. and Personality, 33, 105–123.
Darker, C. D., French, D. P., Eves, F. F., & Sniehotta, F. F. Fortier, M., & Kowal, J. (2007). The flow state and physi-
(2010). An intervention to promote walking amongst the cal activity behaviour change as motivational outcomes:
general population based on an ‘extended’ theory of planned A self-determination theory perspective. In M. S. Hagger
behaviour: A waiting list randomised controlled trial. Psy- & N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and
chology and Health, 25, 71–88. self-determination theory in exercise and sport (pp. 113–125).
Deci, E. L., Eghrari, H., Patrick, B. C., & Leone, D. R. (1994). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Facilitating internalization: The self-determination theory French, D. P., & Hankins, M. (2003). The expectancy-value mud-
perspective. Journal of Personality, 62, 119–142. dle in the theory of planned behaviour—and some proposed
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985a). The general causality ori- solutions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 37–55.
entations scale: Self-determination in personality. Journal of Godin, G., Conner, M., & Sheeran, P. (2005). Bridging the
Research in Personality, 19, 109–134. intention-behaviour ‘gap’: The role of moral norm. British
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985b). Intrinsic motivation and self- Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 497–512.
determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Press. Godin, G., Valois, R., Jobin, J., & Ross, A. (1991). Prediction of
Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of intention to exercise of individuals who have suffered from
goal pursuits: Human needs and the self-determination of coronary heart disease. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47,
behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 227–268. 762–772.
De Vet, E., Oenema, A., Sheeran, P., & Brug, J. (2009). Should Gollwitzer, P. M., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Implementation inten-
implementation intentions interventions be implemented tions and goal achievement: A meta-analysis of effects and
in obesity prevention: The impact of if-then plans on daily processes. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 38,
physical activity in Dutch adults. International Journal of 69–119.
Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 6, 11. Goudas, M., & Biddle, S. J. H. (1994). Perceived motivational
Doll, J., & Ajzen, I. (1992). Accessibility and stability of predic- climate and intrinsic motivation in school physical educa-
tors in the theory of planned behavior. Journal of Personality tion classes. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 9,
and Social Psychology, 63, 754–765. 241–250.
Dweck, C. S. (1986). Motivational processes affecting learning. Goudas, M., Biddle, S. J. H., & Fox, K. R. (1994). Perceived
American Psychologist, 41, 1040–1048. locus of causality, goal orientations, and perceived compe-
Edmunds, J. K., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. L. (2007). Per- tence in school physical education classes. British Journal of
ceived autonomy support and psychological need satisfac- Educational Psychology, 64, 453–563.
tion in exercise. In M. S. Hagger & N. L. D. Chatzisarantis Goudas, M., Biddle, S. J. H., & Underwood, M. (1995).
(Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in exercise A prospective study of the relationships between motiva-
and sport (pp. 35–51). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. tional orientations and perceived competence with intrinsic
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and motivation and achievement in a teacher education course.
achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189. Educational Psychology Review, 5, 89–96.
Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. (1997). A hierarchical model of Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D.,
approach and avoidance achievement motivation. Journal of Nosek, B. A., & Mellott, D. S. (2002). A unified theory of
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 218–232. implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept.
Elliot, A. J., & Conroy, D. E. (2005). Beyond the dichotomous Psychological Review, 109, 3–25.
model of achievement goals in sport and exercise psychology. Guay, F., Mageau, G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003). On the
Sport and Exercise Psychology Review, 1, 17–25. hierarchical structure of self-determined motivation: A test
Elliot, A. J., Cury, F., Fryer, J. W., & Huguet, P. (2006). Achieve- of top-down, bottom-up, reciprocal, and horizontal effects.
ment goals, self-handicapping, and performance attainment: Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 992–1004.
A mediational analysis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychol- Hagger, M. S. (2009). Theoretical integration in health psychol-
ogy, 28, 344–361. ogy: Unifying ideas and complementary explanations. British
Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 189–194.
goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Hagger, M. S. (2010a). Current issues and new directions in
80, 501–519. psychology and health: Physical activity research showcasing
Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H. A., & Thrash, T. M. (2002). The need theory into practice. Psychology and Health, 25, 1–5.
for competence. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Hand- Hagger, M. S. (2010b). Health Psychology Review: Advancing
book of self-determination research (pp. 361–387). Rochester, theory and research in health psychology and behavioural
NY: University of Rochester Press. medicine. Health Psychology Review, 4, 1–5.
Elliot, A. J., & Murayama, K. (2008). On the measurement of Hagger, M. S. (2010c). Self-regulation: An important construct
achievement goals: Critique, illustration, and application. in health psychology research and practice. Health Psychology
Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 613–628. Review, 4, 57–65.
Escarti, A., & Gutierrez, M. (2001). Influence of motivational Hagger, M. S., Anderson, M., Kyriakaki, M., & Darkings, S.
climate in physical education on the intention to practice (2007). Aspects of identity and their influence on intentional
physical activity or sport. European Journal of Sport Science, behaviour: Comparing effects for three health behaviours.
1, 1–12. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 355–367.
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (2009). Predicting and changing behavior: Hagger, M. S., & Armitage, C. (2004). The influence of per-
The reasoned action approach. New York: Psychology Press. ceived loci of control and causality in the theory of planned

h ag g er 
behavior in a leisure-time exercise context. Journal of Applied A trans-contextual model. Journal of Educational Psychology,
Biobehavioral Research, 9, 45–64. 95, 784–795.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2001). The Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Harris, J. (2006).
influence of self-efficacy and past behaviour on the physical From psychological need satisfaction to intentional behavior:
activity intentions of young people. Journal of Sports Sciences, Testing a motivational sequence in two behavioral contexts.
19, 711–725. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 131–138.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2005). First- Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Hein, V., Pihu, M.,
and higher-order models of attitudes, normative influence, Soós, I., Karsai, I., . . . Leemans, S. (2009). Teacher, peer, and
and perceived behavioural control in the theory of planned parent autonomy support in physical education and leisure-
behaviour. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 513–535. time physical activity: A trans-contextual model of motiva-
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2006). Self-identity tion in four cultures. Psychology and Health, 24, 689–711.
and the theory of planned behaviour: Between-and within- Hagger, M. S., Hein, V., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2011).
participants analyses. British Journal of Social Psychology, 45, Achievement goals, physical self-concept and social physique
731–757. anxiety in a physical activity context. Journal of Applied Social
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2007a). Advances Psychology, 41, 1299–1339.
in self-determination theory research in sport and exercise. Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 597–599. (2009). The strength model of self-regulation failure and
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2007b). Self- health-related behavior. Health Psychology Review, 3, 208–
determination theory and the theory of planned behavior: 238.
An integrative approach toward a more complete model of Hagger, M. S., Wood, C., Stiff, C., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D.
motivation. In L. V. Brown (Ed.), Psychology of motivation (2010). Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control:
(pp. 83–98). Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science. A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 495–525.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2007c). The Hardeman, W., Johnston, M., Johnston, D. W., Bonetti, D.,
trans-contextual model of motivation. In M. S. Hagger & Wareham, N. J., & Kinmonth, A. L. (2002). Application of
N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and self- the theory of planned behaviour change interventions: A sys-
determination in exercise and sport (pp. 53–70). Champaign, tematic review. Psychology and Health, 17, 123–158.
IL: Human Kinetics. Hardeman, W., Michie, S., Fanshawe, T., Prevost, T., Mclough-
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (Eds.). (2007d). lin, K., & Kinmonth, A. L. (2007). Fidelity of delivery of
Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in exercise and a physical activity intervention: Predictors and consequences
sport. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Psychology and Health, 23, 11–24.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2008). Self- Harris, J. (2008). Psychological need satisfaction and the prediction of
determination theory and the psychology of exercise. Inter- behaviour. PhD thesis, University of Essex, Colchester, UK.
national Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 79–103. Harris, K. C., Kuramoto, L. K., Schulzer, M., & Retallack, J. E.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2009a). Assump- (2009). Effect of school-based physical activity interventions
tions in research in sport and exercise psychology. Psychology on body mass index in children: A meta-analysis. Canadian
of Sport and Exercise, 10, 511–519. Medical Association Journal, 180, 719–726.
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2009b). Integrat- Heckhausen, H., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (1987). Thought contents
ing the theory of planned behaviour and self-determination and cognitive functioning in motivational and volitional
theory in health behaviour: A meta-analysis. British Journal states of mind. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 101–120.
of Health Psychology, 14, 275–302. Hein, V., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). Global self-esteem, goal
Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. D. (2011). Causality achievement orientations and self-determined behavioural
orientations moderate the undermining effect of rewards on regulations in physical education setting. Journal of Sports
intrinsic motivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychol- Sciences, 25, 149–259.
ogy, 47, 485–489 Hein, V., & Koka, A. (2007). Perceived feedback and motivation
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Barkoukis, V., in physical education and physical activity. In M. S. Hagger &
Wang, C. K. J., & Baranowski, J. (2005). Perceived auton- N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and self-
omy support in physical education and leisure-time physical determination in exercise and sport (pp. 127–140). Cham-
activity: A cross-cultural evaluation of the trans-contextual paign, IL: Human Kinetics.
model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 376–390. Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Strack, F. (2009). Impulse and self-
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. control from a dual-systems perspective. Perspectives on Psy-
(2002a). The influence of autonomous and controlling chological Science, 4, 162–176.
motives on physical activity intentions within the theory of Hofmann, W., Friese, M., & Wiers, R. W. (2008). Impulsive
planned behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, versus reflective influences on health behavior: A theoretical
283–297. framework and empirical review. Health Psychology Review,
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. 2, 111–137.
(2002b). A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned Hoyt, A. L., Rhodes, R. E., Hausenblas, H. A., & Giacobbi, P. R., Jr.
action and planned behavior in physical activity: Predictive (2009). Integrating five-factor model facet-level traits with
validity and the contribution of additional variables. Journal the theory of planned behavior and exercise. Psychology of
of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 3–32. Sport and Exercise, 10, 565–572.
Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Culverhouse, T., & Jenkins, A., Christensen, H., Walker, J. G., & Dear, K. (2009).
Biddle, S. J. H. (2003). The processes by which per- The effectiveness of distance interventions for increasing
ceived autonomy support in physical education promotes physical activity: A review. American Journal of Health Pro-
leisure-time physical activity intentions and behavior: motion, 24, 102–117.

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Jeon, C. Y., Lokken, R. P., Hu, F. B., & van Dam, R. M. (2007). Michie, S., Johnston, M., Abraham, C., Lawton, R., Parker, D., &
Physical activity of moderate intensity and risk of type 2 dia- Walker, A. (2005). Making psychological theory useful
betes. Diabetes Care, 30, 744–752. for implementing evidence based practice: A consensus
Kagan, J. (1972). Motives and development. Journal of Personal- approach. Quality and Safety in Health Care, 14, 26–33.
ity and Social Psychology, 22, 51–66. Michie, S., Johnston, M., Francis, J., Hardeman, W., & Eccles, M.
Kavussanu, M., & Roberts, G. C. (1996). Motivation in physical (2008). From theory to intervention: Mapping theoretically
activity contexts: The relationship of perceived motivational derived behavioural determinants to behaviour change tech-
climate to intrinsic motivation and self-efficacy. Journal of niques. Applied Psychology-an International Review-Psychologie
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 18, 264–280. Appliquee-Revue Internationale, 57, 660–680.
Keatley, D., Clarke, D., & Hagger, M. S. (2011). Assessing the Michie, S., & Prestwich, A. (2010). Are interventions theory-
predictive validity of implicit and explicit measures of moti- based? Development of a theory coding scheme. Health Psy-
vation on condom use, physical activity, and healthy eating. chology, 29, 1–8.
Psychology and Health. Advance online publication. doi: Milne, S. E., Orbell, S., & Sheeran, P. (2002). Combining moti-
10.1080/08870446.2011.605451. vational and volitional interventions to promote exercise par-
Kehr, H. M. (2004). Implicit/explicit motive discrepancies and ticipation: Protection motivation theory and implementation
volitional depletion among managers. Personality and Social intentions. British Journal of Health Psychology, 7, 163–184.
Psychology Bulletin, 30, 315–327. Moss-Morris, R., & Yardley, L. (2008). Current issues and new
Koka, A., & Hein, V. (2003). Perceptions of teacher’s feedback and directions in Psychology and Health: Contributions to trans-
learning environment as components of motivation in physi- lational research. Psychology and Health, 23, 1–4.
cal education. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 333–346. Murgraff, V., Abraham, C., & McDermott, M. (2007). Reduc-
Lam, S. (1999). Predicting intentions to conserve water from the ing Friday alcohol consumption among moderate, women
theory of planned behavior, perceived moral obligation, and drinkers: Evaluation of a brief evidence-based intervention.
perceived water right. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, Alcohol and Alcoholism, 42, 37–41.
1058–1071. Newton, M. L., & Duda, J. L. (1999). The interaction of motiva-
Levesque, C., & Pelletier, L. G. (2003). On the investigation of tional climate, dispositional goal orientations, and perceived
primed and chronic autonomous and heteronomous motiva- ability in predicting indices of motivation. International Jour-
tional orientations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, nal of Sport Psychology, 30, 63–82.
29, 1570–1584. Nicholls, J. G. (1984). Achievement motivation: Conceptions of
Lowe, R., Eves, F., & Carroll, D. (2002). The influence of affec- ability, subjective experience, task choice, and performance.
tive and instrumental beliefs on exercise intentions and Psychological Review, 91, 328–346.
behavior: A longitudinal analysis. Journal of Applied Social Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic edu-
Psychology, 32, 1241–1252. cation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Lundahl, B. W., Kunz, C., Brownell, C., Tollefson, D., & Burke, Norman, P., Armitage, C. J., & Quigley, C. (2007). The the-
B. L. (2010). A meta-analysis of motivational interviewing: ory of planned behavior and binge drinking: Assessing the
Twenty-five years of empirical studies. Research on Social impact of binge drinker prototypes. Addictive Behaviors, 32,
Work Practice, 20, 137–160. 1753–1768.
Luszczynska, A. (2006). An implementation intentions inter- Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The
vention, the use of a planning strategy, and physical activity implicit association test at age 7: A methodological and con-
after myocardial infarction. Social Science and Medicine, 62, ceptual review. In J. A. Bargh (Ed.), Automatic processes in
900–908. social thinking and behavior (pp. 265–292). New York: Psy-
Luszczynska, A., & Haynes, C. (2009). Changing nutrition, chology Press.
physical activity and body weight among student nurses and Ntoumanis, N. (2001). A self-determination approach to the
midwives: Effects of a planning intervention and self-efficacy understanding of motivation in physical education. British
beliefs. Journal of Health Psychology, 14, 1075–1084. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 225–242.
Marteau, T. M., Dieppe, P., Foy, R., Kinmonth, A-L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A prospective study of participa-
Schneiderman, N. (2006). Behavioural medicine: Changing tion in optional school physical education based on self-
our behaviour. British Medical Journal, 332, 437–438. determination theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97,
Martinez-Gonzalez, M. A., Varo, J. J., Santos, J. L., De Irala, J., 444–453.
Gibney, M., Kearney, J., & Martinez, J. A. (2001). Preva- Ntoumanis, N., & Biddle, S. J. H. (1999). A review of motiva-
lence of physical activity during leisure time in the Euro- tional climate in physical activity. Journal of Sports Sciences,
pean Union. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 33, 17, 543–665.
1142–1146. Orbell, S., Hodgkins, S., & Sheeran, P. (1997). Implementation
McLachlan, S. (2011). The role of autonomy support and integration intentions and the theory of planned behavior. Personality
in predicting and changing behaviour: theoretical and practical and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 945–954.
perspectives on self-determination theory. PhD thesis, School of Papaioannou, A. (2004). A multi-level approach to motivational
Psychology, University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK. climate in physical education and sport settings: An indi-
Michie, S. (2008). What works and how? Designing more effec- vidual or group level construct? Journal of Sport and Exercise
tive interventions needs answers to both questions. Addic- Psychology, 26, 90–118.
tion, 103, 886–887. Pekrun, R., Elliot, A. J., & Maier, M. A. (2006). Achievement
Michie, S., & Abraham, C. (2008). Advancing the science of goals and discrete achievement emotions: A theoretical
behaviour change: A plea for scientific reporting. Addiction, model and prospective test. Journal of Educational Psychology,
103, 1409–1410. 98, 583–597.

h ag g er 
Pelletier, L. G., Dion, S. C., Slovinec-D’Angelo, M., & Reid, R. Rogers, R. W. (1975). A protection motivation theory of fear appeals
(2004). Why do you regulate what you eat? Relationships and attitude change. Journal of Psychology, 91, 93–114.
between forms of regulation, eating behaviors, sustained Ross, R., Freeman, J. A., & Janssen, I. (2000). Exercise alone
dietary behavior change, and psychological adjustment. is an effective strategy for reducing obesity and related
Motivation and Emotion, 28, 245–277. comorbidities. Exercise and Sport Science Review, 28,
Perugini, M. (2005). Predictive models of implicit and explicit 165–170.
attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 29–45 Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of cau-
Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2001). The role of desires and sality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in
anticipated emotions in goal-directed behaviours: Broaden- two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
ing and deepening the theory of planned behavior. British 749–761.
Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 79–98. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1989). Bridging the research tradi-
Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2004). The distinction between tions of task/ego involvement and intrinsic/extrinsic moti-
desires and intentions. European Journal of Social Psychology, vation: Comment on Butler (1987). Journal of Educational
34, 69–84. Psychology, 81, 265–268.
Perugini, M., & Conner, M. (2000). Predicting and understanding Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2007). Active human nature:
behavioral volitions: The interplay between goals and behav- Self-determination theory and the promotion and main-
iors. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 705–731. tenance of sport, exercise, and health. In M. S. Hagger &
Phillips, P., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2003). Personality, cog- N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and self-
nition, and university students’ examination performance. determination in exercise and sport (pp. 1–20). Champaign,
European Journal of Personality, 17, 435–448. IL: Human Kinetics.
Pintrich, P. (2000). An achievement goal theory perspective on Scholz, U., Schuz, B., Ziegelmann, J. R., Lippke, S., & Schwarzer,
issues in motivation terminology, theory, and research. Con- R. (2008). Beyond behavioural intentions: Planning medi-
temporary Educational Psychology, 25, 92–104. ates between intentions and physical activity. British Journal
Povey, R., Conner, M., Sparks, P., James, R., & Shepherd, R. of Health Psychology, 13, 479–494.
(2000). Application of the theory of planned behaviour to Sheeran, P. (2002). Intention-behavior relations: A conceptual and
two dietary behaviours: Roles of perceived control and self- empirical review. In W. Stroebe & M. Hewstone (Eds.), Euro-
efficacy. British Journal of Health Psychology, 5, 121–139. pean review of social psychology (pp. 1–36). London: Wiley.
Prestwich, A., Ayres, K., & Lawton, R. (2008). Crossing two types Sheeran, P., Norman, P., & Orbell, S. (1999). Evidence that
of implementation intentions with a protection motivation intentions based on attitudes better predict behaviour than
intervention for the reduction of saturated fat intake: A ran- intentions based on subjective norms. European Journal of
domized trial. Social Science and Medicine, 67, 1550–1558. Social Psychology, 29, 403–406.
Prestwich, A., Conner, M., Lawton, R., Bailey, W., Litman, J., & Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999a). Augmenting the theory of
Molyneaux, V. (2005). Individual and collaborative imple- planned behavior: Roles for anticipated regret and descriptive
mentation intentions and the promotion of breast self- norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 29, 2107–2142.
examination. Psychology and Health, 20, 743–760. Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (1999b). Implementation intentions
Prestwich, A., Lawton, R., & Conner, M. (2003). The use of imple- and repeated behaviour: Augmenting the predictive valid-
mentation intentions and the decision balance sheet in promot- ity of the theory of planned behaviour. European Journal of
ing exercise behaviour. Psychology and Health, 10, 707–721. Social Psychology, 29, 349–369.
Prestwich, A., Perugini, M., & Hurling, R. (2009). Can the Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000a). Self schemas and the theory
effects of implementation intentions on exercise be enhanced of planned behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology,
using text messages? Psychology and Health, 24, 677–687. 30, 533–550.
Rawsthorne, L. J., & Elliot, A. J. (1999). Achievement goals and Sheeran, P., & Orbell, S. (2000b). Using implementation inten-
intrinsic motivation: A meta-analytic review. Personality and tions to increase attendance for cervical cancer screening.
Social Psychology Review, 3, 326–344. Health Psychology, 19, 283–289.
Rhodes, R. E., & Courneya, K. S. (2003). Relationships between Sheeran, P., Orbell, S., & Trafimow, D. (1999). Does the tem-
personality, an extended theory of planned behaviour model poral stability of behavioral intentions moderate intention-
and exercise behaviour. British Journal of Health Psychology, behavior and past behavior-future behavior relations?
8, 19–36. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 721–730.
Rhodes, R. E., Courneya, K. S., & Jones, L. W. (2002). Personal- Sheeran, P., Trafimow, D., Finlay, K. A., & Norman, P. (2002).
ity, the theory of planned behavior, and exercise: A unique Evidence that the type of person affects the strength of the
role for extroversion’s activity facet. Journal of Applied Social perceived behavioural control-intention relationship. British
Psychology, 32, 1721–1736. Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 253–270.
Rhodes, R. E., Courneya, K. S., & Jones, L. W. (2003). Translat- Sheldon, K. M. (2002). The self-concordance model of health
ing exercise intentions into behavior: Personality and social goal striving: When personal goals correctly represent the
cognitive correlates. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 449–460. person. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
Rhodes, R. E., Plotnikoff, R., & Courneya, K. (2008). Predicting self-determination research (pp. 65–86). Rochester, NY: Uni-
the physical activity intention-behavior profiles of adopters versity of Rochester Press.
and maintainers using three social cognition models. Annals Sheldon, K. M., Elliot, A. J., Kim, Y., & Kasser, T. (2001). What
of Behavioral Medicine, 36, 244–252. is satisfying about satisfying events? Testing 10 candidate
Rivis, A., & Sheeran, P. (2003). Descriptive norms as an addi- psychological needs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
tional predictor in the theory of planned behaviour: A meta- ogy, 80, 325–339.
analysis. Current Psychology, 22, 218–233. Shen, B., McCaughtry, N., & Martin, J. (2007). The influence
of self-determination in physical education on leisure-time

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


physical activity behavior Research Quarterly for Exercise and Trafimow, D., & Finlay, K. A. (1996). The importance of sub-
Sport, 78, 328–338. jective norms for a minority of people: Between-subjects
Shen, B., McCaughtry, N., & Martin, J. (2008). Urban ado- and within-subjects effects. Personality and Social Psychology
lescents’ exercise intentions and behaviors: An exploratory Bulletin, 22, 820–828.
study of a trans-contextual model. Contemporary Educational Trafimow, D., & Sheeran, P. (1998). Some tests of the distinction
Psychology, 33, 841–858. between cognitive and affective beliefs. Journal of Experimen-
Sniehotta, F. F., Scholz, U., & Schwarzer, R. (2005). Bridging tal Social Psychology, 34, 378–397.
the intention—behaviour gap: Planning, self-efficacy, and Treasure, D. C., & Roberts, G. C. (2001). Students’ perceptions
action control in the adoption and maintenance of physical of the motivational climate, achievement beliefs and satisfac-
exercise. Psychology and Health, 20, 143–160. tion in physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Sniehotta, F. F., Scholz, U., Schwarzer, R., Fuhrmann, B., Kiwus, Sport, 72, 165–175.
U., & Voller, H. (2005). Long-term effects of two psycho- U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (1996).
logical interventions on physical exercise and self-regulation Physical activity and health: A report of the surgeon general.
following coronary rehabilitation. International Journal of Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,
Behavioral Medicine, 12, 244–255. National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health
Spence, A., & Townsend, E. (2007). Predicting behaviour towards Promotion.
genetically modified (GM) food using implicit and explicit Vallerand, R. J. (2000). Deci and Ryan’s self-determination
attitudes. British Journal of Social Psychology, 46, 437–457. theory: A view from the hierarchical model of intrinsic and
Stadler, G., Oettingen, G., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2009). Physical extrinsic motivation. Psychological Inquiry, 11, 312–318.
activity in women effects of a self-regulation intervention. Van Hooft, E. A. J., & De Jong, M. (2009). Predicting job seek-
American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 36, 29–34. ing for temporary employment using the theory of planned
Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2003a). A model behaviour: The moderating role of individualism and collec-
of contextual motivation in physical education: Using con- tivism. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychol-
structs from self-determination and achievement goal theories ogy, 82, 295–316.
to predict physical activity intentions. Journal of Educational van Osch, L., Beenackers, M., Reubsaet, A., Lechner, L., Can-
Psychology, 95, 97–110. del, M., & de Vries, H. (2009). Action planning as predictor
Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2003b). Pre- of health protective and health risk behavior: An investiga-
dicting motivational regulations in physical education: The tion of fruit and snack consumption. International Journal of
interplay between dispositional goal orientations, motiva- Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 6, 69.
tional climate and perceived competence. Journal of Sports Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Soenens, B., & Lens, W. (2004).
Sciences, 21, 631–647. How to become a persevering exerciser? Providing a clear,
Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A test of future intrinsic goal in an autonomy-supportive way. Journal
self-determination theory in school physical education. Brit- of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26, 232–249.
ish Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 411–433. Verplanken, B., & Faes, S. (1999). Good intentions, bad hab-
Standage, M., Gillison, F. B., & Treasure, D. C. (2007). Self- its, and effects of forming implementation intentions on
determination and motivation in physical education. In M. healthy eating. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29,
S. Hagger & N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motiva- 591–604.
tion and self-determination in exercise and sport (pp. 71–85). Verplanken, B., Hofstee, G., & Janssen, H. (1998). Accessibility
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. of affective versus cognitive components of attitudes. Euro-
Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive deter- pean Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 23–35.
minants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Vlachopoulos, S., & Biddle, S. J. H. (1997). Modeling the rela-
Review, 8, 220–247. tion of goal orientations to achievement-related affect in
Symons Downs, D., & Hausenblas, H. A. (2005). The theories physical activity: Does perceived ability matter? Journal of
of reasoned action and planned behavior applied to exer- Sport and Exercise Psychology, 19, 169–187.
cise: A meta-analytic update. Journal of Physical Activity and Walker, G. J., Courneya, K. S., & Deng, J. (2006). Ethnicity,
Health, 2, 76–97. gender, and the theory of planned behavior: The case of play-
Taylor, S. E. (2008). Current issues and new directions in psy- ing the lottery. Journal of Leisure Research, 38, 224–248.
chology and health: Bringing basic and applied research Wang, C. K. J., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2003). Intrinsic motivation
together to address underlying mechanisms. Psychology and towards sports in Singaporean students: The role of sport
Health, 23, 131–134. ability beliefs. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 515–523.
Terry, D. J., Hogg, M. A., & White, K. M. (2000). Attitude- Wang, C. K. J., Biddle, S. J. H., & Elliot, A. J. (2007). The 2×2
behavior relations: Social identity and group membership. achievement goal framework in a physical education context.
In D. J. Terry & M. A. Hogg (Eds.), Attitudes, behavior, and Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 147–168.
social context: The role of norms and group membership (pp. Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral
67–93). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the
Terry, D. J., & O’Leary, J. E. (1995). The theory of planned experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249–268.
behaviour: The effects of perceived behavioural control White, K. M., Hogg, M. A., & Terry, D. J. (2002). Improv-
and self-efficacy. British Journal of Social Psychology, 34, ing attitude-behavior correspondence through exposure to
199–220. normative support from a salient ingroup. Basic and Applied
Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C., Ntoumanis, N., & Nikitaras, N. Social Psychology, 24, 91–103.
(2010). Unhealthy weight control behaviours in adolescent Williams, G. C., Gagne, M., Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002).
girls: A process model based on self-determination theory. Facilitating autonomous motivation for smoking cessation.
Psychology and Health, 25, 535–550. Health Psychology, 21, 40–50.

h ag g er 
Williams, G. C., McGregor, H. A., Zeldman, A., & Freedman, (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in exercise and
Z. R. (2004). Testing a self-determination theory process sport (pp. 101–112). Champiagn, IL: Human Kinetics.
model for promoting glycemic control through diabetes Wilson, P. M., Rodgers, W. M., Blanchard, C. M., &
self-management. Health Psychology, 23, 58–66. Gessell, J. (2003). The relationship between psychological
Williams, P. T. (2001). Physical fitness and activity as separate needs, self-determined motivation, exercise attitudes, and
heart disease risk factors: A meta-analysis. Medicine and Sci- physical fitness. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33,
ence in Sports and Exercise, 33, 754–761. 2373–2392.
Wilson, P. M., & Rodgers, W. M. (2004). The relationship World Health Organization (WHO). (2004). Global strategy on
between perceived autonomy support, exercise regulations diet, physical activity and health. Geneva: Author.
and behavioral intentions in women. Psychology of Sport and Wu, T., Gao, X., Chen, M., & van Dam, R. M. (2009). Long-
Exercise, 5, 229–242. term effectiveness of diet-plus-exercise interventions vs. diet-
Wilson, P. M., & Rodgers, W. M. (2007). Self-determination theory, only interventions for weight loss: A meta-analysis. Obesity
exercise and well-being. In M. S. Hagger & N. L. D. Chatzisarantis Reviews, 10, 313–323.

 advances i n motivation in exercise a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


CHAPTER

Work Motivation: Directing,


28 Energizing, and Maintaining
Effort (and Research)
Adam M. Grant and Jihae Shin

Abstract
This chapter provides an overview of contemporary research on work motivation. We start by
identifying the central premises, controversies, and unanswered questions related to five core theoretical
perspectives on work motivation: expectancy theory, equity theory, goal-setting theory, job design, and
self-determination theory. We then discuss four current topics and new directions: collective motivation
and organizing, temporal dynamics, creativity, and the effects of rewards.
Key Words: work motivation, expectancy theory, equity theory, goal setting, job design,
self-determination theory, organizing, creativity, rewards

Introduction overview of core theoretical perspectives, key stud-


Work motivation is an important phenomenon ies, important controversies and unanswered ques-
for both scholars and practitioners to understand. It tions, as well as call attention to hot topics and new
helps to explain what drove Thomas Edison to invent directions for work motivation theory and research.
the first light bulb, Florence Nightingale to improve We start by discussing five core theoretical perspec-
nursing practices, Nelson Mandela to become the tives on work motivation: expectancy theory, equity
president of South Africa, Benjamin Franklin to theory, goal-setting theory, job design, and self-
create fire and police departments, Maya Angelou determination theory. We then turn our attention
to write poetry, and Michelangelo to paint the Sis- to four new directions and underexplored topics for
tine Chapel. Knowledge of work motivation also work motivation research: group motivation and
has the potential to shed light on major collective organizing, motivation over time, motivation and
accomplishments such as discovering flight, landing creativity, and the effects of rewards.
on the moon, curing river blindness, and inventing
the telephone and the computer. Underlying all of Core Theoretical Perspectives on
these accomplishments is a desire to take action. Work Motivation
Work motivation is described as the psychological Scholars have distinguished between two prin-
processes that direct, energize, and maintain action cipal types of work motivation theories: endog-
toward a job, task, role, or project (Campbell & enous process theories and exogenous cause theories
Pritchard, 1976; Kanfer, 1990). Our chapter is not (Katzell & Thompson, 1990). Endogenous pro-
designed to be exhaustive; comprehensive reviews cess theories focus primarily on the psychological
of work motivation theory and research are avail- mechanisms that explain motivation inside employ-
able in other outlets (e.g., Ambrose & Kulik, 1999; ees’ heads, while exogenous cause theories focus
Diefendorff & Chandler, 2010; Kanfer, Chen, & primarily on contextual influences on work moti-
Pritchard, 2008; Latham & Pinder, 2005; Mitchell vation that can be changed and altered. We begin
& Daniels, 2003). Rather, our goal is to provide an with a consideration of two key endogenous process


theories: expectancy theory and equity theory. Next, ity of multiplicative measures. Moreover, the meta-
we cover two central exogenous cause theories: goal analysis provided little information about causality,
setting and job design. Finally, we examine self- as most studies have been correlational rather than
determination theory as a hybrid perspective that experimental. Nevertheless, the overall results sug-
places equivalent emphasis on endogenous processes gest that expectancy, instrumentality, and valence
and exogenous causes. beliefs do take a valuable step toward explaining
variance in work motivation.
Expectancy Theory Research on expectancy theory has generated
According to expectancy theory, employees several controversies and unanswered questions.
choose to invest effort in courses of action by weigh- In light of evidence that expectancy, instrumental-
ing their relative utilities—that is, their probabilities ity, and valence beliefs leave considerable variance
of achieving desired outcomes (Vroom, 1964). Effort in motivation unexplained (Van Eerde & Thierry,
is a function of three beliefs: expectancy (effort will 1996), it is critical to understand other forces that
lead to performance), instrumentality (performance influence motivation. The theory of planned behav-
will lead to outcomes), and valence (these outcomes ior (Ajzen, 1991) takes a productive step in this
are important or valued). These beliefs are thought direction. According to this theory, planned actions
to interactively influence effort, such that if any are directly caused by intentions as micromediators
one of the beliefs is missing, the course of action of the belief–behavior relationship. Intentions are in
will not be selected (Porter & Lawler, 1968). With- turn a function of perceived behavioral control over
out expectancy beliefs, employees feel that effort is the behavior, attitudes toward the behavior, and
futile; without instrumentality and valence beliefs, subjective norms about the behavior.1
employees question whether performance is worth Comparing the planned behavior and expec-
the effort. Critically, expectancy theory is designed tancy theories reveals both similarities and useful
to account for the within-person decisions that distinctions. Perceived behavioral control, which is
employees make about whether, where, and how to akin to self-efficacy (Bandura, 1977)2, corresponds
invest their time and energy, rather than for differ- to expectancy beliefs, as both describe employees’
ences in effort between employees. judgments about whether they are capable of per-
Expectancy theory has been tested in many stud- forming if they expend effort. Attitudes, which
ies, but it is more often used as an organizing frame- capture the extent to which an employee evaluates
work for generating and testing context-specific the behavior favorably, appear to overlap with both
hypotheses. For example, researchers have applied instrumentality and valence beliefs, which—in
expectancy theory to guide the development of tandem—connote that the behavior will lead to
models to explain variations in DUI arrests among favorable outcomes. Moving beyond expectancy
police officers (Mastrofski, Ritti, & Snipes, 1994), theory, the theory of planned behavior adds subjec-
efforts by middle managers to champion issues for tive norms, or social expectations and pressure to
senior executives to pursue (Ashford, Rothbard, engage in the behavior. The underlying premise is
Piderit, & Dutton, 1998), home runs hit by major that employees derive utility not only from personal
league baseball players (Harder, 1991), and strategic outcomes but also from social rewards that con-
decisions in competitive markets (Chen & Miller, vey approval, respect, and community and social
1994). In a meta-analysis of 77 studies, Van Eerde punishments that convey disapproval, disrespect,
and Thierry (1996) found that expectancy, instru- and alienation. In a meta-analysis of 185 studies,
mentality, and valence beliefs were better predictors Armitage and Conner (2001) found that perceived
of psychological indicators of motivation (inten- behavioral control, attitudes, subjective norms, and
tions and preferences) than of behavioral indicators intentions combined to explain 27% of the variance
(performance, effort, and choices), which may be an in behaviors (31% when self-reported and 21%
artifact of common method and source biases. Sup- when objectively measured or observer-rated) and
porting one fundamental tenet of the theory, they 39% of the variance in intentions. Both subjective
found that expectancy, instrumentality, and valence norms and intentions explained unique variance
beliefs were more accurate predictors of within- in behaviors after accounting for perceived behav-
person than between-person differences in criteria. ioral control and attitudes, which highlights the
However, they found that the multiplicative model potential value of including these two psychologi-
explained little variance over and above the additive cal constructs to expand the predictive validity of
model. This may be an artifact of the low reliabil- expectancy theory.

 work m otivation


A second limitation of expectancy theory is identifying the key psychological forces that guide
that it is often viewed as overly calculative (Ash- decisions about effort and understanding their con-
ford et al., 1998; Mitchell & Daniels, 2003; Staw, sequences, rather than specifying their causes or
1984). Although the theory is reasonably effective fluctuations. Despite these limitations, expectancy
in predicting motivation and behavior, it creates a theory is appealing in its theoretical parsimony and
caricature of how employees actually make deci- its applications to diagnosing and resolving moti-
sions and experience motivation. With the possible vational problems in organizations, and thus it
exceptions of mathematicians, engineers, financial remains a popular and widely used theory.
analysts, and economists, rarely have we seen an
employee sit down and calculate the probabilities Equity Theory
of effort leading to performance and performance Equity theory (Adams, 1963, 1965) takes a step
leading to outcomes, and the utility of these out- toward placing motivation more squarely in a social
comes. It would be even more uncommon for an context. The central assumption of equity theory
employee to perform these calculations for multiple is that employees are motivated when their inputs
possible courses of action. With this limitation in (e.g., effort, knowledge, skill, loyalty) are matched
mind, scholars have begun to incorporate “hot” by outcomes (e.g., pay, bonuses, benefits, recogni-
affective components into expectancy theory (Seo, tion), which creates a sense of equity or fairness.
Barrett, & Bartunek, 2004). For example, Erez and When outcomes do not match inputs, the resulting
Isen (2002) demonstrated that positive affect can perceptions of inequity lead to distress, which moti-
increase expectancy, instrumentality, and valence vates employees to take action to reduce it. When
beliefs, but only under task conditions that are sup- employees feel underrewarded, they may restore
portive of these beliefs (e.g., working on a task in perceived equity by reducing their inputs (slacking
which performance is based on effort rather than off), attempting to reduce others’ inputs (convincing
chance). This research takes a step toward capturing coworkers to do less work or sabotaging their efforts
the real-time, affect-laden processes through which to be productive), seeking to increase their outcomes
expectancy, instrumentality, and valence judgments (asking for a raise or vacation time), or aiming to
are made (see also Seo et al., 2004). decrease coworkers’ outcomes (asking them to take
Expectancy theory has also been criticized for a pay cut or lobbying a boss to standardize salaries).
failing to specify the nature and sources of variations When employees feel overrewarded, they may restore
in employees’ beliefs and judgments. Employees can perceived equity by increasing their inputs (working
attach valence not only to outcomes of performance harder) or reducing their outcomes (requesting a pay
but also to effort and performance as ends in and of cut or redistributing their salaries to coworkers).
themselves. For example, Eisenberger’s (1992) the- How do employees make judgments of equity?
ory of learned industriousness explains how, when To evaluate input-outcome ratios, employees can
employees are rewarded for effort over time, hard make a range of comparisons (Adams, 1963, 1965).
work can take on secondary reward properties, such One set of comparisons is between outcomes and
that employees naturally enjoy the very experience inputs such as effort (the time and energy that I
of expending effort. In addition, employees tend to invested), ability (my knowledge, skills, and talents),
view performance as a reward in and of itself when and seniority (my tenure and loyalty). Another set
they are growth oriented (Hackman & Oldham, of comparisons is of the input-outcome ratios to
1976), conscientious (Grant, 2008b), and achieve- other input-outcome ratios, including my own past
ment motivated (McClelland, 1961), suggesting input-outcome ratios (what I have received else-
that they will place valence on performance even where or before, relative to my contributions) and
when there are no external outcomes attached to it. others’ input-outcome ratios (are mine appropriate
Finally, expectancy theory falls short of explain- in light of the ratios of similar others?). This last
ing how employees update and change their beliefs comparison, the social comparison, is often viewed
over time (Mitchell & Biglan, 1971). For example, as the central theoretical insight offered by equity
valence beliefs can change as employees realize that theory (Weick, 1966): Even when employees receive
their actual satisfaction with an outcome is different outcomes that match their inputs, their motivation
(e.g., lower or higher) than the satisfaction that they can suffer when they perceive others as maintaining
anticipated (e.g., Wilson & Gilbert, 2005). As an more favorable input-outcome ratios. For example,
endogenous process theory (Katzell & Thompson, studies have shown that higher pay dispersion—the
1990), the focus of expectancy theory has been on disparity in compensation between the highest-paid

g r a n t, s h i n 
and lowest-paid employees in an organization— visible, but under high instrumentality, individuals
predicts greater manager and employee turnover maintained their performance: “individuals faced
(Bloom & Michel, 2002), lower job satisfaction, with inequitable underreward will choose the avenue
productivity, and collaboration (Pfeffer & Langton, of decreased performance to the extent that it does
1993), and in major league baseball teams, fewer not affect future rewards. If decreasing performance
runs scored, more runs given up by pitchers, and will adversely affect future rewards, then alternative
more losses (Bloom, 1999). avenues for restoring equity will be undertaken”
Equity theory assumes that both underreward- (Harder, 1991, pp. 463–464).
ing employees and overrewarding employees can Another issue facing equity theory concerns how
be detrimental to motivation. Although research organizations and employees handle inconsistencies
has consistently shown negative motivational and in equity that emerge between different types of com-
behavioral effects of underreward inequity, evi- parisons. For example, when pay dispersion is high,
dence reveals mixed results about the consequences star performers making self-comparisons perceive
of overreward inequity: Some employees appear to high equity, but average and low performers making
decrease their motivation, others increase it, and social comparisons may perceive low equity. In gen-
still others show no significant changes (Ambrose eral, research suggests that in some circumstances, the
& Kulik, 1999). One approach to resolving these costs of perceived inequity among the latter group can
conflicting findings has involved understanding outweigh the benefits of perceived equity among the
individual differences in equity sensitivity. Huse- former group (Bloom, 1999; Messersmith, Guthrie,
man, Hatfield, and Miles (1987) proposed that Ji, & Lee, 2011; Siegel & Hambrick, 2005). How-
employees can be classified into one of three cat- ever, this research has yet to identify conditions under
egories of equity preferences: benevolent (preferring which organizations can create favorable perceptions
a lower outcome/input ratio than comparison oth- of equity for different groups of employees. One prac-
ers), equity sensitive (preferring an equal outcome/ tical solution, pay secrecy, appears to be a mixed bag,
input ratio to comparison others), and entitled as employees often view it as a signal of inequity and
(preferring a higher outcome/input ratio than com- resist by going out of their way to publicize their sala-
parison others). Accordingly, overreward inequity ries (Colella, Paetzold, Zardkoohi, & Wesson, 2007).
leads to higher motivation among benevolent and
equity-sensitive employees than entitled employees Goal-Setting Theory
(Miles, Hatfield, & Huseman, 1989). Benevolent One criticism of both expectancy and equity
and equity-sensitive employees are willing to work theories is that they focus primarily on psychologi-
to restore fairness, whereas entitled employees may cal processes involved in work motivation, provid-
be quite content with receiving more than they ing little explicit theory and guidance for explaining
contribute. the role of contextual forces (Katzell & Thompson,
A key controversy in work motivation research 1990). Goal-setting theory overcomes these limi-
concerns competing predictions between equity tations by focusing on the motivational effects of
and expectancy theories in situations character- goals, or targets for action. Extensive research has
ized by the combination of perceived underreward shown that difficult, specific goals motivate high
inequity and high instrumentality beliefs (Harder, performance by focusing attention, increasing effort
1991). According to equity theory, when instru- and persistence, and encouraging the development
mentality is high, employees who feel underre- of novel task strategies (Locke & Latham, 1990).
warded will be distressed by perceived inequity and For instance, classic studies showed that setting
may reduce their effort to create a more appropriate specific, difficult goals—relative to “do your best,”
balance between their inputs and outcomes. On the easy, or no goals—for 36 truck drivers transport-
other hand, expectancy theory predicts that when ing logs led them to increase from 60% to 90% of
instrumentality is high, employees who feel under- legal allowable weight, saving the company approxi-
rewarded will be motivated to achieve higher per- mately $250,000 in less than a year (for a review, see
formance, as they are confident that this will result Locke & Latham, 2002). In another study, Latham
in the rewards they feel they deserve. Harder (1991) and Saari (1982) gave 39 truck drivers the goal
provided a theoretical and empirical resolution of of enhancing the number of daily trips that they
this controversy in a study of major league baseball took to the mill, which yielded 15% average daily
free agents. He found that under low instrumen- increases in trips and saved the company approxi-
tality, negative performance effects of inequity were mately $2.7 million in less than 4 months.

 work m otivation


Difficult, specific goals are most likely to pro- motivates higher performance (Latham, Erez, &
duce these effects when employees are committed to Locke, 1988). Subsequent studies suggested that
them, when they receive feedback, and when tasks participation may achieve these benefits not only
are simple rather than complex. Without commit- through motivational mechanisms but also through
ment, employees question whether it is worthwhile cognitive mechanisms of enabling employees to
to work toward difficult goals. Without feedback, share information about task strategies and building
employees cannot gauge their progress and adjust self-efficacy (Locke & Latham, 2002). Moreover,
effort, persistence, and task strategies accordingly. employees who have high self-efficacy with respect
When tasks are simple, effort is a key determinant to assigned goals tend to set higher goals, experience
of performance, but when tasks are complex, ability greater goal commitment, choose better task strate-
and task strategies become more influential, reduc- gies, and maintain goal pursuit in the face of nega-
ing the performance effects of goal setting as a moti- tive feedback (Locke & Latham, 2002).
vational technique (Locke & Latham, 2002). Of course, if employees’ goals are not aligned with
At first glance, the principle of difficult goals organizational goals, goal setting can reduce rather
motivating higher performance than easy goals than increase performance (Locke & Latham, 2002).
appears to conflict with expectancy theory. From This raises important ethical issues, as employees can
an expectancy theory standpoint, easy goals yield take unethical or illegal shortcuts to achieve goals.
greater effort-to-performance expectancy beliefs, For example, Schweitzer, Ordoñez, and Douma
and thus greater motivation and performance, than (2004) conducted a laboratory experiment show-
difficult goals. Researchers have resolved this ten- ing that participants were more likely to cheat by
sion by showing that when goal difficulty is held overstating their productivity when they had unmet
constant, higher expectancy beliefs are associated goals than when they were simply asked to do their
with higher performance, but when goal difficulty best. These effects were observed for goals with and
varies, more difficult goals are linked with higher without monetary incentives, and they were particu-
performance, as the attention, effort, persistence, larly pronounced when participants narrowly missed
and task strategy benefits of difficult goals appear goal accomplishment (Schweitzer et al., 2004). A
to outweigh the costs of lower expectancy beliefs heated debate has ensued about whether goal-setting
(Locke, Motowidlo, & Bobko, 1986). Furthermore, theory adequately addresses and accounts for these
expectancy beliefs moderate the effects of goal dif- and other risks of goal setting, such as tunnel vision,
ficulty on performance, such that setting difficult stress, reduced learning and intrinsic motivation,
goals only motivates employees to take action if and excessive risk taking and competition (Latham
they believe such action has the potential to achieve & Locke, 2009; Locke & Latham, 2009; Ordoñez,
the goals (Locke & Latham, 2002). Schweitzer, Galinsky, & Bazerman, 2009a, 2009b).
As goal-setting theory gained prominence, schol- We are sympathetic to the arguments of both sides.
ars began to raise concerns about managers using On one hand, goal-setting theorists have acknowl-
goals as manipulative tools, and they expressed edged many of these risks, and demonstrating that
growing interest in understanding the motivational goals can increase unethical behavior is consistent
effects of goals that were self-set by employees. This with a premise of goal-setting theory that when
yielded a major controversy about whether partici- employees are committed to goals, they will be
pation in goal setting increases motivation and per- motivated to discover and create task strategies for
formance. Holding goal difficulty constant, studies achieving them (Locke & Latham, 2002). After all,
by Latham and colleagues showed null effects of unethical behavior is a task strategy. On the other
participation, whereas studies by Erez and col- hand, although much is known about the motivation
leagues identified significant benefits. The authors and performance effects of goal setting, substantially
collaborated, with Locke as a mediator (not a mod- less theory and research has addressed the condi-
erator), to jointly design experiments to resolve the tions under which goals are more versus less likely to
dispute. They discovered that the effects of partici- encourage unethical behavior and other unintended
pation in goal setting depend on goal commitment. consequences (e.g., Barsky, 2008). This represents
When the purpose of the goals is clear, participation an important direction for future research: Scholars
offers little benefit, but when the purpose is unclear, should systematically build and test theories about
allowing employees to participate serves the func- the factors that amplify and mitigate the negative
tion of increasing goal commitment and thereby effects of goal setting.

g r a n t, s h i n 
Job Design 1991) and workday schedules (Elsbach & Harga-
Goals are one important contextual influence on don, 2006), include knowledge and learning as
motivation, but how employees’ jobs are structured well as motivational mechanisms for explaining
also has a substantial impact on their motivation (for job design effects (Parker, Wall, & Jackson, 1997;
reviews, see Fried, Levi, & Laurence, 2008; Grant & Parker, Wall, & Cordery, 2001), and examine how
Parker, 2009; Morgeson & Humphrey, 2008; Old- motivational approaches to job design from orga-
ham & Hackman, 2010; Parker & Ohly, 2008). Clas- nizational psychology may involve tradeoffs with
sic research on job design focused on the principle of respect to mechanistic approaches from industrial
job enrichment, which refers to altering the structural engineering, perceptual-motor approaches from
characteristics of employees’ tasks to increase their human factors and cognitive psychology, and bio-
motivating potential (Herzberg, 1959). The domi- logical approaches from medicine (Campion &
nant approach to job enrichment is based on the Job McClelland, 1993; Morgeson & Campion, 2002).
Characteristics Model (Hackman & Oldham, 1976, From a motivational standpoint, one critique of
1980), which proposes that motivation, satisfaction, the Job Characteristics Model is that it focused on
performance quality, and withdrawal behaviors such the enrichment of assigned tasks, overlooking the
as absenteeism and turnover are a function of three important role that interpersonal relationships play
critical psychological states: experienced meaningful- in motivation (for a review, see Grant & Parker,
ness, responsibility for outcomes, and knowledge of 2009). Although early research included relational
results. Experienced meaningfulness is thought to be characteristics of jobs such as interactions with others
determined by three core job characteristics: skill vari- and friendship opportunities (Hackman & Lawler,
ety (being challenged to use a variety of one’s capabil- 1971; Trist & Bamforth, 1951; Turner & Lawrence,
ities), task identity (completing a whole, identifiable 1965), they fell out of favor as Hackman and Old-
piece of work from start to finish), and task signifi- ham (1976) sought to focus squarely on the task
cance (having an impact on other people inside or characteristics that composed jobs. Recent research
outside the organization). Experienced responsibility has examined the motivational effects of redesigning
is thought to be shaped by the job characteristic of jobs to connect employees to their impact on the
autonomy (freedom and discretion about when and beneficiaries of their work—the clients, customers,
how to complete the work) and experienced knowl- patients, and other who are affected by their efforts
edge of results by the job characteristic of feedback (Grant, 2007). Studies have shown, for example,
(information from completing the work itself about that when employees even have a short interaction
one’s progress and effectiveness). Thus, from a moti- with an end user of their work, they come to per-
vational standpoint, well-designed jobs are high in ceive their actions as having a greater impact and as
at least one of the dimensions of skill variety, task more socially valued, and feel more committed to
identity, and task significance, as well as in auton- their end users in general, which motivates them to
omy and feedback. These effects are moderated by work harder and achieve higher performance and
individual differences in growth need strength, such productivity (Grant, 2008b; Grant et al., 2007).
that employees who value learning and development As will be discussed in more detail later, this opens
should be more responsive to both the enriched job up the opportunity to understand how jobs can be
characteristics and the critical psychological states, as designed not only to enhance intrinsic motivation
well as by knowledge, skill, and satisfaction with the but also to foster prosocial motivation—the desire
work context. to protect and promote the well-being of others
Field experiments and meta-analytic reviews (Grant, 2007).
have shown that as a whole, these job characteristics Similar to the growing attention to self-set as
have good explanatory power for work motivation opposed to manager-set goals, scholars have observed
(Fried & Ferris, 1987; Griffin, 1983). At the same that managers are not the only architects of jobs;
time, the model has been critiqued and expanded employees also take initiative to proactively alter
on a number of grounds to include curvilinear the characteristics of their own jobs (for a review,
effects of jobs that are “too” enriched (Xie & Johns, see Grant & Parker, 2009). Scholars have developed
1995), consider how job perceptions are shaped by conceptual frameworks to explain the factors that
social information as well as objective task struc- motivate employees to adjust their roles (Nicholson,
tures (Salancik & Pfeffer, 1978; Zalesny & Ford, 1984) and craft or modify their jobs (Wrzesniewski
1990), account for variations between the different & Dutton, 2001). Recent research has revealed how
tasks that employees perform (Wong & Campion, employees take initiative to craft their jobs in pursuit

 work m otivation


of “unanswered callings” (Berg, Grant, & Johnson, reward (Gagné & Forest, 2008), as well as the sym-
2010), craft their jobs not only in isolation but also bolic features of rewards—who distributes them, why,
in collaboration (Leana, Appelbaum, & Shevchuk, how, and to whom (Mickel & Barron, 2008)—may
2009), and experience and respond to challenges affect self-determination and intrinsic motivation.
encountered in job crafting (Berg, Wrzesniewski, & Self-determination theory also makes a valuable
Dutton, 2010). Research has also explored how man- contribution to our understanding of work motiva-
agers and employees work together to negotiate “idio- tion by elaborating our understanding of extrinsic
syncratic deals” about the motivational characteristics motivation. Rather than viewing extrinsic motiva-
of jobs (Hornung, Rousseau, Glaser, Angerer, & tion as a unitary construct, Ryan and Deci (2000)
Weigl, 2010; Rousseau, Ho, & Greenberg, 2006). proposed that extrinsic motivation is a matter of
degree, varying along a continuum of autonomous
Self-Determination Theory regulation. They identified four different types of
Scholars have long viewed intrinsic motiva- extrinsic motivation that employees experience as
tion—a desire to act based on interest and enjoy- successively less controlled and more autonomous:
ment of the work itself—as a key influence on external (based on outside reward and punish-
work motivation, especially in the literatures on job ment contingencies), introjected (based on internal
design (Hackman & Oldham, 1980) and creativity reward and punishment contingencies, such as guilt
(Amabile & Mueller, 2007; George, 2007; Shalley, and self-esteem), identified (based on consistency
Zhou, & Oldham, 2004). Self-determination the- with a personal value), and integrated (assimilated
ory has begun to play a central role in expanding our into one’s system of values).
understanding of intrinsic motivation and inform- In the work domain, researchers have proposed
ing work motivation research more generally (for that since external reward and incentive contingen-
a review, see Gagné & Deci, 2005). In work moti- cies are virtually omnipresent, extrinsic and intrin-
vation research, self-determination theory has been sic motivations often coexist (Adler & Chen, 2009;
particularly useful in resolving controversies about Staw, 1984). If this is true, employees might be
the conditions under which rewards and incentives expected to invest more time and energy in their
have positive versus negative effects. According to work when they find it both intrinsically motivat-
self-determination theory, employees have three ing and are able to identify or integrate it with their
basic psychological needs: autonomy, competence, values (e.g., with prosocial values related to helping
and relatedness (Ryan & Deci, 2000). Autonomy others). Consistent with this prediction, research
refers to the feeling of choice and discretion, com- has shown that intrinsic and prosocial motivations
petence refers to feeling capable and efficacious, and interact synergistically to predict higher levels of
relatedness refers to feelings of connectedness and persistence, performance, and productivity among
belongingness with others. firefighters and fundraisers (Grant, 2008a), as well
Self-determination theorists propose that when as higher levels of creativity achieved by military
these three psychological needs are fulfilled, employ- security officers, water treatment employees, and
ees are more likely to be intrinsically motivated and participants in an experiment helping a local band
internalize external goals and objectives. Thus, when make money (Grant & Berry, 2011). Thus, intrinsic
rewards and incentives are delivered in a manner that and identified-integrated motivations appear to be
threatens feelings of autonomy, competence, and/or particularly potent in combination. Other research
relatedness, employees will tend to react negatively. has shown that autonomous motivations (intrin-
For example, explaining a reward system in a control- sic, integrated, identified) are more important for
ling rather than supportive manner can compromise performance on complex rather than simple tasks,
employees’ feelings of autonomy and relatedness (e.g., where autonomous motivations encourage explo-
Deci, Connell, & Ryan, 1989; Deci, Koestner, & ration and persistence (for a review, see Gagné &
Ryan, 1999; see also Kramer, 1999). On the other Deci, 2005).
hand, as long as rewards and incentives are delivered in Organizational scholars have also used self-
a manner that supports autonomy, competence, and determination theory to explain the motivational
relatedness, intrinsic motivation and internalization effects of transformational leadership—acting to
are more likely (e.g., Amabile, 1993). Other research inspire employees, model important values, and
suggests that additional features of compensation sys- provide individualized consideration and intellec-
tems, such as variable versus fixed pay ratios and the tual stimulation. Bono and Judge (2003) conducted
number of people whose performance determines the a field study and a laboratory experiment showing

g r a n t, s h i n 
that transformational leaders encouraged employ- is conducive to effective task performance and
ees to set autonomous rather than controlled goals, organizational citizenship behaviors (Gagné &
resulting in more positive attitudes and higher per- Deci, 2005).
formance. Interestingly, their field study suggested
that transformational leadership was associated Motivating New Directions
positively with autonomous motivation but had no Beyond these core theoretical perspectives, we
relationship with controlled motivation, while their see a range of contemporary issues and unanswered
lab experiment indicated that transformational lead- questions for work motivation research to address.
ership reduced controlled motivation more strongly In the following sections, we discuss four key cur-
than it increased autonomous motivation. Further rent and new directions for motivation research:
research is still needed to explain this discrepancy, group motivation and organizing, motivation over
but the difference in the strength and content of time, motivation and creativity, and the effects
rewards and incentives between the field and lab of rewards.
may be one key factor (Bono & Judge, 2003).
Integrating job design and self-determination Group Motivation and Organizing
theories, we know much more about how intrin- Moving beyond the dominant emphasis on
sic motivation is influenced by the structure than individual-level motivation, scholars have paid
the content of employees’ tasks. According to self- increasing attention to the role of motivation in
determination theory, feelings of autonomy, com- work groups and teams. The most comprehensive
petence, and relatedness are important for intrinsic perspective on this phenomenon is Chen and Kan-
motivation. However, intrinsic motivation depends fer’s (2006) theoretical model integrating individual-
on enjoying the work itself, and some tasks are level, group-level, and cross-level processes. They
experienced by employees as “not in themselves adopt a systems perspective to explain how, at both
interesting” (Gagné & Deci, 2005, p. 347). Thus, individual and team levels, motivational states affect
even when employees feel autonomous, compe- goal generation and goal striving, and thus influence
tent, and connected to others, they may not experi- performance. They propose that individual motiva-
ence intrinsic motivation in tasks that they do not tional states are a function of employees’ traits, work
find interesting or enjoyable. Currently, we lack experience, the quality of relationships with their
a theoretical framework for specifying how particular leaders, and individual feedback, while team moti-
task contents are more intrinsically interesting than vational states are a function of leadership climate,
others, and how different employees find different group norms, work design, and team feedback.
types of tasks interesting. It may be the case that one They further discuss how team and individual moti-
of the benefits of providing employees with auton- vational processes reciprocally influence each other,
omy is that it gives them the freedom and discretion as do individual and team performance. Recent
to craft their jobs in ways that they find intrinsically research provides support for the general premises
motivating, but this has yet to be studied. of the model. For example, Chen, Kanfer, DeShon,
Finally, little research has explored the costs Mathieu, and Kozlowski (2009) demonstrated the
of intrinsic motivation in organizational settings. cross-level influence of prior team performance on
Research suggests that intrinsic motivation is less subsequent individual performance in two samples.
effective for performance in tasks that are simple They found that prior team performance influences
or require considerable self-control and discipline self-efficacy by shaping prior individual perfor-
(Gagné & Deci, 2005; Koestner & Losier, 2002). mance and team efficacy, that team efficacy affects
Scholars have begun to speculate that intrinsic goal striving through self-efficacy and team action
motivation can distract attention away from orga- processes (e.g., helping and cooperation), and that
nizational goals, or at the very least, is not neces- these team action processes influence individual
sarily aligned with them (Grant & Berry, 2011; performance by shaping individual goal striving.
Osterloh & Frey, 2000). In addition, scholars have One exciting pathway for extending the Chen
raised concerns that employees can be intrinsi- and Kanfer model involves examining the influence
cally motivated toward activities that are directly of motivation on organizing. Organizing refers to
destructive or harmful, such as theft and sabotage the processes through which individual members
(Osterloh & Frey, 2000). As we noted for goal set- coordinate their actions to achieve collective goals
ting, more research is needed on the contingencies (Weick, 1979), and it is among the most important
that affect whether and when intrinsic motivation yet neglected topics in all of organizational research

 work m otivation


(Heath & Sitkin, 2001). Surprisingly little research addition, recent developments in knowledge about
has examined the impact of motivation on organiz- proactive motivation—the desire to take anticipa-
ing. For example, there is a large literature on “high- tory action to create change (for reviews, see Grant &
reliability organizing” that examines how groups Parker, 2009; Parker, Bindl, & Strauss, 2010)—may
coordinate actions to achieve consistent, safe perfor- help to explain the disproportionate influence of
mance in uncertain, complex, consequential, high- particular individual efforts on organizing.
risk contexts such as nuclear power plants, wildland
firefighting, hospital emergency departments, and Motivation Over Time
aircraft carriers (e.g., Hofmann & Stetzer, 1998; In response to critiques that most motivation
Waller & Roberts, 2003; Weick & Roberts, 1993). theory and research is overly static, scholars have
Traditional approaches to increasing reliability have begun to examine dynamic and temporal perspec-
focused on building collective capabilities for sys- tives on motivation. One dynamic view adopts
tems to manage unexpected events through the an adult development perspective to explain how
structuring of roles, routines, and norms (e.g., Bierly motivations change across the life span (Kanfer &
& Spender, 1995; Bigley & Roberts, 2001; Vogus Ackerman, 2004). These authors review research
& Welbourne, 2003; Weick, Sutcliffe, & Obstfeld, suggesting that aging is associated with declines in
1999, 2005; Zohar & Luria, 2003). However, fluid intelligence (working memory, abstract rea-
these collective capabilities are nearly useless soning, attention, and processing of novel infor-
if employees are not motivated to put them into mation), but increases in crystallized intelligence
action. Researchers have yet to explore how indi- (educational and experiential knowledge). They pro-
vidual and team motivational processes affect the pose that as employees age, these changes increase
effective implementation of collective capabilities the likelihood of enhancing effort to cope with jobs
for high reliability. Moreover, individual and team that place heavy demands on fluid intelligence, but
motivational processes may be important catalysts this may compromise motivation and performance,
of the decision to create and develop collective capa- as declining performance in the face of increased
bilities in the first place. effort can reduce self-efficacy. In contrast, aging
More generally, the impact of motivation on may be associated with more effective maintenance
organizing has been neglected. One notable excep- of motivation and performance in jobs that primar-
tion to this trend is the fascinating work by Adler ily require crystallized intelligence, as employees are
and Chen (2009) on large-scale collaborative cre- able to sustain high performance in the absence of
ativity. These authors focus on how social collectives greater effort. As a result, from an instrumental-
are able to organize or coordinate efforts to develop ity and valence standpoint, stronger rewards and
and implement novel, useful solutions to problems, incentives may be necessary to increase the perfor-
such as when hundreds or thousands of software mance of midlife employees (above current levels)
developers collaborate to introduce a new computer in jobs requiring crystallized intelligence, compared
program, aircraft engineers collaborate to develop a to younger workers. Kanfer and Ackerman (2004)
new design, and scientists create new pharmaceu- further propose that aging reduces the valence that
tical drugs. Building on self-determination theory, employees place on effort and on increasing job per-
Adler and Chen (2009) present propositions to formance, although the latter effect can be attenu-
explain how large-scale collaborative creativity can ated by performance standards that fit age-graded
be organized effectively through simultaneously values, such as rising emphasis on social rather than
activating intrinsic and identified motivations. We technical competence.
hope to see more research follow suit by examin- Aging also has important implications for
ing how individual-level and team-level motivations how employees grapple with death awareness and
influence the propensity and capacity to organize. respond to mortality cues, and Grant and Wade-
Research on social motivations that are directed Benzoni (2009) proposed that these changes can
toward others, such as collectivistic work motiva- have substantial effects on work motivation. These
tion (Shamir, 1990, 1991), motivation to innovate authors distinguished between two states of death
(Amabile, 1988), and prosocial motivation (Grant, awareness—the “hot” death anxiety typically stud-
2007, 2008a; Grant & Berry, 2011), may prove ied by terror management theorists and the “cool”
especially useful in drawing sharper theoretical and death reflection typically studied by generativity
empirical links between motivation and the organiz- and posttraumatic growth theorists. They proposed
ing of individual efforts into collective outcomes. In that death anxiety is likely to motivate withdrawal

g r a n t, s h i n 
behaviors from work, such as absenteeism, tardi- These perspectives on life-span development and
ness, and turnover, except when work serves as generational differences emphasize relatively macro-
an escape from mortality cues. They argued that scopic changes in motivation, but it is also impor-
in contrast, death reflection has the potential to tant to understand more microscopic changes in
motivate generative work behaviors, such as help- motivation. Compared to research on the direction
ing, mentoring, and transitions to more prosocially and intensity of effort, few theoretical models and
focused or service-oriented occupations, especially empirical studies have focused on the maintenance
for employees who view work as a calling. However, or persistence of effort (e.g., Grant et al., 2007;
since empirical research has yet to test, challenge, Penner, Dovidio, Piliavin, & Schroeder, 2005). Are
complicate, and expand the propositions developed the factors that sustain motivation different from
by Kanfer and Ackerman (2004) and Grant and those that initiate it—and if so, how, why, and
Wade-Benzoni (2009), we encourage future stud- when? Furthermore, little research has examined
ies on the impact of aging and death awareness on the factors that influence changes in the valence that
work motivation. employees place on different outcomes over time.
A different perspective on temporal changes For example, outside of changes in job designs and
in motivation appears in research on generational incentives, what leads employees to develop more
differences in work values. Twenge, Campbell, intrinsic motivation toward a specific occupation,
Hoffman, and Lance (2010) used a nationally job, project, or task? As another example, research-
representative sample of U.S. high school seniors ers have established that employees vary in their
in 1976, 1991, and 2006 to compare mean work orientations toward work as a job, a career, or a call-
values between the Baby Boomer, Generation X, ing (Wrzesniewski, McCauley, Rozin, & Schwartz,
and Millennial generations. A key feature of their 1997; see also Bunderson & Thompson, 2009).
analytic approach is that while cross-sectional stud- However, we know little about what leads employ-
ies confound generational cohorts with age and life ees to shift from viewing work as a job to a career or
experience, longitudinal studies comparing par- a career to a calling. We need a deeper understand-
ticipants at the same ages can isolate these factors. ing of the factors that shift employees’ motivational
They discovered that on average, leisure values have orientations toward work over time.
increased with each new generation, correspond-
ing with declines in work centrality. Extrinsic val- Motivation and Creativity
ues, although highest among Generation X, remain Motivation is known to play a central role in
higher among Millennials than Baby Boomers. Mil- creativity, or the production of novel and useful
lennials appear to place less importance on social ideas, which is a topic of increasing interest and
and intrinsic work values than Baby Boomers, and importance to organizations as the pace and uncer-
there were no significant differences in emphasis tainty of work continue to accelerate. Amabile and
placed on altruistic work values. colleagues have developed a componential theory
There is a heavy debate about the practical signif- of creativity that features intrinsic motivation
icance of the effect sizes in this program of research prominently as an important influence on the cre-
(e.g., Trzesniewski & Donnellan, 2010), and how ative process (Amabile, 1996; Amabile & Mueller,
they may be small in comparison to those of devel- 2007). Intrinsic motivation is thought to enhance
opmental and age effects (e.g., Roberts, Edmonds, creativity by encouraging exploration and risk tak-
& Grijalva, 2010). However, because of its ability to ing (Amabile, Hennessey, & Grossman, 1986; Hen-
isolate generational differences from age differences, nessey, Amabile, & Martinage, 1989), psychological
this is the most rigorous study to date of genera- engagement in work and in learning (Amabile, Hill,
tional differences in work values. The Twenge et al. Hennessey, & Tighe, 1994), and active processing
(2010) findings raise interesting questions about of information and selection of novel, challenging
whether, on average, employees from the Millennial tasks (Conti, Amabile, & Pollack, 1995), as well
generation will display stronger responses to moti- as persistence (Shalley, Zhou, & Oldham, 2004).
vational interventions focusing on leisure rewards Interestingly, research has shown mixed effects of
(e.g., time off, paid vacations) and weaker responses intrinsic motivation on creativity, with some labora-
to social rewards (e.g., making contacts and friend- tory and field studies indicating a positive relation-
ship opportunities) and intrinsic rewards (e.g., ship, and others suggesting a null relationship (e.g.,
learning, interesting work, creative challenges). George, 2007; Shalley et al., 2004).

 work m otivation


Grant and Berry (2011) sought to resolve this This raises a more general issue with respect to
discrepancy by examining whether the effect of rewards: We believe that lumping all rewards into
intrinsic motivation on creativity is contingent on a common category has obscured the importance
prosocial motivation. They proposed that while of understanding the effects of different types of
intrinsic motivation fosters a focus on novel ideas, rewards on motivation. In particular, researchers have
prosocial motivation is important for encouraging focused primarily on pay and financial incentives,
perspective taking about what others find useful giving far less emphasis to more symbolic rewards
(Grant & Berry, 2011). They found support for such as recognition and appreciation, even though
these hypotheses across field studies of military these rewards are frequently intended to motivate
officers and water treatment employees, as well as and can be effective (Stajkovic & Luthans, 2001;
in a laboratory experiment. We hope to see further see also Frey, 2007, and Grant & Gino, 2010). We
research build on this evidence to examine other hope to see scholars build and test theories about
motivational processes that foster a focus on use- the motivational effects of different types of recog-
fulness, complementing the attention to novelty nition systems.
cultivated by intrinsic motivation. Such investiga-
tions will further enhance our understanding of the Conclusion
effects of motivation on creativity. Scholars have explored many other issues related
More broadly, we hope to see scholars investi- to work motivation that fall outside the scope of this
gate the impact of motivation on a wider range of chapter. For example, important developments have
dependent variables. Our discussion of creativity examined how motivation is shaped by needs and
aligns with increasing attention of organizational motives (for reviews, see Ambrose & Kulik, 1999;
researchers to employee behaviors that go beyond Steers, Mowday, & Shapiro, 2004), personality traits
core task requirements: organizational citizen- (e.g., Barrick, Stewart, & Piotrowski, 2002; Judge
ship behaviors such as helping and sportsmanship & Ilies, 2002), culture (Erez, 2010), and nonwork
(e.g., Organ, 1988; Podsakoff, MacKenzie, Paine, factors (George & Brief, 1996; Kossek & Misra,
& Bachrach, 2000), proactive behaviors such as 2008; Rothbard, 2001). Furthermore, extensive
voicing suggestions and taking charge to introduce treatments of the role of self-regulation processes
new work methods (Grant & Parker, 2009; Parker are available elsewhere (Diefendorff & Chandler,
et al., 2010), and counterproductive behaviors such 2010). In addition, some scholars have developed
as aggression, theft, and sabotage (Griffin & Lopez, integrative perspectives and models that bring
2005; Spector & Fox, 2010). Different motivations together multiple motivation theories (Locke &
may play a key role in shaping which of these behav- Latham, 2004; Mitchell & Daniels, 2003; Steel &
ioral directions employees pursue. König, 2006). It remains to be seen whether an
integrative model of motivation is desirable, or even
Rewards and Motivation possible. Our own view is that given the complex-
Another key direction involves identifying the ity of psychological, social, and situational influ-
conditions under which rewards increase motiva- ences on motivation, researchers are best advised to
tion. A debate currently exists about whether man- develop, test, and elaborate middle-range theories
agers underestimate the power of intrinsic relative to (Weick, 1974) that are problem driven—designed
extrinsic rewards for motivating employees (Heath, to explain particular phenomena and outcomes,
1999), or whether there is a discrepancy between rather than seeking to generalize to all outcomes
what employees say and what they do, such that (Lawrence, 1992). Nevertheless, we hope this chap-
employees report that extrinsic rewards are relatively ter is useful in summarizing key trends in the study
unimportant, but the preferences revealed by their of work motivation and directing, energizing, and
behaviors suggest otherwise (Rynes, Gerhart, & maintaining future research.
Minette, 2004). Scholars may take steps to resolve
this debate by attending not only to the instrumen- Notes
tal features of rewards but also to their symbolic 1. More recent work suggests that the psychological processes
features. For example, Mickel and Barron (2008) underlying the model can be further illuminated by including
propose that rewards will be more likely to increase desires as micromediators of the effects of beliefs on intentions
(Perugini & Bagozzi, 2001).
motivation when they are distributed by high-status 2. Some scholars have challenged the theoretical and empiri-
authority figures, for high performance and accom- cal appropriateness of lumping self-efficacy and perceived
plishments, and in public ceremonies. behavioral control together. The central distinction lies in that

g r a n t, s h i n 
self-efficacy describes judgments of one’s internal capability to Berg, J. M., Grant, A. M., & Johnson, V. (2010). When call-
perform a behavior, whereas perceived behavioral control also ings are calling: Crafting work and leisure in pursuit of
incorporates judgments of whether external forces may limit the unanswered occupational callings. Organization Science, 21,
controllability of the behavior (see Armitage & Conner, 2001, 973–994.
p. 476), which suggests that perceived behavioral control and Berg, J. M., Wrzesniewski, A., & Dutton, J. E. (2010). Perceiv-
expectancy beliefs share greater similarity with each other than ing and responding to challenges in job crafting at different
they do with self-efficacy. ranks: When proactivity requires adaptivity. Journal of Orga-
nizational Behavior, 31, 158–186.
Bierly, P. E., & Spender, J-C. (1995). Culture and high reli-
References ability organizations: The case of the nuclear submarine.
Adams, J. S. (1963). Toward an understanding of inequity. Jour- Journal of Management, 21, 639–656.
nal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 422–436. Bigley, G. A., & Roberts K. H. (2001). The incident command
Adams, J. S. (1965). Inequity in social exchange. In L. Berkowitz system: High-reliability organizing for complex and vola-
(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. tile task environments. Academy of Management Journal, 44,
267–299). New York: Academic Press. 1281–1299.
Adler, P. S., & Chen, C. X. (2009). Beyond intrinsic motivation: Bloom, M. (1999). The performance effects of pay dispersion
On the nature of individual motivation in large-scale collab- on individuals and organizations. Academy of Management
orative creativity. Social Science Research Network. Retrieved Journal, 42, 25–40.
August 2, 2011, from https://msbfile03.usc.edu/digitalmea- Bloom, M., & Michel, J. G. (2002). The relationships among
sures/padler/intellcont/BEYOND%20INTRINSIC%20 organizational context, pay dispersion, and managerial turn-
MOTIVATION-1.pdf over. Academy of Management Journal, 45, 33–42.
Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Bono, J. E., & Judge, T. A. (2003). Self-concordance at work:
Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179–211. Toward understanding the motivational effects of trans-
Amabile, T. M. (1988). A model of creativity and innovation in orga- formational leaders. Academy of Management Journal, 46,
nizations. Research in Organizational Behavior, 10, 123–167. 554–571.
Amabile, T. M. (1993). Motivational synergy: Toward new concep- Bunderson, J. S., & Thompson, J. A. (2009). The call of the
tualizations of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation in the work- wild: Zookeepers, callings, and the double-edged sword of
place. Human Resource Management Review, 3, 185–201. deeply meaningful work. Administrative Science Quarterly,
Amabile, T.M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder, CO: 54, 32–57.
Westview Press. Campbell, D. J., & Pritchard, R. (1976). Motivation theory in
Amabile, T. M., Hennessey, B. A., & Grossman, B. S. (1986). industrial and organizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette
Social influences on creativity: The effects of contracted- (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and organizational psychology
for reward. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, (pp. 63–130). Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.
14–23. Campion, M. A., & McClelland, C. L. (1993). Follow-up
Amabile, T. M., Hill, K. G., Hennessey, B. A., & Tighe, E. M. and extension of the interdisciplinary costs and benefits of
(1994). The Work Preference Inventory: Assessing intrinsic enlarged jobs. Journal of Applied Psychology, 78, 339–351.
and extrinsic motivational orientations. Journal of Personality Chen, G., & Kanfer, R. (2006). Toward a systems theory of
and Social Psychology, 66, 950–967. motivated behavior in work teams. Research in Organiza-
Amabile, T. M., & Mueller, J. S. (2007). Studying creativity, its tional Behavior, 27, 223–267.
processes, and its antecedents: An exploration of the compo- Chen, G., Kanfer, R., DeShon, R. P., Mathieu, J. E., & Kozlowski,
nential theory of creativity. In J. Zhou & C. Shalley (Eds.), S. W. J. (2009). The motivating potential of teams: Test and
Handbook of organizational creativity (pp. 31–62). Mahwah, extension of Chen & Kanfer’s (2006) cross-level model of
NJ: Erlbaum. motivation in teams. Organizational Behavior and Human
Ambrose, M. L., & Kulik, C. T. (1999). Old friends, new Decision Processes, 110, 45–55.
faces: Motivation in the 1990s. Journal of Management, 25, Chen, M. J., & Miller, D. (1994). Competitive attack, retali-
231–292. ation and performance: An expectancy-valence framework.
Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of Strategic Management Journal, 15, 85–102.
planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Colella, A., Paetzold, R. L., Zardkoohi, A., & Wesson, M. J.
Social Psychology, 40, 471–499. (2007). Exposing pay secrecy. Academy of Management
Ashford, S. J., Rothbard, N. P., Piderit, S. K., & Dutton, J. E. Review, 32, 55–71.
(1998). Out on a limb: The role of context and impression Conti, R., Amabile, T. M., & Pollack, S. (1995). Enhancing
management in selling gender-equity issues. Administrative intrinsic motivation, learning, and creativity. Personality and
Science Quarterly, 43, 23–57. Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 1107–1116.
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of Deci, E. L., Connell, J. P., & Ryan, R. M. (1989). Self-
behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215. determination in a work organization. Journal of Applied
Barrick, M. R., Stewart, G. L., & Piotrowski, M. (2002). Per- Psychology, 74, 580–590.
sonality and job performance: Test of the mediating effects Deci, E. L., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. M. (1999). A meta-analytic
of motivation among sales representatives. Journal of Applied review of experiments examining the effects of extrinsic
Psychology, 87, 43–51. rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125,
Barsky, A. (2008). Understanding the ethical cost of organiza- 627–668.
tional goal-setting: A review and theory development. Jour- Diefendorff, J. M., & Chandler, M. M. (2010). Motivating
nal of Business Ethics, 81, 63–81. employees. In S. Zedeck (Ed.), Handbook of industrial and

 work m otivation


organizational psychology (pp. 65–135). Washington, DC: Griffin, R. W. (1983). Objective and social sources of informa-
American Psychological Association. tion in task redesign: A field experiment. Administrative Sci-
Eisenberger, R. (1992). Learned industriousness. Psychological ence Quarterly, 28, 184–200.
Review, 99, 248–267. Griffin, R. W., & Lopez, Y. P. (2005). ‘Bad behavior’ in organiza-
Elsbach, K. D., & Hargadon, A. B. (2006). Enhancing creativity tions: A review and typology for future research. Journal of
through “mindless” work: A framework of workday design. Management, 31, 988–1005.
Organization Science, 17, 470–483. Hackman, J. R., & Lawler, E. E. (1971). Employee reactions
Erez, M. (2010). Culture and job design. Journal of Organiza- to job characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 55,
tional Behavior, 31, 389–400. 259–286.
Erez, A., & Isen, A. M. (2002). The influence of positive affect Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1976). Motivation through
on the components of expectancy motivation. Journal of the design of work: Test of a theory. Organizational Behavior
Applied Psychology, 87, 1055–1067. and Human Performance, 16, 250–279.
Frey, B. S. (2007). Awards as compensation. European Manage- Hackman, J. R., & Oldham, G. R. (1980). Work redesign. Read-
ment Review, 4, 6–14. ing, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Fried, Y., & Ferris, G. R. (1987). The validity of the job char- Harder, J. W. (1991). Equity theory versus expectancy theory:
acteristics model: A review and meta-analysis. Personnel Psy- The case of major league baseball free agents. Journal of
chology, 40, 287–322. Applied Psychology, 76, 458–464.
Fried, Y., Levi, A. S., & Laurence, G. (2008). Motivation and Heath, C. (1999). On the social psychology of agency relation-
job design in the new world of work. In S. Cartwright and ships: Lay theories of motivation overemphasize extrinsic
C. L. Cooper (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of personnel psy- incentives. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision
chology (Vol. 24, pp. 586–611). Oxford, England: Oxford Processes, 78, 25–62.
University Press. Heath, C., & Sitkin, S. (2001). Big-B versus Big-O: What is
Gagné, M., & Deci, E. L. (2005). Self-determination theory and organizational about organizational behavior? Journal of
work motivation. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 26, Organizational Behavior, 22, 43–58.
331–362. Hennessey, B. A., Amabile, T. M., & Martinage, M. (1989).
Gagné, M., & Forest, J. (2008). The study of compensation sys- Immunizing children against the negative effects of reward.
tems through the lens of self-determination theory: Reconcil- Contemporary Educational Psychology, 14, 212–227.
ing 35 years of debate. Canadian Psychology, 49, 225–232. Herzberg, F. (1959). The motivation to work. New York: Wiley.
George, J. M. (2007). Creativity in organizations. Academy of Hofmann, D. A., & Stetzer, A. (1998). The role of safety climate
Management Annals, 1, 439–477. and communication in accident interpretation: Implications
George, J. M., & Brief, A. P. (1996). Motivational agendas in for learning from negative events. Academy of Management
the workplace: The effects of feelings on focus of attention Journal, 41, 644-657.
and work motivation. Research in Organizational Behavior, Hornung, S., Rousseau, D. M., Glaser, J., Angerer, P., & Weigl,
18, 75–109. M. (2010). Beyond top-down and bottom-up work redesign:
Grant, A. M. (2007). Relational job design and the motiva- Customizing job content through idiosyncratic deals. Journal
tion to make a prosocial difference. Academy of Management of Organizational Behavior, 31, 187–215.
Review, 32, 393–417. Huseman, R. C., Hatfield, J. D., & Miles, E. W. (1987). A new
Grant, A. M. (2008a). Does intrinsic motivation fuel the proso- perspective on equity theory: The equity sensitivity construct.
cial fire? Motivational synergy in predicting persistence, per- Academy of Management Review, 12, 222–234.
formance, and productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, Judge, T. A., & Ilies, R. (2002). Relationship of personality to
48–58. performance motivation: A meta-analytic review. Journal of
Grant, A. M. (2008b). The significance of task significance: Job Applied Psychology, 87, 797–807.
performance effects, relational mechanisms, and boundary Kanfer, R. (1990). Motivation theory and industrial and orga-
conditions. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 108–124. nizational psychology. In M. D. Dunnette (Ed.), Handbook
Grant, A. M., & Berry, J. (2011). The necessity of others is the of industrial and organizational psychology (Vol. 1, 2nd ed.,
mother of invention: Intrinsic and prosocial motivations, pp. 75–130). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists
perspective-taking, and creativity. Academy of Management Press.
Journal, 54, 73–96. Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). Aging, adult develop-
Grant, A. M., Campbell, E. M., Chen, G., Cottone, K., Lape- ment, and work motivation. Academy of Management Review,
dis, D., & Lee, K. (2007). Impact and the art of motiva- 29, 440–458.
tion maintenance: The effects of contact with beneficiaries Kanfer, R., Chen, G., & Pritchard, R. D. (Eds.). (2008). Work
on persistence behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human motivation: Past, present, and future. New York: Taylor and
Decision Processes, 103, 53–67. Francis Group.
Grant, A. M., & Gino, F. (2010). A little thanks goes a long Katzell, R. A., & Thompson, D. E. (1990). Work motivation:
way: Explaining why gratitude expressions motivate proso- Theory and practice. American Psychologist, 45, 144–153.
cial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 98. Koestner, R., & Losier, G. F. (2002). Distinguishing three ways
Grant, A. M., & Parker, S. K. (2009). Redesigning work design of being highly motivated: A closer look at introjection,
theories: The rise of relational and proactive perspectives. identification, and intrinsic motivation. In R. Koestner, G.
Academy of Management Annals, 3, 317–375. F. Losier, E. L. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
Grant, A. M., & Wade-Benzoni, K. (2009). The hot and cool self-determination research (pp. 101–121). Rochester, NY:
of death awareness at work: Mortality cues, aging, and self- University of Rochester Press.
protective and prosocial motivations. Academy of Manage- Kossek, E., & Misra, K. (2008). Non-work influences on work
ment Review, 34, 600–622. motivation. In R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. Pritchard (Eds.),

g r a n t, s h i n 
Work motivation: Past, present, and future (pp. 471–500). Morgeson, F. P., & Campion, M. A. (2002). Avoiding tradeoffs
New York: Taylor and Francis Group. when redesigning work: Evidence from a longitudinal quasi-
Kramer, R. (1999). Trust and distrust in organizations: Emerging experiment. Personnel Psychology, 55, 589–612.
perspectives, enduring questions. Annual Review of Psychol- Morgeson, F. P., & Humphrey, S. E. (2008). Job and team
ogy, 50, 569–598. design: Toward a more integrative conceptualization of work
Latham, G. P., Erez, M., & Locke, E. A. (1988). Resolving scien- design. In J. Martocchio (Ed.), Research in personnel and
tific disputes by the joint design of crucial experiments by the human resource management (Vol. 27, pp. 39–92). Bingley,
antagonists: Application to the Erez-Latham dispute regard- England: Emerald Group Publishing Limited.
ing participation in goal setting. Journal of Applied Psychol- Nicholson, N. (1984). A theory of work role transitions. Admin-
ogy, 73, 753–772. istrative Science Quarterly, 29, 172–191.
Latham, G. P., & Locke, E. A. (2009). Science and ethics: What Oldham, G. R., & Hackman, J. R. (2010). Not what it was and
should count as evidence against the use of goal setting? not what it will be: The future of job design research. Journal
Academy of Management Perspectives, 23, 88–91. of Organizational Behavior, 31, 463–479.
Latham, G. P., & Pinder, C. C. (2005). Work motivation theory Ordoñez, L. D., Schweitzer, M. E., Galinsky, A. D. & Bazerman,
and research at the dawn of the twenty-first century. Annual M. H. (2009a). Goals gone wild: The systematic side effects
Review of Psychology, 56, 495–516. of overprescribing goal setting. Academy of Management Per-
Latham, G. P., & Saari, L. M. (1982). The importance of union spectives, 23, 6–22.
acceptance for productivity improvement through goal set- Ordoñez, L. D., Schweitzer, M. E., Galinsky, A. D., & Bazerman,
ting. Personnel Psychology, 35, 781–787. M. H. (2009b). On good scholarship, goal setting, and scholars
Lawrence, P. R. (1992). The challenge of problem-oriented gone wild. Academy of Management Perspectives, 23, 82–87.
research. Journal of Management Inquiry, 1, 139–142. Organ, D. W. (1988). Organizational citizenship behavior: The
Leana, C., Appelbaum, E., & Shevchuk, I. (2009). Work pro- good soldier syndrome. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
cess and quality of care in early childhood education: The Osterloh, M., & Frey, B. (2000). Motivation, knowledge transfer
role of job crafting. Academy of Management Journal, 52, and organizational form. Organization Science, 11, 538–550.
1169–1192. Parker, S. K., Bindl, U. K., & Strauss, K. (2010). Making things
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal-setting and happen: A model of proactive motivation. Journal of Manage-
task performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. ment, 36, 827–856.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2002). Building a practically Parker, S. K., & Ohly, S. (2008). Designing motivating jobs. In
useful theory of goal setting and task motivation: A 35-year R. Kanfer, G. Chen, & R. Pritchard (Eds.), Work motiva-
odyssey. American Psychologist, 57, 705–717. tion: Past, present, and future (pp. 233–284). New York: LEA/
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2004). What should we do Psychology Press.
about motivation theory? Six recommendations for the Parker, S. K., Wall, T. D., & Cordery, J. L. (2001). Future work
twenty-first century. Academy of Management Review, 29, design research and practice: Towards an elaborated model
388–403. of work design. Journal of Occupational and Organizational
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (2009). Has goal setting gone Psychology, 74, 413–440.
wild, or have its attackers abandoned good scholarship? Parker, S. K., Wall, T. D., & Jackson, P. R. (1997). ‘That’s not my
Academy of Management Perspectives, 23, 17–23. job’: Developing flexible employee work orientations. Acad-
Locke, E. A., Motowidlo, S. J., & Bobko, P. (1986). Using self- emy of Management Journal, 40, 899–929.
efficacy theory to resolve the conflict between goal-setting Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Piliavin, J. A., & Schroeder, D. A.
theory and expectancy theory in organizational behavior and (2005). Prosocial behavior: Multilevel perspectives. Annual
industrial/organizational psychology. Journal of Social and Review of Psychology, 56, 365–392.
Clinical Psychology, 4, 328–338. Perugini, M., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2001). The role of desires and
Mastrofski, S. D., Ritti, R. R., & Snipes, J. B. (1994). Expec- anticipated emotions in goal-directed behaviours: Broaden-
tancy theory and police productivity in DUI enforcement. ing and deepening the theory of planned behavior. British
Law and Society Review, 28, 113–148. Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 79–98.
McClelland, D. C. (1961). The achieving society. New York: Van Pfeffer, J., & Langton, N. (1993). The effect of wage dispersion
Nostrom Reinhold. on satisfaction, productivity, and working collaboratively:
Messersmith, J. G., Guthrie, J. P., Ji, Y.-Y., & Lee, J.-Y. (2011). Evidence from college and university faculty. Administrative
Executive turnover: The influence of dispersion and other Science Quarterly, 38, 382–407.
pay system characteristics. Journal of Applied Psychology, 96, Podsakoff, P. M., MacKenzie, S. B., Paine, J. B., & Bachrach, D. G.
457–469. (2000). Organizational citizenship behaviors: A critical review
Mickel, A. E., & Barron, L. A. (2008). Getting “more bang for of the theoretical and empirical literature and suggestions for
the buck”: Symbolic value of monetary rewards in organiza- future research. Journal of Management, 26, 513–563.
tions. Journal of Management Inquiry, 17, 329–338. Porter, L. W., & Lawler, E. E., III. (1968). Managerial attitudes
Miles, E. W., Hatfield, J. D., & Huseman, R. C. (1989). The and performance. Homewood, IL: Dorsey Press.
equity sensitivity construct: Potential implications for worker Roberts, B. W., Edmonds, G., & Grijalva, E. (2010). It is devel-
performance. Journal of Management, 15, 581–588. opmental me, not generation me: Developmental changes are
Mitchell, T. R., & Biglan, A. (1971). Instrumentality theories: Cur- more important than generational changes in narcissism—
rent uses in psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 76, 432–454. comment on Trzesniewski & Donnellan (2010). Perspectives
Mitchell, T. R., & Daniels, D. (2003). Motivation. In W. Bor- on Psychological Science, 5, 97–102.
man, D. Ilgen, & R. Klimoski (Eds.), Handbook of psychology. Rothbard, N. P. (2001). Enriching or depleting? The dynamics of
Vol. 12: Industrial/organizational psychology (pp. 225–254). engagement in work and family roles. Administrative Science
New York: Wiley. Quarterly, 46, 655–684.

 work m otivation


Rousseau, D. M., Ho, V. T., & Greenberg, J. (2006). I-deals: Turner, A. N., & Lawrence, P. R. (1965). Industrial jobs and the
Idiosyncratic terms in employment relationships. Academy of worker. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press.
Management Review, 31, 977–994. Twenge, J. M., Campbell, S. M., Hoffman, B. J., & Lance, C. E.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self-determination theory (2010). Generational differences in work values: Leisure and
and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop- extrinsic values increasing, social and intrinsic values decreas-
ment, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78. ing. Journal of Management, 36, 1117–1142.
Rynes, S. L., Gerhart, B., & Minette, K. A. (2004). The impor- Van Eerde, W., & Thierry, H. (1996). Vroom’s expectancy mod-
tance of pay in employee motivation: Discrepancies between els and work-related criteria: A meta-analysis. Journal of
what people say and what they do. Human Resource Manage- Applied Psychology, 81, 575–586.
ment, 43, 381–394. Vogus, T. J., & Welbourne, T. M. (2003). Structuring for
Salancik, G. R., & Pfeffer, J. (1978). A social information pro- high reliability: HR practices and mindful processes in
cessing approach to job attitudes and task design. Adminis- reliability-seeking organizations. Journal of Organizational
trative Science Quarterly, 23, 224–253. Behavior, 24, 877–903.
Schweitzer, M. E., Ordoñez, L., & Douma, B. (2004). Goal set- Vroom, V. H. (1964). Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.
ting as a motivator of unethical behavior. Academy of Man- Waller, M. J., & Roberts, K. H. 2003. High reliability and orga-
agement Journal, 47, 422–432. nizational behavior: Finally the twain must meet. Journal of
Seo, M., Barrett, L. F., & Bartunek, J. M. (2004). The role of Organizational Behavior, 24, 813–814.
affective experience in work motivation. Academy of Manage- Weick, K. E. (1966). The concept of equity in the perception of
ment Review, 29, 423–439. pay. Administrative Science Quarterly, 11, 414–439.
Shalley, C. E., Zhou, J., & Oldham, G. R. (2004). The effects Weick, K. E. (1974). Middle range theories of social systems.
of personal and contextual characteristics on creativity: Behavioral Science, 19, 357–367.
Where should we go from here? Journal of Management, 30, Weick, K. E. (1979). The social psychology of organizing (2nd ed.).
933–958. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
Shamir, B. (1990). Calculations, values, and identities: The Weick, K. E., & Roberts, K. (1993). Collective mind in organi-
sources of collectivistic work motivation. Human Relations, zations: Heedful interrelating on flight decks. Administrative
43, 313–332. Science Quarterly, 38, 357–381.
Shamir, B. (1991). Meaning, self and motivation in organiza- Weick, K. E., Sutcliffe, K. M., & Obstfeld, D. (1999). Orga-
tions. Organization Studies, 12, 405–424. nizing for high reliability: Processes of collective mindful-
Siegel, P. A., & Hambrick, D. C. (2005). Pay disparities within ness. Research in Organizational Behavior, 21, 81–123.
top management groups: Evidence of harmful effects on per- Weick, K. E., Sutcliffe, K. M., & Obstfeld, D. (2005). Orga-
formance of high-technology firms. Organization Science, 16, nizing and the process of sensemaking. Organization Sci-
259-274. ence, 16, 409–421.
Spector, P. E., & Fox, S. (2010). Theorizing about the devi- Wilson, T. D., & Gilbert, D. T. (2005). Affective forecasting:
ant citizen: An attributional explanation of the inter- Knowing what to want. Current Directions in Psychological
play of organizational citizenship and counterproductive Science, 14, 131–134.
work behavior. Human Resource Management Review, 20, Wong, C., & Campion, M. A. (1991). Development and test
132–143. of a task level model of motivational job design. Journal of
Stajkovic, A. D., & Luthans, F. (2001). Differential effects of Applied Psychology, 76, 825–837.
incentive motivators on work performance. Academy of Man- Wrzesniewski, A., & Dutton, J. E. (2001). Crafting a job: Revi-
agement Journal, 44, 580–590. sioning employees as active crafters of their work. Academy of
Staw, B. M. (1984). Organizational behavior: A review and refor- Management Review, 26, 179–201.
mulation of the field’s outcome variables. Annual review of Wrzesniewski, A., McCauley, C. R., Rozin, P., & Schwartz, B.
Psychology, 35, 627–666. (1997). Jobs, careers, and callings: People’s relations to their
Steel, P., & König, C. J. (2006). Integrating theories of motiva- work. Journal of Research in Personality, 31, 21–33.
tion. Academy of Management Review, 31, 889–913. Xie, J. L., & Johns, G. (1995). Job scope and stress: Can job
Steers, R., Mowday, R., & Shapiro, D. (2004). The future of scope be too high? Academy of Management Journal, 38,
work motivation theory. Academy of Management Review, 29, 1288–1309.
379–387. Zalesny, M. D., & Ford, J. K. (1990). Extending the social infor-
Trist, E. L., & Bamforth, K. M. (1951). Some social and psycho- mation processing perspective: New links to attitudes, behav-
logical consequences of the longwall method of coal-getting. iors, and perceptions. Organizational Behavior and Human
Human Relations, 4, 3–38. Decision Processes, 47, 205–246.
Trzesniewski, K. H., & Donnellan, M. B. (2010). Rethinking Zohar, D., & Luria, G. (2003). Organizational meta-scripts as
“Generation Me”: A study of cohort effects from 1976–2006. a source of high reliability: The case of an army armored
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 58–75. brigade. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 24, 837–859.

g r a n t, s h i n 
CHAPTER

Youth Motivation and Participation in


29 Sport and Physical Activity

Maureen R. Weiss, Anthony J. Amorose, and Lindsay E. Kipp

Abstract
Sports and physical activities are ubiquitous achievement contexts for children and adolescents
worldwide. Motivation to initiate, continue, and sustain physical activity involvement is important for
ensuring positive developmental and healthy outcomes. In this chapter we synthesize and consolidate
theory-driven knowledge about determinants and outcomes of youth motivation and participation in
sport and physical activity. First, we discuss relevant theoretical frameworks for understanding youth
motivation in the physical activity domain. Second, we review empirical research on social-contextual
factors and individual differences related to youths’ desire to continue physical activity and opportunities
for accruing the many benefits from such participation. Finally, based on our review of literature, we
offer several avenues for future research that address gaps in the knowledge base about mechanisms of
influence on youths’ motivation and participation in physical activity.
Key Words: parental influence, coaching behaviors, teacher influence, peer acceptance, friendship,
self-esteem, perceived competence, enjoyment, anxiety, physical activity interventions

“Get up and play an hour a day!” The television sufficient physical activity to attain positive devel-
ad featuring LPGA (Ladies Professional Golf Asso- opmental outcomes.
ciation) players made it clear that children and ado- We define motivation in terms of because
lescents can glean physical, social, and psychological answers to why questions (Weiss & Amorose, 2008;
benefits from engaging in at least 1 hour of physical Weiss & Williams, 2004). In physical activity, such
activity per day (U.S. Department of Health and why questions include the following: (a) Why do
Human Services [USDHHS], 2008). Other sport some individuals regularly play sports or engage in
organizations have followed suit with catchy phrases physical activity, whereas others are not sufficiently
delivered by sport role models. The onslaught of active? (b) Why do some individuals participate
media attention has been fueled by efforts to stem for the inherent pleasure they get out of activ-
the obesity epidemic among youth, with a focus on ity, whereas others participate primarily to obtain
sufficient types and levels of physical activity cou- some external goal? (c) Why do some youth exert
pled with healthy eating patterns. Because of the maximal effort and persevere in physical activities,
frequent news reports, doesn’t everyone know youth whereas others give up easily or even discontinue
should be physically active to accrue health benefits? altogether? Answers to these questions begin with
But if everybody knows that regular physical activity the word because and depend, in part, on the theo-
is important, why isn’t this knowledge translated to retical framework adopted for understanding youth
actual behavior? One reason is that youth vary in physical activity motivation and participation. In
their motivation to initiate, continue, and sustain this chapter we review several theories and empirical


studies that sharpen our lens for answering these social acceptance or approval, and (c) to experience
why questions. But first we define the scope of our fun and enjoyment (Reeve & Weiss, 2006; Weiss
chapter in terms of physical activity contexts and & Williams, 2004). Second, empirical research
populations of interest. provides strong evidence that these theories, which
The term physical activity applies to a range of were originally developed for the academic domain,
structured and unstructured contexts (Weiss & are applicable to youth motivation and participa-
Wiese-Bjornstal, 2009). Structured activities include tion in physical activity. Finally, they are deemed
school and community sport programs, school “practical theories” of motivation (Gill & Williams,
physical education, after-school youth programs, 2008); that is, theoretical constructs and their rela-
and community classes (e.g., dance, martial arts). tionships are easily translated to behavioral interven-
Unstructured activities include recess, free play, rec- tions, which is essential for considering links among
reational activities, and active transport (e.g., riding physical activity motivation, participation behavior,
a bike to school). We focus our review on struc- and health-related outcomes (Reeve & Weiss, 2006;
tured activities for youth, namely organized sport Stuntz & Weiss, 2010).
and physical education, but we also consider cor-
relates of physical activity behavior in general. Sport Competence Motivation Theory
is a ubiquitous phenomenon among children and Susan Harter’s (1978) classic paper revitalized
youth worldwide (De Knop, Engstrom, Skirstad, Robert White’s (1959) concept of effectance moti-
& Weiss, 1996), and physical education is inclusive vation and set in motion her line of research that
of all children, regardless of skill level or external expanded and operationally defined theoretical con-
resources (Payne & Morrow, 2009). These settings structs. White argued that individuals are motivated
consist of social and contextual factors (e.g., adults to have an effect on their environment, and they
and peers, psychological climate) that are conducive subsequently engage in mastery attempts to develop
to modifying motivation and participation behav- or demonstrate competence. Such mastery attempts
ior. We review research on elementary-age children are influenced by the desire for challenge, curiosity,
through emerging adults (i.e., college students) and independence. If challenge-seeking behavior
because most studies in structured contexts have is successful, then feelings of efficacy and inherent
been conducted with these age groups. pleasure are experienced that serve to maintain or
We begin the chapter with an overview of theo- enhance effectance or competence motivation.
retical frameworks that are applicable to understand- Harter (1978, 1981a) revised White’s (1959)
ing youth motivation in physical activity. Next we original model in several ways (see Fig. 29.1). First,
discuss research on determinants and consequences she specified that competence motivation is domain
of physical activity motivation, with an eye toward specific. Children will differ in desire, curiosity, and
robust findings on social-environmental (parent, interest to demonstrate competence depending on
coach, peer influence) and individual difference fac- academic, physical, or social skills. In fact, Harter
tors (self-perceptions, emotions). We conclude with (1981b) explicitly identified sports and physical
suggesting several research directions based on our activities as a salient achievement domain. Second,
extensive review. In sum, physical activity is a very Harter contended that competence or success at
important context for understanding youth motiva- optimal challenges—difficult but realistic skills—is
tion in our efforts to maximize participation levels most likely to contribute to positive emotions and
and achieve positive benefits of an active lifestyle. intrinsic motivation. Third, Harter highlighted
significant adults and peers as central to developing
Theoretical Frameworks for Understanding children’s mastery motivation. She clarified that
Physical Activity Motivation1 socializers, especially parents, must reinforce inde-
We purposefully selected competence motiva- pendent mastery attempts and not just praise suc-
tion (Harter, 1978), self-determination (Deci & cessful outcomes. Fourth, Harter added perceptions
Ryan, 1985), achievement goal (Nicholls, 1989), of competence and control as individual factors that
and expectancy-value (Eccles et al., 1983) theories influence affective responses and competence moti-
for our comprehensive review. First, each theory vation. Fifth, Harter situated intrinsic pleasure or
highlights major reasons why youth are motivated positive affect in response to successful task mastery
to engage in physical activity: (a) to develop or in the center of her model. Harter’s call to “restore
demonstrate physical competence, (b) to attain affect and emotion to its rightful place, as central to

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


Competence
Motivation

Perceptions of
Competence and
Control
Mastery
Positive Affect Attempts

Success at
Optimal Challenges
Socializers’
Approval, Modeling,
Reinforcement

Fig. 29.1. Model of competence motivation theory. Reprinted, with permission, from Weiss, M. R., & Amorose, A. J. (2008). Motivational
orientation and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (3rd ed., p. 119). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

an understanding of behavior” (Harter, 1981b, p. 4) is a strong predictor of cognitive (e.g., self-esteem),


resonates with enjoyment being consistently named affective (e.g., enjoyment, anxiety), and behavioral
as a primary reason for youth physical activity moti- (e.g., effort, persistence) outcomes (e.g., Amorose,
vation (Weiss & Williams, 2004). 2001; Ebbeck & Weiss, 1998; Ferrer Caja & Weiss,
Within physical activity contexts, parents and 2000, 2002; Weiss, Bredemeier, & Shewchuk,
coaches have been frequently studied as important 1986). Because perceived competence is strongly
sources of competence motivation among chil- related to physical activity motivation, many stud-
dren and adolescents (see Weiss & Amorose, 2008; ies have also focused on identifying the informa-
Weiss & Williams, 2004). Mechanisms of paren- tion sources children and adolescents use to form
tal influence, such as providing positive feedback judgments about how physically competent they
for effort and improvement, showing confidence are (e.g., Horn & Weiss, 1991; Weiss & Amorose,
in their child’s potential, and modeling positive 2005; see Horn, 2004; Horn & Amorose, 1998).
attitudes and behaviors, are associated with youth Competence motivation is a multidimensional
reporting higher perceived competence, enjoyment, construct that is influenced by cognitive (perceived
and intrinsic motivation for physical activity (e.g., competence), affective (joy), and social (parents,
Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Bois, Sarrazin, Brustad, coaches) factors. Starting with the box labeled
Trouilloud, & Cury, 2002; Brustad, 1993). Simi- competence motivation in Figure 29.1, we see that
larly, youth report more favorable self-perceptions, a child’s desire to demonstrate competence will
affective responses, and motivational orientations lead her to attempt mastering physical skills. If
when they indicate coaches provide more frequent she is successful, and if significant adults and peers
informational feedback, respond positively to per- respond to her efforts with approval, she will expe-
formance attempts, and place greater emphasis on rience heightened perceived competence and control
a mastery climate (e.g., Black & Weiss, 1992; R.E. and positive affect such as joy, pride, and pleasure.
Smith, Smoll, & Cumming, 2007; Weiss, Amorose, & Positive self-perceptions and affective reactions will
Wilko, 2009). maintain or enhance competence motivation, or
In youth physical activity motivation research, the child’s desire to continue seeking challenge and
perceived competence is the most frequently stud- mastery in physical activities. Maintaining physical
ied individual difference factor (see Weiss & Amo- activity motivation, in turn, will optimize acquiring
rose, 2008; Weiss & Williams, 2004). Perceived the physical, social, and psychological benefits that
physical competence (or sport-specific competence) are afforded from such participation.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Self-Determination Theory is initiated and controlled by internal versus external
Self-determination theory, developed and refined sources) and the degree to which the behavior is regu-
by Deci and Ryan (1985, Ryan & Deci, 2000, lated autonomously (i.e., the extent to which action
2002), is a popular framework for understanding emanates from the self).
youth motivation in sport and physical activity. Self- Intrinsic motivation represents the most self-
determination theory is really a meta-theory com- determined form of behavioral regulation and is
prised of four related mini-theories (i.e., cognitive defined as engaging in an activity for the pleasure and
evaluation theory, organismic integration theory, satisfaction derived from the activity itself (Ryan &
causality orientation theory, and basic needs theory), Deci, 2000, 2007). When intrinsically motivated, indi-
which together identify and explain principles and viduals will freely engage in physical activity and expe-
processes for understanding motivation, personality rience a sense of enjoyment while doing so as opposed
and social development, and overall psychological to performing to obtain some separable outcome (e.g.,
functioning (see Ryan & Deci, 2000, 2002). Given to please others). The most non-self-determined form
the extensiveness of the theory, a comprehensive of motivation is amotivation, which refers to lack of
description is beyond the scope of this chapter (see intention to act and absence of motivated behavior,
Ryan & Deci, 2002, Weiss & Amorose, 2008, for resulting from devaluing an activity, feeling incompe-
reviews). Rather, our focus will be on elements of tent, or not perceiving contingency between actions
the overall theory that have been used to understand and desired outcomes (Ryan & Deci, 2007).
youth motivation in sport and physical activity. Extrinsic motivation, situated between amotivation
One of the main contributions of self-determination and intrinsic motivation on the self-determination
theory to understanding physical activity motivation continuum, involves engaging in an activity for
is distinguishing between various forms of behavioral some instrumental reason (Vallerand, 1997). Ryan
regulation. As mentioned, youth engage in sport and and colleagues (Ryan & Connell, 1989; Ryan &
physical activity to develop or demonstrate physical Deci, 2000, 2002) identified four specific types of
competence, gain social acceptance or approval, and extrinsic motivation that range on a continuum
enjoy one’s experiences (see Weiss & Amorose, 2008). from lower to higher levels of self-determined behav-
Self-determination theory categorizes each specific ior. The two forms of extrinsic motivation falling on
reason into one of three global types of motivation— the non-self-determined side include external and
intrinsic, extrinsic, and amotivation. Each form of introjected regulation. External regulation involves
motivation is situated along a continuum of self- behavior that is controlled by external means such
determination (see Fig. 29.2) insomuch as the motives as rewards or punishments, whereas introjected regu-
vary in terms of locus of causality (i.e., whether behavior lation refers to behavior regulated by internalized

Type of Motivation Amotivation Extrinsic Motivation Intrinsic Motivation

Type of Regulation non-regulation external introjected identified integrated intrinsic

Example Motive “I have nothing “because “I don’t “it will “it helps “I love the rush I feel
“I participate in sport better to do my par- want to help open to confirm when running down
because…” with myself.” ents are let others doors for my iden- the field.”
making down by my future tity as an
me.” quitting.” career as athlete”
a coach.”

Locus of Causality impersonal external somewhat some- internal internal


external what
internal

Degree of non-self- self-determined


Autonomy determined

Fig. 29.2. Continuum of motivation according to self-determination theory. Reprinted, with permission, from Weiss, M. R., & Amorose,
A. J. (2008). Motivational orientation and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (3rd ed., p. 133). Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


pressure such as worry or shame and/or a desire to behavior (Ryan & Deci, 2002). A central tenet of
enhance one’s ego. Identified and integrated regu- the theory is that functionally significant events—
lations represent self-determined or autonomous intrapersonal or social-contextual—that impact a
forms of extrinsic motivation. Identified regulation person’s sense of competence, autonomy, and relat-
involves behavior performed out of choice because edness will affect the type of motivation she or he
an individual values the activity or perceives ben- develops for an activity (i.e., self-determined or
efits from involvement. Integrated regulation reflects non-self-determined). The extent to which these
behavior that has been fully integrated into the self needs are fulfilled provides a mechanism by which
and assimilated with one’s values, goals, and needs intrapersonal events and social-contextual factors
(e.g., identity as a physically active person). affect motivated behavior and personal well-being
Youth will likely have multiple reasons, both (Ryan & Deci, 2002, 2007). Situational events (e.g.,
intrinsic and extrinsic, for their participation in sport coach/teacher styles, motivational climate) that
and physical activity (Weiss & Amorose, 2008). What satisfy psychological needs will promote optimal
is most critical is the extent to which the pattern of functioning, such as self-determined motivation,
motives tends to be more or less self-determined in while those that inhibit need satisfaction will lead to
nature. In other words, a central aspect of understand- nonoptimal outcomes in physical activity contexts
ing motivation from a self-determination perspective (e.g., Blanchard et al., 2009; Cox & Williams, 2008;
centers on whether one has a self-determined or Hollembeak & Amorose, 2005; Ntoumanis, 2005;
non-self-determined motivational orientation for an Standage et al., 2006).
activity. Numerous behavioral, cognitive, and affec- In summary, self-determination theory makes
tive benefits are associated with engaging in physical distinctions among the motives that underlie
activities for self-determined reasons (e.g., Blanchard, youths’ actions and specifies that behavioral regula-
Amiot, Perreault, Vallerand, & Provencher, 2009; tion varies along a continuum of self-determination.
Ferrer Caja & Weiss, 2000, 2002; Gagné, Ryan, & The theory also describes how social-contextual and
Bargmann, 2003; Ntoumanis, 2001, 2005; Pelletier, intrapersonal events can facilitate or thwart people’s
Fortier, Vallerand, & Briére, 2001; Standage, Duda, natural tendency toward psychological growth and
& Ntoumanis, 2006). As such, an important goal self-regulated behavior. Specifically, the extent to
of self-determination theory is to understand the which events impact one’s needs for competence,
processes by which youth develop self-determined autonomy, and relatedness will ultimately influence
motivation, particularly when the initial motive for motivational orientation, behavior, and psychologi-
an activity is not inherently enjoyable but rather cal functioning. A model summarizing theoretical
instrumental in nature (Ryan & Deci, 2002). While relationships is seen in Figure 29.3.
a number of interpersonal and social-contextual fac-
tors contribute to variations in motivational regula- Achievement Goal Theory
tions (e.g., coaching behaviors, motivational climate), The achievement goal perspective has been one
the theory highlights the central role of psychological of the more popular approaches to understanding
needs for competence, autonomy, and relatedness. youth motivation and participation in physical
Satisfaction of these needs is essential to maintaining activity over the past 25 years (Harwood, Spray,
and enhancing self-determined motivation. & Keegan, 2008; Roberts, Treasure, & Conroy,
The need for competence reflects a desire to per- 2007). This perspective is actually a cluster of
ceive our behavior and interaction with the social related yet distinct theories proffered by a num-
environment as effective (Deci, 1975; Harter, 1978, ber of scholars, notably Nicholls (1989), Dweck
White, 1959). The need for autonomy refers to per- (1999), Ames (1992), and Elliot (1999). As with
ceiving behaviors as freely chosen and that we are other theories (e.g., competence motivation, self-
the origins of our actions (deCharms, 1968; Deci determination, expectancy-value), perceptions of
& Ryan, 1987; Ryan & Connell, 1989). The need competence are considered a critical determinant
for relatedness reflects our desire to feel connected of motivated behavior. In fact, a goal of develop-
to others and experience a sense of belonging ing or demonstrating competence or avoiding the
(Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Ryan, 1995). Accord- demonstration of low competence is considered
ing to self-determination theory, the degree to the primary energizing force guiding engagement
which youths’ needs for competence, autonomy, and in achievement-related activities (Harwood et al.,
relatedness are supported will facilitate or obstruct 2008; Roberts et al., 2007). This approach also
their tendencies toward autonomous regulation of highlights the means by which youth construe

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


FUNCTIONALLY NEED MOTIVATIONAL
SIGNFICANT EVENTS SATISFACTION OUTCOMES

COMPETENCE
MOTIVATIONAL
ORIENTATION

INTRAPERSONAL
MOTIVATED
AUTONOMY BEHAVIOR
SOCIAL-
CONTEXTUAL
PSYCHOLOGICAL
FUNCTIONING AND
RELATEDNESS WELL-BEING

Fig. 29.3. Summary model of key relationships in self-determination theory. Reprinted, with permission, from Weiss, M. R., & Amorose,
A. J. (2008). Motivational orientation and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (3rd ed., p. 137). Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics.

competence-related beliefs. That is, understanding 2003, 2009). Nevertheless, the majority of scholarship
motivational orientations and behaviors requires in physical activity from an achievement goal perspec-
knowledge of the criteria used to judge whether tive has focused exclusively on task and ego goals.
performance attempts are successful or unsuccess- These subjective definitions of success and failure
ful—otherwise known as achievement goals. govern achievement-related beliefs and are the central
The labels used to distinguish between different influence guiding motivational processes in achieve-
subjective definitions of success and failure varies ment settings (Roberts et al., 2007). For instance,
across the specific theories; however, task and ego achievement goals combined with perceived compe-
goals are predominantly adopted in physical activity tence are predicted to influence motivated behavior
settings (Nicholls, 1989). People who adopt task- (Dweck, 1999; Nicholls, 1989). Specifically, youth
oriented goals—also referred to as mastery or learn- seek optimal challenges, exert high levels of effort,
ing goals—construe competence in self-referenced and persist even in the face of difficulty when their
terms. Experiencing success is rooted in exerting high achievement goal is task oriented regardless of their
levels of effort and learning and improving skills. perceived competence at the activity. This same
Ego-oriented goals—also referred to as performance adaptive motivational pattern would be expected
or outcome goals—are based in social comparison. for those who adopt an ego-oriented goal as long
Successful demonstration of competence is derived as they possess high expectations of success. Highly
from outperforming others at a task, especially if ego-oriented individuals who doubt their ability,
accomplished with relatively less effort. Achievement however, are expected to demonstrate a helpless
goals are presumed to function at both a dispositional motivational pattern such as avoiding challenge and
and state level. We can distinguish achievement goal demonstrating low effort and persistence. A num-
orientations, which reflect people’s dispositional ber of studies conducted in physical activity settings
proneness to define success and failure in specific provide support for these predictions (e.g., Cury,
ways, from people’s goal involvement, which reflect Biddle, Sarrazin, & Famose, 1997; Sarrazin, Rob-
the achievement goal adopted in a particular situa- erts, Cury, Biddle, & Famose, 2002).
tion (Harwood et al., 2008; Roberts et al., 2007). Achievement goal orientations have been linked to
In early writings, social goals were also considered as a a variety of motivational outcomes among youth (e.g.,
means of defining success, such as attaining acceptance level of perceived competence, motivational orienta-
and approval from significant adults or peers (Maehr & tion, enjoyment and interest, burnout, effort, and per-
Nicholls, 1980). This goal faded away in the literature formance) (see Harwood et al., 2008; Roberts et al.,
for awhile, but heeding the call of Urdan and Maehr 2007; Weiss & Ferrer Caja, 2002). Positive outcomes
(1995) to revive social goals, researchers have included are associated with adopting higher task-oriented
social along with task and ego goals in studies of youth goals, either alone or in combination with a higher
participants’ psychosocial and behavioral outcomes in ego orientation. Conversely, negative motivational
physical activity (e.g., Allen, 2003; Stuntz & Weiss, outcomes are associated with a higher ego orientation,
wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 
especially when paired with a lower task orientation. distinguishing mastery and performance climates
Recent work also shows that positive motivational include the following: (a) the nature and design of
outcomes are associated with the adoption of social tasks, (b) who is given authority for making deci-
goals (e.g., Stuntz & Weiss, 2009). sions, (c) how participants are evaluated and recog-
Given the critical motivational implications of nized, (d) how much time is allotted for learning
achievement goals, a host of intrapersonal and social- skills, and (e) how people are grouped (see Ames,
contextual factors impact adoption of achievement 1992; Duda & Balaguer, 2007; Harwood et al.,
goals, such as cognitive development, implicit theo- 2008; Ntoumanis & Biddle, 1999). Ames asserts
ries of ability, and motivational climate (see Elliot, that individuals’ perceptions of the motivational cli-
1999; Roberts et al., 2007). Because motivational mate is the key factor driving motivation; that is, the
climate is especially relevant for the physical activity subjective meaning attached to significant others’
context, we focus our remaining discussion on this behaviors within the environment will ultimately
construct. According to Ames (1992), motivational affect achievement goals and patterns of motivated
climate refers to how success is defined in a social behavior.
environment such as a physical education class or In summary, situational (e.g., motivational cli-
athletic team. Two primary climates, which paral- mate) and intrapersonal variations (e.g., goal ori-
lel the two main achievement goals, are presumed entation, perceived competence) in how success
to function in achievement settings. A mastery (also and failure are defined are central to understanding
called task-involving) motivational climate describes motivational processes and outcomes according to
an environment that defines success in terms of achievement goal theory. A summary model identi-
learning, improvement, and effort. In contrast, fying some of the key motivational constructs and
a performance (also called ego-involving) motivational their relationships is presented in Figure 29.4.
climate emphasizes competition, winning, and out-
performing others as the basis for defining success Expectancy-Value Theory
(see Duda & Balaguer, 2007; Harwood et al., 2008; Eccles et al.’s (1983) expectancy-value theory
Roberts et al., 2007). describes and explains variations in achievement choices
The prevailing motivational climate will be a and behaviors across multiple domains. Achievement
function of various factors. For instance, dimensions motivation is predicted directly by an individual’s

ACHIEVEMENT ACHEIVEMENT-RELATED
ANTECEDENTS GOALS OUTCOMES

SOCIAL-
CONTEXTUAL
socialization processes
motivational climate BEHAVIORS
choice, effort, persistence
learning and performance
MASTERY (TASK)-
INVOLVED

PERFORMANCE (EGO)- COGNITIONS AND AFFECTIVE


INVOLVED RESPONSES
positive affect (enjoyment, interest)
negative affect (anxiety, boredom)
INTRAPERSONAL perceived competence
goal orientations motivational orientations
perceived competence beliefs about causes of success
cognitive development beliefs about the purposes of sport
implicit theories of
ability
need for achievement
fear of failure

Fig. 29.4. Summary model of the achievement goal perspective.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


expectations of success and subjective task values, and three positive task values (attainment, interest, util-
it is indirectly influenced by socializers’ beliefs and ity), but cost of participating in especially high-level
behaviors; gender and activity stereotypes; children’s sport is a salient factor explaining variations in moti-
schema, goals, and interests; and past achievement vated behavior (e.g., W.M. Weiss & Weiss, 2003,
experiences, among other influences (see Fig. 29.5). 2006, 2007; W.M. Weiss, Weiss, & Amorose, 2010).
Expectancy-value theory has been an especially pro- Consistent with theoretical predictions, expectancies
ductive approach for investigating parental influence and values are strongly related to youths’ achievement
on youths’ physical activity beliefs and behaviors (see behaviors in physical activity (e.g., Cox & Whaley,
Fredricks & Eccles, 2004; Weiss & Amorose, 2008). 2004; Fredricks & Eccles, 2005).
Expectation of success is defined as the individual’s Expectancy-value theory originated as a means of
belief in successfully completing a task or mastering explaining gender differences in achievement choices
an activity, and thus it is synonymous with the con- and behaviors (Eccles et al., 1983). Eccles and col-
struct of perceived competence (Eccles et al., 1983). leagues conducted several studies that revealed
Subjective task value generally refers to the impor- variations in physical activity motivation among
tance placed on being successful in an achievement males and females that were linked to expectations
domain. Eccles and colleagues (1983) identified four of success and subjective task values (e.g., Eccles
components of subjective task value: attainment value, et al., 1983; Eccles & Harold, 1991; Fredricks &
interest value, utility value, and cost. Attainment value Eccles, 2002, 2005). Girls reported lower expec-
refers to personal importance of doing well in a certain tancies, value, and participation in physical
achievement domain that confirms one’s self-identity, activities and sports than boys. In addition to differ-
such as opportunities for demonstrating or developing ential expectancies and task values, gender differences
competence. Interest value refers to how much youth in expectancy-value constructs are also linked to par-
enjoy participating in the activity (i.e., how intrinsi- ents’ beliefs and behaviors about the value of various
cally rewarding is the activity?). Utility value describes achievement domains.
one’s perception of how useful the activity is relative Fredricks and Eccles (2004) classified three mecha-
to short- or long-term goals (i.e., extrinsic value of the nisms of parental influence: (a) providers of experience
activity). Finally, cost refers to one’s appraisal of the (e.g., tangible support, encouragement), (b) interpret-
time, effort, and other resources that would be lost ers of experience (e.g., conveying beliefs about child’s
by engaging in a particular achievement activity. Most domain-specific competence, goal orientation, or how
research by Eccles and colleagues has emphasized the success is defined), and (c) role models (e.g., expressing

Gender & Expectations of


Activity
Success
Stereotypes

Child’s Perceptions
& Interpretations
of Experiences

Achievement
Socializers’ Beliefs Behaviors
and Behaviors

Child’s
Self-Schema
and Goals
Subjective Task
Past Achievement Value
Experiences

Across Time

Fig. 29.5. Relationships in expectancy-value theory. Reprinted, with permission, from Weiss, M. R., & Amorose, A. J. (2008). Motivational
orientation and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (3rd ed., p. 139). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. [Previ-
ously adapted, with permission of Taylor & Francis, Inc., from Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R.D. (1991). Gender differences in sport involvement:
Applying Eccles’ expectancy-value model, Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 32, 7–35.]

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


attitudes and demonstrating behaviors that endorse parent–child relationship for expectancies, task values,
value of a domain). In several studies, youths’ percep- and motivation is bidirectional and reciprocal. This
tions of their parents’ beliefs about participating and latter aspect has not been studied as frequently as the
performing well in sport, and parents’ importance parent-to-child pathway but denotes a particularly rel-
ratings for boys and girls being good in sports, were evant area to pursue in the physical activity domain.
related to youths’ self-judgments of sport competence
(e.g., Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Bois et al., 2002; Fre- Research on Youth Motivation and
dricks & Eccles, 2002; Sabiston & Crocker, 2008). Participation in Physical Activity
Because of strong linkages between parental The four theoretical frameworks highlighted for
beliefs and behaviors with youths’ expectations of explaining youth physical activity motivation pos-
success, task values, and achievement behaviors, sess two common threads of similarity. First, they
Eccles situated expectancy-value constructs within identify social and contextual factors that influence
a comprehensive model of parental influence motivational orientations and behaviors, including
(Eccles, Wigfield, & Schiefele, 1998; Fredricks & parents’ beliefs and behaviors, peer influence, and
Eccles, 2004). Compatible with the original theory, coach–athlete relationships. Second they recognize
this model (see Fig. 29.6) accentuates the role of key individual differences that directly or indirectly
family characteristics (e.g., culture, siblings), general impact participation motivation—notably perceived
parental beliefs and behaviors (e.g., gender stereo- competence and affective responses. In this section
types, parenting styles), and parents’ child-specific we review research on social-contextual factors and
beliefs and behaviors on children’s achievement out- individual differences as they relate to youths’ physi-
comes (perceived competence, task values, partici- cal activity motivation and behavior. We report
pation behavior). Parent–child relationships among findings that cut across theoretical frameworks in
competence beliefs, task values, and achievement an effort to be parsimonious and systematic.
behavior as specified by Eccles’ theory have been
Social-Contextual Factors
a focus of research in physical activity contexts (see
This section is divided into parental influence,
Horn & Horn, 2007; Weiss & Amorose, 2008).
coach–athlete relationships, and peer relationships.
In sum, expectancy-value theory identifies social
We identify robust findings on these topics, thereby
and contextual factors that influence youths’ expectan-
revealing consistent mechanisms of influence on youth
cies of success and subjective task values that, in turn,
motivation and participation in physical activity.
relate to domain-specific achievement behaviors. It
should be noted that the developmental emphasis of parental influence on youth physical
this theory suggests that children’s achievement behav- activity motivation
iors (e.g., participation, performance) can, in turn, In 1992, Brustad published an influential paper in
modify socializers’ beliefs and behaviors. That is, the which he advocated using appropriate theory-driven

Parent, Family, Parents’ General Parent-Specific


Neighborhood Beliefs & Behavior Behaviors
Characteristics - encouragement
- stereotypes
- education - parenting styles - provision of
- culture experiences

Child & Sibling Parents’ Child- Child Outcomes


Characteristics Specific Beliefs - self-perceptions
- gender - expectations - personal values
- birth order - competency - activity choice

Figure 29.6. Model of parental influence (Eccles et al., 1998). Reprinted, with permission, from Weiss, M. R., & Amorose, A. J.
2008. Motivational orientation and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (3rd ed., p. 144). Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics. [Previously adapted, with permission of John Wiley & Sons, from Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998).
Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and
personality development (5th ed., pp. 1017–1095).]

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


approaches to integrate socialization and motiva- Smith, 2006, 2009; M.R. Weiss & Hayashi, 1995;
tional factors into the study of youth in sport. He W.M. Weiss & Weiss, 2003, 2006). For example,
lamented that few studies delved into parents as Ullrich-French and Smith (2006, 2009) investigated
sources of motivation, and that existing descriptive parent–child relationship quality and motivational
studies were limited compared to explanatory theo- constructs among youth soccer players. In the first
ries that would reveal points of intervention. Now study they found that stronger mother–child and
20 years later, the literature abounds with theory- father–child relationship quality, defined as emo-
driven studies investigating mechanisms of parental tional, loyalty, intimacy, and companionship sup-
influence on children’s motivational orientations. port, was positively related to children’s perceived
We organized empirical research on parental influ- competence, enjoyment, and self-determined moti-
ence and children’s psychosocial and behavioral out- vation. One year later, mother–child relationship
comes along Eccles et al.’s (1983, 1998) notion of quality (along with perceived competence and close
parents as providers of experience, as role models, and friendships) distinguished participants who contin-
as interpreters of experience. ued versus discontinued their soccer involvement.
Within these categories, we discuss specific W.M. Weiss and Weiss (2003) investigated sources
parental behaviors that relate to children’s physi- of attraction- and entrapment-based commitment
cal activity motivation. These include (a) providing to participate among adolescent gymnasts. In the
social support, (b) modeling attitudes and behaviors, first study, gymnasts who showed attraction-based
(c) expressing beliefs about the child’s competence, commitment (high enjoyment and perceived ben-
(d) expressing beliefs about the value of physical efits, low perceived costs) reported higher emotional
activity, and (e) conveying pressure to perform or be support from parents than entrapped gymnasts (low
successful (see Garcia Bengoechea & Strean, 2007). enjoyment and perceived benefits, high perceived
Other reviews provide additional perspectives on costs) as well as higher intrinsic motivation and
the role of the family in youth physical activity greater effort and persistence in the gym.
(Brustad, 2010; Fredricks & Eccles, 2004; Horn &
Horn, 2007). Modeling Attitudes and Behaviors
Modeling or observational learning is a powerful
Providing Social Support mechanism of transmitting attitudes and behaviors
Forms of social support include logistical, emo- in the physical activity domain (see McCullagh &
tional, intimacy, affection, companionship, and Weiss, 2002). As role models, parents communicate
instrumental support (Furman & Buhrmester, through words and actions how valuable they believe
1985; Garcia Bengoechea & Strean, 2007). Logisti- it is for their child to be successful in the physical
cal support includes signing children up for sports domain. Some authors have assessed modeling
teams and transporting them to practices and effects as a correlation between parents’ and child’s
games. Emotional support refers to encouragement level of physical activity (e.g., Dempsey, Kimiecik,
for playing a sport and giving one’s best effort and & Horn, 1993; Fredricks & Eccles, 2002; Freed-
responding positively to mastery attempts. Instru- son & Evenson, 1991), but observational learning
mental support includes teaching children sport effects go beyond simple correspondence of activ-
skills and being actively involved in their experi- ity levels to parents’ specific expressions of attitudes
ences, while intimacy and affection support refer and behaviors. Parents’ enjoyment of doing physical
to unconditional warmth, admiration, and respect activities, comments made about the importance
between parent and child. Companionship support and utility of physical activity as a healthy behav-
might include parents and children doing physical ior, and past or present involvement as an athlete
activities and attending sporting events together. or coach exemplify ways in which parents serve as
In line with parents as providers of experience, physical activity models for their children (e.g., Bab-
mothers and fathers who show greater social support kes & Weiss, 1999; Bois, Sarrazin, Brustad, Troul-
for their child’s activity involvement are associated liard, & Cury, 2005; Brustad, 1993; Davison et al.,
with youth reporting favorable ability perceptions, 2003; Davison & Jago, 2009; Weiss & Fretwell,
value toward physical activity, and emotional expe- 2005). The diverse ways in which modeling has
riences, and greater intrinsic motivation and physi- been defined and assessed may explain why some
cal activity behavior (e.g., Bhalla & Weiss, 2010; authors conclude that equivocal support exists for
Brustad, 1993; Davison, Cutting, & Birch, 2003; parents as role models of children’s physical activity
Sabiston & Crocker, 2008; Ullrich-French & behavior (Fredricks & Eccles, 2004).

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


In reality, it is difficult to separate parent behav- Expressing Beliefs About Child’s Competence in
iors neatly into categories of providers, interpreters, Physical Activity
or models of physical activity experiences. When As interpreters of experience, parents communi-
parents facilitate participation opportunities and cate directly or indirectly how confident they are in
encourage children to continue involvement (i.e., their child’s potential to be successful in sport and
provide experiences), or express confidence about how physically talented they believe their child to
their child’s ability to be successful and discuss the be. Parents’ appraisals of their child’s ability influ-
importance of being physically active to stay fit ence children’s perceptions of their parents’ ability
(i.e., interpret experiences), we would argue that beliefs, which ultimately affect their self-appraisals
parents are modeling attitudes and behaviors they of ability (e.g., Amorose, 2002; Bois, Sarrazin,
want their child to emulate. That is, parents are Brustad, Chanal, & Trouilloud, 2005). By far, par-
communicating beliefs and exhibiting behaviors ents’ expression of beliefs about their child’s compe-
that provide children with information and moti- tence is one of the most robust findings related to
vation to embrace physical activity as an integral children’s self-competence beliefs and participation
part of their lifestyle. In line with this theorizing, motivation (e.g., Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Bois et al.,
some researchers have included social support, role 2002; Fredricks & Eccles, 2002, 2005; Kimiecik &
modeling, and competence/value beliefs as indices Horn, 1998; Kimiecik, Horn, & Shurin, 1996). In
of a broader parental influence construct (Davison, these studies, youths’ perceptions of parents’ beliefs
Symons Downs, & Birch, 2006; Fredricks & Eccles, about their sport or fitness competence were strongly
2005; Sabiston & Crocker, 2008). For example, related to their own self-reported ability ratings and
Sabiston and Crocker tested a model of relation- physical activity behavior.
ships among social influence (parents, best friend), Some studies show that parents hold gender-
expectancy-value constructs, and physical activity stereotyped beliefs about children’s competence.
behavior. They situated emotional support, value Jacobs and Eccles (1992) found that mothers who
beliefs, and role modeling as observed indicators of held stronger gender-stereotyped beliefs assigned
a latent construct of parental influence, and they boys higher ability ratings for math and sports
examined associations with adolescent girls’ and and girls higher ratings in social activities. Moth-
boys’ perceived competence, subjective task values, ers’ appraisals, in turn, predicted children’s self-
and physical activity. Strong support emerged for an perceptions of ability in these domains. Fredricks
indirect effect of parents’ beliefs and behaviors on and Eccles (2005) examined parent-child beliefs
youths’ physical activity as mediated by perceived and behaviors in sport among children in grades 2,
competence and task values. 4, and 5. A total support score was based on parents’
Studies that operationally defined parent model- beliefs (e.g., child’s sport ability, value of sport) and
ing in more inclusive terms (e.g., show enjoyment behaviors (e.g., encouragement, time involved in
of physical activity, participate in activities with sports). Boys recorded a greater number of family
children, be a parent-coach, have athletic experi- supports, which were positively associated with chil-
ence) provide substantive support for parents as dren’s perceptions of ability, value toward sport, and
influential role models. For example, Davison and time spent in sports. It should be noted that other
Jago (2009) assessed modeling effects in terms of studies did not find differential parent beliefs and
parent-reported enjoyment of physical activity, fre- behaviors for sons and daughters (Babkes & Weiss,
quency of physical activity, family using sport as 1999; Bois, Sarrazin, Brustad, Trouilloud, & Cury,
recreation, and use of own behavior to encourage 2005; Kimiecik & Horn, 1998; Sabiston & Crocker,
activity. In a longitudinal investigation, they found 2008). Factors that distinguish when and why gen-
that girls who maintained physical activity levels der differences in physical activity motivation occur
from ages 9 to 15 had parents who reported higher are important directions for future research.
scores on these facets of modeling than girls who The finding that parents’ and children’s compe-
did not maintain physical activity levels. Thus, even tence beliefs are interrelated is typically interpreted
though parent modeling has been assessed using as a parent-to-child pathway. An alternative expla-
varied definitions (level of physical activity vs. other nation is that the pathway is bidirectional or recip-
behaviors and attitudes) and methods (objective, rocal (e.g., Davison et al., 2006; Dorsch, Smith, &
parent report, child report), ample evidence exists McDonough, 2009; Eccles et al., 1998; M.R. Weiss &
to support modeling as a salient mechanism of Hayashi, 1995). Children showing interest, talent,
parental influence. and confidence in sport may spark parents’ interest

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


in their child’s participation, influence competence and useful (or not). Among the sources that
beliefs, and inspire them to invest time facilitating emerged were parents’ positive or negative influ-
their child’s experiences. Davison et al. tested two ences—participants with high sport value said
models linking parental support, child’s perceived they were sparked by their parents’ interest in sport
athletic competence, and child’s physical activity. (“grew up around sports”), whereas the low-value
The “traditional” pathway specified that parental group spoke about unsupportive parents, parents’
support at age 9 predicts perceived competence at lack of sport experiences, and parents not provid-
age 11, which predicts physical activity at age 11. ing choices. Bhalla and Weiss (2010) interviewed
The “child elicitation” pathway specified that per- adolescent girls of Anglo and East Indian ethnicity
ceived competence at age 9 predicts parental sup- about parents’ value toward sport. Although Anglo
port at age 11, which predicts physical activity at age parents were seen as placing greater value on sports
11. Path analyses and tests of mediation provided than East Indian parents, themes such as “bring
support for the model in which child characteristics honor to the family” (i.e., family is recognized when
elicit a response from parents (i.e., social support for child achieves in sport), “proud of accomplish-
sport participation) that, in turn, impacts the child’s ments” (i.e., telling others that sport achievement
physical activity behavior. Dorsch et al. also found is special), and “change routine” (i.e., makes adjust-
support for bidirectional socialization influences in ment to schedule to pick up daughter from practice)
sport. Focus group responses by youth sport parents suggest that East Indian parents modify their value
revealed changes in cognitions (knowledge about beliefs as part of the acculturation process, and these
sport), behaviors (physical activity), and emo- beliefs made a positive impact on their daughters’
tions (pride, anxiety) as a result of being socialized sport participation.
through their child’s participation.
In sum, evidence exists for multiple pathways in Conveying Pressure to Perform or Be Successful
the parent–child socialization process. Children’s Social sources of stress and burnout have long
perceptions of parents’ competence beliefs influ- been topics of interest in youth sport (see Crocker,
ence their own self-beliefs and motivated behavior, Hoar, McDonough, Kowalski, & Niefer, 2004).
and children’s perceptions of physical competence Several studies have shown that young athletes
and skill-related behavior influence parents’ percep- worry about fear of failure and negative evaluations
tions of the child’s competence and their behaviors from parents (e.g., Gould, Horn, & Spreeman,
toward their child. 1983; Scanlan & Lewthwaite, 1984; Weiss, Wiese,
& Klint, 1989). For example, Weiss et al. found
Expressing Beliefs About the Value of Physical Activity that youth gymnasts identified parents’ expectations
Parallel with parents as interpreters of experience about performance as a frequent source of worry
by conveying beliefs about their child’s sport com- prior to competition, such as “what my parents will
petence, parents also express thoughts and feelings say” and “letting my parents down.”
about the value they place on being successful in Parents can unknowingly place pressure or unre-
physical activity domains. Similar to the parent– alistic expectations on youth to perform and be suc-
child linkages in beliefs about sport competence, so cessful in sport. This is a fine line—parents might
too is the connection between parents’ importance think and say they are being “supportive” while
and utility value of physical activity with children’s children interpret the same behaviors as “pressure”
self-reported task values and participation behavior (Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Fredricks & Eccles, 2004;
(e.g., Bhalla & Weiss, 2010; Fredricks & Eccles, Gould, Udry, Tuffey, & Loehr, 1996). Because
2002, 2005; Kimiecik & Horn, 1998; Kimiecik youths’ interpretations of parents’ behaviors are the
et al., 1996; Stuart, 2003). In the Fredricks and important factor explaining behavioral outcomes,
Eccles (2002, 2005) studies, children’s perceptions parents’ verbal and nonverbal actions that are trans-
of the value their parents place on being successful lated by youth as pressuring should have negative
in sport were related to their own ratings of impor- psychosocial and behavioral consequences.
tance and utility value as well as time spent in sports Indeed, many studies show a negative relation-
activities. ship between perceived parental pressure and youths’
Using an innovative design, Stuart (2003) enjoyment, motivation, and participation behav-
divided adolescent boys and girls into low and high ior (e.g., Babkes & Weiss, 1999; Bois, Lalanne, &
sport value groups, then interviewed them about Delforge, 2009; Brustad, 1988; Sagar & Lavallee,
why they thought sport was interesting, important, 2010; Scanlan & Lewthwaite, 1984; W.M. Weiss &

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


Weiss, 2007). For example, Bois et al. found that Feedback Patterns
parents’ presence at competitive events was perceived All the theories we highlighted either implicitly
as pressure by adolescent tennis and basketball play- or explicitly suggest that performance-related feed-
ers, resulting in elevated precompetitive anxiety lev- back from significant others will influence perform-
els. If such anxiety levels persist, we would expect ers’ motivation. Studies in sport and physical activity
lower motivation to continue sport. Sagar and settings have explored various dimensions of feed-
Lavallee interviewed adolescent athletes and their back—mostly focusing on the motivational implica-
parents about contributors to fear of failure. Themes tions of the content and frequency of feedback (Horn,
included parents’ use of punitive behavior (e.g., crit- 2008). In general, coaches/teachers who provide
icism), controlling behavior (e.g., attending train- frequent performance-contingent praise and techni-
ings and competitions), and high expectations (e.g., cal information should be associated with positive
reaching top national ranking). Chronic exposure to motivational outcomes among youth participants
such negative parent behaviors is likely to result in (e.g., higher perceived competence, self-esteem, and
heightened stress and dropping out of sport. self-determined motivation), whereas criticizing
athletes/students or ignoring their performance alto-
coach/teacher influence on youth gether should have the opposite effect (see Amorose,
motivation and participation in 2007; Hein & Koka, 2007; Horn, 2008).
physical activity An illustrative example of this type of work comes
At all competitive levels—from youth to col- from the systematic line of research by R.E. Smith,
legiate sport—coaches can significantly impact Smoll, and colleagues (see Smith & Smoll, 2007;
the behaviors, cognitions, and affective responses Smoll & Smith, 2002). Based on extensive observa-
of their athletes (see Amorose, 2007; Chelladurai, tions of youth sport coaches, Smith and Smoll noted
2007; Horn, 2008; Mageau & Vallerand, 2003; the common types of feedback coaches use in prac-
Smoll & Smith, 2002). The way in which coaches tices and games and examined the degree to which
structure practices and games, make decisions, pro- different types of feedback were related to partici-
vide quality and quantity of feedback in response to pants’ psychosocial and behavioral outcomes. Using
performances, establish relationships with athletes, this information, they developed what they termed
and use techniques to motivate players have impor- the “positive approach” to coaching—meaning high
tant implications for psychological development frequencies of reinforcement for effort and good
and motivational orientations. While the physical performances, encouragement following errors, and
education context is different than sport (e.g., stu- mistake-contingent instruction, while at the same time
dents are more variable in ability; focus is on skill minimizing punitive behaviors and nonresponses. In
development, not outcomes), teachers function in a a number of field-based experiments, Smith, Smoll,
similar role as coaches—they structure the setting, and colleagues found that coaches who were trained
use teaching styles, give informational and motiva- to and actually engaged in these behaviors had play-
tional feedback, and form teacher–student relation- ers who reported higher self-esteem, enjoyment, and
ships. Thus, teachers’ instructional behaviors and intentions of returning the following season, and lower
interpersonal interactions also contribute impor- anxiety and attrition rates (e.g., Barnett, Smoll, &
tant motivational consequences for their students Smith, 1992; Smith, Smoll, & Barnett, 1995; Smoll,
(see Biddle, 2001; Hein & Koka, 2007; Standage, Smith, Barnett, & Everett, 1993).
Gillison, & Treasure, 2007). Smith and Smoll’s scholarship stimulated con-
We overview key findings from research explor- siderable research on coaching behaviors and youth
ing the motivational influences of coaches and teach- motivation (e.g., Allen & Howe, 1998; Amorose &
ers. The review is broken down by mechanisms of Horn, 2000; Black & Weiss, 1992; Coatsworth &
coaching/teaching influence: (a) feedback patterns, Conroy, 2006) and to a lesser extent teacher behaviors
(b) general leadership styles, (c) motivational cli- in physical education (e.g., Nicaise, Bois, Fairclough,
mate, and (d) autonomy-supportive versus control- Amorose, & Cogérino, 2007; Nicaise, Cogérino,
ling behaviors. Other important aspects of teaching Bois, & Amorose, 2006). In general, results of
and coaching behavior are motivationally relevant these studies support the motivational benefits of
(e.g., see Jowett & Poczwardowski, 2007); however, adopting a positive approach to providing feedback.
the behavioral mechanisms we review are the ones Despite the intuitive implications of these find-
most systematically examined in youth sport and ings (e.g., coaches and teachers should provide lots
physical activity (Amorose, 2007; Horn, 2008). of praise and encouragement and avoid criticism),

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


it is important to acknowledge that providing effec- reported lower levels of autonomy, relatedness, and
tive feedback is considerably more complex. For intrinsic motivation.
instance, providing seemingly effective feedback, A recent study in the physical education setting
such as praise and technical instruction, may actu- by Koka and Hagger (2010) shows that general
ally lead to negative motivational outcomes if the leadership behaviors of secondary school teach-
feedback is not given contingent or appropriate ers are related to students’ motivation. Specifi-
to performance attempts, is given in a controlling cally, these researchers found that teachers’ positive
or demeaning manner, and is insincere or conde- feedback predicted satisfaction of competence,
scending. Similarly, negative motivational effects are autonomy, and relatedness needs and indirectly
likely when feedback provided to athletes suggests predicted students’ self-determined motivation.
unrealistic performance expectations and promotes Higher autocratic behavior and negative nonverbal
ego involvement (see Henderlong & Lepper, 2002; feedback predicted lower self-determined motiva-
Horn, 2008; Mageau & Vallerand, 2003). tion, whereas frequency of instruction and a teach-
ing style that provided for situational considerations
General Leadership Styles (e.g., accounting for students’ abilities when set-
Studies also provide support for the motiva- ting goals) showed positive relationships with self-
tional influence of general leadership styles exhib- determination constructs. Finally, higher autonomy
ited by coaches and teachers (see Amorose, 2007; need satisfaction was predicted by greater use of
Chelladurai, 2007, Horn, 2008). This research, a democratic teaching style, whereas higher compe-
conducted mainly with coaches, has focused pri- tence need satisfaction was related to less negative
marily on leadership styles assessed with the Leader- nonverbal feedback.
ship Scale for Sport (Chelladurai & Saleh, 1980). Overall, research shows that perceived leadership
With this measure, participants indicate their per- styles exhibited by coaches and teachers have impor-
ceptions of the coach’s general decision-making tant motivational implications (see Amorose, 2007;
style (democratic and autocratic), motivational ten- Chelladurai, 2007; Horn, 2008). From a practical
dencies (social support and positive feedback), and standpoint, getting teachers and coaches to pro-
instructional tendencies (training and instruction). vide social support and positive feedback, engage in
In general, positive athlete outcomes are associated high levels of training and instruction, and adopt
with each of these dimensions, with the excep- a democratic decision-making style should facilitate
tion of autocratic behavior (see Amorose, 2007; self-determined forms of motivation. It should be
Chelladurai, 2007; Horn, 2008). For example, Price noted, however, that these recommendations may
and Weiss (2000) found that adolescent female soc- be too simplistic. For instance, researchers contend
cer players who rated their coaches higher in training that the most effective leadership styles will depend
and instruction, social support, positive feedback, on other factors such as characteristics and prefer-
and democratic decisions, and lower in autocratic ences of athletes/students and the situation or con-
behavior, reported more positive (i.e., perceived text in which coaches/teachers and athletes/students
competence, enjoyment) and fewer negative (i.e., are interacting (e.g., level of competition, type of
anxiety, burnout) motivational outcomes. sport, practice versus game context) (Amorose,
A series of studies by Amorose and colleagues 2007; Chelladurai, 2007; Horn, 2008).
(Amorose & Horn, 2000, 2001; Hollembeak &
Amorose, 2005) linked these leadership styles with Motivational Climate
college athletes’ intrinsic motivation. For instance, The way coaches and teachers structure learning
Hollembeak and Amorose found that the various experiences, how they provide feedback and give
dimensions, with the exception of social support, recognition, and strategies they adopt for group-
were predictive of athletes’ perceptions of compe- ing athletes or students help to establish what has
tence, autonomy, and relatedness, which in turn been referred to as the motivational climate (Ames,
predicted athletes’ level of intrinsic motivation. 1992). The climate refers to how success and failure
The motivational effect was particularly strong for are defined and emphasized in the social environ-
coaches’ decision-making style—athletes perceiv- ment (Duda & Balaguer, 2007). A mastery (task-
ing their coaches as more democratic in decision- involved) motivational climate focuses on learning,
making style reported higher levels of autonomy improvement, and effort as ways of conceptualizing
and intrinsic motivation, while athletes who success. Behaviors exhibited by coaches and teach-
perceived coaches higher in autocratic behavior ers who create this type of climate will provide

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


optimally challenging and meaningful learning and enjoyment of physical education and negatively
activities, reward and encourage progress toward related to boredom. A performance climate, on the
individualized goals, evaluate performance based other hand, was positively associated with an ego
on effort and skill improvement, and promote goal orientation and boredom. The positive effect
cooperation among group members. Conversely, of a mastery compared to a performance climate
a performance (ego-involved) motivational climate was also supported by Papaioannou, Marsh, and
describes an environment where the focus is on Theodorakis (2004), who found that motivational
defining success and failure in terms of favorable climate at the beginning of a school year was pre-
comparison to others and other norm-referenced dictive of goal orientations, enjoyment, effort, per-
criteria. In this climate, coaches and teachers rein- ceived control, physical self-concept, and exercise
force players/students for demonstrating superior intentions at the end of the year.
performance relative to others, use punishment Because motivational climate contributes sig-
for making mistakes, and encourage intrateam nificantly to variations in affective, cognitive, and
rivalry as a means to achieve norm-referenced goals behavioral outcomes in sport and physical activity,
(Newton, Duda, & Yin, 2000). coaches and teachers should engage in instructional
Considerable research has demonstrated that practices and interpersonal behaviors that emphasize
the climate created by teachers and coaches carries a mastery relative to a performance climate (Duda &
motivational significance for youth participants Treasure, 2010; Standage et al., 2007). This is more
(Biddle, 2001; Duda & Balaguer, 2007; Harwood easily accomplished in physical education com-
et al., 2008; Horn, 2008; Roberts et al., 2007). For pared to competitive sport settings, and thus more
instance, youth who report a higher mastery cli- research is merited on how to balance mastery and
mate within their sport/physical activity setting also performance climates in contexts where favorable
report positive achievement-related outcomes such outcomes and being the best are considered bench-
as higher perceptions of competence and enjoyment, marks of successful achievement.
lower anxiety, more adaptive coping strategies, and
higher self-determined motivation, whereas partici- Autonomy-Supportive Versus Controlling Behaviors
pating under a higher performance climate generally One of the more frequently studied aspects of
is associated with less positive achievement-related coach and teacher behavior is the degree to which
outcomes. autonomy-supportive versus controlling behav-
We present a few studies demonstrating the iors are used in their interactions with athletes and
diversity of achievement-related outcomes associ- students. An autonomy-supportive coach/teacher
ated with coaches’ and teachers’ structuring of the engages in behaviors that acknowledge athletes’/
motivational climate. A longitudinal study of ado- students’ thoughts and feelings; encourages choice,
lescent female handball players (ages 13–15 years) self-initiation, and self-regulation; and minimizes
provides a nice illustration of the effects of perceived the use of pressure and demands to control others
motivational climate (Sarrazin, Vallerand, Guillet, (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1987). A controlling inter-
Pelletier, & Cury, 2002). Findings revealed that personal style, on the other hand, is characterized
higher perceived mastery climate predicted higher by pressuring another to think, feel, and act in
levels of the three psychological needs (i.e., perceived a way consistent with the coach’s/teacher’s needs
competence, autonomy, and relatedness), whereas and wants (Deci & Ryan, 1985, 1987).
a higher performance climate negatively predicted In the sport setting, Mageau and Vallerand
perceived autonomy. In turn, need satisfaction pre- (2003) contend that autonomy-supportive coaches
dicted level of self-determined motivation, which engage in a number of specific behaviors to opti-
was directly and negatively related to intention to mize motivation in athletes. These include (a)
drop out and indirectly related to actual dropout providing choice to athletes within specific limits
behavior 21 months later. and rules, (b) providing athletes with a meaning-
Studies in the physical education context also ful rationale for activities, limits, and rules, (c) ask-
highlight the relevance of the motivational climate. ing about and acknowledging athletes’ feelings, (d)
For instance, Barkoukis, Ntoumanis, and Thøgersen- providing opportunity for athletes to take initiative
Ntoumanis (2010) explored changes in junior high and act independently, (e) providing noncontrolling
school students’ achievement motivation over the performance feedback, (f ) avoiding overt control,
course of 3 years. A mastery motivational climate guilt-induced criticism and controlling statements,
was positively associated with a task goal orientation and (g) minimizing behaviors that promote ego

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


involvement. A controlling interpersonal coach- studies looked at both controlling and autonomy-
ing style, according to Bartholomew, Ntoumanis, supportive behaviors given these dimensions are
and Thøgersen-Ntoumani (2009), includes the fol- relatively independent (see Bartholomew et al.,
lowing behavioral dimensions: (a) using rewards 2009, 2010; Tessier, Sarrazin, & Ntoumanis, 2008).
to manipulate athletes’ behavior, (b) using overly For example, Pelletier and colleagues found positive
critical feedback in an attempt to motivate athletes associations between perceived autonomy support
to perform better, (c) attempting to influence ath- and self-determined forms of motivation in a sample
letes’ behaviors and lives outside the sport setting, of 13–22-year-old swimmers, whereas controlling
(d) using power assertive techniques to force athlete coaching behaviors related to less self-determined
compliance, (e) using social comparison as the ref- forms of motivation. They also reported that swim-
erence for evaluating athletes, and (f ) recognizing mers who dropped out over the course of the 2-year
athletes when they are performing well and with- study reported lower autonomy support and greater
drawing attention when athletes are struggling. controlling behaviors from their coaches relative to
Considerable research has shown that teach- swimmers who maintained their participation.
ers and coaches who exhibit more autonomy- It is clear that an autonomy-supportive inter-
supportive and less controlling behaviors facilitate personal style is an effective motivational technique
positive motivational outcomes in students/athletes for coaches and physical educators. Unfortunately,
(see Amorose, 2007; Bartholomew et al., 2009; many teachers and coaches tend to rely on a more
Mageau & Vallerand, 2003; Standage et al., 2007). controlling interpersonal style and may actually be
Studies in sport contexts have consistently shown seen as effective leaders (see Mageau & Vallerand,
that autonomy-supportive behaviors are effective 2003). The data-based evidence, however, implies
in promoting a wide range of positive psychologi- numerous benefits if coaches and teachers are more
cal and behavioral outcomes (e.g., Adie, Duda, & autonomy supportive in their instructional style
Ntoumanis, 2008; Amorose & Anderson-Butcher, (see Amorose, 2007; Mageau & Vallerand, 2003;
2007; Coatsworth & Conroy, 2009; Gagné, Ryan, Standage et al., 2007).
& Bargmann, 2003; Gillet, Vallerand, Amoura, &
Baldes, 2010; Smith, Ntoumanis, & Duda, 2007). peer influence on youth physical
Similar findings occur in physical education settings activity motivation
(e.g., Hagger, Chatzisarantis, Barkoukis, Wang, & One’s peers, such as classmates, teammates, and
Baranowski, 2005; Ntoumanis, 2005; Standage friends, are just as important as significant adults
et al., 2006; Standage & Gillison, 2007; Vierling, when it comes to physical activity motivation, espe-
Standage, & Treasure, 2007). For instance, Standage cially as youth seek to demonstrate autonomy from
and colleagues (2006) showed that students’ (ages adult figures (A.L. Smith, 2003; Weiss & Stuntz,
11–14) perceptions of greater autonomy-supportive 2004). The two main peer constructs in the develop-
behaviors by their physical education teacher pre- mental literature are peer group acceptance and friend-
dicted higher perceived competence, autonomy, ship (Bukowski & Hoza, 1989; Rubin, Bukowski, &
and relatedness in students, which in turn posi- Parker, 1998). Peer group acceptance, or popularity,
tively related to students’ motivational orienta- refers to how much a child is liked or accepted by
tion for physical education. Furthermore, higher members of the peer group (e.g., teammates). It is a
levels of self-determined motivation related to general, unilateral view that entails how well the peer
greater effort and persistence in physical education. group regards one of its members. Friendship refers
A similar pattern of results was reported by Amo- to a close, dyadic relationship (e.g., best friends) and
rose and Anderson-Butcher (2007), who found that represents a specific, reciprocated view of experi-
the degree to which high school and college athletes ences between two persons (e.g., intimacy, loyalty).
perceived their coaches to be autonomy-supportive These constructs are conceptually distinct and thus
positively related to athletes’ perceived competence, examined separately as determinants of youth physi-
autonomy, and relatedness, which in turn predicted cal activity motivation (Weiss & Stuntz, 2004).
self-determined motivation. Over the last decade, more attention has been
The effects of controlling behaviors on moti- given to the role of peer groups and close friend-
vational outcomes have also been examined (e.g., ships in physical activity motivation. This is essen-
Blanchard et al., 2009; Conroy & Coatsworth, 2007; tial because peer interactions and relationships are
Pelletier, Fortier, Vallerand, & Brière, 2001; Smith, best understood within the social context in which
Ntoumanis, & Duda, 2010). In many cases, these they occur (e.g., Zarbatany, Ghesquiere, & Mohr,

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


1992). Sport and physical activity is a unique mediation of positive affect. By contrast, Cox et al.
social context in that behavior and performance are found that perceived peer acceptance was predic-
highly visible. It is not surprising, then, that peer tive of enjoyment and self-determined motivation
comparison and evaluation are salient sources of for adolescent physical education students through
information whereby youth participants judge how perceived relatedness (feeling supported and valued
physically competent they are (Horn, 2004; Horn in the classroom).
& Amorose, 1998). In turn, self-judgments about Social acceptance and approval by peers has
ability are strongly related to motivation to con- also been studied within the concept of social goal
tinue participation. Consistent with our discussion orientations (e.g., Allen, 2003; Petlichkoff, 1993;
of adult influence, we organize our review around Lewthwaite & Piparo, 1993; Schilling & Hayashi,
mechanisms of peer influence: (a) social acceptance 2001; Stuntz & Weiss, 2003, 2009). Social goal
and approval, (b) social support/friendship quality, orientations, like task and ego orientations, refer to
(c) observational learning, (d) leadership behaviors, how one defines success in an achievement domain;
and (e) negative evaluative behaviors. individuals who are high in social goal orientations
define success as having positive relationships with
Social Acceptance and Approval others. Stuntz and Weiss (2009) examined three
One of the most robust findings is that chil- social goal orientations among adolescent sport
dren and adolescents are motivated to participate participants. Group acceptance orientation refers to
in physical activity for social reasons—to be with defining success as being liked and regarded highly
and make friends, attain acceptance and approval by peers; friendship orientation refers to defin-
from peers, and feel part of a group or team (Weiss ing success in terms of developing a close, mutual
& Petlichkoff, 1989; Weiss & Williams, 2004). relationship; and coach praise orientation refers to
These findings generalize across age, gender, race/ defining success as gaining approval from a coach.
ethnicity, socioeconomic status, culture, and sport They assessed social, task, and ego orientations in
type (e.g., Brodkin & Weiss, 1990; Gill, Gross, & relation to motivational outcomes and found that
Huddleston, 1983; Hayashi & Weiss, 1994; Sirard, youth scoring higher in group acceptance and
Pfeiffer, & Pate, 2006; Wilson, Williams, Evans, friendship orientations reported more adaptive
Mixon, & Rheaume, 2005). For example, Brodkin outcomes (perceived competence, enjoyment, and
and Weiss assessed participation motives among intrinsic motivation).
age-group swimmers ranging from 6 to 60+ years Social cohesion is a complementary construct for
old. Participating to attain social status was highest understanding peer group acceptance and approval
for 15–22-year-olds, to obtain friends’ approval was in youth physical activity. Carron and Brawley
highest for 6–9 and 10–14-year-olds, and affiliation (2008) define cohesion as a tendency for a group
reasons were important for all age groups. Klint and (e.g., team) to remain unified in its efforts to achieve
Weiss (1987) found that higher peer acceptance was instrumental (task cohesion) and social goals (i.e.,
associated with higher affiliation motives among social cohesion). Social cohesion refers to develop-
8–16-year-old gymnasts, suggesting that youth who ing and maintaining relationships within the group
feel socially accepted are motivated to demonstrate and is assessed by individuals’ perceptions of how
ability in getting along with teammates. well the group gets along and members like each
Theory-driven studies show that peer group other. Thus, the construct of social cohesion is con-
acceptance is positively related to perceived compe- ceptually consistent with peer group acceptance and
tence, affective responses, motivational orientations, approval. Adolescents’ ratings of greater team har-
and physical activity behavior (e.g., Cox, Duncheon, mony, satisfaction, and mutual liking are associated
& McDavid, 2009; A.L. Smith, 1999; A.L. Smith, with effective coach and peer leadership behaviors
Ullrich-French, Walker, & Hurley, 2006; Ullrich- (e.g., Price & Weiss, 2011b; Westre & Weiss, 1991;
French & Smith, 2006; Weiss & Duncan, 1992). Zacharatos, Barling, & Kelloway, 2000).
For example, A.L. Smith (1999) tested a model of
relationships between peer and motivational vari- Social Support/Friendship Quality
ables and found that adolescents higher in peer Considerable evidence exists that peers serve
acceptance reported higher physical self-worth, pos- as sources of companionship, instrumental, affec-
itive affect, and intrinsic motivation. In turn, higher tion, admiration, and emotional support, which
physical self-worth was indirectly related to intrinsic relate to physical activity motivation (see Weiss &
motivation and physical activity level through the Stuntz, 2004). For example, British adolescents

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


were interviewed to uncover reasons influencing competence, enjoyment, and self-determined moti-
their decisions to maintain or discontinue physi- vation, and less anxiety and self-presentational
cal activity (Coakley & White, 1992). Social sup- concerns—than youth with negative profiles (lower
port from same-sex friends (i.e., companionship peer acceptance, lower friendship quality, higher or
and emotional support) and social constraints from lower conflict).
opposite-sex friends (i.e., peer pressure) emerged as Taking a different approach, Stuntz and Spear-
common themes, with girls indicating these factors ance (2010) explored cross-domain relationships by
as more influential than boys for determining activ- determining whether teammate–athlete interactions
ity choices. Duncan (1993) found that perceptions extend outside the sport context (e.g., school, fam-
of greater companionship and esteem support in ily, goals). Path analyses revealed that positive cross-
physical education predicted adolescent students’ domain teammate relationships were predictive of
enjoyment within the classroom and choices to be high sport enjoyment for collegiate athletes and high
active outside of class. sport commitment for youth and collegiate athletes.
To understand the nature and significance of The concept of cross-domain relationships reso-
friendships on youths’ psychosocial and behavioral nates with a positive youth development approach,
outcomes, Hartup (1995) identified three perspec- whereby caring and supportive peers (and adults)
tives: (a) whether one has a close friendship, (b) who contribute to psychosocial growth experiences in
one’s friends are, and (c) the quality of one’s friend- one domain (e.g., physical activity) that transcend
ships. Friendship quality is essentially synonymous to other domains (Weiss & Wiese-Bjornstal, 2009).
with friendship support and refers to positive and
negative dimensions that characterize one’s relation- Observational Learning
ship. Weiss and colleagues conducted interrelated According to Bandura (1986), modeling or obser-
studies to provide conceptual clarity to the con- vational learning is acknowledged “to be one of the
struct of “sport friendship quality” (Weiss & Smith, most powerful means of transmitting values, attitudes,
1999, 2002; Weiss, Smith, & Theeboom, 1996). and patterns of thought and behavior” (p. 47). Peer
In the first study, Weiss et al. interviewed 8–16- models are similar in age, gender, competence level,
year-old sport participants about their friendships and other characteristics (Schunk, 1998), and they
in physical activities. Twelve positive dimensions connote effective sources of physical activity informa-
(e.g., companionship, intimacy, loyalty, self-esteem tion and motivation (McCullagh & Weiss, 2002). A
enhancement) and four negative dimensions (e.g., similar model is thought to elicit selective attention
conflict, betrayal) emerged that were similar to in observers and change in their competence beliefs,
social support types in the developmental litera- emotions, and motivated behavior through an atti-
ture, whereas context-specific higher-order themes tude of “if he or she can do it, so can I!”
showed unique qualities (e.g., “we help each other Schunk (1998) contends that peer models convey
in sport,” “he motivates me in sports,” “negative information and motivation to observers about learn-
competitiveness”). Dimensions and themes were ing strategies, self-regulation skills, outcome expecta-
used to develop a pool of items and provide factorial tions, and self-efficacy. These psychological processes
and construct validity for a measure of sport friend- translate to motivated behavior and improved perfor-
ship quality (Weiss & Smith, 1999, 2002). mance. Peer models may be effective because observ-
Subsequent studies investigated friendship qual- ers identify better with skills and learning strategies
ity or peer social support in relation to psychosocial conveyed by similar others, and they may be espe-
and motivational variables in the physical domain cially helpful when observers are uncertain about
(e.g., Cox et al., 2009; Moran & Weiss, 2006; A.L. their abilities to do a task, are unfamiliar with skills,
Smith, Ullrich-French, et al., 2006; Ullrich-French & or have experienced difficulty or anxiety in past per-
Smith, 2006, 2009; W.M. Weiss & Weiss, 2003, formance attempts. Weiss, McCullagh, Smith, and
2006, 2007). For example, A.L. Smith, Ullrich- Berlant (1998) conducted an experimental study to
French, et al. assessed 10–14-year-old sport par- determine effects of peer modeling on motivation
ticipants on positive friendship quality, conflict, and skill development of children who were fearful of
and peer acceptance, and they compared relation- and lacked confidence in swimming. Children (ages
ship profiles to motivational outcomes. Youth with 5–8 years) viewed a peer mastery, peer coping, or no
a positive relationship profile (higher peer accep- model (control) in combination with swim instruc-
tance, higher friendship quality, and lower conflict) tion for 3 days. Peer modeling groups showed stronger
showed more adaptive outcomes—higher perceived pre- to postintervention improvements than controls

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


in swim skills, self-efficacy, and fear of swimming, a formal leadership role on sport teams, but team
and differences held up in a retention test. Results members also represent sources of leadership that
highlight the utility of an intervention that combines can motivate others to achieve instrumental and
peer modeling and swim lessons to enhance motiva- social goals (e.g., team harmony) (Glenn & Horn,
tion and skill learning. 1993; Moran & Weiss, 2006; Price & Weiss,
Other researchers have examined modeling effects 2011b). Peer leaders can fulfill roles such as team
through correlating physical activity levels of youth captain or they can emerge from within the group
and their peers or identifying whether youth had by demonstrating ability to inspire group members’
friends who participated in sport or physical activity effort and performance (i.e., lead by example). Thus,
(e.g., Denault & Poulin, 2009; King, Tergerson, & peer leadership represents an important mechanism
Wilson, 2008; Salvy et al., 2008, 2009; Schofield, of peer influence on teammates’ individual (e.g.,
Mummery, Schofield, & Hopkins, 2007). For self-determined) and group motivation (e.g., task
example, adolescents who indicated they had friends and social cohesion).
who exercise or participate in sports reported being Early research focused on correlates of peer
more physically active and spending more hours leadership, with those higher in interpersonal
doing sports than their counterparts (Denault & attraction and sociability being rated higher in
Poulin, 2009; King et al., 2008). Schofield et al. leadership attributes (e.g., Tropp & Landers, 1979;
used an objective measure (pedometry) to assess Yukelson, Weinberg, Richardson, & Jackson,
adolescent girls’ physical activity with that of three 1983). Interpersonal attraction is akin to peer
closest friends, including reciprocated (mutually group acceptance and shows a linkage with peer
nominated) and nonreciprocated friends. Girls with variables of leadership and sociability. Recent
a larger number of active friends were more likely research has adopted theory-driven approaches to
to reach the criterion of 10,000 steps than those understand qualities of effective peer leaders and
with no active friends; and a moderate relationship relationships between leadership behaviors and
emerged for a girl’s physical activity level with that team outcomes (e.g., Glenn & Horn, 1993; Moran
of a reciprocated friend, whereas a trivial correlation & Weiss, 2006; Price and Weiss, 2011a, b; Zacha-
was found for nonreciprocated friendships. ratos et al., 2000).
Davison (2004) investigated sources and types of Several studies show that effective peer leaders are
support for physical activity among middle school characterized by both instrumental (task-oriented)
girls and boys. Interestingly, items for her “peer sup- and expressive (social-oriented) behaviors, such as
port” variable included the following items: “friends’ facilitating teammates in attaining group goals and
level of activity,” “importance friends assign to being promoting a positive and accepting environment
physically active,” “frequency of being active with (e.g., Eys, Loughead, & Hardy, 2007; Glenn &
friends,” “friends’ admiration of people who are Horn, 1993; Price & Weiss, 2011a; Rees, 1983).
active,” and “friends’ admiration of people who are Extending the social-oriented behaviors of peer
athletic.” The content of these items refers to friends’ leaders, studies show that peer attributes of group
attitudes and behaviors for endorsing physical activ- acceptance, positive friendship quality, and leader-
ity, thereby consistent with definition and character- ship are intertwined (Moran & Weiss, 2006; Price &
istics of peer modeling discussed earlier. She found Weiss, 2011a; Yukelson et al., 1983; Zacharatos
that high-active girls reported greater peer support/ et al., 2000). Team members who were rated higher
modeling than low active girls (along with other in leadership qualities and behaviors were also liked
sources of social support). Salvy et al. (2008, 2009) by their teammates, scored higher on social support
found that doing physical activity with a friend was (e.g., loyalty, intimacy, self-esteem enhancement),
particularly effective for overweight youth, who and maintained sport friendships “off the field.”
showed increased motivation to be active and a Based on these links among peer constructs, Moran
higher intensity level in physical activity. Concomi- and Weiss suggested that social competence is the
tant effects were not significant for lean youth; that glue that binds these characteristics and behaviors
is, doing physical activity alone or with a friend was together. Individuals who are outgoing, get along
equally effective. with others, and enjoy and respect the “company
they keep” (Bukowski, Newcomb, & Hartup, 1996)
Leadership Behaviors are favorably appraised by their peers, are chosen as
An exciting area of research that has gained friends, engage in high-quality friendships, and are
momentum is peer leadership. Coaches occupy seen as team leaders.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Transformational leadership theory (Bass, 1998) examples of negative affect and low motivation for
has been embraced as an appropriate and useful sport activities. These disparate opportunities and
framework for understanding peer and coach leader- behaviors denied low-skilled boys from developing
ship in sport (e.g., Price & Weiss, 2011a, b; Rowold, physically (i.e., improve sport skills) and socially
2006). According to this theory, leaders motivate fol- (i.e., attain acceptance, strengthen friendships), and
lowers to adopt attitudes and behaviors that maximize are examples of factors that squelch motivation for
achieving group goals. This occurs through engag- continuing in sport activities.
ing in four behaviors: (a) inspirational motivation Kunesh et al. (1992) conducted a naturalistic
(setting high achievement standards and exhibiting study of 11–12-year-old girls and their peer groups
confidence in attaining them), (b) idealized influ- using multiple methods (observations, sociometric
ence (modeling desirable attitudes and behaviors), (c) ratings, interviews), contexts (school, neighbor-
intellectual stimulation (facilitating problem solving hood), and activities (sport, games, unstructured
among teammates), and (d) individualized consid- physical activity). Male classmates rated the girls
eration (recognizing the needs and interests of each low in peer acceptance (i.e., rejected, neglected)
teammate). Price examined the unique and combined and treated them negatively, such as teasing, criti-
influence of peer and coach leadership on individual cizing, name-calling, taunting, and excluding. Girls
and team outcomes for adolescent female soccer disclosed feelings of anxiety and embarrassment
players. For the unique influence of peer leadership, as a result of the boys’ behaviors at school, which
transformational behaviors were positively related to resulted in girls’ attempts to avoid physical activity
athletes’ enjoyment, intrinsic motivation, task and (e.g., PE, games at recess). By contrast, girls reported
social cohesion, and collective efficacy. When peer having fun doing unstructured physical activity in
and coach leadership were simultaneously modeled, their neighborhood with close friends and viewed
coach transformational behaviors were positively the few negative peer interactions in this setting as
related to individual and team outcomes while peer natural because they were with reciprocated friends.
transformational behaviors were positively related Importantly, the girls continued to be motivated to
only to task and social cohesion. These findings dem- play games and be physically active in their home
onstrate the utility of considering the interaction of environments. The girls’ age group (11–12 years)
peer and coach leadership, because inspirational, ide- was an important feature of the study as evidence
alized, intellectual, and individualized behaviors were shows that girls starting at this age show a decline in
related to team members’ psychosocial attributes and physical activity that continues over the adolescent
team dynamics differently depending on the source years (USDHHS, 2008). Negative peer interactions
of leadership influence. that promote high anxiety and embarrassment and
low motivation for physical activity are counter to
Negative Evaluative Behaviors our efforts to maintain and enhance a physically
Unfortunately interactions and relationships active lifestyle and associated health benefits.
among peers are not always positive. Peer group The study by Kunesh et al. (1992) is reminiscent
relationships can be characterized as low in social of peer victimization, a subject of concern in school
acceptance (e.g., rejected, neglected), high in nega- contexts. Peer victimization refers to verbal, physi-
tive friendship quality (e.g., conflict, betrayal), and cal, and relational aggressive behaviors toward others
low in positive friendship quality (e.g., loyalty, inti- (e.g., Crick, Bigbee, & Howes, 1996; Ladd & Price,
macy) (Bukowski et al., 1996; Rubin et al., 1998). 1993). Youth who are victims of peer aggression
Such can also be the case in sport and physical activ- report anxiety, loneliness, and depression, which is
ity (e.g., Evans & Roberts, 1987; Jensen & Steele, related to lower self-evaluations and decreased moti-
2009; Kunesh, Hasbrook, & Lewthwaite, 1992; vation in achievement situations. The literature on
A.L. Smith, Ullrich-French, et al., 2006; Storch moral development in sport indicates that organized
et al., 2007). For example, Evans and Roberts sport is a context where physical and verbal aggres-
observed elementary-age boys (grades 3–6) while sion occurs frequently and is even encouraged in
they interacted with their peers on the school play- some sports (Weiss, Smith, & Stuntz, 2008). How-
ground and interviewed them about their experi- ever, such behaviors have been studied in relation
ences. Low-skilled boys were chosen last for teams, to sources of social influence and individual differ-
were relegated to noncentral positions (e.g., right ences (e.g., moral reasoning) and not motivation
field), and were often denied playing time. Inter- per se. However, youth who are fearful and anxious
view responses from these boys were vivid with about the physical nature of sports are likely to be

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


prone to dropping out, but little empirical evidence motivation. A major focus is on physical activity as
exists to verify a link between aggressive behaviors a context for promoting positive cognitive, affective,
in sport and motivation to participate. and behavioral outcomes as shown through inter-
A few studies have investigated links between vention and longitudinal studies.
peer victimization, psychosocial variables, and phys-
ical activity among overweight youth (e.g., Jensen self-perceptions and youth physical
& Steele, 2009; Storch et al., 2007). Children who activity motivation
are overweight or at risk of overweight have much In the physical activity domain, self-perceptions
to benefit from regular physical activity, not only have been examined as antecedents of participation
the obvious physical health benefits but social (e.g., motivation and as consequences of physical activ-
making friends, attaining high friendship quality) ity participation (see Fox & Wilson, 2008; Horn,
and psychological (e.g., self-esteem, positive affect, 2004; Weiss, Bhalla, & Price, 2008). We discuss the
motivation) benefits as well. Storch et al. found that, constructs of perceived competence and self-esteem
among 8–18-year-old youth, higher ratings of peer as predictors of physical activity motivation and as
victimization were related to lower physical activ- outcomes derived from one’s experiences in physi-
ity levels, and depressive symptoms and loneliness cal activity. Perceived competence is a belief about
mediated this relationship. Similarly, Jensen and one’s ability in a particular achievement domain
Steele found that fifth- and sixth-grade girls who (e.g., academic, physical, social) or subdomain (e.g.,
reported high levels of weight criticism combined math, soccer, peer acceptance). Individuals appraise
with body dissatisfaction were much less physically their abilities in domains that they value, and these
active than girls who experienced criticism in the domain-specific appraisals make up one’s global
absence of body dissatisfaction. These same results sense of self, or self-esteem. Self-esteem is multi-
were not applicable to boys. Given a major focus of dimensional meaning that individuals can have
physical activity researchers on strategies for reduc- domain-specific self-evaluations as well as more
ing or preventing overweight and obesity, it seems global self-evaluations that are not tied to specific
reasonable to suggest that peer interventions are abilities. In all four theories we highlighted, self-
essential for helping to stem the tide. evaluations of ability and one’s overall self-concept
are important individual differences explaining
summary motivational orientations and behaviors.
Parents, coaches/teachers, and peers connote
salient sources of social influence on young people’s Perceived Competence and Self-Esteem as
motivational orientations and behaviors in physical Determinants of Physical Activity Motivation
activity contexts. Through expressing beliefs about Perceived physical competence is strongly related
competence and task value, as well as exhibiting to self-determined forms of motivation for both
behaviors that convey information, inspiration, and physical education students (e.g., Cox & Williams,
evaluation, significant adults and peers influence 2008; Ferrer Caja & Weiss, 2000, 2002; Ntoumanis,
participants’ psychological needs, behavioral regula- 2001; Standage, Duda, & Ntoumanis, 2003, 2006;
tions, and physical activity levels. In the next section Standage & Gillison, 2007; Taylor, Ntoumanis,
we highlight individual difference factors that are Standage, & Spray, 2010) and sport participants
associated with motivational orientations and par- (e.g., Amorose, 2001; Amorose & Anderson-Butcher,
ticipation behavior. 2007; Hollembeak & Amorose, 2005; Kipp & Amo-
rose, 2008; Sarrazin, Vallerand, Guillet, Pelletier, &
Individual Difference Factors Cury, 2002). In these studies, self-determined forms
In sport and physical activity, self-perceptions, of motivation include both intrinsic motivation and
notably global self-esteem and domain-specific self- self-determined forms of extrinsic motivation (i.e.,
evaluations, are consistently strong predictors of identified and integrated regulations).
motivational orientations and behaviors. Emotional Physical education contexts have provided
responses, such as enjoyment and anxiety, are also strong support for the link between perceived phys-
prevalent in the physical domain and frequently ical competence and self-determined motivation,
mediate the relationship between social influence whereas the sport context has received less attention.
and motivational outcomes. In the following sec- In the physical education domain, Ferrer Caja and
tions, we discuss self-perceptions and emotions as Weiss (2000, 2002) surveyed high school students
antecedents and as consequences of physical activity who took physical education as either a requirement

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


(2000 study) or as an elective (2002 study). Model Students with positive and stable self-evaluations
testing showed that perceived physical competence over time reported higher levels of intrinsic motiva-
(along with task orientation and mastery motiva- tion compared to students with less positive and less
tional climate) was a significant predictor of intrin- stable self-evaluations. Standage and Gillison took
sic motivation for both groups of students. Cox and a different approach by examining self-determined
Williams (2008) administered surveys to fifth- and motivation as a predictor of self-esteem. They
sixth-grade physical education students and found assessed adolescent physical education students on
that perceived physical competence (along with per- self-determination constructs—perceptions of com-
ceived autonomy and relatedness) was a significant petence, autonomy, and relatedness; teacher’s use of
predictor of self-determined motivation. The rela- autonomy-supportive behavior; self-determined
tionship was the strongest when perceived physi- motivation; self-esteem; and health-related quality
cal competence mediated the relationship between of life. In all model testing, perceived competence
mastery climate and self-determined motivation. In predicted self-determined motivation. Further-
the sport domain, Amorose and Anderson-Butcher more, three models supported differing relation-
(2007) assessed high school and college athletes on ships between self-determined motivation and
self-determination constructs and found that per- self-esteem. In model 1, self-determined motivation
ceived competence (as well as perceived autonomy directly predicted self-esteem, which in turn pre-
and relatedness) mediated the relationship between dicted quality of life. In model 2, self-determined
perceived autonomy support by the coach and self- motivation indirectly predicted self-esteem through
determined motivation. quality of life. In model 3, self-determined motiva-
Perceived competence has also been examined as tion directly predicted both self-esteem and quality
a determinant of motivational behaviors in physi- of life. These results mean that self-esteem, quality
cal activity, such as effort, intention to participate in of life, and self-determined motivation are intri-
future activity, persistence, and dropout (Davison, cately related.
Symons Downs, & Birch, 2006; Ferrer Caja & To improve levels of self-determined motivation
Weiss, 2000, 2002; Ntoumanis, 2001; Sarrazin and motivated behavior in physical activity con-
et al., 2002; Standage et al., 2003, 2006; Taylor et al., texts, research clearly shows that maintaining and
2010; Ullrich-French & Smith, 2009). For example, enhancing perceived competence and self-esteem
Ferrer Caja and Weiss also included teacher ratings are important points of intervention. The next ques-
of students’ effort and persistence in physical educa- tion is: How do we improve physical activity partic-
tion. In both studies (2000, 2002), intrinsic moti- ipants’ self-perceptions? Sport and physical activity
vation mediated the relationship between perceived program intervention studies have been helpful in
physical competence and effort and persistence in answering this question.
physical education. In another study, Taylor and col-
leagues assessed adolescent physical education stu- Perceived Competence and Self-Esteem as
dents’ perceived physical competence, motivational Outcome of Physical Activity Experiences
orientations, effort in physical education, intention Several studies have shown that self-evaluations
to exercise outside of physical education class, and can be improved as a result of theory-driven physi-
leisure-time physical activity at three times over cal activity interventions (e.g., Ebbeck & Gibbons,
a school trimester. Results revealed that perceived 1998; Marsh & Peart, 1988; Schneider, Fridlund
physical competence and self-determined motiva- Dunton, & Cooper, 2008; Smoll, Smith, Barnett,
tion were the strongest predictors of effort, inten- & Everett, 1993; Taymoori & Lubans, 2008; Thee-
tions, and physical activity levels. boom, De Knop, & Weiss, 1995). In line with
Global self-evaluations like self-esteem and physi- research on coaches’ feedback patterns and moti-
cal self-worth have also been examined in relation to vational climate, several studies have focused on
self-determined motivation (Amorose, 2001; Hein & coach-training interventions to bring about positive
Hagger, 2007; Standage & Gillison, 2007). For self-evaluations in young sport participants (e.g.,
example, Amorose surveyed middle school physi- Smoll et al., 1993; Theeboom et al., 1995). Smoll
cal education students, most who indicated they and colleagues trained 10–12-year-old boys’ base-
participated in organized sports. Both global self- ball coaches through a coach effectiveness workshop
evaluations (self-worth) and domain-specific self- designed to emphasize reinforcement for good per-
evaluations (physical self-worth, perceived physical formance and effort, mistake-contingent encour-
competence) were related to intrinsic motivation. agement, and corrective and technical instruction.

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


Coaches were also asked to avoid or minimize using the 9-month school-based intervention. Changes
punishment and punitive instruction. At the end of in study variables were analyzed in relation to
the season, boys who played for the trained coaches a comparison group, which included physical edu-
viewed their coaches as more supportive compared cation students at another school. Results showed
to boys in the control group. Importantly for the that intervention group participants who improved
present discussion, boys who played for the trained their fitness levels showed significant increases in
coaches and started the season with low self-esteem global physical self-concept over the school year,
showed significant increases in self-esteem over the and they suggest that organized physical activity
season, whereas those in the control group did not. that improves fitness levels can promote positive
No differences in trained versus control groups self-evaluations.
occurred for youth who scored moderate or high Collectively the intervention studies discussed
in self-esteem at the beginning of the season. These in this section suggest causal links between physi-
results suggest that children who have the most to cal activity participation and improvement in self-
gain from a positive sport experience (i.e., those evaluations. It is clear that quality physical activity
who are low in self-esteem) benefited greatly from programs can positively impact participants’ per-
having a coach who used positive forms of instruc- ceived physical competence and self-esteem. In
tion and evaluative feedback. addition, studies have supported links between
Other intervention studies have emphasized higher perceived competence and self-esteem with
cooperation among peers in physical education greater self-determined motivation and physical
classes to bring about positive self-evaluations activity participation. Coaches, teachers, and physi-
(Ebbeck & Gibbons, 1998; Marsh & Peart, 1988). cal activity instructors are in an important position
For example, Ebbeck and Gibbons conducted an to structure the environment to positively shape the
intervention where middle school physical educa- self-perceptions of their participants. Parents can
tion teachers were trained in using team-building also help shape their children’s perceptions of physi-
challenges with their students throughout the cal competence. As shown in earlier sections, signif-
school year. At the end of the intervention, boys and icant adults who model physical activity, emphasize
girls in the intervention group were significantly a mastery climate, and exhibit autonomy-supportive
higher on perceptions of global self-worth, ath- behavior can positively impact participants’ self-
letic competence, physical appearance, and social perceptions. In sum, theory-driven physical activity
acceptance than the control group (regular physical programs can effect positive changes in participants’
education activities). In addition, female students sense of self. In turn, higher levels of perceived com-
in the intervention group were significantly higher petence and self-esteem can ultimately promote
on perceived academic competence and behavioral more self-determined reasons for participation,
conduct than those in the control group. Effect sizes greater effort and persistence in physical activity,
for group differences were large and meaningful. and higher physical activity levels.
Thus, a physical activity intervention that required
group interdependence and positive peer interac- emotions and youth physical
tions was successful in modifying a range of global activity motivation
and domain-specific self-evaluations. The physical domain provides a unique con-
Some recent studies have incorporated health text for experiencing emotions, both positive and
promotion interventions to increase physical activ- negative. People often experience joy and pride after
ity and improve self-perceptions (Schneider et al., successful physical performances and frustration,
2008; Taymoori & Lubans, 2008). Schneider and anxiety, and anger following poor performances.
colleagues conducted a 9-month, school-based Emotions have been examined as antecedents of
physical activity intervention for sedentary ado- participation motivation and as consequences of
lescent girls. Physical education classes designed participation in physical activity (see Crocker,
exclusively for study participants included super- Hoar, McDonough, Kowalski, & Niefer, 2004;
vised activity four times per week (e.g., aero- Crocker, Kowalski, Hoar, & McDonough, 2004).
bic dance, yoga, basketball, swimming, Tae Bo) In this section, we discuss consistent findings about
and educational instruction one day per week to the relationship between emotions and motivation.
promote physical activity outside of school. Self- Participants’ feeling states are important individual
concept, physical activity level, and cardiovascu- difference factors that help explain motivation and
lar fitness were assessed before, during, and after participation in physical activity.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Research on emotional responses and physi- competence motivation constructs (peer influence,
cal activity motivation has focused mainly on self-perceptions, affect, motivation). Among the
enjoyment, anxiety, stress, and burnout. However, findings, model testing showed that greater positive
definitions of emotion have not always been con- affect toward physical activity was related to higher
ceptually clear, with terms such as feeling states, intrinsic motivation and physical activity behavior.
affect, moods, and emotions used interchangeably. Negative emotions in sport, notably performance
Crocker, Kowalski, et al. (2004) provide a guide for anxiety, competitive stress, and burnout, have been
defining emotions: quick onset, cognitive appraisal, linked to a range of social and motivational outcomes
distinct physiological patterns, subjective feeling among athletes (e.g., Gould, Tuffey, Udry, & Loehr,
states, and facial or bodily expression (e.g., happy, 1996; Gould, Udry, Tuffey, & Loehr, 1996; Price
angry, anxious, and excited). Affect is described as a & Weiss, 2000; Raedeke, 1997; Raedeke & Smith,
global feeling state that can vary in tone (pleasant to 2001, 2004; Vealey, Armstrong, Comar, & Green-
unpleasant) and intensity (low to high). leaf, 1998). For example, Price and Weiss found
To understand emotions in the physical domain, that adolescent female athletes who reported higher
early research identified sources of enjoyment such anxiety and burnout rated their coaches as exhibit-
as perceived competence, positive peer interactions, ing lower frequency of instruction, social support,
perceived coach support, effort, and skill mas- positive feedback, and democratic decision-making
tery (e.g., Scanlan, Carpenter, Lobel, & Simons, behaviors, and greater autocratic behaviors. Athlete
1993; Scanlan, Ravizza, & Stein, 1989; Scanlan & burnout has been a topic of great concern among
Simons, 1992; Wankel & Kreisel, 1985). Sources of scholars and practitioners due to the demands and
anxiety, competitive stress, and burnout have also pressures often present in competitive sport (see
been frequently studied, such as negative perfor- Reeve & Weiss, 2006; Schmidt & Stein, 1991). Ath-
mance expectancies, negative social evaluation, per- lete burnout is defined as (a) emotional and physical
formance evaluation potential, and negative coach exhaustion, (b) reduced sense of accomplishment,
relationships (e.g., Gould, Horn, & Spreeman, and (c) sport devaluation (e.g., Raedeke, 1997; Rae-
1983; Passer, 1988; Raedeke, 1997; Scanlan & deke & Smith, 2001). For example, Raedeke and
Passer, 1979; Scanlan, Stein, & Ravizza, 1991; Smith (2004) found that adolescent swimmers who
see Gould, 1993). Research describing the nature reported higher levels of stress in their sport scored
of positive and negative emotions in the physical higher on burnout. In addition, results provided
domain led to theory-driven studies on the emotion– support for stress as a mediator of the relationship
motivation relationship (see Crocker, Hoar, et al., between coping behaviors and social support satisfac-
2004; Crocker, Kowalski, et al., 2004). tion with burnout. These contextual factors (coping
behaviors and social support) may help reduce stress
Emotions and Affect as Determinants of Participation levels and in turn promote more adaptive forms of
Motivation and Motivated Behaviors motivation for physical activity participants.
Enjoyment and positive affect strongly predict
self-determined motivation, psychological commit- Emotions and Affect as Consequences of Motivational
ment to physical activity, and motivated behaviors Orientations and Physical Activity Participation
like effort and persistence (Raedeke, 1997; Sch- Recently, researchers have examined physical
neider, Dunn, & Cooper, 2009; A.L. Smith, 1999; activity in relation to aspects of well-being such as
Weiss, Kimmel, & Smith, 2001; W.M. Weiss & positive affect, subjective vitality, satisfaction, self-
Weiss, 2003, 2006, 2007; W.M. Weiss, Weiss, & esteem, depressive symptoms, and physical symp-
Amorose, 2010). For example, W.M. Weiss and toms of illness. Emotional aspects of well-being
Weiss (2003, 2006) used cluster analysis to group (e.g., positive affect, depressive symptoms, anxi-
10–18-year-old gymnasts into commitment pro- ety, enjoyment) have been studied as outcomes of
files. Gymnasts with an adaptive profile (commit- motivational orientations and physical activity lev-
ted to sport for attraction reasons) reported higher els (e.g., Blanchard, Amiot, Perriault, Vallerand,
enjoyment, intrinsic motivation, and effort and & Provencher, 2009; Boone & Leadbeater, 2006;
persistence in their training behaviors, compared Cox, Duncheon, & McDavid, 2009; Gore, Farrell,
to gymnasts with a maladaptive profile (committed & Gordon, 2001; Sanders, Field, Diego, & Kaplan,
to sport because they feel entrapped). In another 2000; Vierling, Standage, & Treasure, 2007).
study, A.L. Smith (1999) surveyed adolescents For example, Cox and colleagues found that self-
involved in organized sport and physical activity on determined motivation predicted greater enjoyment

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


and less worry among sixth- through eighth-grade group reported greater enjoyment, perceived com-
physical education students. Blanchard et al. tested petence, and intrinsic motivation, and they were
a model of relationships among team cohesion; rated higher in physical skills, compared to the con-
coach behaviors; perceived competence, autonomy, trol group. MacPhail and colleagues (2008) incorpo-
and relatedness; self-determined motivation; and rated a 16-week sport education unit in elementary
emotional outcomes among 16–22-year-old basket- school physical education classes. The program con-
ball players. Among other findings, self-determined sisted of activities that encouraged autonomy, such
motivation predicted greater levels of positive emo- as choosing a team name, choosing a role on the
tions while playing basketball and satisfaction with team, and taking part in determining winners in the
their sport participation. Boone and Leadbeater tournament. Students were interviewed about their
found that greater positive team sport involve- experiences throughout the program and several
ment among adolescents predicted fewer depressive themes emerged: fun and enjoyment, team affilia-
symptoms, and that perceived social acceptance and tion, autonomy, perceived sport competence, and
body dissatisfaction predicted depressive symp- formal competition. Survey responses for motiva-
toms through positive team sport involvement. tional variables reinforced past research on sources
These studies provide evidence of interrelationships of enjoyment: affiliation, autonomy, competition,
among emotions, motivational orientations, and and learning skills. These intervention studies show
physical activity involvement. that participation in theory-driven physical activity
Several intervention studies provide further programs can foster positive emotional outcomes,
evidence for the role of sport and physical activity including increased enjoyment and reduced anxiety.
in fostering emotional well-being. Theory-based In sum, emotions have been examined in the
physical activity programs have positively changed sport domain as both antecedents and conse-
participants’ emotional experiences (MacPhail, quences of motivational orientations and physical
Gorely, Kirk, & Kinchin, 2008; R.E. Smith, Smoll, activity participation. It is clear that the more youth
& Barnett, 1995; R.E. Smith, Smoll, & Cumming, enjoy their sport experiences, the more they will
2007; Theeboom, De Knop, & Weiss, 1995). R.E. be motivated for self-determined reasons and exert
Smith and colleagues (1995) trained youth baseball greater effort and persistence. In turn, greater self-
coaches using their coach effectiveness program at determined motivation and skill mastery should
the beginning of a season. The training program result in greater positive emotions. Intervention
emphasized providing social support, encourag- studies contribute to our understanding of how
ing and reinforcing effort, and deemphasizing the physical activity can impact emotional well-being.
importance of winning. Thus, the intervention was Programs that emphasize improvement rather than
hypothesized to reduce athletes’ competitive anxi- winning, provide opportunities to be autonomous,
ety levels. At the end of the season, players in the and provide social support bring about greater levels
experimental group evaluated their coaches more of enjoyment and lower levels of anxiety in physical
positively, reported having more fun, and expe- activity participants. These contextual factors can be
rienced reduced trait anxiety compared to players fostered by coaches and parents and can lead to over-
in the control group. R.E. Smith et al. (2007) con- all well-being (e.g., improved self-esteem, greater
ducted a similar intervention and added a mastery perceived competence, higher intrinsic motivation,
climate component in the coach training (empha- and successful skill learning).
sis on effort and improvement and positive control
rather than aversive control). Over the course of a Future Research Directions
basketball season, boys and girls in the experimental In this chapter we extensively reviewed the
group showed decreases in cognitive, somatic, and knowledge base on antecedents, correlates, and out-
overall anxiety, while control group players reported comes of physical activity motivation. Still there is
increases in anxiety. much yet to be known about factors that influence
Other intervention studies were conducted in individuals to initiate and maintain their involve-
physical education-type settings. Theeboom and ment in sport and physical activities. It is not pos-
colleagues (1995) examined the effect of mastery sible within the scope of this chapter to provide
(experimental group) and performance (control a comprehensive set of recommendations for future
group) motivational climates with youth ages 8–12 research. Thus, we limit our suggestions to a few
years in a summer sports program. After a 3-week that we consider to be major areas necessitating
program in martial arts, youth in the intervention attention in the literature. Readers are directed to

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


other sources for more detailed discussion of future The value in expanding our understanding of
research ideas (Amorose, 2007; Horn, 2008; Horn antecedents of coaching and teaching behaviors is
& Horn, 2007; Weiss & Amorose, 2008; Weiss & that it will be useful in designing effective interven-
Stuntz, 2004; Weiss & Williams, 2004). tion programs (Horn, 2008). While examples of
Since Brustad’s (1992) plea for conducting interventions exist that have manipulated key coach-
research blending socialization and motivation ing and teaching behaviors (e.g., Barnett et al., 1992;
influences, an explosion of studies occurred in the Coatsworth & Conroy, 2006; Chatzisarantis &
past decade on mechanisms of parental influence Hagger, 2009; Jaakkola & Liukkonen, 2006; Tes-
and youths’ physical activity experiences. Still much sier, Sarrazin, & Ntoumanis, 2008; Theeboom
is unknown about the role of the family on par- et al., 1995), scholars need to expand this type of
ticipants’ physical activity- and achievement-related research. Such research will help us (a) understand
outcomes (Fredricks & Eccles, 2004; Horn & aspects of intervention that are more or less effective
Horn, 2007). Based on Eccles et al.’s (1998) model for modifying motivational outcomes, (b) deter-
of parental influence, family characteristics (e.g., mine the optimal length of time required to acquire
education, number of children, culture, employ- meaningful changes in motivation, and (c) examine
ment status) and child characteristics (e.g., gender, the most effective ways to train coaches and teachers
birth order, sibling characteristics) should influence to adopt these behaviors.
parents’ beliefs and behaviors and subsequently the Peers such as teammates, classmates, and close
child’s beliefs and behaviors. Additionally, children friends represent a salient source of competence and
can socialize and motivate parents through their motivational information for physical activity partici-
active involvement in sports. Yet most studies have pants. In our review, we teased out potential behavioral
been conducted with intact two-parent families mechanisms based on a limited amount of empirical
(mother and father), of mostly European American research in the physical domain. Thus, considerably
ethnicity, and with little attention to sibling influ- more theory-driven research is needed to determine
ence (Horn & Horn, 2007). Clearly more empiri- how and why peer groups and friendships make an
cal study is needed on variations in family structure impact on youths’ self-perceptions, emotions, and
(e.g., single-parent, step-parents, same-sex parents), physical activity motivation (see A.L. Smith, 2003;
bidirectional and reciprocal parent-child influences, Weiss & Stuntz, 2004). All the theories we highlighted
and family characteristics (e.g., socioeconomic sta- explicitly acknowledge the powerful contribution of
tus, culture, number and birth order of siblings) on significant others on motivational processes; thus,
youth participants’ physical activity motivation. A questions couched within competence motivation,
few studies have initiated momentum on these issues self-determination, achievement goal, and expectancy-
(e.g., Bhalla & Weiss, 2010; Davison et al., 2006; value theories present attractive opportunities to inves-
Dorsch et al., 2009), but clearly more complete tigate peer interactions and relationships as sources of
testing is needed of Eccles et al.’s model of parental physical activity motivation.
influence in the context of physical activity. Given that children and adolescents possess an
Coaches and teachers clearly have an important entire social network of significant adults and peers,
influence on the athletes and students with whom an important direction is to simultaneously exam-
they work, yet many questions remain to be answered ine children’s relationships with all relevant socializ-
in the study of these social-contextual determinants of ers and their physical activity motivation (see Weiss
motivation (see Amorose, 2007; Horn, 2008; Mageau & Amorose, 2008; Weiss & Stuntz, 2004). Recent
& Vallerand, 2003). For instance, we know relatively studies have begun to assess the contribution of two
little about why coaches and physical education teach- sources of social influence on youth participants’
ers act the way they do. A number of scholars have motivation-related constructs using statistical mod-
identified potentially important determinants such eling techniques (e.g., Cox et al., 2009; Price &
as characteristics of the coach/teacher, the situation, Weiss, 2011b; Sabiston & Crocker, 2008; Ullrich-
and characteristics of the athletes/students (see Chel- French & Smith, 2006, 2009). However, investigat-
ladurai, 2007; Horn, 2008; Horn, Lox, & Labrador, ing the influence of children’s social networks on
2010; Mageau & Vallerand, 2003; Smoll & Smith, psychosocial and behavioral outcomes will no doubt
2002). However, with the exception of a few stud- necessitate a variety of methodologies, including
ies (e.g., Price & Weiss, 2000; Taylor, Ntoumanis, & ethnographic, interview, observational, and survey
Smith, 2009; Taylor, Ntoumanis, & Standage, 2008), techniques, to obtain a complete picture of this pro-
these relationships remain understudied. cess (see Garcia Bengoechea & Strean, 2007).

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


Concluding Remarks N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and self-
determination in exercise and sport (pp. 209–227). Cham-
Physical activity is a unique context for pro-
paign, IL: Human Kinetics.
moting positive youth development, including Amorose, A. J., & Anderson-Butcher, D. (2007). Autonomy-
self-perceptions, emotional responses, motivational supportive coaching and self-determined motivation in high
orientations, and participation behaviors (Weiss & school and college athletes: A test of self-determination the-
Wiese-Bjornstal, 2009). We cannot assume find- ory. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 654–670.
Amorose, A. J., & Horn, T. S. (2000). Intrinsic motivation: Rela-
ings from other domains such as school, neighbor-
tionships with collegiate athletes’ gender, scholarship status,
hood, and community clubs translate to youths’ and perceptions of their coaches’ behavior. Journal of Sport
experiences in sport and physical activity. It is nec- and Exercise Psychology, 22, 63–84.
essary to consider the nuances and complexities of Amorose, A. J., & Horn, T. S. (2001). Pre- to post-season
this context that is so ubiquitous among boys’ and changes in the intrinsic motivation of first year college ath-
letes: Relationships with coaching behavior and scholarship
girls’ involvement today. Practical theories for the
status. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 13, 355–373.
physical activity domain include competence moti- Babkes, M. L., & Weiss, M. R. (1999). Parental influence on
vation, self-determination, achievement goal, and cognitive and affective responses in children’s competitive
expectancy-value theories. All have been supported soccer participation. Pediatric Exercise Science, 11, 44–62.
through model testing that reveals theoretically Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A
social cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
consistent relationships among social, psychologi-
Barkoukis, V., Ntoumanis, N., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani, C.
cal, and motivational constructs. However, experi- (2010). Developmental changes in achievement motivation
mental, intervention, longitudinal, and qualitative and affect in physical education: Growth trajectories and
designs and methodologies can contribute substan- demographic differences. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11,
tially to filling the gaps in our understanding of tem- 83–90.
Barnett, N. P., Smoll, F. L., & Smith, R. E. (1992). Effects of
poral, short- versus long-lasting, and strength and
enhancing coach-athlete relationships on youth sport attri-
meaningfulness of effects on youths’ physical activ- tion. The Sport Psychologist, 6, 111–127.
ity motivation. Such work can notably elevate our Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani,
knowledge about determinants of physical activity C. (2009). A review of controlling motivation strategies from
motivation and, in turn, health and well-being. a self-determination theory perspective: Implications for
coaches. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology,
2, 215–233.
Note Bartholomew, K. J., Ntoumanis, N., & Thøgersen-Ntoumani,
1. Other theoretical frameworks have been used to study C. (2010). The controlling interpersonal style in a coach-
youth sport and physical activity motivation, such as the sport ing context: Development and initial validation of a psy-
commitment model and causal attribution theory (see Crocker, chometric scale. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 32,
Hoar, et al., 2004; Weiss & Amorose, 2008). It is beyond the 193–216.
scope of this chapter to review all theories, so we selected ones Bass, B. M. (1998). Transformational leadership: Industry, mili-
that have been most productive for understanding physical activ- tary, and educational impact. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
ity motivation among youth. Baumeister, R., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong:
Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental
References human motive. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529.
Adie, J. W., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2008). Autonomy Bhalla, J. A., & Weiss, M. R. (2010). A cross-cultural perspective
support, basic need satisfaction and the optimal functioning of parental influence on achievement beliefs and behaviors in
of adult male and female sport participants: A test of basic sport and school domains. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
needs theory. Motivation and Emotion, 32, 189–199. Sport, 81, 494–505.
Allen, J. B. (2003). Social motivation in youth sport. Journal of Biddle, S. J. H. (2001). Enhancing motivation in physical educa-
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, 551–567. tion. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Advances in motivation in sport
Ames, C. (1992). Achievement goals, motivational climate, and and exercise (pp. 101–128). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
motivational processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation Black, S. J., & Weiss, M. R. (1992). The relationship among per-
in sport and exercise (pp. 161–176). Champaign, IL: Human ceived coaching behaviors, perceptions of ability, and moti-
Kinetics. vation in competitive age-group swimmers. Journal of Sport
Amorose, A. J. (2001). Intraindividual variability of self- and Exercise Psychology, 14, 309–325.
evaluations in the physical domain: Prevalence, consequences, Blanchard, C. M., Amiot, C. E., Perreault, S., Vallerand, R. J.,
and antecedents. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 23, & Provencher, P. (2009). Cohesiveness, coach’s interper-
222–244. sonal style and psychological needs: Their effects on self-
Amorose, A. J. (2002). The influence of reflected appraisals on determination and athletes’ subjective well-being. Psychology
middle school and high school athletes’ self-perceptions of of Sport and Exercise, 10, 545–551.
sport competence. Pediatric Exercise Science, 14, 377–390. Bois, J. E., Lalanne, J., & Delforge, C. (2009). The influence of
Amorose, A. J. (2007). Coaching effectiveness: Exploring the parenting practices and parental presence on children’s and
relationship between coaching behavior and motivation from adolescents’ pre-competitive anxiety. Journal of Sports Sci-
a self-determination theory perspective. In M. S. Hagger & ences, 27, 995–1005.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Bois, J. E., Sarrazin, P. G., Brustad, R. J., Trouilloud, D. O., & Conroy, D. E., & Coatsworth, J. D. (2007). Coaching behaviors
Cury, F. (2002). Mothers’ expectancies and young adoles- associated with changes in fear of failure: Changes in self-
cents’ perceived physical competence: A yearlong study. Jour- talk and need satisfaction as potential mechanisms. Journal of
nal of Early Adolescence, 22, 384–406. Personality, 75, 383–419.
Bois, J. E., Sarrazin, P. G., Brustad, R. J., Chanal, J. P., & Trouil- Cox, A., Duncheon, N., & McDavid, L. (2009). Teachers as
loud, D. O. (2005). Parents’ appraisals, reflected appraisals, sources of relatedness perceptions, motivation, and affective
and children’s self-appraisals of sport competence: A yearlong responses in physical education. Research Quarterly for Exer-
study. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 17, 273–289. cise and Sport, 80, 765–773.
Bois, J. E., Sarrazin, P. G., Brustad, R. J., Trouilloud, D. O., & Cox, A. E., & Whaley, D. E. (2004). The influence of task value,
Cury, F. (2005). Elementary schoolchildren’s perceived compe- expectancies for success, and identity on athletes’ achievement
tence and physical activity involvement: The influence of par- behaviors. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 103–117.
ents’ role modeling behaviours and perceptions of their child’s Cox, A., & Williams, L. (2008). The roles of perceived teacher
competence. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 6, 381–397. support, motivational climate, and psychological need satis-
Boone, E. M., & Leadbeater, B. J. (2006). Game on: Dimin- faction in students’ physical education motivation. Journal of
ishing risks for depressive symptoms in early adolescence Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30, 222–239.
through positive involvement in team sports. Journal of Crick, N. R., Bigbee, M. A., & Howes, C. (1996). Gender differ-
Research on Adolescence, 16, 79–90. ences in children’s normative beliefs about aggression: How
Brodkin, P., & Weiss, M. R. (1990). Developmental differences do I hurt thee? Let me count the ways. Child Development,
in motivation for participating in competitive swimming. 67, 1003–1014.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 12, 248–263. Crocker, P. R. E., Hoar, S. D., McDonough, M. H., Kowalski, K. C., &
Brustad, R. J. (1992). Integrating socialization influences into Niefer, C. B. (2004). Emotional experience in youth sport.
the study of children’s motivation in sport. Journal of Sport In M. R. Weiss (Ed.), Developmental sport and exercise psy-
and Exercise Psychology, 14, 59–77. chology: A lifespan perspective (pp. 197–221). Morgantown,
Brustad, R. J. (1993). Who will go out and play? Parental and WV: Fitness Information Technology.
psychological influences on children’s attraction to physical Crocker, P. R. E., Kowalski, K. C., Hoar, S. D., & McDonough,
activity. Pediatric Exercise Science, 5, 210–223. M. H. (2004). Emotion in sport across adulthood. In M.
Brustad, R. J. (2010). The role of family in promoting physi- R. Weiss (Ed.), Developmental sport and exercise psychology:
cal activity. President’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports A lifespan perspective (pp. 197–221). Morgantown, WV: Fit-
Research Digest, Series 10 (3), 1–8. ness Information Technology.
Bukowski, W. M., & Hoza, B. (1989). Popularity and friend- Cury, F., Biddle, S. J. H., Sarrazin, P., & Famose, J. P. (1997).
ship: Issues in theory, measurement, and outcome. In Achievement goals and perceived ability predict investment
T. J. Berndt & G. W. Ladd (Eds.), Peer relationships in child in learning a sport task. British Journal of Educational Psychol-
development (pp. 15–45). New York: Wiley. ogy, 67, 293–309.
Bukowski, W. M., Newcomb, A. F., & Hartup, W. W. (1996). Davison, K. K. (2004). Activity-related support from parents,
Friendship and its significance in childhood and adoles- peers, and siblings and adolescents’ physical activity: Are
cence: Introduction and comment. In W. M. Bukowski, there gender differences? Journal of Physical Activity and
A. F. Newcomb, & W. W. Hartup (Eds.), The company they Health, 1, 363–376.
keep: Friendship in childhood and adolescence (pp. 1–15). Davison, K. K., Cutting, T. M., & Birch, L. L. (2003). Par-
New York: Cambridge University Press. ents’ activity-related parenting practices predict girls’ physi-
Carron, A. V., & Brawley, L. R. (2008). Group dynamics in sport cal activity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 35,
and physical activity. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport 1589–1595.
psychology (3rd ed., pp. 213–237). Champaign, IL: Human Davison, K. K., & Jago, R. (2009). Change in parent and peer
Kinetics. support across ages 9 to 15 yr and adolescent girls’ physi-
Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Hagger, M. S. (2009). Effects of cal activity. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise, 41,
an intervention based on self-determination theory on self- 1816–1825.
reported leisure-time physical activity participation. Psychol- Davison, K. K., Symons Downs, D., & Birch, L. L. (2006).
ogy and Health, 24, 29–48. Pathways linking perceived athletic competence and paren-
Chelladurai, P. (2007). Leadership in sports. In G. Tenenbaum tal support at age 9 years to girls’ physical activity at age 11
& R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (3rd ed., years. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 77, 23–31.
pp. 113–135). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley. deCharms, R. (1968). Personal causation: The internal affective
Chelladurai, P., & Saleh, S. D. (1980). Dimensions of leader determinants of behavior. New York: Academic Press.
behavior in sports: Development of a leadership scale. Jour- De Knop, P., Engstrom, L. M., Skirstad, B., & Weiss, M. R.
nal of Sport Psychology, 2, 34–45. (Eds.). (1996). Worldwide trends in youth sport. Champaign,
Coakley, J. J., & White, A. (1992). Making decisions: Gender IL: Human Kinetics.
and sport participation among British adolescents. Sociology Deci, E. L. (1975). Intrinsic motivation. New York: Plenum.
of Sport Journal, 9, 20–35. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-
Coatsworth, J. D., & Conroy, D. E. (2006). Enhancing self- determination in human behavior. New York: Plenum Pub-
esteem of youth swimmers through coach training: Gender lishing Corporation.
and age effects. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 173–192. Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (1987). The support of autonomy
Coatsworth, J. D., & Conroy, D. E. (2009). The effect of and the control of behavior. Journal of Personality and Social
autonomy-supportive coaching, need satisfaction, and self- Psychology, 53, 1024–1037.
perceptions on initiative and identity in youth swimmers. Dempsey, J. M., Kimiecik, J. C., & Horn, T. S. (1993). Parental
Developmental Psychology, 45, 320–328. influence on children’s moderate to vigorous physical activity

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


participation: An expectancy-value approach. Pediatric Exer- Growth trajectories in two male-sex-typed domains. Devel-
cise Science, 5, 151–167. opmental Psychology, 38, 519–533.
Denault, A-S., & Poulin, F. (2009). Predictors of adolescent Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Parental influences on
participation in organized activities: A five-year longitudinal youth involvement in sports. In M. R. Weiss (Ed.), Devel-
study. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 19, 287–311. opmental sport and exercise psychology: A lifespan perspective
Dorsch, T. E., Smith, A. L., & McDonough, M. H. (2009). (pp. 145–164). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Information
Parents’ perceptions of child-to-parent socialization in orga- Technology.
nized youth sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2005). Family socialization, gen-
31, 444–468. der, and sport motivation and involvement. Journal of Sport
Duda, J. L., & Balaguer, I. (2007). Coach-created motivational and Exercise Psychology, 27, 3–31.
climate. In S. Jowett & D. Lavallee (Eds.), Social psychology Freedson, P. S., & Evenson, S. (1991). Familial aggregation in
in sport (pp. 117–130). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. physical activity. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 62,
Duda, J. L, & Treasure, D. C. (2010). Motivational processes 384–389.
and the facilitation of quality engagement in sport. In Furman, W., & Buhrmester, D. (1985). Children’s perceptions
J. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal growth of the personal relationships in their social networks. Devel-
to peak performance (6th ed., pp. 59–80). Boston, MA: opmental Psychology, 21, 1016–1024.
McGraw Hill. Gagné, M., Ryan, R. M., & Bargmann, K. (2003). Autonomy
Duncan, S. C. (1993). The role of cognitive appraisal and friend- support and need satisfaction in the motivation and well-
ship provisions in adolescents’ affect and motivation toward being of gymnasts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15,
activity in physical education. Research Quarterly for Exercise 372–389.
and Sport, 64, 314–323. Garcia Bengoechea, E., & Strean, W. B. (2007). On the interper-
Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self theories: Their role in motivation, person- sonal context of adolescents’ sport motivation. Psychology of
ality, and development. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. Sport and Exercise, 8, 195–217.
Ebbeck, V., & Gibbons, S. L. (1998). The effect of a team build- Gill, D. L., Gross, J. B., & Huddleston, S. (1983). Participation
ing program on the self-conceptions of grade 6 and 7 physical motivation in youth sports. International Journal of Sport Psy-
education students. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, chology, 14, 1–14.
20, 300–310. Gill, D. L., & Williams, L. (2008). Psychological dynamics of sport
Ebbeck, V., & Weiss, M. R. (1998). Determinants of children’s and exercise (3rd ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
self-esteem: An examination of perceived competence and Gillet, N., Vallerand, R. J., Amoura, S., & Baldes, B. (2010).
affect in sport. Pediatric Exercise Science, 10, 285–298. Influence of coaches’ autonomy support on athletes’ motiva-
Eccles, J. S., Adler, T. E., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., tion and sport performance: A test of the hierarchical model
Meece, J. L., & Midgley, C. (1983). Expectancies, values, of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Psychology of Sport and
and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement Exercise, 11, 155–161.
and achievement motivation (pp. 75–146). San Francisco, Glenn, S. D., & Horn, T. S. (1993). Psychological and personal
CA: W.H. Freeman. predictors of leadership behavior in female soccer athletes.
Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R. D. (1991). Gender differences in Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 5, 17–34.
sport involvement: Applying the Eccles’ expectancy-value Gore, S., Farrell, F., & Gordon, J. (2001). Sports involvement
model. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 3, 7–35. as protection against depressed mood. Journal of Research on
Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A. W., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motiva- Adolescence, 11, 119–130.
tion to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg Gould, D. (1993). Intensive sport participation and the pre-
(Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology. Vol. 3: Social, emo- pubescent athlete: Competitive stress and burnout. In
tional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 1017–1095). B. R. Cahill & A. J. Pearl (Eds.), Intensive participation
New York: Wiley. in children’s sports (pp. 19–38). Champaign, IL: Human
Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motivation and Kinetics.
achievement goals. Educational Psychologist, 34, 169–189. Gould, D., Horn, T., & Spreeman, J. (1983). Sources of stress in
Evans, J., & Roberts, G. C. (1987). Physical competence and the junior elite wrestlers. Journal of Sport Psychology, 5, 159–171.
development of children’s peer relations. Quest, 39, 23–35. Gould, D., Tuffey, S., Udry, E., & Loehr, J. (1996). Burnout
Eys, M. A., Loughead, T. M., & Hardy, J. (2007). Athlete leader- in competitive junior tennis players: II. Qualitative analysis.
ship dispersion and satisfaction in interactive sport teams. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 341–366.
Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 281–296. Gould, D., Udry, E., Tuffey, S., & Loehr, J. (1996). Burnout in
Ferrer Caja, E., & Weiss, M. R. (2000). Predictors of intrinsic competitive junior tennis players: I. A quantitative psycho-
motivation among adolescent students in physical education. logical assessment. The Sport Psychologist, 10, 322–340.
Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 71, 267–279. Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., Barkoukis, V., Wang,
Ferrer Caja, E., & Weiss, M. R. (2002). Cross-validation of C. K. J., & Baranowski, J. (2005). Perceived autonomy sup-
a model of intrinsic motivation with students enrolled in port in physical education and leisure-time physical activity:
high school elective courses. The Journal of Experimental A cross-cultural evaluation of the trans-contextual model.
Education, 71, 41–65. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 376–390.
Fox, K. R., & Wilson, P. M. (2008). Self-perceptual systems Harter, S. (1978). Effectance motivation reconsidered. Human
and physical activity. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport Development, 21, 34–64.
psychology (3rd. ed., pp. 49–64). Champaign, IL: Human Harter, S. (1981a). A model of intrinsic mastery motivation in
Kinetics. children: Individual differences and developmental change.
Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Children’s competence In W. A. Collins (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psy-
and value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: chology (Vol. 14, pp. 215–255). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Harter, S. (1981b). The development of competence motivation ability perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
in the mastery of cognitive and physical skills: Is there still ogy, 63, 932–944.
a place for joy? In G. C. Roberts & D. M. Landers (Eds.), Jensen, C. D., & Steele, R. G. (2009). Body dissatisfaction,
Psychology of motor behavior and sport—1980 (pp. 3–29). weight criticism, and self-reported physical activity in pre-
Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. adolescent children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34,
Hartup, W. W. (1995). The three faces of friendship. Journal of 822–826.
Social and Personal Relationships, 12, 569–574. Jowett, S., & Poczwardowski, A. (2007), Understanding the
Harwood, C., Spray, C. M., & Keegan, R. (2008). Achievement coach-athlete relationship. In S. Jowett & D. Lavallee (Eds.),
goal theories in sport. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport Social psychology in sport (pp. 3–14). Champaign, IL: Human
psychology (3rd. ed., pp. 157–186). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Kinetics. Kimiecik, J. C., & Horn, T. S. (1998). Parental beliefs and chil-
Hayashi, C. T., & Weiss, M. R. (1994). A cross-cultural analy- dren’s moderate-to-vigorous physical activity. Research Quar-
sis of achievement motivation in Anglo and Japanese mara- terly for Exercise and Sport, 69, 163–175.
thon runners. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 25, Kimiecik, J. C., Horn, T. S., & Shurin, C. S. (1996). Relation-
187–202. ships among children’s beliefs, perceptions of their parents’
Hein, V., & Hagger, M. S. (2007). Global self-esteem, goal beliefs, and their moderate-to-vigorous physical activity.
achievement orientations, and self-determined behavioral Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 67, 324–336.
regulations in a physical education setting. Journal of Sport King, K. A., Tergerson, J. L., & Wilson, B. R. (2008). Effect
Sciences, 25, 149–159. of social support on adolescents’ perceptions of and engage-
Hein, V., & Koka, A. (2007), Perceived feedback and motivation ment in physical activity. Journal of Physical Activity and
in physical education and physical activity. In M. S. Hagger Health, 5, 374–384.
& N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and Kipp, L. E., & Amorose, A. J. (2008). Perceived motivational cli-
self-determination in exercise and sport (pp. 127–140). Cham- mate and self-determined motivation in female high-school
paign, IL: Human Kinetics. athletes. Journal of Sport Behavior, 31, 108–129.
Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The effects of praise Klint, K. A., & Weiss, M. R. (1987). Perceived competence and
on children’s intrinsic motivation: A review and synthesis. motives for participating in youth sports: A test of Harter’s
Psychological Bulletin, 128, 774–795. competence motivation theory. Journal of Sport Psychology,
Hollembeak, J., & Amorose, A. J. (2005). Perceived coaching 9, 55–65.
behaviors and college athletes’ intrinsic motivation: A test of Koka, A., & Hagger, M. (2010). Perceived teaching behaviors
self-determination theory. Journal of Applied Sport Psycholo- and self-determined motivation in physical education: A test
gy.17, 20–36. of self-determination theory. Research Quarterly for Exercise
Horn, T. S. (2004). Developmental perspectives on self- and Sport, 81, 74–86.
perceptions in children and adolescents. In M. R. Weiss Kunesh, M. A., Hasbrook, C. A., & Lewthwaite, R. (1992).
(Ed.), Developmental sport and exercise psychology: A lifespan Physical activity socialization: Peer interactions and affective
perspective (pp. 101–143). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Infor- responses among a sample of sixth grade girls. Sociology of
mation Technology. Sport Journal, 9, 385–396.
Horn, T. S. (2008). Coaching effectiveness in the sport domain. Ladd, G. W., & Price, J. M. (1993). Playstyles of peer-
In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology (3rd. ed., pp. accepted and peer-rejected children on the playground. In
240–267). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. C. H. Hart (Ed.), Children on playgrounds: Research perspec-
Horn, T. S., & Amorose, A. J. (1998). Sources of competence tives and applications (pp. 130–161). Albany: State Univer-
information. In J. L. Duda (Ed.), Advances in sport and exer- sity of New York Press.
cise psychology measurement (pp. 49–64). Morgantown, WV: Lewthwaite, R., & Piparo, A. J. (1993). Goal orientations in
Fitness Information Technology. young competitive athletes: Physical achievement, social-
Horn, T. S., & Horn, J. L. (2007). Family influences on chil- relational, and experiential concerns. Journal of Research in
dren’s sport and physical activity participation, behavior, and Personality, 27, 103–117.
psychosocial responses. In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund MacPhail, A., Gorely, T., Kirk, D., & Kinchin, G. (2008). Chil-
(Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology (pp. 685–711). Hoboken, dren’s experiences of fun and enjoyment during a season of
NJ: Wiley. sport education. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 79,
Horn, T. S., Lox, C. L. & Labrador, F. (2010). The self-fulfilling 344–355.
prophecy theory: When coaches’ expectations become real- Mageau, G. A., & Vallerand, R. J. (2003). The coach-athlete
ity. In J. M. Williams (Ed.), Applied sport psychology: Personal relationship: A motivational model. Journal of Sports Sciences,
growth to peak performance (6th ed., pp. 81–105). Palo Alto, 21, 883–904.
CA: Mayfield. Maehr, M. L., & Nicholls, J. G. (1980). Culture and achieve-
Horn, T. S., & Weiss, M. R. (1991). A developmental analysis ment motivation: A second look. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies
of children’s self-ability judgments. Pediatric Exercise Science, in cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 221–267). New York:
3, 312–328. Academic Press.
Jaakkola, T., & Liukkonen, J. (2006). Changes in students’ self- Marsh, H. W., & Peart, N. D. (1988). Competitive and coop-
determined motivation and goal orientation as a result of erative physical fitness training programs for girls: Effects on
motivational climate intervention within high school physi- physical fitness and multidimensional self-concepts. Journal
cal education classes. International Journal of Sport and Exer- of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 10, 390–407.
cise Psychology, 4, 302–324. McCullagh, P., & Weiss, M. R. (2002). Observational learning:
Jacobs, J. E., & Eccles, J. S. (1992). The impact of mothers’ The forgotten psychological method in sport psychology. In
gender-role stereotypic beliefs on mothers’ and children’s J. Van Raalte & B. W. Brewer (Eds.), Exploring sport and

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


exercise psychology (2nd ed., pp. 131–149). Washington, DC: Raedeke, T. D., & Smith, A. L. (2001). Development and pre-
American Psychological Association. liminary validation of an athlete burnout measure. Journal of
Moran, M. M., & Weiss, M. R. (2006). Peer leadership in sport: Sport and Exercise Psychology, 23, 281–306.
Links with friendship, peer acceptance, psychological charac- Raedeke, T. D., & Smith, A. L. (2004). Coping resources and
teristics, and athletic ability. Journal of Applied Sport Psychol- athlete burnout: An examination of stress mediated and
ogy, 2, 97–113. moderation hypothesis. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychol-
Newton, M., Duda, J. L., & Yin, Z. N. (2000). Examination ogy, 26, 525–541.
of the psychometric properties of the Perceived Motivational Rees, C. R. (1983). Instrumental and expressive leadership in
Climate in Sport Questionnaire-2 in a sample of female ath- team sports: A test of leadership role differentiation theory.
letes. Journal of Sport Sciences, 18, 275–290. Journal of Sport Behavior, 6, 17–27.
Nicaise, V, Bois, J., Fairclough, S. J., Amorose, A. J., & Cogérino, Reeve, R. E., & Weiss, M. R. (2006). Sports and physical activi-
G. (2007). Girls’ and boys’ perceptions of physical education ties. In G. G. Bear & K. M. Minke (Eds.), Children’s needs
teachers’ feedback: Effects on performance and psychological III: Development, prevention, and intervention (pp. 485–498).
responses. Journal of Sports Sciences, 25, 915–926. Bethesda, MD: National Association of School Psychologists.
Nicaise, V, Cogérino, G., Bois, J., & Amorose, A. J. (2006). Stu- Roberts, G. C., Treasure, D. C., & Conroy, D. E. (2007). Under-
dents’ perceptions of teacher’s feedback and physical compe- standing the dynamics of motivation in sport and physical
tence in physical education classes: Gender effects. Journal of activity: An achievement goal interpretation. In G. Tenen-
Teaching in Physical Education, 25, 36–57. baum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.), Handbook of sport psychology
Nicholls, J. G. (1989). The competitive ethos and democratic edu- (3rd ed., pp. 3–30). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
cation. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Rowold, J. (2006). Transformational and transactional leader-
Ntoumanis, N. (2001). A self-determination approach to the ship in martial arts. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 18,
understanding of motivation in physical education. British 312–325.
Journal of Educational Psychology, 71, 225–242. Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W. M., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer
Ntoumanis, N. (2005). A prospective study of participation in interactions, relationships, and groups. In W. Damon (Series
optional school physical education using a self-determination Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychol-
theory framework. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, ogy. Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (pp.
444–453. 619–700). New York: Wiley.
Ntoumanis, N., & Biddle, S. (1999). A review of motivational Ryan, R. M. (1995). Psychological need and the facilitation of
climate in physical activity. Journal of Sports Sciences, 17, integrative processes. Journal of Personality, 63, 397–427.
643–665. Ryan, R. M., & Connell, J. P. (1989). Perceived locus of cau-
Papaioannou, A., Marsh, H., & Theodorakis, Y. (2004). A multi- sality and internalization: Examining reasons for acting in
level approach to motivational climate in physical education two domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57,
and sport settings: An individual or group level construct? 749–761.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 26, 90–118. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2000). Self determination theory
Passer, M. W. (1988). Determinants and consequences of chil- and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social develop-
dren’s competitive stress. In F. L. Smoll (Ed.), Children in ment, and well being. American Psychologist, 55, 68–78.
sport (3rd ed., pp. 203–227). Champaign, IL: Human Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). An overview of self-
Kinetics. determination theory: An organismic-dialectical perspec-
Payne, V. G., & Morrow, J. R., Jr. (2009). School physical educa- tive. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of
tion as a viable change agent to increase youth physical activ- self-determination research (pp. 3–33). Rochester, NY: The
ity. President’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports Research University of Rochester Press.
Digest, 10 (2), 1–8. Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2007). Active human nature:
Pelletier, L. G., Fortier, M. S., Vallerand, R. J., & Briére, N. M. Self-determination theory and the promotion and main-
(2001). Associations among perceived autonomy support, tenance of sport, exercise, and health. In M. S. Hagger &
forms of self-regulation, and persistence: A prospective study. N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic motivation and self-
Motivation and Emotion, 25, 279–306. determination in exercise and sport (pp. 1–20). Champaign,
Petlichkoff, L. M. (1993). Relationship of player status and time IL: Human Kinetics.
of season to achievement goals and perceived ability in inter- Sabiston, C. M., & Crocker, P. R. E. (2008). Exploring self-
scholastic athletes. Pediatric Exercise Science, 5, 242–252. perceptions and social influences as correlates of adolescent
Price, M. S., & Weiss, M. R. (2000). Relationships among leisure-time physical activity. Journal of Sport and Exercise
coach burnout, coach behaviors, and athletes’ psychological Psychology, 30, 3–22.
responses. The Sport Psychologist, 14, 391–409. Sagar, S. S., & Lavallee, D. (2010). The developmental origins
Price, M. S., & Weiss, M. R. (2011a). Peer leadership in sport: of fear of failure in adolescent athletes: Examining parental
Relationships among personal characteristics, leader behav- practices. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 11, 177–187.
iors, and team outcomes. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, Salvy, S-J., Bowker, J. W., Roemmich, J. N., Romero, N., Kief-
23, 49–64. fer, E., Paluch, R., & Epstein, L. H. (2008). Peer influence
Price, M. S., & Weiss, M. R. (2011b). Relationships among on children’s physical activity: An experience sampling study.
coach leadership, peer leadership, and adolescent athletes’ Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 33, 29–49.
psychosocial and team outcomes: A test of transformational Salvy, S-J., Roemmich, J. N., Bowker, J. C., Romero, N. D.,
leadership theory. Manuscript submitted for publication. Stadler, P. J., & Epstein, L. H. (2009). Effect of peers
Raedeke, T. D. (1997). Is athlete burnout more than just stress? and friends on youth physical activity and motivation to
A sport commitment perspective. Journal of Sport and Exer- be physically active. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34,
cise Psychology, 19, 396–417. 217–225.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Sanders, C. E., Field, T. M., Diego, M., & Kaplan, M. (2000). tion intentions as predictors of well-being in sport. Journal of
Moderate involvement in sports is related to lower depres- Applied Sport Psychology, 22, 17–33.
sion levels among adolescents. Adolescence, 35, 793–797. Smith, A. L. (1999). Perceptions of peer relationships and physi-
Sarrazin, P., Roberts, G. C., Cury, F., Biddle, S. J. H., & Famose, cal activity participation in early adolescence. Journal of Sport
J. P. (2002). Exerted effort and performance in climbing and Exercise Psychology, 21, 329–350.
among boys: The influence of achievement goals, perceived Smith, A. L. (2003). Peer relationships in physical activity con-
ability, and task difficulty. Research Quarterly for Exercise and texts: A road less traveled in youth sport and exercise psychol-
Sport, 73, 425–436. ogy research. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 4, 25–39.
Sarrazin, P., Vallerand, R., Guillet, E., Pelletier, L., & Cury, F. Smith, A. L., Ullrich-French, S., Walker, E., II, & Hurley, K. S.
(2002). Motivation and dropout in female handballers: (2006). Peer relationship profiles and motivation in youth
A 21-month prospective study. European Journal of Social sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 28, 362–382.
Psychology, 32, 395–418. Smith, R. E., & Smoll, F. L. (2007). Social-cognitive approach
Scanlan, T. K., Carpenter, P. J., Lobel, M., & Simons, J. P. to coaching behaviors. In S. Jowett & D. Lavallee (Eds.),
(1993). Sources of enjoyment for youth sport athletes. Pedi- Social psychology in sport (pp. 3–14). Champaign, IL: Human
atric Exercise Science, 5, 275–285. Kinetics.
Scanlan, T. K., & Lewthwaite, R. (1984). Social psychological Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L, & Barnett, N. P. (1995). Reduction of
aspects of competition for male youth sport participants: children’s sport performance anxiety through social support
I: Predictors of competitive stress. Journal of Sport Psychology, and stress-reduction training for coaches. Journal of Applied
6, 208–226. Developmental Psychology, 16, 125–142.
Scanlan, T. K., & Passer, M. W. (1979). Sources of competitive Smith, R. E., Smoll, F. L., & Cumming, S. P. (2007). Effects of
stress in young female athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology, a motivational climate intervention for coaches on young
1, 151–159. athletes’ sport performance anxiety. Journal of Sport and Exer-
Scanlan, T. K., Ravizza, K. & Stein, G. L. (1989). An in-depth cise Psychology, 29, 39–59.
study of former elite figure skaters: I. Introduction to the Smoll, F. L., & Smith, R. E. (2002). Coaching behavior research
project. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 54–64. and intervention in youth sport. In F. L. Smoll & R. E. Smith
Scanlan, T. K., & Simons, J. P. (1992). The construct of sport (Eds.), Children and youth in sport (pp. 211–231). Dubuque,
enjoyment. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.), Motivation in sport and IA: Kendall/Hunt.
exercise (pp. 199–216). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Smoll, F. L., Smith, R. E., Barnett, N. P., & Everett, J. J. (1993).
Scanlan, T. K., Stein, G. L., & Ravizza, K. (1991). An in-depth Enhancement of children’s self-esteem through social sup-
study of former elite figure skaters: II. Sources of stress. Jour- port training for youth sport coaches. Journal of Applied Psy-
nal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 13, 102–120. chology, 78, 602–610.
Schilling, T. A., & Hayashi, C. T. (2001). Achievement moti- Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2003). A model
vation among high school basketball and cross-country ath- of contextual motivation in physical education: Using con-
letes: A personal investment perspective. Journal of Applied structs from self-determination and achievement goal theories
Sport Psychology, 13, 103–128. to predict physical activity intentions. Journal of Educational
Schmidt, G. W., & Stein, G. L. (1991). Sport Commitment: Psychology, 95, 97–110.
A model integrating enjoyment, dropout, and burnout. Jour- Standage, M., Duda, J. L., & Ntoumanis, N. (2006). Students’
nal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 8, 254–265. motivational processes and their relationship to teacher rat-
Schneider, M., Dunn, A., & Cooper, D. (2009). Affect, exercise, ings in school physical education: A self-determination the-
and physical activity among healthy adolescents. Journal of ory approach. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 77,
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 31, 706–723. 100–110.
Schneider, M., Fridlund Dunton, G., & Cooper, D. M. (2008). Standage, M., & Gillison, F. (2007). Students’ motivational
Physical activity and physical self-concept among sedentary responses toward school physical education and their rela-
adolescent females: An intervention study. Psychology of Sport tionship to general self-esteem and health-related quality of
and Exercise, 9, 1–14. life. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 704–721.
Schofield, L., Mummery, W. K., Schofield, G., & Hopkins, W. Standage, M., Gillison, F., & Treasure, D. C. (2007), Self-
(2007). The association of objectively determined physical determination theory and motivation in physical education.
activity behavior among adolescent female friends. Research In M. S. Hagger & N. L. D. Chatzisarantis (Eds.), Intrinsic
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 9–15. motivation and self-determination in exercise and sport (pp.
Schunk, D. H. (1998). Peer modeling. In K. Topping & S. Ehly 71–86). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
(Eds.), Peer-assisted learning (pp. 185–202). Mahwah, NJ: Storch, E. A., Milsom, V. A., DeBraganza, N., Lewin, A. B.,
Erlbaum. Geffken, G. R., & Silverstein, J. H. (2007). Peer victimiza-
Sirard, J. R., Pfeiffer, K. A., & Pate, R. R. (2006). Motiva- tion, psychological adjustment, and physical activity in over-
tional factors associated with sports program participation weight and at-risk-for-overweight youth. Journal of Pediatric
in middle school students. Journal of Adolescent Health, 38, Psychology, 32, 80–89.
696–703. Stuart, M. E. (2003). Sources of subjective task value in sport:
Smith, A., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. (2007). Goal striving, An examination of adolescences with high or low value for
goal attainment, and well-being: Adapting and testing the sport. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15, 239–255.
self-concordance model in sport. Journal of Sport and Exercise Stuntz, C. P., & Spearance, A. L. (2010). Cross-domain rela-
Psychology, 29, 763–782. tionships in two sport populations: Measurement validation
Smith, A., Ntoumanis, N., & Duda, J. (2010). An investiga- including prediction of motivation-related variables. Psychol-
tion of coaching behavior, goal motives, and implementa- ogy of Sport and Exercise, 11, 267–274.

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


Stuntz, C. P., & Weiss, M. R. (2003). Influence of social goal Wankel, L. M., & Kreisel, P. S. J. (1985). Factors underlying
orientations and peers on unsportsmanlike play. Research enjoyment of youth sports: Sport and age group compari-
Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 74, 421–435. sons. Journal of Sport Psychology, 7, 51–64.
Stuntz, C. P., & Weiss, M. R. (2009). Achievement goal orienta- Weiss, M. R., & Amorose, A. J. (2005). Children’s self-perceptions
tions and motivational outcomes in youth sport: The role in the physical domain: Between- and within-age variability
of social orientations. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, in level, accuracy, and sources of perceived competence. Jour-
255–262. nal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 27, 226–244.
Stuntz, C. P., & Weiss, M. R. (2010). Motivating children and Weiss, M. R., & Amorose, A. J. (2008). Motivational orienta-
adolescents to sustain a physically active lifestyle. American tions and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in
Journal of Lifestyle Medicine, 4, 433–444. sport psychology (3rd ed., pp. 115–155). Champaign, IL:
Taylor, I. M., Ntoumanis, N., & Smith, B. (2009). The social Human Kinetics.
context as a determinant of teacher motivational strategies Weiss, M. R., Amorose, A. J., & Wilko, A. M. (2009). Coaching
in physical education. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 10, behaviors, motivational climate, and psychosocial outcomes
235–243. among female adolescent athletes. Pediatric Exercise Science,
Taylor, I. M., Ntoumanis, N., & Standage, M. (2008). 21, 475–492.
A self-determination theory approach to understanding the Weiss, M. R., Bhalla, J. A., & Price, M. S. (2007). Developing
antecedents of teachers’ motivational strategies in physi- positive self-perceptions through youth sport participation.
cal education. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 30, In H. Hebestreit & O. Bar-Or (Eds.), The encyclopaedia of
75–94. sports medicine. Vol. 10: The young athlete (pp. 302–318).
Taylor, I. M., Ntoumanis, N., Standage, M., & Spray, C. M. Oxford, England: Blackwell.
(2010). Motivational predictors of physical education stu- Weiss, M. R., Bredemeier, B. J., & Shewchuk, R. M. (1986). The
dents’ effort, exercise intentions, and leisure-time physical dynamics of perceived competence, perceived control, and
activity: A multilevel linear growth analysis. Journal of Sport motivational orientation in youth sports. In M. R. Weiss &
and Exercise Psychology, 32, 99–120. D. Gould (Eds.), Sport for children and youths (pp. 89–101).
Taymoori, P., & Lubans, D. R. (2008). Mediators of behavior Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
change in two tailored physical activity interventions for ado- Weiss, M. R., & Duncan, S. C. (1992). The relation between
lescent girls. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 9, 605–619. physical competence and peer acceptance in the context of
Tessier, D., Sarrazin, P., & Ntoumanis, N. (2008). The effects of children’s sport participation. Journal of Sport and Exercise
an experimental programme to support students’ autonomy Psychology, 14, 177–191.
on the overt behaviors of physical education teachers. Euro- Weiss, M. R., & Ferrer Caja, E. (2002). Motivational orienta-
pean Journal of Psychology of Education, 23, 239–253. tions and sport behavior. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in
Theeboom, M., De Knop, P., & Weiss, M. R. (1995). Motiva- sport psychology (2nd ed., pp. 101–184). Champaign, IL:
tional climate, psychosocial responses, and motor skill devel- Human Kinetics.
opment in children’s sport: A field-based intervention study. Weiss, M. R., & Fretwell, S. D. (2005). The parent-coach/child-
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 17, 294–311. athlete relationship in youth sport: Cordial, contentious, or
Tropp, K. J., & Landers, D. M. (1979). Team interaction and the conundrum? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 76,
emergence of leadership and interpersonal attraction in field 286–305.
hockey. Journal of Sport Psychology, 1, 228–240. Weiss, M. R., & Hayashi, C. T. (1995). All in the family: Parent-
U.S. Department of Health and Human Services (2008). 2008 child socialization influences in competitive youth gymnas-
Physical Activity Guidelines for Americans. Washington, DC. tics. Pediatric Exercise Science, 7, 36–48.
Ullrich-French, S., & Smith, A. L. (2006). Perceptions of rela- Weiss, M. R., Kimmel, L. A., & Smith, A. L. (2001). Deter-
tionships with parents and peers in youth sport: Independent minants of sport commitment among junior tennis players:
and combined prediction of motivational outcomes. Psychol- Enjoyment as a mediating variable. Pediatric Exercise Science,
ogy of Sport and Exercise, 7, 193–214. 13, 131–144.
Ullrich-French, S., & Smith, A. L. (2009). Social and motiva- Weiss, M. R., McCullagh, P., Smith, A. L., & Berlant, A. R.
tional predictors of continued youth sport participation. Psy- (1998). Observational learning and the fearful child: Influ-
chology of Sport and Exercise, 10, 87–95. ence of peer models on swimming skill performance and
Urdan, T. C., & Maehr, M. L. (1995). Beyond a two-goal the- psychological responses. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
ory of motivation and achievement: A case for social goals. Sport, 69, 380–394.
Review of Educational Research, 65, 213–243. Weiss, M. R., & Petlichkoff, L. M. (1989). Children’s motivation
Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic for participation in and withdrawal from sport: Identifying
and extrinsic motivation. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in the missing links. Pediatric Exercise Science, 1, 195–211.
experimental social psychology (pp. 271–360). San Diego, CA: Weiss, M. R., & Smith, A. L. (1999). Quality of youth sport
Academic Press. friendships: Measurement development and validation. Jour-
Vealey, R. S., Armstrong, L., Comar, W., & Greenleaf, C. A. nal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 145–166.
(1998). Influence of perceived coaching behaviors on burn- Weiss, M. R., & Smith, A. L. (2002). Friendship quality in youth
out and competitive anxiety in female college athletes. Jour- sport: Relationship to age, gender, and motivation variables.
nal of Applied Sport Psychology, 10, 297–318. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 24, 420–437.
Vierling, K. K., Standage, M., & Treasure, D. C. (2007). Predict- Weiss, M. R., Smith, A. L., & Stuntz, C. P. (2008). Moral devel-
ing attitudes and physical activity in an “at-risk” minority opment in sport and physical activity: Theory, research, and
youth sample: A test of self-determination theory. Psychology intervention. In T. S. Horn (Ed.), Advances in sport psychology
of Sport and Exercise, 8, 795–817. (3rd ed. pp. 187–210). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

 youth m otivation and participation i n s p ort a n d ph ys i c a l ac ti vi t y


Weiss, M. R., Smith, A. L., & Theeboom, M. (1996). “That’s Weiss, W. M., & Weiss, M. R. (2006). A longitudinal analysis
what friends are for”: Children’s and teenagers’ perceptions of of sport commitment among competitive female gymnasts.
peer relationships in the sport domain. Journal of Sport and Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 7, 309–323.
Exercise Psychology, 18, 347–379. Weiss, W. M., & Weiss, M. R. (2007). Sport commitment among
Weiss, M. R., & Stuntz, C. P. (2004). A little friendly compe- competitive female gymnasts: A developmental perspective.
tition: Peer relationships and psychosocial development in Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 78, 90–102.
youth sport and physical activity contexts. In M. R. Weiss Weiss, W. M., Weiss, M. R., & Amorose, A. J. (2010). Sport
(Ed.), Developmental sport and exercise psychology: A lifespan commitment among competitive female athletes: Test of an
perspective (pp. 165–196). Morgantown, WV: Fitness Infor- expanded model. Journal of Sports Sciences, 28, 423–434.
mation Technology. Westre, K. R., & Weiss, M. R. (1991). The relationship between
Weiss, M. R., & Wiese-Bjornstal, D. M. (2009). Promoting perceived coaching behaviors and group cohesion in high
positive youth development through physical activity. Presi- school football teams. The Sport Psychologist, 5, 41–54.
dent’s Council on Physical Fitness and Sports Research Digest, White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of
10(3), 1–8. competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–330.
Weiss, M. R., Wiese, D. M., & Klint, K. A. (1989). Head over Wilson, D. K., Williams, J., Evans, A., Mixon, G., & Rheaume,
heels with success: The relationship between self-efficacy and C. (2005). A qualitative study of gender preferences and
performance in competitive youth gymnastics. Journal of motivational factors for physical activity in underserved ado-
Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11, 444–451. lescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30, 293–297.
Weiss, M. R., & Williams, L. (2004). The why of youth sport Yukelson, D., Weinberg, R., Richardson, P., & Jackson,
involvement: A developmental perspective on motivational A. (1983). Interpersonal attraction and leadership within
processes. In M. R. Weiss (Ed.), Developmental sport and collegiate sport teams. Journal of Sport Behavior, 6, 28–36.
exercise psychology: A lifespan perspective (pp. 223–268). Mor- Zacharatos, A., Barling, J., & Kelloway, E. K. (2000). Devel-
gantown, WV: Fitness Information Technology. opment and effects of transformational leadership in adoles-
Weiss, W. M., & Weiss, M. R. (2003). Attraction- and cents. The Leadership Quarterly, 11, 211–226.
entrapment-based commitment among competitive female Zarbatany, L., Ghesquiere, K., & Mohr, K. (1992). A context
gymnasts. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 25, perspective on early adolescents’ friendship expectations.
229–247. Journal of Early Adolescence, 12, 111–126.

wei s s , a moros e, ki p p 


CHAPTER

Through a Fly’s Eye: Multiple Yet


30 Overlapping Perspectives on
Future Directions for Human
Motivation Research
Richard M. Ryan and Nicole Legate

Abstract
In this final chapter we examine future directions in motivation research by looking through the
individual lenses of our volume authors. We review each chapter for viewpoints on new directions for
research. Each chapter offers some unique ideas relevant to the particular area of inquiry, but there
is also overlapping emphasis on several issues facing the field as a whole. The most widely cited future
direction was for more research into dual-process models of motivation. There were also frequent calls
for more intervention research, especially interventions in which process variables and active ingredients
can be carefully assessed. A desire for more developmental and neuropsychological studies of motivation
was also common among this selected group. The centrality of motivation for human adaptation and
wellness makes the pursuit of these topics a central task for psychology.
Key Words: human motivation, dual processes, interventions, development, well-being

The centrality of motivation in human function- human motivation involves the fluid interplay of
ing and wellness is clear, and it supplies the rationale biological, psychological and sociocultural determi-
for this volume. As the papers included here high- nants of what moves us to action.
light, motivation plays an essential role in adaptation Given the importance and the level of complexity
and in both individual and collective well-being. In inherent in the study of human motivation, there is
addition, many of the common assumptions about clearly only going to be increasing research activity
human motivation are wrong or overly simplistic, on this topic in the years ahead. Where is the field
and contemporary research is yielding new insights headed? What gaps do motivation studies need to
into what moves us into action, for better or for address? What are the proximal and distal problems
worse. in line for exploration and discovery?
Collectively these papers also underscore another Each Oxford volume in this series finishes with a
fact: The factors that both underlie and influence “future directions” chapter, typically expressing the
motivation are complex and multilayered. In this views of the editor(s). Although we could finish this
volume, motivation is variously described as being volume with a chapter centering on our own views
molded through evolution; engendered by culture; on the future of motivation research, our sensibilities
facilitated or undermined by parents; impacted by suggest that we provide readers with a more demo-
social contexts such as work, school, and leisure cratic offering. So in this chapter, rather than let-
settings; and dependent upon underlying neuro- ting the editor’s singular (and rather myopic) views
logical mechanisms. In other words, the science of predominate, we will look through the multiple and


independent lenses of our chapter authors to provide a Future Directions in Motivation:
“compound eye” view of the field’s future directions. Assembling a Compound View
To really see the benefits of presenting a “com- Each of the authors of this volume highlights
pound eye” view on future directions in motivation, important new directions for the study of motiva-
it is important to understand a bit more about this tion as he or she sees it. We present these in the
system. A compound eye has some advantages, espe- order they appear in the volume. Some of these rec-
cially when looking ahead. For example, flies see ommendations for future research are specific to the
through such a system, equipped with a convex sur- theoretical framework reviewed in the chapter, but
face carpeted by multiple “eyes” called ommatidia. just as often authors also pointed to common gaps
Each ommatidium has its own lens, and it is pointed in motivation research that currently leave impor-
in a slightly different direction, much like the cur- tant questions less than fully answered.
rent perspectives represented in these handbook
chapters. Yet in the fly, what each of the separate General Theories of Human Motivation
“eyes” senses significantly overlaps with those next social cognitive theory
to it (see, e.g., Riley, Harmann, Barrett, & Wright, and motivation
2008). These sensory inputs come together so that Dale H. Schunk and Ellen L. Usher
the fly perceives one image. Such a complex system
• Schunk and Usher raised questions about
of vision has both scope and redundancy built into
the applicability of social cognitive theory
it, contributing to the fly’s highly skilled capacity to
across all age groups. Because some learning and
navigate and “find the sugar.”
experiences of self-efficacy may often require
Within the present volume are 29 chapters from
complex cognitive capacities, the authors believe
articulate leaders in the field of motivation, each peer-
that understanding developmental constraints
ing into the future of our field. They are pursuing
on these motivational processes represents an
separate but interrelated theoretical questions, and in
important area of further research. Longitudinal
doing so harnessing and refining the scientific tools
methods may help elucidate these questions
available. Each takes a unique perspective, but there
about developmental changes in self-efficacy and
is also overlap in visions of where the field should go.
learning.
Why not let each of these ommatidum contribute its
• Moreover, because most social cognitive
own uniquely weighted input to our perception? No
research in motivation has been conducted in
doubt each will be distinct, but they may also over-
Western societies, Shunck and Usher called for a
lap and offer redundancies that highlight the most
broader examination of the cross-cultural relevancy
pressing issues for future work in human motivation
of the theory.
research. What compound vision might result?
• Specific to social cognitive theory, Schunk
Accordingly, in what follows we have reviewed
and Usher believe that future research should
each of the chapters in this volume to distill some of
focus on how modeled observations can combine
the salient directions for future research in the field
with learner practice to “optimize motivational
of human motivation the chapter authors believe
effects.” They pointed to the importance of
should be pursued. It is important to recognize that
technology in carrying out this objective. Making
the authors, when invited to write for this volume,
modeling more accessible to learners through
were not specifically asked to reflect on future direc-
computers and hand-held devices could make
tions in their chapters. Though some said little in
new and diverse modeling opportunities possible.
this regard, many devoted significant attention to
Moreover, having learners watch their own
raising issues that they believe merit future pur-
performance on video could improve their ability
suit. Some explicitly commented on the important
to self-model.
questions needing to be further explored in their
specific areas of research, and many reflected on the
methodological and substantive directions the field
cybernetic control and
of human motivation more generally ought to fol-
self-regulation of behavior
Charles S. Carver and Michael F. Scheier
low. Once presenting these “nutshell” summaries of
the authors’ reflections on future directions, we will • In their discussion of the self-regulation
see whether we can benefit from their compound of behavior and emotion, Carver and Scheier
vision. reexamined some previous assumptions about

rya n , leg ate 


the hierarchy of behavioral controls. New insights to explore in more depth how death can be a
about the dual modes of functioning, and “constructive, empowering force” for people.
dimensionality within emotions, have prompted
this reconsideration. They suggested that a model too much of a good thing? trade-offs
of hierarchical organization of the self and its goals in promotion and prevention focus
likely involves “pressures toward compatibility” Abigail A. Scholer and E. Tory Higgins
among values and attitudes. At the same time,
• Scholer and Higgins hoped to see future
lower levels of self-regulation may operate
research move beyond analysis at the individual
independently of higher levels of self-regulation
level to explore how groups of individuals with
and may sometimes be in conflict with them.
different levels of promotion and prevention
Testing this idea of compatibility (which in our
orientations work together. This line of research
work in self-determination theory we might think
could help elucidate what work environments
of in terms of integration) is seen by Carver and
look like when they are maximizing the benefits
Scheier as an important area for investigation.
of these two motivational systems.
• The authors also raised questions concerning
• Understanding how promotion and
other compatibility-related conflicts in self-control.
prevention motivations work together with other
For example, does self-control pit longer and
motivations (namely locomotion and assessment
shorter term goals against each other, as usually
motivations) was another area identified for
thought, or does self-control pit the two mental
future investigation. The authors reasoned that
modes against each other (automatic tendencies vs.
understanding the bigger picture of how these
planful effort to restrain behavior)?
different regulatory systems interact could help
• They also wondered whether future research
to maximize the benefits and minimize the costs
might apply their feedback theory to more fully
of each particular motivational system. This is in
address the core motivational processes involved
line with their main argument throughout the
in growth and optimal functioning. For example,
chapter that more motivation isn’t necessarily
perhaps enjoyment signals that engaging in the
better—there are trade-offs involved with each
experience is moving the person toward another
motivational system.
goal that is already part of the self. More generally
understanding ties between affective feedback and
motivation, personality, and
growth functions is an agenda for future research.
development within embedded
social contexts: an overview of
terror management theory self-determination theory
Pelin Kasebir and Tom Pyszczynski Edward L. Deci and Richard M. Ryan
• In line with terror management theory’s • A theme of this chapter was how
(TMT) tradition of employing innovative people exist within embedded social contexts
methods, Kasebir and Pyszczynski called for that affect their psychological need satisfactions
more new and creative methods, beyond death- and wellness both directly and indirectly. Deci
thought accessibility methodology, to explore and Ryan were thus especially interested in
new territory in terror management processes. developing a better understanding of how
• The authors saw TMT’s applications to different levels of social influence (e.g.,
psychopathology and fostering peace as other interpersonal, institutional, cultural, politico-
important future directions of research. economic) interact to impact motivational
• They also saw further investigation into outcomes and well-being.
meaning and certainty, both epistemic and • There was special interest in both new
existential forms, as helping to reconcile TMT historical and technological trends that are
with other theories about meaning (Heine, changing the traditional avenues of socialization
Proulx, & Vohs, 2006; Lind & van den Bos, and modes of influence on goals and values.
2002). Particularly noted was the extending reach of
• Recognizing that death reminders can have a corporate capitalism, and increasing exposure
positive impact on some individuals, the authors to media and interactive technologies, and
suggest that TMT move beyond only looking at their role in facilitating or undermining
the “darker side” of human motivation and begin basic need satisfactions.

 through a fly’s eye


• Their comments also focused on the study implicit-explicit motivation
of forces that thwart or facilitate peoples’ basic congruence
needs within and across diverse settings as Todd M. Thrash, Laura A. Maruskin, and
potentially informing policy and interventions Chris C. Martin
aimed at fostering individual and community
• In their chapter, Thrash, Maruskin and
wellness.
Martin pointed out a lot of variation in how
congruence in implicit and explicit motivation
Motivational Processes
is operationalized and modeled across studies.
ego depletion: theory and evidence
Because these differences lead to different
Mark Muraven
conceptualizations and different robustness of
• Muraven sees a better understanding of how findings, the authors called for a more careful
practicing self-control helps to build self-control and explicit rationale when operationalizing the
as a critical next step in ego-depletion research. construct and presenting one’s analytic approach
Understanding this pathway will be valuable in future studies on congruence.
for the theory and for designing interventions. • They also raised an interesting question that
Knowing more about practice effects can specifically we have sometimes wondered ourselves: Why not
inform interventions by identifying tasks that are cite Freud? Many of Freud’s writings can speak
most effective to practice, and by specifying the to congruence in implicit and explicit motives,
optimum practice time and frequency that leads to and the authors argue that ignoring these insights
improvements in self-control. Interventions that “undermines rather than serves scientific credibility
aim to build self-control could have value for the and progress.” Thrash et al. suggested that when
individual for society, as self-control is involved possible, researchers should better understand and
in many important behaviors like controlling utilize historical insights.
aggression, getting along with others, and • Practicing what they preach, the authors
resisting temptation. highlight insights from Freud about integrating
• Another direction for future research that incongruence as a direction for future research.
Muraven identified involves the finding that Though acceptance is typically thought to be the
depletion leads to greater passivity. Exploring this only way to integrate incongruence, there are two
finding further and connecting it to changes in the other “healthy” ways to integrate incongruent
brain may help create a more comprehensive and motives posited by Freud that merit exploration:
unified theory of depletion. rejection and sublimation. The authors encouraged
researchers to consider these other options as
flow a means of integration, as both rejection and
Susan A. Jackson sublimation of motives (especially implicit ones)
can be accomplished in self-determined and
• Jackson pointed to the importance of
mindful ways.
investigating neurological and psychophysiological
• Another future direction of research in
correlates of flow for the theory’s advancement.
congruence that the authors identified is to
Utilizing such methods, she argues, is crucial to a
move beyond a between-persons level of analysis.
deeper understanding about what systems are in
Exploring how an individual varies across time
play when an individual is in a state of flow.
and across “content domains” in the congruence
• She also recommended that future research
of their implicit and explicit motives represents an
continue to examine both individual differences
important and unexplored area of investigation.
and situational factors (e.g., competition)
that facilitate and hinder the flow experience.
curiosity and motivation
Furthermore, Jackson also raised questions as
Paul J. Silvia
to how person and situational variables interact
to affect the different dimensions of flow. • Silvia wondered whether the different lines
Additionally, how do these dimensions of flow of thought on curiosity couldn’t be connected
shift across contexts and within individuals? in future pursuits on curiosity and motivation.
She identified this interplay of context and He suggested some connections between self-
person as one of the most important directions determination theory and emotion psychology,
for the future of flow research. for example. He encouraged researchers to be

rya n , leg ate 


open to bridging theories in an effort to better • The authors also called for broader
understand the motive of curiosity, with the methodologies such as priming, diary
risk that doing so might result in changes to methodologies, and continued work on
the concept of curiosity. developing interventions.
• Silvia also pointed to exploring the interplay • Other areas of needed investigation
of traits and states as a direction of future research identified by the authors involved a more fine-
in curiosity. Looking at how “curious traits tuned understanding of the interdependent
influence curious states” using a variety of new relations between achievement goals.
methods could advance the field of curiosity • They also suggested that understanding
in motivation. He also suggested examining how situational factors may affect achievement
the “midrange” level of curiosity (the level of goals and understanding potential cultural
idiosyncratic interests), especially how it develops, differences in these effects would be an
as a future direction. important area for future research.

interest and its development goal regulation and implementation:


K. Ann Renninger and Stephanie Su goal setting and goal striving
Peter M. Gollwitzer and Gabriele Oettingen
• Renninger and Su suggest that future
interest research focus more on developmental • Gollwitzer and Oettingen saw future
transformations. For example, can the meaning of directions in goal regulation and implementation
factors like novelty vary across phases of interest as better understanding how mental contrasting
and across age groups? and implementation intentions can best help
• The authors also wondered about the role people create goals that help them fulfill their
of contextual supports in facilitating interest at wishes.
different phases of interest development, and they • The authors discussed an intervention that
suggested this too as an important future area of taught people how to effectively set and implement
research. goals by themselves, and carrying out more of these
• They also encouraged interest researchers to interventions would be a worthy pursuit for future
draw upon the existing body of work in interest goal research.
to better understand differences in studies using • They also called for more focus on potential
different measures and methods. This could mediators of the effects of goal regulation and
help provide a more unified and comprehensive implementation, such as increased efficacy or
understanding of interest. control beliefs.

Goals and Motivation unconscious goal pursuit:


achievement goals: examining nonconscious goal regulation
the thoughts, attitudes, and and motivation
behaviors that characterize Henk Aarts and Ruud Custers
people’s competence-based
• Aarts and Custers encouraged
pursuits
more investigation of the role of awareness
Kou Murayama, Andrew J. Elliot, and Ron
of goals in producing behavior. Many questions
Friedman
remained unanswered, such as whether
• Murayama, Elliot, and Friedman identified consciousness mediates goal-priming effects.
the processes underlying goal pursuit as an To answer such questions, they recommend
important priority for future investigation in that manipulation checks always be used, and
achievement goals. Understanding this process eventually researchers should develop more
could help inform interventions. refined methods to examine just how conscious
• They also detailed ways to advance people are of goal primes directing their behavior
achievement goal work, including extending (e.g., Seth, Dienes, Cleeremans, Overgaard, &
the framework by using a “3x2 framework” to Pessoa, 2008).
understand different types of achievement goals, • The authors highlighted another challenging
and better understanding the consequences of question for future research: How do unconscious
mastery-avoidance goals. goals “flexibly control” behavior?

 through a fly’s eye


• The authors also raised many questions shifts in goal orientation research are relatively
about when consciousness can play a role in goal new, future research should continue to examine
pursuit. They wondered whether consciousness the trajectories of people’s orientation of goal
may facilitate performance in some contexts more focus across the life span using longer term
than others, and whether consciousness may be longitudinal designs.
especially important in stopping, or overriding • The authors also state that future research
behavior. will have to show the incremental validity of
• Moreover, the authors suggested that an goal focus above and beyond well-established
important avenue of future research is determining motivational constructs such as intrinsic and
whether consciously and unconsciously activated extrinsic motivation.
goals stimulate cognitions and the brain in similar
or distinct ways. Diverse methods and levels of Motivation in Relationships
analysis, especially at the neurological level, are self-enhancement and self-protection
likely needed to answer these questions and yield motives
additional insights on goal-related processes and Constantine Sedikides and Mark D. Alicke
efficacy.
• Sedikides and Alicke believe that exploring
the dynamic between self-enhancement and
the motivational complexity of
self-protection motives is an important pursuit
choosing: a review of theory
for new research. As these motives can operate
and research
independently, and may also interact in unknown
Erika A. Patall
ways, they recommended that researchers treat
• Patall believes that future research in choice these motives as separate dimensions rather than as
should explore whether the effects of choice go two ends of a continuum. Treating these motives
beyond the effect of having one’s preferences. as separate dimensions can help answer questions
• She also thought it was important that about how they may facilitate or impede one
researchers focus on factors that enhance or inhibit another in different contexts and also help
choice effects. She raised the important issue to examine their interplay with other self-
of systematically testing, through experimental evaluation motives.
design, certain assumptions that are made in the • The authors wondered about the relations
literature about choice, such as about the effort between implicit and explicit self-enhancement
required for making different types of choices. and self-protection motives. They advocated
• Future research in choice should also test using new methodologies to help elucidate these
how factors like interest, perceived competence, relations and their functional effects.
and developmental age interact to affect different • They also called for future research exploring
motivational outcomes. the situational factors and individual differences
• Patall also saw research on the mechanisms that constrain self-enhancement and self-
and pathways through which choice leads to protection.
different motivational outcomes as another
important direction. the gendered body project:
motivational components of
on gains and losses, means and ends: objectification theory
goal orientation and goal focus Tomi-Ann Roberts and Patricia L. Waters
across adulthood
• In discussing self-objectification and its many
Alexandra M. Freund, Marie Hennecke, and
adverse consequences to health and psychological
Maida Mustafić
well-being, Roberts and Waters wondered why
• Freund, Hennecke, and Mustafić discussed some individuals are motivated to engage in self-
the potential benefits and mechanisms of a process objectification behaviors and others aren’t.
focus as opposed to an outcome focus in people’s • Given the current cultural climate,
goal orientation. They found that a process focus understanding the factors that promote resilience
helps with adaptation after failure, and they in the face of societal pressures to conform is
encouraged future research on goal focus to test critical for developing interventions that could
this relatively new idea. Yet because age-related start to bolster resilience to self-objectification

rya n , leg ate 


in girls and women. Designing interventions to goals, especially those low in approach or high
reduce objectification of girls and women at the in avoidance goals. Interventions on people’s
community and societal level, and interventions relationship goal tendencies, such as by cueing
to promote resilience to societal pressures at the incentives, could be important for improving
individual level, represent important next steps to relationship functioning and wellness.
reduce adverse outcomes like disordered eating, • The authors called for more careful study
body shame, and depressive symptoms. of how motives and goals operate in long-term
• The authors suggested that self-objectification relationships over time.
can be thought of as motivated behavior but also as
a type of amotivation, or external locus of control Evolutionary and Biological Perspectives
with respect to norms of physical attractiveness. neuropsychology and human
Testing these ideas in relation to other theories of motivation
motivation will be important in advancing self- Johnmarshall Reeve and Woogul Lee
objectification theory.
• A goal of Revee and Lee’s chapter was to
show the relevance of neuroscience to motivation
parents and motivation: the role of
research in an effort to expand the field through
relatedness
introducing new methodology and theoretical
Eva M. Pomerantz, Cecilia Sin-Sze Cheung, and
conceptualizations. They wondered about the
Lili Qin
future of motivation and neuroscience, and how
• Many themes emerged in Pomerantz, much the two fields will come to influence one
Cheung, and Qin’s chapter about parenting and another. They put this future in the hands of
motivation that they identified as important motivation researchers, who will need to be open
empirical questions to test. For example, and willing to learn more about neuroscience and
understanding how different phases of the its methods for this advancement to occur.
parent–child relationship impact children’s • The authors raised many questions that
academic functioning, and understanding the will require a good deal of empirical support
limits of relatedness to children’s academic to eventually answer. For example, they asked
functioning represent two closely related lines whether neuroscience is relevant to only some
of future research discussed by the authors. parts of motivation or whether it is relevant to
• The authors also believe that further more general and complex types of motivation.
understanding of developmental pathways, such Another question they posed as meriting inquiry:
as how children’s sense of responsibility to parents Can the brain generate motivation of its own? Or
may have an increased effect on their motivation is the motivation at the neurological level always a
over time, is needed in future research. They also response to environmental events?
called for examination of this issue cross-culturally. • They also encouraged more research on
the compatibility of dependent measures in
avoiding the pitfalls and approaching neuroscience, such as reaction times and cortical
the promises of close relationships activations, with typical outcomes in motivation
Shelly L. Gable and Thery Prok research. Research in motivational neuroscience
has begun to identify neural bases of different
• Gable and Prok called for the field of
motivational states, but completing such an
approach and avoidance social motivation to move
endeavor might arguably be the biggest
beyond examining individual differences to look at
challenge facing this field.
the situational factors, such as environmental cues
for incentive or threat, that influence an individual
evolved individual differences
to pursue interpersonal goals. Understanding cues
in human emotion
in the environment and other situational influences
Larry C. Bernard
represents a fruitful area of future research.
• They considered it important for future • Bernard advocates for more support from
research to examine implicit incentives and threats other areas of investigation, including comparative
and their impacts on motivation. psychology and behavioral genetics, to further
• They also wondered whether it is possible for the study of evolved individual differences in
people to learn to focus on different relationship personality and motivation.

 through a fly’s eye


• He believes that future research should • They wondered whether awareness might also
test multiple and conflicting motives to explain some of the mechanisms through which
understand how they may interact to shape certain effort mobilization effects occur.
behavior. Moreover, it is important to test • The authors suggested that future research
how different strengths in motive dimensions examine the roles of personality and individual
shape social cognition. differences, and situational variables like task
• Bernard highlighted experimental context and task framing on effort intensity and
personality research as a direction of future mobilization.
research. • They also encouraged future researchers
• He also saw potential utility of evolved to conduct studies using other physiological
individual differences in applied areas such as correlates, such as brain activity, in tandem with
education, industrial/organizational, health, and cardiovascular measurements to better understand
clinical psychology. how the central and autonomic nervous systems
interact to mobilize effort.
moods of energy and tension
that motivate Motivation in Application
Robert E. Thayer motivation in psychotherapy
Martin Grosse Holtforth and Johannes Michalak
• Thayer hoped that future investigations
could help clarify how complex moods arise. • Grosse Holtforth and Michalak asserted
He discussed “seemingly paradoxical effects” that motivation is critical for psychotherapy in
resulting from biopsychological states that all patients, and as such it should inform many
simultaneously activate and deactivate the aspects of therapy. A great deal of research is
body, and thus suggested investigating these needed to answer the questions that they raise
biopsychological states further as an important about motivational factors in psychotherapy. For
avenue in future research. He implied that example, insofar as motivational factors are linked
moving beyond cross-sectional designs could help to the onset and maintenance of psychopathology,
clarify the interplay of energetic and tense what are the mechanisms underlying these links?
arousal producing complex moods. Experience Also on the issue of mechanism, the authors
or event sampling methodologies could be wonder about the mechanisms that underlie
useful to future work. change in different therapeutic approaches.
• Another unanswered issue that Thayer • Are there changes in both implicit and
raised as a direction for future research concerns explicit motivation during psychotherapy
determining the “moderate point at which interventions? Do different types of therapies
increasing tension leads first to increased energy change these two motivations in different ways?
but at some point to reduced energy.” • To the extent that cultural factors impact
motivation, how do they impact treatment?
effort intensity: insights from the • Addressing the authors’ question about brain
cardiovascular system changes corresponding to changes in motivation
Guido H. E. Gendolla, Rex A. Wright, and during the course of therapy necessitates
Michael Richter incorporating neuroscience methods into
treatment outcome studies.
• Gendolla, Wright, and Richter
• Clearly, diverse methodologies are needed
identified more investigation into the roles that
to answer these complex questions. The authors
consciousness and automaticity play in expending
proposed that experimental and longitudinal
effort as an important future direction. Do people
designs can help clarify these questions and
always consciously determine how much effort
advance future research in this field.
to mobilize for a behavior? The authors asserted
that effort could become learned to the point of
motivation in education
automaticity. Although the authors reviewed
Allan Wigfield, Jenna Cambria, and Jacquelynne
some evidence supporting this, they suggested
S. Eccles
that more research needs to be conducted
before clear claims can be made about • Wigfield, Cambria, and Eccles believe
implicit effort mobilization. that the issue of how individual differences

rya n , leg ate 


in children, namely gender and ethnic may also help elucidate other conditions that can
differences, and different classroom contexts facilitate motivation.
impact student’s motivation merits attention • Grant and Shin suggest that moving beyond
for future research. the individual level of analysis is an important area
• They encouraged future research to move for advancement of work motivation. Examining
beyond self-report measures of motivations and how motivation operates in work groups and
outcomes. They noted that conducting interviews teams, for example, merits more research attention.
and having multiple informants could help create a • They also encouraged future research focusing
more complete picture of children’s motivation in on the issue of worker motivation over time.
the classroom. Research that employs longitudinal methods could
• They urged researchers to continue to do address this issue.
intervention work, especially quasi-experimental
designs and randomized trials, in classrooms at all motivation in sport and physical
education levels (from early elementary grades to activity
high school). Maureen R. Weiss, Anthony J. Amorose, and
• The authors also identified collaboration Lindsay E. Kipp
with policy makers as an important next step to
make sure that findings from motivation research, • Weiss, Amorose, and Kipp identified the
especially interventions, can better inform issue of how family dynamics affect physical
school reform in an effort to optimize children’s activity motivation in youth as a needed direction
motivation in school. for future research. For example, family structure
differences (e.g., single- vs. two-parent households)
advances in motivation in exercise and and family characteristics such as socioeconomic
physical activity status may differentially impact sports and physical
Martin S. Hagger activity motivation. Understanding how these
factors interact to facilitate or hinder motivation
• Hagger identified interventions to change may serve to elucidate risk and protective factors,
physical activity behavior as a main avenue for and help inform interventions and populations to
future research. He believes that careful evaluation be targeted.
of physical activity interventions is critical for • Furthermore, these authors would like
understanding the “active ingredients” of change to understand in a fine-tuned way the parts of
and of basic mechanisms of motivation. interventions that are more or less effective,
• He prioritized replicating and manualizing such as the optimum length of time for a
interventions as a direction for future studies, and coaching intervention. They encouraged further
as such called upon researchers to detail all aspects investigation into the “active ingredients”
of their interventions, including how they evaluate producing change in coaching and teaching
treatment fidelity. behaviors that lead to enhanced motivation in
• Hagger also pointed to research about children.
implicit and explicit motivational processes on • They also thought that it is important for
physical activity behavior as another valuable future research to determine how and why peer
future area. groups and friendships affect physical activity
motivation.
work motivation: directing, energizing, • They proposed that a variety of methodologies
and maintaining research are necessary to accomplish these objectives,
Adam M. Grant and Jihae Shin including ethnographic, interview, observational,
• Grant and Shin hope to see researchers extend and survey methods.
the scope of outcomes of work motivation into
more specific topics such as creativity and task Motivation’s Future: What’s the Buzz?
persistence. Remembering that our Oxford Handbook of
• They also would like to see more work on Human Motivation authors were not explicitly
the effects of a broader range of rewards, such asked to write about future directions, most none-
as recognition and appreciation, on motivation. theless did make some forward-looking comments.
Understanding these other reward structures In our review of articles we tried to cull these visions

 through a fly’s eye


into a cohesive picture of the future of motivation over time—changing in its qualities and complex-
work. Of course, each of our “ommatidium” pro- ity. Understanding these transformations and the
vided some unique ideas, typically connected with systematic influences of maturation, context, and
the specific area of research. But some overlap, or culture on motivational changes and manifestations
redundancy occurred, and we focus on that. over time clearly concerns our volume authors.
Perhaps the most widely cited future direction Following these “big three” themes of dual
that emerged was, at least for us, a somewhat sur- process, intervention and developmental research,
prising one. Mentioned more than any other area emerging as important future directions were calls
for future research was investigations of dual-process for greater integration between biological and psy-
models or more study of the distinctions and relations chological methods and theorizing. As Reeve and
between automatic, or implicit, and deliberative, or Lee point out in their chapter on the neuroscience
explicit, goals. The fact that this interest emerged of human motivation, more of a two-way street
in so many papers reflects motivation researchers’ needs to develop between neuropsychology and
renewed interest in nonconscious processes and the behavioral scientists, one that navigates between
motivated behavior they can organize. We would the dual hazards of reductionism on the one hand
add to this the strong interest in the dynamic nature and “floating” unanchored psychological constructs
of motivation, as implicit and explicit processes can on the other. The excitement here is that mapping
operate congruently or be in conflict. So despite our of psychological processes onto real-time biological
surprise it should have been of little wonder that correspondents offers opportunities to test hypo-
this was the most saliently expressed future direc- thetical processes with a level of detail and resolu-
tion in the field, since it has both basic research and tion not previously accessible in our science. We
broad applied implications. might add here that this call for more integration
Alongside more examination of implicit and was not limited to neuroscience. It is clear that we
explicit processes, perhaps the next most frequently more broadly need to attach our motivation theo-
mentioned future direction was a call for more ries to biological functioning, including physiologi-
intervention research, including controlled or ran- cal measures of effort, exertion, arousal, and fatigue,
domized clinical trials. The ideas expressed in this as exemplified in the work reviewed by Gendolla,
vein were not simply calls to “do good” with our Wright, and Richter in this volume.
knowledge. Instead, oft echoed was the idea that The only final big category that spanned across
through intervention research we can significantly the majority of chapters was a desire for more
advance the basic science of human motivation. understanding of how individual differences influ-
In attempting to test the efficacy and “stickiness” ence motivation. Here we include both calls for
of interventions we gain greater understanding of more studies of behavioral genetics, as well as more
mechanisms and basic processes at work, especially measurement of traits and stable characteristics that
if we are careful to both appropriately randomize emerge in development from interactions of the
and measure potential mediators and moderators of genome with cultural and environmental factors.
obtained effects. Insofar as many theoretical tradi- Individual differences are indeed understudied in a
tions in the field of motivation are experimentally field that tends to focus on experimental method-
based, intervention research can also help establish ologies and situational manipulations and effects.
the generalizability and relevance of theory to repre- But clearly our experimental effects are frequently
sentative populations and everyday contexts. moderated by individual differences, many of which
A third frequently cited direction for future are still to be identified in their importance and
research was the call for more developmental and mechanisms of influence. Put differently, individual
longitudinal research. Reflecting again the fact that differences qualify even the most common effects
so much theory in this volume is primarily founded we study in this field, from the effects of mortality
upon experimental methods, and therefore focuses salience on defenses, to the impact of interpersonal
on short timeframes and proximal outcomes, the controls on intrinsic motivation. Our authors iden-
call for longitudinal research has at least two impli- tify our lack of focus on these moderating differ-
cations. First, longitudinal research advances our ences (other than as control variables) as a major
causal models because it can allow for some quasi- gap in our knowledge.
causal modeling and hypothesis testing. More Among the other topics for future direction that
important perhaps, developmental research would were mentioned frequently were the following: more
take seriously the idea that motivation transforms studies of within-person changes over time, more

rya n , leg ate 


studies of cultural and economic system influences biologists and comparative psychologists, and engage
on motivation, more studies of group (as opposed to in more informative, research-intensive interven-
individual) motivational processes, and more atten- tions, among other important future directions. Our
tion to motivation at the interface between humans hope is that the contributions within this volume
and technology. And of course most every author help researchers envision new ways forward, so that
called for more refined and sharper tools for digging we can satisfy not only our curiosity about human
into their particular plots within this field of study. nature but also optimize our derived knowledge to
When all of these authors’ perspectives coalesce help enhance human well-being, adaptation, and
into one compound eye, it is clear that there is plenty our collective quality of life. So let’s get buzzing.
of territory yet to explore within the field of human
motivation. To get there, the fly’s eye view suggests
Reference
that we will especially need to intensify research Riley, D. T., Harmann, W. M., Barrett, S. F., & Wright, C. H.
efforts with regard to nonconscious motivational (2008). Musca domestica inspired machine vision sensor with
processes, accomplish more integrative work with hyperacuity. Bioinspiration and Biomimetics, 3, 026003.

 through a fly’s eye


INDEX

A achievement motivation theory, lack of willpower and, 223


ability, effort intensity and, 424–425 choice in, 250 motive congruence and, 151
academic function. See also student The Achievement Motive (McClelland/ negative, priority management and, 30
motivation Atkinson/Clark/Lowell), 142–143 parental, in SDT, 339–340
achievement goals for, 200 ACT. See Acceptance and Commitment positive, as counterintuitive, 35–36
autonomy climates, 95 Therapy priority management and, 36
choice as benefit for, 254–255, 255 action, affect and, 34 reference criterion for, 33
high-stakes rewards for, 99–100 actional phase, for motivations, 291, affiliation, need for, 353
lack of willpower for, 222 292–293 affiliation motives, 353
responsibility of children to parents action awareness, in flow, 128–129 approach, 353
and, 341–342, 344–345 activated freeze response, 416 avoidance, 353
Acceptance and Commitment Therapy activation, of motives, 8 age
(ACT), 453, 454–455, 457 activity. See physical activity adult goal development by, 283
achievement goals, 558 adaptation change goals and, 289–290
approach-avoidance distinctions in, for adult development goals, 283 gender development for girls by,
194–195 of goal focus, 289–290 328–329
in classroom setting, 200 in objectification theory, 324–325 goal focus and, 285–288
competence and, 195–196 addictive behaviors. See also alcohol outcome focus for goals and, 290
contextual effects of, 199–201 consumption; tobacco use process focus for goals and, 290
definition of, 195–196 self-control and, 112 responsibility of children to parents
dichotomous model of, 191–193 ADHD. See attention-deficit hyperactivity by, 344
extrinsic, 196 disorder stability goals and, 290
future research on, methodological adult development, goals for, 281 work motivation influenced by,
expansion in, 201–202 adaptation in, 283 513–514
goal pursuit and, 234 age as influence on, 283 agency
historical development of, 191–195 compensation in, 282–283 collective, 15
integrated model for, 193 goal focus and, 285–288 neural foundation for, 372–373
learning-performance model of, goal selection and, 282 reciprocal interactions and, 14–15
192 multidirectionality of, 281–283 aggressive behavior
measurement of, 197–198 multiple goal management and, 282 ego depletion and, 116
in multiple goals model, 198 optimization for, 282–283 evolved individual motives for,
as omnibus construct, 195 resource conservation in, 282–283 392–393
performance-approach, 198–199 affect. See also complex moods; moods; AIM-Q. See Assessment of Individual
performance-avoidance, 198–199 negative affect; positive affect Motives-Questionnaire
social, 196–197 action and, 34 akrasia, 221. See also willpower, lack of
as specific aim, 195–196 approach-related, 34 alcohol consumption
task-ego model of, 192–193 associative learning and, 369 dysfunctional goals and, 448
as theory, physical activity and, avoidance-related, 34 ego depletion and, 114–115
485–487, 492–493, 524–526 biological mechanisms for, 32–33 self-awareness and, 57
trichotomous model of, 194 bipolar dimensions, 34–35 Alliance Fostering Therapy, 449
2 x 2 model of, 194–195 dimensions of, 33–34 Allport, Gordon, 304
work-avoidance, 196 emotion and, 32 altruism, 395
achievement goal theory, youth motivation feedback control and, 32–36 Ames, C., 193
and, 524–526 interest development and, 176 analytical processing, prevention-focused

565
individuals and, 77 for children, in SDT, 340 controllability of traits in, 308
anxiety-buffer hypothesis, 47 controlling climates for, 94–95 egocentrism and, 309–310
PTSD and, 51 ego depletion and, 121 focalism and, 310
anxiety disorders, in prevention-focused intrinsic motivation and, 87–88 individuated entities in, 310
individuals, 70 in personal relationships, 95–96 nonmotivational explanations for,
appearance motive, 393 purpose of, 85 309–311
approach affiliation motives, 353 in SDT, 85–86 as pancultural, 308
approach-related affect, 34 self-awareness and, 102 prevalence of, among populations,
approach versus avoidance processes self-control and, 121 307–308
in achievement goal models, 194–195 supportive climates for, 94–95 psychological health and, 314
affect and, 34, 34 autonomy-controlling climates, 94–95 psychological interests and, 315
for close relationship formation, in health care settings, 95 self-affirmation and, 309
352–353, 355–356 in school settings, 95 self-threat and, 309
eagerness-related approach strategies, social contexts for, 95 verifiability of attributes in, 308–309
215 in workplace settings, 95 Beyond Boredom and Anxiety
in feedback control, 32 autonomy orientation, 90–91 (Csikszentmihalyi), 130
in goal focus, 288 autonomy-supportive climates, 94–95 bias. See encoding biases; self-serving bias
in goal striving, 215 in health care settings, 95 Billings, Josh, 311
Aristotle, 221 relational supports in, 94 binge eating, self-awareness and, 57
Arnold, Felix, 159 in school settings, 95 Binswanger, Ludwig, 48
aspirations. See also extrinsic life goals; social contexts for, 95 BIS. See Behavioral Inhibition System
goals; intrinsic life goals in workplace settings, 95 breasts, sexual objectification of, 326–327
cultural contexts for, 97 autotelic experience, 129–130 Brumberg, Joan, 323
in goal striving, 215 avoidance affiliation motives, 353 BTAE. See better-than-average-effect
Assessment of Individual Motives- avoidance goals, well-being and, 447
Questionnaire (AIM-Q), 398 avoidance processes. See approach C
assimilation, in BTAE, 310–311 versus avoidance processes; illness calm energy, 414
associative learning, as motivational state, avoidance; performance-avoidance flow and, 414
369–370 goals; threat avoidance, in close calm tiredness, 415
affect and, 369 relationships; work-avoidance goals capitalism. See corporate capitalism
environmental incentives in, 369 avoidance-related affect, 34 cardiovascular reactivity, to effort intensity,
liking in, 369–370 423
Atkinson, J.W., 142–143 B career choice, self-efficacy and, 23
attachment, personal relationships and, balanced selection for phenotypes, causality orientations, 87, 90–91
53–54 in FFM, 384 autonomy orientation, 90–91
existential threats and, response to, 54 BAS. See Behavioral Activity System challenge in flow, skills in balance with,
attachment theory, 336–339 basic psychological needs, in SDT, 87, 128, 130
parental relationships in, 337–338 101–102 change goals, 289–290
parental sensitivity in, 338 autonomy as, 87 children. See also relatedness, for children;
peer relationships in, 338 for children, 339 youth motivation, for physical activity
security perceptions in, 343 extrinsic life goals and, as antagonistic adoption of parental values, 342
teacher relationships in, 338 to, 92 attachment theory perspective on,
attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder intrinsic life goals for, 92 336–339
(ADHD), 219 physical activity and, 484–485, autonomy for, in SDT, 340
attitudes, in planned behavior theory, 481 488–489, 524 basic psychological needs of, in SDT,
attribution theory, choice in, 252 weekend effect and, 101 339
autonomous motivation. See also extrinsic Becker, Ernest, 45 investment and engagement in, 336
motivation; intrinsic motivation Behavioral Activity System (BAS), 356 parental affect and, in SDT, 339–340
benefits of, 90 behavioral control, self-efficacy and, parental relationships for, in
causality orientations for, 87, 90–91 515–516 attachment theory, 337–338
within corporate capitalism, 99 Behavioral Inhibition System (BIS), 356 parental sensitivity towards, in
cultural influences on, 89–90, 96–97 behavioral syndrome theory, 382 attachment theory, 338
feedback for, 94 in evolved individual difference theory, peer relationships for, in attachment
identified regulation in, 89 389–390 theory, 338
integrated regulation in, 89 extraversion in, 386 responsibility to parents, 341–342
introjection in, 89 FFM and, 385–386 SDT perspective on, 339–341
outcomes from, 89–90 behavior theory, curiosity in, 160 self-efficacy in, 25
in psychotherapy, 443, 448–449 better-than-average-effect (BTAE), socialization of, in SDT, 339
social-contextual effects of, 93–96 307–311 teacher relationships for, in attachment
wellness and, 96–97 aggregate comparisons in, 310 theory, 338
autonomy. See also choice assimilation in, 310–311 choices, 559
as basic psychological need, 87 attribute valence in, 308 in academic achievement motivation,
causality orientations for, 87, 90–91 contrast in, 310–311 254–255, 255

566 index
in achievement motivation theory, 250 approach versus avoidance processes in, competitions, lack of willpower, 222
in attribution theory, 252 352–353, 355–356 complex moods, 413–415
autonomy and, 258–260, 264 BAS for, 356 calm energy, 414
characteristics of, 257–263 BIS for, 356 calm tiredness, 415
cognitive dissonance and, 252 disadvantages of, 351–352 tense energy, 415
competence from, perceptions of, 261, emotion in, 357–358 tense tiredness, 414–415
263–264 evolutionary development of, 351 computer-mediated environments, flow
control and, 258 functional development of, 351 in, 132
correspondence with target and, goal type and, 355 concentration, in flow, 129
269–270 incentives for, 354 Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction
cultural differences as influence on, interpersonal outcomes, in approach (CORI), 472
265–267 versus avoidance motivation, conditioning. See classical conditioning
as demotivator, 255–257 355–356 conscientiousness, willpower and,
by developmental level, 267 intimacy motivation in, 353–354 224–225
disadvantages of, 256 outcome-goal link processes for, consciousness. See private body
in dissonance theory, 252 356–358 consciousness
in drive theories, 249 social bonds as, 350–351 consilience, in motivation studies, 6
effort level for, 260–262 social information in, 356–357, 357 consistency, in motives, 150–151
ego depletion from, 256 social motivation and, 353–354 context learning, stimulus-response rules
in expectancy-value theories, 249– threat avoidance for, 354 and, 236
250, 250 coasting, positive affect and, 35 control. See also control theory; feedback
as experience, 251–257 cognition control; self-control
frame of reference for, 262 ego depletion and, 116 choice and, 258
future research on, 271–272, 272 energetic arousal and, 411–412 in flow, 129
individual orientations for, 250–251, environmental inquisitiveness and, 392 in planned behavior theory, 481–482
263–268 cognitive modeling, for vicarious self-efficacy and, 515–516
interest level and, 263 learning, 17 student motivation and, 464
intrinsic motivation and, 253 collective agency, in reciprocal controlled motivations. See extrinsic
maladaptive, 249 interactions, 15 motivation
maximization strategies for, 264–265 collective efficacy, 24 control theory, 6
motivations for, 249–251 sources of, 24 individual differences in motivation,
as motivator, 251–255 for teachers, 24 400
multiplicity of, influence of, 261–262 commitment, as tradeoff cooperative learning, 471
through nonconscious processes, 251 benefits of, 74–75 Core flow scales, 135
outcome desirability and, 269 evolved individual motives for, CORI. See Concept-Oriented Reading
perceived versus actual, 257 394–395 Instruction
perceptions of, 257–260 in mental contrasting, 212 corporate capitalism, 98
in planned behavior theory, 250 in prevention-focused individuals, autonomous motivation within, 99
power of, 248 72–75 extrinsic life goals and, 99
preferences as influence on, 270–271 signal detection paradigms, 74 materialism from, 99
in psychological theory, 248 in women, 395 reward contingencies in, 98–99,
realism of setting and, 268–269 comparative psychology, in FFM, 99–100
in regulatory focus theory, 265 384–385 correspondence of content, for motives,
as reward-based, 249 compensation, in adult development 147–148
satisficing strategies for, 264–265 goals, 282–283 creativity
in SDT, 253 competence flow and, 132
self-efficacy from, 254, 264 achievement goals and, 195–196 in promotion-focused individuals,
in self-perception theory, 252–253 from choice, 261, 263–264 76–77
in self-presentational enhancement in physical activity motivation, within work motivation, 514–515
situations, 268 parental expression of, 530–531 Csikszentmihalyi, M., 130
self-regulation and, 250, 256 student motivation and, 464 cultural values
situational characteristics and, youth physical activity motivation evaluation of, 97
268–270 and, 540–542 female self-objectification and, 330
in social learning theory, 250 competence motivation theory need satisfaction and, 97
socioeconomic differences influenced as domain specific, 521 culture
by, 265–267 effectance motivation in, 521 aspirations and, contexts for, 97
task type and, 269 peer influence in, 521 autonomous motivation influenced by,
Clarification Oriented Therapy, 453, 457 for physical activity, 521–522 89–90, 96–97
Clark, R.A., 142–143 positive effect in, 521–522 BTAE and, as pancultural, 308
classical conditioning, unconscious goal self-perception of competence in, 521 choice influenced by, 265–267
pursuit and, 233 skill mastery in, 521 female self-objectification and, 330
close relationships, 560 youths and, for physical activity, group belonging and, 52–53
advantages of, 351–352 521–522 need satisfaction and, 97

index 567
responsibility of children to parents delegation hypothesis, 219 education, 561–562. See also academic
and, 345 democratic systems, 100–101 function; learning; student
SDT and, 86–87, 96–97 Demosthenes, 304 motivation
social cognitive theory and, relevance depression grading systems and, 470
of, 25–26 failure and, 69–70 transition to schools and, 469–471
student motivation influenced by, hedonic incentive and, 431–432 as value, in student motivation, 465
473–474 personal goals and, 447–448 EEG. See electroencephalography
values in, 97, 97 priority management and, 36 effectance motivation, 521
worldview validation through, 52 promotion-focused individuals efficacy. See collective efficacy; self-efficacy
curiosity, 557–558 and, 70 effortful control, in feedback control, 37
in behavior theory, 160 self-system theory and, 70 effort intensity, 561
diversive, 159 desirability, motivation and, 209 ability and, 424–425
drive-reduction model for, 158 developmental psychology cardiovascular reactivity to, 423
emotional theory for, 160–161 children’s responsibility to parents difficulty in, variability of status for,
as “for its own sake” motivation, in, 344 421–422, 423–427
159–161 choice in, 267 ego involvement in, 430–431
future research applications for, effortful control in, 37 empirical evidence for, 423–432
163–164 Dewey, John, 159–160, 160 fatigue and, 425–426
global, 162 DFI. See Decision-Fostering Intervention future research implications for,
I-D model of, 162 DFS-2. See dispositional flow scale-2 433–435
individual differences in, 161–163 diet and nutrition, energetic arousal hedonic incentive for, 431–432
information gaps model for, 159 and, 410 implicit determination of, 433–434
interest as influence on, 161 Dinnerstein, Dorothy, 326 individual differences in, 434
novelty reduction and, 157–159 discrepancy-enlarging feedback loop, measurement for, 422–423
openness to experience and, 162–163 29–30 mood and, 426–427, 431–432
reward-aversion model for, 160 for overt behavior, 30 in motivation intensity theory,
specific, 159 in personality psychology, 30 420–422
uncertainty reduction and, 157–159 in social psychology, 30 outcome expectancy for, 428–429
cybernetic processes, for self-regulation, discrepancy-reducing feedback loop, 29 personality and, 434
28–29, 555–556 for overt behavior, 30 physiological correlates of, 435
feedback control, 29–31 in personality psychology, 30 self-evaluation of, 431
hierarchy for, 29 in social psychology, 30 social evaluation of, 429–430
for motivations, 38–40 disinhibition effects, in vicarious success in, variables for, 427–432
theory development for, 28–29 learning, 16 task context and, 434
Cyrenaics, 304 dispositional flow scale-2 (DFS-2), 134 theory development for, 418
dissonance theory, choice in, 252 egocentrism, BTAE and, 309–310
D cognitive dissonance, 252 ego depletion, 7, 557
death, awareness of. See also existential diversive curiosity, 159 aggressive behavior and, 116
anxiety; existential psychology diversive exploration, 158 alcohol consumption and, 114–115
DTA hypothesis and, 47 dopamine system, in reward-based autonomy and, 121
escaping from, 56–58 motivated action, 367–368 as biologically mediated, 118
flight fantasies and, 49 dopamine hypothesis of positive causes of, 113–114
from frailty of human body, 48–49 affect, 370 from choice, 256
health-promoting behaviors from, expectancy and, 376 cognitive effects of, 116
48–49 drive-reduction model, 158 consequences of, 114–117
religious belief and, 55 diversive exploration as revision conservation of resources and,
self-esteem and, 49–51 of, 158 119–120
in TMT, 46 optimal arousal approach to, 158–159 decision making with, 116
in wilderness settings, 49 drive theories, choice in, 249 EEG methodology for, 117
death-thought accessibility (DTA) DTA hypothesis. See death-thought ego strength and, 112–113
hypothesis, 47 accessibility hypothesis EMG activation and, 116–117
Decision-Fostering Intervention (DFI), Dweck, C.S., 192 expectancy accounts in, 117–118
453–454 dysfunctional goals, alcohol future research models for, 123
decision making consumption and, 448 glucose levels and, 118
dopamine hypothesis of positive implementation intentions in, 120
affect and, 370 E inhibitions of urges with, 115
ego depletion and, 116 eagerness-related approach strategies, 215 interpersonal behaviors and, 113–114
implicit motivational states for, neural early grade school, student motivation interpersonal effects from, 115–116
bases of, 370–371 during, 469–471 moderators of, 120–121
rational motivational states for, neural economic systems, in SDT, 97–99 motivation and, 119
bases of, 371–372 corporate capitalism, 98 operations for, 117–118
somatic marker hypothesis for, 371 human capital in, 98 physiological markers of, 116–117
delay of gratification tasks, 221–222 The Economist, 50–51 positive affect and, 120–121

568 index
after rest, 120–121 for altruism, 395 in SSB, 306
risk taking and, 116 for appearance, 393 success and, from effort intensity,
self-affirmation and, 121 background for, 386–387 428–429
self-control and, 111 behavioral syndromes and, 389–390 expectancy theory, work motivation and,
as self-fulfilling, 117–118 for commitment, 394–395 506–507
self-perception with, 116 components of, 388 critical assessment of, 506–507
from suppression of self, 114 in dyadic domains, 392–394 employee belief changes under, 507
susceptibility to persuasion with, 115 for environmental inquisitiveness, 392 planned behavior theory and, 506
ego involvement for illness avoidance, 391–392 expectancy-value theories
in achievement goal model, 192–193 in individual domains, 391–392 attainment values in, 527
success and, 430–431 for interpersonal inquisitiveness, 392 choice in, 249–250, 250
ego strength in large group domains, 395–396 gender in, 527
development of, 121–122 MAT for, 387 interest values in, 527
from self-control, 112–113, 122 for meaning, 397 in neuroscience, 373–374
electroencephalography (EEG), 117 for mental efforts, 393–394 parental influence in, 527–528
electromyographic (EMG) activation, motives in, 389–390 utility values in, 527
116–117 in multilevel selection theory, 395 for youth motivation, for physical
embedded contexts, motivations in, 86–87 operational definitions for, 389–390 activity, 526–528
EMG activation. See electromyographic physical stature and status, 394 Experience Sampling Method (ESM),
activation PRF for, 387 130, 133–134
emotions, 560–561 sexuality and, 393 experiential system, in feedback control,
affect and, 32 for social exchange, 395–396 37
in close relationships, 357–358 theory development for, 387–391 explicit motives, 557
curiosity and, 160–161 for threat avoidance, 391 consistency in, 150–151
interest and, 172 tradeoffs in, 391–397 correspondence of content for,
mood compared to, 408 in very large group domains, 396–397 147–148
priority management and, influenced for wealth accumulation, 394 disattenuation approaches in,
by, 36 existential anxiety, 48–58 148–149
self-efficacy and, 22 flight fantasies and, 49 incongruence in, 154
in social interaction, 357–358 future research on, 58–59 independence of, 154
youth physical activity motivation group belonging and, 52–53 integrative general model for, 145–146
and, 542–544 health-promoting behaviors from, measure methodology for, 142–146,
employees, in expectancy theory, 507 48–49 148–149
enactive learning, 15 hero worship and, 56 MTMM analysis of, 149
encoding biases, 311–312 from human sexuality, 49 omnibus effect of multiple
End Poem (Rilke), 59 materialism and, 49 methodological factors, 146–147,
energetic arousal, 409–412 after 9/11 attacks, 57 154
calm energy mood, 414 personal dimensions of, 49–51 private body consciousness in,
cognition and, 411–412 personal relationships and, 53–54 150–151
complex moods from, 413–415 physical dimensions of, 48–49 in psychotherapeutic motivation, 442
diet and, 410 from problems of body, 48–49 self-monitoring of, 150–151
endogenous cycle of, 409 psychopathology of, 51 statistical independence in, 144
physical activity and, 411 religious belief and, 55 exploration, as tradeoff
sleep and, 410–411 self-esteem and, 49–51 through materialism, 75
stress and, 412 social dimension of, 52–54 maximization desires and, 75
subjective, 409–410 spiritual dimension of, 54–56 for promotion-focused individuals,
well-being and, 409–410 SSB and, 50 75–76
environmental incentives, 369 symbolic immortality and, 50–51 unintended losses from, 76
environmental inquisitiveness, 392 worldview validation in, 52–53 external regulation, in OIT, 484
cognitive function and, 392 existential psychology, 45 extraversion
Epicureans, 304 objectification theory and, 326–327 in behavioral syndrome theory, 386
episodic memory, self-efficacy and, 376 TMT and, 45–48 in fluctuating phenotype selection,
equity theory, work motivation and, existential self-awareness, 45 for FFM, 384
507–508 anxiety from, motivational role of, in personality psychology, 381
critical assessment of, 508 48–58 extrinsic achievement goals, 196
ESM. See Experience Sampling Method expectancy process focus for goals and, 284
evolutionary psychology, sexual dopamine systems and, influence on, extrinsic life goals, 91–93
objectification in, 324–325 376 attainment of, 92–93
evolutionary theory, individual differences ego depletion and, 117–118 basic psychological needs and, as
in motivation in, 382 neural foundation for, 375–376 antagonistic to, 92
evolved individual differences, in reward prediction error and, 375 corporate capitalism and, 99
motivation, 386–397 in self-enhancement, 313 manipulation of, 93
with aggression, 392–393 in self-protection, 313 psychological health influenced by, 92

index 569
pursuit of, 92 females. See women fluctuating selection for phenotypes,
research on, 93 feminism, sexual objectification and, 324 in FFM, 383–384
extrinsic motivation, 88–89 Five-Factor model of personality (FFM), extraversion in, 384
as external regulation, 88 382–386 focalism, BTAE and, 310
identified regulation in, 89 background of, 382 forethought, 376
integrated regulation in, 89 balanced selection in, for phenotypes, “for its own sake” motivation, 159–161
introjection in, 89 384 Four-Phase model, of interest, 167,
neural foundation for, 374–375 behavioral syndromes and, 385–386 169–171
outcome focus for goals and, 284 comparative psychology in, 384–385 developmental factors in, 170–171
for physical activity, 523–524 criticisms of, 386 individual interest in, 169
social context effects on, 96 evolution mechanisms for, 382–386 learner characteristics in, 175
work motivation and, 511 fluctuating selection in, for situational interest in, 169
phenotypes, 383–384 Frenzel, A.C., 178–179
F life history theory and, 385 Freud, Anna, 304
failure, as tradeoff, 67, 67–71 stabilizing selection in, for phenotypes, Freud, Sigmund, 153–154, 304
affective consequences of, 295–296 382–383 friendships, youth motivation influenced
behavioral consequences of, 294–295 tradeoffs in, 383–384 by, 536–537
depression and, 69–70 flight fantasies, 49 FSS. See Flow State Scale
emotional intensity for, 68 flow, as concept, 57, 557
emotional quality of, 68–69 action awareness in, 128–129 G
goal focus after, consequences of, autotelic experience and, 129–130 game structures, 455
294–296 calm energy mood and, 414 game theory, 400
happiness and, 69 challenge-skills balance in, 128, 130 gender. See also objectification theory
as means-related, 294 in computer-mediated environments, in expectancy-value theories, 527
as outcome-related, 294 132 vicarious learning and, 17
strategic preferences and, 71–72 control in, 129 gender development, for girls
fantasy realization, theory of, 211 creativity and, 132 by age level, 328–329
fatigue, effort intensity and, 425–426 definition of, 127–128 cognitive developmental theories for,
mental, 426 dimensions of, 128–130 328–330
feedback ESM for, 130, 133–134 media influences on, 331
autonomous motivation and, 94 facilitation of, 136–137 sexual objectification and, 328–330
in flow, 129 feedback in, 129 socialization theories for, 328
self-enhancement and, 313–314 Flow Scales for, 132, 134–136, gendered body project, 323, 331
self-protection and, 313–314 137–138 self-esteem and, 331
in unconscious goal pursuit, 239 FSS for, 132 general bodily arousal, 415–416
for youth motivation, 532–533 future research applications for, General Psychotherapy (GPT) model, 451
feedback control, 29–31. See also negative 137–138 generational differences, in work
feedback; positive feedback goal clarity in, 129 motivation, 514
affect and, 32–36 hypnotic susceptibility and, 137 global curiosity, 162
approach and avoidance processes in, 32 identification of, 127 glucose levels, ego depletion and, 118
effortful control in, 37 influential factors for, 136 goals. See also achievement goals; goal
experiential system in, 37 macro experiences, 128 orientation; goal pursuit; goal
goal sequences in, 31, 38 measurement of, 133–136 setting; goal striving; learning goals;
hierarchical organization of, 31–32, model for, 128 performance goals
37–38 motive congruence and, 152 abstractness of, 210–211
impulsive systems in, 37 neural foundation for, 375 for adult development, 281
levels of abstraction in, 31 perfectionism and, 132 avoidance, 447
loops in, 29–30 qualitative methodology for, 133 change, 289–290
in overt behavior, 30–31 quantitative methodology for, close relationships and, 355
planfulness in, 38 133–136 definition of, 4, 18, 191
principles in, 31 research examples of, 130–132 difficulty of, 18
processes in, 29–30, 30–31 self-consciousness in, loss of, 129 dysfunctional, 448
programs in, 31 self-reporting measures for, 138 extrinsic, in life, 91–93
rational system in, 37 in sports settings, 131 extrinsic achievement, 196
reflecting systems in, 37 task concentration with, 129 feedback control and, as sequences,
self-control and, 38 time perception and, 129 31, 38
two-mode models for, 37–38 Flow Scales, 132, 134–136, 137–138 in flow, 129
feedback loops, 29–30 Core, 135 habits and, as automatic, 237
concurrent function for, 34 Long, 134, 134–135 inequality of, 281
discrepancy-enlarging, 29–30 potential uses of, 135–136 inhibition of, in prevention
discrepancy-reducing, 29 Short, 135 orientation, 69
elements of, 29 Flow State Scale (FSS), 132 inhibition of, in promotion
homeostasis in, 30–31 FSS-2, 134 orientation, 69

570 index
interference of, 239 by life domain, 212 High Performance Learning Communities
intervention studies for, 225–226 mental contrasting in, 212, 212–213 Project (Project HiPlace), 472
intrinsic, in life, 91–93 for performance goals, 210 high school, student motivation during,
learning, 18–19 psychopathology and, 447 470
of motivational psychotherapy, 456 self-regulation of, 211–214 high-stakes rewards, 99–100
motivation compared to, 208–210 as theory, for work motivation, economic malfeasance from, 100
new, sources of, 39–40 508–509 in SDT, 99–100
performance-approach, 198–199 theory of fantasy realization and, 211 homeostasis, in feedback loops, 30–31
performance-avoidance, 198–199 goal striving, 214–225, 558 human capital, 98
shielding of, 217 approach versus avoidance processes human sexuality. See sexuality
social, for physical activity, 525 in, 215 hypnotic susceptibility, flow and, 137
social achievement, 196–197 aspirational standards and, 215
in social cognitive theory, 18–19 content as influence on, 214 I
stability, 290 delegation hypothesis for, 219 IAT. See Implicit Association Test
work-avoidance, 196 determinants of, 214–218 identified regulation, 89
goal focus, 283–296, 559. See also goal eagerness-related approach strategies in OIT, 484
orientation; outcome focus, for in, 215 for physical activity, 524
goals; process focus, for goals functions of, 214 identity-relatedness, in goal striving, 216
adaptation of, 289–290 future research applications for, identity status, 152
affective consequences after failure, 225–226 I-D model, of curiosity, 162
295–296 identity-relatedness in, 216 illness avoidance, 391–392
age and, 285–288 implementation intentions in, 218– immortality
approach versus avoidance processes 219, 219–221 through religious belief, 55
in, 288 intervention studies for, 225–226 symbolic, 50–51
behavioral consequences after failure, lack of willpower and, 221–225 implementation approach, to physical
294–295 learning-performance model for, 215 activity, 487–488
for change goals, 289–290 orientation framing in, 215 implementation intentions, in goal
change in, 288–290 positive affect and, 217 striving, 218–219, 219–221
after failure, consequences of, power status and, 217–218 ADHD and, 219
294–296 in self-completion theory, 216 lack of willpower, 224
maintenance in, 286 self-regulation of, 218–225 Implicit Association Test (IAT), 494
means usage frequency in, 289 shielding in, 217 implicit motives, 8, 557
motivational phase and, 291–293 structural features of goals in, consistency in, 150–151
resources for, 287, 289 214–215 correspondence of content for,
in self-system theory, 287–288 Goetz, T., 178–179 147–148
for stability goals, 290 GPT. See General Psychotherapy model disattenuation approaches in, 148–149
stability of, 288–290 grading systems, student motivation and, IAT for, 494
time perspectives and, 287–288 470 incongruence in, 154
goal-interference, 239 groups, work motivation for, 512–513 independence of, 154
goal orientation, 281, 559 group belonging, 52–53 integrative general model for, 145–146
adaptation of, 283 culture and, 52–53 measure methodology for, 142–146,
age as influence on, 283 group learning, student motivation 148–149
compensation in, 282–283 and, 469 MTMM analysis of, 149
goal selection and, 282 omnibus effect of multiple
multidirectionality of, 281–283 H methodological factors, 146–147,
multiple goal management and, 282 habits 154
optimization for, 282–283 as automatic goal-directed behavior, 237 for physical activity, 493–494
outcome focus for goals and, 284–285 stimulus-response rules and, 235–236 private body consciousness in,
process focus for goals and, 284–285 in unconscious goal pursuit, 235–238 150–151
resource conservation in, 282–283 happiness, 69 in psychotherapeutic motivation, 442
student motivation and, 466 Harter, Susan, 521 self-monitoring of, 150–151
goal pursuit, 558–559 health care, autonomy climates in, 95 statistical independence in, 144
achievement goals and, 234 health-promoting behaviors, 48–49 impression management, in SSB, 307
performance and, 281 Heckhausen, Heinz, 291, 292, 373 impulsive systems, in feedback control, 37
research on, 233, 234–235 Heckhausen, J., 283, 291 incentives, motivation and, 209
triggers for, 234 hedonic incentive, for success, 431–432 for close relationships, 354
goal setting, 558 hedonism, 304 current, 354
determinants of, for content and hegemonic masculinity environmental, 369
structure, 210–211 definition of, 325 hedonic, for success, 431–432
future research applications for, power and, 325 material, for success, 427–428
225–226 sexual objectification of women and, incongruence, of motives, 154
intervention studies for, 225–226 325–326 individual differences, in motivation,
for learning goals, 210 hero worship, 56 560–561, 563–564

index 571
AIM-Q for, 398 knowledge in, 181 L
assessment strategy development for, learning and, 177 lack of willpower. See willpower, lack of
398–399 Punnett square for, 182–183 leadership
in behavioral syndrome theory, 382 research on, 175–181 Leadership Scale for Sport, 533
in control theory, 400 shifts in, 176, 177–178 transformational, 511–512
for effort intensity, 434 study methodology for, 173, 175–181 for youth motivation, 533, 538–539
in evolutionary theory, 382 survey methodology for, 173 Leadership Scale for Sport, 533
evolved, 386–397 sustainment factors, 176, 177–178 learners, in Four-Phase model of interest,
in FFM, 382–386 triggers for, 175–176, 176–177 175
future research applications for, International Association for Positive learning. See also vicarious learning
399–400 Psychology (IPPA), 127 in achievement goal models, 192
in game theory, 400 interpersonal inquisitiveness, 392 associative, as motivational state,
for legacies, 396–397 interpersonal motives, model for, 455 369–370
in life history theory, 382 interventions context, stimulus-response rules and,
in personality psychology, 381–382 DFI, 453–454 236
reliability of, 398 for physical activity, mapping of, cooperative, 471
theory development for, 381–382 495–496 enactive, 15
validity of, 398–399 in psychotherapeutic motivation, goals, 18, 18–19
for youth physical activity, 540–544 449–450 in goal striving, 215
in Zurich Model of Social Motivation, for student motivation, 471–473, 474 in groups, student motivation and,
400 intervention studies, for goals, 225–226 469
individual interest, 169 interviews. See motivational interviewing interest development and, 177
information gaps model, for curiosity, 159 intimacy motivation, 353–354 observational, 16, 16, 537–538
inhibition effects, in vicarious learning, 16 intrinsic life goals, 91–93 performance compared to, 15
inquisitiveness attainment of, 92–93 social influences on, 463–464
environmental, 392 for basic psychological needs, 92 learning goals, 18, 18–19
interpersonal, 392 manipulation of, 93 goal setting for, 210
integrated regulation, 89 pursuit of, 92 learning-performance model
in physical activity, 524 research on, 93 of achievement goals, 192
intention, physical activity and, 480 intrinsic motivation, 87–88 for goal striving, 215
in planned behavior theory, 483 choice and, 253 learning theory, motivation in, 209
interest. See also interest development external pressure and, 94 legacy motive, 396–397
awareness of, 168–169 information internalization in, 88 Lewin, Kurt, 251
choice influenced by, 263 neural foundation for, 374–375 life history theory, 382
conceptualizations of, 171–172 in OIT, 484 FFM and, 385
curiosity influenced by, 161 organizational goal-setting and, 512 liking, in associative learning, 369–370
definition of, 169 outcome focus for goals and, 284 Linnenbrink-Garcia, L., 179–180
development of, 170–171, 175–181 outcomes from, 89–90 Long flow Scales, 134, 134–135
emotions and, 172 for physical activity, 523 DFS-2, 134
Four-Phase model of, 167, 169–171 as pleasurable, 88 psychometric characteristics
future research applications for, process focus for goals and, 284 of, 134–135
183–184 social context effects on, 96 Lowell, E.L., 142–143
individual, 169 for students, 464–466, 467
knowledge and, 169 work motivation and, 511 M
measurement considerations for, introjection, 89 MacKinnon, Catherine, 325
172–175 in OIT, 484 macro flow experiences, 128
Punnett square for, 182–183 IPPA. See International Association for maladaptive choices, 249
research on, 168–169 Positive Psychology Manzy, C., 179–180
situational, 169 mastery experiences
student motivation and, 465 J outcome focus for goals and, 284–285
study methodology for, 173, 175–181 James, William, 48, 304 process focus for goals and, 284–285
survey methodology for, 173 Job Characteristics Model, 510 self-efficacy and, 23–24
task competence and, 172 job design, work motivation and, in student motivation, 467
vocational, 172 510–511 MAT. See Motivation Analysis Test
Interest and Effort in Education (Dewey), 160 from Job Characteristics Model, 510 material incentives, for success, 427–428
interest development, 169, 170–171, materialism. See also wealth accumulation,
558 K evolved individual motives for
affect and, 176 Keillor, Garrison, 307 from corporate capitalism, 99
case studies for, 178–179, 179–180, Keller, Helen, 167–168, 167–168 existential anxiety and, 49
180–181 knowledge exploration through, in promotion-
fluctuations in, 176, 177–178 interest and, 169 focused individuals, 75
future research applications for, in interest development, 181 maximization desires, exploration and, 75
183–184 Koskey, K.L., 179–180 Mayer, Richard, 365

572 index
McClelland, David, 141–142, 142–143 activation of, 8 difficulty in, variability of status for,
independence interpretation of, appearance, 393 421–422, 423–427
143–145 biological purpose of, 4–5 fatigue in, 425–426
McDougall, William, 161–162 for choice, 249–251 mood in, 426–427
meaning schemas cybernetic approach to, 38–40 motives. See also explicit motives;
evolved individual motives for, 397 definition of, 13, 367 implicit motives
through religious belief, 55 desirability and, 209 activation of, 8
media, sexual objectification of women drive-reduction model for, 158 affiliation, 353
and, 331 ego depletion and, 119 appearance, 393
memory. See episodic memory, self-efficacy within embedded contexts, 86–87 congruence, 142
and; selective self-memory “for its own sake,” 159–161 consistency in, 150–151
menstruation, sexual objectification and, goal focus and, 291–293 correspondence of content for, 147–148
327 goals compared to, 208–210 definition of, 141
mental contrasting, in goal setting, 212, as implicit, 8 disattenuation approaches in,
212–213 incentives and, 209 148–149
commitment in, 212 individual differences for, 563–564 in evolved individual difference theory,
mediating processes for, 213 intimacy, 353–354 389–390
motivation and, 213 in learning theory, 209 game structures as, in personality
for negative futures, 212–213 mental contrasting and, 213 disorders, 455
for present reality associations, 213 nonconscious, 8, 8, 251 incongruence and, 154
as problem-solving strategy, 213 in observational learning, 16 integrative general model for, 145–146
mental fatigue, 426 for physical activity, theoretical interpersonal model, 455
middle school, student motivation during, frameworks for, 521–528 legacy, 396–397
470–471 postactional phase for, 291 measure methodology for, 142–146,
Mill, John Stuart, 304 preactional phase for, 291, 292 148–149
Mischel, Walter, 222 predecisional phase for, 291, 291–292 MTMM analysis of, 149
mnemic neglect, 312 priming methods for, 8 omnibus effect of multiple
moods, 561. See also depression psychological models for, 5–7 methodological factors, 146–147,
alternative models for, 412–413 social, 353–354 154
arousal systems, 113, 409–412 in social cognitive theory, processes private body consciousness in, 150–151
cognition and, 411–412 for, 18, 17–24, 209 PSEs for, 141–142
complex, formation of, 413–415 for student, 464–466 questionnaires for, 141–142
diet and nutrition and, 410 tradeoffs for, 67–78 self-determination in, 150
effort intensity and, 426–427, 431–432 in unconscious goal pursuit, 239–242 self-monitoring of, 150–151
emotion compared to, 408 motivation, studies on social exchange, 395–396
energetic arousal, 409–412 academic resurgence for, 7–9 statistical independence in
function of, 408 causal explanations in, 5 measurement for, 143
future research applications, 418 consilience in, 6 TAT for, 141
general bodily arousal, 415–416 coordinated analyses for, 9 motive congruence, 142
in motivation intensity theory, 426–427 experimental methods in, 8–9 affect regulation and, 151
muscular tension and, 416 interdisciplinary nature of, 4–7 analysis of, 154
pleasantness and activation model practical applications for, 9–10 antecedents of, 151
for, 412 psychological models in, 5–7 consequences of, 151–153
pleasure/displeasure model for, 412 statistical methodology changes for, 8 flow and, 152
positive and negative activation model Motivational Attunement approach, 449 Freud and, 153–154
for, 412 motivational climate identity status and, 152
positive and negative affect model for physical activity, 492, 533–534 incongruence and, 154
for, 412 for SDT, 492 need satisfaction in, 151
self-regulation of, 416–417 for youths, for physical activity, 526, research models for, 153
task difficulty and, 426–427 533–534 stress and, 151
tense arousal, 412 motivational interviewing, 453 unintegration of, 154
two-dimensional models for, 412 motivational theory, 4. See also control volitional strength and, 152
mortality salience (MS) hypothesis, 46–47 theory; regulatory focus theory; well-being and, 152–153
motivations. See also curiosity; evolved self-determination theory; MS hypothesis. See mortality salience
individual differences, in social cognitive theory; terror hypothesis
motivation; extrinsic motivation; management theory MTMM analysis. See multitrait-method
goals; intrinsic motivation; cognitive approaches to, 3 analysis framework
motivational theory; neuroscience, coordinated analyses for, 9 multilevel selection theory, 395
motivation and; psychotherapy, psychological models in, 5–7 multiple goals model, 198
motivation in; relatedness, for Motivation Analysis Test (MAT), 387 multitrait-method (MTMM) analysis
children; rewards, motivation and; motivation intensity theory, 420–422 framework, 149
individual differences, in motivation ability in, 424–425 Murray, J. Clark, 159
actional phase for, 291, 292–293 benefit in, 421 muscular tension, 416

index 573
N openness to experience, curiosity and, perceived versus actual choice, 257
need for affiliation. See affiliation, need for 162–163 perfectionism, flow and, 132
need satisfaction, 151. See also basic optimal arousal approach, to drive- performance
psychological needs, in SDT reduction model, 158–159 in achievement goal models, 192
cultural values and, 97 optimal experience, 127. See also flow, as goal pursuit and, 281
need thwarting, 488–489 concept in goal striving, 215
negative affect organismic integration theory (OIT), learning compared to, 15
goal striving and, 216–217 484 outcome focus for goals and, 284–285
priority management and, 30 external regulation in, 484 process focus for goals and, 284–285
unconscious goal pursuit and, 243 identified regulation in, 484 self-efficacy and, 21
negative feedback, for overt behavior, 30 intrinsic motivation in, 484 tradeoffs in, 76–78
neuroscience, motivation and, 560. See introjection regulation in, 484 performance-approach goals, 198–199
also decision making organization performance-avoidance goals, 198–199
for agency, 372–373 intrinsic motivation and, 512 performance goals, 18–19
associative learning states for, purpose of, 3 goal setting for, 210
369–370 work motivation and, 513 Perkun, R., 178–179
automatic states for, 368–369 outcome expectations persistence, self-efficacy and, 23
dopamine hypothesis of positive affect, choice and, 269 personal goals
370 self-efficacy and, 19, 19, 23 alcohol consumption and, dysfunction
dopamine system in, 367–368 in social cognitive theory, 19 in, 448
for expectancy, 375–376 outcome focus, for goals content dimension of, 446–447
in expectancy-value theory, 373–374 age and, 290 depression and, 447–448
for extrinsic motivation, 374–375 definition of, 284 in psychotherapeutic motivation,
for flow, 375 extrinsic motivation and, 284 442–443, 444–445
future research applications in, goal orientation and, 284–285 in teleonomic model, 446
377–378 intrinsic motivation and, 284 well-being as, 445–448
implicit states for, 370–371 performance and, 284–285 personality
for intrinsic motivation, 374–375 process focus compared to, 285, 285 effort intensity and, 434
key constructs in, 372–377 An Outline of Intellectual Rubbish lack of willpower and, 224
neural core for, 367–372 (Russell), 315–316 structure, 143
neurotransmitters in, 378 overt behavior, 30–31 personality disorders, 455
priming in, 370–371 discrepancy-enlarging feedback loop game structures in, 455
rational motivational states, 371–372 for, 30 model of interpersonal motives, 455
for self-efficacy, 376 for discrepancy-reducing feedback personality psychology
for self-regulation, 376–377 loop, 30 comparative psychology for, 384–385
somatic marker hypothesis, 371 negative feedback processes for, 30 discrepancy-enlarging feedback loop
taxonomy for, 366–367 positive feedback processes for, 30 in, 30
for value, 373–374 discrepancy-reducing feedback loop
for volition, 373 P in, 30
Nicholls, J.G., 192–193 parents. See also attachment, personal extraversion in, 381
9/11 attacks, existential anxiety after, 57 relationships and; attachment feedback control hierarchy in, 32
nonconscious motivations, 8, 8 theory; relatedness, for children FFM in, 382–386
choice and, 251 in attachment theory, 337–338 focus of, 386
Nussbaum, Martha, 324 parental affect, in SDT, 339–340 individual differences in motivation,
physical activity motivation, influence 381–382
O on, 528–532 Personality Research Form (PRF), 387
objectification theory, 559–560 pressure by, for physical activity, personal relationships. See also attachment
evolutionary adaptation in, 324–325 531–532 theory; parental relationships,
existential motivations in, 326–327 responsibility of children to, 341–342, in attachment theory; peer
framework of, 324–327 344–345 relationships; teacher relationships
gendered body project and, 323, 331 sensitivity of, in attachment theory, attachment and, 53–54
girls’ gender development and, 338 existential anxiety and, 53–54
328–330 parental relationships, in attachment mutual autonomy support in, 95–96
hegemonic masculinity maintenance theory, 337–338 teacher-student, student motivation
in, 325–326 sensitivity towards child in, 338 and, 468, 466–469
media influences in, 331 PDP. See Process Dissociation Procedure persuasive messages
power in, role of, 325 peer relationships ego depletion and, 115
self-objectification in, 327–331 in attachment theory, 338 from self-efficacy, 21–22, 24
objective self-awareness theory, 44 in competence motivation theory, 521 physical activity, 562
observational learning, 16 mutual autonomy support in, 95–96 achievement goal theory and,
motivation in, 16 student motivation and, 468–469 485–487, 492–493, 524–526
for youth motivation, 537–538 youth motivation influenced by, basic psychological needs and,
OIT. See organismic integration theory 535–540 484–485, 488–489, 524

574 index
competence motivation theory for, dopamine hypothesis, 370 intrinsic motivation and, 284
521–522 ego depletion and, 120–121 outcome focus compared to, 285, 285
energetic arousal and, 411 goal striving and, 217 performance and, 284–285
expectancy-value theories for, with multiple concerns, 36 programs, in feedback control, 31
526–528 unconscious goal pursuit and, 240, Project HiPlace. See High Performance
extrinsic motivation for, 523–524 241–242 Learning Communities Project
identified regulation of, 524 positive and negative activation model, for promotion orientation, in regulatory focus
implementation intention approach mood, 412 theory, 6, 66. See also failure, as
to, 487–488 positive and negative affect model, for tradeoff; success, as tradeoff
implicit motivation for, 493–494 mood, 412 creativity and, 76–77
integrated regulation of, 524 positive feedback, for overt behavior, 30 depression and, 70
intention and, 480 postactional phase, for motivations, 291 desired end states for, 66–67
intervention mapping for, 495–496 posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), 51 exploration and, 75–76
intrinsic motivation for, 523 power. See also willpower, lack of for failure, 67, 67–71
measurement methodology for, of choices, 248 happiness and, 69
493–496 goal striving and, through status, intergoal inhibition in, 69
motivational climate for, 492, 217–218 life experiences and, 71–72
533–534 in objectification theory, 325 performance and, 76–78
need thwarting and, 488–489 rape and, 325–326 strategic preferences for, 71–72
in planned behavior theory, 481–484, Powers, William, 29 for success, 67, 67–71
489–491 preactional phase, for motivations, 291, system constraints for, 78–80
psychology of, 480 292 tradeoffs, for motivations, 67–78
range of, 521 implementational mindset for, 292 PSEs. See Picture-Story Exercises
RCTs for, 494–495 predecisional phase, for motivations, 291, psychological distance, in goal striving,
in SDT, 484–485, 488–489, 291–292 216
489–491, 492–493, 523–524 preferences, choice influenced by, psychopathology, development of, 446
self-efficacy and, 480 270–271 goal setting and, 447
social goals for, 525 prevention orientation, in regulatory focus psychotherapy, motivation in, 561
theoretical advances for, 487–493 theory, 6, 66. See also failure, as ACT, 453, 454–455, 457
theoretical motivation frameworks for, tradeoff; success, as tradeoff Alliance Fostering Therapy, 449
521–528 analytical processing and, 77 autonomous motivation approach to,
theory integration for, 489–493 anxiety disorders and, 70 443, 448–449
2 x 2 model for, 486–487, 492–493 commitment and, 72–75 Clarification Oriented Therapy, 453,
as value, 531 desired end states for, 66–67 457
well-being and, 479 for failure, 67 clinically relevant constructs in,
youth motivation for, 528–540 happiness and, 69 442–443
physical stature and status, evolved intergoal inhibition in, 69 DFI in, 453–454
individual motives for, 394 life experiences and, 71–72 experience as influence on, 443
Picture-Story Exercises (PSEs), 141–142 performance and, 76–78 explicit motives in, 442
statistical independence in, 143 safety as focus in, 78 factors in, 448–455
Plan Analysis, 445 satisfactory state as motivational future research applications for,
planfulness, 38 necessity for, 70 457–458
planned behavior theory, 481–484 strategic preferences for, 71–72 general models of change in, 451–452
attitudes in, 481 for success, 67 goals of, 456
choice in, 250 system constraints for, 78–80 GPT, 451
control beliefs in, 481–482 tradeoffs, for motivations, 67–78 implicit motives in, 442
critical assessment of, 482, 483–484 PRF. See Personality Research Form intervention facilitation in, 449–450
expectancy theory and, 506 priming methods, for motives, 8 Motivational Attunement approach,
intention in, 483 neural basis for, 370–371 449
SDT and, integration of, 489–491 principles, in feedback control, 31 motivational construct assessment in,
subjective norms in, 481 priority management 443–445
pleasantness and activation model, for affect and, 36 motivational interviewing in, 453
mood, 412 depression and, 36 patient ambivalence over, 453
pleasure centers, of brain, 235–236 emotions and, influence on, 36 patient relationship in, 449
pleasure/displeasure model, for mood, 412 private body consciousness, 150–151 personal goals in, 442–443, 444–445
political systems, 100–101 problem-solving strategies, mental Plan Analysis in, 445
democratic, 100–101 contrasting as, 213 Rubicon Model of Action Phases in, 451
totalitarianism, 100 Process Dissociation Procedure (PDP), 223 SDT in, 444
positive affect process focus, for goals SIM in, 448–449
coasting and, 35 age and, 290 SST, 452–453, 454, 457
in competence motivation theory, definition of, 284 theory development for, 441–442
521–522 extrinsic motivation and, 284 treatment goals in, 444, 443, 445,
as counterintuitive, 35–36 goal orientation and, 284–285 449–450

index 575
TTM, 452 responsibility, of children to parents, purpose of, 43
values in, 442 341–342 in SDT, 102
WBT, 454 academic functioning and, 341–342, self-regulation and, 44, 58
well-being as goal in, 445–448 344–345 TMT and, 57
PTSD. See posttraumatic stress disorder cultural variations of, 345 well-being and, 102
Pugh, K.J., 179–180 by developmental stage, 344 self-awareness theory, success in, 431
Punnett square, for interest, 182–183 parental values and, adoption of, 342 self-completion theory, 216
resting self-consciousness, in flow, 129
R ego depletion after, 120–121 self-control
randomized controlled trials (RCTs), energetic arousal after, 410–411 addictive behaviors and, 112
494–495 retention, for vicarious learning, 16 autonomy and, 121
treatment fidelity in, 495–496 rewards, motivation and conservation of resources and,
rape, feelings of power from, 325–326 automatic states for, 368–369 119–120
rational system, in feedback control, 37 choice based on, 249 ego depletion and, 111
RCTs. See randomized controlled trials dopamine system-influenced, ego strength from, 112–113, 122
reciprocal interactions, 14–15 367–368 feedback control and, 38
agency and, 14–15 neural foundations for, in motivated mood arousal and, 113
collective agency in, 15 actions, 367–372 as process, 111
self-efficacy and, 14 neurotransmitters for, 378 self-regulation compared to, 111–112
triadic reciprocality, 14 reward prediction error, 375 self-determination, in motives, 150
reflecting systems, in feedback control, 37 sources of, 368–372 self-determination theory (SDT), 4, 7,
reflection, in self-regulation, 376 in work settings, 515 556–557. See also autonomous
reform, of student motivation practices, reward-aversion model, for curiosity, 160 motivation; extrinsic motivation;
470–471 reward contingencies, in corporate intrinsic motivation
research implications for, 474–475 capitalism, 98–99 autonomy in, 85–86, 340
regulation. See external regulation, in as high-stakes, 99–100 basic psychological needs in, 87,
OIT; identified regulation; reward prediction error, 375 101–102, 484–485
integrated regulation; reward processing, in unconscious goal causality orientations in, 87, 90–91
self-regulation pursuit, 240–241 for children, 339–341
regulatory fit, 6 Rilke, Rainer Maria, 59 choice in, 253
regulatory focus theory, 6, 66–67 risk taking, ego depletion and, 116 cultural contexts for, 86–87, 96–97
choice in, 265 Rubicon Model of Action Phases, 451 economic systems under, 97–99
prevention orientation in, 6, 66 Russell, Bertrand, 315–316 embedded contexts and, motivations
promotion orientation in, 6, 66 safety, as focus, 78 in, 86–87
regulatory fit in, 6 extrinsic life goals in, 91–93
tradeoffs in, for motivations, 67–78 S high-stakes rewards in, 99–100
relatedness, for children, 560. See also SDT. See self-determination theory intrinsic life goals in, 91–93
attachment, personal relationships selective self-memory motivational climate for, 492
and from encoding bias, 311–312 OIT as subtheory of, 484
alternative factors to, 345–346 prevalence of, 311 overview of, 85
attachment theory and, 336–339 psychological health and, 314–315 parental affect in, 339–340
contextual variations, 344–345 psychological interests and, 315 physical activity in, 484–485, 488–
early, 342–344 the self 489, 489–491, 492–493, 523–524
function of, 335 ego depletion and, from suppression, planned behavior theory and,
responsibility to parents and, 341–342 114 integration of, 489–491
SDT and, 339–341 four constituents of, 48 political systems under, 100–101
theory integration for, 342–346 self-affirmation in psychotherapeutic motivation, 444
relationships. See attachment theory; close BTAE and, 309 self-awareness in, 102
relationships; peer relationships; ego depletion and, 121 social contexts in, 87
personal relationships self-enhancement and, 306, 312 socialization of children, 339
religious beliefs. See also spirituality self-protection and, 312 transformational leadership and,
existential anxiety and, 55 SSB and, 306 511–512
immortality and, 55 self-awareness, 44–45. See also existential 2 x 2 model and, integration of,
intrinsic, 55 anxiety; existential psychology 492–493
meaning schemas through, 55 autonomy and, 102 work motivation and, 511–512
psychological consequences of, 55 avoidance behaviors, 57 youth motivation and, 523–524
worldview validation through, 55 behavioral flexibility and, 44–45 self-efficacy. See also collective efficacy
resources escaping from, 56–58 behavioral control and, 515–516
in adult goal development, 282–283 existential issues with, 45 career choice and, 23
ego depletion and, 119–120 in flow, 128–129 in children, 25
for goal focus, 287 flow concept and, 57 from choice, 254, 264
self-control and, 119–120 functions of, 44 effects of, 22–23, 23
response facilitation, 16 in objective self-awareness theory, 44 emotional reactions and, 22

576 index
environmental factors for, 14 self-perception. See also self-awareness mnemic neglect and, 312
episodic memory and, 376 in competence motivation theory, 521 SSB and, 305–306
informational sources for, 21, 20–22 ego depletion and, 116 sexuality
mastery experiences and, 23–24 for youth physical activity motivation, evolved individual motives for, 393
multiplicative influences on, 22 540–542 existential anxiety and, 49
neural foundation for, 376 self-perception theory, choice in, 252–253 sexual masochism, self-awareness and, 57
outcome expectations and, 19, 19, 23 self-presentation, choice and, 268 sexual objectification, of women. See also
performance behavior and, 21 self-protection, 559 objectification theory
persistence and, 23 BTAE in, 307–311 breasts and, 326–327
personal factors for, 14 definition of, 303 components of, 324
from persuasive messages, 21–22, 24 expectancy differentials in, 313 definition of, 324
physical activity and, 480 feedback inconsistency and, 313–314 as evolutionary adaptation, 324–325
reciprocal interactions and, 14 future research development for, 316 existential motivations for, 326–327
research evidence for, 23–24 in hedonism, 304 feminism and, 324
self-modeling for, 21 information valences in, 313–314 gendered body project and, 323, 331
self-regulation and, 23 instantiations of, 304–314 hegemonic masculinity maintenance
in social cognitive theory, 20–24 nonmotivational explanations for, from, 325–326
social comparisons and, 20, 21 312–314 media influences in, 331
student motivation and, 464 psychological health and, 314–315 menstruation and, 327
task choice and, 23 psychological interests and, 315 power from, 325
self-enhancement, 559 selective self-memory and, 311–312 theoretical frameworks for, 324–327
BTAE in, 307–311 self-affirmation and, 312 by women, 327–331
definition of, 303 self-threat and, 312 shielding, of goals, 217
expectancy differentials in, 313 self-view valences in, 313–314 Short flow scales, 135
feedback inconsistency and, 313–314 SSB in, 305–307 SIM. See Striving Instrumentality Matrix
future research development for, 316 theory development for, 304 situational interest, 169
in hedonism, 304 self-regulation. See also commitment, as skills
information valences in, 313–314 tradeoff; feedback control; self- in competence motivation theory, 521
instantiations of, 304–314 control; self-determination in flow, challenges in balance with,
nonmotivational explanations for, choice and, 250, 256 128, 130
312–314 cybernetic processes for, 28–29, in unconscious goal pursuit, 236–238
psychological health and, 314–315 555–556 sleep, energetic arousal and, 410–411. See
psychological interests and, 315 forethought in, 376 also resting
selective self-memory and, 311–312 of goal setting, 211–214 Smith R.E., 532
self-affirmation and, 306, 312 of goal striving, 218–225 social achievement goals, 196–197
self-threat and, 312 happiness and, 69 social bonds, 350–351
self-view valences in, 313–314 of mood, 416–417 social isolation and, 350–351
SSB in, 305–307 neural foundation for, 376–377 social cognitive theory, 6, 555
theory development for, 304 reflection in, 376 collective efficacy and, 24
self-esteem self-awareness and, 44 conceptual framework for, 14–15
existential anxiety and, 49–51 self-control compared to, 111–112 cross-cultural relevance of, 25–26
gendered body project and, 331 self-efficacy and, 23 developmental appropriateness of, 25
youth physical activity motivation in social cognitive theory, 17 enactive learning in, 15
and, 540–542 unconscious goal pursuit and, 243 future research applications for, 24–26
self-evaluation. See also self-enhancement; self-serving bias (SSB), 50 goals in, 18–19
self-protection expectancies in, 306 modeled observations in, benefits
of effort intensity, 431 impression management in, 307 of, 25
in social cognitive theory, 19 nonmotivational explanations for, motivational processes in, 18, 17–24,
social comparisons and, 20 306–307 209
of success, 431 prevalence of, among populations, 305 outcome expectations in, 19
self-modeling, for vicarious learning, 17 psychological health and, 314 performance in, learning compared
self-monitoring, of motives, 150–151 psychological interests and, 315 to, 15
self-objectification, by women, 327–331 self-affirmation and, 306 reciprocal interactions in, 14–15
cognitive developmental theories for, in self-enhancement, 305–307 self-efficacy in, 20–24
328–330 in self-protection, 305–307 self-evaluation in, 19
cultural messages and, 330 self-threat and, 305–306 self-regulatory processes in, 17
factors for, 327–328 self-system theory social comparisons in, 20
gendered body project and, 323, 331 depression and, 70 symbolic processes in, 17
girls’ gender development and, goal focus in, 287–288 values in, 19–20
328–330 Self-System Therapy (SST), 452–453, vicarious learning in, 15, 15–17
through media, 331 454, 457 social comparisons
situational motivators for, 330 self-threat, 312 development of, 20
socialization theories for, 328 BTAE and, 309 self-efficacy and, 20, 21

index 577
self-evaluation and, 20 in early grade school, 469–471 task-ego model, of achievement goals,
in social cognitive theory, 20 education as value in, 465 192–193
social competence, student motivation goal orientation and, 466 TAT. See Thematic Apperception Test
and, 468–469 by grade level, 469–470 teacher relationships
social contexts, in SDT, 87 grading systems and, 470 children and, in attachment theory, 338
social exchange motive, 395–396 group learning and, 469 collective efficacy for, 24
social goals, for youth physical activity, in high school, 470 student motivation and, 468,
525 interest and, 465 466–469
social information, in close relationships interventions for, 471–473, 474 for youth motivation, for physical
interpretation of, 356–357 intrinsic motivation and, 464–466, 467 activity, 532–535
weight of, 357 mastery experiences as part of, 467 teleonomic model, of well-being, 446
social interaction, emotion in, 357–358 in middle school, 470–471 personal goals in, 446
social isolation, negative effects of, peer influence on, 468–469 television consumption, self-awareness
350–351 through Project HiPlace, 472 and, 57
socialization, of children, 339 reform efforts for, 470–471, 474–475 tense arousal, 412
social learning theory, choice in, 250 in research study, 474 calm energy mood, 414
social motivation school influence on, 466–469 complex moods from, 413–415
affiliation motives in, 353 self-efficacy and, 464 muscular tension and, 416
Zurich Model, 400 social competence and, 468–469 tense energy, 415
social psychology TARGET approach to, 467–468 tense tiredness, 414–415
discrepancy-enlarging feedback loop task assignment and, 466–467 terror management theory (TMT), 4,
in, 30 teachers’ influence on, 468, 466–469 6–7, 45–48, 556
discrepancy-reducing feedback loop transition to school and, 470, anxiety-buffer hypothesis in, 47
in, 30 469–471 criticism of, 58–59
feedback control hierarchy in, 32 subjective energy, 409–410 death as inevitability in, 46
socioeconomics, choice influenced subjective norms, in planned behavior development of, 45–46
by, 265–267 theory, 481 DTA hypothesis in, 47
somatic marker hypothesis, 371 success, as tradeoff, 67, 67–71 existential anxiety in, 48–58
specific curiosity, 159 ego involvement in, 430–431 future research on, 58–59
spirituality, 56 emotional intensity for, 68 group belonging and, 52–53
existential anxiety and, 54–56 emotional quality of, 68–69 MS hypothesis in, 46–47
sports, flow in, 131. See also youth happiness and, 69 psychopathology of, 51
motivation, for physical activity material incentives for, 427–428 PTSD and, 51
SSB. See self-serving bias outcome expectancy for, 428–429 purpose of, 43
SST. See Self-System Therapy in self-awareness theory, 431 research strategy for, 46–48
stability goals, 290 self-evaluation of, 431 self-awareness and, 57
stabilizing selection for phenotypes, social evaluation of, 429–430 worldview validation in, 52–53
in FFM, 382–383 strategic preferences and, 71–72 Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), 141
statistical independence, in motive success, effort intensity and, 427–432 theory of fantasy realization. See fantasy
measurement, 143 material incentives for, 427–428 realization, theory of
in explicit motives, 144 Sullivan, Anne, 167 theory of planned behavior. See planned
in implicit motives, 144 symbolic immortality, 50–51 behavior theory
McClelland’s interpretation of, 143–145 hero worship and, 56 The Story of My Life (Keller), 167–168
in personality structure, 143 thirst, as motivational state, 368–369
problems in, 143 T threat avoidance
Steinbeck, John, 50 TARGET approach, to student in close relationships, 354
Stewart, V.C., 179–180 motivation, 467–468 evolved individual motives for, 391
stimulus-response rules, 235–236 evaluation techniques in, 468 time
context learning and, 236 grouping arrangements in, 468 goal focus and, 287–288
in pleasure centers, of brain, 235–236 task design in, 468 perception of, flow and, 129
stress time allocation in, 468 in TARGET approach, 468
energetic arousal and, 412 tasks work motivation influenced by,
motive congruence and, 151 in achievement goal model, 192–193 513–514
Striving Instrumentality Matrix (SIM), choice and, by type, 269 TMT. See terror management theory
448–449 delay of gratification, 221–222 tobacco use, self-control tasks and, 122
student motivation, 464–466 effort intensity by, 434 Tomkins, Silvan, 160–161
classroom practices, 466–467, 473 flow and, 129 totalitarianism, 100
competence and, 464 interest in, competence and, 172 tradeoffs, for motivations, 67–78, 556.
control and, 464 mood and, 426–427 See also prevention orientation, in
cooperative learning approach to, 471 self-control, with tobacco use, 122 regulatory focus theory; promotion
with CORI, 472 self-efficacy from, 23 orientation, in regulatory focus theory
cultural influences on, 473–474 student motivation from, 466–467 commitment as, 72–75
development of, 466 in TARGET approach, 468 in emotional life, 67–72

578 index
within evolved individual differences, through observation, 16 for groups, 512–513
391–397 production processes of, 16 intrinsic motivation and, 511
exploration as, 75–76 response facilitation in, 16 job design and, 510–511
for failure, 67, 67–71 retention of, 16 organization through, 513
in FFM, 383–384 self-modeling for, 17 parameters of, 505
in performance, 76–78 types of, 16, 16 rewards in, 515
for success, 67, 67–71 violence, from threats to worldview SDT and, 511–512
transformational leadership, 511–512 validation, 53 temporal perspectives on, 513–514
transition to schools, student vocational interest, 172 theoretical perspectives on, 505–512
motivation and volition transformational leadership and,
in grade school, 469–471 neural foundation for, 373 511–512
in high school, 470 strength of, 152 worldview validation, 52–53
Transtheoretical Model (TTM), 452 through culture, 52
triadic reciprocality, 14 W through religious belief, 55
trichotomous model, of achievement Watt, H.M.G., 178–179 threats to, violence from, 53
goals, 194 WBT. See Well-Being Therapy
TTM. See Transtheoretical Model wealth accumulation, evolved individual Y
2 x 2 model, of achievement goals, motives for, 394 Yalom, Irvin, 45, 49, 51. See also
194–195 weekend effect, 101 existential anxiety
for physical activity, 486–487 well-being Young, Iris Marion, 326–327
SDT and, integration of, 492–493 avoidance goals and, 447 youth motivation, for physical activity,
energetic arousal and, 409–410 528–540, 562
U motive congruence and, 152–153 achievement goal theory and,
unconscious goal pursuit. See also goal as personal goal, 445–448 524–526
setting; goal striving physical activity and, 479 attitude and behavioral modeling for,
academic development of, theories for, self-awareness and, 102 529–530
232–233 social isolation and, 350–351 autonomy-supportive behaviors for,
active maintenance of representations teleonomic model of, 446 534–535
in, 238–239 Well-Being Therapy (WBT), 454 coach/teacher influence on, 532–535
classical conditioning and, 233 wellness, autonomous motivation and, competence for, 540–542
conceptual parameters of, 233–235 96–97 competence motivation theory and,
control of, 235–239 White, Robert, 521 521–522
feedback processing in, 239 will, unconscious goal pursuit and, 240 controlling behaviors in, 534–535
flexibility of, 238–239, 243 willpower, lack of, 221–225 emotional influences on, 542–544
goal-interference and, inhibition in academic settings, 222 expectancy-value theories for,
of, 239 affective responses and, 223 526–528
habits in, 235–238 behavioral responses and, 223–224 expressions of competence for, 530–531
monitoring in, 239 cognitive responses and, 223 feedback patterns for, 532–533
motivation in, 239–242 competition and, 222 friendship quality as influence on,
negative affect and, 243 conscientiousness and, 224–225 536–537
positive affect and, 240, 241–242 delay of gratification tasks and, future research applications for,
research on, 233–234 221–222 544–545
reward processing in, 240–241 habitual responses and, 222–223 individual differences in, 540–544
self-regulation and, 243 implementation intention and, 224 leadership styles for, 533, 538–539
skills adjustment in, 236–238 PDP for, 223 motivational climate for, 526,
stimulus-response rules and, 235–236 personality attributes and, 224 533–534
will and, 240 women. See also gender; gender negative evaluative behaviors,
urges, with ego depletion, 115 development, for girls; 539–540
objectification theory; sexual observational learning for, 537–538
V objectification, of women parental influence on, 528–532
values commitment motivations in, 395 peer influence on, 535–540
education as, 465 in media, sexual objectification of, 331 perceived competence and, 540–542
as internalized process, 374 self-objectification by, 327–331 pressure by parents for, 531–532
neural foundations for, 373–374 work-avoidance goals, 196 SDT and, 523–524
physical activity as, 531 work motivation, 562 self-esteem and, 540–542
in psychotherapeutic motivation, 442 aging of employees and, 513–514 self-perceptions and, 540–542
in social cognitive theory, 19–20 autonomy climates in, 95 social-contextual factors for, 528–540
vicarious learning, 15, 15–17 creativity within, 514–515 social goals for, 525
cognitive modeling for, 17 equity theory and, 507–508 social support for, 529, 536
disinhibition effects in, 16 expectancy theory and, 506–507 as value, 531
gender and, 17 extrinsic motivation and, 511
inhibition effects in, 16 generational differences in, 514 Z
modeling for, 16–17 goal setting theory and, 508–509 Zurich Model of Social Motivation, 400

index 579

Potrebbero piacerti anche