Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Early Predictors of Adolescent Violence

A B S T R A C T Phyllis L. Ellickson, PhD, and Kimberly A. McGuigan, PhD

Objectives. This study sought to During the last decade, violence has estimated that slightly more than one half had
identify early predictors of adolescent received increasing attention as a major pub- engaged in some form of relational or preda-
violence and to assess whether they lic health issue for Americans of all ages.1 Of tory violence in the previous year, and about
vary by sex and across different types particular concern is the degree to which vio- 20% had engaged in multiple and persistent
and levels of violence. lence affects the lives of youth, either as the violence.9
Methods. Data from a 5-year lon- perpetrators or as the victims of violence. The widespread nature of youth vio-
gitudinal self-report survey of more Between 1985 and 1990, arrests for murder, lence suggests that efforts to combat it need
than 4300 high school seniors and manslaughter, and aggravated assault rose by to be broad as well, to reach youth from dif-
dropouts from California and Oregon 60% for children younger than 18 years.2 ferent racial and social class groups in urban,
were used to regress measures of rela- Between 1985 and 1991, homicide arrest suburban, and rural communities. However,
tional, predatory, and overall violence rates actually declined among those older although violence prevention programs are
on predictors measured 5 years earlier. than 25 years, but they doubled among proliferating, few have been rigorously evalu-
younger males.3 ated, and even fewer have been shown to
Results. Deviant behavior in grade
These high rates of violence are mir- yield positive results.10 To improve our ability
7, poor grades, and weak bonds with
rored by high rates of youth victimization, to prevent or curb violence among youth, we
middle school predicted violent behav-
and violence and victimization tend to have need a better understanding of how it comes
ior 5 years later. Attending a middle
common antecedents.4 Moreover, despite the about, the factors that promote it, and the fac-
school with comparatively high levels fact that rates of violent crime have declined tors that inhibit it. Some of our understanding
of cigarette and marijuana use was also across all age groups since 1994, adolescents of the etiology of youth violence comes from
linked with subsequent violence. Early between the ages of 12 and 19 years remain studies of youth in the criminal justice sys-
drug use and peer drug use predicted at highest risk for victimization by violent tem,11,12 but these studies do not allow us to
increased levels of predatory violence crime.5 identify factors that discriminate between
but not its simple occurrence. Girls Among the general population of adoles- violent and nonviolent youth.
with low self-esteem during early ado- cents, as opposed to those apprehended by the In addition, although research on the
lescence were more likely to hit others criminal justice system, violent behavior is predictors of general delinquency yields
later on; boys who attended multiple also becoming increasingly common. Esti- potentially important clues about the causes
elementary schools were also more mates of self-reported assaults among 17-year- of violence, violent and nonviolent delin-
likely to engage in relational violence. olds who responded to the National High quents may differ from each other in signifi-
Conclusions. Violence prevention School Senior Study show that assault rates cant ways.13 Because very few studies distin-
programs for younger adolescents increased by at least 20% between 1975 and guish the two, we still do not know whether
should include efforts to prevent or 1985.6 Reports of violent victimization at the predictors of general delinquency and
reduce troublesome behavior in school school increased substantially between 1989 those of violent behavior are the same.14
and poor academic performance. Ado- and 1995.7 We also need better information on
lescent girls may also prof it from In 1997, results from the Youth Risk whether boys and girls are differentially vul-
efforts to raise self-esteem; adolescent Behavior Surveillance System Survey showed nerable to environmental and individual char-
boys may need extra training in resist- that 37% of students nationwide had engaged acteristics that might foster later violence.
ing influences that encourage deviant in a physical fight in the previous year, with Because boys are far more prone to both gen-
behavior. Programs aimed at preventing the prevalence rates for local school-based eral delinquency and violence,15–17 studies of
drug use may yield an added violence- surveys varying between 27% and nearly what leads to either behavior have often
reduction bonus. (Am J Public Health. 50%.8 Male students were nearly twice as focused solely on boys or failed to ask how
2000;90:566–572) likely as female students to have been in a
physical fight (46% vs 26%); African Ameri-
The authors are with RAND, Santa Monica, Calif.
cans and Hispanics had substantially higher Requests for reprints should be sent to Phyllis
rates than Whites (43% and 41% vs 34%).8 A L. Ellickson, PhD, RAND, PO Box 2138, Santa
study of high school seniors and dropouts Monica, CA 90407-2138.
from 8 California and Oregon communities This article was accepted November 5, 1999.

566 American Journal of Public Health April 2000, Vol. 90, No. 4
Predictors of Violence

the antecedents of delinquency or violence 12th grade (or of comparable age), 4390 stu- elementary schools attended taps disruption
may vary by sex.12,18 However, recent evi- dents (67%) completed surveys mailed to in the child’s relationship to school; it may
dence suggests that teenaged girls are as their homes. Of these students, 10.4% had also reflect the family’s residential or mari-
likely as teenaged boys to hit family members dropped out of high school. The sample was tal stability.29 We measured family bonds by
and that girls with violent or deviant behavior composed of 3128 White students (71%), whether the student was raised in a nuclear or
are differentially vulnerable to family prob- 345 African Americans (8%), 387 Hispanics disrupted family (with the latter defined as
lems and poor mental health.9,19 These find- (9%), 392 Asians (9%), and 133 Native one in which 1 or both natural parents were
ings suggest the need for further examination Americans or students who identified them- absent) and whether the student reported that
of sex differences in violence predictors. selves as having other racial/ethnic back- he or she discussed personal problems with
The f inding that boys and girls are grounds (3%). Nonresponse weights were his or her parents.
equally likely to be violent toward family developed to minimize attrition bias (as Other problem behaviors were repre-
members also suggests the need to ask described subsequently). sented by early deviance and substance use.
whether different types of violence have dif- Deviance at grade 7 was measured with a
ferent antecedents. Two dimensions of partic- Measures 4-item scale that includes stealing, skipping
ular interest are relational and predatory vio- school, cheating on tests, and being sent out
lence. The former refers to violence that For the dependent variables, we used of class (Cronbach α = 0.64); substance use
arises from interpersonal disputes between dichotomous and continuous measures of was an average of 3 items tapping the fre-
family members, friends, and acquaintances; 3 types of violence assessed at grade 12: rela- quency—from never to daily—of using alco-
the latter refers to violent behaviors, such as tional violence (persistent hitting), predatory hol, cigarettes, and marijuana (α = 0.71). We
mugging, robbery, and gang assaults, that are violence, and overall violence.9 These mea- measured exposure to deviant social influ-
“perpetrated intentionally to obtain some sures were derived from confirmatory factor ences (i.e., drug users) according to perceived
gain or as part of a pattern of criminal or anti- analysis, which showed evidence of an over- prevalence of friends and peers who use ciga-
social behavior.”10(p9) Among adolescents, all violence factor and 2 subfactors similar to rettes, alcohol, and marijuana (α = 0.81) and
relational violence is far more common than the constructs of relational and predatory reported number of offers of these substances
predatory violence.9,10 Whether it also has violence discussed by Tolan and Guerra.10 (α=0.77).
roots in different childhood experiences and Two items contributed to relational violence: Personality and attitude scales included
characteristics remains relatively unexplored. hitting or threatening to hit a family member self-esteem and rebelliousness (2 items
In this study we used data from a longi- or someone outside the family. Four items each). Although these items have low relia-
tudinal panel study of more than 4000 high contributed to predatory violence: past-year bility (α = 0.55 and 0.42, respectively), we
school seniors and dropouts to address the use of force or strong-arm methods to obtain included them because they represent theo-
following questions: What characteristics of money or things from people, involvement in retically important predictors. Sociodemo-
seventh graders and their environments dis- gang fights, attacking someone with the graphic characteristics included the child’s
criminate between those who will be violent intent of seriously hurting or killing them, reported age at baseline, sex, parental edu-
5 years later and those who will not be vio- and carrying a hidden weapon. All 6 items cation, and race/ethnicity (with separate
lent? Are the predictors of violence different contributed to overall violence. dummy variables for White, African Ameri-
for adolescent girls and boys? for relational Because the hitting items included can, Hispanic, Asian, Native American, or
vs predatory violence? for any violence vs threats, we required a response of 3 or more multiracial). Parental education (average of
amount of violence? times in the previous year to either item for a mother’s and father’s educational level) was a
positive code on the dichotomous measure of proxy for household socioeconomic level.
relational violence. Subjects who responded Contextual measures were ecologic and were
Methods positively to 1 or more of the relevant scale assessed at the school and neighborhood lev-
items were given a score of 1 on the dichoto- els: actual prevalence of drug use (cigarettes
Subjects mous variables for predatory and overall vio- and marijuana) among eighth graders in the
lence. We examined predictors of any violence subject’s middle school and socioeconomic
Subjects included in this study were par- for each category and, given the occurrence of status of the middle school’s catchment area,
ticipants in the RAND Adolescent Panel violence, the amount (or frequency) of each indicated by census data (see Table 1).
Study, a multiyear examination of substance type of violence in the previous year (ranging
use and related health-compromising behav- from 1 time to 20 or more times). Nonresponse Weights and Missing
iors. The study sample included students We chose predictor variables represent- Data Imputation
originally drawn from 30 California and Ore- ing 7 key domains identified as influential in
gon middle or junior high schools. Schools theories of adolescent development and One third of the baseline sample sub-
were selected to reflect a broad range of empirical research on delinquency: school jects did not return a survey at grade 12;
community environments, including urban, bonds, family bonds, other problem behav- nonrespondents were more likely than respon-
suburban, and rural school districts. Nine iors, exposure to deviant social influences, dents to be male, to have poor grades, to
schools had minority populations of 50% or personality and attitudes, school and neigh- have used marijuana or cigarettes by grade
greater, and 18 schools drew from areas with borhood context, and sociodemographic 7, and to be African American or Hispanic.
household incomes below the state median. characteristics. 20–27 Each predictor was To correct for this attrition bias, we created
In this study we used data from the first assessed at grade 7, 5 years before comple- nonresponse weights derived from a logistic
and seventh data collection waves, conducted tion of the outcome measures (Table 1). regression model that regressed grade 12
in 1985 and 1990. At baseline, when the stu- We treated poor school performance survey return on multiple baseline character-
dents were in the 7th grade, 6527 subjects (represented by self-reported grades) as an istics (sex, race/ethnicity, disrupted family
completed surveys administered in school; indicator of weak commitment or bonding structure, academic ability, deviance, and
5 years later, when the students were in the to the academic environment.28 Number of drug-related beliefs). The resulting weights

April 2000, Vol. 90, No. 4 American Journal of Public Health 567
Ellickson and McGuigan

TABLE 1—Means and Zero-Order Correlations of Predictor Variables With Violence at 18 Years of Age: California
and Oregon Adolescents 1985 and 1990

Zero-Order Correlation

Any Relational Any Predatory


Mean (SD) No. of Items Any Violence Violence Violence

Dependent variable (grade 12)


Any violence 0.50 (0.50) 6 ... ... ...
Any relational violence (persistent hitting) 0.21 (0.41) 2 ... ... ...
Any predatory violence 0.20 (0.40) 4 ... ... ...
Amount (ln) of overall violencea 0.87 (0.82) 6 ... ... ...
Amount (ln) of relational violencea 0.63 (0.66) 2 ... ... ...
Amount (ln) of predatory violencea 0.75 (0.79) 4 ... ... ...
Predictor variable (grade 7)
School bonds
Poor grades (A = 1, F = 5) 2.00 (0.80) 1 0.19 0.15 0.21
No. of elementary schools attended 2.10 (1.16) 1 0.07 0.08 0.07
Family bonds
Nuclear family 0.64 (0.48) 1 −0.09 −0.07 −0.08
Talks to parents 0.63 (0.48) 1 −0.06 −0.03 −0.07
Problem behavior
Deviance 0.47 (0.58) 4 0.20 0.17 0.23
Drug use frequencyb 0.00 (0.80) 3 0.13 0.12 0.17
Social influences
Perceived peer drug useb −0.10 (0.88) 6 0.09 0.09 0.09
Drug offers 0.98 (1.11) 3 0.16 0.13 0.17
Personality and attitudes
Self-esteem (low) 2.20 (0.90) 2 0.09 0.07 0.08
Rebelliousnessb −0.04 (0.79) 2 0.08 0.09 0.08
Sociodemographics
Age at baseline 12.70 (0.54) 1 0.04 0.02c 0.09
Sex (female) 0.54 (.50) 1 −0.22 −0.14 −0.25
Race
White 0.71 (0.45) 1 −0.05 −0.04 −0.09
Black 0.08 (0.27) 1 0.06 0.05 0.07
Hispanic 0.09 (0.28) 1 0.02c 0.03c 0.06
Asian 0.09 (0.29) 1 −0.02c −0.01c 0.00c
Native American 0.02 (0.13) 1 0.01c −0.01 0.01c
Multiracial 0.01 (0.12) 1 0.02c 0.01c 0.01c
Parent education 2.00 (1.01) 4 −0.06 −0.04 −0.07
Contextual
Neighborhood socioeconomic statusd 0.34 (0.83) 3 −0.09 −0.06 −0.09
School drug use prevalencee 122.20 (32.17) 2 0.08 0.06 0.08

Note. Unless otherwise indicated, correlations are statistically significant at P < .05. ln = natural logarithm.
a
Scale derived from confirmatory factor analysis.
b
Scale created as the average of individual items that have been standardized with mean = 0 and standard deviation = 1.
c
Not statistically significant.
d
Weighted sum of standardized median family income, average education of adults, and percentage of families with both parents present in
census tract of subject’s middle school.
e
Sum of percentage of school’s eighth graders who indicated that they had ever used cigarettes or marijuana and percentage who had used
each substance in the previous month.

were the reciprocals of the predicted proba- missing at random (once the predictors in the these cases would have resulted in a loss of
bilities of returning a survey at grade 12.30 model are controlled), the weights cannot cor- 15% of the subjects. To reduce subject loss
Note that weighting produces larger standard rect for any additional nonresponse bias that is due to item nonresponse, therefore, we
errors of estimates and, thus, more conserva- not associated with the model’s baseline pre- replaced missing values with regression
tive hypothesis tests. dictors.32 The weights allowed us to general- imputation, using the other nonmissing pre-
The weights removed 90% or more of ize to the baseline participants rather than to dictors to estimate these values.
the bias discussed earlier.31 For example, the the entire 7th-grade cohort of each school.
weighted 12th-grade sample yielded nearly Nevertheless, nonresponse at baseline had lit- Model Development
the same estimate (49.7%) of baseline ciga- tle effect on sample characteristics, because
rette use as did the original group of 7th-grade baseline respondents closely resembled the We used a 2-part model to analyze the
respondents (50.2%), whereas the unweighted entire cohort.33 prospective determinants of violence.34 This
estimates (44.8%) understated the actual Although the percentages of missing approach is appropriate when (1) the out-
prevalence of cigarette use. However, because cases for any single predictor ranged from come variable’s distribution has a large pro-
this strategy assumes that nonrespondents are 1% to 6% of the sample, listwise deletion of portion of zeros and a positive skew and (2)

568 American Journal of Public Health April 2000, Vol. 90, No. 4
Predictors of Violence

the determinants of “any” occurrence of the the continuous measures. To guard against from each domain were related to both gen-
outcome may differ from the determinants of type I errors (findings of significance due eral and more specific measures of violence.
the level of the same outcome. For example, to chance), we then cross validated the mod- All of the predictors were signif icantly
boys and girls may be equally likely to els on the remaining 50% of the sample. The related to the occurrence of any violence
engage in relational violence, but boys may decision rules used for variable inclusion except those tapping membership in 4 racial/
do it more frequently than girls. The 2-part with cross validation ensured that our type I ethnic groups: Asian, Hispanic, Native Amer-
model would indicate that the data are consis- error rate would be less than 0.0075 for a ican, and multiracial. The results were similar
tent with this expectation if the predictor single hypothesis test. To account for both for relational and predatory violence, with
“gender” was not statistically significant in clustering of students within schools and the the following exceptions: age was not a sig-
the logistic model (the first part) but was sig- use of nonresponse weights, we adjusted the nificant predictor of persistent hitting, while
nif icant in the subsequent least squares standard errors for all estimates with the being Hispanic was a significant predictor of
regression (the second part). We used logistic Huber correction in the Stata 4.0 software predatory violence.
regression to model whether any violence package (Stata Corp, College Station, Tex).
had occurred and least squares regression to Multivariate Predictors of Violence
model amount of violence, conditional on
any violence having occurred. Results When we controlled for all of the vari-
We used explanatory variables mea- ables simultaneously, fewer predictors re-
sured at grade 7 to predict each of the 3 types Bivariate Findings mained significant. Table 2 shows the logistic
of violence at grade 12: overall, relational, regression results for the dichotomous mea-
and predatory. We developed the models Table 1 shows the zero-order correla- sures of violence and the ordinary least
with a randomly selected sample of 50% of tions between the dichotomous measures of squares regression results for the continuous
the observations, using stepwise logistic violence (any overall violence, relational vio- measures. Three characteristics measured
regression for the “any violence” measures lence, and predatory violence) and the hypoth- during grade 7 consistently foretold the oc-
and stepwise least squares regression for esized predictors. As we predicted, variables currence of violence (overall, relational, and

TABLE 2—Grade 7 Predictors of Violence at Age 18 Years (Multivariate Models): California and Oregon Adolescents,
1985 and 1990
Presence of Violence at 18 Years of Age (Logistic Regression) Amount of Violence at 18 Years of Age (OLS Regression)

Any Violence Any Relational Any Predatory Overall Violence (ln) Relational Predatory
(n = 4380; Violence (n = 4326; Violence (n = 4390, (n = 2161a; Violence (ln) Violence (ln)
Pseudo-R 2 = 0.082) Pseudo-R = 0.060) Pseudo-R 2 = 0.125)
2
R 2 = 0.057) (n = 903 ; R = 0.005) (n = 743a; R 2 = 0.080)
a 2

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) β (SE) β (SE) β (SE)

School bonds
Poor grades 1.34 (1.26, 1.43) 1.32 (1.16, 1.52) 1.49 (1.34, 1.65) 0.09 (0.021)**** NS NS
No. of elementary schools 1.12 (1.06, 1.17) 1.14 (1.06, 1.23) NS 0.04 (0.022*) NS NS
attended
Family bonds
Nuclear family NS NS NS NS NS 0.12 (0.069)**
Talks to parents NS NS NS NS NS NS
Problem behavior
Deviance 1.62 (1.37, 1.90) 1.46 (1.32, 1.63) 1.64 (1.46, 1.84) 0.13 (0.030)**** NS NS
Drug use frequency NS NS NS NS NS 0.11 (0.028)****
Social influences
Perceived peer drug use NS NS NS NS NS 0.07 (0.026)***
Drug offers NS NS NS NS NS NS
Personality and attitudes
Self-esteem (low) NS 1.13 (1.03, 1.23) 1.14 (1.03, 1.26) NS NS NS
Rebelliousness NS NS NS NS NS 0.07 (0.029)***
Sociodemographics
Age at baseline NS 0.85 (0.74, 0.96) NS NS NS NS
Sex (female) 0.43 (0.38, 0.49) 0.53 (0.46, 0.62) 0.29 (0.24, 0.36) −0.23 (0.034)**** NS −0.38 (0.062)****
Race
White NS NS 0.68 (0.57, 0.80) NS NS NS
Black NS NS NS NS NS NS
Hispanic NS NS NS NS NS NS
Asian NS NS NS NS NS NS
Native American NS NS NS NS NS NS
Multiracial NS NS NS NS NS 0.44 (0.101)****
Parent education NS NS NS NS NS NS
Contextual
Neighborhood socio- NS NS NS NS NS NS
economic status
School drug use 1.004 (1.002, 1.007) 1.004 (1.001, 1.007) NS NS 0.0015 (0.00051)*** NS
prevalence

Note. OLS = ordinary least squares; OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval; ln = natural logarithm.
a
Subjects with a value of 1 in the logistic regression model and no missing data.
*P < .10; **P < .05; ***P < .01; ****P < .001.

April 2000, Vol. 90, No. 4 American Journal of Public Health 569
Ellickson and McGuigan

predatory) by the end of the high school less than 6% of the variance for overall vio- doing poorly in middle school, and low self-
years: doing poorly in school, early deviant lence, less than 1% for relational violence, esteem; both are inhibited by being female.
behavior, and being male. Middle school and 8% for predatory violence. Only 1 vari- Unique predictors of relational violence
context and high mobility during elementary able, actual prevalence of drug use in the ado- included having attended 2 or more elemen-
school also predicted later violence; adoles- lescent’s middle school, predicted the amount tary schools and attending a middle school
cents who went to middle schools with rela- of relational violence 5 years later. Four vari- with comparatively high levels of drug use
tively high levels of drug use among the stu- ables—poor grades, high elementary school among its enrollees. Unique predictors of
dent population and those who had shifted mobility, early deviance, and sex—predicted amount of predatory violence included early
from one elementary school to another were amount of overall violence. drug use and high perceived levels of drug
more likely to engage in both overall and For predatory violence, there were 6 pre- use among one’s middle school friends and
relational violence. Low self-esteem pre- dictors: frequency of using alcohol, ciga- peers.
dicted both relational and predatory violence, rettes, and marijuana during grade 7; higher
but rebelliousness dropped out as a predictor. levels of perceived drug use by one’s middle Multivariate Predictors by Sex
In contrast to the bivariate results, greater school peers; being male; being multiracial;
maturity (as measured by being compara- coming from a nuclear family; and rebel- Table 3 shows the degree to which sex
tively older for one’s grade level) acted as a liousness. However, the last 2 variables had differences in predictors of violence emerge
damper for relational violence once we con- an impact that was contrary to our predic- during early adolescence. Variables with simi-
trolled for other predictors. tions, with adolescents from nuclear families lar effects on whether seventh-grade boys and
more likely to be frequent perpetrators of girls exhibited violence in the future included
Multivariate Predictors of Amount predatory violence and rebellious youth less engaging in deviant behavior as younger ado-
of Violence likely to be frequent perpetrators of predatory lescents and attending middle schools with
violence. comparatively high levels of drug use. Having
Our ability to predict the amount of vio- As Table 2 shows, different types of poor grades in middle school was also impor-
lence exhibited by an adolescent, given that violence have both common and unique tant for both sexes, but it increased the odds for
he or she had engaged in some violence, was antecedents. Both relational and predatory different types of violence: relational violence
limited (see Table 2). The models explained violence are fostered by early deviance, for girls and predatory violence for boys.

TABLE 3—Grade 7 Predictors of Any Violence at 18 Years of Age, by Sex (Multivariate Logit Models): California and Oregon
Adolescents, 1985 and 1990

Female Subjects Male Subjects

Any Violence Any Relational Any Predatory Any Violence Any Relational Any Predatory
(n = 2350, Violence (n = 2322, Violence(n = 2354, (n = 2030, Violence (n = 2004, Violence (n = 2036,
Pseudo-R = >0.038) Pseudo-R = >0.054) Pseudo-R 2 = 0.058)
2 2
Pseudo-R 2 = >0.041) Pseudo-R = 0.04) Pseudo-R 2= >0.065)
2

OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI)

School bonds
Poor grades 1.37 (1.23, 1.52) 1.44 (1.19, 1.73) NS NS NS 1.53 (1.35, 1.73)
No. of elementary schools NS NS NS 1.20 (1.10, 1.32) 1.15 (1.04, 1.27) NS
attended
Family bonds
Nuclear family NS NS NS NS NS NS
Talks to parents NS NS NS NS NS NS
Problem behavior
Deviance 1.60 (1.32, 1.94) 1.41 (1.16, 1.73) 1.78 (1.40, 2.26) 1.84 (1.46, 2.33) 1.31 (1.10, 1.56) 1.72 (1.45, 2.02)
Drug use frequency NS NS NS NS NS NS
Social influences
Perceived peer drug use NS NS NS NS NS NS
Drug offers NS NS NS NS 1.29 (1.15, 1.44) NS
Personality and attitudes
Self-esteem (low) NS 1.21 (1.05, 1.38) NS NS NS NS
Rebelliousness NS NS NS NS NS NS
Sociodemographics
Age at baseline NS 0.67 (0.53, 0.83) NS NS NS NS
Sex (female) NS NS NS NS NS NS
Race
White NS 0.67 (0.49, 0.91) 0.57 (0.43, 0.76) NS NS 0.77 (0.62, 0.94)
Black NS NS NS NS NS NS
Hispanic NS NS NS NS NS NS
Asian NS NS NS NS NS NS
Native American NS NS NS NS NS NS
Multiracial NS NS NS NS NS NS
Parent education NS NS NS NS NS NS
Contextual
Neighborhood socio- NS 0.79 (0.69, 0.91) 0.70 (0.61, 0.80) NS NS NS
economic status
School drug use prevalence 1.005 (1.002, 1.008) NS NS 1.005 (1.003, 1.007) NS NS

Note. OR = odds ratio; CI = confidence interval.

570 American Journal of Public Health April 2000, Vol. 90, No. 4
Predictors of Violence

Other variables acted as risk factors for and relational violence as older teenagers. Because early deviance and poor grades
girls but not for boys, and vice versa. Adoles- Similarly, “bad” school environments— provide useful warning signals of later vio-
cent girls were more vulnerable to having low specifically, those middle schools character- lence, these results suggest that violence pre-
self-esteem as seventh graders and living in ized by comparatively high levels of cigarette vention programs aimed at younger adoles-
neighborhoods of low socioeconomic status: and marijuana use—also promoted subse- cents should include efforts to prevent or
the former predicted relational violence for quent violence. Such school environments reduce troublesome behavior in school and
girls but not for boys; the latter predicted provided an added stimulus to violent behav- poor academic performance. Such efforts
relational and predatory violence for girls ior even after we accounted for individual per- should begin in elementary school, reflect-
alone. Adolescent boys, on the other hand, ceptions and behavior. ing the fact that the deviant behavior and
were more susceptible to the effects of high Background characteristics that pre- poor academic orientation that we measured
mobility during elementary school and being dicted subsequent violence included 2 pro- had started at least as early as grade 6 and
offered drugs during the seventh grade, both tective factors, being White and being fe- probably earlier. The fact that greater matu-
of which predicted future relational violence male. Being White lowered the probability of rity acted as a damper on later violence,
for male students but not for female students. engaging in predatory (but not relational) once we controlled for early deviance and
Being White lowered the odds of predatory violence. However, identification with other other factors, also supports early prevention
violence for both sexes but diminished the racial/ethnic groups had no impact on vio- and intervention.
likelihood of relational violence for girls lence once we controlled for early behavioral Little in this analysis argues for differ-
alone. problems and environmental influences. ential violence prevention efforts by race/
When we considered the amount of vio- Being female lowered the probability of ethnicity or social class. Most of the bivari-
lence in which these adolescents engaged engaging in both relational and predatory ate relationships between racial group and
(data not shown), the most telling sex differ- violence and, if engaged in, the amount of violent behavior disappeared when we con-
ence involved the number of predictors: 6 for predatory (but not relational) violence. Thus, trolled for behavioral and environmental fac-
boys and 2 for girls. These differences reflect girls who stepped over the threshold and tors, as did the links between parental socio-
the fact that both the frequency and the range engaged in relational violence (hitting others economic status and violence. However, the
of scores for amount of violence were lower at least 3 times or more in the previous year) differential impact of certain predictors by
for girls than for boys. Nevertheless, sex were as likely as their male counterparts to sex suggests that violence prevention efforts
similarities and differences that were consis- engage in such violence frequently. Being should be sensitive to the special needs of
tent with the results for any violence also comparatively older for one’s grade emerged both sexes, particularly the higher-risk pro-
emerged. Doing poorly in school raised the as a damper on subsequent relational vio- files of girls with low self-esteem and of
frequency of relational violence for girls and lence when we controlled for behavioral and boys who have experienced substantial dis-
of overall violence for both girls and boys, environmental predictors. continuity in their early school environment.
while being White acted as a protective factor Boys and girls were differentially suscep- In addition, the link between exposure to
against increased relational violence only for tible to certain individual and environmental pro-drug environments and subsequent vio-
girls. Boys, but not girls, engaged in more characteristics. Girls who exhibited low self- lence for boys suggests that they may profit
frequent violence if they had attended multi- esteem as early as grade 7 were more likely to from extra training in how to resist social
ple schools during their elementary school engage in relational violence 5 years later; pressures that encourage deviant behavior.
years and had been exposed to deviant social those who attended schools in neighborhoods The results of this study also suggest
influences in middle school (offers of drugs of low socioeconomic status were more likely that programs aimed at preventing drug use
and perceived exposure to peers who use to engage in both relational and predatory vio- may yield an added violence-reduction
them). The sex analyses also showed that the lence. Neither of these variables was signifi- bonus. Because middle schools with high
counterintuitive effects noted earlier—those cant for boys. Boys, on the other hand, were rates of drug use foster later violence, reduc-
for family disruption and rebelliousness— particularly vulnerable to repeated moves that ing overall levels of drug use in the middle
occurred only for boys. involved attending different elementary school population might limit the subsequent
schools and to exposure to pro-drug social contextual impact of “bad” school environ-
influences (drug offers). The first raised the ments. Because exposure to drug offers
Discussion odds of persistently hitting other people along increases the likelihood of more frequent
with the amount of hitting that occurred; the relational or predatory violence, helping mid-
Analyses of predictors of violence second increased the likelihood of engaging in dle school children—particularly boys—
among high school seniors and dropouts show relational violence and the amount of both learn how to resist such offers might have the
that early deviant behavior, poor grades, weak relational and predatory violence. added benefit of reducing levels of violence
elementary school bonds, and pro-drug mid- Two other drug-related variables, early several years later. Future research is needed
dle school environments fostered violent be- use of drugs and perceived prevalence of to determine whether drug prevention pro-
havior several years later. Adolescents who drug use among one’s middle school peers, grams actually yield these added benefits.
acted out by stealing or getting in trouble at affected the amount of violence in which In terms of study limitations, we ob-
school in grade 7 were significantly more teenagers subsequently engaged but did not served a good deal of unexplained variation
likely to be violent 5 years later than those predict its simple occurrence. The greater the for each of the models, which may be partly
who did not. Coupled with poor grades, these frequency of one’s own drug use during mid- attributable to the nature of the dependent
deviant behaviors predicted relational and dle school and the higher the perceived level and independent variables, the types of mod-
predatory violence, as well as overall vio- of drug use among one’s peers, the greater the els used, and the magnitude of elapsed time
lence. In addition, adolescents who attended likelihood of frequent predatory violence. As between measuring predictors and outcomes.
several elementary schools, and who thus with drug offers, the social influence variable In the part 1 (logistic regression) models, the
may not have developed strong bonds at any (perceived drug use among one’s peers) was outcome variables were dichotomous and
of them, were more likely to engage in overall significant for boys alone. had a restricted range of variation that could

April 2000, Vol. 90, No. 4 American Journal of Public Health 571
Ellickson and McGuigan

be explained; hence, the pseudo-R 2 measure petration and victimization. J Adolesc Health. analysis of age- and gender-differentiated effects.
would typically be smaller in logistic regres- 1997;21:291–302. J Quant Criminology. 1995;11(2):167–193.
sion than in linear regression. Because they 5. National Crime Victimization Survey: Changes 20. Huizinga D, Esbensen F-A, Weiher AW. Are
in Criminal Victimization, 1994–95. Washington, there multiple paths to delinquency? Denver
were limited to explaining amount of vio-
DC: US Dept of Justice; 1997. Youth Survey. J Criminal Law Criminology.
lence given that some violence had already 6. Osgood DW, O’Malley PM, Bachman JG, John- 1991;82(1):83–105.
occurred, the part 2 (linear regression) mod- ston LD. Time trends and age trends in arrests 21. Hawkins J, Weis J. The social development
els also had a restricted range of variation to and self-reported illegal behavior. Criminology. model: an integrated approach to delinquency
be explained. In addition, we would expect a 1989;27:389–417. prevention. J Primary Prev. 1985;6:73–97.
considerable amount of unexplained varia- 7. Chandler KA, Chapman CD, Rand MR, Taylor 22. Bandura A. Social Learning Theory. Englewood
BM. Students’ Reports of School Crime: 1989 Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1977.
tion in behavior simply because we predicted
and 1995. Washington, DC: US Dept of Educa- 23. Jessor R, Jessor S. Problem Behavior and Psy-
violent behavior over a long time horizon tion; 1998. NCES publication 98-241/NCJ- chosocial Development. New York, NY: Acade-
(5 years). 169607. mic Press Inc; 1977.
Nevertheless, better measures of the 8. Kann L, Kinchen SA, Williams BI, et al. Youth 24. Kaplan H. Toward a general theory of drug
theoretical constructs might also have im- risk behavior surveillance—United States, 1997. abuse and other deviant adaptations. J Drug
proved the models’ predictive power. For MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 1998;47(SS-3): Issues. 1985;15(4):477–492.
example, we lacked measures of family vio- 1–89. 25. Elliott DS, Huizinga D, Ageton SS. Explaining
9. Ellickson PL, Saner H, McGuigan KA. Profiles Delinquency and Drug Use. Beverly Hills,
lence and parental supervision, both of which of violent youth, substance use, and other con-
have been linked with later violence among Calif: Sage Publications; 1985.
current problems. Am J Public Health. 1996;86: 26. White HR, Pandina RJ, LaGrange RL. Longitu-
children.35,36 In addition, low reliabilities for 985–991. dinal predictors of serious substance use and
the rebelliousness and self-esteem scales could 10. Tolan P, Guerra N. What Works in Reducing Ado- delinquency. Criminology. 1987;25(3):715–740.
account for the unanticipated negative rela- lescent Violence: An Empirical Review of the 27. Shepherd JP, Farrington DP. Preventing crime
tionship between violence and rebelliousness Field. Chicago, Ill: Center for the Study and Pre- and violence. BMJ. 1995;310:271–272.
vention of Violence; 1994.
and for our failure to find consistent associa- 28. Saner H, Ellickson PL. Concurrent risk factors
11. Chaiken JM, Chaiken MR. Drugs and predatory
tions between self-esteem and later violence for adolescent violence. J Adolesc Health. 1996;
crime. In: Tonry M, Wilson JQ, eds. Drugs and
for both sexes. Future research would also 19:94–103.
Crime. Chicago, Ill: University of Chicago Press;
29. Stern D, Catterall J, Alhadeff C, Ash M. Reduc-
benefit from improved operationalization of 1990:203–239.
ing the High School Dropout Rate in Califor-
the underlying theoretical constructs. 12. Fagan J, Hansen KV, Jang M. Profiles of chroni-
nia: Why We Should and How We May. Berke-
cally violent juvenile offenders: an empirical test
ley, Calif: Institute of Governmental Studies,
of an integrated theory of violent delinquency.
University of California; 1986.
Contributors In: Kluegel JR, ed. Evaluating Juvenile Justice.
30. Cassel CM, Sarndal CE, Wretman JH. Some
Beverly Hills, Calif: Sage Publications; 1983:
Both authors were involved in the design and analy- uses of statistical models in connection with
91–119.
sis. P. L. Ellickson wrote the drafts; K. A. McGuigan nonresponse problems. In: Madow WG, Olkin I,
13. Tolan PH, Loeber RL. Antisocial behavior. In:
offered revisions. eds. Incomplete Data in Sample Surveys, Vol-
Tolan PH, Cohler BJ, eds. Handbook of Clinical
Research and Practice With Adolescents. New ume III: Symposium on Incomplete Data, Pro-
York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1993: ceedings. New York, NY: Academic Press Inc;
Acknowledgments 307–331. 1983:143–170.
This research was funded by a grant from the Cali- 14. Henggelar SW. Delinquency in Adolescence. 31. Ellickson PL, McGuigan KA, Adams V, Bell
fornia Wellness Foundation. New York, NY: Sage Publications; 1989. RM, Hays RD. Teenagers and alcohol misuse in
We are indebted to Bob Bell and Alaida Rod- 15. Elliott DS. Youth Violence: An Overview. Boul- the United States: by any definition, it’s a big
riguez for their assistance. der, Colo: Center for the Study and Prevention problem. Addiction. 1996;19:1489–1503.
of Violence; 1994. 32. Little RJA, Rubin DB. Statistical Analysis With
16. Elliott DS, Huizinga D, Morse B. Self-reported Missing Data. New York, NY: John Wiley &
References violent offending: a descriptive analysis of juve- Sons Inc; 1987.
1. Rosenberg ML, O’Carroll PW, Powell KE. Let’s nile violent offenders and their offending careers. 33. Bell RM, Gareleck C, Ellickson PL. Baseline
be clear; violence is a public health problem. J Interpersonal Violence. 1986;4:472–514. Nonresponse in Project ALERT: Does It Matter?
JAMA. 1992;267:3071–3072. 17. Orpinas PK, Basen-Engquist K, Grunbaum JA, Santa Monica, Calif: RAND Corp; 1990.
2. Yoshikawa H. Prevention as cumulative protec- Parcel GS. The co-morbidity of violence- 34. Duan N, Manning WG Jr, Morris CN, New-
tion: effects of early family support and edu- related behaviors with health-risk behaviors in a house JP. A Comparison of Alternative Models
cation on chronic delinquency and its risks. population of high school students. J Adolesc for the Demand for Medical Care. Washington,
Psychol Bull. 1994;115:28–54. Health.1995;16:216–225. DC: US Dept of Health and Human Services;
3. Maguire K, Pastore AL, Flanagan TJ. Source- 18. Salts CJ, Lindholm BW, Goddard HW, Duncan 1982.
book of Criminal Justice Statistics—1992. S. Predictive variables of violent behavior in 35. Widom CS. The Cycle of Violence. Washington,
Washington, DC: US Dept of Justice, Bureau of adolescent males. Youth Soc. 1995;26:377–399. DC: US Dept of Justice; 1992.
Justice Statistics; 1993. 19. Johnson RA, Su SS, Gernstein DR, Shin HC, 36. Widom CS. Does violence beget violence? A
4. Malik S, Sorenson SB, Aneshensel CS. Commu- Hoffmann JP. Parental influence on deviant critical examination of the literature. Psychol
nity and dating violence among adolescents: per- behavior in early adolescence: a logistic response Bull. 1989;21:129–135.

572 American Journal of Public Health April 2000, Vol. 90, No. 4

Potrebbero piacerti anche