Sei sulla pagina 1di 13

Developmental Psychology © 2009 American Psychological Association

2009, Vol. 45, No. 6, 1761–1773 0012-1649/09/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/a0016426

A Process Analysis of the Transmission of Distress from Interparental


Conflict to Parenting: Adult Relationship Security as
an Explanatory Mechanism

Patrick T. Davies, Melissa L. Sturge-Apple, E. Mark Cummings


and Meredith J. Woitach University of Notre Dame
University of Rochester

Toward advancing conceptualizations of the spillover hypothesis, this study examined the conditions and
mechanisms underlying the transmission of distress from the interparental relationship to parenting
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

difficulties over a 2-year period in a sample of 233 mothers (M ⫽ 35.0 years) and fathers (M ⫽ 36.8
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

years) of kindergarten children. Findings from autoregressive structural equation models indicated that
parents’ gender moderated associations between interparental conflict and parental psychological control
and insensitivity to children’s negative affect. Pathways between interparental conflict and parenting
difficulties over the 2-year period were significant for fathers but not mothers. Analysis of insecurity and
depressive symptoms as affective mechanisms of spillover revealed that adult relationship insecurity was
a significant mediator in the pathways between interparental conflict and parenting difficulties experi-
enced by fathers.

Keywords: interparental conflict, parenting, adult depression, emotion regulation, relationship insecurity

As the relationship hub of the family system, the quality of the yield any enhancement in interparental relationship quality (P. A.
interparental relationship has significant implications for how Cowan & Cowan, 2002). Taken together, the various findings
adults approach the developmental challenge of raising children. provide strong support for the thesis that interparental conflict
For example, novel use of experimental designs has shown that progressively undermines child-rearing practices (C. P. Cowan,
mothers randomly assigned to participate in a conflictual interac- Cowan, Pruett, & Pruett, 2007; Mikulincer & Goodman, 2006).
tion with their partners were less attentive to their sons in an However, the question remains as to why interparental conflict
ensuing parent– child interaction task than mothers who partici- compromises parenting. The objective of this study is to examine
pated in a nonconflictual interaction (Jouriles & Farris, 1992). interparental relationship security as an explanatory mechanism in
Longitudinal research has shown that observations of destructive the link between interparental conflict and child-rearing practices.
interparental conflict predicted subsequent decreases in parents’ As the prevailing model guiding research on the interplay be-
emotional availability 1 year later (Sturge-Apple, Davies, & Cum- tween interparental conflict and parenting processes, the spillover
mings, 2006). Lending further support to the impact of marital hypothesis proposes that “the emotions, affect, and mood gener-
conflict on parenting, parent participation in interventions that are ated in the marital realm transfers to the parent– child relationship”
specifically designed to improve the marriage has been shown to (Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000, p. 26). Research documenting
promote more effective parenting practices. In contrast, participa- concurrent and longitudinal associations between marital and
tion in another program designed to improve parenting did not parent– child difficulties is commonly cited as supporting the te-
nets of the spillover hypothesis (e.g., Engfer, 1988; Erel & Bur-
man, 1995; Gerard, Krishnakumar, & Buehler, 2006; Krishnaku-
mar & Buehler, 2000; Lindsey, MacKinnon-Lewis, Campbell,
Patrick T. Davies and Meredith J. Woitach, Department of Clinical and
Social Sciences in Psychology, University of Rochester; Melissa L. Sturge-
Frabutt, & Lamb, 2002; Sturge-Apple et al., 2006). However, these
Apple, Mt. Hope Family Center, University of Rochester; and E. Mark studies have not tested the core tenet that affective mechanisms
Cummings, Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame. resulting from marital conflict actually predict subsequent impair-
This research was supported by a grant from the National Institute of ments in parenting. Moreover, this general assumption provides
Mental Health (Project R01 MH57318) awarded to Patrick T. Davies and little guidance in identifying exactly how and why anger and
E. Mark Cummings. We are grateful to the children, parents, teachers, and distress in the interparental relationship is transmitted to the
school administrators who participated in this project. We also express our parent– child subsystem. As a first step toward advancing a
gratitude to project staff, including: Courtney Forbes, Courtney Henry, process-oriented model of spillover, we examine the relative via-
Marcie Goeke-Morey, Amy Keller, Michelle Sutton, Alice Schermerhorn,
bility of two potential affective processes as explanatory mecha-
and the graduate and undergraduate students at the University of Rochester
and University of Notre Dame.
nisms in pathways between interparental conflict and parenting
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Patrick processes over the span of 2 years: adult negative affect and adult
T. Davies, Department of Clinical and Social Sciences in Psychology, insecure representations of their intimate relationship.
University of Rochester, Rochester, NY 14627. E-mail: patrick.davies@ The central role of mood difficulties as a mediator of associa-
rochester.edu tions between interparental conflict and parenting problems is

1761
1762 DAVIES, STURGE-APPLE, WOITACH, AND CUMMINGS

evident in many conventional formulations of the spillover hypoth- partners is intimately linked with the ability of the partners to use
esis. For example, the conceptualization of spillover by Krishna- each other as sources of support in the family, particularly when
kumar and Buehler (2000) underscores the role of “emotions, they occur during stressful or conflictual times (P. A. Cowan,
affect, and mood” (p. 26) as operative mechanisms. Similarly, Erel Cohn, Cowan, & Pearson, 1996; Crowell et al., 2002; Mikulincer,
and Burman (1995) define spillover as the process in which a Florian, Cowan, & Cowan, 2002; Treboux, Crowell, & Waters,
“conflictual marital relationship may cause parents to be irritable 2004). Expressions of hostility and disengagement during conflicts
and emotionally drained and therefore less attentive and sensitive may specifically serve as signals that the partner in the relationship
to their children” (p. 109; also see Easterbrooks & Emde, 1988). cannot reliably serve as a secure base and, in some relationships,
Drawing from these conceptualizations, a plausible thesis is that may even pose a threat to the welfare of the adult. Moreover, as
protracted experiences with conflict in the interparental relation- relatively stable relational processes, underlying adult worries
ship may increase the likelihood of subsequent parenting problems about the security of their relationship with their spouse may
by progressively amplifying parental depressive symptoms char- progressively undermine abilities to serve as an effective and
acterized by negative mood, anhedonia, lethargy, and helplessness. responsive caregiver. In support of this hypothesis, adult endorse-
Negative affect may further undermine parenting through the am- ment of insecurity in romantic relationships during pregnancy was
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

plification of negative appraisals and attributions of child behavior, associated with less desire to have children and greater parenting
constriction of attention resources devoted to child-rearing activ- stress when the children were 6 months of age. Moreover, under-
ities, disruption of problem-solving abilities, and facilitation of scoring the potential robustness of the adult attachment as a
reflexive, dysregulated response patterns (Dix, 1991). predictor, these associations remained even after taking into ac-
By the same token, it is questionable whether relatively short- count potential third variables, including the participants’ and their
term feelings or displays of negative affect are primary processes partners’ depressive symptoms and marital dissatisfaction (Rholes,
underlying the occurrence of spillover over relatively long devel- Simpson, & Friedman, 2006). In light of this support for attach-
opmental lags of months or years. Although many seminal ac- ment as a carrier of spillover, the goal of this study was to examine
counts of the spillover hypothesis conceptualized negative affect the relative roles of adult attachment insecurity and depressive
as a temporary, dynamic state stemming from conflict between symptoms as intermediary mechanisms in pathways between in-
parents (e.g., Almeida, Wethington, & Chandler, 1999; Chris- terparental conflict and parenting difficulties.
tensen & Margolin, 1988; Jouriles & Farris, 1992; Mahoney, To maximize the power to identify spillover mechanisms, we
Boggio, & Jouriles, 1996), spillover from the interparental to focused on examining two affective dimensions of parenting dur-
parent– child relationship in this sense may only be particularly ing the early elementary school years. Focus on the developmental
pronounced within narrow developmental windows of minutes, period of middle childhood is supported by the results of a quan-
hours, or days. Alternatively, the notion of spillover may be titative review of the literature indicating that the strength of
viewed as a shorthand or even a valuable metaphor for the active associations between interparental conflict and parenting difficul-
processes, but it remains that the active processes must be identi- ties is accentuated during middle childhood (Krishnakumar &
fied. That is, it is unlikely that relatively brief, transient, changes Buehler, 2000). In addition, if adult attachment insecurity and
in mood or emotion can account for lasting changes in parenting. depressive symptoms serve as the operative underpinnings of the
Thus, to cogently affect parenting over time, the dynamic emo- spillover process, then it follows that mediational pathways be-
tional underpinnings of spillover must transform into stable indi- tween interparental conflict and parenting should be more consis-
vidual differences, such as negative affect (e.g., depressive symp- tent when the child-rearing domain is shaped explicitly by emo-
toms), resulting in cumulative decrements in parenting over tional relationship processes in the family. More specifically,
periods of months or years. These questions have yet to be exam- marital conflict would be most likely to impact emotion-laden
ined at an empirical level. For example, consistent with the pro- domains of parenting by virtue of the difficulties in regulating
posed link in the chain between marital conflict and lasting emotion and using the partner as a base of support. Lending
changes in parental negative affect, prior research has shown that evidence to this contention, a recent review indicated that associ-
marital distress increases depressive symptoms (Beach, Katz, Kim, ations between interparental conflict and parenting across the body
& Brody, 2003; Fincham, Beach, Harold, & Osborne, 1997; Whis- of literature were significantly stronger for child-rearing domains
man, 2001). Supporting another link in this pathway, depressive reflecting low levels of acceptance and harsh punishment than for
symptoms are associated with decrements in domains of parenting lax control (Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000). Accordingly, this
ranging from diminished parental warmth and sensitivity to diffi- study focused on parental insensitivity to child expressions of
culties in disciplining and managing children (Cummings & negative emotion and attempts to control and discipline the child in
Davies, 1994; Lovejoy, Graczyk, O’Hare, & Neuman, 2000). psychologically controlling ways characterized by guilt induction,
Skepticism about whether relatively transient changes, such as instilling anxiety, and intrusiveness.
increases in negative affect, can serve as a primary underpinning of However, although a fundamental assumption of the spillover
spillover encourages identifying how other dynamic properties of hypothesis is that interparental discord plays an etiological role in
the emotional aftermath of interparental conflict might become organizing the affective mechanisms that undermine parenting, an
organized into stable psychological structures that may progres- alternative hypothesis is that the proposed mediational paths in-
sively erode effective parenting patterns over time. From an emo- volving interparental conflict are spurious artifacts resulting from
tional security perspective, adult representations of the security of their noncausal association with the more active, transactional
the interparental relationship may serve as another mechanism roles of adult relationship insecurity and depressive symptoms.
linking interparental conflict and parenting difficulties. Life span Adult depressive symptoms may play a primary etiological role in
formulations of attachment theory posit that conflict between adult promoting attachment insecurity in intimate relationships by fuel-
INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT AND PARENTING 1763

ing negative interpersonal appraisals and evoking negative re- mediator, and outcome (Cole & Maxwell, 2003). Therefore, tem-
sponses from partners (Coyne, 1976; Sacco & Vaughan, 2006). porally ordered prospective analyses were possible using Wave 1
Likewise, attachment theory posits that insecurity in adult rela- measures of interparental conflict, Wave 2 assessments of adult
tionships may engender elevated depressive symptoms by under- relationship insecurity and depressive symptoms, and Wave 3
mining emotion regulation abilities and reinforcing negative work- assessments of parenting difficulties. Autoregressive paths control-
ing models of the self and close relationships (Bowlby, 1988; ling for prior values of the affective mediators and parenting
Carnelley, Pietromonaco, & Jaffe, 1994; Mikulincer & Florian, difficulties were also specified to more definitively identify direc-
1998; Roberts, Gotlib, & Kassel, 1996). Supporting these paths, tionality of relations among the constructs (Gollob & Reichardt,
several studies suggest relations between insecure attachment in 1987).
adult relations and elevated depressive symptoms (J. Feeney,
Alexander, Noller, & Hohaus, 2003; Whiffen, Kallos-Lilly, & Method
MacDonald, 2001).
Thus, as a significant challenge to the spillover hypothesis, this
Participants
body of work raises the possibility that adult depressive symptoms Participants were drawn from a larger project that originally
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and attachment insecurity are such strong mediators of one another included 235 parents and children recruited through local school
in models of parenting difficulties that they supersede any role of districts and community centers in a moderate-sized metropolitan
interparental conflict as a factor in their prediction. That is, adult area in the Northeast and a small city in the Midwest. Interested
depressive symptoms and relationship insecurity may serve as families were included in the project if they met the following
mediators of both interparental conflict and one another in pre- eligibility criteria: (a) the primary caregivers had a child in kin-
dicting interparental conflict. Therefore, this study tests the tenets dergarten; (b) the kindergarten child and two primary caregivers
of the spillover hypothesis and possibly viable alternative models. lived together for at least the preceding 3 years; and (c) the primary
Conceptual models and literature reviews have also proposed caregivers and child were fluent in English. Across the three
that differences in the magnitude of the spillover process across annual measurement occasions, 20 families who initially partici-
families differ for mothers and fathers. The father vulnerability pated in Wave 1 did not participate by Wave 3 (8.5%). Compar-
hypothesis specifically postulates that paternal parenting practices isons of families who did and did not participate in all three waves
are more susceptible to deteriorating in the face of interparental (i.e., n ⫽ 215 and n ⫽ 20, respectively) along the 22 primary
conflict than maternal parenting (Belsky, Gilstrap, & Rovine, variables at Wave 1 (i.e., interparental conflict, adult insecurity
1984; Cummings, Goeke-Morey, & Raymond, 2004; Cummings and depressive symptoms, parenting) yielded one significant dif-
& O’Reilly, 1997). In support of this prediction, some studies have ference: Mothers who did not participate in all three waves re-
reported that associations between interparental conflict and par- ported higher levels of parental intrusiveness (M ⫽ 21.55, SD ⫽
enting difficulties are stronger or more consistent for fathers than 2.61) than mothers who participated in all three waves (M ⫽ 20.03,
they are for mothers (see Davies & Lindsay, 2001; Krishnakumar SD ⫽ 3.07), t(231) ⫽ 2.41, p ⬍ .05. Because differences between
& Buehler, 2000). However, the role of parent gender in the the two groups of families were lower than would be expected by
spillover process has yet to be definitively delineated. For exam- chance (4.5%), we estimated missing data for all 235 families in
ple, the meta-analysis conducted by Erel and Burman (1995) the larger project using an expectation maximization algorithm.
revealed that these gender effects were inconsistent across groups Two families were excluded from analyses because they were
of studies that differed in methodological characteristics. There- identified as multivariate outliers. Accordingly, the final sample
fore, as a step toward resolving this inconsistency, we examine for this article consisted of 233 mothers (M age ⫽ 35.0 years,
whether the mediational pathways between interparental conflict, SD ⫽ 5.61; range: 22–52) and fathers (M age ⫽ 36.8 years, SD ⫽
adult relationship insecurity and depressive symptoms, and par- 6.17; range: 22–52) of kindergarten children (127 girls and 102
enting difficulties are moderated by gender. Further complicating boys; M age at Wave 1 ⫽ 6.0 years, SD ⫽ .48; range 5– 8).
the picture, it is also plausible that spillover may vary as a function Median family income for the 233 families was in the range of
of child gender or the gender composition of the parent– child dyad $40,000 to $54,000, with 13% of the sample reporting household
(e.g., Davies & Lindsay, 2001; Erel & Burman, 1995; Krishnaku- income below $23,000. The average years of education completed
mar & Buehler, 2000). For example, there is some, albeit incon- by mothers and fathers were 14.48 (SD ⫽ 2.34) and 14.61 (SD ⫽
sistent, evidence to suggest that spillover processes may be par- 2.70), respectively. A large proportion of the parents reported
ticularly pronounced for daughters (e.g., Erel & Burman, 1995; being White (77%), followed by smaller percentages of Black
Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000) and for father– daughter dyads (17%), Hispanic (4%), Asian American (1%), mixed (1%), and
(e.g., Davies & Lindsay, 2001; Erel & Burman, 1995). To address other races (1%). Parents were generally married (89.3%) and
these possibilities, we also examine the alternative predictions that lived together an average of 11 years prior to Wave 1 (SD ⫽ 4.88).
child gender and the gender composition of the parent– child dyad Most caregivers reported being the biological parents of their
moderate spillover processes. children (91.0%), followed by smaller numbers of stepparents
In summary, our goal is to identify the affective mechanisms (3%), adoptive parents (3%), and other types of guardianship (3%).
underlying emotional spillover by examining adult relationship In accordance with the goal of obtaining diversity in the experi-
insecurity and depressive symptoms as mediators of associations ences with interparental discord, 33% of mothers and 43% of
between interparental conflict and subsequent changes in parenting fathers in our sample could be classified as maritally dissatisfied
over a 2-year period. Inclusion of three measurement occasions on the basis of scores below 100 on the Short Marital Adjustment
spaced 1 year apart generated rigorous tests of our mediator Test at Wave 1 (Locke & Wallace, 1959), with 53% of the couples
predictions by temporally disaggregating the putative risk factor, containing at least one maritally dissatisfied partner.
1764 DAVIES, STURGE-APPLE, WOITACH, AND CUMMINGS

Procedures sion scale from the Conflict and Problem Solving Scales (CPS;
Kerig, 1996). Parents rate each of the items (e.g., “name-calling,
Families visited the laboratories at the research sites two times cursing, insulting”) along 4-point scales ranging from 0 (never) to
spaced 1 week apart at each of three waves spaced 1 year apart. 3 (often) in relation to both their own and their partner’s use of
Visits lasted about 3 hr. At Wave 1, mothers and fathers partici- verbally aggressive behaviors during interparental conflicts. Inter-
pated in an interparental interaction task. Following similar pro- nal consistency, test–retest reliability, and various forms of valid-
cedures used in previous research (Du Rocher Schudlich, Papp, & ity for this measure are well established (Kerig, 1996, 1998).
Cummings, 2004), spouses independently selected three topics that Mothers’ and fathers’ composite ratings of self and partner’s
they perceived as problematic in their marriage. After selecting verbally aggressive conflict behaviors were significantly corre-
one topic they both felt comfortable discussing from each of their lated with one another (r ⫽ .49). Accordingly, the scales were
lists, the couples discussed each topic for 10 min while they were summed to form a single composite of parent report on overall use
alone in the laboratory room. Videotaped records of the session of verbal aggression in the interparental dyad. Internal consistency
were subsequently coded for interparental conflict behavior. To of parent report of verbal aggression in this sample as indexed by
examine the validity of the assumption that the interparental in- Cronbach’s ␣ was .92.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

teraction task assessed characteristic ways of managing conflict in


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Adult depressive symptoms. Mothers and fathers reported on


the interparental relationship, we asked mothers and fathers indi- their depressive symptoms at Waves 1 and 2 using the Center for
vidually to respond to the question, “Overall, how much did the Epidemiological Studies–Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff,
discussion resemble disagreements that usually occur between you 1977). The CES-D is a 20-item self-report measure designed to
and your partner at home?” immediately following the interaction assess depressive symptoms, particularly negative affect (e.g.,
task. Response alternatives ranged from 1 (a lot more negative) to “bothered by things,” “could not shake off the blues,” “sad,”
7 (a lot more positive). Supporting the comparability of the inter- “depressed,” “fearful”), in community samples. For each item,
actions during the laboratory visit to conflicts that occur in the mothers and fathers used a 4-point response format ranging from
home, the means of mother and father responses fell between 0 (rarely or none of the time) to 3 (most of the time) to indicate the
about the same and a little more positive on the 7-point scale (M ⫽ way they felt or behaved during the past week. The CES-D has
4.71, SD ⫽ .91 and M ⫽ 4.76, SD ⫽ 1.11, respectively). Across all well-established psychometric properties, including internal con-
three waves, mothers and fathers also independently completed sistency, test–retest reliability, and convergent validity with clin-
questionnaires to assess interparental conflict, adult relationship ical and self-reports of depression (Radloff, 1977; Weissman,
security and depressive symptoms, and parenting difficulties. The Sholomskas, Pottenger, Prusoff, & Locke, 1977). Internal consis-
study was approved by the institutional review board at each tency coefficients for the CES-D in this sample were satisfactory
research site. for mothers (␣s ⫽ .87 at Wave 1 and .90 at Wave 2) and fathers
(␣s ⫽ .85 at Wave 1 and .90 at Wave 2).
Adult relationship insecurity. To assess adult insecurity in the
Measures
interparental relationship, mothers and fathers were asked to com-
Interparental conflict. We used observations derived from the plete the Attachment Style questionnaire at Waves 1 and 2 (AS;
interparental interaction task and parent reports of interparental Becker, Billings, Eveleth, & Gilbert, 1997). The development of
conflict in the home to obtain a multimethod latent construct of the AS consisted of adapting selective items from established adult
interparental conflict at Wave 1. For the observational component attachment questionnaires (i.e., Bartholomew & Horowitz, 1991;
of the measurement battery, trained coders provided behavioral Collins & Read, 1990; Hazan & Shaver, 1987; Mikulincer, Flo-
ratings using three codes from the System for Coding Interactions rian, & Tolmacz, 1990). The AS contains 19 items that assess three
and Dyads (SCID; Malik & Lindahl, 2004). The SCID yields scales, including: (a) Preoccupied Attachment (i.e., “I often want
molar ratings of discord for each of the two marital interactions, to get closer to my partner than s/he wants to get to me”), (b)
ranging from 1 (very low) to 5 (high). Coders separately rated Fearful Attachment (i.e., “I am not sure that I can always depend
maternal and paternal negativity and conflict, which reflects the on my partner to be there when I need him/her”), and (c) Secure
extent to which each individual in the dyad displays anger, frus- Attachment (i.e., “I find it relatively easy to get close to my
tration, and tension. At a dyadic level of analysis, coders provided partner.”). Items were rated on a 7-point scale ranging from 1
ratings of couple cohesiveness. Low cohesiveness scores indicate (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Internal consistency
overall interpersonal disengagement, aloofness, or indifference in coefficients among the three AS scales in this sample were satis-
the dyad as a unit. Therefore, for the subsequent analyses, cohe- factory for mothers (␣s ⫽ .74 to .85 for Wave 1 and .78 to .87 for
siveness was reverse scored and labeled as disengagement so that Wave 2) and fathers (␣s ⫽ .74 to .84 at Wave 1 and .84 to .85 at
higher scores indicate greater withdrawal. For each conflict code, Wave 2). The three scales were highly correlated for mothers
ratings were averaged across the two interparental interactions to (mean r ⫽ .58; range ⫽ .50 to .71) and fathers (mean r ⫽ .54;
obtain more comprehensive, parsimonious measures. Ratings from range ⫽ .43 to .70). Therefore, to retain methodological compa-
the primary coder, who rated all the marital interactions, were used rability with the manifest measure of depressive symptoms, the
in the analyses. To assess interrater reliability, another coder three scales were averaged together after reverse-scoring the Se-
independently rated 25% of the tapes. The resulting intraclass cure Attachment Scale to correspond with the directionality of the
correlation coefficients for the three codes of interparental conflict other two scales.
ranged from .95 to .96. Parental insensitivity to child negative affect. Measures of
For the parent report component of the measurement battery, parental insensitivity to child distress were obtained through ma-
mothers and fathers each completed the eight-item Verbal Aggres- ternal and paternal reports of their nonsupportive reactions on two
INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT AND PARENTING 1765

subscales (i.e., Minimization Reactions, Punitive Reactions) of the Persistent Anxiety (e.g., “If your child breaks a promise, you don’t
Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale (CCNES; Fabes, trust him/her again for a long time”), and Intrusive (e.g., “You ask
Poulin, Eisenberg, & Madden-Derdich, 2003). The CCNES con- your child to tell you everything that happens when s/he is away
sists of 12 hypothetical vignettes depicting child distress or anger from home”) scales from the parent form of Child Report of
(e.g., “If my child is playing with other children and one of them Parenting Behaviors Inventory at Waves 1 and 3 (CRPBI; Margo-
calls him/her names, and my child then begins to tremble and lies & Weintraub, 1977; Schludermann & Schludermann, 1970).
become tearful, I would ___”). After reading each vignette, parents Items on each of the five-item subscales were rated on 5-point
independently rated the likelihood of responding to their children’s Likert scales ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (always). The subscales
distress in each of six ways using a 7-point scale ranging from 1 have satisfactory internal consistency, and previous research has
(very unlikely) to 7 (very likely). Whereas the Minimization Re- supported the convergent and discriminant validity of the measures
actions subscale reflects parental dispositions to downplay or (Fauber, Forehand, Thomas, & Wierson, 1990). Internal consis-
devalue their children’s distress (e.g., “tell my child not to make a tency coefficients among the three CRPBI scales in this sample
big deal out of it”), the Punitive Reactions subscale denotes parent were adequate for mothers (␣s ⫽ .66 to 71 at Wave 1 and .63 to.68
tendencies to punish or reprimand their children for expressing at Wave 2) and fathers (␣ ⫽ .70 to .75 at Wave 1 and .73 to .80
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

negative affect (e.g., “tell my child to behave or we’ll have to go at Wave 3). The three indicators for mothers and fathers were
home right away”). The validity of these nonsupportive scales is retained as manifest indicators of latent constructs of maternal and
supported by their associations with indices of children’s social paternal psychological control.
and emotional adjustment (e.g., Fabes et al., 2003; Jones, Eisen-
berg, Fabes, & MacKinnon, 2002) and negligible links with indi-
Results
ces of social desirability (Fabes et al., 2003). Reliability for the
Minimization and Punitive Reactions subscales was good for Descriptive Analyses
mothers (␣s ⫽ .80 and .74 for Wave 1 and .86 and .77 for Wave
3) and fathers (␣s ⫽ .82 and .81 at Wave 1 and .82 to .78 at Wave Tables 1 and 2 show the means, standard deviations, and cor-
3). Both of the two subscales were used as indicators of latent relations among the main variables for mothers and fathers, re-
constructs of maternal and paternal insensitivity to child distress. spectively. The moderate to high magnitude of correlations among
Parental psychological control. Mothers and fathers reported the proposed manifest indicators of the main constructs (i.e., mean
on their own use of psychologically controlling parenting behav- r ⫽ .48 and .49 for mothers and fathers, respectively) provided
iors using the Control Through Guilt (e.g., “You tell your child that initial support for the operationalization of the measurement
s/he would do what you want if s/he loved you”), Instilling model. Consistent with the proposed mediational pathways be-

Table 1
Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations of the Main Variables for Mothers in the Primary Analyses

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18

Wave 1
1. F-SCID Neg. 2.06 0.98 —
2. M-SCID Neg. 1.75 0.90 .55ⴱ —
3. SCID Dis. 2.89 0.94 .42ⴱ .40ⴱ —

4. CPS-Agg. 12.01 3.66 .36 .33ⴱ .30ⴱ —
5. CES-D 8.91 7.96 .12 .10 .26ⴱ .29ⴱ —
6. AS 14.62 6.96 .30ⴱ .34ⴱ .32ⴱ
.46ⴱ .35ⴱ —
7. CCNES Pun. 26.73 8.43 .07 .06 .07 .04 .25ⴱ .22ⴱ —
8. CCNES Min. 30.78 9.99 .09 .04 .18ⴱ .10 .14ⴱ
.14ⴱ .67ⴱ —
9. CRPBI Int. 20.16 3.06 .02 ⫺.06 .10 ⫺.02 .09 ⫺.02 .05 .09 —
10. CRPBI Gui. 12.15 3.27 .01 ⫺.08 .19ⴱ .08 .17ⴱ .10 .19ⴱ .19ⴱ .46ⴱ —
11. CRPBI Anx. 10.55 3.61 .11 ⫺.03 .15ⴱ .09 .25ⴱ .12 .33 .30 .39ⴱ .66ⴱ
ⴱ ⴱ

Wave 2
12. CES-D 9.54 9.00 .04 .05 .17ⴱ .13ⴱ .49ⴱ .28ⴱ .23ⴱ .08 .10 .18ⴱ .19ⴱ —
13. AS 14.66 7.02 .25ⴱ .36ⴱ .30ⴱ .37ⴱ .26ⴱ .70ⴱ .20ⴱ .13ⴱ .02 .06 .12 .39ⴱ —
Wave 3
14. CCNES Pun. 26.60 8.77 .13ⴱ .05 .04 .04 .10 .14ⴱ .66ⴱ .54ⴱ .01 .18ⴱ .28ⴱ .10 .14ⴱ —
15. CCNES Min. 29.61 9.60 .11 .01 .09 .02 .13ⴱ .13ⴱ .63ⴱ .72ⴱ .03 .10 .21ⴱ .05 .12 .68ⴱ —
16. CRPBI Int. 19.70 2.91 .10 .02 .10 .02 .16ⴱ .07 .10 .14ⴱ .53ⴱ .40ⴱ .39ⴱ .06 .01 .07 .10 —
17. CRPBI Gui. 11.66 2.85 ⫺.01 ⫺.09 .08 .07 .13ⴱ .01 .22ⴱ .21ⴱ .29ⴱ .68ⴱ .56ⴱ .08 .00 .22ⴱ .13ⴱ .47ⴱ —
18. CRPBI Anx. 10.02 3.02 .08 ⫺.08 .12 .08 .13ⴱ .20ⴱ .40ⴱ .35ⴱ .19ⴱ .50ⴱ .63ⴱ .14ⴱ .19ⴱ .40ⴱ .33ⴱ .36ⴱ .63ⴱ —

Note. M ⫽ mother; F ⫽ father; SCID ⫽ System for Coding Interactions and Dyads; SCID Neg. ⫽ observation of negativity in interparental conflict;
SCID Dis. ⫽ observation of interparental disengagement; CPS-Agg ⫽ Conflict and Problem Solving Scales Verbal Aggression Scale; CES-D ⫽ Center
for Epidemiological Studies–Depression Scale; AS Pre. ⫽ Attachment Style Preoccupied Scale; AS ⫽ Attachment Style Scale; CCNES ⫽ Coping with
Children’s Negative Emotions Scale; CCNES Pun. ⫽ CCNES Parent Punitive; CCNES Min. ⫽ CCNES Parent Minimization; CRPBI ⫽ Child Report of
Parenting Behaviors Inventory; CRPBI Int. ⫽ CRPBI Parental Intrusiveness Scale; CRPBI Gui. ⫽ CRPBI Parental Control Through Guilt; CRPBI
Anx. ⫽ CRPBI Instilling Persistent Anxiety.

p ⬍ .05.
1766 DAVIES, STURGE-APPLE, WOITACH, AND CUMMINGS

Table 2
Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations of the Main Variables for Fathers in the Primary Analyses

Variables M SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18

Wave 1
1. F-SCID Neg. 2.06 0.98 —
2. M-SCID Neg. 1.75 0.90 .55ⴱ —
3. SCID Dis. 2.89 0.94 .42ⴱ .40ⴱ —
4. CPS-Agg. 12.01 3.66 .36ⴱ .33ⴱ .30ⴱ —
5. CES-D 8.29 7.33 .07 ⫺.02 .14ⴱ .16ⴱ —
6. AS 14.53 6.15 .14ⴱ .22ⴱ .23ⴱ .42ⴱ .27ⴱ —
7. CCNES Pun. 30.61 9.79 ⫺.01 .12 .15ⴱ .14ⴱ .11 .18ⴱ —
8. CCNES Min. 36.84 11.20 .05 .16ⴱ .12 .10 .04 .08 .67ⴱ —
9. CRPBI Int. 18.39 3.47 .10 .02 .10 .03 ⫺.03 .03 .08 .09 —
10. CRPBI Gui. 12.01 3.49 .09 ⫺.01 .12 .12 .13ⴱ .11 .27ⴱ .25ⴱ .50ⴱ —
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

11. CRPBI Anx. 11.55 3.77 .19ⴱ .08 .10 .15ⴱ .28ⴱ .21ⴱ .27ⴱ .22ⴱ .42ⴱ .61ⴱ —
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Wave 2
ⴱ ⴱ ⴱ ⴱ ⴱ
12. CES-D 9.61 9.02 .12 .09 .15 .18 .58 .23 .11 .11 ⫺.17 .06 .12 —
13. AS 15.02 6.58 .22ⴱ .25ⴱ .26ⴱ .38ⴱ .26ⴱ .67ⴱ .20ⴱ .16ⴱ ⫺.04 .09 .18ⴱ .39ⴱ —
Wave 3
14. CCNES Pun. 29.94 9.01 .14ⴱ .14ⴱ .18ⴱ .11 .19ⴱ .15ⴱ .63ⴱ .49ⴱ .03 .17ⴱ .28ⴱ .19ⴱ .29ⴱ —
15. CCNES Min. 35.75 10.35 .17ⴱ .16ⴱ .17ⴱ .12 .09 .16ⴱ .42ⴱ .53ⴱ ⫺.01 .09 .26ⴱ .13 .28ⴱ .73ⴱ —
16. CRPBI Int. 18.12 3.82 .21ⴱ .06 .17ⴱ .09 .01 .07 .05 .07 .58ⴱ .23ⴱ .26ⴱ ⫺.15ⴱ .08 .02 .02 —
17. CRPBI Gui. 11.77 3.63 .20ⴱ ⫺.04 .12 .21ⴱ .22ⴱ .21ⴱ .22ⴱ .20ⴱ .35ⴱ .52ⴱ .44ⴱ .12 .23ⴱ .22ⴱ .20ⴱ .50ⴱ —
18. CRPBI Anx. 11.04 3.66 .24 .09 .16 .19 .30 .14 .29 .27 .35 .39 .62 .20 .22 .32 .24 .43 .68 —

Note. M ⫽ mother; F ⫽ father; SCID ⫽ System for Coding Interactions and Dyads; SCID Neg. ⫽ observation of negativity in interparental conflict; SCID
Dis. ⫽ observation of interparental disengagement; CPS-Agg ⫽ Conflict and Problem Solving Scales Verbal Aggression Scale; CES-D ⫽ Center for
Epidemiological Studies–Depression Scale; AS ⫽ Attachment Style Scale; CCNES ⫽ Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scale; CCNES Pun. ⫽
CCNES Parent Punitive subscale; CCNES Min. ⫽ CCNES Parent Minimization subscale; CRPBI ⫽ Child Report of Parenting Behaviors Inventory; CRPBI
Int. ⫽ CRPBI Parental Intrusiveness Scale; CRPBI Gui. ⫽ CRPBI Parental Control Through Guilt subscale; CRPBI Anx. ⫽ CRPBI Instilling Persistent
Anxiety subscale.

p ⬍ .05.

tween the main constructs, significant interrelationships were ev- comparison for the structural paths in Figures 1 and 2 consisted of
ident among dimensions of interparental conflict and Wave 2 adult comparing a model in which all parameters were allowed to vary
relationship insecurity and depressive symptoms for both mothers freely with a model in which comparable paths across the relevant
and fathers. Moreover, Wave 1 interparental conflict and Wave 2 groups of child gender, parent gender, and parent gender by child
adult relationship insecurity and depressive symptoms were more gender were constrained to be equal.
consistently associated with greater parenting difficulties for fa- The results indicated that the free-to-vary models for child
thers than for mothers. gender and the parent gender by child gender models did not
generate significantly better representations of the data than the
Analysis Plan models constraining paths across gender to equality. Therefore, the
results indicated that child gender or the configuration of parent
We conducted structural equation modeling (SEM) using max- and child gender did not moderate the proposed mediational path-
imum likelihood estimation with AMOS software (Arbuckle, ways. However, the multigroup comparisons for parent gender
2006) to examine adult relationship insecurity and depressive indicated that the free-to-vary model provided a significantly bet-
symptoms as explanatory mechanisms in links between interpa- ter fit than the constrained model in the prediction of parental
rental conflict and parenting difficulties. As a first step toward insensitivity to child distress, ⌬␹2(7, N ⫽ 466) ⫽ 15.25, p ⬍ .05,
parsimoniously testing the mediational hypotheses, we conducted and psychological control, ⌬␹2(7, N ⫽ 466) ⫽ 14.70, p ⬍ .05.
three sets of multigroup comparisons to evaluate whether the Because the mediational pathways varied as a function of parent
proposed pathways differed as a function of child gender, parent gender, the primary analyses consisted of multigroup comparisons
gender, and the combination of parent and child gender. Due to the designed to simultaneously estimate the mediational paths for
complexity of the multimediator models in relation to our sample mothers and fathers in the prediction of each type of parenting
size, separate multigroup SEM analyses were performed for each difficulty.
type of parenting difficulty. For two of the three analyses involving
child gender, the small sample sizes resulting from splitting the
Primary Analyses
data by child gender necessitated the specification of path analyses
using manifest, rather than latent, composites for our measures of Mediational tests often require that a significant association
interparental conflict, adult insecurity, and parenting difficulties. exist between the predictor and outcome (Baron & Kenny, 1986;
Manifest variables of these primary constructs were constructed by MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002). There-
first standardizing their respective manifest indicators and subse- fore, our first pair of SEMs constrained paths between each of the
quently summing them to create composites. Each multiple group mediators (i.e., adult relationship insecurity and depressive symp-
INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT AND PARENTING 1767

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3

-.03/.09
Interparental
Conflict
.70 .72 .71 .70 .58 .58 .55 .53
-.08/.12
M-SCID F-SCID SCID CPS-
Neg. Neg. Dis. Agg.

.27*/.11
Adult Depressive Adult Depressive
.58*/.36* Symptoms .45*/.55* Symptoms

.01/.08 .17*/.19*
* *
.37 /.32 .32*/.29*
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.16*/.05
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Adult Adult
.22*/.06 .61*/.58* -.07/.04
Insecurity Insecurity

.20*/.12

.03/.15*

Parental Parental
Insensitivity .87*/.62* Insensitivity

.92 .84 .73 .80 .84 .92 .81 .79

CCNES-.Pun CCNES-Min. CCNES-.Pun CCNES-Min.

Figure 1. A multigroup structural equation model testing adult relationship insecurity and depressive symp-
toms as intermediary mechanisms in associations between interparental conflict and mother and father insen-
sitivity to child negative affect. Parameter estimates for the structural paths are standardized path coefficients.
Bolded loadings and path coefficients reflect estimates for mothers, whereas italicized figures reflect the
estimates for fathers. M ⫽ mother; F ⫽ father; SCID ⫽ System for Coding Interactions and Dyads; SCID
Neg. ⫽ observation of negativity in interparental conflict; SCID Dis. ⫽ observation of interparental disengage-
ment; CPS-Agg ⫽ Conflict and Problem Solving Scales Verbal Aggression Scale; CCNES ⫽ Coping with
Children’s Negative Emotions Scale; CCNES Pun. ⫽ CCNES Parent Punitive; CCNES Min. ⫽ CCNES Parent
Minimization. ⴱ p ⬍ .05.

toms) and parenting difficulties to zero. In the same model, we which the paths between each of the proposed mediators and
estimated the direct, predictive paths between (a) Wave 1 interpa- parenting difficulties were freely estimated. Although results did
rental conflict and the proposed mediators at Wave 2 and (b) the not yield initial support for mediation in the maternal model, we
Wave 2 proposed mediators and Wave 3 parenting difficulties. To estimated the full model for mothers and fathers simultaneously
provide a conservative test of prospective associations, we speci- through multigroup comparisons on the basis of two consider-
fied autoregressive paths for the proposed mediators from Wave 1 ations. First, even if interparental conflict is unrelated to parenting
to Wave 2 and each parenting difficulty from Wave 1 to Wave 3. difficulties for mothers, it is still possible that interparental conflict
In support of the precondition for testing mediation, interparental may be indirectly associated with greater parenting difficulties by
conflict at Wave 1 was a significant predictor of increases in undermining maternal depressive symptoms and adult relationship
paternal insensitivity, ␤ ⫽ .18, p ⬍ .05, and psychological control, insecurity. Second, the multigroup analysis provided an examina-
␤ ⫽ .16, p ⬍ .05, from Wave 1 to Wave 3. In contrast, comparable tion of the mediational pathways involving depressive symptoms
associations between interparental conflict and maternal insensi- and adult relationship insecurity as a function of parent gender. In
tivity, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.03, and psychological control, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.03, were addition, the full model also estimated cross-lagged, longitudinal
negligible and nonsignificant. Tests of parent gender as a moder- paths between the proposed mediators to test our model in relation
ator through estimation of pairwise parameter comparisons further to an alternative model that proposes that adult depressive symp-
revealed that interparental conflict was a significantly stronger toms and attachment insecurity are such strong predictors of one
predictor of parental insensitivity, z ⫽ 2.43, p ⬍ .05, and psycho- another in models of parenting difficulties that they supersede any
logical control, z ⫽ 2.26, p ⬍ .01, for fathers in comparison to role of interparental conflict as a factor in their prediction.
mothers. Parental insensitivity results. The multigroup SEM results for
Given support for the direct paths between interparental conflict the model predicting parental insensitivity are shown in Figure 1.
and paternal parenting, we proceeded to test the full model in The model provided a good representation of the data, ␹2(80, N ⫽
1768 DAVIES, STURGE-APPLE, WOITACH, AND CUMMINGS

Wave 1 Wave 2 Wave 3

-.04/.08
Interparental
Conflict
.70 .72 .71 .70 .58 .58 .55 .53
-.08/.12
M-SCID F-SCID SCID CPS-
Neg. Neg. Dis. Agg.

.27*/.11
Adult Depressive Adult Depressive
.53*/.34* Symptoms .45*/.55* Symptoms

.01/.08 .17*/.19*
.08/.16 * *
.35 /.27 .32*/.29*
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

.16*/.05
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Adult Adult
.24*/.20* .61*/.58* -.06/.03
Insecurity Insecurity

.10/.17*

.03/.14*

Parent Psych. Parent Psych.


Control .81*/.60* Control

.79 .73 .85 .83 .50 .60 .71 .76 .87 .90 .57 .55

CRPBI CRPBI CRPBI CRPBI CRPBI CRPBI


Anx. Guilt Intr. Anx. Guilt Intr.

Figure 2. A multigroup structural equation model testing adult relationship insecurity and depressive symp-
toms as intermediary mechanisms in associations between interparental conflict and mother and father psycho-
logical control. Parameter estimates for the structural paths are standardized path coefficients. Bolded loadings
and path coefficients reflect estimates for mothers, whereas italicized figures reflect the estimates for fathers.
M ⫽ mother; F ⫽ father; SCID ⫽ System for Coding Interactions and Dyads; SCID
Neg. ⫽ observation of negativity in interparental conflict; SCID Dis. ⫽ observation of interparental disengage-
ment; CPS-Agg ⫽ Conflict and Problem Solving Scales Verbal Aggression Scale; CRPBI ⫽ Child Report of
Parenting Behaviors Inventory; CRPBI Int. ⫽ CRPBI Parental Intrusiveness Scale; CRPBI Gui. ⫽ CRPBI
Parental Control Through Guilt subscale; CRPBI Anx. ⫽ CRPBI Instilling Persistent Anxiety subscale. ⴱ p ⬍
.05.

466) ⫽ 149.85, p ⬍ .001, RMSEA ⫽ .04, and ␹2/df ratio ⫽ 1.87, from .18, p ⬍ .05 to .09, p ⫽ ns). In further support of the
CFI ⫽ .97, and TLI ⫽ .94. Results for mothers and fathers are mediational role of adult relationship insecurity, follow-up tests
depicted in the same model for the sake of comparison, with using MacKinnon et al. (2002) procedures for indirect effects
maternal findings bolded to differentiate them from the paternal indicated that the indirect pathways involving interparental con-
findings. In support of the measurement models for both mothers flict and adult relationship insecurity were significant in the pre-
and fathers, all loadings of the manifest indicators onto their diction of paternal insensitivity at Wave 3, z⬘ ⫽ 1.82, p ⬍ .05.
respective latent constructs were significant (i.e., p ⬍ .001) and In accordance with the father model, the results for mothers
moderate to high in magnitude (i.e., .53 to .92). indicated that Wave 1 interparental conflict was a predictor of
Consistent with mediational hypotheses, analysis of the struc- increased insecurity in the interparental relationship from Wave 1
tural paths in the father model indicated that Wave 1 interparental to Wave 2, ␤ ⫽ .17, p ⬍ .05. In addition, Wave 1 maternal
conflict predicted Wave 2 paternal relationship insecurity, ␤ ⫽ .19, relationship insecurity was a predictor of increased depressive
p ⬍ .05 even after specifying autogressive paths and the cross- symptoms from Wave 1 to Wave 2, ␤ ⫽ .16, p ⬍ .05. However,
lagged paths between depressive symptoms and relationship inse- maternal depressive symptoms and adult relationship insecurity at
curity (see Figure 1). Adult relationship insecurity at Wave 2, in Wave 2 were not significant predictors of maternal insensitivity at
turn, was a significant predictor of Wave 3 parental insensitivity, Wave 3.
␤ ⫽ .15, p ⬍ .05. Inclusion of the structural paths between the Parental psychological control results. Figure 2 depicts the
proposed mediators and parenting difficulties resulted in notable results of the SEM multigroup comparison analyses in the predic-
reductions in the magnitude of the path coefficients between tion of parental psychological control. Results for mothers are
interparental conflict and paternal insensitivity (␤ reduced by 50% represented by the bolded values to distinguish them from the
INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT AND PARENTING 1769

findings for the fathers. The overall fit of the model was good, gender as a moderator has rarely been tested directly in studies.
␹2(232, N ⫽ 466) ⫽ 257.41, p ⬍ .001, RMSEA ⫽ .05, and ␹2/df Following calls by Cummings et al. (2004), our study separately
ratio ⫽ 2.11, CFI ⫽ .95, and TLI ⫽ .92. As with the parental tested and compared the strength of the spillover pathways across
sensitivity model, support for the measurement models for paternal child gender, parent gender, and the gender composition of the
and maternal psychological control is evidenced by the moderate parent– child dyad. The results showed that pathways between
to high loadings of the manifest indicators onto their proposed interparental conflict and parenting varied specifically as a func-
latent constructs (range: .50 to .90; p ⬍ .001). tion of parent gender. In accord with prior findings (Jouriles &
The structural paths for the psychological control model in Farris, 1992; Kitzmann, 2000; Owen & Cox, 1997), interparental
Figure 2 evidence a similar pattern of findings to the parental conflict was associated with greater parenting difficulties for fa-
sensitivity analyses. For fathers, Wave 1 interparental conflict was thers but not mothers. However, against a backdrop of primarily
a significant predictor of increases in paternal relationship insecu- cross-sectional and short-term experimental designs that do not
rity, ␤ ⫽ .19, p ⬍ .05 from Wave 1 to Wave 2. In turn, Wave 2 explicitly examine parent gender as a moderator, a novel finding of
adult relationship insecurity was associated with Wave 3 paternal this article is that the magnitude of the paths between interparental
psychological control, ␤ ⫽ .14, p ⬍ .05. In contrast, Wave 2 conflict and subsequent increases in parental psychological control
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

depressive symptoms were not significantly related to Wave 3 and insensitivity were significantly stronger for fathers than they
father psychological control, ␤ ⫽ .03, ns. Inclusion of the medi- were for mothers over a 2-year time period. In drawing from the
ational paths resulted in a considerable reduction in the magnitude father vulnerability hypothesis (Cummings et al., 2004), one in-
of the standardized path between Wave 1 interparental conflict and terpretation for the greater susceptibility of fathers is rooted in the
Wave 3 psychological control (␤ dropped by 53% from .17, p ⬍ notion that mothers may be better able to compartmentalize their
.05 to .08, p ⫽ ns). Significance tests for mediation outlined by spousal and parental roles, resulting in less carry-over from marital
MacKinnon et al. (2002) further revealed that the indirect pathway relations to parenting (Belsky, Youngblade, Rovine, & Volling,
among interparental conflict, adult relationship insecurity, and 1991; Lindahl, Clements, & Markman, 1997). Similarly, in keep-
paternal psychological control was significant, z⬘ ⫽ 1.84, p ⬍ .05. ing with traditional gender roles and the disproportionate preva-
Similar to the findings from maternal sensitivity, Wave 1 inter- lence of women as primary caregivers, parenthood might be re-
parental conflict predicted subsequent increases in maternal rela- garded as a more fundamental role by women than by men (Davies
tionship insecurity, ␤ ⫽ .17, p ⬍ .05, but not maternal depressive & Lindsay, 2001; Thompson & Walker, 1989).
symptoms, ␤ ⫽ ⫺.08, ns. Moreover, no support was found for Analysis of the mediational role of adult relationship insecurity
maternal depressive symptoms or adult relationship insecurity as and depressive symptoms in the spillover process further eluci-
predictors of subsequent parenting difficulties. dated why paternal parenting is more likely to progressively de-
teriorate following experiences with interparental conflict. In ex-
Discussion amining the first autoregressive path in the mediational chain,
interparental conflict predicted subsequent increases in interparen-
The spillover hypothesis has garnered considerable empirical tal relationship insecurity, but not depressive symptoms, over a
and theoretical attention, but the conditions and mechanisms un- 1-year period for fathers. However, the father vulnerability hy-
derlying the transmission of distress from interparental relations to pothesis does not propose that the first link in the mediational
parenting difficulties remain largely unidentified. Moreover, be- chain is the source of their greater vulnerability. In fact, the tacit
cause the spillover hypothesis was originally developed to under- assumption of this model is that interparental conflict should incur
stand why interparental discord was related to parenting difficul- a similar emotional toll on both mothers and fathers (Cummings &
ties over relatively small developmental periods (e.g., minutes, O’Reilly, 1997). Thus, the finding that interparental conflict also
hours), even less is known about when and why interparental predicted increases in adult relationship insecurity for mothers is
conflict may insidiously undermine parenting practices over rela- consistent with father vulnerability formulations of spillover.
tively lengthy developmental spans of years. The goal of our Within the father vulnerability hypothesis, the locus of the gender
longitudinal study was to address this gap by identifying affective specificity in the transmission of distress lies in the associations
mechanisms that mediate prospective associations between inter- between emotional spillover mechanisms (i.e., relationship inse-
parental conflict and parenting difficulties within a broader curity, depressive symptoms) and parenting difficulties. The
gender-differentiated model. Our findings indicated the interparen- greater salience of the parenting role and more effective compart-
tal conflict predicted increases in parental psychological control mentalization of spousal and parenting roles by mothers relative to
and insensitivity to child distress for fathers but not mothers. fathers should specifically be evidenced in the ability to counteract
Moreover, in supporting the moderating role of parent gender, the unfolding cascade of negativity into child-rearing contexts
interparental conflict was a significantly stronger predictor of (Belsky et al., 1991; Cummings et al., 2004; Thompson & Walker,
increases in parental psychological control and insensitivity to 1989). Consistent with this prediction, the findings in the second
child distress for fathers than for mothers. Mediational analyses link of the proposed mediational chain indicated that adult rela-
further revealed that fathers’ susceptibility to experiencing in- tionship insecurity predicted subsequent increases in both parental
creases in adult relationship insecurity explained why interparental insensitivity to child negative affect and psychological control for
conflict was associated with their greater parenting problems over fathers but not mothers.
time. In attesting to its strength as an explanatory mechanism of
Although gender is increasingly regarded as critical factor for spillover processes in fathers, the mediational role of adult rela-
advancing an understanding of the spillover hypothesis (Cum- tionship insecurity in pathways between interparental discord and
mings et al., 2004; Krishnakumar & Buehler, 2000), the role of fathering difficulties remained even when estimating adult depres-
1770 DAVIES, STURGE-APPLE, WOITACH, AND CUMMINGS

sive symptoms as a possible mediator and rival predictor in the involvement and maternal gatekeeping were not assessed, it re-
analytic models. Thus, the results lend support to the notion that mains for future research to test whether maternal gatekeeping and
more global forms of negative affect are not necessarily the oper- consequent diminished paternal involvement are factors in rela-
ative processes underlying the transmission of disturbances from tions between adult relationship insecurity and spillover to parent-
the interparental to parent– child relationship. Relative to a trait ing for fathers.
conceptualization of negative affect (i.e., depressive symptoms), Drawing from an evolutionary perspective, another explanation
our findings not only demonstrate intriguing specificity in adult is that perturbations in the balance between security and caregiving
relationship insecurity as a carrier of spillover but also substan- concerns in the interparental relationship may be more pronounced
tially reduce the plausibility of competing, methodological expla- for men than for women. Evolutionary models of pair-bonding
nations. For example, in extending trait negative affectivity models specifically theorize that maximizing reproductive fitness may
(Harold & Conger, 1997; Watson & Pennebaker, 1989), associa- demand different strategies for men and women (Geary & Flinn,
tions between interparental conflict, adult emotional distress, and 2001). For women, the ability to definitively determine their
parenting difficulties may simply be an artifact of the tendency for maternity permits the development of the caregiving system within
individuals with high levels of trait negativity to inflate reports of the context of the parent– child relationship, in which effort is
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

adverse experiences with others and their own negative attributes. devoted toward protection and support of the child (Draper, 1989;
However, the failure to identify depressive symptoms as a medi- Geary, 2000). In contrast, paternal uncertainty for men may pull
ator within a broader autoregressive design that controls for prior for the development of the caregiving system within the context of
values of the primary constructs militates against the notion that a lengthy pair-bond relationship with their partner. Caregiving
the findings are simply an artifact of a negative affectivity trait. behaviors that reinforce the pair-bond relationship serve the adap-
By the same token, the results beg the question of why adult tive function of increasing reproductive fitness by insuring con-
relationship insecurity serves as a specific affective mechanism of tinued access to a reproductive partner and by increasing certainty
spillover even after taking into account global depressive symp- of paternity in potential offspring through reducing her exposure to
toms. According to ethological formulations of security (Ain- and desire to mate with other adult men. A corollary of this
sworth, 1985; Crowell et al., 2002; Davies & Sturge-Apple, 2007; assumption is that the adaptive value of male parental investment
Mikulincer, Shaver, Gillath, & Nitzberg, 2005), the attachment depends on signals of monogamy from the female. Conflict or
system is so closely intertwined with the caregiving system and its withdrawal between parents may represent a lack of pair-bond en-
adaptive function of protecting dependents through sensitivity and hancing behaviors, which could signal a potential change in the
responsiveness to dependent needs that it is commonly viewed as balance of costs and benefits for reproductive effort. Thus, the sig-
a component of the adult attachment relationship. By extension, nificance of the intimate adult relationship as a context for the
attachment insecurity fostered by doubts about the availability of development of the caregiving system in men may help to explain
one’s partner as a source of support may also undermine the why mediational paths between interparental conflict, relationship
caregiving system, impeding adults’ ability to be sensitive and insecurity, and parenting were only applicable to fathers.
responsive to the needs of both their partner and, potentially, their Interpretation of the present findings, however, must be quali-
children. The primacy of self-protective systems (e.g., attachment, fied by several limitations of the study. First, despite some diver-
social defense or wariness) in the context of recurrent threats posed sity in sociodemographic (e.g., race, income, education) and fam-
by interparental conflict may limit opportunities to use the adult ily risk (i.e., high levels of interparental distress), care should be
partnership as a training ground for developing and refining strat- taken in generalizing the findings to other samples. Second, al-
egies in the caregiving system (Davies & Sturge-Apple, 2007), though interparental conflict was assessed using multiple methods
thereby coalescing into stable patterns in which the caregiving and informants, maternal and paternal depressive symptoms, rela-
system is consistently superseded by security concerns (B. C. tionship insecurity, and parenting practices were assessed using
Feeney & Collins, 2001; Mikulincer et al., 2005). Therefore, self-report instruments. Although controlling for earlier levels of
increasing tendencies to use punitive, minimizing, and intrusive depressive symptoms, adult relationship insecurity, and parenting
child-rearing practices may be a manifestation of disturbances in practices in our autoregressive models substantially reduces com-
the caregiving system. mon method and informant variance (e.g., Steinberg, Lamborn,
However, this interpretation, in itself, does not address the Darling, Mounts, & Dornbusch, 1994), multimethod or multi-
question of why relationship insecurity served as a mediator of informant assessments of each of the key constructs may help to
associations between interparental conflict and parenting only for further clarify spillover processes from the interparental to parent–
fathers. The maternal gatekeeping hypothesis may provide one child relationship. For example, our self-report assessment of adult
explanation for this pattern of findings. In assuming a greater role relationship insecurity only captures one facet of a multifaceted
in organizing caregiving activities and opportunities within the attachment system. Because cognitive, affective, and behavioral
family (Allen & Hawkins, 1999; Parke, 2002), mothers are theo- components of the attachment system also operate outside of
rized to act as gatekeepers who increase or decrease (a) paternal conscious awareness (Mikulincer et al., 2002), obtaining more
access to opportunities to acquire parenting skills from them and comprehensive assessments of adult relationship security through
(b) paternal autonomy to take on caregiving responsibilities for semistructured interview or observation techniques is an important
their children (Grossman, Pollack, & Golding, 1988). High levels direction for future research (e.g., Crowell et al., 2002; Treboux et
of interparental conflict may be associated with diminished pater- al., 2004; Wampler, Riggs, & Kimball, 2004). Third, as is common
nal involvement as mothers in distressed marriages become in- in lagged panel designs (e.g., Cui, Donnellan, & Conger, 2007),
creasingly reluctant to collaborate with fathers in caregiving re- the magnitude of the mediational pathways in our study was
sponsibilities (De Luccie, 1995). However, because paternal relatively modest. However, comparable effect sizes in prior re-
INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT AND PARENTING 1771

search are still regarded as having considerable practical and Becker, T. E., Billings, R. S., Eveleth, D. M., & Gilbert, N. W. (1997).
theoretical implications (for a discussion, see Cui et al., 2007). Validity of scores on three attachment style scales: Exploratory and
As a further qualification in interpreting the results, the focus on confirmatory evidence. Educational and Psychological Measurement,
examining mediational pathways between interparental conflict, 57, 477– 493.
adult depressive symptoms and relationship insecurity, and par- Belsky, J., Gilstrap, B., & Rovine, M. (1984). The Pennsylvania Infant and
Family Development Project: I. Stability and change in mother–infant
enting difficulties does not reduce the plausibility of other patterns
and father–infant interaction in a family setting at one, three, and nine
of family process. For example, although the results support the months. Child Development, 55, 692–705.
role of interparental conflict as a predictor of depressive symptoms Belsky, J., Youngblade, L., Rovine, M., & Volling, B. (1991). Patterns of
and adult relationship insecurity, previous research underscores marital change and parent– child interaction. Journal of Marriage and
other patterns of directionality between these family characteris- the Family, 53, 487– 498.
tics. For example, consistent with prior empirical documentation Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent– child attachment and healthy
of concurrent and prospective associations (Carnelley et al., 1994; human development. New York: Basic Books.
J. Feeney et al., 2003; Roberts et al., 1996), maternal relationship Carnelley, K. B., Pietromonaco, P. R., & Jaffe, K. (1994). Depression,
insecurity predicted increases in their depressive symptoms over a working models of others, and relationship functioning. Journal of
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 127–140.


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

1-year period. Likewise, research has also shown that adults ex-
hibiting elevated depressive symptoms experience subsequent in- Christensen, A., & Margolin, G. (1988). Conflict and alliance in distressed
and non-distressed families. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde
creases in distress in their marital relationships (Davies, Dumenci,
(Eds.), Relationships within families: Mutual influences (pp. 283–310).
& Windle, 1999; Kouros, Papp, & Cummings, 2008). In addition,
Oxford, England: Clarendon.
because adult relationship insecurity is likely to be one component Cole, D. A., & Maxwell, S. E. (2003). Testing mediational models with
of a multitude of mediational pathways involved in spillover, longitudinal data: Questions and tips in the use of structural equation
testing the role of other attributes (e.g., maternal gatekeeping, modeling. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 112, 558 –577.
paternal involvement) as explanatory mechanisms and broadening Collins, N. L., & Read, S. J. (1990). Adult attachment, working models,
the conceptual scope of spillover to incorporate more supportive and relationship quality in dating couples. Journal of Personality and
and constructive relationship and emotional processes are impor- Social Psychology, 58, 644 – 663.
tant directions for future research (Simons, Lorenz, Wu, & Conger, Cowan, C. P., Cowan, P. A., Pruett, M. K., & Pruett, K. (2007). An
1993). approach to preventing coparenting conflict and divorce in low-income
In conclusion, our study constitutes a first step toward beginning to families: Strengthening couple relationships and fostering fathers’ in-
elucidate the affective underpinnings of the spillover hypothesis—that volvement. Family Process, 46, 109 –121.
Cowan, P. A., Cohn, D., Cowan, C. P., & Pearson, J. L. (1996). Parents’
is, the cumulative transmission of difficulties from the interparen-
attachment histories and children’s internalizing and externalizing be-
tal to the parent– child relationship. Over the relatively lengthy
havior: Exploring family systems models of linkage. Journal of Con-
developmental period of 2 years, interparental conflict was a sulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 53– 63.
significant predictor of subsequent parental difficulties only for Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002). Interventions as tests of family
fathers. Consistent with evolutionary models of parenting, autore- systems theories: Marital and family relationships in children’s devel-
gressive SEM models indicated that greater paternal susceptibility opment and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 14,
to affective forms of parenting problems (i.e., psychological con- 731–759.
trol, insensitivity to child negative affect) following experiences Coyne, J. C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression.
with interparental discord were mediated by increases in their Psychiatry, 39, 28 – 40.
interparental relationship insecurity. Crowell, J. A., Treboux, D., Gao, Y., Fyffe, C., Pan, H., & Waters, E.
(2002). Assessing secure base behavior in adulthood: Development of a
measure, links to adult attachment representations, and relations to
References couples’ communication and reports of relationships. Developmental
Ainsworth, M. D. (1985). Attachment across the lifespan. Bulletin of the Psychology, 38, 679 – 693.
New York Academy of Medicine, 61, 792– 812. Cui, M., Donnellan, M., B., & Conger, R. D. (2007). Reciprocal influences
Allen, S. M., & Hawkins, A. J. (1999). Maternal gatekeeping: Mothers’ between parents’ marital problems and adolescent internalizing and
beliefs and behaviors that inhibit greater father involvement in family externalizing behavior. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1544 –1552.
work. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61, 199 –212. Cummings, E. M., & Davies. P. T. (1994). Maternal depression and child
Almeida, D. M., Wethington, E., & Chandler, A. L. (1999). Daily trans- development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 73–122.
mission of tensions between marital dyads and parent– child dyads. Cummings, E. M., Goeke-Morey, M. C., & Raymond, J. (2004). Fathers in
Journal of Marriage and the Family, 61(1), 49 – 61. family context: Effects of marital quality and marital conflict. In M. E.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2006). Amos 7.0 user’s guide. Chicago: SPSS. Lamb (Ed.), The role of the father in child development (4th ed., pp.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable 196 –221). New York: Wiley.
distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and Cummings, E. M., & O’Reilly, A. W. (1997). Fathers and family context:
statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Effects of marital quality on child adjustment. In M. E. Lamb (Ed.), The
51, 1173–1182. role of fathers in child development (3rd ed., pp. 49 – 65). New York:
Bartholomew, K., & Horowitz, L. M. (1991). Attachment styles among Wiley.
adults: A test of a four-category model. Journal of Personality and Davies, P. T., Dumenci, L., & Windle, M. (1999). The interplay between
Social Psychology, 61, 226 –244. maternal depressive symptoms and marital distress in the prediction of
Beach, S. R. H., Katz, J., Kim, S., & Brody, G. H. (2003). Prospective adolescent adjustment problems. Journal of Marriage and the Family,
effects of marital satisfaction on depressive symptoms in established 61, 238 –254.
marriages: A dyadic model. Journal of Social and Personal Relation- Davies, P. T., & Lindsay, L. L. (2001). Does gender moderate the effects
ships, 20, 355–371. of marital conflict on children? In J. H. Grych & F. D. Fincham (Eds.),
1772 DAVIES, STURGE-APPLE, WOITACH, AND CUMMINGS

Interparental conflict and child development: Theory, research, and Parents’ reactions to elementary school children’s negative emotions:
applications (pp. 64 –97). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Relations to social and emotional functioning at school. Merrill-Palmer
Press. Quarterly, 48, 133–159.
Davies, P. T., & Sturge-Apple, M. L. (2007). Advances in the formulation Jouriles, E. N., & Farris, A. M. (1992). Effects of marital conflict on
of emotional security theory: An ethological perspective. Advances in subsequent parent–son interactions. Behavior Therapy, 23, 355–374.
Child Development and Behavior, 35, 87–137. Kerig, P. K. (1996). Assessing the links between interparental conflict and
De Luccie, M. F. (1995). Mothers as gatekeepers: A model of maternal child adjustment: The Conflict and Problem Solving Scales. Journal of
mediators of father involvement. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 156, Family Psychology, 10, 454 – 473.
115–131. Kerig, P. K. (1998). Moderators and mediators of the effects of interpa-
Dix, T. (1991). The affective organization of parenting: Adaptive and rental conflict on children’s adjustment. Journal of Abnormal Child
maladaptive processes. Psychological Bulletin, 110, 3–25. Psychology, 26, 199 –212.
Draper, P. (1989). African marriage systems: Perspectives from evolution- Kitzmann, K. M. (2000). Effects of marital conflict on subsequent triadic
ary ecology. Ethology & Sociobiology, 10, 145–169. family interactions and parenting. Developmental Psychology, 36, 3–13.
Du Rocher Schudlich, T. D., Papp, L. M., & Cummings, E. M. (2004). Kouros, C. D., Papp, L. M., & Cummings, E. M. (2008). Interrelations and
Relations of husbands and wives’ dysphoria to marital conflict resolu- moderators of longitudinal links between marital satisfaction and de-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tion strategies. Journal of Family Psychology, 18, 171–183. pressive symptoms among couples in established relationships. Journal
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Easterbrooks, M. A., & Emde, R. N. (1988). Marital and parent– child of Family Psychology, 22, 667– 677.
relationships: The role of affect in the family system. In R. A. Hinde & Krishnakumar, A., & Buehler, C. (2000). Interparental conflict and par-
J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships within families: Mutual influ- enting behaviors: A meta-analytic review. Family Relations, 49(1),
ences (pp. 83–103). Oxford, England: Clarendon Press. 25– 44.
Engfer, A. (1988). The interrelatedness of marriage and the mother– child Lindahl, K. M., Clements, M., & Markman, H. (1997). Predicting marital
relationship. In R. A. Hinde & J. Stevenson-Hinde (Eds.), Relationships and parent functioning in dyads and triads: A longitudinal investigation
within families: Mutual influences (pp. 105–118). Oxford, England: of marital processes. Journal of Family Psychology, 11, 139 –151.
Clarendon Press. Lindsey, E. W., MacKinnon-Lewis, C., Campbell, J., Frabutt, J. M., &
Erel, O., & Burman, B. (1995). Interrelatedness of marital relations and Lamb, M. E. (2002). Marital conflict and boys’ peer relationships: The
parent– child relations: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, mediating role of mother–son emotional reciprocity. Journal of Family
118, 108 –132. Psychology, 16, 466 – 477.
Fabes, R. A., Poulin, R. E., Eisenberg, N., & Madden-Derdich, D. A. (2003). Locke, H. J., & Wallace, K. M. (1959). Short marital adjustment tests:
The Coping with Children’s Negative Emotions Scales (CCNES): Psycho- Their reliability and validity. Marriage and Family Living, 21, 251–255.
metric properties and relations with children’s emotional competence. In Lovejoy, M. C., Graczyk, P. A., O’Hare, E., & Neuman, G. (2000).
R. A. Fabes (Ed.), Emotions and the family (pp. 285–310). Binghamton, Maternal depression and parenting behavior: A meta-analytic review.
NY: Hawthorne Press. Clinical Psychology Review, 20, 561–592.
Fauber, R., Forehand, R., Thomas, A. M., & Wierson, M. (1990). A MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., &
mediational model of the impact of marital conflict on adolescent Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other
adjustment in intact and divorced families: The role of disrupted par- intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83–104.
enting. Child Development, 61, 1112–1123. Mahoney, A., Boggio, R. M., & Jouriles, E. N. (1996). Effects of verbal
Feeney, B. C., & Collins, N. L. (2001). Predictors of caregiving in adult marital conflict on subsequent mother–son interactions in a child clinical
intimate relationship: An attachment theoretical perspective. Journal of sample. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 25, 262–271.
Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 972–994. Malik, N. M., & Lindahl, K. M. (2004). Systems for coding interactions in
Feeney, J., Alexander, R., Noller, P., & Hohaus, L. (2003). Attachment dyads (SCID). In P. K. Kerig & D. H. Baucom (Eds.), Couple obser-
insecurity, depression, and the transition to parenthood. Personal Rela- vational coding systems (pp. 173–188). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
tionships, 10, 475– 493. Margolies, P. J., & Weintraub, S. (1977). The revised 56-item CRPBI as a
Fincham, F. D., Beach, S. R. H., Harold, G. T., & Osborne, L. N. (1997). research instrument: Reliability and factor structure. Journal of Clinical
Marital satisfaction and depression: Different causal relationships for Psychology, 33, 472– 476.
men and women? Psychological Science, 8, 351–357. Mikulincer, M., & Florian, V. (1998). The relationship between adult
Geary, D. C. (2000). Evolution and proximate expression of human pater- attachment styles and emotional and cognitive reactions to stressful
nal investment. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 55–77. events. In J. A. Simpson & W. S. Rholes (Eds.), Attachment theory and
Geary, D. C., & Flinn, M. V. (2001). Evolution of human parental behavior close relationships (pp. 143–165). New York: Guilford Press.
and the human family. Parenting, 1, 5– 61. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., Cowan, P. A., & Cowan, C. P. (2002).
Gerard, J. M., Krishnakumar, A., & Buehler, C. (2006). Marital conflict, Attachment security in couple relationships: A systemic model and its
parent– child relations, and youth maladjustment. Journal of Family implications for family dynamics. Family Process, 41, 405– 434.
Issues, 27, 951–975. Mikulincer, M., Florian, V., & Tolmacz, R. (1990). Attachment styles and
Gollob, H. F., & Reichardt, C. S. (1987). Taking account of time lags in fear of personal death: A case study of affect regulation. Journal of
causal models. Child Development, 58, 80 –92. Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 273–280.
Grossman, F. K., Pollack, W. S., & Golding, E. (1988). Fathers and Mikulincer, M., & Goodman, G. S. (2006). Dynamics of romantic love:
children: Predicting the quality and quantity of fathering. Developmental Attachment, caregiving, and sex. New York: Guilford Press.
Psychology, 24, 82–91. Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Gillath, O., & Nitzberg, R. A. (2005).
Harold, G. T., & Conger, R. D. (1997). Marital conflict and adolescent Attachment, caregiving, and altruism: Boosting attachment security in-
distress: The role of adolescent awareness. Child Development, 68, creases compassion and helping. Journal of Personality and Social
333–350. Psychology, 89, 817– 839.
Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an Owen, M. T., & Cox, M. J. (1997). Marital conflict and the development
attachment process. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, of infant–parent attachment relationships. Journal of Family Psychol-
511–524. ogy, 11, 152–164.
Jones, S., Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & MacKinnon, D. P. (2002). Parke, R. D. (2002). Fathers and families. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.),
INTERPARENTAL CONFLICT AND PARENTING 1773

Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Being and becoming a parent (2nd ed., unavailability and children’s adjustment difficulties. Child Development,
pp. 27–74). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 77, 1623–1641.
Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale. Applied Psychological Measure- Thompson, L., & Walker, A. J. (1989). Gender in families: Women and
ment, 1, 385– 401. men in marriage, work, and parenthood. Journal of Marriage and the
Rholes, W. S., Simpson, J. A., & Friedman, M. (2006). Avoidant attach- Family, 51, 845– 871.
ment and the experience of parenting. Personality and Social Psychol- Treboux, D., Crowell, J. A., & Waters, E. (2004). When “new” meets “old”:
ogy Bulletin, 32, 275–285. Configurations of adult attachment representations and their implications for
Roberts, J. E., Gotlib, I. H., & Kassel, J. D. (1996). Adult attachment marital functioning. Developmental Psychology, 40, 295–314.
security and symptoms of depression: The mediating roles of dysfunc- Wampler, K. S., Riggs, B., & Kimball, T. G. (2004). Observing attachment
tional attitudes and low self-esteem. Journal of Personality and Social behavior in couples: The Adult Attachment Behavior Q-Set (AABQ).
Psychology, 70, 310 –320. Family Process, 43, 315–335.
Watson, D., & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health, complaints, stress, and
Sacco, W. P., & Vaughan, C. A. (2006). Depression and the response of
distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological
others: A social– cognitive interpersonal process model. In T. E. Joiner,
Review, 96, 234 –254.
J. S. Brown, and J. Kisner (Eds.), The interpersonal, cognitive, and
Weissman, M. M., Sholomskas, D., Pottenger, M., Prusoff, B. A., & Locke,
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

social nature of depression (pp. 101–132). New York: Routledge.


B. Z. (1977). Assessing depressive symptoms in five psychiatric popu-
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Schludermann, E., & Schludermann, S. (1970). Replicability of factors in


lations: A validation study. American Journal of Epidemiology, 106,
children’s reports of parent behavior. Journal of Psychology, 76, 239 – 203–214.
249. Whiffen, V. E., Kallos-Lilly, A. V., & MacDonald, B. J. (2001). Depres-
Simons, R. L., Lorenz, F. O., Wu, C., & Conger, R. D. (1993). Social sion and attachment in couples. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25,
network and marital support as mediators and moderators of the impact 577–590.
of stress and depression on parental behavior. Developmental Psychol- Whisman, M. A. (2001). The association between depression and marital
ogy, 29, 368 –381. dissatisfaction. In S. R. H. Beach (Ed.), Marital and family processes in
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Darling, N., Mounts, N. S., & Dornbusch, depression: A scientific foundation for clinical practice (pp. 3–24).
S. M. (1994). Over-time changes in adjustment and competence among Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
adolescents from authoritative, authoritarian, indulgent, and neglectful
families. Child Development, 65, 754 –770. Received June 5, 2008
Sturge-Apple, M. L., Davies, P. T., & Cummings, E. M. (2006). Impact of Revision received January 22, 2009
hostility and withdrawal in interparental conflict on parental emotional Accepted April 8, 2009 䡲

Potrebbero piacerti anche