Sei sulla pagina 1di 9

Journal of Ctmsulting and Clinical I%ychology ~ t 1987 by the American Psycholo8ical Association, Inc.

1987, Vol. 55, No. 6, 889-897 0022-006X/87/$00.75

Cognitive-Behavioral Treatment of Women's


Body-Image Dissatisfaction
Jonathan W. Butters T h o m a s E Cash
Harlem Valley Psychiatric Center Old Dominion University

Body-image dissatisfaction is a problem that affectsa substantial minority ofwomen and cuts across
various diagnostic groups. Collegewomen with a significant level ofbody-image dissatisfaction were
randomly assigned to either a cognitive-behavioral treatment (CBT) program (n = 15) or to a wait-
ing-list control group (n = 16). The CBT program consisted of six structured, individual sessions
that applied cognitive-behavioral procedures to the problem of negative body image. At pretest,
posttest, and 7-week foUow-up,multiple aspects of body image and other areas of psychosocial func-
tioning were assessed. Relative to the control condition, the CBT program successfully improved
affectivebody image, weakened maladaptive body-image cognitions, and enhanced social self-esteem
and feelingsabout physical fitness and sexuality. Treatment effectswere largely maintained at follow-
up. After posttest, the control group received a 3-week treatment with immediate effects that gener-
ally replicated those obtained in the 6-wer program.

A substantial amount of research literature (cf. Cash, 1981, ally quite prevalent in the general population. A recent large-
1985; Hatfield & Sprecher, 1986) bas verified that one's objec- sample national survey (Cash et al., 1986) of adolescents and
tive level of physical attractiveness can influence one's psycho- adults indicated that 31% of female respondents and 24% of
social experiences and deveiopment. The cumulative adverse maie respondents reported negative attitudes about their physi-
effects of unattractiveness may undermine one's mental health cal appearance. For both sexes, dissatisfaction was strongest
(Archer & Cash, 1985; Cash, 1985; Farina et al., 1977; Farina, with weight-sensitive body areas; for example, the midtorso
Burns, Austad, Bugglin, & Fischer, 1986; O'Grady, 1982). How- area elicited dissatisfaction for 57% of the women and 50% of
ever, individuals' self-perceptions of their physical appearance the men. This survey also round that a negative body image was
bear only modest resemblance to reliable objective perceptions clearly associated with lower levels of self-reported psychosocial
of their physical attractiveness, and self-perceptions bave even well-being. Comparisons with earlier survey data (Berscheid,
more salient implications for mental health (Cash, 1985; Noies, Walster, & Bohrnstedt, 1973) have pointed to an increase in the
Cash, & Winstead, 1985). problem over the past decade (Cash et al., 1986).
Among clinical and nonclinical samples, both men and Despite the growing prevalence of body-image problems and
women who perceive themselves as physically unattractive or, their clinical significance, researchers have largely neglected to
more generaUy, who report negative or dysphoric attitudes to- develop and evaluate therapeutic interventions for the improve-
ward their physical appearance (i.e., a negative body image) ment of a dysfunctional body image. A recent review (Butters,
typically bave been found to have poor self-esteem, social anxie- 1985) revealed that among the 18 studies evaluating a wide
ries and inhibitions, sexual difflculties, and a vulnerability to range of treatments for the improvement of body image, no
depression (e.g., Archer & Cash, 1985; Cash, 1985; Cash, Cash, study met ail three methodological criteria of (a) including a
& Butters, 1983; Cash, Winstead, & Janda, 1986; Lerner, Orlos, control group, (b) randomly assigning subjects, and (c) collect-
& Knapp, 1976; Noies et al., 1985; Rosen & Ross, 1968; Secord ing follow-up data. Ofthe seven studies that did include a con-
& Jourard, 1953). Moreover, persons who exhibit disordered trol group, rive round that the interventions failed to produce
eating behaviors ofien have dysfunctional perceptions of their significantly different effects on body-image indices. Moreover,
physical appearance (Cash & Brown, in press; Garfinkel & Gar- researchers have often relied on a single measure of body image
ner, 1982). The problem of body-image dissatisfaction is actu- and have failed to recognize the multidimensional nature of the
body-image construct (Cash & Brown, in press; Keeton, Cash,
& Lewis, 1987; McCrea, Summerfield, & Rosen, 1982).
This study was completed by the first author under the supervision of Accordingly, the present controlled investigation examined
the second author in partial fulfillment of requirements for the doctor the effectiveness of treatments designed to ameliorate negative
of psychology degree from the Virginia Consortium for Professional body image. Because a dysphoric body image is more closely
Psycbology.The autbors wish to thank Timothy Brown, Gary Cundiff, linked with appearance-related cognitions than with physical
Lisa Lee, and Julie Shelton for serving as therapists. A previous version
realities (Cash, 1985; Cash et al., 1986), the problem seemed to
ofthis article was presented at the 1985 convention ofthe Association
for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy. be a suitable application of cognitive-behavior therapy. Fur-
Correspondence concerning this article sbould be addressed to thermore, cognitive-behavioral techniques have demonstrated
Thomas E Cash, Department of Psychology,OId Dominion University, therapeutic eflicacy for a variety of dysphoric conditions (Kaz-
Norfolk, Virginia 23508. din & Wilson, 1978; Kendall & Hollon, 1979; Miller & Berman,

889
890 JONATHAN W. BUTTERS AND THOMAS E CASH

1983) known to be associated with a negative body image (e.g., vention, elicited from the client what was most personally meaningful,
depression, interpersonal anxieties, sexual dysfunctions, eating and assigned a homework task.
disorders). Therefore, the present study describes the design and Experimental treatment group. In Session 1, clients received infor-
evaluation o f two brief cognitive-behavioral treatment pro- mation concerning the problem ofbody-image dissatisfaction (e.g., pos-
sible causes, prevalence, effects on personal happiness) and the underly-
grains for body-image dissatisfaction a m o n g young women,
ing rationale for the treatment program. In the remainder of the session,
who are especially likely to report a negative body image (Ber-
audiotaped instructions taught clients progressive muscle relaxation
scheid et al., 1973; Cash et al., 1986; Fallon & Rozin, 1985). A plus the use of autogenic suggestions and pleasant imagery (Goldfried
6-week, individual cognitive-behavioral treatment was com- & Davison, 1976). The importance of relaxation for managing body-
pared with a waiting-list control condition whose members image dysphoria was emphasized. Each client was given a copy of the
were subsequently treated in an abbreviated 3-week program. A tape and was assigned to practice relaxation daily during the week.
7-week foUow-up o f the initial treatment was carried out. Focal In Session 2, clients constructed a personalized hierarchy of rive body
effects on multiple body-image parameters, including affective parts or areas, ranging from those with which the client was moderately
and cognitive components, and generalization to other aspects satisfied to those with which she was least satisfied. Following a detailed
ofpsychosocial functioning were evaluated. script, the therapist then guided the client through imaginal systematic
desensitization (Goldfried & Davison, 1976; Wolpe, 1973) for her anxi-
ety concerning body areas and the body as a whole. The idea of desensi-
Method tization as a method for taking control (Goldfried & Davison, 1976)
of body-image feelings was emphasized, and daily practice of imaginal
Subjects desensitization was assigne&
Session 3 stressed applying the techniques to real-life situations. The
Sixteen undergraduate psychology classes containing 186 female stu- therapist guided in vivo desensitization in which the client Iooked at
dents were tested using the Body-self Relations Questionnaire (BSRQ; herself in a full-length mirror in the soundproof room. The assigned
Winstead & Cash, 1984) and the Symptom Checklist-90-Revised (SCL- homework was the practice of in vivo desensitization privately at home,
90-R; Derogatis, 1977). ~ A project description and a form to request clothed and unclothed, in front o f a mirror. Also, the client was intro-
body-image therapy wr also included. Women who met ail of the fol- duced to the concept of automatic thoughts (Beck, 1976) and was asked
Iowing inclusionary and exclusionary cr9 were telephoned: (a) a de- to monitor and record her automatic thoughts as she Iooked at her body
sire to receive body-image therapy; (b) a score below the 25th percentile in the mirror.
ofthe BSRQ Appearance Evaluation subscale norms for college women In Session 4, the focus was on understanding the foie that thinidng
(Winstead & Cash, 1984); (c) T scores under 70 on both the Psychoti- plays in maintaining body-image dissatisfaction. The A-B-C (anteced-
cism and Paranoid Ideation scales of the SCk-90-R (to exclude more ents--beliefs--emotional consequences) model of Ellis (1962) was ar-
severely disturbed persons); (d) sr weight that was not more ticulated, and the concepts of irrational beliefs (Eliis, 1962) and cogni-
than 25% above or below the median desirable weight for her height tive errors (Beck, 1976) were presented on audiotape. Several cognitive
(Metropolitan Life Insurance tables, 1983); (e) denials of a serious medi- errors regarding appearance were detailed, and clients identified those
cal condition or of current receipt of counseling or psychotherapy. Of that were personally applicable. The therapist taught the client rational
the 186 women tested, 36 met ail eriteria, and from these 32 were ran- counterarguments to combat her cognitive errors concerning appear-
domly selected. ance. Homework required the subject to write several counterargu-
The subjects' average age was 21.3 years (SI) = 5.0). Thirty subjects ments for the beliefs covered in the session. In addition, the subject was
were White, 1 was Black, and 1 was Asian. They were informed that taught and assigned Beck's (1976) triple-column technique to use
due to the program's popularity and the number of therapists available, whenever she felt dissatisfied with some aspect ofher appearance: (a) to
halfofthe participants would begin therapy in a week and the other half monitor negative body-image affect in specific situations, (b) to identify
would start midway through the semester. Subjects then drew lots to the automatic thoughts and cognitive errors that occur, and (c) to dis-
be randomly assigned to one of the two conditions and to one of four pute or correct the thoughts with counterarguments and to monitor the
therapists. AU but one subject participated for the study's duration. This affective ontcome.
subject, assigned to the experimental group, was dropped from the In Session 5, the homework was reviewed and the cognitive therapy
study after one session and was referred to a therapist due to reports of concepts and their practical applications were further elaborated and
suicidal ideation. rehearsed. An audiotape discussed the notions of engaging in body-re-
lated behaviors that give one a sense ofmastery or pleasure (Beck, 1976)
and viewiag the body hOt only as an aesthetic object but also for its
Procedure capabilities (e.~, fimess, sensory experience, health). For homework,
clients completed the Body-Related Activities Inventory (modeled after
Four clinical psychology doctoral students (two men and two women) the Pleasant Events Sehedule; MacPhillamy & Lewinsohn, 1974), rat-
served as trained therapists in the study. Therapists did not conduct th• ing the frequency oftheir involvement in various physical activities and
research assessments and were blind to subjects' data. Each therapist their feelings of personal mastery and pleasure associated with each.
was assignr to treat 4 subjects in the experimental gronp and 4 subjects During the week, clients were assigned at ieast three such activities.
in the treated control group. In Session 6, after the homework was reviewed, an audiotape reviewed
Individuat treatment consistr of structured l-hr weekly sessions. the skills taught in the program plus several strategies for posttreatment
Each session began with the therapist's 10-15-min meeting with the maintenance of a more positive body image. These included the use of
client to establish rapport, to review any homework 8 the prior week, stress-inoculation techniques (Meichenbaum, 1985) and relapse-pre-
and to introduce the session's topics. The client was then escorted to a vention strategies (Marlatt & Gordon, 1985) when confronted with an
soundproof room where ail auditory stimulation could be controlled by
the therapist from an adjacent room. While comfortably reclined in a
chair, the client received an approximate 30-min intervention that var- Additional information and materials concerning treatment pro-
ied in focus from session to session. Finally, the subject returned to the grams and assessment instruments are available by request from the
consulting room where the therapist reviewed the highlights of the inter- second author.
BODY-IMAGE TREATMENT 891

apparent failure to cope with negative body-image thoughts and feel- Woipe & I.azarus, 1966) from 0 to 100 (100 = extreme distress). She
ings. FinaUy, the therapist and client reviewed her progress in the pro- was instructed to not fixate her gaze on any one body feature but to
grain and discussed coping strategies tailored to her needs. look at ail ofher body features.
The high degree of structure in the program (i.e., liberal use of audio- Photo self-ratings. Color photographs of individual subjects were
taped interventions, written handouts for clients, plus specific written taken at the first assessment session using a Polaroid camera, with each
scripts for each session and each therapist) assured the delivery of a subjeet posed with a smile for a standard frontal, fuU-body photograph.
consistent treatment package across clients and therapists. Each client At eaeh assessment, each subject viewed her (pretest) photograph and
attended ail six sessions. Checks on the completion oftherapeutic home- rated how attractive, on a 9-point scale (9 = extremelyphysically attrac-
work assignments further verified clients' treatment compliance. tive), she expeeted peers would rate her photograph. This measure (Self-
Waiting-list control (WLC) group. Control subjects were pre- and PA) refleets subjeets' beliefs about how others view their physical attrac-
posttested on the same schedule as the CBT group and began treatment tiveness.
a week alter the second assessment. This treated waiting-list control Observers" ratings of physical attractiveness. To determine how oth-
group (TWLC) received an abbreviated, 3-week treatment rather than ers actually rated subjects' attractiveness, the four therapists, the experi-
the full CBT treatment because of insuflicient time in the semester to menter, and the second author independently rated subjects' photo-
conduct two 6-week treatments plus assessments. FoUowing their sec- graphs using the same 9-point scale of physical attractiveness. This
ond assessment session, these individuals were asked to spend lime prior truth-by-consensus method is the standard operational definition used
to their first treatment session looking at themselves in the mirror, rating in physical attractiveness research (Cash, 198 l; Hatfield & Sprecher,
their level of distress, and monitoring automatic thoughts as explained 1986). Only the experimenter had met subjects at the lime ofhis ratings,
in a handout. Each TWLC client received an extended 90-min flrst ses- but he had not seen subjects' Self-PA ratings. The Cronbach's alpha for
sion in order to prefacr the initial cognitive treatment session with the judges' ralings was 0.88, indicating high interrater reliability.
audiotape of the program's rationale. Other than these differences and Body Image Detection Device (BIDD). This overhead projector ap-
a change in what was reviewed during the final session (because the paratus provides an inexpensive and simple measure of an individual's
TWLC group had not received relaxation training or desensitization), estimation ofthe width ofher body at different points (sec Ruff& Bar-
the t h r ~ TWLC sessions were idenlical to Sessions 4, 5, and 6 for the rios, 1986). When these estimations are compared to the actual widths,
CBT group.
a measure of body-size estimation accuracy is obtained. In addition, a
measure is obtained of how the subject judges her widths to compare
with those of most women her age and height (the Subjective Rating
Assessment Instruments a n d Measures Index, SRI). In the absence of available interjudge reliability data for
actuai body widths in this study, however, only the SRI is reported. For
Ail subjects were assessed during Weeks 1, 8, and 13. Thus, the CBT
this assessment, the subject rated her subjective appraisal of the width
and TWLC groups' posttests were held 2 wer after the final treatment
of her face, chest, waist, hips, and thighs on scales of 0 (grossly br
session (to reduce demand eharacteristics); and follow-up assessment
the normal physical dimension given her height, age, and gender) to 100
was held 7 weeks afler the CBT group's final treatment session. AH as-
sessments afler mass testing were conducted on an individual basis by (grossly above the norm), with 50 representing normal physical dimen-
the first author, who was n o t a therapist in the study. Honest responding sions. The SRI measure is caiculated for each body area as (subjective
was stressed, and subjeet code numbers were used. Subjects were in- ratingJ50) • 100 and is averaged across the rive areas (Ruff& Barrios,
formed that the therapists would not see these data. 1986). Thus, the SRI taps a cognitive aspect of body image--beliefs
Body--SelfRelations Questionnaire (BSRQ). The BSRQ (Cash et al., about size normativeness.
1986; Winstead & Cash, i 984) contains 140 items rated on 5-point Lik- PersonalAppearance Beliefs Test (PABT). This instrument contains
ert-type scales. Itis an attitudinal measure of body image that yields rive items developed for the present study to measure the extent to which
data on three attitudinal dimensions (Evaluation, Attention/Impor- individuals subscribe to presumably dysfunctional beliefs about their
tance, Activity) for three different somatic domains (Appearance, Fit- appearance. Items were constructed to conform to the specific cognitive
ness, Health) plus a Sexuality Evaluation score. The four subscales per- errors described by Beck, Rush, Shaw, and Emery (1979) or to other
tinent to the study's hypothesized effects and their internal consisteneies cognitive processes known to diminish body image (Cash et al., 1983).
(Cronbach's alphas) in the present study were Appearanee Evaluation Subjects made 5-point Likert-type ratings of the extent of their agree-
(18 items), 0.82; Appearanoe Attention/Importance (20 items), 0.89; ment with these statements: (a) "If there are people around who are
Fitness Evaluation (19 items), 0.85; and Sexuality Evaluation (7 items), more attractive than I, then I don't feel attractive" (social comparison);
0.70. Evaluation scales are scored in the positive direction; higher Ap- (b) " I f I feel unattractive, it usually means that I don't look attractive"
pearance Attention/Importance scores reflect greater cognitive focus on (emotional reasoning); (c) "IfI'm not beautiful, then I must be unattrac-
one's physieal appearance. Previous research has attested to the tempo- tive" (dichotomous thinking); (d) "If there's one flaw in my appextrance
rai stability and the convergent, discriminant, and construct validity of then I don't feel attractive" (magnification); and (e) "How I feel about
these BSRQ subscales (e.g., Cash & Green, 1986; Cash et al., 1986; myself is usually related to how 1 feel about my appearance" (selective
Noies et al., 1985; Winstead & Cash, 1984). abstraction).
Body Parts Satisfaction Scale (BPSS). The BPSS (Berscheid et al., Texas Social Behavior Inventory (TSBI). This instrument consists
1973; Bohrnstedt, 1977) is a 25-item measure ofbody satisfaction that of 16 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale (Helmreich & Stapp, 1974).
yields six factor seores relating to satisfaction with particular groups of It produces a single, internally consistent score for social self-esteem
body parts as weU as a single overall appearance satisfaction rating. In (y = 0.91 ), with higher scores reflecting self-perceived confidence, social
addition, an internally consistent (alpha = 0.86) total body parts satis- dominance, and social compr
faction score is obtained by computing the mean of ail 6-point ratings SCL-90-R. The SCL-90-R as a 90-item self-report inventory ofpsy-
(from extremely dissatisfied to extremely satisfied). chological symptoms (Derogatis, ! 977). Each item is rated on a 5-point
Mirror distress rating. This is a measure deveioped specifically for seale to indicate the distress occurring over a specified period. Although
this study to provide a simple, face-valid measure of body-image dis- this instrument can be seored for 10 symptom dimensions, a Global
tress. With the examiner absent, the subject was instructed to gaze at Severity Index (GSI) can be computed as a mean rating across symp-
herself in a full-length mirror for 89min and then to rate the level of toms to provide a composite, internally consistent index of general psy-
anxiety experienced on a scale of subjective units of distress (SUDS; chopathoiogy. At the first ~ m e n t , subjects rated their symptoms
892 JONATHAN W. BUTTERS AND THOMAS E CASH

over the past month, and T scores greater than or equai to 70 on the tion with Face (p < .001), Extremities (p < .001 ), Breasts (p <
Psychoticism and Paranoid Ideation scales were used to screen out se- .001), Midtorso (p < .001), and Lower Torso (p < .006). Thus,
verely disturbed volunteers. Subsequently,the SCL-90-Rwas completed with the exception of the Height factor (whieh was rarely a
in reference to a l-week period, and GSI scores were computed. source of body-image dissatisfaction), the CBT program en-
ClientSatisfactionQuestionnaire(CSQ). The 8-item CSQ is accepta- hanced clients' reported satisfaction with ail bodily regions.
bly reliable and valid as a general measure of client satisfaction with
health and human services (Larsen, Attkisson, Hargreave~ & Nguyen, Analyses ofthe 7-week foUow-up data for the CBT group (sec
1979) and is scored with the composite of 4-point Likert-type scales Table 1) were conducted to evaluate the maintenance of treat-
with higher scores indicating greater satisfaction. The item, "Ifyou were ment gains on body-image affect. As predicted, ail one-way, re-
to seek help again, would you corne back to out program?" was deleted peated-measures ANOVAS were significant (ps < .001), and
because the program was being offered only once. Cronbach's alphas Newman-Keuls eomparisons revealed differences between pre-
were 0.92 for the CBT group and 0.77 for the TWLC group. test and follow-up means but not between posttest and follow-
up means on each ofthe four body-image affeet measures. Sim-
Results ilarly, treatment effeets were maintained for the BPSS factor
stores.
Outcome variables were organized into three conceptual
clusters: measures regarding affective evaluations of appear- C B T Effects on Body-Image Cognitions
anee, measures pertaining to appearanee-related eognitions,
and measures related to other body-image domains or areas of Several ofthe outcome variables were essentially cognitive in
psychosocial functioning. The general strategy for data analysis nature, including (a) beliefs about how physically attractive one
within each eluster involved 2 (CBT vs. WLC) • 2 (pretest vs. is to one's peers (Self-PA); (b) appraisals of the normativeness
posttest) multivariate analyses of variance (MANOVAS using of one's body size (BIDD SRI); (c) cognitive focus (attending
Wilks's lambda criterion) that were followed, where appropri- to, thinking about, and placing importance) on physical appear-
ate, by univariate analyses of variance (ANOVAS) to examine ance (BSRQ Appearance Attention/Importance); and (d) cog-
significant simple effects. These analyses permitted evaluation nitive errors concerning personal appearance (the rive PABT
of any pretest differences between groups and of pre- to posttest items). Means, standard deviations, and effects are presented in
changes for both treatment and control groups. With pretest Table 2.
scores as covariates, analyses of covariance (ANCOW,s) were For the MANOVA and for each of the eight measures, the
used to test between-groups differences at posttest. One-way re- Group • Time effect was significant (p < .03 t o p < .001). Sim-
peated-measures ANOVASfollowed by Newman-Keuls analyses ple effects analyses indicated that, although there were no pre-
evaluated the follow-up maintenance of any significant treat- to posttest changes for the WLC group, the CBT group changed
ment changes. 2 significantly on ail measures. Following treatment, CBT clients
believed that others would find them more attractive (p < .002),
appraised their body size as smaller and doser to the norm
C B T Effects on Body-Image Affect (p < .03), reported less cognitive focus on appearance (p < .03),
Four measures of body-image affect were analyzed to deter- and espoused each cognitive error to a weaker extent (i.e., social
mine the CBT's effeetiveness in altering clients' body-image dis- comparison, p < .001; emotional reasoning, p < .009; dichoto-
satisfaction: the BSRQ Appearance Evaluation subseale, the mous thinking, p < .02; magnification, p < .003; selective ab-
BPSS rating of appearanee satisfaction, the BPSS eomposite straction, p < .002). Comparisons of CBT and WLC groups at
score for body parts satisfaction, and the mirror SUDS rating. posttest were significant for all these cognitive variables, except
Comparing the CBT and WLC groups at pre- and posttest, the for a marginal effeet (p < .07) on dichotomous thinking.
2 • 2 MANOW, yielded a significant (p < .003) Group • Time Additional analysis was carried out to examine the veridical-
interaction, which was also significant for each dependent vari- ity of clients" judgments of their physical attractiveness in rela-
able. Simple effects analyses revealed no between-groups tion to actual observer ratings. Groups did not differ on observ-
differences at pretest and no pre- to posttest changes for the er-rated attractiveness, with CBT subjects averaging 5.2 (SD =
WLC group. 1.0) and WLC subjects averaging 5.0 (SD = 1.4). Discrepancy
As shown in Table 1, the CBT produced significant pre- to scores (PA discrepancy) were calculated by subtracting the ob-
posttest increases on BSRQ Appearance Evaluation (p < .002), server rating from the Self-PA rating and conducting a 2 • 2
BPSS appearance satisfaction (p < .001), and BPSS body parts ANOVA, which produced a significant Group • Time interac-
satisfaction (p < .001) as well as significant reductions in dis- tion (p < .001). The two groups did not differ significantly at
tress on the mirror SUDS ratings (p < .001). Between-groups pretest, when they underestimated their attractiveness (Ms =
ANCOVAcomparisons at posttest indicated that thesr improve- - 0 . 2 0 and - 0 . 4 0 for CBT and WLC groups, respectively), but
ments produced significant (p < .005) differences in relation to differed significantly at posttest (p < .02), with no change for
the WLC group. the control subjects (M = -0.52) and a significant shift to over-
Similar multivariate and univariate analyses were performed estimation (M = 1.33) for treated subjects (p < .002).
to ascertain effects for the six BPSS factor scores: Face, Extremi-
ries, Lower Torso, Midtorso, Height, and Breasts. The results 2 Initial data analyses included the therapist factor as part ofthe de-
indicated significant increases in satisfaction for the CBT group sign. However,becausr the multivariate main and interactive effects of
on ail factors (p < .005) except Height. Relative to the WLC therapists were consistently nonsignificant, the reported analyses were
group at posttest, CBT4reated clients reported greater satisfac- collapsed across this factor.
BODY-IMAGE TREATMENT 893

Table 1
Affective Body-Image Means, Standard Deviations, and Treatment Effects
Assessment time"

1 2 3
Intergroup Intragroup
Measure and condition M SD M SD M SD comparisonsb comparisonsc

BSRQ Appr Evaluation 19.97"**


CBT 2.52 0.37 3.33 0.62 3.24 0.63 16.30***
WLC/TWLC 2.43 0.53 2.50 0.59 3.15 0.54 53.80***
BPSS Appearance Satisfaction 17.24***
CBT 2.83 0.96 4.27 0.80 3.93 0.88 22.25***
WLC]TW'LC 3.00 1.21 3.06 1.18 4.06 1.06 40.00***
BPSS Body Parts Satisfaction 25.81 ***
CBT 3.50 0.57 4.39 0.50 4.28 0.54 24.25***
WLC/TWLC 3.64 0.63 3.66 0.44 4.23 0.66 45.37***
Mirror SUDS rating 12.75"*
CBT 54.1 25.8 19.2 13.6 23.8 19.8 14.59"**
WLCfrWLC 54.2 23.2 43.6 18.9 26.8 20.3 7.64*

Note. BPSS = Body Parts Satisfaction Scale; BSRQ = Body-Self Relations Questionnaire; CBT = cognitive-behavioral treatment; SUDS = subjec-
tive units ofdistress; TWLC = treated waiting-list eontrol; WLC = waiting-fist control.
a For the CBT group, Assessments 1, 2, and 3 were pretest, posttest, and follow-up. For the WLC group, Assessments 1 and 2 were pre- and posttest
and Assessment 3 was posttest for the treated control subjects, b Comparisons between CBT and WLC groups at posttest were adjusted for pretest
stores (df= 1, 28). c For the CBT group, pre- and posttest and follow-up were compared (df= 2, 28). For the TWLC group, pre- and posttest were
compared ( d f = 1, i 5).
*p<.05. **p<.Ol. ***p<.O01.

Repeated-measures analyses that included follow-up data in- levels (ps < .01) but did not differ from posttest. No change
dicated that ail significant outcomes were maintained, without from posttest was evident for the GSI measure.
further gains or subsequent losses, on the cognitive variables,
except for a decline (p < .01) in Self-PA rating (and in the auto- Client Satisfaction With the CBT Program
correlated PA discrepancy index). Although hOt significantly
different from baseline, Self-PA ratings were somewhat more On the 7-item CSQ, the CBT group rated the program with
accurate at follow-up ( M = 0.20) than at the posttest level of a mean of 3.5 (SD = 0.5) on a 4-point scale. Specifically, 93%
overestimation. rated the services good to excellent, 100% definitely or generally
received the services desired, 80% indicated their needs were
Generalization of CBT Effects met, 87% would recommend the program to friends, 87% were
satisfied with the help received, 93% felt they were dealing more
Four measures concerned the generalization of treatment effectively with their body-image problems, and 100% reported
effects beyond appearance-related parameters to self-evalua- overall satisfaction with the service they obtained. Client satis-
tions of physical fitness, sexuality, social self-esteem, and gen- faction was maintained at follow-up.
eral psychosocial functioning. Respectively, these effects were
examined on the BSRQ Fitness Evaluation and Sexuality Eval-
Effects of Treatment of the Control Group
uation subscales, the TSBI, and the SCL-90-R (see Table 3). Be-
cause of differences in the administration of the SCL-90-R at The abbreviated treatment of the control group (i.e., the
pretest screening and other assessments, comparisons between TWLC group) was evaluated for each set of measures by one-
these particular administrations could not be made. way MANOVASand ANOVAS comparing clients at pretest (sec-
A multivariate Group x Time effect was significant (p < .03), ond assessment) versus posttest (third assessment) and compar-
and 2 • 2 ANOVASyielded significant interaction effects on Fit- ing posttests ofthe two treated groups. Regarding the latter, no
ness Evaluation (p < .002), Sexuality Evaluation (p < .001), posttest differenees between CBT and TWLC groups were
and TSBI (p < .005). Analyses of simple effects revealed sig- found. However, the 3-week treatment did produee significant
nificant pre- to posttest differences for the CBT group but not multivariate changes in each set of measures.
for the WLC group on Fitness Evaluation (p < .001), Sexuality For body-image affect (see Table 1), ail four measures re-
Evaluation (p < .001), and TSBI (p < .001). Between-groups flected improvement: BSRQ Appearance Evaluation (p <
posttest comparisons were also significant for Fitness Evalua- .001), BPSS appearance satisfaction (p < .001), BPSS body
tion (p < .006), for Sexuality Evaluation (p < .003), and for parts satisfaction (p < .001), and mirror SUDS (p < .02). The
TSBI (p < .02). Groups failed to differ on GSI posttest scores brief treatment increased satisfaction on rive of the six BPSS
o f the SCL-90-R. factor scores (ps < .01) and showed a nonsignifieant trend for
Maintenance of the three observed effects for the CBT group the Height factor (p < .09). On the cognitive measures (see Ta-
was supported at foUow-up, when stores differed from pretest ble 2), the TWLC group showed several signifieant changes in
894 JONATHAN W. BUTI'ERS AND THOMAS F. CASH

Table 2
Cognitive Body-Image Means, Standard Deviations, and Treatment Effects
Assessmenttime"

1 2 3
Intergroup Intragroup
Measure and condition M SD M SD M SD comparisonsb comparisonsc

Self-PArating 24.61 ***


CBT 5.00 1.31 6.53 1.12 5.40 1.92 8.56**
WLC/TWLC 4.56 1.71 4.44 1.90 5.00 1.75 1.06
BIDD SRI 6.34*
CBT 127.4 15.6 114.9 13.2 119.9 18.3 6.11"*
WLC/TWLC 124.8 25.6 124.1 21.5 116.7 18.3 10.42"*
BSRQ Attention/Importance 8.94**
CBT 4.20 0.36 3.93 0.42 4.01 0.41 4.02*
WLCfrWLC 4.32 0.39 4.34 0.39 4.08 0.36 10.48"*
PABT SocialComparison 15.31"**
CBT 4.20 0.78 2.53 0.92 3.13 1.06 18.76***
WLCfrWLC 4.31 0.87 3.81 0.83 3.00 0.97 18.78"**
PABT Emotional Reasoning 13.43**
CBT 4.00 1.07 2.93 1.03 3.07 1.03 6.49"*
WLCfrWLC 3.88 0.88 4.00 0.63 3.00 1.03 20.00***
PABT Diehotomous Thinking 2.24
CBT 3.00 1.20 2.00 0.93 2.27 1.34 4.34*
WLC/TWLC 2. i 9 1.11 2.12 0.88 1.94 0.85 1.90
PABT Magnification 8.84**
CBT 3.40 1.18 2.20 0.68 2.33 0.90 8.38***
WLC/TWLC 3.19 1.11 3.12 0.96 2.62 1.02 3.75
PABT SeleetiveAbstraction 9.92**
CBT 4.33 0.62 3.27 0.96 3.60 0.74 9.57***
WLCfrWLC 4.19 0.54 4.25 0.78 3.75 0.93 2.50

Note. BIDD SRI = Body Image Detection Deviee SubjectiveRating Index; BSRQ = Body-Self Relations Questionnaire; CBT = cognitive-behav-
ioral treatment; PABT = Personal Appearanee BeliefsTest; PA = physical attractiveness; TWLC = treated waiting-listcontrol; WLC = waiting-list
control.
"For the CBT group, Assessments 1, 2, and 3 were pretest, posttest, and follow-up. For the WLC group, Assessments I and 2 were pre- and posttest
and Assessment3 was posttest for the treated control subjects, bComparisons between CBT and WLC groups at posttest were adjusted for pretest
stores (df= 1,28). c For the CBT group, pre- and posttest and follow-upwere compared (df= 2, 28). For the TWLC group, pre- and posttest were
compared (df= 1, 15).
*p<.05. **p<.01. ***p<.001.

the predicted direction: BIDD SRI (p < .007), BSRQ Appear- of their presenting target complaint--feelings of unattractive-
ance Attention/Importance (p < .007), and PABT social com- ness and dissatisfaction with their physical appearance. As a
parison and emotional reasoning items (ps < .00 I), with a trend result of the treatment program, participants reported more
shown for the magnification item (p < .07). No significant positive evaluations of their appearance, feelings of greater sat-
changes were found for Self-PA ratings or the remaining two isfaction with various aspects oftheir physical appearance, and
PABT items. Among the four generalization variables (see Table less discomfort when privately viewing themselves in a mirror.
3), the TWLC group improved significantly on each: Fitness These outcomes were maintained approximately 2 months af-
Evaluation (p < .001), Sexuality Evaluation (p < .01), TSBI ter the final therapy session.
(p < .00 I), and the GSI ofthe SCL-9O-R (p < .008). Consistent with a cognitive interpretation ofbody-image dis-
The CSQ scores reflected a high level of client satisfaction satisfaction (e.g., Cash & Butters, in press; Fallon & Rozin,
with the 3-week program (M = 3.5, SD = 0.4) that was very 1985; Gainer & Bemis, 1982; Noles et al., 1985), the treatment
similar to that for the longer CBT program. Ninety-four percent program had a significant impact on several cognitive parame-
of the TWLC participants indicated the program met most or ters of body image. Following treatment and at follow-up, cli-
ail of their needs. ents judged their body size to be smallcr and closer to the norm,
whereas before therapy they believed they wcre over 25% wider
Discussion than peers their age, gender, and height. 3 The CBT program also
significantlyreduced clients' extremely high level of preoccupa-
The results confirm the therapeutic efficacy of cognitive-be- tion with appearance, as reflected by reductions in BSRQ Ap-
havioral procedures for altering the dysfunctional affective and
cognitive aspects ofa negative body image. Relative to their own
pretreatment levels as wcll as to the untreated control subjects, 3Although women typically appraise their body size as somewhat
the CBT clients made significant gains on the multiple measures wider or heavier than the norm of their actual size (Cash & Brown, in
BODY-IMAGE TREATMENT 895

Table 3
Generalization Variable Means, Standard Deviations, and Treatment Effects

Assessmenttime"

1 2
Intergroup Intragroup
Measure and condition M SI) M SI) M SD comparisons b comparisonsr
TSBI Self-Esteem 7.31"
CBT 31.3 9.2 40.3 8.2 38.7 9.4 16.43"**
WLC/TWLC 34.9 5.1 36.4 3.8 42.2 5.5 22.88***
SCL-90-R GSI
CBT B
0.55 0.43 0.48 0.34
WLC/TWLC m
0.64 0.41 0.44 0.23 9.93**
BSRQ Fitness Evaluation 9.68**
CBT 2.72 0.65 3.34 0.60 3.33 0.60 20.75***
WLC/TWLC 2.89 0.46 3.01 0.45 3.35 0.37 22.41 ***
BSRQ SexualityEvaluation 11.57"*
CBT 3.14 0.56 3.81 0.70 3.70 0.67 13.17"**
WLC/TWLC 3.39 0.57 3.48 0.50 3.70 0.52 9.14"*

Note. BSRQ = Body-Sr Relations Questionnaire; CBT = cognitive-behavioral treatment; GSI = Global Severity Index; SCL-90-R = Symptom
Checklist-90-Revised; TSBI = TexasSocial Behavior Inventory; TWLC = treatr waitin_g-listcontroi; WLC = waiting-listcontrol.
a For the CBT group, Assessments 1, 2, and 3 were pretest, posttest, and follow-up. For the WLC group, Assessments 1 and 2 were pre- and posttest
and Assessment3 was posttest for the treated control subjects, b Comparisons betwer CBT and WLC groups at posttest were adjusted for pretest
scores (dr= 1, 28). r For the CBT group, pre- and posttest and foUow-upwere compared (dr= 2, 28). For the TWLC group, pre- and posttest were
compared (df= 1, 15).
*p<.05. **p<.Ol. ***p<.O01.

pearance Attention/Importance subscale scores from the 70th outcomes produced by the 6-week program, supporting the
percentile at pretest to the 50th percentile at follow-up. More- effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral therapy in the ameliora-
over, after treatment and at follow-up, CBT clients were less tion of a negative body image. As consumers, participants re-
likely to endorse a variety of dysfunctional or erroneous beliefs ported a high level of satisfaction and goal-attainment with each
about physical appearance that paraUel more general cognitive program. Any specific comparisons of outcomes for the two
errors formulated by Beck et al. (1979). The only cognitive out- treatments must be ruade cautiously due to differences in the
corne not maintained at follow-up was clients' judgments of components, duration, and time ofyear oftreatments; the num-
how attractive peers would rate their photo. Participants under- ber of pretreatment assessments; and the experience of thera-
estimated their actual physical attractiveness before treatment pists in conducting each program. However, the combination of
and greafly overestimated it immediately after CBT. At follow- the 6-week CBT program desensitization procedures with the
up, clients' estimations were more accurate albeit not signifi- largely cognitive interventions of the 3-week program make the
cantly different from pretreatment levels. The accuracy of self- conclusions of a recent meta-analytic review noteworthy (Ber-
estimates of attractiveness, although associated with a variety man, Miller, & Massman, 1985): that cognitive and desensitiza-
of affective states, may be a more fragile, less stable measure tion treatments are roughly equal and not additive in efi9
than other body-image indices (Cash, 1985; Cash et al., 1983; Of course, differential effectiveness may depend on the particu-
Noies et al., 1985). 4 lar problems treated, and none ofthe 25 meta-analyzed studies
The treatment program exerted generalized effects beyond concerned treatment ofbody-image dissatisfaction. Until com-
the focal outcomes on appearance-related aspects of body im- parative, dismantling research is conducted (Kazdin, 1983) that
age. Treatment enhanced clients' social self-esteem, improved equates groups on ail variables except the components ofstudy,
their sexual interest and feelings, and promoted more positive the tentative conclusion from the present study is that both vari-
evaluations of physical fitness. Examination of these changes ations of the treatment are effective. Future researchers should
in relation to local norms for college women (Butters, 1985; evaluate the CBT's therapeutic efl9 in relation to other in-
Winstead & Cash, 1984) indicated that clients became quite terventions that serve as better controls than a waiting-list group
similar to their average peer on each oftbese measures. for nonspecific and demand-characteristic effects (Kazdin,
With several minor variations, the abbreviated cognitive-be- 1983; Kendall & Norton-Ford, 1982). Self-report assessments
havioral treatment of control subjects generally replicated the should be supplemented by the development of unobtrusive be-
havioral and Ix'er-report indices of body image. Longer term

prr Cash et al., 1986), participants in the present study judged their 4 With respect to participants' observer-rated physical attractiveness,
size as much wider at prctest than the average appraisals for samples of it is noteworthy that they were by no means uniformly unattractive.
normal control subjectsas wellas widcr than bulimics described by Ruff Among these women who sought therapy to feel more attractive, 55%
and Barrios (1986) or binge eatcrs describr by Koeton et al. (1987). were actually rated as slightlyto very attractive.
896 JONATHAN W. BUTTERS AND THOMAS E CASH

follow-up will also be important in future replications and re- versus systematic dr Is one treatment superior? Psycho-
finements. logical Bulletin, 97. 451--461.
The quasidinical nature ofthis investigation also necessitates Br E., Walster, E., & Bohrnstr G. (1973). The happy Ameri-
caution in the extrapolation offindings to various patient popu- can body: A survey report. Psychology Today, 7, 119-131.
lations. Although clients were volunteers whose scores on the Bohrnstedt, G. W. (1977). On measuring body satisfaction. Unpub-
lished manuscript, Institute of Social Research, Indiana University,
BSRQ and otber baseline body-image measures reflected sub-
Bloomington, IN.
stantial unhappiness with appearance, 5 in actual dinical set- Butters, J. W. (1985). The controlled evaluation of a cognitive-behav-
tings, body-image problems are most often presented as a facet ioral treatment program for body-image dissatisfaction in college
of the phenomenology or symptomatology of other psychologi- women, Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Virginia Consortium for
cal disorders (Cash, 1985). Accordingly, one imperative direc- Professional Psychology, Norfolk, VA.
tion for future clinical research is to consider the utility of the Cash, T. E (1981). Physical attractiveness: An annotated bibliography
cognitive-behavioral body-image interventions as a component of theory and research in the behavioral sciences. JSAS Catalog of
of a broader therapeutic regimen for affective, anxiety, sexual, Selected Documents in Psycholog~, / /(Ms. No. 2370).
and eating disorders. For example, becausr residual body-image Cash, T. E (1985). Physical appearance and mental health. In J. A. Gra-
disturbances are predictive of relapse in the treatment o f an- haro & A. Kligman (Eds.), The psychology of cosmetic treatments (pp.
orexia and bulimia nervosa (Cash & Brown, in press; Garner & 196-2 ! 6). New York: Praeger.
Cash, T. E, & Brown, T. A. (in press). Body image in anorexia nervosa
Bemis, 1982; Garner & Garfinkel, 1981), body-image t r o t -
and bulimia nervosa: A review of the literature. Behavior Modifica-
ment may contribute significantly to clinical outcomes with
tion.
these patients. Although several clinical researchers have incor- Cash, T. E, & Butters, J. W. (in press). Poor body image: Helping the
porated procedures for altering a dysfunctional body image in patient change. Medical Aspects of Human Sexuality.
their eating-disorder treatment programs (e.g., Garner, 1986; Cash, T. E, Cash, D. W., & Butters, J. W. (1983). "Mirror, mirror on
Weiss, Katzman, & Wolchik, 1985; Wooley & Kearney-Cooke, thr w a l l . . . ? : " Contrast effects and sr162 of physical at-
1986), the specific incremental efficacy of these procedures t~ctiveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 9, 351-358.
awaits controlled investigation. Cash, T. E, & Green, G. K. (1986). Body weight and body image among
Tbe scientific study ofhow best to help the diverse individuals college women: Perception, cognition, and affect. Journal of Personal-
who surfer from distressing thoughts and feelings of physical ity Assessment, 50, 290-301.
unattractiveness is one of considerable opportunity. In record Cash, T. E, & Horton, C. E. (I 983). Aesthetic surgery: Effects of rhin~
numbers today, persons consult plastic surgeons for anomalies plasty on the social perception of patients by others. Plastic and Re-
constructive Surger~, 72, 543-548.
of physical appearance that range from major congenital or
Cash, T E, Winstead, B. A., & Janda, L. H. (1986). The grr American
traumatic disfigurements to minor deviations from aesthetic
shape-up: Body image survey report. Psychology Today, 20(4), 30-
norms to imagined defects in appearance (Cash & Horton, 37.
1983; Goin & Goin, 198 l). Cancer treatments can bave appear- Derogatis, L. R. (1977). SCL-90: Administration, scoring and proce-
ance-altering effects that adversely affect body image and t r o t - dures manual for the revised version. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins
ment compliance (Mulready & Lamb, 1985). For many people, University Sehool of Medidne.
the pursuit of strategies for weight loss is perhaps less for the Ellis, A. (1962). Reason and emotion in psyrhotherap~. Secaucus, NJ:
purpose ofshedding pounds than for shedding social stigma and Citadel Press.
a negative body image (Cash, 1985). Our hope is that the present Fallon, A. E., & Rozin, P. (1985). Sex differences in perceptions ofdesir-
investigation and its findings will encourage much-needed re- able body shape. JournalofAbnormal Psycholog~, 94, 102-105.
search on a problem that is far-reaching. Farina, A., Burns, G. L., Austad, C., Bugglin, C., & Fischer, E. H.
(1986). The foie of physical attractiveness in the rr of dis-
charged psychiatrie patients. Journal of Abnormat Psychologs 95,
In fact, subjects"pretest scooeson the BSRQ Appearance Evaluation 139-143.
subscale and on BPSS measures were approximately 1 standard dr Farina, A., Fischer, E., Sherman, S., Smith, W., Groh, T., & Mermin, P.
tion Iowerthan means obtained locallyfor moderately depressed college (1977). Physical attractiveness and mental illnr Journal of Abnor-
students (Noies et al., 1985) and for binge eaters (Keeton et al., 1987). mal Psycholog~, 86, 510-517.
Subjects' BSRQ prr162 mean stores placed them at the 7th percentile Gartinkel, la. E., & Garner, D. M. (1982). Anorexia nervosa: A muhidi-
for collr women. Self-r was at the ! 6th percentile on the TSBI mensionalperspective. New York: Bruner-Mazel.
(Butters, 1985), and global psychological symptomatology on the SCL- Garner, D. M. (1986). Cognitive therapy for anorexia nervosa. In K. D.
90-R was about t standard deviation higher than the norm for nonpa- Brownell & J. P. Foreyt (Eds.), Handbook of eating disorders (pp.
tient women (Derogatis, 1977).
301-327). New York: Basie Books.
Garner, D. M., & Br K. (1982). A cognitive-br approach
References to anorexia nervosa. Cognitive TherapyandResearch, 6, 1-27.
Garner, D. M., & Garfinkel, P. E. (1981). Body image in anorexia ner-
Archer, R. P., & Cash, T. E (1985). Physical attractiveness and malad- vosa: Measurement, theory, and clinical implications. International
justment among psychiatrieinpatients. JournalofSocialand Clinical Journal of Psychiatry in Medicine, 1t, 263-284.
Psycholog~, 3, 170-180. Goin, J. M., & Goin, M. K. (1981). Changing the body: Psychological
Bock, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New effects of plastic surgers Baltimore, MD: Williams & Williams.
York: International Universities Press. Goldfried, M. R., & Davison, G. C. (1976). Clinical behavior therap)~.
Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. E, & Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
therapy ofdepression. New York: Guilford Prr Hatfield, E., & Sprecher, S. (1986). Mirror, mirror...: The importance
Berman, J. S., Miller, R. C., & Massman, P. J. (1985). Cognitive therapy oflooks in •veryday lire. Albany: State University of New York Press.
BODY-IMAGE TREATMENT 897

Heimreich, R., & Stapp, J. (1974). Short forms ofthe Texas Social Be- Metropolitan Life Insurance Company. (1983). Metropolitan life insur-
havior Inventory (TSBI): An objective measure of self-esteem. Bulle- ance table of desirable weight. New York: Author.
tin of the Psychonomic Society, 4, 473--475. Miller, R. C., & Berman, J. S. (1983). The elticacy of cognitive behavior
Kazdin, A. E. (1983). Treatment research: The investigation and evalu- therapies: A quantitative review ofthe research evidence. Psychologi-
ation of psychotherapy. In M. Hersen, A. E. Kazdin, & A. S. Bellack cal Bulletin, 94, 39-53.
(Eds.), The clinicalpsychology handbook. New York: Pergamon Press. Mulready, P. M., & Lamb, J. M. 0985). Cosmetics therapy for female
Kazdin, A. E., & Wilson, G. T. (1978). Evaluation ofbehavior therapy: chemotherapy patients. In M. R. Solomon (Ed.), The psychology of
Issues, evidence and research strategies. Lincoln: University of Ne- fashion (pp. 255-263). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
braska Press. Noies, S. W., Cash, T. E, & Winstead, B. A. (1985). Body image, physi-
cal attractiveness, and depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
Keeton, E, Cash, T. E, & Lewis, R. J. (1987, March). The multidimen- Psychologs 53, 88-94.
sional assessment of body image among college students. Paper pre- O'Grady, K. E. (1982). Sex, physical attractiveness, and perceived risk
sented at the Southeastr Psyehological Convention, Aflanta, GA. for mental illness. Journal of Personality and Socml Psychologs 43,
Kendall, E C., & Hollon, S. D. (Eds.). (1979). Cognitive-behavioral in- 1064-1071.
terventions: Theors research, and procedures. New York: Academie Rosen, G. M., & Ross, A. O. (1968). Relationship ofbody image to self-
Press. concept. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 32, l O0.
Kendall, E C., & Norton-Ford, J. D. (1982). Therapy outcome researeh Ruff, G. A., & Barrios, B. A. (1986). Realistic assessment ofbody image.
methods. In P. C. Kendall & J. N. Butcher (Eds.), Handbook ofre- Behavioral Assessment, 8, 237-251.
search methods in clinicalpsychology (pp. 429--460). New York: Wi- Secord, E E, & Jourard, S. M. (1953). The appraisal of body-cathexis:
ley. Body-cathexis and the self. Journal of Consulting Psychologs 17,
Larsen, D. L., Attkisson, C. C., Hargreaves, W. A., & Nguyen, T. D. 343-347.
(1979). Assessment of client/patient satisfaction: Development of a Weiss, L., Katzman, M., & Wolehik, S. (1985). Treating bulimia: A psy-
choeducational approach. New York: Pergamon Press.
general scale. Evaluation and Program Planning, 2, 197-207.
Winstead, B. A., & Cash, T. E (1984, March). Reliability and validity
Lerner, R. M., Orlos, J. B., & Knapp, J. R. (1976). Physical attractive- of the Body Self-Relations Questionnaire: A new measure of body im-
ness, physical effectiveness and self-concept in iate adolescents. Ad- age. Paper presented at the Southeastern Psychological Association
olescence, 1I, 313-326. Convention, New Orleans, LA.
MacPhiUamy, D. J., & Lewinsohn, E M. (1974). Depression as a func- Wolpe, J. (1973). Thepracticeofbehaviortherapy(2nd ed.). New York:
tion of desired and obtained pleasure. Journal ofAbnormal Psychol- Pergamon Press.
ogy. 83, 651-657. Wolpe, J., & Lazarus, A. A. (1966). Behavior theral~ techniques. New
Marlatt, G. A., & Gordon, J. R. (Eds.). (1985). Relapse prevention: York: Pergamon Press.
Maintenance strategies in the treatment of addictive behaviors. New Wooley, S. C., & Kearney-Cooke, A. (1986). Intensive treatment ofbu-
York: Guilford Press. limia and body-image disturbance. In K. D. Brownell & J. P. Foreyt
McCrea, C. W., Summerfield, A. B., & Rosen, B. (1982). Body image: (Eds.), Handbook ofeating disorders (pp. 476-502). New York: Basic
Books.
A selective review of existing measurement techniques. British Jour-
nal of Medical Psychologs 55, 225-233. Received September 21, 1986
Meichenbaum, D. (1985). Stress inoculation training. New York: Perga- Revision received March 16, 1987
mon Press. Accepted March 30, 1987 9

Potrebbero piacerti anche