Sei sulla pagina 1di 14

Journal of Educational Psychology © 2017 American Psychological Association

2018, Vol. 110, No. 1, 119 –132 0022-0663/18/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000192

The Role of Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting in the Early


Academic Achievement of Latino Students

Yeonwoo Kim and Esther J. Calzada R. Gabriela Barajas-Gonzalez, Keng-Yen Huang,


The University of Texas at Austin Laurie M. Brotman, Ashley Castro, and
Catherine Pichardo
New York University School of Medicine

Early academic achievement has been shown to predict high school completion, but there have been few
studies of the predictors of early academic success focused on Latino students. Using longitudinal data
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

from 750 Mexican and Dominican American families, this study examined a cultural model of parenting
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and early academic achievement. While Latino students were achieving in the average range as a whole,
certain subgroups (e.g., Dominicans, boys) were at higher risk for underachievement. Results highlighted
the protective role of authoritative parenting, which was associated with academic and social-emotional
school readiness, both of which predicted higher achievement at the end of first grade. The role of respeto
and authoritarian parenting practices in academic achievement at first grade differed between Mexican
and Dominican American families. Findings advance understanding of early achievement and parenting
among Latino families from a cultural perspective.

Educational Impact and Implications Statement


Certain subgroups of Latino students (e.g., Dominican-origin, boys) are at greater risk for under-
achievement than their peers (e.g., Mexican-origin, girls). Authoritative parenting practices are a
protective factor for academic achievement in first grade. Efforts to support Latina mothers in how
they socialize and interact with their young children may have positive effects on their child’s later
academic achievement.

Keywords: academic achievement, early childhood, Latino students, parenting

According to the life course perspective of high school dropout prospective, longitudinal study of children from 6 months through
(Alexander, Entwisle, & Kabbani, 2001), the successful comple- 19 years, Egeland and colleagues (Jimerson, Egeland, Sroufe, &
tion of high school is determined by the interplay of individual Carlson, 2000) examined characteristics prior to school entry and
child, family, and school factors that unfold well before students throughout children’s schooling as predictors of high school drop-
enter high school. In support of this model, a host of indicators in out. In their sample of non-Latino white and black families living
first grade, including behavior problems, school performance, in poverty, the home environment and quality of early parenting
grade retention, parent involvement, and family stressors have uniquely predicted dropout status, even when accounting for pre-
been shown to predict dropout (e.g., Brooks-Gunn, Guo, & Furst- dictors measured at later timepoints. Despite the policy and prac-
enberg, 1993; Cairns & Cairns, 1994; Ensminger & Slusarcick, tice implications of these findings, it is not known whether such
1992; Garnier, Stein, & Jacobs, 1997; Haveman & Wolfe, 1994; findings apply to Latino student populations, a gap that is partic-
Roderick, 1994). In fact, even before they enter formal schooling, ularly notable in light of the disproportionate risk for school
children have had a wealth of experiences that set the stage for the dropout experienced by Latinos (Aud, Fox, & KewalRamani,
ways in which they will transition to and navigate through school 2010). To address this limitation in the literature, the present study
(Finn, Gerber, & Boyd-Zaharias, 2005; Garnier et al., 1997). In a examined parenting as a predictor of early academic achievement
in a sample of Latino students and their mothers.

This article was published Online First March 30, 2017. Latino Parenting
Yeonwoo Kim and Esther J. Calzada, School of Social Work, The
University of Texas at Austin; R. Gabriela Barajas-Gonzalez, Keng-Yen
Huang, Laurie M. Brotman, Ashley Castro, and Catherine Pichardo, The Authoritative and Authoritarian Parenting Practices
Department of Population Health, New York University School of Medi-
cine. A vast literature shows the strong and direct effects that parent-
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Esther J. ing has on child development. Traditional parenting theory em-
Calzada, School of Social Work, The University of Texas at Austin, 1925 phasizes responsiveness (i.e., warmth and nurturance) and de-
San Jacinto Boulevard, Austin, TX 78712. E-mail: esther.calzada@ mandingness (i.e., behavior management practices) as key
austin.utexas.edu dimensions underlying parenting styles. These global dimensions

119
120 KIM ET AL.

of parenting appear to be cross-culturally robust in that parents


from all cultures place demands on and respond to the needs of
their children, but some research suggests that different levels of
demandingness and responsiveness may be seen across ethnic
groups (e.g., Cardona, Nicholson, & Fox, 2000). More impor-
tantly, relations between parenting and child outcomes appear to
be moderated by ethnicity (Hill, Bush, & Roosa, 2003; Steinberg,
Lamborn, Dornbusch, & Darling, 1992).
For non-Latino White children, authoritative parenting practices
(i.e., high responsiveness and demandingness) seem to promote,
and authoritarian practices (i.e., low responsiveness and high de-
mandingness) to hinder the developmental competencies needed to
achieve academically. Among Latinos, the literature is inconclu-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

sive regarding the nature and influence of parenting practices


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

according to a number of studies with largely Mexican-origin


parents, the largest Latino subgroup in the U.S. (note, though, that
studies rarely disaggregate findings by country of origin). Some Figure 1. Cultural framework of Latino parenting and student academic
studies suggest that Latino parents are highly authoritarian, and achievement. Straight lines indicate positive hypothesized associations,
that their elementary- and high school–age children may not be and dashed lines indicate negative hypothesized associations.
impacted negatively by this parenting approach (Hillstrom, 2009;
Moon, Kang, & An, 2009; Pong, Hao, & Gardner, 2005), whereas
studies with Mexican- and Dominican-origin preschoolers find value of independence appears to go hand-in-hand with authorita-
negative effects of authoritarian parenting (Calzada, Barajas- tive parenting practices. Thus, according to this framework (see
Gonzalez, Huang, & Brotman, 2015; Calzada, Huang, Anicama, Figure 1), mothers’ acculturative status is expected to influence
Fernandez, & Brotman, 2012). These inconsistencies may reflect socialization messages of respeto or independence and the author-
the differential impact of parenting practices across developmental itarian or authoritative parenting practices that reinforce them;
stages; during early childhood, it may be expected that authoritar- both, in turn, are expected to influence child developmental out-
ian parenting is especially maladaptive. For example, it seems comes.
likely that high levels of parental control may confer risk when No studies have examined the relation of cultural socialization
children transition to formal schooling if controlling parenting to children’s early academic functioning but theoretically, respeto
inhibits the autonomy children need to be successful in adapting to may make children more “teachable” in the classroom in that they
a classroom setting. At the same time, although high levels of respect authority and conform to rules (Harwood, 1992). Alter-
authoritarian control may be protective among adolescents who are nately, it may leave children ill-prepared for schooling in the U.S.
vulnerable to negative peer and neighborhood influences, high educational system because they have been less encouraged to
control may not be necessary with preschoolers who have limited question, problem-solve or negotiate. Unexpectedly, in a correla-
exposure to such risk factors. tional study of Latino preschoolers, Calzada and colleagues (2012)
found respeto to be negatively associated with socioemotional
Latino Cultural Socialization functioning (a component of school readiness) among Mexican-
origin (MA) and Dominican-origin (DA) students, mediated by
Like all family processes, parenting is rooted in culture (i.e., the authoritarian parenting practices, a finding that was later replicated
shared values, beliefs and experiences that determine a group’s using longitudinal data (Calzada et al., 2015). In contrast, inde-
behavioral norms; Guarnaccia & Rodriguez, 1996). In the present pendence positively influenced socioemotional functioning,
study, we consider a cultural framework (Calzada, Fernandez, & though only among MA children, mediated by authoritative par-
Cortes, 2010; see Figure 1) that expands the focus of parenting enting practices. The authors speculated that the influence of
research to emphasize cultural socialization as the impetus for the cultural socialization could vary across MA and DA groups be-
intentional use of authoritarian or authoritative parenting practices. cause they occur within unique ecological circumstances (e.g., an
Cultural socialization is the process through which parents trans- ethnic enclave vs. a diverse neighborhood; see Calzada et al., 2012
mit cultural values, beliefs, traditions, and behavioral norms to for further discussion). These studies not only highlight ethnic
their children (Hughes et al., 2006). For Latino parents, adherence subgroup differences but also demonstrate the value of studying
to the cultural value of respeto, which emphasizes obedience and socialization messages in relation to the developmental competen-
deference to adults (Calzada, 2010; Calzada et al., 2010), appears cies that Latino children need to be successful in school.
to go hand-in-hand with an authoritarian parenting style. Depend-
ing in part on their acculturative status (i.e., acculturation, or
The Role of School Readiness
adaptation to mainstream culture; and enculturation, or mainte-
nance of one’s culture of origin), Latino parents may emphasize School readiness, or the extent to which children enter school
respeto to varying degrees (Gonzales et al., 2008). More accultur- with the foundational academic and social-emotional competen-
ated Latina mothers may be more likely to embrace the main- cies that allows them to learn, predicts later achievement (Duncan
stream U.S. cultural value of independence, socializing their child et al., 2007). The academic domain of school readiness includes
to be assertive and autonomous, even during early childhood. The early language, understanding of concepts, and motor skills that
PARENTING AND LATINO ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 121

serve as building blocks for emergent reading and math skills later-generation families from more than a dozen Spanish-
(National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early speaking countries of origin. To provide a nuanced understanding
Child Care Research Network, 2000). The social-emotional learn- of the pan-Latino population, the present study sampled purpo-
ing (SEL) domain includes developmental skills—such as inter- sively and exclusively from the Mexican- and Dominican-origin
acting positively with others and regulating emotions, attention populations. The majority of U.S. Latinos (65%) come from Mex-
and behavior—that promote on-task behavior and executive func- ico, and though they have not historically resided in the Northeast,
tioning (Rhoades, Warren, Domitrovich, & Greenberg, 2011). MAs are poised to quickly become the largest subgroup in New
Children without the requisite SEL skills and who display behavior York City (NYC; Bergad, 2011). In contrast, the Dominican pop-
problems are less likely to engage in appropriate learning activities ulation has long represented one of the largest subgroups in NYC,
or exhibit a desire to learn and succeed (Wentzel, 1993), and their where 1 in 5 Latinos is DA. The two groups differ significantly
teachers are less likely to provide them with instruction and across various contextual characteristics, and citywide statistics
positive feedback (McEvoy & Welker, 2000). Over time, as chil- show lower academic achievement in MA relative to DA students
dren act out when presented with difficult tasks, their teachers tend (Sáenz & Ponjuan, 2011).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

to withdraw demands and provide fewer learning opportunities


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

(Arnold et al., 1999; Carr, Taylor, & Robinson, 1991). In one of Gender
the few longitudinal studies of young Latino children’s academic
development, children with problem behavior in preschool had Second, we examine gender differences, given the well-
lower academic achievement in first grade (Oades-Sese, Esquivel, established disparities in achievement that favor girls nationally
Kaliski, & Maniatis, 2011). and within ethnic groups (NCES, 2000). Compared to girls, boys
Overall, though, Latino students appear to be at relatively low are more likely to repeat a grade, be suspended or expelled, be
risk for early behavior problems, in part because of a strong referred for special education, and be diagnosed with behavior
cultural emphasis on cultivating interpersonal (e.g., social) skills problems that interfere with learning. The cumulative toll of these
beginning at a very young age (Galindo & Fuller, 2010; Guerrero obstacles contributes to lower rates of high school graduation and
et al., 2013). In contrast, in the earliest years of schooling, Latinos postsecondary education among Latino males than females (Sáenz
lag behind their non-Latino peers across various indicators of & Ponjuan, 2011). For example, only 57% of Latino boys who
academic school readiness. Latino pre-K students have lower attend NYC public schools graduate from high school, compared
levels of letter, number and shape recognition (Aud et al., 2010), with 67% of Latina girls (Villavicencio, Bhattacharya, & Guidry,
and Latino kindergarten students are rated as less engaged and 2013).
attentive (Llagas, 2003). Latino students score significantly below
the national average on standardized achievement tests (Fryer & Grade
Levitt, 2004; Lee & Burkam, 2002). By fourth grade, 78% of
Latino students fall below the proficient range on national stan- Finally, we examine differences between students who entered
dards of math and reading, and these rates remain stable through their assigned elementary school in prekindergarten versus kinder-
Grade 12, when 80% of Latinos have not reached proficiency in garten. The benefits of pre-K have been described extensively in
math and reading (Hemphill, Vanneman, & Rahman, 2011). These the literature (Gormley, Gayer, Phillips, & Dawson, 2005), but
achievement gaps, which have remained relatively intractable over only about half of Latino children attend pre-K (Espinosa, 2008).
time (Hemphill et al., 2011), have dire implications for high school Estimates are even lower for children from Spanish-speaking
graduation rates (Alexander et al., 2001). Latino families (Espinosa, 2008), making the grade in which
students start formal schooling especially pertinent in immigrant
samples.
The Present Study Based on these literatures, we explored mean-level differences
In response to the compelling need to identify malleable pre- based on ethnicity, gender, and grade in our first aim of describing
dictors that may be targeted in interventions to support the aca- academic achievement at the end of first grade. We expected DA
demic success and high school completion of Latino students, the students, girls, and children who entered school as pre-K students
present study uses a longitudinal design to examine early academic to be higher achieving than MA students, boys, and children who
achievement and its relation to parenting practices, conceptualized entered school as kindergarten students. For Aim 2 (model test-
as cultural socialization messages and the authoritative/authoritar- ing), we tested a 4-step sequential model which considers maternal
ian practices that reinforce them, that are shaped by mothers’ acculturative status as a predictor of children’s academic achieve-
acculturative status. First, we describe academic achievement at ment, through its association with parenting (cultural socializa-
the end of first grade in a sample of MA and DA students in New tion messages, parenting practices) and, in turn, the association
York City. Second, we test a cultural framework of Latino parent- of parenting with student school readiness, separately for MA
ing to identify predictors of student academic achievement at the and DA.
end of first grade. In all analyses, we consider the role of ethnicity,
gender, and grade. Method

Ethnicity Participants
Considerable diversity characterizes the Latino population, Data were drawn from a longitudinal study of the early devel-
which is made up of approximately 52 million immigrant and opment of Latino children conducted by the authors in NYC. The
122 KIM ET AL.

study took place in 24 public elementary schools, where eligible 4- assessment at baseline. Mother’s English and Spanish proficiency
to 5-year-old children were enrolled between 2010 –2013. Chil- was measured using the language competence subscales of the
dren were eligible if their mothers identified as Mexican (MA) or AMAS (described further below; Zea, Asner-Self, Birman, &
Dominican (DA) and if they were newly enrolled in prekindergar- Buki, 2003). Nine items for each language that assessed expressive
ten or kindergarten in one of our partner schools. The final sample and receptive, as well as written and oral, language competence
included 414 MA and 336 DA students and their mothers (N ⫽ across contexts (e.g., work, home) were rated from 1 (not at all) to
750 mother-child pairs). Consistent with previous reports on these 4 (extremely well). The items were averaged for each language to
two subsamples, MA (n ⫽ 414) and DA (n ⫽ 336) families create an English and a Spanish language competence scale. Mother’s
differed on several demographic characteristics (see Table 1). education level was coded as high school graduate or less (⫽ 0) or
Specifically, MA mothers were more likely to be immigrant (p ⬍ college or more (⫽ 1). Mother’s employment was coded as not
.001), have less than a high school education (p ⬍ .001), and live working for pay (⫽ 0) or working for pay (⫽ 1). To calculate a family
in poverty (p ⬍ .001) in comparison with DA mothers. MA income-to-needs ratio, we considered income relative to number of
children were younger than DA children (p ⬍ .001), and there family members living in the home for whom the mother was finan-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

were no significant difference between the MA and DA samples cially responsible or with whom she was sharing household expenses
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

on child gender.
using the federal poverty guidelines.
Acculturative status. The Abbreviated Multidimensional Ac-
Measures culturation Scale (AMAS; Zea et al., 2003) was used as a self-
Demographic form. A comprehensive demographic form report measure of acculturative status (i.e., acculturation, encul-
was administered to mothers. Child age and mother’s age was turation). The AMAS can be used with any ethnic group; the use
measured in years as a continuous variable at baseline. Child of the term status recognizes that acculturation/enculturation
gender was coded as boy (⫽ 0) or girl (⫽ 1). Mothers’ self- change over time and that the current measure reflects mothers’
identified ethnicity was coded as Mexican American (⫽ 0) or acculturation/enculturation at a specific point in time. This study
Dominican American (⫽ 1). Immigrant status for mothers and used two domains of the AMAS: identity and cultural knowledge,
children was coded as U.S.-born (⫽ 0) or immigrant (⫽ 1). both of which are measured for the culture of origin (enculturation)
Children’s language preference was coded as English (⫽ 0) or as well as mainstream/“U.S. American” culture (acculturation),
Spanish (⫽ 1), depending on the child’s choice of language for the resulting in four subscales (2 of enculturation and 2 of accultura-

Table 1
Descriptive Characteristics of the Sample, by Child Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade

Ethnicity differences Gender differences Grade differences


M ⫾ SD/n (%) M ⫾ SD/n (%) M ⫾ SD/n (%)
Descriptive statisticsa
Characteristic M ⫾ SD/n (%) MAb DAc Boysd Girlse Pre-Kf Kg

Child age 4.43 ⫾ .60 4.39 ⫾ .60 4.48 ⫾ .59 4.46 ⫾ .60 4.39 ⫾ .60 3.88 ⫾ .33 4.85 ⫾ .38
(t test ⫽ ⫺2.05ⴱ) (t test ⫽ 1.52) (t test ⫽ ⫺36.74ⴱⴱⴱ)
Child gender (boys) 367 (48.9) 198 (47.8) 169 (50.3) — — 151 (46.3) 216 (50.9)
(␹2 ⫽ .45) — (␹2 ⫽ 1.58)
Child foreign-born 55 (7.5) 8 (2.0) 47 (14.4) 32 (9.0) 23 (6.1) 15 (4.7) 40 (9.7)
(␹2 ⫽ 39.64ⴱⴱⴱ) (␹2 ⫽ 2.14) (␹2 ⫽ 6.26ⴱⴱ)
Mothers’ age 32.23 ⫾ 6.73 31.12 ⫾ 5.66 33.61 ⫾ 7.64 32.54 ⫾ 7.19 31.94 ⫾ 6.26 31.92 ⫾ 6.01 32.48 ⫾ 7.24
(t test ⫽ ⫺4.94ⴱⴱⴱ) (t test ⫽ 1.20) (t test ⫽ ⫺1.11)
Child language preference (English) 375 (50.5) 145 (35.3) 230 (69.3) 185 (51.1) 190 (49.9) 137 (42.9) 238 (56.1)
(␹2 ⫽ 84.91ⴱⴱⴱ) (␹2 ⫽ .11) (␹2 ⫽ 12.66ⴱⴱⴱ)
Mothers’ ethnicity (MA) 414 (55.2) — — 198 (54.0) 216 (56.4) 185 (56.7) 229 (54.0)
— (␹2 ⫽ .45) (␹2 ⫽ .56)
Mother education (% ⱕ high school) 541 (72.7) 378 (92.0) 163 (49.0) 267 (73.4) 274 (72.1) 218 (66.9) 323 (77.3)
(␹2 ⫽ 171.61ⴱⴱⴱ) (␹2 ⫽ .15) (␹2 ⫽ 9.99ⴱⴱ)
Mother works for pay 345 (46.7) 126 (31.0) 219 (66.0) 170 (47.0) 175 (46.4) 128 (39.5) 217 (52.3)
(␹2 ⫽ 90.02ⴱⴱⴱ) (␹2 ⫽ .88) (␹2 ⫽ 11.95ⴱⴱ)
Mother foreign-born 686 (92.2) 405 (98.5) 281 (84.4) 338 (93.1) 348 (91.3) 299 (92.0) 387 (92.4)
(␹2 ⫽ 51.28ⴱⴱⴱ) (␹2 ⫽ .81) (␹2 ⫽ .03)
Mothers’ English proficiency 2.15 ⫾ .86 1.80 ⫾ .59 2.58 ⫾ .94 2.12 ⫾ .84 2.18 ⫾ .88 2.22 ⫾ .88 2.10 ⫾ .84
(t test ⫽-13.28ⴱⴱⴱ) (t test ⫽-81) (t test ⫽ 1.97ⴱ)
Mothers’ Spanish proficiency 3.63 ⫾ .51 3.58 ⫾ .55 3.70 ⫾ .45 3.64 ⫾ .51 3.63 ⫾ .51 3.64 ⫾ .48 3.63 ⫾ .53
(t test ⫽⫺3.32ⴱⴱ) (t test ⫽ .25) (t test ⫽ .37)
Family poverty status 498 (70.2) 317 (82.1) 181 (56.0) 247 (71.4) 251 (69.1) 215 (70.3) 283 (70.2)
(␹2 ⫽ 57.25ⴱⴱⴱ) (␹2 ⫽ .43) (␹2 ⫽ .00)
Note. Dash means that no bivariate analysis occurred. MA ⫽ Mexican American; DA ⫽ Dominican American; Pre-K ⫽ Pre-kindergarten; K ⫽
Kindergarten. Because a full information procedure (FIML) estimates parameters for missing data based on all available data instead of imputing, the
number of cases differs.
a
n ⫽ 709 to 750. b n ⫽ 386 to 414. c n ⫽ 323 to 336. d n ⫽ 346 to 367. e n ⫽ 363 to 383. f n ⫽ 306 to 326. g n ⫽ 403 to 424.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
PARENTING AND LATINO ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 123

tion). The 24 items are rated from “not at all” (⫽ 1) to “extremely itative: MA ⫽ .86; DA ⫽ .83; authoritarian: MA ⫽ .69; DA ⫽
well” (⫽ 4). Sample items include “Being U.S. American/Domin- .66).
ican/Mexican plays an important part in my life,” (identity); “How School readiness.
well do you know the national heroes of U.S./Mexico/the Domin- Academic school readiness. The Developmental Indicators
ican Republic?” (cultural knowledge). Raw scores of six items for the Assessment of Learning-Third edition (DIAL-3; Mardell-
were used to specify each latent variable of subscale (i.e., U.S.
Czudnowski & Goldenber, 1998) is an individually administered
identity, ethnic identity, U.S. cultural knowledge, and ethnic
test that was administered as a measure of academic school read-
cultural knowledge). Internal consistencies were high for all
iness. The DIAL-3, and its abbreviated version, the Speed DIAL
subscales for both groups (MA: ␣ range .89 –.91; DA: ␣ range
(used in the present study), assess motor, conceptual, and language
.91–.92).
development that is considered the foundation for successful aca-
Cultural socialization messages. To assess socialization
demic learning. Ten concepts are tested including copying (motor),
messages, mothers completed the Cultural Socialization of Latino
identifying colors (concepts), and letter naming (language). The
Children (CSLC; Calzada et al., 2012), a measure that taps into the
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

DIAL has well-established psychometric properties, includes in-


behavioral manifestations of the cultural value of respeto and the
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

U.S. American value of independence (Calzada et al., 2010). Two dicators of potential developmental delays and is available in
factor-derived scales drawn from a principal component factor Spanish and English. The Speed DIAL, which was used in the
analysis with Varimax rotation were used in the present study: the present study, consists of three subscales (motor, concepts, and
respeto scale included 5 items (e.g., “I tell my child to show language subscales) and was designed to be used with children
respect by greeting elders politely.”), and the independence scale ages 3.0-6.11. The Speed DIAL is predictive of math and English
included 6 items (e.g., “I teach my child to share his own ideas and test scores in third grade (Walk, 2005). In the present study,
opinions.”). Raw scores of 5 items and 6 items were used to average scores were calculated for each motor, concepts, and
specify latent variables of respeto and independence, respectively. language subscales to specify a latent variable of academic read-
Items are rated on a Likert scale from “strongly disagree (⫽ 1)” to iness. On the Speed DIAL, the means in the standardization
“strongly agree (⫽ 5).” Internal consistency was adequate for both sample were: 20.0 (6.6) for children ages 4 – 0 through 4 –5; 23.8
scales and was similar for the MA and DA samples (␣ were .72 (6.0) for children ages 4 – 6 through 5– 0; 27.2 (6.9) for children
and .73 for MA, and.78 and .76 for DA, for respeto and indepen- ages 5– 0 through 5–5; and 30.9 (6.0) for children ages 5– 6
dence, respectively). through 6.
Parenting practices. The Parenting Styles and Dimensions SEL school readiness. The Behavior Assessment System for
(PSD; Robinson, Mandleco, Olsen, & Hart, 1995) was admin- Children-2 (BASC-2; Reynolds & Kamphaus, 2004), a measure
istered as a self-report measure of parenting practices corre- of social-emotional and behavioral functioning with well-
sponding to Baumrind’s (1995) authoritarian and authoritative established psychometric properties, was administered to teach-
parenting style constructs. Parents respond to each item on a ers. Teachers rate 139 items in terms of how often the child has
5-point Likert scale anchored by “never (⫽ 1),” “once in a engaged in a behavior during the past 4 weeks on a 4-point scale
while (⫽ 2),” “about half the time (⫽ 3),” “very often (⫽ 4),” (never ⫽ 0; sometimes ⫽ 1; often ⫽ 2; and almost always ⫽ 3),
and “always (⫽ 5).” The PSD has been shown to have strong and T scores are calculated based on child age (M ⫽ 50, SD ⫽
face validity, concurrent validity, and predictive validity for 10). In the present study, two latent variables were used:
schoolchildren, including preschool students, in the U.S. (Oli- teacher-rated adaptive behaviors based on three measured vari-
vari, Tagliabue, & Confalonieri, 2013). The PSD has been ables (i.e., social skills, adaptability, communication) and
standardized for parents of young children, and has been used teacher-rated total problem behaviors based on three measured
with samples of various ethnic backgrounds including Latina
variables (i.e., the depression, aggression, hyperactivity sub-
mothers from Puerto Rico, the Dominican Republic and Mexico
scales of the BASC).
(Calzada & Eyberg, 2002; Calzada et al., 2012; Calzada et al.,
2015). This study used the authoritative and authoritarian par- Child academic achievement. The Kaufman Test of Edu-
enting scales. The authoritative parenting scale consists of three cational Achievement, Second Edition, Brief Form (KTEA-II;
dimensions: connection (5 items; e.g., “I am responsive to my Kaufman & Kaufman, 2005) provides a quick, reliable estima-
child’s feelings or needs”), regulation (5 items: e.g., “I explain tion of global academic skills by assessing the achievement
to my child how I feel about his or her good and bad behavior”), domains of reading, mathematics, and written language. Stan-
and autonomy granting (5 items; e.g., “I take my child’s desires dardization of the KTEA included a Latino subsample. The
into account before asking him or her to do something”). The Reading subtest includes word recognition (27 items) and read-
authoritarian scale also consists of three dimensions: physical ing comprehension (46 items). The Math and Writing subtests
coercion (4 items; e.g., “I use physical punishment as a way of consist of 67 and 46 items, respectively. The KTEA-II Brief
disciplining my child”), verbal hostility (4 items; e.g., “I yell or Form also derives a Brief Achievement Composite score. Stan-
shout when my child misbehaves”), and nonreasoning/punitive dard scores are based on a mean of 100 and a standard deviation
(4 items; e.g., “I punish by taking privileges away from my of 15. In the present study, children who scored ⬎1 SD below
child with little if any explanation”). Average scores were the mean were categorized at risk. For model testing, we created
calculated for these six dimensions, which were used as the a latent variable of child academic achievement using three
specified latent variables for authoritative and authoritarian measured variables of mean scores corresponding to the read-
parenting. Alpha coefficients were acceptable to good (author- ing, math, and writing subtests of the KTEA.
124 KIM ET AL.

Procedure After the mother and child assessments, teachers of participating


children were contacted (all mothers consented to the collection of
Enrollment of school partners. All data were drawn from teacher report) and their consent was obtained in person. Consent-
schools that served MA and DA students in NYC. Public elemen- ing teachers (92%) completed a teacher report packet on the child
tary schools were approached for partnership in the project via (e.g., child behavior) and mother (e.g., parent involvement) for
informational letters and phone calls if they (a) housed a universal each participating child in that classroom. Most (93%) consenting
pre-K program with at least 2 pre-K classes, and (b) had at least teachers returned their packets, resulting in 202 teachers who
20% Latino students according to Department of Education statis- provided data on 368 pre-K and kindergarten classrooms and 702
tics. The 24 partner schools were classified as either a “Mexican” (93.6%) participant children. There were no differences on demo-
(n ⫽ 13) or a “Dominican” (n ⫽ 11) school depending on the graphics (i.e., ethnicity, mother education, marital status, family
predominant ethnic group of its Latino students as determined by poverty, child gender), or parent-rated child functioning (i.e., ex-
neighborhood-level census data because Department of Education ternalizing, internalizing problems) between children who had
records do not have information on students’ country of origin; all teacher ratings and those who did not (p ⬎ .05).
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

MA participants were drawn exclusively from “Mexican” schools


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

and all DA participants were drawn exclusively from “Dominican”


schools. Importantly, MA and DA schools did not differ on the Analytic Approach
contextual characteristics measured in the present study (Calzada We used SPSS and AMOS software programs for the analyses
et al., 2015). Both MA and DA students attended large public in the present study. For our first aim, to describe academic
elementary schools (⬎650 students per school). The schools were achievement and its predictors, we conducted descriptive and
highly segregated, with an average of 95% students of color and bivariate statistics on the variables for the full sample and for
95% eligible for free or reduced lunch. subgroups based on ethnicity, gender, and grade when children
Participant enrollment. We sampled from students entering entered school (i.e., in pre-K or kindergarten).
their zoned public elementary school, whether as pre-K or kinder- For our second aim, to test a model of parenting and academic
garten students, to characterize Latino children’s experiences dur- achievement, we used two time points: baseline data for predictor
ing the transition to formal schooling. Participant enrollment took variables (acculturative status, socialization messages, parenting
place exclusively in the initial 3-month period of the school year. practices, school readiness) and follow-up data collected at the end
At partner schools, research staff, fluent in Spanish and English, of the child’s first grade school year for our outcome variable
attended parent meetings and were present during daily school (academic achievement test scores). To account for missingness,
drop-off and pick-up times to inform mothers of the study. The we used a full information procedure (FIML). Instead of imputing
recruitment rate was 79% (range across schools: 59 – 98%), with for missing data, FIML derives likelihood functions based on all
no differences between MA and DA schools. available data and then estimates parameters for all data points
Data collection. As part of a larger, longitudinal study, moth- (Enders & Bandalos, 2001).
ers were interviewed, teachers completed questionnaires, and chil- Next, we examined bivariate correlations between the study
dren were assessed in the fall when they entered school (i.e., in variables for MA and DA students separately (see Table 4). We
pre-K or kindergarten) and in the spring as they completed first found significant associations consistent with our conceptual
grade. In the present study, data on predictor variables (mothers’ model, and although there were many ethnic group similarities in
acculturative status, parenting, student school readiness) were col- the pattern of associations, there were also some differences. Thus,
lected at baseline (i.e., in the Fall when children first entered we conducted a measurement invariance test between MAs and
school) and data on outcome variables (academic achievement) DAs on all measures.
were collected at the end of first grade. We then tested a structural equation model (SEM; see Figure 1)
Participant mothers were scheduled for an appointment with a using the maximum likelihood estimation method. Although indi-
bilingual research assistant at their child’s school. Mothers were vidual items were used to specify the latent variables of accultura-
asked which language they preferred to be interviewed in and were tive status and cultural socialization messages, subscale scores
consented before beginning any research activities; all forms and from each of the measures were used to specify the latent variables
measures were available in both Spanish and English. The majority of parenting practices, school readiness, and academic achieve-
of mothers (98% of MA and 76% of DA) chose to be interviewed ment in the model. We chose an SEM analytic approach over
in Spanish. Interviews lasted approximately 2 hours and mothers multiple regression analyses because of its ability (a) to examine
received a stipend for their participation. All child testing occurred sequential links from mothers’ acculturation and enculturation, to
during the school day at a time agreeable to the teacher. For the socialization messages, to parenting practices, to school readiness,
baseline assessment, testing was conducted by a bilingual research and finally to academic achievement and (b) to correct for mea-
assistant in an hour-long session (broken into shorter sessions surement error and residual error (Byrne, 2001). Our data consist
when needed) in the child’s primary language (determined based of 750 children within 388 classrooms and 24 schools. We did not
on mother report, child report and research staff observation of use multilevel analysis for two reasons. First, the intraclass corre-
child’s language use in informal interactions). All children were at lation (ICC) for academic achievement at the school level was .058
least 4 years old at the time of testing. Approximately 50% of MA (␶00 ⫽ 71.89; p ⬎ .05) in MA and .045 (␶00 ⫽ 77.34; p ⬎ .05 in
and DA children (both MA and DA) were tested in Spanish. The DA. According to Lee (2000), multilevel modeling is recom-
final assessment in the spring of first grade followed the same mended when ICC is greater than .10. Also, the ‘30/30 rule,’ which
procedure, except that all children were tested in English, the stipulates that a sample of at least 30 groups with at least 30
language in which they were evaluated by schools. individuals per group is needed for multilevel modeling (Kreft,
PARENTING AND LATINO ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 125

1996), was not met in the present study. More than half of the parents, MA parents were younger (31.12 vs. 33.61 years) and
classrooms (195 classes) had one participating child, and another more likely to be foreign-born (98.5% vs. 84.4%), less educated
quarter (97 classes) had two participating children. (92.0% vs. 49.0% high school graduates), and living in poverty
To test our model, we used multigroup structural equation (82.1% vs. 56.0%). There were no differences based on child
modeling and controlled for child’s grade when they entered gender. However, baseline characteristics showed that com-
school, child gender, child’s language of assessment at baseline, pared with those in pre-K, children in kindergarten were more
and family poverty (i.e., income-to-needs ratio). First, our concep- likely to be foreign-born (9.7% vs. 4.7%) and their mothers
tual model was tested with MA and DA children separately to were more likely to have low educational levels (77.3% vs.
establish the baseline model fit and examine possible structural 66.9% high school graduates) and to work outside of the home
differences between the two groups. Four indices were used to (52.3% vs. 39.5%).
assess the goodness of fit of the hypothesized model to the actual Table 2 shows descriptive statistics for parenting variables as
data in multi-Group SEM per Keith (2014) and Kline (2005): well as bivariate statistics with the endogenous variable (academic
chi-square (␹2 ⬎ .05 or ␹2/df ratio less than 3.0), Adjusted Root achievement in first grade). Mothers reported high levels of ethnic
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Mean Square Error of Approximation (adjusted RMSEA ⬍ .05), identity and ethnic cultural knowledge, and moderate levels of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Comparative Fit Index (CFI ⬎ .90), and Tucker-Lewis index U.S. American identity and cultural knowledge; high levels of
(TLI ⬎ .90). Then, we conducted a measurement invariance test, socialization to both respeto and independence; and high levels
a prerequisite of multigroup structural equation modeling, between
of authoritative and low levels of authoritarian parenting. Com-
ethnic groups (i.e., MAs and DAs). The measurement invariance
pared to MA mothers, DA mothers reported significantly higher
test set factor loadings from all latent to measured variables to be
levels of U.S. identity (2.99 vs. 2.19), U.S. cultural knowledge
the same across groups. The test determines whether measures are
(2.44 vs. 1.63), ethnic cultural knowledge (2.81 vs. 2.63), social-
functioning in a comparable way across groups. We used the
ization messages of independence (4.49 vs. 4.38), and authoritative
chi-square difference test, RMSEA, and TLI to compare the fit of
parenting (4.51 vs. 4.25). There were no significant differences in
nested models (Hong & Ho, 2005; Hong, Malik, & Lee, 2003).
socialization messages of independence or parenting practices
Once measurement invariance was supported, we then conducted
multigroup structural equation modeling by ethnicity to test where (authoritative, authoritarian) between mothers of boys and girls, or
structural path differences occurred. All the constraints from the mothers of pre-K and kindergarten children.
measurement invariance test were retained, and structural paths There were robust ethnic group differences on school readiness
were constrained one at a time. A constraint of structural path (see Table 3). Specifically, DA children were rated as engaging in
means that that path will be freely estimated but that the unstan- more problem behaviors (48.54 vs. 46.13) but also in more adap-
dardized path will be constrained to be equal across groups (Keith, tive behaviors (47.54 vs. 45.57) in the classroom, and they scored
2014). A statistical significant degradation in the fit of the model higher on a test of academic readiness (23.86 vs. 20.35) than MA
indicates a significantly different path coefficient across groups. children. In comparing boys and girls, boys showed more problem
behaviors (48.15 vs. 46.24) and less adaptive behaviors (45.58 vs.
Results 47.28 points) in the classroom than girls. Finally, children in
kindergarten scored higher in academic readiness than children in
pre-K (age-adjusted scores of 25.83 vs. 16.75). Notably, there were
Preliminary Analyses
no significant differences in mean-level scores of first grade aca-
Table 1 presents the baseline characteristics for the full sample demic achievement by child ethnicity or gender. However, at the
and for the three subgroups based on ethnicity (55.2% MA), end of first grade, children from the kindergarten cohort were more
gender (48.9% boys), and grade (i.e., when they entered school, in likely than those from the pre-K cohort to have lower mean-level
pre-K or kindergarten; 56.5% kindergarten). Compared with DA scores of first grade achievement (96.28 vs. 98.50) and to have

Table 2
Descriptive Analysis of Latent Variables of the Sample, by Child Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade

Ethnicity differences M ⫾ SD Gender differences M ⫾ SD Grade differences M ⫾ SD


Descriptive statisticsa
Variable M ⫾ SD MAb DAc t test Boysd Girlse t test Pre-Kf Kg t test

U.S. identity 2.55 ⫾ .93 2.19 ⫾ .85 2.99 ⫾ .84 ⫺12.9ⴱⴱⴱ 2.61 ⫾ .92 2.50 ⫾ .95 1.6 2.52 ⫾ .94 2.58 ⫾ .93 ⫺.8
Ethnic identity 3.87 ⫾ .37 3.91 ⫾ .31 3.82 ⫾ .43 3.3ⴱⴱ 3.89 ⫾ .35 3.85 ⫾ .39 1.3 3.85 ⫾ .42 3.88 ⫾ .32 ⫺.9
U.S. cultural knowledge 1.99 ⫾ .74 1.63 ⫾ .53 2.44 ⫾ .71 ⫺17.29ⴱⴱⴱ 1.98 ⫾ .74 2.00 ⫾ .74 ⫺.3 2.01 ⫾ .74 1.97 ⫾ .74 .7
Ethnic cultural knowledge 2.71 ⫾ .77 2.63 ⫾ .74 2.81 ⫾ .79 ⫺3.2ⴱⴱ 2.73 ⫾ .77 2.70 ⫾ .77 .5 2.67 ⫾ .76 2.75 ⫾ .78 ⫺1.3
Socialization of independence 4.43 ⫾ .43 4.38 ⫾ .42 4.49 ⫾ .43 ⫺3.2ⴱⴱ 4.44 ⫾ .43 4.42 ⫾ .43 .9 4.43 ⫾ .41 4.43 ⫾ .44 ⫺.1
Socialization of respeto 4.33 ⫾ .52 4.30 ⫾ .51 4.36 ⫾ .52 ⫺1.5 4.35 ⫾ .51 4.30 ⫾ .52 1.4 4.28 ⫾ .52 4.36 ⫾ .51 ⫺2.2ⴱ
Authoritative practices 4.12 ⫾ .64 4.25 ⫾ .67 4.51 ⫾ .53 ⫺5.8ⴱⴱⴱ 4.36 ⫾ .64 4.37 ⫾ .61 ⫺.2 4.37 ⫾ .59 4.37 ⫾ .65 .0
Authoritarian practices 1.75 ⫾ .46 1.63 ⫾ .52 1.49 ⫾ .47 3.9ⴱⴱⴱ 1.60 ⫾ .51 1.53 ⫾ .48 1.9 1.57 ⫾ .48 1.56 ⫾ .51 .3
Note. MA ⫽ Mexican American; DA ⫽ Dominican American; Pre-K ⫽ Pre-kindergarten; K ⫽ Kindergarten; Because a full information procedure
(FIML) estimates parameters for missing data based on all available data instead of imputing, the number of cases differs.
a
n ⫽ 733 to 745. b n ⫽ 401 to 411. c n ⫽ 328 to 334. d n ⫽ 356 to 364. e n ⫽ 372 to 381. f n ⫽ 317 to 326. g n ⫽ 414 to 419.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

126
Table 3
School Readiness and Academic Achievement by Child Ethnicity, Gender, and Grade

Full samplea MAb M ⫾ SD DAc M ⫾ SD t test or Boysd Girlse t test or Pre-Kf Kg M ⫾ SD t test or
Variable M ⫾ SD (%) (%) (%) ␹2 M ⫾ SD (%) M ⫾ SD (%) ␹2 M ⫾ SD (%) (%) ␹2

SEL school readiness


Problem behaviors 47.19 ⫾ 7.37 46.13 ⫾ 6.50 48.54 ⫾ 8.17 ⫺4.31ⴱⴱⴱ 48.15 ⫾ 8.21 46.24 ⫾ 6.31 3.43ⴱⴱ 46.05 ⫾ 6.22 48.08 ⫾ 8.05 ⫺3.61ⴱⴱⴱ
Adaptive behaviors 46.44 ⫾ 7.95 45.57 ⫾ 7.62 47.54 ⫾ 8.24 ⫺3.22ⴱⴱ 45.58 ⫾ 7.63 47.28 ⫾ 8.18 ⫺2.78ⴱⴱ 45.93 ⫾ 7.69 46.83 ⫾ 8.14 ⫺1.46

Academic school readiness


Speed DIAL-3 21.92 ⫾ 7.37 20.35 ⫾ 7.24 23.86 ⫾ 7.07 ⫺6.65ⴱⴱⴱ 21.71 ⫾ 7.42 22.11 ⫾ 7.32 ⫺.74 16.75 ⫾ 6.16 25.83 ⫾ 5.58 ⫺20.73ⴱⴱⴱ

Academic achievement at the end of first grade (KTEA)


KTEA 97.15 ⫾ 12.43 96.73 ⫾ 11.33 97.73 ⫾ 13.81 ⫺.90 96.99 ⫾ 13.07 97.29 ⫾ 11.85 ⫺.28 98.50 ⫾ 11.20 96.28 ⫾ 13.10 2.05ⴱ
% at risk on KTEA (18.5%) (16.3%) (21.7%) (2.42) (20.7%) (16.7%) (1.36) (11.8%) (22.9%) (10.01ⴱⴱ)
Note. DIAL-3 was measured by the abbreviated version of the Developmental Indicators for the Assessment of Learning-Third edition (Speed DIAL-3) at baseline. KTEA was measured by the
Kaufman test of Educational Achievement, Second Edition, Brief Form (KTEA-II) at the end of first grade. MA ⫽ Mexican American; DA ⫽ Dominican American; Pre-K ⫽ Pre-kindergarten; K ⫽
Kindergarten. Because a full information procedure (FIML) estimates parameters for missing data based on all available data instead of imputing, the number of cases differs.
a
n ⫽ 518 to 744. b n ⫽ 301 to 411. c n ⫽ 217 to 333. d n ⫽ 242 to 364. e n ⫽ 276 to 381. f n ⫽ 203 to 326. g n ⫽ 315 to 419.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
KIM ET AL.

Table 4
Correlations Among Latent Variables by Ethnicity Group

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12
ⴱⴱⴱ ⴱⴱⴱ
1. U.S. identity 1 ⫺.073 .318 ⫺.164 .075 .032 .086 ⫺.027 .038 .067 ⫺.018 ⫺.085
2. Ethnic identity ⫺.077 1 ⫺.210ⴱⴱⴱ .293ⴱⴱⴱ .007 .173ⴱⴱ .059 .011 ⫺.043 .100 .024 ⫺.051
3. U.S. cultural knowledge .216ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.021 1 ⫺.071 .119ⴱ ⫺.083 .122ⴱ .029 .151ⴱⴱ ⫺.030 .105 .082
4. Ethnic cultural knowledge ⫺.066 .162ⴱⴱ .222ⴱⴱⴱ 1 .168ⴱⴱ .340ⴱⴱⴱ .133ⴱ ⫺.119ⴱ ⫺.068 ⫺.021 ⫺.052 ⫺.034
5. Socialization of independence ⫺.056 .081 .083 .187ⴱⴱⴱ 1 .532ⴱⴱⴱ .398ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.153ⴱⴱ ⫺.157 .025 .033 ⫺.021
6. Socialization of respeto ⫺.099ⴱ .081 ⫺.023 .158ⴱⴱ .546ⴱⴱⴱ 1 .295ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.137ⴱ ⫺.066 .017 ⫺.010 ⫺.144ⴱ
7. Authoritative practices ⫺.062 .030 .129ⴱ .263ⴱⴱⴱ .410ⴱⴱⴱ .302ⴱⴱⴱ 1 ⫺.127ⴱ ⫺.042 .090 .056 .076
8. Authoritarian practices .019 .063 ⫺.106ⴱ .060 ⫺.026 ⫺.024 ⫺.020 1 .104 ⫺.016 .039 .013
9. Problem behaviors .013 .039 ⫺.079 ⫺.055ⴱ ⫺.035 .073 .032 .129ⴱ 1 ⫺.415ⴱⴱⴱ ⫺.062 ⫺.165ⴱ
10. Adaptive behaviors ⫺.034 ⫺.027 .123ⴱ .026 .037 ⫺.091 .032 ⫺.062 ⫺.239ⴱⴱⴱ 1 .395ⴱⴱⴱ .387ⴱⴱⴱ
11. Academic readiness ⫺.030 ⫺.060 .068 .020 .071 ⫺.028 .084 ⫺.021 .055 .361ⴱⴱⴱ 1 .468ⴱⴱⴱ
12. Academic achievement at the
end of first grade ⫺.085 ⫺.047 .087 ⫺.015 .010 ⫺.006 .176ⴱⴱ ⫺.004 ⫺.163ⴱⴱ .371ⴱⴱⴱ .361ⴱⴱⴱ 1
Note. Correlations for the Mexican sample presented below the diagonal; correlations for the Dominican sample presented above the diagonal.

p ⬍ .05. ⴱⴱ p ⬍ .01. ⴱⴱⴱ p ⬍ .001.
PARENTING AND LATINO ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 127
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Figure 2. Structural model with standardized estimates. Bold lines and numbers indicate significant paths and
standardized coefficients.

scores at least one standard deviation below the mean, or at respeto (standardized coefficient ⫽ .16); socialization of indepen-
academic risk (22.9% vs. 11.8%). dence was associated with more authoritative parenting practices
(standardized coefficient ⫽ .62); and more authoritative parenting
Multi-Group Structural Equation Modeling was associated with better adaptive behaviors (standardized coef-
The model was tested with MA and DA children separately. The ficient ⫽ .13) and higher academic readiness (standardized coef-
fit indices of the structural model were acceptable (␹2/df ⫽ 1.55, ficient ⫽ .14) in students. Authoritarian parenting practices, in
adjusted RMSEA ⫽ .04, TLI ⫽ .91, CFI ⫽ .92). The standardized contrast, were associated with more problem behaviors (standard-
coefficients and R2 are presented in Figure 2. For MA students, ized coefficient ⫽ .21), fewer adaptive behaviors (standardized
mothers’ ethnic cultural knowledge was positively related to their coefficient ⫽ ⫺.22), and lower academic readiness (standardized
socialization of independence (standardized coefficient ⫽ .22) and coefficient ⫽ ⫺.14) in students. Finally, academic readiness and
128 KIM ET AL.

adaptive behaviors at baseline predicted higher academic achieve- who started formal schooling in pre-K versus kindergarten. Past
ment at the end of first grade (standardized coefficient ⫽ .93 and studies have found that Latino students enter pre-K and kinder-
.18, respectively). garten with low levels of academic skills (Fryer & Levitt, 2004;
For DA students, mothers’ ethnic cultural knowledge was pos- Lee & Burkam, 2002). The achievement gap has been shown to
itively associated with their socialization of independence (stan- narrow, however, between pre-K, kindergarten and first grade
dardized coefficient ⫽ .29) and respeto (standardized coeffi- (Reardon & Galindo, 2009). Similarly, in the present study, mean-
cient ⫽ .42), and their U.S. identity was positively associated with level descriptive statistics show that even students with limited
their socialization of respeto (standardized coefficient ⫽ .16). academic readiness skills (e.g., MA students) or low social-
Socialization of independence was associated with more authori- emotional school readiness (e.g., boys) at the start of school were,
tative parenting practices (standardized coefficient ⫽ .55), as a whole, achieving at grade level by the spring of first grade.
whereas socialization of respeto was associated with more author- Still, 18.5% of students were in the at-risk range (defined as 1 ⫹
itarian parenting practices (standardized coefficient ⫽ .41). More SD below the mean on standardized testing) at the end of first
authoritative parenting practices were associated with better adap- grade, with notable group differences observed. First, children who
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

tive behaviors (standardized coefficient ⫽ .14), and more author- entered formal schooling in kindergarten rather than pre-K were
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

itarian parenting practices were associated with more problem twice as likely to be at risk for academic underachievement at the
behaviors (standardized coefficient ⫽ .17) in students. Finally, end of first grade (23% compared with 12%). This pattern is
academic readiness and adaptive behaviors were significant pre- consistent with past studies showing the importance of preschool
dictors of academic achievement (standardized coefficient ⫽ .86 for academic success, especially among students from non-English
and .23, respectively). The standardized direct and indirect effects speaking backgrounds (Howes et al., 2008; Wong, Cook, Barnett,
on academic achievement are shown in the Appendix. & Jung, 2008). Second, according to post hoc descriptive analyses
A measurement invariance test was conducted as a prerequisite on ethnic and gender subgroups, 28% of DA boys were at risk for
of multigroup structural equation modeling. For a measurement academic underachievement at the end of first grade, compared
invariance test between MAs and DAs, we constrained the factor with approximately 15% of MA boys, MA girls and DA girls.
loadings to be equal. The constraints increased the chi-square Future research is needed to examine the academic trajectories of
value from 4,543 to 4,613 and were statistically significant at alpha diverse Latino students, with attention to what risk factors may
.05. Although the chi-square difference test is often used to com- disproportionately affect DA boys. For example, characteristics of
pare the fit of nested models, the additional consideration of the classroom, including student-teacher relationships, will be im-
RMSEA and TLI is recommended (Hong & Ho, 2005; Hong et al., portant to explore in future work (Hughes & Kwok, 2007; Suárez-
2003). The baseline model and the model with metric invariance Orozco, Suárez-Orozco, & Qin-Hillard, 2005). Also, though the
showed the same TLI values (baseline model ⫽ .91; metric in- question of gender socialization and schooling in young Latino
variance model ⫽ .91) and adjusted RMSEA (baseline model ⫽ students remains largely unexplored, there is some modest evi-
.04; metric invariance model ⫽ .04), indicating that metric invari- dence that gender influences academic motivation and supports
ance was in fact supported. Given that the condition of measure- among Latino students at older ages (e.g., Alfaro, Umaña-Taylor,
ment invariance was met, we conducted a multigroup structural & Bámaca, 2006; Yowell, 2000; Williams, Alvarez, & Hauck,
equation modeling by ethnicity. Two of the 12 the specified path 2002).
coefficients were significantly different across ethnic groups: from
U.S. identity to respeto, ⌬␹2 ⫽ 7.05, p ⬍ .01; and from cultural
Parenting and Academic Achievement
knowledge to respeto, ⌬␹2 ⫽ 7.11, p ⬍ .01.
Our test of a conceptual model of parenting is consistent with
past studies (see Calzada et al., 2010; Calzada et al., 2012; Calzada
Discussion
et al., 2015) in highlighting how maternal acculturative status is
The present study examined academic achievement among La- associated with parenting—including cultural socialization mes-
tino students, focusing on variations by ethnicity, gender and grade sages and authoritative/authoritarian practices. Unique to the pres-
when they entered school, and tested a model of parenting as a ent study is parenting as a predictor of children’s academic
predictor of Latino students’ early achievement. To our knowl- achievement through school readiness in academic and social-
edge, this is the first study to examine longitudinal associations emotional domains. Findings illustrate the protective role of au-
between parenting and standardized achievement test scores in a thoritative parenting practices for both MA and DA students.
sample of young (first grade) Latino students. Our findings high- Specifically, authoritative parenting was associated with better
light ethnic subgroup differences and the role of parenting, which social-emotional school readiness in DA and MA students, and
was found to predict academic achievement indirectly, via aca- with academic school readiness in MA students. For all students,
demic and social-emotional school readiness skills in pre-K or school readiness was associated with better academic achievement
kindergarten. later on. Authoritarian parenting practices, on the other hand, were
associated with lower social-emotional school readiness for DA
and MA students and with lower academic readiness in MA
Early Academic Achievement Among Latino Students
students, though its link to later underachievement was significant
In assessing academic achievement at the end of first grade, we only among MA students.
found that the Latino students in our sample were generally It is not clear from our data why these ethnic group differences
achieving at grade level (i.e., mean of 96.56 on the KTEA) and this exist, but our findings mirror inconsistencies in the broader liter-
held true for MA and DA students, boys and girls, and students ature on authoritarian parenting practices in Latino families (Cal-
PARENTING AND LATINO ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 129

zada et al., 2012; Calzada et al., 2015; Gorman-Smith, Tolan, enting practices was based on a Western typology (i.e., authori-
Henry, & Florsheim, 2000; Knight, Virdin, & Roosa, 1994). Thus, tarian, authoritative), and some scholars have questioned its appli-
it is likely that the impact of parenting varies across developmental cability to Latino families (Domenech Rodríguez, Donovick, &
outcomes, time points and contexts (i.e., MA and DA families Crowley, 2009). Indeed, the authoritarian scale used in this study
differed on almost all contextual variables measured in the present had modest reliability. Furthermore, we relied on mothers’ self-
study). How these seemingly complex and dynamic effects of risk report, which is subject to rater bias. Thus, although the present
and protection may play out is an important area for ongoing study suggests that research on authoritarian/authoritative parent-
research. ing in Latinos can be useful in understanding developmental
The field must also carefully consider the ways in which it outcomes, more scholarship on Latino parenting theory is needed
characterizes Latino parents, who are often described as harsh and to ensure methodological rigor and cultural sensitivity. Third, our
controlling based on Westernized notions of parenting (Cardona et study used a more global index of problem behaviors because
al., 2000; Guilamo-Ramos et al., 2007; Rodriguez & Olswang, conceptually, “social emotional school readiness” is a domain that
2003). It is notable that although we relied on a Westernized
encompasses all problem behaviors, whether externalizing or in-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

typology of parenting (e.g., authoritarian/authoritative parenting)


ternalizing. Problem behaviors were not a significant predictor of
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

in the present study, we found high levels of (self-reported) au-


later academic achievement in the present study. Given that there
thoritative and low levels of authoritarian parenting, especially
could be differences in how internalizing behaviors are associated
among DA mothers. These results suggest that harsh, controlling
with parenting and school readiness relative to externalizing be-
parenting may not be the norm for all Latino parents and that in
fact, authoritative parenting may well be culturally congruent, at havior problems, future research should explore problem domain-
least during the preschool years. specific associations with school readiness and academic achieve-
Still, consistent with past research (Calzada et al., 2010; Calzada ment.
et al., 2012; Calzada et al., 2015), culture was found to play an Fourth, our model was limited to select family level variables,
important role in parenting, with mothers’ acculturative status and did not consider other ecological variables that undoubtedly
associated with socialization messages and parenting practices. For influence academic achievement, including characteristics of the
both MA and DA mothers, ethnic cultural knowledge was associ- classroom and school. Our model explained a modest amount of
ated with more socialization to the U.S. American value of inde- the variance in teacher-reported problem and adaptive behavior,
pendence and to the cultural value of respeto, and socialization indicating the presence of other variables in shaping child func-
messages of independence, in turn, was associated with more tioning in the classroom. For example, quality of instruction (i.e.,
authoritative parenting. For DA mothers only, socialization to the teaching practices), cultural competence of school staff, bilingual
cultural value of respeto was associated with more authoritarian education resources, and the demographic characteristics of
parenting. Theoretically, socialization messages of independence schools are likely important determinants of school readiness and
(e.g., “I encourage my child to tell me when s/he disagrees with academic achievement (Caldas & Bankston, 1997; Ma & Klinger,
me”) align with the use of authoritative parenting practices (e.g., “I 2000; Rivkin, Hanushek, & Kain, 2005). Outside of the classroom,
give my child reasons why rules should be obeyed”), and social- the neighborhood context (e.g., poverty, safety, segregation) has
ization messages of respeto (e.g., “I correct my child when he or also been found to influence academic achievement (Hanson et al.,
she does not offer to help elders”) align with the use of authori- 2011; Lapointe, Ford, & Zumbo, 2007; Leventhal & Brooks-Gunn,
tarian parenting practices (e.g., “I use physical punishment as a 2000) and should be considered in future research with Latinos.
way of disciplining my child”; Calzada et al., 2010). Empirically, Finally, our sample was limited to MA and DA families, and
higher levels of acculturation and enculturation appear to be as- study findings cannot be generalized to other Latino families. The
sociated indiscriminately with higher levels of socialization to U.S. tremendous diversity of the Latino population has been well-
and Latino values. Perhaps both constructs capture, at least in part, described in the literature, and although we accounted for some
a mother’s underlying approach to social interactions (e.g., ac-
sources of heterogeneity (i.e., ethnicity, child gender, grade when
tively engaging with others, whether it is members of her culture,
they entered school), we were not able to account for others such
members of mainstream society, or her child; Calzada et al., 2012).
as immigrant status (the vast majority of students had foreign-born
We are currently exploring other ways of studying acculturation
parents) of students, important next steps in this line of research.
(e.g., by examining acculturative stress rather than acculturation
Although more work is needed to understand early academic
levels) that may better inform family processes.
achievement in Latino students, the present study makes a
significant contribution to identifying malleable factors that
Limitations and Conclusions may be associated with early achievement. Among low-income
There are several notable limitations to the present study. First, families, parenting practices represent a particularly important
although we used longitudinal data to examine children’s aca- malleable factor because they have the potential to buffer
demic achievement, we relied on cross-sectional data when exam- children from the negative impact of socioeconomic challenges
ining associations between acculturative status, socialization mes- (Hill, 2001). Findings from the present study suggest that
sages, parenting practices, and children’s school readiness. Thus, parenting that aims to encourage age-appropriate independence
although our data support a conceptual model in which accultura- in young children and that relies on authoritative practices may
tive status shapes socialization messages and parenting practices, promote the development of children’s early academic and
we were not able to establish temporal ordering and causality social-emotional competencies, with positive effects on aca-
cannot be inferred. Second, as noted above, our measure of par- demic achievement in later years.
130 KIM ET AL.

References Domenech Rodríguez, M. M., Donovick, M. R., & Crowley, S. L. (2009).


Parenting styles in a cultural context: Observations of “protective par-
Alexander, K., Entwisle, D., & Kabbani, N. (2001). The dropout process in enting” in first-generation Latinos. Family Process, 48, 195–210. http://
life course perspective: Early risk factors at home and school. Teachers dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.2009.01277.x
College Record, 103, 760 – 822. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/0161-4681 Duncan, G. J., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, A., Magnuson, K., Huston, A. C.,
.00134 Klebanov, P., . . . Japel, C. (2007). School readiness and later achieve-
Alfaro, E. C., Umaña-Taylor, A. J., & Bámaca, M. Y. (2006). The influ- ment. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1428 –1446. http://dx.doi.org/10
ence of academic support on Latino adolescents’ academic motivation.
.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428
Family Relations, 55, 279 –291. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1741-3729
Enders, C. K., & Bandalos, D. L. (2001). The relative performance of full
.2006.00402.x
information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in struc-
Arnold, D. H., Ortiz, C., Curry, J. C., Stowe, R. M., Goldstein, N. E.,
tural equation models. Structural Equation Modeling, 8, 430 – 457.
Fisher, P. H., . . . Yershova, K. (1999). Promoting academic success and
http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15328007SEM0803_5
preventing disruptive behavior disorders through community partner-
Ensminger, M. E., & Slusarcick, A. L. (1992). Paths to high school
ship. Journal of Community Psychology, 27, 589 –598. http://dx.doi.org/
graduation or dropout: A longitudinal study of a first-grade cohort.
10.1002/(SICI)1520-6629(199909)27:5⬍589::AID-JCOP6⬎3.0.CO;
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

2-Y Sociology of Education, 65, 95–113. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2112677


This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Aud, S., Fox, M. A., & KewalRamani, A. (2010). Status and trends in the Espinosa, L. M. (2008). Challenging common myths about young English
education of racial and ethnic groups. NCES 2010 – 015. National Cen- language learners. New York, NY: Foundation for Child Development.
ter for Education Statistics. Finn, J. D., Gerber, S. B., & Boyd-Zaharias, J. (2005). Small classes in the
Baumrind, D. (1995). Child rearing dimensions relevant to child maltreat- early grades, academic achievement, and graduating from high school.
ment. In R. Lerner (Ed.), Child maltreatment and optimal care giving in Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 214 –223. http://dx.doi.org/10
social contexts (pp. 55–73). New York, NY: Garland. .1037/0022-0663.97.2.214
Bergad, L. W. (2011). Mexicans in New York City, 1990 –2009: A visual Fryer, R. G., Jr., & Levitt, S. D. (2004). Understanding the black-white test
database. Center for Latin American, Caribbean & Latino Studies, score gap in the first two years of school. The Review of Economics and
CUNY Graduate Center. Statistics, 86, 447– 464. http://dx.doi.org/10.1162/003465304323031049
Brooks-Gunn, J., Guo, G., & Furstenberg, F. F., Jr. (1993). Who drops out Galindo, C., & Fuller, B. (2010). The social competence of Latino kinder-
of and who continues beyond high school? A 20-year follow-up of black gartners and growth in mathematical understanding. Developmental
urban youth. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 3, 271–294. http:// Psychology, 46, 579 –592. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0017821
dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327795jra0303_4 Garnier, H. E., Stein, J. A., & Jacobs, J. K. (1997). The process of
Byrne, B. M. (2001). Structural equation modeling with AMOS: Basic dropping out of high school: A 19-year perspective. American Edu-
concepts, applications and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. cational Research Journal, 34, 395– 419. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/
Cairns, R. B., & Cairns, B. D. (1994). Lifelines and risks: Pathways of 00028312034002395
youth in our time. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Gonzales, N. A., Germán, M., Kim, S. Y., George, P., Fabrett, F. C.,
Caldas, S. J., & Bankston, C. (1997). Effect of school population socio- Millsap, R., & Dumka, L. E. (2008). Mexican American adolescents’
economic status on individual academic achievement. The Journal of cultural orientation, externalizing behavior and academic engagement:
Educational Research, 90, 269 –277. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ The role of traditional cultural values. American Journal of Community
00220671.1997.10544583 Psychology, 41, 151–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10464-007-9152-x
Calzada, E. J. (2010). Bringing culture into parent training with Latinos. Gorman-Smith, D., Tolan, P. H., Henry, D. B., & Florsheim, P. (2000).
Cognitive and Behavioral Practice, 17, 167–175. http://dx.doi.org/10 Patterns of family functioning and adolescent outcomes among urban
.1016/j.cbpra.2010.01.003 African American and Mexican American families. Journal of Family
Calzada, E., Barajas-Gonzalez, R. G., Huang, K. Y., & Brotman, L. (2015). Psychology, 14, 436 – 457. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.14.3
Early childhood internalizing problems in Mexican-and Dominican- .436
origin children: The role of cultural socialization and parenting practices. Gormley, W. T., Jr., Gayer, T., Phillips, D., & Dawson, B. (2005). The
Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology. Advance online
effects of universal pre-K on cognitive development. Developmental
publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1041593
Psychology, 41, 872– 884. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.6
Calzada, E. J., & Eyberg, S. M. (2002). Self-reported parenting practices in
.872
Dominican and Puerto Rican mothers of young children. Journal of
Guarnaccia, P. J., & Rodriguez, O. (1996). Concepts of culture and their
Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 31, 354 –363. http://dx.doi
role in the development of culturally competent mental health services.
.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3103_07
Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences, 18, 419 – 443. http://dx.doi
Calzada, E. J., Fernandez, Y., & Cortes, D. E. (2010). Incorporating the
.org/10.1177/07399863960184001
cultural value of respeto into a framework of Latino parenting. Cultural
Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology, 16, 77– 86. http://dx.doi.org/ Guerrero, A. D., Fuller, B., Chu, L., Kim, A., Franke, T., Bridges, M., &
10.1037/a0016071 Kuo, A. (2013). Early growth of Mexican-American children: Lagging
Calzada, E. J., Huang, K. Y., Anicama, C., Fernandez, Y., & Brotman, in preliteracy skills but not social development. Maternal and Child
L. M. (2012). Test of a cultural framework of parenting with Latino Health Journal, 17, 1701–1711. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10995-012-
families of young children. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority 1184-7
Psychology, 18, 285–296. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0028694 Guilamo-Ramos, V., Dittus, P., Jaccard, J., Johansson, M., Bouris, A., &
Cardona, P. G., Nicholson, B. C., & Fox, R. A. (2000). Parenting among Acosta, N. (2007). Parenting practices among Dominican and Puerto
Hispanic and Anglo-American mothers with young children. The Jour- Rican mothers. Social Work, 52, 17–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/sw/52
nal of Social Psychology, 140, 357–365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/ .1.17
00224540009600476 Hanson, M. J., Miller, A. D., Diamond, K., Odom, S., Lieber, J., Butera,
Carr, E. G., Taylor, J. C., & Robinson, S. (1991). The effects of severe G., . . . Fleming, K. (2011). Neighborhood community risk influences
behavior problems in children on the teaching behavior of adults. Jour- on preschool children’s development and school readiness. Infants &
nal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 523–535. http://dx.doi.org/10 Young Children, 24, 87–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/IYC
.1901/jaba.1991.24-523 .0b013e3182008dd0
PARENTING AND LATINO ACADEMIC ACHIEVEMENT 131

Harwood, R. L. (1992). The influence of culturally derived values on kindergarten children. Early Education and Development, 18, 473–
Anglo and Puerto Rican mothers’ perceptions of attachment behavior. 495. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/10409280701610846
Child Development, 63, 822– 839. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131236 Lee, V. E. (2000). Using hierarchical linear modeling to study social
Haveman, R., & Wolfe, B. (1994). Succeeding generations: On the effects contexts: The case of school effects. Educational Psychologist, 35,
of investments in children. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. 125–141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/S15326985EP3502_6
Hemphill, F., Vanneman, A., & Rahman, T. (2011). How Hispanic and Lee, V. E., & Burkam, D. T. (2002). Inequality at the starting gate: Social
White students in public schools perform in mathematics and reading on background differences in achievement as children begin school. Wash-
the national assessment of educational progress (No. 2011– 459). NCES ington, DC: Economic Policy Institute.
Report. Leventhal, T., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2000). The neighborhoods they live in:
Hill, N. E. (2001). Parenting and academic socialization as they relate to The effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent out-
school readiness: The roles of ethnicity and family income. Journal of comes. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 309 –337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
Educational Psychology, 93, 686 – 697. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022- 0033-2909.126.2.309
0663.93.4.686 Llagas, C. (2003). Status and trends in the education of Hispanics (NCES
Hill, N. E., Bush, K. R., & Roosa, M. W. (2003). Parenting and family 2003– 008). U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Educa-
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

socialization strategies and children’s mental health: Low-income tion Statistics. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

Mexican-American and Euro-American mothers and children. Child Ma, X., & Klinger, D. A. (2000). Hierarchical linear modeling of student
Development, 74, 189 –204. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1- and school effects on academic achievement. Canadian Journal of
00530 Education, 25, 41–55. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1585867
Hillstrom, K. A. (2009). Are acculturation and parenting styles related to Mardell-Czudnowski, C., & Goldenberg, D. S. (1998). Developmental
academic achievement among Latino students? (Unpublished disserta- indicators for the assessment of learning. Monterey, CA: American
tion). University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California. Guidance Service.
Hong, S., & Ho, H. Z. (2005). Direct and indirect longitudinal effects of McEvoy, A., & Welker, R. (2000). Antisocial behavior, academic
parental involvement on student achievement: Second-order latent failure, and school climate: A critical review. Journal of Emotional
growth modeling across ethnic groups. Journal of Educational Psychol- and Behavioral Disorders, 8, 130 –140. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/
ogy, 97, 32– 42. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.1.32 106342660000800301
Hong, S., Malik, M. L., & Lee, M. K. (2003). Testing configural, Moon, S. S., Kang, S. Y., & An, S. (2009). Predictors of Immigrant
metric, scalar, and latent mean invariance across genders in sociot- children’s school achievement: A comparative study. Journal of Re-
ropy and autonomy using a non-Western sample. Educational and search in Childhood Education, 23, 278 –289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
Psychological Measurement, 63, 636 – 654. http://dx.doi.org/10 02568540909594661
.1177/0013164403251332 National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child
Howes, C., Burchinal, M., Pianta, R., Bryant, D., Early, D., Clifford, R., & Care Research Network. (2000). The relation of child care to cognitive
Barbarin, O. (2008). Ready to learn? Children’s pre-academic achieve- and language development. Child Development, 71, 960 –980. http://dx
ment in pre-kindergarten programs. Early Childhood Research Quar- .doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00202
terly, 23, 27–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2007.05.002 Oades-Sese, G. V., Esquivel, G. B., Kaliski, P. K., & Maniatis, L. (2011).
Hughes, D., Rodriguez, J., Smith, E. P., Johnson, D. J., Stevenson, H. C., A longitudinal study of the social and academic competence of econom-
& Spicer, P. (2006). Parents’ ethnic-racial socialization practices: A ically disadvantaged bilingual preschool children. Developmental Psy-
review of research and directions for future study. Developmental Psy- chology, 47, 747–764. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0021380
chology, 42, 747–770. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.5.747 Olivari, M. G., Tagliabue, S., & Confalonieri, E. (2013). Parenting style
Hughes, J., & Kwok, O. M. (2007). Influence of student-teacher and and dimensions questionnaire: A review of reliability and validity.
parent-teacher relationships on lower achieving readers’ engagement Marriage & Family Review, 49, 465– 490. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/
and achievement in the primary grades. Journal of Educational Psychol- 01494929.2013.770812
ogy, 99, 39 –51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.99.1.39 Pong, S. L., Hao, L., & Gardner, E. (2005). The roles of parenting styles
Jimerson, S., Egeland, B., Sroufe, L. A., & Carlson, B. (2000). A prospec- and social capital in the school performance of immigrant Asian and
tive longitudinal study of high school dropouts examining multiple Hispanic adolescents. Social Science Quarterly, 86, 928 –950. http://dx
predictors across development. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 525– .doi.org/10.1111/j.0038-4941.2005.00364.x
549. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0022-4405(00)00051-0 Reardon, S., & Galindo, C. (2009). The Hispanic-White achievement gap
Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (2005). Kaufman Test of Educational in math and reading in the elementary grades. American Educational
Achievement–Brief form manual (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Research Journal, 46, 853– 891. http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/
Publishing. 0002831209333184
Keith, T. Z. (2014). Multiple regression and beyond: An Introduction to Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, R. W. (2004). BASC-2: Behavior Assess-
multiple regression and structural equation modeling. New York, NY: ment System for Children (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: AGS Publishing.
Routledge. Rhoades, B. L., Warren, H. K., Domitrovich, C. E., & Greenberg, M. T.
Kline, R. B. (2005). Principles and practice of structuralequation model- (2011). Examining the link between preschool social– emotional com-
ing (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press. petence and first grade academic achievement: The role of attention
Knight, G. P., Virdin, L. M., & Roosa, M. (1994). Socialization and family skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 182–191. http://dx.doi
correlates of mental health outcomes among Hispanic and Anglo Amer- .org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.07.003
ican children: Consideration of cross-ethnic scalar equivalence. Child Rivkin, S. G., Hanushek, E. A., & Kain, J. F. (2005). Teachers, schools,
Development, 65, 212–224. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131376 and academic achievement. Econometrica, 73, 417– 458. http://dx.doi
Kreft, I. G. G. (1996). Are multilevel techniques necessary? An overview, .org/10.1111/j.1468-0262.2005.00584.x
including simulation studies (Unpublished Report). Los Angeles, CA: Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., & Hart, C. H. (1995).
California State University. Available from http://www.eric.ed.gov Authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting practices: Devel-
Lapointe, V. R., Ford, L., & Zumbo, B. D. (2007). Examining the rela- opment of a new measure. Psychological Reports, 77, 819 – 830. http://
tionship between neighborhood environment and school readiness for dx.doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1995.77.3.819
132 KIM ET AL.

Roderick, M. (1994). Grade retention and school dropout: Investigating the Learning (Doctoral dissertation). East Tennessee State University, United
association. American Educational Research Journal, 31, 729 –759. States. Retrieved from Dissertations & Theses: Full Text database.
http://dx.doi.org/10.3102/00028312031004729 Wentzel, K. R. (1993). Does being good make the grade? Social
Rodriguez, B. L., & Olswang, L. B. (2003). Mexican-American and behavior and academic competence in middle school. Journal of
Anglo-American mothers’ beliefs and values about child rearing, edu- Educational Psychology, 85, 357–364. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/
cation, and language impairment. American Journal of Speech- 0022-0663.85.2.357
Language Pathology, 12, 452– 462. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1058- Williams, L. S., Alvarez, S. D., & Hauck, K. S. A. (2002). My name is not
0360(2003/091) Maria: Young Latinas seeking home in the heartland. Social Problems,
Sáenz, V. B., & Ponjuan, L. (2011). Men of color: Ensuring the academic
49, 563–584. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/sp.2002.49.4.563
success of Latino males in higher education. Washington, DC: Institute
Wong, V. C., Cook, T. D., Barnett, W. S., & Jung, K. (2008). An
for Higher Education Policy.
effectiveness-based evaluation of five state pre-kindergarten programs.
Steinberg, L., Lamborn, S. D., Dornbusch, S. M., & Darling, N. (1992).
Impact of parenting practices on adolescent achievement: Authoritative Journal of Policy Analysis and Management, 27, 122–154. http://dx.doi
parenting, school involvement, and encouragement to succeed. Child .org/10.1002/pam.20310
Yowell, C. M. (2000). Possible selves and future orientation: Exploring
This article is intended solely for the personal use of the individual user and is not to be disseminated broadly.

Development, 63, 1266 –1281. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1131532


Suárez-Orozco, M. M., Suárez-Orozco, C., & Qin-Hilliard, D. (2005). The new hopes and fears of Latino boys and girls. The Journal of Early
This document is copyrighted by the American Psychological Association or one of its allied publishers.

immigration: An interdisciplinary reader. New York, NY: Routledge. Adolescence, 20, 245–280. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/027243160002000
Villavicencio, A., Bhattacharya, D., & Guidry, B. (2013). Moving the 3001
needle: Exploring key levers to boost college readiness among Black Zea, M. C., Asner-Self, K. K., Birman, D., & Buki, L. P. (2003). The
and Latino males in New York City. The Research Alliance for New abbreviated multidimensional acculturation scale: Empirical valida-
York City Schools. tion with two Latino/Latina samples. Cultural Diversity and Ethnic
Walk, R. A. (2005). Evaluating the predictive validity of the Speed DIAL Minority Psychology, 9, 107–126. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/1099-
version of the DIAL-3, Developmental Indicators for the Assessment of 9809.9.2.107

Appendix
Standardized Direct, Indirect, and Total Effects on Academic Achievement at the End of First Grade by
Ethnic Group

MA children DA children
Direct Indirect Total Direct Indirect Total
Variable effect effect effect effect effect effect

1. U.S. identity — ⫺.01 ⫺.01 — .00 .00


2. Ethnic identity — .00 .00 — ⫺.00 ⫺.00
3. U.S. cultural knowledge — .01 .01 — .01 .01
4. Ethnic cultural knowledge — .02 .02 — .01 .01
5. Socialization of independence — .12 .12 — .07 .07
6. Socialization of respeto — ⫺.06 ⫺.06 — ⫺.02 ⫺.02
7. Authoritative practices — .16 .16 — .09 .09
8. Authoritarian practices — ⫺.19 ⫺.19 — ⫺.06 ⫺.06
9. Problem behaviors ⫺.11 — ⫺.11 ⫺.09 — ⫺.09
10. Adaptive behaviors .18 — .18 .23 — .23
11. Academic readiness .93 — .93 .86 — .86

Received May 24, 2016


Revision received January 9, 2017
Accepted January 9, 2017 䡲

Potrebbero piacerti anche