Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

GEORGE R.

FRANKE and JEONG-EUN PARK*

The authors combine findings from 155 samples of more than 31,000
salespeople to test alternative models of antecedents and consequences
of adaptive selling behavior (ASB) and customer orientation (CO). A
random-effects meta-analysis yields average values for 28 different
correlations ranging from –.16 to .35, 19 of which are significant.
Controlling for salesperson gender and selling experience, structural
equation modeling indicates that ASB increases self-rated, manager-
rated, and objective measures of performance, whereas CO increases
only self-rated performance. Both ASB and CO increase job satisfaction.
Tests of reciprocal relationships indicate that ASB increases CO and job
satisfaction increases performance rather than vice versa. Selling
experience increases performance but not job satisfaction, and
saleswomen rate their performance and satisfaction slightly higher than
salesmen do. The magnitudes of the relationships indicate that ASB and
selling experience have greater effects than CO and gender on
salesperson performance.

Salesperson Adaptive Selling Behavior and


Customer Orientation: A Meta-Analysis

Understanding the characteristics of effective salespeople cept at the level of the individual salesperson and customer”
has been a long-standing goal of managers and researchers. (Saxe and Weitz 1982, p. 343). This approach emphasizes
Quantitative syntheses of hundreds of empirical studies long-term customer satisfaction rather than short-term sales
indicate that personal characteristics, role perceptions, and objectives.
job attributes typically account for 10% or less of the vari- The purpose of this study is to provide an integrative
ance in salesperson performance and job satisfaction (e.g., meta-analysis of research on both adaptive selling and
Brown and Peterson 1993; Churchill et al. 1985). Therefore, customer-oriented selling. Using correlations obtained from
identifying additional useful predictors could prove helpful journal articles, dissertations, and other sources, we test a
in selecting, training, and managing salespeople. structural model in which performance and satisfaction are
Two salesperson characteristics that have been the focus outcomes of adaptive selling behaviors (ASB) and customer
of prominent research streams in sales force research have orientation (CO). We extend previous research on the rela-
not yet been examined in a meta-analysis. One stream tionships between objective and subjective performance
examines adaptive selling, that is, “the altering of sales measures (Rich et al. 1999) by examining objective out-
behaviors during a customer interaction or across customer comes, self-ratings, and managerial ratings of performance.
interactions based on perceived information about the We also assess the effects of salesperson gender and selling
nature of the selling situation” (Weitz, Sujan, and Sujan experience. Finally, we compare alternative model specifi-
1986, p. 175). This approach enables salespeople to tailor cations and test various potential moderators of the relation-
messages to fit individual customers’ needs and prefer- ships in the model. The results should suggest useful
ences. The other stream involves customer-oriented selling, avenues for sales force management and research.
which “can be viewed as the practice of the marketing con-
BACKGROUND AND HYPOTHESES
Figure 1 presents an overview of the model. Hypothesis
*George R. Franke is Professor and Reese Phifer Fellow of Marketing,
Department of Management and Marketing, University of Alabama
numbers indicate proposed antecedents and consequences
(e-mail: gfranke@cba.ua.edu). Jeong-Eun Park is an assistant professor, of ASB and CO. Because the model allows these relation-
Marketing Department, University of New Hampshire (e-mail: John. ships to be completely or partially mediated by intervening
Park@unh.edu). Tom Brown, Eric Harris, and Charles Pettijohn provided variables, we consider total effects rather than direct effects
valuable unpublished correlations for use in the meta-analysis. The authors in testing the hypotheses. We include additional paths in the
also gratefully acknowledge the helpful comments of David Ford, the
anonymous JMR reviewers, and the late Dick Wittink. model because of their substantive relevance and to avoid
confounding of other variables’ effects with those of ASB

© 2006, American Marketing Association Journal of Marketing Research


ISSN: 0022-2437 (print), 1547-7193 (electronic) 693 Vol. XLIII (November 2006), 693–702
694 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, NOVEMBER 2006

Figure 1
INITIAL AND ALTERNATIVE MODELS OF ANTECEDENTS AND CONSEQUENCES OF ASB AND CO

and CO. The dashed arrows indicate alternative structural Weitz 1982). In general, empirical findings indicate a strong
models, though we also test the implied reciprocal relation- correspondence between customer perceptions of sales-
ships with further model modifications. person CO and satisfaction, trust, and salesperson relation-
ship development (e.g., Stock and Hoyer 2005; Williams
ASB
and Attaway 1996). However, as with ASB, the benefits of
Salespeople can use information they gather about customer-oriented selling must be weighed against its costs,
prospects before or during an interaction to customize the such as the salesperson’s time spent identifying customer
content and format of their messages for more effective problems and solutions, possibly reduced margins or
communications. Weitz (1981) discusses ASB in his contin- increased service costs entailed in satisfying customers, and
gency model of salesperson effectiveness, and Spiro and lost sales that might have been made with more aggressive
Weitz (1990) develop the concept further in their presenta- sales approaches. In their presentation of the selling
tion of the adaptive selling (ADAPTS) scale. They argue orientation–customer orientation (SOCO) scale, Saxe and
that the benefits of ASB are likely to outweigh the costs of Weitz (1982) report a significant, positive correlation
gathering and responding to information under particular between CO scores and performance for the salespeople
circumstances, such as when the salesperson has the neces- who are above average on their ability to help the customer
sary resources, when buying tasks are complex and may and the strength of their customer relations. Otherwise, the
result in large orders, and when customer relationships have correlation is negative or nonsignificant, and it is nonsignif-
little conflict and are expected to continue in the future. icant within each of four sales forces and for the sample as
However, even simple adaptations in sales interactions, a whole (Saxe 1979).
such as reactions to questions, comments, body language, Despite the limited initial results, subsequent authors
and so on, may improve rapport and reduce objections. have argued for a positive effect of CO on salesperson per-
Accordingly, research largely concludes that ASB improves formance (e.g., Keillor, Parker, and Pettijohn 2000).
salesperson performance regardless of the circumstances Schwepker (2003) concludes from his narrative review of
(e.g., Boorom, Goolsby, and Ramsey 1998; Spiro and Weitz the literature on CO that this relationship holds for both
1990; Weitz, Sujan, and Sujan 1986). Therefore, we pro- business-to-business and retail sales. Therefore, we propose
pose the following: the following:
H1: Adaptive selling increases salesperson performance. H3: Customer-oriented selling increases salesperson
performance.
The practice of adaptive selling may be intellectually
stimulating, increasing the salesperson’s enjoyment of the The literature suggests that there is a positive relationship
selling experience (Sujan, Weitz, and Sujan 1988). In addi- between salesperson CO and job satisfaction. Customer-
tion, the conscious effort to notice and respond to differ- oriented salespeople should find the process of satisfying
ences between prospects may give salespeople a feeling of customers’ needs intrinsically pleasing (e.g., Brown et al.
satisfaction with their performance (Keillor, Parker, and 2002). In addition, salespeople who are high in CO may
Pettijohn 1999). Work effort tends to increase job satisfac- better understand their service role and experience less role
tion in both sales and nonsales contexts (Brown and Peter- stress than salespeople who are low in CO, so that high CO
son 1994). Therefore, we propose the following: should reduce role conflict and ambiguity (e.g., Saxe and
Weitz 1982). In turn, this would increase job satisfaction.
H2: Adaptive selling increases salesperson job satisfaction.
Therefore, we propose the following:
CO H4: Customer-oriented selling increases salesperson job
satisfaction.
With its emphasis on long-term customer satisfaction, a
high CO reflects a high concern for others, low-pressure Adapting to customers entails focusing on their individ-
selling, and problem–solution selling approaches (Saxe and ual needs and preferences, which may lead to a customer-
Salesperson Adaptive Selling Behavior and Customer Orientation 695

centered, problem-solving orientation. An empathetic rela- Job Performance and Satisfaction


tionship between the salesperson and the customer may also An enduring question in sales force research is the rela-
result from the process of adaptive selling (Siguaw 1991). tionship between job performance and satisfaction. The
Therefore, a possible antecedent of customer-oriented sell- majority view is that performance leads to desired out-
ing is ASB: comes, such as pay, recognition, and promotion, which in
H5: Adaptive selling behavior increases customer-oriented turn lead to satisfaction (Yilmaz 2002). Effective sales-
selling. person job performance should be intrinsically satisfying,
and if the organization’s systems for performance measure-
A different perspective is that CO entails concern for
ment and compensation are not defective, it should also be
solving customers’ specific problems and therefore entails
extrinsically rewarding. The opposing view is that satisfac-
gathering information and adapting to individual customers’
tion affects job-related attitudes and emotions, which in
needs (Bodkin 1989). The effect of CO could be similar to
turn influence performance. To test both perspectives, we
that of empathy, which Spiro and Weitz (1990) hypothesize
examine alternative models in which performance affects
and find to be positively correlated with ASB. Accordingly,
satisfaction, and vice versa.
H5 competes with an alternative hypothesis that reverses the
direction of influence: Potential Moderator Variables
H5r: Customer-oriented selling increases ASB. In general, prior research has shown little evidence of
moderators of ASB or CO. One exception is the work of
Stock and Hoyer (2005), who show that empathy, expertise,
Gender and Experience
and other factors moderate the effect of the salesperson’s
Women tend to be less aggressive and more agreeable, customer-oriented attitudes on customer perceptions of the
empathetic, and relationship-oriented than men (e.g., Fein- salesperson’s customer-oriented behavior. The current
gold 1994). Empathy and other characteristics that increase meta-analysis covers a wide range of sales force samples
awareness of situational factors and the willingness to adapt and research procedures, which may facilitate the detection
behaviors accordingly may increase ASB (Spiro and Weitz of moderator effects on antecedents or consequences of CO.
1990). Female salespeople may also tend to be more con- We examine the following potential moderators: customer
cerned with customer satisfaction and opposed to “hard- type (organizational or consumer), product type (goods or
sell” techniques, making them higher in CO (e.g., O’Hara, services), salesperson gender and selling experience, meas-
Boles, and Johnston 1991). Therefore, gender may influ- ures used (ADAPTS or others, and SOCO or others), publi-
ence both ASB and CO: cation year, and publication outlet (journal or others). As is
the case with the other moderators, we assess gender and
H6: Female salespeople practice adaptive selling more than
male salespeople.
experience relative to studies rather than to individuals. We
H7: Female salespeople are more customer oriented than male examine gender as the proportion of women in the sample,
salespeople. and experience is the average years of total selling experi-
ence in the sample. Unfortunately, we cannot test
Adaptive selling draws on skills and knowledge of selling individual-level interactions, such as ASB × CO, with the
situations, customer types, and selling strategies that data available for the meta-analysis.
increase with experience (e.g., Spiro and Weitz 1990; Weitz,
METHOD
Sujan, and Sujan 1986). Experienced salespeople may also
have a greater ability to identify ways to help satisfy cus- Collection and Coding of Studies
tomer needs, a longer-term orientation, and more repeat To limit potential cultural influences on model relation-
customers. These factors should increase CO (O’Hara, ships, the meta-analysis uses only studies of native English-
Boles, and Johnston 1991; Pettijohn, Pettijohn, and Taylor speaking sales forces. In addition, to reduce redundancy
2002; Saxe and Weitz 1982). Therefore, we propose the with Churchill and colleagues’ (1985) meta-analysis and to
following: correspond with the date of Saxe’s (1979) development of
the SOCO scale, we use only studies dated 1979 or later.
H8: Increased levels of sales experience increase adaptive
selling. Subject to these constraints, we identified potentially rele-
H9: Increased levels of sales experience increase customer- vant studies in multiple ways. We posted a request for arti-
oriented selling. cles and working papers on the electronic marketing list-
server ELMAR. We searched ABI/Inform, ProQuest Digital
There is no obvious reason to predict an effect of gender Dissertations, and other computerized databases using key-
on salesperson performance or job satisfaction except words related to adaptive selling, CO, and personal selling
through the mediating effects of ASB and CO (cf. Siguaw in general. We examined the Social Sciences Citation Index
and Honeycutt 1995). However, sales experience would for articles that cited Saxe and Weitz’s (1982) and Spiro and
logically appear to be an antecedent of both job perform- Weitz’s (1990) seminal studies. We manually searched jour-
ance and satisfaction. Ineffective or unhappy salespeople nals, such as Industrial Marketing Management and Jour-
may eventually look for a more suitable occupation and nal of Personal Selling & Sales Management. When we
consequently not be represented among the more experi- identified relevant studies, we obtained them when possible
enced respondents to a sales force survey (cf. Churchill, and examined their references for additional sources.
Ford, and Walker 1976). Therefore, the model allows for We treated multiple publications based on the same sam-
direct effects of experience on performance and satisfaction ple as a single study, and we treated results from distinct
in testing the antecedents and consequences of ASB and samples within a single study as independent. In all, articles
CO. in 33 different journals, 48 dissertations, and six conference
696 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, NOVEMBER 2006

proceedings and working papers yielded correlations from standard errors must be divided by the square root of the
155 different samples that included 31,428 salespeople. The mean square error to yield standard normal variates (z sta-
samples represent multiple sales approaches, such as tele- tistics) for accurate tests of significance (Hedges and Olkin
marketing, door-to-door, organizational, and retail sales, 1985).
and products, such as pharmaceuticals, real estate, cars,
computers and computer services, insurance and financial Structural Model Analysis
services, and advertising products and services. We We analyzed the mean correlations obtained in the meta-
obtained ASB correlations from 42 studies and CO correla- analysis with LISREL 8.54 to test the models that appear in
tions from 40 studies, and 10 studies reported both ASB and Figure 1. The harmonic mean of the correlations’ total sam-
CO. The remaining studies provided one or more correla- ple sizes is specified as the sample size for the analyses
tions between gender, experience, performance, and/or (Viswesvaran and Ones 1995). Because the different per-
satisfaction. formance indicators are likely to be related in ways that are
The key data taken from each source were the pairwise not fully accounted for by their common antecedents, we
correlations between variables in the model. We recorded allowed their structural errors in the LISREL psi matrix to
measure reliability when it was reported or when it was correlate. We assumed that gender and sales experience
based on the Spearman–Brown prophecy formula for corre- were measured without error. With one exception, we set
lations determined as the composite of related individual the error terms for the other constructs equal to 1 less the
measures (e.g., Nunnally 1978, Ch. 5). We also recorded the mean reliabilities obtained in the meta-analysis (ASB, .82;
values of the aforementioned moderator variables when CO, .84; self-rated performance, .84; manager-rated per-
possible. We discussed the codings, and analyses are based formance, .86; objective performance, .69; job satisfaction,
on consensus decisions. .85). The exception is the objective-performance variable.
Objective performance is often measured with a single indi-
Meta-Analytic Analysis Procedures cator, for which no reliability estimate is available. When it
We took a random-effects perspective, which Hunter and is a composite of just two or three indicators, such as sales
Schmidt (2000) recommend for routine use in meta- and quota achievement, reliability estimates will be low
analysis, when calculating mean correlations and perform- unless the indicators are highly correlated. Therefore, we
ing tests of moderators. In this approach, the variability in used a higher value of .85 as the reliability of objective per-
findings across studies is treated as arising from both the formance. This value is consistent with Rich and col-
sampling of studies, reflecting between-studies variance, leagues’ (1999) meta-analysis, it is similar to the reliabili-
and the sampling of individuals within studies, reflecting ties of the other performance measures, and it may avoid
sampling error. In comparison, the fixed-effects perspective overcorrecting for measure unreliability in estimating the
treats studies as differing only in terms of the participants structural model relationships.
examined, plus perhaps a few identifiable study characteris-
tics that are taken into account as moderators. The random- RESULTS
effects approach simplifies to the fixed-effects approach
when the studies’ outcomes are homogeneous (i.e., when all Mean Correlations
the between-studies variance is attributable to sampling The 28 correlations that appear in Table 1 are based on
error from the selection of study participants). When the 11.6 samples and 2338 salespeople on average. They range
assumptions of the fixed-effects approach are not met, the from .01 to .35 in absolute value, and 19 are significant. The
random-effects approach produces more generalizable heterogeneity variance, which shows the variance due to
results and is less subject to Type I errors in testing mean sampling of studies (as opposed to sampling of research
effect sizes (e.g., Hunter and Schmidt 2000). participants), equals .000 for only three of the distributions.
As Hunter and Schmidt (1990) and Schulze (2004) rec- This variance equals zero in the homogeneous case that is
ommend, we analyzed the correlations in their raw form assumed in fixed-effects models. Both Hunter and
rather than after applying Fisher’s r-to-z transformation. Schmidt’s (1990) and Shadish and Haddock’s (1994; not
Schulze’s extensive simulations that compare alternative shown to conserve space) tests of homogeneity show that
meta-analytic approaches indicate that Hunter and the variance is significant for 20 of the relationships. Con-
Schmidt’s (1990) procedures are effective for estimating sequently, a fixed-effects analysis, which assumes homoge-
average effect sizes, testing the significance of average neous effects, would inappropriately weight the study
effects, and estimating the variance of the effect size in the results and exaggerate the statistical significance of the
overall population. However, their homogeneity test mean correlations for a majority of the relationships in the
(Hunter and Schmidt 1990, p. 111) has relatively low power model.
to distinguish between fixed- and random-effects distribu- The only significant, negative correlation is between gen-
tions. Therefore, we used (though we do not report it in der and experience (r = –.16; saleswomen have less selling
detail) a potentially more powerful approach that Shadish experience on average than salesmen). The largest positive
and Haddock (1994, Equation 18-23, p. 275) discuss as an correlation is between manager-rated and objective per-
additional test of homogeneity. formance (r = .35); ten other correlations range from .20 to
We used weighted regression procedures to test the mod- .26. The correlational results are largely consistent with the
erator variables. The weights are the inverse of the sum of hypotheses and other effects that appear in Figure 1. The
the standard error of the study effect and the variance of the exceptions to the hypotheses are H3 (CO is positively
population effect size (if greater than zero). This approach related to self-rated performance but not to manager-rated
produces correct coefficient estimates, but the resulting or objective performance), H6 (gender is uncorrelated with
Salesperson Adaptive Selling Behavior and Customer Orientation 697

Table 1
META-ANALYTIC CORRELATIONS

Relationship
Number of Heterogeneity
Hypothesis Variable 1 Variable 2 Estimates Total n Mean r z Qa Variance
H1 ASB Self-rated performance 26 5822 .261 11.32** 92.16** .010
H1 ASB Manager-rated performance 6 970 .089 2.46* 7.75 .002
H1 ASB Objective performance 14 2453 .149 3.99** 49.48** .014
H2 ASB Job satisfaction 3 652 .254 5.13** 5.47 .003
H3 CO Self-rated performance 15 3254 .194 4.62** 92.24** .022
H3 CO Manager-rated performance 3 542 .013 .39 1.86 .000
H3 CO Objective performance 15 3311 .021 .49 92.20** .023
H4 CO Job satisfaction 11 2452 .221 6.52** 33.92** .009
H5/H5r ASB CO 10 2155 .259 5.81** 48.84** .016
H6 Genderb ASB 5 1266 –.030 –.92 6.68 .001
H7 Genderb CO 7 1119 .165 2.52* 35.23** .024
H8 Experience ASB 10 2635 .137 4.90** 21.16* .004
H9 Experience CO 9 981 .040 1.58 5.61 .000
— Experience Self-rated performance 17 2978 .220 9.32** 30.95* .004
— Experience Manager-rated performance 2 426 .201 2.71** 5.06* .007
— Experience Objective performance 8 1597 .262 3.71** 73.35** .036
— Experience Job satisfaction 9 1745 .033 .76 28.93** .011
— Self-rated performance Job satisfaction 24 5584 .212 9.92** 66.85** .007
— Manager-rated performance Job satisfaction 13 2856 .139 4.87** 31.29** .006
— Objective performance Job satisfaction 19 4769 .126 4.11** 86.75** .014
— Gender Experience 14 2712 –.161 –4.69** 47.02** .012
— Gender Self-rated performance 11 2138 –.010 –.54 8.03 .000
— Gender Manager-rated performance 8 1475 –.034 –1.21 9.40 .001
— Gender Objective performance 9 1482 –.042 –.95 25.60** .011
— Gender Job satisfaction 12 2529 .041 1.37 27.50** .006
— Self-rated performance Manager-rated performance 9 1423 .246 8.07** 13.40 .003
— Self-rated performance Objective performance 13 2470 .263 6.29** 64.66** .018
— Manager-rated performance Objective performance 22 3674 .352 10.65** 113.68** .019
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
aQ is a chi-square statistic that indicates whether the heterogeneity variance is significantly greater than zero.
bGender is coded as male = 0, and female = 1.

ASB), and H9 (experience is uncorrelated with CO). effect plus an indirect effect mediated by ASB to yield a
Experience is also unrelated to job satisfaction, contrary to significant total effect, contrary to the correlational results.
expectations. Experience also increases performance, though it does not
increase job satisfaction. Both self-rated and manager-rated
Structural Model Analyses performance increase job satisfaction. Finally, the mediat-
The harmonic mean of the sample sizes for the estimates ing effect of CO leads to small but significant total effects
in Table 1 is 1544, giving substantial power to detect poor of gender on self-rated performance and job satisfaction.
model fit and obtain significant parameter estimates in the The second model in Table 2 allows for reciprocal paths
LISREL analysis. We report parameter estimates for three between ASB and CO and between performance and satis-
alternative models in Table 2. The initial model is as it faction to help clarify the direction of the relationships
appears in Figure 1, with ASB causing CO and performance between variables (cf. Bagozzi 1980; Brown and Peterson
causing job satisfaction. The fit of this model is excellent: 1993). Further modifications make the model identified and
χ2 = .576, d.f. = 6; root mean square error of approxima- more parsimonious. The paths from gender to ASB, experi-
tion = 0; nonnormed fit index = 1.02; and standardized root ence to CO, and experience to satisfaction are omitted, and
mean square residual = .003. The variance explained is the effects of self-rated, manager-rated, and objective per-
2.3% for ASB, 13.5% for CO, 15.7% for self-rated perform- formance on satisfaction are constrained to be equal, as are
ance, 5.3% for manager-rated performance, 10.1% for the effects of satisfaction on performance. The resulting
objective performance, and 15.9% for job satisfaction. model fits the data well (χ2 = 2.37, d.f. = 9; root mean
As with the correlational results, the total effects support square error of approximation = 0; nonnormed fit index =
many of the hypothesized relationships shown in Figure 1. 1.02; and standardized root mean square residual = .006).
Adaptive selling behavior increases all three performance The direct and total effects in this model are mostly similar
measures (H1), job satisfaction (H2), and CO (H5). Cus- in significance and magnitude to the initial results. The total
tomer orientation increases self-rated performance (H3) and effect of ASB on CO is significant and positive (effect =
job satisfaction (H4), though contrary to H3, it has no effect .39, t = 4.77), whereas the influence of CO on ASB is non-
on manager-rated or objective performance. Gender has no significant (effect = –.10, t = –.82). Therefore, H5 is sup-
effect on ASB, contrary to H6, but it increases CO (H7). ported and H5r is not. However, the results show that satis-
Experience has a positive effect on ASB (H8) and CO (H9). faction leads to performance (effect = .16, t = 2.79) rather
The influence on CO combines a nonsignificant direct than performance to satisfaction (effect = –.02, t = –.43).
698

Table 2
DIRECT AND TOTAL EFFECTS FOR STRUCTURAL MODELS

Initial Model Reciprocal Model Final Model


Direct Total Direct Total Direct Total
Hypothesis Relationship Effect t Effect t Effect t Effect t Effect t Effect t
H1 ASB → self-rated performance 0.24 07.65**0 0.29 09.66** 0.20 05.66** 0.29 10.12** 0.20 06.26** 0.29 09.66**
H1 ASB → manager-rated performance 0.08 02.52**0 0.08 02.50** 0.04 01.14** 0.07 02.43** 0.04 01.28** 0.08 02.50**
H1 ASB → objective performance 0.15 04.67**0 0.14 04.65** 0.11 02.98** 0.13 04.21** 0.12 03.63** 0.14 04.65**
H2 ASB → job satisfaction 0.21 06.30**0 0.31 10.20** 0.26 06.54** 0.31 10.82** 0.25 07.85** 0.31 10.20**
H3 CO → self-rated performance 0.15 04.83**0 0.15 04.83** 0.12 03.58** 0.12 02.51** 0.12 03.87** 0.15 04.83**
H3 CO → manager-rated performance –.02 0–.60**0 –.02 0–.60** –.05 –1.48** –.03 0–.79** –.05 –1.50** –.02 0–.60**
H3 CO → objective performance –.03 –1.06**0 –.03 –1.06** –.06 –1.94** –.05 –1.33** –.05 –1.73** –.03 –1.06**
H4 CO → job satisfaction 0.17 05.52**0 0.18 05.99** 0.19 05.99** 0.15 03.08** 0.18 05.99** 0.18 05.99**
H5 ASB → CO 0.31 10.57**0 0.31 10.57** 0.40 03.74** 0.39 04.77** 0.31 10.57** 0.31 10.57**
H5r CO → ASB — — — — –.10 0–.78** –.10 0–.82** — — — —
H6 Gender → ASB –.01 0–.27**0 –.01 0–.27** — — –.02 0–.79** –.01 0–.27** –.01 0–.27**
H7 Gender → CO 0.19 07.28**0 0.19 06.96** 0.19 07.11** 0.19 07.12** 0.19 07.28** 0.19 06.96**
H8 Experience → ASB 0.15 05.32**0 0.15 05.32** 0.16 05.45** 0.15 05.34** 0.15 05.32** 0.15 05.32**
H9 Experience → CO 0.03 01.02**0 0.08 02.72** — — 0.06 03.43** 0.03 01.02** 0.08 02.72**
— Experience → self-rated performance 0.20 07.46**0 0.24 09.01** 0.20 07.59** 0.24 09.00** 0.20 07.59** 0.24 09.01**
— Experience → manager-rated performance 0.21 07.503** 0.22 08.02** 0.21 07.63** 0.22 08.05** 0.21 07.65** 0.22 08.02**
— Experience → objective performance 0.26 09.81**0 0.28 10.62** 0.26 09.89** 0.28 10.61** 0.26 09.90** 0.28 10.62**
— Experience → job satisfaction –.07 –2.41**0 0.04 01.48** — — 0.04 01.43** –.01 0–.39** 0.04 01.48**
— Self-rated performance → job satisfaction 0.12 03.57**0 0.12 03.57** –.02a 0–.42** –.02 0–.43** — — — —
— Manager-rated performance → job satisfaction 0.10 03.03**0 0.10 03.03** –.02a 0–.42** –.02 0–.43** — — — —
— Objective performance → job satisfaction 0.05 01.43**0 0.05 01.43** –.02a 0–.42** –.02 0–.43** — — — —
— Job satisfaction → self-rated performance — — — — 0.17b 02.64** 0.16 02.79** .15 04.94** 0.15 04.94**
— Job satisfaction → manager-rated performance — — — — 0.17b 02.64** 0.16 02.79** .16 04.91** 0.16 04.91**
— Job satisfaction → objective performance — — — — 0.17b 02.64** 0.16 02.79** .12 03.72** 0.12 03.72**
— Gender → self-rated performance — — 0.03 02.50** — — 0.02 02.50** — — 0.03 02.50**
— Gender →manager-rated performance — — –.00 0–.65** — — –.01 0–.78** — — –.00 0–.65**
— Gender → objective performance — — –.01 –1.03** — — –.01 –1.28** — — –.01 –1.03**
— Gender → job satisfaction — — 0.03 02.95** — — 0.03 03.02** — — 0.03 02.95**
*p < .05.
**p < .01.
Notes: Parameter estimates with the same superscript are constrained to be equal. Gender is coded as male = 0, and female = 1.
JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, NOVEMBER 2006
Salesperson Adaptive Selling Behavior and Customer Orientation 699

Therefore, reversing the performance–satisfaction relation- tionship between ASB and self-rated performance is based
ship in the initial model leads to a preferable final model. on 26 samples that include more than 5800 salespeople.
The initial and final models are equivalent in the sense Others are more susceptible to change as new evidence
that they would fit any given data exactly the same (Mac- accumulates, such as the relationship between CO and
Callum et al. 1993). The models also yield identical total manager-rated performance based on three samples of 542
effects for all relationships, except between performance salespeople.
and satisfaction, and therefore are interchangeable with Tests of moderators, which compare effects across levels
respect to the tests of hypotheses and effects of gender and of various predictor variables, are also heavily influenced
experience. The important difference is that in the final by the number of studies available for analysis. The results
model, job satisfaction has a significant, positive effect on do not show any strong evidence of effects varying over
all three measures of performance, and performance has no time, product or customer type, or other study characteris-
influence on job satisfaction. As with the initial model, the tics. These negative findings could be due to limited statisti-
final model accounts for 2.3% of the variance in ASB and cal power, considering that only three tests had as many as
13.5% of the variance in CO. It accounts for 17.7%, 7.4%, 24 degrees of freedom for error, and most had fewer than
and 11.2% of the variance in self-rated, manager-rated, and 10. As further studies become available, the ability to detect
objective performance, respectively, and 12.5% of the vari- moderators will increase. In addition, further research may
ance in job satisfaction. perform insightful tests by examining within-study modera-
tors. For example, primary research could test for interac-
Moderator Analyses tive effects of ASB and CO, which was not possible in the
We examined potential moderators for 13 correlations: meta-analysis, or examine ASB × experience or CO × gen-
ASB and CO with each other, with their two antecedents der with possibly hundreds of degrees of freedom.
(gender and experience), and with four consequences (job Subject to these important caveats, this study updates and
satisfaction and three measures of performance). The eight extends several previous meta-analyses of sales force
moderators we considered were type of customer, type of research and provides new evidence on the antecedents and
product sold, average selling experience, proportion of consequences of adaptive selling and CO. As in prior inves-
males in the sample, publication year and source, and which tigations, the predictors do not account for large amounts of
measures of ASB and CO were used. We tested each mod- variance in salesperson performance: less than 7% for any
erator separately to maximize the number of usable obser- single predictor and less than 18% for any structural model.
vations. Of the 104 weighted regressions run, one is signifi- However, Dunlap’s (1994) common language effect size
cant at p < .01, and six others are significant at p < .05. indicator for bivariate normal correlations, or CLR, shows
Three indicate that the relationship between ASB and self- that even small effects may be managerially relevant. For
rated performance has declined over time, is lower in jour- two variables correlated r > 0, CLR = sin–1(r)/π + .5 (with
nal articles than other sources, and is lower for more experi- the sine in radians) can be interpreted as the probability that
enced salespeople. Another indicates that the relationship between two randomly selected people, the one who is
between ASB and sales experience is lower for the original higher on one of the variables will also be higher on the sec-
ADAPTS scale than for alternative measures of ASB. How- ond variable. When variables are uncorrelated, the probabil-
ever, when we eliminate one dissertation that reports unusu- ity is simply 50%. The .261 correlation between ASB and
ally high correlations for ASB, all four moderators become self-rated performance has an associated CLR of .584, sug-
nonsignificant. The other significant moderators suggest gesting that a salesperson who is higher in ASB than
that the correlation between CO and objective performance another salesperson has almost a 60% probability of also
is higher in samples with greater proportions of women and being higher in self-rated performance. It is also possible to
that over time, the relationship between CO and experience interpret CLR for a single person relative to the average lev-
has decreased, whereas the relationship between CO and els of two measures, such that a salesperson who is above
self-rated performance has increased. As with ASB, remov- average in ASB has a 58% probability (r = .254) of being
ing one study from each regression makes these CO moder- above average in job satisfaction. These are not large differ-
ators become nonsignificant. Given that we found only a ences, but if the costs of selecting, training, and motivating
few more significant moderators than would be expected salespeople to practice more adaptive selling are relatively
due to chance and that none of the results survived removal small, they may be more than justified by the expected
of even a single study, the findings give little evidence of benefits in terms of increased performance and job
moderating effects on relationships between ASB, CO, and satisfaction.
other variables. Both the correlations and the structural coefficients show
that ASB is related more to self-rated performance than to
manager-rated or objective performance. The higher corre-
DISCUSSION lation between two self-rated attributes could result from
The fixed-effects perspective on meta-analysis assumes response tendencies and common method biases (e.g., Pod-
that differences across homogeneous studies are due solely sakoff et al. 2003). More substantively, salespeople may
to random sampling of subjects. The more realistic random- consider adaptiveness an aspect or cause of performance,
effects perspective acknowledges that observed effects will which would lead to positive correlations with self-ratings
vary as a result of the sampling of studies and the sampling of performance. The smaller correlations found with
of subjects. Therefore, conclusions from a meta-analysis are manager- and self-rated performance could result if both
strengthened when the results are based on large samples of managers and salespeople believe that ASB contributes to
studies and large numbers of subjects. Some of the current performance, but manager ratings differ from self-ratings of
findings should be robust; for example, the estimated rela- ASB. If so, better communication between salespeople and
700 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, NOVEMBER 2006

managers about these (and other performance factors) regardless of the customer’s long-term satisfaction. Distin-
might lead to more consistent performance ratings (cf. Rich guishing between customer-oriented attitudes and behaviors
et al. 1999). and examining the roles of empathy and other personal
Adaptive selling behavior has a positive direct effect on characteristics may shed additional light on the relationship
satisfaction and an indirect effect mediated by CO. Adap- between ASB and CO (cf. Stock and Hoyer 2005).
tive selling is less monotonous than repeating the same Weitz, Sujan, and Sujan’s (1986) framework for adaptive
message to all prospects, it takes more concentration while selling implies that ASB increases with experience, and our
interacting with prospects, and it may also require more results support this. Experience gives salespeople the
precall planning and information gathering. Therefore, part chance to encounter a wider variety of selling situations,
of the effect of ASB on job satisfaction may be mediated by develop a broader repertoire of selling strategies, and apply
effort, which tends to increase both performance and satis- more information-acquisition skills. They also have had
faction (Brown and Peterson 1994). more time to observe a positive effect of ASB on perform-
Customer orientation increases self-rated performance ance. However, the CLR for the experience–ASB relation-
and job satisfaction. The effects are not large, but as with ship is small (only .544). Thus, although the more experi-
ASB, salespeople who are high in CO believe that they are enced salesperson will be higher in ASB on average, there
doing a better job and are more satisfied with their job than will be many exceptions to this pattern in most sales forces.
salespeople who are low in CO. Apparently, though, The same is true with the effect of experience on perform-
customer-oriented selling does not consistently lead to sales ance. The correlations across the three performance types
or other results that managers value, because its effects on range from .20 to .26, and associated CLR values range
manager-rated and objective performance are nonsignifi- from .564 to .584. These correlations are similar to the
cant. Salespeople may believe that customer-oriented sell- value of .20 that Ford and colleagues (1987) find for objec-
ing has long-term benefits, even if their short-term results tive data and manager or peer ratings in studies dating as far
are not recognized by managers or reflected in objective back as 1918, indicating considerable stability across stud-
sales measures. In addition, the costs of implementing ies and periods.
customer-oriented selling may be higher than salespeople Sales experience has a nonsignificant correlation with
realize. Another possibility is that salespeople are not good and direct effect on CO, but it has a small positive total
judges of their CO or that they do not communicate their influence. Therefore, the results support H9, though the
CO effectively to customers and managers. The correlation rationale given for the hypothesis does not account for the
between CO and manager-rated performance used in the mediating effect of ASB. Experience also has a nonsignifi-
structural analysis is based on 3 studies of more than 500 cant effect on job satisfaction. Extremely dissatisfied sales-
salespeople, and the results for CO and objective outcomes people should eventually switch to more fulfilling occupa-
are based on 15 studies of more than 3300 salespeople, so tions, which by itself should lead to a positive experience–
the findings may be reasonably generalizable. Therefore, satisfaction relationship. Therefore, the nonsignificant
the meta-analysis raises questions about how effectively effect suggests that some previously satisfied salespeople
customer-oriented selling implements the marketing con- become less satisfied over time, perhaps because of disap-
cept at the salesperson–customer level (Saxe and Weitz pointment with promotional opportunities or financial
1982). recognition (Churchill, Ford, and Walker 1976).
Empathetic salespeople may better identify and respond In sales force research, the relationship between perform-
to customers’ concerns, and Siguaw (1991) suggests that ance and satisfaction is generally small and sometimes non-
adapting to different customers can lead salespeople to significant, depending on what antecedent variables are
empathize with them. In addition, women tend to be more taken into account (e.g., Brown and Peterson 1993, 1994).
empathetic and relationship oriented than men (Feingold The direction of the relationship is also controversial,
1994). Therefore, as expected, saleswomen and salespeople though the majority view is that performance leads to satis-
who are high in ASB tend to be higher in CO. However, faction (Yilmaz 2002). However, the final model in the cur-
gender has no influence on ASB, possibly because women rent study suggests that satisfaction influences performance
tend to be lower in self-monitoring than men (Day et al. rather than the reverse. The effects are similar for self-rated,
2002). Self-monitoring, which reflects the degree to which manager-rated, and objective performance measures, which
people monitor others’ responses to themselves and modify implies that failing to distinguish between types of perform-
their self-presentation to fit the social climate, is positively ance has not been a limitation of previous performance–
linked to ASB (e.g., Spiro and Weitz 1990). Thus, lower satisfaction investigations. However, self-rated and
self-monitoring could reduce any tendency for saleswomen manager-rated performance may reflect dimensions of
to be more adaptive than salesmen. extra-role performance that objective measures do not cap-
It is conceptually plausible that CO influences ASB ture. If so, despite their consistent relationship to job satis-
rather than vice versa (Bodkin 1989), and this is empirically faction, the three performance measures may show different
supported when models equivalent to the ones in Table 2 are relationships with other outcomes, such as organizational
analyzed. However, when reciprocal relationships are commitment and turnover (cf. MacKenzie, Podsakoff, and
allowed, the evidence supports ASB as a cause rather than Ahearne 1998).
an effect of CO. If ASB increases effectiveness, “sales- It is worth noting that the small gender–performance
people with both high and low customer orientation will relationship found in the literature through 1982 (Ford et al.
adapt sales presentations to customer interests” (Saxe and 1987) has largely disappeared in studies from 1979 to the
Weitz 1982, p. 344). That is, both customer and selling ori- present. The total effects in the structural model show that
entations could lead to ASB but for different reasons, such saleswomen have significantly higher self-rated perform-
as to satisfy customer needs or to make an immediate sale ance and job satisfaction than salesmen, but the coefficient
Salesperson Adaptive Selling Behavior and Customer Orientation 701

for each is only .03. Therefore, the findings suggest that REFERENCES
men and women are equally suited for sales careers. Bagozzi, Richard P. (1980), “Performance and Satisfaction in an
Industrial Sales Force: An Examination of Their Antecedents
CONCLUSION
and Simultaneity,” Journal of Marketing, 44 (Spring), 65–77.
The results provide current empirical generalizations Bodkin, Charles D. (1989), “The Adaptive Behavior Construct and
about several widely studied variables in sales force Its Effect on Salesperson Performance,” doctoral dissertation,
research. A key finding is that ASBs have stronger effects Department of Marketing, Virginia Tech University.
than customer-oriented selling on salesperson performance Boorom, Michael L., Jerry R. Goolsby, and Rosemary P. Ramsey
and satisfaction, though the strength of the effect depends (1998), “Relational Communication Traits and Their Effect on
Adaptiveness and Sales Performance,” Journal of the Academy
on the performance measure used. Sales experience of Marketing Science, 26 (Winter), 16–30.
increases performance but not job satisfaction, and gender Brown, Steven P. and Robert A. Peterson (1993), “Antecedents and
has no important influence on either. Satisfaction increases Consequences of Salesperson Job Satisfaction: Meta-Analysis
all three measures of performance. Because some variables and Assessment of Causal Effects,” Journal of Marketing
show consistent relationships across performance measures Research, 30 (February), 63–77.
whereas others do not, the findings support Churchill and ——— and ——— (1994), “The Effect of Effort on Sales Perfor-
colleagues’ (1985, p. 117) recommendation “that the use of mance and Job Satisfaction,” Journal of Marketing, 58 (April),
multiple indicators of different types to assess the perform- 70–80.
ance construct is desirable.” Brown, Tom J., John C. Mowen, D. Todd Donavan, and Jane W.
Further research could extend the findings in several Licata (2002), “The Customer Orientation of Service Workers:
Personality Trait Effects on Self- and Supervisor Performance
ways. Longitudinal and experimental designs could help Ratings,” Journal of Marketing Research, 39 (February),
justify the causal interpretation and clarify the directions of 110–19.
the relationships between ASB, CO, and other variables. Churchill, Gilbert A., Jr., Neil M. Ford, Steven W. Hartley, and
Measuring variables we did not include in the meta- Orville C. Walker Jr. (1985), “The Determinants of Salesperson
analysis, such as empathy, effort, role conflict and ambigu- Performance: A Meta-Analysis,” Journal of Marketing
ity, and in-role versus extra-role performance, could reveal Research, 22 (May), 103–18.
how the relationships between certain variables are medi- ———, ———, and Orville C. Walker Jr. (1976), “Organizational
ated or moderated by other variables. Comparing the effects Climate and Job Satisfaction in the Salesforce,” Journal of Mar-
of self-rated, manager-rated, and customer-rated CO on per- keting Research, 13 (November), 323–32.
formance could have valuable implications for sales force Day, David V., Deidra J. Schleicher, Amy L. Unckless, and Nathan
J. Hiller (2002), “Self-Monitoring Personality at Work: A Meta-
training and management. Within-study analyses could bet- Analytic Investigation of Construct Validity,” Journal of Applied
ter measure and test potential moderators, such as the nature Psychology, 87 (April), 390–401.
of the buying task, the strength of the customer relationship, Dunlap, William P. (1994), “Generalizing the Common Language
years of selling experience, or interactive effects of ASB Effect Size Indicator to Bivariate Normal Correlations,” Psycho-
and CO. Replications of prior research and reports of corre- logical Bulletin, 116 (November), 509–511.
lations between variables in addition to any other relevant Feingold, Alan (1994), “Gender Differences in Personality: A
findings would strengthen the empirical foundations for Meta-Analysis,” Psychological Bulletin, 116 (November),
some of the relationships we studied in this meta-analysis. 429–56.
In many cases, authors of prior studies could make a useful Ford, Neil M., Orville C. Walker Jr., Gilbert A. Churchill Jr., and
contribution to the literature simply by reanalyzing existing Steven W. Hartley (1987), “Selecting Successful Salespeople: A
Meta-Analysis of Biographical and Psychological Selection
data sets and presenting their findings in additional detail. Criteria,” in Review of Marketing 1987, Michael J. Houston, ed.
Further research could also examine alternatives to the Chicago: American Marketing Association, 90–131.
causal sequences we investigated here. For example, adap- Hedges, Larry V. and Ingram Olkin (1985), Statistical Methods for
tive salespeople may be better able to use feedback from Meta-Analysis. San Diego: Academic Press.
their manager or customers to improve their performance. If Hunter, John E. and Frank L. Schmidt (1990), Methods of Meta-
so, performance outcomes of selling efforts could influence Analysis: Correcting Error and Bias in Research Findings.
and be influenced by ASB. Another modification to the Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
final model in Table 2 could make job satisfaction a cause ——— and ——— (2000), “Fixed Effects Vs. Random Effects
of CO or both CO and ASB. Satisfaction logically appears Meta-Analysis Models: Implications for Cumulative Research
to derive directly from working to adapt to customers and Knowledge,” International Journal of Selection and Assess-
ment, 8 (December), 275–92.
meet their needs, but Siguaw (1991) argues that job satis- Keillor, Bruce D., R. Stephen Parker, and Charles E. Pettijohn
faction increases organizational commitment, effort, empa- (1999), “Sales Force Performance Satisfaction and Aspects of
thy, consideration, and other traits that motivate salespeople Relational Selling: Implications for Sales Managers,” Journal of
to be concerned with meeting the needs of their organiza- Marketing Theory and Practice, 7 (Winter), 101–115.
tion and customers. Therefore, job satisfaction could be a ———, ———, and ——— (2000), “Relationship-Oriented
cause of or both a cause and effect of ASB and CO. Characteristics and Individual Salesperson Performance,” Jour-
Meta-analysis is a way of taking stock of the available nal of Business & Industrial Marketing, 15 (1), 7–22.
knowledge in a research area. Although further research MacCallum, Robert C., Duane T. Wegener, Bert N. Uchino, and
may enrich or contradict the results we obtained in this Leandre R. Fabrigar (1993), “The Problem of Equivalent Mod-
study, the extant evidence raises questions about the influ- els in Applications of Covariance Structure Analysis,” Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 114 (July), 185–99.
ence of self-perceived CO on important performance out- MacKenzie, Scott B., Philip M. Podsakoff, and Michael Ahearne
comes. Instead, the literature indicates that adaptive and (1998), “Some Possible Antecedents and Consequences of In-
experienced salespeople are likely to outperform their col- Role and Extra-Role Salesperson Performance,” Journal of
leagues on average. Marketing, 62 (July), 87–98.
702 JOURNAL OF MARKETING RESEARCH, NOVEMBER 2006

Nunnally, Jum C. (1978), Psychometric Theory, 2d ed. New York: Siguaw, Judy A.J. (1991), “The Relationships of Customer Orien-
McGraw-Hill. tation, Adaptive Selling and Selected Antecedents: A Causal
O’Hara, Bradley S., James S. Boles, and Mark W. Johnston Model,” doctoral dissertation, College of Administration and
(1991), “The Influence of Personal Variables on Salesperson Business, Louisiana Tech University.
Selling Orientation,” Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Man- ——— and Earl D. Honeycutt Jr. (1995), “An Examination of
agement, 11 (Winter), 61–67. Gender Differences in Selling Behaviors and Job Attitudes,”
Pettijohn, Charles E., Linda S. Pettijohn, and A.J. Taylor (2002), Industrial Marketing Management, 24 (January), 45–52.
“The Influence of Salesperson Skill, Motivation, and Training Spiro, Rosann L. and Barton A. Weitz (1990), “Adaptive Selling:
on the Practice of Customer-Oriented Selling,” Psychology & Conceptualization, Measurement, and Nomological Validity,”
Marketing, 19 (September), 743–57. Journal of Marketing Research, 27 (February), 61–69.
Podsakoff, Philip M., Scott B. MacKenzie, Jeong-Yeon Lee, and Stock, Ruth Maria and Wayne D. Hoyer (2005), “An Attitude-
Nathan P. Podsakoff (2003), “Common Method Biases in Behavior Model of Salespeople’s Customer Orientation,” Jour-
Behavioral Research: A Critical Review of the Literature and nal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 33 (Fall), 536–52.
Recommended Remedies,” Journal of Applied Psychology, 88 Sujan, Harish, Barton A. Weitz, and Mita Sujan (1988), “Increas-
(October), 879–903. ing Sales Productivity by Getting Salespeople to Work
Rich, Gregory A., William H. Bommer, Scott B. MacKenzie, Smarter,” Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 8
Philip M. Podsakoff, and Jonathan L. Johnson (1999), “Apples (August), 9–19.
and Apples or Apples and Oranges? A Meta-Analysis of Objec- Viswesvaran, Chockalingam and Deniz S. Ones (1995), “Theory
tive and Subjective Measures of Salesperson Performance,” Testing: Combining Psychometric Meta-Analysis and Structural
Journal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 19 (Fall), Equations Modeling,” Personnel Psychology, 48 (Winter),
41–52. 865–85.
Saxe, Robert (1979), “The Customer Orientation of Salespeople,” Weitz, Barton A. (1981), “Effectiveness in Sales Interactions: A
doctoral dissertation, Anderson School of Management, Univer- Contingency Framework,” Journal of Marketing, 45 (Winter),
sity of California, Los Angeles. 85–103.
——— and Barton A. Weitz (1982), “The SOCO Scale: A Mea- ———, Harish Sujan, and Mita Sujan (1986), “Knowledge, Moti-
sure of the Customer Orientation of Salespeople,” Journal of vation, and Adaptive Behavior: A Framework for Improving
Marketing Research, 19 (August), 343–51. Selling Effectiveness,” Journal of Marketing, 50 (October),
Schulze, Ralph (2004), Meta-Analysis: A Comparison of 174–91.
Approaches. Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe and Huber. Williams, Michael R. and Jill S. Attaway (1996), “Exploring
Schwepker, Charles H., Jr. (2003), “Customer-Oriented Selling: A Salespersons’ Customer Orientation as a Mediator of Organiza-
Review, Extension, and Directions for Future Research,” Jour- tional Culture’s Influence on Buyer-Seller Relationships,” Jour-
nal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 23 (Spring), nal of Personal Selling & Sales Management, 16 (Fall), 33–52.
151–71. Yilmaz, Cengiz (2002), “Salesperson Performance and Job Atti-
Shadish, William R. and C. Keith Haddock (1994), “Combining tudes Revisited: An Extended Model and Effects of Potential
Estimates of Effect Size,” in The Handbook of Research Synthe- Moderators,” European Journal of Marketing, 36 (11–12),
sis, Harris Cooper and Larry V. Hedges, eds. New York: Russell 1389–1414.
Sage Foundation, 261–81.

Potrebbero piacerti anche