Sei sulla pagina 1di 7

Accident Analysis and Prevention 70 (2014) 188–194

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Accident Analysis and Prevention


journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/aap

Preventing adolescent drowning: Understanding water safety


knowledge, attitudes and swimming ability. The effect of a short
water safety intervention
Lauren A. Petrass ∗ , Jennifer D. Blitvich
Faculty of Health Federation University Australia Mt Helen, Ballarat, VIC, Australia

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: Worldwide, epidemiological data indicate that children are a high-risk group for drowning and while
Received 15 August 2013 progress has been made in understanding toddler drownings, there is a lack of empirical evidence
Received in revised form 1 April 2014 regarding the drowning risk and protective factors inherent for adolescents and young adults. This study
Accepted 10 April 2014
used a self-report questionnaire to establish swimming and water safety knowledge and attitudes of
young adults and objectively measured their actual swimming ability using formal practical testing
Keywords:
procedures. Participants then completed a short, 12-week intervention that encompassed swimming,
Adolescent drowning
survival and rescue skills, along with water safety knowledge applicable to a range of aquatic environ-
Swim ability
Aquatic knowledge
ments. Knowledge, attitudes and swimming ability were then re-measured following the intervention
Water safety attitudes to evaluate its effectiveness. The Wilcoxon matched pairs signed ranks test was performed to detect
whether there were significant differences between knowledge, attitude and swim ability scores pre-
intervention and post-intervention. A total of 135 participants completed the baseline and follow up
questionnaire and all practical testing. Results indicated that these young adults had a very low level of
water safety knowledge pre-intervention, although the majority had sound swimming and water safety
skills and attitudes. Overall, significant improvements were evident in knowledge (p < 0.001) and swim
ability (p < 0.001) post-intervention, although no changes were observed in attitudes (p = 0.079). Previ-
ous participation in formal swimming lessons and/or swimming within the school curriculum had no
significant impact on water safety knowledge, skills or attitudes of these young adults, and there were
few significant gender differences. While it is important to conduct further studies to confirm that these
findings are consistent with a more representative sample of young adults, our findings are the first to
provide empirical evidence of the value of a comprehensive aquatic education program as a drowning
prevention strategy for young adults.
© 2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd.

1. Introduction 2008). In contrast, there is a lack of empirical evidence regarding


the drowning risk and protective factors inherent in the later age
Worldwide, epidemiological data indicate that children are a groups and the diversity of drowning locations and activities within
high-risk group for drowning (Taneja et al., 2008). In Australia, these groups makes prevention a significant challenge.
for example, between July 2002 and June 2011, there were 589 Research relating to drowning deaths among young adults (both
drowning deaths in the 0–19 year age group. Significant progress male and female) has predominantly focused on a small num-
has been made in understanding toddler drownings in Australia ber of key risk factors including alcohol and drug use; risk-taking
and other high income counties. Drowning prevention measures behaviour; and swimming ability. The negative effect of alco-
for toddlers and young children typically include strategies such hol consumption in and around aquatic environments has been
as isolation swimming pool fencing (Thompson and Rivara, 1998), widely documented (American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee
supervision (Petrass et al., 2011) and child safe play areas (Fragar on Injury, Violence, and Poison Prevention 2003; Driscoll et al.,
et al., 2003), and teaching parents skills such as CPR (Marchant et al., 2004; Quan and Cummings, 2003). A recent Australian study inves-
tigating drowning in the 15–19 years age group identified that
alcohol was involved in 21% of all drowning deaths, most com-
∗ Corresponding author. Tel.: +61 3 5327 9392. monly at rivers, creeks and streams (Royal Life Saving Society
E-mail address: l.petrass@federation.edu.au (L.A. Petrass). Australia [RLSSA], 2012a), and this has been identified as a priority

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.04.006
0001-4575/© 2014 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
L.A. Petrass, J.D. Blitvich / Accident Analysis and Prevention 70 (2014) 188–194 189

concern (Australian Water Safety Council, 2012). Further, during Sports Science students were invited to participate as the swim-
adolescence there is a complex interaction among psychological, ming and water safety intervention program was embedded within
cognitive and physical developmental processes which cumulates the Swimming and Water Safety course that formed part of the
in increased risk taking and novelty seeking behaviours (Steinberg, students’ undergraduate degree. General background information
2004). This, combined with increasing autonomy and the need relating to the study was provided in the first practical class
to gain peer approval and acceptance, results in heightened vul- and students interested in the intervention completed a substan-
nerability to injury (Johnson and Jones, 2011; Steinberg, 2004), tially extended version of the “Can You Swim” self-report survey
particularly drowning in aquatic settings. Supervision remains as a (described in Section 2.2) which provided baseline data. Follow-
drowning prevention strategy for young adults however, due to ing the completion of the survey, practical swimming assessment
their increased autonomy, supervision changes from being pro- commenced (to gather baseline practical skills data) and all practi-
vided by parents to almost entirely being provided by lifeguards cal assessment was completed within the first four practical classes,
and peers (Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2012a). well before the specific skills were introduced, to eliminate the pos-
Accordingly, young adults need to be equipped with sufficient sibility of learning in the course impacting pre-intervention results.
swimming and water safety knowledge and skills to make informed To decrease the likelihood of participant response bias, students
decisions and take appropriate action in a variety of situations were not informed that some of the survey questions paralleled
where a drowning risk is present. the practical test items. To enable comparisons between pre- and
While limited research connects increased swimming ability post-intervention, in the final practical session of the swimming
(though some form of instruction) to a decrease in drownings and water safety intervention program the “Can You Swim Inter-
(Smith, 1995), swimming ability continues to be considered a pre- vention” self-report survey and practical assessment items were
ventative strategy that may be beneficial for persons of all ages repeated. Ethical approval was granted by the University Human
(Brenner et al., 2003, 2009). Accordingly, RLSSA promotes that Research Ethics Committee and informed consent was obtained
every child should have access to a quality swimming and water from all participants prior to the completion of the survey and
safety education program that includes swimming, survival and practical swimming assessment.
rescue skills, and water safety knowledge suitable for use in a range
of aquatic settings (Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA],
2012b). For young children, research indicates that swimming skills 2.2. Instruments
are more readily attained when motor development is at least the
level of a 5 year old (Blanksby et al., 1995; Parker and Blanksby, The “Can You Swim Intervention” self-report survey was an
1997). However, it remains unknown whether the swimming and extended version of the survey validated for similar use and applied
water safety skills and knowledge acquired through participation in a large international study that included New Zealand, Norway,
in formal swimming lessons as a young child are retained at older Japan and Australia (Moran et al., 2012; Petrass et al., 2012). Modifi-
ages. Further, it is also unknown whether swimming and water cations to the original survey included the addition of ten multiple
safety education programs conducted through the primary and sec- choice questions that assessed swimming, water safety and aquatic
ondary school years impact on young adults’ (18–34 years) water knowledge (e.g., which of the following categories of drowning
safety skills and knowledge. victims is least likely to wave or call for help?). Twelve attitude
Recent international research investigating the relationship statements were included, with participants required to indicate
between perceived and real swimming ability of first year colle- the extent of agreement to each statement on a five-point Lik-
giate physical education students (aged 17–29 years) in Australia, ert scale (ranging from strongly agree to strongly disagree). For
Japan, New Zealand and Norway confirmed that most students had example, it is safe to swim at surf beaches and rivers after consum-
a sound aquatic skill base. This was expected, given that partici- ing more than 1–2 standard alcoholic drinks. Eight closed-ended
pants were undertaking a program where aquatic activities were response questions were also added that enabled characterisation
part of their professional development (Moran et al., 2012; Petrass of aquatic participation and behaviour (e.g., in the past year how
et al., 2012). In contrast, a self-report study that examined the often did you swim at the following venues: home swimming pool;
swimming ability of US adults, which claimed to be representative public swimming pool; surf beach; lake, pond, water hole; river,
of the US population, indicated that 37% of respondents reported creek).
limited swimming ability (defined as unable to swim/could not To establish content validity of the additional 30 questions in
swim the length of a standard pool [24 yd or 22 m]) (Irwin et al., the “Can You Swim Intervention” survey, the entire instrument was
2009). Equivalent research on the swimming competence of young reviewed by a panel of eight experts with extensive swim coaching
Australian adults is lacking, and to our knowledge no research has and teaching experience. Two academics with psychology back-
considered the swimming and water safety knowledge and atti- grounds also reviewed the instrument, specifically focussing on the
tudes of this population. Therefore this study fulfils three purposes: attitude and behaviour questions. Experts were invited to provide
(1) to ascertain the water safety knowledge, attitudes and swim- suggestions for modifications, additions or deletions to the sur-
ming ability of young Australian adults; (2) to explore demographic vey content. Expert feedback was collated and condensed and a
variables that significantly influence water safety knowledge, atti- small number of modifications were made to the wording within
tudes and swimming ability; and (3) to determine the effect of a the survey. The amended version was piloted with a small num-
12-week swimming and water safety intervention on young adults’ ber of Bachelor of Education students, a group similar to the study
swimming and water safety knowledge, attitudes and ability of participant group. This pilot trial enabled face validity to be deter-
young adults. mined, ambiguity within questions identified and addressed, and
instructions and question wording clarified where necessary. All
feedback received from the pilot study was addressed for ques-
2. Method tionnaire improvement.
Test–retest reliability of the extended “Can You Swim Inter-
2.1. Participants and procedure vention” survey was conducted one month prior to the data
collection. Reliability was assessed through repeat completion of
The “Can You Swim Intervention” study was conducted between the questionnaire over a short time interval (mean 14 days) with a
February and June 2010. All first year Bachelor of Exercise and group of 50 Bachelor of Education students. Kappa statistics were
190 L.A. Petrass, J.D. Blitvich / Accident Analysis and Prevention 70 (2014) 188–194

Table 1
The teaching sequence and content included in the 12-week intervention program.

Week Theory sessions Practical 1 Practical 2

1 Introduction to swimming and water safety Water confidence games; breathing Body orientation; basic sculling
2 Drowning epidemiology Survival sculling, sculling for movement; Backstroke
synchronised swimming skills
3 Fluid mechanics Backstroke continued Freestyle
4 Fluid mechanics Freestyle continued Breaststroke kick, survival backstroke
5 Entries, focussing on teaching safer diving Breaststroke continued Entries; safe diving skills
skills
6 Survival issues and techniques Sidestroke Survival skills (clothed); PFDs; HELP and
huddle positions
7 Survival issues and techniques Practical survival test Butterfly
8 Starts and turns Butterfly continued Competitive starts and turns
9 Rescue Introduction to rescues: rope throw; dry rescue Victim simulation; defence and escape
positions
10 Spinal injury management Accompanied rescues; non-contact rescues Contact rescues and spinal cord injury
management
11 Aquatic instruction, and designing sessions for Initiative and judgement Initiative and judgement
different settings
12 Revision of qualitative analysis of swimming Squad organisation and training Practical 200 m individual medley time trial
strokes

used to establish test–retest reliability and values were categorised critical thinking and there is persuasive evidence that students
according to the scale of Landis and Koch (1977). engaged in collaborative learning achieve higher levels of thought
The practical skills assessment consisted of five skills that and retain information longer than students who work individu-
addressed individual swimming, survival, and rescue skills and cor- ally, (Johnson and Johnson, 1986; Totten et al., 1991) and this was
responded to the extended “Can You Swim Intervention” survey. considered desirable.
The items assessed included a continuous swim of up to 425 m The intervention program was conducted over 12 weeks (to
(participant choice of stroke, no speed requirement) assessed on an fit in with the University semester) and each week all par-
eight-point scale ranging from 1, does not attempt test to 8, com- ticipants were required to attend two scheduled 1-h practical
pleted 425 m; a 100 m swim on the back (assessed on a 7-point swimming sessions and one, 1-h theory session where atten-
scale ranging from 1, does not attempt test to 7, completes task dance was expected, although not compulsory. All students could
easily with excellent form and pace; floating, with minimal swim- also attend an additional 1-h optional practical swimming session
ming action (up to 10 min, with the time in minutes recorded as for further assistance, although this session was primarily imple-
the participants’ score). An underwater swim (up to 25 m); and a mented for students with low level entry skills. Five qualified
dive entry (into 2 m water depth) were both assessed on a 6-point AUSTSWIM instructors with extensive experience teaching swim-
scale. A 25 m contact rescue of a simulated unconscious person was ming and water safety and experience teaching at a tertiary level
also included but is not reported in this paper. Following the prac- were employed to conduct the scheduled practical sessions. Each
tical testing all scores were re-coded on a scale of 0, minimum to of the practical sessions contained a maximum of 30 students and
10, maximum to enable comparison of practical items on the same was team-taught by two of the AUSTSWIM qualified instructors.
scale. For further details of the practical test items and how consis- The 1 h theory session was also conducted by an AUSTSWIM quali-
tency in assessment of practical items was established, see Moran fied instructor and course presenter with extensive teaching and
et al. (2012) and Petrass et al. (2012). research experience in injury prevention and risk management,
particularly within the area of water safety and drowning preven-
tion.
2.3. Intervention program

The swimming and water safety intervention program was 2.4. Statistical analysis
embedded within the Swimming and Water Safety course that
formed part of the students’ undergraduate degree. The inter- To enable comparison between pre-intervention and post-
vention was a comprehensive program that encompassed a intervention survey and practical data, each participant was
combination of personal swimming, survival and rescue skills along allocated a unique eight digit identification number at the com-
with water safety knowledge applicable to a range of aquatic envi- mencement of the study. All data were double entered and cleaned
ronments (Table 1). This was considered important, particularly as in Microsoft Excel and the cleaned data were transferred to SPSS
Australian drowning statistics indicate that older children (5–19 (version 19) for statistical analysis.
years) are likely to be swimming and recreating at inland water- Frequencies and percentages were used to summarise categor-
ways including rivers, streams and creeks immediately prior to ical variables, particularly demographic information and aquatic
drowning (Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2012a). participation and behaviour data. As the data were categorical and
The program also focused on developing student understanding ordinal, non-parametric statistics were conducted. The Wilcoxon
of teaching strategies and teaching sequences for aquatic skills matched pairs signed ranks test was performed to detect any sig-
including survival and competitive swimming strokes (Table 1). A nificant differences between knowledge, swimming ability and
collaborative and social learning pedagogy (i.e., participative learn- attitude scores pre- and post-intervention. Mann–Whitney tests
ing) was implemented as this enabled students to exchange ideas in were used to establish whether there were significant differences
small groups, engage in discussion, and take responsibility for their in swimming ability, attitudes and knowledge pre-intervention
own learning (for example, students were frequently required to and post-intervention for male and females and also for those
observe peers, assess causes of faulty technique and apply effec- who had previously participated in formal swimming lessons
tive teaching practices and strategies to correct the identified and/or swimming as part of the school curriculum and those that
faults). Research indicates that this pedagogical approach promotes had not. Finally, Spearman’s correlation coefficient was used to
L.A. Petrass, J.D. Blitvich / Accident Analysis and Prevention 70 (2014) 188–194 191

determine the relationship between participants’ baseline self-


rated swimming ability and pre-intervention practical testing and
the equivalent post-intervention measures.

3. Results

Test–retest assessment indicated that the majority (40.9%) of the


new questions had substantial agreement. Other questions were
calculated to have slight (7.6%), fair (4.5%), moderate (30.3%) or
almost perfect (16.7%) agreement. Accordingly, as less than 10%
of questions had slight agreement (and these were predominately
related to the attitude questions) the extended “Can You Swim
Intervention” survey was considered appropriate for this study.
A total of 154 students were enrolled in the Swimming and
Water Safety course, however, removal of missing data (e.g., where
the student left the Bachelor of Exercise and Sport Science course;
was injured part-way through the intervention program; did not Fig. 1. Median scores (out of a maximum score of 10) on the five practical test
items pre-intervention and post-intervention (both males and females, combined).
attend 90% of practical swimming sessions; or was absent at *
Denotes significant improvement from pre-intervention to post-intervention,
the time of follow-up testing) reduced the sample size to 135 p < 0.001.
(88%). Gender balance was relatively even (56.4% males and 43.6%
females) and the majority were aged between 18 and 19 years
(82.2%) with a few participants aged 20–24 years (17.8%). Slightly post-intervention (Table 2). For those statements that were signifi-
more than half (57.5%) reported that they had undertaken formal cantly different, with the exception of one statement (regardless of
swimming lessons (outside of school) however, of these only 55.8% your swim ability, you should always swim between the flags) par-
reported the length of time they were enrolled in lessons. The ticipants reported a more conservative response post-intervention
majority (67.4%) reported that they had completed ≤2 years, whilst (Table 2). Mann–Whitney tests indicated that females were sig-
32.6% had completed >2 years. Almost half (44.8%) of the partici- nificantly more conservative in their pre-intervention attitudes
pants reported that they had taken part in high school swimming (Mdn = 40) than males (Mdn = 38) (U = 1575.5, z = −2.73, p = 0.006,
and close to one fifth (18.7%) reported that they had participated r = −0.24), but there were no significant gender differences post-
in squad level swimming (defined as advanced swimming instruc- intervention (U = 1994.0, z = −0.82, p = 0.410, r = −0.07).
tion focussing on stroke correction and fitness). Reported level of Compared with the pre-intervention practical testing, post-
participation in aquatic activities within the last 12 months varied intervention showed statistically significant improvement in
according to the aquatic environment, for example the majority of the participants’ overall swimming ability (z = −8.87, p < 0.001,
participants reported never or not often swimming at rivers, creeks r = −0.77). When analysed according to each individual skill, sta-
(69.9%) and lakes or ponds (72.6%). In contrast, over half the par- tistically significant improvement was evident in all skills except
ticipants reported swimming often or very often at public pools the float, which was essentially at ceiling level pre-intervention
and beaches (51.9% and 54.9%, respectively). Very few participants (Fig. 1).
held a pool lifeguard certificate (11.9%) or an AUSTSWIM Teacher Prior to and following the intervention, participants rated their
of Swimming and Water Safety (5.9%) qualification. swimming ability on a 10 point scale from 0, not at all confident to
At baseline, participants typically had a low level of swim- 10, very confident. Results indicated that participants’ perceptions
ming, water safety and aquatic knowledge (Mdn = 40). In relation of their own swimming ability mirrored the change that was evi-
to gender differences, males were significantly more likely to dent in the practical swimming ability pre-and post-intervention.
receive a higher knowledge score than females (Mdn = 40 versus 30, At baseline, perceived swimming ability was rated significantly
respectively) on the baseline survey (U = 1677.5, z = −2.20, p = 0.028, lower (Mdn = 6) than after the intervention (Mdn = 8) (z = −4.22,
r = 0.19). Neither formal swimming lessons (Mdn = 35) (adjusted p = <0.001, r = 0.36). Further analysis indicated that participant
for the length of time enrolled) nor participation in swimming baseline self-rated swimming ability was positively correlated with
lessons/classes within the school curriculum (Mdn = 40) had a sig- their pre-intervention practical testing (rs = 0.48, p < 0.001) as were
nificant impact on participants’ baseline swimming, water safety the equivalent post-intervention measures (rs = 0.41, p < 0.001).
and aquatic knowledge (U = 177.0, z = −0.52, p = 0.601, r = 0.08 and, There was no significant difference in the overall pre-
U = 89.5, z = −1.49, p = 0.137, r = 0.19, respectively). Following the intervention swimming ability of participants who reported
12-week intervention program there was a significant increase undertaking formal swimming lessons (Mdn = 40) or swimming
(z = −1.49, p < 0.001, r = 0.13) in participants’ swimming, water lessons/classes within the school curriculum (Mdn = 40) and those
safety and aquatic knowledge, evidenced by an improved median who did not (Mdn = 41) (U = 1744.0, z = −1.88, p = 0.600, r = 0.16
score of 70. There were no statistically significant post-intervention and, U = 106.0, z = −1.08, p = 0.278, r = 0.19, respectively). Further,
differences in the level of knowledge between male and female there was no difference in swimming ability when length of
participants (Mdn = 70.00 for both males and females, U = 1980.0, time in formal lessons was accounted for (Mdn = 40, U = 142.5,
z = −0.74, p = 0.460, r = 0.06) or for participants who had reported z = −1.57, p = 0.116, r = 0.24). However, the participants who had
that they had undertaken formal swimming lessons (Mdn = 70, undertaken either formal swimming lessons (Mdn = 43.8) or swim-
U = 151.5, z = −1.37, p = 0.172, r = 0.21) or swimming lessons/classes ming lessons/classes within the school curriculum (Mdn = 42.1)
within school (Mdn = 70, U = 127.0, z = −0.52, p = 0.600, r = 0.06). were of a significantly higher standard following the interven-
Evaluation of participant attitudes pre (Mdn = 39) and post- tion (U = 1684.5, z = −2.30, p = 0.021, r = 0.28 and U = 55.0, z = −2.35,
intervention (Mdn = 40) indicated there was no significant p = 0.019, r = 0.29, respectively), regardless of whether they were
change in attitudes overall (z = −1.75, p = 0.079, r = −0.15). When involved for ≤2 years or >2 years. Unlike the knowledge compo-
statements were analysed individually, only one third of state- nent, there were no statistically significant pre-post intervention
ments had a significantly different response pre-intervention to differences in the level of swimming ability between male and
192 L.A. Petrass, J.D. Blitvich / Accident Analysis and Prevention 70 (2014) 188–194

Table 2
Changes in attitude statements from pre-intervention to post-intervention measured by a five point scale (1 = strongly agree, 5 = strongly disagree).

Attitude statement Median pre (IQRˆ) Median post (IQR) z Value p Value

Regardless of your swim ability, you should always swim between the flags 1 (1) 2 (1) −2.71 0.007*
Drinking alcohol on a small boat or yacht is ok if skipper does not drink 3 (2) 3 (1) −2.98 0.003*
It is ok to swim under the influence of drugs or alcohol 5 (1) 5 (1) −2.61 0.009*
Swimming in the surf after the surf patrol is finished is ok if other people 3 (1) 3 (1) −2.19 0.029*
are still in the water
Swimming outside the flags is ok if the surf looks safe and you’re a good 4 (1) 4 (1) −0.30 0.762
swimmer
It is ok to dive into water without knowing the depth if you know how to 5 (1) 4 (1) −1.91 0.057
perform a safe dive
It is safe to swim at surf beaches and rivers after consuming more than 1–2 4 (1) 4 (1) −1.51 0.130
standard alcoholic drinks
You should never ignore water safety signs or swim in prohibited aquatic 2 (1) 2 (1) −0.96 0.338
settings
Swimming at night is only risky if you are not familiar with the 4 (2) 4 (1) −1.01 0.311
environment
Consuming alcoholic drinks in and around a private pool is safe, if 4 (1) 4 (1) −0.239 0.811
everyone can touch the bottom
Swimming alone is risky even if you are a good swimmer 4 (1) 2 (0) −1.46 0.143
If you are a competent swimmer you should not have to wear a PFD when 4 (1) 4 (1) −1.62 0.105
jet skiing, water skiing or boating

ÎQR = interquartile range.

female participants (pre U = 2134.0, z = −0.05, p = 0.956, r = 0.01; aquatic environment, and personal safety and survival techniques
post U = 2031.0, z = −0.66, p = 0.512, r = 0.05). (Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2010). The lack of
water safety knowledge exhibited by young adults in this study was
4. Discussion therefore disconcerting, and more so as there was no knowledge
difference between individuals who reported previous participa-
There is consensus among the water safety education commu- tion in formal swimming and water safety lessons or swimming as
nity that teaching water safety knowledge and skills will shape part of the school curriculum. This lack of awareness is especially
positive water safety attitudes, perceptions and behaviour in alarming because in Australia, rivers, creeks and streams are the
aquatic environments (Moran, 2008). Further, it is thought the leading location for drowning deaths in the 15–19 year age group
swimming and water safety skills that are taught to children at (Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2012a), however over
a young age (from the age of 5 years) can provide young adults half (54.1%) of study participants indicated that private swimming
with the skills required to safely enjoy the aquatic environment, pools were the main location for drownings. Furthermore, when
and potentially provide the rescue and resuscitation skills nec- asked when swimming in a river or lake should be attempted, the
essary to assist others in aquatic emergencies (Australian Water majority (70.7%) selected one of the least safe options. The limited
Safety Council, 2012). To our knowledge however, no studies to understanding that these young adults have of rivers, creeks and
date have investigated the water safety knowledge, attitudes and streams may be associated with their lack of familiarity in these
swimming ability of young Australian adults or considered whether settings, as evidenced by the majority of participants reporting
participation in formal swimming and water safety lessons and/or that they had never, or not often swam at rivers, creeks, lakes or
involvement in swimming programs within the school curriculum ponds. This lack of aquatic knowledge and familiarity within these
has a significant impact on the water safety knowledge, attitudes settings, coupled with increased risk-taking that is characteristic
and swimming ability of young adults. of this age group (Johnson and Jones, 2011; Steinberg, 2004) is
This study established the swimming and water safety knowl- likely to contribute to increased drowning risk in these aquatic
edge and attitudes of a group of young adults and directly environments.
measured their actual swimming ability using formal testing pro- A lack of knowledge was also evident in relation to personal
cedures. Knowledge, attitudes and swimming ability were then safety and survival techniques, despite the fact that this is included
re-measured following a comprehensive 12-week swimming and in the latter levels of the RLSSA swim and survive program (Active
water safety intervention program, taught by highly qualified and Award 6: Swim and Survive; and Active Award 7: Senior Swim
experienced instructors, to determine its effectiveness. A large and Survive) (Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2010).
number of young adults in this study reported they had undertaken It remains unclear whether the poor knowledge is associated with
previous formal swimming and water safety lessons and/or had a lack of retention of information or whether water safety knowl-
involvement in swimming programs within the school curriculum. edge is not actually taught within some swimming and water safety
This could be influenced by the availability of aquatic environments programs. Results from a recent survey of Australian swim schools
and learn to swim centres in Australia; the existence of a National indicated that over half reported that they incorporate water safety
swimming and water safety program; or a bias associated with a knowledge in every lesson either through water safety tips fol-
sample drawn from a Bachelor of Exercise and Sport Science cohort. lowed by oral questioning or dry land activities (Royal Life Saving
The effectiveness of school swimming programs in educat- and AUSTSWIM Ltd., 2010). However, confirmation of the accuracy
ing students in a wide variety of swimming and water safety of reporting and evaluation of the effectiveness of this approach
skills that includes general swimming techniques and treading to teaching water safety knowledge should be considered. Many
water, survival techniques and strategies, floating and rescue skills swim schools also reported that they conducted a water safety
is unknown. However, according to the “Royal Life Saving Soci- week once per school term to cover all the water safety content but
ety Australia Swim and Survive Program”, participants should be also commented that these are the lessons that children are most
exposed to the aforementioned swimming and water safety skills likely to miss (Royal Life Saving and AUSTSWIM Ltd., 2010). For
and also obtain knowledge in specific areas including dangers in the this reason, RLSSA promotes that water safety skills and knowledge
L.A. Petrass, J.D. Blitvich / Accident Analysis and Prevention 70 (2014) 188–194 193

should be integrated throughout every lesson within a program Overall, this study provides insight into swimming and water
(Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2012b). Education of safety knowledge, attitudes and the actual swimming ability of
parents and young adults is also required, and must highlight the young adults and also illustrates that a short, comprehensive water
importance of swimming and water safety knowledge, combined safety intervention significantly improved water safety knowledge
with adequate swimming and survival skills to decrease drowning and swimming ability in this group. As with all studies, there are
risk. several limitations that must be acknowledged and that merit con-
Despite the low level of water safety knowledge identified in sideration when planning future studies. First, although this study
this study, the findings from this research show that the major- had a high response rate and a relatively large cohort completed
ity of young adults had sound swimming and water safety skills, both questionnaires and practical swimming assessment pre- and
and an excellent ability to float in deep water with all partici- post-intervention, the sample was drawn from a very active group
pants able to float for 10 minutes. This result is consistent with that were doing this program as part of their studies and as such,
research on skill learning, which has demonstrated that physi- were more likely to be motivated to participate than other young
cal tasks display less skill decay than cognitive tasks following a adults. Future work should therefore consider recruiting a more
period of non-practice or non-use (Arthur et al., 1998). When swim- representative national or state sample as this will help draw more
ming ability was analysed by gender, there were no significant skill solid conclusions related to the water safety knowledge, attitudes
differences evident between males and females prior to, or fol- and ability of young adults. Second, self-reporting of involvement
lowing the intervention, a finding which is in contrast to studies in formal swimming and water safety lessons may be viewed as
that have measured self-reported swimming ability, where adoles- problematic. While these young adults appeared to be able to recall
cent males typically report higher levels of swimming ability than whether they had undertaken lessons, questions requiring them to
females (Howland et al., 1996; McCool et al., 2008; Morgan et al., recall the duration of time they were enrolled in lessons and the
2009). age they commenced lessons were poorly answered and may have
Further, there were no differences in swimming and water impacted on the findings of this study. How best to collect accu-
safety skills prior to the intervention for participants who reported rate information relating to young adults’ participation in formal
undertaking formal swimming lessons or swimming within the swimming and water safety lessons requires further consideration.
school curriculum. These participants were however, of a signif- In addition, information regarding the content covered within for-
icantly higher standard following the intervention. It has been mal swimming lessons and school swimming programs, and the
recognised that skill decay is problematic in circumstance where ethos of the organisation would enable a better understanding
individuals receive skills and knowledge that they then may not of the value of swimming and water safety lessons for drowning
use for an extended period of time (Arthur et al., 1998), and this prevention. Finally, as this study primarily aimed to ascertain the
may in part explain why there was no difference in swimming abil- water safety knowledge, attitudes and swimming ability of young
ity at the start of this study. One of the main challenges of working Australian adults and determine the effect of a short swimming
with young adults is capturing this age group for refresher train- and water safety intervention, post-testing was completed in the
ing. Wisher et al. (1991) found that after formal training, only a final week of the program to increase the response rate. How-
limited amount of refresher training, if any, is needed to reacquire ever, as research indicates that there is substantial skill loss with
any skill that has been lost among military recruits. Accordingly, non-practice and non-use, future studies should consider imple-
the intervention in this study is likely to have been a refresher menting a longitudinal study design to track skill retention and
for participants who had undertaken formal swimming lessons or decay. It would also be of interest to identify whether a core set
swimming within the school curriculum, and therefore these par- of major factors influence the retention or decay of swimming
ticipants may have experienced a greater level of overlearning, thus and water safety skills and knowledge over extended periods of
enabling them to move beyond a competent level of skill acquisi- non-use.
tion to a proficient or expert level. While Universities are an ideal
place to capture this age group for further formal and/or refresher
training, a large portion of the population do not attend university 5. Conclusion
and thus strategies to more broadly recruit the young adult age
group are required. Results indicate that young adults in this study had a very low
It has been well documented that attitude has an important level of water safety knowledge, which, if coupled with increased
influence on behaviour (Fishbein and Ajzen, 1975), however there is risk-taking that is characteristic of this age group is likely to con-
lack of evidence of interventions that influence attitude change over tribute to increased drowning risk in aquatic environments. Despite
a period of time. It has been recognised that adult education may the low level of water safety knowledge, findings showed that the
play a role in changing attitudes, although there is little evidence majority of young adults had sound swimming and water safety
to support how or why this occurs (Preston and Feinstein, 2004). skills and attitudes. This study suggests that participation in formal
Results of this study demonstrated that there was no significant swimming lessons or swimming within the school curriculum did
change in attitudes overall, following the intervention. This finding not influence the pre-intervention water safety knowledge, skills
corroborates with findings from a school-based water safety inter- or attitudes of young adults, and there were few significant gen-
vention conducted in Greater Athens, where no significant changes der differences. Following the intervention, participants who had
were observed in high school students’ attitudes pre- and post- reported previous swimming lesson experience demonstrated a
intervention (Terzidis et al., 2007). When results from the current significantly higher practical skill level than those without a his-
study were analysed according to gender, female attitudes were ini- tory of previous lessons. Importantly, this study also identified that
tially very conservative and they changed to be less conservative a short but comprehensive swimming and water safety interven-
post-intervention. This change may be suitable, given the increased tion, comprised of a combination of swimming, survival and rescue
water safety skills and knowledge obtained through the interven- skills along with water safety knowledge, played a positive role in
tion program. Because little other information exists regarding how shaping the water safety knowledge and swimming and survival
attitude impacts on young adults’ behaviour and risk taking in skills of young adults, with significantly improved scores following
aquatic environments, or how to effectively change young adults the intervention. While it is important to conduct further studies
water safety attitudes, there is a need for further research to more to confirm that these findings are consistent with a more represen-
fully understand this area. tative sample of young adults, our findings are the first to provide
194 L.A. Petrass, J.D. Blitvich / Accident Analysis and Prevention 70 (2014) 188–194

empirical evidence of the value of a comprehensive aquatic educa- Marchant, J., Cheng, N.G., Lam, L.T., Fahy, F.E., Soundappan, S.V., Cass, D.T., Browne,
tion program as a drowning prevention strategy for young adults. G.J., 2008. Bystander basic life support: an important link in the chain of survival
for children suffering a drowning or near-drowning episode. Med. J. Aust. 188
(8), 484–485.
Acknowledgements McCool, J.P., Moran, K., Ameratunga, S., Robinson, E., 2008. New Zealand beachgoers’
swimming behaviors, swimming abilities, and perception of drowning risk. Int.
J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 2 (1), 7–15.
This study would not have been possible without the active Moran, K., 2008. Will they sink or swim? New Zealand youth water safety knowledge
involvement of the University of Ballarat Bachelor of Exercise and and skills. IJARE 2 (2), 114–127.
Sports Science students. The authors would also like to thank Dr. Moran, K., Stallman, R., Kjendlie, P., Dahl, D., Blitvich, J.D., Petrass, L.A., McElroy,
G.K., Goya, T., Teramoto, K., Matsui, A., Shimongata, S., 2012. Can you swim?
Peter Kremer (Deakin University) and Dr. Peta White (University An exploration of measuring real and perceived water competency. IJARE 6 (2),
of Ballarat) for their contributions to the extended “Can You Swim 122–135.
Intervention” survey and Holly Burton for assistance with data col- Morgan, D., Ozanne-Smith, J., Triggs, T., 2009. Self-reported water and drowning risk
exposure at surf beaches. Aust. N.Z. J. Public Health 33 (2), 180–188.
lection.
Parker, H.E., Blanksby, B.A., 1997. Starting age and aquatic skill learning in young
children: mastery of prerequisite water confidence and basic locomotion skills.
References Aust. J. Sci. Med. Sport 29 (3), 83–87.
Petrass, L.A., Blitvich, J.D., Finch, C.F., 2011. A lack of caregiver supervision: a con-
tributing factor in Australian unintentional child drowning deaths, 2000–2009.
American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Injury, Violence, and Poison Pre-
Med. J. Aust. 194 (5), 228–231.
vention, 2003. Prevention of drowning in infants, children, and adolescents.
Petrass, L.A., Blitvich, J.D., McElroy, G.K., Harvey, J., Moran, K., 2012. Can you swim?
Pediatrics 112 (2), 440–445.
Self-report and actual swimming competence among young adults in Ballarat,
Arthur, W., Bennett, W., Stanush, P.L., McNelly, T.L., 1998. Factors that influence skill
Australia. IJARE 6 (2), 136–148.
decay and retention: a quantitative review and analysis. Hum. Perform. 11 (1),
Preston, J., Feinstein, L., 2004. Adult Education and Attitude Change No. Wider Ben-
57–101.
efits of Learning Research Report, No. 11. The Centre for Research on the Wider
Australian Water Safety Council, 2012. Australian Water Safety Strategy 2012–2015.
Benefits of Learning Institute of Education, London.
Australian Water Safety Council, Sydney, Australia.
Quan, L., Cummings, P., 2003. Characteristics of drowning by different age groups.
Blanksby, B.A., Parker, H.E., Bradley, S., Ong, S., 1995. Children’s readiness for learning
Inj. Prev. 9, 163–168.
front crawl swimming. JSAMS 27, 34–37.
Royal Life Saving and AUSTSWIM Ltd., 2010. 2010 Survey of Swim School Managers
Brenner, R.A., Saluja, G., Smith, G.S., 2003. Swimming lessons, swimming ability, and
Report. Rotal Life Saving Society and AUSTSWIM Ltd., Sydney, Australia.
the risk of drowning. Inj. Control Saf. Promot. 10 (4), 211–216.
Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2010. Swimming and Lifesaving: Water
Brenner, R.A., Taneja, G., Haynie, D.L., Trumble, A.C., Qian, C., Klinger, R.M., Klebanoff,
Safety for all Australians, sixth ed. Elsevier Mosby, Sydney, Australia.
M.A., 2009. Association between swimming lessons and drowning in childhood:
Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2012a. The Forgotten 50%: Analysis of
a case-control study. Arch. Pediatr. Adolesc. Med. 136 (3), 203–210.
Drowning in Children Aged 5–19 Years in Australia. Royal Life Saving Society
Driscoll, T.R., Harrison, J.E., Steenkamp, M., 2004. Alcohol and drowning in Australia.
Australia, Sydney, Australia.
Inj. Control Saf. Promot. 11 (3), 175–181.
Royal Life Saving Society Australia [RLSSA], 2012b. No Child to Miss Out: Basic Swim-
Fishbein, M., Ajzen, I., 1975. Belief, Attitude, Intention, and Behavior: An Introduction
ming and Water Safety Education—The Right of all Australian Children. Royal Life
to Theory and Research. Addison-Wesley, Reading, MA.
Saving Society Australia, Sydney, Australia.
Fragar, L., Gibson, C., Henderson, A., Franklin, R., 2003. Farmsafe Farms for Kids:
Smith, G.S., 1995. Drowning prevention in children: the need for new strategies. Inj.
Evidence Based Solutions for Child Safety on Australian Farms. Moree: Aus-
Prev. 1 (4), 216–217.
tralian Centre for Agricultural Health and Safety, http://www.aghealth.org.au/
Steinberg, L., 2004. Risk taking in adolescence: what changes and why? Ann. N.Y.
tinymce fm/uploaded/Child%20Safety%20Resources/farmsafe farms kids
Acad. Sci. 1021 (1), 51–57.
solutions child injury.pdf.
Taneja, G., Van Beeck, E., Brenner, R.A., 2008. Drowning. In: Peden, M., Oyegbite, K.,
Howland, J., Hingson, R., Mangione, T.W., Bell, N., Bak, S., 1996. Why are most drown-
Ozanne-Smith, J., Hyder, A.A., Branche, C., Rahman, F., Rivara, F.P., Bartolomeos,
ing victims men? Sex differences in aquatic skills and behaviors. Am. J. Public
K. (Eds.), World Report on Child Injury Prevention. World Health Organisation,
Health 86 (1), 93–96.
Geneva, Switzerland, pp. 59–77.
Irwin, C.C., Irwin, R.L., Ryan, T.D., Drayer, J., 2009. Urban minority youth swim-
Terzidis, A., Koutroumpa, A., Skalkidis, I., Matzavakis, I., Malliori, M., Frangakis, C.E.,
ming (in)ability in the united states and associated demographic characteristics:
DiScala, C., Petridou, E., 2007. Water safety: age-specific changes in knowledge
toward a drowning prevention plan. Inj. Prev. 15 (4), 234–239.
and attitudes following a school-based intervention. Inj. Prev. 13, 120–124.
Johnson, R.T., Johnson, D.W., 1986. Action research: cooperative learning in the
Thompson, D.C., Rivara, F.P., 1998. Pool fencing for preventing drowning in children.
science classroom. Sci. Child. 24 (2), 31–32.
Cochrane Database of Syst. Rev. (1).
Johnson, S.B., Jones, V.C., 2011. Adolescent development and risk of injury:
Totten, S., Sills, T., Digby, A., Russ, P., 1991. Cooperative learning: a guide to research.
using developmental science to improve interventions. Inj. Prev. 17, 50–
Garland, New York, NY.
54.
Wisher, R.A., Sabol, M.A., Sukenik, H.K., Kern, R.P., 1991. Individual Ready Reserve
Landis, J.R., Koch, G.G., 1977. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical
(IRR) Call-up: Skill Decay No. Research Report 1595. U.S Arm Research Institute,
data. Biometrics 33 (1), 159–174.
Alexandria, VA.

Potrebbero piacerti anche