Sei sulla pagina 1di 11

J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267

DOI 10.1007/s10896-016-9825-z

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Big Five Personality Traits and Physical Aggression


between Siblings in South Korea: an Actor-Partner
Interdependence Analysis
Jeong Jin Yu 1 & Gum Ok Lim 2 & Wendy C. Gamble 3

Published online: 19 May 2016


# Springer Science+Business Media New York 2016

Abstract The present study examined actor and partner ef- and van Aken 2003; Gleason et al. 2004; Pursell et al. 2008;
fects of the Big Five personality traits, assessed by the NEO Tani et al. 2003) or general aggression (Barlett and Anderson
Five-Factor Inventory, on physical aggression within sibling 2012; Heaven 1996; Miller et al. 2003; Sharpe and Desai
dyadic interactions. Data were collected from 86 target ado- 2001; Tremblay and Ewart 2005). Yet, to our knowledge no
lescents receiving counseling services, their mothers, and research has been conducted to determine whether personality
closest-age siblings in South Korea. Mothers rated their chil- traits are related to physical aggression between siblings. This
dren’s personalities. Target adolescents and siblings reported issue is worth exploring given that biological siblings share,
their own personalities, as well as their sibling’s and their own on average, 50 % of their genes and family environmental
perpetration of physical aggression against one another. influences that can result in similar attitudes, values, and per-
Substantial self-other (i.e., mother and sibling) agreement sonality traits. Siblings exert influence on one another in their
was found for personality traits. Both actor and partner effects everyday social exchanges and share considerable physical
were found for the negative associations between extraver- aggression (Caffaro 2014; Yu and Gamble 2008, 2012).
sion, openness to experience, and conscientiousness and phys- Thus, the main purpose of this research was to examine the
ical aggression among siblings. The current findings increase association between personality traits and physical aggression
our understanding of personality traits implicated in physical among siblings in order to advance our understanding of the
aggression in general, and specifically aggression among underlying etiology of sibling aggression from a dyadic
siblings. perspective.

Keywords Big Five personality traits . Physical aggression .


Siblings . Actor-partner interdependence model The Big Five Personality Traits

Strong evidence has accumulated in the last decade linking Personality can be defined as relatively consistent and stable
personality traits with physical aggression. Most of this work characteristics that determine how people think, feel, act, and
has focused on either aggressiveness with peers (Asendorpf behave (McCrae and Costa 1990). Although the last several
decades have seen the development of a substantial number of
measures of personality traits, the most popular and widely
accepted model of classifying personality traits is the Big Five
* Jeong Jin Yu (McCrae and Costa 1997; McCrae et al. 2005). Big-Five the-
yjj72@daum.net
ory identifies five broad but distinct dimensions of personality
traits that include: Neuroticism, Extraversion, Openness to
1
Department of Psychology, Umeå University, Experience, Agreeableness, and Conscientiousness (Costa
SE-901 87 Umeå, Sweden and McCrae 1992). Neuroticism refers to an individual’s pro-
2
Gyeonggi Youth Counseling and Welfare Center, Suwon, Gyeonggi, pensity to experience psychological distress and irrational
South Korea thinking. Extraversion is concerned with an individual’s
3
Fun Science for Kids, Tucson, AZ, USA proneness to positive emotions, excitement, stimulation, and
258 J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267

sociability. Openness to experience reflects an individual’s and Roscoe 1990). Similarly, 48 % of undergraduate students
willingness to consider different ideas, values, attitudes, and reported being victims of physical aggression by siblings, and
experiences. Agreeableness is defined as an individual’s incli- 41 % reported being perpetrators of such behaviors during
nation towards interpersonal relationships. Conscientiousness childhood (Hardy 2001). Albeit to a lesser degree, adolescents
denotes an individual’s ability to exercise control over im- reported that they frequently hit or pushed around (24 %) or
pulses and delay gratification. Researchers have established beat up (11 %) their siblings (Duncan 1999). A recent survey
strong reliability and validity of the Big Five personality traits of high school students also reported that 16 % engaged in a
in the child and adolescent age range, gender, as well as across physical fight with siblings over the past 30 days (Johnson et
cultures (McCrae and Costa 1997; McCrae et al. 2005, 2010; al. 2015). While these are all findings based on U.S. samples,
Mervielde et al. 1995; Piedmont and Chae 1997; Yoon et al. those from other countries are scarce. Tippett and Wolke’s
2002). This evidence has been extended to Korean samples, (2015) study is one of the limited studies focusing upon
where a five-factor model (FFM) of personality similar to that sibling aggression among young adolescents in the United
of U.S. samples has been found (Piedmont and Chae 1997; Kingdom, and they found that 20 % of the respondents
Yoon et al. 2002). The FFM also has been successfully repli- perpetrated physical aggression toward siblings and 28 %
cated in six other countries, including South Korea (McCrae were victimized physically by siblings. Using a sample of
and Costa 1997). Given the array of evidence, all these authors university students in Portugal, Relva et al. (2013) docu-
convincingly argue that personality structures are universal mented that 73 % reported physical assault toward siblings,
across cultures. and 71 % were physically assaulted by siblings in the pre-
vious 12 months of this study. However, no known studies
exist on sibling physical aggression during adolescence in
Agreement on Personality Traits among Siblings South Korea.
and Parents There is some literature that reports that physical aggres-
sion between siblings is linked to a variety of maladaptive
While most research on personality traits relies on self-report outcomes. Characteristics of sibling relationships may carry
measures, exploring the extent to which self-ratings of person- over into other relationships, as evidenced in associations be-
ality converge with those provided by significant others par- tween sibling physical aggression and peer bullying, while
tially addresses the issue of reliability and validity on person- physical sibling victimization is linked to peer victimization
ality traits. Although self-reported personality traits may show (Bowes et al. 2014; Johnson et al. 2015; Tippett and Wolke
correspondence with individual behaviors and attitudes (e.g., 2015; Yu and Gamble 2009). Physical aggression between
Barlett and Anderson 2012; Sharpe and Desai 2001), other siblings during childhood is also predictive of later maladjust-
family members’ perceptions could be at least equally predic- ment, such as delinquency, antisocial behavior, depression,
tive of aggression in the context of family dynamics (Yu and self-harm, and anxiety during adolescence (Bank et al. 1996;
Gamble 2008, 2009). Ratings from a sibling may serve as a Bowes et al. 2014; Button and Gealt 2010; Stocker et al.
valuable source of personality assessment because siblings 2002). Thus, these findings call attention to the importance
spend a great deal of time with each other, witness a wide of sibling physical aggression. By extension, an investigation
range of behavior, and judge each other on a daily basis into adolescent sibling aggression in South Korea would be
(Gamble and Yu 2008; Gamble et al. 2011; Lanthier 2000; fruitful, given the literature reviewed and the cultural evidence
Yu and Gamble 2012). While there is still very scant evidence that there is a hierarchical relation among siblings, whereby
for interjudge agreement between siblings on personality older siblings are responsible for the care and support for
traits, Lanthier (2000, p. 404) reported moderate agreement younger siblings and younger siblings respect older siblings,
(mean r = .41; rs = .31 [agreeableness] to .54 [extraversion]) heavily influenced by Confucian values (Barr 2016).
across five personality traits between young adult siblings.

Association between the Big Five Personality Traits


Sibling Physical Aggression and Physical Aggression

Physical aggression among siblings can be defined as any Investigators have demonstrated some interest in identifying
deliberate physical acts or harm directed against siblings, the Big Five personality traits associated with physical aggres-
and this is a pervasive and widespread phenomenon (Caffaro sion, and they seem to have much to offer in this regard. As a
2014; Straus et al. 1996; Tippett and Wolke 2015; Yu and start, personality traits represent general tendencies to act or
Gamble 2012). For example, in one study, approximately react in certain ways and offer some clues to ways of coping
65 % of high school students engaged in physically aggressive with stress (Penley and Tomaka 2002; McCrae and Costa
behavior against siblings during the last 12 months (Goodwin 1997). Indeed, empirical evidence has accumulated over the
J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267 259

past decades suggesting that both agreeableness and consci- personality are associated with their own (actor effects) and
entiousness are negatively associated with physical aggression sibling’s physical assault (partner effects) using structural
among children (Asendorpf and van Aken 2003; Book, Volk, equation modeling (SEM). In the same manner, we estimated
& Hosker, 2012), adolescents (Fossati et al. 2012; Gleason et how much the siblings’ scores on each of the Big Five dimen-
al. 2004; Miller et al. 2003; Pursell et al. 2008), or undergrad- sions are associated with their own (actor effects) and target
uate students (Sharpe and Desai 2001; Tremblay and Ewart adolescent’s physical assault (partner effects).
2005). Some studies show that extraversion is negatively as- Because researchers in this area often rely on respondents’
sociated with physical aggression among undergraduate stu- self-reported data as the only source of information on person-
dents (Sharpe and Desai 2001), while others show the oppo- ality traits, we know too little about how personality traits
site association (Barlett and Anderson 2012; Fossati et al. rated by well-acquainted observers, such as siblings and par-
2012). The results of analysis looking for associations be- ents, may be related to physical aggression in sibling relation-
tween neuroticism and physical aggression have similarly ships. Therefore, both self- and informant reports of personal-
been inconsistent, with some studies showing that neuroticism ity were employed to provide more objective and integrated
is positively associated with physical aggression among ado- information. More specifically, the present study utilized six
lescents (Book et al., 2012; Fossati et al. 2012; Miller et al. sets of personality trait ratings: (a) the target’s self-rated per-
2003; Pursell et al. 2008) or undergraduate students (Sharpe sonality, (b) the target’s sibling-rated personality, (c) the tar-
and Desai 2001), and others reporting the opposite (Barlett get’s mother-rated personality, (d) the sibling’s self-rated per-
and Anderson 2012). Openness, although the least studied sonality, (e) the sibling’s target-rated personality, and (f) the
trait, has positively predicted physical aggression among un- sibling’s mother-rated personality.
dergraduate students (Barlett and Anderson 2012).
Even aside from the fact that all of the above studies have
only examined samples from Western industrialized countries, Method
it is not yet clear whether or to what extent these results are
applicable to the sibling context. This warrants attention given Participants
that most siblings share the same parents, grow up in the same
household, and compete for parental resources, or possess Target adolescents were recruited from Gyeonggi Youth
characteristics that can amplify personalities and conflict. Of Counseling and Welfare Center, Suwon, South Korea where-
particular importance is the fact that physical aggression by counselors provide support and advice on various issues
among siblings is widespread but has been less studied than that youth are struggling with or facing. Target adolescents
any other type of family violence (Caffaro 2014; Yu and were receiving counseling services there because of their emo-
Gamble 2008, 2012), and this holds true in South Korea as tional and behavioral problems. Target adolescents, their
well. Taking all of these things together, exploring how the mother, and sibling closest in age participated in this study.
Big Five personality constructs operate with regards to sibling A total of 86 families participated with each member of the
physical aggression among Korean adolescents would, thus, triad successfully completing the surveys. The target adoles-
make a unique contribution to the literature. cents (41 male) ranged in age from 12 to 20 years
(M = 16.1 years, SD = 1.72), whereas their siblings (45 male)
ranged in age from 12 to 22 years (M = 17.3 years, SD = 3.10).
Current Study Thirty-one target adolescents (36 %) were older than their
siblings. Sixteen of the target adolescents had dropped out of
Despite ongoing efforts to investigate the relations between school. There was, however, no statistically significant differ-
the Big Five personality traits and physically aggressive be- ence was found between the target adolescents who had
havior in other relationships, sibling relationships have largely dropped out and those who were attending school with respect
been ignored. To what extent do the Big Five personality traits to personality traits or physical assault toward siblings. The
relate to physical aggression against siblings in a sample of sibling gender composition (older-younger) was 21 male-
youth? In attempting to answer this question in the context of male, 21 female-female, 20 male-female, and 24 female-
sibling dyads, it is important to bear in mind that each sibling male pairs. Mothers’ ages ranged from 35 to 55 years
within the dyad is in an interdependent relationship that (M = 44.4 years, SD = 4.04). Most were married (n = 59,
should be considered simultaneously within a single model 69 %), remarried (n = 2, 2 %), or cohabitating (n = 1, 1 %),
(Greer et al. 2015; Kenny et al. 2006; Yu and Gamble 2008, whereas the remaining were separated or divorced (n = 20,
2012). With such issues in mind, the Actor-Partner 23 %) or widowed (n = 4, 5 %). Sixty-six percent of the
Interdependence Model (APIM: Kenny et al. 2006; Kashy mothers (n = 57) were high school graduates, 19 % (n = 16)
and Kenny 2000) was used to investigate the extent to which college graduates, 2 % (n = 2) some college education, and
target adolescents’ scores on each of the five dimensions of 13 % (n = 11) less than a high school education. Most of the
260 J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267

participants came from low- to lower-middle income families. Results


Specifically, the majority (n = 54, 63 %) reported annual
household incomes of less than ₩30,000,000 (equivalent to Cross-Informant Correlations
approximately $27,100 U.S. dollars), 23 % (n = 20) reported
incomes between ₩30,000,000–₩39,990,000 (between ap- We first calculated the correlations between the study variables
proximately 27,100 and 36,124 U.S. dollars), 4 % (n = 3) (see Table 1). The cross-informant correlations ranged from .51
reported incomes between ₩40,000,000–₩49,990,000 (be- to .85 for personality traits, all p < .001. Target adolescent-
tween approximately 36,134 and 45,158 U.S. dollars), and mother agreement for the target adolescent’s personality traits
11 % (n = 9) reported incomes greater than ₩50,000,000 ranged from .64 (conscientiousness and openness to experi-
(equivalent to approximately $45,167 U.S. dollars).1 ence) to .82 (neuroticism and extraversion), target adolescent-
sibling ranged from .51 (openness to experience) to .85 (extra-
Measures version), and mother-sibling ranged from .53 (openness to ex-
perience) to .82 (extraversion). Similarly, sibling-mother agree-
Personality Personality traits were measured by the NEO ment on the sibling’s personality traits ranged from .70
Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; Costa and McCrae 1992) (agreeableness) to .85 (conscientiousness), sibling-target ado-
designed to assess the Big-Five dimensions: neuroticism, lescent ranged from .65 (agreeableness) to .85 (extraversion),
extraversion, openness, conscientiousness, and agreeable- and mother-target adolescent ranged from .56 (openness to ex-
ness. Mothers completed ratings of their children’s person- perience) to .83 (extraversion). Correlations between the two
alities using the Korean version of the NEO-FFI (Ahn and different informants’ ratings of minor and severe forms of phys-
Lee 1996), which consists of 60 items rated on a 5-point ical violence ranged from .57 to .68, all p < .001.
Likert format ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5
(strongly agree). This is a well-validated and widely used Paired Samples T-Tests
Korean version of the scale that has been shown to be
appropriate for use with adolescents (Kim et al. 2006). Paired-samples t-tests were calculated to compare personality
Both target adolescents and siblings completed the ques- traits or sibling physical aggression in target adolescents with
tionnaires on their own and their siblings’ personalities. closest-age siblings. Target adolescents self-reported signifi-
Cronbach’s alphas ranged from .70 to .96 for mothers, cantly higher neuroticism and significantly lower extraver-
from .69 to .96 for target adolescents, and from .72 to .95 sion, agreeableness, and conscientiousness than siblings did.
for siblings. Target adolescents scored significantly higher on mothers’
ratings of neuroticism and significantly lower on mothers’
Sibling Physical Aggression The Physical Assault scale of ratings of extraversion, openness to experience, agreeable-
the revised Conflict Tactics Scales-sibling version or ness, and conscientiousness than siblings. In the same manner,
CTS2-SP (Straus et al. 1996) was used to measure physical target adolescents scored significantly higher on sibling’s rat-
aggression between siblings. The CTS2, consisting of 12 ings of neuroticism and significantly lower on sibling’s ratings
items, distinguishes between minor (e.g., grabbing, throw- of extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and
ing; five items) and severe (e.g., kicking, choking; seven conscientiousness than they rated siblings. With regard to per-
items) assaults (Straus et al. 1996). Straus et al. (1996) petration, siblings were more likely than target adolescents to
suggest that the CTS2 can be used for siblings after modi- report a higher frequency of severe physical assault (see Table
fying the wording slightly, which produces the CTS2-SP. It 2). However, in terms of prevalence rates, neither target ado-
has been translated into Korean and has been adequately lescents nor siblings were more likely to engage in minor
validated (e.g., Cha and Sohn 2006). The target adolescents (χ2(1) = .75, ns) or severe (χ2(1) = .43, ns) physical assaults.
and their siblings were asked to disclose perpetrating phys- In addition, 66 (76.7 %; 19 were older than their siblings) and
ical aggression towards each other on a scale of 1 (this has 61 (70.9 %; 22 were older than their siblings) target adoles-
never happened) to 6 (more than 20 times in the past year). cents reported perpetrating minor and severe physical assaults
Cronbach’s alphas for the target adolescents and siblings on their siblings, respectively, while 61 (70.9 %; 44 were older
were .71 (minor), .58 (severe) and .84 (minor), .78 (severe), than target adolescents) and 57 siblings (66.3 %; 40 were older
respectively. than target adolescents) did so in the past 12 months. Chi-
square analyses revealed that among both target adolescents
(χ2(1) = 4.06, p < .05 ) and siblings (χ2(1) = 6.09, p < .05 ),
1
According to the Statistics Korea (2012), average monthly household older siblings were more likely than younger siblings to per-
income was ₩3,842,000 in 2011 (annual income of approximately ₩46,
petrate minor physical aggression, whereas there were no such
104,000). Retrieved from http://kostat.go.kr/portal/english/news/1/7/
index.board?bmode=read&bSeq=&aSeq=254308&pageNo= differences in severe physical aggression among target ado-
1&rowNum=10&navCount=10&currPg=&sTarget=title&sTxt= lescents (χ2(1) = .00, ns) or siblings (χ2(1) = 2.84, p < .10).
Table 1 Intercorrelations for the NEO-FFI and sibling physical aggression

Variable 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18

1. mtN
2. mtE −.66***
3. mtO −.03 .12
4. mtA −.11 −.02 .54***
5. mtC −.17*** −.11 .58*** .61***
6. mnN −.35** .43*** −.19† −.38*** −.33**
J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267

7. mnE .13 −.29** .27* .50*** .44*** −.86***


8. mnO .10 −.19† .25* .44*** .34** −.65*** .69***
9. mnA .02 −.07 .22* .40*** .14 −.42*** .43*** .64***
10. mnC .14 −.15 .11 .24* .19† −.62*** .48*** .75*** .58***
11. tN .82*** −.57*** .20† .08 .06 −.38*** .23* .15 .10 .12
12. tE −.71*** .82*** .09 .06 −.08 .29** −.15 −.06 .09 −.03 −.71***
13. tO .02 .01 .64*** .29** .43*** −.16 .21† −.19† .19† .11 .21† .10
14. tA −.24* .14 .41*** .76*** .34** −.13 .32*** .27* .31** .18 −.14 .30** .31**
15. tC −.35** .14 .45*** .41*** .64*** −.12 .26* .25* .21† .09 −.29** .32** .50*** .46***
16. tsN −.34** .37*** −.15 −.30** −.21† .79*** −.72*** −.51*** −.25* −.52*** −.31** .32** −.02 −.17 −.10
17. tsE .17 −.30** .16 .44*** .35** −.71*** .83*** .50*** .19 .31** .16 −.19† .13 .37*** .29** .77***
18. tsO .02 −.15 .17 .22* .36** −.34** .39*** .56*** .31** .45*** .02 −.09 .22* .17 .29** −.37*** .48***
19. tsA −.11 .11 .14 .41*** .19† −.19† .32** .54*** .65*** .39*** −.07 .22* .12 .43*** .33** −.24* .33** .56***
20. tsC .09 −.10 .10 .21† .20† −.45*** .40*** .62*** .38*** .78*** −.05 .04 .06 .20† .25* −.57*** .46*** .71***
21. sN −.40*** .42*** −.25* −.30** −.21† .80*** −.66*** −.44*** −.20† −.45 −.37*** .32** −.13 −.18† −.08 .82*** −.72*** −.31**
22. sE .16 −.32** .25* .44*** .34** −.77*** .82*** .57*** .26* .43*** .18 −.16 .14 .35** .22* −.74*** .85*** .42***
23. sO .12 −.31** .23* .25* .35** −.48*** .45*** .71*** .39*** .64*** .09 −.13 .23* .14 .29* −.48*** .41*** .69***
24. sA .07 −.13 .28** .27* .21† −.33** .31** .66*** .70*** .59*** .08 .06 .24* .29** .28** −.31** .21† .53***
25. sC .16 −.19† .09 .09 .13 −.49 .35** .60*** .42*** .85*** .10 −.07 .12 .09 .11 −.51*** .30** .53***
26. stN .72*** −.54*** .07 −.15 −.03 −.32** .08 .08 −.05 .17 .69*** −.67*** .11 −.29** −.25* −.38*** .18 .20†
27. stE −.70*** .82*** .03 .05 −.10 .41*** −.21 −.13 .01 −.10 −.64*** .85*** −.00 .27* .15 .39*** −.31** −.20†
28. stO −.07 −.08 .53*** .34** .45*** −.10 .14 .06 .11 −.05 .16 −.04 .51*** .26* .28** .01 .02 .03
29. stA −.22* −.00 .44*** .81*** .48*** −.31** .50*** .32** .31** .10 −.08 .17 .31** .77*** .44*** −.21† .44*** .13
30. stC −.21† −.02 .46*** .60*** .69*** −.30** .51*** .34** .33** .04 .05 .07 .42*** .45*** .60*** −.16 .36** .14
31. tM .63 .20† −.19 −.37** −.34** .27* −.36** −.29** −.29** −.15 −.08 .10 −.23* −.33** −.26* .23* −.33** −.25*
32. tS −.00 .21† −.15 −.28* −.32** −.31** −.35** −.29** −.21† −.20† −.12 .10 −.31** −.25* −.26* .24* −.32** −.24*
33. sM −.02 .24* −.33** −.26* −.40*** .24* −.31** −.41*** −.38*** −.18 −.20† .12 −.32** −.12 −.27* .12 −.24* −.42***
34. sS −.02 .19† −.35** −.32** −.36** .25* −.32** −.48*** −.50*** −.26* −.18 .10 −.34** −.20† −.31** .14 −.22* −.43***

Variable 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34

1. mtN
2. mtE
3. mtO
4. mtA
5. mtC
6. mnN
261
Table 1 (continued)
262

Variable 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34

7. mnE
8. mnO
9. mnA
10. mnC
11. tN
12. tE
13. tO
14. tA
15. tC
16. tsN
17. tsE
18. tsO
19. tsA
20. tsC .53***
21. sN −.07 −.41***
22. sE .23* .45*** −.83***
23. sO .40*** .70*** −.46*** .56***
24. sA .65*** .59*** −.25* .35** .69***
25. sC .31** .80*** −.48*** .43*** .68*** .58***
26. stN −.11† .17 −.43*** .14 .23* .10 .31**
27. stE .23* −.10 .47*** −.29** −.25* −.06 −.23* −.76***
28. stO .05 −.11 .01 .00 .13 .13 −.10 .05 .02
29. stA .36** .11 −.23* .45*** .11 .17 −.08 −.39*** .22* .44***
30. stC .32** −.01 −.12 .34** .13 .15 −.11 −.34** .14 .50*** .74***
31. tM −.24* −.20† .18 −.31** −.27* −.28* −.12 .08 .12 −.25* −.41*** −.44***
32. tS −.11 −.24* .22* −.27* −.28** −.22* −.19† −.04 .19† −.22 −.30** −.35** .86***
33. sM −.26* −.21† .13 −.30** −.48*** −.44*** −.17 −.02 .20† −.23* −.22** −.42*** .63*** .57***
34. sS −.34** −.28** .12 −.26* −.51*** −.53*** −.23* −.06 .18† −.26* −.24* −.38*** .68*** .57*** .89***

mt mother’s ratings for target child, mn mother’s ratings for nontarget child, t target child’s self-report, ts target child’s ratings for sibling, s sibling’s self-report, st sibling’s ratings for target child, N
neuroticism, E extraversion, O openness, A agreeableness, C conscientiousness, M ratings of minor forms of physical violence, S ratings of severe forms of physical violence
†p < .10. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001
J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267
J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267 263

Table 2 Descriptive statistics of the Big Five personality traits and .071; Openness for Experience, χ2(30) = 50.47, p < .05,
physical assault (N = 86 dyads)
CFI = .96, TLI = .94, RMSEA = .090; Agreeableness,
Measure Target Sibling Paired t χ2(31) = 42.46, p < .10, CFI = .98, TLI = .97, RMSEA =
adolescent tests .066; Conscientiousness, χ2(30) = 57.28, p < .01, CFI = .96,
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) TLI = .94, RMSEA = .103).
Mother report Neuroticism 3.56 (.56) 2.75 (.61) 7.84***
We then proceeded to separately estimate the five structural
Extraversion 2.80 (.69) 3.18 (.67) −3.24**
models. Because the number of parameters in the structural
models is the same as that in the measurement models, all fit
Openness 2.84 (.39) 3.07 (.38) −4.64***
indices for the structural models remain the same as those for
Agreeableness 2.71 (.59) 3.37 (.56) −9.62***
the measurement models. The unstandardized path coeffi-
Conscientiousness 2.18 (.66) 3.18 (.74) −10.42***
cients are presented in Table 3. Results revealed that neuroti-
Self-report Neuroticism 3.66 (.69) 2.74 (.64) 7.76***
cism had no actor effects on physical assault between siblings.
Extraversion 2.79 (.80) 3.27 (.61) −4.13***
While target adolescents’ neuroticism was not significantly
Openness 3.10 (.40) 3.19 (.43) −1.53
associated with their siblings’ physical assault, siblings’ neu-
Agreeableness 3.03 (.40) 3.37 (.35) −7.12***
roticism was significantly positively associated with target
Conscientiousness 2.75 (.53) 3.37 (.65) −7.23***
adolescents’ physical assault. Siblings’ extraversion was sig-
Sibling report Neuroticism 3.57 (.63) 2.66 (.63) 8.06***
nificantly negatively related to their own physical assault,
Extraversion 2.74 (.80) 3.15 (.64) −3.27**
while no such actor effect was found for target adolescents.
Openness 2.82 (.42) 3.00 (.39) −2.81**
Siblings’ extraversion was negatively associated with target
Agreeableness 2.47 (.65) 3.06 (.60) −7.67**
adolescents’ physical assault, whereas no significant partner
Conscientiousness 2.18 (.69) 3.09 (.75) −8.22***
effect was discovered for siblings’ physical assault. Whereas
Minor physical assault 1.65 (.65) 1.78 (1.00) −1.58 siblings’ openness to experience was significantly negatively
Severe physical assault 1.39 (.42) 1.53 (.71) −2.23* related to their own physical assault, target adolescents’ open-
df = 85. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001 ness to experience was not significantly related to their own
physical assault. Target adolescents’ openness to experience
was significantly negatively related to siblings’ physical as-
Measurement and Structural Models sault, and siblings’ openness to experience was also signifi-
cantly negatively related to target adolescents’ physical as-
The general SEM model comprises two parts, a measurement sault. Actor effects were found for both target adolescents
model and a structural model. Measurement models that de- and siblings, as their agreeableness was significantly negative-
scribe the relations between the observed measures and the ly associated with their own physical assault. However, no
underlying latent constructs were first developed via confir- partner effects were found between one partner’s agreeable-
matory factor analysis and then structural models that test the ness and the other partner’s physical assault. Similarly, two
hypothesized relationships among the latent variables shown significant actor effects were found for both target adolescents
in Fig. 1 were tested using AMOS 18.0 (Arbuckle 2009). and siblings as their conscientiousness was found to be nega-
Multiple fit indices were used to evaluate overall model fit: tively associated with their own physical assault. Whereas
the χ2 statistic, the comparative fit indices (CFI), Tucker- target adolescents’ conscientiousness was negatively associat-
Lewis Index (TLI), and the root mean squared error approxi- ed with their siblings’ physical assault, no significant partner
mation (RMSEA). Models with CFI and TLI values greater effect was found for siblings’ physical assault.
than .95 and RMSEA values less than .06 are deemed accept-
able (Hu and Bentler 1999).
We estimated the five separate measurement models; how- Discussion
ever, we found negative residual variances for the target ado-
lescents’ minor physical aggression or siblings’ serious phys- The current work fills a void in the literature by examining
ical aggression variables. To overcome this problem, we re- actor and partner effects of the Big Five personality traits on
estimated the model with this variance fixed to zero. Because physical aggression between sibling pairs based on a sample
the magnitude of the negative variance estimate was small, our of Korean youth utilizing the APIM analytical technique.
results remained essentially the same. All factor loadings (in- Multiple sources (i.e., self, sibling, and mother) of information
cluding personality traits and physical aggression) were great- on personality traits lend greater credibility to the findings. As
er than .65 (all ps < .001). Each of the five models fitted the such, our findings provide important new insights into under-
data adequately (Neuroticism, χ2(30) = 41.17, p < .10, standing physical aggression between siblings from a dyadic
CFI = .98, TLI = .98, RMSEA = .066; Extraversion, perspective by explicitly taking into account both younger and
χ2(30) = 42.96, p < .10, CFI = .98, TLI = .97, RMSEA = older siblings’ personality traits. In a broad sense, we found
264 J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267

e e e e e

Mother Self- Sibling Minor Serious


report report report

Target adolescent’s a Target adolescent’s


personality physical assault

p
d

p
d

Sibling’s a Sibling’s
personality physical assault

Mother Self- Target adoles-


report cent report Minor Serious
report

e e e e e

Fig. 1 Illustration of relations between personality traits and physical aggression among siblings using APIM. Paths a represent actor effects, while paths
p indicate partner effects. Structural path coefficients and critical ratios are presented in Table 2. e = error, d = disturbance term

significant actor and partner effects of personality on sibling higher mean neuroticism score, but significantly lower mean
physical aggression. extraversion, openness to experience, agreeableness, and con-
There was moderate to high interrater consistency for the scientiousness scores. It has been well documented that per-
target’s personality factors, consistent with previous reports sonality traits exert direct and indirect effects on the develop-
(Connolly et al. 2007). More specifically, the self-other corre- ment of maladjustment (DeNeve and Cooper 1998). For ex-
lation tended to be highest for extraversion and lowest for ample, high neuroticism and low extraversion are associated
openness to experience. Similarly, interrater agreement for with maladaptive coping strategies, hopelessness, anxiety, and
the sibling’s personality factors was found to be moderate to depression (DeNeve and Cooper 1998; Vreeke and Muris
high. The self-other correlation tended to be highest for extra- 2012). Adding to prior work, current findings suggest that
version or conscientiousness and lowest for agreeableness or further attention to personality traits is warranted to screen
openness to experience. Our results are partially consistent potential clients for appropriateness for therapy.
with the results of a meta-analysis reporting highest agreement Although no differences between target adolescents and
for extraversion and lowest agreement for agreeableness based their closest-age siblings were found in prevalence rates of
on 36 studies of adult samples (Connolly et al. 2007). In the minor or severe forms of physical aggression, siblings report-
same vein, college students and their nearest-age sibling have ed higher levels of severe physical aggression towards target
reported highest agreement for extraversion and lowest agree- adolescents than vice versa. This is somewhat contrary to our
ment for agreeableness (Lanthier 2000). In other words, the expectation because siblings in the present study were per-
present study adds to the literature by showing that extraver- ceived as having more favorable personalities. Although the
sion is more easily observable and identifiable than other per- CTS2 used in the present study differentiates between minor
sonality traits, such as openness to experience and agreeable- and severe acts of physical aggression, measures of other
ness within the family context (e.g., Funder and Dobroth types (e.g., reactive vs. proactive) or aspects (e.g., duration)
1987). Thus, the present study shows that not only mothers of physical aggression between siblings would shed light on
but also adolescent siblings can serve as reliable informants on the bigger picture. In terms of sibling birth order, it is worth
personality traits. noting that although no significant differences were observed
Target adolescents receiving counseling services differed in severe forms of physical assault, older siblings, whether
from their siblings closest in age in having a significantly they were target adolescents or not, were more likely than
J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267 265

Table 3 Parameter estimates from the APIM (N = 86 dyads) neuroticism were positively related to target adolescents’
APIM parameters Estimate Z physical aggression, in line with data obtained from several
studies (Book et al., 2012; Fossati et al. 2012; Miller et al.
Actor effects 2003; Pursell et al. 2008; Sharpe and Desai 2001).
Neuroticismt → Physical assaultt .21 1.34 Neuroticism is the general tendency toward negative affective
Neuroticisms → Physical assaults .30 1.39 states, irrational thinking, and impulsive behavior (DeNeve
Partner effects and Cooper 1998; Vreeke and Muris 2012). Thus, those high
Neuroticismt → Physical assaults −.02 −.10 on trait neuroticism may be more prone to psychological dis-
Neuroticisms → Physical assaultt .39** 2.60 tress and impulsiveness. The significant actor and partner ef-
Actor effects fects indicated that siblings higher in extraversion were less
Extraversiont → Physical assaultt .05 .44 likely to perpetrate physical aggression and to elicit target
Extraversions → Physical assaults −.41* −2.32 adolescents’ physical assault in line with a previous study
Partner effects (Sharpe and Desai 2001). An extraverted person is likely to
Extraversiont → Physical assaults .15 .86 seek and enjoy the company of others and be sociable and
Extraversions → Physical assaultt −.37** −3.10 upbeat in general; thus, youth with high extraversion may
Actor effects create fewer physically conflictual situations in the sibling
Opennesst → Physical assaultt −.36 −1.57 context too. While there are few studies that have found a
Opennesss → Physical assaults −1.56*** −4.37 significant association between openness to experience and
Partner effects physical aggression, the current findings revealed that sib-
Opennesst → Physical assaults −.70* −2.37 lings’ openness was significantly and negatively linked to
Opennesss → Physical assaultt −.57* −2.18 their own physical aggression against target adolescents. In
Actor effects addition, target adolescents’ and siblings’ openness was sig-
Agreeablenesst → Physical assaultt −.42** −2.92
nificantly and negatively related to each others’ physical ag-
Agreeablenesss → Physical assaults −1.05*** −4.50
gression. Thus, the actor effect was found for siblings and
Partner effects
partner effects were found for both target adolescents and
siblings. It appears that openness to experience characterized
Agreeablenesst → Physical assaults −.08 −.45
by greater tolerance for different behavior, attitudes, or dispo-
Agreeablenesss → Physical assaultt −.25 −1.54
sitions, particularly, may serve as a significant deterrent to
Actor effects
physical aggression between siblings. The significant actor
Conscientiousnesst → Physical assaultt −.50*** −3.79
effects indicated that target adolescents and siblings higher
Conscientiousnesss → Physical assaults −.32* −2.18
in agreeableness were less likely to exhibit physical aggres-
Partner effects
sion toward each other. Indeed, agreeableness has been found
Conscientiousnesst → Physical assaults −.70*** −3.75
to be closely linked to motivation to maintain harmonious
Conscientiousnesss → Physical assaultt −.11 −1.07
interpersonal relations and to seek a constructive resolution
t target adolescent, s sibling. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001 (Jensen-Campbell et al. 2003). Conscientiousness may predis-
Unstandardized regression coefficients are presented pose youth to act less impulsively toward their siblings by
controlling themselves, and both actor and partner effects sup-
ported this. That is, significant actor effects were found for
younger siblings to perpetrate minor forms of physical assault. both target adolescents and siblings, such that those high in
Because Confucianism that endorses older siblings’ control conscientiousness were less likely to perpetrate physical ag-
and leadership over younger siblings still remains a fairly gression against their sibling. In addition, significant partner
substantial cultural tradition in South Korea, a rigid hierarchi- effects were found for siblings such that target adolescents
cal power structure between older and younger siblings might high in conscientiousness were less likely to elicit their sib-
contribute to the greater aggressiveness of older siblings. ling’s physical aggression.
Alternatively, older siblings, whose physical development The results taken as a whole suggest that not only the
was more advanced than that of younger siblings, might ex- target adolescent’s own personality traits but also the sib-
ploit their physical advantage. Future researchers might want ling’s personality traits have a sizeable impact on one’s
to explore this issue with a scale that measures adherence to physical aggression toward siblings. Given the fact that
Confucianism. sibling relationships are interdependent in nature, the pres-
The present study examined actor and partner effects of ent study echoes the importance of dyadic research ap-
each sibling’s personality traits on both target adolescent’s proaches to understanding the dynamics of sibling interac-
own and his/her sibling’s physical aggression in a single tions, including physical aggression (Kenny et al. 2006; Yu
APIM analysis. Results showed that sibling’s levels of and Gamble 2008, 2012).
266 J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267

Our findings should be interpreted with caution due to types of aggression between siblings and the present study
some limitations; limitations that nevertheless draw attention may serve as a springboard for future investigations on these
to avenues for future research. First, data collected at a single issues.
point in time precluded causality or directionality and thus
future longitudinal research is needed to lend further credence
to our findings. Second, the fact that the sample size in our
study was small and target adolescents were recipients of References
counseling services may impose limits on the generalizability
of our findings to other clinically ascertained samples or more Ahn, C., & Lee, K. (1996). Clinical application of NEO personality in-
diverse community samples. Therefore, our findings await ventory. The Korean Journal of Counseling and Psychotherapy, 8,
65–75.
replication with a larger sample of sibling dyads. By further
Arbuckle, J. L. (2009). Amos 18 user’s guide. Crawfordville: Amos
extension, cultural values (e.g., Confucianism) may moderate Development Corporation.
the association between personality traits and physical aggres- Asendorpf, J. B., & van Aken, M. A. G. (2003). Validity of big five
sion among siblings. Thus, it would be interesting for future personality judgments in childhood: a 9 year longitudinal study.
research to examine whether the present findings are moder- European Journal of Personality, 17, 1–17.
Bank, L., Patterson, G. R., & Reid, J. B. (1996). Negative sibling inter-
ated by cultural values. Finally, self-reported aggression mea-
action patterns as predictors of later adjustment problems in adoles-
sures are prone to social desirability bias, which might com- cent and young adult males. In G. H. Brody (Ed.), Sibling relation-
promise the results of the study. Researchers will need to ame- ships: Their causes and consequences (pp. 197–229). Norwood:
liorate these effects through careful methodological consider- Ablex.
ations, such as sibling ratings of aggressiveness, self-ratings of Barlett, C. P., & Anderson, C. A. (2012). Direct and indirect relations
between the Big 5 personality traits and aggressive and violent be-
victimization, and controlling for social desirability (Tan and havior. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 870–875.
Grace 2008). Barr, M. D. (2016). Confucianism: Classical, neo- and Bnew^. In J.
Haynes (Ed.), Routledge handbook of religion and politics (2nd
ed., pp. 55–69). New York: Routledge.
Conclusion Book, A. S., Volk, A. A., & Hosker, A. (2012). Adolescent bullying and
personality: An adative approach. Personality and Individual
Differences, 52, 218–223.
The present study is the first, to our knowledge, to evaluate Bowes, L., Wolke, D., Joinson, C., Lereya, S. T., & Lewis, G. (2014).
actor and partner effects for relations between the Big Five Sibling bullying and risk of depression, anxiety, and self-harm: a
personality traits obtained from multiple informants related to prospective cohort study. Pediatrics, 134, e1032–e1039.
physical aggression among sibling dyads. Self- and other- Button, D. M., & Gealt, R. (2010). High risk behaviors among victims of
sibling violence. Journal of Family Violence, 25, 131–140.
ratings of the target’s personality produced very similar results
Caffaro, J. V. (2014). Sibling abuse trauma: Assessment and intervention
and thus, the present study shows that mothers and adolescent strategies for children, families, and adults (2nd ed.). New York:
siblings can serve as reliable reporters of personality. Of par- Routledge.
ticular importance are the significant associations between Cha, B. K., & Sohn, J. N. (2006). Stress, anger, anger expression, and
personality traits and physical aggression between siblings depression between battered women and non-battered women.
Journal of Korean Academy of Psychiatric and Mental Health
from a dyadic perspective, including actor and partner effects. Nursing, 15, 187–196.
It is not only an adolescent’s own personality but also a sib- Connolly, J. E., Kavanagh, E. J., & Viswesvaran, C. (2007). The conver-
ling’s personality that serve as possible correlates of physical gent validity between self and observer ratings of personality: a
aggression between siblings. Our findings have direct links to meta-analytic review. International Journal of Selection and
practical implications to prevent or reduce physical aggression Assessment, 15, 110–117.
Costa Jr., P. T., & McCrae, R. R. (1992). Professional manual: Revised
between siblings. At the most basic level, it will be more NEO personality inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO five-factor inven-
efficient to have both younger and older siblings in various tory (NEO-FFI). Lutz: Psychological Assessment Resources.
interventions to regulate physical aggression against siblings DeNeve, K. M., & Cooper, H. (1998). The happy personality: a meta-
and to further enhance their well-being. The next recommen- analysis of 137 personality traits and subjective well-being.
dation is to rigorously assess both siblings’ personality traits at Psychological Bulletin, 124, 197–229.
Duncan, R. D. (1999). Peer and sibling aggression: an investigation of
the outset of therapy or counseling for screening purposes to intra- and extra-familial bullying. Journal of Interpersonal Violence,
identify their own strengths and weaknesses and further help 14, 871–886.
them capitalize on their strengths and bolster their weak- Fossati, A., Borroni, S., & Maffei, C. (2012). Bullying as a style of
nesses. Generally, not only can extreme levels of personality personal relating: personality characteristics and interpersonal as-
pects of self-reports of bullying behaviours among Italian adolescent
traits be identified early in life, but they can also be amelio-
high school students. Personality and Mental Health, 6, 325–339.
rated at a younger age through proper parenting. It is our hope Funder, D. C., & Dobroth, K. M. (1987). Differences between traits:
that future research will delve further into personality traits properties associated with interjudge agreement. Journal of
and their associations with not only physical but also other Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 409–418.
J Fam Viol (2017) 32:257–267 267

Gamble, W. C., & Yu, J. J. (2008). Adolescent siblings’ looking glass Penley, J. A., & Tomaka, J. (2002). Associations among the Big Five,
self-orientations: patterns of liabilities and associations with parent- emotional responses, and coping with acute stress. Personality and
ing. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 37, 860–874. Individual Differences, 32, 1215–1228.
Gamble, W. C., Yu, J. J., & Kuehn, E. D. (2011). Adolescent sibling Piedmont, R. L., & Chae, J.-H. (1997). Cross-cultural generalizability of
relationship quality and adjustment: sibling trustworthiness and the five-factor model of personality: development and validation of
modeling, as factors directly and indirectly influencing these asso- the NEO PI-R for Koreans. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology,
ciations. Social Development, 20, 605–623. 28, 131–155.
Gleason, K. A., Jensen-Campbell, L. A., & Richardson, D. S. (2004). Pursell, G. R., Laursen, B., Rubin, K. H., Booth-LaForce, C., & Rose-
Agreeableness as a predictor of aggression in adolescence. Krasnor, L. (2008). Gender differences in patterns of association
Aggressive Behavior, 30, 43–61. between prosocial behavior, personality, and externalizing prob-
Goodwin, M. P., & Roscoe, B. (1990). Sibling violence and agonistic lems. Journal of Research in Personality, 42, 472–481.
interactions among middle adolescents. Adolescence, 25, 451–467. Relva, I. C., Fernandes, O. M., & Costa, R. (2013). Psychometric prop-
Greer, K. B., Campione-Barr, N., & Lindell, A. K. (2015). Body talk: erties of revised conflict tactics scales: Portuguese sibling version
siblings’ use of positive and negative body self-disclosure and asso- (CTS2-SP). Journal of Family Violence, 27, 577–585.
ciations with sibling relationship quality and body-esteem. Journal Sharpe, J. P., & Desai, S. (2001). The revised NEO personality inventory
of Youth and Adolescence, 44, 1567–1579. and the MMPI-2 psychopathology five in the prediction of aggres-
Hardy, M. S. (2001). Physical aggression and sexual behavior among sib- sion. Personality and Individual Differences, 31, 505–518.
lings: a retrospective study. Journal of Family Violence, 16, 255–268.
Statistics Korea. (2012). Household Income & Expenditure. Retrieved
Heaven, P. C. L. (1996). Personality and self-reported delinquency: anal-
from http://kostat.go.kr/portal/english/news/1/7/index.board?
ysis of the BBig Five^ personality dimensions. Personality and
bmode=read&bSeq=&aSeq=254308&pageNo=1&rowNum=
Individual Differences, 20, 47–54.
10&navCount=10&currPg=&sTarget=title&sTxt=.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covari-
ance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus new alterna- Stocker, C. M., Burwell, R. A., & Briggs, M. L. (2002). Sibling conflict in
tives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. middle childhood predicts children’s adjustment in early adoles-
Jensen-Campbell, L. A., Gleason, K. A., Adams, R., & Malcolm, K. T. cence. Journal of Family Psychology, 16, 50–57.
(2003). Interpersonal conflict, agreeableness, and personality devel- Straus, M. A., Hamby, S. L., Boney-McCoy, S., & Sugarman, D. B.
opment. Journal of Personality, 71, 1059–1085. (1996). The revised conflict tactics scales (CTS2): development
Johnson, R. M., Duncan, D. T., Rothman, E. F., Gilreath, T. D., and preliminary psychometric data. Journal of Family Issues, 17,
Hemenway, D., Molnar, B. E., et al. (2015). Fighting with siblings 283–316.
and with peers among urban high school students. Journal of Tan, L., & Grace, R. C. (2008). Social desirability and sexual offenders: a
Interpersonal Violence, 30, 2221–2237. review. Sexual Abuse: Journal of Research and Treatment, 20, 61–
Kashy, D. A., & Kenny, D. A. (2000). The analysis of data from dyads 87.
and groups. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of re- Tani, F., Greenman, P. S., Schneider, B. H., & Fregoso, M. (2003).
search methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 451–477). Bullying and the big five: a study of childhood personality and
New York: Cambridge University Press. participant roles in bullying incidents. School Psychology
Kenny, D. A., Kashy, D. A., & Cook, W. L. (2006). Dyadic data analysis. International, 24, 131–146.
New York: Guilford Press. Tippett, N., & Wolke, D. (2015). Aggression between siblings: associa-
Kim, D.-I., Hong, S., & In, H.-Y. (2006). A validation study of personality tions with home environment and peer bullying. Aggressive
assessment inventory based on Big Five factors for adolescents in Behavior, 41, 14–24.
Korea. Journal of Yeolin Education, 14, 289–305. Tremblay, P. F., & Ewart, L. A. (2005). The Buss and Perry aggression
Lanthier, R. L. (2000). Intersibling agreement for Golderberg’s big five questionnaire and its relation to values, the Big Five, provoking
adjective markers. Psychological Reports, 86, 400–406. hypothetical situations, alcohol consumption patterns, and alcohol
McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr., P. T. (1990). Personality in adulthood. New expectancies. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 337–346.
York: Guilford. Vreeke, L. J., & Muris, P. (2012). Relations between behavioral inhibi-
McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr., P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a tion, Big Five personality factors, and anxiety disorder symptoms in
human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516. non-clinical and clinically anxious children. Child Psychiatry and
McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., & 79 Members of the Personality Profiles Human Development, 43, 884–894.
of Cultures Project (2005). Personality profiles of cultures: Yoon, K., Schmidt, F., & Ilies, R. (2002). Cross-cultural construct validity
Aggregate personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social of the five-factor model of personality among Korean employees.
Psychology, 89, 407–425. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33, 217–235.
McCrae, R. R., Terracciano, A., De Fruyt, F., De Bolle, M., Gelfand, M. Yu, J. J., & Gamble, W. C. (2008). Familial correlates of overt and rela-
J., Costa Jr., P. T., et al. (2010). The validity and structure of culture- tional aggression between young adolescent siblings. Journal of
level personality scores: data from ratings of young adolescents. Youth and Adolescence, 37, 655–673.
Journal of Personality, 78, 815–838.
Yu, J. J., & Gamble, W. C. (2009). Adolescent relations with their
Mervielde, I., Buyst, V., & De Fruyt, F. (1995). The validity of the Big-
mothers, siblings, and peers: an exploration of the roles of maternal
Five as a model for teachers’ ratings of individual differences among
and adolescent self-criticism. Journal of Clinical Child and
children aged 4–22 years. Personality and Individual Differences,
Adolescent Psychology, 38, 672–683.
18, 525–534.
Miller, J. D., Lynam, D., & Leukefeld, C. (2003). Examining antisocial Yu, J. J., & Gamble, W. C. (2012). Sibling maltreatment. In R. J.
behavior through the lens of the five factor model of personality. Levesque (Ed.), Encyclopedia of adolescence (pp. 2717–2725).
Aggressive Behavior, 29, 497–514. New York: Springer.

Potrebbero piacerti anche