Sei sulla pagina 1di 20

Interview and Observation Methods

in Functional Assessment 7
Alison M. Kozlowski and Johnny L. Matson

Act Amendments of, 1997, 20 U.S.C. Section


Introduction 1400 et seq, 1997; Individuals with Disabilities
Education Act Amendments of, 2004, 11 Stat. 37
Challenging behaviors are evinced by individuals
U.S.C. Section 1401, 2004). However, the meth-
who have a variety of disabilities including those
ods of conducting a functional behavioral assess-
with intellectual disability (ID; McClintock, Hall,
ment need not be identical across cases.
& Oliver, 2003; Poppes, van der Putten, &
Techniques are often individualized depending
Vlaskamp, 2010) and/or autism spectrum disor-
on the frequency and severity of the challenging
ders (ASD; Matson, Wilkins, & Macken, 2009;
behavior, availability of resources, and informa-
Mudford et al., 2008; Murphy, Healy, & Leader,
tion that has already been acquired regarding the
2009), as well as those individuals who have mild
challenging behavior and its function(s).
disabilities or do not possess any documented
Functional behavioral assessment, in general,
disabilities (Gettinger & Stoiber, 2006; Kinch,
refers to methods of ascertaining the maintaining
Lewis-Palmer, Hagan-Burke, & Sugai, 2001).
variables of challenging behaviors through both
When formulating treatment plans for these indi-
experimental and nonexperimental means, and it
viduals, clinicians often state that conducting a
comprises three main categories: indirect or anec-
functional behavioral assessment is an integral
dotal assessments, descriptive or naturalistic
part of the process and assists in treatment plan-
observational assessments, and experimental
ning. In fact, federal law in the USA currently
functional analysis (EFA) (Iwata, Vollmer,
mandates that treatment of all challenging behav-
Zarcone, & Rodgers, 1993; Neidert, Dozier,
iors is based on the results of a functional behav-
Iwata, & Hafen, 2010). Although only the former
ioral assessment as stated in the Individuals with
two is discussed in this chapter, it is first critical
Disabilities Education Act Amendments of, 1997
to understand the difference between functional
and 2004 (Individuals with Disabilities Education
behavioral assessment in general and EFA. While
these terms may seem synonymous and are often
confused as such, they are not and should not be
A.M. Kozlowski (*) used interchangeably. Functional behavioral
Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, assessment includes a group of possible strategies
LA 70803, USA
used to determine the function(s) of challenging
e-mail: alikoz@gmail.com
behavior, whereas EFA, which is one type of
J.L. Matson
functional behavioral assessment, refers solely to
Department of Psychology, Louisiana State University,
Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA the experimental manipulation of environmen-
e-mail: johnmatson@aol.com tal variables to achieve this same information.

J.L. Matson (ed.), Functional Assessment for Challenging Behaviors, 105


Autism and Child Psychopathology Series, DOI 10.1007/978-1-4614-3037-7_7,
© Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2012
106 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

The remaining functional behavioral assessment assessment methods tend to be necessary and/or
techniques do not incorporate experimental preferred in many cases.
manipulation of variables. The focus of this chapter is on interview and
EFA is commonly viewed as the hallmark of observations methods that may be completed as a
functional behavioral assessment (Hanley, Iwata, part of the functional behavioral assessment pro-
McCord, 2003; Iwata, Dorsey, Slifer, Bauman, & cess. First, overall interview methods is addressed
Richman, 1982/1994; Neidert et al., 2010). This with descriptions of some of the most commonly
is largely in part due to the fact that EFA is the used and most researched interviews currently
only established way in which a causal relation- available being provided. Next, a similar over-
ship can be determined between different func- view is given for direct observation methods and
tions and behaviors, while other functional their examples. Then, since many studies explor-
behavioral assessment strategies only indicate ing the psychometrics of both interviews and
which functions and behaviors correlate with one direct observation methods are in comparison to
another. An earlier chapter of this book provides one another as well as other functional behavioral
an in depth review of EFA and its components, so assessment methods, psychometric data, advan-
it will not be discussed thoroughly here. However, tages, and disadvantages of these tools are offered
it is important to note here that, despite its elite and compared in subsequent sections.
status, EFA is not always practical or safe to
employ. In these circumstances, other functional
behavioral assessment methods, such as those Interview Methods
that will be reviewed here, are necessary.
Instances in which EFA would not be deemed Interviews are among the most common func-
appropriate include when the behavior is not tional behavioral assessment strategies employed
occurring frequently enough to adequately assess (Ellingson, Miltenberger, & Long, 1999; Rojahn,
it in such a setting (Matson & Minshawi, 2007). Whittaker, Hoch, & González, 2007). Use of
If the challenging behavior is occurring only interviews allows clinicians to collect a variety of
rarely, the chances of it occurring within a con- information regarding the challenging behavior(s)
trived setting are also low. The safety of the indi- and bypasses many of the concerns with EFA.
vidual and others also needs to be given Such methodology does not require the target
consideration when conducting a functional behavior to be exhibited during the assessment
behavioral assessment. Severe behaviors that process, which permits assessment of less fre-
may cause injury to the self or others are not ide- quently occurring behaviors and those behaviors
ally assessed through EFA. This is because an that pose serious danger or risk to the self or oth-
EFA requires that the challenging behavior occurs ers. Furthermore, an interview of this kind could
without interruption. Therefore, safety parame- be viewed as a broadband functional assessment
ters frequently employed in the naturalistic set- measure. In contrast to EFA and many scaling
ting would actually interfere with identifying the methods such as the Questions About Behavioral
function of the behavior. Another concern with Function (QABF; Matson & Vollmer, 1995) and
EFA is that factors related to the challenging Motivation Assessment Scale (MAS; Durand &
behavior may be unable to be integrated into the Crimmins, 1992), all of which are thoroughly
assessment process, such as the behavior occur- reviewed in other chapters of this book, the results
ring with specific caregivers (English & Anderson, provide clinicians with comprehensive informa-
2004). Furthermore, EFA typically requires large tion surrounding the target behavior that may
amounts of resources including trained staff, otherwise not be considered. Responses are typi-
significant periods of time, reinforcers, and work cally open-ended and are, therefore, not limited
space that is not always readily available to clini- or restricted by confounding variables or specific
cians or facilities (Matson & Minshawi, 2007). categories of functions. However, as will be dis-
Therefore, alternative functional behavioral cussed later on with respect to the interviews
7 Interview and Observation Methods 107

reviewed herein, there are also drawbacks with The interview was created by modifying the FAIF
functional behavioral assessment interviews, as (O’Neill et al., 1997) and is administered in a
with any other assessment strategy. similar fashion. However, rather than requiring
45–90 min to complete, the FACTS only requires
10–25 min, with administration time dependent
Teacher/Caregiver Interviews on how knowledgeable the informant is with
respect to the student being assessed and the
Functional Analysis Interview Form number and complexity of challenging behaviors
The Functional Analysis Interview Form (FAIF) in question (McIntosh et al., 2008). The interview
is one of the most popular and frequently used comprises two parts: Part A begins by collecting
interview measures for assessing the function(s) narrative information regarding strengths of the
of challenging behaviors. The interview is admin- individual, identifying problem behaviors, and
istered to someone familiar with the individual identifying routines during which the behaviors
being assessed (e.g., parent and caregiver) and most commonly occur (e.g., when, where, and
takes approximately 45–90 min to complete with whom). The last section is completed by
(O’Neill et al., 1997). The FAIF is a paper-and- asking the respondent to provide the interviewer
pencil interview and primarily elicits information with the student’s daily schedule including activi-
through open-ended questions related to the ties, individuals present during different activi-
behaviors in question. It comprises 11 sections ties, the specific problem behaviors elicited at
which probe for information regarding the fol- different times, and the likelihood of these behav-
lowing: (1) descriptions of the behaviors, (2) iors occurring during these times, which is rated
potential bioenvironmental events that may affect on a scale from 1 (low) to 6 (high). Up to three
the behaviors, (3) events and situations that pre- routines are then selected for further assessment
dict the presence of the behaviors, (4) the func- in Part B based on similar behaviors being likely
tions or consequences maintaining the behaviors, to occur during certain conditions.
(5) the efficiency of the behaviors, (6) functional During Part B, each routine identified during
alternative behaviors the individual already dis- Part A is examined separately. Therefore, up to
plays, (7) the individual’s communicative abili- three Part B assessments may be conducted for
ties, (8) things to do and avoid when working the individual. After identifying which routine
with the person to increase their success, (9) rein- will be examined during each specific Part B
forcing items, activities, or events for the indi- assessment, more details regarding the problem
vidual, (10) behavior and treatment history, and behavior are gather through open-ended ques-
(11) a diagram to summarize the information col- tions (e.g., operationally defining the behavior,
lected regarding predictors and/or consequences frequency, duration, and intensity). Next, predic-
of challenging behaviors. Interviewers pose the tors/antecedents and then consequences are
questions to respondents and record the respon- explored with many options being made avail-
dent’s answers in the appropriate space. Follow-up able as well as giving the respondent the opportu-
questions may be asked as needed throughout the nity to include self-identified predictors and/or
interview. consequences. A summary of the behavior is then
compiled, which integrates the antecedents,
Functional Assessment Checklist: behavior, and consequences. This information is
Teachers and Staff later used for development of a treatment plan.
Although its name may imply otherwise, the The respondent rates their confidence in the com-
Functional Assessment Checklist: Teachers and piled summary statement on a scale of 1 (not very
Staff (FACTS) (March et al., 2000) is a semi- confident) to 6 (very confident). Lastly, strategies
structured interview to be used for functional previously and/or currently used for preventing
behavioral assessments with student populations. and treating the problem behavior are named.
108 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Functional Assessment and Intervention Student-Assisted Functional


Program Assessment Interview
The Functional Assessment and Intervention The Student-Assisted Functional Assessment
Program (FAIP) is a computer-based functional Interview (Kern, Dunlap, Clarke, & Childs, 1994)
behavioral interview program originally devel- was the first interview of its kind. The interview
oped for use in school settings (University of is divided into four sections and takes approxi-
Utah, Utah State University, & Utah State mately 20–30 min to administer. The first section
Department of Education, 1999 as cited in contains 12 questions regarding the student’s
Hartwig, Tuesday Heathfield, & Jenson, 2004). schoolwork and classroom to which the student
The program guides the interviewee through five can respond “always,” “sometimes,” or “never.”
sections pertaining to a specific individual and In the second section, open-ended questions are
his/her targeted challenging behavior. In the first posed to the student to assess why and when the
three sections, the interviewee is asked to provide targeted challenging behavior occurs, what
information regarding identifying and setting changes could be made within the school setting
information, antecedents, and consequences. to alleviate the student’s difficulties and to iden-
Prior to continuing, the interviewee is then tify rewards/activities that the student enjoys.
prompted to confirm or disconfirm all anteced- Next, the student is asked to rate all of their
ents and consequences the program has identified classes in terms of how much they enjoy the sub-
based on the information provided. In the fourth ject using a Likert scale with ratings 1–5 where 1
section, the program integrates the identified indicates “not at all” and 5 corresponds to “very
antecedents and consequences to formulate much.” In the final section, what the student likes
hypothesized functions. Up to four possible func- and dislikes about each subject is explored
tions may be elicited from the program: gain through a series of open-ended questions.
attention, obtain access to tangibles, escape/avoid
demands, and sensory stimulation. At this time, Student-Guided Functional
the interviewee either confirms or disconfirms Assessment Interview
each hypothesized function. In the last section, The Student-Guided Functional Assessment
the interviewee is given function-based and Interview (Reed, Thomas, Sprague, & Horner,
research-supported interventions that are specific 1997) was developed for use in school settings
to the individual based on identifying informa- when children are engaging in challenging behav-
tion provided earlier. The interviewee is then able iors within the classroom, mainly talking out of
to choose from these options. turn, teasing/bullying, not following directions,
and not completing work. The interview is bro-
ken down into multiple sections and typically
Student Interviews administered to both the student and teacher.
First, the individual is asked to define the target
Although the majority of functional behavioral behaviors. Next, problematic settings and/or
assessment interviews rely on parents, caregiv- classes are noted by instructing the individual to
ers, or teachers as informants, a more recent complete a daily schedule. The schedule contains
development in the field has incorporated gather- each class or other activity the student partici-
ing information from students/individuals engag- pates in throughout the day as well as the instruc-
ing in challenging behaviors. Being able to derive tor for that class or activity. The individual is then
information from this source allows clinicians asked to rate the likelihood and intensity of the
better insight into challenging behaviors includ- student engaging in the target behavior during
ing the potential for a wider breadth of data. To that class or activity on a scale of 1 (least difficult)
date, several variations of a student-guided func- to 6 (most difficult). A blank diagram is then pre-
tional assessment interview exist which are com- sented to be completed by the individual with
monly adapted from one another. respect to events surrounding the target behavior
7 Interview and Observation Methods 109

(e.g., class demands, teacher demands, receiving ment. While conducting this observation, real-time
attention, and noise/distractions). Events are doc- data is recorded, thereby eliminating the biased
umented in the order in which they occur before effects of retrospective report. Contingency event
or after the target behavior. Lastly, a support plan recording was originally developed to be com-
is developed in a similar diagram where setting pleted in an unstructured format where data is
events and predictors are manipulated, replace- collected by documenting the date, time, anteced-
ment behaviors are contrived, and consequences ent event(s), target behavior (i.e., the challenging
are given for engagement in the challenging behavior), and consequent event(s) in separate
behavior versus the desired behavior. columns. Antecedents refer to the events occur-
ring prior to the display of the target behavior,
while consequent events are those occurring after
Direct Observation Methods the individual has already begun engaging in the
target behavior. The data is descriptive in nature
Although interviews provide clinicians with a detailing the sequence of events in the observer’s
wealth of information, best practice suggests that own words; therefore, it is commonly dubbed the
multiple methods of functional behavioral assess- descriptive or narrative recording format (Cooper,
ment be integrated to determine the function(s) of Heron, & Heward, 2007; Miltenberger, 2001).
challenging behaviors. Observation methods are Additional columns may be included depending
yet another option frequently incorporated into on the goal of the assessment with data in other
comprehensive functional behavioral assess- categories related to the challenging behavior,
ments. While observations certainly involve such as other individuals present at the time of the
direct examination of the individual, it is impor- challenging behavior or the location in which the
tant to understand that observation methods are behavior occurred, being recorded (Rojahn,
not synonymous with EFA. However, in contrast Schroeder, & Hoch, 2008). For an example of a
to EFA, direct observations occur within the natu- blank A–B–C recording sheet of this kind, refer
ral environment of the individual thus allowing to Appendix A.
clinicians the ability to assess situations in which Subsequently, contingency event recording
challenging behaviors actually occur. Furthermore, was expanded to include A–B–C checklists, also
unlike alternative methods of functional behav- known as structured A–B–C data collection. The
ioral assessment, direct observations do not rely premise of the data collection is synonymous
on retrospective report or memory, thereby elimi- with the original A–B–C recording sheets except
nating confounds associated with such reports. that now narrative report for the antecedent
event(s), target behavior, and consequent event(s)
is replaced with options for the observer to sim-
Contingency Event Recording ply check-off based on occurrence. An example
(A–B–C Data/Recording) of an A–B–C checklist is presented in Appendix
B. One such checklist was developed by O’Neill
Contingency event recording, more commonly and colleagues (1997), named the Functional
referred to as Antecedent–Behavior–Consequence Assessment Observation Form (FAOF). This
(A–B–C) data/recording, is by far the most preva- specific observation form contains eight sections.
lent form of nonexperimental observation meth- First, the individual being observed and dates of
ods used in functional behavioral assessment. observation are noted, with the possibility of
This method was actually one of the first func- observations spanning over more than just 1 day.
tional behavioral assessment techniques intro- Next, predetermined time intervals are decided
duced in applied settings and was developed by upon and labeled on the form. These time inter-
Bijou, Peterson, and Ault (1968). Contingency vals are dependent on the individual being
event recording involves direct observation of the observed and may coincide with specific activi-
individual being assessed in their natural environ- ties throughout the day, similar to scatter plot
110 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

data collection which will be discussed shortly. on A–B–C checklists, and it also provides the
This form actually differs from traditional contin- observer with the ability to describe all of the
gency event recordings by including this compo- events regardless of their perceived effect on
nent. The next sections (i.e., behaviors, predictors, the target behavior. However, structured check-
perceived functions, and actual consequences) lists may cue the observer to notice specific ante-
are presented in a checklist format. All targeted cedents or consequences that they may have
challenging behaviors are listed followed by pre- otherwise overlooked or considered irrelevant to
dictors, also known as antecedents. The perceived the situation. Another clearly positive property of
functions section, which also differs from typical checklists is that they are easy and quick to com-
contingency event recordings, prompts the plete. If an observer collecting A–B–C data is also
observer to endorse which listed function he/she working with the individual who is exhibiting the
believes to have brought about the behavior. challenging behavior, which is quite common
Finally, in the actual consequences section, the since parents, teachers, and therapists are often
observer checks the column aptly describing what those collecting the data, it may not be feasible to
occurred following the individual engaging in the expect the observer to provide a narrative on the
targeted challenging behavior. The authors events. This would be especially true if there is a
encourage clinicians to first conduct an interview greater frequency of the challenging behavior.
to choose which behaviors, predictors, perceived Despite the advantages and disadvantages of
functions, and actual consequences should be each A–B–C data collection method, very little
displayed as options on the data collection form. research has yet to examine the differences
With more recent advances in technology, addi- between structured and unstructured A–B–C data.
tional contingency event recording strategies Based on the results of one study assessing the
using a structured format have become available. accuracy and preference of both formats among
For example, similar to A–B–C checklists docu- 16 special education teachers and paraprofession-
menting information on paper, personal data als, the structured format yielded slightly greater
assistants have been used to electronically collect accuracy and was more preferred among teachers
observational data (Tarbox et al., 2009). When (Lerman, Hovanetz, Strobel, & Tetreault, 2009).
using these devices, antecedents, behaviors, and Overall, the accuracy of data collected across
consequences are documented similar to a paper- both methods was only modest due to the teach-
and-pencil checklist format. ers’ lack of knowledge regarding functional
Both contingency event recording formats behavioral assessment. Therefore, further train-
have advantages and disadvantages that clinicians ing in functional behavioral assessment is neces-
should be aware of prior to choosing which sary for teachers, especially those working with
specific data collection method to utilize. Ideally, children who display behavioral difficulties.
individuals with a strong background in functional Contingency event recording data is com-
behavioral assessment will be called upon for the monly interpreted in one of two ways. The sim-
collection of behavioral data. Unfortunately, this plest method is based on a correlational visual
is seldom possible in practice, and parents and inspection of the frequencies of the antecedents
teachers are commonly required to collect the and consequences related to specific challenging
appropriate data. Therefore, one of the more behaviors (Tarbox et al., 2009). If the antecedents
immediate considerations should be the compe- and consequences serve the same function for the
tency of the observer who will be collecting the same behavior, it is sufficient to say that the occur-
data. When using the unstructured, narrative for- rence of the behavior served that single function.
mat, observers are able to describe in their own Then, the most frequently occurring of those
words what events occurred prior to and follow- functions for that specific behavior may be
ing the target challenging behavior. This elimi- hypothesized to maintain the behavior. If anteced-
nates the confound of the observer not ents and consequences do not coincide during a
understanding specific terms commonly located single occurrence of a behavior, the interpretation
7 Interview and Observation Methods 111

becomes more complicated. In these cases, the Once again, the 30-s window is a suggestion
behavior may be maintained by multiple func- which may be modified. As was discussed with
tions or irrelevant correlating antecedents and respect to interpretation of contingency event
consequences may be coinciding with the behav- recording data, conditional probabilities may also
ior. As such, interpretations should be made with be calculated for continuous event recordings.
caution. On the other hand, contingency event Also, due to the nature of continuous event
recording data may also be analyzed by calculat- recording data collection allowing for additional
ing conditional probabilities (Lerman & Iwata, variables to be collected, other calculations may
1993; Mace & Lalli, 1991). First, the proportion also be possible. For example, intervals during
of times the target behavior followed each ante- which a specific antecedent preceded the target
cedent out of all of the times the target behavior behavior divided by the number of intervals con-
occurred is calculated. In addition, the percentage taining that specific antecedent can also be calcu-
of times each consequence followed the target lated since all antecedents are documented
behavior is also calculated. As will be discussed regardless of whether they are antecedents to the
in the next section, conditional probabilities may target behavior. Therefore, this data allows the
also be calculated for continuous event recording clinician to determine how often the target behav-
with additional calculations possible. ior actually followed the antecedent—perhaps
the antecedent occurred frequently without a sub-
sequent occurrence of the target behavior.
Continuous Event Recording Information of this kind can be quite valuable.

Based on Bijou and colleagues’ (1968) original


work on contingency event recording, continuous Scatter Plot Analysis
event recording was subsequently developed
(Mace, Lalli, & Lalli, 1991). Data collection While slightly less direct than data collected
begins by an individual first compiling a list of through contingency or continuous event record-
possible antecedents, challenging behaviors, and ings, scatter plot analysis is yet another observa-
consequences during observation periods. All tional method of collecting data related to the
categories may be broad or narrow depending on function(s) of challenging behavior. To collect
the specific individual being assessed. Then pre- this type of data, predetermined time intervals are
determined time intervals for data collection are decided upon before beginning data collection
established and divided into equal time segments (Touchette, MacDonald, & Langer, 1985).
for data collection. Mace and colleagues suggest Although these time intervals can be as simple as
that 15–60-min observation periods be used with hour or half-hour blocks of time throughout the
10-s time intervals. Therefore, if the designated day, it is strongly suggested that the time periods
observation period is 15 min, the entire period represent different activities occurring during the
can be divided into 90 10-s time intervals. During day or even other changes in the environment,
the direct observation periods, observers use a such as staff shift changes. Recording data
partial-interval recording procedure. If any of the according to differing environmental aspects will
antecedents or behaviors occur during a 10-s allow for easier interpretation of the data. Scatter
period, the appropriate box is marked. This is the plot data is simpler to collect in comparison to
distinguishing difference between contingency contingency or continuous event recording data
and continuous event recording – all antecedents because its collection only requires that an indi-
are recorded regardless of if they are following vidual denote whether the target behavior
by engagement in the target behavior. occurred during the specified time interval rather
Consequences are documented somewhat differ- than supply a descriptive narrative account or
ently; only consequences occurring up to 30 s determine the antecedents or consequences of the
following challenging behaviors are documented. behavior. Data collection can be implemented in
112 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

two ways: either frequency data can be collected conclusions cannot be derived from scatter plot
with a tally mark being placed in the time period data. When Kahng et al. were unable to decipher a
during which the target behavior occurred or data temporal pattern from several visual displays of
can be plotted on a grid during the observation scatter plot data, the authors constructed a control
period. If the tally mark method is chosen, the chart (Pfadt & Wheeler, 1995) for each scatter plot
data is later compiled into a graph with the time to statistically interpret the data. Control charts are
period along the X-axis and the frequency of the commonly used as one of many statistical proce-
challenging behavior along the Y-axis similar to dures to improve industrial organization production.
the visual presentation of the grid data collection However, Pfadt and Wheeler suggest that these
method. This method may also be more feasible statistical procedures may also be applied to the
in settings where training in data collection is behavioral sciences to analyze behavior patterns.
limited since frequency data is often collected to The statistical analysis aims to identify patterns of
monitor progress regardless of its inclusion in a variability that are considered “out of statistical
scatter plot. control.” That is, they are statistically sufficiently
Utilizing the grid option eliminates the need deviant from the mean so as to be significantly
for two steps in the scatter plot process; however, different. Applying this statistical analysis to the
it requires greater time investment during actual same 15 sets of data which had been impervious to
data collection. On the grid, successive days are scatter plot analyses resulted in a temporal pattern
presented along the X-axis, while the time peri- being identified for 12 of the 15 data sets. Although
ods are displayed on the Y-axis. Then for each Kahng et al. state that needing to apply this statisti-
time period over the course of each day, the cal analysis to scatter plot data compromises one
appropriate block is shaded accordingly. of the main advantages of scatter plot analysis (i.e.,
Typically, an empty cell indicates that the target being able to visually interpret the data with ease),
behavior was absent, while a filled cell marks its addition still allows clinicians to identify tem-
presence of the behavior. However, depending on poral patterns of behavior, which is the goal of
the frequency of the target behavior, variations of scatter plot analysis.
this method can be used (Kahng et al., 1998;
Touchette et al., 1985). For example, a blank cell
may represent an absence of the behavior, while a Psychometric Properties
shaded cell denotes low frequencies of the behav- of Interview and Direct Observation
ior and a filled cell indicates high frequencies of Methods
the behavior. The difference between low and
high frequencies of the behavior would be based Since the current chapter focuses on two func-
on predetermined criteria. Although more than tional behavioral assessment methods (i.e., inter-
three different codes can be used, some have views and direct observations), and these two
found this to compromise the interpretability of methods are often compared to one another in
the data (Touchette et al.). studies, data regarding the reliability and validity
Interpretation of scatter plot data involves of the aforementioned methods will be discussed
inspection of the visual display to determine time in a similar fashion. First, some examples of stud-
periods, which correlate with specific events, dur- ies only addressing one form of functional behav-
ing which the target behavior is more likely to ioral assessment will be presented. Subsequently,
occur. Although some researchers find scatter plots examples of studies examining multiple func-
to be advantageous in that they are easily inter- tional behavioral assessment strategies will be
preted visually, simple visual interpretation of reviewed. Please note that the review of psycho-
scatter plot data does not always arrive at a conclu- metric properties presented is not an all inclusive
sion regarding temporal periods during which the compilation of studies regarding the specific
target behavior is most likely to occur (Kahng assessment method, but rather a demonstration of
et al., 1998). However, this is not to say that these recent research.
7 Interview and Observation Methods 113

Interviews students. Inter-rater reliability for the entire FAIP


averaged 63.9% agreement, while inter-rater reli-
FACTS ability for the derived functions averaged 70.96%
An excellent review completed by McIntosh et al. agreement across participants. Test–retest reli-
(2008) provides a wealth of information on the ability was calculated by having one set of 19
psychometric properties of the FACTS. The participants complete the FAIP for a second time,
review aggregated the results of nine separate approximately 30 days following its first admin-
studies assessing the properties of the FACTS in istration. Test–retest reliability averaged 72.66%
a total of 41 children attending public preschools, for the entire FAIP and 81.4% agreement for the
elementary schools, and middle schools. The derived functions. Concurrent validity was
test–retest reliability was found to be strong with assessed by having multiple respondents com-
respect to antecedents, functions, and total behav- plete the FAIP, MAS, and FAIF. There was
ioral hypotheses, while the test–retest reliability 69.44% agreement between the FAIP and MAS,
for setting events was moderate. Inter-rater reli- and 76.34% agreement between the FAIP and
ability was also moderate across respondents. In FAIF. The clinical utility of all three assessments
terms of validity, convergent validity has been was also measured, with results indicating that
most commonly explored by comparing the professionals most preferred the FAIP overall
FACTS to either direct observations or an EFA. when compared with the MAS and FAIF.
Complete agreement between the FACTS and
direct observations reached 90%, while the Student-Guided Functional
FACTS and EFA agreed on functions for 53% of Assessment Interview
the cases. However, it should be noted that there Reed and colleagues (1997) assessed the inter-
were some instances in which there was partial rater reliability of the Student-Guided Functional
agreement between assessment methods. For Assessment Interview by administering the inter-
example, for 5% of validation cases between the view to ten students in the fifth through eighth
FACTS and direct observations, the direct obser- grades, and their corresponding teachers, who
vations pointed toward multiple functions, one of had a history of exhibiting challenging behaviors
which was consistent with the function identified within the school setting. All interviews were
by the FACTS. Similarly, for 24% of the valida- administered first to teachers and then to the cor-
tion cases between the FACTS and EFA, the EFA responding students within 3 days of the original
indicated multiple functions, one of which was interview. When conducting interviews with the
also indicated by the FACTS. Validity based on students, prompting questions were frequently
treatment utility was also explored for 15 stu- incorporated as students often needed guidance
dents. All treatment plans developed based on the throughout the assessment. These were used as
identified function from the FACTS resulted in a follow-up questions to the main questions asked
decrease in targeted challenging behaviors. The during the interview and were standard for all
majority of students experienced at least a 50% interviews with students.
reduction in problem behaviors. Taken collectively, there was 60% teacher–stu-
dent agreement on the entire functional behavioral
FAIP assessment portion of the interview. When break-
A sample of 59 school psychologists, social ing down the results according to the different
workers, and teachers participated in the stan- aspects of the functional behavioral assessment
dardization of the FAIP using a sample of chil- section, agreement was variable. Teachers and
dren in the third through sixth grades who students demonstrated agreement on 81.5% of
engaged in challenging behaviors within the challenging behaviors, with students identifying
classroom setting (Hartwig et al., 2004). For more behaviors than did teachers. The behaviors
inter-rater reliability, 19 pairs of participants that were reported only by the students and not the
were asked to complete the FAIP on 19 separate teachers appear to be those that were not necessarily
114 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

observable to teachers within the classroom possible that the results derived through the EFAs
setting (e.g., possession of inappropriate items), were similarly invalid.
thus at least minimally explaining the discrepancy.
While there was 77% agreement for predictors Scatter Plot Analysis
and consequences of challenging behaviors, there Touchette and colleagues (1985) reported excel-
was only 23% agreement on setting events. lent inter-rater reliability between observers in
Overall, there was 38% agreement on the support collection of data that was used in scatter plot
plan portion of the interview, with agreement analyses for three individuals with ASD. For two
varying between 25% and 48% across prevention of the three children assessed, functions main-
strategies, teaching strategies, consequences, and taining the challenging behavior were identified
setting changes. However, consistency between through scatter plot analysis, thereby causing
the functional assessment and support plan por- function-based interventions to be implemented.
tions for teacher and student interviews was 78% A subsequent reduction in challenging behaviors
and 70%, respectively, suggesting that there was was observed for all clients.
moderate to good ability on behalf of the infor- Symons, McDonald, and Wehby (1998) used
mants to develop treatment plans consistent with scatter plot analysis in two behavior management
their hypothesized functions. Taken collectively, classrooms in Canada for two boys who engaged
there was a 22% agreement across the entire inter- in challenging behaviors frequently throughout
view between teachers and students. the day (i.e., more than 10 times per day). Each of
the two classroom teachers was instructed to col-
lect frequency data for each 30-min interval
Direct Observations throughout the school day. During the study, the
first author (Symons) collected interobserver
Continuous Event Recording agreement data with each teacher for a minimum
Lerman and Iwata (1993) investigated the ability of 20% of school days to ensure inter-rater reli-
of continuous event recordings to identify the ability; the average agreement was 93.0%. The
function of self-injurious behaviors in six adults first author then made a scatter plot visual display
with profound intellectual disability. For five of of each student’s behavior data using symbols to
the individuals, continuous event recordings were denote low, medium, and high frequencies of the
completed for a total of 24 h. For one individual, behavior based on preestablished criteria for each
assessment was conducted for a total of 48 h to individual student. These scatter plots were
determine whether a lengthier assessment period updated on a weekly basis. To assess the validity
would clarify the results. In addition, EFAs were of scatter plot data, team meetings were held
completed for all participants independent of the approximately once each week with the first
continuous event recording results. While EFAs author, teacher, and teacher’s aide present to ana-
were found to identify the maintaining variables lyze the data and identify time periods of con-
of self-injurious behavior in all of the partici- cern, if any. Once one or more time periods of
pants, continuous event recordings appeared to concern were noted, hypotheses regarding the
be successful only in differentiating social versus elevation in the presence of the target behavior
nonsocial functions. Whether attention, escape, during these time periods were proposed, and an
or another social contingency maintained the appropriate intervention was then implemented
behavior could not be discerned through the for one of the time periods based on this hypoth-
descriptive assessment. Additionally, a lengthier esis. For both students, implementation of an
assessment period did not prove effective in fur- intervention based on scatter plot analysis resulted
ther clarifying the results of a descriptive assess- in a moderate decrease in challenging behaviors,
ment. However, it should be noted that EFA thereby supporting the effectiveness of scatter
was held as the gold standard in this assessment plot analysis in functional behavioral assessment
and its results were not validated. Therefore, it is within the classroom setting.
7 Interview and Observation Methods 115

Maas, Didden, Bouts, Smits, and Curfs (2009) Comparisons of Multiple Assessment
used scatter plot analysis to determine the tempo- Methods
ral characteristics of excessive daytime sleepi-
ness and disruptive behaviors in seven adults with Arndorfer, Miltenberger, Woster, Rortvedt, and
Prader-Willi Syndrome. Frequency data were Gaffaney (1994) used a multi-assessment method
collected by parents and/or caregivers across a to assess the maintaining variables of challenging
4-week period between normal waking hours. behaviors in five children ages 2–13 years who
Time periods were broken down into 2-h inter- had varying levels of intellectual impairment,
vals, and within the 2-h time period the presence developmental delays, and/or other psychologi-
of behaviors was rated across two separate situa- cal disorders. The first phase of the study was
tions—activities versus no activities. Each behav- termed the “descriptive assessment” and included
ior received one of three scores; 0 indicated not assessment methods such as administration of the
sleepy/no disruptive behavior, 1 indicated some- MAS and FAIF, as well as contingency event
what sleepy/somewhat disruptive behavior, and 2 recording data collected through direct observa-
indicated very sleepy or asleep/severe disruptive tions independently by the parents and research-
behavior. All codes were operational defined for ers. For one child, all four assessments arrived at
the observers. Interobserver agreement for data the same function. For the remaining four chil-
collection was deemed good. Separate scatter dren, the FAIF and A–B–C data indicated identi-
plots for excessive daytime sleepiness and dis- cal functions while the MAS was inconsistent.
ruptive behaviors were then constructed for each The descriptive assessment data was then com-
participant during activities and during the piled for each child, so that hypotheses regarding
absence of activities. The time intervals were the function(s) of the challenging behaviors could
segmented vertically with activity and non-activ- be made, with the hypothesized function being
ity periods separated, and successive days were chosen as the one supported by the most assess-
segmented horizontally. Scatter plot analysis ments. As such, the FAIF’s and A–B–C data’s
indicated that individuals with Prader-Willi identified function was always the one chosen for
Syndrome exhibited excessive daytime sleepi- manipulation. EFAs were then completed to
ness more commonly during the late afternoon assess the validity of the hypothesized functions
and evening hours, especially when no activities derived through the descriptive assessments. All
were planned. Excessive daytime sleepiness was children’s descriptive assessment results were
also more common on Saturdays, also increasing validated through 90–120 min EFAs. Functional
when there was no activity involvement. A less Communication Training (FCT), a treatment pro-
distinct pattern emerged for disruptive behaviors; tocol frequently implemented to teach individu-
engagement in disruptive behavior was relatively als to appropriately communicate to achieve the
consistent across days and activity involvement. same function their challenging behavior had
However, there was a slight elevation during been maintained by, was then implemented for
weekends when no activities were provided. The two of the children according to the validated
results of this study have many implications for function. Teaching these children to verbally
the use of scatter plot analysis. First, as the request the attention or tangible they desired
authors themselves point out, more concrete significantly decreased the rate of their challeng-
results may have been obtained through the use ing behaviors. Thus, implementation of FCT fur-
of shorter time intervals (e.g., Touchette et al., ther validated the results of both these children’s
1985). Secondly, since the authors based much of descriptive assessments and EFAs. Therefore,
their analysis on the hypothesis that the targeted both the results of the FAIF and contingency
behaviors would increase during periods of inac- event recordings were validated through the
tivity, the information obtained through the anal- findings in this study.
ysis was somewhat limited. Specific activities Cunningham and O’Neill (2000) conducted a
were not considered nor were other possible similar study with three boys aged 3–5 years who
influential factors, such as staff preference. were diagnosed with an ASD. Each child engaged
116 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

in challenging behaviors to include biting self, services for a behavior disorder. The teacher and
physical aggression, and tantrums. Four func- student interviews were developed specifically
tional behavioral assessment techniques were for the study, while the FAOF was used to collect
compared: EFA, an interview (FAIF), contin- contingency event recording data. The interviews
gency event recording (FAOF), and a scaling were administered separately to groups of teach-
method (MAS). While multiple functions were ers and individual students, and they solicited
identified for each child, all four assessment information regarding the behaviors, their ante-
methods arrived at the same primary function for cedents, and their consequences. Summary state-
two of the children. For the third child, the EFA ments were then derived based on the information
and FAIF arrived at the same primary function, collected, and teachers and students were then
while contingency event recording and the MAS individually asked to rank these statements as to
arrived at another identical primary function. In which scenarios were the most problematic, thus
this example, the secondary function identified the most likely to be maintaining the challenging
by the EFA and FAIF was also the same and behavior. With respect to data collected through
served as the primary function identified by con- the FAOF, rankings were made similarly by cal-
tingency event recording and the MAS, and vice culating the percent occurrence of each function
versa. Therefore, although the sample size within across all observations. Interobserver agreement
the study was quite small, a limitation that will be was also calculated for the direct observations,
discussed later, these findings suggest that the with an average agreement of 80%. Results indi-
aforementioned functional behavioral assessment cated a significant discrepancy between the chal-
methods were able to reliably identify the same lenging behaviors identified by teachers and
function albeit at different rankings. students with only a 30% rate of agreement.
Alter, Conroy, Mancil, and Haydon (2008) Much like the study conducted by Reed and col-
implemented four different functional behavioral leagues (1997), teachers were less likely to iden-
assessment techniques with four children who tify behaviors that were not easily observable
were at risk for emotional and behavioral disor- within the classroom setting. Overall, there was a
ders. The FAIF, MAS, and A–B–C recordings 64% agreement across all three functional behav-
were all compared to EFA, which was designated ioral assessment methods (i.e., teacher interview,
as the most valid method of assessment. When student interview, and contingency event record-
compared, the FAIF, MAS, and A–B–C recording ing) with respect to accuracy of identified func-
methods all demonstrated low agreement with one tion as well as rank order of that function with
another. Furthermore, the FAIF and MAS also respect to other noted functions. The remaining
demonstrated low consistency with EFA. 36% of cases displayed agreement between
Therefore, within this sample, the FAIF was not teacher interviews and contingency event record-
deemed a valid assessment of maintaining vari- ing data but not with student interviews.
ables of challenging behaviors. On the other hand, Newcomer and Lewis (2004) investigated the
A–B–C recordings were designated as the only validity of descriptive assessment methods (i.e.,
assessment method which corroborated the teacher interviews, student interviews, scatter
findings of an EFA for all four children. Therefore, plots, and A–B–C recordings) in three children
although contingency event recording does not ages 9–11 years old attending public elementary
involve experimental manipulation of variables schools who were displaying behavioral
present within the individual’s immediate envi- difficulties putting them at risk for failure that
ronment as does EFA, this study indicated that the school year. Assessment occurred in three
results of these two assessments are quite similar. phases—the first phase explored functions utiliz-
Murdock, O’Neill, and Cunningham (2005) ing the aforementioned descriptive assessment
assessed the reliability and validity of teacher methods, the second phase generated hypotheses
interviews, student interviews, and contingency based on the descriptive assessment methods,
event recordings. Eight boys ages 12–15 partici- and an EFA was conducted during the third phase
pated in the study, all of which were receiving to confirm the hypotheses generated. Across all
7 Interview and Observation Methods 117

three children, the A–B–C recordings, scatter nonsocial variables. No discernable pattern of
plots, student interviews, and EFA demonstrated object mouthing was seen across conditions,
convergent validity. The teacher interview, which thereby confirming the results of contingency
was conducted using an adapted FAIF, corrobo- event recordings in that the behavior was main-
rated the findings for two of the three children, tained by nonsocial variables.
while the third child’s FAIF indicated the child’s Ervin, DuPaul, Kern, and Friman (1998) uti-
primary function as his secondary one. Therefore, lized a teacher interview, student interview
taken together, it appears that all of the descrip- (Student Assisted Functional Assessment
tive assessment methods were valid in identify- Interview), and direct observations to formulate
ing the maintaining variable of these three hypotheses regarding the function of two teenage
children’s challenging behaviors. Based on these boys’ off-task behavior within the classroom.
maintaining variables, function-based treatments Both boys met diagnostic criteria for attention-
and nonfunction-based treatments were imple- deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional
mented. For all children, function-based treat- defiant disorder at the time of the study. Based on
ments resulted in a significant decrease in the cumulative results of the descriptive assess-
challenging behaviors when compared with base- ments, which all pointed toward identical func-
line. Nonfunction-based treatments were met tions, function-based intervention plans were
with increases and significant variability in chal- implemented for both of the boys. A significant
lenging behaviors for two of the students, and a decrease was seen in the off-task behavior of both
slight decrease in one student. However, for the boys during intervention phases with an increase
student who experienced decreases in challeng- in the behaviors occurring during reversal proce-
ing behaviors both during function- and nonfunc- dures. Therefore, the cumulative results of the
tion-based treatments, the gains were greater with comprehensive functional behavioral assessment
the former. appeared valid in identifying the variables main-
Mueller and Kafka (2006) completed a com- taining both of the boys’ off-task behavior, allow-
prehensive functional behavioral assessment for ing for appropriate interventions to be put in
a 4-year-old girl who engaged in object mouthing place.
within the classroom setting. Techniques
employed included parent and teacher interviews,
contingency event recording, and EFA. The inter- Overview of Interviews in Functional
views conducted were not according to a specified Behavioral Assessment
protocol, but did elicit information regarding
antecedents and consequences of object mouth- Given that interviews are among the most popular
ing. Taken together, the parent and teacher inter- method of functional behavioral assessment,
views were relatively inconclusive in identifying attention needs to be given to their potential use in
specific antecedents likely to precede object identifying maintaining variables of challenging
mouthing. However, an attention function was behaviors and aiding in implementation of appro-
hypothesized based on information acquired priate interventions. To date, minimal research has
regarding consequences since the consequence to been conducted on the psychometric properties of
mouthing was always a verbal reprimand. Based various interviews, and the results of studies that
on contingency event recordings, mouthing was have been completed are relatively inconsistent
only potentially maintained by attention in the with one another. While some have found parent,
form of verbal reprimands, but most likely main- caregiver, and/or teacher interviews to be quite
tained by a nonsocial function as it occurred beneficial in identifying the function of challeng-
across various situations without discrimination. ing behaviors (e.g., Cunningham & O’Neill, 2000;
Finally, an EFA was conducted with attention and McIntosh et al., 2008; Newcomer & Lewis, 2004),
alone conditions to distinguish whether object others tend to find that these assessment methods
mouthing was maintained by attention or are invalid (e.g., Alter et al., 2008).
118 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Many variables may play a role in the differ- challenging behavior most commonly occurred,
ences found between studies. O’Neill and col- and self-assessed experience with functional
leagues (1997) assert that the FAIF should be behavioral assessment. At the conclusion of the
administered by a professional with training in interview, the teachers and staff were asked to rate
functional assessment. Although this is a rela- how confident they were that their interview had
tively undisputable claim with respect to all func- identified the correct function on a 6-point Likert
tional assessment methods, it appears appropriate scale ranging from 1 (not confident) to 6 (very
to say that this may hold even more truth for func- confident). Then, an EFA was conducted to
tional assessment interviews, as opposed to rating identify the function of the behavior.
scales, due to the unstructured nature of the assess- Although the vast majority of informants
ment process as well as the clinical judgment reported possessing little to no experience with
needed to interpret the results. While the FAIF conducting a functional behavioral assessment or
and other interview methods are exceedingly thor- developing a treatment plan, it was found that
ough, they produces a much more complex set of 91.4% of respondents rated their confidence as a
data when compared with rating scales due to the 4 or higher. However, the only significant finding
open-ended format of the interview as well as the was that those individuals who were highly
lack of a scoring algorithm (Sturmey, 1994). confident and identified the correct function had
Therefore, it is possible that the findings of stud- significantly more exposure to the student both
ies differed based on the training of those admin- throughout the school day and during times in
istering the interviews. In fact, despite its popular which the student engaged in the targeted behav-
use, research has also identified interviews to be ior. Therefore, when choosing a respondent for a
the assessment method with which clinicians have functional behavioral assessment interview, it
had the least amount of training (Ellingson et al., seems appropriate that those being interviewed
1999). Furthermore, when asked to rate how easy should be individuals who have considerable
different functional behavioral assessment strate- exposure to the student. However, exposure is not
gies were to use, interviews were rated as being sufficient. The respondent must also indicate that
more difficult to use than scaling methods and they are confident in their ratings. Unfortunately,
direct observations. Interviews were also rated confidence ratings cannot be obtained until the
less effective in determining the function(s) of interview is complete. Though this is without
behavior and less useful when compared with question a limitation of this finding, this informa-
EFA and direct observation, but to be more effec- tion is still valuable in determining if the already
tive and more useful in comparison to rating scales. administered interview is likely valid. Clearly,
In addition to these concerns about interviews more research needs to be conducted to investi-
overall, another major limitation of interviews is gate this relationship. In the meantime, it is sug-
that they rely on retrospective report. gested that informants be those who are familiar
The utility of interviews in functional behav- with the individual both during and outside of
ioral assessment does not solely rely on the specific behavioral challenges. Furthermore, if confidence
interview administered nor its psychometrics but ratings are later found to be weak, those conduct-
also the respondent participating in the interview ing the interview are advised to interpret the
process. Borgmeier and Horner (2006) investi- results with caution or to weigh the results of
gated the predictive validity of confidence ratings other interviews more heavily.
made by the respondents. A total of 63 teachers Experience with functional behavioral assess-
and staff participated in completing the FACTS ment is another factor that may affect the validity
for nine students. Five to eight teachers or staff of interview results. Although Borgmeier and
completed the interview for each of the nine Horner (2006) did not find a correlation between
students, all of whom varied in their exposure to the validity of interview results and experience
the student during the school day, exposure to with functional behavioral assessment, a
the student during periods when the targeted significant flaw with this finding is that the
7 Interview and Observation Methods 119

experience of functional behavioral assessment type of functional behavioral assessment to be


was self-assessed on a Likert scale. Therefore, it deemed inappropriate. Therefore, it is highly
may be that participants rated their experience suggested that although the effectiveness of stu-
and knowledge with functional behavioral assess- dent interviews should continue to be explored,
ment based on different factors. Other research they should not be used in isolation, even with
has found that training informants on aspects of individuals without intellectual impairments.
functional behavioral assessment actually does With respect to standard parent, teacher, and
lead to an increased ability to accurately identify caregiver interviews, the FAI and FACTS both
the functions of challenging behaviors (McNeill, have moderate to strong research support with
Watson, Henington, & Meeks, 2002). Therefore, respect to their reliability and validity. At the
conducting interviews with informants who have same time, this is not to say that some research
at least some background in functional behav- has not suggested otherwise or that a sufficient
ioral assessment would prove to be beneficial. amount of research has been conducted as of yet.
The strong suggestion to choose informants For example, all studies exploring these inter-
who have knowledge of functional behavioral views have only included a small number of par-
assessment clearly speaks against the idea of ticipants. Therefore, reliability and validity
including students or individuals engaging in the findings need to be interpreted with caution.
challenging behaviors in the interview process. However, although these interviews may be less
However, this is not necessarily the case. systematic and have less research to support their
Collecting information from those engaging in psychometric reliability and validity, they are not
challenging behaviors may be quite beneficial, without their advantages. As was previously
with students having the ability to identify inter- stated, interviews do not require the individual in
vention strategies that may assist them person- question to be present nor that the targeted behav-
ally. Furthermore, as was commonly seen when ior, which may pose danger to the individual and/
conducting student interviews, students are likely or others, be exhibited. Interviews also require
to identify more behavior problems than teachers significantly less time to complete than EFAs.
due to exposure limitations on the part of teach- The majority of functional behavioral assess-
ers (Murdock et al., 2005; Reed et al., 1997). Yet, ments likely include at least a minor interview
there was some discrepancy between studies with regardless of its effectiveness in identifying func-
respect to teacher and student interviews corrob- tions. This is because interviews prompt infor-
orating each other’s findings (Ervin et al., 1998; mants to supply basic information that EFA,
Murdock et al.; Newcomer & Lewis, 2004; Reed direct observations, and scaling methods do not.
et al.). Additionally, despite the possible advan- Most interviews, such as the FAIF and FACTS,
tages of including the student or individual require the respondent to operationally define the
engaging in the challenging behavior in the func- targeted behavior, a critical piece of information
tional behavioral assessment interview process, that other functional behavioral assessment
this assessment method may not be appropriate methods do not incorporate. It is not that these
for all populations. At present, research has only alternative methods are overlooking the importance
documented its use among individuals, primarily of this information, but rather that it is assumed
students, who have either a mild disability or no that this information has already been gathered
diagnostic label. Therefore, future research needs through an interview. Therefore, at least a brief
to explore whether individuals with intellectual interview should be mandatory when beginning a
and/or developmental disabilities would benefit functional behavioral assessment as it may be
from participation in this form of functional seen as a starting point to any functional behav-
behavioral assessment. It is likely that deficits ioral assessment, especially when the function(s)
associated with intellectual and/or developmental of behavior are elusive to the assessor.
disabilities may hinder the individual’s ability to Although very little research has been
provide accurate information, thus causing this conducted on the FAIP to date, the results of the
120 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

preliminary study are exciting and provide initial their status as correlational assessment methods.
evidence that the FAIP may be a useful interview In comparison to EFA, these methods require
assessment for use when conducting functional considerably less resources. Relatively little
behavioral assessments. It differs from the major- training is required to collect and interpret the
ity of other interviews by interpreting the narra- data, and supplies needed are of little or no cost.
tive reports from respondents, thus formulating Furthermore, since direct observation data is col-
hypotheses regarding the function(s) of behav- lected during the individual’s regular activities, it
iors. Since the lack of a scoring algorithm is one does not often require extra personnel or time to
of the more prominent disadvantages to func- complete. Although direct observations are cri-
tional behavioral assessment interviews, the tiqued for only being able to identify correlations
FAIP’s built in scoring program is a major advan- between behaviors and antecedents/conse-
tage. Not only does it sidestep the difficulty in quences, a major limitation of EFAs, the fact that
interpretation of the results, but also it does not the behaviors are not occurring in the natural
require significant expertise in the area of func- environment, is overcome by this method of
tional behavioral assessment. Furthermore, since assessment.
the FAIP is administered by a computer as Despite the similarity in the overall method of
opposed to a professional, its use significantly collecting direct observation data, there are
reduces the need for personnel resources that significant differences between the three methods
may not be available. The cost of administering discussed within this chapter that warrant consid-
the FAIP is also another likely advantage of the eration. Direct observations vary in their simplic-
interview due to less resources being needed and ity, range of data, and validity. While scatter plot
because a one-time fee would be in place as data is the simplest to collect, its results are more
opposed to purchasing of multiple assessment ambiguous than either contingency or continuous
measures. However, this is not to say that the event recordings since the presence of targeted
FAIP is not without its disadvantages. Clearly, a behaviors is merely correlated with different time
great deal of more research is needed. Also, in periods, which is in turn correlated with different
contrast to other interviews which appear to be activities occurring throughout the day. Though
more broad based in nature, as they will probe some have found the results of such analyses to
about many different factors within the individual’s be beneficial and helpful in formulating treatment
environment that may be affecting his/her behav- plans, results are somewhat speculative in nature.
ior, the FAIP narrows its results down to four On the other hand, contingency and continuous
general hypotheses. It should also be noted that event recordings provide significantly more infor-
although the FAIP makes a significant contribu- mation to the clinician and can also actually inte-
tion to treatment by providing function- and grate some aspects of a scatter plot analysis since
research-based interventions, a professional is the time of day can similarly be documented.
needed to implement and monitor the effective- However, contingency and continuous event
ness of these treatments. recordings also require more training in the area
of functional behavioral assessment to ensure
that accurate data is being collected. Furthermore,
Overview of Direct Observation these methods require more time and attention be
Methods in Functional Behavioral given to data collection while conducting obser-
Assessment vations since significantly more documentation is
required. Although continuous event recordings
Direct observation data, including contingency would appear to be superior over contingency
event recordings, continuous event recordings, event recordings, the former may be just as unrea-
and scatter plots, can greatly assist in the func- sonable to conduct as EFAs since it requires con-
tional behavioral assessment process despite stant documentation throughout a predetermined
7 Interview and Observation Methods 121

observation period. In fact, it is rather unlikely alternative functional behavioral assessment


that staff working with an individual to be techniques exist, none are without their flaws.
observed would be capable of completing a con- While each of these overall methods and their
tinuous event recording while working with the specific strategies have different advantages and
individual. disadvantages to consider prior to beginning any
Based on the advantages and disadvantages of functional behavioral assessment, the key to a
the three direct observation methods discussed comprehensive functional behavioral assessment
here in comparison to one another, it is not sur- does not rely on only one method but rather a col-
prising that contingency event recordings are laboration of different methodologies to assist in
often chosen in lieu of either of the other two the treatment planning process. Furthermore,
methods. This, of course, means that there has functional behavioral assessments should be indi-
also been less research conducted on either of the vidualized so that one set protocol is unlikely to
other two methods. The research that has been be appropriate for all cases.
conducted on scatter plot analyses and continu- Didden (2007) suggests a seven-step plan for
ous event recordings to date is at best inconsis- conducting a thorough functional behavioral
tent. However, the most common of the direct assessment: (1) identify and operationally defined
observation methods, contingency event record- the targeted challenging behaviors, (2) utilize
ing, is also the direct observation method with the direct observation methods such as contingency
most evidence to support its use in functional event recordings and scatter plots, (3) administer
behavioral assessment (e.g., Alter et al., 2008; interviews and scales to those familiar with the
Newcomer & Lewis, 2004). Although contin- individual, (4) complete an EFA, (5) integrate
gency event recording cannot be considered syn- results from functional behavioral assessment to
onymous with EFA by any means, the results formulate hypotheses regarding the function(s)
have repeatedly been found to corroborate those of targeted challenging behaviors, (6) develop a
found through an EFA. Therefore, contingency treatment plan based on the derived function
event recordings should routinely be completed maintaining the targeted challenging behavior(s),
when conducting a functional behavioral assess- and (7) monitor effectiveness of treatment inter-
ment, especially if resources do not permit that ventions. Although this seven-step plan appears
an EFA or more comprehensive assessment be to be without question the ideal assessment, in
conducted. most cases it is not practical. Therefore, in cases
in which a comprehensive functional behavioral
assessment involving an EFA or solely an EFA
Conclusion cannot be conducted, it is proposed that alterna-
tive, brief functional behavioral assessment strat-
EFA, which is often deemed the gold standard of egies be used initially with a progression to more
functional behavioral assessment methods, is not time-consuming and labor-intensive methods
always practical, safe, or even possible. Therefore, as deemed necessary (Vollmer et al., 1995).
alternative functional behavioral assessment With this progression as the basis to functional
techniques are often deemed necessary. Within behavioral assessment, a brief interview and
this chapter, various interview and direct obser- some form of direct observation, preferably
vation methods commonly used to aid in func- contingency event recordings, should undoubt-
tional behavioral assessment have been reviewed. edly be included with more intense methods
All of these assessments have their own strengths being incorporated when needed. In this manner,
and weaknesses, which have been addressed the most parsimonious way of identifying func-
accordingly. Based on the information presented tions maintaining challenging behaviors can be
herein, it should be apparent that although many accomplished.
122 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Appendix A References1
Alter, P. J., Conroy, M. A., Mancil, G. R., & Haydon, T.
Antecedents Consequence (2008). A comparison of functional behavior assess-
Date/ (what happened Target (what happened ment methodologies with young children: Descriptive
time right before?) behavior right after?) methods and functional analysis. Journal of Behavioral
Education, 17, 200–219.
Arndorfer, R. E., Miltenberger, R. G., Woster, S. H.,
Rortvedt, A. K., & Gaffaney, T. (1994). Home-based
descriptive and experimental analysis of problem
behaviors in children. Topics in Early Childhood
Special Education, 14, 64–87.
*Bergstrom, M. K. (2003). Efficacy of school-based teams
conducting functional behavioral assessment in the
general education environment. Unpublished doctoral
dissertation, University of Oregon.
Bijou, S. W., Peterson, R. F., & Ault, M. H. (1968).
A method to integrate descriptive and experimental
field studies at the level of data and empirical concepts.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 175–191.
*Borgmeier, C. & Horner, R. H. (2006). An evaluation of
Appendix B the predictive validity of confidence ratings in identi-
fying accurate functional behavioral assessment
hypothesis statements. Journal of Positive Behavior
Interventions, 8, 100–105.
Date/time *Carter, D. L. & Horner, R. H. (2007). Adding functional
Staff initials behavioral assessment to First Step to Success: A case
Behavior study. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 9,
Physical aggression 229–238.
Out of seat Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007).
Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle
Location
River: Pearson.
Classroom Cunningham, E., & O’Neill, R. E. (2000). Comparison of
Hallway results of functional assessment and analysis methods
Bathroom with young children with autism. Education and
Cafeteria Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental
Disabilities, 35, 406–414.
Playground
Didden, R. (2007). Functional analysis methodology in
Antecedents developmental disabilities. In P. Sturmey (Ed.),
Direction given Functional analysis in clinical treatment (pp. 65–86).
Preferred item removed Burlington: Academic.
Transition Durand, V. M., & Crimmins, D. B. (1992). The Motivation
Denied request Assessment Scale administrative guide. Topeka:
Monaco & Associates.
No staff attention
Ellingson, S. A., Miltenberger, R. G., & Long, E. S.
Other (write on back) (1999). A survey of the use of functional assessment
Consequences procedures in agencies serving individuals with devel-
Verbal reprimand opmental disabilities. Behavioral Interventions, 14,
Redirection to current task 187–198.
Ignored
Given tangible 1
References marked with an asterisk indicate studies
Allowed to escape activity
included in the McIntosh et al. (2008) review paper dis-
Other (write on back) cussed within.
7 Interview and Observation Methods 123

English, C. L., & Anderson, C. M. (2004). Effects of injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
familiar versus unfamiliar therapists on responding in Analysis, 26, 293–319.
the analog functional analysis. Research in Maas, A. P. H. M., Didden, R., Bouts, L., Smits, M. G., &
Developmental Disabilities, 25, 39–55. Curfs, L. M. G. (2009). Scatter plot analysis of exces-
Ervin, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., Kern, L., & Friman, P. C. sive daytime sleepiness and severe disruptive behav-
(1998). Classroom-based functional and adjunctive ior in adults with Prader-Willi syndrome: A pilot
assessments: Proactive approaches to intervention study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 30,
selection for adolescents with attention deficit hyper- 529–537.
activity disorder. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, Mace, F. C., & Lalli, J. S. (1991). Linking descriptive and
31, 65–78. experimental analyses in the treatment of bizarre
*Filter, K. J., & Horner, R. H. (2009). Function-based aca- speech. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24,
demic interventions for problem behavior. Education 553–562.
and Treatment of Children, 11, 222–234. Mace, F. C., Lalli, J. S., & Lalli, E. P. (1991). Functional
Gettinger, M., & Stoiber, K. C. (2006). Functional assess- analysis and treatment of aberrant behavior. Research
ment, collaboration, and evidence-based treatment: in Developmental Disabilities, 12, 155–180.
Analysis of a team approach for addressing challeng- *March, R. E. & Horner, R. H. (2002). Feasibility and
ing behaviors in young children. Journal of School contributions of functional behavioral assessment in
Psychology, 44, 231–252. schools. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral
Hanley, G. P., Iwata, B. A., & McCord, B. E. (2003). Disorders, 10, 158–170.
Functional analysis of problem behavior: A review. March, R. E., Horner, R. H., Lewis-Palmer, T., Brown, D.,
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 147–185. Crone, D., Todd, A. W., et al. (2000). Functional
Hartwig, L., Tuesday Heathfield, L., & Jenson, W. R. Assessment Checklist: Teachers and Staff (FACTS).
(2004). Standardization of the Functional Assessment Eugene: Educational and Community Supports.
and Intervention Program (FAIP) with children who Matson, J. L., & Minshawi, N. F. (2007). Functional
have externalizing behaviors. School Psychology assessment of challenging behavior: Toward a strategy
Quarterly, 19, 272–287. for applied settings. Research in Developmental
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Amendments Disabilities, 28, 353–361.
of 1997, 20 U.S.C. Section 1400 et seq. (1997). Matson, J. L., & Vollmer, T. (1995). Questions About
Individuals with Disabilities Education Act Amendments Behavioral Function (QABF). Baton Rouge: Disability
of 2004, 11 Stat. 37 U.S.C. Section 1401. (2004). Consultants, LLC.
Iwata, B. A., Dorsey, M. F., Slifer, K. J., Bauman, K. E., & Matson, J. L., Wilkins, J., & Macken, J. (2009). The rela-
Richman, G. S. (1994). Toward a functional analysis tionship of challenging behaviors to severity and
of self-injury. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, symptoms of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of
27, 197–209 (Reprinted from Analysis and Intervention Mental Health Research in Intellectual Disabilities, 2,
in Developmental Disabilities, 2, 3–20, 1982). 29–44.
Iwata, B. A., Vollmer, T. R., Zarcone, J. R., & Rodgers, T. McClintock, K., Hall, S., & Oliver, C. (2003). Risk mark-
A. (1993). Treatment classification and selection based ers associated with challenging behaviours in people
on behavioral function. In R. Van Houten & S. Axelrod with intellectual disabilities: A meta-analytic study.
(Eds.), Behavior analysis and treatment (pp. 102–125). Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47,
New York: Plenum. 405–416.
Kahng, S., Iwata, B. A., Fischer, S. M., Page, T. J., McIntosh, K., Borgmeier, C., Anderson, C. M., Horner, R.
Treadwell, K. R. H., Williams, D. E., et al. (1998). H., Rodriguez, B. J., & Tobin, T. J. (2008). Technical
Temporal distributions of problem behavior based on adequacy of the Functional Assessment Checklist:
scatter plot analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Teachers and Staff (FACTS) FBA interview measure.
Analysis, 31, 593–604. Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 10,
Kern, L., Dunlap, G., Clarke, S., & Childs, K. E. (1994). 33–45.
Student-assisted functional assessment interview. *McKenna, M. K. (2006). The role of function-based
Diagnostique, 19, 29–39. academic and behavior supports to improve reading
Kinch, C., Lewis-Palmer, T., Hagan-Burke, S., & Sugai, achievement. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
G. (2001). A comparison of teacher and student func- University of Oregon.
tional behavior assessment interview information from McNeill, S. L., Watson, T. S., Henington, C., & Meeks, C.
low-risk and high-risk classrooms. Education and (2002). The effects of training parents in functional
Treatment of Children, 24, 480–494. behavior assessment on problem identification, prob-
Lerman, D. C., Hovanetz, A., Strobel, M., & Tetreault, A. lem analysis, and intervention design. Behavior
(2009). Accuracy of teacher-collected descriptive Modification, 26, 499–515.
analysis data: A comparison of narrative and struc- Miltenberger, R. G. (2001). Behavior Modification: prin-
tured recording formats. Journal of Behavioral ciples and procedures (2nd ed.). Belmont: Wadsworth/
Education, 18, 157–172. Thomson Learning.
Lerman, D. C., & Iwata, B. A. (1993). Descriptive and Mudford, O. C., Arnold-Saritepe, A. M., Phillips, K. J.,
experimental analyses of variables maintaining self- Locke, J. M., Ho, I. C. S., & Taylor, S. A. (2008).
124 A.M. Kozlowski and J.L. Matson

Challenging behaviors. In J. L. Matson (Ed.), Clinical Rojahn, J., Schroeder, S. R., & Hoch, T. A. (2008). Self-
assessment and intervention for autism spectrum dis- injurious behavior in intellectual disabilities.
orders (pp. 267–297). London: Elsevier. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
Mueller, M. M., & Kafka, C. (2006). Assessment and Rojahn, J., Whittaker, K., Hoch, T., & González, M. L.
treatment of object mouthing in a public school class- (2007). Assessment of self-injurious and aggressive
room. Behavioral Interventions, 21, 137–154. behavior. In L. Glidden & J. L. Matson (Eds.),
Murdock, S. G., O’Neill, R. E., & Cunningham, E. (2005). International review of research in mental retardation
A comparison of results and acceptability of functional (Handbook of assessment in persons with intellectual
behavioral assessment procedures with a group of mid- disability, Vol. 34, pp. 281–319). Amsterdam: Elsevier.
dle school students with emotional/behavioral disorders *Salentine, S. P. (2003, May). The impact of “contextual fit”
(E/BD). Journal of Behavioral Education, 14, 5–18. on the selection of behavior support plan procedures. In
Murphy, O., Healy, O., & Leader, G. (2009). Risk factors R. H. Horner (Chair), Moving from functional assess-
for challenging behaviors among 157 children with ment to the design of behavior support. Symposium
autism spectrum disorder in Ireland. Research in conducted at the conference of the International
Autism Spectrum Disorders, 3, 474–482. Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco.
Neidert, P. L., Dozier, C. L., Iwata, B. A., & Hafen, M. *Schindler, H. R. & Horner, R. H. (2005). Generalized
(2010). Behavior analysis in intellectual and develop- reduction of problem behavior of young children with
mental disabilities. Psycholgoical Services, 7, autism: Building transsituational interventions.
103–113. American Joumal of Mental Retardation, 110, 36–47.
Newcomer, L. L., & Lewis, T. J. (2004). Functional Sturmey, P. (1994). Assessing the functions of aberrant
behavioral assessment: An investigation of assessment behaviors: A review of psychotropic instruments.
reliability and effectiveness of function-based inter- Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24,
ventions. Journal of Emotional and Behavioral 293–304.
Disorders, 12, 168–181. Symons, F. J., McDonald, L. M., & Wehby, J. H. (1998).
O’Neill, R. E., Horner, R. H., Albin, R. W., Sprague, J. R., Functional assessment and teacher collected data.
Storey, K., & Newton, J. S. (1997). Functional assess- Education and Treatment of Children, 21, 135–159.
ment and program development for problem behavior: Tarbox, J., Wilke, A. E., Najdowski, A. C., Findel-Pyles,
A practical handbook. Pacific Grove: Brooks/Cole. R. S., Balasanyan, S., Caveney, A. C., et al. (2009).
Pfadt, A., & Wheeler, D. J. (1995). Using statistical pro- Comparing indirect, descriptive, and experimental
cess control to make data-based clinical decisions. functional assessments of challenging behavior in
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 28, 349–370. children with autism. Journal of Developmental and
Poppes, P., van der Putten, A. J. J., & Vlaskamp, C. (2010). Physical Disabilities, 21, 493–514.
Frequency and severity of challenging behaviour in Touchette, P. E., MacDonald, R. F., & Langer, S. N.
people with profound intellectual and multiple dis- (1985). A scatter plot for identifying stimulus control
abilities. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 31, of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
1269–1275. Analysis, 18, 343–351.
*Preciado, J. A. (2006). Using a function-based approach University of Utah, Utah State University, & Utah State
to decrease problem behavior and increase reading Department of Education. (1999). Functional
academic engagement for Latino English language Assessment and Intervention Program. Longmont:
learners. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University Sopris West.
of Oregon. Vollmer, T. R., Marcus, B. A., Ringdahl, J. E., & Roane,
Reed, H., Thomas, E., Sprague, J. R., & Horner, R. H. H. S. (1995). Progressing from brief assessments to
(1997). The Student Guided Functional Assessment extended experimental analyses in the evaluation of
Interview: An analysis student and teacher agreement. aberrant behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior
Journal of Behavioral Education, 7, 33–49. Analysis, 28, 561–576.

Potrebbero piacerti anche