Sei sulla pagina 1di 5

Personality and Individual Differences 108 (2017) 45–49

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Personality and Individual Differences

journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid

The social contagion of incremental and entity trait beliefs


Edward Burkley a,⁎, Jessica Curtis b, Thomas Hatvany a
a
Oklahoma State University, United States
b
Arkansas State University, United States

a r t i c l e i n f o a b s t r a c t

Article history: This study examined if people's own beliefs regarding the malleability of traits is influenced by the beliefs of sur-
Received 30 July 2016 rounding others. Consistent with the idea of social contagion, people who read a vignette of someone espousing
Received in revised form 28 November 2016 an incremental view (i.e., perceive traits as malleable) were more likely to endorse an incremental view them-
Accepted 29 November 2016
selves than those who read a vignette of someone with an entity view (i.e., perceive traits as fixed). Results indi-
Available online xxxx
cated this contagion effect is not domain specific and can spread from one skill domain (e.g., athletics) to another
Keywords:
(e.g., mathematics). Furthermore, others who espoused an incremental view were perceived to be more inspiring
Implicit theories and therefore more likely to serve as positive role models than those who espoused an entity view. Overall, these
Incremental beliefs results provide a bridge between the largely disparate literature areas of implicit trait beliefs, social contagion,
Entity beliefs and role models and indicate one potential source for the origination of these trait beliefs.
Social contagion © 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
Role models

1. Introduction people to adopt this belief for themselves. We also sought to examine
the boundaries of this effect by examining if the contagion process is do-
Imagine you are reading an interview with a highly successful ath- main specific. That is, does exposure to a person who purports a belief in
lete. When asked what his skills were like as a child, he states that he one domain (e.g., athletics) impact a person's own beliefs in a different
had a natural aptitude for sports and that he thinks athletic skills are domain (e.g., mathematics)? Lastly, we sought to bridge the trait belief
something you have to be born with. Now instead imagine reading and role model literature by examining whether people who espouse a
the same article but the interviewee states that as a child, he was not particular belief are more inspiring and therefore more likely to serve as
very skilled at sports, that he had to work hard to improve his abilities, a role model. Prior research indicates that successful others are more
and that he thinks you can change and improve your athletic skills over likely to serve as role models if people view their success as attainable
time. How would reading these articles impact your own beliefs about (Lockwood & Kunda, 1997). Because an incremental perspective frames
whether traits are fixed or malleable? How inspired would you feel the success as more attainable than an entity perspective, we examined
after reading one article versus the other? if people would be more inspired when exposed to a person espousing
These examples demonstrate the difference between entity and in- an incremental belief.
cremental trait beliefs (Dweck, 1999). The former represents an entity
perspective – the belief that traits are fixed at birth and do not change 1.1. Incremental versus entity trait beliefs
much over time. Conversely, the latter represents an incremental per-
spective – the belief that traits are malleable and improvable over the According to Dweck's (1999) implicit personality theories model,
lifetime. A wealth of research has examined the consequences of these people differ in their lay beliefs about the malleability of personal attri-
beliefs, finding they have a strong impact on motivation (Dweck, butes. Entity people believe they are either born with abilities or not and
1999; Dweck & Leggett, 1988). However, less is known regarding the that no amount of work will enable them to improve. Instead, incre-
causes or origins of these beliefs. mental people believe their abilities are developed through education
The present work examines a unique potential source of people's and practice and that they can always improve. Differences in these
trait beliefs: the views of others. Consistent with social contagion theory trait beliefs are important because they have been shown to strongly in-
(Le Bon, 1903; Levy & Nail, 1993), we examined if trait beliefs are “con- fluence behavior (for reviews, see Dweck, 1999; Dweck & Leggett,
tagious” in that they may spread from one person to another. Thus, we 1988). Because entity individuals believe their traits are unchanging,
tested if exposure to a person who purports a particular belief leads they view failure as an indication of low ability. This attribution pro-
duces negative emotions and leads to task avoidance, as well as reduced
⁎ Corresponding author at: 116 North Murray, Oklahoma State University, Stillwater,
expectations, effort, persistence, and performance. Conversely, incre-
OK 74048, United States. mental individuals view failure as an indication of lack of effort. This at-
E-mail address: ed.burkley@okstate.edu (E. Burkley). tribution produces feelings of optimism, leads to attempts at self-

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2016.11.063
0191-8869/© 2016 Published by Elsevier Ltd.
46 E. Burkley et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 108 (2017) 45–49

improvement, and results in sustained or enhanced expectations, effort, they had learned in the activity. The results indicated a chain of social
persistence and performance. For these reasons, people who hold an in- contagion. First-generation learners who thought their teacher was in-
cremental perspective are typically more successful than people who trinsically motivated demonstrated greater intrinsic motivation them-
hold an entity perspective (Dweck, 1999; Dweck & Leggett, 1988). For selves. Furthermore, the second-generation learners who were taught
example, Blackwell, Trzesniewski, and Dweck (2007) found that junior by first-generation learners who thought their teacher was intrinsically
high students who believed intelligence was malleable showed an up- motivated also demonstrated greater intrinsic motivation.
ward trajectory in math grades over two years, whereas those who be- To date, no studies have examined if trait beliefs spread from person
lieved intelligence was fixed showed a flat trajectory. to the person in a similar manner. We sought to address this gap in the
Given the many benefits of an incremental perspective, researchers literature by examining if people exposed to a person who purports
tried to examine ways to systematically alter these implicit beliefs. having a particular belief are more likely to adopt this belief for
Such inductions of trait beliefs not only allow researchers to examine themselves.
causal relationships, they also give clues as to the potential origins of
such beliefs. The earliest study that attempted to alter trait beliefs did 1.3. Role models
so via feedback in the form of praise (Mueller & Dweck, 1998). Results
indicated that children who were praised in an entity way (e.g., “You Because we believed that people who attributed their success to in-
must be smart at these problems”) were more likely to adopt an entity cremental reasons would encourage others to adopt this incremental
perspective of intelligence, whereas those who were praised in an incre- belief, we also expected such people would be more likely to be viewed
mental way (e.g., “You must have worked hard at these problems”) as a role model compared to those who attributed their success to entity
were more likely to adopt an incremental perspective. However, the reasons. This prediction was based on the role models literature that
most common technique used to alter trait beliefs in the literature is demonstrates how successful people have the ability to be both inspir-
through the explicit presentation of scientific data that supposedly ing and deflating (e.g., Lockwood & Kunda, 1997; Mussweiler, 2003;
demonstrates how traits are either fixed or malleable (e.g., Aronson, Suls, Martin, & Wheeler, 2002). According to Lockwood and Kunda
Fried, & Good, 2002; Burkley et al., 2014; Chiu, Hong, & Dweck, 1997; (1997), the primary factor that determines which outcome will occur
Heslin, Latham, & VandeWalle, 2005; Levy, Stroessner, & Dweck, is the perceived attainability of the upward comparison's success.
1998). For example, Levy et al. (1998) presented participants with a fic- In support of this assertion, research demonstrates that factors
titious scientific article that reported on case studies, intervention pro- which promote the belief that one could obtain the same outcome as
grams and longitudinal data that suggested personality was either the successful other tend to increase the likelihood that the successful
fixed (titled “Personality, Like Plaster, Is Pretty Stable Over Time”) or other will serve as an inspiring role models (Lockwood & Kunda,
malleable (titled “Personality is Changeable and Can Be Developed”). 1997; Suls et al., 2002). One factor known to impact perceived attain-
Results indicated that people who read the fixed article adopted an en- ability is having enough time to achieve the success. For example,
tity perspective whereas those who read the malleable article adopted Lockwood and Kunda (1997) found that first-year college students
an incremental perspective. A similar approach has been used to alter were inspired by a successful graduating senior, whereas fourth-year
beliefs about intelligence (Aronson et al., 2002), personality traits students were deflated. This pattern presumably occurred because the
(Chiu et al., 1997), general abilities (Heslin et al., 2005), math abilities first-year students perceived the other's success as attainable because
(Burns & Isbell, 2007), and natural beauty (Burkley et al., 2014). they still had enough time to achieve it whereas the fourth-year stu-
Although praise and scientific data may reflect two factors that influ- dents did not. More relevant to the current discussion, a second factor
ence people's trait beliefs, we believe another possible source—one that that impacts perceived attainability is the participant's trait beliefs. Peo-
is more indirect but just as influential—is the perspective of others. Spe- ple with an incremental view of intelligence are more inspired by a suc-
cifically, we assert that trait beliefs are likely to spread from one person cessful other than those with an entity view (Hoyt, Burnette, & Innella,
to another, such that exposure to another's beliefs can influence your 2012; Lockwood & Kunda, 1997).
own. This assertion is consistent with the notion of social contagion. In addition to the participant's trait beliefs, we assert that another
factor that may convey attainability is the successful other's trait beliefs.
1.2. Social contagion When successful others espouse an incremental belief, they encourage
the perception that others can also attain their same level of success if
Social contagion refers to the spreading of thoughts, emotions, and they work hard enough. As such, we predicted that successful others
behaviors from one person (“initiator”) to another (“recipient”; Levy & who espouse an incremental belief would be more inspiring role models
Nail, 1993). Unlike other forms of social influence that involve a clear in- than those who espouse an entity belief.
tention on the part of the initiator to impact the recipient (e.g., persua-
sion, obedience), social contagion is thought to occur in a more indirect 1.4. Present study
manner (Levy & Nail, 1993). Historically, social contagion was examined
primarily in terms of behaviors (e.g., aggression; Bandura, 1973; Le Bon, The goal of the present study was twofold. First, we sought to exam-
1903; Wheeler & Caggiula, 1966) and emotions (Le Bon, 1903; Sullins, ine if trait beliefs are “contagious” in that they spread from one person
1991). to another. Based on prior evidence of social contagion for other types
More recently, researchers have examined how thoughts also have a of thought, we predicted that people exposed to a person who espouses
tendency to spread from person to person (Lynch, 1996). The conta- an incremental belief would be more likely to adopt this belief for them-
gious nature of thoughts (sometimes referred to as memes; Dawkins, selves than people exposed to a person who espouses an entity belief.
1976) has been demonstrated for a wide range of cognitive processes, Second, we sought to test the boundaries of this effect by examining
including beliefs, motivations, memories, and goals (e.g., Aarts, whether the contagion is domain specific. Specifically, we examined if
Gollwitzer, & Hassin, 2004; Lynch, 1996; Radel, Sarrazin, Legrain, & exposure to a person who purports a belief in one domain (e.g., athlet-
Wild, 2010; Roediger, Meade, & Bergman, 2001; Wild & Enzle, 2002). ics) would impact a person's own beliefs in a different domain (e.g.,
For example, Radel et al. (2010) examined how intrinsic motivation mathematics). Because social contagion effects tend to be indirect and
can spread from teacher to student. In their study, students learned far reaching (Wild & Enzle, 2002), we predicted trait beliefs would
that their teacher was either intrinsically or extrinsically motivated to spread from one domain to another. However, there is reason to believe
demonstrate a learning activity. After listening to the lecture, those stu- this may not happen. Prior work indicates that people's own beliefs tend
dents (“first-generation learners”) then served as teachers for another to be domain-specific, such that the same person may hold an entity
student (“second-generation learners”) regarding the information belief about one domain and an incremental belief about another
E. Burkley et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 108 (2017) 45–49 47

(Dweck, Chiu, & Hong, 1995; Dweck, Hong, & Chiu, 1993). Because there successful in this domain, espousing that his skills were something he
is theoretical support both for and against the idea of domain spreading, improved upon with effort and practice.
our study serves as a “critical test” of these two competing possibilities.
To test this first prediction, we chose to focus specifically on partici-
pants' trait beliefs regarding mathematics (Burkley, Parker, Stermer, & 2.2.2. Manipulation check of target's trait beliefs about math
Burkley, 2010; Burns & Isbell, 2007; Rattan, Good, & Dweck, 2012; After reading the interview, participants indicated the extent they
Shively & Ryan, 2013). Focusing on a domain-specific trait belief (rather thought the target in the interview held an entity or incremental view
than a general belief) allowed us to determine if trait beliefs in this par- of math. Recall that our prediction was that trait beliefs espoused by
ticular domain would be equally influenced by a successful other's trait the target in one domain would spread to a new domain; however, an
beliefs regarding the same domain (mathematics) or a different domain alternative explanation is that our manipulation of the target's espousal
(athletics). Furthermore, research has shown that trait beliefs regarding may unintentionally communicate that the target believes all traits are
math have important real-world implications (Burkley et al., 2010; entity- or incremental-based. To ensure this was not the case, we
Burns & Isbell, 2007; Luo, Lee, Ng, & Ong, 2014; Rattan et al., 2012). included these manipulation check items. Specifically, participants
For instance, Burkley et al. (2010) found that women who held an entity answered two questions about the target's beliefs about math (i.e., To
belief about math were more likely to disengage from the math domain what extent does Mr. Baron believe that math skills can be improved
following failure whereas those with an incremental belief were not. upon?; To what extent does Mr. Baron believe that math skills can
Thus, seeking out new ways to encourage women to adopt an incre- change over time?). Ratings were made on a 1 (not at all) to 7 (very
mental math belief may be helpful in reducing the gender gap common- much so) scale. The two items (r = 0.76) were then averaged together
ly seen in the STEM sciences. to create a composite, with higher scores indicating a more incremental
A second goal of the present work was to bridge the trait belief and view.
role model literatures by examining if people who espouse a particular
trait belief are more inspiring. Prior research indicates that successful
2.2.3. Participants' trait beliefs about math
others are more likely to serve as role models if observers view their
After some filler items, participants completed a three-item measure
success as attainable (Hoyt et al., 2012; Lockwood & Kunda, 1997). Be-
of their own trait beliefs regarding the math domain (Burkley et al.,
cause an incremental perspective frames the success as more attainable
2010; e.g., You can learn new things, but you can't really change your
than an entity perspective, we predicted that people would be more in-
basic math ability). Ratings were made on a 1 (strongly disagree) to 9
spired when exposed to a person espousing an incremental belief. To
(strongly agree) scale. However, to make comparisons to the target's be-
date, this prediction has not been tested directly. However a study by
lief measure easier, the items were reverse scored such that agreement
Buunk (2006) within the relationship domain found that couples ex-
implied a more incremental view of math. These items (α = .77) were
posed to a successful married couple who attributed their success to
averaged to form a composite score.
hard work experienced more positive affect than those exposed to a
couple who did not attribute their success to hard work. This suggests
that when successful people emphasize their effort in achieving their
2.2.4. Perceived inspiration
successes (i.e., espouse an incremental belief), they are more likely to
Participants also completed a three-item assessment of the extent
serve as positive role models for others. Our study was designed to di-
that they found the target inspiring (e.g., To what extent did you feel in-
rectly test this prediction.
spired by the article?; Would you like to someday be like Mr. Baron?).
Ratings were made on a 1 (not at all) to 7 (very much so) scale. The
2. Method
items (α = .81) were averaged to create a composite, with higher
scores indicating greater inspiration.
2.1. Participants and design

One hundred and sixty-one undergraduate students (120 women; 3. Results


75% White, mean age = 18.99, SD = 1.50) from a large Southwestern
university participated in the study in exchange for course credit. The All data were analyzed using a 2 (domain: athletic vs. mathemat-
study utilized a 2 (domain: athletic vs. mathematic) × 3 (target's belief: ic) × 3 (target's belief: control vs. entity vs. incremental) factorial anal-
control vs. entity vs. incremental) factorial design. ysis of variance (ANOVA).

2.2. Procedure and measures


3.1. Manipulation check of target's trait beliefs about math
2.2.1. Target's trait beliefs manipulation
All participants read an excerpt of a magazine article that supposedly There was a significant main effect of domain, F(1, 155) = 13.61,
depicted an interview with a successful target named Malcolm Baron. p b .001, η2p = 0.08, and a main effect of target's belief, F(2, 155) =
Based on random assignment, participants in the athletic condition 35.36, p b .001, η2p = 0.31. However, as expected, this was qualified
read an article supposedly featured in a sports magazine that featured by a significant interaction, F(2, 155) = 19.98, p b .001, η2p = 0.21. In
an interview with a “successful and highly respected American soccer the athletic domain, there were no significant differences in perceived
player.” Participants in the mathematic condition read an article suppos- trait beliefs across the control, entity and incremental conditions, F(2,
edly featured in a money magazine that featured an interview with a 155) = 1.43, p = .24. Conversely, in the mathematics domain, there
“successful and highly respected American real estate developer.” Par- were significant differences, F(2, 155) = 45.28, p b .001. Participants
ticipants in the control condition read an interview in which the target in the incremental condition perceived the target held a greater incre-
only mentioned his career accomplishments. mental view (M = 6.60, SD = 0.74) than those in the control condition
Additionally, participants in the entity condition read an interview in (M = 2.88, SD = 1.54), p b .001, d = 3.08, who in turn perceived the tar-
which the target mentioned his career accomplishments and indicated get held a greater incremental view than those in the entity condition
he had always been successful in this domain, espousing that his skills (M = 2.12, SD = 1.44), p = .05, d = 0.51. The fact that there were no
were not something that could be changed. Instead, participants in the differences in the athletic conditions confirms that our manipulation
incremental condition read an interview in which the target mentioned specifically altered the target's beliefs about math, rather than uninten-
his career accomplishments and indicated he had not always been tionally communicating the target's beliefs about all traits.
48 E. Burkley et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 108 (2017) 45–49

3.2. Participant's trait beliefs about math

As expected, the only significant effect to emerge was a main effect


of target's belief, F(2, 155) = 3.39, p = .04, η2p = 0.04. Planned contrasts
revealed that regardless of the domain, participants in the incremental
condition held a more incremental view (M = 6.61, SD = 1.30) than
those in the entity condition (M = 5.65, SD = 1.99), t(158) = 2.55,
p = .01, d = 0.57 (Figure 1). Furthermore, participants in the incremen-
tal condition held a marginally more incremental view than those in the
control condition (M = 6.00, SD = 1.89), t(158) = 1.81, p = .07, d =
0.39. However, participants in the entity condition did not significantly
differ from those in the control condition, t(158) = 0.94, p = .39. This
pattern demonstrates evidence of a social contagion effect and the fact
that it emerged in both athletic and mathematic domains (i.e., only a
main effect of target's belief) indicates that, as expected, this contagion
effect is not domain-specific.

Fig. 2. Perceived inspiration of the target as a function of the target's espoused view and
3.3. Perceived inspiration success domain. Higher scores represent greater inspiration. Error bars represent 95%
confidence intervals.
Only the two main effects were significant. First, there was a signif-
icant main effect of domain, F(1, 155) = 10.75, p b .001, η2p = 0.07,
such that participants in the math domain were more inspired by the incremental view were more likely to endorse an incremental view
successful target (M = 3.74, SD = 1.60) than those in the athletic do- themselves compared to those who read a vignette of someone espous-
main (M = 3.20, SD = 1.49). This may have occurred because our col- ing an entity view. Furthermore, our results indicated this contagion ef-
lege student sample likely perceives math successes as more fect does not appear to be domain specific and can spread from one
attainable than professional athletic successes. Second, there was a sig- domain to another. Thus, reading about an athlete espousing an entity
nificant main effect of target's belief, F(2, 155) = 29.24, p b .001, η2p = view of athletic skills led people to adopt an entity view regarding
0.27. Planned contrasts revealed that regardless of the domain, partici- their own math skills. This contagious spreading of trait beliefs helps ex-
pants were more inspired in the incremental condition (M = 4.36, plain why these beliefs are often shared within groups or cultures (Choi,
SD = 1.32) than the entity condition (M = 2.42, SD = 1.37), Nisbett, & Norenzayan, 1999).
t(158) = 2.32, p = .02, d = 1.44 (Figure 2). Participants in the incre- We also found that people who espouse an incremental view are
mental condition were also more inspired than those in the control con- more inspiring and therefore more likely to serve as positive role
dition (M = 3.73, SD = 1.39), t(158) = 6.80, p b .001, d = 0.46, who in models than people who espouse an entity view. This pattern is consis-
turn were more inspired that those in the entity espousing condition, tent with research indicating that the more attainable another's suc-
t(158) = 5.18, p b .001, d = 0.95. Thus, participants were more inspired cesses are perceived to be, the more likely they are to serve as positive
by a successful other who espoused an incremental view than an entity role models (Lockwood & Kunda, 1997).
view.
4.1. Implications, limitations and future directions
4. Discussion
Although a great deal of research has examined the consequences of
Consistent with the concept of social contagion (Levy & Nail, 1993), entity versus incremental beliefs, only a few studies have examined po-
we found that trait beliefs are “contagious” in that they spread from one tential sources or causes of these beliefs (e.g., praise, scientific informa-
person to another. People who read a vignette of someone espousing an tion). Importantly, the present study adds to this work by suggesting
that people in our social environment can have an important impact
on our own beliefs. In everyday life, we are constantly bombarded
with statements regarding others' trait beliefs: Beyoncé espoused an en-
tity perspective of her beauty when she sang, “I woke up like this;” Mi-
chael Jordon espoused an incremental perspective of his athletic skills
when in a Nike commercial he stated, “Maybe I led you to believe that
basketball was a God-given gift and not something I worked for every
single day of my life;” Regardless of which perspective is expressed
and in what domain, what is clear from the present work is that these
statements have sway on our own beliefs. Although the impact of others
is more indirect than the factors shown to impact trait beliefs in prior re-
search, our work suggests they are just as influential. And when it comes
to real world interactions, people's own trait beliefs are probably more
likely to come from indirect, social sources than from the direct presen-
tation of scientific data that has typically been studied in the literature.
Thus, people who want to serve as positive role models and inspire
others, whether they be teachers, coaches, managers or parents, should
emphasize an incremental perspective when communicating to their
audience.
Although the present work provides an important bridge between
Fig. 1. Participants' trait beliefs about math as a function of the target's espoused view.
the largely disparate literature areas of implicit trait beliefs, social conta-
Higher scores represent a more incremental view. Error bars represent 95% confidence gion, and role models, several questions remain. First, this study's sam-
intervals. ple was composed predominantly of women. Future research should
E. Burkley et al. / Personality and Individual Differences 108 (2017) 45–49 49

examine if the gender of the participant has an impact on these re- Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality and development.
Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
sponses. Second, this study only examined the contagion effects that Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. (1988). A social cognitive approach to motivation and person-
occur when observing a successful other's comments. It may be that peo- ality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273.
ple are less likely to adopt another's beliefs if the initiators are instead Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995). Implicit theories and their role in judgments and
reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.
espousing a reason for their failure. Future research should explore Dweck, C. S., Hong, Y., & Chiu, C. (1993). Implicit theories: Individual differences in the
this potential boundary to the contagion effect. Third, it is likely that cer- likelihood and meaning of dispositional inference [Special issue: On inferring person-
tain variables may moderate the likelihood that trait beliefs will spread al dispositions from behavior]. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 644–656.
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype ex-
from one person to another. For example, prior work on other forms of pression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. Journal of Personality and
contagion indicates that people are more likely to adopt another's per- Social Psychology, 78, 708–724.
spective if they feel a sense of connection with the initiator. Factors Goldstein, N. J., & Cialdini, R. B. (2007). The spyglass self: A model of vicarious self-percep-
tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 402–417.
known to increase this sense of connection include shared similarities,
Heslin, P. A., Latham, G. P., & VandeWalle, D. M. (2005). The effect of implicit person the-
an overlap between the self and other, and actively taking the other ory on performance appraisals. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90, 842–856.
person's perspective (Galinsky & Moskowitz, 2000; Goldstein & Hoyt, C. L., Burnette, J. L., & Innella, A. N. (2012). I can do that: The impact of implicit the-
Cialdini, 2007). Future research should explore if these and other factors ories on leadership role model effectiveness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
38, 257–268.
increase or decrease the likelihood that trait beliefs will spread from Le Bon, G. (1903). The crowd: A study of the popular mind (4th imprinting). London: T. Fish-
person to person. Finally, it is possible that there are other unexplored er Unwin (Originally published: Psychologie des foules, 1895).
potential consequences that occur from this contagion effect. For in- Levy, D. A., & Nail, P. R. (1993). Contagion: A theoretical and empirical review and recon-
ceptualization. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 119, 233–284.
stance, it may be that reading about someone with an incremental per- Levy, S., Stroessner, S., & Dweck, C. (1998). Stereotype formation and endorsement: The
spective not only inspires people but actually facilitates them to role of implicit theories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1421–1436.
perform better on a relevant task. Future research should explore this Lockwood, P., & Kunda, Z. (1997). Superstars and me: Predicting the impact of role
models on the self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 91–103.
and other potential implications from the present work. Luo, W., Lee, K., Ng, P. T., & Ong, J. X. W. (2014). Incremental beliefs of ability, achievement
emotions and learning of Singapore students. Educational Psychology, 34, 619–634.
References Lynch, A. (1996). Thought contagion: How belief spreads through society. New York: Basic
Books.
Aarts, H., Gollwitzer, P. M., & Hassin, R. R. (2004). Goal contagion: Perceiving is for pursu- Mueller, C. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1998). Intelligence praise can undermine motivation and
ing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 23–37. performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 33–52.
Aronson, J., Fried, C. B., & Good, C. (2002). Reducing the effects of stereotype threat on Af- Mussweiler, T. (2003). Comparison processes in social judgment: Mechanisms and conse-
rican American college students by shaping theories of intelligence. Journal of quences. Psychological Review, 110, 472–489.
Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 113–125. Radel, R., Sarrazin, P., Legrain, P., & Wild, T. C. (2010). Social contagion of motivation be-
Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice- tween teacher and student: Analyzing underlying processes. Journal of Educational
Hall. Psychology, 102, 577–587.
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence Rattan, A., Good, C., & Dweck, C. S. (2012). “It's ok—Not everyone can be good at math”:
predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an in- Instructors with an entity theory comfort (and demotivate) students. Journal of
tervention. Child Development, 78, 246–263. Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 731–737.
Burkley, M., Burkley, E., Stermer, P. S., Andrade, A., Bell, A. C., & Curtis, J. (2014). The ugly Roediger, H. L., Meade, M. L., & Bergman, E. T. (2001). Social contagion of memory.
duckling effect: Examining fixed versus malleable beliefs about beauty. Social Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 365–371.
Cognition, 32, 466–483. Shively, R. L., & Ryan, C. S. (2013). Longitudinal changes in college math students' implicit
Burkley, M., Parker, J., Stermer, P. S., & Burkley, E. (2010). Trait beliefs that make women theories of intelligence. Social Psychology of Education, 16, 241–256.
vulnerable to math disengagement. Personality and Individual Differences, 48, Sullins, E. S. (1991). Emotional contagion revisited: Effects of social comparison and ex-
234–238. pressive style on mood convergence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 17,
Burns, K. C., & Isbell, L. M. (2007). Promoting malleability is not one size fits all: Priming 166–174.
implicit theories of intelligence as a function of self-theories. Self and Identity, 6, Suls, J., Martin, R., & Wheeler, L. (2002). Social comparison: Why, with whom, and with
51–63. what effect? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 159–163.
Buunk, A. (2006). Responses to a happily married other: The role of relationship satisfac- Wheeler, L., & Caggiula, A. R. (1966). The contagion of aggression. Journal of Experimental
tion and social comparison orientation. Personal Relationships, 13, 397–409. Social Psychology, 2, 1–10.
Chiu, C., Hong, Y., & Dweck, C. S. (1997). Lay dispositionism and implicit theories of per- Wild, T. C., & Enzle, M. E. (2002). Social contagion of motivational orientations. In E. L.
sonality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 19–30. Deci, & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination research (pp. 141–157).
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Varia- Rochester, NY: University of Rochester Press.
tion and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 47–63.
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Potrebbero piacerti anche