Sei sulla pagina 1di 6

Cerebellum (2008) 7:589–594

DOI 10.1007/s12311-008-0074-4

Cerebellum: Connections and Functions


Mitchell Glickstein & Karl Doron

Published online: 11 November 2008


# Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2008

Abstract In addition to its role in motor control, reflex lesions. Fifteen years later, Flourens [2] argued that, after
adaptation, and motor learning, three sorts of evidence have such lesions animals can still move, but their coordination
been put forward to support the idea that the cerebellum is impaired. In the following 70 years, there were many
may also be involved in cognition. Patients with cerebellar attempts to characterize more precisely the nature of the
lesions are reported to have deficits in performing one or deficit. In the most extensive of these studies, Luciani [3]
another cognitive task. The cerebellum is often seen to be related the deficits to a fundamental impairment of muscle
activated when normal subjects perform such tasks. There control. According to Luciani’s careful observations and
are connections to and from areas of the prefrontal cortex conclusions, lesions of the cerebellum cause atonia or loss
that may be involved in cognition. In this paper, we review of muscle tone, asthenia, or muscular weakness and astasia,
the anatomical evidence to support the claim. We suggest which, according to Luciani, manifests itself as tremor,
that there are only minor connections with cognitive areas oscillation, and lack of coordination.
of the cerebral cortex and that some of the imaging Although no one denied the obvious motor symptoms
evidence may reflect the cerebellum’s role in the control that are caused by lesions of the cerebellum, there was an
of eye movements rather than cognition. occasional claim that they may also produce cognitive
deficits.
Keywords Cerebellum . Eye movements . Pontine nuclei . André-Thomas [4] dismissed such claims, basing his
DTI . Cerebral peduncle conclusions on a systematic description of clinical symp-
toms and postmortem evidence. He wrote:
“While it is true that in a sufficiently large number of
Introduction
observations the coincidence of intellectual disturbances
with cerebellar lesions has been noted, how many times
From the earliest experimental studies in animals, it was
has a relation of cause and effect been established in a
apparent that cerebellar lesions cause impairment in the
rigorously scientific manner between the two? Those
control of movement. Rolando [1] described the resultant
who have thought that they have found this relation have
profound motor deficit that was caused by cerebellar
not taken into consideration the possibility of the
coexistence of cerebral lesions, or the insufficiency of
M. Glickstein (*)
Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, the examination of the cerebrum.”
University College London,
For normal posture and movements, inputs from the
Gower Street,
London WC1E 6BT, UK vestibular system as well as muscle, tendon, and joint
e-mail: m.glickstein@ucl.ac.uk afferents are required. Sherrington [5] interpreted the
deficits that are caused by cerebellar lesions as being due
K. Doron
to loss of proprioceptive input to the motor system. The
Department of Psychology,
University of California at Santa Barbara, cerebellum, he said, is the head ganglion of the proprio-
Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660, USA ceptive system.
590 Cerebellum (2008) 7:589–594

At the time that these interpretations were put forward, activity in those same structures. Just as functional magnetic
there were few anatomical techniques available for estab- resonance imaging (fMRI) activity may be associated with
lishing fiber connections in the nervous system. Ramon y planned as well as actual movement, so planned eye
Cajal [6] would sometimes attempt to follow the course and movement may be associated with the activation of the
termination of fiber tracts using the Golgi stain, but this same brain structures that control eye movement. Some
method is not really suitable for tracing long pathways. of the inputs from the cerebral cortex to the cerebellum
Such connections were typically studied either by gross that appear to be cognitive in function may be primarily
dissection or by using the degeneration method of Marchi. involved in eye movement control.
But the Marchi technique stains degenerating myelin,
hence, it is strongly biased in favor of the largest diameter
axons. Gross Connections of the Cerebellum

The cerebellum is connected by three prominent paired


Recent Suggestions of a Cognitive Function stalks, the cerebellar peduncles, which link it to the rest of
for the Cerebellum the brain. The inferior peduncle is largely afferent from
the inferior olivary nucleus as well as spinocerebellar and
The idea that the cerebellum may be involved in cognition vestibular systems. Few would argue that the inferior
has reappeared from time to time, and recently seems to peduncle is a link to cognitive structures. The alleged input
have become popular again [7]. Following the suggestions from cognitive areas of the cerebral cortex would reach the
in Leiner’s paper, there are now hundreds of reports which cerebellum by way of a relay in the pontine nuclei. The axons
argue for a cognitive role for the cerebellum. The original of pontine cells project to the cerebellar cortex by way of
syllogism seems to be that the cerebellum, and particularly the middle cerebellar peduncle, which is by far the largest
the cerebellar hemispheres, is particularly large in humans, of the three peduncles in the human brain.
monkeys, and apes; humans, apes, and monkeys are clever, The output from the cerebellum is by way of the axons
so the cerebellum is a likely brain structure for cleverness. of the cerebellar nuclei, most of which travel in the superior
(An alternative syllogism might be that humans, apes, and cerebellar peduncle to the red nucleus and the thalamus.
monkeys are most skillful in the use of their fingers…). The thalamus, in turn, relays that input to the cerebral
Three sorts of evidence have been put forward to support cortex. In this paper, we examine the nature of the input to
the idea of a role for the cerebellum in cognitive functions; the pontine nuclei from the cerebral cortex and the output
neuropsychological deficits in patients with cerebellar targets of the cerebellar nuclei to the cerebral cortex.
lesions, activation of the cerebellum in normal subjects as
they perform a cognitive task, and anatomical connections
showing links to and from the cerebellum of structures in Input to the Cerebellum
the cerebral cortex that are known or thought to be involved
in cognition. Some of this evidence is summarized in a In humans and the higher primates, by far the largest source
paper by Schmahmann [8]. of input to the pontine nuclei is from the cerebral cortex.
In the past hundred years, there have been major advances Two obvious questions can be asked about that input:
in the techniques available for studying connections within which cells in the cerebral cortex project to the pontine
the brain, and this evidence should help to clarify the nuclei and which areas of the cortex project there? The
functions of the cerebellum. We can ask: what are the inputs second questions bears directly on the issue of a possible
to the cerebellum and where does it project? In this paper, we role of the cerebellum in cognition. If an area of the
restrict ourselves largely to the anatomical evidence, al- cerebral cortex that is known to function in cognitive tasks
though we share with André-Thomas a skeptical view of the projects to the pons or receives an input from the cerebellar
alleged clinical evidence for a role for the cerebellum in nuclei, that would be evidence for a role for the cerebellum
cognition and the associated claims based on imaging in such tasks. Both questions, which cells and how they are
techniques. distributed, can be addressed by filling the pontine nuclei
The cerebellum is concerned with the direct ongoing with a retrograde tracer and identifying the location and
regulation of movement, planning of movements, and motor distribution of retrogradely labeled cells in the cerebral
learning. Actual and planned movements may masquerade cortex. The answer to the first question is easy. All input
as cognition. For example, much of what we do is preceded from the cerebral cortex to the pontine nuclei arises from
by an eye movement [9], and the frontal cortex has several layer V pyramidal cells. Pontine-projecting cells often form
regions that are involved in the control of eye movements a continuous sublamina within layer V of the cerebral
[10–12]. What we think we may do may be preceded by cortex [13, 16] (Fig. 1).
Cerebellum (2008) 7:589–594 591

Fig. 1. Distribution of labeled


cells in the cerebral cortex of a
monkey after an injection of a
retrograde tracer (W.G.A.H.R.P.)
into the pontine nuclei. a Plane
of sections illustrated in b. b
Extent of primary injection site
in pontine nuclei. c Location of
retrogradely labeled cells in the
cerebral cortex; each dot repre-
sents 25 cells. d Representative
cross sections of cortex to show
location of labeled cells (from
Glickstein et al. [16])

Corticopontine fibers enter the internal capsule and region of the peduncle. Parietal lobe-originating fibers are
proceed ventrally to join the cerebral peduncle at the base of between these two. In the rat barrel field, two sublaminae of
the midbrain. In rats, the projection from the cerebral cortex layer V can be distinguished. Cells in the superficial
through the cerebral peduncle is spatially ordered [14]. Fibers sublamina Va project to the basal ganglia. Cells in the deeper
arising from cells in the temporal and occipital cortex travel layer Vb project to the pontine nuclei [13]. The cells in layer
in the dorsolateral region of the cerebral peduncle. Fibers Vb appear darker in cytochrome oxidase preparations, and
originating from the frontal cortex travel in the ventromedial unlike Va, Vb receives a direct projection from the thalamus.
592 Cerebellum (2008) 7:589–594

Which Cortical Areas Project to the Pons in Monkeys? In rats, all of the cerebral cortex projects to the pons. The
projection is orderly. It is as if there is a miniature
Layer V cells provides the input to the pontine nuclei in representation of the cerebral cortex in the cerebral peduncle
all mammals that have been studied, but there are major [14]. Because of that arrangement, it is possible to selectively
species differences among mammals in the number of cut the fibers from different cortical areas. Rats were trained
cortical areas that give rise to corticopontine fibers. In rats, to jump across a 16 cm gap between two platforms; a
all of the cerebral cortex projects to the pontine nuclei [15]; distance that they could just manage to reach with their
in monkey, only about half of the cerebral cortex projects to whiskers [21]. If the second platform was 1 cm beyond the
the pontine nuclei [16]. reach of the whiskers, the rat would refuse to jump. We cut
The differential projection from cortical visual areas the fibers within the cerebral peduncle on one side,
provides a clue to the function of the corticopontocerebellar disconnecting the barrel field connection to the pontine
system. In monkeys, there is a dense projection to the pons nuclei, thus leaving the rat with only one set of whiskers
from the dorsal visual stream of extrastriate visual areas, a connected to the pons. When the whiskers connected to the
region in which most of the neurons are motion-sensitive. pons were cut, the rat refused to jump. In contrast, cutting the
There are few or no inputs from areas in the ventral stream whiskers that were disconnected from the pons had no effect
of cortical visual areas whose cells are involved in higher on jumping. When the whiskers connected to the pons were
visual processes, such as face recognition and form allowed to regrow, the rat would jump again. In all cases,
discrimination. Lesions of the dorsal stream of visual areas jumping in the light was unimpaired. The sensory informa-
impair skilled visually guided use of the hand and fingers. tion reaching the cerebellum from the rat barrel cortex plays
Control lesions of the ventral stream visual areas do not an important role for judging distance for jumping. It seems
[17]. likely that other cortical projections play a similar role in
The kind of visual information that is sent to the motor control.
cerebellum in cats is similar to that which is sent in monkeys. In addition to direct sensory control of movement, the
Corticopontine [18] and pontine visual cells of cats [19] are anatomical study of the corticopontine system suggest
sensitive to the direction and velocity of moving targets; they another important role for this system. Ramon y Cajal and
are relatively uninfluenced by the precise shape or orienta- later Ugolini and Kuypers [22] showed that pyramidal tract
tion of those targets. fibers, as they descend through the pons, give off collaterals
that connect to cells in the pontine nuclei. These collaterals
are an obvious candidate to serve as an efference copy of a
Disconnection of Sensory Areas of the Cerebral Cortex given movement.
from the Cerebellum Impairs the Sensory Guidance The densest area of cells projecting to the pontine nuclei
of Movement from the cerebral cortex is from the primary motor and
premotor cortex. There is also a strong projection from area
A patient in Professor Hans Joachim Freund’s group [20] 8, the frontal eye field. There is a much weaker projection
suffered a stroke in the caudal limb of the internal capsule from more rostral prefrontal cortical areas [16]. Some of
without damage either to the cerebral cortex or to cortico- these prefrontal regions are cognitive in function, but some
cortical fiber systems. The lesion led to a profound deficit may be more closely related to the control of eye move-
in visuomotor control, most marked when the patient ments [12].
attempted to use the hand opposite the side of the stroke.
Recently, we used diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) in a set
of 20 normal brains to confirm the origin and course of Afferent Connections, Thalamic targets of the Cerebellar
the fibers in this region of the internal capsule. We Nuclei, on the Thalamic Input to Frontal Eye Fields
replicated the lesion site from the Classen et al. case in
stereotaxic space and demonstrated a subcortical circuit All links from the cerebellum to the cerebral cortex
whose interruptions at the site of the lesion blocked dorsal originate from the cerebellar nuclei and relay to the cerebral
stream visual input to the pontine nuclei and cerebellum. cortex by way of the thalamus. Middleton and Strick [23]
Fibers in this region of the internal capsule receive their injected the prefrontal cortex of monkeys, areas 46 and 9,
input from the dorsal stream of visual areas. In the case of with a virus-based label and showed that there were
this DTI study, we found that these fibers arise from the retrograde transneuronal labeled cells in the ventral dentate
superior parietal lobule. The fibers from this area trace nucleus. Their data are unequivocal in demonstrating that
ventrally and caudally to the lateral edge of the cerebral there is a link between the cerebellar hemispheres, which
peduncle, which is consistent with monkey and human project to the dentate nucleus and prefrontal cortex. We
neuropathology evidence. suggest that this connection may be part of an eye
Cerebellum (2008) 7:589–594 593

movements’ circuit. May et al. [24] injected a retrograde Many of the papers demonstrating cognitive deficits follow-
tracer into the superior colliculus and showed that this area ing cerebellar damage may be due to concomitant damage to
receives a direct projection from the cerebellar dentate other brain structures. Neural activity in the cerebellum
nucleus. See, for example, their Fig. 1 sections K and L and during cognitive tasks may be associated with actual or
Fig. 3 sections H, I, and J. planned eye movements.
An alternative way to study the connections would be to
fill all of the cerebellar deep nuclei with an orthograde
tracer and map the distribution of the orthogradely labeled
References
fibers in the thalamus. This evidence could then be
compared with the pattern of retrograde label in the
1. Rolando L (1968) Saggio sopra la vera struttura del cervello
thalamus nuclei after an injection is made in the frontal dell’uomo e degl’animali e sopra le funzionei del sistema nervosa:
cortex. Sakai et al. [25] injected all three cerebellar nuclei Sassari: 1809. English translation in Clarke E, O’Malley C. The
and traced the fibers to their termination in the thalamus. human brain and spinal cord. California University Press,
Her interest was in comparing the distribution of cerebellar Berkeley and Los Angeles, pp 653–656
2. Flourens P (1968) Recherche experimentales sur les proprietes et
input to the thalamus with that from the basal ganglia, but les fonctions du systeme nerveux dans les animaux vertebres.
her data serve to outline the thalamic targets of the cerebellar Paris: Crevot 1824. English translation in Clark E, O’Malley C.
nuclei. The great majority of the terminations that she saw The human brain and spinal cord. California University Press,
were in the ventral thalamus. There was a small extension Berkeley and Los Angeles
3. Luciani L (1891) Il cereveletto. Successori Le Monniere, Firenze
into the nucleus medialis dorsalis which was restricted to its 4. André-Thomas J (1912) Cerebellar functions. Journal of Nervous
far lateral region, adjacent to the external medullary lamina. and Mental Disease Publishing Company, New York (Nervous
The cortical target of this thalamic area is the frontal eye and Mental Disease Monograph Series No. 12. Translated from
fields. She did not show a projection to the more central the French by W. Conyers Herring)
5. Sherrington C (1906) The integrative action of the nervous
parvocellular region of medialis dorsalis. system. Yale, New Haven
Middleton and Strick injected area 46 in some cases and 6. Ramon y Cajal S (1955) Histologie du Nysteme Nerveux. CSIC,
area 9 in others. Lynch [12] showed that area 46 in the Madrid
monkey and a corresponding cortical area in the human 7. Leiner H, Leiner A, Dow R (1989) Reappraising the cerebellum:
what does the hindbrain contribute to the forebrain? Behav
brain is part of an eye movement controlling area. Neurosci 103:989–1008
If prefrontal cortical areas known to be involved in 8. Schmahmann J (1991) An emerging concept. The cerebellar
cognitive functioning project to the pontine nuclei, then the contribution to higher function. Arch Neurol 48:1178–1187
case might be made that the cerebellum is involved in 9. Land M (2006) Eye movements and the control of actions in
everyday life. Prog Retin Eye Res 25:296–324
cognition. Ramnani and his colleagues [26] used DTI to 10. Moschovakis A et al (2004) Oculomotor areas of the primate
study efferent pathways from the cortex. They found that frontal lobes: a transneuronal transfer of rabies virus and (14C)-2-
nearly half of the cerebral peduncle contains efferent fibers deoxyglucose functional imaging study. J Neurosci 24:5726–5740
from the human prefrontal areas. But the areas that they 11. Rosano C et al (2002) Pursuit and saccadic eye movement
subregions in human frontal eye field: a high-resolution fMRI
studied are known to be involved in eye movement control investigation. Cereb Cortex 12:107–115
as well as cognition. Their study of the efferent pathway 12. Lynch JC, Tian JR (2005) Cortico-cortical networks and cortico-
would not have distinguished between the two functions. In subcortical loops for the higher control of eye movements. Prog
an ongoing study, we divided the prefrontal cortical areas in Brain Res 151:461–501
13. Mercier B, Legg C, Glickstein M (1990) Basal ganglia and
stereotaxic space into six regions. We then used DTI cerebellum receive different somatosensory information in rats.
tractography to confirm that, in humans, the origin of the Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 87:4388–4392
prefrontal projections to pons arise primarily from the 14. Glickstein M, Kralj-Hans I, Legg C, Mercier B, Ramna-Rayan M,
premotor regions and dorsal precentral sulcus. Our findings Vaudano E (1992) The organisation of fibres within the rat basis
pedunculi. Neurosci Lett 135:75–79
to date suggest that the prefrontal areas that project to the 15. Legg C, Mercier B, Glickstein M (1989) Corticopontine projec-
pons are similar to those that are seen in fMRI studies of tion in the rat: the distribution of labelled cortical cells after large
eye movements [11]. injections of horseradish peroxidase in the pontine nuclei. J Comp
Neurol 286:427–441
16. Glickstein M, May J, Mercier B (1985) Corticopontine projection
in the macaque: the distribution of labelled cortical cells after
Conclusion large injections of horseradish peroxidase in the pontine nuclei.
J Comp Neurol 235:343–359
The cerebellum is involved in the regulation of movement 17. Glickstein M, May J, Buchbinder S (1997) Visual control of the
arm, the wrist, and the fingers; Pathways through the brain.
of the limbs, the eyes, and the fingers [27]. Anatomical Neuropsychologia 36:981–1001
evidence at best reveals only a weak connection between the 18. Gibson A, Baker J, Mower G, Glickstein M (1978) Corticopontine
cerebellum and cerebral cortical areas involved in cognition. cells in area 18 of the cat. J Neurophysiol 41:484–495
594 Cerebellum (2008) 7:589–594

19. Baker J, Gibson A, Glickstein M, Stein J (1976) Visual cells in the 24. May P et al (1990) Cerebellotectal pathways in the macaque:
pontine nuclei of the cat. J Physiol 255:415–433 implications for collicular generation of saccades. Neurosci
20. Classen J et al (1995) Subcortical origin of visuo-motor apraxia. 36:305–324
Brain 118:1365–1374 25. Sakai S, Inase M, Tanji J (1996) Comparison of cerebellothalamic
21. Jenkinson E, Glickstein M (2000) Whiskers, barrels, and cortical and pallidothalamic projections in the monkey (Macaca fuscata):
efferent pathways in gap-crossing by rats. J. Neurophysiol a double anterograde labelling study. J Comp Neurol 368:215–
84:1781–1789 228
22. Ugolini G, Kuypers HG (1986) Collaterals of corticospinal and 26. Ramnani N et al (2006) The evolution of prefrontal inputs to the
pyramidal fibres to the pontine grey demonstrated by a new cortico-pontine system: diffusion imaging evidence from macaque
application of the fluorescent fibre labelling technique. Brain Res monkeys and human. Cereb Cortex 16:811–818
365:211–227 27. Glickstein M, Waller J, Baizer J, Brown B, Timmann D
23. Middleton F, Strick P (2001) Cerebellar projections to prefrontal (2005) Cerebellum lesions and finger use. Cerebellum 4:189–
cortex of the primate. J Neurosci 21:700–712 197

Potrebbero piacerti anche